knowledge sharing: an empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf!...

305
Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive factors in an organizational setting by Michael Max Evans A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Faculty of Information University of Toronto © Copyright by M. Max Evans 2012

Upload: vukhuong

Post on 21-Mar-2018

223 views

Category:

Documents


4 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

           

Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive factors in an organizational setting

by

Michael Max Evans

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Faculty of Information University of Toronto

© Copyright by M. Max Evans 2012

Page 2: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  ii  

Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive factors in an organizational setting

M. Max Evans

Doctor of Philosophy Faculty of Information University of Toronto

2012

Abstract Effective knowledge sharing within project teams is critical to knowledge-intensive

professional service firms. Prior research studies indicate a positive association between

trust, social-cognitive factors, and effective knowledge sharing among co-workers. The

conceptual framework proposed here builds on these studies, and draws from theoretical

foundations from the organizational behavior, psychology, information studies,

sociology, and management literature on organizational trust and knowledge sharing, and

identifies the most significant factors found to influence organizational knowledge

sharing directly and indirectly through trust. The study makes methodological

contributions in the form of conceptualizations for knowledge sharing behavior, trust, and

tie strength. Also, it provides a more nuanced and focused analysis, by factoring for

knowledge type and co-worker working relationship.

Data were collected from 275 knowledge workers (‘legal professionals’ and paralegals)

engaged in shared legal project work, at one of Canada’s largest multijurisdictional law

firms. The nature of their work required a significant reliance on co-workers, for both

explicit and tacit knowledge. Multiple regression analysis, among other statistical

techniques, was used to test the hypotheses and determine significant relationships.

Page 3: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  iii  

Of the factors examined in the study, the three found to have the strongest effect on

respondents’ trust in their co-workers were shared vision, shared language, and tie

strength. Furthermore, the two factors found to have the strongest effect on organizational

knowledge sharing behavior were trust and shared vision. Overall trust was also found to

have a mediating effect between shared vision and knowledge sharing behavior, and

between shared language and knowledge sharing behavior.

A significant implication for practitioners is that effective knowledge sharing among co-

workers requires a nurturing manager to work on developing co-worker trust and shared

vision. Furthermore, a manager wanting to promote trust between co-workers must

nurture shared language and shared vision.

Page 4: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  iv  

Table of Contents Abstract ...........................................................................................................................................ii

List of Figures ................................................................................................................................vi

List of Tables.................................................................................................................................vii

Acknowledgments...........................................................................................................................x

Chapter 1: Introduction.................................................................................................................1 1.0 Chapter Overview .........................................................................................................................1 1.1 Problem Statement and Motivation .............................................................................................1 1.2 Purpose and Scope .......................................................................................................................2 1.3 Research Approach ......................................................................................................................3 1.4 Significance of the Study..............................................................................................................4 1.5 Structure of the Dissertation ........................................................................................................5

Chapter 2: Literature Review and Theoretical Framework ......................................................7 2.0 Chapter Overview .........................................................................................................................7 2.1 Knowledge in an Organizational Setting .....................................................................................7

2.1.1 Data, Information, and Knowledge .......................................................................................8 2.1.2 Tacit and Explicit Forms of Knowledge ..............................................................................11 2.1.3 Knowledge Sharing and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Defined .........................................18

2.2 Organizational Trust and Knowledge Sharing .........................................................................20 2.2.1 Understanding Organizational Trust...................................................................................22 2.2.2 Trust and Knowledge Sharing Behavior..............................................................................28

2.3 Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust ...........................................................................................35 2.3.1 Homophily and Trust ...........................................................................................................35 2.3.2 Shared Perspective and Trust ..............................................................................................41 2.3.3 Tie Strength and Trust .........................................................................................................42 2.3.4 Relationship Length and Trust.............................................................................................43

2.4 Social-Cognitive Factors and Knowledge Sharing ...................................................................45 2.4.1 Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior ....................................................................45 2.4.2 Shared Perspective and Knowledge Sharing Behavior .......................................................49 2.4.3 Tie Strength and Knowledge Sharing Behavior ..................................................................51 2.4.4 Relationship Length and Knowledge Sharing Behavior ......................................................52

2.5 Conceptual Framework ..............................................................................................................52

Chapter 3: Research Design and Methodology .........................................................................57 3.0 Chapter Overview .......................................................................................................................57 3.1 Research Questions and Hypotheses .........................................................................................57 3.2 Operationalization of Variables .................................................................................................58

3.2.1 Social-Cognitive Variables (Independent Variables) ..........................................................58 3.2.2 Trust (Mediating Variable) ..................................................................................................65 3.2.3 Knowledge Sharing Behaviors (Dependent Variables) .......................................................74

3.3 Nature of Co-worker Relationship.............................................................................................82 3.4 Reliability and Validity ...............................................................................................................82 3.5 Data Analysis Strategy ...............................................................................................................83 3.6 Selection of Study Population ....................................................................................................84 3.7 Data Collection ...........................................................................................................................85

Chapter 4: Results ........................................................................................................................87 4.0 Chapter Overview .......................................................................................................................87 4.1 Survey Sample Size .....................................................................................................................87 4.2 Survey Respondents ....................................................................................................................87

4.2.1 Respondent Profile by Role and Department ......................................................................88

Page 5: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  v  

4.2.2 Respondent Profile by Gender .............................................................................................89 4.2.3 Respondent Profile by Birth, Country, Citizenship, and Ethnicity ......................................90 4.2.4 Respondent Profile by Education and Age ..........................................................................90

4.3 Analyses of Measures .................................................................................................................91 4.3.1 Knowledge Sharing Behavior (KSB) ...................................................................................91 4.3.2 Overall Trust ........................................................................................................................95 4.3.3 Shared Language and Shared Vision...................................................................................98 4.3.4 Relationship Length and Tie Strength ...............................................................................101 4.3.5 Homophily..........................................................................................................................104

4.4 Summary of Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables ............................................................107 4.5 Hypothesis Testing....................................................................................................................109

4.5.1 Age, Gender, and Educational Homophily ........................................................................109 4.5.2 Independent Variable 4 – Shared Language .....................................................................134 4.5.3 Independent Variable 5 – Shared Vision ...........................................................................140 4.5.4 Independent Variable 6 – Relationship Length .................................................................146 4.5.5 Independent Variable 7 – Tie Strength ..............................................................................150 4.5.6 Independent Variable 8 – Overall Trust ............................................................................158 4.5.7 Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Knowledge Sharing Behavior....................................................................................................................................................162 4.5.8 Mediating Effect of Overall Trust ......................................................................................168

4.6 Summary of Hypothesis Tests ..................................................................................................180 4.7 Summary of Results by Research Question.............................................................................182

Chapter 5: Discussion and Summary .......................................................................................185 5.0 Chapter Overview .....................................................................................................................185 5.1 Discussion of the Research Findings ......................................................................................191

5.1.1 Relationships Between Social-Cognitive Variables and Trust ..........................................191 5.1.2 Research Question 1 Summary ..........................................................................................204 5.1.3 Relationships Between Social-Cognitive Variables and Knowledge Sharing Behavior ...204 5.1.4 Research Question 2 Summary ..........................................................................................219 5.1.5 Relationships Between Trust and Knowledge Sharing Behavior ......................................219 5.1.6 Research Question 3 Summary ..........................................................................................223 5.1.7 Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Knowledge Sharing Behavior....................................................................................................................................................223 5.1.8 Research Question 4 Summary ..........................................................................................230 5.1.9 Mediating Effects of Trust Between Social-Cognitive Factors and Knowledge Sharing Behavior ......................................................................................................................................231 5.1.10 Research Question 5 Summary ........................................................................................237

5.2 Discussion of the Main Findings and Research Contributions .............................................238 5.3 Limitations of the Study ...........................................................................................................243 5.4 Implications for Practice ..........................................................................................................243 5.5 Conclusion and Future Work ..................................................................................................247

References ...................................................................................................................................250

Appendix .....................................................................................................................................269 A.1 Knowledge Definition Components.........................................................................................269 A.2 Survey Instrument....................................................................................................................279 A.3 Firm-wide Invitation Email .....................................................................................................291 A.4 University of Toronto Ethics Approval Letter ........................................................................293

     

Page 6: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  vi  

List of Figures Figure 2.1 Boisot’s Data, Information, and Knowledge Relationship ...........................................10 Figure 2.2 Tsoukas’ Representation of Polanyi’s Personal (Tacit) Knowledge ............................14 Figure 2.3 Original KSB Literature Map .......................................................................................21 Figure 2.4 Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman’s Proposed Model of Trust ............................................25 Figure 2.5 Conceptual Framework Based on Literature Review ...................................................56 Figure 2.6 Conceptual Framework of Decomposed Homophily Factors.......................................56 Figure 4.1 The Focal Relationship and Trust as a Mediating Variable........................................168 Figure 4.2 Significant Relationships and Corresponding Hypotheses for Positive Referents .....171 Figure 4.3 Significant Relationships and Corresponding Hypotheses for Negative Referents....176 Figure 5.1 Conceptual Framework...............................................................................................186 Figure 5.2 Collective Effect of Factors on Knowledge Sharing Behavior for Positive Referents

.............................................................................................................................................225 Figure 5.3 Collective Effect of Factors on Knowledge Sharing Behavior for Negative Referents

.............................................................................................................................................226 Figure 5.4 Summary of the Overall Mediating Effects of Trust for Positive Referents and

Negative Referents...............................................................................................................233

Page 7: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  vii  

List of Tables Table 2.1 Synonyms of Explicit Knowledge..................................................................................15  Table 3.1 Research Questions and Associated Hypotheses ...........................................................58 Table 3.2 Operationalization and Measurement of the Shared Perspective Research Variables...61 Table 3.3 Operationalization and Measurement of the Tie Strength Research Variable ...............64 Table 3.4 Operationalization and Measurement of the Ability-Based/Competence-Based Trust .69 Table 3.5 Operationalization and Measurement of the Benevolence-Based Trust ........................72 Table 3.6 Operationalization and Measurement of the Integrity-Based Trust ...............................73 Table 3.7 Operationalization and Measurement of the Propensity to Trust...................................74 Table 3.8 Operationalization and Measurement of Willingness to Share and Use Knowledge ....78 Table 3.9 Reliability Results for Measures of Holste’s Knowledge Variables..............................79 Table 3.10 Operationalization and Measurement of Levin and Cross’ Perceived Receipt of Useful

Knowledge Research Variable ..............................................................................................82 Table 4.1 Respondents by Role ......................................................................................................88 Table 4.2 Respondents by Role within the Firm by Department ...................................................89 Table 4.3 Respondents by Age.......................................................................................................90 Table 4.4 Factor Analysis Results for Knowledge Sharing Behavior............................................92 Table 4.5 Reliability Results for Combined Dependent Variables ................................................93 Table 4.6 Descriptive Statistics for Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables ...............................95 Table 4.7 Factor Analysis Results for Trust...................................................................................96 Table 4.8 Reliability Results for Trust Variables...........................................................................97 Table 4.9 Descriptive Statistics for Trust Variables.......................................................................98 Table 4.10 Factor Analysis Results for Shared Language and Shared Vision...............................99 Table 4.11 Reliability Results for Combined Shared Language and Shared Vision Variables ...100 Table 4.12 Descriptive Statistics for Shared Language and Shared Vision Variables.................100 Table 4.13 Factor Analysis Results for Tie Strength Variables ...................................................102 Table 4.14 Reliability Results for Combined Tie Strength Variables..........................................103 Table 4.15 Descriptive Statistics for Tie Strength and Relationship Length Variables...............103 Table 4.16 Descriptive Statistics for Age and Age Difference ....................................................105 Table 4.17 Descriptive Statistics for Gender and Gender Difference ..........................................106 Table 4.18 Descriptive Statistics for Education and Educational Gap.........................................107 Table 4.19 Descriptive Statistics for Study Variables..................................................................109 Table 4.20 Correlations Between Age and Trust Variables .........................................................110 Table 4.21 Regression of Trust on Age and Other Independent Variables..................................111 Table 4.22 Correlations Between Age and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables..................113 Table 4.23 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Age and Other Independent Variables

.............................................................................................................................................114 Table 4.24 Correlations Between Age Difference and Trust Variables.......................................116 Table 4.25 Regression of Trust on Age Difference and Other Independent Variables................117 Table 4.26 Correlations Between Age Difference and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables 117 Table 4.27 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Age Difference and Other

Independent Variables .........................................................................................................118 Table 4.28 Correlations Between Education and Trust Variables ...............................................120 Table 4.29 Regression of Trust on Education and Other Independent Variables ........................121 Table 4.30 Correlations Between Education and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables .......121 Table 4.31 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Education and Other Independent

Variables ..............................................................................................................................122 Table 4.32 Correlations Between Educational Gap and Trust Variables.....................................123

Page 8: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  viii  

Table 4.33 Regression of Trust on Educational Gap and Other Independent Variables..............124 Table 4.34 Correlations Between Educational Gap and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables

.............................................................................................................................................125 Table 4.35 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Educational Gap and Other

Independent Variables .........................................................................................................126 Table 4.36 Correlations Between Gender and Trust Variables....................................................127 Table 4.37 Regression of Trust on Gender and Other Independent Variables.............................127 Table 4.38 Correlations Between Gender and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables.............128 Table 4.39 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Gender and Other Independent

Variables ..............................................................................................................................129 Table 4.40 Correlations Between Gender Gap and Trust Variables ............................................131 Table 4.41 Regression of Trust on Gender Gap and Other Independent Variables .....................132 Table 4.42 Correlations Between Gender Gap and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables .....133 Table 4.43 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Gender Gap and Other Independent

Variables ..............................................................................................................................134 Table 4.44 Correlations Between Shared Language and Trust Variables....................................135 Table 4.45 Regression of Trust on Shared Language and Other Independent Variables ............136 Table 4.46 Correlations Between Shared Language and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables

.............................................................................................................................................138 Table 4.47 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Shared Language and Other

Independent Variables .........................................................................................................139 Table 4.48 Correlations Between Shared Vision and Trust Variables.........................................140 Table 4.49 Regression of Trust on Shared Vision and Other Independent Variables..................142 Table 4.50 Correlations Between Shared Language and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables

.............................................................................................................................................143 Table 4.51 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Shared Vision and Other Independent

Variables ..............................................................................................................................145 Table 4.52 Correlations Between Relationship Length and Trust Variables ...............................146 Table 4.53 Regression of Trust on Relationship Length and Other Independent Variables........148 Table 4.54 Correlations Between Relationship Length and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Variables ..............................................................................................................................149 Table 4.55 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Relationship Length and Other

Independent Variables .........................................................................................................150 Table 4.56 Correlations Between Tie Strength and Trust Variables............................................152 Table 4.57 Regression of Trust on Tie Strength (Prior to)/Tie Strength (While on) and Other

Independent Variables .........................................................................................................153 Table 4.58 Correlations Between Tie Strength (Prior to) / Tie Strength (While on) and

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables .............................................................................155 Table 4.59 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Tie Strength (Prior to) / Tie Strength

(While on) and Other Independent Variables ......................................................................158 Table 4.60 Correlations Between Overall Trust and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables ...159 Table 4.61 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Overall Trust and Other Independent

Variables ..............................................................................................................................161 Table 4.62 Group 1 MRA Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results................................162 Table 4.63 Group 2 MRA Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results ...............................163 Table 4.64 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Overall Willingness to Share

Knowledge in Groups 1 and 2 .............................................................................................164 Table 4.65 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Willingness to Share Explicit and

Tacit Forms of Knowledge in Group 1................................................................................164

Page 9: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  ix  

Table 4.66 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Willingness to Share Explicit and Tacit Forms of Knowledge in Group 2................................................................................165

Table 4.67 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Overall Willingness to Use Knowledge in Groups 1 and 2 .............................................................................................165

Table 4.68 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Willingness to Use Explicit and Tacit Forms of Knowledge in Group 1................................................................................166

Table 4.69 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Willingness to Use Explicit and Tacit Forms of Knowledge in Group 2................................................................................167

Table 4.70 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge in Groups 1 and 2 .............................................................................................167

Table 4.71 Results of the HMRA for Positive Referents (Group 1) ............................................172 Table 4.72 HMRA Model Change Summary for Positive Referents (Group 1) ..........................172 Table 4.73 Group 1 HMRA Results for Shared Language on Knowledge Sharing Behavior.....173 Table 4.74 Group 1 HMRA Results for Shared Vision ...............................................................174 Table 4.75 Results of the HMRA for Negative Referents (Group 2)...........................................177 Table 4.76 HMRA Model Change Summary for Negative Referents (Group 2) ........................177 Table 4.77 Group 2 HMRA Results for Shared Vision ...............................................................178 Table 4.78 Summary of the Mediating Effects of Overall Trust..................................................180 Table 4.79 Summary of Data Analysis and whether the Hypotheses were Supported ................181  Table 5.1 Significant Relationships Unique to the Nature of the Working Relationship ............242 Table 5.2 Summary of Abrams, Cross, Lesser and Levin’s Trust Builders and Associated

Managerial Actions..............................................................................................................245

Page 10: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  x  

Acknowledgments I am immensely proud of the work in this dissertation and I would never change the path

I took to get to this point. Especially because along the way, I have met many wonderful

people to whom I owe my gratitude for all their support, guidance, and encouragement

over the years. Without them, I could not have reached this incredible milestone in my

journey.

In moving to the United States, my parents made the decision to give up everything they

had and start a new life in a country they could not even speak the language of. Without

their incredibly brave decision, my opportunities and directions in life would have

certainly been quite different. I love you very much mom and dad and I understand and

truly appreciate the sacrifice you made on my behalf! I would also like to acknowledge

and dedicate this work to my grandmothers, who instilled in me the value of a good

education and the importance of being a well-rounded, cultured person. Most importantly

they inspired me to be patient, kind, and benevolent and to have strong values and ethics.

This may have taken me some time to learn, but better late than never! I would like to

thank Beata, who bravely endured hearing and reading more about my work than any

human alive. More importantly, she had to deal with my thesis-related anxiety over the

years, which she did very calmly and lovingly. You were right - everything turned out

great. Love you for being so supportive! Next, I would like to thank my Canadian

expatriate family, who were the driving force behind my coming to The University of

Toronto to complete my Masters and subsequent PhD (thanks for the idea, Len). Their

support and love provided much needed strength throughout this process. Finally, I would

like to thank Mark and Lora who have treated me like a member of their own family,

since the first day we met, and always had confidence in my abilities and have shown me

tremendous support and love. I am proud to have you as family.

My co-supervisors and committee members were exceptional at guiding me through the

scholarly process, yet still allowing me to learn, grow, and make my own decisions. I can

never choose the right words or write enough of them to thank Anthony and Chun Wei

for the amount of time and effort they invested in me, from my Masters onward. I truly

Page 11: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  xi  

look up to both these wonderful people and consider them to be exemplar academics,

personal mentors, heroes, and friends. The same may be said about Kelly, who, while

joining later in the process, was vital in shaping my research and helping me through the

most significant phases. I will never forget the belief they all had in my work and in me,

or the commitment and sacrifice they have made. It is true that we stand on the shoulders

of those that came before us.

There are several other people who were instrumental to the completion of the study and

who I owe an incredible debt of gratitude and acknowledgment. First, I would like to

thank my great friend Catalin, for his tireless comments, edits, support (both mental and

professional), encouragement, consultation, and of course English lessons. Everyone

should be so lucky as to have a friend like Cat in their life. Next, a big Thank You to

Joel, for making an invaluable introduction and for being a wonderful friend and

colleague for many years. Your encouragement meant a lot. Next, I would like to thank

Norm and Andrew for believing in the value of my research, for convincing senior

management of this value, and most importantly for helping me execute an incredibly

successful yet very elaborate survey. I would never have dreamt that we would get so

many professionals to take the time to complete such an intricate survey. Finally, thank

you to Melissa for generously and patiently teaching me how to clean up my data and for

showing me how to use a statistical tool I was unfamiliar with.

I would also like to acknowledge a number of individuals who were instrumental in

shaping and supporting my education over the years. Each of these people took from their

time to answer my questions, always listened and addressed my concerns, were always

up for a spirited debate, and most importantly provided excellent mentorship, guidance,

and support. First, thank you to all of the memorable and influential professors I had in

my undergraduate (especially Dr. T), my Masters, and my PhD (especially Lynne and

Bill). Thank you Dr. Scott, Dr. Herman, Dr. Joseph, and especially Dr. Steve who, as

senior doctoral students helped me understand and prepare for the challenges I would

face in my PhD. Thank you to my colleagues and visiting scholars from the KMRC

(especially Dr. Riva), from KMDI (the main reason I elected UofT), and from my cohort.

Page 12: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  xii  

Thank you to the Faculty of Information, KMDI, and UofT administrative staff, who

never tired of my questions. A special thank you to all my co-workers at UTM, who I am

proud to call friends. I have tremendously enjoyed teaching there over the years, but of

course it is easy to love your job when you are surrounded by wonderful co-workers.

Finally, thank you to all my students who have helped me shape and refine my teaching

skills and who keep me feeling young.

Lastly, I would like to acknowledge my friends, who are such an important part of my

life. First, to my friends in Chicago, who have continued to make an effort to stay close,

despite the hundreds of miles separating us (especially Andrew, Stanley and Joshua). I

know how easy it is to lose touch, and I appreciate it. A special thank you to Michael and

Shurik for their support throughout the years. They really define how good friends should

treat each other. I would also like to thank my friends in Toronto for welcoming me to

your city, including me in your circles of friends, and of course getting me addicted to

golf. I have had a great time studying in Toronto over the years. I would also like to

express my gratitude to Anthony and Karen who have always made me feel a part of their

family and their home. Last but most certainly not least, I would like to acknowledge

Sammy, who bravely made the trek to Toronto with me and has faithfully been by my

side ever since.

 

Page 13: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  1  

Chapter 1: Introduction 1.0 Chapter Overview

The first chapter introduces the present study, which examines factors that influence the

knowledge sharing behavior of knowledge workers in a professional service firm, directly

and indirectly through trust. The chapter begins with a brief statement of the research

problem and discussion of research motivation, purpose, and scope. Next, an overview of

the research approach and the significance of the study are discussed. The chapter

concludes with a summary of the structure of the dissertation.

1.1 Problem Statement and Motivation

A fundamental assumption of the knowledge-based view of the firm (Grant, 2002) is that

knowledge has market value and is one of the most productive resources for

organizations. In addition, knowledge is subject to economies of scale (i.e. initial creation

costs are higher than replication costs) and is a necessary resource for the production of

goods and services for the marketplace (Grant, 2002). The central importance of

knowledge to the production and the creation of value is an important area of study for

researchers in management disciplines, such as strategy, organizational science, and

organizational behavior. A better understanding of the nature of knowledge, its creation,

use, sharing, combination, and value, facilitates an improved understanding of the

functioning of firms in particular, and contemporary developed economies in general.

Such an understanding is both valuable in itself and also a potential source of strategies

and methodologies leading to the improved performance of firms and the economies

within which they are embedded.

It is difficult to argue with the fact that the effective sharing of knowledge has numerous

benefits for the organization and the individuals involved. Cyr and Choo (2010) give a

few examples of the organizational benefits, which include building on past experience

and knowledge, responding more quickly and efficiently to problems, developing new

ideas and insights, and avoiding reinventing the wheel or repeating past mistakes. These

benefits, however, are hard earned for the organization, since finding ways to promote

effective knowledge sharing behavior between employees can be a significant challenge.

Page 14: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  2  

One reason for this is because “knowledge-sharing behaviors in organizations are highly

complex social interactions” (Dalkir & Wiseman, 2004, p. 64), which are influenced by

trust and other social or cognitive human factors. The study of these human factors is a

relatively new focus area for knowledge management (KM) theorists, which means that

there is not much empirical evidence, or organizational protocol, that assists researchers

in understanding (or practitioners in promoting) effective knowledge sharing behaviors in

an organizational setting (Ruggles, 1998). “While KM is currently experiencing

enormous growth in popularity, little empirical research and development has been done

to better understand the factors influencing knowledge sharing” (Dalkir & Wiseman,

2004, p. 59).

1.2 Purpose and Scope

The phenomenon of interest in the present study is the knowledge sharing behavior

(KSB) of employees in organizations. Specifically, the research examines knowledge

workers on projects in a professional service firm. Of specific interest are trust and other

social factors that have been found to influence or inhibit knowledge sharing behavior in

this setting.

Since a majority of the existing empirical and theoretical research in this area point to

trust as an influencing factor for knowledge sharing behavior, trust was included as a

central factor to the study. For this research, the most important role of trust in the

organization is its ability to support or facilitate knowledge sharing behavior. “A clear

understanding of trust and its causes can facilitate cohesion and collaboration between

people” (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 710-711). Past research has shown trust to have this effect

through an increase in willingness to share information and ideas with others (Davenport

& Prusak, 1998; Empson, 2001; McDermott & O’Dell, 2001; Husted & Michailova,

2002; Hendricks, 1999; Hinds & Pfeffer, 2003; Szulanski 1995; 1996) and decrease in

the time and effort associated with information search and processing (Zaheer, McEvily,

Page 15: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  3  

& Perrone, 1998; Limerick & Cunnington, 1993; Roberts & O’Reily, 1974; Husted &

Michailova, 2002; Orlikowski, 1993; Mayer et al., 1995; Uzzi, 1997; Burt, 1992).1

This study seeks to expand the understanding of the relationship between trust and

knowledge sharing behavior by answering the overarching research question: what are

the factors that influence knowledge sharing behavior directly and indirectly through

trust? Additional influencing factors were identified through a review of organizational

literature and empirical studies that focused on social or cognitive factors, which

influence organizational trust, especially in the context of knowledge sharing. Numerous

factors were identified and an additional five constructs were added to the study. These

include shared language, shared vision, homophily, tie strength, and relationship

duration. The study sought to test the direct influence of each of these social and

cognitive factors (constructs) on both trust and knowledge sharing behavior. In addition,

the mediating effect of trust was tested between each of these factors and knowledge

sharing behavior. Finally, the collective effect of these factors and trust on knowledge

sharing behavior was tested.

1.3 Research Approach

In the present study, an exploratory approach was used to conduct empirical research on

knowledge workers, at a large legal professional service firm. The study collected

quantitative data from 275 participants, using a web-based survey as a primary research

instrument. The goal of the survey was to understand which of the social and/or cognitive

factors influenced knowledge sharing behavior, directly and indirectly through trust.

Statistical analyses were used to test the hypotheses, which assisted in answering the

overarching and sub-research questions. Statistical analyses of the data included factor

and reliability analysis, correlation analysis, t-tests, and multiple regression analysis. The

mediating (i.e. indirect) effect of trust was tested using hierarchical multiple regression

analysis and steps outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986). Additional data, gathered from

                                                                                                               1 A complete discussion of the relationships between trust and knowledge sharing behavior is discussed in Chapter 2.

Page 16: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  4  

site visits and un-structured interviews with senior executives at the firm, were used to

interpret the findings of the study.

1.4 Significance of the Study

In theoretical terms, the present study contributes to earlier research on knowledge

sharing behavior and trust by building a conceptual framework that includes several of

the most significant factors found to influence or inhibit knowledge sharing behavior

and/or trust in previous empirical studies. This study extends previous research by

investigating the direct influence of these factors on knowledge sharing behavior and

trust. The study also tests whether trust exerts a mediating influence between these

factors and knowledge sharing behavior. This is important, since few research studies

deem trust as a mediating variable; and no known study examines the mediating

influence of this number of factors through trust, onto knowledge sharing behavior. In

addition, no known study considers this number of factors, or tests their collective

influence on knowledge sharing behavior.

The use of a larger set of social and cognitive factors in this study is significant for at

least three reasons. First, measuring the influence of each of the factors on trust and

knowledge sharing behavior can provide a comprehensive analysis of the motivators of

both (as well as their relative levels of influence). Second, this larger set of factors can

form the basis for an empirical model that better predicts, or explains, the existence and

the level of trust/knowledge sharing behavior between parties, in a work relationship.

Finally, the examination of the inter-relatedness of trust, social and cognitive factors, and

knowledge sharing behavior provides a better understanding of the true influencing and

inhibiting factors on effective knowledge sharing behavior in an organizational setting.

The present study also extends the understanding of knowledge sharing behavior found in

previous studies. This perspective takes into account three separate knowledge sharing

behaviors, including: the willingness of the knowledge owner to share their knowledge;

the willingness of the knowledge receiver to use the knowledge shared; and the perceived

benefit or utility of the shared knowledge. Each of these knowledge sharing behaviors has

Page 17: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  5  

been operationalized and used in previous organizational studies, but this is the first study

where all three elements are included, for a better understanding of organizational

knowledge sharing behavior. An in-depth comprehension of these behaviors was also the

reasoning behind asking participants to answer survey items about two co-workers they

mentally selected based on the nature of their working relationship. For example, the

respondent is asked to comment on one co-worker with whom he or she had a positive

working relationship, and another with whom he or she had a negative working

relationship with. Other important distinctions the study makes include between the form

of knowledge transferred (overall, explicit, and tacit) and the type of trust (overall,

ability-based, integrity-based, and benevolence-based).

In practical terms, the research study helps practitioners in professional service firms

understand the factors that lead to effective knowledge sharing behavior and, more

importantly, suggests methods to foster, promote, and improve them. The reality is that it

can be expensive and time consuming to implement strategies that promote trust and

every social or cognitive factor found to positively influence knowledge sharing behavior

in the firm. In addition, many of the previously identified influencing factors may not

appear as significant, when others are considered at the same time. The results of the

research study provide practitioners guidance and direction with respect to which factors

are most important, or most significant, for the firm to focus its resources on. The study

also offers some suggestions, for practitioners, on the ways they may promote and

nurture the most important factors found.

1.5 Structure of the Dissertation

The rest of this dissertation is organized as follows. Chapter 2 introduces the significant

research questions and the supporting literature review, which is presented in four parts:

Section 2.1 introduces the main phenomenon of interest by defining organizational

knowledge, distinguishing between explicit and tacit knowledge, and defining knowledge

sharing behavior; Section 2.2 examines the literature and theories on relationships

between trust and knowledge sharing behavior; Section 2.3 defines each of the identified

social-cognitive factors and explores the literature on relationships between these factors

Page 18: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  6  

and trust; and Section 2.4 examines the literature on relationships between the identified

social-cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior. Chapter 2 concludes by

presenting a conceptual framework for the research study. Chapter 3 details the research

design and methodology used in the study, beginning with a discussion of the research

questions and associated hypotheses. Next, data measurement and data analysis strategies

are discussed. The chapter concludes with a discussion of the study population, site, and

data collection methods. Chapter 4 presents the results of the statistical analyses and the

testing of the hypotheses using correlation, regression, and mediation analysis. The

chapter closes with a summary of the results by research question and a summary of

whether the hypotheses were upheld. Chapter 5 discusses each of the research findings,

compares them to previous research, and suggests possible reasoning behind the

relationships. The chapter concludes with a discussion of the main findings and research

contributions, the limitations of the study, and the implications for future research as well

as practice.

Page 19: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  7  

Chapter 2: Literature Review and Theoretical Framework 2.0 Chapter Overview

The following is a review of the literature addressing the main research question: what

are the factors that influence knowledge sharing behavior directly and indirectly

through trust? To further understand this research question, three additional questions

were posed. What are the significant relationships between trust and knowledge sharing

behavior? What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and

trust? And, what are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and

knowledge sharing behavior?

Based on these questions, the literature review is presented in four parts: Section 2.1

Knowledge in an Organizational Setting introduces the main phenomenon of interest by

defining organizational knowledge, distinguishing between explicit and tacit knowledge,

and defining knowledge sharing behavior; Section 2.2 Trust and Knowledge Sharing

defines trust and examines the literature and theories on relationships between trust and

knowledge sharing behavior; Section 2.3 Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust defines

each of the identified social-cognitive factors and explores the literature on relationships

between these factors and trust; and finally, Section 2.4 Social-Cognitive Factors and

Knowledge Sharing examines the literature on relationships between the identified social-

cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior. The chapter concludes by presenting a

conceptual framework for the research study.

2.1 Knowledge in an Organizational Setting

A great deal of research in knowledge management (KM) is concerned with

epistemological issues relating to how knowledge is acquired and how it might be

differentiated from opinion, belief, and other related concepts. Although these issues are

fundamentally important to explore and explicate for any researcher in KM, they are not

the focus of this research. This research is concerned with examining organizational

knowledge sharing behavior, the lack of which has been identified by Hendricks (1999)

as a significant barrier to effective knowledge management. To fully understand, define,

and frame knowledge sharing behavior, it must first be differentiated from other similar

Page 20: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  8  

constructs in the field of knowledge management, including data, information,

knowledge, and knowledge sharing.

2.1.1 Data, Information, and Knowledge

Some authors (Alavi & Leidner, 2001; Earl, 2001) feel comfortable using the terms

information and knowledge interchangeably, since they argue for little practicality in

making a distinction. This approach, perhaps, tends to arise from a systems or computer

science background and although it might prove to be easier for scientific inquiry (i.e.

being easily quantifiable and measurable), it does not reflect significant distinctions,

which are discussed below (Tsoukas 2005a, Boisot 1998, 2002; Choo 1998; Davenport &

Prusak 1998; Nonaka, 2002; Nonaka & Takeuchi 1995; Leonard & Sensiper, 2002;

Thompson & Walsham, 2004; Huber, 1991).

Data, information, and knowledge are three independent constructs that can be

considered as constituent elements of a continuum (Tsoukas, 2005a; Nonaka &

Takeuchi, 1995; Nonaka, 2002; Boisot, 1998, 2002; Leonard & Sensiper, 2002). Boisot

(1998) explains the relationship between the three by simply stating, “knowledge builds

on information that is extracted from data” (p. 12). Leonard and Sensiper (2002) argue

that “knowledge is a subset of information” (p. 485). Nonaka and his colleagues (Nonaka

& Takeuchi, 1995; Nonaka, 2002) view data, information, and knowledge as active

rearrangements of each other, where “information is a flow of messages [or meanings],

while knowledge is created by that very flow of information, anchored in the beliefs and

commitment of its holder” (Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995, pp.58-9). Nonaka (2002) also

asserts that “information is a necessary medium or material for initiating and formalizing

knowledge” (p. 439). Huber (1991) and Boisot (2002) present a similar intellectual

framework, referring to knowledge as interpreted information. For example, Boisot

(2002) notes that:

“[…] it is never knowledge as such that flows between agents, but rather data from which information has to be extracted and internalized. Only when information has been successfully internalized and forms part of an agent's repertoire of expectations and behaviors can it properly be called knowledge” (p. 72).

Page 21: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  9  

Tsoukas (2005a) locates the meaning of the terms data, information, and knowledge

along a continuum, “depending on the extent to which they reflect human involvement

with, and processing of, the reality at hand […] put simply, data require minimal human

judgment, [and] knowledge maximum judgment” (p. 120).

Data

In information systems terminology, one may think of data as inputs and outputs from a

system (e.g. numbers, characters, images). According to Boisot, data is ‘a discrimination

between physical states’ (1998, p. 12) which is ‘located in the world’ (2002, p. 67) and

‘can be characterized as a property of things’ (1998, p. 12). It is not necessary for data to

convey information to agents, and two separate agents could interpret the same piece of

data as two distinct pieces of information. Extracting the patterns within the data is a

creative task of the agent and can be unique for each agent (Boisot, 2002). Data is often

spoken of as being captured, processed, stored, or disseminated. As mentioned earlier, it

is data, as opposed to knowledge, that flow between agents and systems.

Information

Information can be thought of as a ‘flow of messages’ (Nonaka, 2002, p. 438) that

establishes a relationship between things and agents (Boisot, 1998). This relationship is

best described in Boisot’s (1998) diagram (Figure 2.1), which argues that data are

inherent to objects and events (things) and that agents use their ‘perceptual or conceptual

filters’ (p. 12) to create a subset of these data from the objects and events. Once this

subset of data is created within the agent (i.e. interpretation has taken place) an

established relationship between the two (i.e. agent and object) is formed. This

established relationship between data source and agent is called information.

Page 22: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  10  

 Figure 2.1 Boisot’s Data, Information, and Knowledge Relationship (1998, p. 12)

Knowledge

Using an activity theoretical approach, this research defines knowledge as:

The potential of an activity, situated within a socially constructed domain and bounded by the developmental capacity of the individual.

Although this definition is not presented in precisely this form by any other authors, its

components encompass the core ideas of many noted philosophers, epistemologists, and

organizational theorists. This research does not attempt to resolve a two-thousand-year-

old debate on the definition of knowledge, rather to provide a definition suitable for and

consistent with the rest for the research. It covers what this researcher feels are the most

notable components of knowledge.

Elaborating on these components is important but outside the immediate scope of this

research. Therefore, a thorough explanation and justification for this definition is

provided in the Appendix, along with a relevant peer-reviewed literature review. The five

components of the definition were carefully considered as follows:

1. Knowledge is created, interpreted, disseminated, and displayed through activity;

2. Knowledge is situated within a particular domain;

3. Knowledge is socially constructed and interpreted;

4. Knowledge is personal and bounded by developmental capacity;

5. There is a potential to knowledge (e.g. domain, social construction, and

developmental capacity are partial determinants of the potential value of

knowledge).

Page 23: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  11  

2.1.2 Tacit and Explicit Forms of Knowledge

Tacit Knowledge

Tacit knowledge is personal and embodied (Nonaka, 2002; Spender 1996b; Polanyi,

1962, 1966), which makes its codification (formalization) and dissemination very

difficult (Nonaka, 2002). Polanyi (1966) famously explained this by stating “I shall

reconsider human knowledge by starting from the fact that we can know more than we

can tell” (p. 4). Most tacit knowledge will find “involvement in a specific context”

(Nonaka, 2002, p. 439) or closely associate with a particular “social or collective

identity” (Spender, 1996b, p.53). If the agent is not associated with the specific domain of

action, then attaining the tacit knowledge becomes next to impossible.

Tacit knowledge, which is somewhat similar to Aristotle’s concept of phronesis

(Thomson, 1955) or practical wisdom, is rooted in the actions of an agent, and is revealed

to the world through practice, action, or activity (Brown & Duguid, 1998; Spender,

1996b; Nonaka, 2002; Leonard & Sensiper, 2002). Ryle (1949) argued this point by

saying that ‘knowing how’ puts ‘knowing that’ into practice. Possessing the relevant

‘know-how’2 makes the knowledge “actionable and operational” (Brown & Duguid,

1998, p. 95).

According to Nonaka (2002), tacit knowledge involves both technical and cognitive

elements. The cognitive elements are working/mental models created by the agent, to

form an understanding of the world around them. These cognitive elements consist of

“analogies, schemata, paradigms, beliefs, and viewpoints” (p. 439). These elements are

also difficult to formalize and disseminate since they are developed within particular

contexts (domains) and may vary across different domains. The technical elements of

tacit knowledge refer to “concrete know-how, crafts, and skills that apply to specific

contexts” (Nonaka, 2002, p. 439) and might best be described using Polanyi’s/Tsoukas’

concept of subsidiary awareness.

                                                                                                               2 Particulars need not be apparent

Page 24: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  12  

Building on the work of Polanyi3, Tsoukas (2005b) discusses the concept of subsidiary

awareness with respect to the ‘know more than we can tell’ element of tacit knowledge.

Agreeing with Polanyi, Tsoukas (2005b) asserts that tacit skills retain opacity and un-

specificity in terms of their particulars. Since these particulars are unknown, the skill

itself cannot be fully accounted or explicated. In other words, the practitioner is able to

perform the skill, without having theoretical knowledge of the particulars involved. For

Tsoukas (2005b),

“Tacit knowledge consists of a set of particulars of which we are subsidiarily aware as we focus on something else. Tacit knowing is vectorial: we know the particulars by relying on our awareness of them for attending to something else” (p. 158).

Perhaps this is best explained through example. Suppose one is driving in the rain and the

car in front suddenly brakes, causing them to skid-stop. In a classroom setting, the driver

may have been told to slightly depress the brakes and steer in the opposite direction of the

way the car skids. In the heat of the moment, however, it is unlikely that the driver will

recall the theoretical lesson. Instead, the driver would rely on their tacit knowledge of

driving under these and other conditions, accumulated over the course of months and

years. They will also not consciously attend to moving their hands on the steering wheel

and their foot on the brake. Thus, based on prior experience, the driver will form a

reaction, which will vary according to the accumulated knowledge. The less experience,

the less likely for the correct or prudent action to occur.

According to Polanyi (1962), tacit knowing occurs through a process of unconscious trial

and error. This trial and error is a feeling-out process, where the agent is improving in

success, over time, without specifically knowing (in a theoretical sense) how. For Nonaka

(2002) tacit knowing is a continuous activity, which develops through action and

reaction. For Choo (2000), tacit knowing is achieved through “extended periods of

experiencing and doing a task, during which the individual develops a feel for and a

capacity to make intuitive judgments about the successful execution of the activity” (p.

395).

                                                                                                               3 Polanyi’s subsidiary awareness is discussed in the Instrumentalization section

Page 25: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  13  

The ability to display the instrumentalization or skill with a tool, what Aristotle called

techne (Thomson, 1955), is one of the few undeniable ways an agent can put their tacit

knowledge on display for the world to see. According to Polanyi (1966), “we can,

accordingly, interpret the use of tools, of probes, and of pointers as further instances of

the art of knowing” (p. 7). In its most basic form, the instrumentalization of an object/tool

simply means to be able to use it properly and un-problematically. Some may call this the

embodiment of a tool; Yuasa (1987, p.25) called it ‘learning with the body’ (tainin/

taitoku) and Boisot (2002, p.73) ‘learning-by-doing’. To achieve mastery with a tool, one

must first learn to use the tool in its intended, domain specific, manner. Polanyi (1962,

1966; Polanyi & Prosch, 1975) and Tsoukas (2005b) suggested that this is realized

through the agent assimilating and dwelling in the tool (i.e. making it feel as if it is an

extension of the body). In order to ‘dwell’ in the tool the agent must be able to focus their

attention through the tool onto the target. The tool must not be in the agent’s focal

awareness4. Instead it “needs to become an instrument through which [they] act [and] of

which [they] are subsidiarily aware” (Tsoukas, 2005b, p. 149). According to Polanyi

(1966), “in an act of tacit knowing we attend from something for attending to something

else; namely, from the first term [proximal] to the second term [distal] of the tacit

relation” (p. 10) and “we are attending from these internal processes to the qualities of

things outside, [t]he transposition of bodily experiences into the perception of things

outside” (p. 14).

Tsoukas (2005b) provides a couple of examples to illustrate this point:

“I have a subsidiary awareness of my holding the hammer in the act of focusing on hitting the nail. In being subsidiarily aware of holding a hammer I see it as having a meaning that is wiped out if I focus my attention on how I hold the hammer. If a pianist shifts her attention from the piece she is playing to how she moves her fingers; if a speaker focuses his attention on the grammar he is using instead of the act of speaking” (p. 146).

                                                                                                               4 To place an object into focal awareness is to make it the object of attention

Page 26: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  14  

According to Tsoukas (2005b), tacit knowledge forms a triangular relationship between

subsidiary particulars, the knower and the focal target (Figure 2.2). By acting (i.e. taking

action), the knower links the subsidiary particulars to the focal target. “Subsidiary, or

instrumental knowledge, is not known in itself, but is known in terms of something

focally known” (Tsoukas, 2005b, p. 156). Without the knower, there is no way to

integrate subsidiaries to the focal target. Therefore, as previously mentioned, tacit

knowing is personal and rooted in action5.

The result of ongoing usage of the tool is two-fold. As the agent gains experience using

the tool, they become less aware of how to use the tool to achieve optimal results

(Tsoukas, 2005a). In addition, the instrumentalization of actions6 enables the agent to

increase their awareness of the situation and refine and improve their skill with the tool

(Tsoukas, 2005a). In Tsoukas’ (2005a) words, “the ongoing process of transforming

experience into subsidiary awareness […] allows one to reach ever higher levels of

skilful achievement” (p. 128). Choo (2000) proposed that tacit knowledge may be,

“revealed through rich modes of discourse that include the use of analogies, metaphors or

models, and through the communal sharing of stories” (p. 396).

Explicit Knowledge

Explicit knowledge is routinely defined as knowledge that can be expressed formally

using some system of symbols or formal systematic language (Choo, 1998, 2000;                                                                                                                5 Tacit knowing is personal because it requires the knower. It is action oriented, because it requires subsidiaries to be linked to focal targets by the knower (requires the knower to act). 6 Actions must be purposeful and justified

Figure 2.2 Tsoukas’ (2005b) Representation of Polanyi’s Personal (Tacit) Knowledge

Page 27: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  15  

Nonaka, 2002). Explicit knowledge is understood to exist independently from the human

agent who is the knower (De Long & Fahey, 2000). Choo (1998, 2000) further divided

explicit knowledge into object-based or rule-based. Object-based explicit knowledge is

embedded into artifacts and is usually represented using a string of symbols, or is

embodied in the entity itself (Choo, 2000). Some examples given by Choo (2000)

include: products, patents, software code, computer databases, technical drawings, tools,

prototypes, photographs, voice recordings, and films (p. 396). Explicit knowledge is rule-

based when it takes the form of rules, routines, or operating procedures (Choo, 2000).

Other examples of explicit knowledge include: documents, pictures, stories, diagrams and

narratives (Brown & Duguid, 2000, p. 76).

 Synonym Author

Codified Nonaka and Takeuchi (1995); Nonaka (2002); Boisot (1998)

Structured De Long and Fahey (2000) Encoded (Similar to Object-Based)

Blackler (2002); Thompson and Walsham (2004)

Embedded (Similar to Rule-Based)

Blackler (2002)

Articulated Knowledge Choo and Bontis (2002) Table 2.1 Synonyms of Explicit Knowledge

The formal expression of knowledge into a system of categories and symbols is called

codification. According to Boisot (2002), codification “refines the categories that the

agent invokes or creates so that it can use them efficiently and in discriminating ways.

The fewer data an agent has to process to distinguish between categories, the more

codified the categories that it has to draw upon” (p. 68). Following a resource-based view

of knowledge7, it is commonly accepted that the more codified something becomes, the

easier it is to disseminate without loss of integrity (Boisot, 2002; Nonaka & Takeuchi,

1995; Choo, 1998, 2000; Choo & Bontis, 2002; De Carolis, 2002; Spender, 1994). This is

not always true, as there might be issues from the receiving party in decoding,

comprehending, and assessing the value of the new explicit, codified knowledge.

                                                                                                               7 A resource-based view of knowledge views knowledge as an objective transferable commodity

Page 28: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  16  

Making Tacit Knowledge Explicit

It has been argued that the level to which something can be codified determines its

placement on the explicit/tacit knowledge continuum. The problem with this viewpoint is

that it assumes all tacit forms of knowledge can become codified. This school of thought

is rather consistent with an information systems perspective that reduces all knowledge to

that which has been codified and that which has yet to be codified. The belief is that

knowledge can be dragged along a supposititious continuum, from tacit to explicit and

back. In other words, all tacit knowledge may be reduced or articulated to an explicit or

codified form.

Polanyi (1962) insisted that tacit knowledge could not be reduced to explicit. Tsoukas

(2005ab) added that tacit knowledge may also not be converted, captured, or translated,

but merely put on display. One reason is that some dimensions of knowledge can never

become fully explicated (Leonard & Sensiper, 2002; Boisot, 2002). This is what Polanyi

(1966) referred to, when he said that “we know more than we can tell” (p. 4). De Carolis

(2002) explained this by arguing that difficulties arise when the knower makes an attempt

to communicate their tacit knowledge, because they realize that they cannot fully

articulate it (i.e. part of it remains tacit). The reality is that agents often know more than

they even realize, making it impossible for the knower to fully articulate what is known.

Any attempt to codify the tacit knowledge will ultimately result in an incomplete

representation, since some of the knowledge stays with the knower (Boisot, 2002).

Hence, one should consider explicit and tacit as two interdependent forms of knowledge,

necessary in all acts of knowing. As Duguid (2005) asserted, “codification is remarkably

powerful, but its power is only released through the corresponding knowing how, which

explains how we get to know and learn to do” (p. 114). Polanyi (1962) also argued that

all codified knowledge requires the skill of a knower, before it can be put into practice.

Tsoukas (2005b) explained this by stating that “even the most theoretical form of

knowledge, such as pure mathematics, cannot be a completely formalized system, since it

is based for its application and development on the skills of mathematicians and how

such skills are used in practice” (p. 142). Duguid (2005) made a similar case arguing that

Page 29: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  17  

“while knowledge may include codified content, to be used it requires the disposition to

apply it” (p. 111). In other words, to know requires both knowing how something is done

and how to do it (which only resides in the agent). In Blackler’s (2002) words, codified

knowledge “require[s] operators to interpret the selective, de-contextualized and abstract

symbols that machines [and other codified sources] present to them” (p. 53).

Polanyi (1962, p. 50) and Tsoukas (2005b, p. 155) further suggested that the rules of art

do not determine the practice of an art. Rather, rules are only maxims that serve as a

guide to an art, only if they can be integrated into practical knowledge. In other words, it

is necessary for the agent to be able to understand and ‘re-attach’ the information found

in databases or books, in order to put the knowledge into practice. If the agent does not

understand the domain or context of the explicated knowledge, they can make no

valuable use of it. In the words of De Long and Fahey (2000),

“[Codified] resources, no matter how highly analyzed, only become practical knowledge when individuals can apply their own experience and contextual understanding to interpret the details and implications for action” (p. 115).

Choo and Bontis (2002) rephrased:

“The application of explicit knowledge often requires individuals who can interpret, elaborate, demonstrate, or instantiate the formal knowledge with respect to a particular problem setting” (p. 12).

The two forms of knowledge are not in opposition (Tsoukas, 2005b), nor are they

alternative forms of knowing (Spender, 1996b), rather they complement each other in the

knowing process. As Tsoukas (2005b) stated, “two sides of the same coin” (p. 158).

Neither can exist without the other. Explicated artifacts act as guiding lights in providing

meaning and interpretation to a tacit activity. “Uncodified knowledge provides

background context and warrants for assessing the codified. Background no longer works

as background when it is foregrounded” (Duguid, 2005, p. 112). Using Ryle’s (1949)

vernacular, one may say that knowing how helps to make knowing that actionable.

However, getting more ‘know that’ (i.e. explicit, codified information) does not

Page 30: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  18  

necessarily lead to ‘know how’, which is traditionally generated through practice

(Duguid, 2005).

2.1.3 Knowledge Sharing and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Defined

In a strict and literal sense, knowledge cannot be shared; in that it is not like a

commodity, which can be freely passed around (Hendricks, 1999). Instead, knowledge

sharing is a process involving at least two actors, which has no identifiable starting or

ending point (Boer, van Baalen, & Kumar, 2002). The knowledge owner begins the

knowledge sharing process through an act of externalization, which might or not be a

conscious act. The knowledge receiver then conducts an act of internalization, to absorb

the new stimulus (Hendricks, 1999). Davenport and Prusak (1998) reflected this

perspective in their formula for transfer, which states: Transfer = Transmission +

Absorption.

Perhaps the best way to understand the externalization and internalization processes is to

refer to Boisot’s (2002) concept of ‘resonance’ or Fiol’s (1994) idea of ‘common

understanding’. Boisot (2002) argued that knowledge sharing is no more than “some

degree of resonance being achieved between the knowledge states of two or more agents

following some sharing of data among them” (p. 68). Boisot (2002) further noted that in

addition to resonance, an act of reconstruction is needed for knowledge to be successfully

shared. Knowledge may also be reconstructed through practice and observation

(Hendricks, 1999). The knowledge receiver may then display the reconstruction of

knowledge through action (Duguid, 2005). Without the receiver engaging in some

behavior, it is difficult to determine whether knowledge has been shared, though the

specification of the required behaviors may also be difficult to specify (Wittgenstein,

1953).

The level of success in the reconstruction of knowledge can be quantified, from the

organization’s perspective, in two ways: first, through the actual use of knowledge

shared; and second, through the performance outcomes that result from the knowledge

shared. However, accurately measuring either can be very challenging. Some researchers

Page 31: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  19  

have approached this problem by measuring the data/information flow or information

awareness (Cross & Cummings, 2004). These approaches could also be conceptually

problematic, as the flow of data or information, alone, does not guarantee that knowledge

was being shared. Additionally, knowing who possesses certain domain knowledge,

alone, does not guarantee that this person is accessible or willing to help. In reality,

neither of these approaches accurately measures knowledge sharing, only remnants of the

knowledge sharing process. These approaches may also raise privacy concerns by mining

data flow (e.g. email) or by asking respondents to identify, name, and rate people in their

professional network, a rather impossible task when trying to keep anonymity.

To better understand knowledge sharing, in this research, an alternative approach was

used; one that considered three important conditions necessary for effective knowledge

sharing behavior to take place. First, the knowledge source must be willing to share the

knowledge they posses. Second, the knowledge receiver must be willing to receive and

use the knowledge that is shared. Finally, the knowledge receiver must perceive the

knowledge shared as being useful to their individual work, the project, or the

organization as a whole. These are important conditions, since all knowledge sharing

requires a willingness to participate in the knowledge sharing process from both ends. For

example, when a person is approached to share what they know, they are asked to make

an investment of their valuable time, often without any likelihood of reward or

recognition. This investment of time may be significant, as extensive interaction may

have to take place to ensure that the knowledge seeker understands. Clearly this

requirement for the investment of time may reduce an individual’s willingness to share

knowledge or use the knowledge shared.

In this research, the three knowledge sharing conditions will be collectively considered as

representing effective knowledge sharing behavior. However, in a pure sense these

conditions do not actually represent behaviors, rather behavioral precursors (i.e.

willingness or intentions) and post-behavioral outcomes (i.e. perceived usefulness). A

more complete discussion of this distinction can be found in Section 3.2.3.

Page 32: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  20  

2.2 Organizational Trust and Knowledge Sharing

To further understand knowledge sharing and knowledge sharing behavior, a

comprehensive literature review of knowledge sharing factors was conducted (Evans,

2008). Knowledge sharing factors were identified from three different bodies of literature

including studies where authors took a philosophical or theoretical approach; behavioral

approach, and management science or information science (organizational behavior)

approach. Two important assumptions were made: an explicit definition of knowledge

need not be stated in the work, and if a definition for knowledge is provided it need not

be universal.

This initial literature review identified numerous factors as motivators or inhibitors to

knowledge sharing and led to the development of an overview model, or literature map,

representing the main motivators and inhibitors to knowledge sharing (Figure 2.3). The

factors have been organized using a distinction made by Duguid (2005) between factors

that can or cannot be shared (i.e. ability factors) and those that will or will not be shared

(i.e. willingness factors). Given the complexity of the model, creating a study that

investigates all the factors summarized in the figure is not feasible, thus a decision had to

be made on which ones to focus. Of all these factors, trust kept emerging in the literature

and was predominantly the most recurring factor across all three bodies of literature.

Much of this theoretical and empirical work discussed trust as both directly influencing

knowledge sharing and as an important antecedent or precondition, reaffirming that it

should be a central construct in understanding knowledge sharing behavior.

Page 33: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  21  

 Figure 2.3 Original KSB Literature Map8

A second round of literature review of trust studies identified 14 research variables that

were found to have a major effect on trust, especially in the context of knowledge

sharing. Special attention was given to those trust studies that related to knowledge

sharing within an organizational setting. These 14 variables included: group norms,

shared language, shared vision, value homophily, status homophily, tie strength,

reciprocity, formal sanctions, informal sanctions, intrinsic rewards, extrinsic rewards,

relationship duration, tertius gaudens orientation, and tertius iungens orientation. For the

present study, certain variables were combined and others were excluded because of the

concern they posed on the ability to record, measure, and analyze them without affecting

participant anonymity. Therefore, the original list of 14 variables was reduced to five that

jointly will be referred to in this study as social-cognitive factors. These social-cognitive

factors are: shared language, shared vision, homophily, tie strength, and relationship

duration.

                                                                                                               8 Figure 2.3 depicts factors that have been found to be motivators or inhibitors to knowledge sharing behavior. The arrows represent significant relationships found in the literature.

Page 34: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  22  

The following sections explore the existing literature on the interrelatedness of trust, the

identified social-cognitive factors, and knowledge sharing behavior. The sections will

define each of these variables and discuss direct relationships found in previous research

between the social-cognitive factors, trust, and knowledge sharing behavior. The review

will begin with organizational trust, as it is a central construct to this research.

2.2.1 Understanding Organizational Trust

Trust is a construct that has been examined by numerous social science fields of study,

including history, anthropology, psychology, political science, economics, sociology,

information studies, and knowledge management with each of these disciplines applying

their own perspectives and approaches (lenses). Lewicki and Bunker (1996) point out that

“little effort has been made to integrate these different [trust] perspectives or articulate

the key role trust plays in critical social processes (e.g. cooperation, coordination,

performance)” (p. 115). Even though several studies have been conducted since, the role

trust plays in social processes remains an important area of research needing exploration.

Worchel (1979) argued that all these different perspectives on trust may be categorized

into three broad research approaches, which Lewicki and Bunker (1995; 1996) expanded

on. The first research approach, proposed by Worchel (1979), is consistent with the view

of personality theorists. This viewpoint is rooted in early psychological development and

focuses on ‘developmental and social contextual factors’ that shape trust (Lewicki &

Bunker, 1996, p. 115). The second perspective focuses on trust as an institutional

phenomenon and is consistent with research approaches in sociology and economics

(Lewicki & Bunker, 1995; 1996). In this perspective, trust is studied within institutions,

across institutions, or as an individual’s trust in the institution. The final category,

proposed by Worchel (1979), is consistent with the approach of social psychologists and

examines interpersonal relationships and transactions. This is the focus adopted in the

present study. Of specific interest is the “contextual factors that serve to either enhance or

inhibit the development and maintenance of trust” (Lewicki & Bunker, 1996, p.116).

Page 35: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  23  

According to Lewicki and Bunker (1996), recent work on trust has attempted to make a

distinction between personal and professional relationships. The former suggests that the

research focuses on the development of trust in close personal contexts (e.g. romantic

relationships, friendships, acquaintances). The latter suggests a focus on the development

of trust in working or professional relationships. The authors (Lewicki & Bunker, 1996)

explain that a distinction should be made, because the way these relationships form and

develop is radically different. “In professional relationships [as opposed to personal

ones], trust does not begin with the development of intense emotionality” (Lewicki &

Bunker, 1996, p. 118). The present study focuses only on professional or working

relationships and Mayer et al.’s (1995) model (described in detail below; see Figure 2.4)

is appropriate for use with this type of research, as it is specifically formulated for use

within the organizational setting.

As there are numerous approaches and disciplines to the study of trust, there are also

several definitions. Any definition used must be consistent with, and appropriate to, the

perspective of trust the research intends on selecting. Since the present study emphasizes

a social-psychological perspective, an appropriate definition for trust is one that perceives

it in an interpersonal organizational context.

For most social psychologists, trust is based on “expectations set within particular

contextual parameters and constraints” (Lewicki & Bunker, 1996, p.116). Deutsch (1960)

suggested that an individual decides to trust another when three situational parameters

exist: an uncertain future course of action; an outcome depending on the behavior of

others; and the strength of the detrimental event that is greater than the strength of the

beneficial event. Using similar parameters, Schlenker, Helm, and Tedeschi (1973)

defined trust as the “reliance upon information received from another person about

uncertain environmental states and their accompanying outcomes in a risky situation” (p.

419). Johnson-George and Swap (1982) noted that a “willingness to take risks may be

one of the few characteristics common to all trust situations” (p. 1306). Boon and

Holmes’ (1991) interpretation of trust also focused on risk, defining trust as “a state

involving confident positive expectations about another’s motives with respect to oneself

Page 36: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  24  

in situations entailing risk” (p. 194). Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis’ (1996b) definition

interprets risk, which the authors describe as “an important component in a model of

trust”, through ‘vulnerability’ (1996, p. 340). According to the authors, “making oneself

vulnerable is taking risk. Trust is not taking risk per se, but rather it is a willingness to

take risk” (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995, p. 712) or a “willingness to engage in

risk-taking with the focal party” (Mayer & Davis, 1999, p. 124). According to the

authors, their definition and corresponding model “complements the risk literature by

clarifying the role of interpersonal trust in risk taking” (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman,

1995, p. 711).

Mayer et al. (1995) define trust as

“[…] the willingness of a party to be vulnerable9 to the actions of another party based on the expectation that the other will perform a particular action important to the trustor, irrespective of the ability to monitor or control that other party” (p. 712).

