differential conditioning of alpha amplitude: a fresh look at an old phenomenon

11
Differential conditioning of alpha amplitude: A fresh look at an old phenomenon Jillene B. Harris * School of Behavioural Sciences, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia Accepted 9 February 2005 Available online 28 March 2005 Abstract Objective: To determine the latency and development of conditional suppression of alpha amplitude and its relationship to behaviour, alpha amplitude (8–13 Hz) was measured in a differential conditioning procedure. Methods: The CSC/K were tones and the US was a photic checkerboard. Alpha amplitude, CNV, RT and verbal responses were recorded from 12 participants. Results: The CSC/K difference in acquisition was greatest from 250 ms before the US. It was greatest from the trial where RT declined and participants could report the CSC/US relationship. There was an amplitude increase in lower band activity 230 ms after the US. This looked like a VEP but was produced by phase-locked activity starting before the US. Conclusions: Predicting the US led to cortical priming. Amplitude change in acquisition is congruent with CNV, RT and verbal performance. Significance: Prediction, expectancy and motor preparation are reflected in changes in alpha activity. These results provide converging evidence for the functional role of 8–10 Hz activity. They complement the emerging picture of the role of alpha activity in cognition, indicating that it extends to the acquisition of predictive knowledge. q 2005 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Keywords: Alpha desynchronisation; Event-related desynchronisation; Pavlovian conditioning; Associative learning; Differential conditioning; EEG Detecting and responding to predictive signals in our environment is critical for our survival. Predicting the quality of food on the basis of its colour, the presence of predators from the evidence of their scent, and the onset of winter from the fall of leaves, are all cases where learning a predictive signal has contributed to the survival of our ancestors. Less dramatic, though no less important examples occur throughout each day as people learn to predict what will happen next. The past decade has witnessed a significant increase in knowledge about the way that predictive sequences of events are encoded. This increase has been facilitated by technology with PET, fMRI, ERP, MEG and EEG all contributing. The research reported here focused on EEG alpha activity in a human differential conditioning procedure. The study of Pavlovian conditioning (Pavlov, 1927/1960) provided a basis for understanding how predictive learning might take place. By presenting the learner with two consecutive stimuli, a conditional stimulus (CS), which does not elicit a response, and an unconditional stimulus (US), which is response evoking, a conditional response (CR) is elicited to the CS. It is now widely acknowledged that the CR indicates that a predictive relationship has been learned between the CS and US (Dickinson and Shanks, 1989; Mackintosh, 1977; Rescorla, 1988; Turkan, 1989; Wasserman and Miller, 1997). The study of conditional changes in alpha amplitude com- menced soon after the alpha rhythm was first reported (cf. Putney, 1966). In these studies, a photic visual stimulus was used as the US as it blocks occipital alpha, or reduces its amplitude. Conditioning occurs by preceding US onset with Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–1443 www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph 1388-2457/$30.00 q 2005 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2005.02.003 * Address: School of Social Sciences and Liberal Studies, Charles Sturt University, The McDonogh Building, Bathurst, NSW 2795, Australia. Tel.: C61 2 6338 4295; fax: C61 2 6338 4401. E-mail address: [email protected]

Upload: independent

Post on 27-Nov-2023

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Differential conditioning of alpha amplitude: A fresh

look at an old phenomenon

Jillene B. Harris*

School of Behavioural Sciences, University of Newcastle, Callaghan, NSW, Australia

Accepted 9 February 2005

Available online 28 March 2005

Abstract

Objective: To determine the latency and development of conditional suppression of alpha amplitude and its relationship to behaviour, alpha

amplitude (8–13 Hz) was measured in a differential conditioning procedure.

Methods: The CSC/K were tones and the US was a photic checkerboard. Alpha amplitude, CNV, RT and verbal responses were recorded

from 12 participants.

Results: The CSC/K difference in acquisition was greatest from 250 ms before the US. It was greatest from the trial where RT declined and

participants could report the CSC/US relationship. There was an amplitude increase in lower band activity 230 ms after the US. This looked

like a VEP but was produced by phase-locked activity starting before the US.

Conclusions: Predicting the US led to cortical priming. Amplitude change in acquisition is congruent with CNV, RT and verbal performance.

Significance: Prediction, expectancy and motor preparation are reflected in changes in alpha activity. These results provide converging

evidence for the functional role of 8–10 Hz activity. They complement the emerging picture of the role of alpha activity in cognition,

indicating that it extends to the acquisition of predictive knowledge.

q 2005 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Alpha desynchronisation; Event-related desynchronisation; Pavlovian conditioning; Associative learning; Differential conditioning; EEG

Detecting and responding to predictive signals in our

environment is critical for our survival. Predicting the

quality of food on the basis of its colour, the presence of

predators from the evidence of their scent, and the onset of

winter from the fall of leaves, are all cases where learning a

predictive signal has contributed to the survival of our

ancestors. Less dramatic, though no less important

examples occur throughout each day as people learn to

predict what will happen next. The past decade has

witnessed a significant increase in knowledge about the

way that predictive sequences of events are encoded. This

increase has been facilitated by technology with PET, fMRI,

ERP, MEG and EEG all contributing. The research reported

1388-2457/$30.00 q 2005 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology.

doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2005.02.003

* Address: School of Social Sciences and Liberal Studies, Charles Sturt

University, The McDonogh Building, Bathurst, NSW 2795, Australia.

Tel.: C61 2 6338 4295; fax: C61 2 6338 4401.

E-mail address: [email protected]

here focused on EEG alpha activity in a human differential

conditioning procedure.

The study of Pavlovian conditioning (Pavlov,

1927/1960) provided a basis for understanding how

predictive learning might take place. By presenting the

learner with two consecutive stimuli, a conditional stimulus

(CS), which does not elicit a response, and an unconditional

stimulus (US), which is response evoking, a conditional

response (CR) is elicited to the CS. It is now widely

acknowledged that the CR indicates that a predictive

relationship has been learned between the CS and US

(Dickinson and Shanks, 1989; Mackintosh, 1977; Rescorla,

1988; Turkan, 1989; Wasserman and Miller, 1997). The

study of conditional changes in alpha amplitude com-

menced soon after the alpha rhythm was first reported

(cf. Putney, 1966). In these studies, a photic visual stimulus

was used as the US as it blocks occipital alpha, or reduces its

amplitude. Conditioning occurs by preceding US onset with

Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–1443

www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph

Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–14431434

a neutral auditory or tactile CS, after which the CS came to

elicit conditional alpha suppression.

