the display of “dominant” nonverbal cues in negotiation ... · the display of “dominant”...

29
© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2011 DOI: 10.1163/157180611X592950 International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 brill.nl/iner e Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: e Role of Culture and Gender Zhaleh Semnani-Azad* and Wendi L. Adair** Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, 200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3G1 (E-mail: [email protected]; [email protected]) Received 23 December 2010; accepted 26 March 2011 Abstract e current study extends prior negotiation research on culture and verbal behavior by investigating the display of nonverbal behaviors associated with dominance by male and female Canadian and Chinese negotiators. We draw from existing literature on culture, gender, communication, and display rules to predict both culture and gender variation in negotiators’ display of three nonverbal behaviors typically associated with dominance: relaxed posture, use of space, and facial display of negative emotion. Partici- pants engaged in a dyadic transactional negotiation simulation which we videotaped and coded for non- verbal expression. Our findings indicated that male Canadian negotiators engaged in more relaxed postures and displayed more negative emotion, while male Chinese negotiators occupied more space at the negotiation table. In addition, use of space and negative emotion partially mediated the relationship between culture and joint gains, as well as satisfaction with negotiation process. We discuss contributions to cross-cultural negotiation literature, implications for cross-cultural negotiation challenges, as well as future studies to address cultural variation in the interpretation of nonverbal cues. Keywords dominant behavior, negotiation, culture, gender, nonverbal communication International business negotiators face a complex array of challenges including cultural differences in communication styles, strategic repertoires, and cognitive schemas. ese differences often lead to a culture clash, due to negotiators either failing to adapt or over-adapting to their partner, which can cause a variety of * ) Zhaleh Semnani-Azad is a Graduate Student in Industrial Organizational Psychology at the University of Waterloo, Canada. Zhaleh’s research interests include cross-cultural negotiation, the role of context and cultural norms in verbal and nonverbal communication, behavioral adaptation, and universality of decision-making biases across cultures. ** ) Wendi Adair is a Professor in Industrial Organizational Psychology at the University of Waterloo, Canada. Her research focuses on negotiation and conflict management in the global marketplace. Dr. Adair’s research on reciprocity in cross-cultural negotiations has received awards from both the Amer- ican Psychological Association and the International Association for Conflict Management.

Upload: nguyenliem

Post on 10-Jun-2018

225 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

© Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2011 DOI: 10.1163/157180611X592950

International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 brill.nl/iner

The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: The Role of Culture and Gender

Zhaleh Semnani-Azad* and Wendi L. Adair**Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo,

200 University Avenue West, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3G1(E-mail: [email protected]; [email protected])

Received 23 December 2010; accepted 26 March 2011

AbstractThe current study extends prior negotiation research on culture and verbal behavior by investigating the display of nonverbal behaviors associated with dominance by male and female Canadian and Chinese negotiators. We draw from existing literature on culture, gender, communication, and display rules to predict both culture and gender variation in negotiators’ display of three nonverbal behaviors typically associated with dominance: relaxed posture, use of space, and facial display of negative emotion. Partici-pants engaged in a dyadic transactional negotiation simulation which we videotaped and coded for non-verbal expression. Our findings indicated that male Canadian negotiators engaged in more relaxed postures and displayed more negative emotion, while male Chinese negotiators occupied more space at the negotiation table. In addition, use of space and negative emotion partially mediated the relationship between culture and joint gains, as well as satisfaction with negotiation process. We discuss contributions to cross-cultural negotiation literature, implications for cross-cultural negotiation challenges, as well as future studies to address cultural variation in the interpretation of nonverbal cues.

Keywordsdominant behavior, negotiation, culture, gender, nonverbal communication

International business negotiators face a complex array of challenges including cultural differences in communication styles, strategic repertoires, and cognitive schemas. These differences often lead to a culture clash, due to negotiators either failing to adapt or over-adapting to their partner, which can cause a variety of

*) Zhaleh Semnani-Azad is a Graduate Student in Industrial Organizational Psychology at the University of Waterloo, Canada. Zhaleh’s research interests include cross-cultural negotiation, the role of context and cultural norms in verbal and nonverbal communication, behavioral adaptation, and universality of decision-making biases across cultures.**) Wendi Adair is a Professor in Industrial Organizational Psychology at the University of Waterloo, Canada. Her research focuses on negotiation and conflict management in the global marketplace. Dr. Adair’s research on reciprocity in cross-cultural negotiations has received awards from both the Amer-ican Psychological Association and the International Association for Conflict Management.

Page 2: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

452 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

misunderstandings, misattributions, and conflict (Adler and Graham 1989; Mor-ris et al. 1998; Tinsley, Taylor, and Adair 2009). Thus, one goal of international negotiation research is to model and understand distinct intracultural negotiation processes so that we can anticipate and manage challenges that occur in intercul-tural negotiations. The current study takes a first step in investigating the chal-lenges that intercultural negotiators may face from nonverbal communication clashes, by examining variation in the display of nonverbal behaviors associated with dominance in dyadic, intracultural Canadian and Chinese negotiations.

Prior research on cross-cultural negotiation processes has focused almost exclu-sively on cultural differences in verbal communication styles (Adair, et al. 2001; Giebels, De Dreu, and Van De Vliert 2000; Taylor and Thomas 2008; Tinsley 2001). Yet understanding cultural variation in negotiators’ nonverbal communi-cation is also important since it is estimated that as much as 80% of communica-tion is conveyed through nonverbal channels (Birdwhistell 1955; Lieberman and Rosenthal 2001; Mehrabian 1981). People use nonverbal cues to express mes-sages that verbal communication cannot (Ting-Toomy 1999). In addition, given that nonverbal behavior is assumed to be unconscious, it is often trusted more than verbal messages, especially when these channels of communication are in conflict (Afifi 2007; Ting-Toomy 1999). Thus, nonverbal communication is an essential part of how negotiators search for information and try to understand the other party’s goals, intentions, and underlying interests (Thompson 2009).

Prior studies have documented cultural variation in both the display rules that influence the nonverbal cues people display and also how people interpret non-verbal behavior (Ekman and Friesen 1975; Matsumoto and Juang 2004; Ting-Toomy 1999). For example, in a study by Graham and Sano (1984), American managers assumed that silence from their Japanese counterparts signaled consent, when it actually did not. Within the negotiation context, silence may be used as a persuasive tactic to elicit concessions or as an indirect way of saying “no” by the Japanese negotiators (Graham 1985; Ueda 1974). This however, is not the case for North American negotiators who tend to be more verbose and uncomfortable with silence (Graham 1985). Cultural differences have also been reported in negotiators’ facial expressions. For example, Russian negotiators tend to start off with grim faces, but later in the interaction they smile and appear more relaxed, which is associated with relationship development (Richmond 1996; Ting-Toomy 1999). Americans in contrast, tend to start negotiation in a friendly manner with open smiles (Ting-Toomy 1999), something a Russian counterpart may interpret as ingenuine.

Cultural variation in the display and interpretation of nonverbal cues is also evident in eye contact. Previous studies illustrate that Japanese managers make less eye contact than American managers (Hawrysh and Zaichkowsky 1990). This has also been illustrated in the negotiation context, where eye contact and facial gazing (gazing at partner’s face) is less frequent amongst Japanese negotia-tors compared to American negotiators (Graham 1985). Western negotiators may

Page 3: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 453

interpret this lack of eye contact as a knowledge deficiency and/or deceit (Argyle and Cook 1976; Collett 1971; McCarthy, Lee, Itakura, and Muir 2006). Further-more, East Asian negotiators may interpret higher levels of eye contact by North American negotiators as a dominating and competitive stance. This in turn may foster a distributive negotiation, lowering potential for joint gains and integrative agreements (Hawrysh and Zaichkowsky 1990).

Our study extends this prior research by investigating cultural variation in nonverbal cues that convey dominance. Dominance is described as an action tendency that stems from emotion and involves asserting power over one’s coun-terpart (Davitz 1969; Kumar 2004). A dominant action tendency is associated with a competitive negotiation approach, which is characterized by a high con-cern for the self and a low concern for one’s partner (Pruitt and Rubin 1986). In addition, feelings of power and negative emotion may both prompt negotiators to display dominance (Butt and Choi 2010; Kumar, 2004). We are interested in how negotiators from different cultures express dominance nonverbally because 1) all negotiations involve some element of competition, which could elicit expression of dominance by negotiators (Lax and Sebenius 1986); 2) verbal expressions of dominance constitute a major threat to integrative negotiation (Brett, Shaprio, and Lytle 1998; Olekalns, Smith, and Walsh 1996), and 3) uncer-tainty, perceived dissimilarity, and cultural distance in cross-cultural negotiations are likely to elicit a negative affect that may prompt expressions of dominance (Kumar 2004).

In the realm of nonverbal communication, behaviors that signify dominance include a relaxed posture, for example sitting back in one’s chair, the use of space, for example spreading one’s work out all over the table, and the expression of negative emotion such as anger (Mehrabian 1972; Remland 2009). Based on existing literature on culture and communication, we develop hypotheses pre-dicting culture and gender variation in the display of these nonverbal cues. Since nonverbal cues such as posture and facial expression can predict behavioral outcomes (Ambady and Rosenthal 1992), we also test whether such nonverbal displays impact negotiation outcome. Our article contributes to the international negotiation literature by illustrating cultural variation in negotiators’ display of nonverbal behaviors that significantly impact negotiation performance and negotiator satisfaction. We discuss implications for negotiation training, manag-ing international negotiations, and future research on the interpretation of non-verbal cues.

