mphil thesis tharaka dassanayake

Upload: anand-rasane

Post on 01-Jun-2018

230 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    1/185

     

    Cognitive effects of

    organophosphorus insecticide poisoning

    studied using reaction time,

    evoked potentials and

    event-related potentials 

    Waidyaratne Dassanayake Mudiyanselage Tharaka Lagath

    Dassanayake 

    Master of Philosophy June 2007

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    2/185

    Cognitive effects of

    organophosphorus insecticide poisoning

    studied using reaction time,

    evoked potentials and

    event-related potentials 

    Waidyaratne Dassanayake Mudiyanselage Tharaka Lagath

    Dassanayake

    MBBS (Sri Lanka)

    Department of Physiology

    A thesis submitted to the Faculty of Medicine in fulfilment of the

    requirements for the Degree of

    Master of Philosophy

    of the

    University of Peradeniya

    Sri Lanka

    June 2007

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    3/185

      i

    Declaration

    I hereby certify that the work reported in this thesis was carried out by me under the

    supervision of Dr. V. S. Weerasinghe, Dr. U. Dangahadeniya and Professor Nimal

    Senanayake.

    It describes the results obtained from my own research work except where due reference

    has been made in the text. No part of this thesis has been presented for any other degree

    in this or any other University.

    Date:................................... ...................................................

    Dr. W. D. M. T. L. Dassanayake

    Certified by:

    1. Supervisor: Dr. V. S. Weerasinghe Date: ………………..

    ……………………………………

    2. Supervisor: Dr. U. Dangahadeniya Date: ……………….

    ………………………………….

    3. Supervisor: Prof. N. Senanayake Date: ……………….

    ..………………………………….

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    4/185

      ii

    Acknowledgements

    Although this thesis presents my own research work, I would not be able to complete it

    without the guidance and the support of many individuals and organisations to which I

    must express my sincere gratitude.

    I am very much obliged to my academic supervisor and Head of the Department of

    Physiology Dr. Vajira Weerasinghe, for introducing me into scientific research and the

    field of neurophysiology. He always encouraged me and rendered intellectual and

    technical guidance from the day of inception of the thinking behind this work. Prof.

     Nimal Senanayake sharpened my thinking by critically evaluating my work and giving

    invaluable advice throughout the study. I am fortunate to have an exemplary researcher

    like him to supervise my research degree. As an academic supervisor, Dr. Udaya

    Dangahadeniya always welcomed me and provided prompt advice and guidance when I

    was in need. I am much indebted to him.

    I am thankful to my teachers and senior colleagues in the Department of Physiology;

    Prof. Malini Udupihille, Prof. Premalatha Balasooriya, Dr. Jayantha Rajaratne, Dr.

    Shamila Rajaratne, Dr. Anula Kariyawasam, Dr. Indu Nanayakkara, Dr. Anoja

    Ariyasinghe and Dr. Sudheera Kalupahana for their encouraging me and sharing my

    departmental commitments during the period of this study. Dr. Indu Nanayakkara and Dr.

    Sudheera Kalupahana provided valuable suggestions in writing up the thesis.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    5/185

      iii

     

    This project would not have been possible without the financial and intellectual support

    of the South Asian Clinical Toxicology Collaboration (SACTRC). A very special thank

    should go to Prof. Andrew Dawson, who provided numerous opportunities for me to

     present my work in the scientific community. He was a great person to work with. I am

    also thankful to all the colleagues of the SACTRC research team for supporting me in

    many ways.

    I must also acknowledge the Staff of the Poisoning Management Unit, the Neurology

    Ward and the Medical Wards, Teaching Hospital, Peradeniya who supported me in many

    ways in recruiting the study participants. I am especially grateful to Dr. Keerthi Kularatne

    who advised and helped me in clinical assessment of the patients.

    I greatly appreciate all the patients and the volunteers who consented to participate in the

    study without any hesitation.

    Finally, I deeply admire my wife Dewasmika and my mother who energized my work

    with their understanding, encouragement and love.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    6/185

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    7/185

      v

     paracetamol overdose (n=11). The second part was a prospective study where the OP

     poisoned patients were followed up after six months of poisoning and the findings were

    compared with their immediate post cholinergic phase measurements. The tests used to

    assess visuomotor information processing were simple visual reaction time, recognition

    visual reaction time, visual evoked potentials (VEP) and motor evoked potentials. The

    term “cognitive processing time (CPT)” was used to denote the time taken from initial

    cortical perception of a stimulus to initiation of descending motor impulses. CPT of each

    type of visual reactions was calculated by subtracting the sum of the visual impulse

    duration and the motor impulse duration from reaction time (CPT = Reaction time –

    (P100 VEP latency + total motor conduction time)). Auditory P300 cognitive event-

    related potential (ERP) was recorded, quantified and analysed to assess cognitive

     processing of auditory information.

    Results: Patients with OP insecticide poisoning showed significant delays in CPT of

    simple visual reactions (CPTSVR ) (p=0.037), CPT of recognition visual reactions

    (CPTRVR ) (p=0.024) and P300 latency (p=0.003) compared to healthy controls. The

     patients also had a significant impairment in CPTSVR  (p=0.017), CPTRVR  (p=0.002) and

    P300 latency (p=0.009) compared to the control group with paracetamol overdose.

    Comparison of the initial and follow-up findings of the patients revealed that the

    impairment in CPTSVR  (p=0.527) and P300 latency (p=0.867) remained unchanged even

    six months after poisoning. However, CPTRVR   showed a significant improvement

    (p=0.012). Visual and motor conduction latencies or P300 amplitude were similar

     between the groups and between the two assessments of the patients with OP poisoning.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    8/185

      vi

    Conclusions: OP insecticide poisoning appears to slow the cognitive processes involved

    in visuomotor information processing and auditory stimulus evaluation. These effects

     persist beyond the clinical recovery from the cholinergic phase, and the deficits in

    auditory stimulus evaluation and cognitive processing in simple visual reactions appear to

     be persistent even six months after exposure. These findings are compatible with the

    cognitive deficits observed in some previous human studies. The neural substrates of the

    affected cognitive processes are largely compatible with the topography of the

    neuropathological lesions that have been observed in OP exposed experimental animals.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    9/185

      vii

    Table of contents

    Declaration ………………………………………………………………… …………... i

    Acknowledgements ……………………………………………………………………... ii

    Abstract …………………………………………………………………………………. iv

    List of tables …………………………………………………………………………... xii

    List of figures …………………………………………………………………………. xiii

    List of abbreviations …………………………………………………………………... xvi

    Section 1

    INTRODUCTION …………………………………………………………………….. 1

    1.1. 

    Organophosphorus insecticide poisoning ……………………………………... 1

    1.1.1.  Epidemiology ..………………………………………………………… 1

    1.1.2.  Pathophysiology ..……………………………………………………… 3

    1.2. Behaviour and cognition ..………………………………………………………. 5

    1.3. Cognitive processing of sensory information and integration between

    cortical areas ……………………………………………………………………. 8

    1.3.1. 

    Visual perception ..…………………………………………………… 12

    1.3.2.  Auditory perception ..………………………………………………… 15

    1.4. Measurement of information processing in human brain ……………..………. 17

    1.4.1.  Reaction time ..……………………………………………………….. 17

    1.4.2. 

    Event-related potentials (ERPs) and P300 ..………………………….. 21

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    10/185

      viii

    1.5. Delayed cognitive effects of acute OP poisoning: evidence from previous

    studies and their limitations …………………………………………………… 25

    1.6.  Rationale, aims and objectives of the present study ………………………….. 29

    Section 2

    METHODOLOGY ………………………………………………………………….... 31

    2.1. Study design………………………………………………………………….… 31

    2.2. Ethical considerations …………………………………………………….…… 32

    2.3. Subjects ………………………………………………………………………... 33

    2.3.1.  Test group ……………………………………………………………... 33

    2.3.1.1.Inclusion criteria …………………………………………………… 33

    2.3.1.2.Exclusion criteria …………………………………………………... 33

    2.3.2.  Healthy control group ………………………………………………….. 34

    2.3.2.1.Inclusion criteria …………………………………………………… 34

    2.3.2.2.Exclusion criteria ………………………………………………….. 34

    2.3.3.  Hospitalised control group ……………………………………………... 34

    2.3.3.1.Inclusion criteria …………………………………………………… 34

    2.3.3.2.Exclusion criteria …………………………………………………... 34

    2.4. Background information ………………………………………………………. 35

    2.4.1.  Test group ……………………………………………………………... 35

    2.4.2.  Healthy controls ……………………………………………………...... 36

    2.4.3. 

