a psychometric evaluation of an achievement motivation

113
\ A PSYCHOMETRIC EVALUATION OF AN· ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION QUESTIONNAIRE (PMV) by ERICA LEVER SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF ARTS (COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY) in the FACULTY OF ARTS at the RAND AFRIKAANS UNIVERSITY SUPERVISOR: PROFESSOR F.P. HUGO CO-SUPERVISOR: PROFESSOR J.D. VAN WYK

Upload: others

Post on 18-Dec-2021

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

\

1~

A PSYCHOMETRIC EVALUATION OF AN·

ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION QUESTIONNAIRE (PMV)

by

ERICA LEVER

SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE

REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

MASTER OF ARTS

(COUNSELLING PSYCHOLOGY)

in the

FACULTY OF ARTS

at the

RAND AFRIKAANS UNIVERSITY

SUPERVISOR: PROFESSOR F.P. HUGO

CO-SUPERVISOR: PROFESSOR J.D. VAN WYK

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I am indebted to the following people who have contributed to the

completion of this study. In particular, I wish to thank:

Professor Franscois Hugo, my Supervisor, for his invaluable interest

and friendly guidance in the field of academic achievement.

Professor Deon van Wyk, my Co-supervisor, for his statistical

guidance, and his willingness to accommodate me in his busy schedule.

The Department for Higher Education at the Rand Afrikaans Universi ty

for their co-operation.

Heleen Heydenrych, for her assistance in translation.

My brother, Selwyn, for his invaluable and much appreciated

statistical guidance and for ensuring the grammatical correctness of

the text.

Jennifer Lewis for her expert typing and endless patience.

My parents, sister, Rene, and brothers, Selwyn and Jeffrey, for their

love and constant encouragement.

Gavin Hartley, for his love, humour, kindness and support.

PAGE iii

I dedicate this work to

G-d for sparing

my life.

liv .....

\

I N D E X

CHAPTER 1 : INTRODUCTION

1.1 INTRODUCTION

1.2 PREDICTION STUDIES

CHAPTER 2 : OVERVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

2.1 INTRODUCTION

2.2 ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT IN A UNIVERSITY SETTING.

2.2.1 Study habi ts and atti tudes

2.2.2 Intelligence

2.2.3 Achievement motivation

2.2.4 Personality factors

2.2.5 Anxiety

2.2.6 Adjustment

2.3 DEFINITIONS OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION

2.4 THEORIES OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION

a) Introduction

b) Main concepts

2.4.1 Murray

2.4.1.1 Introduction

2.4.1.2 The concept of the need

a) Characteristics of human needs

b) Types of human needs

c) Motives

2.4.1.3 A brief evaluation of Murray's work

2.4.2 McClelland: The affective-arousal model

2.4.2.1 Introduction

2.4.2.2 The concept of the cue

2.4.2.3 Affect as a basis for motives

PAGE iv

PAGE

1

1

2

4

4

4

4

5

6

6

7

8

9

10

10

10

11

11

12

12

13

14

14

15

15

15

15

2.4.2.4 Antecedent conditions for affective arousal16

a) The pleasure-pain principle

b) The discrepancy hypothesis

c) The discrepancy-persistencehypothesis

16

17

17

2.4.2.5 Discrepancies from adaptation level (AL) 18

2.4.2.6 The acquisition of motives 18

2.4.2:7 The achievement motive 18

2.4.2.8 Measurement of nAch (need forachieveme~t) by the TAT 20

INDEX (corrt )

2.4.3 Atkinson

2.4.3.1 Introduction: Main concepts

2.4.3.2 Motives to achieve success (Ms)

PAGE vPAGE

21

21

22

36

a) Introduction 22

b) The tendency to achieve success (Ts) 22

c) The probability of success 23

d) The incentive value of success (Is) 23

2.4.3.3 The motive to avoid failure 24

a) Introduction 24

b) The tendency to avoid failure (Taf) 24

2.4.3.4 Resultant achievement motivation (Tr) 25

2.4.3.5 Critique and conclusion 26

2.4~3.6 Elaborations of Atkinson's Theory 28

a) Perceived instrumentality 28

b) Inertial tendency (Tg) 29

c) The motive to avoid success (M-s) 30

2.4.4 A cognitive approach to achievement motivationtheory : A general overview 31

2.4.4.1 The perceived causes of success andfailure 33

2.4.4.2 Antecedents of causal ascription 34

2.4.4.3 The consequences of causal ascription 35

a) Causal ascriptions and goalexpectancy 35

b) Causal ascriptions and effectivereactions 36

2.4.4.4 A brief evaluation of Weiner'scognitive approach

2.4.5 Hermans 37

2.4.5.1 Introduction 37

2.4.5.2 Characteristics of negative failure-anxious individuals 37

2.4.5.3 Characteristics of positive failure-anxious individuals 38

2.4.5.4 Conclusion and critique 38

CHAPTER 3 : EMPIRICAL RESEARCH FINDINGS 40

3.1 INTRODUCTION: CHAPTER OUTLINE 40

3.2 RESEARCH ON ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT IN A UNIVERSITY SETTING 40

3.2.1 Intelligence

3.2.2 Study habits and attitudes

40

41

jvi .•...

\

INDEX (cont )

3.2.3 Personality factors

3.2.4 Anxiety

3.2.5 Adjustment

3.2.6 Achievement motivation

3.3 THEORIES OF MOTIVATION CURRENT RESEARCH

3.3.1 McClelland

3.3.2 Atkinson

a) Risk preference

b) The incentive value of success

c) Task performance efficiency

d) Persistence level

e) Educational implications of Atkinson's theory

f) Extensions of Atkinson's theory

i) Contingent pathways

ii) Inertial tendencies . ,

iii) Motive to avoid success (M-s)

3.3.3 Hermans

3.4 PROPOSITIONS

PAGE vi

42

44

44

45

46

46

48

49

49

50

50

51

51

51

52

52

53

54

CHAPTER 4 : RESEARCH DESIGN 55

4.1 HYPOTHESES 55

4.2 SUBJECTS 56

4.3 MEASURING INSTRUMENTS 58

4.3.1 The Achievement Motivation Questionnaire 58

4.3.2 The New South African Group Test 59

4.3.3 Survey of Study Habits and Attitudes 59

4.3.4 The Personal, Home Social and Formal Relations

Questionnaire 59

4.3.5 The IPAT Anxiety Scale 60

4.3.6 The 16 Personality Factor Questionnaire 60

4.4 EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE 60

4.4.1 Goal of the study 60

4.4.1.1 Experimental procedure 60

Ivii .....

INDEX (cont )

CHAPTER 5 : STATISTICAL RESULTS

5.1 RELIABILITY (item analysis)

5.2 VALIDITY

5.2.1 Bravais Pearson correlation coefficients

5.2.2 Simple Regression Analysis

5.2.3 t Tests

PAGE vii

62

62

63

62

66

66

\ CHAPTER 6 DISCUSSION OF RESULTS AND LIMITATIONS

OF THE STUDY AND RECOMMENDATIONS 68

6.1 INTRODUCTION 68

6.2 ITEM ANALYSIS 68

6.3 BRAVAIS PEARSON CORRELATIONS 68

6.3.1 Achievement motivation and academic achievement: 68

6.3.2 Achievement motivation and intelligence 69

6.3.3 Achievement motivation and study orientation 69

6.3.4 Achievement motivation and personality 69

6.3.5 Achievement motivation and intrapersonal

adjustment 70

6.3.6 Achievement motivation and anxiety 70

6.3.7 Academic achievement and intelligence 70

6.4 THE PREDICTION OF ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT ON THE BASIS OF

IQ SCORE 70

6.5 A COMPARISON OF UNDER-ACHIEVERS VERSUS OVER-ACHIEVERS

ON ANXIETY & STUDY ORIENTATION 70

6.5.1 Anxiety 71

6.5.2 Study orientation 71

6.6 LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY AND RECOMMENDATIONS 71

CHAPTER 7 : SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION

7.1 SUMMARY

7.2 CONCLUSION

74

74

75

jvi:i i. ....

TABLE A

TABLE B

TABLE C

TABLE D

TABLE E

TABLE F

TABLE G

LIST OF TABLES

A general overview of the attributional

interpretation of expectancy-value theory

Weiner et al (1972) two-dimensional

taxonomy of causal ascription

Frequency Distribution for Sex

Frequency Distribution for Faculty

Pearson Correlation Coefficients between

Achievement Motivation and Academic

Achievement, Intelligence, Study

Orientation and Personality Factors

Pearson Correlation Coefficient

between Academic Achievement and

Intelligence

The significance of differences in

means of over-achievers versus under­

achievers on anxiety and study

orientation

PAGE viii

PAGE

32

33

:57

57

62

63

66

/ix .....

\

o P S 0 M MIN G

. PAGE ix

Onderprestasie in Suid Afrikaanse universi tei te is 'n verskynsel wat

ernstige afmetings aanneem en huidige navorsing in hierdie verband

toon aan dat dit verrykende implikasies tot gevolg het. Daar bestaan

gevolglik 'n dringende behoefte aan 'n instrument waarvolgens die

potensHHe onderpresteerder betyds geidentifiseer kan word. By die

Randse Afrikaanse Universiteit is dit verpligtend dat alle eerstejaar­

studente 'n reeks diagnostiese psigometriese toetse voor die aanvang

van die akademiese studiejaar afle. Een van hierdie psigometriese

toetse is die PMV (prestasiemotiveringsvraelys). Aangesien die

betroubaarheid en geldigheid van die PMV tot op hede nog onbekend is,

was die doel van hierdie studie om dit te bereken.

Verskillende teoriee van prestasiemotivering is bespreek, insluitend,

Murray (1938), McClelland (1976) se affektiewe opwekkingsmodel, Weiner

(1974) se kognitiewe benadering tot motivering en laastens, die teorie

van Hermans, Bergen & Eijssen (1982).

Hierna volg die statistiese bewerkinge van die studie. Die eksperi­

mentele groep het ui t 1729 blanke, Afrikaanssprekende, voorgraadse

eerstejaarstudente wat in 1985 geregistreer het, bestaan. Ander

meetinstrumente wat in hierdie navorsing gebruik is, is onder andere,

die nuwe Suid Afrikaanse Groepstoets (NGAG), die Opname van Studie­

gewoontesen Houdings (OSGH), die 16 PF, die Persoonlike Huislike,

Sosiale en Formele Verhoudingsvraelys (PHSF) en die Ipat Angsskaal.

Die betroubaardheid van die PMV is deur middel van 'n item analise

bepaal (NP-50 program). Bravais Pearson korrelasiekoeffisiente. 'n

Eenvoudige regressieanalise en t-toetse is gebruik om die geldigheid

van die vraelys te bereken.

Statisties beduidende resultate is verkry in al drie die bewerkinge.

Die resul tate wat verkry is met behulp van die eenvoudige regressie

analise, maak die voorspelling van akademiese prestasie op grond van 'n

verkree intelligensiekwosient moontlik, asook die identifiseering van

oor- en onder-presteerders. Resultate wat verkry is met behulp van die

t-toetse, het aangetoon dat daar 120 oorpresterders en 287 onderpres­

teerders was. Hierdie twee groepe proefpersone se gemiddelde tellings

ten opsigte van die Ipat angsskaal, en die OSGH is met mekaar

vergelyk, waar volgens daar TI beduidende verskil ten opsigte van slegs

die OSGH tellings gevind is.

Ix .....

OPSOMMING (verv . )

PAGE x

Verder, word beperkinge en leemtes in hierdie navorsingstudie bespreek

en sluit onder andere in dat n uitgesoekte studentegroep gebruik is,

en waar die resultate nie veralgemeen kan word na die algemene

populasie nie. 'n Verdere leemte is die fei t dat daar nie vanuit nontwikkelingsperspektief gekyk is na die verskynsel van motivering en

prestasie by studente nie. Lae motivering van die proefpersone om die

diagnostiese toetse te vol tooi het ook daartoe bygedra dat die data

wat verkry is, waarskynlik onsuiwer was. Aanbevelings met betrekking

tot verdere navorsing is gedoen en behels onder andere datdaar in die

voorspelling van akademiese prestasie n nie-linieere benadering tot

prestasiemotivering gevolg word.

CHAPTER 1.

1.1 INTRODUCTION

The phenomenon of under-achievement in first year students assumes

staggering proportions.

Research by Marais (1980) proves that under-achievement is a serious and

perturbing phenomenon. Research was completed at the Rand Afrikaans

University (R.A.U.) and results showed that:

146 of the 377 students who failed had an intelligence quotient of at

least 120. Forty of these students had an intelligence quotient of 130

and higher.

the department which yielded the highest failure rate was the Natural

Sciences Department, where 43,4% were male failure students and 59,3%

were female students. The above students were intellectually gifted,

with an intelligence quotient of 130 and higher.

Marais (1981) identified 86 intellectually gifted students. Of these 86

students, 25 discontinued or failed their studies. Thirteen students did not

return to the uni versi ty. The mean IQ for those that had failed was 141. At

this time, these failures meant a financial loss for the university and a

loss of potential manpower for South Africa.

The conclusion to be drawn from these studies is that academic failure has

immense financial implications. Furthermore, in view of the economic

situation in South Africa today, academic failure has even greater financial

impact for the university, the individual and the state.

The concern for the failure and under-achievement in fi~st year students is

reflected in the words of Koornhof (1978, in Marais, 1980): "The high rate of

failure among university undergraduates, particularly first years, remains a

constant source of concern to the state, the universities and the students

themselves." (p. 3)

From the above, it is evident that it is important to be able to predict

academic achievement.

/2 .....

PAGE 2

1.2 PREDICTION STUDIES

In the past, various attempts have been made to predict academic

achievement. Intelligence and aptitude were amongst the most popular of

predictors. The reason for this is that these variables could explain

maximum variance in school achievement. According to the literature,

(Schoeman, 1982), intelligence and aptitude could only explain between 20%

and 36% of the variance in academic achievement.

Because intelligence and aptitude were not considered adequate predictors of

variance in academic achievement, researchers began investigating the role of

non-intellectual factors, as predictors of academic achievement.

A study researching the role of non-intellectual factors in academic

achievement was completed by Engelbrecht (1974). This study revealed that

factors such as anxiety, introversion/extroversion and study orientation

played an important role in the examination achievement of girls with above

average intellect. Engelbrecht (1974) maintains that we should not dismiss

intellectual factors as predictors of academic achievement, but that we

should bear in mind the important role that non-intellectual factors play inthis regard.

Schoeman (1982) has. summed up the role that intellectual and non-intellectual

factors play in academic achievement in the following manner. He claims that

non-intellectual factors, together with cognitive variables are only capable

of explaining between 45% and 50% of variance in academic achievement. In

other words, these percentages are seen as the "cei1ing" for prediction

studies. "Hierdie persentasies word as 'n soort "plafon" vir voorspell ings

studies gesien." (p.l).

According to Schoeman (1982), it appears that the creation of homogenous

subgroups is the most promising approach in an attempt to explain academic

achievement.

These prediction studies can all be said to have one conclusion, reflected in

the words of Schoeman (1982):

/3 .....

PAGE 3

"Aangesien die voorspelling van akademiese prestasie deur die jare

al hoe belangriker geword het en vandag 'n absolute noodsaaklikheid

is, moet daar gesoek word na 'n model of prosedure wat 'n groter

persentasie van die variansie in akademiese prestasie kan ver-kl aar ;"

(p , 3).

The important point is that if one is able to explain variance in academic

achievement, one is able to predict academic achievement. Schoeman (1982)

claims that if this ideal can be reached, it would result in effective

counselling in schools and universities. The ideal would be to identify

a test/measurement that would be able to differentiate between over- and

under-achievers. Such a test has been constructed by Vi Ljoen (1983), but

the reliability and validity of the test have to date; remained unknown

in the university setting. This, therefore, is the goal of the present

research.

/4 .....

-.CHAPTER 2 OVERVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

PAGE 4

2.1. INTRODUCTION

There are many factors which play a role in the prediction of academic

achievement at the tertiary level and since academic achievement is a

concept of such entirety, a limited number of factors have been delineated

for this study. They include study habits and attitudes, intelligence,

achievement motivation, personality variables, adjustment and anxiety. The

above factors will be discussed and following this, are the theories of

achievement motivation.

2.2 ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT IN A UNIVERSITY SETTING

As mentioned above, there are a number of factors which influence academic

achievement and it is these factors which have been utilised in conducting a

prediction study of academic achievement at the tertiary level. It is

important for this study that one consider its general outline to be academic

achievement, with achievement motivation as one aspect of this very broad

field. Furthermore, one should also bear in mind that to see academic

achievement and achievement motivation as distinct entities is impossible

because when one considers an achievement motivated student, there is usually

the implication of an above average achievement score, since achievement

motivation and academic achievement are positively correlated. One should

also bear in mind that the student is in constant interaction with the

broader environmental system at all times and that this exerts a

considerable influence on the student. A study researching this interaction

effect has been completed by Hugo (1984). The point to be made here is that

one cannot be naive about the complexity of the cone ept of achievement

motivation and one must be aware of the complex inter-dependency of the many

variables associated with academic achievement.

2.2.1 Study habits and attitudes

From, the literature, it is evident that study habits and attitudes play

a substantial role in academic achievement (O'Connor-, Chassie & Walter,

1980). It seems logical that there is a posi tive correlation between

academic achievement and study habits, because the SSHA (Survey of Study

Habi ts and Attitudes) is capable of predicting academic achievement

(Holtzman & Brown, 1953, Dobson, 1979).

More specifically, research completed by Zarb (l981) , indicates that

effective study habits are significantly posi tively . correlated with

achievement motivation. This implies that the more motivated a student

is, the more attention he/she will pay to study habits and usually, more

effective study habits and attitudes result in increased academic

performance./5 .....

PAGE 5

It appears that, in general, effective study habits and attitudes are

posi tively correlated with achievement motivation as well as academic

achievement. A positive cycle is established because positive motiva­

tion, together with effective study habits and attitudes in turn result

in increased academic performance, which in turn, motivates the student

and so the cycle repeats itself.

2.2.2 Intelligence

In the past, theorists have attempted to predict academic achievement

with the help of intelligence quotient scores and on this ~atter, there

are still contradictory opinions because there is debate whether IQ is

capable of predicting academic achievement or not. On one hand, some

theorists claim there is a positive correlation between IQ scores and

academic achievement (Sinha, 1966; Dhaliwal & Sharma, 1976), and

consequently, these theorists claim that IQ is a good predictor of

academic achievement. On the other hand, there are theorists who claim

that IQ is not a good predictor of academic achievement and that there.;;-

is not necessarily a positive correlation between IQ and academic

achievement (Ryle & Lungi, (1968); Garbers & Faurie 1972) . Whether or

not IQ is a good predictor of academic achievement is still debatable,

and the issue remains unresolved, perhaps due to the multi-faceted and

complex nature of intelligence itself.

Taking achievement motivation into consideration, it has been found by

Hundal and Jerath (1972) that nAch (need for achievement) was

significantly positively correlated with verbal and non-verbal measures

of intelligence. In other words, students with high~r intelligence tend

to be more achievement motivated than students with lower intelligence.

Perhaps this can be explained by the idea that students with higher IQs

are aware that they have the ability to achieve academically and

therefore are more motivated to attempt academic success.

/6 .....

\.

2.2.3

PAGE 6

Achievement motivation

The theory on the effect of achievement motivation on academic achieve­

ment indicates that nAch as measured by the TAT or pencil-and-paper

tests, is associated with higher grades. It is clear that the student

who is motivated to do well, will achieve higher academic grades than

the student who is not motivated to achieve academically. From the

literature, it is evident that there is a positive correlation between

achievement motivation and academic achievement (Shivarathnamma, 1981,;

Youngblood, Stuart & Martin, 1982,; Romine & Crowell, 1981; Grobe &

Lotta, 1981 j Uguroglu & Walberg, 1979). In general then, the

literature reveals that the more highly achievement motivated a student

is, the higher will be his/her academic achievement. . .

A different perspective is held by Lens & de VoIder (1980), who found an

inverted U-shaped relationship between achievement motivation and

academic .achievement. They hypothesize that the student reaches a point

where he/she is over-motivated and consequently, there is a decrea~e in

academic achievement. This research is still in keeping with the general

idea that achievement motivation and academic achievement are positively

correlated.

2.2.4 Personality factors

The literature reveals that certain personality factors are associated

with student under-achievement (Saenger-Ceha, 1970). It also appears

that there are diverse opinions as to whether there is correlation

between personality factors and academic achievement as well as

achievement motivation. Saenger-Ceha (1970) maintains that factors such

as dependency, lack of self control and lack bf persistence are

associated with academic under-achievement. Other factors associated

wi th academic under-achievement include lack of autonomy; flexibility

and lack of aggression (Lavin, 1965) The literature reveals that

extroverts tend to achieve less academically than introverts.(Entwistle/

1972 ; Goh & Moore, 1978).

/7 .....