In Mayer et al.’s (1995) opinion, previous research and models on trust have not been

clear in differentiating trust, factors that lead to trust, and outcomes of trust. The authors’

model conceptualizes trust in a fashion that distinguishes it from its outcomes and

antecedents (Mayer & Davis, 1999). The model (definition) also considers trust factors

from both the characteristics of the trustor (i.e. propensity to trust) and the collective

perceived characteristics of the trustee (i.e. ability, benevolence, integrity), something

that, the authors argue, other models have neglected (Mayer et al., 1995; Schoorman,

Mayer, & Davis, 1996b). “The failure to clearly specify the trustor [the trusting party]

and the trustee [the party to be trusted] encourages the tendency to change referents and

even level of analysis, which obfuscates the nature of the trust relationship” (Mayer et al.,

1995, p. 711).

                                                                                                               9 Making oneself vulnerable, implies that something important may be lost. Trust is the willingness to take a risk. The level of trust directly relates to the level of perceived risk (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995; Zaheer, McEvily, & Perrone, 1998).

Page 37: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  25  

Mayer et al.’s (1995) proposed model of organizational trust (Figure 2.4) separates the

relationship between trustor and trustee, in an effort to understand the factors underlying

why a trustor would trust a trustee.

 Figure 2.4 Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman’s (1995) Proposed Model of Trust (p. 715)

Mayer et al. (1995) argue that it is the individual traits or characteristics of the trusting

parties that determine the level of trust that may be achieved between them. For instance,

for a trustor to exhibit trust towards a trustee, the trustor must first have the ‘propensity to

trust’10 (Mayer et al., 1995, p.715) that particular trustee, or the propensity to trust in

general (especially when the relationship is new). In return, the trustee must be perceived

as having ability11, benevolence12, and integrity13, which, together, help the trustor

determine the trustee’s ‘trustworthiness’ (Mayer et al., 1995). Any ‘measure’ of a

trustee’s ‘trustworthiness’ is only based on a perception of ‘trustworthiness’ by the

trustor, and not on the actual characteristics or traits of the trustee. In a subsequent paper,

the authors (Schoorman, Mayer, & Davis, 1996b) justify this perspective by claiming that

                                                                                                               10 Propensity to trust is defined as “the general willingness to trust others” (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995, p. 715) 11 Ability is defined as the skills, competencies, and characteristics necessary to have influence in a specific domain. (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995, p. 717) 12 Benevolence is defined as the extent to which a trustor believes the trustee wants to do good to the trustor. Act in a way that is not egocentric. (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995, p. 718) 13 Integrity is determined by the trustor, by making an assessment as to whether or not the trustee will adhere to an acceptable (to the trustor) set of principles. (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995, p. 719)

Page 38: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  26  

it is necessary “to account for why a particular trustee might be highly trusted by one

trustor but not by another” (p. 338).

“A trustor will be willing to be vulnerable to another party based both on the trustor’s propensity to trust other people in general and on the trustor’s perception that the particular trustee is trustworthy” (Mayer & Davis, 1999, p. 124).

To better explain Mayer et al.’s (1995) proposed model of trust, it is best to separate the

trustor’s trust characteristics from those of the trustee. The former considers the trustor’s

“general willingness to trust others” (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 714), which affects the

likelihood that the trustor will trust in general, and presumably carries with the person, as

they interact in different situations (Mayer et al., 1995). “In this approach trust is viewed

as a trait that leads to a generalized expectation about the trustworthiness of others”

(Mayer et al., 1995, p.715). The authors refer to this trait as the ‘propensity to trust’.

“Propensity might be thought of as the general willingness to trust others. Propensity will

influence how much trust one has for a trustee prior to data on that particular party

becoming available” (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 715).

Mayer et al. (1995) also suggest that one should consider the trustee’s characteristics that

convey trustworthiness to the trustor. “Characteristics and actions of the trustee will lead

that person to be more or less trusted” (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 717). To identify these

characteristics Mayer et al. (1995) conducted a review of antecedent factors14 and found

three characteristics to appear most often. Mayer et al. (1995) refer to these three

characteristics (i.e. ability, benevolence, and integrity) as The Factors of Trustworthiness.

“Our decision to treat all three as contributors to trust was based on our view that they

have an additive quality in determining the level of trust. [Nevertheless,] all three

concepts are theoretically distinct” (Schoorman, Mayer, & Davis, 1996b, p. 339).

According to Mayer et al. (1995), “ability is that group of skills, competencies, and

characteristics that enable a party to have influence within some specific domain” (p.

717). If a trustee is perceived as having high domain specific knowledge, then that person

                                                                                                               14 For a complete list of the Mayer et al.’s literature review and antecedent factors see Table 1 Trust Antecedent (1995, p. 718)

Page 39: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  27  

is afforded trust, by the trustor, on tasks relating to that domain (Mayer et al., 1995). In

justifying the inclusion of ability in their model, Mayer et al. (1995) cite a number of

theorists who discuss either the same (i.e. ability15) or similar constructs in their work.

The authors note that similar constructs include competence, expertise, business sense,

and judgment (Mayer et al., 1995). Competence has been a popular synonym in studies

measuring trust and will be discussed in more detail in the next chapter. Replacing one

term for another is not a concern, since “competence and ability are clearly similar”

(Mayer et al., 1995, p. 722). In later work, the authors even use the terms interchangeably

(Schoorman, Mayer, & Davis, 1996b).

Mayer et al. (1995) define benevolence as “the extent to which a trustee is believed to

want to do good to the trustor, aside from an egocentric profit motive” (p. 718).

Benevolence suggests that there is “some specific attachment” (p. 718) of the trustee to

the trustor (e.g. the trust between a mentor and his/her protégé). Unlike ability, which is

domain specific, benevolence implies a personal orientation. Mayer et al. (1995) justify

the inclusion of benevolence, by citing several theorists16 who have also used the exact

term in their interpretations of trust. The authors (Mayer et al,, 1995) also review other

theorists who use different terminology, but essentially have similar perceptions (i.e.

consider trust to be tied to a persons’ altruistic or loyalty-based intentions and/or

motives).

Integrity “involves the trustor’s perception that the trustee adheres to a set of principles

that the trustor finds acceptable” (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 719). According to the authors

(Mayer et al., 1995), the trustor makes a judgment about the trustee’s integrity, in four

ways: through the consistency of the trustee’s past actions; through communication with

others (i.e. others say the trustee is credible); through an assessment of the trustee’s sense

of justice17; and through an assessment of the extent to which the trustee’s actions match

their words. Like in the case of the previous two constructs, the authors (Mayer et al.,

                                                                                                               15 “Cook and Wall (1980), Deutsch (1960), Jones, James, and Bruni (1975), and Sitkin and Roth (1993) all considered ability an essential element of trust.” (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 717) 16 Larzelere & Huston, 1980; Solomon, 1960; Stickland, 1958, as cited in Mayer et al., 1995 17 The trustee is thought as having high integrity when their perceived sense of justice is high

Page 40: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  28  

1995) justify the inclusion of integrity, by citing numerous theorists18 who use it (or a

very similar construct) as an antecedent to trust.

Ability, benevolence, and integrity are all related to one another, but each can be

separated and varies independently of the others (Mayer et al., 1995). All three factors are

important to trust, yet each, by itself, is insufficient for trust. “Each element contributes a

unique perceptual perspective from which the trustor considers the trustee” (Mayer et al.,

1995, p. 722). If all three are perceived as high by the trustor, then the trustee is deemed

trustworthy. Trustworthiness should be understood as a continuous variable, rather than

mutually exclusive (i.e. either present or not). High trust normally presumes a high level

of all three variables, but it is possible for lesser degrees of the three variables to still

yield ‘meaningful’ amounts of trust (Mayer et al., 1995, p. 721).

The extent to which one person is willing to trust another is a function of both the

trustor’s perceived judgment of the trustee (with respect to ability, benevolence, and

integrity) and the trustor’s propensity to trust.

2.2.2 Trust and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

The majority of research examining the impact of trust on knowledge sharing builds upon

previous work in the fields of psychology and behavioral science, that have been

examining trust as an antecedent for social behavior. The consensus of this research

seems to suggest that higher trust among individuals leads to higher and more productive

knowledge sharing behaviors and activities between them. It is also generally accepted

that trust is a prerequisite for knowledge sharing (Nonaka, 1991; Adler, 2002; De Long &

Fahey, 2000; McAllister, 1995). As Mayer et al. (1995) stated, “a clear understanding of

trust and its causes can facilitate cohesion and collaboration between people” (pp. 710-

11).

Before discussing the literature in more detail, it is important to acknowledge a few

limitations in attempting to synthesize a literature review on trust and knowledge sharing.                                                                                                                18 Leiberman, 1981; Sitkin & Roth, 1993; Butler & Cantrell, 1964; Butler, 1991; Gabarro, 1978; Hart, Capps, Cangemi, & Caillouet, 1986, as cited in Mayer et al., 1995

Page 41: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  29  

First, the reviewed studies did not all explore similar settings, respondents, or relationship

dyads; and in no study were these characteristics identical to those proposed in this

research. Second, in most cases, the knowledge sharing constructs in the literature were

conceptually related to the three behavioral conditions proposed in this study for effective

knowledge sharing behavior, yet rarely identical to them. Finally, trust greatly varied in

the way it was defined and measured in the literature, which has been recognized by

McEvily and Tortoriello (2011) as a problem for this area of research – the authors’

review of 171 empirical papers measuring trust in organizational contexts (published

from 1962-2010) found a staggering 207 different psychometric trust measures.

Looking past these limitations, the literature revealed that trust had a positive influence

on effective knowledge transfer, behaviors, and activities. The same can be said for the

positive effect of trust on outcomes associated with effective knowledge transfer (e.g.

decision-making and problem solving). The following sections present findings from the

literature showing the relationship between trust and 1) specific knowledge sharing

behaviors; 2) other motivating behaviors; and 3) resource and information exchange /

decision making and problem solving.

Trust and Specific Knowledge Sharing Behaviors

A number of research studies examined a direct relationship between trust and knowledge

sharing behavior specifically. For example, Renzl’s (2008) study found a direct positive

relationship between trust and knowledge sharing behavior within and between project

groups in two large organizations (15 interviews and 201 survey respondents). Andrews

and Delahaye (2000) study with scientific staff representing 5 partner organizations at a

Bio-Medical Consortium (15 semi-structured interviews) found trustworthiness among

co-workers to positively affect knowledge sharing activities. Trust was also found to

have a significant positive effect on knowledge sharing in two research studies (437

survey respondents) done at three large technology companies implementing KM in

Taiwan (Ho, Kuo, Lin, & Lin, 2010; Ho, Kuo, & Lin, 2011).

Page 42: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  30  

Using a social capital perspective, Nahapiet and Ghoshal (1998) developed a model that

viewed trust as a necessary facet of the relational dimension of social capital.

The authors argued that this relational dimension (with trust as a facet) had a positive

effect and influence on the transfer of knowledge. Tsai and Ghoshal (1998) conducted

research to test this model on 45 managers (3 managers in each of the 15 business units)

in a multinational electronics company. The results found a positive relationship between

trust and the willingness to share knowledge. Other researchers suggested similar positive

relationships between trust and the willingness to share knowledge, including Hinds and

Pfeffer (2003), Van den Hooff and Van Weenen (2004), Lin (2007), and Hislop (2003).

In his doctoral research, Holste (2003) examined the effect of trust on the willingness to

share knowledge and willingness to use knowledge. To gain a comprehensive

understanding of the effect of trust, Holste (2003) also factored for the type of knowledge

(explicit vs. tacit), as well as the nature of relationship with referent (positive vs. negative

referent19). Holste’s (2003) results showed that both affect-based trust and cognition-

based20 trust had positive effects on the willingness to share knowledge and willingness

to use knowledge with positive referents. With negative referents only, affect-based trust

(not cognition-based) was found to have a positive effect on the willingness to share

knowledge (Holste, 2003). However, both affect-based trust and cognition-based trust

had positive effects on the willingness to use knowledge from negative referents (Holste,

2003).

In their study of three divisions at an American pharmaceutical company, a British bank,

and a Canadian oil and gas company, Levin and Cross (2004) found trust (specifically

competence-based trust) to have a positive impact on knowledge transfers involving

                                                                                                               19 Positive referents were those individuals the respondents felt they worked best with and negative referents were those individuals the respondents felt they did not work well with. 20 In his study, McAllister (1995) created new measures to assess affect- and cognition-based trust. These measures were based on the work of Lewis and Weigert (1985) who described social trust as having cognitive and affective foundations. According to the authors (Lewis & Weigert, 1985) “trust is based on a cognitive process which discriminates among persons and institutions that are trustworthy, distrusted, and unknown. In this sense, we cognitively choose whom we will trust” (p. 970). In addition, “trust is also constructed on an emotional base that is complementary to its cognitive base. This affective component of trust consists in an emotional bond among all those who participate in the relationship” (Lewis & Weigert, 1985, p. 971).

Page 43: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  31  

highly tacit knowledge. More interestingly, for transfers involving codified knowledge,

competence-based trust was found not to provide any benefit. Yang and Farn’s (2009)

research suggested similar findings, where affect-based trust (rather than competence-

based) was found to motivate the intention to share tacit knowledge. Parallel findings

were made in a survey of 212 students in the Business Administration department at the

University of Taiwan that found trust in co-workers to be positively related to tacit

knowledge sharing (Lin, 2007). Holste’s (2003) work showed a more complex

relationship between trust and explicit and tacit knowledge sharing behavior, one that

depended on how the respondent perceived the relationship between them and the

referent. For example, with positive referents the effect of trust on the willingness to

share knowledge and willingness to use knowledge consisted more of tacit knowledge

sharing and use, rather than explicit (Holste, 2003). For negative referents, the opposite

was found to be true (Holste, 2003).

Finally, Levin and Cross (2004) found significant positive relationships to exist between

benevolence/competence-based trust and the perceived receipt of useful knowledge.

These results were also consistent with Szulanski, Cappetta, and Jensen (2004), who

examined 122 knowledge transfers in 38 practices and found perceptions of

trustworthiness to have direct effect on the accuracy of knowledge transfer. Other

research also found evidence of the positive effect of trust on knowledge importing

(Andrews & Delahaye, 2000) and knowledge combination (Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998).

Trust and Other Motivating Behaviors

Research examining trust in an organizational setting was not specifically concerned with

knowledge sharing constructs or the knowledge transfer process. Instead, in these studies

the focus was on the effect of trust on other organizational behaviors and actions. It

should be noted that the behaviors below are not specifically knowledge sharing

behaviors; yet these studies were included in the review because the behaviors can be

argued to have a positive relationship on effective knowledge sharing behavior.

Page 44: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  32  

The discussion of these other organizational behaviors will start by examining the role

trust played in minimizing behaviors that might be argued as being detrimental to

effective knowledge sharing behavior. First, trust and trustworthiness between employees

have been positively associated with a decrease in efforts needed for information search

and processing (Zaheer, McEvily, & Perrone, 1998). When trust exists, efforts needed for

information search and processing are minimized, since the receiving party does not have

to scrutinize the quality or veracity of the information (Zaheer, McEvily, & Perrone,

1998). "High levels of trust help reduce transaction costs” (Limerick & Cunnington,

1993, p. 95). Trust and trustworthiness have also been associated with a decrease in

information monitoring and safeguarding behaviors (Zaheer, McEvily, & Perrone, 1998;

Roberts & O’Reily, 1974; Husted & Michailova, 2002; and Orlikowski, 1993). Control

mechanisms are reduced as interaction increases and trust is developed (Mayer et al,

1995). The existence of a trusting relationship reassures the sender that the receiver will

not misappropriate the information entrusted to them, reducing their monitoring,

safeguarding behaviors, and conserving cognitive resources (Uzzi, 1997). Ultimately this

leads to more ‘openness’ in the exchange (Zaheer, McEvily & Perrone, 1998). According

to Limerick and Cunnington (1993) “trust lubricates the smooth, harmonious functioning

of the organization by eliminating friction and minimizing the need for bureaucratic

structures that specify the behavior of participants who do not trust each other” (pp. 95-

6).

Other organizational studies have made similar conclusions. For example, Zand’s (1972)

study examining problem-solving groups in management development programs found

trust to minimize control-based monitoring behavior. McAllister’s (1995) study of 194

managers and professionals reporting on cross-functional dyadic relationships with co-

workers found trust to minimize defensive cooperative behavior; also an empirical

finding of Tsai and Ghoshal (1998) and Hislop (2003). Zaheer, McEvily and Perrone’s

(1998) study of 205 purchasing managers found trust to minimize conflict, as well as

reduce costs associated with negotiation. Finally, Bromiley and Cummings (1995) argued

that trust reduced transaction costs.

Page 45: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  33  

On the contrary, there are a number of organizational trust studies that examined the role

trust plays in motivating behaviors that may be argued as leading to effective knowledge

sharing behavior. For example, trust has been found to influence an individual’s desire to

share information and ideas (Davenport & Prusak, 1998; Empson, 2001; McDermott &

O’Dell, 2001; Husted & Michailova, 2002; Hendricks, 1999; and Hinds & Pfeffer, 2003)

or what Szulanski (1995; 1996) calls a desire to ‘initiate a transfer’. Zand (1972) found

trust to increase confidence in the reliability of others, and the work of Tsai and Ghoshal

(1998) found it to influence the lending of support to co-workers in the achievement of

their goals. McAllister (1995) found that managers who had higher trust in their workers

paid more attention to their needs. Looking at relationships with management in a large

US federal government training organization, McCauley and Kuhnert (1992) found a

positive relationship between trust and willingness to listen. Similarly, Penley and

Hawkins’ (1985) research with employees in the personnel and support services areas of

a large southern US insurance company and from the logistics and support division of a

large military base, found a positive relationship between trust and communication

responsiveness, which they defined as a willingness to listen and respond to issues raised.

The authors’ (1985) findings also suggested a direct effect of trust on task

communication, which they defined as willingness to explain work tasks and policies.

Finally, numerous researchers (Ring & Van de Ven, 1994; Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998; Dirks

& Ferrin, 2002; Kramer, 1999; and Tyler & Degoey, 1996) have found direct positive

connections between trust and cooperation or cooperative behavior.

Trust and Resource/Information Exchange & Trust and Decision Making/Problem

Solving

The final section of trust literature explores the effect of trust on resource/information

exchange, decision-making, and problem-solving within the firm. Even though these

outcomes do not specifically address or relate to knowledge sharing behavior, it could be

argued that they represent the desired residual effects of the organizational knowledge

sharing process. In other words, in knowledge intensive organizations, the desired or

expected outcomes of knowledge sharing would be measured in increased exchange,

better decision-making, and better problem-solving.

Page 46: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  34  

The literature suggests that trust has a positive effect on resource exchange (Tsai &

Ghoshal, 1998). In addition, several other research studies found trust to have a positive

effect on the exchange of accurate, comprehensive, and timely information, including

ideas, goals, opinions, and problems (Zand, 1972; Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998; Mital, Isreal, &

Agarwal, 2010; McCauley & Kuhnert, 1992; Burt, (1992). McCauley and Kuhnert (1992)

explained this relationship as behaviorally driven, where trust had a positive effect on a

feeling of openness in the information exchange. Other authors have made similar

connections to behavioral and cognitive drivers for this relationship with information

exchange, finding trust to have a positive influence on information available (DeLong &

Fahey, 2000; Husted & Michailova, 2002), information commitment (Dirks & Ferrin,

2002; Zand, 1972; Ouchi, 1980), and information disclosure. Zand’s (1972) research also

found trust to have a positive effect on the information search process.

Trust has also been positively linked to effective decision-making and problem solving.

For example, McCauley and Kuhnert’s (1992) study discovered a positive relationship

between trust and willingness to participate in decision-making. Zand (1972) found trust

to have a positive influence on the framing of problem alternatives and effective problem

solving within groups. This may be because there is less scrutiny in the exchange, and the

group members are able to draw better distinctions on the information they have

(Tsoukas 2005a, 2005b), which gives them the ability to reach quicker and better

decisions (Roberts & O’Reily, 1974). “A group within which there is extensive

trustworthiness and extensive trust is able to accomplish more than a comparable group

without the trustworthiness and trust” (Coleman, 1988, p. S101).

Summary of Trust and Knowledge Sharing

Despite a lack of consistency in the definition of trust and knowledge sharing behavior,

the overall literature findings suggest a positive relationship between trust and knowledge

sharing behavior in organizations. This can be witnessed in the sections above, that

summarize the literature showing a positive relationship between trust and 1) specific

Page 47: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  35  

knowledge sharing behaviors; 2) other related motivating behaviors; and 3) resource and

information exchange / decision-making and problem-solving.

2.3 Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust

The following section defines each of the social-cognitive factors identified in the study,

as well as examines the existing literature for relationships between these variables and

trust. The section begins with homophily factors and then discusses shared perspective,

tie strength, and relationship length.

2.3.1 Homophily and Trust

In network terms, homophily implies the existence of a positive relationship between the

degree of similarity between two actors and the strength of the relationship between

them. In other words, greater similarity between two individuals leads to a stronger

relationship between them. McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Cook, (2001) define

homophily as “the principle that contact between similar people occurs at a higher rate

than among dissimilar people” (p. 416). According to Coleman (1988), a higher rate of

contact (caused by homophily) allows for the development of reputation through the

proliferation of obligations and expectations. Further, the development of reputation leads

to higher trust.

According to Toh and Srinivas (2011) “apparent physical cues […] form salient bases of

an individual’s assessment of how much to trust a given target” (p. 3). In his work, Byrne

(1971) discovered that individuals with characteristics similar to those of the respondents

being studied were rated as more attractive and respondents were more willing to trust

those individuals. Several other researchers including Burt (1992), Levin, Whitener, and

Cross (2006), and Chatman and Flynn (2001) have also established direct connections

between rate of contact and homophily, and between trusting behavior and homophily.

In his work, Burt (1992) established a direct connection between homophily and trust,

arguing that similar agents are more likely to trust each other than those that are

dissimilar. Burt (1992) argued that two individuals with high homophily are more likely

Page 48: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  36  

to trust each other because “the operational guide to the formation of close, trusting

relations seems to be that a person more like me is less likely to betray me” (p. 16).

Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2006) suggested a similar relationship stating “trust may be

built on perceived demographic similarities” (p. 1164). Chatman and Flynn (2001)

argued that “people often use immediately apparent physical features, such as race, sex,

and national origin, to categorize others and predict their behavior” (p. 957). Other

researchers have also found that people believe those with demographic similarities to

themselves as being more honest, trustworthy, and cooperative (Brewer, 1979;

McAllister, 1995; Shore, Cleveland, & Goldberg, 2003; Tsui & O’Reilly, 1989). Brewer

(1981) called this a “depersonalized trust based on category membership” (p. 356).

Noted causes of homophily include geography21, family ties22, organizational foci23,

isomorphic sources24, and cognitive processes25 (McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook,

2001). According to the authors (2001), there are two distinct types of homophily: status

homophily26 and value homophily27. However, this research study will only explore status

homophily, as value homophily may be too conceptually similar to shared vision, which

is discussed below. Nevertheless, it is important to note that there is research suggesting

that trust is influenced by homophily on values and goals28 (e.g. Hogg & Terry, 2000;

McAllister, 1995; Sitkin & Roth, 1993; and Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998). The selected

characteristics used to explore the relationship of status homophily with trust include both

ascribed characteristics (age, gender, race, ethnicity and nationality, and immigration)

                                                                                                               21 Geography relates to geographic distance. More likely to have contact with those that are closer 22 Family Ties refers to a family relation (biological tie). Likely to be the same race, ethnicity, and religion 23 Organizational Foci relates to a focused activity which fosters the relationship (e.g. school, work, or voluntary organizations) 24 Isomorphic Sources relates to occupied positions or roles (e.g. workplace roles (status, seniority, functional division), family roles (wives), or political roles (senators)) 25 Cognitive Processes refers to perceived similarity (e.g. people who share similar knowledge domains) 26 Status Homophily is based on informal, formal, and ascribed status. Includes ascribed characteristics (race, ethnicity, gender, age) and acquired characteristics (religion, education, occupation, behavior patterns) 27 Value Homophily is based on values, attitudes, and beliefs 28 These connections are discussed in more detail in the Shared Vision section.

Page 49: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  37  

and acquired characteristics (education, experience, and marital status)29. The review will

begin with the ascribed characteristics.

Age Homophily and Trust

Status homophily motivated by a similarity in age has been prevalent in psychological

studies over the years, especially when homophily was measured through rate of contact.

Existing research on age homophily also seems especially prevalent in studies of

community, friendship networks, and school-related acquaintances. Fischer (1977) and

Verbrugge (1977) found age homophily to be a higher influencer of rate of contact than

any other dimensions among close friends. Feld (1982) and Shrum, Cheek Jr., and Hunter

(1988) found similar results in their studies of superficial friendships and school-related

acquaintances. In his research of personal friend networks, Fisher (1982) expanded on the

influence of age homophily as more than an increase in the rate of contact between

individuals, suggesting that age homophilous ties were closer, longer-lived, and more

personal (in addition to having a higher number of exchanges). Although, once again,

trust was not specifically mentioned, Marsden’s (1988) study, using data from the 1985

General Social Survey (GSS)30, discovered that respondents had a tendency to confide in

someone of the same age. The study (1998) also found that people were less likely to

discuss ’important matters’ with someone farther away from them in age. Both actions

could suggest a clear sign of trusting behavior.

Gender Homophily and Trust

Gender homophily begins when children enter school (Smith-Lovin & McPherson, 1993;

McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook, 2001). As students move from early grades to their

adolescence, boys are more likely to form larger heterogeneous groups, while girls form

smaller, more homogeneous, ones (Shrum et al., 1988). By adulthood, most people have

                                                                                                               29 Another notable network effect of homophily is evidenced by what is termed ‘selective tie dissolution’. This phenomenon arises as a result of low homophily within a group leading to weakened ties and hence a higher probability of subsequent dissolution (McPherson, Smith-Lovin, & Cook, 2001, p. 436). 30 The GSS gathered data on interpersonal environments for a sample of 1534 persons drawn from the non-institutionalized population of the United States.

Page 50: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  38  

gender-integrated networks (37% are perfectly mixed), with only 22% not having cross-

gender confidants (Marsden, 1987).

In their research, Huckfeldt and Sprague (1995) found content-based, less intimate

relationships, like the ones that might traditionally be found in a corporate environment,

to be gender homophilous (e.g. 84% of men discussed politics only with other men).

Research specifically taking place in corporate environments has confirmed the existence

of gender homophily (Bielby & Baron, 1986; Kalleberg, Knoke, Marsden, & Spaeth,

1996), which most frequently occurred among upper-level managers and entrepreneurs

(Ibrarra, 1992, 1997; Brass, 1985). Also, men were found to have more gender

homophilous networks, especially in institutions where men were the majority (Ibrarra,

1992, 1997; Brass, 1985). According to McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Cook, (2001) “this

pattern [of male homophilous networks] is especially strong when we consider

instrumental or status-loaded ties of advice, respect, and mentoring; socio-emotional ties

of friendship and support are much more gender homophilous, in spite of skewed

environments” (p. 424). In these types of situations, one gender is clearly provided an

unfair and unequal access to advice, respect, and mentoring (McPherson, Smith-Lovin, &

Cook, 2001). Without access to these opportunities, trust has little chance to develop.

Race / Ethnicity Homophily and Trust

According to McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Cook (2001) race and ethnicity create the

largest divide among social networks within the United States. Strong homophily based

on race or ethnicity has been discovered in several different types of relationships

including married couples (Kalmijn, 1998), individuals that confide in one another

(Marsden, 1987; 1988), school-related relationships (Shrum et al., 1988; Maccoby, 1998),

and work-related relationships (Lincoln & Miller, 1979; Ibarra, 1995). Interestingly, in

his research, Marsden (1987) found that only 8% of adults sampled discussed important

matters with someone of opposite race. Similar results were found by Blum, Blau, and

their colleagues (Blum, 1984; Blau, Beeker, & Fitzpatrick, 1984; Blau, Blum, &

Schwartz, 1982; Blau, Ruan, & Monika, 1991), when they examined different measures

of ethnicity (e.g. national origin, native tongue, ethnic group, and birth location). With

Page 51: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  39  

respect to the organizational settings, Ibarra (1995) found that ethnic minorities were

much more likely to seek advice and support from their heterogeneous counterparts, than

the ethnic majority.

Even though no direct relationship between ethnicity-based homophily and trust is

specifically mentioned, it is reasonable to assume that discussing important matters,

seeking advice, and seeking support from another individual are all clear signs of trusting

behavior, which appear to be occurring more frequently within ethic homogeneous

relationships.

Citizenship / Immigration Homophily and Trust

Earley and Mosakowski (2000) suggested that “nationality is a superordinate determinant

of a person's self-identity, derived through a meaning system shared with others” (p. 26).

Geringer (1988), Oetzel (1995), and Bornhorst et al. (2010) further explained this

relationship by suggesting that nationality determines communication patterns and styles

of interaction. In addition, shared “nationality can become a key driver of social

categorization” (Mäkelä, Andersson, and Seppälä, 2012, p. 442) through in-group

association, which in turn influences trust among members. However, empirical evidence

of this relationship was not very consistent.

In their research involving almost 900 students from a Dutch university, Curseu and

Schruijer (2010) found nationality diversity to be negatively related to trust. In other

words, respondents trusted those individuals of similar nationalities more than those with

different nationalities. Research by Uslaner (2011) found that similar nationality

influences trust, but only for certain backgrounds. Banai and Reisel’s (1999) research

found host-country nationals to report high levels of trust for their superiors with similar

nationalities in nationally homogeneous settings. However in nationally heterogeneous

settings, trust between employees and their superiors was not affected by nationality.

Finally, Mäkelä, Andersson, and Seppälä’s (2012) study found no relationship between

similar nationality and trust.

Page 52: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  40  

Educational Homophily and Trust

Unlike the homophily characteristics previously discussed, which are all ascribed,

education, occupation, experience, and marital status are acquired or achieved by the

individual. However, similar to the presence of homophily based on ascribed

characteristics, research also shows evidence of homophily based on acquired

characteristics. This would seem only logical, since learning institutions locate

educationally like-minded students in similar physical settings and create opportunities

for networking and social exchange through group discussions and projects. For example,

as students begin their university education, they are separated into particular degrees. As

they progress through their degrees, they are further narrowed into different faculties,

schools (e.g. a school of management), and specialty programs (e.g. strategy

management). This occurs again at the graduate level, when individuals seek a focused

advanced education (e.g. a law degree). With each step, the students’ network becomes

more educationally homogeneous. This segregating behavior seems to continue into the

work environment, as organizations tend to group employees with similar professional

experience or education together (e.g. accountants, sales persons). As with specific

faculties, these professional groups are also traditionally physically grouped together, in a

department or industry. Routine work then affords these employees opportunities for

social interaction and exchange, through projects or shared work. Overtime, close ties

and friendships are established in such environments, starting at young ages. One

outcome of the new close ties and friendships is an increased trust between the

individuals (Coleman, 1988; Burt, 1992).

Existing research found similar connections between close personal ties and educational

homophily. For example, Marsden’s (1987) results showed about 30% of the close

personal networks to be homophilous on education (with a standard deviation of less than

1 year of education) (p. 126) with extremely high and low educational levels showing the

greatest tendency for homophily (Marsden, 1988). Louch (2000) found similar results,

suggesting that individuals were more likely to form close network ties with others who

share a similar education. Several other authors found a positive relationship between

cooperative ties and educational homophily among individuals. Galaskiewicz and Shatin

Page 53: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  41  

(1981) found this relationship in turbulent communities, Laumann, Gagnon, Michael, and

Michaels (1994) in romantic relationships, Verbrugge (1977) in friendship networks, and

Reagans (2005) in a small corporate setting. Marsden’s (1987) study found that people

were more likely to confide in those with similar educational levels. Finally, Wright’s

(1997) study, which was not directly related to education or educational level, similarly

found the difference in skill level to be a significant barrier to friendship31.

Marital Homophily and Trust

Although no specific study examines marital status as a homophily factor in an

organizational setting, a number of researchers have noted marital status to be an

important homophily characteristic. For example, McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Cook

(2001) mention marital status several times in their review of homophily in social

networks, but provide no empirical contribution. In their study, van Duijn, van

Busschbach, and Snijders (1999) found friendships whose members were similar in

marital status to be more stable. Finally, in her study, Popielarz (1999) found that in all

female groups relationships between individuals were more likely to be homophilous on

marital status than in all-male or mixed groups. One might argue that there is a

relationship between marital status and trust, yet further specific research is needed to

bring forth empirical evidence.

2.3.2 Shared Perspective and Trust

Organizational shared perspective between employees is best conceptualized by Levin,

Whitener, and Cross (2006) who defined it as an amalgamation of shared language and

shared vision between co-workers. The authors (2006) defined shared language as the

extent to which the “knowledge receiver and source seem on the same wavelength” (p.

1166). ‘Same wavelength’ was an idiom used to describe a situation in which the sender

and receiver were able to easily understand, communicate, and agree. According to the

authors (Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2006), “as a manifestation of shared perspective,

parties develop a sense of the extent to which they share the same language or jargon” (p.

1166). Argyres (1999) referred to shared language as “a ‘technical grammar’ for                                                                                                                31 Other research studies (Marsden, 1987; Campbell, Marsden, & Hurlbert, 1986; Campbell, 1988; Fischer, 1982) have shown evidence of higher educated males as having more diverse networks.

Page 54: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  42  

communication” (p. 162). Shared vision is defined as the extent to which a source and

receiver (in the eyes of the receiver) share goals, concerns, and purpose (Levin, Whitener,

& Cross, 2006; Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998). Tsai and Ghoshal (1998) explained shared vision

as embodying “the collective goals and aspirations of the members of an organization;

[…] we can view a shared vision as a bonding mechanism that helps different parts of an

organization to integrate or to combine resources” (p. 467).

According to Levin, Cross, and Abrams (2003), “both of these elements […] are among

the most important factors in relation to who trust[s] whom in the knowledge transfer

context” (p. 24). Bracken and Oughton (2006) argued for this relationship by stating that

“if there is to be any chance of success in developing common understandings, the first

step is the development of trust” (p. 380). One possible explanation for this connection

was suggested by Lee (2009), who argued that shared language “overlaps with informal

interaction during daily business activity [which] helps to develop general levels of

solidarity32 [and motivate] benevolence of individual actors” (p. 258). The positive

relationship between shared perspective and trust was empirically supported by Mäkelä

and Brewster (2009), and Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2006). In addition, Tsai and

Ghoshal (1998) found shared vision to have a significant positive effect on perceived

trustworthiness.

2.3.3 Tie Strength and Trust

Tie strength was defined using Granovetter (1973), who conceptualized it as “a

combination of the amount of time, the emotional intensity, the intimacy, and the

reciprocal services which characterize the tie” (p. 1361). Granovetter (1973) further

added that each of the three determinants act independently, although the set, as a whole,

are highly intra-correlated. Several other researchers (Hansen, 1999; Levin & Cross,

2004; McFadyen & Cannella, 2004) followed similar conceptualizations, defining tie

strength as a mix of interaction frequency and closeness. Interaction frequency can

practically be thought of as synonymous with communication frequency and, in a

corporate setting, could presumably include interactions that are face-to-face, by phone,

                                                                                                               32 Solidarity is the basis for the formation of trust

Page 55: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  43  

through email, etc. Closeness relates to an emotional intensity felt by the individuals in

the relationship towards one another, which Marsden and Campbell (1984) argued is “on

balance the best indicator of the concept of tie strength” (p. 498). In fact, the authors

(1984) warned that measures of tie strength based on interaction frequency alone do not

accurately capture tie strength among co-workers. In a more recent paper, Levin, Walter,

and Murnighan (2011) supported Marsden and Campbell’s (1984) claim by arguing that

interaction frequency is not a perfect indicator of tie strength, because not all interactions

are “the same length, do not occur at a constant rate, and have unequal emotional impact”

(2011, p. 924) The variation in these ultimately causes different lasting outcomes.

With respect to trust, Levin and Cross (2004) found that tie strength had a positive impact

on benevolence-based and competence-based (i.e. ability-based) trust in organizational

settings. The authors also found benevolence and competence based trust to act as

mediator between strong ties and the perception that the knowledge received from the

coworker was useful. Krackhardt’s (1992) research suggested similar conclusions,

finding strong ties to be more accessible and more willing to help than weak ones.

2.3.4 Relationship Length and Trust

Relationship length was defined by Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2004) as how long one

co-worker has known another. Knowing someone for a longer period of time should

theoretically lead to more trust, since it provides the individuals with ample opportunities

to develop trust between them. However, according to the authors (2004) the “construct

of relationship length has been largely neglected in the trust literature” (p. I1). For

example, Dirks and Ferrin’s (2002) meta-analysis of studies and papers written over the

last four decades on trust and its implications on leadership, only found 5 of the 106

studies to contain a measure for relationship length.

Dirks and Ferrin (2002) suggested that, “the level of trust may be greater in a relationship

of long duration than in a relationship of short duration owing to the level of knowledge

and familiarity acquired” (p. 615). Lewicki and Bunker (1996) made similar claims,

arguing that levels of trust in a relationship increase and develop over time. The authors

Page 56: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  44  

(Lewicki & Bunker, 1996) proposed that trust (specifically in professional relationships)

becomes deeper as it moves through identified stages, which they call “The Stagewise

Evolution of Trust” (p. 124). The movement through these stages occurs over time, as

individuals develop a history of interaction. "In professional relationships, trust develops

gradually as the parties move from one stage to another" (Lewicki and Bunker, 1996, p.

124). The authors (1996) argued that trust is "a dynamic phenomenon which takes on a

different character in the early, developing, and ‘mature’ stages of a relationship" (p.

118).

Similar connections between trust and relationship length can be found in Coleman’s

(1988) work on network closure. Coleman (1988) reasoned that a reputation was required

to build trust between individuals, which developed over time through the proliferation of

obligations and expectations, a process similar to Dirks and Ferrin’s (2002) ‘knowledge

and familiarity acquired’ or Lewicki and Bunker’s (1996) history of interaction. Levin,

Whitener, and Cross (2004) explained these interactions as a “chance to gather

information about each other’s idiosyncrasies and perspectives, expectations can be

rooted in knowing if they share the same goals, perspective, and self-identity” (p. I3).

Much like Coleman (1988), Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2004) argued that, early in a

relationship, the trustor does not have accurate or reliable information about the trustee to

gauge benevolence. The authors propose that, over time and with direct social interaction,

the trust between the individuals becomes rooted in expectations that are based on actual

observations of behavior. Their study (Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2004) found that “[the]

relationship length significantly moderated the bases of benevolence trust [and it] did not

have a direct association with a person’s trust in another party’s benevolence, but rather a

complex and curvilinear one” (pp. I4-5).

“We found that, in new/early relationships, the bases of trust in another party’s benevolence are rooted primarily in expectations associated with demographic prototypes33; in medium-length relationships, they are rooted primarily in behavioral expectations gathered from moderate social interaction; and in old/long

                                                                                                               33 E.g. gender similarity was significantly associated

Page 57: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  45  

relationships, they are rooted primarily in personal knowledge of shared perspectives” (Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2004 p. I5).

Interestingly, the authors’ (Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2004) research findings showed

trustworthy behaviors to be the greatest in relationships that were neither new nor old, but

in-between.

2.4 Social-Cognitive Factors and Knowledge Sharing

The following section explores existing literature for relationships between the previously

discussed social-cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior. As with the previous

section, the review will begin with homophily factors and then discuss relationships with

shared perspective, tie strength, and relationship length. Each social-cognitive factor was

defined in the previous section.

2.4.1 Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Below is a brief summary of the literature exploring the relationships between status

homophily and knowledge sharing or knowledge sharing behavior. Both ascribed and

acquired characteristics are explored, with ascribed characteristics including age, gender,

race, ethnicity, and citizen and immigration data, and acquired characteristics including

education, experience, and marital status.

Age Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Research that found a positive relationship between age homophily and rate of contact

was previously discussed (Fisher, 1977; Verbrugge, 1977; Feld, 1982; Shrum, Cheek Jr.,

& Hunter, 1988). Building on this research, one may assume that an increase in the rate

of contact between co-workers gives them ample opportunities for knowledge sharing,

use, and explanation. Research studies conducted by Ojha (2005) and Riege (2005) may

suggest evidence for this theory, with findings showing that the more age compatible a

team was, the more likely that team would be to engage in effective knowledge sharing.

Furthermore, both authors provided evidence that age differences were likely to suppress

knowledge sharing. Ojha (2005) explained this relationship by suggesting that persons of

similar age were more likely to group together and interact freely.

Page 58: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  46  

Gender Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

As previously discussed, the work of Huckfeldt and Sprague (1995) found that less

intimate and more content-based relationships (like the ones one may find in a

professional organization) tend to be significantly more gender segregated. Other studies

set in professional environments also found evidence of gender segregation (e.g. Bielby

& Baron, 1986; Kalleberg, Knoke, Marsden, & Spaeth, 1996). Ibarra’s (1992) work

found that gender homophily played a role in inequality for access to professional

interaction networks and their knowledge sources and contents. Connelly and Kelloway

(2003) offer a nice illustration of this relationship by stating “if knowledge sharing is

most likely to occur among friends, and employees are most likely to become friends

with similar others (e.g. of the same gender), then employees of a minority gender may

be less likely to share knowledge freely” (p. 300). In his work on knowledge sharing in

software project teams, Ojha (2005) discovered that the more compatible a person was

with their group in terms of gender, the more likely they were to practice knowledge

sharing.

Race / Ethnicity Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Mehra, Kilduff, and Brass (1998) suggest that “research on the patterning of social

relations in organizations has shown the importance of visible categories such as race as

basis for identification and network formation” (p. 441) and “[as] basis for shared identity

and social interaction” (p. 450). Along similar lines, it was previously discussed that

strong homophily on race and ethnicity has been discovered in marriages (Kalmijn,

1998), individuals that confide in each other (Marsden, 1987; 1988), school-related

relationships (Shrum et al., 1988; Maccoby, 1998), and work-related relationships

(Lincoln & Miller, 1979; Ibarra, 1995). Further, Marsden (1987), Blum (1984), Blau and

his colleagues (Blau, Beeker, & Fitzpatrick, 1984; Blau, Blum, & Schwartz, 1982; Blau,

Ruan, & Monika, 1991) found race to predict with whom individuals shared important

matters.

Within the organizational setting, Ibarra (1993) argues that “cross-race developmental

relationships [e.g. mentor/apprentice] appear to be harder to develop, and they provide a

Page 59: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  47  

narrower range of benefits for minorities” (p. 70). Ironically, Ibarra (1993) insists that it

is not that these cross-race contacts are not available to their counterparts; it is just that,

statistically, these relationships seem more difficult and less likely to form in the

organizational setting (1993). Ibarra (1993) further argues that this difficultly in forming

relationships limits the knowledge sharing opportunities provided to these employees

(e.g. often visible minorities) in the form of mentoring. In subsequent research, Ibarra

(1995) found that ethnic minorities were much more likely (than the ethnic majority) to

seek advice and support from their heterogeneous counterparts. Other organizational

studies (Brass, Galaskiewicz, Greve, & Tsai, 2004; Mehra, Kilduff, & Brass, 1998) have

suggested similar findings, even citing race homophily as a driver for effective

organizational knowledge sharing (Makela, Kalla, & Piekkari, 2007).

Citizenship / Immigration Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Geringer (1988), Oetzel (1995), and Bornhorst et al. (2010) found that similarity in

nationality influenced communication patterns and styles of interaction, which arguably

play a role in the sharing and use of knowledge. In their study, Manev and Stevenson

(2001) found shared nationality to be a foundation on which managers create and sustain

strong network ties. These strong network ties create opportunities for interaction, which

further open opportunities for knowledge sharing to take place. According to Mäkelä,

Andersson, and Seppälä (2012) “knowledge sharing is a bi-product of interaction: the

more extensive interaction there is between two people, the more opportunities they have

for both intentional and serendipitous knowledge sharing” (p. 442). The authors also

argued that network ties tend to be stronger between employees coming from similar

national backgrounds, because nationality can become a key driver for social

categorization. Their findings supported their reasoning, showing evidence that

individuals of the same nationality shared more knowledge across unit boundaries.

Educational Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

As previously mentioned, individuals of similar education and experience are more likely

to form network ties (Louch, 2000) and cooperative ties (Galaskiewicz & Shatin, 1981;

Laumann, Gagnon, Michael & Michaels, 1994; Verbrugge, 1977). Ojha’s (2005) research

Page 60: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  48  

found that individuals with greater differences in their levels of education were less likely

to share. Ojha (2005) also found differences in the type of education to be an inhibitor for

participating in knowledge sharing. Finally, Riege’s (2005) work established a

contributory relationship between educational level and the likelihood to share

knowledge.

Marital Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Fischer (1982) found significant evidence of marital status homophily in his study of

personal networks, where married respondents more frequently named married referents,

never married named the never married referents, and the divorced named divorced.

Kalmijn and Vermunt (2007) offered an explanation for this, by arguing that “people may

have a preference for interacting with others in the same marital status category [because

those] in the same marital status position may better understand, they may have more

relevant information for each other, and they may share a certain lifestyle which increases

possibilities for joint activities” (p. 27). The authors (2007) also suggested that this may

be because individuals want to interact with people who went through similar life

transitions, or they are also going through these life transitions. “If a person’s friends start

getting married, for example, this may speed up this person’s decision to get married as

well, thereby increasing the degree of marital status homogeneity. Friends who marry

will become disconnected from friends who remain single, and friends who divorce will

become disconnected from friends who remain married” (Kalmijn & Vermunt, 2007, p.

27).

There was no research found to discuss the direct effect of marital status on knowledge

sharing or knowledge sharing behavior. However, there are some studies that suggest

planning groups, such as Parent Teacher Associations (PTAs), to be homogeneous with

respect to marital status (McPherson & Smith-Lovin, 1987). Also, in their review of

homophily, Rogers and Bhowmik (1970) mentioned a 1967 study at the University of

Chicago, conducted by Palmore that found individuals living in the Chicago ghettos to

share family planning ideas with others of like marital status.

Page 61: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  49  

2.4.2 Shared Perspective and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

As previously discussed, knowledge is highly contextual and circumstantial (Goman,

2002); it is always developed in a specific context and is rarely interpreted by the receiver

in the exact way as it was intended by the transmitter (Husted & Michailova, 2002). A

key problem is representing the context in which knowledge is created and is relevant

(Choo, 2000), which makes transferring knowledge problematic (Brown & Duguid,

1991; Kogut & Zander, 1992; Empson, 2001). Reasons for contextual mismatches

include differences in mental/conceptual frameworks or culture and language (Hendricks,

1999). For this reason, several researchers have identified a shared perspective as a driver

for knowledge sharing (e.g. Nahapiet & Ghoshal, 1998; Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995;

Brown & Duguid, 2000; Mäkelä & Brewster, 2009).

Knowledge is easier to transfer when it is rooted in the domain or practice of the

individuals participating (Brown & Duguid, 1998). Nonaka (2002) makes a similar

argument with information: “the mere transfer of information will often make little sense

if it is abstracted from embedded emotions and nuanced contexts that are associated with

shared experiences” (p. 442). For knowledge to be shared, the receiver and the transmitter

must share a contextual base (Mäkelä & Brewster, 2009). Brown and Duguid (2000)

called this “a shared framework for interpretation” (p. 107). The receiver must possess

what Swap et al. (2001) called a hook or receptor, which assimilates the information

provided by the transmitter. Argyres (1999) called this “a ‘technical grammar’ for

communication” (p. 162).

Shared Language and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Triandis (1960) found connections between similarity in language and effective

knowledge understanding and use. Tsoukas and Vladimirou (2001) proclaimed a

common language as the most important cultural tool needed in assisting an individual in

‘drawing distinctions’ within a collectively generated ‘domain of action’. Nonaka (1994)

argued that shared language was paramount to the transfer and integration of tacit

knowledge rooted in the sharing of common schemata and frameworks, such as stories,

analogies, and metaphors.

Page 62: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  50  

Research also found that shared language facilitated knowledge sharing behavior

through: a common understanding of how to act (Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998); an ability to

gain access to the right people for information (Nahapiet & Ghoshal, 1998); a “common

conceptual apparatus for evaluating the likely benefits of exchange” (Chiu, Hsu, &

Wang, 2006, p. 1878); and a common framework for the combination of knowledge

(Nahapiet & Ghoshal, 1998). Several of these benefits were represented in Zenger and

Lawrence’s (1989) research, which found that shared language determined the efficiency

of communication, by acting as guide for how information was interpreted and responded

to. Henderson’s (2005) study of multinational corporation international project teams also

concluded that language diversity affected knowledge sharing through interpretation,

ultimately influencing overall team performance. Ojha’s (2005) research, which collected

data on 83 software project teams in 20 organizations, found a significant relationship

between language compatibility and the likelihood of participating in knowledge sharing

behavior. Chiu, Hsu, and Wang (2006) added a distinction between the differential

effects shared language had on the quality and quantity of knowledge, suggesting shared

language to have a significant positive effect on the quality of knowledge shared, and no

effect on quantity.

Shared Vision and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Research also found that shared vision helps workers see the potential value of their

knowledge exchange (Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998) and provides a guideline for understanding

which knowledge was worth acquiring and disseminating (Hoe & McShane, 2002). Also,

if shared vision was high among co-workers, then the knowledge sharing process could

tolerate a certain degree of ‘inefficiency’, as long as the bulk of employee actions were

pointed in a unified direction (Hoe & McShane, 2002).

Tsai and Ghoshal (1998) found that management teams who shared a vision were more

likely to participate in knowledge sharing and resource exchange activities. Chang et al.

(2011) showed that shared vision was a necessary precondition for knowledge sharing,

and that it had a positive effect on the overall willingness to share knowledge, ideas, and

opinions, and to answer colleague questions. Similarly, Hoe and McShane’s (2002) study

Page 63: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  51  

of IT sales, customer service, and technical consulting employees in 11 business units of

a large ICT firm, found shared vision to be a strong predictor for knowledge sharing.

Finally, Chiu, Hsu, and Wang (2006) found shared vision to have a significant positive

effect on the quality of knowledge shared and, more interestingly, a significant negative

effect on quantity. This finding suggests that having a higher shared vision encouraged

more succinct meaningful exchanges between individuals.

2.4.3 Tie Strength and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

In his seminal paper, Granovetter (1973) concluded that weak ties were more likely to act

as a source for unique and useful information. Granovetter (1973) reasoned that an

individual’s strong ties likely had the same or similar information and network access to

those already in the network. On the other hand, weak ties provided the opportunity to

access new network ties, as well as useful novel information. “Weaker ties reflect a path

along which new information or novel insights are more likely to travel in comparison to

stronger ones” (Levin & Cross, 2004 p. 1480). Subsequent research by Granovetter

(1982), as well as Rogers (1995), found that weak ties were also instrumental in the

diffusion of ideas. Research on weak ties has also demonstrated they are beneficial in the

dissemination of information (Uzzi & Lancaster, 2003; Cross & Cummings, 2004) and

technical advice (Constant, Sproull, & Kiesler, 1996). Hansen (1999) found weak ties

useful, because they were, for the individual, less costly to maintain than strong ties.

Other research suggested the exact opposite relationship to exist. For instance,

Krackhardt (1992) suggested that strong ties were more important than weak ones to the

individual, because these were the ties that made themselves accessible and, more

importantly, were willing to help. Numerous other studies have confirmed this claim, by

suggesting that strong ties are channels through which useful knowledge travels, and that

these paths have greater knowledge exchange occurring within them (Hansen, 1999;

Szulanski, 1996; Ghoshal, Korine, & Szulanski, 1994; Uzzi, 1996, 1997). Levin and

Cross’s (2004) research even found a positive statistical relationship between strong ties

and the perception that the knowledge received was useful. Much like Krackhardt (1992),

Levin and Cross (2004) reasoned that this occurred because “strong ties [were] more

Page 64: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  52  

likely to expend effort to ensure that a knowledge seeker sufficiently understands and can

put into use newly acquired knowledge” (p. 1479).

2.4.4 Relationship Length and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Very few research studies were found that tested the length of relationship, and a direct

relationship between relationship length and knowledge sharing behavior. In their

examination of interpersonal relationships and knowledge sharing at multinational

corporations, Mäkelä and Brewster (2009) found that relationship length influenced the

extent of knowledge sharing. In subsequent research Mäkelä, Andersson, and Seppälä

(2012) found knowledge sharing to be “through enhanced interaction extensiveness

[which is] the product of ‘frequency of interaction’ and ‘length of the relationship’” (p.

447).

Other researchers expanded their theoretical models to include knowledge sharing

behaviors, by proposing a more complex relationship between knowledge sharing

behavior and relationship length, mediated through trust (Renzl, 2008; Levin, Whitener,

& Cross, 2006; Coleman 1988).

2.5 Conceptual Framework

This section presents the conceptual framework for examining the effect social-cognitive

factors and trust have on organizational knowledge sharing behavior. The conceptual

framework is based on research studies, presented in the previous sections, which

explored various relationships between trust and knowledge sharing (or knowledge

sharing behavior) in organizations. It is also based on research studies, also presented in

the previous sections, which identified social or cognitive factors that have been found to

influence trust, knowledge sharing behavior or both. Research studies conducted within

organizational settings were given priority in the review, but were not always available.

Knowledge sharing behavior was identified in the conceptual framework using three

important behavioral conditions (presented in Section 2.1.3) necessary for effective

knowledge sharing behavior to take place. First, the knowledge source must be willing to

Page 65: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  53  

share the knowledge they posses (i.e. willingness to share knowledge). Second, the

knowledge receiver must be willing to receive and use the knowledge that is shared (i.e.

willingness to use knowledge). Finally, the knowledge receiver must perceive the

knowledge shared as being useful to their individual work, the project, or the

organization as a whole (i.e. perceived receipt of useful knowledge). In addition,

willingness to share and use knowledge are further separated to consider overall

knowledge, tacit knowledge, and explicit knowledge.

The first social-cognitive factor identified in the literature and appearing in the

conceptual framework (Figure 2.5) is homophily. According to existing research,

homophily (or similarity) between two individuals based on certain ascribed and acquired

characteristics (i.e. age, gender, race/ethnicity, citizenship/ immigration, education, and

marital status) has been found to have a positive effect on the level of trust the two

individuals perceive to have for one another (Section 2.3.1). Homophily based on these

characteristics has also been found to positively influence effective knowledge sharing

activities and behaviors (Section 2.4.1). Figure 2.6 shows a decomposition of the

homophily factor, noting each of the ascribed and acquired characteristics identified in

the study, and their proposed relationship with trust and knowledge sharing behavior.

Arrows in Figures 2.5 and 2.6 indicate these relationships. The influence of each type of

homophily factor on trust and knowledge sharing behavior will be explored in the study.

The second and third social-cognitive factors identified in the conceptual framework are

shared language and shared vision between co-workers (Figure 2.5). These factors were

based on Levin, Whitener, and Cross’ (2006) concept of organizational shared

perspective, which is an amalgamation of the two. Several previous organizational

studies (Section 2.3.2) have found positive relationships between shared language and

trust, as well as between shared vision and trust. Specifically, co-workers who shared a

higher degree of language or vision were found to have higher trust for one another.

Previous studies (Section 2.4.2) have also found shared language and shared vision to

have a positive influence on effective knowledge sharing activities and behaviors. Arrows

in Figure 2.5 indicate these relationships.

Page 66: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  54  

The fourth social-cognitive factor identified in the literature review is tie strength

between co-workers. Tie strength was primarily based on the work of Granovetter (1973),

who conceptualized it as “a combination of the amount of time, the emotional intensity,

the intimacy, and the reciprocal services which characterize the tie” (p. 1361). Research

by Levin and Cross (2004) and Krackhardt (1992) suggested that a stronger tie strength

between individuals led to higher perceived trust among them. However, the relationship

between tie strength and knowledge sharing behaviors has not been entirely

straightforward. For example, Granovetter (1973), Levin and Cross (2004), Rogers

(1995) and other authors (Uzzi & Lancaster, 2003; Cross & Cummings, 2004; Constant,

Sproull, & Kiesler, 1996) found weak ties to provide access to more information and

channels for knowledge, as well as new knowledge. These ties are also more efficient,

since they are less costly (e.g. time) to maintain (Hansen, 1999). On the other hand,

several other researchers (Krackhardt, 1992; Hansen, 1999; Szulanski, 1996; Ghoshal,

Korine, & Szulanski, 1994; Uzzi, 1996, 1997) found strong ties to promote more

effective knowledge sharing activities and behaviors because these are the ties that are

more accessible and willing to help. The conceptual framework in this study takes into

consideration both possibilities by simply identifying some type of relationship as

existing between tie strength and knowledge sharing behavior and between tie strength

and trust.

The final social-cognitive factor identified in the literature review, and included in the

conceptual framework (Figure 2.5), is relationship length. Relationship length was

defined using Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2004) as how long one co-worker has known

another. Numerous studies (Levin, Whitener, & Cross 2004; Dirks & Ferrin, 2002;

Lewicki & Bunker, 1996; Coleman, 1988; Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2004) have

suggested that the longer an individual knows another, the more they should theoretically

trust them, since, over time, the individual becomes more familiar with their co-worker

through interaction. This interaction allows for both individuals to learn about each other

(Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2004) and build reputations (Coleman, 1988). As previously

mentioned, few studies (Mäkelä & Brewster, 2009; Mäkelä, Andersson, & Seppälä,

2012) were found that discovered a direct influence of relationship length on knowledge

Page 67: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  55  

sharing behavior but several studies suggested a relationship existed (and was mediated)

through trust (Renzl, 2008; Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2006; Coleman 1988). The

conceptual framework in this study considers both types of relationships by simply

indicating some type of relationship as existing between relationship length and

knowledge sharing behavior and between relationship length and trust.

The final factor included in the conceptual framework found to influence knowledge

sharing behavior is trust (Figure 2.5). Trust was defined using Mayer et al.’s (1995)

proposed model of organizational trust, which includes benevolence-based trust, ability-

based trust, integrity-based trust, and propensity to trust. The literature review (Section

2.2.2) identified an overwhelming positive relationship between trust and specific

knowledge sharing behaviors, other organizational behaviors (which might be deemed as

beneficial for knowledge sharing), resource/information exchange, and decision-making

and problem solving (which are outcomes of knowledge sharing behavior). Arrows in

Figure 2.5 indicate this relationship.

Since each of the social-cognitive factors has been found in the literature to have some

direct effect on both knowledge sharing behavior and trust, in addition to depicting these

direct relationships, the conceptual framework also considers the indirect or mediating

effect through trust onto knowledge sharing behavior.

Each of the concepts explored in the present study, and possible relationships between

them, are depicted in the conceptual framework in Figure 2.5. Factors examined, along

with trust, include homophily, shared language, shared vision, tie strength, and

relationship length. Figure 2.6 depicts a decomposition of the homophily factor,

separating ascribed and acquired characteristics.

Page 68: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  56  

 Figure 2.5 Conceptual Framework Based on Literature Review

 Figure 2.6 Conceptual Framework of Decomposed Homophily Factors

Page 69: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  57  

Chapter 3: Research Design and Methodology 3.0 Chapter Overview

This chapter details the research design and methodology used in this study. The chapter

begins with a discussion of the research questions and associated hypotheses. Next, the

data measurement strategies are discussed, with a clarification of the variables and

operationalization. The chapter continues with data analysis strategies, elaborating the

validity and reliability of measures, as well as how hypotheses were tested. The chapter

concludes with a discussion of population, site, and data collection methods.

3.1 Research Questions and Hypotheses

As previously mentioned, the overarching research question was what are the factors

that influence knowledge sharing behavior directly and indirectly through trust? To

elaborate this research question, three additional questions were posed. What are the

significant relationships between trust and knowledge sharing behavior? What are the

significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and trust? And, what are the

significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and knowledge sharing

behavior?

To better understand the direct and indirect effects of social-cognitive variables and trust

on knowledge sharing behavior, two additional research questions were posed. The first

examined the collective effect social-cognitive factors and trust had on knowledge

sharing behavior (i.e. what is the collective effect of the identified social-cognitive

variables and trust on knowledge sharing behavior?). The second additional research

question was concerned with the indirect or intervening effect trust had between social-

cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior (i.e. does trust act as a mediating

variable between social-cognitive variables and knowledge sharing behavior?).

Based on the findings from the literature review, the relationships between the

independent and dependent variables were hypothesized. Testing these hypothesized

relationships formed the basis through which the research questions were answered. A

summary of the five research questions and associated hypotheses is shown in Table 3.1.

Page 70: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  58  

Research Question Associated Hypotheses

RQ1: What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and trust?