    Hospitalised controls ………………………………………………….... 36

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    11/185

      ix

    2.5. Assessment of cognitive processing …………………………………………. 40

    2.5.1. 

    Basis of assessment paradigms ……………………………………….. 40

    2.5.2.  Equipment …………………………………………………………….. 42

    2.5.3.  Details of the techniques ……………………………………………… 44

    2.5.4. 

    Testing protocol ………………………………………………………. 53

    2.6. Data analysis …………………………………………………………………. 58

    2.6.1.  Calculated outcome variables …………………………………………. 58

    2.6.2.  Statistical analysis ……………………………………………………... 59

    Section 3

    RESULTS …………………………………………………………………………….. 61

    3.1. Data analysis ………………………………………………………………….. 61

    3.1.1.  Test group …………………..…………………………………………. 61

    3.1.2.  Healthy control group ..………………………………………………... 61

    3.1.3.  Hospitalised control group …………………………………………….. 62

    3.2. Episode of poisoning, management measures and complications ……………. 64

    3.2.1. 

    Test group …………………………………………………………....... 64

    3.2.2. 

    Hospitalised control group …………………………………………….. 67

    3.3. Intergroup comparison of measures of cognitive processing, visual and

    motor components ……………………………………………………………. 68

    3.3.1.  Visual reaction time …………………………………………………… 69

    3.3.2. 

    Components of visual reaction time …………………………………... 72

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    12/185

      x

    3.3.2.1.Visual conduction …………………………………………………. 72

    3.3.2.2.Motor component ………………………………………………….. 73

    3.3.2.3.CPT of visual reactions ……………………………………………. 76

    3.3.2.3.1.  CPT(SVR) …………………………………………………… 76

    3.3.2.3.2. 

    CPT(RVR) …………………………………………………… 76

    3.3.3.  P300 ERP ……………………………………………………………… 78

    3.3.4.  Summary of intergroup comparison of measures of information

     processing ………………………………………………………………... 81

    3.3.5.  Correlation between measures of cognitive processing ……………….. 84

    3.4. Follow up assessment of the patients with OP insecticide poisoning ………… 86

    3.4.1.  Visual reaction time ……………………………………………………. 86

    3.4.2.  Components of visual reaction time …………………………………… 87

    3.4.2.1.Visual conduction ………………………………………………….. 87

    3.4.2.2.Motor component ………………………………………………….. 88

    3.4.2.3.CPT of visual reactions ……………………………………………. 89

    3.4.3.  P300 ERP ………………………………………………………………. 90

    3.4.4.  Summary of the comparison of initial and follow up assessment of the

     parameters of cognitive processing, afferent and efferent conduction ….... 92

    Section 4

    DISCUSSION ………………………………………………………………………… 94

    4.1. 

    Effects of OP on visual reaction time …………………………………………….. 95

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    13/185

      xi

    4.2. Importance of analysis of individual components of visual reactions ………......... 96

    4.3. 

    Cognitive processes and the flow of information in visuomotor reactions ….......... 97

    4.4. Long-term effects of OP pesticide poisoning on visuomotor information

     processing ………………………………………………………………………... 101

    4.5. 

    Effects of OP poisoning on P300 event-related potential ………………………... 106

    4.6. OP induced brain damage: corroborating evidence from human studies

    and animal experiments ………………………………………………………….. 109

    4.7. Limitations of the study ……………………………………………………......... 111

    Section 5

    CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE DIRECTIONS ………………………………… 114

    List of references ……………………………………………………………………... 116

    Appendices …………………………………………………………………………… 138

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    14/185

      xii

     

    List of tables

    3.1. Comparison of age: Test group and healthy control group ………………………. 62

    3.2. 

    Age distribution of organophosphorus poisoned patients, matched hospitalised

    control group and healthy controls ……………………………………………… 63

    3.3. 

    The type of organophosphorus (OP) insecticide ingested by patients …………... 65

    3.4. 

     Numbers of subjects whose measurements were taken in tests of information

     processing ……………………………………………………………………….. 69

    3.5. Comparison of the measures of information processing: test group (initial

    assessment) vs. healthy controls ………………………………………………… 82

    3.6. 

    Comparison of the measures of information processing: hospitalised controls

    vs. matched test group subjects and healthy controls …………………………… 83

    3.7. Correlation between parameters of cognitive processing in all study participants.. 84

    3.8. Comparison of parameters of information processing of the test group patients

    in the initial post-cholinergic phase and the follow up ………………………….. 93

    4.1.  Neuropsychological findings of the epidemiological studies on chronic effects

    of acute OP pesticide poisoning ………………………………………………… 103

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    15/185

      xiii

    List of figures

    1.1. Chemical structure of organophosphorus compounds ……………………………. 4

    1.2.  Neuropsychological classification of dimensions of behaviour and cognition …... 6

    1.3. Generic scheme of information processing in sensory pathways ………………… 9

    2.1. 

    Selection and comparison of study samples ……………………………………... 39

    2.2. Calculation of cognitive processing time ………………………………………… 41

    2.3. Magstim Model 200TM magnetic stimulator with circular 90mm coil …………... 43

    2.4. Signal averaging machines ……………………………………………………… 43

    2.5. Electrode placement in VEP recording ………………………………………….. 46

    2.6. A normal VEP waveform ………………………………………………………… 47

    2.7. Magnetic field and the induced current produced by the current flowing

    through the coil of the magnetic stimulator ……………………………………… 49

    2.8. A standard  MEP waveform ……………………………………………………… 50

    2.9. Electrode placement in recording P300 component of ERPs …………………… 52

    2.10. 

    A normal P300 ERP waveform ………………………………………………… 53

    2.11. 

    Reaction time test ………………………………………………………………. 55

    2.12.  Recording auditory P300 event-related potential …………………………….... 55

    2.13.  Recording pattern reversal visual evoked potentials ………………………….. 57

    2.14.  Transcranial magnetic stimulation and motor evoked potential recording …… 57

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    16/185

      xiv

    3.1. Age distribution of the test group and healthy controls ………………………….. 62

    3.2. 

     Number of patients that showed each cholinergic feature ………………………. 67

    3.3. VRT in the test group and healthy controls …………………………………....... 71

    3.4. VRT in the test subjects, matched hospitalised controls and healthy controls ….. 71

    3.5. 

    VEPs of a patient with OP poisoning …………………………………………… 72

    3.6. Distribution of P100 latency …………………………………………………….. 73

    3.7. MEPs in a patient with OP poisoning …………………………………………… 74

    3.8. Motor conduction times in patients and healthy controls ……………………….. 75

    3.9. Motor conduction times in test subjects, matched hospitalised controls

    and matched healthy controls ……………………………………………………. 75

    3.10.  CPT(SVR) and CPT(RVR) in the test group and healthy control group …………... 77

    3.11.  CPT(SVR) and CPT(RVR): test subjects, matched hospitalised controls and

    healthy controls ………………………………………………………………….. 78

    3.12.  Grand average ERP waveforms for target tones .……………………………… 79

    3.13.  Distribution of P300 latencies …………………………………………………. 80

    3.14.  Distribution of P300 amplitudes ………………………………………………. 81

    3.15.  Correlation between CPT(SVR) and CPT(RVR) …………………………………... 85

    3.16. 

    Correlation between CPT(RVR) and P300 latency ……………………………… 85

    3.17.  Changes in VRT from initial post-cholinergic phase assessment to the

    follow up assessment ……………………………………………………………. 87

    3.18.  Changes in P100 VEP latency from initial post-cholinergic phase

    assessment to the follow up assessment …………………………………………. 88

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    17/185

      xv

    3.19.  Measures of motor conduction in the first assessment and the follow up in the

     patients with OP poisoning ……………………………………………………… 89

    3.20.  CPT of visual reactions in the patients in the first assessment and

    the follow up assessment ………………………………………………………... 90

    3.21. 