Nonverbal Display of Dominance in Negotiation

Negotiation is a process of social interaction by which parties interdependently make decisions about how to distribute resources and/or resolve conflicts (Thomp-son and Hastie 1990). Models of conflict management and negotiation styles

Page 4: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

454 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

typically include a cooperative/competitive dichotomy (Deutsch 1973; Lax and Sebenius 1986), that is used to distinguish five strategic approaches including accommodating, avoiding, competing, compromising, and integrating (Pruitt and Carnevale 1993; Pruitt and Rubin 1986). In the “competing style” (high self-concern and low other-concern), a negotiator is assertive, dominant, and uncoop-erative (Kirkbride, Tang, and Westwood 1991; Morris et al. 1998). This style is typically measured by such verbal strategies and behaviors as demands, threats, and aggression (De Dreu, Weingart, and Kwon 2000; Pruitt 1981). However, negotiation researchers have yet to examine nonverbal behaviors that display dominance or a competitive stance.

In addition to competitive goals, interpersonal processes and emotion may elicit dominant displays by negotiators. Research on negotiation process has found that negotiators are likely to reciprocate distributive tactics and negative emotion (Brett et al. 1998; Van Kleef et al. 2004). Thus, negotiators who face a competitive partner or who experience negative affect may consciously or uncon-sciously reciprocate or mirror their partner’s displays of dominance (Thompson, Nadler, and Kim 1999). Models of emotion in negotiation also note that negative affect resulting from goal conflict or unmet expectations may elicit dominant action tendencies in negotiators (Barry and Oliver 1996; Butt, Choi, and Jaeger 2005; Kumar 1999). Dominance is generally associated with being forceful, assertive, and expressive (Burgoon and Dunbar 2000; Manusov 2005), the non-verbal display of which includes the use of space, relaxed posture, and emotion expressiveness, especially negative emotions (Mehrabian 1974; Remland 2009).

According to Remland (1981), powerful and dominant individuals tend to have more access to space and larger territories. Previous studies illustrate that when people expand themselves by occupying space, they are perceived as domi-nant, and postural expansion is more likely to occur amongst high status individu-als (Argyle 1988; Aries, Gold, and Weigel 1983; Mehrabian 1972; Tiedens and Fragale 2003). Space is thus associated with a visible indication of status and power. Occupying space through body expansion can be achieved by moving one’s limbs (arms and legs) away from oneself (Tiedens and Fragale 2003). Hence, dominance may be expressed by occupying more physical space, by sitting in open body posi-tions, and by using expansive gestures (Manusov and Pattersons 2006).

Interestingly, another indicator of dominance is the ability to be relaxed and poised (Manusov and Patterson 2006). Dominant personality dispositions have been shown to correlate with relaxed behavior, and dominant communicators tend to exhibit a relaxed, yet confident guise (Burgoon et al. 1990; Manusov 2005; Mehrabian 1972). In general, high status is associated with a relaxed, easy-going demeanor because a person who feels powerful is confident and more able to be relaxed in social interactions (Burgoo 1999; Gifford 1994; Manusov and Patterson 2006; Remland 2009). Nonverbal cues associated with the confident side of dominance include relaxed and expansive postures (Manusov and Patter-

Page 5: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 455

son 2006; Mehrabian 1972; Remland 2009). According to Mehrabian (1972), superiors are more likely to lean back in the chair, use an open-arms position, stretch out, and place their arms and legs in relaxed positions (Remland 2009).

A third nonverbal indicator of dominance is the expression of emotion, par-ticularly negative emotion such as anger. Individuals with power and status have a tendency to disregard display rules and so, they may be more visibly expressive than those of lower status (Remland 2009). They may exhibit dominance by yell-ing, frowning, staring angrily, not joining in laughter, and engaging in other emo-tional expressiveness (Remland 2009). A person expressing anger is thought to be dominant, competent, smart, and persuasive (Clark, Parakil, and Carver 1996; Gallois 1993; Labott et al. 1991). Tiedens (2001) likewise reported that the expression of anger led to status conferral and maintenance of status.1 This prior research suggests that the display of negative emotion on one’s face is a nonverbal cue often associated with dominance.

As noted above, all negotiations inherently include some element of competi-tion. Even negotiators with cooperative motives and a concern for the other party typically still want to get a good deal for themselves (Lax and Sebenius 1986). Thus, we expect all negotiators to display some form of dominance by occupying space, using expansive gestures, engaging in relaxed postures, and displaying neg-ative emotions. Yet, we argue that the level and form of dominant nonverbals will vary depending on a negotiator’s culture and gender.

Culture and the Display of Dominant Nonverbal Behaviors in Negotiation

Culture can be defined as the unique nature of a social group with regards to values, norms, practices, and institutions (Lytle, Brett, Barness, Tinsley, and Jans-sens 1995). Culture influences individual behaviors as well as the mental models of how things work (Hofstede 1980). Within the negotiation context, culture has been shown to influence not only negotiation goals (Gelfand and Realo 1999; Tinsley and Pillutla 1998; Triandis et al. 1988), strategies (Adair, Weingart, and Brett 2006; Gelfand and Christakopoulou 1999), and cognitive schemas (Adair, Taylor, and Tinsley 2009; Gelfand and McCusker 2002), but also communica-tion styles (Hall 1976; 1987) and normative behaviors (Ellsworth and Ludwig 1972; Matsumoto and Juang 2004).

1) We recognize that a negotiator’s nonverbal display of dominance may have multiple antecedents, including self-emotions, partner emotions, partner behaviors and strategic intent (Barry and Oliver 1996; Butt, Choi, and Jaeger 2005; Kopelman, Rosette, and Thompson 2006; Kumar 1997); as well as conse-quences, including reciprocal dominance, yielding, and a conflict spiral (Brett, Shpairo, and Lytle 1998; Butt et al. 2005; Van Kleef, De Dreu, and Manstead 2004). However, as the current study examines culture and gender as predictors of negotiators’ nonverbal displays of dominance, we reserve a look at the role of emotion in predicting nonverbal displays for future studies.

Page 6: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

456 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

There are two reasons why we expect cultural differences in the display of dom-inant behaviors. First, prior research suggests that compared to North Americans, East Asians tend to be more cooperative when dealing with in-groups (Morris et al. 1998). Second, North Americans are more likely to vary their posture and display emotion compared to East Asians, who restrain their posture, and mask the display of negative emotion (Kudoh and Matsumoto 1985; Matsumoto and Kupperbusch 2001). While our study will compare samples of Canadian and Chinese negotiators, our literature review will cover research addressing North American and East Asian cultures more generally.

In a conflict setting, East Asians tend to be more cooperative with in-groups compared to North Americans (Wade-Benzoni, Okumura, Brett, Moore, Ten-brunsel, and Bazerman 2002). These cultural differences in conflict management styles have been explained by Hofstede’s (1980) cultural dimension of individual-ism-collectivism (Brett 2000; Oetzel and Ting-Toomey 2003). In individualistic cultures (e.g. Canada), autonomy, independence, and self-assertion is promoted, while in collectivistic cultures (e.g. China), interdependence, social obligation, and relationship harmony is promoted (Butler, Lee, and Gross 2007). According to Triandis (1988, 1989), compared to collectivists, individualists tend to give preference to their individual goals over group goals. Thus, cooperative behavior tends to be stronger amongst collectivists when interacting with an in-group, than individualists. In fact, several studies illustrate that compared to U.S. Amer-icans; Japanese tend to be more cooperative and are likely to adopt an equal allocation distribution strategy when interacting with in-group members (Leung and Bond 1984; Wade-Benzoni et al. 2002).2

Given that for in-group interactions individualists are more self-interested and less cooperative than collectivists, they may employ more dominating conflict strategies (Oetzel and Ting-Toomey 2003; Ting-Toomey and Kurogi 1998). In a study by Ohbuchi et al. (1999), U.S. American students preferred direct confron-tation more than avoidance in a conflict situation. Moreover, dominating styles are positively associated with independence, while avoiding and compromising styles are positively linked with interdependence (Oetzel 1998). So, we expect North American negotiators to engage in more dominant behaviors than East Asian negotiators.

We also expect cultural differences in negotiators’ display of dominant behav-iors since research illustrates that people’s body postures and gestures are influ-enced by their culture (Kleinsmith, De Silva, and Berthouze 2006; Matsumoto and Kudoh 1987). Compared to North Americans, Japanese individuals tend to

2) Please note that in some contexts (e.g. buyer-seller bargaining), Chinese negotiators have a more com-petitive bargaining style than North Americans since sellers are viewed as an out-group (Lee 2000). This bargaining style is, however, not applicable in our study since our negotiation simulation did not involve a buyer and seller. Instead, participants were asked to negotiate about different issues pertaining to open-ing a catering business.

Page 7: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 457

use more restrained gestures in emotional situations. In communicating interper-sonal positiveness (affiliation and liking), Kudoh and Matsumoto (1985) found that compared to Americans, who leaned forward, Japanese participants displayed more restrained postures by straightening their backs. Bond and Shiraishi (1974) also reported that compared to Westerners, the Japanese display far fewer gestures and use more simple postures. Accordingly, we expect North American negotia-tors will take up more physical space and display more relaxed postures than East Asian negotiators, who will exhibit more rigid and reserved postures.

Culture also affects the extent to which people display facial expressions. West-ern European cultural values, such as independence and self-assertion, promote open emotion expression (Butler at al. 2007). East Asian cultural values, such as interdependence and relationship harmony, promote emotion suppression, the active reduction of emotion-expressive behavior during emotional arousal (Gross and Levenson 1993, 1997). Previous studies illustrate that collectivists attenuate negative emotions and mask those emotions with smiles (Gross and John 1998; Matsumoto et al. 1997; Matsumoto and Kupperbusch 2001). In a study examin-ing emotional expression in four cultures, Matsumoto et al. (1998) found that Americans scored significantly lower on controlling their expressions than Kore-ans and Japanese.3 Provided that East Asians are more reserved and mask their negative emotions, we expect that compared to North Americans, East Asians will display fewer negative emotions.

Hypothesis 1: Canadian negotiators will engage in more dominant nonverbal behav-iors than Chinese negotiators. Hence, Canadians will use more physical space (H1a), exhibit more relaxed postures (H1b), and express more negative emotion (H1c) than Chinese negotiators.