H1: Age homophily will be positively related to Trust H3: Educational homophily will be positively related to Trust H5: Gender homophily will be positively related to Trust H18: Race/Ethnic Homophily will be positively related to Trust H20: Citizen/Immigration homophily will be positively related to Trust H22: Marital Homophily will be positively related to Trust H7: Shared Language will be positively related to Trust H9: Shared Vision will be positively related to Trust H11: Relationship Length will be positively related to Trust H13: Tie Strength will be positively related to Trust

RQ2: What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and knowledge sharing behavior?

H2: Age Homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H4: Educational Homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H6: Gender Homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H19: Race/Ethnic Homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H21: Citizen/Immigration homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H23: Marital Homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H8: Shared Language will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H10: Shared Vision will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H12: Relationship Length will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior H14: Tie Strength will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

RQ3: What are the significant relationships between trust and knowledge sharing behavior?

H15: Trust will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

RQ4: What is the collective effect of the identified social-cognitive variables and trust on knowledge sharing behavior?

H16: Overall Trust and Social-Cognitive Factors explain Knowledge Sharing Behavior

RQ5: Does trust act as a mediating variable between social-cognitive variables and knowledge sharing behavior?

H17:Overall Trust will be a mediating variable between Social-Cognitive Factors and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Table 3.1 Research Questions and Associated Hypotheses

3.2 Operationalization of Variables

3.2.1 Social-Cognitive Variables (Independent Variables)

Homophily Measures

To measure status homophily between individuals, this research used variables to capture

both ascribed and acquired characteristics. Each respondent was asked to answer

questions that provided information related to characteristics about themselves; and then,

as best as they could, answer questions describing characteristics about two fellow co-

workers they mentally selected. Ascribed characteristic variables of interest included

measures of age, gender, race/ethnicity, as well as citizen/immigration data. Acquired

characteristic variables of interest included educational history, experience, and marital

Page 71: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  59  

status. Since questions regarding both acquired and ascribed characteristics may be

sensitive in nature, appropriate wording became imperative. To avoid such concerns, this

study used Statistics Canada’s 2006 Census questions (see Survey Instrument in the

Appendix), to capture all homophily related survey items (Statistics Canada, 2006). This

research did not introduce or measure any variables that attempt to capture value

homophily, as these measures may be conceptually too similar to shared vision.

Shared Perspective (Shared Language and Shared Vision)

Based on the work of Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2006), two variables were introduced

to measure the extent to which a shared perspective (shared language and shared vision)

existed between participants in the study. To measure shared perspective, Levin,

Whitener, and Cross (2006) conducted a cross-sectional survey of employees working in

a knowledge intensive division of a U.S. pharmaceutical company, a British bank, and a

Canadian oil and gas company. A total of six items were used: three items to measure

shared language (Cronbach’s alpha of .67) and three items for shared vision (Cronbach’s

alpha of .78). Table 3.2 summarizes the items used by Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2006)

for shared language, the authors’ study population, and the proposed adapted items that

were used in the research presented here. The proposed adapted measure for shared

language is also indicated using a three-item measure. Items included: “I could

understand completely what this person meant when he or she was talking”; “I was

familiar with the jargon/terminology that he or she used”; and, “it felt like we could

communicate on the same wavelength”. All shared language items were measured using

a five-point scale (1 – Strongly Disagree, 5 – Strongly Agree).

Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2006) also developed three items to measure shared vision,

which, they claim, were motivated by, and are similar to, the two-item measure for

shared vision used by Tsai and Ghoshal (1998), in their research of management teams in

a multinational electronics company. Table 3.2 summarizes the items used by Levin,

Whitener, and Cross (2006) and Tsai and Ghoshal (1998) for shared vision, the authors’

study population, and the proposed adapted items that were used in the research presented

here.

Page 72: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  60  

Based on the work of Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2006) and Tsai and Ghoshal (1998), in

this study, shared vision was measured using a 5-item scale. The items were: “I felt like

this person and I were working toward completely different goals”; “I assumed that this

person and I cared about the same issues”; “I believed that this person and I shared a

commitment to a common purpose”; “I believed that this person and I shared the same

ambitions and vision”; and, “I believed that this person and I shared enthusiasm about

pursuing the collective goals and missions of the whole organization”. All shared vision

items were measured on a five-point scale (1 – Strongly Disagree, 5 – Strongly Agree).

Page 73: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  61  

Author(s) Factor Study Population

Authors’ Item Adapted Item (final question items in

bold)

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Shared Language

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to seeking information/advice from this person on this project, I could understand completely what this person meant when he or she was talking.

I could understand completely what this person meant when he or she was talking.

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Shared Language

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to seeking information/advice from this person on this project, I was familiar with the jargon/terminology that he or she used.

I was familiar with the jargon/terminology that he or she used.

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Shared Language

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to my seeking information/advice from this person on this project, it felt like we could communicate on the same "wavelength”.

It felt like we could communicate on the same "wavelength”.

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Shared Vision

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to seeking information/advice from this person on this project, I felt like this person and I were working toward completely different goals. [reverse coded]

I felt like this person and I were working toward completely different goals.

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Shared Vision

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to seeking information/advice from this person on this project, I assumed that this person and I cared about the same issues.

I assumed that this person and I cared about the same issues.

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Shared Vision

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to seeking information/advice from this person on this project, I believed that this person and I shared a commitment to a common purpose.

I believed that this person and I shared a commitment to a common purpose.

Tsai and Ghoshal, 1998

Shared Vision

Management Teams

Our unit shares the same ambitions and vision with other units at work

I believed that this person and I shared the same ambitions and vision.

Tsai and Ghoshal, 1998

Shared Vision

Management Teams

People in our unit are enthusiastic about pursuing the collective goals and missions of the whole organization.

I believed that this person and I shared enthusiasm about pursuing the collective goals and missions of the whole organization.

Table 3.2 Operationalization and Measurement of the Shared Perspective Research Variables

Tie Strength

Based on the work of Granovetter (1973), and two items used by Hansen (1999) in his

study for inter-unit tie weakness (i.e. one for interaction frequency and another for

closeness; see Table 3.3), Levin and Cross (2004) developed their measure for

organizational tie strength. Using pre-test feedback, the authors (Levin & Cross, 2004)

Page 74: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  62  

clarified Hansen’s (1999) scales, and introduced new ties for those participants with no

prior contact. Levin and Cross (2004) also included a third item for communication

frequency, to increase reliability. Since the three measures (Table 3.3) used different

scales, Levin and Cross (2004) normalized each of them, before creating the overall

variable. Cronbach’s alpha for Levin and Cross’ (2004) three-item operationalization of

tie strength was .9. For the present study, the authors’ (Levin & Cross, 2004) items

(Table 3.3) were further adapted, as a result of conversations with Daniel Levin (Evans,

personal communication, October, 2010), who, with his colleagues, suggested a

rethinking of Marsden and Campbell’s (1984) understanding on tie strength. Levin,

Walter, and Murnighan (2011) suggested that emotion-based (or closeness) measures

were, at the very least, as important as the commonly used interaction and

communication frequency measures. With this in mind, a fourth item relating to closeness

was introduced in this study, to increase reliability (Table 3.3) (i.e. two of the four tie

strength items adapted for use in the present study related to closeness and the other two

items to interaction/communication frequency).

In addition, it was reasoned that tie strength prior to working on a project could be

conceptually different from tie strength developed while working on a project. The

perceived difference between tie strength prior to and while on could be justified by

considering that the frequency with which two co-workers interact can easily vary from

project to project, based on the specifics of the project and its requirements. Also, the

closeness two co-workers feel for each other can change, as they continue to work

together and have opportunities to interact. It is possible that high levels of interaction

frequency exist between co-workers, prior to working on the project, and still have no

relation to the tie strength felt while on a new project. Making this distinction is

important, since one practical application for understanding tie strength may be choosing

to allocate workers to projects based on their prior communication or interaction

frequency. However, past interaction or communication may not guarantee, or be a

predictor for, tie strength between the same co-workers while they are working on a new

project together. Since this conceptual difference may have a different effect on the

dependent variables, a distinction between the two was made. Therefore, each of the tie

Page 75: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  63  

strength items appeared twice on the survey; once asking the respondent about their

relationship with their co-workers prior to the project and once again to measure the

same relationship while on the project.

In this study, tie strength was measured using an eight-item scale. Items were: “Prior to

(While) working with each of the co-workers on the project you shared, how close was

your working relationship?”; “My relationship with each of the co-workers I mentally

selected was a very intense, strong relationship prior to (while) working on the matter or

firm-related project we shared”; “Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers

on the project you shared, how often did you communicate?”; “Prior to (While) working

with each of the co-workers on the project you shared, to what extent did you typically

interact with each of them?”. The response categories for the four tie strength items

varied (see Survey Instrument in the Appendix for specific scales).

Page 76: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  64  

Author(s) Factor Study Population

Authors’ Item Adapted Item (final question items in

bold)

Levin and Cross, 2004

Tie Strength

Knowledge workers (Pharmaceutical)

Prior to seeking information/advice from this person on this project how close was your working relationship with each person? If you had no prior contact at all with this person before you sought information /advice from him or her on this project, please choose 7. (1 = very close; 4 = somewhat close; 7 = distant)

Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers on the project you shared, how close was your working relationship?

Levin and Cross, 2004

Tie Strength

Knowledge workers (Pharmaceutical)

Prior to seeking information/advice from this person on this project how often did you communicate with each person? If you had no prior contact at all with this person before you sought information /advice from him or her on this project, please choose 7. (1 = daily; 2 = twice a week; 3 = once a week; 4 = twice a month; 5 = once a month; 6 = once every 2nd month; 7 = once every 3 months or less (or never))

Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers on the project you shared, how often did you communicate?

Levin and Cross, 2004

Tie Strength

Knowledge workers (Pharmaceutical)

Prior to seeking information/advice from this person on this project to what extent did you typically interact with each person? (1 = to no extent; 2 = to little extent; 3 = to some extent; 4 = to a great extent; 5 = to a very great extent)

Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers on the project you shared, to what extent did you typically interact with each of them?”

Hansen, 1999

Interunit Tie Weakness

Divisions in a large electronics company

How frequently do (did) people in your division interact with this division (on average over the past two years)? (0 = once a day, 1 = twice a week, 2 = once a week, 3 = twice a month, 4 = once a month, 5 = once every 2nd month, 6 = once every 3 months.)

Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers on the project you shared, to what extent did you typically interact with each of them?” *Repeated item

Hansen, 1999

Interunit Tie Weakness

Divisions in a large electronics company

How close is (was) the working relationship between your division and this division? (0 = "Very close, practically like being in the same work group," 3 = "Somewhat close, like discussing and solving issues together," 6 = "Distant, like an arm's-length delivery of the input".)

Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers on the project you shared, how close was your working relationship? *Repeated item

New Item Tie Strength

N/A N/A My relationship with each of the co-workers I mentally selected was a very intense, strong relationship prior to (while) working on the matter or firm-related project we shared.

Table 3.3 Operationalization and Measurement of the Tie Strength Research Variable

Page 77: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  65  

Relationship Duration (Length)

Based on the work of Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2004; 2006), a one-item variable was

used to measure relationship duration/length between co-workers. The authors (2006)

simply asked “How long had you known this person prior to your seeking

information/advice from him or her on this project (in months and years)?” For the

present study, a similar item was used: “Approximately, how long have you known each

of the co-workers?” This study recognizes that relationship duration may not be a

predictor for interaction pattern, although, as Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2004; 2006)

argue, it is one important proxy for the extent of interaction.

3.2.2 Trust (Mediating Variable)

In Mayer et al.’s (1995) opinion, previous research, models, and measures for trust have

not been clear in differentiating trust, factors that led to trust, and the outcomes of trust.

However, the authors’ model conceptualizes trust in a fashion that distinguishes it from

its outcomes and from its antecedents. Their model and definition also attempts to

measure trust factors with respect to the characteristics of the trustor (i.e. propensity) and

the perceived characteristics of the trustee (i.e. ability, benevolence, and integrity),

something the authors argue other models have neglected (Mayer et al., 1995;

Schoorman, Mayer, & Davis, 1996b). “The failure to clearly specify the trustor [the

trusting party] and the trustee [the party to be trusted] encourages the tendency to change

referents and even level of analysis, which obfuscates the nature of the trust relationship”

(Mayer et al, 1995, p. 711). As discussed in the literature review chapter (Section 2.2.1),

the model suggests a separate measure for ‘propensity to trust’ (p. 715), which captures a

personal characteristic of the trustor and perceived trustworthiness, a set of characteristics

specific to the trustee (i.e. ability, benevolence, and integrity) (Mayer et al., 1995).

Using Mayer et al.’s (1995) distinction, the present research proposed to measure trust by

first asking respondents to answer items relating to their ‘propensity to trust’, and then

asking the respondents to make an assessment of their co-worker’s factors of perceived

trustworthiness (i.e. ability, benevolence, and integrity). This allowed for the study to

explore the relationship of collective or overall trust (i.e. the sum of the three perceived

Page 78: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  66  

trustworthiness factors) and each of the individual trustworthiness factors, against other

independent variables in the study, as well as dependent variables. The operationalization

of each type of trust factor is presented below.

Ability-based Trust (Competence-based trust)

Based on the work of several researchers (Mayer & Davis, 1999; Levin & Cross, 2004;

McAllister, 1995; Chattopadhyay, 1999), in this study, nine items (see Table 3.4) were

introduced to measure perceived ability-based trust in a co-worker. To increase

reliability, items for ability-based trust were adapted and combined from similar

constructs, including Mayer and Davis’ (1999) ability-based trust, Levin and Cross’

(2004) competence-based trust, McAllister’s (1995) cognition-based trust, and

Chattopadhyay (1999) trust.

In their study of employee trust for top management, Mayer and Davis (1999) introduced

a measure to reflect ability-based trust in top management’s domain specific skills and

competencies. Ability-based trust was rated as high when management’s decisions

showed competence, or when management demonstrated skills in understanding

problems and resolving employee work-related issues. To measure ability-based trust,

Mayer and Davis (1999) used six items, which are summarized in Table 3.4. Cronbach’s

alpha for these six measures was .85 for the first wave of the study and .88 for the second

wave (Mayer & Davis, 1999).

Mayer, Davis, and Schoorman (1995; 1996ab) argue that ability-based trust and

competence-based trust are conceptually similar constructs, which suggests that

competence-based trust items may be included alongside ability-based ones. Similarly,

competence-based trust may be considered synonymous with McAllister’s (1995)

cognition-based trust. For example, Levin and Cross (2004) measured competence-based

trust using a measure adapted from two top loading items for cognition-based trust,

developed and used by McAllister (1995) in his study. Cronbach’s alpha for competence-

based trust measures was .80 (Levin & Cross, 2004). Like Levin and Cross (2004),

Chattopadhyay (1999) also adapted McAllister’s (1995) instrument, in his measurement

Page 79: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  67  

of trust as a mediating factor between demographic characteristics and organizational

citizenship behavior. Chattopadhyay (1999) used four of McAllister’s (1995) items,

including an adaptation of the three suggested for this research (Table 3.4).

McAllister’s (1995) work on trust suggested that there are two principal forms of trust:

cognition-based and affect based. To develop measures for these two forms of trust,

McAllister (1995) conducted a literature review on available measures of trust. Eleven

behavioral scholars were then asked to classify the initial pool of 48 items, into the

suggested two forms of trust. McAllister (1995) used these evaluations to create a subset

of twenty items, ten for each form of trust. Through exploratory factor analysis of pre-test

data on M.B.A. and business students, McAllister (1995) reduced the number of items to

the eleven ‘strongest-loaded items’ (p. 36). Cronbach’s alphas were .91 for cognition-

based trust items (six in total) and .89 for affect-based items (five in total).

According to McAllister (1995), cognition-based trust relates to the cognitive foundations

of trust. This form of trust is circumstantial and is based on a choice made for “good

reasons” (p. 25). These good reasons are usually based on past evidence of behavior, of

which ability and/or competence is a part. “Measures of trust in organizational settings

suggest that competence and responsibility are central elements” (McAllister, 1995, p.

26). Although not all of McAllister’s (1995) items fit within the current study, three items

of the cognition-based trust (Table 3.4) did relate to ability/competence-based trust.

Page 80: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  68  

Author(s) Factor Study Population  

Authors’ Item Adapted Item (final question items in bold)  

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Ability Top Management & Employees

Top management is very capable of performing its job.

This person is very capable of performing his/her job.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Ability Top Management & Employees

Top management is known to be successful at the things it tries to do.

This person is known to be successful at the things he/she tries to do.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Ability Top Management & Employees

Top management has much knowledge about the work that needs done.

This person has much knowledge about the work that needs done.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Ability Top Management & Employees

I feel confident about top management’s skills.

I feel confident about this person’s skills.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Ability Top Management & Employees

Top management has specialized capabilities that can increase performance.

This person has specialized capabilities that can increase performance.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Ability Top Management & Employees

Top management is well qualified.

This person is well qualified.

Levin and Cross, 2004

Competence Knowledge workers

Prior to seeking information / advice from this person, I believed that this person approached his or her job with professionalism and dedication.

I believed that this person approached his or her job with professionalism and dedication.

Levin and Cross, 2004

Competence Knowledge workers

Prior to seeking information / advice from this person, given his or her track record, I saw no reason to doubt this person’s competence and preparation.

Given his or her track record, I saw no reason to doubt this person’s competence and preparation.

McAllister, 1995

Cognition Manager and knowledge workers

This person approaches his or her job with professionalism and dedication.

I believed that this person approached his or her job with professionalism and dedication. *Repeated item

McAllister, 1995

Cognition Manager and knowledge workers

Given this person’s track record, I see no reason to doubt his/her competence and preparation.

Given his or her track record, I saw no reason to doubt this person’s competence and preparation. *Repeated item

McAllister, 1995

Cognition Manager and knowledge workers

I can rely on this person not to make my job more difficult by careless work.

I can rely on this person not to make my job more difficult by careless work.

Page 81: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  69  

Chattopadhyay, 1999

Trust Workgroups (employees and peers)

The members of my work group approach their jobs with professionalism and dedication.

I believed that this person approached his or her job with professionalism and dedication. *Repeated item

Chattopadhyay, 1999

Trust Workgroups (employees and peers)

Given my work group’s track record, I see no reason to doubt his/her competence and preparation.

Given his or her track record, I saw no reason to doubt this person’s competence and preparation. *Repeated item

Chattopadhyay, 1999

Trust Workgroups (employees and peers)

I can rely on my group members not to make my job more difficult by careless work.

I can rely on this person not to make my job more difficult by careless work. *Repeated item

Table 3.4 Operationalization and Measurement of the Ability-Based / Competence-Based Trust

In this study, ability-based trust was measured with a nine-item scale (Table 3.4). Items

were: “This person is very capable of performing his/her job”; “This person is known to

be successful at the things he/she tries to do”; “This person has much knowledge about

the work that needs done”; “I feel confident about this person’s skills”; “This person has

specialized capabilities that can increase performance”; “This person is well qualified”;

“I believed that this person approached his or her job with professionalism and

dedication”; “Given his or her track record, I saw no reason to doubt this person’s

competence and preparation”; and, “I can rely on this person not to make my job more

difficult by careless work”. All ability-based trust items were measured on a five-point

scale (1 – Strongly Disagree, 5 – Strongly Agree).

Benevolence-Based Trust

Based on the work of several researchers (Mayer & Davis, 1999; Levin, Whitener, &

Cross, 2006; Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, & Takenouchi, 1996), in this study, nine items

(Table 3.5) were adapted to measure perceived benevolence-based trust in a co-worker.

To increase reliability for this measure, items for benevolence-based trust were adapted

and combined from similar items used by Mayer and Davis (1999); Levin, Whitener, and

Cross (2006) and Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, and Takenouchi (1996).

In their study, Mayer and Davis (1999) explained benevolence as the extent to which an

employee believed their manager cared about their interests and acted in an altruistic

Page 82: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  70  

manner toward them (i.e. acted in a good way towards the employee). The authors

(Mayer & Davis, 1999) operationalized benevolence into five items (Table 3.5) that

collectively had Cronbach’s alpha of .87 for the second wave of the study and .89 for the

third wave. Other researchers have also used similar constructs, in their work on trust in

organizations. For example, in their research on relationship length and trust, Levin,

Whitener, and Cross (2006) measured perceived trustworthiness as a “perception of

trustworthiness in terms of benevolence” (p. 1166). The authors (Levin, Whitener, &

Cross, 2006) used three items (Table 3.5) to measure perceived trustworthiness between

employees. Collectively, the three items had a Cronbach’s alpha of .84 (Levin, Whitener,

& Cross, 2006).

Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, and Takenouchi’s (1996) work on trust, in strategic alliances

between US and Japanese firms, also recognized benevolence-based trust, by dividing

trust into two dimensions: credibility and benevolence. The authors (Johnson et al., 1996)

operationalized the benevolence dimension by using four items (Table 3.5) adapted from

the work of Ganesan (1994). Factor and reliability analyses were not presented for this

specific dimension, but the authors did get Cronbach’s alphas of .94 for the overall

measure of trust in US firms, and .92 in their Japanese partners (Johnson et al., 1996).

In this study, benevolence-based trust was measured using a nine-item scale (Table 3.5).

Items were: “This person is very concerned about my welfare”; “My needs and desires

are very important to this person”; “This person would not knowingly do anything to hurt

me”; “This person really looks out for what is important to me”; “This person will go out

of his or her way to help me”; “This person would always look out for my interests”;

“This person would go out of his or her way to make sure I am not damaged or harmed in

this relationship”; “I feel like this person cares what happens to me”; and, “I feel like this

person is on my side”. All benevolence-based trust items were measured on a five-point

scale (1 – Strongly Disagree, 5 – Strongly Agree).

Page 83: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  71  

Author(s) Factor Study Population

Authors’ Item Adapted Item (final question items in bold)

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Benevolence Top Management & Employees

Top management is very concerned about my welfare.

This person is very concerned about my welfare.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Benevolence Top Management & Employees

My needs and desires are very important to top management.

My needs and desires are very important to this person.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Benevolence Top Management & Employees

Top management would not knowingly do anything to hurt me.

This person would not knowingly do anything to hurt me.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Benevolence Top Management & Employees

Top management really looks out for what is important to me.

This person really looks out for what is important to me.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Benevolence Top Management & Employees

Top management will go out of its way to help me.

This person will go out of his or her way to help me.

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Perceived Trustworthiness

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to seeking information / advice from this person on this project, I assumed that he or she would always look out for my interests.

This person would always look out for my interests.

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Perceived Trustworthiness

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to seeking information / advice from this person on this project, I assumed that he or she would go out of his or her way to make sure I was not damaged or harmed.

This person would go out of his or her way to make sure I am not damaged or harmed in this relationship.

Levin, Whitener, and Cross, 2006

Perceived Trustworthiness

Knowledge workers on a project

Prior to seeking information / advice from this person on this project, I felt like he or she cared what happened to me.

I feel like this person cares what happens to me.

Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, and Takenouchi, 1996

Benevolence Dimension of Trust

US and Japanese strategic business partners

Our Japanese/US partner would go out of its way to make sure our firm is not damaged or harmed in this relationship.

This person would go out of his or her way to make sure I am not damaged or harmed in this relationship. *Repeated item

Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, and Takenouchi, 1996

Benevolence Dimension of Trust

US and Japanese strategic business partners

In this relationship, we feel like our Japanese/US partner cares what happens to us.

I feel like this person cares what happens to me. *Repeated item

Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, and Takenouchi, 1996

Benevolence Dimension of Trust

US and Japanese strategic business partners

Our Japanese/US partner always looks out for our interests in this alliance.

This person would always look out for my interests. *Repeated item

Page 84: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  72  

Johnson, Cullen, Sakano, and Takenouchi, 1996

Benevolence Dimension of Trust

US and Japanese strategic business partners

We feel like our Japanese/US partner is on our side.

I feel like this person is on my side.

Table 3.5 Operationalization and Measurement of the Benevolence-Based Trust Integrity-Based Trust

Based on the work of Mayer and Davis (1999) and Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis

(1996a) six items (Table 3.6) were adapted to measure perceived integrity-based trust in

a co-worker. Mayer and Davis (1999) operationalized integrity-based trust into six items

(Table 3.6), using slightly altered measures, originally developed by Schoorman, Mayer,

and Davis (1996a). The authors’ (Mayer & Davis, 1999) items intend to measure justice,

honesty, fairness, and consistency. The items are also used to gauge adherence to the

general perceived values or principles of the trustee. The six items used by Mayer and

Davis (1999) were found to have Cronbach’s alphas of .82 for the first wave of the study

and .88 for the second wave.

In this study, integrity-based trust was measured using a similar six-item scale to that of

the one used by Mayer and Davis (1999) (Table 3.5). Items were: “This person has a

strong sense of justice”; “I never have to wonder whether this person will stick to his/her

word”; “This person tries hard to be fair in dealings with others”; “This person’s actions

and behaviors are not very consistent” (reverse coded); “I like this person’s values”; and

“Sound principles seem to guide this person’s behavior”. All integrity-based trust items

were measured on a five-point scale (1 – Strongly Disagree, 5 – Strongly Agree).

Page 85: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  73  

Author(s) Factor Study Population

Authors’ Item Adapted Item (final question items in bold)  

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Integrity Top Management & Employees

Top management has a strong sense of justice.

This person has a strong sense of justice.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Integrity Top Management & Employees

I never have to wonder whether top management will stick to its word.

I never have to wonder whether this person will stick to his/her word.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Integrity Top Management & Employees

Top management tries hard to be fair in dealings with others.

This person tries hard to be fair in dealings with others.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Integrity Top Management & Employees

Top management’s actions and behaviors are not very consistent. (reverse coded)

This person’s actions and behaviors are not very consistent. (reverse coded)

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Integrity Top Management & Employees

I like top management’s values. I like this person’s values.

Mayer and Davis, 1999

Integrity Top Management & Employees

Sound principles seem to guide top management’s behavior.

Sound principles seem to guide this person’s behavior.

Table 3.6 Operationalization and Measurement of the Integrity-Based Trust

Propensity to Trust

Based on the work of Mayer and Davis (1999) and Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis

(1996a) eight items (Table 3.7) were adapted to measure the respondent’s propensity to

trust. Mayer and Davis (1999) operationalized propensity to trust, using the same eight

items used by Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis (1996a) in their study of trust and the

delegation of risky tasks by veterinarians to their staff (Table 3.7). The authors’ (Mayer

& Davis, 1999) items were found to have Cronbach’s alphas of .55 for the first wave of

the study and .66 for the second wave. Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis’s (1996a) earlier

work yielded a slightly higher Cronbach’s alpha of .71.

In this study, propensity to trust was measured using a similar eight-item scale (Table

3.7) to the one used by Mayer and Davis (1999) and Schoorman, Mayer, and Davis

(1996a). Items were: “One should be very cautious with strangers”; “Most experts tell the

truth about the limits of their knowledge”; “Most people can be counted on to do what

they say they will do”; “These days, you must be alert or someone is likely to take

advantage of you”; “Most salespeople are honest in describing their products”; “Most

Page 86: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  74  

repair people will not overcharge people who are ignorant of their specialty”; “Most

people answer public opinion polls honestly”; and “Most adults are competent at their

jobs”. All propensity to trust items were measured on a five-point scale (1 – Strongly

Disagree, 5 – Strongly Agree).

Author(s) Factor Study Population Authors’ Item Adapted Item (final question items in

bold)  

Mayer and Davis, 1999 Schoorman, Mayer and Davis, 1996a

Propensity to Trust

Top Management & Employees Veterinarians & hospital staff

One should be very cautious with strangers.

One should be very cautious with strangers.

Mayer and Davis, 1999 Schoorman, Mayer and Davis, 1996a

Propensity to Trust

Top Management & Employees Veterinarians & hospital staff

Most experts tell the truth about the limits of their knowledge.

Most experts tell the truth about the limits of their knowledge.

Mayer and Davis, 1999 Schoorman, Mayer and Davis, 1996a

Propensity to Trust

Top Management & Employees Veterinarians & hospital staff

Most people can be counted on to do what they say they will do.

Most people can be counted on to do what they say they will do.

Mayer and Davis, 1999 Schoorman, Mayer and Davis, 1996a

Propensity to Trust

Top Management & Employees Veterinarians & hospital staff

These days, you must be alert or someone is likely to take advantage of you.

These days, you must be alert or someone is likely to take advantage of you.

Mayer and Davis, 1999 Schoorman, Mayer and Davis, 1996a

Propensity to Trust

Top Management & Employees Veterinarians & hospital staff

Most salespeople are honest in describing their products.

Most salespeople are honest in describing their products.

Mayer and Davis, 1999 Schoorman, Mayer and Davis, 1996a

Propensity to Trust

Top Management & Employees Veterinarians & hospital staff

Most repair people will not overcharge people who are ignorant of their specialty.

Most repair people will not overcharge people who are ignorant of their specialty.

Mayer and Davis, 1999 Schoorman, Mayer and Davis, 1996a

Propensity to Trust

Top Management & Employees Veterinarians & hospital staff

Most people answer public opinion polls honestly.

Most people answer public opinion polls honestly.

Mayer and Davis, 1999 Schoorman, Mayer and Davis, 1996a

Propensity to Trust

Top Management & Employees Veterinarians & hospital staff

Most adults are competent at their jobs.

Most adults are competent at their jobs.

Table 3.7 Operationalization and Measurement of the Propensity to Trust

3.2.3 Knowledge Sharing Behaviors (Dependent Variables)

As discussed in the previous chapter, for the present research, three knowledge sharing

conditions were adopted to collectively understand knowledge sharing behavior. These

Page 87: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  75  

conditions were: the knowledge source must be willing to share the knowledge they

posses; the knowledge receiver must be willing to receive and use the knowledge that is

shared; and the knowledge receiver must perceive the knowledge shared as being useful

to their individual work, the project, or the firm. Even though the present study refers to

these conditions as knowledge sharing behaviors, as previously mentioned, in a pure

sense these conditions do not represent actual employee behaviors, rather self-reported

behavioral precursors (i.e. willingness or intentions) and post-behavioral outcomes (i.e.

perceived usefulness). It is believed that this approach would be appropriate in

understanding knowledge sharing behaviors, as these types of measures have often been

used as close surrogates to indicate or measure behavior (e.g. the technology acceptance

model (TAM), interpersonal trust measures, etc.). In addition, the actual knowledge

sharing behavior could not be captured, as there was no practical way of measuring actual

employee behavior.

Based on this perspective, three variables were introduced to measure knowledge sharing

behavior in this study: willingness to share knowledge; willingness to use knowledge; and

perceived receipt of useful knowledge. The operationalization of each of these three

knowledge sharing behavior variables is discussed below.

Willingness to Share Knowledge and Willingness to Use Knowledge

As part of his doctoral dissertation on knowledge sharing and trust, Holste (2003)

operationalized knowledge sharing into four categories of measures, based on direction

and knowledge type (i.e. sharing explicit knowledge, sharing tacit knowledge, using

explicit knowledge, and using tacit knowledge). Holste (2003) created sixteen items (four

for each category; see Table 3.8), using examples of explicit and tacit knowledge34

identified in his literature review. “Each statement simply asks the respondent to indicate

his [or her] willingness to share or use a specific example of explicit or tacit knowledge

identified by experts in the literature” (Holste, 2003, p. 75). Holste (2003) calculated the

score for each of his four knowledge-sharing dependent variables by “determining the

                                                                                                               34 A complete list of Holte’s descriptions and examples of explicit and tacit knowledge can be found in Table 3 of his work (Holste, 2003, pp. 23-25).

Page 88: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  76  

mean of his/her responses to the items associated with each dependent variable” (p. 77).

Since the sixteen items were unique to Holste’s (2003) study, he also conducted factor

analysis, to determine if he was measuring four distinct variables. Each of the factors had

clean loadings, and reliability analysis of the four variables and two combined knowledge

measures yielded high Cronbach’s alphas (Table 3.9).

In this study, willingness to share knowledge and willingness to use knowledge were each

measured using five-item scales, similar to those used in Holste’s (2003) study (Table

3.8). The one notable difference is that the eight items Holste (2003) used to measure

willingness to share and use explicit knowledge were reduced to two items, because the

remaining examples of explicit knowledge were not relevant to this study (e.g. lectures,

databases, spreadsheets, etc.). Items for willingness to share knowledge were: “I would

take the initiative to provide this individual with useful tools I have developed (e.g.

precedents, memos, client information, industry information)”; “I would allow this

individual to spend significant time observing me in order for them to better understand

and learn from my work”; “I would willingly share with this person rules of thumb, tricks

of the trade, and other insights into the work of my office and that of the organization I

have learned”; “I would willingly share my new ideas with this individual”; and “I would

willingly share with this individual the latest organizational rumors, if significant”. Items

for willingness to use knowledge were: “I would eagerly receive and use tools developed

by this person including precedents, memos, client information, and industry

information”; “I would welcome the opportunity to spend significant time observing and

collaborating with this individual in order for me to better understand and learn from

their work”; “I would welcome and use any rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other

insights they have learned”; “I would eagerly receive and consider any new ideas this

individual might have”; and “I would tend to believe organizational rumors shared by

this individual and would use such knowledge as appropriate”. All willingness to share

knowledge and willingness to use knowledge items were measured on a five-point scale

(1 – Strongly Disagree, 5 – Strongly Agree).

Page 89: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  77  

Author(s) Factor Authors’ Item Adapted Item (final question items in bold)  

Holste (2003); adapted from Choo (2000)

Willingness to share explicit organizational knowledge

I would take the initiative to provide this individual with tools I have developed in connection with my work that I believe would be useful to him/her.

I would take the initiative to provide this individual with useful tools I have developed (e.g. precedents, memos, client information, industry information).

Holste (2003); adapted from Haldin-Herrgard, 2000

Willingness to share explicit organizational knowledge

I would take the initiative to provide this individual with lectures/presentations I have prepared that I believe would be useful to him/her.

*Item not used in the present study

Holste (2003); adapted from Choo, 2000; Clarke & Rollo, 2001; Epstein, 2000

Willingness to share explicit organizational knowledge

Assuming I had permission to do so, I would take the initiative to provide this individual with data/databases/spreadsheets I am maintaining that I believe would be useful to him/her.

*Item not used in the present study

Holste (2003); adapted from Smith, 2001; Wong & Radcliffe, 2000

Willingness to share explicit organizational knowledge

Assuming I had permission to do so, I would take the initiative to provide this individual with printed or electronic copies of documents and/or manuals I have produced that I believe would be useful to him/her.

*Item not used in the present study

Holste (2003); adapted from Choo, 2000 Haldin-Herrgard, 2000

Willingness to use explicit organizational knowledge

I would eagerly receive and use tools developed by this person, if relevant to my work.

I would eagerly receive and use tools developed by this person including precedents, memos, client information, and industry information.

Holste (2003); adapted from Haldin-Herrgard, 2000

Willingness to use explicit organizational knowledge

I would eagerly receive and use lectures/presentations prepared by this person, if relevant to my work.

*Item not used in the present study

Holste (2003); adapted from Choo, 2000; Clarke & Rollo, 2001; Epstein, 2000

Willingness to use explicit organizational knowledge

I would eagerly receive and use data/databases/spreadsheets developed by this person, if relevant to my work.

*Item not used in the present study

Holste (2003); adapted from Smith, 2001; Wong & Radcliffe, 2000

Willingness to use explicit organizational knowledge

I would eagerly receive and use printed or electronic copies of documents and/or manuals produced by this person, if relevant to my work.

*Item not used in the present study

Page 90: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  78  

Holste (2003); adapted from Choo, 2000; Clarke & Rollo, 2001; Davenport & Grover, 2001; Scott, 2000

Willingness to share tacit organizational knowledge

If requested to do so, I would allow this individual to spend significant time observing and collaborating with me in order for him/her to better understand and learn from my work.

I would allow this individual to spend significant time observing me in order for them to better understand and learn from my work.

Holste (2003); adapted from Haldin-Herrgard, 2000; Wong & Radcliffe, 2000

Willingness to share tacit organizational knowledge

I would willingly share with this person rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other insights into the work of my office and that of the organization I have learned.

I would willingly share with this person rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other insights into the work of my office and that of the organization I have learned.

Holste (2003); adapted from Epstein, 2000

Willingness to share tacit organizational knowledge

I would willingly share my new ideas with this individual.

I would willingly share my new ideas with this individual.

Holste (2003); adapted from Epstein, 2000

Willingness to share tacit organizational knowledge

I would willingly share with this individual the latest organizational rumors, if significant.

I would willingly share with this individual the latest organizational rumors, if significant.

Holste (2003); adapted from Choo, 2000; Clarke & Rollo, 2001; Davenport & Grover, 2001; Scott, 2000

Willingness to use tacit organizational knowledge

If relevant to my work, I would welcome the opportunity to spend significant time observing and collaborating with this individual in order for me to better understand and learn from his/her work.

I would welcome the opportunity to spend significant time observing and collaborating with this individual in order for me to better understand and learn from their work.

Holste (2003); adapted from Haldin-Herrgard, 2000; Wong & Radcliffe, 2000

Willingness to use tacit organizational knowledge

If relevant to my work, I would welcome and use any rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other insights he/she has learned.

I would welcome and use any rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other insights they have learned.

Holste (2003); adapted from Epstein, 2000

Willingness to use tacit organizational knowledge

I would eagerly receive and consider any new ideas this individual might have.

I would eagerly receive and consider any new ideas this individual might have.

Holste (2003); adapted from Epstein, 2000

Willingness to use tacit organizational knowledge

I would tend to believe organizational rumors shared by this individual and would use such knowledge as appropriate.

I would tend to believe organizational rumors shared by this individual and would use such knowledge as appropriate.

Table 3.8 Operationalization and Measurement of Willingness to Share and Use Knowledge

Page 91: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  79  

Holste’s Knowledge Measure Cronbach’s Alpha

Explicit Knowledge Sharing .90 Explicit Knowledge Use .94 Tacit Knowledge Sharing .85 Tacit Knowledge Use .84 Combined Knowledge Sharing .95 Combined Knowledge Use .94

Table 3.9 Reliability Results for Measures of Holste’s (2003) Knowledge Variables Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The previous measures for willingness to share knowledge and willingness to use

knowledge only consider characteristics of the exchange itself, and do not address the

effectiveness of the shared knowledge. To be considered successful, the result of the

interaction must have a positive impact on the individuals involved in the exchange, the

project, or the organization. In other words, the knowledge shared must be useful or have

utility.

In their work on weak ties, trust, and knowledge transfer, Levin and Cross (2004) adapted

a measure to capture this type of utility for project-related work, which they called

perceived receipt of useful knowledge. They operationalized this variable by creating

eight unique items (Table 3.10) adapted from four organizational knowledge sharing

research studies35. “These eight items asked to what extent the knowledge received from

each person hurt or helped key aspects of the project’s outcomes” (Levin & Cross, 2004,

p. 1482). Reliability analysis for these eight measures (receipt of useful knowledge)

produced a Cronbach’s alpha of .93 (Levin & Cross, 2004).

In this study, perceived receipt of useful knowledge was measured using a five-item scale

similar to the one used by Levin and Cross (2004) (Table 3.10). The one notable

difference is the exclusion of three items used by Levin and Cross (2004), which

specifically related to the overall project budget, overall project time, and overall project

cost. For most respondents, this type of information may be unknown. Therefore, these

three items used by Levin and Cross (2004) were reduced and adapted into one item,

                                                                                                               35 Item 1 is adapted from Szulanski (1996); 2-6, Keller (1994); 7, Haas and Hansen (2007); 8, Hansen (1999).

Page 92: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  80  

asking respondents to reflect on the cost and time it took them to complete the part of the

project they were responsible for.

Items for perceived receipt of useful knowledge were: “The information I received from

each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to: client

satisfaction with the project; this projects quality; the project teams overall performance;

the overall success of FIRM NAME; the cost and/or time it took to complete the portion

of the project I am responsible for; [and] my individual performance on the project.” All

perceived receipt of useful knowledge items were measured on a five-point scale (1 –

Very negative, 5 – Very positive).

Page 93: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  81  

Author(s) Factor Authors’ Item Adapted Item (final question items in bold)  

Levin and Cross, 2004; adapted from Szulanski, 1996

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The information/advice I received from this person made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to client satisfaction with this project.

The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to client satisfaction with the project.

Levin and Cross, 2004; adapted from Keller, 1994

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The information/advice I received from this person made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to this project's quality.

The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to this project’s quality.

Levin and Cross, 2004; adapted from Keller, 1994

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The information/advice I received from this person made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to this project team's overall performance.

The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to the project team’s overall performance.

Levin and Cross, 2004; adapted from Keller, 1994

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The information/advice I received from this person made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to my organization.

The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to the overall success of FIRM NAME.

Levin and Cross, 2004; adapted from Keller, 1994

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The information/advice I received from this person made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to this project's coming in on budget or closer to coming in on budget.

The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to the cost and/or time it took to complete the portion of the project I am responsible for.

Levin and Cross, 2004; adapted from Keller, 1994

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The information/advice I received from this person made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to reducing costs on this project.

The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to the cost and/or time it took to complete the portion of the project I am responsible for. *Repeated item

Levin and Cross, 2004; adapted from Haas and Hansen, 2007

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The information/advice I received from this person made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to my being able to spend less time on this project.

The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to my individual performance on the project.

Levin and Cross, 2004; adapted from Hansen, 1999

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

The information/advice I received from this person made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to shortening the time this project took.

The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to the cost and/or time it took to complete the portion of the project I am responsible for. *Repeated item

Page 94: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  82  

(1=Contributed very negatively; 2=Contributed negatively; 3= Contributed somewhat negatively; 4=Contributed neither positively nor negatively; 5=Contributed somewhat positively; 6=Contributed positively; 7=Contributed very positively)

Table 3.10 Operationalization and Measurement of Levin and Cross’ (2004) Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge Research Variable

3.3 Nature of Co-worker Relationship

In this study, the complete survey was divided into three sections: an individual section,

asking the respondents to answer questions about themselves and their background; a co-

worker section, asking the respondents to answer questions about a positive referent (i.e.

someone they worked best with, on a project they worked on recently); and finally

another co-worker section, asking respondents to answer questions about a negative

referent (i.e. someone they did not work well with, on a project they worked together on

recently). This approach of getting respondents to comment on both a positive and

negative referent was based on similar distinctions made in previous research studies

(McAllister, 1995; Tsui, 1984, 1986; Holste, 2003). This approach was also motivated by

a conceptual distinction in the types of relationships that occur within these settings. For

example, one of the interesting features of knowledge-intensive organizations is that their

employees rarely have free choice in deciding whom they work with, and whom they are

required to share knowledge with. To achieve project objectives, in most instances,

employees are required to share knowledge with both individuals they work well with

and those who they do not work well with.

3.4 Reliability and Validity

The validity of the study primarily relied on construct validity, since all adapted scales

and items had been previously used in empirical organizational studies that confirmed

their operationalizations through factor and/or reliability analysis. An indicator of

reliability (i.e. Cronbach’s alpha) was used for each of the original measures above and it

exceeded the minimal acceptable range of 0.65–0.70 (DeVellis, 1991, p. 85). The present

study aimed to confirm the operationalization of each of the measures, using exploratory

factor analysis and reliability analysis (i.e. Cronbach’s alpha). Any weak-loading, or

incorrect loading, item discovered using factor analysis was excluded from further

analysis. In addition, any measure that did not exceed the minimal acceptable Cronbach’s

alpha range (DeVellis, 1991) was also excluded from further analysis. This method

Page 95: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  83  

assured the reliability and validity of measures and provided construct validity for future

studies.

3.5 Data Analysis Strategy

Each of the hypotheses identified above was tested using statistical techniques. Initially,

correlation analysis was used to measure bivariate relationships between independent and

dependent variables, and then t-tests were used to test differences between the means of

variables (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). To measure correlation between variables, the

Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient (r) was used. Multiple regression

analysis was performed to examine relationships between dependent variables and the

focal independent variable, while controlling for all the other variables in the model. The

standardized regression coefficients, or βetas (β), represented the amount of net change

that occurred in the dependent variable(s) for an independent variable change of one

standard deviation (Bohrnstedt & Knoke, 1994, p. 519). A t-test was conducted to assess

whether the βeta coefficients were significantly different than zero. Significance was

tested for the regression coefficient, as well as for the overall equation (p < .05). Finally,

an F-test was done to “assess whether the proportion of variance of the dependent

variable that [was] accounted for by the regression on the independent variables [was]

statistically significant” (Chan, 2002, p. 117). If the results of the regression analysis

contradicted the results of the correlation analysis, or t-tests, the results of the regression

analysis were given priority and accepted, as multiple-regression analysis controlled for

all the variables in the conceptual framework of the study, and therefore represented the

more stringent analytical framework.

To test for the mediating effect of trust, hierarchical multiple regression analysis was

used, in accordance with a testing method suggested by Baron and Kenny (1986),

whereby four conditions were met. As first step, the direct effect of each social-cognitive

factor was tested against the mediating variable (overall trust) and each of the dependent

variables (knowledge sharing behaviors). To qualify for inclusion, the social-cognitive

factor needed to have a significant relationship with overall trust and at least one

knowledge sharing behavior. Next, hierarchical multiple regression equations were run,

Page 96: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  84  

using a two-step model (i.e. one with and one without the mediating variable), to

determine whether the relationship between the social-cognitive factor and knowledge

sharing behavior was reduced, when overall trust was introduced. If, after introducing

overall trust, the original path between the social-cognitive factor and knowledge sharing

behavior became insignificant (i.e. complete mediating effect), or was reduced (i.e.

partial mediating effect), it could be concluded that overall trust had a mediating effect.

3.6 Selection of Study Population

Professional service firms (PSFs) rely greatly on their employees’ knowledge for

developing solutions to difficult and complex problems (Hitt, Bierman, Shimizu, &

Kochhar, 2001; Criscuolo, Salter, & Ter Wal, 2010). Developing solutions, however,

depends on individual knowledge and on successfully sharing knowledge with co-

workers and applying it effectively (Cross & Sproull, 2004).

The legal profession is one of several situated in PSFs. Other situated professions include

accountancy, medicine, architecture, and engineering (Clegg & Bailey, 2008). The legal

profession is knowledge-based, as “in its core, [it] is about providing specialized

knowledge and services in a variety of ways to a variety of clients” (du Plessis & du Toit,

2006, p. 360). Effective knowledge sharing practices are perhaps the most important

assets of PSFs.

The firm selected for this study is one of Canada's preeminent and largest law firms, with

offices in six cities across Canada, making it an excellent example of a large PSF. The

firm has implemented an “Excellent Knowledge Management” initiative, aimed at

leveraging and sharing the intellectual assets of the firm, to better serve their clients. As

explained by one of the senior partners, the philosophy of the firm is “thought leadership,

knowledge, and collaboration [to achieve] service excellence. […] our firm aims to build,

manage, and share our specialized knowledge and broad expertise” (Evans, personal

communication, October, 2010).

Page 97: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  85  

The firm manages a large number of project teams, whose composition consists of

knowledge workers working on projects (i.e. legal matters). The nature of these projects

allow survey respondents to objectively evaluate the project’s outcomes, giving a better

sense of the resulting effects of knowledge shared, as well as a better understanding of

knowledge sharing behavior

The selection of legal professionals to participate in the study flowed naturally from the

selection of the firm, as they are the heart of the firm and are the key individuals who

provide knowledge-based services to their clients. Another important reason was because

of the nature of their work, which is primarily knowledge-intensive and, more

importantly, relies heavily on the co-workers’ knowledge sharing and use behaviors. As

such, the firm provides an ideal environment for studying knowledge sharing behavior

between knowledge workers.

3.7 Data Collection

A senior partner at the national multijurisdictional law firm was contacted to discuss the

opportunity of conducting this study. The objectives of the study were explained to senior

partners and the Director of Knowledge Management at the firm, after which the firm

agreed to participate. Before launching the survey at the study site, a pre-test was

conducted with twenty knowledge workers, who were not affiliated with the firm. The

purpose of the pre-test was to analyze the survey instrument and ensure that respondents

understood and could answer the questions. The knowledge workers made several

valuable suggestions, with respect to delivery method, survey instructions, and wording

of questions. The updated survey was then published on the web, using a private web-

based academic survey tool called Qualtrics. Once the survey was online, it was pre-

tested again with ten additional knowledge workers, who were not affiliated with the

firm, and with three senior executives working at the firm. The purpose of the second

pre-test was to evaluate the delivery mechanism, time the survey, and re-evaluate the

wording of the questions. The second pre-test group reported no issues with the delivery

mechanism and an approximate time of 15-20 minutes per survey, which was deemed

appropriate. However, based on subsequent interviews with the three senior executives

Page 98: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  86  

involved in the pre-test, some questions were further re-worded, to match the vernacular

of the firm.

After all the changes were in place, a senior partner at the firm sent a firm-wide email

(Appendix A.3), asking all ‘legal professionals’ and paralegals/law clerks in six

nationally distributed offices to participate in the survey (approximately 900 employees

in total). All the employees contacted were knowledge workers engaged in knowledge-

intensive legal project work, the nature of which required a significant reliance on others,

for both explicit and tacit forms of knowledge. The survey was active from January 26th,

2011 to February 25th, 2011 (31 days,) and respondents could participate at anytime

between those dates. In addition, the senior partner sent out two reminder emails during

the active survey period. The first email was sent two weeks after the survey was

launched and the second email was sent three days before the closing date. Respondents

who completed the survey were rewarded for participation with a $5 gift card to a

popular coffee shop.

Once the data were analyzed, the site (firm) was visited to conduct 90-120 minute un-

structured interviews with senior partners and the Director of Knowledge Management,

to gauge whether the survey captured a representative sample of the firm, and to further

understand and interpret the unique significant relationships found during analysis. In

several cases, unique relationships found between variables were due to the specific

industry or firm characteristics, which these interviews helped understand and shape. The

results of the analysis are presented in the next chapter.

Page 99: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  87  

Chapter 4: Results 4.0 Chapter Overview

This chapter begins by presenting information about the size of the study and its

respondents, describing the organization as a whole. Next, it will discuss how measures

were analyzed using factor and reliability analysis. Descriptive statistics are presented for

each variable. The chapter then focuses on the testing of the hypotheses using correlation

and regression analysis. The chapter closes with summaries of the research questions and

whether the hypotheses were upheld.

4.1 Survey Sample Size

As discussed in the previous chapter, adequate sample size (N) was determined using

power analysis. Based on Cohen and Cohen’s (1983) power of significance test analysis

table (Table F, p. 528), to attain a statistical power of .80 with a population effect size of r

= .30, the study needed to have at least 84 respondents. Tabachnick and Fidell’s (2007, p.

123) more stringent sample size heuristic (N ≥ 50 + 8(number of independent variables))

required at least 146 respondents36. The study sample size of 275 well exceeded both

heuristics.

4.2 Survey Respondents

Approximately 900 invitations were sent to all ‘legal professionals’ and paralegals/law

clerks working at one of Canada’s largest multijurisdictional law firms. Of the 900

invitations, 775 were distributed to ‘legal professionals’, of whom 735 were lawyers, 30

trademark or patent agents, 5 or 6 accountants, and 5 or 6 (GR) governmental

professional. Approximately 37% of all the legal professionals were associates and 63%

were partners. In addition, 120 invitations were sent to all ‘paralegals’ and ‘law clerks’.

No administrative staff were distributed invitations to participate.

                                                                                                               36 146 respondents were based on the 12 independent variables used in the proceeding hypothesis testing, including demographic variables. Variables included are: Age, Age Difference, Gender, Gender Gap, Education, Educational Gap, Shared Language, Shared Vision, Tie Strength (Prior to), Tie Strength (While on), Relationship Length, and Overall Trust.

Page 100: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  88  

A total of 275 completed questionnaires were received from the population of 900

eligible knowledge professionals who had worked on a project or matter, for a response

rate of 30.6%. A breakdown of respondent by role appears in Table 4.1.

Role N % of Sample

Clerk / Paralegal 89 32.36% Articling Student 4 1.45% Associate 91 33.09% Partner 88 32.00% No Answer 3 1.09%

Table 4.1 Respondents by Role

Four respondents identified themselves as ’students‘, because at the time of the survey

they were officially designated as such by the firm. However, according to an interview

with one of the senior partners, these respondents should be considered associate lawyers,

since this was their role on projects.

4.2.1 Respondent Profile by Role and Department

Approximately 24% of all ‘legal professionals’ completed the questionnaire (i.e. 183 of

the 775 invited to participate). Compared to the actual firm distribution, partners were

underrepresented (i.e. 88 of 183 or 48.1%) and associates overrepresented (i.e. 95 of 183

or 51.9%). This should not be surprising, since partners tended to be busier and less

motivated to complete this type of questionnaire. One senior partner explained this, in an

interview, by saying that “an associate who sees an email from a partner saying fill this

out is more likely to do it than a partner who sees an email from a partner saying please

fill this out” (Evans, personal communication, August, 2011). For similar reasons, legal

clerks and paralegals were highly overrepresented in the survey, with a response

percentage of 74.2% (i.e. 89 of 120 invited to participate completed the survey). There

were no anticipated implications of the overrepresentation of law clerks/paralegals and

the underrepresentation of partners, as the intention of this study was to capture the

general knowledge sharing behavior of knowledge workers on projects, regardless of

their role within the firm. However, this distinction may be made in future research.

The professional law services of the firm were separated into two departments: business

and litigation. The actual firm distribution, at the time of the survey, was about 55% in

Page 101: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  89  

the business services department and 45% in the litigation services department. This was

similar to the distribution found in the survey respondents (i.e. business department was

57% and litigation department was 42%).

Role Business Litigation

Clerk / Paralegal 49 40 Articling Student 4 0 Associate 50 40 Partner 54 34

Table 4.2 Respondents by Role within the Firm by Department Table 4.2 shows the role within the firm by department. According to a senior partner at

the firm, clerks/paralegals and associates were accurately represented in the survey

respondents, for both departments. On the other hand, litigation partners were

underrepresented in the survey respondents. One senior partner explained this by

reasoning that litigators spend more time out of the office, on depositions and motions,

mediations, and trials. Corporate (i.e. business) lawyers tend to spend more time in the

office, which gives them more opportunities to complete the questionnaire.

4.2.2 Respondent Profile by Gender

According to one senior manager, when compared to the actual distribution in the firm,

women were overrepresented in the survey respondents (i.e. women accounted for 62%

of all respondents). In actuality, the distribution of associates was approximately 55%

men and 45% women and the distribution of partners was approximately 70% men and

30% women.

Based on the actual firm distribution, associate women were slightly overrepresented in

the survey respondents (i.e. 53% as opposed to 45%). Women partners were also

overrepresented, as 41% of all partners who completed the survey were women (as

opposed to 30%). However, the higher number of women respondents who were

paralegals or clerks could explain the generally larger percentage of women in the survey

respondents. According to a senior partner at the firm, the position of clerk/paralegal was

predominantly held by women, who outnumbered men in the same role by 10-1, both in

the firm and in the survey respondents (i.e. men ~3% / women ~30%).

Page 102: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  90  

4.2.3 Respondent Profile by Birth, Country, Citizenship, and Ethnicity

Almost 85% of those who completed the survey were born in Canada (233 of 275) with

3% from the US (8 of 275) and 3% from the UK (7 of 275). Approximately 9% (25 of

275) were born in other countries and less than 1% (2 of 275) did not answer. More than

98% (270 of 275) of respondents had Canadian citizenship (1.5% other; >.4% no

answer). Almost 87% (239 of 275) of respondents identified themselves as, at least

partially, Caucasian (Chinese = ~4%; South Asian = ~3%; Arab = ~2%; Prefer not to say

= ~2%)37

According to senior executives at the firm, the survey respondents accurately represented

the birth country, citizenship, and ethnic distributions present in those invited to

participate.

4.2.4 Respondent Profile by Education and Age

Of all the respondents that completed the survey, more than half (139 of 275) achieved

some sort of professional degree (e.g. JD/LLB, MD). In addition, 3% earned a doctoral

degree (8 of 275), 13% a Masters degree (36 of 275), 13% a 4-year degree (36 of 275)

and 17% of respondents (47 of 275) finished some college. Less than 3% only finished

high school and only one respondent did not provide their education. The age breakdown

of respondents is presented in Table 4.3. According to senior executives at the firm, the

survey respondents accurately represented the education and age distributions of those

invited to participate.

Age N %

21-30 54 19.64% 31-40 102 37.09% 41-50 68 24.73% 51-60 37 13.45% Over 61 9 3.27% No Answer 5 1.82%

Table 4.3 Respondents by Age

                                                                                                               37 The percentages are approximate, as a person may have identified themselves with more than one ethnicity/race

Page 103: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  91  

4.3 Analyses of Measures

4.3.1 Knowledge Sharing Behavior (KSB)

Factor Analysis

As discussed in the previous chapter, this study used three sets of measures for

knowledge sharing behavior, which included willingness to share knowledge, willingness

to use knowledge, and perceived receipt of useful knowledge.

Overall willingness to share knowledge and overall willingness to use knowledge were

each measured using five survey items. Four of the five items, in each group, were

intended to measure tacit knowledge and one item measured explicit knowledge. Overall

willingness to share knowledge (WSO) was separated into willingness to share explicit

knowledge (WSE) and willingness to share tacit knowledge (WST). Similarly, overall

willingness to use knowledge (WUO) was separated into willingness to use explicit

knowledge (WUE) and willingness to use tacit knowledge (WST). With perceived receipt

of useful knowledge (PRUK), a total of seven variables were used to measure knowledge

sharing behavior in the study. To analyze the variance in these knowledge sharing

behavior items, SPSS was used to perform an exploratory factor analysis (i.e. principal

components analysis with varimax rotation). Table 4.4 presents the results of the factor

analysis for Knowledge Sharing Behavior.

Positive Referents Negative Referents

Component Component Willingness to Share Knowledge 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 I would take the initiative to provide

this individual with useful tools I have developed (e.g. precedents, memos, client information, industry information).

0.83 0.842

2 I would allow this individual to spend significant time observing me in order for them to better understand and learn from my work.

0.783 0.87

3 I would willingly share with this person rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other insights into the work of my office and that of the organization I have learned.

0.842 0.902

Page 104: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  92  

4 I would willingly share my new ideas with this individual.

0.66 0.844

5 I would willingly share with this individual the latest organizational rumors, if significant.

0.218 0.485

Willingness to Use Knowledge 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 I would eagerly receive and use tools

developed by this person including precedents, memos, client information and industry information.

0.702 0.75

2 I would welcome the opportunity to spend significant time observing this individual in order for me to better understand and learn from their work.

0.836 0.784

3 I would welcome and use any rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other insights they have learned.

0.859 0.873

4 I would eagerly receive and consider any new ideas this individual might have.

0.754 0.827

5 I would tend to believe organizational rumors shared by this individual and would use such knowledge as appropriate.

0.704 0.673

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 1 2 3 1 2 3 The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contributions to:

1 Client satisfaction with the matter/project

0.818 0.856

2 The matter's/project's quality 0.838 0.882 3 The project team's overall

performance 0.868 0.876

4 The overall success of BLG 0.853 0.871 5 The cost and/or the time it took to

complete the portion of the matter/project I am responsible for

0.716 0.715

6 My individual performance on the matter/project

0.715 0.744

Table 4.4 Factor Analysis Results for Knowledge Sharing Behavior Based on Comrey and Lee (1992), a factor loading above 0.55 (30% overlapping

variance) was set as cutoff point for inclusion of an item in a factor. The results of the

varimax rotation (Table 4.4) suggested that, with both positive (Group 1) and negative

(Group 2) referents, factor analysis distinguished principal factors comprising measures

Page 105: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  93  

for overall willingness to share knowledge, overall willingness to use knowledge, and

perceived receipt of useful knowledge. However, within overall willingness to share

knowledge, the fifth survey item (i.e. “I would willingly share with this individual the

latest organizational rumors, if significant”) had ‘poor’ factor loadings in Group 1 and

‘fair’ factor loadings in Group 238. Since these loadings were lower than the set cutoff

point, the fifth item was excluded from all subsequent data analyses involving overall

willingness to share knowledge or willingness to share tacit knowledge.

Reliability Analysis

Reliability analysis was performed on the three combined dependent measures for

knowledge sharing behavior (i.e. overall willingness to share knowledge39, overall

willingness to use knowledge, and perceived receipt of useful knowledge). Additionally,

reliability analysis was performed for willingness to share tacit knowledge and

willingness to use tacit knowledge. As noted in Table 4.5, the values of Cronbach’s α

well exceeded the minimally acceptable range of 0.65–0.70 (DeVellis, 1991, p. 85), in

both study groups.