    Grand average ERP waveforms of the first assessment and the follow up

    assessment of the patients with OP poisoning …………………………………... 91

    3.22.  P300 ERP changes in patients with OP poisoning: first assessment vs.

    follow up assessment ……………………………………………………………. 92

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    18/185

      xvi

    List of abbreviations

    AChE – acetylcholinesterase

    ACh – acetylcholine

    CI – confidence intervals

    CMCT – central motor conduction time

    CPT – cognitive processing time

    CPTRVR  – cognitive processing time of recognition visual reactions

    CPTSVR  – cognitive processing time of simple visual reactions

    dB – decibels

    EP – evoked potential

    ERP – cognitive event-related potential

    FDS – Flexor digitorum superficialis

    Hz – Hertz

    IQR – inter-quartile range

    M – magnocellular

    MEP – motor evoked potential

    ms – milliseconds

    MT – movement time

    n – number of subjects

    OP – organophosphorus

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    19/185

      xvii

    P – parvocellular

    PET - positron emission tomography

    PMCT –peripheral motor conduction time

    r – Pearson’s correlation coefficient

    RAS - reticular activating system

    RRT – recognition reaction time

    RT – reaction time

    RVRT – recognition visual reaction time

    SE – standard error of the mean

    SRT – simple reaction time

    SVRT – simple visual reaction time

    TMCT – total motor conduction time

    TMS – transcranial magnetic stimulation

    V1 – primary visual cortex

    V2 – secondary visual cortex

    VEP – visual evoked potential

    WHO – World Health Organisation

    µV – microvolt

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    20/185

    1

     

    Section 1

    INTRODUCTION

    1.1.  Organophosphorus insecticide poisoning

    1.1.1. Epidemiology

    Production of synthetic pesticide formulations has a history of over half-a-century.

    In year 2000 the global production was 3.75 million metric tons (Tilman et al., 2001).

    About one-fourth of formulated pesticides are used in the developing countries

    (Jeyaratnam, 1984). Organophosphorus (OP) compounds have been the principal means

    of agricultural pest control throughout the world since 1980s (Stephens, 1995).

    In 1992, World Health Organization (WHO) reported that roughly three million

     pesticide poisonings occur annually resulting in 220,000 deaths worldwide. Poisonings

    are far more frequent in the developing world in comparison to the developed countries.

     Ninety-nine per cent of more than 200,000 pesticide-related deaths occur in the

    developing world (Jeyaratnam, 1990). Ingestion of pesticides, particularly OP

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    21/185

    2

    compounds, is a widely prevalent method of deliberate self-harm in agricultural

    communities across Asia. According to WHO reports in 2002, the estimated annual death

    rate from self-harm in the Asian region is half-a-million (World Health Organisation

    2002). Of these, ingestion of pesticides accounts for 300,000 deaths (Eddleston, 2000).

    About two-thirds of the instances (i.e. 200,000 deaths) the causative pesticide is an OP

    insecticide (Eddleston, 2000; Eddleston and Phillips, 2004).

    Like in many other Asian countries, pesticide poisoning is a major clinical and

     public health problem in Sri Lanka (Jeyaratnam et al., 1982; Eddleston et al., 1998; Van

    der Hoek et al., 1998; Fernando & Hewagalage, 1999). According to the Annual Health

    Bulletin year 2000, pesticide poisoning is the leading cause of death in many agricultural

    districts (Ministry of Health, 2001). Although pesticides are widely used in agricultural

    communities, most clinically documented acute poisonings in Sri Lanka are not due to

    occupational overexposure, but episodes of deliberate ingestion. In a study carried out in

     North-Central province, out of 526 pesticide-related poisonings 68% cases were self-

    inflicted, 19% were spraying-related and 13% were accidental (Van der Hoek et al.,

    1998). Young adults form the major proportion of deliberate self-poisoning victims (de

    Alwis, 1988; Van der Hoek et al., 1998). Occupational exposure leading to intoxication

    through inhalation and skin contact is also seen in agricultural communities. Pesticide

    sprayers who do not use protective gear are particularly at risk.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    22/185

    3

    Following the common pattern of the Asian region (Eddleston, 2000; Eddleston and

    Phillips, 2004), OP insecticides are responsible for the majority of pesticide poisonings in

    Sri Lanka. By late 1970s and early 80s, OP compounds became the main causative agents

    in pesticide poisoning associated hospital admissions in many parts of Sri Lanka

    [Colombo 1976: 26 out of 46 (55%), Peradeniya 1984/85: 92 out of 117 (79%) and Jaffna

    259 (89%) out of 291] (Senanayake, 1986). In the following decade the islandwide

    hospital statistics showed that about 75% of poisoning related admissions and 85% of

    deaths from poisoning were caused by OP insecticides (Senanayake, 1998).

    1.1.2. Pathophysiology

    All OP compounds share a common chemical structure (figure 1.1). The basic

    components include a phosphorus atom that forms a double bond with either oxygen (P =

    O) or sulphur (P = S), and three side chains. Group X differs widely in individual OP

    agents and to a great extent determines the physical and chemical characteristics of the

    compounds (Erdman, 2004). The R 1 and R 2 side chains are typically alkoxy groups, but

    may be aliphatic or aromatic hydrocarbons. The biological action of OP depends on the

     phosphorylating ability of the OP, which in turn is determined by the electrophilicity of

    the phosphorus atom. Electrophilicity is mostly determined by the substituent groups in

    the OP molecule.  In vivo activity of the OP depends also on the lipid solubility of the OP

    compound. Highly lipid soluble OP compounds can cross the biological membranes and

    the blood brain barrier, to reach the neural tissues where the OP can exert its main action.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    23/185

    4

     

    Figure 1.1: Chemical structure of organophosphorus compounds.

    OP insecticides inhibit acetylcholinesterase (AChE), the enzyme which degrades

    the neurotransmitter acetylcholine (ACh) released by cholinergic nerve endings.

    Resulting accumulation of ACh leads to different effects depending on the site. It causes

    depolarisation block of the nicotinic cholinergic receptors at the neuromuscular junction.

    Cholinergic overactivity in the autonomic nervous system principally causes

     parasympathetic stimulation through muscarinic cholinergic receptors. Accumulation of

    ACh also affects the cholinergic neural circuits in the central nervous system. Classically,

    three main syndromes of acute OP poisoning have been identified, viz. acute cholinergic

    crisis, intermediate syndrome and delayed polyneuropathy. The latter two are seen only

    in a proportion of the victims who develop acute cholinergic crisis. To a large extent, the

    amount and the type of the OP compound determines the occurrence of intermediate

    syndrome and delayed polyneuropathy.

    In addition to these manifestations, research over last few decades suggests the

     possibility of delayed sequelae of OP on ‘higher functions’ of the nervous system. The

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    24/185

    5

    observations have variously referred in literature as “neurobehavioural effects”,

    “neuropsychological abnormalities” or “neuropsychiatric changes” (Abou-donia, 2003).

    Particularly, the cognitive sequelae have drawn attention of researchers engaged in

    toxicology as well as in many other fields including neurology, neuropsychology and

     psychophysiology.

    1.2.  Behaviour and cognition

    Behaviour can be conceptualised in three dimensions, viz. cognition, emotionality

    and executive functions (Figure 1.2) (Lezak, 1995).

    1.  Cognition is defined as ‘the information handling aspect of behaviour’. In general

    terms, cognition is described as knowing, perceiving or conceiving as a faculty

    distinct from emotion and volition (Oxford Dictionary, 1994).

    2.   Emotionality concerns with feelings and motivation.

    3.   Executive functions  deal with the ways in which the mental operations are

    executed. The observable motor activity comes under this domain.

    Cognitive functions may be studied under four faculties (Figure 1.2):

    1.   Receptive functions  encompass abilities to select, acquire and integrate

    information. Major part of sensory information perception falls into this category.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    25/185

    6

    2.   Memory and learning  refers to encoding, storage, mental representations and

    retrieval of information.

    3.  Thinking, at least in a neuropsychological perspective, can be classified under the

    dimension of cognition. It involves voluntary mental organisation and

    reorganisation of information. In this view, any cognitive operation that relates

    two or more bits of information explicitly (e.g. adding two numbers) or implicitly

    (e.g. judging an action as ‘good’ or ‘bad’) can be regarded as a thinking process.

    4.   Expressive functions  encompass the mental operations dealing with means

    through which information is communicated or acted upon (e.g. speech, writing

    and drawing, gestures and manipulating). Thus, the expressive aspect of cognition

    forms the immediate mental substrate for executive functions of behaviour.

    Figure 1.2: Neuropsychological classification of dimensions of behaviour and cognition.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    26/185

    7

    However, the mental tasks cannot be strictly compartmentalised in any one of these

    functional subdivisions of cognition. Even in ‘simple’ tasks such as face-recognition,

    many of them operate in unison. The initial cortical processing involves analysis of

     perceptual properties of the visual stimulus such as lines, colour of the face and the

    spatial relationship of its parts. This mainly demands receptive functional resources. At a

    higher level of recognition, stored memory traces are retrieved to match the perceptual

    representations of the face. These latter stages of processing attribute a semantic value for

    initial visual signals (Gazzaniga et al., 1998). Therefore, visual recognition necessitates

    two facets of cognition (viz. receptive functions and memory) to work in harmony.