PAGE 7

On the other hand, there is conflicting literature which suggests that

there is no significant correlation between academic achievement and

personality variables (Saenger-Ceha, 1970). This perspective is comple­

tely contradictory to McClelland's Taf (tendency to avoid failure) and

Ts (tendency to achi eve success) persons and Hermans I pos i tive and

negative failure-anxious individuals. The literature on the effect of

neuroticism on academic achievement is contradictory. On one hand, some

theorists claim that there is a negative relationship between

neurotocism and academic achievement (Saenger-Ceha) 1970).Contradictory

views are held by other theorists. They claim that neuroticism and

academic achievement are positively related (Wankowki & Cox, 1973; Ryle

1968). Perhaps the reason for contradictory views is due to the fact

that neuroticism (if seen as a specific aspect of anxiety), has both

facilitating and debilitating aspects. In addition, the literature also

suggests a positive and significant correlation between: introversion

and achievement motivation (Entwistle, 1972; Goh & Moore 1978).

More specifically, there appears tci be a negative correlation between

neuroticism and achievement motivation (Hafeez & Shantamani, 1972). In

other words, those students who are less anxious and neurotic will tend

to be more motivated to achieve academically.

2.2.5 Anxiety

Sarason & Mandler (1952) maintain that when a stimulus situation

contains elements which specifically arouse test anxiety. This i~crease

in anxiety drive will lead to decreased performance in individuals who

have test irrelevant (incompatible/interfering) anxiety responses in

their response repertory. For individuals without such response

repertories, these stimulus elements wi11 rai se their gener-al drive

level and will resul t in improved performance. Thi s dual approach to

anxiety is comparable to that of Hermans, Bergen;; Eijssen (1982) who

differentiated between pos Lt i ve and negative failure anxiety, as well as

Alpert and Haber's (1960) concepts of facilitating and debilitating

anxiety.

/8 .....

PAGE 8

From the literature, it is clear that most theorists have not yet

differentiated between positive and negative failure anxiety or

debilitating and facilitating anxiety. Most theorists claim that there

is a negative correlation between anxiety (in general) and academic

achievement. In other words, those students who are less anxious, tend

to do better academically (Lynne, '1977 i Kanekar, ,1976 ).

More specifically, anxiety is said to correlate positively with

achievement motivation (Gissrau, 1976). It appears that anxiety is

facili tative when required for motivation, but in excess can play a

debilitative role in academic achievement.

2.2.6 Adjustment

From the literature, there is no doubt and it is evident" that academic

under-achievement is associated with overall lack of adjustment

(Saenger-Ceha, 1970)

LncLuded : under the heading of adjustment is that of the self concept.

Literature on the influence of self concept shows that students who see

themselves positively, having a good self concept, tend to be academic

achievers, whereas, students with a low self concept tend to be

under-achievers (Purkey, 1970 FarIs, ~967; Tuel & Wursten 196= ;

Bailey, 1971 and Lynne, 1977). Zarb. (1981) maintains that it is

more specifically academic self concept which is positively related to

academic achievement, whereas general self concept is not necessarily

correlated with academic achievement.

The literature on the relationship between achievement motivation and

self concept indicates that achievement motivation and self'concept are

significantly and positively correlated (Srivastava, 1979)

/9 .....

PAGE 9

2.3. DEFINITIONS OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION

Heckhausen (1967), has defined achievement motivation as follows:- "The

striving to increase, or keep as high as possible, one's own capability in all

activities in which a standard of excellence is thought to apply and where the

execution of such activites can, therefore, succeed er- fail." (p . 5).

In a similar line of thought, McClelland (1958, in Weiner, 1972) describes

achievement motivation as: "the pursuit of an achievement goal, where an

achievement goal is regarded as success in competition with some standard of

excellence" (p , 110).

Yet another definition of achievement motivation has been proposed by Atkinson,

(1964) who maintains that the concept of achievement motivation attempts to

account for:

"the determinants of direction, magnitude and persistence of behaviour in a

limited domain of human activites. It applies only when an individual knows

that his performance will be evaluated (by himself and others) in terms of

some standard of excellence, and that the consequences of his action will

be either a favourable evaluation (success) or an unfavourable evaluation

(failure)" (p , 240).

The following definition of achievement motivation, as proposed by Winterbottom

(1958, in Atkinson, 1964) demonstrates that achievement motivation is a multi­

faceted concept in that it highlights another facet of achievement motivation,

which has not yet been covered by previous definitions, namely, the issue of

pride. Winterbottom (1958), maintains that: "the disposition called the

achievement motive might be conceived as a capacity for taking pride in an

accomplishment when success at one or another activity:is achieved." (p . 241).

/10 .....

PAGE 10

With all the above definitions of achievement motivation in mind. the author

of this thesis has defined achievement motivation in her own terms. The

achievement motive is seen as a motive to maintain one's own level of capa­

bility and to compete (with others or with oneself). once a personal goal has

been set. with a subjective. internal standard of excellence in mind. It

applies only when the individual is aware of an evaluation of performance

(which may be positive or negative) and the consequences thereof (success or

failure) •\

~·4. THEORIES OF ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION

a) Introduction

Historically. the theory of achievement motivation began with the works of

Lewin. Tolman & Freud, whose theories revolved essentially around the drive

and the instinct. Following this. there developed theories centering around

the need. as proposed by Murray. (1938. in Weiner. 1972). The concept of the

need for achievement was elaborated upon by McClelland. (1955) and Atkinson.

(1964). Lastly. Hermans. Bergen & Eijssen (1982) elaborated further by adding

the dimension of anxiety to the concept of achievement motivation. This

section is a theoretical and historical overview of the above theories of

achievement motivation from 1938 and the works of Murray to 1982 and the

works of Hermans et ale

b) Main concepts

Murray is an eclectic motivation theorist in that his views are an

integration of the works of earlier theorists such as Freud,' Lewin and

Tolman. Works on achi evement motivation rather than motivation in general

began with the works of Murray. He is essentially a need theorist. for he

viewed needs as the core of goal-directed behaviour. :Thus. Murray's main

concept is that of the need.

The need to achieve was then elaborated upon by McClelland, (1955) for whom

motives are the most important concept. The essence of McClelland's theory is

that the achievement motive is learned through persistent repeti tive

affective associations between stimulus situation and behavioural patterns.

/11. ....

PAGE" 11

McClelland and Atkinson, began working concurrently on the concept of the

need for achievement. Atkinson concentrated on the role of individual

differences for an understanding of motivational processes, maintaining that

the person, the environment and human experience are the determinants of

behaviour. These two theories became partially linked and known as the

McClelland-Atkinson model of achievement motivation (Weiner, 1972).

The last theorist to be discussed is Hermans, et. a1. (l982) whose theory

revolves around the dimension of anxiety as it relates to achievement

motivation.

2.4.1 Murray

2.4.1.1 Introduction

Murray's theory of achievement motivation began as a theory of

personality - as an attempt to explain individual differences.

Historically, Murray's theory stems from the works of a theorist

by the name of McDougall, (1908, in Weiner, 1972) who conceptua­

lised the individual as a bundle of instincts, otherwise known as

goal-directed dispositions. Murray, (1938, in Weiner, 1972)

parted with McDougall's ideas that one's needs were instinctive.

He argued that a better understanding of the individual could be

attained through description of one in terms of the effects one

generally strove to bring about, rather than in terms of one ' s

generalised habits/traits, which revealed little of one's

personal aims and goals.

Since Murray's theory of achievement motivation is rooted in

personality theory, it is logical that his theory revolves

around human individual differences, since it is these very

specific aspects of the individual which can be grouped together

to form one's personal i ty. In other words, Murray saw human

behaviour and one's needs as part of one's total personality.

"According to Murray, the behaviour of an individual person

reveals rhythms of rest and activity. These cycles of behaviour

are interwoven in a dense network of total behaviour, whi ch

Murray refers to as the lexicon of behaviour. The goal of this

/12 .....

PAGE 12

behaviour is to take the organism from some prior state into some

consequent state. "That is, a motivating force carries the organism away

from a prior or inihating condi tion into a state like satiation in

which the force disappears." (Bolles, ,1975:, p , 81). These trends

(discussed above) which are apparent in one's general behaviour are due

to drives and needs which operate homeostatically .•

2.4.1.2 The concept of the need

Needs are the core of goal-directed behaviour and it is the

environment which determines whether they will be expressed.

Murray (1938, in Weiner, 1972) defines a need as: "an organic

potentiality or readiness to respond in a certain way under given

conditions" (p , 170). Given the definition of a heed, it is

important to be aware of the fact that needs are

homeostatic. Needs are considered homeostatic in the sense that

the achievement of a demotivated state is only possible when a

goal has been achieved and the need is dispelled, resulting in

the re-establishment of an equilibrium. Needs, in Murray's system

may be physiological in origin but usually they are aroused by

particular events in the environment that offer certain threats

or promises to the individual. These stimulus actions upon the

individual are what Murray calls "presses". In essence then , it

is the environmental "press acting in conjunction with a need

which determines whether a need will be expressed in order to

reach a goal." (p . 173) •.

a) The characteristics of human needs

As mentioned above, Murray maintains that needs may be related to

physiological disturbances, as in the case of hunger, thirst and

sex. These types of needs, Murray refers to as viscerogenic

needs. Needs which are not crucial for the long-term adaptation

of the organism, but are important in understanding human

behaviour, Murray refers to as psychogenic needs. These are the

individual's psychological needs, . of which Murray has 1 isted

about 30. Al though psychogenic needs are not biological, Murray

maintains that they may be innate.

/13 .....

PAGE 13

Psychogenic needs are usually exhibited in overt action when

they are aroused and it is this type of behaviour which forms

the basis of the individual's daily schedule. These are termed

mani fest!obj ecti fied needs. However, some needs are unexpressed

in overt action and these are termed latent needs!subjectified

needs. This distinction is s i gnd f'Lcant; for Murray, because it

is important for him to take note of those needs which are

repeatedly expressed and those which are frustrated (unexpres­

sed). Frustration of a need may lead to inhibition of a need

or to a re-striving. This is most important in understanding

the expression/lack of expression of the achievement need.

Manifest and latent needs may also be conscious or unconscious.

By conscious, Murray means that the need is immediately retros­

pective, otherwise known as introspective awareness. In other

words, whatever a subject can report upon, is considered

conscious and everything else is considered unconscious. Both

:conscious and unconscious needs may be either subjectified

(unexpressed) or objectified (expressed).

b) Types of human needs

Murray has listed about 30 psychogenic needs which are either

expressed (objectified) or remain latent (subjectified) in each

indivi dual. These range from needs associated with inanimate

objects (for example, the need for acquisition) to needs asso­

ciated with affection between people (for example, the need

for affiliation). The most important group of needs for this

study. are these relating to ambi tion. The need for ambition

consists of a broad need known as the need for superiority,

which is composed of two interrelated needs known as the need

for achievement and the need for recognition. The need for

achievement has been defined as: (in McClelland, 1955), "The

need to overcome obstacles, to exercise power,to strive to

do something difficult as well and as quickly as possible" (p.

64) •

/14 .....

\

PAGE 14

The need for achievement is usually fused wi th complementary

needs, like needs involving the defence of norms, and/or the

avoidance of humiliation. These complementary needs include the

need for inviolacy - (the need for self respect and pride and to

preserve one's 'good name') and the need for counter-action - (to

overcome defeat by restricting and retal iating, and to defend

one's honour in action).

c) Motives

Murray maintains that a motive is comprised of a number of

needs. These needs become a characteristic of the individual and

therefore appear in varying situations. In oth~r words, (in

Bolles, 1967) "The assumption underlying the assessment of

motives is that if an individual is characterised by a few

salient needs, then these needs should keep reappearing in a

number of different contexts" (p . 83) . In essence, motives

(comprised of needs), once created, affect all behaviour until it

is satisfied and a new equilibrium established.

2.4.1.3 A brief evaluation of Murray's work

Murray's theory has loopholes in the sense that man is not

. always provided with practical ways of implementing his theory

and as a result, his theory does not provide a whole approach to

understanding human behaviour. For example, it is unknown how

different individuals come to react differently. to environ­

mental presses, or why failure causes need abasement in one and

need for achievement in another. This point is made clearer by

the f'o l Low i ng quote (in Bolles ,1975 ) ::. "Even though Murray

tells us that such differences constitute the basic source of

difference between individual personalities, he provides no

analytical tools for explaining them" (p . 84).

For the author of thi s thesis, the most redeeming feature of

Murray's works is that he took account of the fact that a large

number of energy systems operate and interact in any individual.

It is evident that he did not see the need for achievement as

simply a unitary drive.

/15 .....

PAGE 15

2.4.2 McClelland - The affective arousal model

2.4.2.1 Introduction

There are certain stimuli which involve discrepancies between

expectation and perception (L, e. that which one expects to get

and that which one believes one is getting). These are the

sources of primary, unlearned affect. For McClelland, motives are

based on affective arousal and all motives are learned by pairing

cues with affective states.

2.4.2.2 The concept of the cue

Cues which are paired with these affective states become capable

of general ising a state derived from the original affective

si tuation. To give an example, this means that if a buzzer is

associated with eating a cube of sugar, the buzzer will in time

be capable of evoking a motive involving positive affective

change. In other words, the buzzer becomes capable of eliciting

pos i tive affect even though the sugar is not present. The cue

which is present will then serve as a motive, for example, the

buzzer will serve as a motive in the absence of the sugar cube. A

cue is said to have increased motivating power if it has been

associated with a change in affect.

2.4.2.3 Affect as a basis for motives

McClelland has used affect as a basis for:'. motives because it

provides the concept of motivation with some kind of limited

base. Stated otherwise (McClelland, 1955): "It seems apparent

that the motive concept will be useful only if it has some kind

of limited base. If all associations are motivating, there seems

no particular reason to introduce the concept of motivation to

apply to a particular subclass of association" (p. 30).

In other terms, the concept of matiyes would be meaningless

without discriminative associations.

/16 .....

PAGE 16

Furthermore. McClelland claims that affective states are

important in controlling behaviour. in the sense that the

pleasure-pain or pleasurable/unpleasurable affect is easy to

manipulate in the laboratory. Pain and pleasure are considered

the very core of those antecedent conditions necessary for

affective arousal. This concept will be dealt with in more detail

below.

2.4.2.4 Antecedent conditions for affective arousal

The task of identifying antecedent conditions which produce

affective arousal is important. for if one knows them. one is in

a position. according to the theory. of knowing how to create a

motive. McClelland hypothesized that a motive could be created by

arousal at a sensory level and subsequent persistent

discrepancies from it.

a) The pleasure-pain principle and the discrepancy hypothesis

In essence. McClelland sees affective arousal as a consequence of

a sensory stimulus and the perception thereof by the

individual. In McClelland's words. he puts it as follows: (in

McClelland. Atkinson. Clark and Lowell. 1976) "Affective

arousal is the innate consequence of certain sensory or

perceptual events" (p . 43). McClelland hypothesized that this was

probably the basic mechanism which gives rise to sensory

pleasantness (for example, sweetness) and unpleasantness (for

example. bitterness) at a more complex perceptual level.

Posi tive affect is experienced if, for the individual.

discrepancies from one's adaptation level - :(one's usual level of

sensory expectation) are small. Similarly, negative affect is

experienced if discrepancies from one's adaptation level (abbre­

viated as AL) are large. In other words, the size of the

discrepancy between the stimulus (or perception) and the

adaptation level (or expectation) is what is considered

important if viewed on a simple pleasantness - unpleasantness

curve. This is known as the discrepancy hypothesis. One should

/17 .....

PAGE 17

also bear in mind that there are natural adaptation levels for

various sensory receptors. This is essential to the discrepancy

motive mentioned above because some stimuli give rise naturally

to pleasantness and others to unpleasantness. For example, sugar

appears to give rise to pleasurable sensations across the

entire range of stimulus intensity. According to the theory, it

is apparent that a discrepancy from the natural adaptation level

(AL), large enough to produce unpleasantness is impossible. If

the stimulus is bitter, the picture is quite different, in the

sense that nearly all intensities of stimulus concentration

give rise to negative affect. It makes sense that a discrepancy

between the stimulus (perception) and the adaptation level

(expectation) must exist for a length of time before a

pleasant/unpleasant (hedonic) response occurs.

c) The discrepancy-persistence hypothesis

In McClelland's words, the hypothesis can be stated as follows:

(in McClelland, v et al . , 1976): "A discrepancy between

adaptation level and a sensation or event must persist for a

finite length of time before it gives rise to an hedonic

response" (p , 46).

The rationale behind this hypothesis is that sensations such as

taste, smell and pain give rise to affective responses mor-e

quickly than do sensations such as sight or hearing. A possible

explanation would be that there occurs :-a "receptor lag",

otherwise known as an "At lag". Thi s means that the first group

of sensations may change rather slowly, so that the discrepancy

caused by a new stimulus must last a bit longer in order for the

individual to experience pleasantness/unpleasantness (collec­

tively called a hedonic response). McClelland maintains that

/18 .....

(I

\

PAGE 18

an increase in stimulus intensity becomes the basis for a motive

only if it represents a large enough discrepancy from the

adaptation level (AL) to produce positive or negative affect.

2.4.2.5 Discrepancies from adaptation level

For McClelland, it is vi tally important that one bears in. mindI

that it is not the intensity of the stimulus per se whi oh is

important" but the disCrebncy from the adaptation level (AL).

He also maintains that changes in AL, with attendant hedonic

responses (pleasantness/unpleasantness) may be artificially

induced (e vg , chemically/neurologically). Further~ore,I

McClelland is of the opinion that events can also differ; from

expectations on a variety of dimensions.

2.4.2.6 The acquisition of motives

By McClelland's definition, a motive is formed by pairing cues

(stimuli) with affective arousal and as discussed, affect is

the result of certain discrepancies between expectations and

perceptions. For example, if a monkey had to see a de-tached

plaster head (cue), this would produce negative affect, which,

in turn, would cause fear (affective arousal). Fear, in turn,

elicits an avoidance motive. In other words, cues (stimuli)

result in affective arousal, which, in turn, are the bas i s for

motives and more specifically, for this thesis, attention will

be paid to the achievement motive.

2.4.2.7 The achievement motive

a) The development of the achievement motive

i

One of McClelland I s cardinal hypotheses is that the achievement

motive develops out of growing expectations. The expectations

are buil t out of universal experi ences wi th probl em solving;

such as walking, talking and reading. The child gradually ~earns

that these tasks can be done quickly and efficiently or cl~msily

and slowly. In other words, the· child develops his/her own

framework of self evaluation. The child learns what to expect

/1 C! ......J •••••

from him/herself once

been established.

this')

f~ework

PAGE 19

of self evaluation: has,

Similarly, when a child receives a toy car for the first time,I

his expectations are virtually non existent and he can derive

little or no positive/negative affect from manipulating it until

such expectations are developed. If, while playing with a toy

for the first time, his expectations are confirmed, he. will

derive pleasure from the toy. If, however, there are too' many

non confirmations of his expectancies, (if the toy is too

complex), he will not derive pleasure from the toy. If one takes

this example to its logical conclusion, the reason why an adult

does not play with toys is because his expectations become

certainties, confirmation becomes 100% and the ad~lt gets bbred.

In other words, discrepancies from certainty are no longer

sufficient to yield pleasure. In this respect, McClelland is

known as an optimal level theorist, in the sense that poad t.i ve

affect is aroused when stimulation is moderately discrepant

from the AL or from what the person expects. On the other hand,

negative affect results from a large discrepancy in these

factors.

McClelland maintains that as far as the achievement motive is

concerned, there are limits placed on the individual by his

intelligence. For example, the possibilities of a toy will never

be exhausted as far as a moron is concerned, because the

possibil i ties are never certain enough for him/her to be bored

over trying them out. Thus, one would expect some kind of

correlation between the mastery level involved in nAch (need

for achievement) for a given individual and one's intelligence.

There are also limits placed on the development of nAch by the

negative affect which results from too large discrepancies

between expectations and events. For example, if a child's

expectations of a toy are not confirmed at' all, (either the

toy is too simplistic or too complex), the result is negative

affect, and cues associated with these activi t i es evoke

/20 .....

\

PAGE 20

avoidance motives. To develop the achievement approach motive,

parents should provide opportunities for their children, which

are just beyond the child's present knowledge. In summary, for

the development of the achievement motive, it is necessary that

the individual must have a certain level of intelligence and the

discrepancies between expectations and events must provide for an

achievement motivated approach. It is also necessary that the

individual develops some framework of sel f' evaluation. For the

development of stronger achievement motives, children need some

structuring of performance standards,()~9me demands by their

parents and surrounding culture. In McClelland's words, this

process can be stated as follows: "The child must begin to

perceive performance in terms of standards of excellence so

that discrepancies of various sorts from this perceptual frame of

reference (adaptation 1 evel ) can produce posi tive or negative

affect. II (p. 79). (McClelland, 1955, p , 79)

2.4.2.8 Measurement of nAch by the TAT

For some people, a situation in which one's performance can be

evaluated causes negative affect and as a result, withdrawal and

subsequent avoidance responses occur. The person will, as a

resul t, not be motivated to enter such a situation. For other

people, such a situation causes positive affect. For them, an

evaluation of performance leads to an approach response and as a

resul t, this individual will be motivated to enter into such a

si tuation again. McClelland felt that the TAT would be able to

detect such individual differences.