Gender Differences in the Display of Dominant Nonverbal Behaviors

Aside from the display of nonverbal cues varying across cultures, nonverbal behaviors also vary across genders (Frances 1979). Gender refers to the psycho-logical and behavioral characteristics cultures have developed based on sex differ-ences (Matsumoto 2004). Gender roles refer to the degree to which a person adopts the gender specific behaviors ascribed by his or her culture (Matsumoto 2004). Based on gender role stereotypes, men are viewed as dominant, aggressive, and extroverted, whereas females are viewed as nurturing, adaptive, and agreeable (Matsumoto 2004).

3) Please note that these studies do not focus on the feelings that elicit emotional expression, but rather on cultural variation in display rules that encourage or inhibit the expression of emotion in general. An examination of the feelings that elicit emotional expression in various cultural contexts is beyond the scope of the current study.

Page 8: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

458 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

The masculinity-femininity domain of cultural variability proposed by Hofstede (1980) captures the extent to which a culture promotes gender roles and fosters differences between males and females. In masculine cultures gender roles are distinct; men should be concerned with ego enhancement by being tough and assertive, while women should strive for ego-effacing by being tender and modest (Hofstede 1980; 1991). In feminine cultures, gender roles overlap; both men and women are encouraged to be modest and oriented toward quality of life (Hofstede 1991). Hence, compared to masculine cultures, feminine cultures promote flex-ible sex-role behaviors.

Some East Asian countries like Japan, China, and Philippines score higher on the masculinity dimension compared to Canada and some Western countries (Hofstede 1980; Matsumoto 2004). In these East Asian countries, females are encouraged to be passive, carry out domestic duties, raise children, and be “good” daughters-in-law. Men are raised to be aloof, unemotional, and authoritative (Sue 1998). In contrast, Western countries such as the Netherlands, Germany, and Finland, have less differentiation between males and females on various psycho-logical characteristics, and gender differences within the country are relatively small (Williams and Best 1990). Given that China has a masculinity index of 66 and Canada has a masculinity index of 52 (Hofstede, Hofstede, and Minkov 2010), we should observe higher gender role differences amongst the Chinese than Canadians.

In summary, because dominant behavior is more characteristic of males than females, and gender role distinctions should be more prevalent in China than Canada, we predict that male negotiators will display more dominant nonverbal behavior than females, and this gender difference will be larger for Chinese nego-tiators than Canadian negotiators.

Hypothesis 2: Male negotiators will display more dominant nonverbal behaviors than female negotiators (H2a) and this difference will be greater for Chinese negotia-tors than for Canadian negotiators (H2b).

Dominant Nonverbal Behaviors and Negotiation Outcome

As previously mentioned, negotiators employing a competing style tend to be more dominant and assertive. These negotiators tend to be highly concerned with their own outcome in a conflict, and are motivated to win or defeat their oppo-nent (Thomas and Kilmann 1974; Walters et al. 1998). Negotiators with this style typically make large demands and use distributive tactics (Walters et al. 1998). These negotiators may assume that their opponent has opposite interests and preferences, blinding them to common interests and potential value creation opportunities (De Dreu et al. 2000; Pinkley, Griffith, and Northcraft 1995). This

Page 9: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 459

in effect may result in fewer integrative agreements and lower joint gains (Carnev-ale, Pruitt, and Seilheimer 1981).

Since individualists tend to engage in direct confrontation of conflict, and employ more dominating conflict tactics than collectivists (Ohbuchi et al. 1999; Ting-Toomey and Kurogi 1998; Oetzel and Ting-Toomey 2003); we expect North American negotiators to display more dominant behaviors, which will consequently lower their joint gains. Also, given that dominance tends to be asso-ciated with males rather than females, and that the promotion of sex-role stereo-types is higher in China than Canada, we expect males to display more dominant behaviors than females, which will in turn explain their lower joint gains. In other words, we predict that dominant nonverbal behavior will mediate the relation-ship between culture, gender, and joint gains such that more dominant nonverbal behaviors will lead to lower joint gains.

Prior research on negotiation illustrates that when power is unbalanced, nego-tiators with more power tend to gain more profits, make fewer concessions, and be more satisfied with the negotiation process and outcomes (Dwyer and Walker 1981; Ganesan 1993; McAlister Bazerman, and Fader 1986; Neslin and Green-halgh 1983). Hence, dominance is positively related to individual outcome (Bottger 1984; Butt et al. 2005; Littlepage, Schmidt, Whisler, and Frost 1995). A study by Van Kleef and colleagues illustrates one possible mechanism in that they found in a computer-mediated negotiation task, participants were more likely to engage in cooperative behavior and concede when faced with an angry opponent (opponent exhibiting dominance) (Van Kleef, et al. 2004). Thus, dominant behaviors should mediate the relationship between culture, gender, and individ-ual gain, where higher displays of dominant nonverbal cues should lead to higher individual gain.

Hypothesis 3: Dominant nonverbal behaviors will mediate the relationship between culture, gender, and negotiation outcome. The relationship with joint gains will be inversely related, such that the higher the dominant behavior, the lower the joint gains (H3a). Dominant nonverbal behaviors will also mediate the relationship between culture, gender, and individual gain and this relationship will be positive. The higher the dominant behavior, the higher the individual gain (H3b).

Considering the prevalence of fixed-pie bias, whereby parties assume negotiations are distributive, many negotiators believe that negotiation is mostly about com-petition (Bazerman and Neale 1983). When these negotiators display dominant behavior that matches their competitive negotiation schema, they should experi-ence satisfaction because they are enacting a consistent negotiation script. Accord-ing to previous research, people like to behave in ways that are consistent with their beliefs (Aronson 1968; Greenwald and Ronis 1978). When people experience inconsistency between their behavior and cognition, they experience cognitive

Page 10: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

460 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

dissonance, in which a person feels tension and discomfort (Festinger 1957). If negotiators believe that negotiation is competitive, they may behave more domi-nantly to be consistent with their cognitions. This in turn may increase their sat-isfaction with the negotiation process. Thus, we predict that dominant behavior will mediate the relationship between culture, gender, and negotiator satisfaction such that dominant behaviors will lead to greater negotiator satisfaction.

Hypothesis 4: Dominant nonverbal behaviors will mediate the relationship between culture, gender, and negotiator satisfaction such that negotiators who display more dominant nonverbal behavior will report more satisfaction with the negotiation process.

Method

Participants

All East Asian participants were Chinese, and all North American participants were Canadian-Caucasians. Eighty-two Chinese (44 females and 38 males) and eighty-four Canadian (42 females and 42 males) undergraduate students from a Canadian University participated in a study on “Decision Making,” in exchange for 1 course participation credit or $10.4 All Chinese students were born in China, Hong Kong, or Taiwan, had been in Canada for less than ten years, and identified primarily with the Chinese culture.5 When asked with what culture they identi-fied most (1 = Chinese, 2 = Canadian), Chinese participants reported strong identification with the Chinese culture (M = 1.05, SD = .23) and Canadian participants reported strong identification with the Canadian culture (M = 1.99, SD = .11).

Design

The study employed at 2 × 2 factorial design with two levels of culture (Cana-dian/ Chinese) and two levels of gender (male/female). Participants engaged in a dyadic intracultural negotiation and were assigned a partner of their own gender.

4) Chinese participants who identified with the Canadian culture (N = 5) were excluded from the data analysis. 5) Although there are some differences in values and practices amongst sub-regions of Greater China (e.g. People’s Republic of China, Hong Kong, and Taiwan) (House, Hanges, Javidan, Dorfman and Gupta 2004), such as propensity to initiate negotiation (Volkema 2011), we expect similar displays of dominant nonverbal behaviors amongst members of these regions, since prior research illustrate that East Asians (from various regions) are more likely to restrain their posture, and mask the display of negative emotion compared to North Americans (Kudoh and Matsumoto 1985).

Page 11: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 461

Procedure

Participants arrived in pairs at the laboratory and were seated at a table across from each other. Participants were presented with confidential role instructions for “At Your Service,” a negotiation case involving a chef and an entrepreneur negotiating about opening a new catering business (Brett and Gelfand 2008). Participants were given 15 minutes to prepare individually, and were informed that their goal was to maximize their own points and reach agreement on all four issues. Participants were not provided with a negotiation deadline; however, dyads that spent more than 30 minutes were instructed to end the negotiation within 5 minutes. Once an agreement was reached, both parties recorded their agreement and calculated their individual and joint gains. Then, participants individually completed surveys measuring satisfaction and demographics.

The negotiation sessions were videotaped without the awareness of participants because knowledge of the video cameras might have affected nonverbal behavior displays. Upon the completion of the study, all participants were thanked, com-pensated, and debriefed about the purpose of the study and were informed of the video recording. Participants were then asked to read and sign a second consent form allowing researchers to examine the video recordings.

Dependent and Mediating Measures: Dominant Behaviors

We tested dominant nonverbal behaviors as both dependent (H1&2) and medi-ating (H3&4) measures. We created these measures by coding videos of the nego-tiation interaction. Seven independent raters of East Asian and North American cultural backgrounds were trained to reliably identify all the behaviors examined in this study. Raters coded two practice sessions, and for each session, inter-rater reliability was assessed using bivariate correlation. The mean alpha was 0.77, indi-cating a good inter-rater reliability (Portney and Watkins 2009). Then, two of the seven raters coded every session, recording and rating the frequency of dominant nonverbal behaviors, and we averaged their ratings.

Raters watched each session three times without volume. The first time they were asked to get an overall impression of the interaction. The second and third time, coders were asked to focus on one participant at a time and examine that negotiator’s nonverbal cues (See Appendix A). For some of the behaviors, raters counted the number of times those behaviors were displayed. Thus, to measure negotiators’ relaxed posture, we summed frequency counts of “leaning sideways” and “leaning back,” where negotiators leaned against their chair in a relaxed man-ner. For other behaviors, coders made judgments on a 5 point rating scale (1 = behavior did not occur; 5 = behavior was displayed very often).6 To capture the

6) Though we recognize that the meaning of nonverbal behavior is often tied to verbal speech (Guerrero 1996), our approach is to objectively measure the display of dominant nonverbal cues without inter-

Page 12: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

462 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

extent to which negotiators used physical space, we averaged coder’s ratings of each negotiator’s “usage of space,” “hands on table,” and “movement of hands.”