Group 1 Group 2 Reliability Statistics α N α N

Overall Willingness to Share (Excluding Item 5) 0.859 4 0.908 4 Willingness to Share (Tacit Only) 0.799 3 0.889 3 Overall Willingness to Use 0.877 5 0.878 5 Willingness to Use (Tacit Only) 0.857 4 0.869 4 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.917 6 0.917 6

Table 4.5 Reliability Results for Combined Dependent Variables Descriptive Statistics

As per the factor and reliability analysis, knowledge sharing behaviors were grouped into

three constructs: willingness to share knowledge, willingness to use knowledge, and

perceived receipt of useful knowledge. Willingness to share/use knowledge was further

divided by type of knowledge (i.e. explicit vs. tacit). Measures for overall willingness to

                                                                                                               38 One possible explanation as to why item five did not load with the others in WSO is because the idea of sharing “organizational rumors” might have been frowned upon within the organizational culture, and thus respondents did not want to claim they would willingly share any rumors, regardless of whether they were appropriate. 39 Excluding item five

Page 106: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  94  

share knowledge and overall willingness to use knowledge were calculated by adding

explicit and tacit knowledge together. Through interviews with senior partners in the

firm, it was discovered that, in the legal setting, explicit knowledge referred to

precedents, memos, client information, and industry information. Tacit knowledge

included rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, insights, new ideas, and, in some cases,

rumors. Perceived receipt of useful knowledge was a respondent’s assessment of the

usefulness of the knowledge received from their co-workers, on the individual, group,

and firm performance outcomes. Performance outcomes related to the individual, as well

as to client satisfaction, overall project quality, overall project team performance, and the

overall firm success.

Table 4.6 presents the descriptive statistics for the knowledge sharing behavior variables.

The results suggested that individuals within the firm exhibited significantly higher

overall knowledge sharing behavior toward positive referents (i.e. those individuals with

whom respondents felt they worked best). According to these descriptive statistics,

respondents were 21% (4.3854 vs. 3.4644) more willing to share knowledge and 25%

(4.247 vs. 3.2023) more willing to use knowledge with individuals they felt they worked

best with. Interestingly, respondents perceived the knowledge they received from positive

referents to be 28% more useful than knowledge received from negative referents (4.3954

vs. 3.1869).

N Minimum Maximum Mean Std.

Deviation Overall Willingness to Share Knowledge Group 1 264 3 5 4.3854 0.51303 Group 2 260 1 5 3.4644 0.97578 Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge Group 1 267 2 5 4.44 0.588 Group 2 262 1 5 3.58 1.1 Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge Group 1 264 3 5 4.3674 0.52654 Group 2 262 1 5 3.43 0.9985 Overall Willingness to Use Knowledge Group 1 264 1 5 4.247 0.62049 Group 2 260 1 5 3.2023 0.90907 Willingness to Use Explicit Knowledge Group 1 268 1 5 4.36 0.653 Group 2 266 1 5 3.41 1.082

Page 107: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  95  

Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge Group 1 267 1 5 4.2257 0.64516 Group 2 262 1 5 3.1613 0.93311 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge Group 1 263 3 5 4.3954 0.49739 Group 2 263 1 5 3.1869 0.83245

Table 4.6 Descriptive Statistics for Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables

4.3.2 Overall Trust

Factor Analysis

Based on Mayer et al.’s (1995; Mayer & Davis, 1999) construct of perceived

trustworthiness, the measure of overall trust (Otrust) was calculated by combining a

series of items designed to capture three distinct types of perceived trust: ability-based

trust (ABtrust), integrity-based trust (IBtrust)40, and benevolence-based trust (BBtrust).

To analyze the variance and test for three distinct measures of trust, SPSS was used to

perform an exploratory factor analysis (i.e. principal components analysis with varimax

rotation). Table 4.7 presents a summary of the factor analysis results for overall trust, by

type.

Positive Referents Negative Referents Component Component

Ability-Based Trust 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 I believed that this person

approached his or her job with professionalism and dedication.

0.7 0.532

2 Given his or her track record, I saw no reason to doubt this person’s competence and preparation.

0.757 0.735

3 This person is very capable of performing his/her job.

0.818 0.861

4 This person is known to be successful at the things he/she tries to do.

0.778 0.8

5 This person has much knowledge about the work that needs to be done.

0.805 0.843

6 I feel confident about this person’s skills.

0.824 0.88

                                                                                                               40 The procedure for reverse coding involved wording the first IBtrust item, such that high values of the theoretical construct were reflected by low scores on the item, while other IBtrust items were worded such that high values of the construct were reflected by high scores on the item. This was done to encourage respondents to pay attention to the questions. It should also be noted that all trust items were first transformed, so that they were all oriented in the same direction before they were averaged.

Page 108: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  96  

7 This person has specialized capabilities that can increase performance.

0.767 0.795

8 This person is well qualified. 0.841 0.872 9 I can rely on this person not to make

my job more difficult by careless work.

0.647 0.721

Benevolence-Based Trust 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 This person is very concerned about

my welfare. 0.821 0.844

2 My needs and desires are very important to this person.

0.819 0.851

3 This person would not knowingly do anything to hurt me.

0.589 0.777

4 This person really looks out for what is important to me.

0.812 0.846

5 This person would go out of his or her way to help me.

0.759 0.848

6 This person would go out of his or her way to make sure I am not damaged or harmed in this relationship.

0.781 0.81

7 I feel like this person cares what happens to me.

0.827 0.834

8 This person would always look out for my interests.

0.85 0.879

9 I feel like this person is on my side. 0.758 0.815 Integrity-Based Trust 1 2 3 1 2 3 1 This person has a strong sense of

justice. 0.516 0.579

2 I never have to wonder whether this person will stick to his/her word.

0.661 0.573

3 This person tries hard to be fair in dealings with others.

0.618 0.643

4 This person’s actions and behaviors are not very consistent. [reverse coded]

0.391 0.635

5 I like this person’s values. 0.63 0.644 6 Sound principles seem to guide this

person’s behavior. 0.656 0.661

Table 4.7 Factor Analysis Results for Trust

The results of the varimax rotation suggested that, in both groups, factor analysis

distinguished principal factors comprising measures for ability-based trust, benevolence-

based trust, and integrity-based trust. Notably, within Group 1, the first item for

integrity-based trust (i.e. This person has a strong sense of justice.) and the first item for

ability-based trust (i.e. I believed that this person approached his or her job with

Page 109: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  97  

professionalism and dedication.) had slightly lower factor loadings than the set cutoff of

.55 (i.e. .512, .532). Also, the fourth item for integrity-based trust (i.e. This person’s

actions and behaviors are not very consistent.) had a notably lower factor loading than

the set cutoff (i.e. 0.391). However, based on the fact that each of these items had a

strong factor loading in the corresponding group, and on factor loadings found in

previous research, a decision was made not to omit the three trust items from the study.

Reliability Analysis

Reliability analysis was performed on the independent measures used to calculate overall

trust in both groups (i.e. ABtrust, BBtrust, and IBtrust). Reliability analysis was also

performed for the combined measure of Otrust, which added the previous three together.

As noted in Table 4.8, the value of Cronbach’s α well exceeded the acceptable minimum,

for these measures of trust, in both study groups. Cronbach’s α for propensity to trust did

not meet the minimum acceptable range for analysis. Therefore, all propensity to trust

items were excluded from all further analyses. This was not expected to affect the results,

since the remaining measures fully captured trust in a co-worker, regardless of the

respondent’s propensity to trust.

Group 1 Group 2 Reliability Statistics α N α N

All Trust Items (Overall Trust) 0.943 24 0.935 24 Ability-Based Trust 0.932 9 0.927 9 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.943 9 0.956 9 Integrity-Based Trust 0.783 6 0.863 6

Table 4.8 Reliability Results for Trust Variables Descriptive Statistics

Table 4.9 presents descriptive statistics for the trust variables (i.e. Otrust, ABtrust,

BBtrust, and IBtrust). As expected, the results suggested that respondents exhibited

significantly higher trust towards positive referents versus negative ones; averaging 23%

(4.4696 vs. 3.4415) higher in ABtrust, 38.5% higher in BBtrust (4.0868 vs. 2.5115), and

32% (4.1812 vs. 2.841) higher in IBtrust. As a combined measure, Otrust towards

positive referents was 31% (4.2643 vs. 2.9388), higher than Otrust towards negative

ones.

Page 110: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  98  

N Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation Overall Trust Group 1 253 2.71 5 4.2643 0.47851 Group 2 248 1.17 4.5 2.9388 0.65614 Ability-Based Trust Group 1 269 2.78 5 4.4696 0.5228 Group 2 263 1.22 5 3.4415 0.86473 Benevolence-Based Trust Group 1 265 1.56 5 4.0868 0.65311 Group 2 260 1 4.67 2.5115 0.85928 Integrity-Based Trust Group 1 264 2.83 5 4.1812 0.52393 Group 2 262 1 5 2.841 0.79747

Table 4.9 Descriptive Statistics for Trust Variables

4.3.3 Shared Language and Shared Vision

Factor Analysis

The measure of shared language (SL) was calculated by combining three question items

designed by Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2006), to capture the extent to which the

knowledge sender and receiver were able to easily understand and communicate with

each other. The measure of shared vision (SV) was calculated by combining five items

(three designed by Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2006) and two by Tsai and Ghoshal

(1998)), designed to capture the extent to which the knowledge sender and receiver

shared common goals, concerns, and purpose.

To analyze the variance of the two measures, and to confirm that shared language and

shared vision measured distinctly different concepts, SPSS was used to perform an

exploratory factor analysis on the eight items (i.e. principal components analysis with

varimax rotation). Table 4.10 presents a summary of the factor analysis results for SL and

SV.

Page 111: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  99  

Positive Referents

Negative Referents

Component Component Shared Language 1 2 1 2 1 I could understand completely what

this person meant when he or she was talking.

0.844 0.845

2 I was familiar with the jargon/terminology that he or she used.

0.882 0.796

3 It felt like we could communicate on the same "wavelength”.

0.822 0.781

Shared Vision 1 2 1 2 1 I felt like this person and I were

working toward completely different goals. [reverse coded]

0.35 0.422

2 I assumed that this person and I cared about the same issues.

0.759 0.798

3 I believed that this person and I shared a commitment to a common purpose.

0.82 0.874

4 I believed that this person and I shared the same ambitions and vision.

0.862 0.861

5 I believed that this person and I shared enthusiasm about pursuing the collective goals and mission of the whole organization.

0.795 0.751

Table 4.10 Factor Analysis Results for Shared Language and Shared Vision

The results of the varimax rotation suggested that, in both groups, factor analysis

distinguished principal factors comprising measures for shared language and shared

vision. However, within shared vision, the first item (i.e. I felt like this person and I were

working toward completely different goals [reverse coded41]) had factor loadings of

lower than the set cutoff point, for both groups (i.e. .350 in Group 1 and .422 in Group

                                                                                                               41 The first item for shared vision was reverse coding, which involved wording the item such that high values of the theoretical construct were reflected by low scores on the item, while other Shared Vision items were worded such that high values of the construct were reflected by high scores on the item. This was done to encourage respondents to pay attention to the questions. It should also be noted that all Shared Vision items were first transformed, so that they were all oriented in the same direction before they were averaged.

Page 112: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  100  

2)42. Therefore, the item was excluded from all subsequent data analysis involving shared

vision.

Reliability Analysis

Reliability analysis was performed on the combined measures for shared language and

shared vision43. As noted in Table 4.11, the value of Cronbach’s α well exceed the

minimally acceptable range of 0.65–0.70 (DeVellis, 1991, p. 85), in both study groups.

Group 1 Group 2 Reliability Statistics α N α N

Shared Language 0.882 3 0.776 3 Shared Vision 0.887 4 0.862 4

Table 4.11 Reliability Results for Combined Shared Language and Shared Vision Variables

Descriptive Statistics

Table 4.12 presents descriptive statistics for the SL and SV variables. The results

suggested that respondents had significantly higher shared language and shared vision

with positive referents. Specifically, respondents averaged 24% higher in SL (i.e. means

of 4.2388 in Group 1 vs. 3.213 in Group 2) and 24% higher in SV (i.e. means of 4.1783 in

Group 1 vs. 3.166 in Group 2).

N Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation Shared Language Group 1 268 1 5 4.2388 0.57715 Group 2 266 1 5 3.213 0.82603 Shared Vision Group 1 265 2.5 5 4.1783 0.53747 Group 2 262 1 5 3.166 0.83712

Table 4.12 Descriptive Statistics for Shared Language and Shared Vision Variables

                                                                                                               42 One possible explanation as to why item one did not load with the others for SV, was because respondents could not think of a situation where they would be asked to work towards different goals. Or they might never have been put in a situation to work towards different goals. Item one not loading might have also been a factor of the reverse coding. 43 Excluding item one

Page 113: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  101  

4.3.4 Relationship Length and Tie Strength

Factor Analysis

Based on the works of Levin, Whitener, and Cross (2004) and Dirks and Ferrin (2002),

the measure of relationship length (RL) was captured using a single survey item:

“Approximately, how long have you known each of the two co-workers you selected?”

Tie strength was measured by combining four question items influenced by the works of

Levin and Cross (2004) and Hansen (1999). These items were further transformed as a

result of conversations with Daniel Levin (Evans, personal communication, October,

2010), who suggested a rethinking of Marsden and Campbell’s (1984) work on tie

strength. Levin, Walter, and Murnighan (2011) suggested that ’emotion-based‘ (or

closeness) measures were, at the very least, an equally important indicator of tie strength

to the commonly used interaction and communication frequency measures. With this in

mind, two of the four tie strength items used in this study related to ’closeness‘, and the

other two items to interaction frequency. These four items44 were asked of both groups.

Finally, the study examined tie strength both prior to and while working on a project

together.

Tie strength items were combined and standardized following a similar method to the one

used by Levin and Cross (2004). Once the data was collected, the questions with six and

eight response choices were averaged and reduced to five, so that the four tie strength

questions had the same number of response choices. This made it possible for them to be

combined and analyzed.

Next, at the suggestion of Daniel Levin (Evans, personal communication, March, 2011),

an exploratory factor analysis of the four standardized items was done using principal

                                                                                                               44 Tie Strength Closeness items: 1. Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers on the matter or firm-related project you shared, how close was your working relationship? 2. My relationship with each of the co-workers I mentally selected was a very intense, strong relationship prior to (while) working on the matter or firm-related project we shared. Tie Strength Interaction / Communication Frequency items: 1. Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers on the matter or firm-related project you shared, how often did you communicate? 2. Prior to (While) working with each of the co-workers on the matter or firm-related project you shared, to what extent did you typically interact with each of them?

Page 114: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  102  

axis factoring (in SPSS). Based on previous research (Levin & Cross, 2004; Marsden &

Campbell, 1984), it was expected that factor analysis would extract into two factors

(emotion-based and frequency-based tie strength), but in fact, as can be seen in Table

4.13, it only extracted as one component in both groups (prior to and while on the

project). Based on factor analysis, all subsequent analyses of tie strength used one

combined and standardized variable for tie strength (TSp – prior to the project) and one

variable for tie strength (TSw – while on the project).

Positive

Referents Negative Referents

Component Component Tie Strength - Prior to the project 1 1 1 Prior to working with each of the co-workers on the

matter or firm-related project you shared, how close was your working relationship?

0.808 0.826

2 Prior to working with each of the co-workers on the matter or firm-related project you shared, how often did you communicate?

0.876 0.831

3 Prior to working with each of the co-workers on the matter or firm-related project you shared, to what extent did you typically interact with each of them?

0.889 0.881

4 My relationship with each of the co-workers I mentally selected was a very intense, strong relationship prior to working on the matter or firm-related project we shared.

0.758 0.659

Tie Strength – While on the project 1 1 1 While working on the matter or firm-related project

you shared, how close was your working relationship with each of the co-workers you mentally selected?

0.755 0.804

2 While working on the matter or firm-related project you shared, how often did you communicate with the two co-workers you mentally selected?

0.463 0.628

3 While working on the matter or firm-related project you shared, to what extent did you typically interact with the two co-workers you mentally selected?

0.837 0.853

4 My relationship with each of the co-workers I mentally selected was a very intense, strong relationship while working on the matter or firm-related project we shared.

0.611 0.672

Table 4.13 Factor Analysis Results for Tie Strength (Prior to and While on the Project/Matter)

   

Page 115: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  103  

Reliability Analysis

Reliability analysis was performed on the combined and standardized items for TSp and

TSw in both groups. As noted in Table 4.14, the value of Cronbach’s α exceeded the

acceptable range, in both study groups.

Group 1 Group 2 Reliability Statistics α N α N

Tie Strength (Prior to the project) 0.889 4 0.752 4 Tie Strength (While on the project) 0.887 4 0.827 4

Table 4.14 Reliability Results for Combined Tie Strength Variables Descriptive Statistics

Table 4.15 presents descriptive statistics for the tie strength and relationship length

variables.

N Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation Tie Strength (Prior to the project)

Group 1 269 1 5 3.2326 1.19006 Group 2 268 1 5 2.4953 1.04899 Tie strength (While on the project)

Group 1 269 1.5 5 4.0895 0.65364 Group 2 265 1 5 3.3079 0.85965 Relationship Length Group 1 272 2 17 10.17 3.89 Group 2 269 1 17 9.19 3.998

Table 4.15 Descriptive Statistics for Tie Strength and Relationship Length Variables The results suggested that respondents had significantly higher overall tie strength with

positive referents. Specifically, averaging 23% higher TSp (i.e. means of 3.2326 in Group

1 vs. 2.4953 in Group 2) and 19% higher TSw (i.e. means of 4.0895 in Group 1 vs.

3.3079 in Group 2).

Means for relationship length were relatively similar for positive (i.e. 10.17) and negative

referents (i.e. 9.19). The results were expected to be similar, as it is reasonable to assume

that respondents would know co-workers they work well with and those they do not work

well with equal amounts of time.

Page 116: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  104  

4.3.5 Homophily

Factor Analysis

In order to investigate the significance of homophily, the survey instrument was designed

so that it could capture seven types of ascribed or acquired characteristics: age, gender,

ethnicity, education, marital status, country of birth, and citizenship. Each respondent was

asked to identify their own personal characteristics for these seven categories and the

same characteristics again for both positive and negative referents.

Once the data were collected, further examination was conducted on the characteristics,

by reviewing the overall descriptive statistics for the sample. The review led to the

discovery that some of the characteristic variables were not appropriate for further

homophily analysis. Specifically, birth country, citizenship, ethnicity, and marital status

did not have adequate variability or distribution, across the sample, to warrant inclusion.

Section 4.2.3 mentioned that 85% of respondents who completed the survey were born in

Canada and more than 98% of all respondents had Canadian citizenship. With such an

overwhelming majority of Canadian respondents, there was not enough variability to

effectively analyze homophily based on country of birth or country of citizenship.

Similarly, almost 87% of respondents identified themselves as, at least partially,

Caucasian, causing a similar variability concern with homophily based on ethnicity.

Finally, of all the respondents surveyed, 58% were legally married. Due to the lack of

variability in birth country, citizenship, ethnicity, and marital status responses, a decision

was made not to continue with any further analysis of homophily for these variables.

Descriptive Statistics – Age Homophily

To analyze age homophily, the data set was first reduced to include only those cases

where respondents provided a complete set of answers for their age and the ages of the

positive and negative referents. Next, the “prefer not to say” and “I am not able to

assess” responses were eliminated from the new data set, which also reduced the

response choices of all the age items to 5 (i.e. 1 = 21-30, 2 = 31-40, 3 = 41-50, 4 = 51-60

and 5 = Over 61). Finally, two new variables were created to measure the age difference

Page 117: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  105  

between the respondent’s age group and the age groups of the two co-workers they were

asked to think about. For example, age difference in Group 1 was calculated by taking the

absolute value of the respondent’s age subtracted from the age of the person they worked

best with. The resulting factor represented the number of age categories that separated the

two. For example, an age difference of 1 would equal a one-step separation between age

categories, or approximately 10 years.

Table 4.16 presents descriptive statistics for the age and age difference variables. The

table suggests that the survey respondents were, on average, very similar in age to

positive referents (i.e. mean of 2.4 vs. 2.45 in Group 1) and only marginally younger than

negative referents (mean of 2.68 in Group 2). Further examination of the means for age

difference suggested no evidence of age homophily, since the average age gap between

respondents and referents was about one age category, or approximately 10 years (i.e.

with positive referents .984 and with negative referents 1.091).

N Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation Respondent's Age 252 1 5 2.4 1.023 Age in Group 1 252 1 5 2.45 0.95 Age in Group 2 252 1 5 2.68 1.047 Age Difference in Group 1 252 0 3 0.9841 0.85605 Age Difference in Group 2 252 0 4 1.0913 0.89909

Table 4.16 Descriptive Statistics for Age and Age Difference Descriptive Statistics – Gender Homophily

Gender homophily was analyzed using a similar technique to the one used for age

homophily. First, the data set was reduced to include only those cases where respondents

provided a complete set of answers for their gender and the genders of the positive and

negative referents. Next, the “prefer not to say” responses were eliminated from the

updated data set. This also reduced the item scales to have two matching gender

categories (i.e. 1 = Male 2 = Female). Finally, two new variables were created to measure

the gender difference between the respondent and referent. Gender difference was

calculated by taking the absolute value of the respondent’s gender subtracted from the

gender of the referent co-workers. The resulting factor was a binary code suggesting

whether the two genders were the same (i.e. 0 = same, 1 = different). For example, if the

Page 118: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  106  

respondent’s gender was female (coded 2) and the positive referent was also a female

(coded 2), then the resulting gender difference factor would be 0 (ABS(2-2)), or the

same. If, on the other hand, the respondent was male (coded 1) and the positive referent

was female (coded 2), then the resulting gender difference factor would be 1 (ABS(1-2)),

or different.

Table 4.17 presents descriptive statistics for the gender and gender difference variables.

The results suggested that, on average, no one gender was considered better or worse to

work with, as the means were evenly distributed across the survey respondents and the

referents. A gender difference mean of close to zero would have suggested the

possibility of gender homophily. A gender difference mean close to one would have

suggested the opposite effect. The gender difference means in Table 4.15 suggested that

no gender homophily was present in either group.

N Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation Respondent's Gender 266 1 2 1.62 0.485 Gender in Group 1 266 1 2 1.5 0.501 Gender in Group 2 266 1 2 1.51 0.501 Gender Difference in Group 1 266 0 1 0.3459 0.47655 Gender Difference in Group 2 266 0 1 0.4098 0.49272

Table 4.17 Descriptive Statistics for Gender and Gender Difference Descriptive Statistics – Educational Homophily

Educational homophily was analyzed using a similar technique to the one used to identify

age and gender homophily. First, the data set was reduced to include only those cases

where respondents provided a complete set of answers for their education and the

education of the positive and negative referents.

Next, the “I don’t know” responses were eliminated from the updated data set, which also

reduced the response choices of all the education items to six matching education

categories (i.e. 1 = high school/GED, 2 = some college/2-year college degree, 3 = 3 or 4-

year university degree, 4 = professional degree (e.g. JD/LLB, MD), 5 = Masters degree

and 6 = Doctoral degree). Finally, two new variables were created to measure the

educational gap between the respondents and each of the two co-workers they selected.

Page 119: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  107  

Educational gap was calculated by taking the absolute value of the respondent’s

educational level subtracted from the educational level of the two referents. The resulting

factors represented the educational gap that separated the two co-workers. An

educational gap of 1 would equal a one-step separation between educational levels (e.g.

some college (2) to 4-year degree (3)).

Table 4.18 presents descriptive statistics for the education and educational gap variables.

The table suggests that the survey respondents were, on average, almost identical in

educational level to both positive (i.e. mean of 3.75 vs. 3.77 in Group 1) and negative

referents (i.e. mean of 3.74 in Group 2). These means and the ones related to educational

gap suggest the possible existence of homophily based on education, in both groups. For

example, the average educational gaps found between respondents and referents, both

positive (.532) and negative (.612), was a difference of less than one educational

category.

N Minimum Maximum Mean Std. Deviation Respondent's Education 237 1 6 3.75 1.018 Education in Group 1 237 1 6 3.77 0.823 Education in Group 2 237 1 6 3.74 0.942 Educational Gap in Group 1 237 0 3 0.5316 0.77299 Educational Gap in Group 2 237 0 4 0.6118 0.87886

Table 4.18 Descriptive Statistics for Education and Educational Gap 4.4 Summary of Descriptive Statistics of Study Variables

Table 4.19 provides a summary of the descriptive statistics for the independent and

dependent variables analyzed in the study. The table presents the sample sizes,

minimums, maximums, means, and standard deviations for each of the variables

discussed in the previous sections.

N Minimum Maximum Mean Std.

Deviation Overall Willingness to Share Knowledge Group 1 264 3 5 4.3854 0.51303 Group 2 260 1 5 3.4644 0.97578 Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge Group 1 267 2 5 4.44 0.588 Group 2 262 1 5 3.58 1.1

Page 120: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  108  

Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge Group 1 264 3 5 4.3674 0.52654 Group 2 262 1 5 3.43 0.9985 Overall Willingness to Use Knowledge Group 1 264 1 5 4.247 0.62049 Group 2 260 1 5 3.2023 0.90907 Willingness to Use Explicit Knowledge Group 1 268 1 5 4.36 0.653 Group 2 266 1 5 3.41 1.082 Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge Group 1 267 1 5 4.2257 0.64516 Group 2 262 1 5 3.1613 0.93311 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge Group 1 263 3 5 4.3954 0.49739 Group 2 263 1 5 3.1869 0.83245 Overall Trust Group 1 253 2.71 5 4.2643 0.47851 Group 2 248 1.17 4.5 2.9388 0.65614 Ability-Based Trust Group 1 269 2.78 5 4.4696 0.5228 Group 2 263 1.22 5 3.4415 0.86473 Benevolence-Based Trust Group 1 265 1.56 5 4.0868 0.65311 Group 2 260 1 4.67 2.5115 0.85928 Integrity-Based Trust Group 1 264 2.83 5 4.1812 0.52393 Group 2 262 1 5 2.841 0.79747 Shared Language Group 1 268 1 5 4.2388 0.57715 Group 2 266 1 5 3.213 0.82603 Shared Vision Group 1 265 2.5 5 4.1783 0.53747 Group 2 262 1 5 3.166 0.83712 Tie Strength (Prior to) Group 1 269 1 5 3.2326 1.19006 Group 2 268 1 5 2.4953 1.04899 Tie strength (While on) Group 1 269 1.5 5 4.0895 0.65364 Group 2 265 1 5 3.3079 0.85965 Relationship Length Group 1 272 2 17 10.17 3.89 Group 2 269 1 17 9.19 3.998 Respondent's Age 252 1 5 2.4 1.023 Age in Group 1 252 1 5 2.45 0.95 Age in Group 2 252 1 5 2.68 1.047 Age Difference in Group 1 252 0 3 0.9841 0.85605 Age Difference in Group 2 252 0 4 1.0913 0.89909 Respondent's Gender 266 1 2 1.62 0.485 Gender in Group 1 266 1 2 1.5 0.501 Gender in Group 2 266 1 2 1.51 0.501

Page 121: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  109  

Gender Difference in Group 1 266 0 1 0.3459 0.47655 Gender Difference in Group 2 266 0 1 0.4098 0.49272 Respondent's Education 237 1 6 3.75 1.018 Education in Group 1 237 1 6 3.77 0.823 Education in Group 2 237 1 6 3.74 0.942 Educational Gap in Group 1 237 0 3 0.5316 0.77299 Educational Gap in Group 2 237 0 4 0.6118 0.87886

Table 4.19 Descriptive Statistics for Study Variables 4.5 Hypothesis Testing

In the following sections, each of the hypotheses identified in the previous chapter was

tested using statistical techniques. Correlation analysis was used to measure the bivariate

relationships between the independent and dependent variables. Then, t-tests were used to

test differences between the means of the variables. Multiple regression analysis was

used to examine the relationships between the dependent variables and the focal

independent variables, while controlling for all the other variables. And finally,

hierarchical multiple regression analysis was used to test for a mediating effect between

the independent and dependent variables.

If the results of the regression analysis contradicted the results of the correlation analysis,

or t-tests, the results of the regression analysis were given priority and accepted as MRA

controlled for all of the study’s variables in the theoretical framework, and therefore

represented the more stringent analytical approach.

4.5.1 Age, Gender, and Educational Homophily

Hypotheses 1, 3, and 5 were concerned with the direction of the relationship between

trust and age, education, and gender based homophily. Hypotheses 2, 4, and 6 were

concerned with the direction of the relationship between knowledge sharing behaviors

and age, education, and gender based homophily. For all six hypotheses, homophily

variables were expected to have positive direct effects on trust and knowledge sharing

behavior.

4.5.1.1 Independent Variable 1 – Age Homophily

Hypothesis 1 stated that “age homophily will be positively related to trust” and

hypothesis 2 stated that “age homophily will be positively related to knowledge sharing

Page 122: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  110  

behavior”. Correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to test these

hypotheses.

As an initial step in the analysis of age homophily, the demographic variable of age was

considered with each of the dependent variables for trust and knowledge sharing

behavior. As a second step, the variable of age difference was analyzed with each of the

variables for trust and knowledge sharing behavior.

Age and Trust

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between age and

each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.20, the correlation between age

and overall trust (Otrust) was not significant, in either group. However, when age was

analyzed with each type of trust, there was a statistically significant positive correlation

between ability-based trust (ABtrust) and age (.232) in Group 2. No other correlations

were found between age and the other forms of trust, in either group. The results of the

correlation analysis suggested that as the age of the negative referent increased, so did the

respondents ABtrust in that co-worker.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust 0.058 253 0.069 246 Ability-Based Trust 0.068 269 0.232*** 261 Integrity-Based Trust 0.005 264 -0.027 260 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.052 265 -0.074 258

Table 4.20 Correlations Between Age and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

age and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the other

independent variables. MRA of trust on age, along with the remaining independent

Page 123: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  111  

variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+

β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap45.

The adjusted R2 was the proportion of the variance of the dependent variable(s) (i.e. trust)

that was accounted for, by the set of independent variables in the regression equation,

adjusted for the total number of variables in the equation.

Table 4.21 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on age and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated for each of the

trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and benevolence-

based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of age on each type of trust46.

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Age only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust 0.059 1.056 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.091 1.413 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.014 0.237 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.049 .854 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust .176* 2.216 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust .343*** 4.428 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust -0.009 -.111 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.033 -.406 206 0.124 3.629 0

Table 4.21 Regression of Trust on Age and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

The adjusted R2 was then calculated for each type of trust. Adjusted R2 for Otrust was

.527 in Group 1 and .182 in Group 2. Adjusted R2 for ABtrust was .336 in Group 1 and

.158 in Group 2, IBtrust was .489 in Group 1 and .145 in Group 2, and BBtrust was .471

in Group 1 and .124 in Group 2. For both groups, adjusted R2 for each model had p <

.001.

                                                                                                               45 (Age, Edu = Education, Gen=Gender, R.Len = Relationship Length, T.S.p = Tie Strength (prior to), T.S.w = Tie Strength (while on), S. Lan = Shared Language, S.Vis = Shared Vision, AgeDiff = Age Difference, EduGap = Educational Gap, GenGap = Gender Gap) 46 ABtrust, IBtrust, and BBtrust are each a subset of Otrust which is a combination of the three

Page 124: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  112  

For positive referents, the standardized regression coefficient of OTrust on age was found

to be not significant. However, for negative referents, the standardized regression

coefficient of Otrust on age was .176 (p < .05). The results also showed that, for negative

referents, the standardized regression coefficient of ABtrust on age was .343 (p < .001).

No other form of trust was found to be significant, with respect to age, in Group 2 and no

significant relationships were found in Group 1, with age.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that, for the group of negative

referents, respondents had higher Otrust (consisting of ABtrust) in those co-workers who

were older. In other words, as the age of negative referents increased, so did the

respondent’s Otrust and ABtrust in those individuals.

The strength of the relationships between each type of trust and age was determined using

the square of semi-partial coefficient (sr2). In Group 2, the amount of variance in Otrust

that was uniquely explained by age was 3% and the amount of variance in ABtrust that

was uniquely explained by age was 12%. No relationships were found to be significant in

Group 1.

Age and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between age and

each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.22, the

correlation between age and overall willingness to share (WSO) was found to be not

significant in Group 1, and had a correlation of -.196 (p < .01) in Group 2. After running

correlations separately on the two types of knowledge shared, statistically significant

negative correlations were found with both willingness to share explicit knowledge (-

.144, p < .01) and willingness to share tacit knowledge (-.192, p < .01), in Group 2.

Neither WSE or WST was found to be significantly correlated to age in Group 1. The

correlations between age and overall willingness to use knowledge (WUO), willingness to

use explicit knowledge (WUE), willingness to use tacit knowledge (WUT), or perceived

receipt of useful knowledge were found to be not significant in either group (Table 4.22).

The results of the correlation analysis indicated that, as the age of the negative referents

Page 125: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  113  

increased, an overall willingness to share (both explicit and tacit forms of knowledge)

with that co-worker decreased.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share -0.062 264 -0.196** 258 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.004 267 -0.144** 260 Willingness to Share (Tacit) -0.08 264 -0.192** 260 Overall Willingness to Use 0.081 264 -0.031 258 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.008 268 0.004 264 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.099 264 -0.031 260 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.012 263 0.054 261

Table 4.22 Correlations Between Age and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

age and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling for the effect

of the the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on age,

along with the remaining independent variables, took the following form: KSB DV =

β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+

β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust47.

Table 4.23 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior dependent variables on age and other independent variables. The MRA was

repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE,

WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect of age on each

of the knowledge sharing behaviors48.

                                                                                                               47 (Age, Edu = Education, Gen=Gender, R.Len = Relationship Length, T.S.p = Tie Strength (prior to), T.S.w = Tie Strength (while on), S. Lan = Shared Language, S.Vis = Shared Vision, AgeDiff = Age Difference, EduGap = Educational Gap, GenGap = Gender Gap, Otrust = Overall Trust) 48 WSE and WST are each a subset of WSO, which is a combination of the two variables. WUE and WUT are each a subset of WUO, which is a combination of the two variables.

Page 126: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  114  

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Age only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share -0.065 -1.028 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.053 -0.780 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) -0.062 -0.971 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use .201** 2.924 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.019 0.276 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) .234** 3.378 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.037 -0.591 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share -.229** -2.818 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -.212** -2.592 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) -.222** -2.698 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.090 1.149 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.100 1.264 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.075 0.953 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.130 1.671 189 0.247 6.151 0 Table 4.23 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Age and Other Independent

Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001) The adjusted R2 was then calculated for each knowledge sharing behavior. Adjusted R2

for WSO was .424 in Group 1 and .203 in Group 2; WSE was .329 in Group 1 and .177 in

Group 2; WST was .399 in Group 1 and .186 in Group 2. The adjusted R2 for WUO was

.316 in Group 1 and .277 in Group 2; WUE was .288 in Group 1 and .215 in Group 2;

WUT was .298 in Group 1 and .266 in Group 2. The adjusted R2 for PRUK was .434 in

Group 1 and .247 in Group 2. For both groups, the adjusted R2 for each model had p <

.001.

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO, WSE, and WST or PRUK on

age was found to be not significant. However, the standardized regression coefficient of

WUO on age was .201 (p < .01). In addition, the standardized regression coefficient of

WUT on age was .234 (p < .01); WUE on age was found to be not significant. For Group

2, the standardized regression coefficient of WUO, WUE, and WUT or PRUK on age was

found to be not significant. However, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO on

age was -.229 (p < .01). In addition, the standardized regression coefficient of WSE on

age was -.212 (p < .01) and WST on age was -.222 (p < .01).

Page 127: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  115  

The strength of the relationships between each type of knowledge sharing behavior and

age was determined using the square of semi-partial coefficient (sr2). In Group 1, the

amount of variance in WUO that was uniquely explained by age was 4% and the amount

of variance in WUT that was uniquely explained by age was 6%. In Group 2, the amount

of variance in WSO that was uniquely explained by age was 5%. In the same group, the

amount of variance in WSE that was uniquely explained by age was 5% and the amount

of variance in WST that was uniquely explained by age was 5%.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that, for the group of positive

referents, respondents had a higher WUO and WUT from those co-workers who were

older. In the negative referent group, respondents had a higher WSO, WSE, and WST with

those co-workers who were younger.

Age Difference and Trust

Prior to running correlation and regression analysis on the age difference variable, the

entire data set was sorted and reduced to include only those cases where respondents

provided a complete set of answers for their age and the ages of the two co-workers they

mentally selected. Any case not containing complete age information was excluded from

further analysis.

Correlation analysis was then used to examine the bivariate relationships between age

difference and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.24, the correlation

between age difference and Otrust was not significant in Group 1, and .143 (p < .05) in

Group 2. However, when age difference was analyzed with each type of trust separately,

there was a statistically significant positive correlation found, in Group 2, between

ABtrust and age difference (.172, p < .01). No other correlations were found between age

difference and the other forms of trust in Group 2, and no significant correlations were

found between trust and age difference in Group 1. The results of the correlation analysis

suggested that, as the age gap between the respondent and the negative referent increased,

so did overall trust and ability-based trust in that co-worker.

Page 128: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  116  

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust 0.062 234 0.143* 232 Ability-Based Trust 0.05 247 0.172** 246 Integrity-Based Trust 0.048 245 0.124 244 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.062 245 0.05 244

Table 4.24 Correlations Between Age Difference and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

age difference and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the other

independent variables. MRA of trust on age difference, along with the remaining

independent variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+

β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+

β11*GenGap. The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust is discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.25 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on age difference and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated for

each of the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of age difference on

each type of trust49. For both groups, the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust on

age difference was found to be not significant. In addition, no other form of trust was

found to be significant with respect to age difference in either group. Overall, the

multiple regression analysis indicated that age homophily had no significant effect on

trust, in either group.

Trust Dependent Variable(s) β for Age Diff. only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust 0.076 1.443 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.046 0.763 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.096 1.776 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.068 1.246 204 0.471 17.458 0

                                                                                                               49 ABtrust, IBtrust, and BBtrust are each a subset of Otrust which is a combination of the three

Page 129: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  117  

Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust 0.076 1.076 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.073 1.029 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.099 1.389 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.014 0.195 206 0.124 3.629 0

Table 4.25 Regression of Trust on Age Difference and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Therefore, hypothesis 1 was not supported by the results of the analysis in either group.

Correlation analysis suggested that the opposite effect may be true with negative

referents, but the regression analysis showed no significant evidence of age homophily

(as it related to trust) in either group.

Age Difference and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between age

difference and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables. As it can be seen in

Table 4.26, the correlation between age difference and WSO, WSE, or WST was not

significant in either group. The correlation between age difference and WUO, WUE, or

WUT was also not significant in either group. Finally, the correlation between age

difference and PRUK was not significant in Group 1, and .141 (p < .05) in Group 2. The

results of the correlation analysis suggested that, as the age gap between the respondents

and negative referents increased, so did a perception that the knowledge received from

that co-worker was useful and had a positive effect on the performance outcomes.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share 0.023 246 0.042 254 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.017 249 0.060 256 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.023 246 0.032 256 Overall Willingness to Use -0.001 244 0.069 254 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.084 248 0.042 260 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -0.030 246 0.078 256 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.070 243 0.141* 257

Table 4.26 Correlations Between Age Difference and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Page 130: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  118  

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationship between

age difference and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling

for the effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on

age difference, along with the remaining independent variables, took the following form:

KSB DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+

β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The adjusted model R2

for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.27 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior dependent variables on age difference and other independent variables. The

MRA was repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST,

WUO, WUE, WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect

of age difference on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Age Diff. only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.086 1.462 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.062 0.974 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.088 1.460 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use -0.109 -1.692 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.032 0.494 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -.144* -2.222 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.069 1.174 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.094 1.319 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.101 1.409 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.083 1.151 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.009 0.137 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.022 0.308 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.004 0.063 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.069 0.995 189 0.247

6.151 0

Table 4.27 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Age Difference and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficients of WSO, WSE, WST, or PRUK on

age difference was found to be not significant. Further, the standardized regression

coefficient of WUO or WUE on age difference was not significant. However, in Group 1,

Page 131: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  119  

the standardized regression coefficient of WUT on age difference was -.144 (p < .05).

Therefore, in Group 1, there was a 2% variance in WUT that was uniquely explained by

age homophily.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that respondents were more willing to

use tacit knowledge from positive referents, if they were closer to their age. In other

words, a smaller gap in age difference or higher age homophily was found to be a

predictor of a WUT in Group 1. No significant age homophily and knowledge sharing

behavior relationships were found for Group 2.

Therefore, hypothesis 2 was partially supported by the results of the analysis in Group 1

and not supported in Group 2. The correlation analysis showed no significant

relationships between age homophily and willingness to share knowledge, or willingness

to use knowledge, in either group. However, the analysis did reveal a statistically

significant negative relationship between age homophily and perceived receipt of useful

knowledge, with negative referents. The regression analysis did not result in similar

findings; showing no evidence of age homophily with relation to willingness to use

knowledge, perceived receipt of useful knowledge, or overall willingness to use

knowledge. However, the regression analysis did suggest age homophily to be present

with respect to WUT from positive referents.

4.5.1.2 Independent Variable 2 – Educational Homophily

Hypothesis 3 stated that “educational homophily will be positively related to trust” and

hypothesis 4 stated that “educational homophily will be positively related to knowledge

sharing behavior”. Correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to test these

hypotheses.

As an initial step in the analysis of educational homophily, the demographic variable of

education was considered with each of the dependent variables for trust and knowledge

sharing behavior. As second step, the variable of educational gap was analyzed with each

of the variables for trust and knowledge sharing behavior.

Page 132: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  120  

Education and Trust

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between

education and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.28, the correlation

between education and Otrust, ABtrust, IBtrust, or BBtrust was found to be not

significant for either group. The results of the correlation analysis suggested that no

significant relationships existed between trust and education in either group.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust -0.013 252 0.046 246 Ability-Based Trust 0.030 268 -0.035 261 Integrity-Based Trust 0.018 263 0.055 260 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.023 264 0.075 258

Table 4.28 Correlations Between Education and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

education and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the other

independent variables. MRA of trust on education, along with the remaining independent

variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+

β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap.

The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.29 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on education and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated for each

of the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effects of education on each

type of trust.

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Education only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust 0.045 0.861 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.060 0.999 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust .122* 2.295 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.011 -0.208 204 0.471 17.458 0

Page 133: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  121  

Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust 0.025 0.346 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.018 0.261 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.000 0.001 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.029 0.409 206 0.124 3.629 0

Table 4.29 Regression of Trust on Education and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust, ABtrust, or BBtrust on

education was found to be not significant. However, in the same group, the standardized

regression coefficient of IBtrust on education was .122 (p < .05). Therefore, in Group 1,

the amount of variance in IBtrust that was uniquely explained by education was 1%. No

significant education and trust relationships were found in Group 2. Overall, the multiple

regression analysis indicated that respondents had higher integrity-based trust in those

positive referent co-workers who had a higher level of education.

Education and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between

education and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables. As it can be seen in

Table 4.30, the correlation between education and WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE, WUT,

or PRUK was not significant in either group. The results of the correlation analysis

suggested that no significant relationships existed between knowledge sharing behavior

and education in either group.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share -0.111 263 0.024 258 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.069 266 0.027 260 Willingness to Share (Tacit) -0.118 263 0.021 260 Overall Willingness to Use -0.071 263 -0.118 258 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.082 267 -0.136 264 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.064 266 -0.102 260 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.029 262 0.014 261

Table 4.30 Correlations Between Education and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Page 134: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  122  

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

education and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling for the

effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on

education, along with the remaining independent variables, took the following form:

KSB DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+

β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The adjusted model R2

for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.31 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior variables on education and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated

for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE, WUT,

PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect of education on each

of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Education only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.041 0.712 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.077 1.243 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.024 0.397 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.016 0.246 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.052 0.806 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.010 0.151 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.039 -0.668 189 0.434

13.030 0

Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share -0.086 -1.188 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.064 -0.881 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) -0.088 -1.203 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use -0.084 -1.224 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) -0.045 -0.637 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -0.090 -1.296 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.003 0.048 189 0.247 6.151 0

Table 4.31 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Education and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

The standardized regression coefficient of WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE, WUT, or

PRUK on education was found to be not significant, in either group. Overall, the multiple

Page 135: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  123  

regression analysis confirmed the correlation analysis, indicating that no significant

education and knowledge sharing behavior relationships existed in either group.

Educational Gap and Trust

Prior to running correlation and regression analysis on the educational gap variable, the

entire data set was sorted and reduced to include only those cases where respondents

provided a complete set of answers for their education and the education of the two co-

workers they mentally selected. Any case not containing complete educational

information was excluded from further analysis.

Correlation analysis was used to examine the bivariate relationships between educational

gap and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.32, the correlation

between educational gap and Otrust, ABtrust, IBtrust, or BBtrust was not significant in

either group. The results of the correlation analysis suggested that no significant

relationships existed between trust and educational homophily in either group.

Group 1: Positive

Referent Group 2: Negative

Referent Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust -0.031 221 0.009 216 Ability-Based Trust -0.038 233 0.007 231 Integrity-Based Trust 0.019 230 0.012 229 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.028 230 -0.03 228

Table 4.32 Correlations Between Educational Gap and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

educational gap and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the

other independent variables. MRA of trust on educational gap, along with the remaining

independent variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+

β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+

β11*GenGap. The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust was discussed in Section

4.5.1.1.

Page 136: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  124  

Table 4.33 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on educational gap and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated

for each of the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of educational gap on

each type of trust.

The standardized regression coefficient of Otrust, ABtrust, IBtrust, or BBtrust on

educational gap was found to be not significant in either group. Overall, the multiple

regression analysis confirmed the correlation analysis, indicating that no significant

relationships existed between educational homophily and trust, in either group. Since the

correlation analysis and regression analysis showed no significant evidence of

educational homophily (as it relates to trust) in either group, hypothesis 3 was not

supported by the results of the analysis in either group.

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Educational Gap only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust -0.012 -0.242 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust -0.040 -0.691 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.028 0.556 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.001 -0.017 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust 0.043 0.649 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.005 0.077 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.071 1.082 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.034 0.518 206 0.124 3.629 0

Table 4.33 Regression of Trust on Educational Gap and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Educational Gap and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between

educational gap and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables. As it can be seen

in Table 4.34, the correlation between educational gap and WSO, WSE, WST, WUO,

WUE, WUT, or PRUK was not significant in either group. The results of the correlation

analysis suggested that no significant relationships existed between knowledge sharing

behavior and educational homophily, in either group.

Page 137: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  125  

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share -0.031 230 -0.018 230 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.053 233 -0.026 232 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.033 230 -0.001 231 Overall Willingness to Use 0.037 228 0.043 227 Willingness to Use (Explicit) -0.031 232 0.047 233 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.061 231 0.029 229 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.017 227 0.024 231

Table 4.34 Correlations Between Educational Gap and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

educational gap and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling

for the effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on

educational gap, along with the remaining independent variables, took the following

form: KSB DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+

β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The

adjusted model R2 for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.35 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior variables on educational gap and other independent variables. The MRA was

repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE,

WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect of educational

gap on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

The standardized regression coefficient of WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE, WUT, or

PRUK on educational gap was found to be not significant in either group. Overall, the

multiple regression analysis confirmed the correlation analysis, indicating that no

significant educational gap and knowledge sharing behavior relationships existed in

either group. Since the correlation analysis and regression analysis showed no significant

evidence of educational homophily as it related to knowledge sharing behavior, in either

group, hypothesis 4 was not supported by the results of the analysis, in either group.

Page 138: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  126  

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Educational Gap only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.016 0.280 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.042 -0.706 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.035 0.619 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.036 0.588 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) -0.019 -0.303 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.060 0.973 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.011 0.203 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share -0.049 -0.747 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.072 -1.074 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) -0.039 -0.580 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.055 0.852 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.000 -0.007 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.063 0.985 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.069 1.070 189 0.247 6.151 0 Table 4.35 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Educational Gap and Other

Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

4.5.1.3 Independent Variable 3 – Gender Homophily

Hypothesis 5 stated that “gender homophily will be positively related to trust” and

hypothesis 6 stated that “gender homophily will be positively related to knowledge

sharing behavior”. Correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to test these

hypotheses.

As an initial step in the analysis of gender homophily, the demographic variable of

gender was considered with each of the dependent variables for trust and knowledge

sharing behavior. As second step, the variable of educational gap was analyzed with each

of the variables for trust and knowledge sharing behavior.

Gender and Trust

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between gender

and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.36, the correlation between

gender and Otrust, ABtrust, IBtrust, or BBtrust was found to be not significant for either

Page 139: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  127  

group. The results of the correlation analysis suggested that no significant relationships

existed between trust and education, in either group.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust 0.012 253 -0.011 247 Ability-Based Trust -0.013 268 -0.067 262 Integrity-Based Trust 0.069 264 -0.004 261 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.008 265 0.011 259

Table 4.36 Correlations Between Gender and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

gender and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the other

independent variables. MRA of trust on gender, along with the remaining independent

variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+

β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap.

The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.37 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on gender and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated for each of

the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of gender on each

type of trust.

Trust Dependent Variable(s) β for Gender only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust 0.075 1.365 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.017 0.266 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust .183** 3.242 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.039 0.674 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust -0.014 -0.187 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.039 0.565 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust -0.028 -0.384 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.112 -1.545 206 0.124 3.629 0

Table 4.37 Regression of Trust on Gender and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Page 140: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  128  

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust, ABtrust, or BBtrust on

gender was not significant. However, in the same group, the standardized regression

coefficient of IBtrust on gender was .183 (p < .01). In Group 1, the amount of variance in

IBtrust that was uniquely explained by gender was 3%. No significant relationships

between gender and trust were found in Group 2. The results of the multiple regression

analysis indicated that respondents had higher integrity-based trust in those positive

referent co-workers that were women.

Gender and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between gender

and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.38,

the correlation between educational gap and WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE, WUT, or

PRUK was not significant in either group. The results of the correlation analysis

suggested that no significant relationships existed between knowledge sharing behavior

and gender, in either group.

Group 1: Positive

Referent Group 2: Negative

Referent Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share 0.044 264 0.069 259 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.017 267 0.080 261 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.050 264 0.063 261 Overall Willingness to Use -0.034 264 0.052 259 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.017 268 0.061 265 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -0.043 267 0.046 261 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.086 263 0.045 262

Table 4.38 Correlations Between Gender and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

gender and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling for the

effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on

gender, along with the remaining independent variables, took the following form: KSB

DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+

Page 141: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  129  

β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The adjusted model

R2 for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.39 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior variables on gender and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated

for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE, WUT,

PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect of gender on each of

the knowledge sharing behaviors.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Gender only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referent Overall Willingness to Share .131* 2.112 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) .146* 2.198 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.113 1.775 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.051 0.756 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) .138* 2.014 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.033 0.485 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.049 -0.788 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referent Overall Willingness to Share 0.031 0.428 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.066 0.894 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.013 0.183 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use .162* 2.326 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) .179* 2.495 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) .140* 2.000 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.129 1.824 189 0.247 6.151 0

Table 4.39 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Gender and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO on gender was .131 (p <

.05) and the standardized regression coefficient of WSE on gender was .146 (p < .05). In

the same group, the standardized regression coefficient of WUE on gender was .138 (p <

.05). Regression analysis found no other significant relationships between the remaining

KSB (i.e. WST, WUO, WUT or PRUK) variables and gender in this group. In Group 1,

the amount of variance in WSO that was uniquely explained by gender was 2%. Further,

Page 142: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  130  

the amount of variance in WSE that was uniquely explained by gender was 2%. Finally,

the amount of variance in WUE that was uniquely explained by gender was 2%.

For Group 2, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO, WSE, WST, or PRUK on

gender was not significant. However, in the same group, the standardized regression

coefficient of WUO on gender was .162 (p < .05). In addition, the standardized regression

coefficient of WUE on gender was .179 (p < .05) and the standardized regression

coefficient of WUT on gender was .140 (p < .05). In Group 2, the amount of variance in

WUO that was uniquely explained by gender was 3%. Further, the amount of variance in

WUE that was uniquely explained by gender was 3%. Finally, the amount of variance in

WUT that was uniquely explained by gender was 2%.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that respondents had a higher WSO,

WSE, and WUE from positive referents who were women. In Group 2, the multiple

regression analysis indicated that respondents had a higher WUO (both tacit and explicit)

from those individuals who were women.

Gender Gap and Trust

Prior to running correlation and regression analysis on the gender gap variable, the entire

data set was sorted and reduced to include only those cases where respondents provided a

complete set of answers for their gender and the genders of two co-workers they mentally

selected. Any case not containing complete gender information was excluded from

further analysis.

Correlation analysis was used to examine the bivariate relationships between educational

gap and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.40, the correlation

between gender gap and Otrust was not significant in either group. However, there was a

positive correlation found in Group 1 between ABtrust and gender gap of .148 (p < .05)

and a negative correlation in Group 2 between BBtrust and gender gap of -.136 (p < .05).

No other significant correlations were found between gender gap and the other forms of

trust, in either group. The results of the correlation analysis suggested that respondents

Page 143: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  131  

had a higher ability-based trust in positive referents that were of opposite gender. With

negative referents however, respondents had a higher benevolence-based trust in the co-

workers who were of the same gender.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust 0.051 246 -0.11 244 Ability-Based Trust 0.148* 261 -0.029 259 Integrity-Based Trust 0.019 257 -0.114 258 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.008 258 -0.136* 256

Table 4.40 Correlations Between Gender Gap and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

gender gap and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the other

independent variables. MRA of trust on gender gap, along with the remaining

independent variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+

β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+

β11*GenGap. The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust was discussed in Section

4.5.1.1.

Table 4.41 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on gender gap and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated for

each of the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of gender gap on

each type of trust. The standardized regression coefficient of Otrust, ABtrust, IBtrust, or

BBtrust on gender gap was found to be not significant, in either group. Overall, the

multiple regression analysis indicated that no significant relationships existed between

gender homophily and trust, in either group.

Page 144: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  132  

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Gender Gap only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust -.043 -0.808 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust .035 0.565 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust -.038 -0.692 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust -.093 -1.694 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust -.079 -1.107 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust -.003 -0.049 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust -.090 -1.257 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust -.128 -1.804 206 0.124 3.629 0

Table 4.41 Regression of Trust on Gender Gap and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

The correlation analysis suggested that gender homophily was positively related to

BBtrust in Group 2, and negatively related to ABtrust in Group 1. In both cases, however,

the regression analysis showed no significant evidence of gender homophily, as it related

to trust, for either group. Therefore, hypothesis 5 was not supported by the results of the

analysis, in either group.

Gender Gap and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between gender

gap and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables. As it can be seen in Table

4.42, the correlation between gender gap and WUO was .124 (p < .05) in Group 1, and

not significant in Group 2. No other correlations were found to be significant between

gender gap and the remaining KSBs (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST, WUE, WUT, or PRUK), in

either group. The results of the correlation analysis suggested that respondents were more

willing to use knowledge from positive referents, when they were of opposite gender.

Page 145: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  133  

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share 0.032 257 -0.099 256 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.031 260 -0.055 258 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.031 257 -0.107 258 Overall Willingness to Use 0.124* 257 -0.026 256 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.118 261 -0.012 262 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.11 260 -0.018 258 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.103 256 0.008 259

Table 4.42 Correlations Between Gender Gap and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

gender gap and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling for

the effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on

gender gap, along with the remaining independent variables, took the following form:

KSB DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+

β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The adjusted model R2

for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.43 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior dependent variables on gender gap and other independent variables. The MRA

was repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST, WUO,

WUE, WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect of

gender gap on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of WUE on gender gap was .143 (p

< .05). The amount of variance in WUE that was uniquely explained by gender gap was

2%. For Group 2, the standardized regression coefficient of PRUK on gender gap was

.148 (p < .05). The amount of variance in PRUK that was uniquely explained by gender

gap was 2%. No other relationships were found to be significant between gender gap and

the other KSBs, in either group. Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that

respondents perceived the knowledge from negative referents as more useful, when they

Page 146: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  134  

were of different gender. The analyses also indicated that respondents had higher WUE

from those positive referents different from them in gender.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Gender Gap only T N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share .062 1.022 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) .042 0.658 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) .063 1.012 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use .084 1.271 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) .143* 2.146 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.063 0.950 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.075 1.257 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share .033 0.458 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) .100 1.384 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) .004 0.056 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use .116 1.667 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) .067 0.945 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) .119 1.711 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge .148* 2.116 189 0.247 6.151 0

Table 4.43 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Gender Gap and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

The correlation analysis found no effect of gender homophily. Instead, it suggested a

gender heterogeneous effect might be present in relation to WUO, in Group 1. The

regression analysis also showed no significant evidence of gender homophily, as it

related to knowledge sharing behavior, in either group. In fact, regression analysis

showed similar heterogenous relationships with WUE in Group 1, and PRUK in Group 2.

Based on these results, hypothesis 6 was not supported by the analysis, in either group.

4.5.2 Independent Variable 4 – Shared Language

Hypothesis 7 stated that “shared language will be positively related to trust” and

hypothesis 8 stated that “shared language will be positively related to knowledge sharing

behavior”. Correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to test these

hypotheses.

Page 147: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  135  

Shared Language and Trust

Correlation analysis was used to examine the bivariate relationships between shared

language and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.44, the correlation

between shared language and Otrust was .498 (p < .001) in Group 1, and .195 (p < .01)

in Group 2. In Group 1, the correlation between shared language and ABtrust was .435;

between shared language and IBtrust was .517; and between shared language and

BBtrust was .395 (all three correlations p < .001). In Group 2, the correlation between

shared language and ABtrust was .165 (p < .01); between shared language and IBtrust

was .150 (p < .05); and between shared language and BBtrust was .156 (p < .05). The

results of the correlation analysis suggested that respondents had higher trust (of all

types) in those co-workers with whom they shared more of a common language. The

results were consistent in both groups, although they showed weaker correlations

between respondents and negative referents.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust 0.498*** 249 0.195** 258 Ability-Based Trust 0.435*** 263 0.165** 265 Integrity-Based Trust 0.517*** 259 0.150* 264 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.395*** 261 0.156* 261

Table 4.44 Correlations Between Shared Language and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

shared language and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the

other independent variables. MRA of trust on shared language, along with the remaining

independent variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+

β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+

β11*GenGap. The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust was discussed in Section

4.5.1.1.

Table 4.45 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on shared language and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated

for each of the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

Page 148: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  136  

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of shared language

on each type of trust.

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust on shared language was

.259 (p < .001). In addition, the standardized regression coefficient of ABtrust on shared

language was .273 (p < .001); IBtrust on shared language was .330 (p < .001); and

BBtrust on shared language was .149 (p < .05). In Group 1, the amount of variance in

Otrust that was uniquely explained by shared language was 7%. The amount of variance

in ABtrust that was uniquely explained by shared language was 7%. The amount of

variance in IBtrust that was uniquely explained by shared language was 11%. Finally,

the amount of variance in BBtrust that was uniquely explained by shared language was

2%. No significant relationships between shared language and any of the trust variables

were found in Group 2.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that respondents had higher trust (of

all types) in those positive referents with whom they shared more of a common language.

The MRA found no significant relationships between trust and shared language with

negative referents.

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Shared Language only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust .259*** 4.070 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust .273*** 3.719 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust .330*** 5.052 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust .149* 2.294 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust 0.040 0.544 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.012 0.165 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.037 0.503 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.066 0.898 206 0.124 3.629 0 Table 4.45 Regression of Trust on Shared Language and Other Independent Variables

(*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Page 149: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  137  

Even though correlation analysis found statistically significant relationships between trust

and shared language in both groups, the regression analysis showed only significant

effects in Group 1. Therefore, hypothesis 7 was supported by the results of the analysis in

Group 1 and not supported in Group 2.

Shared Language and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between shared

language and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables. As it can be seen in

Table 4.46, the correlation between shared language and WSO was .473 in Group 1 (p <

.001) and .152 in Group 2 (p < .05). In addition, the correlation between shared language

and WSE was .434 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .161 in Group 2 (p < .01). The correlation

between shared language and WST was .454 in Group 1 (p < .001) and not significant in

Group 2. Next, the correlation between shared language and WUO was .343 in Group 1

(p < .001) and .210 in Group 2 (p < .01). The correlation between shared language and

WUE was .333 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .130 in Group 2 (p < .05). The correlation

between shared language and WUT was .322 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .191 in Group 2

(p < .01). Finally, the correlation between shared language and PRUK was .431 in Group

1 (p < .001). No significant correlation was found between shared language and PRUK

in Group 2.