     Nevertheless this classification of behaviour and cognition permits easier practical

    observation, measurement and description of normal and abnormal behaviour, and

    constitutes a framework for organisation of behavioural data. 

    ‘ Activity variables’ determine the efficiency of the above dimensions of behaviour.

    Some neuropsychologists categorise consciousness and attention under activity variables

    (Lezak, 1995). For instance, solving an arithmetic problem involves numerous

    interactions between receptive functions, memory and thinking. When a person is fully

    conscious and directs maximum attention towards the task, he can integrate the above

    dimension of cognition and solve the problem more accurately and faster than when he is

    less attentive. Activity variables do not have any specific behavioural outcomes or end

     products of their own. Thus there are no ‘pure’ behavioural tests to measure activity

    variables. Behavioural correlates of activity variables are always assessed through

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    27/185

    8

     performance of a task related to other behavioural domains mentioned earlier. Thus, a

    given cognitive process involves activation of one or more faculties of cognition, the rate

    of the process being influenced by activity variables.

    1.3.  Cognitive processing of sensory information and integration between cortical

    areas

    Information processed during cognitive operations are either retrieved from

    memory stores, or acquired from the external environment as sensory inputs. Sensory

    signals generated by external stimuli are carried to brain via afferent neuronal pathways.

    These signals cross several synapses and reach the cerebral cortex. Synaptic transmission

     permits the afferent signals to be modified to a certain extent by pre- and/or post-synaptic

    influences. Thalamus, with its specific sensory nuclei, plays a major role as a multimodal

    relay station where sensory perception occurs at an elementary level. On their way to

    cortical areas, sensory pathways send inputs to the reticular activating system (RAS) of

    the brain stem. RAS is involved in maintaining the arousal state of the individual.

    Thalamocortical fibres project to primary sensory cortical areas. Higher-order perceptual

     processing begins in these modality-specific (vision, hearing, somatic sensations etc.)

     primary cortical areas. An abstract schematic representation of this initial information

     processing is illustrated in figure 1.3.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    28/185

    9

     

    Figure 1.3:  Generic scheme of information processing in sensory pathways. (RAS:

    reticular activating system).

    Primary sensory areas are found in different parts of the cerebral cortex, depending

    on the sensory modality they subserve:

    •  Primary visual area: striate cortex of the occipital lobe

    •  Primary auditory area: superior temporal gyrus

    •  Primary somatosensory cortex: postcentral gyrus

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    29/185

    10

    Billions of neurons in primary sensory cortical areas register raw sensory

    information in the form of a ‘map’. These representations per se do not have a semantic

    value. Primary sensory cortical representations are transmitted to adjacent secondary

    association areas which integrate these raw perceptions, increasing the complexity of

    sensory information. The cells in secondary association areas are also modality specific.

    Only a stimulus of the appropriate modality can fire neurons in secondary association

    areas.

    Tertiary association areas are considered responsible for integration of mental

    representations of more than one modality (Lezak, 1995). Posterior association cortex is a

    main area where this supramodal integration of perceptual functions takes place.

    Therefore it is also called heteromodal or multimodal cortex. Having a parieto-temporo-

    occipital distribution, it is in close proximity with somatosensory, auditory and visual

    cortical regions. Inputs from many sensory organs can fire the neuronal populations of

     posterior association cortex. Patients with lesions in posterior parietal cortex may develop

    constructional disorders where multimodal integration of visuospatial information is

    deficient (Black & Bernard, 1984). Transfer of impulses between cortical regions is not

    unidirectional. Feedback loops from secondary and tertiary association areas back-

    innervate more elementary sensory areas. The association areas also have numerous

    connections with subcortical structures.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    30/185

    11

    As information moves from one stage to the next, the cortical representations

     become perceptually elaborate and semantically sophisticated. Findings of single cell

    recording experiments support this. For example, a simple flash of light can stimulate

     primary visual cortical neurons. In contrast, cells of the association areas of inferior

    temporal cortex do not fire on exposure to simple lines or spots of light. Rather, they

    respond to complex stimuli such as an elaborate drawing of a hand (Desimone et al.,

    1990). Thus, visual association areas in the inferior temporal cortex appear to play a role

    is object recognition rather than simple perception of a quantum of light.

    Multimodal representations synthesised in association areas are transmitted to

    limbic and frontal association areas, to be activated into feelings, thoughts and actions

    (Pandya & Yeterian, 1985, 1990). Prefrontal division of the frontal lobe receives

    information about the external environment from postcentral structures. According to one

    estimate, around 60% of these neuronal pathways come from tertiary association areas

    and 25% from secondary association areas (Strub & Black, 1988). Prefrontal cortex also

    receives inputs form limbic system about the internal status of the body. Convergence of

    many types of information from multiple sources (memory storage, visceral arousal

    centres, sensory association areas etc.) enables the prefrontal cortex to process and

    integrate information at the highest order or greatest complexity. The prefrontal division

    interacts with the secondary motor association area in the premotor division and

    subsequently with the primary motor cortex, to bring about executive functions of

     behaviour such as movements, gestures, speech etc.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    31/185

    12

    As discussed earlier, receptive functions do not operate in isolation. There are

    numerous complex interactions with memory systems occupying multiple loci including

    the diencephalon, hippocampus, striatum, amygdala and many parts of the neocortex.

    Two-way information transfer nourishes both these faculties of cognition. Retrieval of

    information from the memory stores helps higher order semantic interpretation of

     perceptual cues, whilst perceptual processing enables encoding and consolidation of new

    external information to strengthen memory networks. In humans, visual and auditory

     perception provide large amount of external information for higher-order processing.

    1.3.1.  Visual perception

    Visual stimuli evoke sensory neural impulses that travel through afferent visual

     pathways to lateral geniculate nuclei and then to the primary visual cortex in the occipital

    lobes. Cerebral neural networks process these signals further to identify their perceptual

    and semantic properties. In the present study, processing of the visual representations

     beyond the primary visual cortex is regarded as cognitive processing of visual

    information.

    Visual perception involves concurrent, parallel analysis of various attributes of the

    stimulus (colour, shape, orientation, motion etc.) through different subsystems

    (Gazzaniga et al., 1998). This parallel processing begins in the lateral geniculate nuclei,

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    32/185

    13

    where magnocellular (M) neurons and parvocellular (P) neurons analyse different

    features of the stimulus. This M-P distinction is maintained at primary (V1 or

    Brodmann’s area 17) and secondary (V2 or Broadmann’s area 18) visual cortical regions

    as demonstrated by histological studies (Bear et al., 1996). M pathways are sensitive to

    changes in contrast and motion. As shown in cytochrome oxidase histological staining

    techniques, P pathway has two subsystems viz. blobs and interblobs in V1, which

    correspond to thin strips and interstrips respectively, in V2. Blobs and thin strips in P

     pathway are mainly sensitive to colour. Interblobs and interstrips are mainly sensitive to

    location and orientation of the stimulus. Additional evidence for parallel processing in

    visual perception comes from single cell recording studies (Hubel & Wiesel, 1968; 

    Maunsell & Van Essen, 1983), visual search task experiments (Treisman & Gelade,

    1980), and more recently, from positron emission tomography (PET) studies (Zeki,

    1993).

    The specialisation hypothesis  of visual perception is based on the above

    observations. It suggests that in a given moment, there are many maps of the same image

    in many visual areas. But each map differs according to the type of information (e.g.

    colour, shape, orientation etc.) they represent. Higher level visual processing engage

    subcortical pathways extending form V1 to adjacent parietal and temporal cortices.

    Superior longitudinal fasciculus (occipito-parietal pathway) contains axons extending to

     posterior parietal cortex. Inferior longitudinal fasciculus (occipito-temporal pathway)

    contains axons terminating in inferior temporal cortex.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    33/185

    14

    Mishkin and Ungerleider first proposed a functional model involving these two

     pathways, to explain extraction of two fundamentally different properties of visual

    information (Mishkin et al., 1982a; 1982b; 1983). According to them, the ventral,

    occipito-temporal pathway is involved in recognising what  we are looking at (i.e. object

    recognition), and hence called “what” pathway. Dorsal, occipito-parietal tracts are

    specialised in spatial perception where we identify the location of an object and the

    spatial relationship of objects in the visual field. Therefore the original researchers called

    it “where” pathway. This “what – where” dichotomy of visual perception is supported by

    electrophysiological evidence (Robinson et al.,1978; Desimone, 1991; Ito et al. 1995),

     behavioural experiments on animals (Pohl, 1973) and PET studies of normal human brain

    (Haxby et al., 1994).