For display of negative emotion, we asked coders to rate how often negotiators displayed all of the negative emotions included in the widely used PANAS scale (Watson, Clark, and Tellegen 1988). Prior researchers have categorized negative emotions into self-induced (e.g. shame, regret) and other induced (e.g. anger, outrage) categories (Butt et al. 2005; Weiner 1986). The self-induced negative emotions on the PANAS scale are less easily associated with dominance (e.g. scared, nervous, ashamed, and sad). However, our original coding scheme did not allow us to separate out these negative emotions from the other-induced emo-tions that are more closely associated with dominance. Thus, we later asked cod-ers to recall the overall display of anger, disgust, irritability, and hostility versus all other PANAS negative emotions in all the videos they coded. Overall, coders reported observing more negative emotions communicating dominance (e.g. anger, disgust, irritable, and hostile) (M = 1.8, SD = .4), compared to the other negative emotions (e.g. scared, nervous, ashamed, and sad), (M = 1.5, SD = .4, t (6) = 2.29, p = .06).

Dependent Measures: Joint Gains, Individual Gains, and Negotiator Satisfaction

The objective negotiation outcomes, individual and joint gains, were dependent measures. Individual gain was computed by adding the points a negotiator received for each issue according to the settlement contract. For joint gains, we added the individual gains for both parties in each dyad. Participants’ satisfaction with negotiation was measured using the Subjective Value Inventory (SVI) ques-tionnaire (Curhan, Elfenbein, and Xu 2006). SVI taps into four components of negotiation satisfaction: feelings about the instrumental outcome, feelings about the self, feelings about the process, and feelings about the relationship. We uti-lized the subscale for satisfaction with negotiation process.

Results

Cultural Comparison of Dominant Nonverbal Behaviors

Hypothesis 1 predicted that Canadian negotiators would exhibit more dominant nonverbal behaviors than Chinese negotiators. We conducted univariate general linear model analyses to examine main effects of culture on the dominant nonver-bal behaviors (see Table 1) as well as culture by gender interactions. Since some of

preting the meaning of the behavior. A more subjective approach, where observers make inferences about the intentions of negotiators’ behaviors or negotiators themselves interpret their behaviors is discussed below under future directions (Burgoon and Dunbar 2000; Manusov 2005).

Page 13: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 463

our measures were frequency counts, we controlled for time spent negotiating, because negotiators who spent longer time negotiating would have had more opportunity to display nonverbal cues.

Results indicate that the use of space was higher for the Chinese participants (M = 3.37, SE = 0.05) than Canadian participants (M = 3.23, SE = 0.05), (F (1, 161) = 4.12, p < .05), a finding that was the opposite of our prediction (H1a). There were no significant cultural differences in relaxed postures (F (1, 161) = .97, p > .05), so H1b was also not supported. However, Canadian negotiators displayed more negative emotion (M = 1.64, SE = 0.06) than Chinese negotiators (M = 1.39, SE = 0.06), (F (1, 161) = 9, p < .05), supporting our final prediction (H1c).

Hypothesis 2 predicted that male negotiators of both Canadian and Chinese cultures would exhibit more dominant nonverbal behaviors than female negotia-tors, and this would be particularly evident for Chinese negotiators. To test this claim, we examined the main effect of gender and the interaction between gender and culture. We did not find a significant main effect of gender on any of our nonverbal measures: use of space (F (1, 163) = .045, p > .05), relaxed posture (F (1, 163) = .001, p > .05), or negative emotion (F (1, 163) = .82, p > .05). Thus, contrary to H2a, male negotiators in general did not engage in more dominant behaviors compared to female negotiators. However, H2b received partial sup-port (Table 2). We found a marginally significant Culture X Gender interaction for use of space (F (1, 161) = 2.84, p = .09), where Chinese males used space the most, while Canadian males used space the least. The amount of space used by females did not vary across culture. A significant interaction emerged for relaxed postures (F (1, 161) = 4.79, p < .05). Again, the extent to which female negotia-tors exhibited relaxed posture was not significant across culture. However, overall Chinese male negotiators scored the lowest on relaxed posture whereas Canadian males scored the highest on this category. We did not find a significant interaction on the expression of negative emotion.

To examine the interactions more closely, we plotted the means. The figures illustrate that Chinese men scored highest in use of space (M = 3.43, SE = .07), while Canadian men scored the lowest (M = 3.17, SE = .07) (see Fig. 1). How-ever, where Canadian men varied their posture the most (M = 6.41, SE = .90),

Table 1. Hypothesis 1: Mean Scores Of Dominant Behaviors across Culture

Nonverbal Behaviors Chinese Canadian

M SE M SEUse of Space* 3.37* .05 3.23* .05Relaxed Posture 4.58 .65 5.5 .64Negative Emotion* 1.39* .06 1.64* .06

* p ≤ .05.

Page 14: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

464 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

Table 2. Culture by Gender Interaction

Male Female

Nonverbal Behaviors ChineseM (S.E.)

CanadianM (S.E.)

ChineseM (S.E.)

CanadianM (S.E.)

Use of Space † 3.43 (.07) 3.17 (.07) 3.31 (.07) 3.28 (.07)Relaxed Posture * 3.54 (1) 6.41 (.9) 5.62 (.9) 4.59 (.92)Negative Emotion 1.29 (.08) 1.64 (.08) 1.5 (.08) 1.63 (.08)

* p ≤ .05.† p ≤ .10.

Fig. 2. Culture by gender interaction: relaxed postures.

Fig. 1. Culture by gender interaction: use of space.

Page 15: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 465

Chinese men varied their posture the least (M = 3.54, SE = 1.00) (see Fig. 2). The scores of Canadian and Chinese women on these dimensions was fairly similar (Can M = 4.59, SE = .92), (Chin M = 5.62, SE = .90), and it was not always less than the male negotiators.

Dominant Behaviors as Mediators of Joint and Individual Gains and Negotiator Satisfaction

To test whether the effects of culture and gender on joint gains and negotiation satisfaction were mediated by dominant nonverbal behaviors, we conducted a series of hierarchical regressions. Culture and gender were each dummy coded (0, 1) since they were categorical predictor variables. We transformed our nonver-bal behavior scores prior to conducting mediation analysis to control for the fact that some dyads spent more time negotiating than others. For frequency count of nonverbal behaviors, we divided each individual’s total count by the number of minutes the dyad negotiated, resulting in a proportion score capturing behaviors per minute (Adair et al. 2001). Negative emotion was rated on a 1–5 scale, which we also transformed using the number of minutes spent negotiating. We used this transformation because we wanted to weight negative emotion more heavily for dyads that spent less time negotiating. Research on negotiation and time shows that negotiators are likely to posture and compete early on, which is offset by later cooperative behaviors (Pruitt 1981; Putnam and Jones 1982). If negotiators only spent 10 minutes negotiating, rather than the maximum of 30 minutes, the expression of negative emotion should have a stronger effect than if negotiations lasted longer and reached the cooperative stages.

H3a predicted that dominant behaviors would mediate the relationship between culture, gender and joint gains (see Table 3); such that the more domi-nant behavior is displayed, the lower the joint gains. To examine this relationship, we employed the sequential regression technique and reporting procedures (Keith 2006; MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, and Sheets 2002; Wade-Benzoni et al. 2002) using the dyad as our unit of analysis. We initially ran a moderated mediation model; however, the interaction (Culture x Gender) was not significantly related to joint gains. So, for the remaining mediation analyses, we included both culture and gender as our predictors. In step 1 of the model, we examined whether the predictor variables, culture and gender, were significantly related to our dependent measure, joint gains. The results indicate a significant negative relationship between culture, β = −.23, gender, β = −.24, and joint gains, (F (2, 70) = 3.88, p < .05), where Canadians had lower joint gains than Chinese negotiators, and females had lower joint gains than male negotiators. Step 2 of our model examined whether the mediators, dominant behaviors, significantly predicted joint gains. Both use of space, β = −.33, (F (1, 71) = 8.46, p < .05), and negative emotion, β = −.31, (F (1, 71) = 7.3, p < .05), were inversely related to

Page 16: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

466 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

joint gains such that higher scores on these behaviors, lowered joint outcomes. We did not find a significant relationship between relaxed postures and joint gains. The full mediation was tested in the final step of our model. After including the predictor and mediating variables, culture was no longer significant and gen-der was less significant. Use of space, β = −.29, (F (3, 69) = 5.22, p < .05), and negative emotion, β = −.25, (F (3, 69) = 4.33, p < .05), were still significant, indi-cating that these dominant behaviors partially mediate the relationship between culture, gender and joint outcomes. Thus, H3a was partially supported.

We conducted similar analysis to test whether dominant nonverbal cues medi-ated the relationship between culture, gender, and individual gains. H3b pre-dicted a positive relationship such that the more a negotiator employs dominant behaviors, the higher his or her negotiation outcome will be. Each participant, rather than the dyad, was our unit of analysis, and we controlled for partner’s negotiation outcome when examining the relationship of culture, gender, and dominant behaviors with individual gain. Overall, we did not find a significant relationship between culture and gender with individual outcome. Yet, the Cul-ture x Gender interaction was significantly related to individual gain. We found a

Table 3. Mediation Analysis Predicting Joint Gains

Variable B Adjusted R 2 Δ R 2 F df for F

Step 1CultureGender

−.23*−.24*

.074 .1 3.88* (2, 70)

Step 2Use of Space −.33**

.094 .11 8.46** (1, 71)

Step 3CultureGenderUse of Space

−.2−.22*−.29**

.15 .19 5.22** (3, 69)

Step 1CultureGender

−.23*−.24*

.074 .1 3.88* (2, 70)

Step 2Negative Emotion −.31*

.081 .093 7.3* (1, 71)

Step 3CultureGenderNegative Emotion

−.16−.22*−.25*

.12 .16 4.33* (3, 69)

* p ≤ .05.** p ≤ .01.