McClelland chose the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), developed

by Henry Murray, to assess individual dif:ferences in the motive

to enter an achievement situation. Murray called this motive the

"need to achieve", and it has become abbr-ev iated as nAch.

Murray had constructed the TAT to measure "psychogenic" needs

/21 .....

PAGE·21

Vof the personality. Subjects were asked to write stories about

pictures shown to them. Each picture was so ambiguous that it did

not suggest one dominant theme. Instead, the theme of the story

was generated by the subject's personality. In other words, the

TAT is a projective test in the sense that the subject

pr9jected his/her own needs into the story. Murray believed that

the subject's needs could be accurately assessed by careful

examination of the story told in response to an ambiguous TAT

picture.

There has been considerable disenchantment with the validity of

the nAch score. Klinger (1966) concluded that nAch scores do not

consti tute an adequate measure of motivation. Similarly,

Entwistle (1972) maintains that the internal consistency

(homogeneity reliability) of the TAT is an abysmal 0,35 and

test/retest reliability is low.

2.4.3 Atkinson

2.4.3.1 Introduction: Main concepts

Achievement related behaviour is conceptualised as resultant of a

conflict situation, in which the two. conflicting behaviours .ar-e

the motivation for success (Ms) and the motive to avoid failure

(Maf). These two motives have been discussed separately in

theory, but in real ity , all peopl e have both motives to some

extent. The difference between the motive to achieve success (ll:s)

and the motive to avoid failure is called "resultant

achievement motivation". This conflict is resolved when the

individual takes a decision to act in a certain way (for

example, to approach an achievement related task)/or some

resolution is achieved. The above then, is in essence, the core

of Atkinson's theory, which will be discussed in detail.

Following this, elaborations to his theory will be discussed.

These include concepts such as perceived instrumentality (PI),

Inertial Tendency (Tg) and the motive to avoid success (M-s).

/22 ..

PAGE 22

Another of Atkinson's main premises is that the individual is

constantly active. The important issue for Atkinson is not the

cause of an initiation of activity or the cessation of activity,

but the cause of a change in activity. In his own words, he has

stated it as follows: "The psychology of motivation should be

primarily concerned with the problem of change in on-going

activi ty ." (Atkinson, 1964, p , 301).

\ 2.4.3.2 Motive to achieve success (Ms)

a) Introduction

Atkinson claims that the motivation to enter a particular

achievement situation is what is termed the tendency to achieve

success, abbreviated as Ts. This motivation is determined by

three factors, namely, the strength of a person's motive to

achieve success, known as Ms. Secondly, it is also comprised of

~he probability the person believes that he/she will success on a

particular task, which is called "probability of success", (Ps),

and thirdly, the incentive value of success (Is) on that

particular task (that which is derived from entering an

achievement related situation).

These three factors combine multiplicatively to determine the

strength of motivation to enter an achievement situation/the

tendency to achieve success (Ts). That is, the tendency to

achieve success (Ts) is equal to the motive to achieve success

(Ms), multiplied by the probability of success (Ps); multiplied

by the incentive value of success (Ts): Ts ~ Ms x Ps x Is. These

factors have been reviewed in more detail below.

:.

b) The tendency to achieve success (Ts)

In order to calculate Ts, (the motivation to enter a particular

achievement situation), one must first know something about that

individual's personali ty - namely, the strength of thi s indivi­

dual's motive to achieve success (Ms). Because this motive

/23 .....

\

PAGE 23

is a personality characteristic, it is ~ranssituational, meaning

that the individual carries it with him/her from one situation to

another. It is viewed by Atkinson as a movi tation to enter a

specific achievement situation, as opposed to Ms, which is a

motive that, as a personality characteristic, is relevant to all

achievement situations that an individual may enter. Ts motiva­

tion, therefore, has three components, one of which is Ms (the

motivation to achieve success).

c) The probability of success (Ps)

Unlike Ms (the motive to achieve success), Ps (the probability of

success) is very different and changes in every situation. Ps is

the individual's subjective probability of success in a given

situation. In other words, it is the individual's own perception

of his/her chances of success in a particular situation. It is

assessed anew each time the individual faces an achievement

situation and hereafter there occurs continual reassessmen~ once

the individual has entered an achievement situation.

d) The incentive value of success (Is)

The incentive value of success is that concession derived by an

individual from an achievement related situation. If a task is

very easy, the incentive value of success will be low, but if a

task is very difficult, the incentive value is said to be high.

The reason for an achievement task having low incentive value

(easy task) is due to a high probabil ity of success (Ps ) , It

follows therefore; that Is and Ps are i nver-se Iy proportional,

because the easier a task, the greater the chances of. success and

the lower the incentive value.

With the above theory in mind, Atkinson postulated that people

/24 .

PAGE 24

who have the motive to achieve success will tend to prefer tasks

of intermediate difficulty, where there is a 50% probability of

success (that is Ps = 0,5). In other words, high nAch

individuals experience highest incentive value of success (Is),

at a Ps of 0,5. Atkinson maintains that it is at this point that

an individual would be most efficient. If the individual is most

motivated at this stage, persistence level is also at its

maximum. This is what has become known as Atkinson's risk

preference theory, or level of aspiration theory.

When people with high Ms perceive an achievement situation, they

perceive the possibil i ty of attaining a success. They have a

motivation to approach such a situation, because it holds out the

possibili ty of successful accomplishment. On th'€! other hand,

there are individuals who view an achievement situation quite

differently - for them, it holds out the possibility of failure,

shame and humiliation. As a result, entering an achievement

:situation elicits anxiety and fear of failure in some people.

They are said to be high in the motive to avoid failure. (Mar),

and since an achievement situation can be failed, they tend to

avoid them.

2.4.3.3 The motive to avoid failure

a) Introduction

Just as Ms (the motive to achieve success) produces Ts (the

tendency to approach success), so does Maf (motive to avoid

failure) produce Taf (tendency to avoid failure). Taf inhibits Ts

because it inhibits an individual's entrance into an

achievement situation. Taf also acts to inhibit performance once

a person has entered an achievement situation (Atkinson, 1964).

b) The tendency to avoid failure (Taf)

The tendency to avoid failure (Taf) has three determinants.

/25 .....

\

2.4.3.4

PAGE 25

The first is the individual's Maf (motive to avoid failure),

while the second is the person's estimate of the probability of

failing (Pf) and the third component is the incentive value of

failure (If) (what failure means to the individual). The tendency

to avoid failure is equal to the motive to avoid failure,

multiplied by the probability of failure, multiplied by the

incentive value of failure. That is: Taf = Maf x Pf x If.

In general, the motive to avoid failure (Mar) produces some

tendency to avoid any achievement situation, regardless of the

probabili ty of failure (Pf). However, Atkinson claims that a

sttuat i on ' in which there is a 50% probabil i ty of failure (Pf =

0,5) is the most anxiety provoking for a Maf person.

Furthermore, the higher a person's Maf is, the more that

person's dislike for situations in which Pf = 0,5. This will

surpass his/her dislike for situations in which Pf is some other

number. Usually these individuals select tasks with a very high

pr-obab i Li ty of failure (because failing at an extremely

difficult task is not humiliating) or a very easy task (where the

probabili ty of failure is low). Consequently, high Maf people

can escape some of their anxiety by choosing very difficult or

very easy tasks.

Resultant achievement motivation (Tr)

Since a person has both an Ms and Maf, every achievement

situation will elicit some Ts and some Taf. How the individual

approaches any given achievement situation depends on which

tendency is greater, Ts or Taf. In addition, the strength of this

moti ve toward or away from an achievement situation depends on

the amount by which the greater t.endency. exceeds the smaller

tendency. This is the very essence of the concept of resul tant

motivation. In other words, the resultant achievement motivation

(Tr) or the net tendency to approach an achievement situation is

equal to the magnitude of the tendency to approach the situation

minus the magnitude of the tendency to avoid it: Tr = Ts - Taf.

In equation form, resultant motivation is represented as follows

/26 .....

PAGE 26

Tr = (Ms x Ps x Is) + (Maf x If x -Pf) (Atkinson, (1964).

The more that Ms exceeds Maf, the more a person prefers

intermediate-difficul ty tasks, where the probability of success

is 50% (Ps = 0,5) (Atkinson, 1964).

Regardless of task difficulty, people in whom Maf exceeds Ms

have negative intrins ic (internal) motivation toward an

achievement situation. Therefore, they will not enter a

si tuation unless there is enough positive extrinsic (external)

motivation (such as money or status), to overcome their negative

intrinsic motivation. The equation Tr = Ts + Taf predicts only

intrinsic motivation - the tendency to approach an achievement

si tuation, independent of any "extra added incentives". Thus,

the total resultant motivation is represented in equation form

as follows: Tr (resultant motivation) = Ts (tendency to achieve

success) + Taf (tendency to avoid failure) + Text (motivation

due to external factors), or Tr = Ts + Taf + Text.

2.4.3.5 Critique and conclusion

One very severe limitation to Atkinson's theory is that it is

a stimulus bound theory of motivation - it encourages us to

think of the immediate stimulus situation as the cause of

arousal of a tendency to act in a certain way. This "cause and

effect" manner of looking at motivation is very simplistic

because it dismisses the cybernetic feedback cycles which are

characteristic of virtually all behaviour (Weiner, 1972).

The impl ication is that without any stimulus to set things in

motion, one would have no motivating tendencies. This approach

is analogous to the stimulus-response (S-R) approach of early

behaviourists. The author of this thesis maintains that

cognitive factors are important mediating variables and

therefore warrant discussion. The cognitive approach will be

presented in the next section.

/27 .....

\

PAGE 27

Atkinson realised that to view the immediate environmental

stimulus as the cause of a response was inadequate. It therefore

becomes necessary to view the particular S-R event as an

incident within a goal-directed trend of behaviour, the episode

having a beginning (a state of motivation), a middle (goal­

striving activity) and an end (attainment of goal, and cessation

of striving). The anticipated goal is also seen as one of the

determinants of the response. This approach is also partially

inadequate because it assumes that what the subject is doing

immediately before the episode is responsible for the initiation

of the entire episode. It also assumes that what the subject does

immediately after the episode has no influence on behaviour of- .

the individual during the episode.

Atkinson maintains that the difficulty lies in a failure to

appreciate what it means to acknowledge that a living organism is

constantly active - that is, an individual's behaviour is always

seen as an expression of the then dominant tendency - even when

the individual is asleep or resting. Resting (inhibi tion of

a response) and sleeping are seen as "motivated" activity.

Atkinson maintains that we should look at the motivation of

an individual as a continuously rolling ball.

Another criticism against Atkinson's theory is that he assumes

that Maf ;> Ms individuals are very anxious, due to the

possibility of failure and consequent humiliation. He has

neglected the fact that Ms > Maf individuals may well enter an

achievement situation and whilst still in it, experience anxiety

due to the possiblity of failure and consequent humiliation.

The author of this thesis maintains that"- this is especially

relevant to the university student, whose symptom is expressed

examination anxiety.

Conclusion

Atkinson maintains that an adequate psychology of motivation

/28 .....

PAGE 28

will one day provide for an integration of the two disciplines

of scientific psychology: the one concerned with individual

differences in personality, and the other, concerned with basic

processes.

2.4.3.6 Elaborations of Atkinson's theory

The theory of achievement motivation has been elaborated upon

during the last decade and most of these elaborations consist

in altering the original equation so that it will remain

applicable under varying

elaborations have been

conditions. Three

included, namely,

essential

perceivedi

instrumentality (Raynor, 1970), inertial tendency (Weiner,

success (Horner, 1972 ) .limited research on these

included because new! and!

1965) and the motive to avoid

Al though as yet there is still

elaborations, they have been

e controversial ideas are critical to the dynamic growth of anyi

field in psychology.

a) Perceived instrumentality refers to the perceived usefu~ness,

of a task in achieving a goaL For example, a course in organic

chemistry is vi tal if one' 5 goal is to become a chemist; but

irrelevant if one I s goal is to become a personnel manager.

Achievement motivation theory predicts that Ms, Maf and Ps ~ould,

be identical and therefore, motivation should be Lderrt Lc a I in

both situations. Raynor (1970) recognised this weakness in

Atkinson's theory and proposed an elaboration of it to take

(incentive

,into account the importance of an achievement activity in lone's

Ichemi st , aslong-range goals. Therefore, for the potential

he/she ponders the Ps (probability of success) and Is

value of success) for the first time, the Ps and Is of all

subsequent biochemistry courses are important. Each new: stepI

is thus contingent on successful compl etion of the prior !step.

This is termed a multistep contingent pathway of which the

opposi te is called a one-step path. All biochemistry courses

are said to be instrumental in achieving the final goal.

/29 .....

PAGE 29

For the Ms :> Maf individual, Tr (resultant achievement

motivation) will always be greater in a multistep contingent path

than in a one-step path. The prediction for the Maf person

is exactly the opposite in the sense that when such an individual

perceives a task as being instrumental in attaining access to

several other achievement tasks, his motivation decreases.

Consequently, a contingent path is more threatening to the Maf'>

Ms individual than a non-contingent path.

Even though a contingent path is more threatening to a Maf::::::'

Ms individual, a contingent path may have some external extrinsic

reward associated with its successful completion. This extrinsic

reward may be so great as to overcome one I s negat.ive intrinsic

motivation, thereby falsifying the prediction that Maf > Ms

individuals prefer a non-contingent path.

'Raynor (1970) believes that this elaboration of achievement

theory is important because it allows for the analysis of real­

life situations not treated adequately by the original

McClelland/Atkinson model. Perhaps the results 'found by Atkinson

and McClelland were confounded by the fact that in the

laboratory, perceived instrumentality is zero, for the subject

perceives the task not to be instrumental in any way to one's

'future plans. It is when the subject undertakes tasks cr'uc i.a I to

his future goals that the theory of perceived instrumentality

makes accurate predictions.

b) Inertial tendency (Tg). The persistence of an unfulfilled wish

is a phenomenon which can be traced back to the works of Freud in

the late 19th century. More recently, a car~ful, analysis of it

was done by Lewin (1951) and his students, who termed it the

Zeigarnik effect. Greater recall of ,unfinished tasks is the core

of the Zeigarnik effect. More recently, Weiner (1965) has

extended this theory to the field of achievement motivation.

/30 .....

PAGE 30

Weiner proposed that success completes the tendency to attain

an achievement related goal, whereas failure allows the

tendency to continue uncompleted. Consequently, if someone fails

. at a task, the resultant tendency will persist following that

failure and will add to the motivation for that task when it

is attempted again. As a result, every time a task is failed,

two adjustments must be made to the resultant-tendency formula:

firstly, Ps (probability of success) is lowered as the

individual realises that the task is more difficult than

initially anticipated. Secondly, in terms of what has been

discussed above, all the motivation of the previous unsuccessful

attempts are added to the resultant tendency f?r the present

trial •. The motivation remaining from an unsuccessful trial and

its resultant added motivation on the following trial is what

is called inertial tendency, abbreviated as Tg. Weiner maintains

that when Tg is taken into account, the formula for resultant

tendency is as follows:

Tr = (Ms x Ps x Is) + (Maf x If x -Pf) + Tg

In other words, resultant motivation is the motivation to

achieve success added to the motivation to avoid failure plus

inertial tendency .

. Bearing the Weiner's inertial-tendency elaboration in mind,

he predicted that Maf > Ms individuals do more poorly: as the ­

number of failures increase because the individual faces the

negative inertial tendency of the prior unsuccessful attempt

plus the negative tendency (Mar) of the present trial.

Consequently, as the number of failures increase, so the

negative motivation increases. Similarly, :Ms :> Maf individuals

should show an increase in motivation following failure because

of the added motivation of previous unsuccessful attempts

(inertial tendency) plus their motivation to succeed (Ms).

c) The motive to avoid success (M-s). Most studies derived from

the theory of achievement motivation have been carried out on

/31 ....•

PAGE 31

male samples only and those studies which have utilised female

subjects, have yielded results which are different and

inconsistent to those for males (Sarason & Smith,1971). It

appears that the concepts of Ms and Maf are insufficient to

explain achievement related behaviour in women.

This deficiency in achievement motivation theory prompted Horner

(1968, in Horner, 1972) to postulate a theory of motivation to

avoid success, (abbreviated as M-s). Horner views the motive to

avoid success as a latent, stable personality characteristic that

is established early in one's development, specifically during

development of the female's sex role identity. : Horner (1968),

has defined the motive to avoid success as "a predisposition to

become anxious in achievement-orientated situations that are

inherently competitive and therefore are not "feminine" (p. 216).

Horner's rationale is that women, more than men, are soCialised

to value social relationships and affiliation, and a

competitive situation creates a conflict that threatens her sex

role identity. As a result, the resultant-tendency equation for

women woul d be al tered as follows to accommodate the motive to

avoid success:

Tr = ()Ms x Ps x Is» + «Maf x If x -Pf») - M-s.

This concept proposes that females have a conflict between

motivation to succeed and sex role identity, to the extent that

one's sex role identity may inhibit achievement related

pursuits. As a result, the resultant-tendency equation would be

altered as follows to accommodate the motive to avoid success:

Tr = «(Ms x Ps x Is») + ((Maf + Pf + If) - M-s.

In other words, resultant achievement motiVation is equal to the

motive to achieve success plus the motive to avoid failure minus

the motive to avoid success.

2.4.4 An attributional interpretation of expectancy-value theory

A cognitive approach to achievement motivation theory

/32 .....

PAGE 32

TABLE A

A general overview o~ the attributional interpretation

o~ expectancy-value theory.

ANTECEDENTS

OF CAUSAL

ASCRIPTION

CAUSAL ASCRIPTION CONSEQUENCES

(What influences

perception

of the reasons

~or success and

f'af Lure )

specific cues

belief systems

individual

predispositions

(The perceived reasons (The consequences'

for success and failure) of perceived success/

ability failure)

- effort - performance

- task difficulty behaviour

- luck

Weiner (1974) maintains that all individuals perceive the causes of

success' and failure in different ways. This is a subjective judgement

process and technically is known as causal ascription of success and

failure. The perceived reasons for success and failure include ability,

effort, task difficulty and luck, as well as less common'ascriptions

like mood, fatigue and illness (antecedents). Those factors which

influence one's perception of the reasons for success and failure

include specific cues such as past history, sot'ial norms and task

diffi culty. In addi tion to specific cues, cogni tive structures, such as

belief systems (causal schemata) and individual disposition also playa

role. Weiner believes that these cognitive processes and structures have

been neglected by expectancy-value theorists ..

How one perceives the causes of success and failure will determine what

goal one sets out to achieve (for example, I have the necessary ability

to achieve an honours degree in Sociology), and what emotional reactions

will occur if this goal is reached (pride) or whether it is not

(humiliation). Consequently, causal ascriptions influence goal anticipa~

tions and affective (emotional) consequences of success and failure.

/33 .....

PAGE 33

Thus, causal ascriptions f'orm a- very basic for performance behaviour.

These are known as the consequences of causal ascription.

2.4.4.1 The perceived causes of success and failure (causal ascription)

Weiner (1974) postulates that individuals utilise four elements

in the process of causal ascription to interpret and predict an

achievement-related event. The four elements are ability, effort,

task difficulty and luck. This means that, in attempting to

explain a prior success or failure of an achievement related

task, one assesses one's own ability level, the amount of effort

that is required, the difficulty of the task and the magnitude

and direction of one I s experienced luck. These are the four

causal sources of any achievement-related task. Similarly,

future expectations of success and failure will depend on

previous experience with achievement outcomes.

Weiner, Heckhausan, Meyer and Cook (1972) have placed the causes

of success and failure within a two-dimensional taxonomy.

Ability and effort are properties internal to the person, while

task difficulty and luck are external factors. This is known as

the internal-external dimension. In addition, ability and task

difficulty are relatively stable factors, (known as invariable

factors), whereas luck and effort are ever-changing factors

(variable). The diagram below indicates this 2 x 2 ; classifica­

tion scheme.

TABLE B

Weiner et al (1972) two-dimensional

taxonomy of causal ascription.

Internal

Stable ability

Unstable effort

External

task difficulty

luck

As shown in this table, ability is an internal, stable cause;

effort is an internal unstable cause; task difficulty is

external and stable and luck is external and unstable.

/34 .• ~ ..

PAGE 34

2.4.~.2 Antecedents of causal ascription

Weiner maintains that those factors that influence one t s

perception of the reasons for success and failure in~lude

specific cues, belief systems and individual predispositions~

Specific cues

I

The main antecedent cues for causal judgements are said to be

specific information including one's past outcome history,

social norms, task difficulty and luck.

iIf the individual has a past history of succ~ss, then : this

individual will tend to enter an achievement related task in

a positive manner, whereas if past history is one of failure,

one will be inclined to expect failure at an achievement related

task. Repeated success or failure indicates whether' the

individual "can" or "cannot".

Outcome information, together with social norms is often used

to infer one's ability level. For example, if one succeeds at

a task where all others fail, then one is likely to be perceived

as very able. Task difficulty is often inferred from the

prevailing social norm as well as from specific task

characteristics (for example, steepness of a mount.afn )", Weiner

(1974) has found that the greater the percentage of others

succeeding at a task, the more likely that a given success will

be ascribed to the ease of the task. Similarly, the greater

the percentage of others failing at a task, the more likely

that a given failure will be attributed. to the difficulty of

the task. Finally, luck is perceived as an apparent lack of

personal control over the outcome of a task. For example , the

number rolled on a die will be ascribed to chance, but the

repeated appearance of the same number suggests personal control

over the outcome a~d would produce ascriptions to ability.