    The purpose of the analysis of individual visual features is to recognise the objects

    in the visual field. According to the Warrington’s two-stage model, analysis of visual

    features helps categorisation of the object we see according to its physical properties (i.e.

    colour, shape etc.), and it is called perceptual categorisation. Patients who are unable to

     perform this perceptual categorisation are incapable of recognising an object as the same

    one when viewed under different lighting conditions or from different viewpoints

    (Gazzaniga et al., 1998). This condition is called appreciative agnosia. At a higher level

    of processing, the perceptual representation of the object undergoes semantic

    categorisation, where the perceptual representation is given a meaningful functional

    value. Those who are unable to perform this semantic categorisation suffer from

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    34/185

    15

    associative agnosia. When they are shown a set of objects and asked to choose two

    objects which are similar in terms of their function they tend to select two which are

    similar in physical appearance (Warrington & Taylor, 1978).

    At these higher levels of visual information processing where the demands are to

    recognise the object, perceptual representations interact with long-term memory traces of

    the known objects. In other words, multiple dimensions of cognition (figure 1.2) viz.

    receptive functions, memory and learning and even thinking, begin to interact strongly

    with one another.

    1.3.2.   Auditory perception

    Auditory afferents enter the brainstem via cochlear nerves. Forming synapses in

    cochlear nuclei, superior olivary nucleus, inferior colliculus and medial geniculate

    nucleus, the neuronal pathways reach the primary auditory cortex (Brodmann’s area 41)

    in the superior temporal gyrus. Secondary auditory cortex occupies the Brodmann’s areas

    42 and 43. Neurons in the auditory areas are arranged to form a tonotopic map. For

    instance, cells in a given area of the primary auditory cortex are maximally sensitive to a

     particular frequency. The frequency of maximal sensitivity changes little by little from

    one cell to the adjacent ones, so that one can map the auditory area into a frequency

    gradient. Therefore the ‘receptive field’ of a neuron involved in auditory processing is

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    35/185

    16

    expressed as a frequency range (in contrast to a visual cortical cell, of which the receptive

    field encompasses a specific area of the visual field, which can be expressed in spatial

    dimensions).

    Basilar membrane near the base of the cochlea is maximally sensitive to high

    frequency sounds, whilst low frequency sounds mainly activate the apex of the cochlea.

    The tuning specificity of auditory receptive fields becomes more refined as the stimulus

    moves through the system. For instance, a cell in cochlea that is maximally sensitive to

    5000Hz stimulus, responds to stimuli ranging from 2000Hz to 10000Hz. A cell in

    auditory cortex that responds maximally to a stimulus of 5000Hz may respond to stimuli

    of 4000Hz to 6000Hz, which is a much narrower range.

    The computational goals of audition are similar to those of vision. The two goals

    are to recognize the nature of the sound (“what?”) and the location of the sound source

    (“where?”). Scientific evidence on mechanisms of auditory information processing is

    much scarce in comparison to those of visual information processing. Auditory cortex is

    involved in determining the nature of the auditory stimulus (“what is the stimulus?”) as

    well as direction from which sound emanates. However evidence based on the

    experimental animal model barn owl, suggests that to a certain extent, the question

    “where is the stimulus?” is solved at the level of brain stem and the auditory cortex

    (Konishi, 1993). Medial superior olivary nucleus plays a major role in detecting the time

    lag between acoustic signals entering the two ears.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    36/185

    17

    1.4.  Measurement of information processing in human brain

    The rapidity of human information processing and scarcity of knowledge on the

    neural correlates of higher-order information processing makes studying the neural

     processes of cognition difficult. However, the discipline of mental chronometry, which

    has origins that date back to more than a century, seeks to measure the time-course of

    mental operations in the human brain. Mental chronometric tasks have been used

    extensively in cognitive neuroscience to elucidate mechanisms underlying cognitive

     processing. From late 1800s to 1950s, assessment of temporal nature of information

     processing was built almost entirely around a single behavioural method: measuring and

    comparing reaction times during simple cognitive tasks.  Later, with the development of

    electrophysiological techniques, evoked potentials (EPs) and cognitive event-related

     potentials (ERPs) started to contribute to elucidation of the time-course of information

     processing in the nervous system. Present study uses visual reaction time (VRT) tests, EP

    and ERP techniques to quantitatively assess the cognitive processing of visual and

    auditory information processing in victims of OP poisoning.

    1.4.1. Reaction time

    Reaction time (RT) is defined as the time that passes from arousal of a sensory

    organ to a motor reaction. Depending on task requirements and complexity, RT tests are

    widely used to evaluate the time-course of various cognitive processes (Gazzaniga et al.,

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    37/185

    18

    1998). All types of reaction tasks demand analysis of certain perceptual and/or semantic

     properties of a presented stimulus (e.g. colour, shape, position in space, tone, meaning of

    a word etc.), and execution of an appropriate motor response. The analysis of perceptual

    and semantic properties of the stimulus involves receptive functions, memory updating

    and thinking, whereas selection and execution of motor response involve expressive

    functions of cognition.

    In simple reaction time (SRT) experiments, there is only one stimulus (e.g. white

    flash) and one response (e.g. pressing a button). Therefore the responder does not have to

    concentrate on specific characteristics of the stimulus (e.g. whether it is a white flash or a

    red flash) and just need to respond to the stimulus as quickly as possible. If the stimulus

    is of visual modality the test is a simple visual reaction time  (SVRT) test.

     Neuropsychologists use SRT as a measure of attention (Lezak, 1995).

    In recognition reaction time (RRT) experiments, there are some stimuli that should

     be responded to, and others that should get no response. For example, in a recognition

    visual reaction time  (RVRT) test, either a white flash or a red flash may appear

    randomly, and the requirement is to respond only to white flashes as soon as possible.

    Thus there is still only one correct response. Before responding the individual has to

    detect the stimulus and also discriminate the target stimulus (i.e. white flash) from the

    distracting stimulus (i.e. red flash).

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    38/185

    19

    Pioneer RT studies were those of F.C. Donders in 1868.  He showed that SRT is

    shorter than RRT. Laming (1968) in his studies, determined that SRT averaged 220ms

    and RRT averaged 384ms. According to Bellis (1933), pressing a key in response to a

    light (i.e. SVRT) is around 220ms in males, and about 260ms in females (Kosinski,

    2000). This is in line with many studies that concluded that complex stimulation

     paradigms, as in RRT experiments, elicit slower RTs (Brebner, 1980, Teichner et al.,

    1974). Over the last century, RT has been used to assess psychomotor speed in variety of

    neurological and psychiatric illnesses (e.g. mental retardation, dementias, depression,

    schizophrenia, drug dependence) and in performance oriented groups (e.g. sportsmen).

    Being a behavioural test, RT includes not only cognitive processing of information

     but also sensory and motor components of the task. Welford (1968) defined three

    components of a psychomotor reaction: ‘first, the time taken by the stimulus to activate

    the sense organ and for impulses to travel from it to the brain; second, the central

     processes concerned with identifying the signal and initiating a response to it; and third,

    the time required to  energize the muscles and to produce an overt recorded response’

    (Danev et al., 1971). With invent of electrophysiological techniques, there are direct

    ways to assess the sensory and motor components. 

    1.  Sensory Component:

    This can be visual, auditory etc., depending on the modality of the stimulus. The

    duration of this component is the time period between stimulation of the sensory

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    39/185

    20

    receptors and arrival of the afferent neuronal impulse to the primary sensory (visual,

    auditory or somatosensory) cortex. If the stimulus is visual, the light strikes the retina,

    stimulates photoreceptors and initiates the afferent nerve impulses that reach the primary

    visual cortex. Transmission of impulses along the visual pathways can be studied using

    visual evoked potential (VEP) techniques. In VEP recordings P100 component is a

    waveform that peaks around a latency of 100ms. Research evidence suggests that it is

    generated by the neuronal activity of the striate cortex when a visual impulse reaches the

    occipital cortex (Seki et al. 1996).  Thus, P100 latency can substitute the duration of

    afferent component of a visual reaction. 