The results of the correlation analysis suggested that respondents had a higher WSO,

WSE, and WST with positive referents, if the two shared more of a common language.

This was also the case between respondents and negative referents, where higher shared

language was correlated to higher WSO and WSE. The results of the correlation analysis

also suggested that respondents had a higher WUO, WUE, and WUT from co-workers, in

both groups, if they shared more of a common language. Finally, the results suggested

that respondents had a higher perception that knowledge received from positive referents

was useful, if they shared a common language with them. This same relationship was not

found with negative referents.

Page 150: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  138  

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Positive

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share 0.473*** 260 0.152* 257 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.434*** 263 0.161** 259 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.454*** 260 0.130 259 Overall Willingness to Use 0.343*** 259 0.210** 256 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.333*** 263 0.130* 262 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.322*** 262 0.191** 258 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.431*** 259 0.108 259

Table 4.46 Correlations Between Shared Language and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

shared language and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling

for the effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on

shared language, along with the remaining independent variables, took the following

form: KSB DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+

β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The

adjusted model R2 for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.47 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior dependent variables on shared language and other independent variables. The

MRA was repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST,

WUO, WUE, WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect

of shared language on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO on shared language was

.320 (p < .001). In the same group, the standardized regression coefficient of WSE on

shared language was .325 (p < .001) and WST on shared language was .294 (p < .001).

In Group 1, the amount of variance in WSO that was uniquely explained by shared

language was 10%. The amount of variance in WSE that was uniquely explained by

shared language was 11%. The amount of variance in WST that was uniquely explained

by shared language was 9%.

Page 151: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  139  

For Group 2, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO on shared language was not

significant. However, the standardized regression coefficient of WSE on shared language

was.146 (p < .05) (i.e. WST on shared language was not significant). The standardized

regression coefficient of WUO, WUE, WUT, or PRUK on shared language was not

significant in either group. In Group 2, the amount of variance in WUE that was uniquely

explained by shared language was 2%.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that respondents had higher WSO,

WSE, and WST with those positive referents they shared more of a common language

with. The MRA also suggested that respondents had a higher WSE with those negative

referents they felt they shared a more of a common language with.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Shared Language only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share .320*** 4.330 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) .325*** 4.074 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) .294*** 3.892 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.047 0.584 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.095 1.156 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.036 0.438 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.123 1.667 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.099 1.362 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) .146* 1.993 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.073 0.999 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use -0.026 -0.371 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) -0.043 -0.595 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -0.026 -0.365 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.039 -0.558 189 0.247 6.151 0 Table 4.47 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Shared Language and Other

Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001) In Group 1, both the correlation and regression analyses suggested statistically significant

relationships between shared language and willingness to share knowledge (overall,

explicit and tacit). However, unlike the correlation analysis, the regression analysis did

Page 152: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  140  

not find significant relationships between shared language and the other KSBs, for

Group 1. In Group 2, correlation analysis found significant relationships between shared

language and all of the KSB variables. However, the regression analysis found only one

of these relationships to be statistically significant (i.e. between shared language and

WSE). Therefore, hypothesis 8 was partially supported in both groups.

4.5.3 Independent Variable 5 – Shared Vision

Hypothesis 9 stated that “shared vision will be positively related to trust” and hypothesis

10 stated that “shared vision will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior”.

Correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to test these hypotheses.

Shared Vision and Trust

Correlation analysis was used to examine the bivariate relationships between shared

vision and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.48, in Group 1 the

correlation between shared vision and Otrust was .637; between shared vision and

ABtrust was .531; between shared vision and IBtrust was .635; and between shared

vision and BBtrust was .535. All four relationships in this group had p < .001. In Group 2,

the correlation between shared vision and Otrust was .465; between shared vision and

ABtrust was .292; between shared vision and IBtrust was .436; and between shared

vision and BBtrust was .360. All four relationships in this group had p < .001. The results

of the correlation analysis suggested that respondents had higher trust (of all types) in

positive and negative referents with whom they shared more of a vision.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust 0.637*** 246 0.465*** 243 Ability-Based Trust 0.531*** 260 0.292*** 257 Integrity-Based Trust 0.635*** 256 0.436*** 254 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.535*** 258 0.360*** 255

Table 4.48 Correlations Between Shared Vision and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

shared vision and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the other

Page 153: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  141  

independent variables. MRA of trust on shared vision, along with the remaining

independent variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+

β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+

β11*GenGap. The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust was discussed in Section

4.5.1.1.

Table 4.49 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on shared vision and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated for

each of the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of shared vision on

each type of trust.

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust on shared vision was .374.

In the same group, the standardized regression coefficient of ABtrust on shared vision

was .362; the standardized regression coefficient of IBtrust on shared vision was .346;

and the standardized regression coefficient of BBtrust on shared vision was .286. All four

relationships in this group had p < .001. In Group 1, the amount of variance in Otrust that

was uniquely explained by shared vision was 14%. The amount of variance in ABtrust

that was uniquely explained by shared vision was 13%. The amount of variance in

IBtrust that was uniquely explained by shared vision was 12%. Finally, the amount of

variance in BBtrust that was uniquely explained by shared vision was 8%.

For Group 2, the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust on shared vision was .375

(p < .001). In the same group, the standardized regression coefficient of ABtrust on

shared vision was .277 (p < .001); the standardized regression coefficient of IBtrust on

shared vision was .322 (p < .001); and the standardized regression coefficient BBtrust on

shared vision on was .251 (p < .01). In Group 2, the amount of variance in Otrust that

was uniquely explained by shared vision was 14%. The amount of variance in ABtrust

that was uniquely explained by shared vision was 8%. The amount of variance in IBtrust

that was uniquely explained by shared vision was 10%. Finally, the amount of variance in

BBtrust that was uniquely explained by shared vision was 6%.

Page 154: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  142  

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that respondents had higher trust (of

all types) in those positive and negative referents with whom they had more of a shared

vision.

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Shared Vision only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust .374*** 5.939 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust .362*** 4.871 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust .346*** 5.313 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust .286*** 4.384 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust .375*** 5.039 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust .277*** 3.773 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust .322*** 4.320 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust .251** 3.329 206 0.124 3.629 0

Table 4.49 Regression of Trust on Shared Vision and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Both the correlation and regression analyses showed statistically significant positive

relationships between shared vision and trust (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based,

and benevolence-based). Therefore, hypothesis 9 was supported by the results of the

analysis, in both groups.

Shared Vision and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between shared

vision and each of the knowledge sharing behaviors. As it can be seen in Table 4.50, the

correlations between shared vision and WSO was .557 in Group 1 and .310 in Group 2;

between shared vision and WSE was .463 in Group 1 and .279 in Group 2; between

shared vision and WST was .553 in Group 1 and .285 in Group 2. Next, the correlations

between shared vision and WUO was .511 in Group 1 and .439 in Group 2; between

shared vision and WUE was .440 in Group 1 and .301 in Group 2; between shared vision

and WUT was .499 in Group 1 and .451 in Group 2. All the correlations between shared

vision and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables had p < .001. The results of

the correlation analysis suggested that, in both groups, having more of a shared vision

among co-workers had a positive effect on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

Page 155: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  143  

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Positive

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share 0.557*** 257 0.310*** 254 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.463*** 260 0.279*** 256 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.553*** 257 0.285*** 256 Overall Willingness to Use 0.511*** 256 0.439*** 254 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.440*** 260 0.301*** 259 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.499*** 259 0.451*** 255 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.631*** 255 0.209*** 256

Table 4.50 Correlations Between Shared Vision and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

shared vision and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling for

the effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on

shared vision, along with the remaining independent variables took the following form:

KSB DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+

β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The adjusted model R2

for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.51 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior dependent variables on shared vision and other independent variables. The

MRA was repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST,

WUO, WUE, WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effects

of shared vision on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO on shared vision was .240 (p

< .05); the standardized regression coefficient of WST on shared vision was .260 (p <

.001), and the standardized regression coefficient of WSE on shared vision was not

significant. For the same group, the standardized regression coefficient of WUO on

shared vision was .265 (p < .01); the standardized regression coefficient of WST on

shared vision was .282 (p < .001); and the standardized regression coefficient of WUE on

Page 156: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  144  

shared vision on was not significant. Finally, in Group 1, the standardized regression

coefficient of PRUK on shared vision was .385 (p < .001).

In Group 1, the amount of variance in WSO that was uniquely explained by shared vision

was 6%. The amount of variance in WST that was uniquely explained by shared vision

was 7%. Next, the amount of variance in WUO that was uniquely explained by shared

vision was 7%, and the amount of variance in WUT that was uniquely explained by

shared language was 8%. Finally, the amount of variance in PRUK that was uniquely

explained by shared vision was 15%.

For Group 2, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO on shared vision was .222 (p

< .01); the standardized regression coefficient of WSE on shared vision was .165 (p <

.05); and the standardized regression coefficient of WST on shared vision was .227 (p <

.01). For the same group, the standardized regression coefficient of WUO on shared

vision was .312 (p < .001); the standardized regression coefficient of WUT on shared

vision was .348 (p < .001); and the standardized regression coefficient of WUE on shared

vision was not significant. Finally, the standardized regression coefficient of PRUK on

shared vision was not significant in Group 2.

In Group 2, the amount of variance in WSO that was uniquely explained by shared vision

was 5%. The amount of variance in WSE that was uniquely explained by shared vision

was 3% and the amount of variance in WST that was uniquely explained by shared vision

was 5%. Finally, the amount of variance in WUO that was uniquely explained by shared

vision was 10%, and the amount of variance in WUT that was uniquely explained by

shared language was 12%.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that respondents had a higher WSO

and WST with those positive referents they felt they shared more of a vision with.

Respondents also had a higher WUO and WUT from positive referents they shared more

of a vision with. More shared vision between respondents and positive referents also

related to a higher perception that the knowledge received from those co-workers was

Page 157: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  145  

useful. Similarly, for negative referents, respondents had a higher WSO, WSE, and WST

with those individuals that they felt they shared more of a vision with. Higher shared

vision between respondents and negative referents also related to a higher WUO and

WUT.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Shared Vision only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share .240* 3.106 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) .153 1.851 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) .260*** 3.293 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use .265** 3.169 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.072 0.846 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) .282*** 3.358 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge .385*** 5.049 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share .222** 2.796 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) .165* 2.064 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) .227** 2.825 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use .312*** 4.031 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.128 1.618 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) .348*** 4.483 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.082 -1.065 189 0.247 6.151 0

Table 4.51 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Shared Vision and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

In both groups, the correlation and regression analyses suggested statistically significant

relationships between shared vision and WSO, as well as between shared vision and WST.

The relationships found using correlation analysis in both groups between shared vision

and WSE were significant, but only remained significant for negative referents after

regression analysis. In both groups, the correlation and regression analyses suggested

statistically significant relationships between shared vision and WUO, as well as between

shared vision and WUT. The relationships between shared vision and WUE were both

significant using correlation analysis, but failed to remain so, in either group, after

regression analysis. Finally, the relationships between shared vision and PRUK were

significant in both groups using correlation analysis, but only remained significant for

positive referents after regression analysis. Therefore, hypothesis 10 was partially

supported in both groups.

Page 158: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  146  

4.5.4 Independent Variable 6 – Relationship Length

Hypothesis 11 stated that “relationship length will be positively related to trust” and

hypothesis 12 stated that “relationship length will be positively related to knowledge

sharing behavior”. Correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to test these

hypotheses.

Relationship Length and Trust

Correlation analysis was used to examine the bivariate relationships between relationship

length and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.52, the correlation

between relationship length and Otrust was .199 in Group 1 (p < .001) and not significant

in Group 2. The correlation between relationship length and ABtrust was not significant

in Group 1 and .147 in Group 2 (p < .05). The correlation between relationship length

and IBtrust was .195 in Group 1 (p < .001) and -.143 in Group 2 (p < .05). The

correlation between relationship length and BBtrust was .194 in Group 1 (p < .01) and

not significant in Group 2. The results of the correlation analysis suggested that the

longer the respondents knew the positive referents, the more Otrust, IBtrust, and BBtrust

they had in those individuals. The results were mixed in the second group, where the

longer respondents knew the negative referents, the more ABtrust and less IBtrust they

had in them.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Trust 0.199*** 252 -0.028 247 Ability-Based Trust 0.115 267 0.147* 262 Integrity-Based Trust 0.195*** 263 -0.143* 261 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.194** 264 -0.116 259

Table 4.52 Correlations Between Relationship Length and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

relationship length and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the

other independent variables. MRA of trust on relationship length, along with the

remaining independent variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+

β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+

Page 159: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  147  

β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap. The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust was discussed in

Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.53 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on relationship length and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated

for each of the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of relationship length

on each type of trust.

In Group 1, no significant trust and relationship length relationships resulted from the

MRA. For Group 2, the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust on relationship

length was -.196 (p < .05). Further, the standardized regression coefficient of IBtrust on

relationship length was -.197 (p < .05) and the standardized regression coefficient of

BBtrust on relationship length was -.190 (p < .05) (ABtrust was not significant). In this

group, the amount of variance in Otrust that was uniquely explained by relationship

length was 4%. The amount of variance in IBtrust that was uniquely explained by

relationship length was 4%, and the amount of variance in BBtrust that was uniquely

explained by relationship length was 4%.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that respondents had higher Otrust,

IBtrust, and BBtrust in those negative referents they had a shorter relationship length

with. This may also be thought in terms of trust decreasing, as relationship length

increased between the respondent and the person they did not work well with.

Page 160: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  148  

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Relationship Length only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust -0.010 -0.172 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.015 0.218 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.054 0.893 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.077 -1.246 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust -0.196* -2.582 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust -0.031 -0.391 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust -0.197* -2.500 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust -0.190* -2.370 206 0.124 3.629 0 Table 4.53 Regression of Trust on Relationship Length and Other Independent Variables

(*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001) In Group 1, the statistically significant relationships found between relationship length

and trust, using correlation analysis, were no longer found to be significant using

regression analysis. This was also the case for ABtrust in Group 2. On the other hand,

IBtrust was found to have a statistically significant negative relationship with

relationship length in Group 2, using both correlation and regression analysis. Otrust and

BBtrust had significant positive relationships with relationship length using correlation

analysis, but were found to have a significant negative relationship using MRA.

Therefore, hypothesis 11 was not supported by the results of the analysis in either group.

Interestingly, in Group 2 the MRA suggested partial support of the opposite effect.

Relationship Length and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was first used to examine the bivariate relationships between

relationship length and each of the knowledge sharing behaviors. As it can be seen in

Table 4.54, the correlation between relationship length and WSO was .164 (p < .01) in

Group 1 and not significant in Group 2. The correlation between relationship length and

WSE was .176 (p < .01) in Group 1 and not significant in Group 2. The correlation

between relationship length and WST was not significant in either group. No significant

correlations were found between relationship length and WUO, WUE, or WUT in either

group. Finally, the correlation between relationship length and PRUK was .140 (p < .05)

in Group 1 and not significant in Group 2.

Page 161: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  149  

The results of the correlation analysis suggested that the longer the respondents knew the

positive referents, the more willing they were to share knowledge (i.e. overall, tacit and

explicit) with them. The results also suggested that the longer the respondents knew the

positive referents, the more they perceived the knowledge received from them to be

useful. No significant relationships were found between relationship length and

knowledge sharing behaviors, with negative referents.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share 0.164** 264 -0.056 260 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.176** 267 -0.004 262 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.146 264 -0.060 262 Overall Willingness to Use 0.075 263 -0.013 259 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.099 267 0.024 265 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.071 266 -0.017 261 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.140* 262 -0.073 262

Table 4.54 Correlations Between Relationship Length and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

relationship length and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while

controlling for the effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing

behavior on relationship length, along with the remaining independent variables, took the

following form: KSB DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+

β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The

adjusted model R2 for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.55 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior dependent variables on relationship length and other independent variables. The

MRA was repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST,

WUO, WUE, WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect

of relationship length on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

Page 162: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  150  

The standardized regression coefficient of each of the KSBs on relationship length was

found to be not significant, in either group. Overall, the multiple regression analysis

indicated that, for both groups, there were no significant relationships between KSB and

relationship length.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Relationship Length only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.009 0.131 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.054 0.757 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) -0.012 -0.170 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use -0.034 -0.468 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.070 0.938 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -0.053 -0.721 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.025 0.376 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.046 0.572 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.092 1.129 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.024 0.296 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use -0.066 -0.838 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) -0.055 -0.685 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -0.063 -0.798 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.117 -1.484 189 0.247 6.151 0

Table 4.55 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Relationship Length and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

In Group 1, the statistically significant relationships, found using correlation analysis,

between KSB and relationship length were no longer significant using regression

analysis (i.e. WSO, WSE, PRUK). Neither correlation nor regression analysis found

significant relationships between relationship length and knowledge sharing behaviors, in

the second group. Therefore, hypothesis 12 was not supported by the results of the

analysis in either group.

4.5.5 Independent Variable 7 – Tie Strength

Hypothesis 13 stated that “tie strength will be positively related to trust” and hypothesis

14 stated that “tie strength will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior”.

Correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to test these hypotheses. Tie

Page 163: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  151  

strength was measured twice for each group, once examining the tie strength between the

respondent and the co-worker prior to the project or matter (i.e. TSp), and once more for

the tie strength between the two co-workers while on the project or matter (i.e. TSw).

Tie Strength and Trust

Correlation analysis was used to examine the bivariate relationships between tie strength

and each of the trust variables. As it can be seen in Table 4.56, the correlation between

TSp and Otrust was .283 in Group 1 (p < .001), and not significant in Group 2. The

correlation between TSw and Otrust was .502 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .148 in Group 2

(p < .05). After controlling for the type of trust, the correlation between TSp and ABtrust

was not significant, in either group. The correlation between TSw and ABtrust was .297 in

Group 1 (p < .001) and not significant in Group 2. The correlation between TSp and

IBtrust was .200 in Group 1 (p < .001) and not significant in Group 2. The correlation

between TSw and IBtrust was .423 in Group 1 (p < .001) and not significant in Group 2.

The correlation between TSp and BBtrust was .345 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .138 in

Group 2 (p < .05). Finally, the correlation between TSw and BBtrust was .533 in Group 1

(p < .001) and .165 in Group 2 (p < .01).

The results of the correlation analysis suggested that respondents had higher trust (Otrust,

IBtrust, and BBtrust) in those positive referents with whom they had higher TSp. In

addition, higher TSp between respondents and negative referents resulted in higher

BBtrust in those co-workers. The results of the correlation analysis also suggested that

respondents had higher trust (of all types) in those positive referents that they had more

TSw. For negative referents, higher TSw was found to positively influence Otrust and

BBtrust in that co-worker.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Trust Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Tie Strength – Prior to the project Overall Trust 0.283*** 249 0.108 246 Ability-Based Trust 0.115 264 0.061 261 Integrity-Based Trust 0.200*** 260 0.028 260 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.345*** 261 0.138* 258

Page 164: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  152  

Tie Strength – While on the project Overall Trust 0.502*** 249 0.148* 243 Ability-Based Trust 0.297*** 264 0.079 258 Integrity-Based Trust 0.423*** 260 0.066 257 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.533*** 261 0.165** 255

Table 4.56 Correlations Between Tie Strength and Trust Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

tie strength and each of the trust variables, while controlling for the effect of the other

independent variables. MRA of trust on tie strength, along with the remaining

independent variables, took the following form: Trust DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+

β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+

β11*GenGap. The adjusted model R2 for each type of trust was discussed in Section

4.5.1.1.

Table 4.57 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the trust dependent

variables on tie strength and other independent variables. The MRA was repeated for

each of the trust dependent variables (i.e. overall, ability-based, integrity-based, and

benevolence-based), which allowed the study to examine the effect of tie strength on each

type of trust.

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust on TSp was not significant.

However, in the same group, the standardized regression coefficient of BBtrust on TSp

was .207 (p < .01) (ABtrust and IBtrust were not significant in this group). For Group 1,

the standardized regression coefficient of Otrust on TSw was .251 (p < .001). In addition,

for this group, the standardized regression coefficient of IBtrust on TSw was .136 (p <

.05), and the standardized regression coefficient of BBtrust on TSw was .353 (p < .001)

(ABtrust was not significant). In this group, the amount of variance in BBtrust that was

uniquely explained by TSp was 4%. The amount of variance in Otrust that was uniquely

explained by TSw was 6%. The amount of variance in IBtrust that was uniquely

explained by TSw was 2%; and the amount of variance in BBtrust that was uniquely

explained by TSw was 13%. For Group 2, the MRA results suggested no statistically

significant relationships between tie strength and any of the trust variables.

Page 165: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  153  

Overall, the multiple regression analysis indicated that for both positive and negative

referents, respondents had higher BBtrust in those individuals they felt they had a

stronger tie strength with (prior to and while on the project). Respondents also had higher

Otrust and IBtrust in positive referents they felt they had stronger tie strength with, while

on the project or matter.

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Tie Strength (Prior to the Project) only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust 0.071 1.175 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust -0.080 -1.143 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust -0.027 -0.426 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust .207** 3.305 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust 0.044 0.550 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.005 0.058 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.025 0.313 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.052 0.661 206 0.124 3.629 0

Trust Dependent Variable(s)

β for Tie Strength (While on the project) only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Trust 0.251*** 4.267 195 0.527 20.657 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.073 1.070 205 0.336 10.369 0 Integrity-Based Trust 0.136* 2.264 203 0.489 18.560 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.353*** 5.909 204 0.471 17.458 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Trust 0.049 0.634 194 0.182 4.906 0 Ability-Based Trust 0.045 0.589 208 0.158 4.527 0 Integrity-Based Trust -0.041 -0.542 205 0.145 4.141 0 Benevolence-Based Trust 0.062 0.803 206 0.124 3.629 0

Table 4.57 Regression of Trust on Tie Strength (Prior to)/Tie Strength (While on) and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

The correlation analysis for Group 1 found significant relationships between TSp and

Otrust, IBtrust, and BBtrust. However, subsequent regression analysis for this group only

confirmed the relationship between TSp and BBtrust. In addition, the correlation analysis

for Group 1 showed statistically significant relationships between TSw and each of the

trust variables. Subsequent regression analysis confirmed the relationships between TSw

and Otrust; between TSw and IBtrust; and between TSw and BBtrust. The correlation

Page 166: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  154  

analysis for Group 2 showed statistically significant relationships between TSp and

BBtrust, that subsequent regression analysis did not confirm. Finally, the correlation

analysis for Group 2 showed statistically significant relationships between TSw and

Otrust, and between TSw and BBtrust. However, subsequent regression analysis did not

confirm either relationship in this group. Therefore, hypothesis 13 was partially

supported by the results of the analysis in Group 1 (both prior to and while on the

project) and not supported in Group 2.

Tie Strength and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Correlation analysis was used to examine the bivariate relationships between tie strength

and each of the knowledge sharing behaviors. As it can be seen in Table 4.58, the

correlation between TSp and WSO was .172 in Group 1 (p < .01) and .179 in Group 2 (p

< .01). The correlation between TSp and WSE was .154 in Group 1 (p < .05) and .148 in

Group 2 (p < .05). The correlation between TSp and WST was .164 in Group 1 (p < .01)

and .173 in Group 2 (p < .01). The correlation between TSp and WUO was .141 in Group

2 (p < .05) and not significant in Group 1. The correlation between TSp and WUE was

.197 in Group 2 (p < .05) and not significant in Group 1. The correlation between TSp

and WUT was .127 (p < .05) and not significant in Group 1. Finally, the correlation

between TSp and PRUK was .197 in Group 1 (p < .01) and not significant in Group 2.

The correlation between TSw and WSO was .373 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .148 in Group

2 (p < .05). The correlation between TSw and WSE was .321 in Group 1 (p < .001) and

.144 in Group 2 (p < .05). The correlation between TSw and WST was .359 in Group 1 (p

< .001) and .136 in Group 2 (p < .05). The correlation between TSw and WUO was .372

in Group 1 (p < .001) and .146 in Group 2 (p < .05). The correlation between TSw and

WUE was .301 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .150 in Group 2 (p < .05). The correlation

between TSw and WUT was .364 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .131 in Group 2 (p < .05).

Finally, the correlation between TSw and PRUK was .343 in Group 1 (p < .001) and .211

in Group 2 (p < .01).

Page 167: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  155  

The results of the correlation analysis suggested that respondents had a higher willingness

to share knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit) with individuals, in both groups, they had

higher tie strength with, prior to and while on the project or matter. The results of the

correlation analysis also suggested that respondents had a higher willingness to use

knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit) with negative referents they had higher tie strength

with, prior to the project or matter; and with co-workers from both groups they had

higher tie strength with, while on the project or matter. Finally, the results of the

correlation analysis suggested that respondents had a higher perception that knowledge

received from positive referents was useful, if they had higher tie strength with that co-

worker, prior to the project or matter. Higher tie strength with both negative and positive

referents, while on the project, also positively influenced the perception that the co-

worker knowledge was useful.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Tie Strength – Prior to the project Overall Willingness to Share 0.172** 260 0.179** 258 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.154* 263 0.148* 260 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.164** 260 0.173** 260 Overall Willingness to Use 0.082 260 0.141* 258 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.047 264 0.197** 264 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.088 263 0.127* 260 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.197** 260 0.09 261 Tie Strength – While on the project Overall Willingness to Share 0.373*** 261 0.148* 256 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.321*** 264 0.144* 258 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.359*** 261 0.136* 258 Overall Willingness to Use 0.372*** 261 0.146* 256 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.301*** 265 0.150* 262 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.364*** 264 0.131* 257 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.343*** 260 0.211** 258

Table 4.58 Correlations Between Tie Strength (Prior to) / Tie Strength (While on) and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

tie strength and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling for

Page 168: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  156  

the effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on tie

strength, along with the remaining independent variables, took the following form: KSB

DV = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+

β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The adjusted model R2

for each type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.59 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior dependent variables on tie strength and other independent variables. The MRA

was repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST, WUO,

WUE, WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect of tie

strength on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

For Group 1, the MRA showed no statistically significant relationships between TSp or

TSw and the knowledge sharing behavior variables. For Group 2, the standardized

regression coefficient of WSO on TSp was .179 (p < .05); and the standardized regression

coefficient of WST on TSp was .189 (p < .05) (WSE on TSp was not significant). For the

same group, the standardized regression coefficient of WUO on TSp was .178 (p < .05);

the standardized regression coefficient of WUE on TSp was .230 (p < .01); and the

standardized regression coefficient of WUT on TSp was .159 (p < .05). The MRA showed

no significant relationships between PRUK and TSp, in either group. In addition, for

Group 2, the standardized regression coefficient of PRUK on TSw was .189 (p < .05). The

MRA showed no other significant relationships between TSw and any other KSB

variable, in this group.

In Group 2, the amount of variance in WSO that was uniquely explained by TSp was 3%.

The amount of variance in WST that was uniquely explained by TSp was 4%. Next, the

amount of variance in WUO that was uniquely explained by TSp was 3%. The amount of

variance in WUE that was uniquely explained by TSp was 5%; and the amount of

variance in WUT that was uniquely explained by TSp was 3%. Finally, in this group, the

amount of variance in PRUK that was uniquely explained by TSw was 4%.

Page 169: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  157  

Overall, the multiple regression analysis results suggested no statistically significant

relationships to exist between TSp or TSw and knowledge sharing behaviors, with

positive referents. However, respondents did have higher WSO and WST with those

negative referents they felt they had a stronger TSp. Respondents also had a higher

willingness to use knowledge (overall, tacit and explicit) from negative referents, when

there were higher levels of TSp between them. Finally, respondents who had higher TSw

with negative referents also had a higher perception that the knowledge received from

those co-workers was useful.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Tie Strength (Prior to the project) only t N

Model Adj. R2 Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share -0.019 -0.280 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.086 -1.181 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.008 0.113 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use -0.082 -1.080 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) -0.135 -1.773 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -0.069 -0.898 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.026 0.383 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.179* 2.230 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.124 1.533 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) .189* 2.338 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use .178* 2.252 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) .230** 2.896 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) .159* 2.004 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.005 0.067 189 0.247 6.151 0

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Tie Strength (While on the project) only T N

Model Adj. R2 Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Positive Referents Overall Willingness to Share 0.036 0.520 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.078 1.059 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.018 0.251 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.030 0.401 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) -0.065 -0.860 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.037 0.492 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge -0.114 -1.653 189 0.434 13.030 0

Page 170: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  158  

Group 2: Negative Referents Overall Willingness to Share -0.109 -1.407 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) -0.066 -0.842 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) -0.118 -1.502 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use -0.029 -0.380 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) -0.003 -0.039 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) -0.043 -0.558 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge .189* 2.517 189 0.247 6.151 0 Table 4.59 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Tie Strength (Prior to) / Tie

Strength (While on) and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001) The correlation analysis for both groups showed significant relationships between TSp /

TSw and willingness to share knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit). Subsequent

regression analysis confirmed statistically significant relationships, with negative

referents, between WSO and TSp, and between WST and TSp. The correlation analysis

also found statistically significant relationships with negative referents between TSp /

TSw and willingness to use knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit), and with positive

referents between WUO and TSw. Subsequent regression analysis confirmed relationships

between willingness to use knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit) and TSp with negative

referents. Finally, the correlation analysis found statistically significant relationships

between TSp and PRUK with positive referents, and between TSw and PRUK in both

groups. Subsequent regression analysis confirmed a statistically significant relationship

between PRUK and TSw with negative referents. Based on these results, hypothesis 14

was not supported for Group 1, either for tie strength prior to or tie strength while on the

project or matter. However, the hypothesis was partially supported for Group 2, both for

tie strength prior to and for tie strength while on the project or matter.

4.5.6 Independent Variable 8 – Overall Trust

Overall Trust and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Hypothesis 15 stated that “trust will be positively related to knowledge sharing

behavior”. Correlation and multiple regression analysis were used to test this hypothesis.

Correlation analysis was used to examine the bivariate relationships between Otrust and

each of the knowledge sharing behaviors. As it can be seen in Table 4.60, the correlation

between Otrust and WSO was .541 in Group 1 and .302 in Group 2. The correlation

Page 171: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  159  

between Otrust and WSE was .469 in Group 1 and .266 in Group ,2 and between Otrust

and WST was .530 in Group 1 and .298 in Group 2. The correlation between Otrust and

WUO was .507 in Group 1 and .468 in Group 2. The correlation between Otrust and

WUE was .491 in Group 1 and .444 in Group 2, and between Otrust and WUT was .485

in Group 1 and .445 in Group 2. Finally, the correlation between Otrust and PRUK was

.586 in Group 1 and .482 in Group 2. All correlations, in both groups, had p < .001. The

results of the correlation analysis suggested that the higher trust respondents had in co-

workers from both groups, the more effective knowledge sharing behaviors were likely to

take place with those co-workers.

Group 1: Positive

Referents Group 2: Negative

Referents Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s) Correlation N Correlation N Overall Willingness to Share 0.541*** 244 0.302*** 240 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.469*** 247 0.266*** 242 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.530*** 244 0.298*** 241 Overall Willingness to Use 0.507*** 244 0.468*** 238 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.491*** 248 0.444*** 244 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.485*** 247 0.445*** 240 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge 0.586*** 244 0.482*** 242

Table 4.60 Correlations Between Overall Trust and Knowledge Sharing Behavior Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Multiple regression analysis (MRA) was then used to examine the relationships between

Otrust and each of the knowledge sharing behavior variables, while controlling for the

effect of the other independent variables. MRA of knowledge sharing behavior on Otrust,

along with the remaining independent variables, took the following form: KSB DV =

β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+

β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust. The adjusted model R2 for each

type of KSB was discussed in Section 4.5.1.1.

Table 4.61 shows the results of the multiple regression analysis of the knowledge sharing

behavior dependent variables on Otrust and other independent variables. The MRA was

repeated for each of the KSB dependent variables (i.e. WSO, WSE, WST, WUO, WUE,

Page 172: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  160  

WUT, PRUK), which allowed the study to individually examine the effect of Otrust on

each of the knowledge sharing behaviors.

For Group 1, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO on Otrust was .178 (p <

.05). The standardized regression coefficient of WST on Otrust was .172 (p < .05) (WSE

on Otrust was found to be not significant). For the same group, the standardized

regression coefficient of WUO on Otrust was .308 (p < .001). The standardized

regression coefficient of WUE on Otrust was .461 (p < .001); and the standardized

regression coefficient of WST on Otrust was .266 (p < .01). Finally, in Group 1, the

standardized regression coefficient of PRUK on Otrust was .309 (p < .001).

In Group 1, the amount of variance in WSO that was uniquely explained by Otrust was

3%. The amount of variance in WST that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 3%.

Next, the amount of variance in WUO that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 10%.

The amount of variance in WUE that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 21%; and the

amount of variance in WUT that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 7%. Finally, in

this group, the amount of variance in PRUK that was uniquely explained by Otrust was

10%.

For Group 2, the standardized regression coefficient of WSO on Otrust was .211 (p <

.01). The standardized regression coefficient of WSE on Otrust was .226 (p < .01); and

the standardized regression coefficient of WST on Otrust was .194 (p < .05). For the same

group, the standardized regression coefficient of WUO on Otrust was .306 (p < .001).

The standardized regression coefficient of WUE on Otrust was .359 (p < .001); and the

standardized regression coefficient of WST on Otrust was .277 (p < .001). Finally, the

standardized regression coefficient of PRUK on Otrust was .462 (p < .001).

In Group 2, the amount of variance in WSO that was uniquely explained by Otrust was

5%. The amount of variance in WSE that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 5%; and

the amount of variance in WST that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 4%. Next, the

amount of variance in WUO that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 9%. The amount

Page 173: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  161  

of variance in WUE that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 13%; and the amount of

variance in WUT that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 8%. Finally, in Group 2, the

amount of variance in PRUK that was uniquely explained by Otrust was 21%.

Overall, the multiple regression analysis suggested that, for positive referents,

respondents had a higher WSO and WST with those co-workers they had a higher Otrust

in. A higher Otrust in positive referents also related to a higher willingness to use

knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit) from them. Additionally, higher Otrust in the

positive referent positively related to a higher perception that the knowledge received

from those co-workers was useful. The MRA also showed that, for negative referents,

respondents had a higher willingness to share knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit) with

those co-workers they had a higher Otrust in. A higher Otrust in those same individuals

also related to a higher willingness to use knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit) from

them. Finally, a higher Otrust by respondents in negative referents positively related to a

higher perception that the knowledge received from those individuals was useful.

Knowledge Sharing Behavior Dependent Variable(s)

β for Overall Trust only t N

Model Adj. R2

Model F

Model Sig.

Group 1: Person they worked best with Overall Willingness to Share 0.178* 2.139 189 0.424 12.540 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.153 1.720 192 0.329 8.789 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.172* 2.029 189 0.399 11.384 0 Overall Willingness to Use 0.308*** 3.401 188 0.316 8.185 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) 0.461*** 5.017 192 0.288 7.444 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) 0.266** 2.921 190 0.298 7.674 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge .309*** 3.790 189 0.434 13.030 0 Group 2: Person they did not work well with Overall Willingness to Share 0.211** 2.848 189 0.203 4.996 0 Willingness to Share (Explicit) 0.226** 3.017 191 0.177 4.416 0 Willingness to Share (Tacit) 0.194* 2.586 189 0.186 4.574 0 Overall Willingness to Use .306*** 4.276 186 0.277 6.896 0 Willingness to Use (Explicit) .359*** 4.901 191 0.215 5.328 0 Willingness to Use (Tacit) .277*** 3.845 187 0.266 6.613 0 Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge .462*** 6.413 189 0.247 6.151 0

Table 4.61 Regression of Knowledge Sharing Behavior on Overall Trust and Other Independent Variables (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Page 174: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  162  

In both groups, the correlation analysis suggested statistically significant relationships

between Otrust and all the knowledge sharing behaviors variables. Subsequent regression

analysis confirmed the correlation analysis in all cases for Group 2, and all cases except

Otrust and WSE in Group 1. Therefore, hypothesis 15 was partially supported in Group 1

and fully supported in Group 2.

4.5.7 Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Knowledge Sharing

Behavior

Hypothesis 16 stated that “overall trust and social-cognitive factors explain knowledge

sharing behavior.” The goal of testing this hypothesis was to assess the collective effect

of the overall model (including Otrust) on knowledge sharing behaviors, in both groups.

The hypothesis was tested for each KSB, in both groups, using regression analysis.

Tables 4.62 and 4.63 summarize the collective model results for each of the knowledge

sharing behaviors in both groups. For each knowledge sharing behavior, the tables

present significant independent variables (based on their β values), β significance,

adjusted R2 for the model, model F value, model significance, and sample size. The semi-

partial coefficient squared (sr2) for each variable is discussed below.

Since KSB was conceptualized in the study as a composite of three distinct knowledge

sharing behaviors, the findings are divided and presented in three sections, consistent

with the three distinct knowledge behaviors: willingness to share knowledge, willingness

to use knowledge, and perceived receipt of useful knowledge.

Group 1 Significant Independent Variables Dependent Variables -

KSB Age β Gen β S.Lan β S.Vis β O.Trust β

Age Diff β

Gender Gap β

Model Adj. R2

F Value Sig. N

KSB - WSO .131* .320*** .240** .178* 0.424 12.54 0 189 KSB - WSE .146* .325*** 0.329 8.789 0 192 KSB - WST .294*** .260** .172* 0.399 11.384 0 189 KSB - WUO .201** .265** .308** 0.316 8.185 0 188 KSB - WUE .138* .461*** .143* 0.288 7.444 0 192 KSB - WUT .234** .282** .266** -.144* 0.298 7.674 0 190 KSB - PRUK .385*** .309*** 0.434 13.03 0 189

Table 4.62 Group 1 MRA Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

 

Page 175: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  163  

Group 2 Significant Independent Variables Dependent Variables

- KSB Age β Gen β

T.S.P. β

T.S.P. β

S.Lan β S.Vis β

O.Trust β

Gender Gap β

Model Adj. R2

F Value Sig N

KSB - WSO -.229** .179* .222** .211** 0.203 4.996 0 189 KSB - WSE -.212** .146* .165* .226** 0.177 4.416 0 191 KSB – WST -.222** .189* .227** .194* 0.186 4.574 0 189 KSB - WUO .162* .178* .312*** .306*** 0.277 6.896 0 186 KSB - WUE .179* .230** .359*** 0.215 5.328 0 191 KSB - WUT .140* .159* .348*** .277*** 0.266 6.613 0 187 KSB - PRUK .189* .462*** .148* 0.247 6.151 0 189

Table 4.63 Group 2 MRA Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

4.5.7.1 Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Willingness to Share

Knowledge

The collective effect of all the independent variables (including overall trust) on

willingness to share knowledge, in both groups, is presented in Table 4.64. Using MRA,

the adjusted model R2 for WSO in the first group was found to be .424, with an F value of

12.54. The Group 1 model had p < .001 and a sample size of 189. Significant

independent variables included gender (β of .131 and sr2 of 2%), shared language (β of

.320 and sr2 of 10%), shared vision (β of .240 and sr2 of 6%), and Otrust (β of .178 and

sr2 of 3%). All βs had p < .05. In Group 2, the adjusted model R2 for WSO was .203 with

an F value of 4.996. The Group 2 model had p < .001 and a sample size of 189.

Significant independent variables included age (β of -.229 and sr2 of 5%), TSp (β of .179

and sr2 of 3%), shared vision (β of .222 and sr2 of 5%), and Otrust (β of .211 and sr2 of

5%). All βs had p < .05.

Page 176: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  164  

Overall Willingness to Share Knowledge (Group 1)

Overall Willingness to Share Knowledge (Group 2)

Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N 0.424 12.54 0 189 0.203 4.996 0 189

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Gender .131 .05 2% Age -.229 .01 5% Shared Language .320 .001 10% Tie Strength (Prior) .179 .05 3% Shared Vision .240 .01 6% Shared Vision .222 .01 5% Overall Trust .178 .05 3% Overall Trust .211 .01 5%

Table 4.64 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Overall Willingness to Share Knowledge in Groups 1 and 2

The collective effect of the overall model on WSE and WST, in both groups, is presented

in Tables 4.65 (Group 1) and 4.66 (Group 2). In Group 1, the adjusted model R2 for WSE

was .329 with an F value of 8.789 (Table 4.65). The Group 1 WSE model had p < .001

and a sample size of 192. Significant independent variables included gender (β of .146

and sr2 of 2%) and shared language (β of .325 and sr2 of 11%). All βs had p < .05. For the

same group, the adjusted model R2 for WST was .399 with an F value of 11.384. The

Group 1 WST model had p < .001 and a sample size of 189. Significant independent

variables included shared language (β of .294 and sr2 of 9%), shared vision (β of .260

and sr2 of 7%), and Otrust (β of .172 and sr2 of 3%). All βs had p < .05.

Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge

(Group 1) Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge

(Group 1) Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N

0.329 8.789 0 192 0.399 11.384 0 189 Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Gender .146 .05 2% Shared Language .294 .001 9% Shared Language .325 .001 11% Shared Vision .260 .01 7% Overall Trust .172 .05 3%

Table 4.65 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Willingness to Share Explicit and Tacit Forms of Knowledge in Group 1

In Group 2, the adjusted model R2 for WSE was .177 with an F value of 4.416 (Table

4.66). The Group 2 WSE model had p < .001 and a sample size of 191. Significant

independent variables included age (β of -.212 and sr2 of 5%), shared language (β of .146

and sr2 of 2%), shared vision (β of .165 and sr2 of 3%), and Otrust (β of .226 and sr2 of

5%). All βs had p < .05. For the same group, the adjusted model R2 for WST was .186,

with an F value of 4.574. The Group 2 WST model had p < .001 and a sample size of 189.

Significant independent variables included age (β of -.222 and sr2 of 5%), TSp (β of .189

Page 177: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  165  

and sr2 of 4%), shared vision (β of .227 and sr2 of 5%), and Otrust (β of .194 and sr2 of

4%). All βs had p <.05.

Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge

(Group 2) Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge

(Group 2) Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N

0.177 4.416 0 191 0.186 4.574 0 189 Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Age -.212 .01 5% Age -.222 .01 5% Shared Language .146 .05 2% Tie Strength (Prior) .189 .05 4% Shared Vision .165 .05 3% Shared Vision .227 .01 5% Overall Trust .226 .01 5% Overall Trust .194 .01 4%

Table 4.66 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Willingness to Share Explicit and Tacit Forms of Knowledge in Group 2

4.5.7.2 Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Willingness to Use

Knowledge

Next, the analysis looked at the collective effect of all the independent variables on

willingness to use knowledge, in both groups. Table 4.67 presents the results of the MRA

for WUO. In Group 1, the adjusted model R2 for WUO was .316, with an F value of

8.185. The Group 1 model had p < .001 and a sample size of 188. Significant

independent variables included age (β of .201 and sr2 of 4%), shared vision (β of .265 and

sr2 of 7%), and Otrust (β of .308 and sr2 of 10%). All βs had p < .05. In Group 2, the

adjusted model R2 for WUO was .277, with an F value of 6.896. The Group 2 model had

p < .001 and a sample size of 186. Significant independent variables included gender (β

of .162 and sr2 of 3%), TSp (β of .178 and sr2 of 3%), shared vision (β of .312 and sr2 of

10%), and Otrust (β of .306 and sr2 of 9%). All βs had p < .05.

Overall Willingness to Use Knowledge

(Group 1) Overall Willingness to Use Knowledge

(Group 2)

Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N Model Adj. R2 F Value Model

Sig. N 0.316 8.185 0 188 0.277 6.896 0 186

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Gender .201 .01 4% Gender .162 .05 3% Shared Vision .265 .01 7% Tie Strength (Prior) .178 .05 3% Overall Trust .308 .01 10% Shared Vision .312 .001 10% Overall Trust .306 .001 9% Table 4.67 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Overall Willingness to Use

Knowledge in Groups 1 and 2

Page 178: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  166  

The collective effect of the overall model on WUE and WUT, in both groups is presented

in Tables 4.68 (Group 1) and 4.69 (Group 2). In Group 1, the adjusted model R2 for WUE

was .288, with an F value of 7.444 (Table 4.68). The Group 1 WUE model had p < .001

and a sample size of 192. Significant independent variables included gender (β of .138

and sr2 of 2%), Otrust (β of .461 and sr2 of 21%), and gender gap (β of .143 and sr2 of

2%). All βs had p < .05. For the same group, the adjusted model R2 for WUT was .298,

with an F value of 7.674. The Group 1 WUT model had p < .001 and a sample size of

190. Significant independent variables included age (β of .234 and sr2 of 6%), shared

vision (β of .282 and sr2 of 8%), Otrust (β of .266 and sr2 of 7%), and age difference (β of

-.144 and sr2 of 2%). All βs had p < .05.

Willingness to Use Explicit Knowledge (Group 1)

Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge (Group 1)

Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N 0.288 7.444 0 192 0.298 7.674 0 190

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Gender .138 .05 2% Age .234 .01 6% Overall Trust .461 .001 21% Shared Vision .282 .01 8% Gender Gap .143 .05 2% Overall Trust .266 .01 7% Age Difference -.144 .05 2%

Table 4.68 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Willingness to Use Explicit and Tacit Forms of Knowledge in Group 1

In Group 2, the adjusted model R2 for WUE was .215, with an F value of 5.328 (Table

4.69). The Group 2 WUE model had p < .001 and a sample size of 191. Significant

independent variables included gender (β of .179 and sr2 of 3%), TSp (β of .230 and sr2 of

5%), and Otrust (β of .359 and sr2 of 13%). All βs had p < .05. For the same group, the

adjusted model R2 for WUT was .266, with an F value of 6.613. The Group 2 WUT model

had p < .001 and a sample size of 187. Significant independent variables included gender

(β of .140 and sr2 of 2%), TSp (β of .159 and sr2 of 3%), shared vision (β of .348 and sr2

of 12%), and Otrust (β of .277 and sr2 of 8%). All βs had p < .05.

Page 179: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  167  

Willingness to Use Explicit Knowledge (Group 2)

Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge (Group 2)

Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N Model Adj. R2 F Value Model

Sig. N 0.215 5.328 0 191 0.266 6.613 0 187

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Gender .179 .05 3% Gender .140 .05 2% Tie Strength (Prior) .230 .01 5% Tie Strength (Prior) .159 .05 3% Overall Trust .359 .001 13% Shared Vision .348 .001 12% Overall Trust .277 .001 8%

Table 4.69 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Willingness to Use Explicit and Tacit Forms of Knowledge in Group 2

4.5.7.3 Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Perceived Receipt of

Useful Knowledge

Finally, the analysis looked at the collective effect of all of the independent variables on

the perceived receipt of useful knowledge, in both groups. Table 4.70 presents the results

of the MRA for PRUK. In Group 1, the adjusted model R2 for PRUK was .434, with an F

value of 13.03. The Group 1 model had p < .001 and a sample size of 189. Significant

independent variables included shared vision (β of .385 and sr2 of 15%) and Otrust (β of

.309 and sr2 of 10%). In Group 2, the adjusted model R2 for PRUK was .247, with an F

value of 6.151. The Group 2 model had p < .001 and a sample size of 189. Significant

independent variables included TSw (β of .189 and sr2 of 4%), Otrust (β of .462 and sr2 of

21%), and gender gap (β of .148 and sr2 of 2%). All βs, in both groups, had p < .05.

Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

(Group 1) Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge

(Group 2)

Model Adj. R2 F Value Model Sig. N Model Adj. R2 F Value Model

Sig. N 0.434 13.03 0 189 0.247 6.151 0 189

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Sig. Independent Variables β β Sig. sr2

Shared Vision .385 .001 15% Tie Strength (While) .189 .05 4% Overall Trust .309 .001 10% Overall Trust .462 .001 21% Gender Gap .148 .05 2%

Table 4.70 Significant IV βs and Collective Model Results on Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge in Groups 1 and 2

Multiple regression analysis showed that overall trust and social-cognitive factors

explained each of the knowledge sharing behaviors identified in the study, with both

positive and negative referents. However, considerably more of knowledge sharing

Page 180: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  168  

behaviors could be explained by the identified IVs in relationships with positive

referents. In either case, hypothesis 16 was fully supported in both groups.

4.5.8 Mediating Effect of Overall Trust

Hypothesis 17 stated that “overall trust will be a mediating variable between social-

cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior.” The theoretical model identified the

role of trust as an intervening or mediating variable, where m mediates a relationship

between x and y (i.e. x → m → y). The independent variables (i.e. social-cognitive factors

x) were predicted to affect the dependent variables (i.e. knowledge sharing behaviors y)

indirectly, through the intervening variable (i.e. overall trust m). This would mean that

social-cognitive factor (SCF) variables (x) have a direct effect on knowledge sharing

behavior (KSB) (y) and a direct effect on overall trust (m); while overall trust (m) also

has a direct effect on knowledge sharing behavior (y). An intervening variable “delineates

the causal mechanisms producing the observed relationship between the focal

independent [i.e. social-cognitive factors] and dependent [i.e. knowledge sharing

behaviors] variables” (Aneshensel, 2002, p. 155). The intervening variable plays two

analytic roles: it is the dependent variable affected by the independent variables

(displayed in connection A in Figure 4.1) and the independent variable affected by the

dependent variables (displayed in connection B in Figure 4.1). Since it must serve a

connective function, both these connections are necessary for Otrust to be confirmed as a

mediating variable. Specifying Otrust as a meditating variable also suggests that the

connection between SCF and KSB is thought to be indirect (i.e. SCFs influences Otrust,

which in turn influences KSBs).

 Figure 4.1 The Focal Relationship and Trust as a Mediating Variable

In addition to an indirect relationship, a mediating variable also has an effect on the direct

relationship that remains between the independent and dependent variables, after the

Page 181: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  169  

indirect relationship is taken into account. Tabachnick and Fidell (2007) called this

relationship (between the IV and DV after “controlling for” the mediator) the direct

effect.

According to Baron and Kenny (1986), four conditions are necessary for a variable to be

confirmed as a mediator:

1. There must be a significant relationship between the IV and the DV;

2. There must be a significant relationship between the IV and the mediator;

3. The mediator must still predict the DV after controlling for the IV and;

4. The relationship between the IV and the DV is reduced when the mediator is in

the equation.

If Hypothesis 17 was to be found valid, then the inclusion of the Otrust variable should

account for some or all of the relationship between SCF and KSB (connection C in

Figure 4.1). Aneshensel (2002) argued that an increase in the indirect component

decreased the direct component. A reduction in, or elimination of, the relationship

between the independent variables (SCF) and the dependent variables (KSB) means that a

part or all of the relationship is explained through the mediating variable (i.e. Otrust). If

the inclusion of Otrust entirely eliminates the relationship between IV and DV, the

relationship is said to be ‘fully elaborated’ and the mediation is said to be perfect (‘full’

or ‘complete’). “This pattern of results tends to occur because the dependent variable is

more proximal to the intervening variable than the focal independent variable and

because proximal effects tend to be stronger than distal effects” (Aneshensel, 2002, p.

164). However, if the inclusion of Otrust into the model only partially accounts for the

relationship, it is said to be ‘partially elaborated’, and the mediation is said to be

‘partial’. In these cases, the results would explain only part of the theory, leaving other

portions unexplained and directing attention to further development of theory. Finally, if

the inclusion of Otrust leaves the focal IV-DV relationship intact, it is said to be

‘unelaborated’. In these cases, it could be ruled out that Otrust is a mediating variable.

Page 182: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  170  

To test the mediating effect of Otrust, hierarchical multiple regression analysis (HMRA)

was used. In the first stage of the HMRA the knowledge sharing behavior variables were

regressed on the independent variables. In this first step, Otrust was excluded from the

model (i.e. KSB = β1*Age+ β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+

β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+ β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust). However,

in the second step of the HMRA, the mediating variable, Otrust, was included as an

independent variable, along with the other independent variables (i.e. KSB = β1*Age+

β2*Edu+ β3*Gen+ β4*R.Len+ β5*T.S.p+ β6*T.S.w+ β7*S.Lan+ β8*S.Vis+ β9*AgeDiff+

β10*EduGap+ β11*GenGap+ β12*O.Trust). The contribution of Otrust as a mediating

variable to the explanation of KSB (y) was assessed using F tests for the increment in R2.

If significant, then βetas and t-tests for each of the individual variables were used.

The focus of the analysis is on what happens to the regression coefficient for the focal

relationships (SCF-KSB), when the mediating variable (Otrust) is added to the model.

Mediation is said to take place when the β of the IV-DV relationship decreases. The

mediation is said to be complete if the focal relationship is no longer significant, after

Otrust is introduced into the model. A partial mediation would still show a statistically

significant relationship, but would have a reduced β, as Otrust explains only part of the

relationship. Finally, the focal relationship is not found to be mediated by Otrust if the

regression coefficient for the relationship is essentially unchanged with the addition of

the mediating variable.

4.5.8.1 Mediating Effect of Overall Trust Results for Positive Referents (Group 1)

Figure 4.2 summarizes the statistically significant relationships found by testing

hypotheses 1-15 for positive referents (i.e. Group 1 or the co-worker the respondent

worked best with). As noted by Baron and Kenny (1986), there must be a significant

relationship between the IV and the DV and between the IV and the mediating variable,

to confirm a variable as a mediator. Of all of the significant relationships in Figure 4.2,

shared language and shared vision were the only two variables that satisfied the first two

conditions set out by Baron and Kenny (1986). Homophily variables, relationship length,

Page 183: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  171  

and tie strength variables did not meet the criterion of being mediating variables, as they

had no significant relationships with either Otrust or KSB.

 Figure 4.2 Significant Relationships and Corresponding Hypotheses for Positive

Referents (Group 1) The final two conditions suggested by Baron and Kenny (1986) to confirm Otrust as a

mediator were tested using a 2-step hierarchical multiple regression analysis. In the

second model (step 2), the effect of Otrust was considered by adding it to the HMRA

Step 1 model, which included the rest of the IVs. The complete results of the HMRA for

positive referents appear in Table 4.71 and Table 4.72.

The results of the analysis showed that Otrust still predicted KSB. Further, the results

showed that the relationships between shared language and KSB, and between shared

vision and KSB, weakened when Otrust was introduced into the model. These results

suggested that Otrust, at least partially, mediated the relationships between shared

language and KSB, as well as between shared vision and KSB. The specifics of these

mediated relationships are discussed in the sections below.

Page 184: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  172  

Table 4.71 Results of the HMRA for Positive Referents (Group 1) (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

  HMRA Model Change Summary

Change R2 F Change Sig. F Change KSB - WSO 0.014 4.576 0.034 KSB - WSE 0.01 2.957 0.087 KSB - WST 0.013 4.117 0.044 KSB - WUO 0.042 11.57 0.001 KSB - WUE 0.094 25.166 0 KSB - WUT 0.032 8.532 0.004 KSB - PRUK 0.043 14.365 0

Table 4.72 HMRA Model Change Summary for Positive Referents (Group 1) Group 1: The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Language and KSB

S. Language Model Summary Step 1 β T Adj. R2 F Value Sig N KSB - WSO 0.366 5.118*** 0.413 13.002 0 189 KSB - WSE 0.364 4.747*** 0.321 8.789 0 192 KSB - WST 0.339 4.638*** 0.388 11.837 0 189 KSB - WUO 0.127 1.590 0.274 7.431 0 188 KSB - WUE 0.213 2.541* 0.193 5.142 0 192 KSB - WUT 0.106 1.330 0.268 7.287 0 190 KSB - PRUK 0.204 2.777** 0.392 12.003 0 189

Page 185: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  173  

Step 2 β T Adj. R2 F Value Sig N Change R2 F Change Sig. F

Change KSB - WSO 0.32 4.330*** 0.424 12.540 0 189 0.014 4.576 0.034 KSB - WSE 0.325 4.074*** 0.329 8.789 0 192 0.01 2.957 0.087 KSB - WST 0.294 3.892*** 0.399 11.384 0 189 0.013 4.117 0.044 KSB - WUO 0.047 0.584 0.316 8.185 0 188 0.042 11.57 0.001 KSB - WUE 0.095 1.156 0.288 7.444 0 192 0.094 25.166 0 KSB - WUT 0.036 0.438 0.298 7.674 0 190 0.032 8.532 0.004 KSB - PRUK 0.123 1.667 0.434 13.030 0 189 0.043 14.365 0

Table 4.73 Group 1 HMRA Results for Shared Language on KSB (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

The Group 1 HMRA results for shared language (on KSB) are shown in Table 4.73 and

summarized below. Otrust was introduced into the model in Step 2 of the HMRA.

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Language and Willingness to

Share Knowledge

The addition of Otrust made a small contribution to the explanation of WSO, as indicated

by the increment of R2 from the Step 1 Model (WSO ΔR2 = 3.3%; p < .001; WSE ΔR2 =

3%; p < .001; WST ΔR2 = 3.3%; p < .001). The addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient

for shared language (on WSO) by 12.6% (Δβ = .366 - .320). Mediation was present, but

only partially, as shared language remained statistically associated with WSO. The

results also showed that the addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient for shared

language with WSE by 10.7% (Δβ = .364 - .325) and with WST by 13.3% (Δβ = .339-

.294). In both these cases the mediation was present, but only partially, as relationships

between shared language and WSE and WST remained statistically significant.

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Language and Willingness to Use

Knowledge

The addition of Otrust made a substantial contribution to the explanation of WUO, as

indicated by the increment of R2 from the Step 1 Model (WSO ΔR2 = 13.3%; p < .001;

WUE ΔR2 = 32.6%; p < .001; WUT ΔR2 = 10.7%; p < .001).

The coefficient for shared language was found to be not significantly associated with

WUO in the first model, or in the second, when Otrust was introduced. However, it was

found that the addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient for shared language with WUE

Page 186: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  174  

by 55.4% (Δβ = .213-.095) and, more interestingly, was no longer statistically significant.

This suggested that Otrust had a complete mediating effect for the relationship between

shared language and WUE. The coefficient for shared language was found to be not

significantly associated with WUT in either model.

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Language and Perceived Receipt

of Useful Knowledge

Otrust made a moderate contribution to the explanation of PRUK, as indicated by the

increment of R2 from the Step 1 Model (PRUK ΔR2 = 9.9%; p < .001).

The addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient for shared language (on PRUK) by 39.7%

(Δβ = .204-.123). Further, because shared language was no longer statistically significant

after Otrust was introduced into the model, it can be concluded that Otrust had a

complete mediating effect for the relationship between shared language and PRUK.

Group 1: The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and KSB

S. Vision Model Summary

Step 1 β T Adj. R2 F Value Sig N KSB - WSO 0.307 4.314*** 0.413 13.002 0 189 KSB - WSE 0.210 2.758** 0.321 8.789 0 192 KSB - WST 0.325 4.474*** 0.388 11.837 0 189 KSB - WUO 0.382 4.855*** 0.274 7.431 0 188 KSB - WUE 0.248 2.986** 0.193 5.142 0 192 KSB - WUT 0.382 4.858*** 0.268 7.287 0 190 KSB - PRUK 0.500 6.877*** 0.392 12.003 0 189

Step 2 β T Adj. R2 F Value Sig N Change R2 F Change Sig. F

Change KSB - WSO 0.24 3.106** 0.424 12.540 0 189 0.014 4.576 0.034 KSB - WSE 0.153 1.851 0.329 8.789 0 192 0.01 2.957 0.087 KSB - WST 0.260 3.293** 0.399 11.384 0 189 0.013 4.117 0.044 KSB - WUO 0.265 3.169** 0.316 8.185 0 188 0.042 11.57 0.001 KSB - WUE 0.072 0.846 0.288 7.444 0 192 0.094 25.166 0 KSB - WUT 0.282 3.358** 0.298 7.674 0 190 0.032 8.532 0.004 KSB - PRUK 0.385 5.049*** 0.434 13.030 0 189 0.043 14.365 0

Table 4.74 Group 1 HMRA Results for Shared Vision (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

The Group 1 HMRA results for shared vision (on KSB) are shown in Table 4.74 and

summarized below. Otrust was introduced into the model in Step 2 of the HMRA.