/35 .....

PAGE 35

In addition to specific cues, Weiner (1974) maintains that

causal schemata (known as belief systems) also influences the

judgement process. Kelley (1972) has defined a causal schemata as

one's belief system about the relationship between an observed

event (for example, graduation) and the perceived causes of that

event (for example, hard work and ability).

Bearing the above in mind, it is natural that there are

individual differences in causal preferences. Some individuals

readily explain success to luck, while others are inclined to

attribute it to ability or hard work. The determinants of causal

ascription can, therefore, be attributed to ~any factors.

Usually, individuals combine and synthesize all the above

information and thereby reach r eI iable causal judgements (Frieze

and Weiner, (1971».

2.4.4.3 The consequences of causal ascription

The perceived reasons for success and failure (causal

ascription) does influence the individual's future goal

expectancy as well as how that individual feels as a result of

having succeeded or failed. Thus, causal ascription will

influence how the individual performs in an achievement related

task because one will have set a goal which is in keep i.ng with

what one expects to achieve as well as one's feelings about one's

chances of success/failure considering prior experiences.

a) Causal ascriptions and goal expectancy

Thus, one can say that the perceived reasons for success and

fail ure do have an effect on future goal attainment. That is,

causal ascription has direct bearing on one's goal expectancy.

For example, following success, expectancy generally rises while

after failure, it usually drops. Thus, after success or failure,

there occurs what is called an expectancy shift.

/36 ..•..

PAGE 36

b) Causal ascriptions and affective reactions

The perceived reasons for success and failure also influence how

the individual will feel once success or failure occurs. This is

Weiner's concept of causal ascription and affective reactions.

Weiner (1974) maintains that pride and shame are maximised when

achievement outcomes are ascribed internally and minimised when

success and failure are attributed to external causes. Thus,

success attributed to high ability or hard work is expected to

produce more pride than success that is due to luck or an easy

task. Similarly, failure perceived as due to low ability or ~ack!

of effort is expected to result in greater shame than failure

that is attributed to a difficult task or bad luck. In sum,

Weiner puts it as follows (in Weiner, 1974): "locus of

causali ty influences the affective consequences of

; achievement behaviours" ( 61)p. .

2.4.4.4 A brief evaluation of Weiner's cognitive approach

It is Weiner's basic argument that the high and low need

achievement individuals differ significantly in that the high

nAch person is more likely to attribute success to his own

efforts. This would, in turn, be more likely to lead to

achievement behaviour in the future and so the cycle would

repeat itself. The author of this thesis maintains that Weiner's

approach takes into account the cybernetic feedback system and

that this is one of the few theories which has ·this redeeming

feature. On the other hand, one must also be aware that this

approach is incapable of explaining achievement behaviour on its

own. Furthermore, one should also never view theories of.moti vation as a static body of 1 i t.er-a tur-e , but as a constantly

changing, dynamic framework which is by no means a complete or

perfect system.

/37 .....

\

PAGE 37

2.4.5 Hermans

2.4.5.1 Introduction

Hermans, Bergen & Eijssen (1982) have not proposed a theory of

achievement motivation, but their ideas on anxiety (angst) have

been presented here because they represent a refinement and

extension of McClelland's Ts and Taf persons.

Hermans et. al. (1982) maintain that failure anxiety is a normal

characteristic of human existence. They also maintain that the

tendency to avoid failure (Taf) can be divided into positive

failure anxiety (positiewe faalangs) and negative failure

anxiety (negatiewe faalangs). Positive failure anxiety enables

the individual to function optimally in a stressful situation,

whereas negative failure anxiety inhibits the individual's

;optimalfunctioning in a stressful situation. Furthermore, the

individual's feelings of being unsure in a stressful situation

and the subsequent decrease in achievement is another aspect of

negative failure anxiety, whereas the existence of a

stressful situation and the accompanying increase in achievement

is known as positive failure anxiety. Positive failure anxiety,

according to Hermans et. al., is synonomous with responsibility

(verantwoordelikheid) and such individuals are said to over­

achieve and are known as motivated, whereas negative' failure

anxiety individuals are known as under-achievers.

2.4.5~2 Characteristics of negative failure-anxious individuals

Whether in fact negative failure-anxiety becomes expressed,

depends on the personality structure of the individual. Such a

person, according to Hermans et. al., has a need for preciseness

in the sense that one's work must be divided into small steps.

This seems to be in keeping with Atkinson's concept of the non­

contingent pathway, in the sense that the individual copes

inadequately with behaviour which has no observable structure in

sight. In addition, a failure anxious person is said to be

/38 .....

PAGE 38

dependent on others for continual feedback on how one is coping

with the task. Another characteristic of failure-anxious

individuals is that they have a need for warm personal relations

especially a warm and informal work climate. They are said to

function at their worst in a cold and formal work climate. In

addi tion, such an individual usually has a large discrepancy

between self concept (the way in which one sees oneself) and

ideal concept (the way in which one would like to see oneself).

Lastly, these individuals have a need for positive expectations

about their work from others. This ties in with what has been

said above, in the sense that any feelings of doubt result in

decreased performance. The individual which Hermans calls a

negative failure-anxious individual, is similar to the Taf

person described by McClelland, but, we should bear in mind that

the individual described above is no less intelligent than the Ts

or the positive failure-anxious individual.

2.4.5.3 Characteristics of positive failure-anxious individuals

As opposed to the negative failure-anxious individual, these

people are independent, ·do not need constant feedback regarding

progress and work best in a cold and formal work climate.

Usually, there is little discrepancy be tween self-concept and

ideal self-concept, and thus, positive failure anxious indivi­

duals are said to be continually striving for progress.

Furthermore, these individuals are said to have a future directed

time perspective and are task orientated.

2.4.5.4 Conclusion and critique

Hermans e t , al . have provided more insight into McClelland's Ts

and Taf individual in the sense that per-sonal Lty variables as

they interact with such a disposition are the key factors to the

expression of success or failure. On the other hand, the author

of this thesis maintains that Hermans et. al. have differentiated

their concepts of negative and positive failure-

/39 .....

PAGE 39

anxiety too rigidly and simplistically. Take for example the

positive failure-anxious individual, who takes responsibility

for his/her tasks and who is achievement orientated but who

does need constant feedback on progress or who works best in

a warm work climate. At this point, it would be useful to

mention Alpert and Haber's (1960) concepts of facilitating and

debilitating anxiety (in van Rooyen, 1983). The point to be

made here is that the positive failure-anxious .individual may

have subtle elements of debilitating anxiety and similarly,

the negative failure-anxious individual may have accompanying

elements of facilitating anxiety.

/L10• • • • •

CHAPTER 3 EMPIRICAL RESEARCH FINDINGS

PAGE 40

3.1 INTRODUCTION CHAPTER OUTLINE

The research presented in Chapter 3 is essentially that which is availabl~ on

the theory discussed in Chapter 2. Firstly, the research which is available

on the achievement motivation theorists has been discussed and following

\ this, research on academic achievement within a university context has been

dealt with. With the theory and research in mind, central ideas have geen

extracted and these have been presented as hypotheses in the follo~ing

chapter.

3.2 RESEARCH ON ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT IN A UNIVERSITY SETTING

As mentioned before, there are many factors which influence academic

achievement and for the purpose of this study, the research on the

following variables has been

atti tudes, personality factors,

tion.

2.1 Intelligence

included: intelligence, study habits 'and

adjustment, anxiety and achievement mot1va-

The research on the effect of intelligence on academic achievement

centres around whether IQ is capable of differentiating between high and

low achieving students and secondly, whether IQ is a good predictor of

academic achievement.

Some researchers have found a significant positive correlation between

IQ and academic achievement, while others have found contradictory

results to these. Savage (1962) found that h i gh achieving medical

students could not be differentiated from low achieving students on the

basis of intelligence test scores. Similar results have been found by

Roe (1953) and MacKinnon (1962). Similarly, it was concluded in a study

by Pentony & Loftus (1970) that students with high intell igence test

scores are not necessarily those who achieved the highest academically.

Identical results have been found by Anderson (1960) and Hudson {1964).

From the results above, it appears that 1Q and academic achievement are

not necessarily positively correlated and as a result, 1Q is not always

a good predictor of academic achievement.

/41 .....

PAGE 41

On the other hand, research completed by Sinha (1966) yielded results

contradictory to the above. This researcher found that high intelligence

scores were significantly positively related to academic achievement and

that IQ is capable of differentiating between high and low achievers.

Similar results have been documented by Eno (1978). This researcher

found that crystalised intelligence was significantly positively

correlated with acadamic achievement. From the research above, it seems

that IQ is positively correlated with academic achievement and as a

result, IQ is a good predictor of academic achievement.

Botha (1971) has come to the conclusion that IQ is capable of

explaining only a small percentage of variance in university academic

achievement. From the research above, it appears that IQ alone is

incapable of differentiating between high and low achievers. As a

resul t, whether IQ is capable of predicting academic achievement is

still debatable.

More specifically, research on the effect of IQ on achievement

motivation indicates that nAch was signi ficantly positively cor-r-el at.ed

with verbal and non-verbal measures of intelligence (Hundal and Jerath,

1972). Thus, it appears that it is the more intelligent student who is

more motivated.

3.2.2 Study habits and attitudes

The literature in Chapter 2 demonstrates that most research centres

around the correlation between academic achievement and study habits and

attitudes. However, there is still some diversion of opinion.

There are various researchers who have conclud~d that there is a

posi tive correlation between academic achievement and study habi ts and

attitudes. These include Wankowski & Cox 1973, Bednar & Weinberg 1970,

Bruwer, 1973, Blumberg, 1969; and Enge 1brecht, 1974. Hol tzman & Brown

(1953) have also found a posi tive correlation between study habits and

attitudes and academic achievement. On the other hand

. /42 .•...

\

PAGE 42

other researchers have a different perspective. They have found that

there is not necessarily a positive correlation between good grades and

study habits and attitudes. Goldfried and D'Zurilla (1973) maintain that

they did not find a positive correlation between academic achievement

(good grades) and study habits and attitudes. They maintain that good

grades are not always a direct consequence of effective study habits.

Similar results have been found by Glock, Ahmann & Smith (1958).

In general, it appears that the majority of research points to a

positive correlation between academic achievement and study habits and

attitudes. However, one must bear in mind that there are differing and

diverse perspectives, because academic achievement is a function of many

factors.

More specifically, research also indicates that effective study habits

are significantly positively correlated with achievement motivation. It

seems logical that the more motivated a student is, the more

attention he/she will pay to effective study habits. Empirical evidence

documenting this has been completed by Zarb (1981) and Wal~erg &Uguroglu (1979).

From the research above,' it appears that empirical evidence points to a

posi tive correlation between academic achievement· and study habits and

attitudes as well as achievement motivation and study habits and

attitudes.

3.2.3 Personality factors

Research on the influence of personality factors: on academic

achievement have concentrated largely on the distinction between

introversion - extroversion and neuroticism and" other miscellaneous

personality factors will be discussed.

/43 .....

PAGE 43

It is clear that extroverts tend to do less well academically at the

university level (Furneaux, 1962; Entwistle, 1972; Banks & Finlayson,

1973; Gotz & Gotz, 1973 Shadbolt, 1978; Entwistle, 1972; Seddan, 1975;

Mann & Rizzo, 1972; Orpen, 1976; Goh & Moore, 1978). Entwistle, (1972)

found that extroverts, particularly stable extroverts, were superior to

introverts up until the age of 14 but that introverts tend to achieve

higher academically than extroverts beyond this age.

An interesting perspective has been added by Entwistle & Brennan (1971)

who have used cluster analysis to compare similarities between people.

These researchers identified twelve personality clusters which were

associated with high and low achievement. Interestingly,

personali ty variables like optimism and dependence usually associated

with academic under-achievement, if combined with factors associated

with academic achievement, like openmindedness and introversion, could

resul t in academic achievement. Similarly, Middleton & Guthrie (1'959)

showed that college success may be achieved in a variety of ways by a

variety'of personality types.

Other personality factors associated with academic achievement are a

tendency to be serious, hardworking and consistent (Romine & Crowell,

1981. Jones (1955) and Lavin (1965) have concluded that flexibility is

associated with academic achievement. Vaughn (1968) found that academic

achievers tend to be persistent, have a deep emotional commitment to

education and are flexible, whereas Wankowski and Cox (1973) maintain

that the academic achiever is independent. In addition, O'Shea (1970)

claims that the academic achiever is hardworking, has a phlegmatic

temperament, is independent, self controlled, sensitive, compliant and

has ego strength.

The research on neuroticism is contradictory. Wi Lson (1973, in Weiner,

1974) found that students who performed well at university were likely

to score high on neuroticism and motivation and s1 ight1y lower on

extroversion than students who were under-achieving. Results contradic-, l

tory to these were found by Wankowski and Cox (1973) and Ry1e (1968).

/44 .....

PAGE 44

These. researchers found that neurotic students tend to achieve less

academically than non-neurotic students. Saenger-Ceha (1970) has offered

an explanation for these contradictory findings on neuroticism. This

researcher claims that certain types of neurotic traits drive the

individual to supreme efforts, while other types inhibit his/her

performance. The explanation is compatible with the theory presented by

Hermans et. al. (1982), who claim that positive failure-anxiety may

drive the student to over-achieve, whereas negative failure-anxiety may

result in under-achievement.

3.2.4 Anxiety

The theory on the effect of anxiety on academic achievement is in a

state of flux, due to the fact that anxiety has been differentiated into

different components by Hermans et. al. (1982), and Barling & Beattie

(1982). Research on the effect of anxiety on academic achievement in

essence shows that anxiety and academic achievement are negatively

related~ In other words, the more anxious the individual, the less well

he/she achieves academically (Grooms & Endler, 1960; Stevenson &. Iscoe

1956; Harlston 1962; Sinick 1956; Endler 1964; Buchin 1966; Sarason &

Mandler 1952; Bernstein 1958; Heald 1970; Russell & Sarason 1965;

Runkel 1959; Engelbrecht 1974 and Marais 1981). In a similar line of

research (Farber & Spence, (1955, in Beard 1980) found that a high

anxiety level facilitates the learning of a simple task, but that it has

the opposite effect when the task is complex.

3.2.5 Adjustment

In this section, the research on adjustment has been .r-evi ewed and

included under the heading of adjustment is that of the self concept.

The research indicates that adjustment is positively correlated with

academic achievement (Jensen 1958; Berger & Sutker 1956; Hoyt & Norman

1954; Yeomons & Lundin 1957 and Burgess 1956).

/45 .....

\

PAGE 45

In a similar vein, there appears to be a positive correlation between

positive self concept and academic achievement (FarIs, 1967; Purkey

1970; Tuel & Wursten 1965; Bad Ley 1971 and Engelbrecht 1973). The above

research seems to lend support to the theory that a posi tive self

concept and more specifically, a positive academic self concept is

positively correlated with academic achievement.

3.2.6 Achievement motivation

The theory on the effect of achievement motivation on academic

achievement reveals that there is a positive correlation between these

variables. In other words, students who are most motivated, tend to

achieve more academically than unmotivated students. A posi ti ve

correlation between achievement motivation and academic achievement has

been found in the following studies: (Wankowski & Cox 1973; Walberg &

Uguroglu 1979; Entwistle, Entwistle & Cowell 1971; Heckhausen 1967;

McClelland, Atkinson, Clark & Lowell 1976; Cattell, Sealy & Sweeney

1966; Danesino & Layman 1969; Green & Farquhar 1965; Bendig 1958 and

Morgan 1952). Another perspective has been added by Botha (1971), who

has shown that highly motivated subjects may have a greater fear of

failure than unmotivated students and that this may detract from

achievement. This may explain a possible negative relationship between

achievement motivation and academic achievement. Hence, it makes good

sense that Malan (1978) and Schoeman (1976) conclude that the

relationship between achievement motivation and academic achievement is

a complex one and warn against over-simpl ification of its. study. In a

similar line of thought, van Dyk (1978) also maintains that achievement

motivation is a difficult variable to measure due to its complexity.

/46 .....

THEORIES OF MOTIVATION

3.3.1 McClelland

CURRENT RESEARCH

PAGE 46

The research on McClelland's work centres around his theories that the

achievement motive is learned and secondly, that achievement motives

have an effect on society.

A number of studies have investigated McClelland I s theory that' the

achievement motive is learned. McClelland and Friedman (1952) found a

significant positive correlation between independence training and nAch

(need for achievement) in children. Similar results were obtained by

McClelland, Rindlisbacher and de Charms (1955). They add that the sex

and educational level of the parents were significantly related to

the age of expected independence training - the highly educated,

especially mothers, expected independence at an early age. In a sim~lar;

line of research, additional perspective is added by Winterbottom (l~68,

in Beard, 1980) who concluded that the mothers of the high and low nAch

boys did not differ in the number of demands for independence, but that

the mothers of high nAch boys made these demands earlier tha,n the

mothers of low nAch boys.

McClelland maintained that if the concept of achievement motivation is

to be useful, it must be clearly measurable.

The first task in devising a method of measuring motivation was to vary

the intensity of a human motive and to measure its effects on

imagination or fantasy. Just as Hull (1943) had experimentally

manipulated drive states in animals, McClelland began by experimentally

manipulating the strength of food motivation in humans. He obtained TAT

stories from gr-oups of navy men who differed in the number of hours

during which they had gone without food. This experiment showed that,

different degrees of hunger were reflected in different amounts of food

imagery in the TAT stories. In other words, fantasy TAT stories could be

used to measure the strength of motivation (Atkinson & McClelland,

1948) .

/47 .....

PAGE 47

McClelland then used the same strategy to study achievement motivation.

The intensity of achievement motivation was varied by giving different

instructions to groups of individuals just before they wrote their TAT

stories. One group was told that people who did well on the fantasy test

were creative, intelligent leaders. The specific kinds of thought which

were present in the achievement group TAT's became the operational

definition of achievement motivation (McClelland, Atkinson, Clark &

Lowell, 1976).

Subsequent research with this measure of achievement motivation

indicated that individuals with high nAch showed certain consistent

patterns of behaviour:

1. High nAch individuals preferred situations in which they had

personel responsibility for the outcomes of events rather than

situations, like gambling, where events were determined by

chance (McClelland, Atkinson, Clark & Lowell, 1953).

2. They chose goals for themselves that were realistic but

challenging as opposed to goals that were either too easy or too

risky (McClelland 1955; Atkinson & Litwin, 1960).

3. They sought situations where they could obtain immediate

concrete feedbacks to determine how well they were doing (Moss &

Kagan, 1961).

These findings, corroborated by other research in such diverse areas as

child-rearing and management, began to indicate that high

achievement motivation was a key factor in the successful strivings for

excellence which characterise the entrepreneur. Achievement

motivation also appeared to influence task-oriented behaviour. This has

been researched by Lowell (1952, in Arkes & Garske, 1977), who assigned

subjects to two experimental groups depending upori" whether they scored

high or low in achieve .•ierrt motivation as reflected in six imaginary

stori es tol d by the subj ects. The groups were then given the task of

/48 .....

\

PAGE 48

unscrambl ing and rearranging letters into meaningful words. The group

that scored high in achievement motivation performed at a significantly

higher level on this task than the group that scored low in achievement

motivation.

One of the most intriguing applications of TAT measures has been the

scoring of U.S. Presidents' nAch in their inaugural addresses (Donley &

Winter (1970, in Arkes & Garske, 1977). According to these researchers,

presidents who were high in both power and achievement motives tended to

be strong and active. Such presidents included John Kennedy, Lyndon

Johnson and Franklin Roosevelt. Presidents who were high in achievement

motivation but low in power, tried to bring about great accomplishments

but lack the political skills to accomplish their goals; -an example is

Herbert Hoover. Presidents who were higher in achievement motivation

than in power preferred taking moderate risks. It appeared that a

constant shift in cabinet personnel eroded their performance. Low

achievement orientated presidents, such as Eisenhower, did not attempt

any major accomplishment. Based on this scoring, Winter (1976),

predicted that Reagan's manner of governing would be most simil ar to

that displayed by Franklin Roosevelt, in the sense that his pattern of

motivation more closely resembl es Roosevel t ' s than it. does that of any

other president.

3.3.2 Atkinso.n

Various concepts of Atkinson's theory have been investigated. The

concepts that will be discussed include Atkinson's theory of risk

preferences, the incentive value of success, task performance

ef~iciency, persistence levels, and the educational implications of his

theory. Furthermore, elaborations of Atkinson's' theory have been

discussed. These include Raynor's concept of contingent pathways,

Weiner's concept of inertial tendency and Horner's concepts of motive to

avoid success.

/49 .....