Page 17: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 467

negative relationship between Culture x Gender interaction, β = −.24, and indi-vidual gain (F (3, 145) = 2.9, p < .05), where Canadians had lower individual gains than Chinese negotiators, especially Canadian females. So, we ran a moder-ated mediation model to examine nonverbal behaviors as mediators. In our anal-yses, none of the dominant behaviors mediated the relationship between culture x gender, and individual gain, thus H3b was not supported.

Hypothesis 4 predicted that dominant nonverbals would mediate the relation-ship between culture, gender, and satisfaction with negotiation process (see Table 4). SVI scores on satisfaction with process were calculated and standardized to z-scores to normalize distribution. Similar to the previous mediation analyses, step 1 of the model explored the relationship of the predictor variables, culture and gender, and our dependent measure, satisfaction with process. The results indicated a significant positive relationship between culture, β = .16, and satisfac-tion with process, (F (1, 144) = 3.86, p < .05), where Canadians reported higher level of satisfaction than Chinese negotiators. The relationship between satisfac-tion with process and gender was not significant, so we did not proceed to analyze gender as a predictor in the mediation model. In step 2, we examined whether the mediators, dominant behaviors, significantly predict satisfaction with negotia-tion. Use of space, β = .22, (F (1, 144) = 7.45), p < .05, and displays of negative emotion, β = .20, (F (1, 144) = 5.84, p < .05), were related to negotiator satisfac-tion such that a higher use of these behaviors led to higher satisfaction. Once more, we did not find a significant relationship between relaxed postures and negotiator satisfaction and thus did not proceed with mediation analysis for relaxed posture. In step 3, we tested the full mediation. Culture was no longer significant while use of space, β = .21, (F (2, 144) = 5.21, p < .05), and displays of negative emotion, β = .17, (F (2, 144) = 3.93, p < .05), were still significant, indicating that these behaviors partially mediate the relationship between culture and joint outcomes. Accordingly, H4 was partially supported.

Discussion

The current study investigated the effect of culture and gender on intracultural negotiators’ display of nonverbal behaviors typically associated with dominance. We predicted differences based on culture, since East Asians are more reserved and less competitive (when interacting with in-groups) than North Americans (Kudoh and Matsumoto 1985; Morris et al. 1998), and gender, since gender role distinctions are stronger in China than in Canada (Hofstede, 1980; Williams Best 1990). Our findings indicate that while Canadian negotiators were more likely to have a relaxed body posture and display negative emotion on their face, Chinese negotiators were more likely to use space at the negotiation table. These results suggest cultural differences in negotiators’ tendencies to display cues

Page 18: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

468 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

typically associated with dominance and offer important implications for both future research and practical advice to negotiators. In addition, we found that the display of negative emotion and the use of space significantly mediated the effect of culture on joint gains and negotiator satisfaction. Thus, our study confirms cultural variation in nonverbal displays of dominance that has implications for negotiation outcome.

Our findings on cultural differences in negotiators’ nonverbal behaviors are consistent with prior research in other contexts, where Canadians displayed more relaxed posture and negative emotion than Chinese negotiators (Butler, Lee, and Gross 2007; Matsumoto et al. 1998). Also, similar to cultural effects in previous studies (Bond and Shiraishi 1974; Matsumoto et al. 1997), our Chinese negotia-tors had more restrained and rigid posture and fewer displays of negative emotion than Canadian negotiators. Our results add to prior research not only by replicat-ing the effects in a negotiation context, but also because we found that along with a reserved and rigid posture, Chinese male negotiators took up more space than Canadian negotiators. Based on Western nonverbal behavior research, taking up space is a sign of dominance (Remland 1981). It seems reasonable that if social norms constrain Chinese male negotiators from expressing dominance with a free and relaxed posture, they may instead reveal it through spreading their things out and taking up space. Similar to the way in which Japanese negotiators seat the most senior executive at the head of the table, facing the door (Hodson, Sano and

Table 4. Mediation Analysis Predicting Satisfaction with Negotiation Process

Variable β Adjusted R 2 Δ R 2 F df for F

Step 1Culture .16*

.02 .026 3.86* (1, 144)

Step 2Use of Space .22**

.043 .05 7.45** (1, 144)

Step 3CultureUse of Space

.14

.21**

.055 .07 5.21** (2, 144)

Step 1Culture .16*

.02 .026 3.86* (1, 144)

Step 2Negative Emotion .2*

.032 .039 5.84* (1, 144)

Step 3CultureNegative Emotion

.12

.17*

.039 .052 3.93* (2, 144)

* p ≤ .05.** p ≤ .01.

Page 19: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 469

Graham 2008), Chinese negotiators may express dominance through the manip-ulation of their environment, rather than their body.

Prior studies on culture and verbal communication have found that East Asian negotiators use more power tactics than North American negotiators (Adair et al. 2004). But North American negotiators tend to have high independence and individualist values that go along with a self-focus and competitive strategy (Brett and Okumura 1998; Gelfand and Christakopoulou 1999; Gelfand and McCusker 2002; Gelfand and Realo 1999). Thus, both East Asian and North American negotiators are likely to display dominance nonverbally. Our study captures how negotiators from Canada and China may display dominance in distinct, culturally-normative ways.

The current study measured the frequency of nonverbal displays, but not the target’s intended meaning or the partner’s interpretation of the nonverbal cue. Future research should test the emotions and strategic intent of Canadian nego-tiators who use a relaxed posture and display negative emotion, and Chinese negotiators who use a lot of space. Our results suggest potential areas of miscom-munication to be studied further in Canadian-Chinese intercultural negotiations. If cross-cultural negotiators display dominance in different ways, negotiators may misinterpret the other party’s approach. For example, a Chinese negotiator may associate a Canadian counterpart’s relaxed posture as disinterest, or the display of emotion as weakness. The Chinese negotiator may walk away or use a concilia-tory strategy when a more effective approach may be reciprocal dominance or yielding (Butt et al. 2005; Van Kleef et al. 2004). There may be a missed oppor-tunity to deflect and redirect dominance, a strategy advocated by Brett and col-leagues (1998) because it was not noticed or was not correctly interpreted.

Our results also revealed that the greatest cultural differences were exhibited by male negotiators, with female negotiators displaying more moderate and similar dominant nonverbal behaviors. In other words, our results support culture and gender role expectations more for male than female negotiators. One reason why we did not find the behaviors associated with traditional female gender roles (i.e. low expression of dominant nonverbal behaviors) may be the context of our study. Negotiation tends to be a more male-dominated field, and so women who engage in negotiation may also try to exhibit more masculine, dominant behaviors (Kray, Thompson, and Galinsky 2001). Also, given that women negotiated with other women, it may have been relatively easy for them to display dominant behaviors as there are fewer social risks (e.g. negative reactions in displaying dominant, masculine behavior by a woman) than negotiating with men (Carli 1990; Deaux and Major 1987; Ridgeway and Berger 1986). This may not have been the case if women negotiated with men. Instead, they may have displayed more traditional female role stereotypes.

We found that the use of space and display of negative emotion mediated the relationship between culture and joint gains, such that the more a negotiator

Page 20: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

470 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

employed these behaviors, the lower the dyad’s joint gains. Given that Chinese negotiators used more space and Canadian negotiators displayed more negative emotions, our data suggest that negative emotion may be a nonverbal associated with dominance for Canadian negotiators and use of space may be a nonverbal associated with dominance for Chinese negotiators.7 This inference is based on previous literature that shows that negotiators employing distributive tactics tend to be more competitive and assertive (Walters et al. 1998) and are thus more likely to display dominance in negotiation. However, since we did not directly measure interpretation of nonverbal cues in this study, linking negotiators’ “use of space” to feelings of dominance in China and “negative emotion” to feelings of dominance in Canada is an empirical one to be tested in future studies.

Use of space and negative emotion also emerged as significant mediators between culture and satisfaction with the negotiation process. Our logic behind this rela-tionship was derived from past literature indicating that people like to behave in a manner that is consistent with their cognition and beliefs (Aronson 1968; Greenwald and Ronis 1978). Given that most people tend to have a fixed-pie belief about negotiations, they may use and display dominant behavior which is congruous with their beliefs and assumptions. This should increase their satisfac-tion with negotiation process since their behavior matches their expectations and intentions. Although we did not test this congruence mechanism directly, our results support this assumption where the more dominant behavior (space and negative emotion) was displayed, the more satisfied that negotiator was with the negotiation process.

Overall, our findings are largely consistent with theories of culture and com-munication. According to researchers, cultures can communicate the same mes-sage in different ways. Low context cultures, typically in the West, engage in explicit, direct information exchange, and are more likely to say things in words and express emotion. In contrast, high context cultures, typically in the East, engage in implicit information exchange such as storytelling and inference mak-ing; and tend to rely on indirect communication and suppress the display of emotion (Hall 1987; Holtgraves 1997; Matsumoto and Juang 2004). Given these cultural differences in verbal communication and information exchange, it may be that Chinese negotiators communicate dominance in a more subtle manner (by taking up space) rather than being more explicit like the Canadians (display-ing negative emotion and relaxed posture).

7) Of course, negative emotion as a nonverbal cue is important for both Chinese and Canadians. How-ever, given cultural differences in display rules, Chinese negotiators are less likely to express negative emotions nonverbally (Butler, Lee, and Gross 2007; Matsumoto et al. 1998) due to loss of face (Graham and Lam 2003).