Page 187: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  175  

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and Willingness to Share

Knowledge

The addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient for shared vision (on WSO) by 21.8% (Δβ

= .307-.240). Mediation was present, but only partially, as shared vision remained

statistically associated with WSO. The results also showed that the addition of Otrust

reduced the coefficient for shared vision with WSE by 27.1% (Δβ = .210-.153) and with

WST by 19.2% (Δβ = .325-.260). In the case of WSE, shared vision was no longer

statistically significant after Otrust was introduced into the model. This suggested that

Otrust had a complete mediating effect for the relationship between shared vision and

WSE. For WST, the mediation was present, but only partially, as the relationship between

shared vision and WST remained statistically significant after Otrust was introduced into

the model.

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and Willingness to Use

Knowledge

The addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient for shared vision (on WUO) by 30.6% (Δβ

= .382-.265). Mediation was present, but only partially, as shared vision remained

statistically associated with WUO. The results also showed that the addition of Otrust

reduced the coefficient for shared vision with WUE by 71% (Δβ = .248-.072) and with

WUT by 26.2% (Δβ = .382-.282). In the case of WUE, shared vision was no longer

statistically significant after Otrust was introduced into the model. This suggested that

Otrust had a complete mediating effect for the relationship between shared vision and

WUE. For WUT, the mediation was present, but only partially, as the relationship

between shared vision and WUT remained statistically significant after Otrust was

introduced into the model.

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and Perceived Receipt of

Useful Knowledge

The addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient for shared vision (on PRUK) by 23% (Δβ

= .500-.385). Mediation was present, but only partially, as the relationship between

Page 188: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  176  

shared vision and PRUK remained statistically significant after Otrust was introduced

into the model.

4.5.8.2 The Mediating Effect of Overall Trust Results for Negative Referents (Group 2)

Figure 4.3 summarizes the statistically significant relationships found by testing

hypotheses 1-15 for negative referents (i.e. Group 2 or the co-workers the respondent did

not work well with). As noted by Baron and Kenny (1986), there must be a significant

relationship between the IV and the DV, and between the IV and the mediating variable,

to confirm a variable as a mediator. Of all of the significant relationships in Figure 4.3,

shared vision was the only variable that satisfied the first two conditions set out by Baron

and Kenny (1986) (i.e. homophily measures, shared language, relationship length, and

tie strength variables did not have significant relationships with either Otrust or KSB).

 Figure 4.3 Significant Relationships and Corresponding Hypotheses for Negative

Referents (Group 2)

Page 189: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  177  

The complete results of the HMRA for Group 2 appear in Table 4.75 and Table 4.76. The

results of the analysis showed that Otrust still predicted KSB. Further, the results showed

that the relationship between shared vision and KSB weakened when Otrust was

introduced into the model. These results suggested that Otrust, at least partially, mediated

the relationship between shared vision and KSB with negative referents. The specifics of

these mediated relationships are discussed in the sections below.

Table 4.75 Results of the HMRA for Negative Referents (Group 2) (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

HMRA Model Change Summary Change R2 F Change Sig. F Change

KSB - WSO 0.034 8.112 0.005 KSB - WSE 0.039 9.103 0.003 KSB - WST 0.029 6.685 0.011 KSB - WUO 0.071 18.284 0 KSB - WUE 0.099 24.023 0 KSB - WUT 0.058 14.784 0 KSB - PRUK 0.165 41.128 0

Table 4.76 HMRA Model Change Summary for Negative Referents (Group 2)

Page 190: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  178  

Group 2: The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and KSB

S. Vision Model Summary Step 1 β T Adj. R2 F Value Sig N KSB - WSO 0.302 3.993*** 0.171 4.531 0 189 KSB - WSE 0.250 3.264** 0.140 3.818 0 191 KSB - WST 0.300 3.941*** 0.160 4.246 0 189 KSB - WUO 0.431 5.690*** 0.205 5.331 0 186 KSB - WUE 0.261 3.315** 0.114 3.215 0 191 KSB - WUT 0.457 6.085*** 0.208 5.441 0 187 KSB - PRUK 0.092 1.156 0.077 2.422 0 189

Step 2 β T Adj. R2 F Value Sig N Change R2 F Change Sig. F

Change KSB - WSO 0.222 2.796** 0.203 4.996 0 189 0.034 8.112 0.005 KSB - WSE 0.165 2.064* 0.177 4.416 0 191 0.039 9.103 0.003 KSB - WST 0.227 2.825** 0.186 4.574 0 189 0.029 6.685 0.011 KSB - WUO 0.312 4.031*** 0.277 6.896 0 186 0.071 18.284 0 KSB - WUE 0.128 1.618 0.215 5.328 0 191 0.099 24.023 0 KSB - WUT 0.348 4.483*** 0.266 6.613 0 187 0.058 14.784 0 KSB - PRUK -0.082 -1.065 0.247 6.151 0 189 0.165 41.128 0 Table 4.77 Group 2 HMRA Results for Shared Vision (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

The Group 2 HMRA results for shared vision (on KSB) are shown in Table 4.77 and

summarized below. Otrust was introduced into the model in Step 2 of the HMRA.

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and Willingness to Share

Knowledge

The addition of Otrust made a substantial contribution to the explanation of WSO

indicated by the increment of R2 from the Step 1 Model (WSO ΔR2 = 16.8%; p < .001;

WSE ΔR2 = 22%; p < .001; WST ΔR2 = 15.6%; p < .001).

The addition of Otrust also reduced the coefficient for shared vision (on WSO) by 26.5%

(Δβ = .302-.222). Mediation was present, but only partially, as shared vision remained

statistically associated with WSO. The results also showed that the addition of Otrust

reduced the coefficient for shared vision with WSE by 34% (Δβ = .250-.165), and with

WST by 24.3% (Δβ = .300-.227). In both cases, the mediation was present, but only

partially, as relationships between shared vision and WSE and WST remained statistically

significant after Otrust was introduced into the model.

Page 191: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  179  

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and Willingness to Use

Knowledge

The addition of Otrust made a substantial contribution to the explanation of WUO as

indicated by the increment of R2 from the Step 1 Model (WUO ΔR2 = 25.6%; p < .001;

WUE ΔR2 = 46%; p < .001; WUT ΔR2 = 21.8%; p < .001).

The addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient for shared vision (on WUO) by 27.6% (Δβ

= .431-.312). Mediation was present, but only partially, as shared vision remained

statistically associated with WUO, after Otrust was introduced into the model. The results

also showed that the addition of Otrust reduced the coefficient for shared vision with

WUE by 51% (Δβ = .261-.128) and with WUT by 23.9% (Δβ = .457-.348). In the case of

WUE, shared vision was no longer statistically significant after Otrust was introduced

into the model. This suggested that Otrust had a complete mediating effect for the

relationship between shared vision and WUE. For WUT, the mediation was present, but

only partially, as the relationship between shared vision and WUT remained statistically

significant after Otrust was introduced into the model.

The Overall Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and Perceived Receipt of

Useful Knowledge

The coefficient for shared vision was found to be not significantly associated with PRUK

in the first model, or in the second, when Otrust was introduced.

4.5.8.3 Summary of the Mediating Effects of Overall Trust

In summary, Otrust was found to mediate some of the effects of the independent

variables on the dependent variables. For positive referents, Otrust was found to have

varying degrees of a mediating effect between shared language and each of knowledge

sharing behaviors tested. In the same group, Otrust was found to also have a mediating

effect between shared vision and each of the knowledge sharing behaviors tested. For

negative referents, Otrust had a mediating effect between shared vision and two of the

three main KSBs (i.e. willingness to share knowledge and willingness to use knowledge).

A complete summary of the mediating effect of Otrust is presented in Table 4.78.

Page 192: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  180  

Group IV DV Type of

mediating effect 1 Shared Language Overall Willingness to Share Knowledge Partial 1 Shared Language Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge Partial 1 Shared Language Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge Partial 1 Shared Language Willingness to Use Explicit Knowledge Complete / Full 1 Shared Language Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge Complete / Full 1 Shared Vision Overall Willingness to Share Knowledge Partial 1 Shared Vision Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge Complete / Full 1 Shared Vision Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge Partial 1 Shared Vision Overall Willingness to Use Knowledge Partial 1 Shared Vision Willingness to Use Explicit Knowledge Complete / Full 1 Shared Vision Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge Partial 1 Shared Vision Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge Partial 2 Shared Vision Overall Willingness to Share Knowledge Partial 2 Shared Vision Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge Partial 2 Shared Vision Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge Partial 2 Shared Vision Overall Willingness to Use Knowledge Partial 2 Shared Vision Willingness to Use Explicit Knowledge Complete / Full 2 Shared Vision Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge Partial

Table 4.78 Summary of the Mediating Effects of Overall Trust Based on the analysis suggested by Baron and Kenny (1986) and the results of the

HMRA, hypothesis 17 was partially supported for positive referents through shared

language and shared vision, and partially supported for negative referents through shared

vision.

4.6 Summary of Hypothesis Tests

Table 4.79 presents a summary of the hypotheses tested, for an overview of the results of

the study.

Hypotheses Support Homophily 1 Age Homophily will be positively related to Trust

Not supported for either group

2 Age Homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Partially supported for Group 1 (Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge) and Not supported for Group 2

3 Educational Homophily will be positively related to Trust

Not supported for either group

4 Educational Homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Not supported for either group

5 Gender Homophily will be positively related to Trust

Not supported for either group

6 Gender Homophily will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Not supported for either group. Partial support of the opposite - Willingness to Use (Explicit) in Group 1 and Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge in Group 2

Page 193: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  181  

Shared Language and Shared Vision 7 Shared Language will be positively related to Trust

Fully Supported for Group 1 (Overall Trust and each form) and Not Supported for Group 2

8 Shared Language will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Partially Supported for Group 1 (Overall Willingness to Share, Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge and Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge) and Partially Supported for Group 2 (Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge)

9 Shared Vision will be positively related to Trust

Fully Supported for both groups (Overall Trust and each form)

10 Shared Vision will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Partially Supported for Group 1 (Overall Willingness to Share, Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge, Overall Willingness to Use, Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge and Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge) and Partially Supported for Group 2 (Overall Willingness to Share, Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge, Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge, Overall Willingness to Use and Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge)

Relationship Length and Tie Strength 11 Relationship Length will be positively related to Trust

Not Supported in either group (Partial Support of the opposite for Group 2: Overall Trust, Integrity-Based Trust, and Benevolence-Based Trust)

12 Relationship Length will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Not supported for either group

13 Tie Strength will be positively related to Trust

Partially Supported for Group 1 both Prior to the project or matter (Benevolence-Based Trust) and While on the project or matter (Overall Trust, Integrity-Based trust and Benevolence-Based Trust). It is not supported for Group 2 Prior to or While on the project or matter.

14 Tie Strength will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Not Supported for Group 1 Prior to the project or matter or While on the project or matter. It is partially supported for Group 2 Prior to (Overall Willingness to Share, Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge, Overall Willingness to Use Knowledge, Willingness to Use Explicit and Tacit Knowledge) and While on the project or matter (Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge)

Trust 15 Overall Trust will be positively related to Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Partially Supported for Group 1 (Overall Willingness to Share, Willingness to Share Tacit Knowledge, Overall Willingness to Use (both explicit and tacit), and Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge) and Fully Supported for Group 2

Collective Effect 16 Overall Trust and Social-Cognitive Factors explain Knowledge Sharing Behavior. Fully Supported for both groups Mediating Effect 17 Overall Trust will be a mediating variable between Social-Cognitive factors and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Partially Supported for Group 1 (through Shared Language and Shared Vision) and Partially Supported for Group 2 (through Shared Vision)

Table 4.79 Summary of Data Analysis and whether the Hypotheses were Supported

Page 194: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  182  

4.7 Summary of Results by Research Question

This research study of trust and knowledge sharing behavior was guided by five research

questions. The following section presents the results of the data analysis, as it pertains to

the research questions.

Summary of Results for Research Question 1

The first research question asked “what are the significant relationships between social-

cognitive variables and trust?” As there were several social-cognitive variables tested in

the study, the answer to this research question varied based on social-cognitive variable.

For example, the data analysis showed that no statistically significant relationships

existed between age, education, or gender homophily and trust, in either group. Next,

shared language was significantly positively related to each trust variable in Group 1 and

not at all related to any trust variable in Group 2. Shared vision was significantly

positively related to each trust variable, in both groups. Next, relationship length was

negatively related to overall trust, integrity-based trust, and benevolence-based trust in

Group 2, and not at all significantly related to trust in Group 1. In Group 1, tie strength,

prior to the project, was positively related to benevolence-based trust. Also in this group,

tie strength, while on the matter or project, was positively related to overall trust,

integrity-based trust, and benevolence-based trust. No significant relationships between

tie strength (prior to or while on) and trust were found in Group 2.

Summary of Results for Research Question 2

The second research question asked “what are the significant relationships between

social-cognitive variables and knowledge sharing behavior?” In Group 1, the data

analysis showed that similarity in age (age homophily) was positively related to

willingness to use tacit knowledge. No age homophily relationships were found in Group

2. Similarity in education (educational homophily) was also not statistically related to

knowledge sharing behavior, in either group. Next, similarity in gender (gender

homophily) was negatively related to willingness to use explicit knowledge from positive

referents, which suggested a gender heterogeneous effect. Gender homophily was also

Page 195: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  183  

found to have a negative effect on a respondent’s perception that the knowledge received

from the negative referents was useful.

The data analysis also showed that in Group 1 shared language was positively related to

overall willingness to share knowledge, willingness to share explicit knowledge, and

willingness to share tacit knowledge. In Group 2, shared language was only found to

have a positive effect on willingness to share explicit knowledge. In Group 1, shared

vision was positively related to overall willingness to share knowledge and willingness to

share tacit knowledge. A significant relationship was also found in that group, between

shared vision and overall willingness to use knowledge and willingness to use tacit

knowledge. Next, shared vision was found to have a positive effect on the respondent’s

perception that the knowledge received from positive referents was useful. In Group 2,

shared vision was positively related to overall willingness to share knowledge,

willingness to share explicit knowledge, and willingness to share tacit knowledge. Shared

vision also had a positive effect on a respondent’s overall willingness to use knowledge

and willingness to use tacit knowledge. The data analysis also showed that no significant

relationships existed between relationship length and any knowledge sharing behavior, in

either group. In addition, no significant relationships were found to exist between tie

strength (prior to or while on) and knowledge sharing behavior in Group 1. In Group 2

however, tie strength, prior to the project, was positively related to a respondent’s overall

willingness to share knowledge and willingness to share tacit knowledge. In this same

group, tie strength, prior to the project, was positively related to a respondent’s overall

willingness to use knowledge, willingness to use explicit knowledge, and willingness to

use tacit knowledge. Finally, in Group 2, tie strength (while on the project) was positively

related to a respondent’s perception that the knowledge received from the negative

referent was useful.

Summary of Results for Research Question 3

The third research question asked “what are the significant relationships between trust

and knowledge sharing behavior?” In Group 1, the data analysis showed that overall

trust was positively related to a respondent’s overall willingness to share knowledge and

Page 196: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  184  

willingness to share tacit knowledge. In the same group, the data analysis also showed

that overall trust had a positive effect on overall willingness to use knowledge,

willingness to use explicit knowledge, and willingness to use tacit knowledge. Finally, in

Group 2, overall trust was positively related to all knowledge sharing behaviors tested in

the study.

Summary of Results for Research Question 4

The fourth research question asked “what is the collective effect of the identified social-

cognitive variables and trust on knowledge sharing behavior?” The data analysis showed

that overall trust and social-cognitive factors explained each of the knowledge sharing

behaviors identified in the study, with both positive and negative referents. Notably, more

knowledge sharing behaviors could be explained by the identified IVs in relationships

with positive referents, than could be explained with negative referents. For example, in

Group 1, the collective model explained 42% of overall willingness to share knowledge,

32% of overall willingness to use knowledge, and 43% of perceived receipt of useful

knowledge. In the Group 2, the collective model explained 20% of overall willingness to

share knowledge, 28% of overall willingness to use knowledge, and 25% of perceived

receipt of useful knowledge.

Summary of Results for Research Question 5

The fifth research question asked “does trust act as a mediating variable between social-

cognitive variables and knowledge sharing behavior?” The data analysis showed that, in

Group 1, overall trust acted (to varying degrees) as a mediating variable between shared

language and knowledge sharing behavior, and as a mediating variable between shared

vision and knowledge sharing behavior. In Group 2, overall trust was found to be a

mediating variable between shared vision and knowledge sharing behavior (also to

varying degrees). Table 4.78 contains the complete details of the mediating effects.

Page 197: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  185  

Chapter 5: Discussion and Summary 5.0 Chapter Overview

As noted in the first chapter the effective sharing of knowledge within organizations

promises many benefits, both to the individual and to the firm. However, in reality, most

companies struggle with sharing knowledge (Ruggles, 1998), because promoting

knowledge sharing within their organizations is a technological and behavioral challenge.

The KM research acknowledges these challenges, by focusing on technological issues

and more recently on human and social factors (Hislop, 2003). Understanding the impact

of these human and social factors on knowledge sharing behavior assists researchers and

practitioners in understanding the question: what makes someone share their knowledge

with others in the firm? These “factors, that motivate people to codify and share

knowledge for the benefit of others, have been identified as a priority area for knowledge

research” (Hall, 2001, p. 140).

Since the mid 1960s, the management literature (e.g. Argyris (1964)) has identified trust

as an important social factor positively linked to the organizational performance. In more

recent literature, trust has been highly regarded as one of the main social factors

facilitating knowledge sharing behavior (Section 2.2). In addition to trust, other research

studies examining knowledge-sharing behavior have identified numerous social or

cognitive factors acting as motivators or inhibitors for knowledge sharing in

organizations. A select few of these social-cognitive factors have been connected to trust

in other studies. However, no study to date has examined the interrelatedness of the three.

For the research presented here, the phenomenon of interest was the knowledge sharing

behavior (KSB) of professionals involved in project-based group work, in a national

professional legal services firm. The study was specifically interested in the significant

direct and indirect relationships that exist between trust, identified social-cognitive

factors, and knowledge sharing behavior, in this setting. The direct and mediating effect

of trust50 was of specific interest, since the literature review identified it as a main factor

                                                                                                               50 Trust was a composite variable based on Mayer et al.’s (1995) measure for perceived trustworthiness; operationalized as the sum of ability-based, integrity-based, and benevolence-based trust.

Page 198: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  186  

to influencing effective knowledge sharing. Also of interest was the collective effect of

social-cognitive factors, together with trust, on knowledge sharing behavior. Social-

cognitive factors tested in the analysis included homophily (gender, age, and education),

shared perspective (shared vision and shared language), tie strength (prior to and while

on the project), and relationship length. Knowledge sharing behaviors included

willingness to share knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit), willingness to use knowledge

(overall, explicit and tacit), and perceived receipt of useful knowledge. The conceptual

framework for the research study is presented in Figure 5.1.

 Figure 5.1 Conceptual Framework

Page 199: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  187  

The overarching research question was: what are the factors that influence knowledge

sharing behavior directly and indirectly through trust? To better answer this research

question, five additional research questions were posed, as follows:

1. What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and

trust?

2. What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and

knowledge sharing behavior?

3. What are the significant relationships between trust and knowledge sharing

behavior?

4. What is the collective effect of the identified social-cognitive variables and trust

on knowledge sharing behavior?

5. Does trust act as a mediating variable between social-cognitive variables and

knowledge sharing behavior?

To answer these research questions, one of Canada’s largest multijurisdictional law firms

was selected as an appropriate site for study. As previously mentioned (Section 3.6) this

firm was selected for the collaboration between knowledge workers on project, its

knowledge intensive environment, and for the nature of the projects they assign.

Before the survey was finalized, a pre-test was conducted with twenty knowledge

workers not affiliated with the sample firm. The survey was then published on the web,

using a private web-based academic survey tool called Qualtrics. Once the survey was

online, it was pre-tested with an additional ten knowledge workers not affiliated with the

sample firm and three senior executives at the firm. Once the pre-tests were complete,

some question wording was adjusted, to match the vernacular of the firm. These changes

were made based on interviews with senior executives at the firm. After the changes were

in place, a senior partner at the firm sent a firm-wide email, asking a sample of

approximately 900 legal professionals and paralegals/law clerks, in six nationally

distributed offices, to participate in the survey. All the employees asked to participate in

the survey were knowledge workers engaged in knowledge-intensive legal project work,

Page 200: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  188  

the nature of which required a significant reliance on others for both explicit and tacit

forms of knowledge. Of the approximately 900 invitations sent, 775 were distributed to

‘legal professionals’ and 120 to ‘paralegals’ and ‘law clerks’. No administrative staff

received the invitation to participate.

The survey took approximately 15-20 minutes to complete and respondents were assured

that all their responses would remain completely confidential and they would remain

anonymous. The survey was divided into three sections: an individual section that asked

respondents to answer questions about themselves and their background; a co-worker

section that asked respondents to answer questions about a positive referent (i.e. someone

they worked best with on a recent project); and finally a co-worker section that asked

respondents to answer questions about a negative referent (i.e. someone they did not

work well with on a recent project). This approach of getting respondents to comment on

both a positive and negative referent was based on similar distinctions made in previous

research studies (McAllister, 1995; Tsui, 1984, 1986; Holste, 2003). The approach was

also motivated by a conceptual distinction in the type of relationships that occur within

these settings. For example, one of the interesting features of knowledge intensive

organizations is that their employees rarely have free choice in deciding with whom they

work, and with whom they are required to share knowledge. To achieve the project

objectives, in most instances, employees would be required to share knowledge with both

individuals they work well with and those with whom they do not work well.

In total, 275 surveys were completed by employees at this national multijurisdictional

law firm, for a response rate of 30.6%. Factor analysis was used to extract underlying

dimensions. Cronbach’s alpha was then used to assess the reliability of the scales used.

Next, each of the hypotheses was tested using statistical techniques. Initially, correlation

analysis was used to measure the bivariate relationships between the independent and

dependent variables, and then t-tests were used to test differences between the means of

the variables. Multiple regression analysis was used to examine the relationships between

the dependent variables and the focal independent variable, while controlling for all the

other variables in the model. If the results of the regression analysis contradicted the

Page 201: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  189  

results of the correlation analysis, or t-tests, the results of the regression analysis were

given priority and accepted as MRA controlled for all of the variables in the theoretical

framework of the study and, therefore, represented the more stringent analytical

framework. To test for the mediating effect of trust, hierarchical multiple regression

analysis was used in accordance with a testing method suggested by Baron and Kenny

(1986).

A summary of the research questions, associated hypotheses, and testing results appears

below. The discussion of these research findings is organized by research question and

presented in greater detail in the sections following. The chapter closes with research

contributions, implications for future research, and implications for practice.

RQ1: What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and

trust?

o H1 Age homophily will be positively related to trust

• Not supported for either group

o H3 Educational homophily will be positively related to trust

• Not supported for either group

o H5 Gender homophily will be positively related to trust

• Not supported for either group

o H7 Shared language will be positively related to trust

• Supported for Group 1 and not supported for Group 2

o H9 Shared vision will be positively related to trust

• Supported for both Groups

o H11 Relationship length will be positively related to trust

• Not supported for either group (i.e. partial support of opposite for Group 2)

o H13 Tie strength will be positively related to trust

• Partially supported for Group 1 both prior to and while on the project; not

supported for Group 2 prior to or while on the project

Page 202: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  190  

RQ2: What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and

knowledge sharing behavior?

o H2 Age homophily will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

• Partially supported for Group 1; not supported for Group 2

o H4 Educational homophily will be positively related to knowledge sharing

behavior

• Not supported for either group

o H6 Gender homophily will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

• Not supported for either group (i.e. partial support of the opposite in both

groups)

o H8 Shared language will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

• Partially supported for both groups

o H10 Shared vision will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

• Partially supported for both groups

o H12 Relationship length will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

• Not supported for either group

o H14 Tie strength will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

• Not supported for Group 1 prior to or while on the project; partially supported

for Group 2 both prior to and while on the project

RQ3: What are the significant relationships between trust and knowledge sharing

behavior?

o H15 Overall trust will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

• Partially supported in Group 1; supported in Group 2

RQ4: What is the collective effect of the identified social-cognitive variables and trust

on knowledge sharing behavior?

o H16 Overall trust and social-cognitive factors explain knowledge sharing behavior

• Supported for both groups

Page 203: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  191  

RQ5: Does trust act as a mediating variable between social-cognitive variables and

knowledge sharing behavior?

o H17 Overall trust will be a mediating variable between social-cognitive factors and

knowledge sharing behavior

• Partially supported for Group 1 (through shared language and shared vision);

partially supported for Group 2 (through shared vision)

5.1 Discussion of the Research Findings

The discussion of the research findings is organized and presented below, by the five sub-

research questions. Each section briefly discusses previous research findings, important

distinctions, notes the expected (hypothesized) results, presents the actual research

findings, and then discusses any divergence with previous findings.

5.1.1 Relationships Between Social-Cognitive Variables and Trust

Research Question 1:

What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and trust?

Hypotheses tested for RQ1:

H1 Age homophily will be positively related to trust (not supported for either group)

H3 Educational homophily will be positively related to trust (not supported for either group)

H5 Gender homophily will be positively related to trust (not supported for either group)

H7 Shared language will be positively related to trust (supported for Group 1 and not

supported for Group 2)

H9 Shared vision will be positively related to trust (supported for both Groups)

H11 Relationship length will be positively related to trust (not supported for either group; with

partial support of opposite for Group 2)

H13 Tie strength will be positively related to trust (partially supported for Group 1 prior to

and while on the project; not supported for Group 2 prior to or while on the project)

To better understand the factors influencing knowledge sharing behavior, directly and

indirectly through trust, the research study began by exploring the significant

relationships between identified social-cognitive factors and trust. As previously

discussed, the construct of trust was based on Mayer et al.’s (1995; Mayer & Davis,

Page 204: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  192  

1999) proposed model for organizational trust, that includes a measure for the trustor’s

‘propensity to trust’ and that trustor’s ‘perceived trustworthiness’ of the trustee (i.e.

measured as the sum of ability-based, benevolence-based, and integrity-based trust in the

trustee). Initially, the study proposed to measure trust by considering the complete Mayer

et al.’s (1995) model, yet during data analysis it was discovered that the measure of

propensity to trust had low Cronbach’s alpha, which determined its exclusion from any

further analysis of trust. Without the inclusion of a measure for the trustor’s propensity to

trust, the aggregate measure of overall trust became very similar to Mayer et al.’s (1995)

measure for perceived trustworthiness, which captures a set of characteristics (i.e. ability,

benevolence, and integrity) the trustor (i.e. respondent) believed the trustee (i.e. co-

worker) to possess.

After an analysis of the literature on trust and knowledge sharing and an assessment of

the research setting, a total of eight social-cognitive factors were identified as appropriate

for study. These factors were further divided into three categories: homophily, shared

language/shared vision, and relationship length/tie strength. Seven hypotheses51 were

tested to answer the first research question. Each hypothesis predicted a positive

relationship between the social-cognitive factor and trust. A complete discussion of the

implications, relating to the first research question, is presented in the sections below.

5.1.1.1 Homophily and Trust

Based on a literature review of the different types of homophily effects on trust (Section

2.3.1), the research study initially sought to test status-based homophily for five ascribed

characteristics (i.e. age, gender, race/ethnicity, citizenship, and immigration) and two

acquired characteristics (i.e. education and martial status). However, after reviewing the

descriptive statistics from the survey data, it was discovered that birth country,

citizenship, ethnicity, and marital status did not have adequate variability or distribution

across the sample to accurately test for or identify homophily in the sample. Specifically,

85% of those who completed the survey were born in Canada, more than 98% had

Canadian citizenship, almost 87% identified themselves as, at least partially, Caucasian,

                                                                                                               51 H1, H3, H5, H7, H9, H11 and H13

Page 205: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  193  

and over 60% were legally married. The exclusion of these characteristics left the study

exploring the role of homophily with a focus on two ascribed characteristics (i.e. age and

gender) and one acquired characteristic (i.e. education).

Age Homophily and Trust

Based on the existing research examining the impact of age homophily on trust (Section

2.3.1) it was anticipated that higher levels of trust would be found among participants

closer in age. Unexpectedly, this was found to be not true, as data analysis showed that

no significant relationships existed between age homophily and trust, in either group. One

possible explanation for this result may be that many of the previous studies, which

discovered age homophily affecting trusting behavior, were among school acquaintances

and friends, and not among co-workers. Age homophily would be expected in those types

of relationships, as schools traditionally organize ages together, into classes. On the other

hand, findings from Marsden’s (1988) study52 suggested that this type of homophily can

be found in non-educational settings. Notably, respondents in Marsden’s (1988) study

were more likely to have met in schools and social groups, rather than corporate

environments. Social groups and community events, such as schools, may have a

tendency of separating individuals into compatible age groups; at least more so than one

would expect this to occur in corporate environments. Based on this separation, it may be

possible that one’s closest friends, and significant others, with whom they discuss

important matters, would be similar to them in age. On the other hand, in knowledge-

intensive organizational settings, employees are assigned into networks and work

relationships based on their skill, experience, and knowledge domain. In this corporate

environment, age plays little to no role in who will work with whom on a project.

Additionally, in social environments people have free will to befriend and “discuss

important matters” with whomever they please. In corporate environments, like the one in

this study, working relationships are far more rigid, rarely having this same fluidity.

Often, project teams are assembled based on either the position of knowledge workers in

a practice group, on availability, by client request, or by the choice of a senior executive.

                                                                                                               52 Marsden’s (1988) study asked respondents to identify those persons with whom they "discussed important matters" within six months prior to the interview. Respondents were then asked more specific questions about the first five people named, including age, education, race/ethnicity, religion, and gender.

Page 206: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  194  

Considering how the forming and functioning of corporate relationships are different

from those in other social environments, it is reasonable to assume age homophily would

not exist in such environments.

A more recent study (Toh & Srinivas, 2011), examining trust between expatriates and

host-country nationals at MNCs, offers a possible substantiation for this line of reasoning.

Despite expecting to, the authors (2011) found no significant interaction effects between

perceived interpersonal similarity (including observable demographics, such as gender

and ethnicity53) and scores for interpersonal trust. Similar to this study, Toh and Srinivas

(2011) expected to discover (based on the existing literature) that trust in an individual

(the expatriate) by their co-worker (the host-country national) would be moderated by the

extent to which the two parties were interpersonally similar. Since the relationships

studied by Toh and Srinivas (2011) were in a similar professional setting, it is reasonable

to assume that the influence of homophily on trust would not be present in such

environments. Future research should focus on making this environmental distinction.

Gender Homophily and Trust

Based on the existing research (Section 2.3.1), it was anticipated that, in this study,

higher levels of trust would be found among those individuals similar in gender.

Unexpectedly, this was found to be not true, as data analysis showed that no significant

relationships existed between gender homophily and trust, in either group. While

unexpected, this result happens to be consistent with Toh and Srinivas’ (2011) study

mentioned in the previous section. In their research, examining gender as a perceived

interpersonal similarity, Toh and Srinivas (2011) also expected to find trust to be affected

by the extent to which two parties were interpersonally similar. However, like in this

study, they found no such relationship.

One possible explanation for the findings in this study was discussed in the previous

section, and involved inherently preventing or reducing the effect of gender homophily

                                                                                                               53 Notably, age was not specifically mentioned in the Toh and Srinivas (2011) study, yet it is reasonable to assume that ten-year age intervals (similar to the ones used in this study) would qualify as a perceived interpersonal similarity or observable demographic.

Page 207: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  195  

through the practice of composing teams based on domain knowledge; a common

practice in professional settings. Another possible explanation for why this study did not

find gender homophily to have an effect on trust may be found in the nature of the

relationship between the ‘legal professional’ and the clerk (or paralegal). According to

interviews with senior partners and executives at the firm, the ‘legal professionals’ and

law clerks (or paralegals) working in the same practice area (e.g. intellectual property)

frequently interact on numerous matters. As these practice groups work over time on

numerous projects (i.e. matters), they build a rapport and a reliance on each other for

support and information. Therefore, the nature of this working relationship requires close

emotional ties, which must be rooted in trust.

Considering the nature of the relationship between the ‘legal professional’ and

clerk/paralegal, as well as the gender composition of the two groups, it is understandable

why gender homophily was not found in this study. For example, of the 180 ‘legal

professionals’ that completed the survey, there was a fairly even distribution among

gender; 52% were men (93/180) and 48% women (87/180). On the contrary, less than 9%

of the 89 clerks (or paralegals) who completed the survey were men (8/89)and 91%

women (81/89). Women in the sample outnumbered men in the clerk/paralegal role by

over 10 to 1. According to interviews with senior executives, this was also an accurate

representation of the firm’s actual gender distribution by role.

Notably, each legal matter (or project) utilizes a mixture of both ‘legal professionals’ and

clerks/paralegals who assist them. Since clerks and paralegals (who are mostly women)

are distributed across each practice group, and are assigned to each matter, each team was

statistically heterogeneous in composition. This group composition, based on industry

practice, may have helped in preventing the effect of gender homophily on trust.

However, it is worth noting that this group composition may be unique to the legal

setting, which makes it difficult to replicate in other professional firm settings, without a

predominantly female-occupied role assigned to each project.

Page 208: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  196  

Educational Homophily and Trust

Based on the findings in the literature review (Section 2.3.1) and on the legal setting of

the organization (i.e. the setting required the individuals who work there to have a similar

education in law), it was anticipated that, in this study, higher levels of trust would be

found among those individuals with similar educational levels. Unexpectedly, data

analysis showed no significant relationships between educational homophily and trust, in

either group.

One possible suggestion for why educational homophily was not discovered in this study

could be that respondents relied on, and had faith in, the organization to hire reliable and

skilled co-workers, regardless of the education level they achieved. Respondents may

have had a predisposed expectation of educational competence in their co-worker, based

on the idea that the firm would not hire someone who did not posses the necessary or

adequate skills and abilities to be trusted to do their job. This faith in the organization

was likely built over time, and may be a factor of the history of hiring, the size of the

firm, its vision, culture, or reputation.

Another possible suggestion for why educational homophily was not discovered could,

once again, stem from the relationship between ‘legal professional’ and clerk/paralegal.

For example, almost 96% (175 of 183) of the ‘legal professionals’ who completed the

survey achieved at least one professional degree. In contrast, 93% (83 of 89) of law

clerks/paralegals who completed the survey achieved a 4-year degree or less, with 61%

(54 of 89) of the law clerks/paralegals not completing a 4-year degree (i.e. having either

“some college” or only a high school degree). Despite this large disparity in education,

the frequent interaction between the ‘legal professional’ and law clerk/paralegal on legal

matters (i.e. projects) required a very close tie and working relationship, which, as

previously mentioned, must be rooted in trusting behavior. The make up of the project

teams requires individuals in the firm to trust both those co-workers with similar and with

different levels of education, to successfully complete their work. Based on this

composition of the project teams, it is understandable why educational homophily was

not found.

Page 209: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  197  

Summary of Homophily and Trust

Contrary to what has been theorized and occasionally found in other research settings,

homophily or similarity in age, gender, and education had no significant effect on a

respondent’s perceived trust in their co-workers, in this setting. Detailed discussions

suggesting why these research findings were not consistent have been presented in the

preceding sections, but generally relate to one or more of three reasons. First, how the

organization assembled project teams or workgroups were different from how they may

have been formed in other settings. This may have prevented or minimized the natural

selection of work partners through homophily. Second, homophily may have not been

found because respondents believed in the organization’s ability to hire domain

knowledgeable, competent individuals. Finally, the role and job description of legal

clerk/paralegal, as well as their relationship with ‘legal professionals’ on each matter (i.e.

project), may inherently have structured each project group to be diverse, with respect to

age, gender, and education. For these reasons, it is believed that no relationship between

trust and homophily were found.

5.1.1.2 Shared Perspective and Trust (Shared Language and Shared Vision)

The constructs of shared language and shared vision were intended to measure the extent

to which a shared perspective exists between respondent and co-worker. More

specifically, the study sought to identify the role that shared perspective played in the

trusting behavior between co-workers in the firm. Understanding this relationship is

important since, given the legal setting of the firm, it is reasonable to assume that when

contextual mismatches occur, trust in the person with whom these occurred (or at the

very least domain or skill specific (ability-based) trust) would decline. Contextual

mismatches and distrust among co-workers could be serious inhibitors to project

outcomes.

Considering the existing research (Section 2.3.2) and the essential need to have a shared

context, language, and vision on legal matters, it was expected that in this study, higher

levels of trust would be found among those individuals similar in shared language and

shared vision. The results with positive referents (i.e. those individuals the respondents

Page 210: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  198  

worked best with) validated these hypotheses, showing that both shared language and

shared vision were significantly positively related to overall trust, ability-based trust,

integrity-based trust, and benevolence-based trust.

The findings for positive referents were consistent with previous research and may be

justified by arguing that respondents would get more meaningful outcomes from the

interactions with co-workers they share language or vision with, especially if they find

the working relationship to be pleasant. The positive working relationship may also

encourage respondents to interact more frequently. More frequent, meaningful, and

pleasant interactions provided ample opportunities to build rapport and trust between co-

workers. Tsai and Ghoshal (1998) explained this by arguing that “when organization

members have the same perceptions about how to interact with one another, they can

avoid possible misunderstandings in their communications and have more opportunities

to exchange their ideas or resources freely” (p. 467). More frequent ideas and resource

exchanges provided ideal opportunities to develop trusting relationships.

For negative referents (i.e. the group of individuals respondents did not work well with),

shared vision was also significantly positively related to overall trust, ability-based trust,

integrity-based trust, and benevolence-based trust; however, no significant relationships

were found between shared language and any type of trust. This suggested that shared

language no longer had an effect on trust, when the respondent deemed the working

relationship between them and co-worker as poor. This was an interesting finding, since

it suggested that having a shared language might not be as important predictor of trust,

especially in lieu of the working relationship. One possible way to explain this result may

be to argue that, unlike shared vision, having a common language is a necessary

prerequisite for working in a legal practice and on a legal team. By virtue of inclusion in

the work group, each respondent may have assumed that their co-workers shared some

degree of common language with them; which did not guarantee they shared a common

goal, and certainly did not guarantee they trusted each other, especially when considering

the effect of the negative working relationship. Based on these findings, having a shared

language was not enough to affect trust, when respondents had to overcome a negative

Page 211: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  199  

working relationship. This finding also suggested the need for further research into the

effect of working relationship on trusting behavior between co-workers.

On the other hand, higher shared vision between respondents and co-workers led to

higher trust in all sampled co-workers, regardless of the working relationship. One

possible explanation for this may have to do with the nature of the legal environment. For

example, the firm’s legal practice-groups begin with organizationally driven, clearly

explicated, shared goals (in terms of legal outcomes) and have a common understanding

of the main concerns and purpose of each matter they work on. Sharing the firm’s overall

vision, as well as the groups’ vision, for the outcomes of the matter, seemed to provide an

excellent platform on which trust could be built. Shared vision also turned out to be a

much stronger predictor of trust than shared language, as it can be witnessed by the

minimal effect from the change in working relationship.

Summary of Shared Perspective and Trust

As expected, the results with positive referents showed a positive relationship between

shared perspective and trust; where higher amounts of both shared language and shared

vision led to a higher trust, by the respondent, in the co-worker they worked best with.

The results from data collected with negative referents showed slightly different results

than those in the first group, offering some interesting perspectives on the role the

working relationship has on shared perspective and trust, in this study. For example, the

results from both groups suggested that the effect shared vision had on trust transcended

the nature of the working relationship between co-workers, reflecting its importance to

trust. Also, the results indicated that sharing a common language with a co-worker was

not necessarily enough to facilitate trust in that person, and that the nature of the working

relationship between the two co-workers may play an important role. This can partially

be explained by arguing that a common language can often be expected on a legal project

and does not guarantee the establishment of a common goal or trust.

Page 212: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  200  

5.1.1.3 Structural Characteristics and Trust (Tie Strength and Relationship Length)

Based on the literature review (Sections 2.3.3 and 2.3.4), this research study sought to

test the effect of two structural factors on trust: tie strength and relationship length.

Relationship length was measured by asking respondents how long they knew each of the

two co-workers they mentally selected. Tie strength was indicated by a 4-item measure,

where two items indicated the closeness of the relationship and the other two indicated

interaction frequency. In addition, based on a perception that tie strength prior to the

project was conceptually different from tie strength while on the project and thus might

have a different effect on trust, each of the four tie strength items appeared twice on the

survey; once asking the respondent about their relationship with their two co-workers

prior to the project and once again to measure the same relationship while on the project.

Tie Strength and Trust

Based on the literature review (Section 2.3.3) it was expected that, in this study, higher

levels of trust would be found among those co-workers who had higher tie strength

between them. This was found to be not entirely true, as the results from positive

referents suggested that there was not a significant relationship between overall trust and

tie strength prior to the project. However, a positive relationship was discovered between

benevolence-based trust and tie strength, prior to the project. This suggested that higher

tie strength with positive referents, prior to the project, was driven by a belief that the co-

worker cared about the respondent’s interests and wanted to do well by the respondent.

That is, if one believed that their co-worker cared about their interests and wanted to do

well by them, they would tend to interact more with this individual and feel more

emotional closeness towards them.

Interestingly, stronger tie strength (prior to) had no effect on trust in the co-

worker’s domain specific skills and competencies (ability-based trust), or adherence to a

set of principles or values acceptable to the respondent (integrity-based trust). This

finding suggested reconsidering how respondents in the study assessed the competence of

co-workers. Perhaps, they assessed their co-workers’ competence with respect to the

outcomes of the interaction, rather than the interaction itself or an emotional intensity

Page 213: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  201  

felt. Stronger tie strength did not necessarily lead to a belief in the competence of co-

workers, because one interaction may have been all that was required. If a respondent

needed some particular knowledge, they may have gone to the co-worker, gotten the

knowledge, and found that it was good knowledge, without the need for additional

emotional intensity or more frequent interaction. Knowing it was good, useful knowledge

may have been all that was needed for trust to occur.

Stronger tie strength, while on the matter or project, was positively related to overall

trust, integrity-based trust, and benevolence-based trust in positive referent co-workers.

These results were similar to those found by Levin and Cross (2004), however, ability or

competence based trust was found to not be affected. As briefly mentioned above, there

may have been several reasons why co-workers interacted on a project. For example, the

projects may have involved multiple types of knowledge and only limited interactions

may have been needed to share this knowledge. However, respondents may have also

needed to interact to get additional support for their analysis and their opinions; to decide

how to manage and proceed with the project; or to express concerns and explore risks.

Reasonably, these types of interactions, and the associated emotional intensity and

frequent interaction, seemed more likely to take place with individuals in whom

respondents had benevolence-based or integrity-based trust (vs. ability-based).

In this study, tie strength had the greatest influence on benevolence-based trust, both

prior to and while on the project. Tie strength had the least influence on competence or

ability-based trust; showing no relationships prior to or while on the project. This seemed

to be in line with what has been discussed above. Just because domain knowledge needed

to be shared, it did not mean that it took much emotional intensity or interaction to share

it. Also, strong ties may have related to interactions that had a purpose other than for

knowledge sharing.

The effect on integrity-based trust changed between the two types of tie strength, where

no significant effect was found with tie strength prior to the project and one was found

with tie strength while on the project. In this case, a common set of principles and values

Page 214: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  202  

acceptable to the respondent (integrity-based trust) led to more frequent interactions and

higher emotional intensity between the co-workers. These results pointed to the fact that

these interactions may have had a purpose other than the transference of skills or

competences (e.g. support). These types of interactions would be far more important and

common in problem solving while on projects, and would rarely take place prior to the

specifics of a project are known.

No significant relationships between tie strength (prior to or while on the project) and

trust were found, with negative referents. This result was interesting, since it pointed out

that the nature of the working relationship clearly played a role in affecting the

relationship between trust and tie strength. One possible reason for this can be found in

associating benevolence / integrity-based trust and working relationship. Perhaps, there is

inherently a connection between thinking a co-worker would “do good” to the trustor

(benevolence-based trust) and positive working relationship. The same may be asked for

whether the co-worker’s set of principles are acceptable to the trustor (integrity-based

trust), since both were found significant with positive referents. Alternatively,

respondents may have interacted with negative referents because they were required to do

so, and not because they chose to. Respondents may have minimized interactions with

this group and, as a result, did not have the opportunity to assess, in the interaction,

whether the co-worker was trustworthy. Any interaction may have been purely

instrumental, just to get the task done, but not to ask any extraneous questions.

Relationship Length and Trust

Based on the literature review (Section 2.3.4) it was expected that, in this study, longer

relationship length would relate to higher levels of trust, by the respondent, in the co-

worker. This was found not to be the case for the first group, where the results of the data

analysis revealed that relationship length was not significantly related to any type of

trust. More interestingly, with negative referents, relationship length was found to be

negatively related to overall trust, integrity-based trust, and benevolence-based trust,

which was the opposite of what was hypothesized; highlighting an interesting distinction

Page 215: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  203  

between the two groups. As with tie strength, ability-based trust was not significantly

related, perhaps for similar reasons.

One possible explanation for the opposite effect found between relationship length and

trust with negative referents can stem from the nature of the corporate relationship. In a

corporate setting, employees participate in mandated, involuntary work groups, where

members do not always have the freedom to choose co-workers, or terminate a poor

working relationship. With positive referents, the involuntary nature of the working

relationship would not be nearly as important factor, since the respondents had a good

working relationship. With negative referents, however, the relationship can be both

involuntary and unpleasant, yet still needed to exist, for the project work to get done. If

the negative referent relationships were completely voluntary, it would be unlikely that a

respondent would continue to work and develop a relationship under conditions where

they felt they did not work well with the co-worker.

Ongoing involuntary relationships with negative referents may explain why the opposite

effect of relationship length on benevolence and integrity-based trust was found.

Benevolence-based trust is driven by a care for the interest of the co-worker and a want to

do good towards them. In poor relationships, it would be easy to understand why a

respondent would not have any interest in doing good towards a co-worker with whom

they do not work well. Alternatively, the respondents may have convinced themselves,

over time, that their principles, values, and beliefs (i.e. drivers for integrity-based trust)

are different from those of negative referent co-workers and that, perhaps, it was even

those perceived differences that caused the poor working relationship. Interestingly, with

negative referents, ability-based trust, which is driven by domain specific skills and

competencies, was found not to be negatively significant, suggesting that it was not a lack

of trust in the skills of the individual that decayed trust in this group.

Summary of Structural Variables and Trust

Tie strength and relationship length were analyzed for relationships between structural

characteristics and trust between co-workers. A holistic view of the data from both

Page 216: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  204  

groups revealed three interesting insights into these relationships. First, in certain

situations, structural characteristics had no significant effect on trust (i.e. tie strength in

Group 2 or relationship length in Group 1). Second, all structural characteristics

measured in the study had an influence only on integrity-based and benevolence-based

trust. Finally, and perhaps most interestingly, no structural characteristic variable had any

effect on ability-based trust.

5.1.2 Research Question 1 Summary

The first research question asked “what are the significant relationships between social-

cognitive variables and trust?” As there were several social-cognitive variables tested in

the study, the answer to this research question varied based on social-cognitive variables.

First, the findings showed there to be no relationships between any types of homophily

and trust. Second, the results showed partial support for a relationship between shared

perspective and trust; with shared vision being fully supported in both groups and shared

language depending on the nature of the working relationship (i.e. was found for positive

referents only). Finally, the results showed the structural variables to have a specific

effect on certain types of trust; where no effect was found on ability-based trust and a

significant effect was found for both integrity and benevolence-based trust. The nature of

the working relationship also played a role in how the structural variables had an impact

on trust.

5.1.3 Relationships Between Social-Cognitive Variables and Knowledge Sharing

Behavior

Research Question 2:

What are the significant relationships between social-cognitive variables and knowledge

sharing behavior?

Hypotheses Tested for RQ2:

H2 Age homophily will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

H4 Educational homophily will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

H6 Gender homophily will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

H8 Shared language will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

H10 Shared vision will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

Page 217: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  205  

H12 Relationship length will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

H14 Tie strength will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

The second research question explored the significant relationships between identified

social-cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior. Knowledge sharing behavior

(KSB) was operationalized as three combined variables measuring willingness to share

knowledge, willingness to use knowledge, and perceived receipt of useful knowledge. The

eight social-cognitive factors tested in the first research question were used, once again,

for the second: homophily, shared perspective (shared language and shared vision), and

structural factors (relationship length and tie strength). Seven hypotheses54 were tested to

answer the second research question, exploring the existence of a positive relationship

between the social-cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior.

A complete discussion of the implications relating to this question is presented in the

sections below.

5.1.3.1 Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

As with trust, certain homophily measures (i.e. birth country, ethnicity, and martial

status) were excluded from the analysis, due to bias in the data (see Section 5.1.1.1 for

details). The exclusion of these factors left the study exploring the role of homophily on

KSBs, based on two ascribed characteristics (age and gender) and one acquired

characteristic (education).

Age Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Given the findings in the existing research (Section 2.4.1) on age homophily and KSB, it

was anticipated that, in this study, higher levels of knowledge sharing behavior55 would

be found among those individuals closer in age. Unexpectedly, this was found to be not

true, as data analysis showed that no significant relationships existed, in either group,

between age homophily and willingness to share knowledge, or between age homophily

and perceived receipt of useful knowledge. In addition, there was no significant

                                                                                                               54 H2, H4, H6, H8, H10, H12 and H14 55 KSB is defined using three separate composite variables: sharing, use, and perceived usefulness

Page 218: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  206  

relationship between age homophily and overall willingness to use knowledge, in either

group. However, there was a significant positive relationship between age homophily and

willingness to use tacit knowledge, with positive referents (none with negative referents).

In Section 5.1.1.1, it was suggested that age homophily was not found in this study

because of the difference between how project groups are structured in corporate

environments and how they may be structured in educational and social environments.

Based on firm practice (or industry-based methodologies), each legal project team’s

composition (on each legal matter) almost always consists of a mix of senior executives,

junior executives, and support staff of varying ages. Therefore, one explanation for why

no relationships were found between age homophily and most knowledge sharing

behaviors could be due to the diverse representation of age groups on each legal matter.

Further, it could be argued that legal knowledge rapidly changes and, hence, age may not

be a good proxy for the quality of knowledge possessed by any employee. Thus,

employees on the project team did not tend to deem age as a reliable basis for either

knowledge sharing behavior, or for considering that the knowledge shared was useful.

There was, however, one noteworthy significant relationship, suggesting that respondents

were more likely to use the tacit knowledge supplied to them by someone they worked

well with, who was closer to them in age. One possible suggestion for this anomaly, from

the rest of the age homophily findings, may be related to the career stage of the

employee. Connelly and Kelloway (2003) argued that an employee’s “age and career

stage may affect their knowledge sharing behaviors through the size and utility of their

social networks; [where] experienced employees may simply be more able to share their

knowledge because they know more of the right people in the organization” (p. 297). In

this setting, it would seem that the longer an employee was at the firm (i.e. the older they

are), the more they used knowledge from others who have been there longer (who,

themselves, are older in age). Using Connelly and Kelloway’s (2003) reasoning, a

younger legal professional may simply not know where to turn to acquire useable tacit

knowledge, because they have an inadequate or ineffectual network. On the other side,

the senior legal professionals may not fully comprehend what tacit knowledge the junior

Page 219: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  207  

executives possess, which causes them to rely on their existing long-standing knowledge

contacts. Alternatively, these senior executive respondents may simply not want to risk

using non-explicit forms of knowledge from less experienced professionals.

Another possible reason age homophily may have been associated with the sharing of

tacit knowledge could be that the quality and usefulness of tacit legal knowledge needed

considerable time to mature and develop, and, hence, was more likely to be shared

between those of similar age and, presumably, experience. It could be postulated that this

tacit knowledge was generally easier to share between those individuals who had some

level of shared experience, either limited or extensive. Alternatively, no relationship was

found with regards to explicit knowledge, because it was much easier to develop and

share among co-workers with varying degrees of experience.

Gender Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Based on the previous research (Section 2.4.1), it was anticipated that, in this study,

higher levels of knowledge sharing behavior would be found among those individuals

similar in gender. Unexpectedly, this was found to be not entirely true, as data analysis

only found small negative relationships between gender homophily and knowledge

sharing behavior. With positive referents, there were no relationships found between

gender homophily and willingness to share knowledge, overall willingness to use

knowledge, or perceived receipt of useful knowledge. However, a negative relationship

was found between willingness to use explicit knowledge and gender similarity (i.e. no

relationship was found with willingness to use tacit knowledge). This finding suggested

that respondents were more willing to use explicit knowledge from those positive referent

co-workers different from them in gender.

The results with positive referents seemed to further suggest that gender homophily did

not exist in the firm, possibly due to an industry practice of how groups are assembled for

projects. As previously mentioned in the gender homophily and trust (Section 5.1.1.1), a

significant reason for why gender homophily was not found may have been due to the

composition of legal project teams. Each team was comprised of ‘legal professionals’

Page 220: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  208  

supported by paralegals and legal clerks. However, the gender distribution of legal

professionals was far more heterogeneous (52% men / 48% women) than that of the

paralegals (9% men / 91% women), yet the two still heavily relied on each other and

actively worked together to bring legal matters to a close. The one significant negative

relationship with positive referents, between gender homophily and willingness to use

explicit knowledge, was likely the result of legal professionals relying on

clerks/paralegals, of the opposite gender, for their supporting legal documents (i.e.

explicit knowledge). This should come as no surprise, since, according to one senior

partner, a significant portion of the paralegal/clerk job description required them to gather

and organize more explicit forms of knowledge. Interestingly, this same relationship was

not present with negative referents, suggesting that respondents preferred to rely on

paralegals or clerks they worked well with, for their useable explicit knowledge.

With negative referents, there were no significant relationships between gender

homophily and willingness to share or use knowledge. There was, however, a negative

relationship between gender homophily and the perceived receipt of useful knowledge.

This finding suggested that respondents perceived knowledge from negative referents,

different from them in gender, as being more useful than knowledge from the co-workers

of the same gender. A reason for this relationship was not entirely clear, since the same

relationship was not found between respondents and positive referents. It might be

possible that there was an interference effect, if the respondents wanted to indicate that

gender was not the basis of their failure to work well with another individual. However,

the results did point to the need to conduct more research in this area and further explore

these potential relationships. As suggested by Connelly and Kelloway (2003), “the

impact of gender on knowledge sharing in organizations has thus far not received much

attention from academics who study knowledge sharing” (p. 300).

Educational Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Based on previous research studies (Section 2.4.1) and on the legal environment (i.e.

participants that have education in the area of law), it was anticipated that, in this study,

higher levels of knowledge sharing behavior would be found among those individuals

Page 221: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  209  

who had similar educational achievements. Unexpectedly, this was found to be not true,

as data analysis showed no significant relationships between educational homophily and

knowledge sharing behavior, in either group.

Both possible suggestions, for why relationships between knowledge sharing behaviors

and educational homophily were not discovered in this study, have been previously

discussed (Section 5.1.1.1). To summarize, first, respondents may have had a belief that

the organization hired skilled co-workers, regardless of the education they achieved. This

confidence in the reliability of the organization’s hiring policies, combined with a display

of skill or competence, by the co-worker, may have been all that was needed for both

parties to be willing to participate in the knowledge exchange. The second, more likely,

reason for why educational homophily was not found, was the significant difference in

educational levels between ‘legal professionals’ and clerks/ paralegals. As previously

mentioned, the education level was, on average, much higher for a ‘legal professional’

than a law clerk/paralegal; yet these two groups interacted extensively on each legal

project, and heavily relied on each other for information, which was vital to the success

of the project.

Summary of Homophily and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Contrary to what has been theorized and found in other research settings, homophily, or

similarity in age, gender, and education, had very little significant effect on a

respondent’s knowledge sharing behaviors, in this study. Interestingly, two of the three

significant effects discovered showed reverse gender homophily to take place (i.e.

willingness to use knowledge from positive referents and perceived receipt of useful

knowledge from negative referents), which was the opposite of what was expected and

found in previous research. As previously mentioned, the results suggested a need to

conduct more research in this area, to further explore these potential relationships. The

single positive homophilous relationship found between age homophily and willingness

to use tacit knowledge was explained by postulating that tacit knowledge was generally

easier to share between those individuals who had a level of shared experience and age.

Page 222: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  210  

5.1.3.2 Shared Perspective and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Shared perspective was defined for the second research question in the same manner as it

was for the first research question. As such, shared perspective was measured using two

separate constructs: shared language and shared vision. A complete discussion of their

effect on knowledge sharing behavior is discussed in the sections below.

Shared Language and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Considering both the existing literature (Section 2.4.2) and the assumption that shared

language was likely to be an important precondition for consideration of any important

legal matter, it was expected that, in this study, higher levels of knowledge sharing

behavior would be found among those individuals who shared a language. The results

with positive referents partially supported the hypothesis, showing that shared language

was significantly positively related to a respondent’s willingness to share knowledge

(overall, explicit and tacit) but not significantly related to a respondent’s willingness to

use knowledge, or a respondent’s perception that the knowledge they received from

positive referents was useful.

Several studies (Section 2.4.2) have found similar positive connections between

willingness to share knowledge (i.e. knowledge sharing behavior) and shared language.

Using these studies, one might extract various reasons explaining the effect of shared

language on willingness to share knowledge. For example, shared language may have

provided reassurance for the respondent that their time would not be wasted, since they

shared a domain with the co-worker (Tsoukas & Vladimirou, 2001; Nonaka, 1994) or

that the co-worker would be able to better interpret the knowledge (Henderson, 2005;

Zenger & Lawrence, 1989). In a legal setting, it may also be possible for shared language

to be instrumental in providing access to knowledge sharing opportunities. For example,

shared language may have been a necessary prerequisite for knowledge sharing to take

place, especially within a specific legal practice area (e.g. intellectual property, maritime

law, etc.). Respondents may have been less willing to share knowledge with those outside

of their practice group (i.e. those with whom they did not share a language), because they

Page 223: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  211  

felt it was a waste of time, or simply because they lacked access (which would be

consistent with the findings of Nahapiet and Ghoshal (1998)).

However, a reason for the lack of relationship between shared language and willingness

to use knowledge was unclear. Perhaps one reason may be elucidated from the work of

Hoe and McShane (2002), who found shared vision to be a strong predictor for informal

knowledge sharing, but not at all associated with informal knowledge acquisition (a

construct similar in nature to willingness to use knowledge). The authors suggested that

this may have been because “informal knowledge acquisition is a more passive or natural

activity whereas informal knowledge dissemination requires more active motivation

guided by shared vision” (p. 289). Extending this idea to shared language, one may argue

that informal knowledge sharing behavior, guided by shared language, may also require

more active motivation (as compared to knowledge use behavior).

The absence of a relationship between shared language and perceived receipt of useful

knowledge is difficult to explain, since it may be expected that the respondents felt that

shared language was a necessary prerequisite for inclusion in a legal practice, on a legal

team, or on a legal matter. Assuming this was the case, each respondent may have gone

into new projects feeling that their co-workers shared a common language with them, by

virtue of inclusion on the project. However, as evidenced by the results, sharing a

language was not a precondition to knowledge sharing, in this setting. Further research

would be required to explore this relationship in more detail.

For the most part, results from negative referents mimicked those from positive referents,

with a notable exception: shared language was no longer related to overall willingness to

share knowledge and willingness to share tacit knowledge. There was, however, a

significant positive relationship found between shared language and willingness to share

explicit knowledge. This result suggested that even when respondents felt there was not a

good working relationship between them and their co-worker, they were still willing to

share explicit forms of knowledge, if the two shared a language. This would confirm

earlier speculations as to the effect of participation in a domain specific legal practice or

Page 224: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  212  

matter. More interestingly, for poor working relationships, respondents were no longer

willing to share tacit knowledge, suggesting that they were no longer interested in

investing the extra time and effort needed to transfer tacit knowledge (Nonaka, 1994).

The results in this group suggested that shared language may not be a sufficient

precondition for the sharing of knowledge with negative referents, especially tacit

knowledge.

Shared Vision and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Based on the literature review (Section 2.4.2), it was expected that, in this study, shared

vision would have a positive effect on knowledge sharing behavior. As predicted, with

positive referents, significant positive relationships were found between shared vision

and overall willingness to share knowledge, overall willingness to use knowledge, and

perceived receipt of useful knowledge. These findings were consistent with those made

by previous authors, and again highlighted the important role shared vision played in the

knowledge transfer process. Building on previous studies, this research aimed to capture

a more comprehensive understanding of the influence of shared vision on knowledge

sharing behavior, by distinguishing between the behaviors of a knowledge transmitter and

receiver (i.e. a willingness to share vs. a willingness to use). This research also

considered knowledge type (explicit vs. tacit) and perceived usefulness of knowledge

shared.