PAGE 49

a) Risk preferences

Atkinson's achievement theory has received its strongest support in the

area of risk preferences. According to the literature, Atkinson

postulated that people who have the motive to achieve success will tend

to prefer intermediate-difficulty tasks. This has been

demonstrated by research completed by McClelland (1955) who showed that

high nAch boys preferred tasks of intermediate difficulty. Similar

results were obtained by Atkinson & Litwin (1960) in a study involving

male college students. Similarly, research completed by Mahone (1960)

and Morris (1966), found that the greater Ms is, the greater will be

one's preference for intermediate-difficulty tasks. Their studies

demonstrated that young adults who are high in nAch (that is, high in

Ms), also aspire towards vocations of intermediate difficulty).

Similar results have been found by Veroff & Peele (1969). In a

ring-to~s game, they found that subjects with high nAch, stand at an

intermediate distance from the ring, whereas subjects with a low nAch,

stand :.either very close to/very far away from the ring. Other studies

designed to test this same principle, have used academic setting. For

example, Isaacson (1964) has shown that Ms > Maf men choose college

majors of intermediate difficulty more often than Maf :> Ms men do.

Similarly, de Charms & Carpenter (1968) showed that Ms ~ Maf children

chose arithmetic probl ems of intermediate di.f'f i cu.I ty and Maf > Ms

children chose problems of extreme difficulty or extreme ease.

b) The incentive value of success

Atkinson t s theory predicts that the incentive val ue of success shoul d

increase steadily as a function of how unl Lke Ly- success is. Litwin

(1958, in Atkinson & Feather, 1966) found evidence that this is the

case. Male subjects were asked to assign a monetary value to success in

a ring-toss game. The money assigned to success at each distance was

linearly related to the probability of success, with the most money

being paid for success at the greatest distance.

/50 .•. . •

PAGE 50

c) Task performance ~fficiency

Atkinson's theory of task performance efficiency has been

investigated. There is some evidence that very high levels of

motivation produce a decrement in task performance-efficiency (Horner,

1972 ). Similarly, {Litwin, Dembo, Festinger & Seas (1944, in Arkes &

Garske, 1977) found results similar to those above: the higher the

difficulty level of an achievement related task, the greater the

incentive value of success.

d) Persistence level

Perhaps the most important study done to test the theory of persistence

as it relates to achievement motivation, was done by Feather (1961) who

measured the number of trials for which subjects would persist on an

impossible task. Feather found that Ms :> Maf subjects persisted longer

when the Ps was initially 0,7. As the subject begins to fail, the Ps

estimate approaches 0,5. This is the level at which the subject shows

maximal motivation and consequently, the subject persists. When the

initial Ps = O,5m, failure causes the Ps estimate to move farther from

the region of maximal motivation. The result is minimal

per-s i.s.tence . The results for the Maf > . Ms men were entirely the

opposite. As the subject fails a task, on which the Ps = 0,7, thereby

lowering the Ps estimate, the Ps will move into the region he most

dislikes - the intermediate difficulty region. As a result, the subject

shows little persistence on such a task. When the initial Ps is 0,5,

failure causes the Ps to become even more extreme. Since such subjects

do not find extreme values aversive, they persist with the task.

Success and fai lure experiences have been shown to affect how hard

subjects try the next time they attempt a similar task. Halisch &

Heckhausen, (1977, in Beard, 1980) have shown that in Ms ~ Maf

chi ldren, failure resulted in increased ef'for-t.j- whereas following

failure, Maf > Ms children decreased' their effort on a similar task.

These results have been explained by Weiner (1972), who claims that the

above effects are a result of the types of attributions people maKe for

their successes and failures. Avc i dance mot i vat.ed subjects blame their

failures on lack of ability, so failure makes them give up, whereas

approach-motivated subjects bl ame failure on lack of effort. Kukla,

I t: ~

.'..J_ ........

PAGE 51

(1977, in Beard, 1980) so these people double their efforts after a

failure. The reasoning is that approach motivated (Ms > Maf) subjects

tend to attribute success to ability, whereas avoidance motivated

subjects attribute success to lucki Berglas & Jones (1978, in Beard,

1980), have found that avoidance motivated (Maf ::> Ms) males who had

experienced success, due to 'luck' rather than effort, often choose to

'self handicap'. As a result, hindered performance may be explained in

terms of this factor. For example, such subj ects may choose to take

drugs that they know will hinder performance, rather than attributing

lack of performance to lack of ability.

e) Educational implications of Atkinson's theory

To some extent, the educational implications of Atkinson ':s theory have

been researched. Atkinson & O'Connor (1963, in Atkinson & Feather.1966)

found that if Ms::> Maf children are in a classroom situation in which

their Ps is very high, that is, far above 0,5, they will not be

maximally motivated. If they are placed in a class in which all the

children have about the same ability, their Ps being about 0,5, they

will :.be maximally motivated. This therefore, is evidence for

mainstreaming. On the other hand, Maf .> Ms children are maximally

inhibited in a class of their own ability level. This is rational if we

recall that Maf:> Ms subjects dislike tasks in which the Ps is about

0,5. In· other words, Ms .> Maf children are maximally motivated if

mainstreamed and Maf ~ Ms children are maximally inhibited if

mainstreamedand thereby placed in a classroom with others with

similar ability.

f) Extensions of Atkinson's theory

i) Contingent pathways

To some extent, the extensions made to Atkinson! s theory have been

researched, including the concepts of contingent pathways, inertial

tendency and motive to avoid success. This research confirms that these

152 •..•.

PAGE 52

extensions to his theory are indeed necessary and most important ­

they are val Ld , The extensions of Atkinson's theory have also been

researched. Raynor (1970) researched the concec'; of contingent

pathways. The abovementioned researcher found that Ms~ Maf psychology

students performed better when they perceived the psychology cause

to be instrumental to future plans (that is, the task was a multistep

contingent pathway). On the other hand, Maf ~ Ms students performed

worse under these circumstances. Similarly, results confirming the

above have been found by Entin & Raynor (1973).

ii) Inertial tendencies

Results which are complementary to those above have been found by Lucas

(1952). His results illustrate the different e:ffects that failure has

on the anxiety levels of different individuals. He found that people

high in anxiety (Mar:::::> Ms) do more poorly after failure and that

people low in anxiety (!vIS"> }~af) do better after failure. In this

exper-i.merrt , Ps (probability of success) was very high, so as failures

occurred, Ps moved into the 0,5 area, causing Ms :> Maf people to enjoy

the t.ask more and Maf::::> Ms peopl e to dislike it. It is possible to

criticise this research by saying that this would be a natural reaction

if Ps is very high, but research by Weiner & Schneider (1971) :found

that the above is true even if the task is very easy or very difficult.

Hence, the literature on inertial tendencies is confirmed by research.

iii) Motive to avoid success (M-s)

Horner (1972) proposed that males and females experience achievement

differently. As a resul t, the concept of motive to avoid .success was

formulated. Horner (1972) concluded that fear of. success does exist

and that it can be used to predict performance, but'research following

Horner's (1972) research is often contradictory. In her earlier work,

.Hor-ner , (1968, in Arkes & Garske, 1977), devised a projective measure

in which subjects wrote stories to a verbal cue that was clearly on

experience of success by a female: 'after first-term finals, Anne finds

herself at the top of her medical school class Among female

subjects, 66% wrote stories cont.e rrn ng M-s imagery, whereas only 9%

of males, given the same cue with a male character did so. Horner

concluded from her r-es ear-ch that M-s was predominantly a pervasive

fer,;cle attribute arid ger.er-e r i y, it was uncharacteristic of men.

lI:.~! ....-- •• ., ••

\

PAGE 53

In subsequent research, Horner (1972) divided women into high and low

M-s imagery groups. She then placed subjects in a non-competitive

situation in which they worked alone and in a competitive situation in

which they worked together with others. Her results supported her

hypotheses: high M-s women performed better when working alone and low

M-s women performed better when competing. In high M-s women, Horner

maintained, the competitive situation aroused an inhibitory anxiety due

to fears that success would be incompatible with femini ty and would

cause social rejection especially from males.

Although Horner's theory appears to be supported by research, there are

researchers who have found results contradictory to those of Horner.

For example, Hoffman (1974), found that M-s imagery in males (77%)

surpassed that in females (65%), suggesting that M-s is not a uniquely

feminine characteristic. Furthermore, research completed by Feather &

Raphelson (1974) and Monahan, Kuhn & Shaver (1974) found that both sexes

projected greater M-s imagery to the Anne cue, suggesting that the

proj ective measure taps the perception of a sex-role stereotype

rathe~.than a stable personality motive.

3.3.-3 Hermans

Very little research has been completed on the works of Hermans, et. all

probably due to the fact that his theories are so recent. Research has

been completed on the scales that he has devised to measure

achievement motivation in children and adults. Waters & Waters (1976)

have found that the Hermans scale was significantly posi ttvely related

to grade point averages, in other words, the Hermans scale is

positively correlated with academic achievement. In South Africa, the

Hermans scale for children (PMT-K) has been succes~fully utilised by van

Rooyen (1983) and Viljoen (1983). Recent research has been

successfully completed in South Africa by Barling and Beattie (1982) who

differentiated between paralysing and motivating stress. They maintain

that there is a positive correlation between motivating stress and

academic achievement and a negative correlation between paralysing

stress and academic achievement.

/54 .....

PAGE 54

3.4 PROPOSITIONS

Various lines of thought or central ides may be extracted from the theory

and its accompanying research. Firstly, the central ideas relating to

intelligence study habits and attitudes and achievement motivation will

be discussed. Thereafter, the central ideas on personal i ty factors,

adjustment, anxiety and residence will be discussed.

It appears that IQ and academic achievement as well as· achievement

motivation are positively related. It also appears that study habits and

atti tudes are positively correlated with academic achievement as well as

achievement motivation. Furthermore, the above research also indicates that

achievement motivation and academic achievement are positively' correlated.

The research previously discussed, reveals and suggests that various

personali ty factors are positively related to academic achievement,

including such factors as introversion, ego strength, dominance and

seriousness. Other personality factors which are positively related to

academic achievement include persistence, conscientiousness, tough

mindedness and aggression. It appears that the successful student would

be practical, down to earth, sensitive, ambitious and disciplined.

Successful students tend to be analytical, critical, free thinking, and

independent. Research also reveals that academic achievement is positively

related to adjustment and more specifically, it is related to self

confidence, self esteem, self control and sociabil i ty. Research and theory

previously discussed suggests that academic achievement is either inversely

or negatively related to anxiety.

With the theory and research in mind, it is clear that certain general

ideas have been extracted which form the very basi s of hypotheses. In the

following chapter these hypotheses will be operationalised and tested

experimentally.

/55 .....

CHAPTER 4 : RESEARCH DESIGNPAGE 55

4.1 HYPOTHESES

\

From Chapter I, it is evident that it is important to be able to predict

academic achievement essentially to decrease the loss of state expenditure

and potential manpower. Furthermore, the whole concept of achievement

motivation in South African universities has, to date, remained unresearched.

Such a test has been constructed by Vilj oen (1983) but its vali di ty and

re 1 iabil i ty have remained unknown. Al though no hypotheses have been

formulated to assess reliability, Hypotheses 1 - 9 have been formulated to

assess the validity of the Achievement Motivation Questionnaire (Prestasie­

motiveringsvraelys) and hypotheses 11 and 12 are addi tional hypotheses on

anxiety and study habits specifically.

Hypothesis 1:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation

and academic achievement (average score of final November 1985 examinations).

Hypothesis 2:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation:.

and intelligence (total intelligence score).

Hypothesis 3:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation

and study orientation (study habi ts and attitudes). (Factor 1 of SSHA). It

seems logical that the most motivated students are those who have a positive

approach to study and who have also developed effective methods of study.

Hypothesis 4:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation

and intrapersonal adjustment. (Factor 1 of PHSF). The student who is

motivated to achieve should have a sense of self confidence, self esteem,

self control and emotional stability.

Hypothesis 5:

There is a s Lgm.f'Lcant; negative correlation between achievement motivation

and personal reservedness (introversion) (factor A of 16PF). Research in

Chapter 3 (Goh & Moore, 1978) found that there is a negative correlation

between the above two variables at the university level.

/56 ..•..

PAGE 56

Hypothesis 6:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation and

assertiveness. (Factor 0 of the 16PF). It seems natural that the student most

motivated to achieve will be bold in outlook and willing to take initiative.

Hypothesis 7:

There is a significant correlation between achievement motivation and

conscientiousness. (Factor G of the 16PF). In order to achieve, it is a necessity

'that the student be willing to apply him/herself.

Hypothesis 8:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation and

independence. (Factor 02 of the 16PF). The rationale is that the student most

motivated to achieve is one who takes initiative and who is self reliant. (One

should bear in mind that a low score on this subscale indicates high independence.)

Hypothesis 9:

There is a significant negative correlation between achievement motivation and

anxiety. (Factor 04 of the 16PF). The rationale is that the student most motivated

to achieve will;.do so if feeling calm and at ease.

Hypothesis 10:

There is a significant positive correlation between academic achievement and

intelligence. It is logical that it is the intellectually stronger student who

is most motivated to achieve.

Hypothesis 11:

There ,is a significant difference between the means of over-achievers versus under­

achievers on anxiety.

Hypothesis 12:

There is a significant difference between the means of over-achievers versus

under-achievers on study orientation (study habits and attitudes). It seems logical

that the over-achiever would be more positive about his/her sVJdies and would have

acquired more effective study methods than the under-achiever.

4.2 SUBJECTS

In total, there w~re 2 311 first-year students registered the Rand

Afrikaans uni versi ty in 198~,. To ensure a homogenous s arr.p l e, 1 729 Whi te,

Afrikaans speaking, undergraduate students were utilised in this study.

/57 ..•..

\

PAGE 57

Of these students, 978 were male (56,6% of the total sample), and 751

(43,4% of the total sample) were commuters and 637 (36,84%) were

residing in the hostel. Below is a summarisation of the data:

TABLE C

Frequency Distribution for Sex

Sex

Male

Female

TOTAL

Frequency

978

751

1729

Percent

56,6

43,4

100,0

TABLE D

Frequency Distribution for Faculty

Facul ty Frequency Percent

Arts 554 32,0

Science 180 10,4

Commerce 603 34,9

Law 99 5,7

Education 203 11,7

Engineering 90 5,2

TOTAL 1729 100,0

/58 .....

PAGE 58

4.3 MEASURING INSTRUMENTS

The following tests were used for this research

\

NSAGT-SSHA

16PF

PHSF

New South African Group Test

Survey of Study Habits and Attitudes

16 Personality Factor Questionnaire

Personal, Home, Social and Formal Relations

Questionnaire

IPAT IPAT Anxiety Questionnaire

PMV Prestasiemotiveringsvraelys (Achievement Motivation

Questionnaire)

4.3.1 The achievement motivation questionnaire (PMV) (See Appendix

A)

The PMV was establ i shed wi th the goal of measuring .achievement

motivation in high school pupils. The questionnaire was

constructed by Viljoen (1983), who found that the reliability

and validity coefficients of the questionnaire, when

administered to high school students, were low. This test is

comprised of 80 questions. The reliability coefficients for

standards 6, 7 and 8 respectively were 0,720, 0,762 and 0,769.

(p , 129). The validity coefficients ranged from 0,16 to 0,40

which was not cons i der-e d ev i deric e of significant correlation

(p , 138). Viljoen (1983) rr.a i rrt a i ns that the PMV should not be

regarded as simply Lnv a l r d , "Die lae, maar hoogs beduidende

kor-r-e Las i ekoef f i s.i er.te wa t in die hoofondersoek v er-kr'y is, dui

op die rnoontlike geldigheid van die prestasiemotiveringsvraelys"

(p . 138).

PAGE 59

4.3.2 The New South African Group Test (NSAGT)

The main objective of a group intelligence scale is to obtain an

impression of a pupil's general intellectual ability. The NSAGT

consists of six sub tests, of which three measure verbal ability and

three, non-verbal ability.

The NSAGT has proved to be a reliable measuring instrument. Research

done by Robbertse (1968, in van der Westhuizen (1983), : obtained the

following reliability coefficients on the total score: 0,86, 0,87 and

0,83. The manual for the NSAGT (p. 19) reports a reliability

coefficient, calculated by KR 21 of 0,84.

4.3.3 Survey of study habits and attitudes (SSHA)

The aim of this questionnaire is to identify students who have

ineffective study habits and attitudes towards their work, thereby

assisting them in realising their latent potentialities. The

reliability coefficients vary from 0,805 to 0,873 (Manual for the

Survey of Study Habits and Atti tudes, (1974), p , 8). As far as the

validi ty of the SSHA is concerned, the manual reports a high and

statistically significant correlation between the SSHA and school

achievement.

4.3.4 The Personal, Home and Formal Relations questionnaire (The PHSF)

The purpose of the PHSF relations questionnaire is to measure, by

relevant components, the personal, home, social and" formal relations of

high school pupils, students and adults, in order to determine level of

adjustment. The reliability of the questionnaire is good. The manual for

the PHSF questionnaire (1971) reports reliability coefficients ranging

from 0,71 to 0,94 (p . 213). The manual (p , 219) also maintains that

research done with the preliminary form of the PHSF, showed that it

possesses a high degree of construct validity.

/60 .....

PAGE 60

4.3.5 The IPAT Anxiety scale

The scale was constructed with the aim of. making an evaluation of

manifest, free, anxiety levels, irrespective of prevalent conditions or

\ immediate situation. The scale is sui table for persons 15 years and

older. The manual for the IPAT Anxiety questionnaire reports

reliability coefficients ranging from 0,83 to 0,88 (test - retest

reliability). Reliability is therefore satisfactory (p. 7).

4.3.6 The 16 Personality Factor questionnaire (16 PF)

Toe 16 PF is a personality questionnaire measuring 16 personal i ty

characteristics. It is designed for the use of adults and consists of

187 items. Cattell, Eber and Tatsuaka (1970) reported'reliability

coefficients ranging from 0,58 to 0,83. Although reliability

coefficients are low (Smit (1983», they are still higher than the

reliab~lity coefficients of tests in everyday use. Smit, 1983, maintains

that validation data consisting of 30 career profiles are available.

Furthermore, Maas, (1975) maintains that the 16 PF complies with

empirical validity: "Cattell en sy medewerkers wys verder daarop dat die

16 PF voldoen aan empiriese geldigheid." (p. 46).

4.4. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE

4.4.1 Goal of the study

The goal of this study is to assess the reliability and validity of the

PMV. In order to achieve this goal, the following experimental procedure

discussed below was utilised.

4.4.1.1. Experimental procedure

At the Rand Afrikaans University, it is compulsory that all

first-year students complete a battery of diagnostic tests before

the commencement of the academic year. All the tests discussed

below were group administered and have been marked using the

standard procedure.

/61. ....

\

PAGE 61

Computation of total academic achievement score

In order to compute academic achievement scores, Naude (1986)

utilised weighted means as a measure of academic achievement,

which is the principle applied here. The total course weight

always equalled 1.00. For example, in the B.Sc. (Natural Science)

area of study, it is stipulated that in one's first year of

study, four subj ects are r'equir-ed, Thus, each subj ect has a

weight of .25. If, for example, a student fails a subject, he/she

earns only .75 (75%) of the potential total of the course total

of 1.00 (100%). Thus, in this case, he/she is penalised by .25

(25%) of a total of 1.00 (100%) for failing one subject.

Similarly, if a student takes more than four subjects in a

B.Sc., he is credited by 0.25 for every subject passed. The

principle is identical for a student who is 'in the other

faculties, where five subjects are required in one's first year.

In these cases, the weight of every course is now .20 (20%) with

a total of 1.00 (100%). Students are thus penalised and credited

by .20 for every subject failed or passed.

/62 .....

CHAPTER 5. STATISTICAL RESULTS

PAGE 62

5.1 Reliability (item analysis)

Presented in Appendix B are the item-test correlations before iteration,

in which case there are a total of 125 items. At this stage, the PMV has

a Kuder Richardson 20 reliability coefficient of 0.387. After 10 iterations,

the Kuder Richardson 20 reliability coefficient had improved to 0.847,

yielding 20 reliable items. This data can be found in Appendix C. Data which

are marked with an asterisk in Appendix C are excluded from the test.

5.2 Validi ty

\ 5.2.1 Bravais Pearson correlation coefficients

One of the ways in which validity was assessed was by computing

Pearson Product Moment correlation coefficient between the PMV and

all the other variables. Data is available for 1054 of the 1729

students. In all the correlations, the minimum level of significance

is 0.05. Presentation and discussion of these results can be found

below :-

TABLE E

PEARSON CORRELATION COEFFICIENTS BETWEEN ACHIEVEMENTMOTIVATION AND ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT, INTELLIGENCE, STUDY ORIENTATION

AND PERSONALITY FACTORS

ACHIEVEMENT

Achievement motivation

MOT I V A T ION

correlated with

Academic achievement

Intelligence

Study orientation

lntrapersonal adjustmen~

Reservedness

Assertiveness

Conscientiousness

Independence

Anxiety

*) Significant at 5% level

**) Significant at 1% level

***) Significant at 0.01% level

r

.0132

.0725

.2239

.3594

.0631

.3278

.0652

-.1099

-.4309

p n Significance

.334 1054

.009 1054 **

.000 1054 ***

.000 1054 ***

.020 1054 *"

.000 1054 ***

.017 1054 *

.000 1054 ***

.000 1054 ***

/63 .....