The term ‘event related desynchronisation’ (ERD) has

been used to describe a reduction in alpha amplitude in a test

epoch (e.g. the ISI) when compared to a baseline level (e.g.

pre-stimulus) (Pfurtscheller, 1977). With the increased

capacity to record data, ERD has been demonstrated to

reflect activation of modality specific cortical regions in

response to the onset of an imperative stimulus (S2) in a

warned RT task. In the period preceding a predicted event,

ERD occurs before a motor response (Pfurtscheller and

Aranibar, 1977; Pfurtscheller and Berghold, 1989; Stancak

and Pfurtscheller, 1996). Further, ERD in the lower band

(8–10 Hz) occurs before S2 in a warned category judgement

task (Klimesch et al., 1992).

The latency of ERD preceding an expected event in these

studies is of interest. This latency has been demonstrated to

coincide with both neural and behavioural activity related to

predicting the US in conditioning. In addition, a delay of

250 ms between CS and US is known to be optimal for

producing conditioned associations (Gormenzano and

Kehoe, 1975). Evidence that significant EEG activity occurs

in this period was more recently demonstrated by Miltner

et al. (1999). They employed a differential conditioning task

where the CS was visual and the US was finger shock and

found that in this period there was coherence between the

area representing fingers and the occipital cortex.

Putney (1973) found that during conditioning, the most

robust conditional alpha suppression occurred just after US

onset. However, given the research on upper and lower band

activity, it seems plausible that a decrease would occur

before the US, and further that it should be greatest in the

lower band. One reason that Putney may have not seen a

decrease before the US is that he did not have a suitable

comparison. It is noteworthy that in Putney’s era, quantify-

ing amplitude involved manual measurement of each alpha

wave, and he used a paradigm which employed a single CS.

Employing a second CS which is not paired with a US

(a CSK) may be a more sensitive way to measure temporal

differences in amplitude. It may also yield more information

about the topography of this response. Moreover, demon-

strating that alpha shows conditional desynchronisation may

help to unite the early conditioning literature on alpha with

the current literature on ERD. It will indicate whether the

results reported for upper and lower band activity extend to

conditioning.

The current research employed a differential design with

a CSC and CSK. The idea was to evaluate the temporal

and topographical characteristics of alpha amplitude during

learning. Participants were required to make a button press

response to the onset of the US. They were also interviewed

at the end of the learning session to see if they could

describe the CSC/US relationship. Additional evidence for

learning was therefore provided by verbal report, RT and the

CNV. The latter is well recognised as being a marker of

learning, with increased negativity occurring in preparation

for a stimulus that requires a response (Hultin et al., 1996;

Rockstroh et al., 1982; Walter et al., 1964).

The current literature distinguishes between different

types of alpha in relation to the onset of a relevant stimulus

(Basar et al., 1997). Evoked alpha occurs in response to a

stimulus and is time-locked to it. This is in contrast to

induced alpha, which occurs in response to a stimulus but is

not time-locked. Basar et al. (1997) have also described

alpha which is emitted. This third type of alpha occurs in

anticipation of a stimulus that is missed. The aim of this

study was to find out what happens to the amplitude of

spontaneous alpha when a person learns to anticipate a

stimulus which suppresses the alpha rhythm. Therefore a

priori, it was difficult to say whether the alpha in question

was evoked, induced or emitted. Further, whether the alpha

response occurred following the US or preceding it, was

also an open question.

The research questions were whether there would be

greater alpha suppression to the CSC when compared to the

CSK, and whether comparing CSC/K would afford

greater sensitivity in detecting amplitude change before

the US. Of specific interest was whether CSC suppression

occurred in the final 250 ms before the US, and whether this

activity would be more prominent within the lower alpha

band. Any change in the amplitude difference between

CSC/K that occurred during acquisition was also of

interest. If alpha can provide information about processes

that occur during acquisition, monitoring change during

learning was a critical observation. The final question

concerned the relationship between the onset of alpha

suppression and behavioural learning: Do they occur

together, or does one precede the other?

1. Method

1.1. Participants

Twelve psychology students from the University of

Newcastle participated in the study in return for course

credit. Their ages ranged from 18 to 39 years. Two

participants were male. Two participants were left-handed.

The study was approved by the Hunter Area Research Ethics

Committee and written informed consent was obtained from

all participants.

1.2. Stimuli

The conditional stimuli (CSC and CSK) were 70 dB

tones with frequencies of 600 and 1200 Hz, counterbalanced

across participants. They were of 2 s duration. The only

difference between these two stimuli was what happened in

acquisition. The CSC was always overlapped by the

unconditional stimulus (US), a photic (10 Hz reversal rate)

black and white checkerboard (rectangles 6.5 cm!5 cm).

It lasted for 1 s duration, and occupied the whole

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–1443 1435

(32 cm!23 cm) computer screen. Throughout all sessions

of the experiment, except when the US occurred, a blue

fixation cross (4 cm wide!3.5 cm high) appeared on the

screen. Stimulus presentations were co-ordinated by Micro

Experimental Laboratory (MEL) software (Psychology

Software Tools, Inc., 1995), run on an IBM PC Pen-

tium133 MHz computer using Fujitsu ICL Ergo Pro monitor

with a 16.6 ms/frame refresh rate. Tones were presented at

70 dB via Hi Tex CB18 speakers situated 1 m in front of the

participant. A MEL (Psychology Software Tools, Inc.,

1995) response box recorded RT in millisecond.

1.3. Procedure

Participants were seated in a chair 1 m eye distance from

the computer screen. They were asked to refrain from

caffeine products for 1 h prior to testing. The experiment

took place in a dimly lit, sound attenuated room. There was

an initial 5 min resting session, where participants were

given instructions about keeping their eyes on the fixation

point.

The experiment consisted of three sessions: habituation,

acquisition and extinction, in set order with a short break

between each. Habituation and extinction sessions were

identical. Both consisted of tone only trials, 15 of each CSCand CSK. The only difference between these two sessions

was that habituation occurred before the acquisition session,

whilst extinction occurred after it. There were also 15 trials

of each CSC and CSK in the acquisition (learning) session.