    2.  Cognitive Processing:

    This comprises the processes that occur in between stimulation of the

    corresponding sensory (visual/auditory) cortex and initiation of the motor signal in the

    area of the primary motor cortex that control the responding part of the body (e.g. hand,

    finger etc.). In this process, more meaningful mental representations of the stimulus are

    linked with neural generators of the appropriate motor response. Thus perception of the

    sensory stimulus comes under receptive functions of cognition whereas selection of the

    appropriate response can be categorised under thinking and expressive functions of

    cognition. In comparison to simple reactions, cognitive processing takes a longer time in

    recognition reactions because the brain needs to discriminate between two or more types

    of stimuli and decide on the appropriate motor response.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    40/185

    21

    3.  Motor component:

    Once the desired motor response (e.g. flexion of the index finger to press a response

     button) is planned, corresponding area of the primary motor cortex is stimulated. The

    efferent signals travel along the corticospinal tracts and stimulate the corresponding

    anterior horn cells and then transmit along the peripheral nerve to generate the endplate

     potentials at the fibres of the target muscles. The time taken for this motor conduction

    can be measured with motor evoked potential (MEP) studies, where the primary motor

    cortex is stimulated and the MEPs are recorded at the skeletal muscles. The duration

     between motor cortical stimulation and the onset of MEP denotes the duration of the

    motor component of the psychomotor reaction. Motor cortical stimulation can be

     performed by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) which is a safe and non-invasive

     procedure (Barker et al., 1985).

    The sensory and motor components involve the same neuronal pathways and their

    durations are expected to be the same in all types of RT tests. This implies that the

    differences in RTs are due to differences in cognitive component (Miller, 2001).

    1.4.2. Event-related potentials (ERPs) and P300

    Recording the average event-related electrical potentials from scalp electrodes

     became a research tool in the 1960s with the advent of analogue and then digital

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    41/185

    22

    computers to accomplish the recording and averaging. The principal use of ERPs is to

    determine the time course of cognitive processes in the human brain (Bressler, 2002).

    The physiological basis of the cortical ERPs lies in fields of potential generated by

    interacting neurons. Activity of the neural circuits of the brain generates electrical fields

    which can be recorded over the scalp as potential differences on a time scale. The

     potential at a given site over the scalp changes every millisecond, reflecting the ever-

    changing activity of underlying neural circuits. This electrical activity is contributed to by

    numerous mental processes which cannot be identified in isolation. Thus, in order to

    observe the neural activity related to a particular cognitive process two main

    requirements have to be fulfilled.

    First, the electrical potentials related to the cognitive process of interest, should be

    captured  during the short time duration it occurs. In ERP tests this is done by applying an

    external event, the properties of which are analysed by the brain. For example, if the tone

    of a presented auditory stimulus needs to be analysed, this stimulus evaluation process

    generates electrical activity in cerebral neural circuits within the next fraction of a

    second. Thus if acquisition of the electrical activity begins at the onset of the stimulus

    and continue for next second or so, the electrical potentials related to the mental process

    of stimulus evaluation can be captured. In other words, in order to capture  a particular

     brain process, the recording epoch has to be time-locked with the stimulus that evokes the

    cognitive process. However, there are so many unrelated neural activities going on even

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    42/185

    23

    during this small time span, and this ‘noise’ (as labelled in electrophysiology), may mask

    the captured mental process of interest.

    Thus, secondly, the captured mental process needs to be highlighted   or isolated .

    This highlighting is done through averaging many time-locked epochs. Unrelated

    electrical activity, having no fixed temporal relationship with the stimulus and the

    recorded time epoch, cancels out with repeated averaging, retaining the potentials of the

    mental process of interest in the recording.

    ERP components could be systematically related to sensory and motor stages of

    information processing. Present study focuses on the auditory ERP waveform P300,

    which is generated in analysing auditory stimuli. P300 was first demonstrated by Sutton

    and colleagues in 1965 (Sutton, et al., 1965). It is a positive ERP deflection which peaks

    around a latency of 300 - 600ms, when a person is exposed to randomly timed two

    different stimuli of the same modality, and required to respond to the infrequent (rare)

    stimulus (also called the target stimulus). Thus the paradigm that is used to elicit P300 is

    called the ‘oddball paradigm’. The response to the target stimulus need not necessarily be

    a motor response. Even mentally counting the target stimuli evoke P300.

    Behavioural correlates of P300 indicate that it reflects a consequence of orienting

    and attending to task-relevant events. Therefore P300 latency is considered a measure of

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    43/185

    24

    ‘stimulus evaluation time’, that is independent of selection of the response (Kutas et al.,

    1977; McCarthy and Donchin, 1981).

    P300 latency was found to have good test-retest reliability in normal individuals, so

    that it has the stability required for clinical and research applications (Polich, 1986;

    Sclare et al., 1984). However, P300 latency is affected by aging. Older subjects were

    found to have longer P300 latencies than young (Marsh & Thompson, 1972; Pfefferbaum

    et al., 1979, 1980a, 1980b; Ford et al., 1979, 1982a, 1982b; Beck et al., 1980). 

    Assessment of P300 latencies over a continuous age range have demonstrated that this

    increase in latency begins at puberty and extends into the eighth decade (Brown et al.,

    1983).

    ERP amplitude variation is considered to reflect the variation in intensity of

    activation of neural structures in the brain by the task variables (Kok, 1990). In

    experimental settings that apply the simple ‘oddball’ paradigm, P300 amplitude also

    depends on the probability of the target stimulus; rarer the target, greater the amplitude of

    the P300 it elicits (Coles, 1995).

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    44/185

    25

    1.5.  Delayed cognitive effects of acute OP poisoning: evidence from previous

    studies and their limitations

    The first reports of chronic neurobehavioural manifestations of OP poisoning date

     back to 1960s. They mainly reported symptoms such as poor memory, confusion,

    anxiety, drowsiness, fatigue, depression and irritability in patients heavily exposed to OP

    compounds (Dille & Smith, 1964; Gerson & Shaw, 1961; Metcalf & Homes, 1969;

    Durham et al., 1965). Although these studies were of little help to reach conclusions, they

    have, for the first time, highlighted the possibility of development of ‘‘chronic OP-

    induced neuropsychiatric disorders’’ (Jamal, 1997). After these early studies, research on

    long-term neurobehavioural effects of OP compounds pursued along two lines:

    1.  The occurrence of chronic neurobehavioural impairment as a consequence of an

    acute OP poisoning (Savage et al., 1988; Rosenstock et al., 1991; Steenland et al.,

    1994; Wesseling, et al., 2002, Stallones & Beseler, 2002)

    2.  The occurrence of neurobehavioural changes as a consequence of prolonged

    exposure without preceding episodes of acute poisoning. Changes with chronic

    low-level exposure were studied in many occupational groups including farmers

    (Fiedler et al., 1997; Stallones & Beseler, 2002), farm-workers (Daniell et al.,

    1992; London et al., 1997), sheep-dippers (Stephens et al., 1995) and termiticide

    applicators (Steenland et al., 2000).

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    45/185

    26

    Present study focuses on delayed cognitive effects of acute OP poisoning. Other

    than initial case studies, only few large scale analytical epidemiological studies have

    assessed the chronic neurological sequelae of acute OP poisoning (Savage et al., 1988;

    Rosenstock et al., 1991; Steenland et al., 1994, Wesseling et al., 2002).

    However, there are several methodological limitations and inconsistencies of the

    findings in the early case studies (Gerson & Shaw, 1961; Dille & Smith, 1964; Durham et

    al., 1965; Metcalf & Homes, 1969) as well as recent case-control studies (Savage et al.,

    1988; Rosenstock et al., 1991; Steenland et al., 1994, Wesseling et al., 2002):

    1.  All these were cross-sectional studies where patients were assessed once, usually

    months to years after poisoning. The large scale epidemiological studies on

    delayed effects of acute poisoning (Savage et al., 1988; Rosenstock et al., 1991;

    Steenland et al., 1994, Wesseling et al., 2002) present little information about the

    acute clinical picture: the type and amount of OP, cholinergic manifestations and

    complications.