Page 21: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 471

Limitations and Future Directions

One limitation of our study is that we operationalized measures of dominant behaviors based on research conducted in the West. And since we wanted to observe negotiators as naturally as possible, we did not interrupt them or ask them for intent or interpretation of nonverbal behavior. For example, some stud-ies illustrate that negative emotions such as anger can be beneficial if expressed in a culturally appropriate manner (Adam, Shirako, and Maddux 2010; Liu 2009; Liu, and Wang 2010). However, since we did not explicitly measure anger (e.g. have negotiators rate their emotions), and have not manipulated anger, we cannot say for sure what the influence of this particular emotion is on negotiation process and outcome. Thus, while we can draw conclusions about the display of “domi-nant” nonverbal cues, we are not able to say for sure if the behaviors displayed by our Eastern negotiators signaled dominance. This can be examined further in future research examining how negotiators from different cultures interpret non-verbal cues. Another limitation of our study is that our negotiators were under-graduate students in a laboratory setting. Despite efforts for experimental realism, some natural nonverbal expressiveness may have been hampered by the artificial setting. It may be expected that our results are on the conservative side, and we may see more evidence of nonverbal expression (or suppression) in videotaped real world negotiations.

Since our study represents an early look at negotiators’ nonverbal displays, we examined nonverbal displays of dominance with the broadest possible lens. Because negotiation is a dynamic process, negotiators’ goals, strategies, and emotions can change over time (Adair and Brett 2005; Barry and Oliver 1996; Kumar 1999; Morris and Keltner 2000; Olekalns Smith, and Walsh 1996). Future work should examine nonverbal displays of dominance at different nego-tiation stages, for example during early periods of posturing or later stages of information exchange. Future work should also investigate interpersonal within-dyad processes such as contagion and mirroring of nonverbal displays (Thomp-son et al. 1999).

In this study, we have not yet tested the many possible antecedents (e.g. emo-tions, strategic intent) and consequences (e.g. yielding, conflict spiral) of negotia-tors’ displays of dominance. There are quite a number of studies examining the influence of anger on negotiation and decision making (Adam et al. 2010; Gib-son, Schweitzer, Callister, and Gray 2009; Liu and Wang 2010; Sinaceur and Tiedens 2006; Van Kleef, De Dreu, Pietroni, and Manstead 2006). These studies, however, illustrate mixed results on negotiation outcomes. For instance, Gibson et al. (2009) suggest that when anger expressions are low on intensity, and are expressed verbally rather than non-verbally, they can result in better outcomes. However, given that some cultures (e.g. East Asia) prefer indirect communication (Hall 1976); verbal expression of anger may not yield positive results. Also, other

Page 22: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

472 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

emotions such as anxiety and tension may be typical in a negotiation context and may influence negotiation outcomes (Graham and Lam 2003; Lee, Yang, and Graham 2006). Moreover, research illustrates different types of negative emotions (Higgins 1987; 2000), and that the type of negative emotion promoted varies across cultures (Kumar 2004; Lee, Aaker, and Gardner 2000). Future research can examine some of these variables by manipulating negotiator emotion and measuring nonverbal displays, or by manipulating nonverbal displays and mea-suring behavioral consequences.

We also hope future research will explore other kinds of nonverbal displays in cross-cultural negotiations. For example, displays of active involvement, happi-ness, or empathy could vary by culture and cause misunderstanding and misat-tribution by cross-cultural negotiators. A closer lens could also be employed to examine cultural variation in how negotiators display different kinds of negative affect nonverbally, for example ego-focused versus other-focused negative emo-tions (Markus and Kitayama 1991) or negative emotions associated with a pre-vention versus promotion focus (Higgins 2000).

By examining nonverbal cues within same-gender, intracultural Chinese and Canadian dyads, we are able to begin constructing a topology of culturally nor-mative nonverbal behaviors in negotiation. Given that 80% of communication is conveyed nonverbally (Lieberman and Rosenthal 2001), by understanding cul-tural differences in nonverbal communication, we can develop a clearer picture of why and how communication problems arise in cross-cultural negotiation. More-over, research findings can be used to train cross-cultural negotiators in the recog-nition, interpretation, and display of culturally normative nonverbal expression to enhance effective communication and maximize integrative outcomes in inter-cultural negotiations.

References

Adam, H., Shirako, A. and Maddux, W.W. (2010). “Cultural Variance in Interpersonal Effects of Anger in Negotiation.” Psychological Science 21, 882–889.

Adair, W.L., Okumura, T. and Brett, J.M. (2001). “Negotiation Behavior When Cultures Collide: The United States and Japan.” Journal of Applied Psychology 86, 3: 371–385.

——, Brett, J.M., Lempereur, A., Okumura, T., Shikhirev, P., Tinsley, C. and Lytle, A. (2004). “Culture and Negotiation Strategy.” Negotiation Journal, 20, 1: 87–111.

Adler, N.J. and Graham, J.L. (1989). “Cross-cultural Interaction: The International Comparison Fallacy.” Journal of International Business Studies 20, 3: 515–537.

Afifi, W.A. (2007). Nonverbal Communication. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Ambady, N. and Rosenthal, R. (1992). “Thin Slices of Expressive Behavior as Predictors of Inter-personal Consequences: A Meta Analysis.” Psychological Bulletin 111, 2: 256–274.

Aries, E.J., Gold, C. and Weigel, R.H. (1983). “Dispositional and Situational Influences on Dom-inance Behavior in Small Groups.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 44, 779–786.

Page 23: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 473

Argyle, Michael. (1988). Bodily Communication (2nd Ed.) Madison: International Universities Press.

—— and Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and Mutual Gaze. Cambridge, UK: Syndics of the Cambridge University Press.

Aronson, E. (1968). “Dissonance Theory: Progress and Problems,” in R.P. Abelson et al., editors, Theories of Cognitive Consistency: A Sourcebook. Chicago: Rand McNally.

Barry, B. and Oliver, R.L. (1996). “Affect in Dyadic Negotiation: a Model and Propositions.” Orga-nizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 67, 127–143.

Bazerman, M.H. and Neale, M.A. (1983). “Heuristics in Negotiation: Limitations to Effective Dispute Resolutions,” in Bazerman M.H., Lewicki, R.J, editor, Negotiating in Organizations. Sage: Beverly Hills, CA; 51–67.

Birdwhistell, R. (1955). “Background in Kinesics.” ETC 13: 10–18.Bond, M.H. and Shiraishi, D. (1974). “The Effect of Body Lean and Status of an Interviewer

on the Non-verbal Behavior of Japanese Interviewees.” International Journal of Psychology 9, 2: 117–128.

Bottger, P.C. (1984). “Expertise and Air Time as Bases of Actual and Perceived Influence in Problem-solving Groups.” Journal of Applied Psychology 69, 214–221.

Brett, J.M. (2000). “Culture and Negotiation.” International Journal of Psychology 35, 2: 97–104.—— and Gelfand, M. (2008). “At Your Service. Negotiation Simulation Case.” Kellogg Dispute

Resolution Resource Center Teaching Materials, Evanston, IL.—— and Okumura, T. (1998). “Inter- and Intracultural Negotiation: U.S. and Japanese Negotia-

tors.” Academy of Management Journal 41, 495–510.——, Shapiro, D.L. and Lytle, A.L. (1998). “Breaking the Bonds of Reciprocity in Negotiations.”

Academy of Management Journal 41, 410–425.Burgoon, J.K. and Dunbar, N. (2000). “An Interactionist Perspective on Dominance-submission:

Interpersonal Dominance as a Dynamically, Situationally Contingent Social Skill.” Communica-tion Monograph 67, 96–121.

——, Johnson, M.L. and Koch, P.T. (1998). “The Nature and Measurement of Interpersonal Dom-inance.” Communication Monographs 65, 309–335.

—— and Le Poire, B.A. (1999). “Nonverbal Cues and Interpersonal Judgments: Participant and Observer Perceptions of Intimacy, Dominance, Composure, and Formality.” Communication Monographs 66, 105–124.

——, Birk, T. and Pfau, M. (1990). “Nonverbal Behaviors, Persuasion, and Credibility.” Human Communication 17, 140–169.

Buss, D.M. (2004). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (2nd Ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Butler, E.A., Lee, T.L. and Gross, J.J. (2007). “Emotion Regulation and Culture: Are the Social Consequences of Suppression Culture-specific?” Emotion 7, 30–48.

Butt, A.Z. and Choi, J.N., (2010) “Does Power Matter? Negotiator Status as a Moderator of the Relationship Between Negotiator Emotion and Behavior,” International Journal of Conflict Man-agement 21, 2: 124–146.

——, Choi, J.N. and Jaeger, A.M. (2005). “The Effects of Self-emotion, Counterpart Emotion, and Counterpart Behavior on Negotiator Behavior: A Comparison on Individual-level and Dyad-level Dynamics.” Journal of Organizational Behavior 26, 681–704.

Carli, L.L. (1990). “Gender, Language, and Influence.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 59, 941–951.

Clark, M.S., Paraki, S.P. and Carver, V. (1996). “Some Thoughts and Findings on Self-Presentation of Emotions in Relationships,” in G.J.O. Fletcher and J. Fitness, editor, Knowledge Structures in Close Relationships: A Social Psychological Approach. Mahwah, NJ; Erlbaum 247–274.

Page 24: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

474 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

Carnevale, P.J., Pruitt, D.G. and Seilheimer, S. 1981. “Looking and Competing: Accountability and Visual Access in Integrative Bargaining.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 40, 111–120.

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S.G. and Aiken, L.S. (2003). Applied Multiple Regression / Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 3rd edition. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Collett, P. (1971). “On Training Englishmen in the Nonverbal Behavior of Arabs: An Experiment in Intercultural Communication.” International Journal of Psychology 6, 209–215.

Curhan, J.R., Elfenbein, H.A. and Xu, H. (2006). “What do People Value When They Negotiate? Mapping the Domain of Subjective Value in Negotiation.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 91, 493–512.

De Dreu, C.K.W., Weingart, L.R. and Kwon, S. (2000). “Influence of Social Motives on Integrative Negotiation: A Meta-analytic Review and Test of Two Theories.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78, 889–905.