The key difference between acquisition and the other

sessions was that the CSC was overlapped 1 s later by the

US. Offset of both CSC and US occurred simultaneously.

Before the start of acquisition the participants were told

that the next part of the experiment was the learning session.

They were asked to try to predict the occurrence of the US,

and to detect a clue that would make prediction easier. The

participants were asked to make a button press response

whenever they saw the US. They were encouraged to

respond quickly. However, in order to keep the epoch

preceding the onset of the US free from movement artifacts,

they were instructed to wait until the US actually occurred.

In each session, trials occurred in pseudo-random order,

with no more than three consecutive presentations of the

same trial type.

A structured interview followed the acquisition session.

It was designed to find out if the participants were aware of

the CSC/US relationship. The questions were:

Could you tell when a checkerboard was about to occur?

How could you tell?

In this experiment there may have been a signal which

indicated that a checkerboard was about to occur. Can

you tell me anything about this signal?

The interview proceeded until it was clear whether or not

the CSC/US relationship could be reported. Those who

could report it were asked how many times they had seen the

checkerboard before they noticed that it was signalled by

one of the tones. Following the interview, the participants

were led to believe that there would be another session

where they would once again be responding to the US. The

extinction session followed with 15 CSC and CSK trials,

identical to the habituation session.

1.4. EEG recording

EEGs were record from sites: Fz, Cz, C3, C4, Pz, P3, P4,

T5, T6, O1, O2 using tin electrodes mounted in an

electrocap, with impedances maintained at 5 kU or less.

The data was referenced offline to averaged mastoids. EEG

signals were amplified via a 32-channel Grass model 12B

amplifier using a bandpass of 0.01–30 Hz and gains of 5000

for frontal and EOG sites and 20,000 for others. Data was

sampled at 500 Hz using Neuroscan Version 4 software.

Eye movements were monitored using four Ag/AgCl

electrodes. Vertical eye movements were recorded above

and below the left eye, and horizontal movements from the

left and right outer canthus.

After a minimum inter-trial interval of 5, 7 or 10 s trial

onset was delayed until the onset of alpha. O1 and O2 were

monitored for alpha activity by a second IBM PC 486DX66

with an RTI800 A/D interface card using Asyst software.

After amplification and bandpass filtering the EEG was

sampled at 50 Hz. The amplitude and latency of peaks and

troughs in the ongoing EEG were recorded.

There were two criteria for alpha:

1.

Three consecutive peak to trough cycles with a duration

of 230–375 ms.

2.

The amplitude of each peak trough cycle was greater

than 10 mV.

Upon detecting alpha, a digital signal was transmitted to

the computer which generated the stimuli. If there was no

alpha after 15 s, a trial was initiated anyway.

This procedure was similar to that documented by Smith

and Putney (1979), however, in this study it was computer

controlled. In addition, a random delay of 50–150 ms was

interposed between the detection of alpha and the initiation

of a trial, so that the presentation of a stimulus was not time-

locked to the phase of the alpha signal.

1.5. Data processing and analyses

1.5.1. Alpha

The continuous EEG data recorded from the 11 sites was

divided into 4096 ms epochs starting 1000 ms before the

onset of the CS until 1096 ms after the offset of the US. Eye

artifact correction was performed using the Gratton et al.

(1983) procedure. The data were baseline corrected over the

entire epoch and linear ramp functions were removed. It

was fast Fourier transformed (FFT), then a bandpass filter

Fig. 1. Mean alpha amplitude to the CSC and CSK in the habituation

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–14431436

(8–13 Hz) was applied. During FFT both amplitude and

phase information were retained so that the data could be

converted back to the time domain using a zero phase

transformation. This process preserves the temporal resol-

ution of the data. In order to average the data, the absolute

values were computed.

1.5.2. CNV

A 2200 ms window was extracted from the data after eye

correction and removal of trials with other artifacts. This

epoch included a pre-stimulus baseline of 200 ms and the

duration of the CSC/K and US onset. The data was

baseline corrected over a 200 ms pre-stimulus interval, and

filtered to remove 50 Hz noise.

1.5.3. Behaviour

To address the question the of relationship between the

CR as measured by the CSC/K difference in alpha and

behavioural learning, a threshold was set for the latter.

Ideally, the threshold would be the trial on which

participants could report the relationship between the

CSC/US. However, verbal report in this experiment was

a somewhat less reliable index of the onset of behavioural

learning than RT because it was obtained at the completion

of the acquisition phase and could therefore be influenced

by forgetting the exact trial that participants detected the

CSC/US relationship. It could also be influenced by

demand characteristics. For this reason, behavioural learn-

ing was determined by RT and two criteria were chosen:

session.

1. A decrease in RT of 200 ms from the first trial, or,

2.

The trial on which RT fell below 200 ms.

Two criteria were adopted instead of one to allow for

extremely fast or slow responders: some participants

responded quickly from Trial 1 and therefore did not

show a reduction of 200 ms, whereas others never

responded at less than 200 ms, but still displayed a dramatic

decrease in RT, and still reported the CSC/US relationship.

1.5.4. Analyses

Being a fully repeated measures design, Vasey and

Thayer’s (1987) recommendation was followed in order to

overcome violations of sphericity. As there were too few

participants to conduct multivariate analyses, univariate

analyses were used and the Huynh and Feldt (1970) epsilon

correction procedure was employed.

2. Results

2.1. Alpha

2.1.1. Comparison of the CSC and CSK across sessions

In order to determine whether suppression was greater to

the CSC in acquisition, the data for each CS condition was

averaged for each of the three sessions. Figs. 1–3 show the

mean alpha amplitude to the CSC and CSK for the

habituation, acquisition and extinction sessions, respect-

ively. A comparison of Figs. 1–3 suggest that in acquisition

the CSC produced greater alpha suppression than the CSK.

This difference was not evident in habituation or extinction.

During acquisition, the difference between CSC/K com-

menced from approximately 500 ms before the onset of the

US and was sustained until after its offset. The difference

between CSC/K appeared greatest at parietal and occipital

sites and smaller in the frontal region. A comparison of

Figs. 1–3 also suggests that the parietal region had higher

amplitude across all three experimental sessions.