With respect to knowledge type, the results showed that, with positive referents, there

were significant relationships only between shared vision and willingness to share and

use tacit knowledge. In fact, when shared vision was analyzed with willingness to share

explicit knowledge and willingness to use explicit knowledge, no significant relationships

were found. This suggested that, with positive referents, shared vision had an important

influence on willingness to transfer (i.e. share and use) tacit knowledge. This was an

important finding, since tacit knowledge is routinely known as being difficult to transfer.

The results showed that the sharing of values or goals may be necessary pre-conditions to

the transfer of tacit knowledge. This finding also built on the work of Chiu, Hsu, and

Wang (2006), in demonstrating that having a shared vision among co-workers led to

Page 225: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  213  

more meaningful, quality exchanges between them (i.e. the transference of tacit

knowledge).

With negative referents, shared vision was also positively related to willingness to share

knowledge (overall, explicit and tacit) and overall willingness to use knowledge. In

addition, shared vision had a positive influence on willingness to use tacit knowledge;

willingness to use explicit knowledge was still found to be not significant. These findings

with negative referents, for the most part, confirmed those with positive referents and

perhaps highlighted an even more complete effect of shared vision on knowledge sharing

behavior with the influence on willingness to share explicit knowledge.

There was another interesting result with respect to the relationship between shared

vision and perceived receipt of useful knowledge. Of all the variables tested in the first

group, shared vision had the strongest effect on perceived receipt of useful knowledge.

However, in the second group, shared vision was found not to have any significant effect

on perceived receipt of useful knowledge. One possible suggestion for this difference

between the groups may have been because respondents could not get past the nature of

the poor working relationship, and generally saw a majority of knowledge from negative

referents as not being useful, even if the two co-workers happened to share a common

vision. Alternatively, this may have been influenced by the significant relationships

found between shared vision and willingness to share explicit knowledge. For example,

with positive referents, only tacit knowledge was found to be significant, which

respondents may have perceived as being more useful than explicit knowledge. However,

in the second group, shared vision was found to also influence willingness to share

explicit knowledge, which respondents may have viewed as being routine or necessary to

the project, more than useful to the outcomes of the matter. Further research would be

warranted to study the usefulness of tacit and explicit knowledge.

Page 226: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  214  

Summary of Shared Perspective and Knowledge Sharing Behavior (Shared Language

and Shared Vision)

Overall, having a shared perspective had a positive effect on knowledge sharing behavior

in the firm. As expected, the results showed a positive relationship between shared

perspective and willingness to share knowledge, in both groups; where higher degrees of

shared language or shared vision led to higher willingness by the respondent to share

knowledge with their co-worker, regardless of the working relationship. The results also

showed an expected positive influence of shared vision on willingness to use knowledge,

in both groups. Surprisingly, no significant relationships were found between shared

language and willingness to use knowledge, in either group.

Perhaps the most interesting finding was the effect of shared vision on perceived receipt

of useful knowledge. The results, with positive referents, suggested that having a shared

vision led to a significantly higher perception that the knowledge received from that co-

worker was useful. However, shared vision did not remain a significant influence with

negative referents, where the working relationship was deemed as poor. This suggested a

need to explore the effect of working relationships further, in subsequent research.

Finally, shared language was not found to influence perceived receipt of useful

knowledge in either group.

5.1.3.3 Structural Characteristics and Knowledge Sharing Behavior (Tie Strength and

Relationship Length)

The structurally oriented social-cognitive factors tested for this research question were

the same as the ones used in the first research question (i.e. tie strength and relationship

length). As with the first research question, tie strength was further analyzed by

separating it into two constructs: tie strength between co-workers prior to the project and

tie strength between co-workers while on the project. A complete discussion of the effect

of structural factors on knowledge sharing behavior is in the sections below.

Page 227: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  215  

Tie Strength and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Based on the literature review (Section 2.4.3), it was difficult to predict the relationships

between tie strength and knowledge sharing behavior, as previous studies have found

contradicting results. However, after considering the setting of this study and fairly

similar settings in previous research, it was expected that tie strength would be positively

related to knowledge sharing behavior. As previously mentioned, tie strength (prior to

the project) was analyzed with each of the knowledge sharing behaviors independently

from tie strength (while on the project).

Tie Strength (Prior to the Project)

The results of the analysis, with positive referents, showed that no significant

relationships were found between tie strength (prior to the project) and any knowledge

sharing behaviors. Thus, for this group, tie strength (prior to the project) was not

positively related to knowledge sharing behavior. The findings, in this group, were

contrary to prior research, since they showed neither weak nor strong ties to have a

significant effect on knowledge sharing behavior. The results, for this group, were also

counter to the work of Levin and Cross (2004), who found that tie strength, mediated by

trust, positively affected knowledge sharing behavior. One possible reason for the results

in this group may have been that, in this professional setting, knowledge sharing did not

depend on the development of close relationships, or on the frequency of interaction, but

on the characteristics of the project itself. Knowledge sharing may have been expected to

take place, as it was an appropriate part of the routine work process, and not because of

prior or present ties between co-workers. For example, the extent of interaction may have

depended on the nature of knowledge exchanged. Just because legal projects were

knowledge-intensive, it did not mean that there necessarily was intensive interaction

between the members of the project. The results, for this group, also suggested that other

factors influenced knowledge sharing behavior, with positive referents, and that the

number of previous interactions or feelings of closeness did not play a role.

The previous suggestion was also supported by the findings, with negative referents,

where stronger tie strength, prior to the project, had a positive influence on knowledge

Page 228: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  216  

sharing behavior. Specifically, in this group, stronger ties, prior to the project, led to

higher overall willingness to share knowledge on the project. This result was peculiar,

even if it was consistent with previous research, since it suggested that strong ties

developed in prior work encounters superseded a negative current working relationship,

to influence knowledge sharing behavior. On the other hand, the results made sense, since

respondents may be more willing to share their knowledge with those negative referents

co-workers with whom they had more prior interaction, or had more prior feelings of

closeness towards. In these cases, the respondents would be more familiar with these co-

workers and could share their knowledge with less effort, or time, invested in working

with someone they felt they did not work well with.

With negative referents, tie strength (prior to the project) was also found to significantly

influence willingness to share tacit knowledge, yet had no effect on willingness to share

explicit knowledge. These findings were consistent with prior research, which also found

strong ties to positively influence tacit knowledge sharing. One possible reason for this

may have been because, as previously discussed, sharing explicit knowledge was

considered a part of normal work routines. This is further reinforced by the fact that the

group members on a particular project likely had very similar backgrounds, with respect

to the interpretation and codification of explicit knowledge. The same cannot be said of

tacit knowledge.

In the same group, stronger ties, prior to the project, also led to more effective knowledge

sharing behavior, in the form of higher willingness to use knowledge (overall, explicit

and tacit). Like in the previous section, strong ties developed on prior work encounters

had more effect on knowledge sharing behavior than the nature of the working

relationship between the co-workers. As previously mentioned, this can be explained by

suggesting that the respondents were more familiar with these co-workers, than those

they had less contact with and, hence, would more easily (or quickly) be able to use

knowledge from them. Finally, stronger ties, prior to the project, had no effect on the

perception that the knowledge received was useful, in either group.

Page 229: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  217  

Tie Strength (While on the Project)

Unlike prior to the project, stronger ties (while on the project) were found to not have any

effect on a respondent’s willingness to share or willingness to use knowledge. However,

with negative referents, stronger ties, while on the project, led respondents to perceiving

knowledge from these co-workers as being more useful. Even though the same

relationship was not found with positive referents, this result with negative referents was

consistent with the literature and made logical sense, since the existence of a strong tie

may have given respondents more confidence in the knowledge that their co-worker

provided. Strong ties may also have led to more emotional attachment, which may have

influenced the confidence a respondent had in the negative referent co-worker’s

knowledge.

Summary of Tie Strength and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

The research produced a number of noteworthy findings from examining the

relationships between tie strength and each of the knowledge sharing behaviors with

negative referents, but found no significant relationships with positive referents. Making

the distinction between positive and negative referents turned out to be valuable in this

study, since significant relationships were only found with negative referents. With

negative referents, relationships were found between tie strength prior to the project and

willingness to share and use knowledge; and between tie strength while on the project

and perceived receipt of useful knowledge. Some suggestions have been made above to

explain why this relationship varied, but further research would be needed to explore this

relationship in more detail.

Finally, the results confirmed that tie strength (prior to the project) and tie strength

(while on the project) were separate constructs; validating their inclusion as control

variables. The two constructs had different effects on knowledge sharing behavior, even

in the same group (i.e. as witnessed by the results with negative referents). More research

is needed to fully explore these differences, but future studies should consider the

importance of making this distinction by controlling for the two variables, along with

Page 230: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  218  

considering the nature of the working relationship (i.e. by incorporating a positive and

negative referent group).

Relationship Length and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Even though very few research studies were found that tested the effect of relationship

length on knowledge sharing behavior, it was believed that, based on the existing studies

with similar constructs (Section 2.4.4), relationship length would be positively related to

knowledge sharing behavior. However, the results from both groups suggested that

relationship length had no effect on any knowledge sharing behavior. This result was

interesting, since it suggested that respondents were willing to share and use explicit and

tacit knowledge both with co-workers they knew a long time and co-workers they only

knew a short time. In addition, respondents did not perceive the knowledge received from

co-workers they knew a long time as being any more useful than the knowledge received

from co-workers they knew only a short time. One possible reason for this may have to

do with project group composition. Group composition was primarily determined by the

firm, who assigned workers to a legal practice-group or legal matter, based on their

domain knowledge, skill, and availability, and not how long they may have known their

co-workers. The resulting legal teams still had to share and use each other’s knowledge,

to successfully accomplish their work. Such a scenario could have resulted in effective

knowledge sharing behaviors with varying relationship lengths between co-workers,

explaining the lack of relationships found.

Summary of Structural Factors and Knowledge Sharing Behavior (Tie Strength and

Relationship Length)

Overall, the structural factors tested in this study had minimal effect on knowledge

sharing behavior, especially after considering the nature of the working relationship. For

example, relationship length had no significant effect on knowledge sharing behavior, in

either group, and tie strength only had a significant positive effect with negative

referents. Tie strength had no effect on any knowledge sharing behavior with positive

referents. With negative referents, tie strength prior to the project had a positive effect on

both willingness to share knowledge and willingness to use knowledge with that co-

Page 231: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  219  

worker. In the same group, tie strength, while on the project, led to a higher perception

that the knowledge received was useful. These findings suggested that tie strength may

have had only a limited effect on knowledge sharing behavior, and was useful only in

relationships with negative referents. Notably, the results did suggest the importance of

considering both tie strength prior to the project and tie strength while on the project, as

their relationships with knowledge sharing behaviors varied. The same can be said for

highlighting the importance of studying a respondent’s relationship with both positive

and negative referents.

5.1.4 Research Question 2 Summary

The second research question asked “what are the significant relationships between

social-cognitive variables and knowledge sharing behavior?” The results of the study

showed there to be a weak partial support for a relationship between homophily (i.e. age

and gender) and willingness to use knowledge, but only with positive referents. The

results also showed a partial support for a positive relationship between shared language

and willingness to share knowledge, in both groups. Next, support was found for a

positive relationship between shared vision and all of the knowledge sharing behaviors,

with positive referents. With negative referents, shared vision had a positive influence on

willingness to share knowledge and willingness to use knowledge. There was no

evidence of any relationships between relationship length and any knowledge sharing

behavior, in either group. There was also no evidence of any relationship between tie

strength (prior to or while on the project) and any knowledge sharing behavior, with

positive referents. However, with negative referents, the results showed there to be

positive relationships between tie strength and all the knowledge sharing behaviors.

5.1.5 Relationships Between Trust and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Research Question 3:

What are the significant relationships between trust and knowledge sharing behavior?

Hypothesis Tested for RQ3:

H15 Overall trust will be positively related to knowledge sharing behavior

Page 232: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  220  

The third research question explored the significant relationships between trust and

knowledge sharing behavior. Knowledge sharing behavior was operationalized in the

same manner as it was in the second research question (i.e. measuring willingness to

share knowledge, willingness to use knowledge, and perceived receipt of useful

knowledge). Similarly, the measure of trust was operationalized in the same manner as it

was for the first research question56. One hypothesis exploring the existence of a positive

effect of overall trust on knowledge sharing behavior was tested to answer the third

research question.

Based on the evidence previously discussed in the literature review on trust and

knowledge sharing behavior (Section 2.2.2), it was expected that if respondents had

higher overall trust in co-workers, then they would also have higher willingness to share

knowledge with them, higher willingness to use knowledge from them, and a higher

perception that knowledge received from them had useful outcomes.

Trust and Willingness to Share Knowledge

The results of the study found that overall trust had a positive influence on overall

willingness to share knowledge, with both positive and negative referents. These findings

were similar to other research that looked at relationships between various types of trust

and willingness to share knowledge (Holste, 2003; Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998; Hinds &

Pfeffer, 2003; Van den Hooff & Van Weenen, 2004; Lin, 2007; Hislop, 2003). These

results were also similar to other research that found trust to have a positive effect on

knowledge sharing behaviors (Renzl, 2008; Ho, Kuo, Lin, & Lin, 2010; Ho, Kuo, & Lin,

2011) and activities (Andrews & Delahaye, 2000).

For positive referents, trust had a positive influence on willingness to share tacit

knowledge, but provided no benefit for sharing explicit or codified knowledge (i.e. no

significant relationships were found). These results were similar to those found in

previous research (e.g. Levin & Cross, 2004; Yang & Farn, 2009; Lin, 2007).

                                                                                                               56 The measure of overall trust was based on Mayer et al.’s (1995) measure for perceived trustworthiness, which is a set of three trust related characteristics (i.e. ability, benevolence, and integrity) the trustor (i.e. respondent) believed the trustee (i.e. co-worker) to possess.

Page 233: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  221  

Interestingly, these results contrasted those of Holste (2003), who found trust to also have

a significant positive effect on a willingness to share both explicit and tacit knowledge,

with positive referents.

One possible explanation for the findings with positive referents may be that respondents

had a higher base level of confidence that the knowledge shared would be used

appropriately by these co-workers, which was independent of trust. With a higher level of

confidence in the positive referent’s ability to internalize the knowledge, there may have

been less need for trust to exist between the individuals. Willingness to share explicit

knowledge may also be considered routine work practice between these individuals, as

respondents would not choose to avoid interacting with positive referents, like they may

with negative ones. This explanation was further justified by the results with negative

referents, where trust had a positive influence on both types of knowledge, with a slightly

stronger influence on willingness to share explicit knowledge, than on willingness to

share tacit knowledge57. In this group, the respondent’s negative feeling towards this co-

worker may have caused them to have less confidence that shared knowledge would be

used appropriately, making trust more important for both forms of knowledge.

Trust and Willingness to Use Knowledge

The results of the study also found that overall trust had a positive influence on overall

willingness to use knowledge, from both positive and negative referents. These findings

were similar to other research, which looked at relationships between various types of

trust and constructs similar to willingness to use knowledge (Holste, 2003; Andrews &

Delahaye, 2000; Tsai & Ghoshal, 1998; Zand, 1972; McAllister, 1995; Bromiley &

Cummings, 1995). Trust also had a noticeably stronger positive effect on overall

willingness to use knowledge, than it did on overall willingness to share knowledge, in

both groups. In addition, the results showed trust to have a positive influence on

willingness to use both explicit and tacit knowledge, with a slightly stronger influence on

tacit knowledge, in both groups. Based on Holste’s (2003) work, this was the expected

result for negative referents, but not for positive ones, where he found trust to have a                                                                                                                57 Holste (2003) found trust to have a greater effect on willingness to share tacit knowledge than on willingness to share explicit knowledge in this group

Page 234: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  222  

moderately greater impact on willingness to use tacit knowledge than on willingness to

use explicit knowledge.

In this study, one possible explanation for the relationships between trust and willingness

to use knowledge may have to do with the level of risk associated with each form of

knowledge. For example, in a legal environment, it may be less risky to use someone’s

tacit knowledge, than his or her explicit knowledge. This can be a challenge, since the

transfer and use of explicit knowledge can be considered quite routine in this

environment. For example, explicit knowledge is more likely to be directly used, and

reused, in project decisions and deliverables. Explicit knowledge is also more likely to be

used by clients, managers, and partners, in evaluating the projects and employees. When

knowledge is made explicit, it is written and made formal, forcing the employee to put

the knowledge “on the record”, which carries accountability and social risk. The

respondent inherently takes a risk by using a co-worker’s explicit knowledge, because

they must ultimately be accountable for the knowledge used. There is less risk that tacit

knowledge would be shown as being used inappropriately, because tacit knowledge is

more difficult to represent in formal legal project deliverables.

Trust and Perceived Reciept of Useful Knowledge

The results from this study found trust to have a positive influence on perceived receipt

of useful knowledge, in both groups. In fact, the influence of trust on perceived receipt of

useful knowledge was the strongest of all the knowledge sharing behaviors tested in the

study. Between the two groups, trust had a significantly stronger impact on perceived

receipt of useful knowledge from negative referents; however, this relationship was

significant in both groups. The finding in this study was similar to the one discovered by

Levin and Cross (2004), although their hypotheses did not set out to test the direct

relationship between trust and perceived receipt of useful knowledge.

As with willingness to use knowledge, one possible explanation for this result may also

involve a respondent’s assessment of risk, which is known as an integral part of

organizational trust (Mayer et al., 1995; 1999). For example, higher levels of trust, by a

Page 235: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  223  

respondent, in their co-worker, may have reduced feelings of risk associated with the

quality of knowledge shared (Mayer et al., 1995). A reduction of this risk, associated with

the quality of knowledge, increased the respondent confidence in the co-workers

knowledge. Together, an increased confidence and decreased risk in the co-worker’s

knowledge, would expectedly raise perceptions that the knowledge received was useful to

the respondent, their project, and the firm.

5.1.6 Research Question 3 Summary

Overall, as expected, trust had a significant positive influence on the combined

knowledge sharing behaviors in the study (i.e. overall willingness to share, overall

willingness to use, and perceived receipt of useful knowledge). Of the knowledge sharing

behaviors, trust had the weakest influence on overall willingness to share knowledge,

with a similar weak influence on willingness to share tacit knowledge, in both groups.

The influence of trust on willingness to share explicit knowledge was only significant for

negative referents. Trust was also found to have a strong positive influence on overall

willingness to use knowledge, willingness to use explicit knowledge, and willingness to

use tacit knowledge. Finally, of all the knowledge sharing behaviors, trust had the largest

positive influence on perceived receipt of useful knowledge, which was significant for

both groups, but about 50% stronger in relationships with negative referents, versus those

with positive referents.

5.1.7 Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Knowledge Sharing

Behavior

Research Question 4:

What is the collective effect of the identified social-cognitive variables and trust on

knowledge sharing behavior?

Hypotheses Tested for RQ4:

H16 Overall trust and social-cognitive factors explain knowledge sharing behavior

The fourth research question explored the collective effect of overall trust and social-

cognitive factors on each of the knowledge sharing behaviors. Knowledge sharing

behavior, overall trust, and each of the social-cognitive factors were operationalized in

Page 236: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  224  

the same manner as they were in the previous research questions. One hypothesis

explored how overall trust and social-cognitive factors explain knowledge sharing

behavior.

It was difficult to predict the resulting effect of the analysis for this research question

based on previous research, since no known organizational study examined the collective

effects of trust, and the specific social-cognitive factors identified in this study, on

knowledge sharing behavior. Also, no other known study has looked at the effect of this

number of social-cognitive factors and trust on knowledge sharing behavior. The research

discussed in the literature review has often identified trust and numerous interrelated

social-cognitive factors as motivators and inhibitors of knowledge sharing behavior, but

have not examined more than few of these constructs collectively.

For the purposes of presenting and discussing the findings relating to this research

question and hypothesis, a social capital framework was adopted. The factors in the study

were organized in an analytical and conceptual way, similar to the one used by Nahapiet

and Ghoshal (1998) in their classification of social capital components or dimensions.

Since the factors used in this study were conceptually similar to the ones used by

Nahapiet and Ghoshal (1998), they could be classified using the same three theoretical

categories: relational (i.e. trust and homophily), cognitive (i.e. shared language and

shared vision), and structural (i.e. tie strength and relationship length). Importantly, this

representation was not conceptually different from the original theoretical model of this

study (Figure 5.1). This representation was only a slightly different way of organizing

and presenting the results, for the purpose of discussion. It was recognized in this

research, as it was by Nahapiet and Ghoshal (1998) that although these three categories

were separated analytically, many of their features may be argued to be highly

interrelated.

Figure 5.2 (Positive Referent Group) and 5.3 (Negative Referent Group) are visual

representations of the findings of this study, with respect to the collective effect of trust

and social-cognitive factors on knowledge sharing behavior. The sections following the

Page 237: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  225  

two figures offer discussion of the fourth research question, elaborating on each specific

knowledge sharing behavior (i.e. willingness to share knowledge, willingness to use

knowledge, and perceived receipt of useful knowledge). The discussion of this research

question closes with a more holistic view of the factors and their effect on knowledge

sharing behavior, as a whole.

Figure 5.2 Collective Effect of Factors on Knowledge Sharing Behavior for Positive

Referents (Group 1) (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Page 238: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  226  

Figure 5.3 Collective Effect of Factors on Knowledge Sharing Behavior for Negative

Referents (Group 2) (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Willingness to Share

Knowledge

In this study, trust, shared language, and shared vision were found to be the most

important factors to influence willingness to share knowledge, with tie strength (prior to

the project) displaying a minimal effect with negative referents.

Beginning with the relational dimension (Figures 5.2 and 5.3), the findings showed that

the effect of trust on overall willingness to share knowledge was significant and of

relatively similar strength, for both groups. These findings confirmed previous research

studies discussed in the literature review (Section 2.2.2). Specifically, with positive

referents, higher trust had a positive effect on overall willingness to share knowledge and

willingness to share tacit knowledge, with little to no effect on explicit knowledge

sharing. In the second group, trust had a positive effect on overall willingness to share

Page 239: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  227  

knowledge, willingness to share explicit knowledge, and willingness to share tacit

knowledge. Homophily, was not found to have any effect on willingness to share

knowledge of any type, in either group.

The cognitive dimension (Figures 5.2 and 5.3) was found to have the strongest effect on

willingness to share knowledge, in both groups. Specifically, with positive referents,

shared language had a positive effect on overall willingness to share knowledge,

willingness to share explicit knowledge, and willingness to share tacit knowledge. With

negative referents, shared language was not as important, with no influence on overall

willingness to share knowledge or willingness to share tacit knowledge, and only a small

influence on willingness to share explicit knowledge.

Shared vision had a stronger influence on willingness to share knowledge, than overall

trust. However, as with overall trust, with positive referents, shared vision had a positive

influence on overall willingness to share knowledge and willingness to share tacit

knowledge, with no effect on explicit knowledge sharing. With negative referents, shared

vision had a positive influence on overall willingness to share knowledge, willingness to

share explicit knowledge, and willingness to share tacit knowledge. Notably, the effect on

willingness to share tacit knowledge was almost twice the effect on willingness to share

explicit knowledge (Figure 5.3). This result suggests that having a shared vision is

generally important in the sharing of tacit knowledge.

With respect to the structural dimension (Figures 5.2 and 5.3), the effect on willingness to

share knowledge was minimal. For example, neither tie strength nor relationship length

were found to have any influence on willingness to share knowledge, with positive

referents. However, with negative referents, tie strength prior to the project was found to

have a small, yet significant, influence on overall willingness to share knowledge and

willingness to share tacit knowledge, with no effect on explicit knowledge. This result

suggested that one possible method of encouraging respondents to share their tacit

knowledge with negative referents may be to create opportunities for prior interactions,

as well as possibilities to develop feelings of closeness. Finally, relationship length was

Page 240: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  228  

not found to have any significant influence on any form of willingness to share

knowledge, with negative referents.

Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Willingness to Use Knowledge

In this study, trust and shared vision were found to have the greatest influence on

willingness to use knowledge, in both groups; with tie strength (prior to the project)

showing a small positive effect, in relationships with negative referents (Figures 5.2 and

5.3). A couple of additional small significant relationships were found between

homophily and willingness to use knowledge, but only for relationships with positive

referents.

Trust was the single most important factor influencing willingness to use knowledge in

this study. In both groups, overall trust was found to have a positive influence on overall

willingness to use knowledge, willingness to use explicit knowledge, and willingness to

use tacit knowledge. These findings showed the importance of trust to the receiving end

of the knowledge sharing process. The findings also showed that respondents would be

equally willing to use knowledge from co-workers they had both good and bad working

relationships with, as long as the respondents trusted them. In other words, the nature of

the working relationships in this study had no effect on the influence of trust on

willingness to share knowledge.

Shared vision was the second most influential factor on willingness to use knowledge.

Specifically, in both groups, shared vision had a positive influence on overall willingness

to use knowledge and willingness to use tacit knowledge, with no effect on willingness to

use explicit knowledge. This was an important finding, since it, once again, showed

shared vision to be a key factor in influencing effective knowledge sharing behavior,

especially for the difficult and time-consuming transfer of tacit knowledge.

A few other factors were found in the study to have a small effect on willingness to use

knowledge. First, age homophily was found to have a small positive effect on willingness

to use tacit knowledge, with positive referents. Next, gender homophily was found to

Page 241: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  229  

have a small negative effect (heterogeneous effect) on willingness to use explicit

knowledge from positive referents. Finally, a small positive effect was found with

negative referents between tie strength (prior to the project) and overall willingness to

use knowledge, willingness to use explicit knowledge, and willingness to use tacit

knowledge. These findings showed small effects, but were interesting because they

seemed to depend on the nature of the working relationship. For example, each of the

homophily effects were only found with positive referents, having no effect with negative

referents. On the other hand, tie strength (prior to the project) had no effect with positive

referents, but impacted willingness to use both types of knowledge with negative

referents. Prior interactions or feelings of closeness may have provided confidence in the

negative referent’s knowledge, or minimized feelings of risk associated with using the

knowledge. Notably, these same interactions were not required with positive referents.

These findings demonstrated the role working relationship played in impacting

knowledge sharing behavior, and made a case for the inclusion of this distinction in

future studies. Future studies are also needed to understand the impact of working

relationship on willingness to use knowledge, in alternative organizational settings.

Collective Effect of Social-Cognitive Factors and Trust on Perceived Receipt of Useful

Knowledge

As with the other two knowledge sharing behaviors, overall trust proved to play a strong

role in influencing perceived receipt of useful knowledge. In fact, of all the factors tested,

overall trust had the strongest effect on perceived receipt of useful knowledge, and was

the only factor to show a significant positive effect in both groups (Figures 5.2 and 5.3).

Interestingly, the effect of overall trust on perceived receipt of useful knowledge was

considerably stronger with negative referents, than it was with positive. This strong effect

from overall trust warranted its consideration, as a central factor, in influencing the

effective transfer of knowledge in the form of useful outcomes. This was especially true

for conditions where the working relationship between the co-workers was negative.

The other factors found to influence perceived receipt of useful knowledge seemed to

highly depend on the nature of the working relationship. For example, with positive

Page 242: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  230  

referents, shared vision had a strong significant effect on perceived receipt of useful

knowledge. However, with negative referents, shared vision had no effect. Next, tie

strength (while on the project) had a small positive effect on perceived receipt of useful

knowledge from negative referents, which was not found to exist with positive referents.

Finally, there was a small reverse effect of gender homophily (or gender heterogeneous

effect) on perceived receipt of useful knowledge from negative referents, but none from

positive referents. Some explanations have already been suggested as to why these results

have occurred, although additional research would be needed to explore the unique effect

of working relationships on perceived receipt of useful knowledge in alternative

organizational settings.

5.1.8 Research Question 4 Summary

Adopting a more holistic view of knowledge sharing behavior, the results revealed

overall trust to be the single most important factor to influence knowledge sharing

behavior. Overall trust had a positive effect on all three knowledge sharing behaviors, in

both groups, and was the single most important factor for influencing willingness to use

knowledge and perceived receipt of useful knowledge. The effect of overall trust on

willingness to share knowledge was also significant, but less than one third that of overall

trust on the other knowledge sharing behaviors.

Shared vision was found to be the second most important single factor influencing

effective knowledge sharing behavior; showing a strong positive influence on all the

knowledge sharing behaviors tested in both groups, with only one exception (i.e. having a

shared vision had no influence on the respondent’s perception that the knowledge

received from negative referents was useful). Shared vision was also the single most

important factor found to influence a respondent’s willingness to share knowledge.

Having a shared language was more important, between the respondent and the positive

referent, in influencing effective knowledge sharing behavior. This was especially the

case with the effect of shared language on the respondent’s willingness to share

Page 243: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  231  

knowledge. However, sharing a language with negative referents did not seem to have

much of an influence58 on effective knowledge sharing behavior.

Tie strength was quite statistically significant between the respondent and the negative

referent; and not at all statistically significant with the positive referent. In fact, between

the respondent and the negative referent, tie strength was one of only two factors (trust

being the other) to have a positive effect on all the tested knowledge sharing behaviors.

On the contrary, relationship length was not found to have any influence on any

knowledge sharing behavior, in either group.

Age and gender homophily were found to have a small effect (positive and negative) on

knowledge sharing behavior, although these results were relatively weak and not

consistent across the three behaviors and two groups, to claim that homophily played a

significant role in effective knowledge sharing behavior. Numerous reasons have been

suggested in this chapter for why some of these results have occurred and, in some cases,

additional research was suggested, because the legal setting may have not been an

appropriate industry for examining status homophily.

5.1.9 Mediating Effects of Trust Between Social-Cognitive Factors and Knowledge

Sharing Behavior

Research Question 5:

Does trust act as a mediating variable between social-cognitive variables and knowledge

sharing behavior?

Hypothesis Tested for RQ5:

H17 Overall trust will be will be a mediating variable between social-cognitive factors and

knowledge sharing behavior

The fifth research question in the study explored the mediating effect of trust between

identified social-cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior. Knowledge sharing

behavior and overall trust were operationalized in the same way as they were in the

                                                                                                               58 There was a small positive effect (β = .146) of shared language on willingness to share explicit forms of knowledge with the individuals the respondent did not work well with.

Page 244: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  232  

previous research questions. A total of eight social-cognitive factors were tested for this

research question. These were also the same ones used in the previous research questions

(i.e. 3 x homophily, shared language, shared vision, relationship length and 2 x tie

strength). To answer this research question, one hypothesis was tested, which explored

the existence and strength of the mediating effect of overall trust between social-

cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior.

Based on the limited research studies available (Sections 2.2.2 and 2.4.4), it was expected

that trust would, at least, have a partial mediating effect on some relationships between

social-cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behaviors. However, it was difficult to

predict the presence or the strength of the mediation effects between specific social-

cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behaviors, because previous studies did not use

similar measures, or include as many independent variables in their analysis. Predicting

the effect of working relationships was also difficult, as the only previous study that

examined trust and knowledge sharing behavior for positive and negative referents

(Holste, 2003) did not measure any other variables similar to the social-cognitive factors

tested in this study. For these reasons, strategies to identify the mediating effects of

overall trust, in the study, were mostly exploratory in nature.

To identify and test the mediating effects that overall trust had on the relationships

between social-cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior, logic developed by

Baron and Kenny (1986) was applied. As first step, the direct effect of each social-

cognitive factor was tested against the mediating variable (overall trust) and each of the

dependent variables (knowledge sharing behaviors). To qualify for inclusion, the social-

cognitive factor needed to have a significant relationship with both overall trust and at

least one knowledge sharing behavior. As it can be seen in Figure 5.4, for positive

referents, two social-cognitive factors met this criterion (i.e. shared language and shared

vision), and for negative referents the conditions were only met by one social-cognitive

factor (i.e. shared vision).

Page 245: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  233  

 Figure 5.4 Summary of the Overall Mediating Effects of Trust for Positive Referents

(Group 1, diagram on left) and Negative Referents (Group 2, diagram on right) (*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001)

Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Language and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

Numerous researchers have suggested that co-workers who share a language have greater

trust in each other (Section 2.3.2) and are more effective in their knowledge sharing

behavior (Section 2.4.2). However, no research has specifically explored the mediating

effect of trust, or the potential interactions among the three concepts. The results of this

study found that, with positive referents, overall trust had a partial mediating effect

between shared language and overall willingness to share knowledge. These results

suggested that, overall trust helped explain the effect of shared language on knowledge

sharing behavior between individuals and the co-workers they worked well with. In other

words, trust partially took the place of shared language in increasing willingness to share

knowledge. Overall trust was also found to have a partial mediating effect between

shared language and willingness to share explicit knowledge, and between shared

language and willingness to share tacit knowledge.

Interestingly, overall trust was found to not have a mediating effect between shared

language and overall willingness to use knowledge. However, overall trust did have a

complete or full mediating effect between shared language and willingness to use explicit

knowledge. This result suggested that overall trust helped explain the effect of shared

language on willingness to use explicit knowledge from positive referents. These results

reaffirm the previous suggestion made in Section 5.1.5 that explicit knowledge may have

Page 246: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  234  

more visible associated risk in the legal setting. A similar relationship was not found for

willingness to use tacit knowledge.

Finally, overall trust was found to have a complete mediating effect between shared

language and perceived receipt of useful knowledge, from positive referents. This result

suggested that when trust was higher among co-workers who worked well together, there

was less need for them to share a language, in order to perceive their co-worker’s

knowledge as being useful. This  result  also  suggested  that  trust  helped  to  explain  the  

effect  of  shared  language  on  perceived  usefulness  of  knowledge  received.  Practically,

this finding highlights the important role trust plays in perceived knowledge outcomes in

the firm.

One possible explanation for these results is that trust affected the extent to which

respondents were forthcoming about their lack of shared language with co-workers. By

doing this, they brought attention to the need to bridge these language barriers,

effectively creating a condition for successful knowledge sharing behavior. This would,

to some extent, explain the partial mediating effect of trust between shared language and

willingness to share knowledge, and the complete mediating effect of trust between

shared language and perceived receipt of useful knowledge. However, it does not explain

the results with willingness to use knowledge, where it was unclear why overall trust was

found to not have a mediating effect for relationships between shared language and

overall willingness to use knowledge, and between shared language and willingness to

share tacit knowledge. One possible explanation for the mediating effect of trust between

shared language and willingness to use explicit knowledge was previously alluded to, and

may have to do with the inherent risk associated with explicit knowledge, in this setting.

Further research would be needed to explore these types of relationships, in alternative

settings.

One interesting point worth noting was how the nature of the working relationship played

a significant role in affecting overall trust, as a mediating variable. Specifically, overall

trust was only found to have a mediating effect between shared language and knowledge

Page 247: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  235  

sharing behaviors, with positive referents, with no significant effects found with negative

referents. The primary reason for this was because no direct relationships were found

between shared language and trust, in this group. Without the existence of a direct

relationship, mediation could not exist. Earlier, it was suggested that having a shared

language may have been a necessary prerequisite for working on a legal project and, by

virtue of inclusion in the work group, each respondent may have assumed that their co-

worker shared some degree of language with them. This, however, did not guarantee that

the two co-workers trusted each other, or that trust would play any role. On the other

hand, there were significant relationships found between shared language and knowledge

sharing behaviors, in this group. This could have been because respondents only involved

negative referents in knowledge sharing behaviors, when the two shared a language, to

simplify and quicken the exchange (e.g. as it was a negative one and respondents did not

want to have extended interaction with someone they did not work well with). Additional

research is needed to further explore the effect of working relationship.

Mediating Effect of Trust Between Shared Vision and Knowledge Sharing Behavior

As with shared language, a number of previous researchers have suggested that co-

workers who share a vision have greater trust in each other (Section 2.3.2) and are more

effective in their knowledge sharing behavior (Section 2.4.2). However, no researchers

have specifically explored the mediating effects of trust between shared vision and

knowledge sharing behavior. Interestingly, unlike with shared language, the results from

this study showed the nature of the working relationship to not matter with shared vision

(i.e. a mediating effect was found in both test groups).

The results of the study found that overall trust had a partial mediating effect between

shared vision and overall willingness to share knowledge, with both positive and negative

referents. This result suggested that overall trust helped explain the effect of shared

vision on willingness to share knowledge with both positive and negative referents.

Overall trust was also found to have a complete mediating effect for the relationship

between shared vision and willingness to share explicit knowledge, with positive

referents, and a partial mediating effect for the relationship between shared vision and

Page 248: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  236  

willingness to share explicit knowledge, with negative referents. Next, overall trust was

found to have a partial mediating effect for the relationship between shared vision and

willingness to share tacit knowledge, in both groups.

Overall trust was also found to have a partial mediating effect between shared vision and

overall willingness to use knowledge, in both groups. Next, overall trust was found to

have a complete mediating effect between shared vision and willingness to use explicit

knowledge, in both groups. In addition, overall trust was found to have a partial

mediating effect for the relationship between shared vision and willingness to use tacit

knowledge, in both groups. Overall trust had the greater mediating effect on willingness

to use explicit knowledge and the effect on both types of knowledge was noteworthy.

This result suggested that overall trust helped explain the effect of shared vision on

willingness to use knowledge from both positive and negative referents.

As with shared language, one possible explanation for the above findings may have been

that trust shaped the extent to which respondents were forthcoming about their lack of

shared vision, creating a condition (i.e. willingness or need) for knowledge sharing

behavior. In other words, with higher trust, there was more transparency for the need to

create opportunities to establish a unified vision. These opportunities to build a shared

vision manifested themselves in the form of increased willingness to share and use

knowledge. In either case, the effect of trust on willingness to share and use knowledge,

in lieu of shared vision, really exhibited its role in organizational knowledge sharing

behavior. This was especially true, because similar mediating effects were found in both

groups (i.e. with both positive and negative referents).

Finally, overall trust was found to have a partial mediating effect between shared vision

and perceived receipt of useful knowledge, with positive referents. Interestingly, overall

trust was found to not have any mediating effects between shared vision and perceived

receipt of useful knowledge, with negative referents. Earlier in the chapter it was

suggested that one possible reason for this may have been that respondents could not get

past the nature of the poor working relationships and generally saw a majority of

Page 249: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  237  

knowledge from negative referents as not being useful, even if the two co-workers

happened to share a common vision.

5.1.10 Research Question 5 Summary

Discovering the mediating effects of overall trust between social-cognitive variables and

knowledge sharing behaviors was largely an exploratory exercise, as many of these

relationships have not been investigated in prior research studies. Interestingly, in one

study by Levin and Cross (2004), where similar variables were used, different results

were discovered59.

Of all the relationships tested, overall trust had the strongest mediating effect between

shared vision and willingness to use knowledge. Only slightly weaker was the mediating

effect of overall trust between shared vision and willingness to share knowledge.

Interestingly, both these effects did not depend on the nature of the working relationships,

and were found to occur in both test groups. In both groups, when there was less trust,

respondents used shared vision to decide whether they were willing to participate in

knowledge sharing behaviors with their co-worker.

Overall trust was also found to have a partial mediating effect on the relationship

between shared vision and perceived receipt of useful knowledge, and full mediating

effect between shared language and perceived receipt of useful knowledge. In both these

cases, the mediating effect was only found with positive referents. When respondents did

not trust these co-workers, they used shared language or shared vision as basis for

deciding how useful the positive referent’s knowledge was to the respondent, the project,

or the firm. Interestingly, this same mediating effect was not found with shared language

or shared vision, with negative referents. Even though possible explanations for the lack

of relationships found with negative referents were given above, these results were

counter-intuitive and worthy of further study. Finally, overall trust was found to have a

partial mediating effect between shared language and willingness to share knowledge,

                                                                                                               59 Levin and Cross (2004), who controlled for homophily and other knowledge related factors, found trust to mediate a relationship between tie strength and perceived receipt of useful knowledge; a finding not discovered in either group of this study.

Page 250: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  238  

and between shared language and willingness to use explicit knowledge; yet again only

with positive referents. The lack of significant results with negative referents was, again,

somewhat counter intuitive and worthy of further study.

With respect to knowledge type, overall trust was found to have a complete mediating

effect on three of the four explicit knowledge sharing behaviors relationships tested, with

a partial mediating effect on the fourth. On the contrary, overall trust only had a partial

mediating effect on three of the four tacit knowledge sharing behaviors tested, with no

effect on the fourth. These results suggested that trust had a slightly stronger mediating

effect on behaviors involving the sharing and using of explicit knowledge, versus those

behaviors involving the sharing and using of tacit knowledge. This may be due to the

proposed associated risk involved with explicit knowledge at the firm. Trust also seemed

more likely to have a mediating effect with positive referents, as opposed to negative

ones, but further research would be required to specifically explore why this was the case.

5.2 Discussion of the Main Findings and Research Contributions

The study set out to better understand knowledge sharing behavior in the context of

knowledge intensive organizational project group work. Of specific interest was the

investigation of the extent to which social-cognitive factors influence knowledge sharing

behavior directly and indirectly, through trust. Also of interest are the direct effects trust

has on knowledge sharing behavior and the collective effects of social-cognitive factors

and trust on knowledge sharing behavior. The following are the main findings and

contributions of this study.

1. Shared vision, shared language, and tie strength were found to have the greatest

influence on a respondent’s trust in their co-worker.

Among all the variables examined in the study, the three factors that were found to

have the strongest effect on respondent’s trust in their co-workers were shared vision,

shared language, and tie strength. Of these three variables, shared vision had the

greatest effect on trust, and was found to influence the perceived trustworthiness of

Page 251: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  239  

both positive (i.e. those co-workers the respondent worked best with) and negative

(i.e. those co-workers the respondent did not work well with) referents. This finding

reflect the importance of creating shared goals, shared concerns, and shared purpose

among co-workers working on a project together, to establish conditions where trust

can thrive (Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2006).

Shared language and tie strength were also found to increase trust in a co-worker, but

only in cases where the working relationship between respondent and co-worker was

positive. Creating a shared language entails building an environment where co-

workers are able to easily understand each other, convey information between each

other, and come to agreement (Levin, Whitener, & Cross, 2006).

2. Trust and shared vision were found to have the greatest influence on a respondent’s

knowledge sharing behaviors.

Among all the variables examined in the study, the two factors found to have the

strongest effect on organizational knowledge sharing behavior were trust and shared

vision. Trust had the greatest influence on knowledge sharing behavior, and was

found to positively influence all three knowledge sharing behaviors tested in the

study, with both positive and negative referents. Shared vision also had a strong

positive influence on all the knowledge sharing behaviors with positive referents and

with two of the three knowledge sharing behaviors with negative referents.

It is also worth mentioning that shared language was found to have a positive

influence on two of the knowledge sharing behaviors, with positive referents; and tie

strength developed prior to the project influenced two of the knowledge sharing

behaviors, with negative referents. Developing tie strength required the two co-

workers to have prior social interaction and experience feelings of “closeness”

towards each other (Levin & Cross, 2004; Marsden & Campbell, 1984).

Page 252: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  240  

3. Trust was found to have a mediating effect between shared vision and knowledge

sharing behavior, as well as between shared language and knowledge sharing

behavior.

This study was one of only few to measure the mediating effect between the

identified social-cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behaviors. The previous

studies that have examined this effect have not done so with this number of social-

cognitive factors, or with such a comprehensive conceptualization of knowledge

sharing behavior. This study found that, when perceived trustworthiness was high in

the referent co-worker, there was less need for the respondent and the co-worker to

have a shared vision. Alternatively, when there was less trust between co-workers,

shared vision was required for them to participate in effective knowledge sharing

behavior with each other. Trust also had a mediating effect between shared language

and knowledge sharing behavior, but this was only found in relationships between

respondents and positive referents.

4. Knowledge sharing behavior was influenced by a combination of trust and one or

more social-cognitive factors.

The collective models revealed that each identified knowledge sharing behavior was

influenced by a combination of trust and one or more of the identified social-

cognitive factors for both positive and negative referents. A fairly large proportion of

knowledge sharing behaviors could be explained by these factors in the first group.

However, in the second group, only a small number were explained. For example,

with positive referents the social-cognitive factors explained more of the knowledge

sharing behaviors, than trust on its own. On the other hand, with negative referents,

the collective model showed that social-cognitive factors made a slightly lower or

equal contribution to that of trust alone, in explaining knowledge sharing behavior.

5. In addition to the empirical research contributions, this study made a number of

methodological contributions in the form of conceptualizations for knowledge sharing

Page 253: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  241  

behavior, trust, and tie strength. Also, it has provided a more nuanced and focused

analysis, by factoring for knowledge type and co-worker working relationship.

First, this study adopted a more comprehensive conceptualization of knowledge

sharing behavior than in previous knowledge sharing research, one that includes

characteristics of both the knowledge sender and knowledge receiver. In

conceptualizing knowledge sharing behavior, this study also considered the perceived

impact of the shared knowledge with respect to usefulness to the individual, the

project, and the firm.

The study also considered explicit and tacit forms of knowledge, a distinction only a

few prior studies have made when examining knowledge sharing behavior. This was

an important distinction, since the ease with which one may be able to transfer the

two types of knowledge may greatly differ (Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995). Further, the

factors that motivate a worker to share one type of knowledge may not be the factors

that motivate the transfer of the other (as the results of the study have shown). The

same could be said for the distinction made between tie strength prior to the project

and tie strength while on the project, the inclusion of which was motivated in this

study by the perception that they could be conceptually differentiated. For example,

the frequency with which two co-workers interacted could significantly vary, based

on the project requirements. Thus, it would also be reasonable to assume that the

closeness two co-workers felt towards each other could change, as they continue to

work together and interact.

Since earlier studies have utilized 207 different psychometric trust measures of

organizational trust (McEvily & Tortoriello, 2011), it was important for this study to

adopt a conceptually sound measure for organizational trust, in order to set precedent

for future studies, and to compare the results with previous studies. Following the

advice of McEvily and Tortoriello (2011), who conducted the most complete analysis

of trust measures, this research adopted Mayer, Davis, and Schoorman’s (1995)

model for organizational trust.

Page 254: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  242  

Finally, very few studies have made the distinction between positive and negative

working relationships. This was an important distinction, since it enhanced the

richness and authenticity of this study, by highlighting the different influence

independent variables had on dependent variables based on the nature of the working

relationship. To highlight these, a summary of the unique relationships is provided in

Table 5.1. In addition, taking this approach provided an opportunity for testing the

identified factors and relationships in two theoretical test groups, in one study.

Independent

Variable Dependent Variable Positive

Referents Negative Referents

Age Homophily Willingness to Use Tacit Knowledge X Gender Homophily Willingness to Use Explicit Knowledge X Gender Homophily Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge X Shared Language All types of Trust X Shared Language Overall Willingness to Share and

Willingness to Share Tacit knowledge X

Shared Vision Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge X Shared Vision Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge X Relationship Length

Overall, Integrity, and Benevolence-Based Trust

X

Tie Strength-Prior Benevolence-Based Trust X Tie Strength-While on

Overall, Integrity, and Benevolence-Based Trust

X

Tie Strength-Prior Overall Willingness to Share, Willingness to Share Tacit, Overall Willingness to Use, Willingness to Use Explicit, Willingness to Use Tacit

X

Tie Strength-While Perceived Receipt of Useful Knowledge X Overall Trust Willingness to Share Explicit Knowledge X

Table 5.1 Significant Relationships Unique to the Nature of the Working Relationship Employees are rarely given a choice in the selection of co-workers. Hence, they

sometimes work with others with whom they have not previously had a positive working

relationship. Although organizations may wish to build project teams of individuals who

have previously worked well together, this may be neither possible nor optimal.

Page 255: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  243  

5.3 Limitations of the Study

There are a few limitations of the study that should be acknowledged. First, the study

may have limited external validity, as it only surveyed knowledge workers in one

organization, within one industry. All survey respondents worked on legal matters in the

role of either a legal professional or paralegal/law clerk. As it was previously suggested,

in some cases, the legal industry practices and routines may have influenced the make up

and outcomes of the working relationships, differently from what one might see in other

organizational settings. Examples of this may be the risk associated with sharing and

using explicit knowledge, or the unique relationship between legal professional and law

clerk/paralegal. It is also noteworthy that 87% of respondents identified themselves, at

least partially, as Caucasian and 98% Canadian in citizenship. Even so, this is not

believed to affect the relationship between trust, social-cognitive factors, and knowledge

sharing behavior. Future research should explore these relationships in other

organizational contexts.

The study was also limited in that it only measured self-reported data (i.e. expressed

willingness and perceived usefulness) for knowledge sharing behavior, and not actual

exchange of knowledge, actual employee sharing behaviors, or actual project outcomes.

Future research could attempt to correlate findings from both expressed and actual

knowledge sharing. However, as previously suggested, measuring actual knowledge

sharing or knowledge sharing behavior may prove difficult. Alternatively, future research

could find ways to determine if expressed willingness to share or use knowledge actually

translates into the sharing of knowledge or if the perceived useful knowledge actually has

tangible useful outcomes for the individual, project, or the firm. These techniques were

not employed in this study, as they would have violated the anonymity of the

respondents.

5.4 Implications for Practice

The most significant implication for practitioners, from this study, is that effective

knowledge sharing behavior among knowledge workers is predominately dependent on

the development of co-worker trust and shared vision. Further, that trust between co-

Page 256: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  244  

workers was primarily influenced by shared language and shared vision. Although other

factors were found to be significant, these three (i.e. trust, shared language, and shared

vision) were the most influential, as witnessed by the direct and indirect relationships

between them. Therefore, one possible suggestion to encourage effective knowledge

sharing behavior is to nurture and promote behaviors and practices that create trust,

shared language, and shared vision among co-workers. Management support is important;

since management must establish and clarify objectives, encourage workers to

participate, and, most importantly, reward workers for their time (i.e. for activities to

create and share language, vision, trust, or knowledge).

Looking specifically at knowledge sharing networks, Abrams, Cross, Lesser, and Levin

(2003) performed a review of existing literature and conducted interviews in 20

organizations, to better understand ways to nurture trust in professional organizations.

Based on their research, the authors identified behaviors and practices that managers may

use in their organizations for promoting trust. Table 5.1 summarizes the authors’ trust

building suggestions and associated managerial actions (p. 67). The authors suggest that

certain behaviors and practices promote different kinds of trust. For example, items 1, 5,

8, and 9 promote benevolence-based trust, item 10 promotes competence-based trust (i.e.

ability-based trust), and the rest of the items promote both (items 2, 3, 4, 6, and 7).

However, the authors warn practitioners that “the right set of trust builders to focus on is

likely unique for each organization [and that] the specific environment that one is in will

dictate which trust builders offer the greatest potential to improve trust” (p. 74).

Ultimately, the authors suggest experimenting with the trust builders, to see which work

best for the organization. Interestingly, the findings from the present research validate the

authors’ suggestion for promoting shared language and shared vision as ways of

nurturing trust (i.e. item 6). For this reason, practical and managerial suggestions for

nurturing shared language and shared vision, in an organizational setting, are briefly

discussed below.

Page 257: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  245  

Trust Builder Managerial Actions Trustworthy Behaviors 1. Act with discretion • Be clear about what information you are expected to keep confidential.

• Don’t reveal information you have said you would not and hold others accountable for this.

2. Be consistent between word and deed

• Be clear about what you have committed to do, so there is no misunderstanding. • Set realistic expectations when committing to do something, and then deliver.

3. Ensure frequent and rich communication

• Make interactions meaningful and memorable. • Consider having some face-to-face (or at least telephone) contact. • Develop close relationships.

4. Engage in collaborative communication

• Avoid being overly critical or judgmental of ideas still in their infancy. • Don’t always demand complete solutions from people trying to solve a problem. • Be willing to work with people to improve jointly on their partially formed ideas.

5. Ensure that decisions are fair and transparent

• Make sure that people know how and why personnel rules are applied and that the rules are applied equally. • Make promotion and rewards criteria clear-cut, so people don’t waste time developing a hidden agenda (or trying to decode everyone else’s).

Organizational Factors 6. Establish and ensure shared vision and language

• Set common goals early on. • Look for opportunities to create common terminology and ways of thinking. • Be on the lookout for misunderstandings due to differences in jargon or thought processes.

7. Hold people accountable for trust

• Explicitly include measures of trustworthiness in performance evaluations. • Resist the urge to reward high performers who are not trustworthy. • Keep publicizing key values such as trust—highlighting both rewarded good examples and punished violations—in multiple forums.

Relational Factors 8. Create personal connections

• Create a “human connection” with someone based on non-work things you have in common. • Maintain a quality connection when you do occasionally run into acquaintances, including discussing non-work topics. • Don’t divulge personal information shared in confidence.

9. Give away something of value

• When appropriate, take risks in sharing your expertise with people. • Be willing to offer others your personal network of contacts when appropriate.

Individual Factor 10. Disclose your expertise and limitations

• Make clear both what you do and don’t know. • Admit it when you don’t know something rather than posture to avoid embarrassment. • Defer to people who know more than you do about a topic.

Table 5.2 Summary of Abrams, Cross, Lesser and Levin’s Trust Builders and Associated Managerial Actions (2003, p. 67)

The results from numerous research studies (described in Section 2.3.2) plus the one

conducted here, suggest that having a shared language and shared vision increases trust

among co-workers. Further, the findings in the present study, as well as others, suggest

that having a greater degree of shared vision among co-workers also increases effective

Page 258: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  246  

knowledge sharing behavior. For this reason, it is important for practitioners to

understand how to create and nurture project and work environments where there are

high degrees of shared vision and shared language (along with trust).

Research by Abrams, Cross, Lesser, and Levin (2003) suggests that one possible method

for establishing and ensuring shared perspective is to initiate all new projects with clearly

explicated goals, with respect to group objectives. These explicated goals and group

objectives require co-workers to share their understandings, opinions, and perceptions

around a common reference point (Daft & Lengel, 1986). This also provides the team

with a unified mandate, preventing them from misinterpreting and misdirection. Abrams

et al. (2003) also suggest using the start of the project as a time to clarify unique

terminology, to make sure individuals from different functional backgrounds place

similar meanings on important words, phrases, and concepts. Carlile (2002), as well as

Mäkelä and Brewster (2009), suggest that using flowcharts and other boundary objects

may help in developing a shared resource, through which project members can have a

common language and common reference point. A boundary object is an “object that

serves to coordinate the perspectives of various constituencies” (Wenger, 1998, p. 106).

Other examples include blueprints, prototypes, process models, and legal precedents or

opinions.

Another way the literature suggests to nurture and promote shared language and shared

vision is to expose co-workers to each other’s domain (Carlile, 2002, 2004; Nonaka &

Takeuchi, 1995). According to Mäkelä and Brewster (2009), this allows project members

to better understand each other’s tacit knowledge, codes, and language systems. The

authors (2009; Mäkelä, 2007) suggest that this type of interaction leads to an inside view

of the co-workers practice, which “facilitates an understanding of the discourse and

expected ways of behaving” (2009, p. 596).

The difficult reality in knowledge intensive project groups is that managers cannot fully

anticipate or predict what the nature of the working relationships between co-workers

may be. On every project, it must be assumed that there may be both positive and

Page 259: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  247  

negative working relationships. Therefore, any strategy that is adopted must have a

positive influence in both types of relationships. Although this study has identified

several other factors that influence knowledge sharing behavior; trust, shared vision, and

shared language (in that order) are the most important to promote and nurture in

organizational settings where individuals must work with both positive and negative

referents.

5.5 Conclusion and Future Work

The phenomenon of interest in the present study was the knowledge sharing of

employees in organizations. Specifically, the research examined knowledge workers, on

projects, in a professional service firm. Of particular interest were trust and other social

and cognitive factors, which have been found to influence or inhibit knowledge sharing

behavior, in this setting.

The overarching research question was what are the factors that influence knowledge

sharing behavior directly and indirectly through trust? To elaborate this research

question, three additional questions were posed. What are the significant relationships

between trust and knowledge sharing behavior? What are the significant relationships

between social-cognitive variables and trust? And, what are the significant relationships

between social-cognitive variables and knowledge sharing behavior? To better

understand the direct and indirect effects of social-cognitive variables and trust on

knowledge sharing behavior, two additional research questions were posed. The first

examined the collective effect social-cognitive factors and trust had on knowledge

sharing behavior (i.e. what is the collective effect of the identified social-cognitive

variables and trust on knowledge sharing behavior?). The second additional research

question was concerned with the indirect or intervening effect trust had between social-

cognitive factors and knowledge sharing behavior (i.e. does trust act as a mediating

variable between social-cognitive variables and knowledge sharing behavior?).

The conceptual framework of the study built on previous research studies, and drew from

theoretical foundations from the organizational behavior, psychology, information

Page 260: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  248  

studies, sociology, and management literature on organizational trust and knowledge

sharing. The framework identified the most significant factors found to influence

organizational knowledge sharing behavior directly and indirectly through trust. Specific

factors examined, along with trust, included homophily, shared language, shared vision,

tie strength, and relationship length. The study made methodological contributions in the

form of conceptualizations for knowledge sharing behavior, trust, and tie strength. Also,

it provided a more nuanced and focused analysis, by factoring for knowledge type and

co-worker working relationship.

An exploratory approach was used to conduct empirical research on knowledge workers,

at a large professional service firm. The study collected quantitative data from 275

participants, using a web-based survey as a primary research instrument. The survey was

active from January 26th, 2011 to February 25th, 2011 (31 days) and respondents could

participate at anytime between those dates. The goal of the survey was to understand

which of the social and/or cognitive factors influenced knowledge sharing behavior,

directly and indirectly through trust, which was done by testing the identified hypotheses.

Statistical analyses were used to test the hypotheses, which assisted in answering the

overarching and sub-research questions. Statistical analyses of the data included factor

and reliability analysis, correlation analysis, t-tests, and multiple regression analysis. The

mediating (i.e. indirect) effect of trust was tested using hierarchical multiple regression

analysis. Additional data were gathered from site visits and 90-120 minutes un-

structured interviews with senior partners and the Director of Knowledge Management at

the firm. The additional data collected from the site visits and interviews were used to

further understand and interpret the findings of the study.

The results of the study showed that, of all the factors examined, the three found to have

the strongest effect on respondent’s trust in their co-workers were shared vision, shared

language, and tie strength. Furthermore, the two factors found to have the strongest effect

on organizational knowledge sharing behavior were trust and shared vision. Trust was

also found to have a mediating effect between shared vision and knowledge sharing

behavior, and between shared language and knowledge sharing behavior. In addition,

Page 261: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  249  

collectively, trust and one or more social-cognitive factors influenced knowledge sharing

behavior.

Throughout the study there have been several implications made for future research. The

most notable included exploring the influence of homophily in the corporate versus social

environments; further distinguishing between tie strength prior to the project from tie

strength while on the project; considering the nature of working relationships (i.e.

positive versus negative); testing the framework in other professional firm contexts; and

finally distinguishing between the reported knowledge sharing behavior and the actual

knowledge sharing behavior.

Significant implications for practitioners were that effective knowledge sharing behavior

among co-workers requires a nurturing manager to work on developing co-worker trust

and shared vision and that a manager wanting to promote trust between co-workers must

nurture shared language and shared vision.

Page 262: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  250  

References

Abrams, L. C., Cross, R., Lesser, E., & Levin, D. Z. (2003). Nurturing interpersonal trust

in knowledge-sharing networks. Academy of Management Executive, 17(4), 64-

77.

Adler, P. S. (2002). Market, hierarchy, and trust: The knowledge economy and the future

of capitalism. In C. W. Choo & N. Bontis (Eds.), The strategic management of

intellectual capital and organizational knowledge (pp. 23-46). New York: Oxford

University Press.

Alavi, M., & Leidner, D. E. (2001). Review: Knowledge management and knowledge

management systems: Conceptual foundations and research issues. MIS

Quarterly, 25(1), 107-136.

Andrews, K. M., & Delahaye, B. L. (2000). Influences on knowledge processes in

organizational learning: The psychosocial filter. Journal of Management Studies,

37(6), 797-810.

Aneshensel, C. S. (2002). Theory-based data analysis for the social sciences. Thousand

Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.

Argyres, N. S. (1999). The impact of information technology on coordination: Evidence

from the B-2 "stealth" bomber. Organization Science, 10(2), 162-180.

Argyris, C. (1964). Integrating the individual and the organization. New York, NY:

Wiley.

Babič, F., & Wagner, J. (2006). Modeling of knowledge creation processes based on

activity theory. Paper presented at the WIKT2006: Workshop on Intelligent and

Knowledge oriented Technologies from http://webocrat.fei.tuke.sk/fei-

cit/publikacie/babic_WIKT2006.pdf

Banai, M., & Reisel, W. D. (1999). Would you trust your foreign manager? An empirical

investigation. The International Journal of Human Resource Management, 10(3),

477-487.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in

social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.