    2.  All the investigators have used symptom inquiry and/or neuropsychological

    testing tools to assess cognitive functions. However, all neuropsychological tests

    require the subjects to perform a particular motor task. Thus the behavioural test

     performance depends largely on cognitive  functions as well as executive 

    functions. In other words, standard neurobehavioural tests (e.g. RT) cannot assess

    the cognitive component in isolation, as the neurobehavioural task duration

    includes the sensory and motor conduction. When these behavioural experiments

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    46/185

    27

    are used to deduce conclusions on cognitive functions, it is assumed that patient’s

    sensory and motor conduction are normal. However normal variations and

     pathological deviations in sensory or motor conduction can affect the behavioural

    test outcome, particularly in those where the speed of the performance is tested

    (e.g. RT tests). In this respect, OP induced delayed neuropathy is a major

    confounding factor. Delayed polyneuropathy manifests after 1-4 weeks of acute

    OP poisoning, causing slowed sensory and motor conduction (Lotti & Moretto,

    2005; Glynn, 2006).

    3.  Techniques such as electrophysiology and functional neuroimaging, which assess

     brain function at a more fundamental level, were hardly ever used in the previous

    studies to elicit chronic cognitive effects of acute OP pesticide poisoning except

    in few, where EEG recordings were analyzed (Metcalf & Holmes, 1969; Savage

    et al., 1988). So far, OP related ERP changes have been reported only in three

    studies: two of them evaluated ERP changes in chronic subclinical exposure in

     pesticide applicators (Teo et al., 1987; Misra et al., 1994) and the other one

    assessed ERP in victims of OP warfare agent sarin (Murata et al., 1997). There

    are no studies that evaluated the delayed effects of acute OP insecticide poisoning

    on ERP.

    4.  Cognitive processing test performance depends on many long-term/stable (e.g.

    age, sex, alcohol abuse etc.) confounding factors, and short-term modifying

    factors (level of arousal, recent alcohol/tobacco intake, sleep deprivation etc.)

    (International Programme on Chemical Safety, 2001). In the previous research

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    47/185

    28

    reports, authors have described the long-term confounding factors. However,

    information is insufficient to ascertain whether the investigators have taken steps

    to minimise the influence of short-term modifying factors before testing the

    subjects.

    5.  Patients have complained more neuropsychiatric symptoms than controls in all

    four epidemiological analytical studies (Savage et al., 1988; Rosenstock et al.,

    1991; Steenland et al., 1994, Wesseling et al., 2002). However neurological

    examination did not reveal any abnormalities in poisoned subjects. Reviewers

    have questioned the validity of the positive symptomatology, suggesting that it

    may be due to recall bias: patients who were exposed to pesticides are more

    motivated to recall or report symptoms (Kamel & Hoppin, 2004).

    6.  Findings of neuropsychological tests are also inconsistent among different

    studies. On one hand, the ambiguity of the results of these studies may be

    attributed to variations in sampling criteria and methodological differences from

    one study to another. On the other hand, some subsequent authors have

    questioned the objectivity of the testing methods and the sensitivity of the existing

    objective testing tools in identifying any subtle impairment in higher brain

    functions (Steenland et al., 2000; Kamel & Hoppin, 2004). However, one

    common feature is that all studies have revealed abnormalities in at least one

    aspect of visual information processing.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    48/185

    29

    1.6.  Rationale, aims and objectives of the present study

    Present study was designed hypothesising that acute OP insecticide poisoning

    impairs cognitive processing of visual and auditory stimuli, even after recovery form the

    cholinergic phase of poisoning. In contrast to cross-sectional studies conducted by

     previous researchers on patients several months after exposure, we enrolled the patients

    during the acute stage of poisoning. Initial assessment was done in the immediate post-

    cholinergic phase and a follow up assessment was done six moths after poisoning.

    Furthermore, selection criteria and study protocols were designed to minimize the effects

    of long-term confounding factors and short-term modifying factors that affect cognitive

     performance. The effect of age and sex on the measures of cognitive processing was

     particularly considered and hence the patients were individually matched with the control

    groups for age and sex.

    Particular attention was given to the psychophysiological indicators of cognitive

     processing of visual and auditory information. We aimed at isolating the cognitive

    component of visual reactions, which is not possible with standard RT measurements.

    The term ‘Cognitive Processing Time (CPT)’ is used in this study to specify the duration

    of the cognitive component of a visual reaction. This component of psychomotor function

    has not been studied to date. As the sensory and the motor components are outside the

    cognitive domain and vary even among healthy individuals, CPT is a more refined and

    accurate measure (in comparison to RT itself) of cognitive processing of information. We

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    49/185

    30

    used ERPs to assess cognitive processing of auditory information. With more refined and

    quantitative tests which closely correlate with actual neuronal processes (viz. cognitive

     processing time in RT tasks, EPs and ERPs), main concerns were to maximize the

    objectivity of the findings and to identify the exact mental processes affected.

    Objectives:

    Specific objectives of this study were to compare the psychophysiological measures

    of cognitive processing of visual information (viz. cognitive processing time of simple

    visual reactions and cognitive processing time of recognition visual reactions) and

    cognitive processing of auditory information (viz. P300 latency and amplitude),

    1.   between patients with OP insecticide poisoning (on recovery from the cholinergic

     phase) and matched controls

    2.   between immediate post-cholinergic phase and six months after poisoning in the

     patients.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    50/185

    31

     

    Section 2

    METHODOLOGY

    2.1.  Study design

    This study consisted of two parts.

    First part  was a case-control study where parameters of cognitive processing were

    compared in patients with OP insecticide poisoning and matched control groups. This

     phase included three groups: the test group and two control groups.

    1. Test group: These were the patients with acute OP poisoning. They were tested twice:

    initially in the immediate post-cholinergic stage of poisoning, and then six months after

     poisoning.

    2. Healthy controls: Healthy individuals from the community, who did not have any

    exposure to OP compounds.

    3. Hospitalised controls: This control group consisted of patients hospitalised with

     paracetamol overdose. This second group was added to match the general wellbeing and

    the psychological status of the test and the control groups at the time of

    neurophysiological assessment. The drug paracetamol was chosen as it has no known

    direct action on cognitive functions.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    51/185

    32

     

    The second part was a follow up study where the patients with OP poisoning were

    tested six months after poisoning. The follow up results of the patients were compared

    with their initial values.

    The study was carried out in medical wards, poisoning management unit and the

    clinical neurophysiology laboratory, Teaching Hospital, Peradeniya. Data were collected

    over a period of 31 months, from July 2004 to February 2007. Patients admitted to the

    medical wards and the poisoning management unit during this period were assessed for

    eligibility for the study.

    2.2.  Ethical considerations

    The study design and protocols complied with the World Medical Association

    Declaration of Helsinki. Informed written consent was obtained from all participants (see

    appendix 1 for patient information sheet and the consent form). Ethical clearance was

    granted by Research and Ethical Review Subcommittee, Faculty of Medicine, University

    of Peradeniya, Sri Lanka, on 18/06/2004.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    52/185

    33

    2.3.  Subjects

     2.3.1.  Test group

    All patients admitted to the hospital with suspected OP poisoning were assessed for

    eligibility.

    2.3.1.1. Inclusion criteria:

    1)  Reliable history of OP exposure

    2)  Any of the clinical features of cholinergic over-activity: fasciculation, miosis,

     bradycardia, excessive sweating / salivation, dyspnoea/lung signs, impaired

    consciousness

    2.3.1.2. Exclusion criteria:

    1)  Chronic exposure to OP compounds (e.g. occupational exposure in farmers, pesticide

    applicators etc.)

    2)  Other pre-existing (diagnosed) neurological illnesses, which can affect cognitive or

    motor functions

    3)  Gross visual or hearing impairment where the patient is unable to identify visual and

    auditory stimuli

    4)  Long-term intake of medications which have the potential to alter cognitive functions

    5)  Consumption of alcohol: more than the upper limit of the safe range (men >

    21units/wk, women > 14units/wk. One unit = equivalent of 8 grams of alcohol)

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    53/185

    34

     2.3.2. Healthy control group

    2.3.2.1. Inclusion criteria:

    1) Age- and sex-matched individuals with similar social background: For the social and

    educational factors to be comparable, patients were instructed to bring a relative or a

    friend of the same age and sex, on attending cognitive function tests. In the instances they

    were unable to do that, appropriate volunteers were tested. The age of the matched

    control was within 3 years of the age of the test group subject.  

    2.3.2.2. Exclusion criteria:

    1)  Past history of OP poisoning

    2)  All the exclusion criteria applied for the test group (criteria 1 to 5 under 2.3.1.2.)

     2.3.3.   Hospitalised controls

    2.3.3.1. Inclusion criteria:

    1) Age- and sex-matched individuals hospitalised with paracetamol overdose. The age of

    the matched control was within 3 years of the age of the test group subject.  