A 3!2!11 repeated measures analysis of variance

(ANOVA) was conducted on an average taken across a

250 ms epoch from K250 to 0 ms before the onset of the

US. The factors were Session (habituation, acquisition,

extinction), CS (CSC, CSK), and Site (Fz, Cz, C3, C4, Pz,

P3, P4, T5, T6, O1, O2). The result of interest was the

interaction between Session and CS, F(1.57, 17.27)Z4.157,

P!0.042, confirming that in acquisition, amplitudes to the

CSC were suppressed in comparison to the CSK. A main

effect for Session, F(2, 22)Z8.232, P!0.002, suggested

amplitudes were lower during acquisition than habituation

Fig. 2. Mean alpha amplitude to the CSC and CSK in the acquisition

session.

Fig. 3. Mean alpha amplitude to the CSC and CSK in the extinction

session.

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–1443 1437

or extinction. There was no main effect for CS, F(1, 11)Z3.78, P!0.08.

A key question in this study was whether there were

differences in alpha amplitude across the scalp, and in

particular during acquisition. There was no three-way

interaction, F(4, 44.05)Z1.65, P!0.180, indicating that

the pattern of greater suppression to the CSC during

acquisition occurred throughout the scalp. However, both

two-way interactions involving Site, Session and Site,

F(7.11, 78.25)Z3.054, P!0.007, and CS and Site,

F(3.46, 38.06)Z3.433, P!0.022, indicated that even

though alpha suppression to the CSC during acquisition

was a distributed effect, the difference between CSC and

CSK was greater in some regions than in others.

One question about the two-way interactions involving

Site is whether they stem from overall differences in alpha

amplitude across the scalp. The data confirmed this idea

with a main effect for Site, F(2.61, 28.68)Z9.296,

P!0.0001. Inspection of the means indicated that within

each region there were no differences between sites. For

example there was no difference between C3, Cz and C4 in

the central region. The main effect occurred because

amplitudes were higher in the parietal region and lower at

Fz. This raised the possibility that the interactions involving

Site were a by-product of this overall power difference.

Inspection of the data indicated that in both of the

interactions involving Site, the region with the highest

amplitude, the parietal region, showed the greatest differ-

ence between CSC and CSK during acquisition. Hence,

regions with greatest alpha power showed effects of greatest

magnitude.

Two other features of these data warrant comment. First,

in both CSC/K conditions and all three phases, there was

an increase in amplitude just before CS onset. This increase

reflected the initiation of trials upon detection of alpha.

Thus, the increase occurred because there was alpha in this

period on the majority of trials. The second feature was a

positive deflection in the CSC condition that occurred in

acquisition, approximately 130 ms after the onset of the US.

The latency and polarity of this deflection suggested that it

was a visual evoked potential (VEP).

2.1.2. Upper and lower band activity

To analyse the upper (11–13 Hz) and lower (8–10 Hz)

frequency bands, three 250 ms epochs were extracted from

the averaged data from the acquisition session. The first was

the period immediately before the US (Pre; K250 to 0 ms),

the second was immediately after it (Post1; 0–250 ms),

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–14431438

and a third, Post2 was from 350 to 600 ms after the US. In

conditioning terms, Pre and Post1 reflect the CR, whereas

Post2 reflects the UR (Putney, 1973). A 2!3!2!11

repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on these data.

The factors were Frequency (upper band (11–13 Hz), and

lower band (8–10 Hz)), Time (Pre, Post1, and Post2), CS

(CSC and CSK), and Site (Fz, Cz, C3, C4, Pz, P3, P4, T5,

T6, O1, and O2).

The main interest in this analysis was whether there were

differences between upper and lower band alpha in relation

to the onset of the US. That is, do the CR and the

unconditional response manifest in a different frequency

range.

Fig. 4 compares the mean CSC/K for upper (11–13 Hz)

and lower (8–10 Hz) band activity for Pre, Post1 and Post2.

It suggests that there was little difference between upper and

lower band suppression in the Pre US epoch, but that the

upper band showed greater suppression than the lower band

at both Post1 and Post2. However, these differences were

not supported by the analysis, as the Frequency!Time!CS interaction, F(1.695, 18.641)Z0.434, P!0.912 was not

significant.

All of the main effects were significant. The effect for

Frequency, F(1, 11)Z5.27, P!0.042, indicated that

8–10 Hz amplitudes were greater. The main effects for

CS, F(1, 11)Z7.98, P!0.017, and Site, F(4.419, 48.604)Z7.56, P!0.0001 supported the previous analysis and

indicated lower amplitudes for the CSC condition, and

higher amplitudes in the parietal region, respectively. The

interaction between CS and Site, F(4.045, 44.494)Z2.86,

PZ0.34, indicated that the difference between CSC and

CSK was greater at some sites than others. As discussed

previously, this was because the parietal region had greater

power and showed differences of greater magnitude than Fz.

The last question in the analysis of upper and lower band

activity was whether there were differences in the

topography of this activity. There was no interaction

between Frequency and Site, indicating that the spatial

Fig. 4. A comparison of upper (11–13 Hz) and lower (8–10 Hz) alpha

activity taken at Pre (K250 to 0 ms) Post1 (0–250 ms) and Post2 (350–

600 ms) epochs. The epochs were calculated from the US onset. The US

only occurred in the CSC condition. The CSK is used for comparison.

distribution was the same for both bands. None of the other

interactions were significant.

One unexpected feature of these results was the increase

in amplitude to the CSC in the lower band in the Post1

epoch. This was interesting because, if anything, the epoch

just after the onset of the US should lead to a greater

decrease in amplitude than in the epoch preceding it.

Inspection of the full 4096 ms epoch for upper and lower

band indicated that it was a result of the VEP-like deflection,

which only occurred in the lower band. By contrast, the

upper band showed a steady decline during US onset. The

latency of the increase in the lower band (i.e. with upper

band activity removed) was approximately 230 ms.

2.1.3. Comparison of the CSC and CSK during acquisition

In order to determine whether the change in the

difference between CSC and CSK could be measured

during acquisition, five 3-Trial averages were taken across

the acquisition session. To reduce the analyses, only the data

from O1 and O2 were considered. As shown in Fig. 5, the

difference in alpha amplitude between the two conditions

increased steadily during acquisition. To confirm this effect,

a trend analysis was conducted using an average taken from

a 50 ms epoch starting 80 ms before the US. It indicated a

linear interaction between CS and Block, F(1, 11)Z11.944,

P!0.005, confirming that the difference in amplitude

between the two conditions developed with repeated

learning trials.