TABLE F

PEARSON CORRELATION COEFFICIENT BETWEENACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT AND INTELLIGENCE

PAGE 63

Academic achievementcorrelated with

Intelligence

r

.2090

p

.000

n

999

Significance

***

\I

***) Significant at 0.1% level

/64 .....

PAGE 64

Hypothesis 1:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation and

academic achievement.

From Table E it is evident that there is a positive but insignificant

correlation between these two variables. For this reason. hypothesis 1 has been

rejected.

!Jypothesis 2:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement: motivation and

intelligence.

From Table E it is evident that there is a positive and significant correlation

between these variables. Thus, this hypothesis has been confirmed.

Hypothesis 3:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation and

study orientation.

From Table E it is evident that there is a positive and significant correlation

between achievement motivation and study orientation (study habits and attitudes).

For this. reason, hypothesis 3 has been confirmed.

Hypothesis 4:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation and

intrapersonal adjustment.

As is evident in Table E, there is a positive and significant correlation between

achievement motivation and intrapersonal adjustment. Thus, hypothesis 4 has been

confirmed.

Hypothesis 5:

There is a significant negative correlation between achievement motivation and

personal reservedness.

From Table E it is evident that there is a positive and significant correlation

between achievement motivation and personal reservedness •..Thus, hypothesis 5 has

been rejected.

/65 .....

PAGE 65

Hypothesis 6:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation and

assertiveness.

It is evident from Table E that there is a positive and significant correlation

between achievement motivation and assertiveness. Thus, hypothesis 6 has been

confirmed.

Hypothesis 7:

\ There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation and

conscientiousness.

From Table E it is evident that there is a positive and significant correlation

between achievement motivation and conscientiousness. Thus, hypothesis 7 has been

confirmed.

Hypothesis 8:

There is a significant positive correlation between achievement motivation and

independence.

From Table E it is clear that there is a significant positive correlation between

achievement motivation and independence. For this reason, hypothesis 8 has been

confirmed.

Hypothesis 9:

There is a significant negative correlation between achievement motivation and

anxiety.

From Table E it is evident that there is a significant negative correlation between

achievement motivation and anxiety. Thus, hypothesis 9 has been confirmed.:

that

and

Hypothesis 10:

There is a significant positive

intell igence •

From Table F it is evident

between academic achievement

confirmed.

correlation between academic achievement and

there is a s Lgnd.f'Lcarrt positive correlation

intelligence. Thus, hypothesis 10 has been

/66 .•..•

\.

5.2.2

PAGE 66

Simple Regression Analysis

Having established that there is a positive and significant

correlation between total academic achievement scores (dependent

variable) and total IQ scores (independent variable), it was possible

to compute a simple regression analysis, with the goal of identifying

over- and under-achievers.

For the purposes of this study, the author of this thesis assumed

that IQ and academic achievement were linearly related due to the

fact that other researchers indicated a positive correlation between

these two variables (Sinha, 1966). The prediction equation is based

on the mathematical equation y == ax + b, where b is a constant and

is the cut on the y axis, whereas a is the slope of the graph. In

this regression, the equation line was computed as; y == 0,3097 (x )

+ 18,0609. In other words, if the x value (intelligence) is 100, the

predicted academic achievement score (y) would equal 0,3097 (100 +

18,0609 49,03. Thus, a student with an IQ of 100 would have a

5.2.3

pre?icted academic achievement score of 49,03%.

De~inition of under- and over-achievers

With the above regression line in mind, the standard error of estimate

was computed, which was 16,539150. Students who fell one standard

error of estimate above the regression line were defined as over­

achievers, of which there were 120, while students who fell one

standard error of estimate below the regression line were defined

as under-achievers, of which there were 287.

t tests

TABLE G

THE SIGNIFICANCE OF DIFFERENCES IN MEANS OFOVER-ACHIEVERS VERSUS UNDER-ACHIEVERS ON ANXIETY

AND STUDY ORIENTATION :

Variable Over-achievers Under-achievers

X S n X S n t

Anxiety 4.375 2.058 120 4.177 2.014 278 .896

Study

Orientation 6.4583 1.802 120 5.0452 2.014 278 6.833*

*) Significant difference ct <:,0; level.__!e

PAGE 67

Hypothesis 11:

There is a significant difference between the means of over-achievers versus

.under-achievers on anxiety.

From Table G above, it is clear that the means between these two groups are,not significantly different for over- and under-achievers. As a result,

hypothesis 11 has not been confirmed.

Hypothesis 12:

There is a significant difference between the means of over-achievers versus

under-achievers on study orientation.

From Table G, it is evident that the means of under-achievers versus over­

achievers are significantly different. For this reason, hypothesis 12 has

been confirmed.

/68 .

CHAPTER 6 .DISCUSSION OF RESULTS. LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY

AND RECOMMENDATIONS

PAGE 68

6.1 INTRODUCTION

From the preceding overview of the literature it is clear that academic

achievement and more specifically, achievement motivation, are index complex and

extremely important factors for any student involved in tertiary education. The

goal of this study is essentially a validity and reliability study of an

achievement motivation questionnaire, which included an item analysis and

Bravais Pearson correlations. Also included was a simple regression analysis

which was used to predict academic achievement on the basis of a given IQ score

and, on the basis of this, two groups were selected, namely, over achievers and

under achievers, whose means on anxiety and study orientation were compared.

Below is a discussion of the statistical results of the above goal and

thereafter, limitations of the study will be discussed.

6.2 ITEM ANALYSIS

After 10 iterations, there are only 20 out of 125 items left, which suggest that

the initial reliability of the test is low. However, this may not be the case,

for although the reliability coefficient before iteration is low, it may simply

be a reflection that the achievement motivation questionnaire (PMV) is

mul tifactorial. It is highly unl ikely that the PMV, as. a measure of such a

complex variable as achievement motivation, can be scored with one single score.

6.3 BRAVAIS PEARSON CORRELATIONS

6.3.1 Achievement motivation and academic achievement

The Bravais Pearson correlation between academic achievement and

achievement motivation indicates that there is a positive but

statistically insignificant correlation between these two variables.

In other words, it is the more motivated student who achieves

academically, but in this study, the corielation is insignificant.

This positive correlation is confirmed by Wankowski and Cox (1973)

who have also found a positive correlation between academic

achievement and achievement motivation. Perhaps the reason for this

statistically insignificant correlation can be explained by the work

of Botha (l971) , who has shown that highly motivated students may

have a greater fear of failure, and that this may detract from their

academic achievement scores.

/69,.. ,.

PAGE 69

6.3.2 Achievement motivation and intelligence

The Pearson correlation between achievement motivation and intelligence

was positive and significant at the 0.05 level. In other words, it is the

more intelligent student who is most motivated to achieve

academically.

6.3.3 Achievement motivation and study orientation

The Pearson correlation coefficient between achievement motivation and

study orientation is positive and significant at the 0.05 level. This

means that the student with effective study habits and a positive

attitude to study is the most motivated to achieve. Similar research,

including Zarb (1981), Uguroglu & Walberg (1979, in Beard, 1980) confirm

the above results. Perhaps the student with a positive:approach to study

realises that this potentially enables him to achieve and therefore, he

becomes motivated to try to achieve academically.

6.3.4 Achievement motivation and personality

The Pearson correlation coefficient between achievement motivation and

reservedness was positive and significant at the 0.05 level. In other

words, it is the extroverted personality who is more motivated to

achieve academically. The above results are contradictory to those of

Entwistle (1972) and Goh & Moore (1978), who found that introverts tend

to be more motivated to achieve academically. Furthermore, there was a

positive and significant correlation between achievement motivation and

assertiveness. Similar results to these have been found by Saenger- Ceha

(1970). The correlation between achievement motivation and

conscientiousness was positive and significant at the' 0.05 level. In

other words, it is the hard-working student who is most motivated to

achieve academically. In addi tion, there was a negative and significant

correlation between achievement motiva t i on and :·independence. Thi s means

that the more independent a student is, the more motivated thi s student

is to achieve academically. Lastly, the carrel ation between achievement

motivation and anxiety was negative and significant at the 0.05 level. In

other words, it is the less anxious student who is most motivated to

achieve academically. Taking the results as a whole, it is the

extroverted, assertive, conscientious, independent and calm student who

is most motivated to achieve academicall~.

/70 .....

"

PAGE 70

6.3.5 Achievement motivation and intrapersonal adjustment

The Pearson correlation coefficient between intrapersonal adjustment and

achievement motivation was positive and significant at the 0.05 level.

This means that it is the well adjusted student who is most motivated

to achieve academically. Similar results to the above have been found by

Jensen (1958), Berger & Sutker (1956) and Burgess (1956).

6.3.6 Achievement motivation and anxiety

The correlation between achievement motivation· and anxiety were negative\

and significant at the 0.05 level. This means that it is the calm student

who is motivated to achieve academically. Similar results have been found

by Lynne (1977) and Kanekar (1976). Conflicting findings to the above

results were found by Gissrau (1976).

6.3.7 Academic achievement and intelligence

The Pearson correlation between achievement motivation and intelligence

indicates that there is a positive and significant correlation at the

0.05 level. This indicates that it is the more intelligent student who

achieves academically. Identical results have been found by Ryle &

Lungi, (1968) and Garbers and Faurie, (1972).

Due:' to the positive and significant correlation between academic

achievement and intelligence, it is possible to predict academic

achievement on the basis of IQ.

6.4 THE PREDICTION OF ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT ON THE BASIS OF IQ SCORE

From the regression equation (Page 66), it is possible to pr-ed i c t academic

achievement on the basis of a given IQ score. However, the predicted academic

achievement score is low. For example, as illustrated on p 66, a student with an

intelligence score of 100, has a predicted academic achievement score of 49,03%.

This may be due to the fact that the regression line is non-linear. In addition,

another possible explanation for the above predicted result is that the

correlation only considers academic achievement and' intelligence, excluding

achievement motivation which may be a factor dri ving students to supreme

achievements.

6.5 A COMPARISON OF UNDER-ACHIEVERS VERSUS OVER-ACHIEVERS ON ANXIETY AND STUDY

ORIENTATION

Once the regression line was computed, those students failing one standard error

of estimate above this line were defined as over-achievers and those who' fell

one standard error of estimate below the regression line were defined as

UDder-achievers. These two groups were then compared on the following

variables, which have been discussed below.:.

/71 .....

\.

PAGE 71

6.5.1 Anxiety

Al though there was no statistically significant difference between the

means of the over-achievers and under-achievers.

6.5.2 Study orientation

The data in Table H (Chapter 5) indicate that there is a statistically

significant difference between the means of over-achievers versus under­

achievers on study orientation. The average score of the over-achievers

is 6,4583, whereas the average score for the under-achievers was 5,0452.

In other words, over-achievers have, in general, a more positive

approach to study than do under-achievers.

6.6 LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Perhaps the most important limitation of this study was that stanine scores were

used in the computation of the Pearson correlation coefficients and some of the

t Tests, because raw scores were unavailable. The use of standard scores should

pr-ef'erab Ly" not be used in r-eaear-ch for certain data is lost in the process and

deeper levels of abstraction are thereby decreased. It is important to note that

the PMV scores and academic achievement scores were available in raw scores.

It is the author's opinion that perhaps a developmental perspective would have

been meaningful and very enlightening in the sense that the first-year student

in the early adulthood stage is very different to the first-year student who is

middle-aged. More specifically, for example, their reasons; for achievement

motivated behaviour are in all likelihood very different. Perhaps, for the

eighteen-year old, achievement motivation and academic:' achievement are seen as

the beginning of a secure career; whereas for the middle-aged student,

achievement motivation and academic achievement may be an important stepping

stone to promotion.

/72 .....

PAGE 72

In addition, one must bear in mind that this study utilised university students

only, who are a very select and intellectually elite group. Perhaps the level of

achievement motivation is higher in the intellectual strata of society than in

non-intellectual groups. The point to be made here is that the results yielded

from the data may not be generalised to any other population group except

Afrikaans speaking, white, undergraduate university students.

Furthermore, the PMV in itself also has its limitations. The results obtained on

the PMV offers no assessment of the student's commitment to achievement

motivation. The diagnostic tests are completed at the beginning of one's firstyear, before the student has been exposed to possible stresses of university

life. In addition, perhaps it is best that the PMV be viewed as a test only for

academic achievement motivation, because it gives no indication of

achievement motivation in areas other than those which are academic.

Also, results from the diagnostic tests may have been influenced by the fact

that the motivation to complete these tests is low. Furthermore, levels of

achievement motivation and academic achievement may have been influenced by the

possibility that often the first-year student finds him/herself in the wrong

facul ty for; which he/she is unsuited. In addition, the diagnostic tests only

give an indication of the behaviour of the student at one point in time,

omitting the fact that the student is an ever-changing entity. The point to be

made here is that the more dynamic aspects of the student are lost through the

use of paper and pencil tests in research.

Bearing the above in mind, it is advisable that researchers utilise raw scores

whenever possible and also to take into account the motivation levels of one's

subjects. It would also be most meaningful if future research based the

prediction of academic achievement on a non-linear regression equation. This

would entail a multivariate approach to academic achievement, which is the

examination of the effect of several factors acting together on a single

variable. In addition, researchers might care to compare a student popul ation

with a non-student population. They may also find it beneficial to examine those

factors which explain variance in achievement motivation, in an attempt to

discover these factors which drive the student to over achieve or slip into the

cycle of under achievement. Furthermore, the future researcher should bear in

mind the works of McClelland (1976) who stated that the achievement motive

develops out of expectations. These include expectations of oneself and the

expectations of f ara i Ly and friends. In other words, the achievement, motive

should be seen within the breader systems context (Minuchin, 1974).

/73 ..... _

PAGE 73

It is also important that data yielded from research on academic achievement be

utilised by the counsellor to preventatively counsel students who find

themselves engulfed in a reality for which they are unprepared. For example, it

may be useful for the student to receive information on:

1) M and T f persons and expected risk levels (Atkinson, 1964).s a

2) The facilitating aspects of the fear of failure. (Atkinson, 1964).

\ 3) The effects of anxiety on academic achievement (Hermans et.al., 1982).

4) The prediction and interpretation of an achievement related event (Weiner et.

a1., 1972).

Research to date has concentrated on static rather than the more dynamic aspects

of achievement motivation and academic achievement. Future res~arch should begin

emphasising this dynamic aspect rather than the field degenerating into a

lifeless body of static literature. This may be achieved by viewing behaviour as

a constant flow of activity and in addition, doing away with a cause-and-effect

explanatin of achievement motivated behaviour. In other words, the researcher

would be doing justice to this field by adopting a non-linear approach to

achievement motivated behaviour. This idea is reflected inn the words of Murray

(1938, in Weiner, 1972): "Human motivation theory began to lose its linear or

simple this-causes-that character and began to assume the form of a network of

functional relationships (p. 84)."

/74 •..•.

1\

CHAPTER 7: SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION

PAGE 74

7.1 SUMMARY

The phenomenon of under-achievement assumes staggering proportions in South

African universities. As a result, there is an urgent need for a questionnaire

whereby the potential under-achiever may be identified at the earliest possible

stage. Such a questionnaire is needed at the Rand Afrikaans University where the

phenomenon of underachievement has immense financial and personal implications.

(Marais, 1980).

The goal of this study is to assess the reliability and validity of such a

questionnaire known as the achievement motivation questionnaire (prestasiemoti­

veringsvraelys). In order to achi eve thi s goal, 1 729 white under-graduate,

Afrikaans-speaking students completed a series of diagnostic tests. These

included the New South African Group Test, the Survey of Study Habits and

Attitudes, the 16PF, the IPAT Anxiety Questionnaire and the Achievement

Motivation Questionnaire (PMV).

In order to assess the reliability of the achievement motivation questionnaire,

an item analysis was used. (NP 50 programme). The validity of the questionnaire

was assesse~ with the use of Bravais Pearson correlation coefficients, a simple

regression analysis and t-tests.

Statistically significant results were found in all three calculations. The

resul ts which were yielded from the simple regression analysis enables one to

predict academic achievement on the basis of a given 1Q score, and also enables

one to identify over-achievers and under-achievers. Results yielded from the

t-tests indicated that there were 120 over-achievers and 287 under- achievers.

The mean scores of these two groups were compared on anxiety and study

orientation. A significant difference between study habi ts and attitudes was

found between these groups with over-achievers having more effective study

habits than under-achievers.

In addition, included in this thesis are the theories of achievement motivation

namely, Murray (1938), McClelland's (1976) affective arousal model, Weiner's

(1974) cogni tive approach to motivation and 1astly, the theory of Hermans,

Bergen & Eijssen (1982). Furthermore, limitations of this study have also been

/75.....

PAGE 75

discussed. They include the use of an elite student group, whose results

cannot be generalised to the general population, the absence of a

developmental approach to motivation and low motivation by the subjects to

complete the diagnostic tests. Recommendatins for further research have been

made, including, amongst other things, the use of a non-l inear approach to

achievement motivation.

7.2 CONCLUSION

This research has shown that the PMV is reliable if reduced to a total of

the specified 20 items, in which case, the KR reliability coefficient is

.847. Furthermore, the questionnaire is also found to be a valid measure

of achievement motivation. Highlighted is the fact that achievement

motivation is very complex, and by virtue of this, it may be multifactorial.

Lastly, there is no doubt that the field of achievement motivated behaviour

still requires considerable research to better understand such complex

behaviour as well as to provide further valuable information applicable to

many facets of life.

/76 .....

PAGE 76

BIBLIOGRAPHY

Alpert, R., & Haber, R.N. (1960). Anxiety in academic achievementsituations. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 61, 207 - 215.

Anderson, A.W. (1960). A note on high intelligence and low academicperformance in the university. The Educand, ~ (1), 111 - 113.

Arkes, H.R. & Garske, J.P. (1977). Psychological theories ofmotivation. California: Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.

Atkinson, J.W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. New Jersey: D.van Nostrand.

Atkinson, J.W. & Feather, N.T. (1966). A theory of achievementmotivation. New York: Wiley.

Atkinson, J. W. & Litwin, G.H.anxiety conceived as motive tofailure. Journal of Abnormal and

(1960). Achievement motive and testapproach success and motive to avoidSocial Psychology. 60, 52 - 63.

Atkinson, J.W. &McClelland, D.C. (1948). The projective expression ofneeds and the effect of different intensities of the hunger drive onthematic apperception. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 643 ­658.

Bailey,:.R.C. (1971). Self concept differences in law and highachieving students. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 27 (2), 188 - 191.

Baird, L.L. (1969). Thestudents' self concept,Guidance Journal, 47 (10),

effects ofgoals and

1015 - 1021.

college residence groups onachievements. Personnel and

Banks, 0., & Finlayson, D. (1971). Success and failure in thesecondary school. London: Methven.

Barling, J., & Beathe, D. (1982). Positive exam results withoutstress. Johannesburg: McGraw-Hill.

Beard, R.M. (1980). Motivating students. London: Routledge and Kegan,Paul.

Bednar, R.W. & Weinberg,treatment programmes forPsychology, 17, 1 - 7.

S.L. (1970). Ingredients of successfulunderachievers. Journa~· of Counselling

Bendig, A.W. (1958). Comparison of the validity .of' two temperamentscales in predicting college achievement. Journal of EducationalResearch, 51, 605 - 609.

Berger, I.W., & Sutker, A.R. (1956). The relationship of emotionaladjustment and intellectual capacity to academic achievement ofcollege students. Me~tal Hygiene, 40, 65 - 77.

/77 .....

BIBLIOGRAPHY (cont )PAGE 77

\

Ber-ns t.e in ,': A.S. (1958). The influence of anxiety level on initial responsepreference in serial nonsense learning. Dissertation Abstracts, 19, 567.

Blumberg, J.C. (1969). The study habits and attitudes of students, withspecial reference to the Johannesburg College of Education. Unpublisheddoctoral thesis, Univerity of South Africa.

Bolles, R.C. (1975). Theory of motivation (2nd Ed.) New York: Harper & Row.

Botha, A.G. (1971). Suksesvolle en minder suksesvolle akademiese presteerders.Unpublished doctoral thesis, (p.64). University of Stellenbosch.

Bruwer, W.J. (1973). Motivering, houding en studie-metode as bepalende faktorein akademiese prestasie. Unpublished doctoral thesis, University ofPotchefstroom.

Buchin, J. (1966). An analysis of the relationship between anxiety and self­concept and college achievement. Dissertation Abstracts, 27, 385.

Burgess, E. (1956). Personality factors of over and under : achievers inengineering. Journal of Educational Psychology, 47, 89 - 99.

Cattell, R.B., Eber, H.W. Tatsuoka, M.M. (1970). Handbook for the sixteenpersonality factor questionnaire. New York: Institute for Personality andAbility Testing, Inc.

Cattell, R.B., Sealy, A.P. & Sweeney, A.B. (1966). What can personality andmotivation source trait measurements add to the prediction of schoolachievement? British Journal of Educational Psychology, 36, 280 - 295.

Chickering, A.W. (1974). Commuting versus resident students. London: Josey BassPublishers.