    2.3.3.2. Exclusion criteria:

    1)  Clinical features of hepatic encephalopathy

    2)  Past history of OP poisoning

    3)  All the exclusion criteria applied for the patient group (criteria 1 to 5 under 2.3.1.2.)

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    54/185

    35

    2.4.  Background information

    Before selecting the participants for cognitive tests, background information were

    collected using structured data sheets (see appendix 2). This aimed at assessing the

    eligibility (criteria under 3.3 above) for the study, finding out the details of poisoning,

    and identifying any confounding factors.

     2.4.1.  Test group

    Clinical interview, examination and inward assessment targeted the information

    related to following areas (see appendix 2.1):

    1.  Personal details: Name, sex, age, postal address, occupation, educational status and

    computer familiarity

    2.  Details of the episode acute poisoning: type of OP, amount, route of exposure, mode

    (intentional/accidental)

    3.  Signs characteristic of OP poisoning: on admission and during hospital-stay

    4.  Management measures: gastric lavage, atropine, pralidoxime, intensive care and other

    supportive treatment

    5.  Any major complications: e.g. respiratory/cardiac arrest, seizures, shock etc.

    6.  Probable confounding factors and other performance modifying factors:

    •   prediagnosed visual/neurological/cognitive impairment

    •   pre-diagnosed psychiatric illnesses

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    55/185

    36

    •  alcohol, tobacco and other addictive drug use: duration, regularity, amount per

    day/week

     2.4.2.   Healthy controls

    Background information included personal details and probable confounding

    factors and other performance modifying factors stated under the information of the test

    group (areas 1 and 6 above) ( Appendix 2.2).

     2.4.3.   Hospitalised controls

    Background information collected was similar to those of the test group:

    1.  Personal details: Name, sex, age, postal address, occupation, educational status and

    computer familiarity.

    2.  Details of paracetamol overdose: amount, mode (intentional/accidental) if intentional,

    the reason

    3.  Clinical features: on admission and during hospital-stay

    4.  Management measures: antidotes (N-acetylcysteine), supportive treatment, intensive

    care

    5.  Any major complications: e.g. liver failure, hepatic encephaolpathy

    6.  Probable confounding factors and other performance modifying factors stated above

    under information of the test group (see appendix 2.3).

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    56/185

    37

    All the patients with intentional poisoning were referred to the hospital psychiatrist.

    The psychiatric assessment and the diagnosis were also included in the record.

    After assessment of background information, the eligible subjects were selected to

     perform tests of cognitive functions. Numbers of recruited subjects and the design of

    inter-group comparison are illustrated in figure 2.1. From July 2004 to September 2006,

    67 patients admitted to Teaching Hospital, Peradeniya with a history of OP poisoning

    were assessed for eligibility for the study. Of these, nine did not meet the inclusion

    criteria either because the evidence of OP exposure was unreliable or because they did

    not show clinical features of intoxication. Of the 58 who fulfilled the inclusion criteria,

    two were excluded due to chronic occupational exposure to OP and seven were excluded

    due to excessive intake of alcohol. These nine patients were males. Of the remaining 49,

    five could not be tested as they left the hospital prematurely. Accordingly, 44 patients (28

    males and 16 females) were assessed after recovery form cholinergic phase (i.e. 1st visit),

    usually on the day of discharge from the hospital. Recovery from acute poisoning was

    considered complete when the patients were free of all signs and symptoms of the acute

    cholinergic syndrome, and free of medication such as atropine. There was at least 24-

    hour gap between the last dose of atropine/benzodiazepines and cognitive function tests.

    The median duration between exposure and cognitive testing was of 10.5 days (Range: 3

    to 47 days).

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    57/185

    38

    The healthy control group that comprised 43 individuals matched for age and sex

    with the patients on one-to-one basis (healthy control number-64 was matched with the

     patient number-18 and number-26). Healthy controls were tested on an appointment

     basis. 

    Recruitment of the hospitalised control group (i.e. patients with paracetamol

     poisoning) was started in April 2006. Patients admitted to the hospital with paracetamol

     poisoning were enrolled in the hospitalised control group only if the patient could be

    matched for sex and age (to nearest 3 years) with a member of the test group.

    Accordingly, the hospitalised control group comprised 11 subjects (three males and nine

    females) who could be matched individually for age and sex with 11 patients in the test

    group. The hospitalised control group was also compared with 11 healthy individuals

    matched with the 11 OP poisoned patients drawn from the test group. This helped to

    determine whether psychological conditions associated with self harm and hospitalisation

    (which was present in patients with paracetamol overdose, but absent in healthy

    individuals) affects the test performance. Hospitalised control group was tested after

    recovery from the acute stage, on the day of discharge from the ward. The median

    duration between ingestion and testing was 5 days (range: 3 to 6 days).

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    58/185

    39

    67 patients with suspected OP

     poisoning assessed for eligibility

    58 fulfilled

    inclusion criteria

    9 excluded:

    2: chronic occupational exposure

    7: excessive alcohol intake

    49 eligible patients

    5 did not participate

    44 assessed for

    cognitive functions

    on recovery from

    cholinergic phase

    43 healthy

    controls

    11 hospitalisedcontrols

    30 assessed for

    cognitive fuctions

    after 6 months

    14 loss of follow up

    Age-and-sex-

    matched

    11 patients drawnfrom test group

    Age-and-sex-

    matched

     

    Figure 2.1: Selection and comparison of study samples.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    59/185

    40

    The subjects were instructed and made to stick to the following criteria before

    attending the cognitive function tests, so that it was possible to minimize the short-term

    factors modifying the cognitive processing speed (International Programme on Chemical

    Safety, 2001): 

    1.  Abstain from alcohol on the day of testing and the day before

    2.  Abstain from smoking and drinking coffee on the day of the test

    3.  Having a sleep of at least 6 hours in the night before the test

    Subjects were inquired for contraindications for magnetic stimulation (intracranial

    metal implants, cardiac pacemakers) but none had any contraindication. A neurological

    examination and a visual acuity test were performed on the day of cognitive assessment.

    2.5.  Assessment of cognitive processing

    After neurological examination, subjects participated in the test battery devised to

    assess cognitive processing of visual information and auditory information.

     2.5.1. 

     Basis of the assessment paradigms

    Visual information processing: Studying the cognitive processing of visual information

    was based on measurement of the cognitive component of visual reactions. We denoted

    the cognitive component of visual reactions as ‘cognitive processing time’ (CPT), and

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    60/185

    41

    quantified the CPT using combined VRT test and EP studies. The collective duration of

    the sensory component, cognitive component and the motor component of the visual

    reaction was measured using VRT tests. VEPs were used to measure the afferent

    conduction time of the visual stimulus, and MEPs to measure the motor component of the

    reaction time. P100 VEP latency was considered the sensory component. We stimulated

    motor cortical neurons using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and recorded the

    MEPs of the muscle that responds in the visual reaction. Then we measured the duration

     between cortical stimulation and the onset of MEPs, which is the total motor conduction

    time (TMCT). This was considered the length of the motor component of the visual

    reaction. Then CPT of the visual reaction was calculated by subtracting the sum of the

    sensory component and the motor component from the VRT (figure 2.2).

    Figure 2.2: Calculation of cognitive processing time (CPT). VRT: visual reaction time.

    TMCT: total motor conduction time.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    61/185

    42

    Auditory information processing: Assessment of cognitive processing of auditory

    information was based on findings of ERPs which are electrophysiological correlates of

    cognition. Applying the auditory oddball paradigm, we recorded ERPs that reflect the

    auditory stimulus evaluation process of the neural circuits.

     2.5.2.   Equipment

    1.  A computer-based test (SermionTM) was used to measure SVRT and RVRT. Visual

    stimuli were displayed on a 14-inch colour monitor.

    2.  A Magstim Model 200TM magnetic stimulator with a circular 90mm coil (Type 9784)

    was used for TMS and spinal stimulation (figure 2.3).

    3.  Medtronic Keypoint® signal averaging machines were used to record and average EPs

    and ERPs (figures 2.4A & 2.4B). MEPs were recorded with pre-gelled surface

    electrodes. 10mm gold-plated cup electrodes were used for VEP and ERP recording.

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassanayake

    62/185

    43

     

    Figure 2.3: Magstim Model 200TM magnetic stimulator with circular 90mm coil.

    Figure 2.4: Signal averaging machines: (A) Medtronic KeypointTM (used to record visual

    evoked potentials and motor evoked potentials). (B) Medtronic Keypoint PortableTM 

    (used to record ERPs).

     A B

     

  • 8/9/2019 MPhil Thesis Tharaka Dassan