However, Fig. 5 suggests that the difference between

conditions was due to an increase in amplitude to the CSKand not a decrease to the CSC. Rather, alpha amplitude to

the CSC was suppressed right from the start of the learning

session. In order to further clarify this, three trial averages

were also calculated for habituation and extinction sessions.

Fig. 6 compares the change in amplitude between CSC and

CSK that occurred during each session. It indicates that at

the start of each session there was initial alpha suppression

in both CS conditions. There was also a trend of increasing

amplitude with repeated trials in all sessions and in both

CSC/K conditions. The only exception was during

acquisition, where amplitude to the CSC remained

suppressed. Learning, or the CR in this experiment, is

reflected by the lack of an increase in amplitude to the CSCduring acquisition.

Fig. 6 also indicates that in the extinction session, any

suppression to the CSC in the first two blocks of trials was

masked by the initial suppression in both conditions. In

Blocks 3 and 4 there is a small CSC/K difference.

However, the response was fully extinguished by Block 5.

2.2. The CNV

All of the participants showed greater negativity to the

CSC than to the CSK during acquisition. The scalp

distribution of this effect differed somewhat between

participants, being more widespread in some than in others.

Fig. 6. Mean alpha amplitude calculated for each three trials for the

habituation, acquisition and extinction sessions. The figure compares CSC

and CSK using data from a 50 ms epoch taken 80 ms before the US. There

were 15 trials of each CS in each session. The figure shows the average of

sites O1 and O2.

Fig. 7. A comparison of the mean CNV for CSC and CSK during

acquisition for the 11 recording sites.

Fig. 5. The development of conditional alpha suppression during

acquisition. The figure compares the mean of each three trials for CSC

and CSK and shows the average of sites O1 and O2.

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–1443 1439

However, in all cases there was greater negativity to the

CSC at the vertex. Fig. 7 shows the distribution of mean

CNV activity for the CSC and CSK conditions in

acquisition. Greater negativity to the CSC starts approxi-

mately 400 ms after CSC onset, and is greatest from

approximately 600 ms after. The difference between

conditions appears greatest at the vertex and nearby sites,

C3, C4 and Pz. A 2!11 (CS!Site) repeated measures

ANOVA was conducted on the mean encompassing a

400 ms epoch from 600 ms after CS onset until US onset. A

main effect for CS, F(1, 11)Z15.85, P!0.002, verified that

there was greater negativity to the CSC than to the CSK,

confirming that the task produced a CNV. There was also a

main effect for Site, F(7.76, 85.30)Z16.06, P!0.0001.

Inspection of the means indicated that Cz showed greater

negativity than other sites, and that the central, parietal and

frontal sites situated around the vertex were more negative

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–14431440

than temporal and occipital sites. There was also an

interaction between CS and Site, F(16.88, 6.12)Z2.55,

P!0.036, which verified that there were greater CSC/Kdifferences between central region and Pz than other sites.

2.3. Behaviour

In the interview, all participants reported learning the

relationship between the CSC and US in one of the first

three trials. RT fell rapidly, so that by Trial 4 it was below

150 ms. In order to respond at 150 ms or less, responses

would have been initiated before US onset (Posner and

Boies, 1971), indicating that by Trial 4 the participants

could predict the US.

2.4. The relationship between alpha and behaviour

Based on the RT data, the trial on which behavioural

evidence of learning occurred (L), and the trial before

(LK1) and after it (LC1) was extracted from the CSCcondition of the alpha data for each participant. Due to the

randomisation of trials, the trial numbers for CSC and

CSK conditions did not always correspond. For example,

sometimes CSK Trial 3 occurred after CSC Trial 5. In

order to make the comparison consistent, LK1, L and LC1

was determined by the CSC, and the corresponding CSKtrial was that immediately following the CSC trial in

question.

The group averages for CSC/K were calculated for the

LK1, L and LC1 using the averaged data from sites O1 and

O2 and the same 50 ms epoch used in the 3-Trial analysis.

The results indicated that a difference between CSC and

CSK occurred on the trial before learning in the direction of

greater suppression to the CSC. This difference was greater

on Trial L, and larger still on the trial following learning.

Thus differences in alpha between the two conditions

closely resemble the changes in RT. A 3!2 (Trial!CS)

repeated measures MANOVA was conducted on these data.

The Trial!CS interaction, F(2, 22)Z1.281, P!0.298, and

the main effect of Trial, F(2, 22)Z1.660, P!0.213, were

not significant. However, the main effect for CS, F(1, 11)Z11.818, P!0.006 was significant indicating that CSCresulted in lower alpha amplitude than the CSK.

3. Discussion

This study provided a fresh look at alpha suppression in a

conditioning paradigm, with the aim of uniting the earlier

alpha conditioning literature with more recent ideas about

the functional significance of alpha. A main focus of this

experiment was to see if a differential technique provided a

more sensitive way to measure conditional suppression

before the US. Early work by Smith and Putney (1979)

reported a longer latency, occurring after the US. However,

they measured consecutive changes in alpha amplitude to

a single CS across the ISI. Without a CSK, a significant

reduction in amplitude was not easily observed until after

US onset. The comparison of CSC/K adopted here had

efficacy. In acquisition, a clear CSC/K difference in the

last 250 ms before US onset was observed. This temporal

period is well recognised as being important and optimal for

producing conditioned associations (Gormenzano and

Kehoe, 1975; Miltner et al., 1999). In addition, there was

a significant difference between these stimuli in acquisition

when compared to habituation and extinction sessions.

Hence, one advantage of the differential design used here

is that conditional suppression before US onset can be

observed. This makes the results for conditioning consistent

with other research which has shown that decreases in

amplitude before a target event provide an index of

expectancy (e.g. Klimesch et al., 1992).

The main question regarding the topography of con-

ditional suppression was whether it could be localised to a

particular region. Suppression was globally distributed.

However, there were some differences between the different

scalp regions, both in the overall level of alpha amplitude

and in the magnitude of suppression during acquisition.

Overall, the parietal region showed greater power than all

other regions, and the magnitude of conditional suppression

was greatest there. Both overall amplitude and conditional

suppression were smallest at Fz.