Danesino, A., & Layman, W.A. (1969). Contrasting personality patterns of highand low achievers among college students of Italian and Irish descent. Journalof Psychology, 71 - 83 •.

o,de Charms, R., & Carpenter, V. (1968). Measuring motivation in culturallydisadvantaged school children. Journal of Experimental Education. 37, 31 - 41.

Dhaliwal, A.S., & Sharma, J.P. (January 1976). The relationship betweenintelligence and academic achievement. Journal of Psychological Research, 20,28 - 32.

Endler, N.S. (1964). Anxiety, aptitude &, academic ach i evemerrt.. Ontario Journalof Educational Research, ~, 85 - 91.

Engelbrecht, S.W.B. (1973). Akademiese prestasie van intellektueelbogemiddelde leerlinge dee 1 een. Pretoria: Human Sciences Research Council.

Engelbrecht, S.W.B. (1974). Akademiese prestasie van intellektueelbogemiddelde leerlinge deel twee. Pretoria: Institute for Manpower Research.

Ena, L. (December, 1978). Predicting achievement and the theory of fluid andcrystallised intelligence. Psychological Reports, 43 (part I), 847 - 852.

/78 .....

BIBLIOGRAPHY (corrt ) PAGE 78

\

Entin, E.E., & Raynor, J.O. (1973). Effects of contingent future orientationand achievement motivation on performance in two kinds of tasks. Journal ofExperimental Research in Personality, ~,314 - 320.

Entwistle, D.R. (1972). To dispel fantasies about fantasy-based measures ofachievement motivation. Psychology Bulletin, 77, 377 - 391.

Entwistle, J., Entwistle, D. & Cowell, M.D. (1971). The academic performanceof students. British Journal of Educational Psychology. 41, 258 - 267.

Entwistle, N.J. (1972). Personality & academic attainment. British Journalof Educational Psychology, 42, 137 - 151.

Entwistle, N.J. & Brennan, T. (1971). The academic performance of studentstypes of successful students. British Journal. of Educational Psychology ,

4!, 268 - 276.

FarIs, R.J. (1967). High and low achievement of intellectually average. intermediate grade students related to self concept and social approval.

Dissertation Abstracts, 28, 1205.

Feather, N.T. & Raphelson,American student groupsPersonality, 42, 190 - 201.

A.C. (1974).motive or

Fear of success in Australian andsex role stereotype. Journal of

Frieze,- I., &:Weiner, B. (1971). Cue utilisation and attributional judgementsfor success and failure. Journal of Personality, 39, 591 - 606.

Furneaux, W~D. (1962). The psychologist and the university. UniversitiesQuarterly, 17, 33 - 47.

Garbers, J.G., & Faure, J.S. (1972). Eerstejaars - universiteitsprestasies,belangrike saamhangende faktore: die patroon van benutting van hoer

opleidingsfasiliteite deur matrikulante in die Kaapprovinsie, Port Elizabeth.Unpublished Master's thesis, University of Port Elizabeth.

Gissrau, B. (August-September 1976). Test anxiety and performance ofjuveniles. Praxis der Kinderpsychologie und Kinderpsychiatrie, 25 (6), 216- 221.

Glock, M.D., Ahmann, J .S., & Smith, W.L. (1958). Predicting academic successin college by means of a study habit and attitude inventory. Educational andPsychological Measurement, ~ (4), 853- 857.

Goh, D.S. '& Moore, C. (1978). Personal i ty and academi c :.achi evement in threeeducational levels. Psychological Reports, 43, 71 - 79.

Goldfried, M.R., & D'Zurillo, T.J. (1973). Prediction of academic competenceby means of the survey of study habi ts & attitudes. Journal of EducationalPsychology, 64, 116 - 122.

Gotz, K.O. & Gotz, K. (1973). Introversion-extroversion and neuroticism ingifted and ungifted art stude~ts. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 36, 675 - 678.

Green, R.L., & Farquhar, W.W. (1965). Negro academic motivation and scholasticachievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 56, 241 - 243.

/79 ... .'.

BIBLIOGRAPHY (corrt ) PAGE,.79.

Grobe, M., & Lotta, R.M. (Spring 1981). Cumulative achievement in a masteryinstructional system the impact of differences in resultant achievementmotivation' and persistence. American Educational Research Journal , ~ (1), 7 ­13.

Grooms, R.R., & Endler, N.S. (1960). The effect of anxiety on academicachievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 51, 299 - 304.

Hafeez, A., & Shantamani, V.S. (1972). A study of the relationship among needachievement, introversion, extroversion & neuroticism. Journal of the IndianAcademy of Applied Psychology, ~ (1), 28 - 32.

Harlestan, D.W. (1962). Test anxiety and performance in problem-solving\ situations. Journal of Personality, 30, 357 - 573.

Heald, H.M. (1970). Thecorrection of errors andAbstracts, 31, 1621 - 1622.

effects of immediate knowledge oftest anxiety upon test performance.

resul ts andDissertation

Heckhausen, H. (1967). The anatomy of achievement motivation. New York:Academic Press.

Hermans, H.J.M., Bergen, T.C.M., & Eijssen, R.W. (1982). Van faa1angst totverantwoordelijkheid het bevorderer von de motivatie in taaksituaties opschool en in het gezin.Lisse: Swetz & Zeitlinger.

Hoffman, L.W. (1974). Fear of success in males and females. Journal ofConsulting and ~linical Psychology. 42, 353 - 358.

Holtzman, W.H., & Brown, W.F. (1953). Study habits and attitudes Ln theprediction of:academic success. American Psychologist, ~' 3~9.

Horner, M. S. (1972). Toward an understanding of achievement-related conflictsin women. Journal of Social Issues, 28, 157 - 175.

Hoyt, D.P., & Norman, W.T. (1954). Adjustment and academic predictability.Journal of Counselling Psychology, 1, 96 - 99.

Hudson, W. (1964). Future open scholars, Nature, 202, 4934.

Hugo, F.P. (1984). Outonomie en prestasie n stelselbenaderingontwikkeling van eerstejaar universiteitsstudente. Unpublishedthesis, University of South Africa.

tot diedoctoral

Hull, C.L. (1943). Principles of behaviour. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts

Hundal, P.S., & Jerath, J.M. (March, 1972). Correlates of projective measure ofachievement motivation and their factorial structure. Indian Journal ofPsychology, 47, 15 - 27.

Isaacson, R.L. (1964). Relation between need for achievement, test anxiety andcurricular choices. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 68, 447 - 452.

/80 .....

BIBLIOGRAPHY (corrt ) PAGE -80

\

Jensen, V. (1958). The influence of personality on academic success. Personneland Guidance Journal, 36, 497 - 500.

Jones, E.S. (1955). The probation student: What he is like and what can be doneabout it. Journal of Educational Research, 49, 93 - 102.

Kanekor, S. (June 1976). Anxiety, intelligence and academic performance.Psychological Reports, 38 (3), part 1, 938.

Kelley, H.H. (1972). Causal schemata and the attribution process. New York:General Learning Press.

Klinger, E. (1966). Fantasy need achievement as a motivational construct.Psychological Bulletin, 66, 291 - 308.

Lavin, D. (1965). The prediction of academic performance. New York: RussellSage Foundation.

Lens, W., & de VoIder, M. (1980). Achievement motivation and intelligent testscores: a test of the Yerkes - Dodson hypothesis. Psychologica Belgica, 20 (1),49 - 59.

Lynne, L. (July 1977). Academic achievement as related to sex, anxiety, selfconcept, aggression and depression. Dissertation Abstracts, 38B, 335.

Maas, F. (1975). Die Persoonlikheidsteorie van Cattell. Pretoria: Institute forPsychometric Research.

,Mackinnon, D.W. (1962). The nature and nurture of creative talent. A~erican

Psychologist,. 17, 484.

Mahone, C.H. (1960). Fear of failure and unrealistic vocational aspiration.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60, 253 - 261.

Malan, J.A. (1978). Prestasiemotivering en selfkonsep. Unpublished master'sthesis, Rand Afrikaans University.

Mann, W.R., & Rizzo, J.L. (1972). Composition of the achiever per-sonaLt tyscale. Psychological Reports, 31, 218.

Marais, F.A.J. (1980). Profile of the first year dropout. Bulletin virDosente. Kwartaalblad oor die Hoeronderwys, 13, 61 - 64.

Marais, L. (1981). Akademiese prestasie van homogene klasse studente gevorm aandie hand van elke persoonlikheidsdimensie. Unpublished Master's thesis, RandAfrikaans University.

Middleton, G., & Guhne, G.M. Personality syndromes and academic achievement.Journal of Educational Psychology, 50, 66 - 69.

Minuchin, S. (1974). Families and family therapy. London: Tavistock Publica­tions.

Monahan, L., Kuhn, D., & Shaver, P. (1974). Intrapsychic versus culturalexplanations of the 'fear of success' motive. ~Tournal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 29, 60 - 64.

/81. ....

BIBLIOGRAPHY (cont) PAGE 81

Morgan, H.H. (1952). A psychometric comparison of achieving and nonachievingcollege students of high quality. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 16, 292 ­298.

Maris, J.L.choice: anPersonality

(1966). Propensity for risk taking as a determinant of vocationalextension of the theory of achievement motivation. Journal of

and Social Psychology, ~' 328 - 335.

Moss, H.A., & Kagan, J. (1961). Stability of achievement and recognition­seeking behaviours from early childhood through adulthood. Journal of abnormaland Social Psychology, 62, 504 - 513.

McClelland, P.C. (1955). Studies in motivation. New York: Appleton-CenturyCrofts.

McClelland, D.C., Atkinson, J.W., Clark, R.A., & Lowell, E.L. (1976). Theachievement motive. New York: Irvington Publishers.

McClelland, D.C., & Friedman, G.A. (1952). A cross cultural study of therelationship between child training practices and achievement 'motivationappearing in folk tales. In Swanson, G.E., Newcomb, T.M. & Hartley, E.L.(Eds). Readings in Social Psychology, New York: McGraw Hill.

McClelland, D.G., Rinalisbacher, A. & de Charms, R. (1955). Religious and othersources of parental attitudes toward independence training, in McClelland,D.C. (Ed.) Studies in Motivation. New York: Appleton-Century Crofts.

Naude, F.P. (1986). Die akademiese prestasie van Afrikaanssprekende en Engels­sprekende studente aan n tweetaalige universiteit. Unpublished doctoralthesis, University of Port Elizabeth.

O'Connor, E.J., & Chassie, M.B. & Walther, F. (December, 1980). Expendedeffort and academic performance. Teaching of Psychology, 7, 231 - 233.

Orpen, C. (1976). Personality and academic attainment: A cross-cultural :study.Bri tish Journal of Educational Psychology, 46, 220'- 222.

O'Shea, A.J. (1970). Low achievement syndrome among bright school boys.Journal of Educational Research, 63 (6), 257 - 262.

Pentony, P. & Loftus, A.R. T. (1970). A predi ction of first year performance ofscience s~udents from IQ and performance in matriculation examination.Australian Journal of Higher Education, ~ (1), 57 - 64.

Purkey, W.W. (1970). Self concept and school achievement. New Jersey: PrenticeHall.

Raynor, J .0. (1970). Relationship between achievement-related motives,orientation & academic performance. Journal of Personality &Psychology, 15, 28 - 33.

futureSocial

Roe, A. (1953). A psychological study of em i nerrt psychologists andanthropologists and a comparison with biological and physical scientists,Psychological Monographs, 67, 3S2.

/82 .....

BIBLIOGRAPHY (cont) PAG::; 82

Romine, P. G., & Crowell, O. C. (1981). Per-sonal i ty correl ates of under- andover-achievement at the university level. Psychological reports, 48, 787­792.

Runkel, P.J. (1959). The social-psychological basis of human relations. Reviewof Educati9nal Research, 29, 317 - 331.

Russell, D.G., & Sarason, I. (1965). Test anxiety, sex & experimentalconditions in relation to anogram solution. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, !, 396 - 493.

Ryle, A. (1968). Student casualties. Bathmore: Penguin.

Ryle, A., & Lunghi, M. (1968). A psychometric study of academic difficultyand psychiatric illness in students. British Journal of Psychiatry. 114, 57- 63.

Saenger-Ceha, M.M. (1970). Psychological and social factors in student drop­~. Amsterdam: Swets and Zeitlinger.

Sarason, S.B., & Mandler, G. (1952). Some correlates of test anxiety. Journalof Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 810 - 817.

Sarason, I.G;·, & Smith, R.E. (1971). Personality Annual Review of Psychology,2?, 393 - 446.

Savage, R.D. (1972). An exploratoryassociated with attainment in medicalEducation, ~ (1), 68 - 77.

study of individual characteristicsschool. British Journal of Medical

Schoeman, A. (1982). Die ontwikkeling van 'n voorspellingsmodel met behulpvan 'n meervetanderlikemetode met die oog op die voorspelling van sukses inst. 10 eksamens. Pretoria: Raad vir Geesteswetenskaplike Navorsing.

Schoeman, W.J. (1976). Die voorspelling van skolastiese sukses. Unpublisheddoctoral thesis, University of the Orange Free State.

Seddan, G.M. (1975). The effects of chronological age on the relationshipof intelligence and academic achievement with extroversion and neuroticism.British Journal of Psychology, 66, 493 - 500.

Shadbolt, D.R. (1978). Interactive relationships between measured personalityand teaching strategy variables. British Journal of Educational Psychology,4~ , 227 - 231.

Shivarathnamma, N. (September 1981). Scholastic achievement in relation toacademic motivation. Indian Journal of Clinical Psychology, ~ (2), 129 - 131.

Sinha, D. (1966). A psychological analysis of some factors associated withsuccess and failure in. university education. Indian Journal Review, !, 1.

Sinick, D. (1956). Two anxiety scales correlated and examined for sexdifferences. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 12, 349 - 395.

Smit, G.J. (1983). Fsigometrika, Pretoria: Haum.

/83 .....

BIBLIOGRAPHY (cont ) PAGE 83

Srivastava, A.K. (April 1979). A study of inter-correlation between somevariables found to be significantly related to under-achievement. IndianJournal of Behaviour, l (3), 26 - 28.

Stevenson, H.W., & Iscoe, 1. (1956). Anxiety and discriminative learning.American Journal of Psychology, 69, 113 - 114.

Tuel, J.K., & Wursten, R. (1965). The influence of intro personal variables onacademic achievement. Californian Journal of Educational Research, ~, 58 - 64.

van der Westhuizen, J .G.L. (1979). Manual for the use of psychological andscholastic tests as aids in school guidance. Pretoria: Human Sciences ResearchCouncil.

van Dyk, T. (1978). Elemente van druipingsprobleem: Faktore wat in die studentgelee is. In Die oorgang van skool na universi tei t Verrigtinge van dieNasionale Simposium, Pretoria, September 1978 (p. 97 - 115) Pretoria: Komiteevan Universiteitshoofde.

van Rooyen, N.M.D. (1983). Strategiee ter verbetering van skolastieseprestasiemotivering. Unpublished Master's thesis, Rand Afrikaans University.

Vaughn, R. (1968). College dropouts: dismissed vs withdrew. Personnel &Guidance Journal. 46, 685 - 689.

Veroff, J., & Peele,achievement motivationIssues, 25, 71 - 92.

S. (1969) . Initi al effects of desegregation on theof Negro elementary school children. Journal of Social

Viljoen, C.F. (1983). Tendense in skolastiese prestasiemotivering by leerlingein sekondere skoolfase. Unpublished Master's thesis, Rand Afrikaans University.Walberg, H.J., & Uguroglu, M.E. (Fall, 1979). Motivation and achievement: aquantative synthesis. American Educational Research Journal, 16, 375 - 389.

Wankowski, J.A., & Cox, J.B. (1973). Temperament, motivation & academicachievement. Birmingham: University of Birmingham Educational Survey.

Warters, L.K., & lliarters, C.W.motivation: their relationshipsthree samples of college males.477 - 481.

(1976). Two measures of resultant achievementto each other -and to academic performance forEducational and Psychological Measurement, 36,

Weiner, B. (1965). Need achievement and the resumption of incompleted tasks.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, !, 165 - 168.

Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation from mechanism to cognition. NewYork: Rand McNally College Publishing Company.

Weiner, B. (1974). Cognitive views of human motivation, New York: AcademicPress.

Weiner, B., Heckhaussen, H., Meyer, M.U., & Cook, R.E. (1972). Causalascriptions and achievement motivation. A conceptual analysis of effort andreanalysis of laws of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,21, 239 - 248.

Weiner, B., & Schneider, K. (1971). Drive versus cognitive theory. A reply toBoor and Harman. Jour~al of Personality and Social Psychology, 18, 258 - 262.

/84 .....

BIBLIOGRAPHY (cont) PAGE 84

The relationship between personalityin male college students. Journal of

Yeomans, W.N., & Lundin, R.W. (1957).adjustment and scholarship achievementGeneral Psychology, 57, 213 - 218.

Youngblood, S.A., Stuart, A., & Matin, B.J. (Winter, 1982). Ability testingand graduate admissions decision process model ing and validation.Educational and Psychological Measurement, 42 (4), 1153 - 1162.

Zarb, J.M. (Winter 1981). Non-academic predictors of successful academicachievement in a normal adolescent sample. Addescence, 16, 891 - 900.

/8::, .....

APPENDIX APAGE 85

VOORBEELD

VraagIn hoe TI mate geniet jy dit om iets saam met jou maats te doen?

1.2.3.

baienogalglad nie Antwoord

As jy baie daarvan hou om iets saam met jou maats te doen, skryfTI 1 langs "antwoord."

1.2.3.

baienogalglad nie Antwoord

As jy nogal daarvan hou om iets saam met jou maats te doen, skryf'n 2 langs "Antwoord".

1.2.3.

baienogalglad nie Antwoord

As jy glad nie daarvan hOll om iets saam met jOll maats te doen nie,skryf'n 3 langs "Antwoord".

1.2.3.

bai:enogalglad nie Antwoord

Jy moet by elke vraag net een nommer langs "Antwoord" skryf. Werkvlot en moenie lank oor 'n antwoord nadink nie.

Kantoor­gebruik

1. Sal jy, wanneer jy hard werk, in jou skoolvakke (1-3 )

1

2

3

gemiddelde punte kry

goed doen

nie goed doen nie Antwoord

1

(4)

(5)

186 .•...

2. Hou jy daarvan as n onderwyse julIe toetspuntein die klas uitlees?

1 meestal

2 miskien

PAGE 86

Kantoor­gebr-uik

3 glad nie Antwoord (6)

3. As dit vir jou redelik seker lyk dat jy moeilikewerk nie gaan regkry nie, sou jy

1 aanhou probeer

2 ophou probeer

3 iemand vra om jou te help Antwoord (7 )

4. Hoe gou wil jy weet wat jou punte is nadat julIen toets geskryf het?

1 enige tyd

2 redelik gou

3 baie gou Antwoord (8 )

5. Hoe gou verloor jy belangstelling wanneer jy niejou werk gou kan afhandel nie?

1 baie gou

2

3

na n rukkie

nie gou nie Antwoord

j

(9 )

6. Wanneer jy 'n werkstuk moet aanpak, beplan jy

1 meestal

2 soms

3 seIde Antwoord (10)

7. Hoe dikwels dink jy oor wat jy in die toekomsgaan doen?

1 baie seIde

2 seIde

3 sorns

4 gereeld Antwoord (11 )

/8 7 •.•••

8. Wie sou jy kies as julIe twee-twee n skooltaakmoet uitvoer?

1 n klasmaat wat altyd baie goeie punte kry

2 n goeie vriend/vriendin, al do en hy/sy niealtyd so goed nie Antwoord

\9. Hoe dikwels vertel jy n leuen?

1 soms

PAG£ &7

Kantoor­gebr-ui l

(12)

2 nooit nie Antwoord ( 13)

10. Dink jy dat julIe in die klas

1 hard genoeg werk

2 harder kan werk

3 baie hard werk Antwoord (14)

11. As jy n moeilik stuk werk kon regkry, sou jytrots voel?

1 miskien

2 ja

3 nie eintlik nie Antwoord (15)

12. Sou jy se dat jy gedurende 'n toets of eksamen

1 effens gespanne is

2 baie gespanne is

3 redelik op jou gemak is Antwoord

13. Veronderstel jy kry 'n probleem met jou skoolwerk;sou jou ouers jou

1 help om dit reg te kry

2 se hoe jy dit miskien kan regkry

(16)

3 op jou eie laat werk Antwoord (17)

14. Bekommer jy jou gewoonl ik nie oor- toetsui tsl ae nie, omdat

1 dit nie saak maak hoe hard n mens leer nie,jou punte bly dieselfde

2 jy weet dat jy hard geleer het

3 toetspunte daarvan afhang of die onderwyservan jou hou of nie Antwoord (18)

/88 ....•

PAGE 8$Kantoor­

gebr-ufk15. Dink jy tydens eksamens daaraan dat jy dalk kan druip?