DePaulo, B.M. and Friedman, H.S. (1998). “Nonverbal Communication,” in D.T. Gilbert, S.T. Fiske, and G. Lindzey, editor, The Handbook of Social Psychology (4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. 2, 3–40.

Deutsch, M. (1973). The Resolution of Conflict: Constructive and Destructive Processes. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Deaux, K. and Major, B. (1987). “Putting Gender into Context: An Interactive Model of Gender-Related Bbehavior.” Psychological Review 94, 369–389.

Dwyer, F.R. and Walker, O.C. (1981). “Bargaining in an Asymmetrical Power Structure.” The Jour-nal of Marketing 45, 1: 104–115.

Ellsworth, P.C. and Ludwig, L.M. (1972). “Visual Behavior in Social Interaction.” Journal of Communication 22, 375–403.

Ekman, P. and Friesen, W.V. (1975). Unmasking the Face: A Guide to Recognizing Emotions from Facial Clues. Engkewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Festinger, L. (1957). A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, Calif.: Stanford University Press.Fischer, A.H. (2000). Gender and Emotion: Social Psychological Perspectives. Cambridge, UK: Cam-

bridge University Press.—— and Ury, W.L. (1983). Getting to Yes: Negotiating Agreement Without Giving In. New York:

Penguin Books.Frances, S.J. (1979). “Sex Differences in Nonverbal Behavior.” Sex Roles 5, 4.Gallois, C. (1993). “The Language and Communication of Emotion.” American Behavioral Scientist

36, 309–338.Ganesan, S. (1993). “Negotiation Strategies and the Nature of Channel Relationships.” Journal of

Marketing Research 30, 2: 183–202.Gelfand, M.J., and Christakopoulou, S. (1999). “Culture and Negotiator Cognition: Judgment

Accuracy and Negotiation Processes in Individualistic and Collectivistic Cultures.” Organization Behavior and Human Decision Processes 79, 248–269.

—— and McCusker, C. (2002). “Culture, Metaphor, and Negotiation,” in M. Gannon and K.L. Newman, editors, Handbook of Cross-cultural Management. Boston: Blackwell.

—— and Realo, A. (1999). “Individualism-Collectivism and Accountability in Intergroup Nego-tiations” Journal of Applied Psychology 84, 721–736.

Gibson, D.E., Schweitzer, M.E., Callister, R.R. and Gray, B. (2009). “The Influence of Anger on Outcomes in Organizations.” Negotiation and Conflict Management Research 2, 236–262.

Giebels, E., De Dreu, C. and Van De Vliert, E. (2000). “Interdependence in Negotiation: Effects of Exit Options and Social Motive on Distributive and Integrative Negotiation.” European Jour-nal of Social Psychology 30, 2: 255–272.

Gifford, R. (1994). “A Lens-mapping Framework for Understanding the Encoding and Decoding Interpersonal Dispositions in Nonverbal Behavior.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66, 398–412.

Page 25: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 475

Graham, J.L. (1985). “Influence of Culture on the Process of Business Negotiations: An Explor-atory Study.” Journal of International Business Studies 16, 81–96.

—— and Lam, N.M., (2003). “The Chinese Negotiation.” Harvard Business Review 81, 10: 82–91.

—— and Sano, Y. (1984). Smart Bargaining: Doing Business with the Japanese, Ballinger Publishing, Cambridge, MA.

Greenwald, A.G. and Ronis, D.L. (1978). “Twenty Years of Cognitive Dissonance: Case Study of the Evolution of a Theory.” Psychological Review 85, 1: 53–57.

Gross, J.J. and John, O.P. (1998). “Mapping the Domain of Expressivity: Multimethod Evidence for a Hierarchical Model.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 74, 170–191.

—— and Levenson, R.W. (1997). “Hiding Feelings: The Acute Effects of Inhibiting Negative and Positive Emotion.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 106, 95–103.

—— and Levenson, R.W. (1993). “Emotional Suppression: Physiology, Self-report, and Expressive Behavior.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 64, 970–986.

Guerrero, L.K. (1996). “Attachment Style Difference in Intimacy and Involvement: A Test of Four Category Model.” Communication Monographs 63, 269–292.

Hall, E.T. (1987). Hidden Differences-Doing Business with the Japanese. New York: Anchor Books.—— (1976). Beyond Culture. New York: Anchor Press.Hawrysh, B.M. and Zaichkowsky, J.L. (1990). “Cultural Approaches to Negotiations: Understand-

ing the Japanese Source.” International Marketing Review 7, 2: 28–42.Higgins, E.T. (2000). “Promotion and Prevention Experiences: Relating Emotions to Non Emo-

tional Motivational States,” in J.P. Forgas, editor, Handbook of Affect and Social Cognition. Mah-wah, NJ: Erlbaum, 186–211.

—— (1987). “Self Discrepancy: A Theory Relating Self and Affect.” Psychological Review 94, 319–340.

Hodson, J.D., Sano, Y., and Graham, J.L. (2008). Doing Business with the New Japan: Succeeding in America’s Richest International Market. Plymouth, United Kingdom, Rowman and Littlefield Publishers, INC.

Hofstede, G. (1991). Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind: Intercultural Cooperation and its Importance for Survival. New York: McGraw-Hill.

—— (1980). Culture’s Consequences: International Differences in Work-related Values. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

——, Hofstede, G.J. and Minkov, M. (2010). Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind: Intercultural Cooperation and its Importance for Survival. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Holtgraves, T. (1997). “Styles of Language Use: Individual and Cultural Variability in Conversa-tional Indirectness.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 73, 3: 624–637.

House, R.J., Hanges, P.J., Javidan, M., Dorfman, P.W. and Gupta, V. (2004). Culture, Leadership, and Organizations: The GLOBE Study of 62 Societies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Keith, T.Z. (2006). Multiple Regression and Beyond. Boston, MA: Pearson Education Inc.Kirkbride, P.S., Tang, S.F.Y. and Westwood, R.I. (1991) “Chinese Conflict Preferences and Negoti-

ating Behavior: Cultural and Psychological Influences.” Organization Studies 12, 3: 365–386.Kleinsmith, A., Ravindra De Silva, P. and Bianchi-Berthouze, N. (2006). “Cross-Cultural Differ-

ences in Recognizing Affect from Body Posture.” Interacting With Computers 18, 6: 1371–1389.Kopelman, S., Rosette, A. and Thompson, L. (2006). “The Three Faces of Eve: Strategic Displays of

Positive Negative and Neutral Emotions in Negotiations.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 99, 1: 81–101.

Kray, L.J., Thompson, L. and Galinsky, A. (2001). “Battle of the Sexes: Gender Stereotype Confir-mation and Reactance in Negotiation.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80, 6: 942–958.

Kudoh, T. and Matsumoto, D. (1985). “Cross-Cultural Examination of the Semantic Dimensions of Body Postures.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48, 6: 1440–1446.

Page 26: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

476 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

Kumar, R. (2004). “Culture and Emotions in Intercultural Negotiations: An Overview,” in M. Gelfand and J.M. Brett, editor, The Handbook of Negotiation and Culture. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press: 95–113.

—— (1999). “Communicative Conflict in Intercultural Negotiations: The Case of American and Japanese Business Negotiations.” International Negotiation Journal 4, 63–78.

—— (1997). “The Role of Affect in Negotiation: An Integrative Overview.” Journal of Applied Behavioral Science 33, 84–100.

Labott, S.M., Martin, R.B., Eason, P.S. and Berkey, E.Y. (1991). “Social Reactions to the Expres-sion of Emotion.” Cognition and Emotion 5, 397–417.

Lax, D.A. and Sebenius, J.K. (1986). The Manager as Negotiator: Bargaining for Cooperation and Competitive Gain. New York, NY: Simon and Schuster Inc.

Lee, A.Y., Aaker, J.L. and Gardner, W.L. (2000). “The Pleasures and Pains for Distinct Self-Construals: The Role of Interdependence in Regulatory Focus.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 78, 1122–1134.

Lee, K., Yang, G. and Graham, J.L. (2006). “Tension and Trust in International Business Negotia-tions: American Executives Negotiating with Chinese Executives.” Journal of International Busi-ness Studies 37, 5: 623–641.

Leung, K. and Bond, M.H. (1984). “The Impact of Cultural Collectivism on Reward Allocation.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 47, 793–804.

Lieberman, M.D. and Rosenthal, R. (2001). “Why Introverts Can’t Always Tell Who Likes Them: Multitasking and Nonverbal Decoding.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80, 294–310.

Littlepage, G.E., Schmidt, G.W., Whisler, E.W. and Frost, A.G. (1995). “An Input-Process-Output Analysis of Influence and Performance in Problem-Solving Groups.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 69, 877–889.

Liu, M. (2009). “The Intrapersonal and Interpersonal Effect of Anger on Negotiation Strategies.” Human Communication Research 35, 148–169.

—— and Wang, W. (2010). “Explaining the Influence of Anger and Compassion on Negotiators Interaction Goals: An Assessment of Trust and Distrust as Two Distinct Mediators.” Communica-tion Research 37, 443–472.

Lund, O.C.H., Tamnes, C.K., Moestue, C., Buss, D.M. and Vollrath, M. (2006). “Tactics of Hier-archy Negotiation.” Journal of Research in Personality 80, 294–310.

Lytle, A.L., Brett, J.M., Barness, Z.I., Tinsley, C.H. and Janssens, M. (1995). “A Paradigm for Confirmatory Cross-Cultural Research in Organizational Behavior,” in L.L Cummings and B.M. Staw, editor, Research in Organizational Behavior. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

MacKinnon, D.P., Lockwood, C.M., Hoffman, J.M., West, S.G. and Sheets, V. (2002). “A Com-parison of Methods to Test Mediation and Other Intervening Variable Effects.” Psychological Methods 7, 83–104.

Manusov, V. (2005). The Sourcebook of Nonverbal Measures: Going Beyond Words. New Jersey, Lon-don: Laurence Erlbaum associates.