The finding that the conditional alpha response (CR) was

greatest in the parietal region invites speculation, because

this region lies over the association cortex. However, there

are several considerations. The most important of these is

the issue of power. The question is whether differences in

the magnitude of conditional suppression are a result of

differences in the overall power between regions. Two

observations are pertinent here. First, participants who had

higher pre-stimulus alpha amplitude also showed greater

unconditional responses to the US. Second, the main effect

for Site indicated that power was greatest in the parietal

region. Together, these results indicate that baseline power

may determine the magnitude of subsequent suppression,

and therefore the magnitude of the CR. This issue has also

been considered by Klimesch et al. (1998). A second

consideration is the paradigm itself. The greater magnitude

of the CR in the posterior scalp could be influenced by the

procedure, which delayed stimulus presentation until

occipital alpha was detected. This wait was imposed to

ensure that there was alpha in the resting record so that

subsequent decreases could be observed. However, the wait

for alpha could potentially bias the results in favour of

greater alpha power at posterior sites, because the CS

occurred at a time when these regions were producing alpha,

whereas anterior regions may not have been simultaneously

producing it. These considerations aside, the most parsimo-

nious interpretation is that the CR is greatest in the parietal

region, a region that lies over the association cortex.

This study also addressed the question of whether

conditional alpha suppression and the unconditional

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–1443 1441

response to the US would manifest at different frequency

ranges. Regarding upper and lower band alpha activity, the

main difference occurred in the interval immediately

following the US (Post1) in the CSC condition, where an

increase in amplitude in the lower band contrasted with the

steadily decreasing amplitude of the upper band. This

difference is related to the VEP, given its polarity and

latency, and because the US was visual. In most other

respects, there was little difference between upper and lower

band alpha. Both displayed greater suppression to the CSCwhen compared to the CSK, and there was little difference

between them in two of the three time periods that were

evaluated (Pre and Post2). They also displayed a similar

spatial distribution. These results seem inconsistent with

Klimesch et al. (1992) research. Instead, these authors

reported that suppression in the upper band is more strictly

localised than the lower band and less active before a target

event. The results from this study could therefore indicate

that, the differences between upper and lower band activity

that occur in warned RT tasks do not occur in conditioning.

However, it seems more plausible that the lack of difference

between frequency bands may have been due to the overlap

of a number of processes all occurring in the period before

the US. Both motor preparation (Pfurtscheller and Berghold,

1989; Pfurtscheller et al., 1996), and preparation for

visual processing (Pfurtscheller et al., 1994), are known to

produce upper band ERD, in modality specific regions. The

current task would engage motor, auditory and visual

systems along with associative processes. As a result, there

may have been suppression at various sub-frequencies and

in distributed modalities, but in sum, they manifested

as suppression across the full 8–13 Hz range in a broad

scalp distribution.

The technique of comparing the CSC/K in considering

upper and lower band activity adopted here differs from the

ERD method that is more typically reported (Pfurtscheller

and Aranibar, 1977). The reason for choosing the current

method was that it provided an optimal way of measuring

conditional amplitude change. Having done so, these results

contribute to the findings of research which has used ERD

and shown decreases in amplitude before a target event (e.g.

Klimesch et al., 1992). In most of this research, the effect of

expectancy has not been assessed with a control for the

effect of the modality of the warning stimulus itself. By

employing a CSK, this study demonstrates that there is

greater alpha suppression in the lead up to an expected

target, even when the effect of the modality of the warning

stimulus is controlled for. It therefore provides converging

evidence for ERD research, and bolsters the finding of

amplitude suppression in expectation of a target event.

A further difference in this study was that the onset of

CSC and CSK stimuli were contingent upon occipital

alpha production within a limited timeframe. This is an

important difference with the potential to bias the results.

For example, as previously mentioned, this may have biased

the topography effects toward greater power and differences

in the posterior scalp. A comparison of both behavioural and

EEG results during alpha present and alpha absent

conditions would provide more reliable results regarding

the functional significance of the alpha oscillations during

conditioning.

A key aim in the current research was to see if

measurement of changes in alpha amplitude were sensitive

enough to monitor differences during the acquisition

session. The goal here was to measure changes in alpha

activity during learning. The data indicated that the

difference in alpha amplitude between CSC and CSKemerged on approximately the fourth trial and increased

with repeated presentations. However, the change between

conditions occurred through an increase in amplitude to the

CSK, while the CSC amplitude remained suppressed.

Examining the data from the habituation and extinction

sessions revealed that the CS initially produces alpha

suppression in all sessions. The most likely interpretation of

this response is orienting, which dishabituates at the start of

each new session. This initial suppression was followed by

an increase in amplitude due to habituation in all conditions

and sessions except for one. Suppression to the CSC in

acquisition did not habituate. This finding suggests that the

CR occurred after several trials in the acquisition session,

and was evident by the lack of an increase in amplitude to

the CSC.

The current experiment therefore demonstrates that the

method is sufficiently sensitive to measure changes in

cortical activation during acquisition. It adds to the growing

knowledge about the neural basis of associative learning

which have investigated neural activity during acquisition

(Armony and Dolan, 2002; Fletcher et al., 2001; Miltner

et al., 1999; Montaldi et al., 1998; Morris et al., 2001;

Ploghaus et al., 2000; Ramnani et al., 2000; Rose et al.,

2001; Sadato et al., 1998).

Converging evidence that alpha suppression in this

experiment was due to predictive learning was provided

by behavioural evidence (RT and verbal report) and by the

CNV which is widely acknowledged as an index of cortical

expectancy (Rockstroh et al., 1982). The group data

indicated greater CNV negativity to the CSC than to the

CSK. Indeed, all of the participants who showed CSC/Kdifferences in alpha amplitude also showed a greater CNV

to the CSC. This invites the question of whether alpha

suppression and the CNV index the same underlying

process.

The differences in the topography of alpha and the CNV

alone suggest that they are underpinned by different

processes. Alpha power and suppression were greatest in

the parietal region, whereas CNV differences were greatest

at the vertex. Evidence from similar studies has also drawn

this conclusion (Grunewald-Zuberbier et al., 1978; Hultin

et al., 1996; Mantanus et al., 1981; Pfurtscheller and

Aranibar, 1977). Only one study, Mantanus et al. (1981),

found a negative correlation between the amplitude of the

early CNV and the percentage of alpha suppression.