1 soms

2 nee

3 ja Antwoord (19)

16. Wanneer jy n probleem in skoolwerk nie kon oplos nie, hoe seker is jydat jy dit die-volgende keer sal regkry?

1 redelik seker

2 baie seker

3 redelik onseker

4 beslis onseker Antwoord (20)

17. Praat jy graag saam as julIe in die klas werk moet bespreek?

1 al tyd

2 dikwels

3 s eLde

4 nooit nie Antwoord (21)

18. Hoe dikwels dink jy meer aan ander mense as aan jousel~?

1 nie sodikwels nie

2 baie gereeld Antwoord (22)

19. Hoe belangrik is dit vir jou.om beter punte as jouklasmaats te kry?

1 nie eintlik belangrik nie

2 nogal belangrik

3 baie belangrik Antwoord (23)

20. As jy sukses behaal het met n taak, sou jy n volgende taak kies wat

1 moeiliker is

2 makliker is

3 min or meer dieselfde is Antwoord (24)

/89 .....

21. Hier volg n beskywing van twee persone: J vra iemand omhom te help wanneer hy n probleem met sy skoolwerk hetjP hou aan met die probleem totdat hy dit regkry. Watter eenis die meeste 5005 jy?

1 J

PAGE 39

Kantoor­gebruik

2 P Antwoord (25)

22. Hoe dikwels is jy haastig om die uitslag van n toetsof eksamen te kry?

1 seIde

2 soms

3 meestal

4 altyd Antwoord (26)

23. Hoe dikwels sou jy se jy is selfsugtig?

1 dikwels

2 nooi t .ni e Antwoord (27)

24. As n stuk huiswerk jou lank besig hou,

1 bly jy daarin geInteresseerd

2 verveel dit jou gou

3 verveel dit jou na n rukkie Antwoord

25. Hoe dikwels bestee jy ekstra tyd om seker te maak dat jynie foute maak nie?

1 nooit nie

2 af en toe

3 meestal Antwoord (29)

26. Wanneer jy aan jou toekoms dink, voel dit vir jou

1 baie ver

2 redelik ver

3 naby

4 baie naby Antwoord (30)

27. Wanneer jy iemand moet kies om jou met n taak te help,wie sou jy kies?

1 n maat a1 kry hy!sy nie goeie punte nie

2 iemand ~at goeie punte kry, al is julIe nie maa~s nie. Antwoord (31 )

28. Dink die onderwysers gewoonlik dat jy in jou skoolwerk

1 redelik hard werk

2 baie hard werk

PAGF. 90Kantoor­

gebruH

3 meer kan werk Antwoord (32 )

29. Wanneer jy nie soveel punte kry as wat jy verwag het nie,pIa dit jou

1 nie eintlik nie

2 effens

3 baie Antwoord (33)

30. Probeer jy afluister wanneer van jou gepraat word?

1 ja

2 neem Antwoord (34)

31. As julIe in enige skoolvak n moeilike stukwerk kry, sal jy

1 net so lank soos jou klasmaats probeer omdit te doen

2' lange as die ander aanhou probeer

3 nie te lank aanhou nie Antwoord (35)

32. Slaap jy voor n eksamen

1 net so goed as gewoonlik

2 effens onrustig

3 nie so goed as gewoonlik nieAntwoord (36)

33. As jou ouers jou nie beloon vir goeie skoolwerk nie, sal jou werk

1 effens verswak

2 baie verswak

3 op dieselfde standaard blyAntwoord

34. Van watter skoolwerk sou jy die meeste hou? Werkwaar

1 jy self vir die resultate verantwoordelik is

(37)

2 toeval of geluk die resultate bepaalAntwoord

,.

~ ..-

(38 )

j~1

PAGE 91Kantoor-

gebr-uak

35. As jy hard ge1eer het vir 'n eksamen dink jy jou punte sal

1 net soveel wees as die meeste van jou klasmaat s'n

2 swak wees

3 goed wees Antwoord (39)

36. Praat jy altyd net goed van ander mense?

1 nee

2 ja Antwoord (40)

37. Gestel jy sou n toets druipi dink jy dat jydie vo1gende toets

1 miskien weer sal druip

2 beslis weer sal druip

3 sal slaag Antwoord

38. Wanneer daar 'n klasbespreking is,

1 bly jy verkieslik stil

(41)

2 neem jy graag deel Antwoord (42)

39. n Leerling'wat beter as sy maats wil doen,

1 wil ~itstyg in die lewe

2 dink hy is beter as ander

3 probeer die onderwyser se guns wenAntwoord (43)

40. Dink jy soms wanneer julIe met skoolwerk besig is dat jyeffens rnoeiliker werk wil doen?

1 ja

2 af en toe

3 nee Antwoord (44)

41. As 'n skoolmaat jou wil help met 'n probleern in jouskoolwerk sal jy sy hulp aanvaar?

1 ja

2 miskien

3 nee Antwoord (45 )

/92 .....

42. How dikwels voel jy teIeurgesteId as n onderwysernie gou ·toetsuitslae teruggee nie?

1 baie dikwels

2 dikwels

3 nie so dikwels nie

PAGE 92-Kam:6or­

gebruH

4 seIde Antwoord (46)

43. Is dit vir jou maklik om te erken as jy n fout gemaak het?

1 nee

2 ja Antwoord (47)

44. n Stuk skoolwerk wat baie tyd neem, is vir jou

1 net soos enige ander stuk werk

2 iets waarin jy gou beIangsteIling verloor

3 redelik interessant Antwoord (48)

45. Hoe belangrik dink jy is dit dat n mens jou werkmoet kontroleer om moontlike foute uit te skakeI?

1 effens belangrik

2 baie belangrik

3 nie belangrik nie Antwoord (49)

46. 'Hoe gereeld dink jy aan jou toekoms?

1 baie gereeld

2 af en toe

3 nie dikwels nie Antwoord (50)

47. Wie kies jy gewoonlik om jou met 'n opdraag te help?

1 enige klasmaat

2 'n goeie vriend of vriendin

3 'n klasmaat wat baie weet van die betrokke vakAntwoord

48. Sou jy se dat jy

1 meer werk as jou klasrr.aats

2 net soveel werk as jou klasmaats

(51)

3 'n bietjie minder werk as die meesteAntwoord (52)

/93 .....

PAGE 93Kantoor-

gebruik49. Sou jy se dat n mens trots kan voel as jy in jou skoolwerk

goed gedoen het?

1 nie eintlik nie

2 miskien

3 ja Antwoord (53)

50. Hoe dikwels vind jy gedurende eksamens dat jy nie kan onthouwat jy geleer het nie?

1 af en toe

2 seIde

3 gereeld Antwoord (54)

51- lIJanneer jy n probleem met jou skoolwerk kry,

1 hou jy op met werk

2 probeer ,jy nog 'n rukkie

3 hou jy aan totdat jy dit regkryAntwoord

52. As jy vir een stuk werk swak punte gekry net, dinkjy dat jy vir jou volgende werk in dieselfde vak

1 beslis weer swak punte sal kry

2 miskien weer swak punte sal kry

(55)

3 dalk beter punte sal kry Antwoord (56 )

53. Voel jy dat dit reg is om iemand wat jou verkulhet ook te kul?

1 ja

2 nee Antwoord (57)

54. Het jou ouers jou redelik jonk toegelaat om selfdinge te doen?

i: jy kan nie onthou nie

2 ja

3 nie eintlik nie Antwoord (58)

55. As 'n mens hard leer aan jou skoolwerk sal jyrneestal

1 goed doen

2 gerniddelde punte kry

3 swak doen Antwoord (59)

Jr, ,.

56. As jy 'iets moeiliks probeer, dink jy jy sal dit

1 meestal nie regkry nie

2 miskien regkry

3 meestal regkry

PAGE 94Kantoor­

gebruil

4 beslis regkry Antwoord (60)

\57. Hou jy van klasbesprekings?

1 af en toe

2 meestal

3 nee Antwoord (61 )

58. Dink jy dat jy graag sou wou uitstyg in die lewe?

1 ja

2 miskien

3 nee Antwoord (62)

59. Watter van die volgende sou jy verkies - werk wat

1 maklik is sodat jy dit beslis sal regkry

2 redelik maklik is, sodat jy dit waarskynliksal regkry

3 effens moeiliker is sodat jy dit miskien niesal regkry nie

Antwoord

60. Laat jy toe dat iemand jou wys wat om te doen as jyn bietjie sukkel om n probleem op te los?

1 soms

2 selde

(63)

3 meestal Antwoord (64)

61. Hoe gou wil jy weet hoeveel punte jy gekry hetwanneer jy n werkstuk ingehandig het?

1 nie noodwendig gou nie

2 die volgende dag

3 redelik gou Antwoord (65)

/95......

62. Wanneer daar TI probleem opduik in n stuk skoolwerkprobeer jy om

1 iemand te kry om jou te help

2 aIleen aan te hou totdat jy die probleemopgelos het

3 met n ander stuk werk aan te gaan

Antwoord

\

63. Wanneer julIe n moeilike toets skryf, is jy gewoonlik

1 gou klaar

2 redelik lank besig

PAGE 95Kantoor->

gebruik

(66)

3 langer as die meeste ander leerlinge besigAntwoord (67)

64. In hoen mate is jy jaloers op iemand met wiejy moet kompeteer?

1 baie

2 glad ni.e Antwoord (68)

65. Hoe dikwels.kontroleer jy of alles nog reg en in ordeis wanneer jy met huiswerk besig is?

1 gereeld

2 soms

3 nie dikwels nie Antwoord (69)

66. Hoeveel van jou onderwysers se vir jou dat jy beterkan doen in jou skoolwerk?

1 baie min

2 n paar

3 die meeste Antwoord (70)

67. As jou rna of pa jou wil help met TI probleem injou skoolwerk, sal jy

1 daarvan hou

2 miskien daarvan hou

3 nie eintlik daarvan hou nieI

Antwoord (71 )

68. As jy n moeilike stuk werk regkry, wat sou jy sehet die grootste rol gespeel?

1 geluk or toeval

2 harde werk

3 jOll eie vermoe,

• Arrtwoor c ( 72)

69. Gestel jy kry n moeilike probleem in jou skoolwerkhoe goed sou jy jou kanse skat om dit reg te kry?

1 goed

2 gemiddeld

PAGE 96

Kantoor­gebruik

3 nie goed nie Antwoord (73)

70. As jy n stuk skoolwerk nie kan regkry nie, dink jy dat jy,wanneer julle weer dieselfde soort werk kry

1 dit miskien sal regkry

2 dit beslis sal regkry

3 miskien dit weer nie sal regkry nie

Antwoord

.71. Gestel jy het n redelik moeilike stuk werk reggekry; sou jywil he jou volgende werk moet

1 net so moeilik wees

2 effens moeiliker wees

(74)

3 effens makliker wees Antwoord (75)

72. Sou jy graag wou weet wat jou onderwysers van jouskoolwerk dink?

1 ja

2 miskien

3 nee Antwoord (76=

73. Wanneer n mens met skoolwerk sukkel, behoort jy

1 liewer met ander werk aan te gaan

2 n maat te vra om jou te help

3 aan te hou totdat jy dit regkry

Antwoord

74. M is 'n leerling watbeplan voordat hy met sy huiswerkbegin; N begin dadelik werk. Wie is die meeste soos jy?

1 M

(77)

2 N Antwoord (78)

75. As jy miskien in n eksamen nie so goed gedoen het aswat jy graag wou nie, sou jy dink dis omdat

1 die vraestel redelik moeilik was

2 jy nie genoeg geleer het nie

ALtwoord (79)

PAGE 97

Kantoor-gebruik

76. Is jy haastig om jou rapport te kry na n eksamen?

1 gewoonlik nie

2 soms

3 meestal Antwoord (80)

77 • Goeie eksamenpunte hang daarvan af of

1 die eksamen maklik of moeilik is\

2 jy slim of nie so slim is nie

3 jy hard gewerk het Antwoord

78. As daar In onderbreking was terwyl jy met jou huiswerkbesig was, is dit vir jou maklik om weer te begin werk?

1 soms

2 meestal

3 nie eintlik nie Antwoord

79. Sou jy hard leer om van die beste punte in jouklas te probeer kry?

1 ja

2 miskien

3 nee Antwoord

80. As jy met h moeilike stuk werk besig is, dink jydat jy dit dalk nie sal regkry nie?

1 soms

2 meestal

(5)

(7)

3 seIde Antwoord (8)

/95 .....

PAGE 98

APPENDIX B

ITEM TEST CORRELATIONS BEFORE ITERATION

-ITEM N. X. SO RxgSg Rxg-........'

1 1048. 1.843 .372 .044 .1182 1048. 1.852 .729 .036 .0503 1048. 2.422 .894 .010 .0114 1048. 2.361 .645 .112 .1735 1048. 2.492 .573 .081 .1416 1048. 1.552 .637 .112 .1757 1048. 3.705 .620 .030 .0498 1048. 1.765 .431 .011 .0269 1048. 1.222 .418 .020 .04810 1048. 1.931 .348 .029 .08311 1048. 1.941 .270 .016 .06012 1048. 1.586 .778 .160 .20513 1048. 2.132 .809 .138 .17114 1048. 1.909 .376 .031 .08415 1048. 1.625 .680 .101 .14816 1048. 1.766 .967 .101 .10417 1048. 2.016 .700 .059 .08418 1048. 1.594 .499 .030 .06119 1048. ·1.767 .661 .017 ~O2520 1048. 1.607 .917 .044 .04821 1048. 1.447 .509 .092 .18122 1048. 2.977 .803 .133 .16623 1048. 1.490 .524 .062 .11824 1048. 2.014 .985 .015 .01525 . 1048. 2.319 .561 .000 .00126 1048. 2.552 .803 .146 .18227 1048. 1.240 .432 .013 .02928 1048. 2.041 .917 .159 .17329 1048. 2.622 .528 .021 .04030 1048. 1.511 .506 .058 .11431 1048. 1.545 .654 .121 ;18532 1048. 1.719 .770 .042 .05533 1048. 2.799 .600 .073 .12134 1048. 1.021 .150 .011 .07635 1048. 2.575 .815 .107 .13136 1048. 1.215 .427 .013 .03237 1048. 2.743 .666 .058 .08838 1048. 1.693 .466 -.022 -.00339 1048. 1.037 .209 .014 .06840 1048. 2.049 .638 .008 .01341 1048. 1.207 .419 .046 .11042 1048. 2.195 .. 868 .057 .06543 1048. 1.684 .469 .023 .05044 1048. 1.832 .905 .159 .17645 1048. 1.889 .340 .032 .09346 1048. 1.242 .446 -.002 -.00447 1048. 2.511 .537 .061 .11348 1048. 1.948 .672 .118 .17549 1048. 2.933 .305 .031 .10150 1048. 1.612 .633 .086 .13751 1048. 2.408 .522 .075 .14352 1048. 2.871 .370 .041 .11153 1048. 1.819 .390 .026 .06754 1048. 2.212 .557 .040 .07255 1048. 1.073 .274 .015 .05556 1048. 2.542 .598 .108 .181

PAGE 99

ITEM TEST CORRELATIONS BEFORE ITERATION (cont)

ITEM N. X. S RxgSg Rxg

57 1048. 1.693 .584 .108 .18558 1048. 1.049 .236 .008 .03359 1048. 2.228 .637 .113 .17860 1048. 2.116 .953 .058 .06061 1048. 2.551 .715 .091 .12862 1048. 1.572 .694 .130 .18863 1048. 2.138 .555 .045 .08264 1048. 1.760 .440 .071 .16265 1048. 1.648 .631 .064 .10166 1048. 1.927 .791 .153 .19367 1048. 1.801 .790 .157 .19968 1048. 2.256 .533 .094 .17769 1048. 1.758 .463 -.004 -.00970 1048. 1.401 .588 .080 .13671 1048. 1.740 .598 .038 .06372 1048. 1.114 .362 .018 .05073 1048. 2.545 .578 .096 .16774 1048. 1.469 .499 .065 .13175 1048. 1.766 .428 .065 .15376 1048. 2.548 .596 .074 .12377 1048. 2.932 .355 .031 .08678 1048. 1.931 .693 .096 .13979 1048. 1.409 .592 .062 .10580 1048. 1.597 .873 .173 .19881 1048. 2.484 .981 .155 .15882 1048. 1.964 .559 -.012 -.02183 1048. 2.035 .848 .006 .00784 1048. 1.696 .777 .017 .02285 1048. 2.576 .688 .099 .14486 1048. 2.247 .569 .082 .14387 1048. 1.986 .769 .110 .14388 1048. 1.787 .579 .070 .12089 1048. 1.580 .494 .059 .11990 1048. 1.412 .494 .083 .16891 1048. 1.969 .589 .095 .16192 1048. 1.736 .661 .024 .03693 1048. 2.042 .739 .026 .03594 1048. 1.386 .493 .003 .00595 1048. 1.901 .633 .085 .13596 1048. 1.587 .642 .052 .08197 1048. 2.057 .740 .181 .24598 1048. 1.927 .579 .120 .20799 1048. 2.073 .772 .051 .066100 1048. 2.200 .570 .. 044 .077101 1048. 1.959 .764 .035 .045102 1048. 1.361 .591 .072 .121103 1048. 2.239 .654 .090 .138104 1048. 1.708 .715 .097 .136105 1048. 1.522 .618 .032 .052106 1048. 1.754 .694 .113 .163107 1048. 1.668 .:'90 .064 .108108 1048. 1.581 .778 .116 .149109 1048. 2.297 .541 .139 .257110 1048. 1.361 .504 .028 .055

/IOD .....

PAGE 100

ITEM TEST CORRELATIONS BEFORE ITERATION (cont)

-. ITEM N. X. S RxgSg Rxg

111 1048. 1.394 .512 .074 .145112 1048. 1.403 .577 .069 .120113 1048. 1.807 .716 .134 .187114 1048. 1.854 .822 .208 .254115 1048. 1.673 .487 .088 .181116 1048. 2.096 .707 .168 .238117 1048. 2.580 .584 .148 .254118 1048. 2.198 .568 .152 .268119 1048. 1.831 .724 .136 .187120 1048. 2.094 .706 .108 .152121 1048. 1.514 .542 .037 .068122 1048. 1.438 .590 .043 .073123 1048. 1.554 .640 .096 .150124 1048. 1.802 .644 .117 .l81125 1048. 2.939 .932 .190 .204

/101. ....

/'

PAGE 101APPENDIX C

ITEM TEST CORRELATIONS AFTER TEN ITERATIONS

ITEM RxgSg Rxg

*1 .030 .082*2 -.102 -.140*3 .013 .015*4 .003 .005*5 .123 .214*6 -.042 -.065*7 .029 .047*8 .002 .004*9 -.009 -.022*10 -.012 -.035*11 .000 -.001*12 .070 .090*13 -.017 -.020*14 .046 .121*15 -.026 -.038*16 -.159 -.164*17 -.141 -.202*18 -.013 -.026*19 -.033 -.051*20 -.140 -.153*2-1 .070 .138*22 ;001 .001*23 .055 .105*24 -.200 -.203*25 .011 .01926 .257 .320*27 -.002 -.004*28 .010 .011*29 -.064 -.121*30 .059 .116*31 .029 .045*32 ,-.282 -.366*33 .027 .044*34 -.005 -.032*35 .060 .074*36 .003 .007*37 .136 .205*38 .106 .228*39 -.006 -.030*40 -.108 -.170*41 .000 - .001*42 .057 .065*43 .047 .101*44 .032 .036*45 .003 :.010*46 -.029 -.064*47 .017 .031*48 .020 .030*49 -.009 -.031*50 -.013 -.021*51 .073 .141*52 .052 .141*53 .014 .036*54 -.062 -.112*55 -.007 -.027

/102 .....

ITEM TEST CORRELATIONS AFTER TEN ITERATIONS (cont )

PAGE 102

ITEM

*56*57*58*59*60*61*62*63*64*65*66*67*68*69*70*71*72*73*74*75*76*77*78"'79*8081*82*83*8485868788*89*9091*92*93*9495*969798*99*100*101*102103*104*105106*107*108

.173

.033-.017

.141-.042

.015-.022-.071

.073-.015

.008

.017

.051-.127

.010

.007

.007

.028-.039

.019

.016

.027-.042

.007

.159

.384-.238-.314-.376

.276

.314

.485

.356

.076

.155

.265-.232-.313

.016

.274-.218.294.295

-.346-.165-.242-.085

.346-.160-.257

.432-.113-.268

.290

.056-.071

.222-.044

.021-.031-.127

.167-.024

.010

.021

.095-.274

.017

.012

.020

.049-.078

.045

.027

.077-.060

.012

.182

.391-.426-.370-.483

.401

.552

.631

.614

.154

.313

.451-.350-.424

.033

.433-.340

.397

.5097. 448-.290-.316-.144

.529-.224-.416

.623-.192-.345

/103 .....

1----- -~------- .---.~~o- ---------

J

ITEM TEST CORRELATIONS AFTER TEN ITERATIONS (cont)

ITEM RxgSg

*109 .194*110 -.142*111 -.150*112 -.148113 .453114 .393115 .272116 .328*117 .027~18 .306119 .478*120 -.196*121 -.216*122 -.103*123 -.253124 .400125 .530

.359-.281-.292-.256

.632

.479

.557

.464

.046

.538

.659-.278-.399-.176-.395

.620

.569

PAGE 103