—— and Patterson, M.L. (2006). The Sage Handbook of Nonverbal Communication. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publication Inc.

Markus, H.R. and Kitayama, S. (1991). “Culture and the Self: Implications for Cognition, Emo-tion and Motivation.” Psychological Review 98, 224–253.

Matsumoto, D. and Juang, L. (2004). Culture and Psychology. Belmont: Thomson. Wadsworth.—— and Kudoh, T. (1987). “Cultural Similarities and Differences in the Semantic Dimensions of

Body Postures.” Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 11, 3: 166–179.—— and Kupperbusch, C. (2001). “Idiocentric and Allocentric Differences in Emotional Expres-

sion and Experience.” Asian Journal of Social Psychology 4, 113–131.

Page 27: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 477

——, Takeuchi, S. and ayani, S., Kouznetsova, N. and Krupp, D. (1998). “The Contribution of Individualism-Collectivism to Cross-National Differences in Display Rules.” Asian Journal of Social Psychology 1, 147–165.

——, Weissman, M., Preston, K., Brown, B. and Kupperbusch, C. (1997). “Context-Specific Mea-surement of Individualism-Collectivism on the Individual Level: The IC Interpersonal Assess-ment Inventory (ICIAI).” Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology 28, 743–767.

McAlister, L., Bazerman, M.H. and Fader, P. (1986). “Power and Goal Setting in Channel Nego-tiation.” Journal of Marketing Research 23, 3: 228–236.

McCarthy, A., Lee, K., Itakura, S. and Muir, D.W. (2006). “Cultural Display Rules Drive Eye Gaze During Thinking.” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 37, 6: 717–722.

Mehrabian, Albert (1981). Silent Messages: Implicit Communication of Emotions and Attitudes (2nd Ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

Mehrabian, A. (1972). Nonverbal Communication. New Jersey: Aldine Transaction.Morris, M.W. and Keltner, D. (2000). “How Emotions Work: The Social Functions of Emotional

Expression in Negotiations,” in B.M. Staw and R.I. Sutton, editors, Research in Organizational Behavior, 11: 1–50. Amsterdam: JAI Press.

——, Williams, K.Y., Leung, K., Larrick, R., Mendoza, T.M., Bhatnagar, D., Li, J., Kondo, M., Lou, J. and Hu, J. (1998). “Conflict Management Style: Accounting for Cross-National Differ-ences.” Journal of International Business Studies 20, 4: 368–379.

Neslin, S.A. and Greenhalgh, L. (1983). “Nash’s Theory of Cooperative Games as a Predictor of Outcomes of Buyer-seller Negotiations: An Experiment in Media Purchasing.” Journal of Mar-keting Research 29, 4: 729–747.

Oetzel, J.G. (1998). “The Effects of Ethnicity and Self-construals on Self Reported Conflict styles.” Communication Reports 11, 133–144.

—— and Ting-Toomey, S. (2003). “Face Concerns in Interpersonal Conflict: A Cross-cultural Empirical Test of the Face Negotiation.” Communication Research 30, 6: 599–624.

Ohbuchi, K., Fukushima, O. and Tedeschi, J.T. (1999). “Cultural Values in Conflict Management: Goal Orientation, Goal Attainment, and Tactical Decision.” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 30, 51–71.

Olekalns, M., Smith, P.L. and Walsh, T. (1996). “The Process of Negotiating: Strategy and Timing as Predictors of Outcomes.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 68, 1: 68–77.

Olson, D.H. and Cromwell, R.E. (1975). Power in Families. Beverly Hills, Calif.: Sage Publications.Pinkley, R.L., Griffith, T.L. and Northcraft, G.B. (1995). “Fixed Pie a la Mode: Information Avail-

ability, Information Processing, and the Negotiation of Suboptimal Agreements.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 62, 1: 101–112.

Portney, L.G. and Watkins, M.P. (2009). Foundations of Clinical Research. Application to Practice. (3rd Ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Practice Hall.

Pruitt, D.G. (1981). Negotiation Behavior. New York: Academic Press.—— and Carnevale, P.J. (1993). Negotiation in Social Conflict. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.—— and Rubin, J.Z. (1986). Social Conflict. New York: McGraw-Hill.Putnam, L.L. and Jones T.S. (1982). “The Role of Communication in Bargaining.” Human Com-

munication Research 8, 262–280.Remland, M.S. (2009). Nonverbal Communication in Everyday Life. (3rd. Ed.). Boston, MA:

Pearson.—— (1981). “Developing Leadership Skills in Nonverbal Communication: A Situational Perspec-

tive.” Journal of Business Communication 18, 18–31.Richmond, Y. (1996). From nyet to da: Understanding the Russians. (2nd Ed.). Yarmouth, ME:

Intercultural Press.

Page 28: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

478 Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479

Ridgeway, C.L. and Berger, J. (1986). “Expectations, Legitimation, and Dominance Behavior in the Task Groups.” American Sociological Review 51, 603–617.

Sue, D. (1998). “The Interplay of Socio-Cultural Factors in the Psychological Development of Asians in America,” in D.R. Atkinson and G. Morten, editor, Counselling American Minorities (5th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill 205–213.

Sinaceur, M. and Tiedens, L.Z. (2006). “Get Mad and Get More than Even: When and Why is Anger Expression Effective in Negotiations.” Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 42, 314–322.

Taylor, P.J. and Thomas, S. (2008). “Linguistic Style Matching and Negotiation Outcome.” Nego-tiation and Conflict Management Research 1, 3: 263–281.

Thomas, K.W. and Kilmann, R.H. (1974). The Thomas-Kilmann Conflict Mode Instrument. Tuxedo, NY: Xicom.

Thompson, L. (1990). “The Influence of Experience on Negotiator Performance.” Journal of Exper-imental Social Psychology 26, 528–544.

—— and Hastie, R. (1990). “Social Perception in Negotiation.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process 47, 98–123.

——, Nadler, J. and Kim, P. (1999). “Some Like it Hot: The Case for the Emotional Eegotiator,” in L. Thompson, J. Levine and D. Messick, editors, Shared Cognition in Organizations: The Man-agement of Knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Tiedences, L.Z. (2001). “Anger and Advancement Versus Sadness and Subjugation: The Effect of Negative Emotion Expression on Social Status Conferral.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 80, 1: 86–94.

—— and Fragale, A.R. (2003). “Power moves: Complementarity in Submissive and Dominant Nonverbal Behavior.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 84, 558–568.

Ting-Toomey, S (1999). Communicating Across Cultures. New York: Guiford Press.—— and Kurogi, A. (1998). “Facework Competence in Intercultural Conflict: An Updated Face-

Negotiation Theory.” International Journal of Intercultural Relations 22, 187–225.Tinsley, C.H. (2001). “How Negotiators Get to Yes: Predicting the Constellation of Conflict Man-

agement Strategies Used Across Cultures.” Journal of Applied Psychology 86, 4: 583–593.—— and Pillutla, M.M. (1998). “Negotiating in the United States and Hong Kong.” Journal of

International Business Studies 29, 711–727.——, Taylor, M.S. and Adair, W.L. (in press). “Culture and International Negotiation Failure,” in

F. Cede and G.O. Faure, editors, Saving Negotiations from Failure. Laxenburg, Austria: IIASA, Processes of International Negotiation Program.

Triandis, H.C. (1989). “The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts.” Psychological Review. 96, 3: 506–520.

——, Bontempo, R., Villareal, M.J., Asai, M. et al. (1988). “Individualism and Collectivism: Cross-Cultural Perspectives on Self in Group Relationships.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54, 323–338.

Ueda, K. (1974). “Sixteen Ways to Avoid Saying No in Japan,” in J.C. Condon and M. Saito, editors, Intercultural Encounters in Japan. Tokyo: Simul Press.

Van Kleef, G.A., De Dreu, C.K.W. and Manstead, A.S.R. (2004). “The Interpersonal Effects of Anger and Happiness in Negotiations.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86, 57–76.

——, De Dreu, C.K.W., Pietroni, D. and Manstead, A.S.R. (2006). “Power and Emotion in Negotiation: Power Moderates the Interpersonal Effects of Anger and Happiness on Concession Making.” European Journal of Social Psychology 36, 557–581.

Volkema, R.J. (2011). “Understanding Initiation Behavior in Chinese Negotiations: An Examina-tion of Distinctions Across Three Subcultures.” International Negotiation Journal 16.

Page 29: The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation ... · The Display of “Dominant” Nonverbal Cues in Negotiation: ... communication, ... negotiation research is to model

Z. Semnani-Azad and W.L. Adair / International Negotiation 16 (2011) 451–479 479

Wade-Benzoni, K.A., Okumura, T., Brett, J.M., Moore, D.A., Tenbrunsel, A.E. and Bazerman, M.H. (2002). “Cognitions and Behavior in Asymmetric Social Dilemmas: A Comparison of Two Cultures.” Journal of Applied Psychology 87, 1: 87–95.

Walters, H.C., Stuhlmacher and Meyer (1998). “Gender and Negotiator Competitiveness: A Meta-analysis.” Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 76, 1: 1–29.

Watson, D., Clark, L.A. and Tellegen, A. (1988). “Development and Validation of Brief Measures of Positive and Negative Affect: The PANAS Scales.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 54, 6: 1063–1070.

Williams, J.E. and Best, D.L. (1990). Measuring Sex Stereotypes: A Multination Study. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Appendix A. Categories of Dominant Behaviors

Categories Nonverbal Behaviors and Descriptions Measure

Use of Space

Space (spreading of arms across table and air)Hands on tableMoved hands (gesturing when speaking)

Scale rating (1 = behavior did not occur; 5 = behavior was displayed very often)

Relaxed Posture

Leaned sideways (posture leaning to left or right)Leaned back (posture leaning back in chair)Body seems relaxed and at ease

Frequency count

Negative Emotion

Angry, nervous, scared, disgusted, ashamed, sad, irritable, hostile

Scale rating (1 = behavior did not occur; 5 = behavior was displayed very often)