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–14431442

Of those studies where alpha suppression and the CNV have

occurred together, fairly simple manipulations of the

paradigm (e.g. removing the motor response) have produced

a dissociation between them (Grunewald-Zuberbier et al.,

1978; Pfurtscheller and Aranibar, 1977). It has been

suggested that the CNV in simple paradigms like the

current one, is most likely a product of motor preparation

(Grunewald- Zuberbier et al., 1978). By contrast, Putney et

al. (1972) have demonstrated that alpha suppression in the

occipital region was attenuated but still significant in the

absence of a motor response to the US. This suggests that

alpha suppression is not solely due to motor preparation.

However, the results reported here did not distinguish

between alphas that are sensitive to particular types of

stimulation. Considering the Mu rhythm which is specific to

motor activity (Kuhlman, 1978), may yield a different result.

Regarding the question of whether alpha is reflecting

significant aspects of associative learning it is useful to

consider these results in relation to those reported in other

frequencies. In learning, the encoding and integration

of associative relationships has also been linked to theta

(4–7 Hz) and gamma (O20 Hz) activity. Regarding theta,

Klimesch (1999) has suggested that synchronisation reflects

the introduction of cortical activation via cortico-hippo-

campal feedback loops, and has demonstrated that task-

related increases in theta power are related to the successful

encoding of new information in episodic memory

(Klimesch, 1999; Klimesch et al., 1994). Klimesch (1999)

has noted that alpha and theta dissociate on tasks of

cognitive performance, with superior cognitive and memory

performance related to an increase in theta power and a

decrease in alpha power. Regarding alpha, Klimesch (1999)

has proposed that widespread desynchronisation at the

scalp, as reflected by a decrease in amplitude in the current

task, indicates decoupling of neural assemblies. He suggests

that this occurs when smaller functional areas are engaging

in Type II synchronisation. This synchronisation occurs in

smaller functional units and is suggested to reflect

information processing. Thus the decrease in amplitude to

the CSC in the current task should coincide with a

simultaneous increase in theta synchronisation, and

Type II alpha synchronisation.

Gamma activity has been implicated in the formation of

associations between event and outcome in both operant and

Pavlovian conditioning. In an operant shaping procedure,

Keil et al. (2001) provided monetary reinforcement for

participants based on their ability to make a response 3 s

after a signal. Performance was compared in three

conditions, in one of these it was contingent on the accuracy

of the timed response. They found that following the

response there was an increase in 20–30 Hz activity in the

left frontal region in the contingent condition. PET studies

have also reported activity in this region, and have

suggested that it is integral in the representation of the

contingency between response and reinforcement (Molchan

et al., 1994). While in a Pavlovian differential conditioning

procedure Miltner et al. (1999) found that greater gamma

coherence occurred between the visual areas receiving

inputs from CSC/K and the region receiving input for

finger shock. This activity occurred in the final 250 ms

before US onset. In conditioning terms this is precisely both

when and where the representation of the association

between CS and US would be expected. The current study

also showed that the difference between CSC/K in alpha

amplitude commenced at approximately the same latency.

The dynamic coherence measures of gamma used by

Miltner et al. (1999) and the measurement of alpha

amplitude reported here are not directly comparable. Even

so, these two results appear to provide converging evidence

of a coupling process in a window well recognised as

optimal for producing CS/US associations.

The question is whether alpha may be reflecting a

specific dimension of stimulus encoding, such as timing of

the US, or whether it reflects the processing of more abstract

features of this relationship? Here Klimesch (1999) has

suggested that alpha and theta activity reflect the integration

of more abstract comparisons between features. The idea

that alpha and theta have a role in top-down processing

related to expectancy and behaviourally relevant stimuli,

has also been proposed by von Stein et al. (2000).

In summary, the current experiment demonstrates that

like S1–S2 paradigms and warned RT tasks, when an event

can be predicted, cortical areas become activated in

preparation (Pfurtscheller and Klimesch, 1992), and further,

that this activity is closely related to behavioural learning.

The finding that changes in alpha closely reflects beha-

vioural performance further exemplifies that the current

method provides a useful index of associative processes.

The current study therefore adds to earlier research reported

by Putney (1973) in three respects. First, it provides an

alternative method to measure the latency of conditional

alpha suppression. Second, it provides information about the

topography of the response. Third, it unites the early

literature on alpha conditioning with the more recent

literature on the functional role of upper and lower band

activity.

References

Armony JL, Dolan RJ. Modulation of spatial attention by fear conditioned

stimuli: an event related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2002;40:

817–26.

Basar E, Schurmann M, Basar-Eroglu C, Karakas S. Alpha oscillations in

brain functioning: an integrative theory. Int J Psychophysiol 1997;26:

5–29.

Dickinson A, Shanks D. Animal conditioning and human causality

judgement. In: Nilsson LG, Archer T, editors. Perspectives on

aversively motivated behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum;

1989. p. 167–91.

Fletcher PC, Anderson JM, Shanks DR, Honey R, Carpenter T, Donovan T,

Papadakis N, Bullmore ET. Responses to human frontal cortex to

surprising events are predicted by formal associative learning theory.

Nat Neurosci 2001;10:1043–8.

J.B. Harris / Clinical Neurophysiology 116 (2005) 1433–1443 1443

Gormenzano I, Kehoe EJ. Classical conditioning: some methodological-

conceptual issues. In: Estes WK, editor. Handbook of learning and

cognitive processes, vol. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1975. p.

143–79.

Gratton G, Coles MGH, Donchin E. A new method for off-line removal of

ocular artifact. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1983;55:468–84.

Grunewald-Zuberbier E, Grunewald G, Rasche A, Netz J. Contingent

negative variation and alpha attenuation responses in children with

different abilities to concentrate. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

1978;44:37–47.

Hultin L, Rossini P, Romani GL, Hogstedt P, Tecchio F, Pizella V.

Neuromagnetic localization of the late component of the negative

contingent variation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1996;98:

435–48.

Huynh H, Feldt LS. Conditions under which the mean square ratios in

repeated measures designs have exact F distributions. J Am Stat Assoc

1970;65:1582–9.

Keil A, Muller MM, Gruber T, Weinbruch C, Elbert T. Human large-scale

oscillatory activity during an operant shaping procedure. Brain Res

Cogn Brain Res 2001;12:397–407.

Klimesch W. EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and

memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res Brain Res Rev

1999;29:169–95.

Klimesch W, Pfurtscheller G, Schimke H. Pre- and post-stimulus processes

in category judgement tasks as measured by event-related desynchro-

nization (ERD). J Psychophysiol 1992;6:185–203.

Klimesch W, Schimke H, Schwaiger J. Episodic and semantic memory: an

analysis in the EEG-theta and alpha band. Electroencephalogr Clin

Neurophysiol 1994;91:428–41.

Klimesch W, Doppelmayr M, Russegger H, Pachinger J, Schwaiger J.

Induced alpha band power changes in the human EEG and attention.

Neurosci Lett 1998;244:73–6.

Kuhlman WN. Functional topography of the human mu rhythm.

Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1978;44:83–93.

Mackintosh NJ. Conditioning as the perception of causal relations. In:

Butts RE, Hintika J, editors. Foundation problems in the special

sciences. Dortrecht: Reidel; 1977. p. 241–50.

Mantanus H, Timsit-Berthier M, Gerono A, von Frenckell R. A correlation

study of contingent negative variation, reaction time and EEG power

spectrum in control and pathological populations. Biol Psychol 1981;

13:227–39.

Miltner WH, Braun C, Arnold M, Witte H, Taub E. Coherence of gamma-

band EEG activity as a basis for associative learning. Nature 1999;397:

434–6.

Molchan SE, Sunderland T, McIntosh AR, Herscovitch P, Schreurs BG. A

functional anatomical study of associative learning in humans. Proc

Natl Acad Sci USA 1994;91:8122–6.

Montaldi D, Mayes AR, Barnes A, Pirie H, Hadley DM, Patterson J,

Wyper DJ. Associative encoding of pictures activates the medial

temporal lobes. Hum Brain Mapp 1998;6:85–104.

Morris JS, Buchel C, Dolan RJ, Wyper DJ. Parallel neural responses in

amygdala subregions and sensory cortex during implicit fear con-

ditioning. Neuroimage 2001;13:1044–52.

Pavlov IP. Conditioned reflexes (GV Anrep Trans). NY: Dover; 1960

(Original work published 1927).

Pfurtscheller G. Graphical display and statistical evaluation of event-related

desynchronization (ERD). Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1977;

43:757–60.

Pfurtscheller G, Aranibar A. Event-related cortical desynchronization

detected by power measurements of scalp EEG. Electroencephalogr

Clin Neurophysiol 1977;42:817–26.

Pfurtscheller G, Berghold A. Patterns of cortical activation during planning

of voluntary movement. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1989;

72:250–8.

Pfurtscheller G, Klimesch W. Event-related synchronisation and desyn-

chronisation of alpha and beta waves in a cognitive task. In: Basar E,

Bullock TH, editors. Induced rhythms in the brain. Boston, MA:

Birkhauser; 1992. p. 117–28.

Pfurtscheller GD, Flotzinger D, Neuper C. Differentiation between finger,

toe and tongue movement in man based on 40 Hz EEG. Electro-

encephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994;90:456–60.

Pfurtscheller G, Stancak Jr A, Neuper Ch. Event-related synchronization

(ERS) in the alpha band—an electrophysiological correlate of cortical

idling: a review. Int J Psychophysiol 1996;24:39–46.

Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Clare S, Gati JS, Rawlins JNP, Mathews PM.

Learning about pain: the neural substrate of the prediction error for

aversive events. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97(16):9281–6.

Posner MI, Boies SJ. Components of attention. Psychol Rev 1971;78:

391–408.

Psychology Software Tools, Inc. Micro experimental laboratory. Pittsburg,

PA: Author; 1995.

Putney RT. A quantitative evaluation of conditioned blocking, enhance-

ment, and inhibition of reinforcement of the alpha rhythm. [Disser-

tation]. Columbia (MO):University of Missouri; 1966. [Dissertation

Abstracts International B27(09)3317:67–930].

Putney RT. Conditioned alpha blocking re-examined with the measurement

of individual wave amplitudes. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

1973;34:485–93.

Putney RT, Erwin TJ, Smith TS. The facilitation of conditioned alpha

blocking with an overt response. Psychon Sci 1972;26:16–18.

Ramnani N, Toni I, Josephs O, Ashburner J, Passingham RE. Learning and

expectation-related changes in the human brain during motor learning.

J Neurophysiol 2000;84(6):3026–35.

Rescorla RA. Pavlovian conditioning: it’s not what you think it is. Am

Psychol 1988;43:151–60.

Rockstroh B, Elbert T, Birbaumer N, Lutzenberger W. Slow brain

potentials and behavior. Baltimore, MD: Urban & Schwartzenberg;

1982.

Rose M, Verleger R, Wascher E. ERP correlates of associative learning.

Psychophysiology 2001;38:440–50.

Sadato N, Nakamura S, Oohashi T, Nishina E, Fuwamoto Y, Waki A,

Yonekura Y. Neural networks for generation and suppression of alpha

rhythm: a PET study. Neuroreport 1998;9:893–7.

Smith ST, Putney RT. Temporal factors in the sensitivity of the alpha

rhythm to habituation and conditioning. Physiol Psychol 1979;7:381–6.

Stancak Jr A, Pfurtscheller G. The effects of handedness and type of

movement on the contralateral preponderance of m-rhythm desynchro-

nisation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1996;99:174–82.

Turkan JS. Classical conditioning: the new hegemony. Behav Brain Sci

1989;12:121–79.

Vasey MW, Thayer JF. The continuing problem of false positives in

repeated measures ANOVA in psychophysiology: a multivariate

solution. Psychophysiology 1987;24:479–86.

von Stein A, Chiang C, Kionig P. Top-down processing mediated by

interareal synchronization. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97:1448–53.

Walter WG, Cooper R, Alderidge VJ, McCallum WC, Winter AL.

Contingent negative variation: an electric sign of sensorimotor

association and expectancy in the human brain. Nature 1964;203:

380–4.

Wasserman EA, Miller RR. What’s elementary about associative learning?

Ann Rev Psychol 1997;48:573–607.