Page 263: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  251  

Bartol, K. M., & Srivastava, A. (2002). Encouraging knowledge sharing: The role of

organizational reward systems. Journal of Leadership and Organizational

Studies, 9(1), 64-77.

Bell, D. (1973). The coming of post-industrial society: A venture in social forecasting.

New York: Basic Books.

Bielby, W. T., & Baron, J. N. (1986). Men and women at work: Sex segregation and

statistical discrimination. American Journal of Sociology, 91(4), 759-799.

Blackler, F. (2002). Knowledge, knowledge work, and organization. In C. W. Choo & N.

Bontis (Eds.), The Strategic Management of Intellectual Capital and

Organizational Knowledge (pp. 47-64). New York: Oxford University Press.

Blackler, F., Crump, N., & Mcdonald, S. (2000). Organizing processes in complex

activity networks. Organization, 7(2), 277-300.

Blau, P. M., Beeker, C., & Fitzpatrick, K. M. (1984). Intersecting social affiliations and

intermarriage. Social Forces, 62(3), 585-605.

Blau, P. M., Blum, T. C., & Schwartz, J. E. (1982). Heterogeneity and intermarriage.

American Sociological Review, 47(February), 45-62.

Blau, P. M., Ruan, D., & Monika, A. (1991). Interpersonal choice and networks in China.

Social Forces, 69(4), 1037-1062.

Blum, T. C. (1984). Racial inequality and salience: An examination of Blau's theory of

social structure. Social Forces, 62(3), 607-617.

Boer, N-I., van Baalen, P. J., & Kumar, K. (2002). An activity theory approach for

studying the situatedness of knowledge sharing. Paper presented at the 35th

Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences Hilton Waikoloa Village,

Big Island, Hawaii.

Bohrnstedt, G. W., & Knoke, D. (1994). Statistics for social data analysis. Itasca,

Illinois: F.E. Peacock Publishers.

Boisot, M. H. (1998). Knowledge assets: Securing competitive advantage in the

information economy. New York: Oxford University Press.

Boisot, M. H. (2002). The creation and sharing of knowledge. In C. W. Choo & N. Bontis

(Eds.), The strategic management of intellectual capital and organizational

knowledge (pp. 65-77). New York: Oxford University Press.

Page 264: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  252  

Boon, S. D., & Holmes, J. G. (1991). The dynamics of interpersonal trust: Resolving

uncertainty in the face of risk. In R. A. Hinde & J. Groebel (Eds.), Cooperation

and prosocial behavior (pp. 190-211). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University

Press.

Bornhorst, F., Ichino, A., Kirchkamp, O., Schlag, K. H., & Winter, E. (2010). Similarities

and differences when building trust: The role of cultures. Experimental

Economics, 13, 260-283.

Bracken, L. J., & Oughton, E. A. (2006). ‘What do you mean?’ The importance of

language in developing interdisciplinary research. Transactions of the Institute of

British Geographers, 31(3), 371–382.

Brass, D. J. (1985). Men's and women's networks: A study of interaction patterns and

influence in an organization. The Academy of Management Journal, 28(2), 327-

343.

Brass, D. J., Galaskiewicz, J., Greve, H. R., & Tsai, W. (2004). Taking stock of networks

and organizations: A multilevel perspective. Academy of Management Journal,

47(6), 795-817.

Brewer, M. B. (1979). In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitive-

motivational analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 307-324.

Brewer, M. B. (1981). Ethnocentrism and its role in interpersonal trust. In M. B. Brewer

& B. E. Collins (Eds.), Scientific Inquiry and the Social Sciences (pp. 345-359).

New York: Jossey-Bass.

Bromiley, P., & Cummings, L. L. (1995). Transaction costs in organizations with trust. In

R. Beis, B. Sheppard & R. Lewicki (Eds.), Research on negotiations in

organizations (Vol. 5, pp. 219-247). Greenwich, CT.

Brown, J. S., & Duguid, P. (1991). Organizational learning and communities of practice:

A unified view of working, learning and innovation. Organization Science, 2(1),

40-56.

Brown, J. S., & Duguid, P. (1998). Organizing knowledge. California Management

Review 40(1), 90-111.

Brown, J. S., & Duguid, P. (2000). Balancing act: Capturing knowledge without killing it.

Harvard Business Review, 78(3), 73–80.

Page 265: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  253  

Burt, R. (1992). Structural holes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Byrne, D. E. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York: Academic Press.

Campbell, K. E. (1988). Gender differences in job-related networks. Work and

Occupations, 15(2), 179-200.

Campbell, K. E., Marsden, P. V., & Hurlbert, J. S. (1986). Social resources and

socioeconomic status. Social Networks, 8(1), 97-117.

Carlile, P. R. (2002). A pragmatic view of knowledge and boundaries: Boundary objects

in new product development. Organization Science, 13(4), 442-455.

Carlile, P. R. (2004). Transferring, translating and trans- forming: An integrative

framework for managing knowledge across boundaries. Organization Science,

15(5), 555-568.

Chan, D. (2002). Maintaining professional competence: Impact of organizational and

individual factors on the updating activities of public reference librarians.

University of Toronto, Toronto.

Chang, C-W., Huang, H.-C., Chiang, C.-Y., Hsu, C.-P., & Chang, C.-C. (2011). Social

capital and knowledge sharing: Effects on patient safety. Journal of Advanced

Nursing, 20. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2648.2011.05871.x

Chatman, J. A., & Flynn, F. J. (2001). The influence of demographic heterogeneity on the

emergence and consequences of cooperative norms in work teams. Academy of

Management Journal, 44, 956-974.

Chattopadhyay, P. (1999). Beyond direct and symmetrical effects: The influence of

demographic dissimilarity on organizational citizenship behavior. Academy of

Management Journal, 42(3), 273-287.

Chiu, C-M., Hsu, M-H., & Wang, E. T. G. (2006). Understanding knowledge sharing in

virtual communities: An integration of social capital and social cognitive theories.

Decision Support Systems, 42, 1872-1888.

Choo, C. W. (1998). The knowing organization: How organizations use information to

construct meaning, create knowledge, and make decisions. New York: Oxford

University Press.

Choo, C. W. (2000). Working with knowledge: How information professional help

organizations manage what they know. Library Management, 21(8), 395-403.

Page 266: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  254  

Choo, C. W., & Bontis, N. (2002). The strategic management of intellectual capital and

organizational knowledge. New York: Oxford University Press.

Clegg, S., & Bailey, J. R. (Eds.). (2008). International encyclopedia of organization

studies. Australia: Sage Publications.

Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression / correlation analysis for the

behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Cohen, W. M., & Levinthal., D. A. (1990). Absorptive capacity: A new perspective on

learning and innovation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 35(1), 128-152.

Coleman, J. S. (1988). Social capital in the creation of human capital. American Journal

of Sociology, 94(Supplement), S95-S120.

Comrey, A. L., & Lee, H. B. (1992). A first course in factor analysis. Hillsdale, New

Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Connelly, C. E., & Kelloway, E. K. (2003). Predictors of employees' perceptions of

knowledge sharing cultures. Leadership & Organization Development Journal,

24(5/6), 294-301.

Constant, D., Sproull, L., & Kiesler, S. (1996). The kindness of strangers: The usefulness

of electronic weak ties for technical advice. Organization Science, 7(2), 119-135.

Criscuolo, P., Salter, A., & Ter Wal, A. (2010, June 16 - 18). The role of proximity in

shaping knowledge sharing in professional service firms. Paper presented at the

Summer conference on opening up innovation: Strategy, organization and

technology, London Business School.

Cross, R., & Cummings, J. N. (2004). Tie and network correlates of individual

performance in knowledge-intensive work. Academy of Management Journal,

47(6), 928-937.

Cross, R., & Sproull, L. (2004). More than an answer: Information relationships for

actionable knowledge. Organization Science, 15(4), 446-462.

Curseu, P. L., & Schruijer, S. G. L. (2010). Does conflict shatter trust or does trust

obliterate conflict? Revisiting the relationships between team diversity, conflict,

and trust. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 14(1), 66-79.

Cyr, S., & Choo, C. W. (2010). The individual and social dynamics of knowledge

sharing: An exploratory study. Journal of Documentation, 66(6), 824 - 846.

Page 267: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  255  

Daft, R. L., & Lengel, R. H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, media

richness and structural design. Management Science, 32(5), 554-571.

Dalkir, K., & Wiseman, E. (2004). Orgnizational storytelling and knowledge

management: A survey. Storytelling, Self, Society, 1(1), 57-73.

Davenport, T. H., & Prusak, L. (1998). Working knowledge: How organizations manage

what they know. Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

De Carolis, D. M. (2002). The role of social capital and organizational knowledge in

enhancing entrepreneurial opportunities in high-technology enviornments. In C.

W. Choo & N. Bontis (Eds.), The strategic management of intellectual capital

and organizational knowledge (pp. 699-709). New York: Oxford University

Press.

De Long, D. W., & Fahey, L. (2000). Diagnosing cultural barriers to knowledge

management. Academy of Management Executive, 14(4), 113-127.

Deutsch, M. (1960). The effect of motivational orientation upon trust and suspicion.

Human Relations, 13(1), 122-139.

DeVellis, R. F. (1991). Scale development. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.

Dirks, K. T., & Ferrin, D. L. (2002). Trust in leadership: Meta-analytic findings and

implications for research and practice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87(4), 611-

628.

Duguid, P. (2005). "The art of knowing": Social and tacit dimensions of knowledge and

the limits of the community of practice. The Information Society, 21(2), 109-118.

duPlessis, T., & duToit, A. S. A. (2006). Knowledge management and legal practice.

International Journal of Information Management, 26(5), 360-371.

Earl, M. (2001). Knowledge management strategies: Toward a taxonomy. Journal of

Management Information Systems, 18(1), 221-233.

Earley, P. C., & Mosakowski, E. (2000). Creating hybrid team cultures: An empirical test

of transnational team functioning. Academy of Management Journal, 43(1), 26-

49.

Elliott, S., & O'Dell, C. (1999). Sharing knowledge & best practices: The hows and whys

of tapping your organization's hidden reservoirs of knowledge. Health Forum

Journal, 42(3), 34-38.

Page 268: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  256  

Empson, L. (2001). Fear of exploitation and fear of contamination: Impediments to

knowledge transfer in mergers between professional service firms. Human

Relations, 54(7), 839-862.

Engeström, Y. (1999). Innovative learning in work teams: Analyzing cycles of

knowledge creation in practice In Y. Engeström, R. Miettinen & R.-L. Punamäki

(Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory (pp. 377-404). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge

University Press.

Evans, M. M. (2008). Determination of research readiness: Knowledge sharing: An

empirical study of the role of trust in an organizational setting. University of

Toronto.

Feld, S. L. (1982). Structural determinants of similarity among associates. American

Sociological Review, 47, 797-801.

Fiol, C. M. (1994). Consensus, diversity, and learning in organizations. Organization

Science, 5(3), 403-420.

Fischer, C. S. (1977). Networks and places: Social relations in the urban setting. New

York: Free Press.

Fischer, C. S. (1982). To dwell among friends. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Galaskiewicz, J., & Shatin, D. (1981). Leadership and networking among neighborhood

human service organizations. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26(3), 434-448.

Ganesan, S. (1994). Determinants of long-term orientation in buyer-seller relationships.

Journal of Marketing, 22(1), 1-19.

Geringer, M. J. (1988). Partner selection criteria for developed country joint ventures.

Business Quarterly, 53(1), 67-84.

Gherardi, S. (2001). From organizational learning to practice-based knowing. Human

Relations, 54(1), 131-139.

Ghoshal, S., Korine, H., & Szulanski, G. (1994). Interunit communication in

multinational corporations. Management Science, 40(1), 96-110.

Goman, C. K. (2002, June 10). Why people don’t tell you what they know. Paper

presented at the IABC International Conference, Hyatt Regency, Chicago, IL

USA.

Page 269: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  257  

Granovetter, M. S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology,

78(6), 1360-1380.

Granovetter, M. S. (1982). The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. In P. V.

Marsden & N. Lin (Eds.), Social Structure and Network Analysis (pp. 105-130).

Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Grant, R. M. (2002). The knowledge-based view of the firm. In N. B. Chun Wei Choo

(Ed.), The strategic management of intellectual capital and organizational

knowledge (pp. 133-148). New York: Oxford University Press.

Haas, M. R., & Hansen, M. T. (2007). Different knowledge, different benefits: Toward a

productivity perspective on knowledge sharing in organizations. Strategic

Management Journal, 28(11), 1133-1153.

Habermas, J. (1971). Knowledge and human interests. Boston: Beacon.

Hall, H. (2001). Input-friendliness: Motivating knowledge sharing across intranets.

Journal of Information Science, 27(3), 139-146.

Hansen, M. T. (1999). The search-transfer problem: The role of weak ties in sharing

knowledge across organization subunits. Administrative Science Quarterly, 44(1),

82-111.

Henderson, J. K. (2005). Language diversity in international management teams.

International Studies of Management and Organization, 35(1), 66-82.

Hendricks, P. (1999). Why share knowledge? The influence of ICT on the motivation for

knowledge sharing. Knowledge and Process Management, 16(2), 91-100.

Hinds, P. J., & Pfeffer, J. (2003). Why organizations don't "know what they know":

Cognitive and motivational factors affecting the transfer of expertise. In M.

Ackerman, V. Pipek & V. Wulf (Eds.), Beyond knowledge management: Sharing

expertise (pp. 3-26). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Hislop, D. (2003). Linking human resource management and knowledge management via

commitment: A review and research agenda. Employee Relations, 25(1/2), 182-

202.

Page 270: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  258  

Hitt, M. A., Bierman, L., Shimizu, K., & Kochhar, R. (2001). Direct and moderating

effects of human capital on strategy and performance in professional service

firms: A resource-based perspective. The Academy of Management Journal,

44(1), 13-28.

Ho, L-A., Kuo, T-H., & Lin, B. (2011). How social identification and trust influence

organizational online knowledge sharing. Internet Research, 22(1), 4-28.

Ho, L-A., Kuo, T-H., Lin, C., & Lin, B. (2010). The mediate effect of trust on

organizational online knowledge sharing: An empirical study. International

Journal of Information Technology & Decision Making, 9(4), 625-644.

Hoe, S. L., & McShane, S. L. (2002). Leadership antecedents of informal knowledge

acquisition and dissemination. International Journal of Organisational

Behaviour, 5(10), 282-291.

Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. (2000). Social identity and self-categorization processes in

organizational contexts. Academy of Management Review, 25(1), 121-140.

Holste, J. S. (2003). A study of the effects of affect-based trust and cognition-based trust

on intra-organizational knowledge sharing and use. Unpublished Ph.D.

Dissertation, Regent University, Virginia Beach, Virginia.

Huber, G. P. (1991). Organizational learning: The contributing processes and the

literatures. Organization Science, 2(1), 88-115.

Huckfeldt, R. R., & Sprague, J. (1995). Citizens, politics and social communication:

Information and influence in an election campaign (Cambridge studies in public

opinion and political psychology). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Husted, K., & Michailova, S. (2002). Diagnosing and fighting knowledge-sharing

hostility Organizational Dynamics, 31(1), 60-73.

Ibarra, H. (1992). Homophily and differential returns: Sex differences in network

structure and access in an advertising firm. Administrative Science Quarterly,

37(3), 422-447.

Ibarra, H. (1993). Personal networks of women and minorities in management: A

conceptual framework. Academy of Management Review, 18(1), 56-87.

Ibarra, H. (1995). Race, opportunity, and diversity of social circles in managerial

networks. Academy of Management Journal, 38(3), 673-703.

Page 271: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  259  

Ibarra, H. (1997). Paving an alternative route: Gender differences in managerial

networks. Social Psychology Quarterly, 60(1), 91-102.

Johnson, J. L., Cullen, J. B., Sakano, T., & Takenouchi, H. (1996). Setting the stage for

trust and strategic integration in Japanese - U.S. cooperative alliances. Journal of

International Business Studies, 27(5, Global Perspectives on Cooperative

Strategies), 981-1004.

Johnson-George, C., & Swap, W. C. (1982). Measurement of specific interpersonal trust:

Construction and validation of a scale to assess trust in a specific other. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 43(6), 1306-1317.

Kalleberg, A. L., Knoke, D., Marsden, P. V., & Spaeth, J. L. (1996). Organizations in

America: Analysing their structures and human resource practices. Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kalmijn, M. (1998). Intermarriage and homogamy: Causes, patterns and trends. Annual

Review of Sociology, 24, 395–421.

Kalmijn, M., & Vermunt, J. K. (2007). Homogeneity of social networks by age and

marital status: A multilevel analysis of ego-centered networks. Social Networks,

29(1), 25-43.

Keller, R. T. (1994). Technology-information processing fit and the performance of R&D

project groups: A test of contingency theory. The Academy of Management

Journal, 37(1), 167-179.

Kogut, B., & Zander, U. (1992). Knowledge of the firm, combinative capabilities, and the

replication of technology. Organization Science, 3, 383-397.

Krackhardt, D. (1992). The strength of strong ties: The importance of philos in

organizations. In N. Nohria & R. G. Eccles (Eds.), Networks and organizations:

Structure, form and action (pp. 216-239). Boston, MA: Harvard Business School

Press.

Kramer, R. M. (1999). Trust and distrust in organizations: Emerging perspectives,

enduring questions. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 569-598.

Latour, B. (1987). Science in action: How to follow scientists and engineers through

society. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.

Page 272: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  260  

Laumann, E., Gagnon, J. H., Michael, R. T., & Michaels, S. (1994). The social

organization of sexuality: Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago:

University of Chicago Press.

Lave, J. (1993). Situating learning in communities of practice. In J. L. L. Resnick, & T.

Teasley (Ed.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition (pp. 63-84). Washington

D. C.: American Psychological Association.

Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation.

Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press.

Lee, R. (2009). Social capital and business and management: Setting a research agenda.

International Journal of Management Reviews, 11(3), 247-273.

Leonard, D., & Sensiper, S. (2002). The role of tacit knowledge in group innovation. In

C. W. Choo & N. Bontis (Eds.), The strategic management of intellectual capital

and organizational knowledge (pp. 485-499). New York: Oxford University

Press.

Leont'ev, A. N. (1978). Activity, consciousness, and personality. Englewood Cliffs, New

Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

Levin, D., Cross, R., & Abrams, L. (2003). Why should I trust you? Predictors of

interpersonal trust in a knowledge transfer context. Rutgers Business School.

Levin, D. Z., & Cross, R. (2004). The strength of weak ties you can trust: The mediating

role of trust in effective knowledge transfer. Management Science, 50(11), 1477-

1490.

Levin, D. Z., Walter, J., & Murnighan, J. K. (2011). Dormant ties: The value of

reconnecting. Organization Science, 22(4), 923-939.

Levin, D. Z., Whitener, E. M., & Cross, R. (2004). Perceived trustworthiness of

knowledge sources: the moderating impact of relationship length. Paper presented

at the Best Papers Proceedings of the Academy of Management, New Orleans.

Levin, D. Z., Whitener, E. M., & Cross, R. (2006). Perceived trustworthiness of

knowledge sources: The moderating impact of relationship length. Journal of

Applied Psychology, 91(5), 1163-1171.

Page 273: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  261  

Lewicki, R. J., & Bunker, B. B. (1995). Trust in relationships: A model of development

and decline. In B. B. Bunker & J. Z. Rubin (Eds.), Conflict cooperation and

justice: Essays inspired by the work of Morton Deutsch (pp. 133-174). San

Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Lewicki, R. J., & Bunker, B. B. (1996). Developing and maintaining trust in work

relationships. In T. R. T. R.M. Kramer (Ed.), Trust in organization: Frontiers of

theory and research (pp. 114-139). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Lewis, J. D., & Weigert, A. (1985). Trust as a social reality. Social Forces, 63(4), 967-

985.

Limerick, D., & Cunnington, B. (1993). Managing the new organisation: A blueprint for

networks and strategic alliances. Chatswood, NSW: Business and Professional

Publishing.

Lin, C-P. (2007). To share or not to share: Modeling tacit knowledge sharing, its

mediators and antecedents. Journal of Business Ethics, 70, 411-428.

Lincoln, J. R., & Miller, J. (1979). Work and friendship ties in organizations: A

comparative analysis of relational networks. Administrative Science Quarterly,

24, 181-199.

Louch, H. (2000). Personal network integration: Transitivity and homophily in strong-tie

relations. Social Networks, 22(1), 45–64.

Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge,

MA: Harvard University Press.

Mäkelä, K. (2007). Knowledge sharing through expatriate relationships: A social capital

perspective. International Studies in Management and Organization, 37(3), 108-

126.

Mäkelä, K., Andersson, U., & Seppala, T. (2012). Interpersonal similarity and knowledge

sharing within multinational organizations. International Business Review, 21(3),

439-451.

Mäkelä, K., & Brewster, C. (2009). Interunit interaction contexts, interpersonal social

capital and the differing levels of knowledge sharing. Human Resource

Management, 48(4), 591–613.

Page 274: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  262  

Makela, K., Kalla, H. K., & Piekkari, R. (2007). Interpersonal similarity as a driver of

knowledge sharing within multinational corporations. International Business

Review, 16(1), 1-22.

Manev, I. M., & Stevenson, W. B. (2001). Nationality, cultural distance, and expatriate

status: Effects on the managerial network in a multinational enterprise. Journal of

International Business Studies, 32(2), 285-303.

Marsden, P. V. (1987). Core discussion networks of Americans. American Sociological

Review, 52, 122-131.

Marsden, P. V. (1988). Homogeneity in confiding relations. Social Networks, 10(1), 57-

76.

Marsden, P. V., & Campbell, K. E. (1984). Measuring tie strength. Social Forces, 63,

482-501.

Marx, K. (1932). The Economic and Philosophical Manuscripts: The Third Manuscript

Written: between April and August 1844 in Paris (G. B. I. 1974, Trans.). First

Published: 1932 in German, by the Institute of Marxism-Leninism in the Union of

Soviet Socialist Republics.

Mayer, R. C., & Davis, J. H. (1999). The effect of the performance appraisal system on

trust for management: A field quasi-experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology,

84(1), 123-136.

Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of

organizational trust. The Academy of Management Review, 20(3), 709-734.

McAllister, D. J. (1995). Affect and cognition-based trust as foundations for interpersonal

cooperation in organizations. The Academy of Management Journal, 38(1), 24-59.

McCauley, D. P., & Kuhnert, K. W. (1992). A theoretical review and empirical

investigation of employee trust in management. Public Administration Quarterly,

16(2), 265-284.

McDermott, R., & O'Dell, C. (2001). Overcoming cultural barriers to sharing knowledge.

Journal of Knowledge Management, 5(1), 76-85.

McEvily, B., & Tortoriello, M. (2011). Measuring trust in an organizational context:

Review and recommendations. Journal of Trust Research, 1(Special Issue), 23-

63.

Page 275: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  263  

McFadyen, M. A., & Cannella, A. A. (2004). Social capital and knowledge creation:

Diminishing returns of the number and strength of exchange relationships.

Academy of Management Journal, 47(5), 735-746.

McPherson, J. M., & Smith-Lovin, L. (1987). Homophily in voluntary organizations:

Status distance and the composition of face-to-face groups. American

Sociological Review, 52(3), 370-379.

McPherson, M., Smith-Lovin, L., & Cook, J. M. (2001). Birds of a feather: Homophily in

social networks. Annual Review of Sociology, 27, 415-444.

Mehra, A., Kilduff, M., & Brass, D. J. (1998). At the margins: A distinctiveness approach

to the social identity and social networks of underrepresented groups. The

Academy of Management Journal, 41(4), 441-452.

Mital, M., Isreal, D., & Agarwal, S. (2010). Information exchange and information

disclosure in social networking web sites. The Learning Organization, 17(6), 479-

489.

Nahapiet, J., & Ghoshal, S. (1998). Social capital, intellectual capital, and the

organizational advantage. Academy of Management Review, 23(2), 242-266.

Nonaka, I. (1991). The knowledge-creating company. Harvard Business Review,

69(November-December), 96-104.

Nonaka, I. (1994). A dynamic theory of organizational knowledge creation. Organization

Science, 5(1), 14-37.

Nonaka, I. (2002). A dynamic theory of organizational knowledge creation. In C. W.

Choo & N. Bontis (Eds.), The strategic management of intellectual capital and

organizational knowledge (pp. 437-462). New York: Oxford University Press.

Nonaka, I., & Takeuchi, H. (1995). The knowledge-creating company: How Japanese

companies create the dynamics of innovation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Oetzel, J. G. (1995). Intercultural small groups: An effective decision-making theory. In

R. L. Wiseman (Ed.), Intercultural communication theories (pp. 247-270).

Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Ojha, A. K. (2005). Impact of team demography on knowledge sharing in software

project teams. South Asian Journal of Management, 12(3), 67-78.

Page 276: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  264  

Orlikowski, W. J. (1993). CASE tools as organizational change: Investigating increment.

MIS Quarterly, 17(3), 309-340.

Ouchi, W. G. (1980). Markets, bureaucracies, and clans. Administrative Science

Quarterly, 25(1), 129-141.

Penley, L. E., & Hawkins, B. (1985). Studying interpersonal communication in

organizations: A leadership application. Academy of Management Journal, 28(2),

309-326.

Polanyi, M. (1962). Personal knowledge. London, UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

Polanyi, M. (1966). The tacit dimension (1st ed.). Garden City, New York: Doubleday

and Company.

Polanyi, M., & Prosch, H. (1975). Meaning. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Popielarz, P. A. (1999). (In)voluntary association: A multilevel analysis of gender

segregation in voluntary organizations. Gender and Society, 13(2), 234-250.

Reagans, R. (2005). Preferences, identity, and competition: predicting tie strength from

demographic data. Management Science, 51(9), 1374–1383.

Renzl, B. (2008). Trust in management and knowledge sharing: The mediating effects of

fear and knowledge documentation. Omega, 36, 206-220.

Riege, A. (2005). Three-dozen knowledge-sharing barriers managers must consider.

Journal of Knowledge Management, 9(3), 18-35.

Ring, P. S., & van de Ven, A. H. (1994). Developmental processes of cooperative

interorganizational relationships. The Academy of Management Review, 19(1), 90-

118.

Roberts, K. H., & O'Reilly III, C. A. (1974). Failures in upward communication in

organizations: Three possible culprits. Academy of Management Journal, 17(2),

205-215.

Rogers, E., & Bhowmik, D. K. (1970). Homophily-heterophily relational concepts for

communication research. Public Opinion Quarterly, 34(4), 523-538.

Rogers, E. M. (1995). Diffusion of innovations (5th ed.). New York: Anchor Day Books.

Ruggles, R. (1998). The state of the notion: Knowledge management in practice.

California Management Review, 40(3), 80-89.

Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. London, UK: Hutchinson & Company.

Page 277: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  265  

Schlenker, B., Helm, B., & Tedeschi, J. T. (1973). The effects of personality and

situational variables on behavioral trust. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 25(3), 419-427.

Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C., & Davis, J. H. (1996). Organizational trust:

Philosophical perspectives and conceptual definitions. Academy of Management

Review, 21(2), 337-340.

Schoorman, F. D., Mayer, R. C., & Davis, J. H. (1996). Empowerment in veterinary

clinics: The role of trust in delegation. Paper presented at the 11th annual meeting

of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology.

Shore, L. M., Cleveland, J. N., & Goldberg, C. B. (2003). Work attitudes and decisions as

a function of manager age and employee age. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88,

529-537.

Shrum, W., Cheek Jr., N. H., & Hunter, S. M. (1988). Friendship in school: Gender and

racial homophily. Sociology of Education, 61(4), 227-239.

Sitkin, S. B., & Roth, N. L. (1993). Explaining the limited effectiveness of legalistic

"remedies" for trust/distrust. Organization Science, 4(3), 367-392.

Smith-Lovin, L., & McPherson, M. (1993). You are who you know: A network

perspective on gender. In P. England (Ed.), Theory on gender. Feminism on

theory: Social institutions and social change (pp. 223-241). New York: Aldine.

Spender, J. C. (1994). Knowing, managing and learning: A dynamic managerial

epistemology. Management Learning, 25(3), 387-412.

Spender, J. C. (1996). Making knowledge the basis of a dynamic theory of the firm.

Strategic Management Journal, 17(Winter Special Issue), 45-62.

Spender, J. C. (1996). Organizational knowledge, learning and memory: Three concepts

in search of a theory. Journal of Organizational Change Management, 9(1), 63-

78.

Statistics Canada (2006). 2006 census questions: And reasons why the questions are

asked Retrieved December 3, 2009, from

http://www12.statcan.ca/english/census06/info/questions/index.cfm?S=11

Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine

communications. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Page 278: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  266  

Swap, W., Leonard, D., & Abrams, L. (2001). Using mentoring and storytelling to

transfer knowledge in the workplace. Journal of Management Information

Systems, 18(1), 95-114.

Szulanski, G. (1995). Unpacking stickiness: An empirical investigation of the barriers to

transfer best practices in the firm. Academy of Management Journal, 38(Special

Issue), 437-441.

Szulanski, G. (1996). Exploring internal stickiness: Impediments to the transfer of best

practice within the firm. Strategic Management Journal, 17(Winter Issue), 27-43.

Szulanski, G., Cappetta, R., & Jensen, R. J. (2004). When and how trustworthiness

matters: Knowledge transfer and the moderating effect of causal ambiguity.

Organization Science, 15(5), 600-613.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Pearson.

Thompson, M. P. A., & Walsham, G. (2004). Placing knowledge management in context.

Journal of Management Studies, 41(5), 725-747.

Thomson, J. A. K. (1955). Aristotle: Nichomachean ethics. London, UK: Penguin.

Toh, S. M., & Srinivas, E. S. (2011). Perceptions of task cohesiveness and organizational

support increase trust and information sharing between host country nationals and

expatriate coworkers in Oman. Journal of World Business, In Press. Retrieved

from http://www.rotman.utoronto.ca/facbios/file/JWB_Toh&Srinivas2011.pdf

Tolman, C. W. (1988). The basic vocabulary of activity theory. Activity Theory, 1, 14-20.

Toulmin, S. (1999). Knowledge as shared procedures. In Y. Engeström, R. Miettinen &

R.L. Punamäki (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory (pp. 53-64). Cambridge,

UK: Cambridge University Press.

Triandis, H. C. (1960). Cognitive similarity and communication in a dyad. Human

Relations, 13(1), 175-183.

Tsai, W., & Ghoshal, S. (1998). Social capital and value creation: The role of intrafirm

networks. The Academy of Management Journal, 41(4), 464-476.

Tsoukas, H. (2005). What is organizational knowledge? In H. Tsoukas (Ed.), Complex

knowledge: Studies in organizational epistemology (pp. 117-140). New York:

Oxford University Press.

Page 279: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  267  

Tsoukas, H. (2005). Do we really understand tacit knowledge? In H. Tsoukas (Ed.),

Complex knowledge: Studies in organizational epistemology (pp. 141-161). New

York: Oxford University Press.

Tsoukas, H., & Vladimirou, E. (2001). What is organizational knowledge? Journal of

Management Studies, 38(7), 973-993.

Tsui, A. S., & O'Reilly, C. A., III (1989). Beyond simple demographic effects: The

importance of relational demography in superior-subordinate dyads. Academy of

Management Journal, 32, 402-423.

Tsui, A. S-Y. (1984). A role-set analysis of managerial reputation. Organizational

Behavior and Human Performance, 34(1), 64-96.

Tsui, A. S-Y. (1986). Interpersonal affect and rating errors. Academy of Management

Journal, 29(3), 586-599.

Tyler, T. R., & Degoey, P. (1996). Trust in organisational authorities: The influence of

motive attributions on willingness to accept decisions. In R. M. Kramer & T. R.

Tyler (Eds.), Trust in organisations: Frontiers of theory and research (pp. 331-

356). Thousand Oaks, CA.: Sage.

Uslaner, E. M. (2011). Trust, diversity, and segregation in the United States and the

United Kingdom. Comparative Sociology, 10(2), 221-247.

Uzzi, B. (1996). The sources and consequences of embeddedness for the economic

performance of organizations: The network effect. American Sociological Review,

61(4), 674-698.

Uzzi, B. (1997). Social structure and competition in interfirm networks: The paradox of

embeddedness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42, 35-67.

Uzzi, B., & Lancaster, R. (2003). Relational embeddedness and learning: The case of

bank loan managers and their clients. Management Science, 49(4), 383-399.

Van De Ven, A., & Johnson, P. E. (2006). Knowledge for theory and practice. Academy

of Management Review, 31(4), 802-821.

van Duijn, M. A. J., van Busschbach, J. T., & Snijders, T. A. B. (1999). Multilevel

analysis of personal networks as dependent variables. Social Networks, 21(2),

187-210.

Page 280: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  268  

vanDenHooff, B., & vanWeenen, F. d. L. (2004). Committed to share: Commitment and

CMC use as antecedents of knowledge sharing. Knowledge and Process

Management, 11(1), 13-24.

Verbrugge, L. M. (1977). The structure of adult friendship choices. Social Forces, 56,

576-597.

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society (M. Cole, V.John-Steiner, S. Scribner, & E.

Souberman ed.). Cambridge,MA: Harvard University Press.

Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice: Learning, meaning, and identity.

Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

Wigg, K. M. (1997). Knowledge management: An introduction and perspective. Journal

of Knowledge Management, 1(1), 6-14.

Wittgenstein, L. (1953). Philosophical investigations (G.E.M. Anscombe & R. Rhees,

Trans.). Oxford: Blackwell Publishers.

Worchel, P. (1979). Trust and distrust. In W. G. Austin & S. Worchel (Eds.), Social

psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 174-187). Monterey, CA Brooks/Cole

Publishing.

Wright, E. O. (1997). Class counts: Comparative studies in class analysis. Cambridge,

MA: Cambridge University Press.

Yang, S., & Farn, C. (2009). Social capital, behavioural control, and tacit knowledge

sharing: A multi-informant design. International Journal of Information

Management, 29(3), 210-218.

Yuasa, Y. (1987). The body: Toward an Eastern mind-body theory (N. Shigenori & T. P.

Kasulis, Trans.). Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.

Zaheer, A., McEvily, B., & Perrone, V. (1998). Does trust matter? Exploring the effects

of interorganizational and interpersonal trust on performance. Organization

Science, 9(2), 141-159.

Zand, D. E. (1972). Trust and managerial problem solving. Administrative Science

Quarterly, 17(2), 229-239.

Zenger, T. R., & Lawrence, B. S. (1989). Organizational demography: The differential

effects of age and tenure distributions on technical communication. The Academy

of Management Journal, 32(2), 353-376.

Page 281: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  269  

Appendix  List  of  Appendices:  

 A.1  Knowledge  Definition  Components  A.2  Survey  Instrument  A.3  Firm-­‐wide  Invitation  Email  A.4  University  of  Toronto  Ethics  Approval  Letter  

 A.1 Knowledge Definition Components

Activity (Action, Practice, and Process)

Polanyi (1966) advanced the notion that knowledge was action-orientated by arguing that

all knowing requires skillful action60. According to Spender (1996b), Polanyi was one of

the first to discuss knowledge as “a form of abstraction that can only be known,

evidenced and communicated through action” (p. 54). This perspective is now commonly

accepted in some form by numerous other social scientists and knowledge management

theorists, including Nonaka and Takeuchi (1995); Choo (1998); Davenport and Prusak

(1998); Leonard and Sensiper (2002); Suchman (1987); Wigg (1997); Tsoukas (2005a,

2005b); Blackler (2002); Spender (1996b); Bartol and Srivastava (2002); Habermas

(1971); Van De Ven and Johnson (2006); and Elliott and O’Dell (1999).

Tsoukas (2005a) refers to a similar component of knowledge, when he explains

knowledge acquisition as “a process for incorporating new experiences and information”

(p. 118). For Nonaka (2002) knowledge is “a dynamic human process of justifying

personal beliefs” (p. 438). A similar view of ‘knowledge as process’61 is also shared by

Latour (1987); Thompson and Walsham (2004); Spender (1996b); and Blackler (2002).

Gherardi (2001) propagates a similar pragmatic theory of knowing, where practice

connects ‘knowing’ with ‘doing’. For Gherardi (2001), “knowledge consequently does

not arise from scientific ‘discoveries’; rather, it is fabricated by situated practices of

knowledge production and reproduction, using the technologies of representation and

mobilization employed by scientists” (p. 136).

                                                                                                               60 For the purpose of this research, the terms activity and action will be considered synonymous. 61 ‘Practice based theory of knowing’ (Blackler, 2002; Thompson & Walsham, 2004)

Page 282: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  270  

The terms practice and process are commonly used interchangeably in the organizational

science and knowledge management literature when defining knowledge. As opposed to

colloquial usage of action, process and practice imply some degree of standardization and

specialization. In this respect, both processes and practices are comprised of a set of

actions (action oriented and action dependent), and have a methodical/standardized

objective. This understanding of process and practice may be used synonymously with

activity, something commonly done in activity theory (e. g. Leont’ev, 1978; Blackler,

Crump, & McDonald, 2000).

Perhaps the best illustration of the activity component of knowledge is represented in a

quote from Blackler (2002) who said that “rather than thinking of knowledge as a thing

that people possess, it is more helpful to analyze knowing as something that people do”

(p. 63).

Domain Situation (Context)

The second component of the proposed definition of knowledge focuses on the idea of

being situated within a socially constructed domain. This section will focus on the idea

of domain situation and the principle of social construction will be discussed in the next

component.

To better understand domain situation, one must first consider the ‘channel’ through

which information is being communicated. ‘Channels’ can consist of physical objects,

such as textbooks, maps, prototypes, or architectural models, or more conceptual objects

such as language, metaphor, analogies, or mental models (Vygotsky 1978; Tsoukas,

2005a; Blackler, 2002; Toulmin, 1999). Traditionally, these more conceptual

representations are referred to as ‘instruments’ (Vygotsky, 1978) or ‘cultural tools’

(Tsoukas, 2005a), yet physical objects may be thought of in a similar way.

The specifics within these ‘channels’ derive their meaning for a particular field or

expertise. Using an quote from Toulmin’s (1999), “language has a definite meaning only

when it is related to a given constellation of practical activities…we understand the

Page 283: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  271  

meaning of the word strike only if we are familiar with the game of baseball” (p. 59).

Using another example from a mathematics textbook, it may be proposed that algebraic

concepts are learned or understood by the students through the use of algebraic equations,

metaphors, and analogies.

The domain or context (i.e. expertise or field) influences and supports the meaning or

understanding of the information. Using the baseball example, if the expressions

“stealing” or “striking” are taken out of the ‘playing baseball’ context, their meaning

dramatically changes. “Playing baseball […] provides the background against which the

word strike has this meaning. The shared intelligibility of any utterance requires it to

have a standard place in a specific practical context” (Toulmin, 1999, p. 60). A specific

context helps to determine interpretation. Thompson and Walsham (2004) explain

context as “the relationally situated ingredients through which knowing occurs” (p. 735).

The appropriate interpretation of the information depends on the nature of the context, as

well as the nature of the community within that context (Duguid, 2005; Leonard &

Sensiper, 2002). According to Nonaka (1994) “what makes sense in one context, can

change or even lose its meaning when communicated to people in a different context” (p.

30). To effectively interact with a domain, one must learn to decode from the perspective

of that domain and community (Duguid, 2005). This idea is consistent with Wittgenstein

(1953), who argues that meaning and practical implication depend on their use and on the

framework in which they exist. Wittgenstein (1953), Spender (1996a), Thompson and

Walsham (2004) would argue that the meaning of all knowledge is tied up within the

context of its development. This reasoning is also supported by Lave’s (1993) and

Blackler’s (2002) argument that knowledge may not be divorced from context and

transmitted as simply abstract data.

According to Bell (1973), individuals use theory to generalize from one context to

another. It is this generalizing and use of theory that assists in exercising judgment

(especially in a foreign domain). Since all people have different ‘relationally situated

ingredients’ or different influencing (meaning-supporting) contexts, it is clear why no

Page 284: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  272  

two agents can ever share exactly the same meaning. According to Boisot (2002), if

people do not share similar meanings, then they will operate with ‘different conceptual

schemes’ (p. 72) causing distortion and rejection of new knowledge.

To interpret meaning within a domain, a person must understand the interrelated contexts

of that domain. For example, to understand the meaning of “the fastball struck out the

third baseman”, the agent must understand both the context of playing baseball as well as

the context use of the English language. This same phrase uttered at a baseball game in

Japan may not have the same interpreted meaning, since the examples only share one of

the two minimum required contexts to interpret meaning from that statement.

Being able to act prudently and correctly within a particular domain implies learning to

make distinctions and connections to the contexts influencing that domain (Tsoukas,

2005a; Wenger, 1998; Van De Ven & Johnson, 2006). In most cases, this learning

requires the person to be a member of the community operating in the domain. This

sentiment is echoed in Lave and Wenger’s (1991) definition of community as

“participation in an activity system about which participants share understandings

concerning what they are doing…the social structure of [their] practice, its power

relations, and its conditions for legitimacy” (p. 97).

Social Construction

Social construction theory62, as it applies to knowledge, finds its roots in early Russian

psychology (Leont'ev, 1978; Vygotsky, 1978) and economics (Marx, 1932). Vygotsky

(1978) and his colleagues argued that human consciousness is both shaped by social

experiences and mediated by culturally established tools. Marx (1932) argued that “the

eye has become a human eye, just as its object has become a social, human object – an

object made by man for man”. Using Leont’ev (1978), the argument is elaborated:

“Isolated activity cannot be understood apart from social ties... Entering into contact with each other, people formulate a language

                                                                                                               62 Social Construction Theory is defined using Spender (1996a) as “the idea that the individual’s consciousness and thinking are fashioned socially” (p. 69).

Page 285: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  273  

that serves to represent the objects, the means, and the very process of work itself. [W]ords, the language signs, are not simply replacements for things, their conditional substitutes. Behind philological meanings is hidden social practice, activity transformed and crystallized in them; only in the process of this activity is objective reality revealed to man” (p. 18).

This premise of socially constructed consciousness is foundational to the work in activity

theory, and is so influential that it is found in their definition of consciousness itself:

“Man is born into the world of objects created by previous generations, and is formed as such only in the process of learning how to use them to a definite end. The mode of his relation to reality is not determined directly by his bodily organisation (as in the case with animals), but by the habits of practical activity acquired solely through communication with other people63 (Tolman, 1988, p. 16).

Influenced by this perspective, many theorists have turned towards social construction

and activity theory to understand and define knowledge, and theorize on how it is created

and disseminated. In general, the social construction viewpoint defines knowledge as ‘the

social practice of knowing’ (Boer, van Baalen, & Kumar, 2002). Gherardi (2001) argues

that “learning and knowing are mediated by social relations [...] knowledge resides in

social relations […and] knowing is part of a surrendering to a social habit” (p. 133).

Nonaka (1994) claims that knowing is something that emerges through continuous

dialogue among practitioners. In fact, Nonaka’s (2002) ‘socialization’64 refers to a

process of creating knowledge through shared experience.

Individuals learn to exercise judgment through a process of socialization, which is based

on a socially constructed shared context (Tsoukas, 2005a). New knowledge is socially

constructed by and becomes meaningful to the community within which it was

constructed (Boer, van Baalen, & Kumar, 2002). In other words, abstract formulations

ultimately depend on collective, socially accepted definitions (Tsoukas, 2005a; Polanyi,

1966; Toulmin, 1999; Blackler, Crump, & McDonald, 2000; Nonaka & Takeuchi, 1995;

                                                                                                               63 Extract from Tolman’s (1988) The basic vocabulary of Activity Theory 64 Socialization is borrowed from Nonaka’s (2002) proposed model for knowledge creation; the SECI model.    

Page 286: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  274  

Spender, 1996). This is what Wittgenstein (1953) called ‘forms of life’ (Lebensformen),

or what Grant (2002) referred to as 'common knowledge'.

Social constructionists understand knowledge to be a product of a collective, something

that is developed communally, over time (Brown & Duguid, 1991; 1998; 2000; Blackler,

2002; Leonard & Sensiper, 2002). In other words, it is the “outcome of people working

together, sharing experiences, and constructing meaning out of what they do” (Choo,

2000, p. 395). Some of these theorists (Leonard & Sensiper, 2002; Polanyi, 1966;

Tsoukas, 2005a) argue that ‘personal knowledge’ exists in collaboration with ‘collective

knowledge’, or that each person socialized within the collective encompasses the

knowledge of the collective. Other theorists (De Carolis, 2002; De Long & Fahey, 2000;

Boer, van Baalen, & Kumar, 2002; Boisot, 2002; Spender, 1994; Brown & Duguid,

1998) believe that socially constructed ‘collective’ knowledge resides (or is embedded) in

and is the possession of the collective itself (independently from the individuals

comprising the collective). This latter perspective suggests that knowledge supersedes

any one individual, and is greater than the sum of the individual knowledge within the

collective.

Developmental Capacity (Personal Ability and Capacity)

If developmental capacity is to be acknowledged as a component of knowledge, one must

first accept Polanyi and Prosch’s (1975) argument that all knowledge is personal and

subjective, despite the fact that its meaning is constructed socially. The authors suggest

that “all knowing is personal knowing” (Polanyi & Prosch, 1975, p. 44). Tsoukas (2005a)

makes a similar argument stating that “all knowledge is personal knowledge” (p. 126).

According to Nonaka (2002), knowledge has a subjective nature, which, at a fundamental

level, is created by the individual. Other authors argue that knowing is mediated through

personal human judgment (Tsoukas, 2005a; 2005b; Polanyi & Prosch, 1975) or a

personal exercising of reason (Spender, 1996b).

Personal judgment involves ‘applying abstract representations of the world’ and making

assessments of the existing gaps within those representations (Tsoukas, 2005b). Personal

Page 287: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  275  

judgments are not guided by strict rules, but through a skillful exercise of the body and

mind, guided by the senses (Polanyi, 1966; Polanyi & Prosh, 1975; Tsoukas, 2005b).

Tsoukas (2005a) describes personal judgment as being associated with the prefix re- (re-

order, re-arrange, and re-design). Judgment involves the personal ability to draw

distinctions or to divide the world into ‘this’ and ‘that’. According to Tsoukas (2005a),

exercising judgment involves “the ability of an individual to draw distinctions and the

location of the individual within a collectively generated and sustained domain of action”

(p.120). For example, a medical student must both be a part of the medical domain and be

able to draw distinctions/make judgments within that domain65. Bell (1973) and Boisot

(1998) take a similar theoretic approach, arguing that knowledge can be conceptualized

as a set of probability distributions that guide reasoned judgment and orient actions.

Being able to select relevant categories for abstraction (draw distinctions) requires the

individual to have prior knowledge of the context/domain in question (Boisot, 2002).

Knowing involves a configuration of context (Thompson & Walsham, 2004) or a unique

integrated set of particulars for which each agent is subsidiarily aware (Tsoukas, 2005a).

Knowing involves continuous interaction with the outside world (Nonaka, 2002), yet

each person’s representation and understanding of that world is personal and different

(Polanyi, 1966). For example, a differential equation cannot alone predict an unknown

function of a variable; it is the application and use of differential equations, in

engineering, physics, and economics, that allows us to make reasoned judgments and

create new knowledge.

If knowing is in fact personal and requires the individual to understand how to exercise

judgments based on historically situated and collectively constructed domains, then it can

be reasoned that biological and developmental capacity can influence the cognitive

ability needed to process new information, draw distinctions, and understand relevant

concepts. Brain functions influence conceptual skills and cognitive abilities. Biological or

physical changes to the brain may influence an individual’s knowledge state. Similarly,

                                                                                                               65 Personal judgments within one domain will enviably encounter messiness and complexity once they are introduced into another domain.

Page 288: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  276  

changes to physical make up or motor abilities may also impact an individual’s ability to

understand and instrumentalize new knowledge.

Potentiality

The reference to potentiality is often alluded to in literature, but rarely explicitly stated,

since the concept of potentiality is difficult to frame. One way to view the potentiality of

knowledge is by considering its dependence on the rest of the definition. In other words,

domain, social construction, and developmental capacity are partial determinants of the

potential value of knowledge. Any variance in these partial determinants will cause some

utility change in the person’s knowledge state. Perhaps this is best explained through

example.

Two students who score the same on a test (getting the same answers right and wrong)

may be considered equal in ability, but are quite different in capability for further

learning. It is naïve to think of these students as having the same knowledge states. As

Boisot (2002) asserts, “no two agents possess identical mental schemas, they will

therefore assimilate and accommodate new knowledge in different ways […] external

data that different agents receive may be identical, what actually gets absorbed by each as

knowledge will differ” (p. 73).

Take, for example, two driver’s education students, sitting at the wheel of a car, for their

first lesson. Neither student has ever driven a car, although student A is an avid gamer

who enjoys playing first person driving simulators. Student B, who does not own a

gaming console, made sure they memorized the rules of the road handbook provided in

class. Student B also pays close attention in class and is attentive to drivers when they are

the passenger. During their first hands-on lesson, each student will process new data

differently, partially due to the differences in their prior experiences. Student A may

relate their driving experience to a stock of knowledge primarily formed out of video

game experiences, whereas student B may relate to a reference from the handbook.

Page 289: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  277  

Boisot (2002) argues that “two agents can never achieve identical dispositions to act and

hence identical knowledge states” (p. 68). Even if the stimuli are identical on the day of

the drive (instructions, supervision, car, driving conditions), student A and B will achieve

different knowledge states. Further, the different knowledge states may vary in effect (or

value), as expressed through action. This is primarily due to their existing different

knowledge states and the way the individuals interpret the meaning of new stimuli. Even

though the two students may have some knowledge state resonance, because of the

differences in how they process data and their prior experiences, they will never reach

identical knowledge states. Similar ideas have been echoed by Cohen and Levinthal’s

(1990) absorptive capacity66 and Vygotsky’s (1978) zone of proximal development67.

Potentiality is also expressed through the ever changing and evolving nature of

knowledge itself. Knowledge undergoes construction and transformation in its use

(Blackler, 2002). When new stimulus (information) is introduced, the existing knowledge

states undergo consolidation or modification. Knowledge is both constructed (created)

and destroyed (forgotten, or made obsolete) during this process (Boisot, 1998; 2002).

This constant transformation makes knowing a ‘continually emergent process’

(Thompson & Walsham, 2004, p. 735). Since knowledge is situated and personal, it will

inevitably change as the situation around the person evolves and develops (Blackler,

2002). The changing situation around the person alters the situated knowledge they

possess, and so on, in a cyclical fashion. Blackler (2002) refers to this type of knowing as

mediated where “changes associated with new information…transform[s] the contexts of

action” (p. 59). Spender (1994) argues that this circular process of ‘learning’ continues,

as long as there is activity. Theoretically, this could be never-ending. In discussing

organizational knowledge creation, Nonaka (2002) mirrors similar sentiments arguing

that knowledge creation is a “never-ending, circular process” (p. 451). Since knowledge

is constantly changing and evolving, the potential knowledge state of the person, at any

given time, may be of more or less value to the firm, and is always in flux.

                                                                                                               66 A theory used to measure a firm's ability to value, assimilate, and apply new knowledge 67 A theory expressing the distance between a person’s actual developmental level as determined through independent problem solving and their potential development as determined through problem solving under guidance or in collaboration with more capable peers  

Page 290: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  278  

For some activity theorists, the idea of potentiality is not new. Vygotsky, Luria, and

Leont’ev wrote extensively on the potential of human cognition. In his discussion of

organizational transformation (organizational knowledge creation/learning), Engeström

(1999) also referred to potentiality, with his concept of ‘expansive learning cycles’. He

argued that “miniature cycles of innovative learning should be regarded as potentially

expansive. A large-scale, expansive cycle of organizational transformation always

consists of small cycles of innovative learning” (Engeström, 1999, p. 385). Extending this

idea, it may be argued that any instance of a knowledge state (commonly referred to as a

piece of knowledge) may be only a fraction or ’small cycle‘ of knowing (where knowing

is a large scale expansive cycle). Potentiality is present in knowing, because these small

cycles will have varying, constantly evolving, degrees of effect (or value) on overall

knowing. This idea is consistent with activity theory, where knowing is constantly

developing in non-static activity systems. Knowledge is created or destroyed, as

contradictions and tensions emerge between the elements within the activity system

(Blackler, 2002; Babič & Wagner, 2006). As more activity systems interlink and more

information is introduced, the person’s potential to alter their existing knowledge state, in

a valuable way, increases.

Page 291: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  279  

A.2 Survey Instrument

Knowledge Sharing Survey: Important information for participants in the survey

(Survey web page designed to resemble institutional letterhead) We are conducting a research study that examines knowledge sharing behavior in group-based work. We are interested in the experiences you and other knowledge workers have with sharing knowledge during everyday activities involved in matter or firm-related project work. The study is led by Max Evans, a Doctoral Candidate at the Faculty of Information (iSchool), University of Toronto, and is part of a doctoral thesis. The research is supervised by Dr. Chun Wei Choo, professor at the Faculty of Information, University of Toronto and Dr. Anthony Wensley, professor at the Rotman School of Management, University of Toronto.

You are invited to complete questionnaire survey, which has three parts. The first is an individual section, which asks you to answer questions about yourself and your background; the second asks you to answer questions about two co-workers: someone you worked best with on a matter or law firm related project the project and someone you did not work well with on a matter or law firm related project. The final section asks a few questions about the general work environment at FIRM NAME. For most questions, you simply select a numbered response that best matches your opinion.

It should take approximately 20 minutes to answer the questions.

The risks associated with completing the survey are minimal and are no greater than those you may encounter in everyday work life. Please note that:

• The questionnaire is completely voluntary. • Your answers will be treated confidentially and with anonymity. • The questionnaire will not ask you to identify yourself, your co-workers, or the

name of the matter or project you are referencing. • No personnel records will be used and no matching or personal characteristics

will be made. Your identity will not be revealed in the reporting of the study’s results.

• The collected data will be housed on a secure server at the University of Toronto. • FIRM NAME will not have access to any raw data. Only members of the

academic research team will have access to this data. • You have the right to withdraw consent and discontinue your participation at any

time. There are no penalties or consequences if you choose not to participate or if you choose to withdraw.

The researchers intend to publish the study’s results in scholarly journals. In all publications, including the summary report, the identity of participants will remain confidential. Should you have any questions or concerns about these procedures or the project in general, please feel free to contact Max Evans ([email protected], 416-

Page 292: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  280  

854-6896), Dr. Chun Wei Choo ([email protected], 416-978-5266), or Dr. Anthony Wensley ([email protected], 905-569-4733). If you have any questions about your rights as a participant, please contact the University of Toronto Office of Research Ethics ([email protected], 416-946-3273). A summary of results would be made available to individual participants upon request. Please email the researcher if you are interested. As a small token of our appreciation, we will also give you a $5 gift card to COFFEE SHOP NAME at the end of the survey. To begin the questionnaire, simply click on the “I consent” button below. Clicking on the “I consent” button gives us your consent for participation. Doing so indicates that you agree to the following statements:

1. I have freely volunteered to participate. 2. I have been informed in advance about the nature of the questionnaire, what my

tasks will be, and what procedures will be followed. 3. I have been given the opportunity to ask questions and have had my questions

answered to my satisfaction. 4. I understand that the information I provide will be treated confidentially and with

anonymity. My identity will not be revealed in the reporting of the study’s results. 5. I am aware that I have the right to withdraw consent and discontinue participation

at any time.

<I Do Not Consent> and <I Consent> buttons on survey web page Section 1: Individual Section Please answer the questions in this section about yourself. 1. Age

21-30 31-40 41-50 51-60 Over 61 Prefer not to say

2. Sex Male Female Prefer not to say

3. What Country were you born in?

Select Country Prefer not to say

4. What Country are you a citizen of? Select Country Prefer not to say

Page 293: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  281  

5. Ethnicity (Mark more than one or specify, if applicable.) White Chinese South Asian (e.g., East Indian, Pakistani, Sri Lankan, etc.) Black Filipino Latin American Southeast Asian (e.g., Vietnamese,

Cambodian, Malaysian, Laotian, etc.) Arab West Asian (e.g., Iranian, Afghan, etc.) Korean Japanese Other Prefer not to say

6. What is the highest level of educational qualification that you have achieved?

High School / GED Some College / 2-year College Degree 3 or 4-year University Degree Professional Degree (JD/LL.B.,MD) Masters Degree Doctoral Degree

7. What is your Marital Status?

Never legally married (single) Legally married (and not separated) Separated, but still legally married Divorced Widowed Common Law Prefer not to say

8. Which department are you a part of?

Business Litigation

9. Which office do you primarily work in?

Toronto / Region of Waterloo Vancouver Calgary Montreal Ottawa

10. What is your role within the firm?

Law Clerk / Paralegal Articling Student Associate Partner

11. Approximately how many years have you worked for FIRM NAME?

Months/Yrs

12. Approximately how many years have you worked in your current role? Months/Yrs

Page 294: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  282  

13. Please indicate your level of agreement with each of the following 8 statements: One should be very cautious with strangers.

1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Most experts tell the truth about the limits of their knowledge. Most people can be counted on to do what they say they will do. These days, you must be alert or someone is likely to take advantage of you. Most salespeople are honest in describing their products. Most repair people will not overcharge people who are ignorant of their specialty. Most people answer public opinion polls honestly. Most adults are competent at their jobs.

For the questions in the next section you are asked to mentally select TWO (2) co-workers with whom you shared a completed matter or firm-related project.

• The first co-worker should be someone you have WORKED BEST with on the matter or firm-related project you shared.

• The second co-worker should be someone you did NOT WORK WELL with on the matter or firm-related project you shared.

• Both co-workers need not come from the same matter or firm-related project, but it is important that you interacted with both individually on whichever matter or firm project you shared.

With BOTH with these people in mind, respond to each of the following questions twice, once for the person you worked best with and once for the person you did not work well with. Co-worker Section – Please respond to each of the questions on this page twice, once for the person you worked best with and once for the person you did not work well with. 14. Age

Person I WORKED BEST with 21-30 31-40 41-50 51-60

Over 61 I am not able to assess

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with 21-30 31-40 41-50 51-60

Over 61 I am not able to assess 15. Sex Person I WORKED BEST with Male Female Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Male Female 16. Country of Birth Person I WORKED BEST with Select Country I don’t know Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Select Country I don’t know

Page 295: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  283  

17. Country of Citizenship Person I WORKED BEST with Select Country I don’t know Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Select Country I don’t know 18. Ethnicity (Mark more than one, if applicable.) Person I WORKED BEST with

White Chinese South Asian (e.g., East Indian, Pakistani, Sri Lankan, etc.) Black Filipino Latin American Southeast Asian (e.g., Vietnamese, Cambodian, Malaysian, Laotian, etc.) Arab West Asian (e.g., Iranian, Afghan, etc.) Korean Japanese Other I don’t know

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with

White Chinese South Asian (e.g., East Indian, Pakistani, Sri Lankan, etc.) Black Filipino Latin American Southeast Asian (e.g., Vietnamese, Cambodian, Malaysian, Laotian, etc.) Arab West Asian (e.g., Iranian, Afghan, etc.) Korean Japanese Other I don’t know

19. Marital Status Person I WORKED BEST with Never legally married (single) Legally

married (and not separated) Separated, but still legally married Divorced Widowed

Common-Law I don’t know Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Never legally married (single) Legally

married (and not separated) Separated, but still legally married Divorced Widowed

Common-Law I don’t know 20. Please select the highest level of educational qualification that each of your co-

workers has achieved (if known) Person I WORKED BEST with High school / GED Some college / 2-

year College Degree 3 or 4-year University Professional Degree (JD/LLB, MD)

Masters Degree Doctoral Degree I don’t know

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with High school / GED Some college / 2-year College Degree 3 or 4-year University

Professional Degree (JD/LLB, MD) Masters Degree Doctoral Degree I don’t know

21. Approximately, how many years has each co-worker worked for FIRM NAME? Person I WORKED BEST with Months/Yrs I don’t know Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Months/Yrs I don’t know 22. Approximately, how many years has each co-worker worked in their current role? Person I WORKED BEST with Months/Yrs I don’t know Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Months/Yrs I don’t know

Page 296: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  284  

23. Approximately, how long have you known each of the co-worker you selected? Person I WORKED BEST with < 1 month 1 < 3 months 3 < 6

months 6 < 9 months 9 months < 1 year 1 < 1.5 years 1.5 < 2 years 2 < 3

years 3 < 4 years 4 < 5 years 5 < 6 years 6 < 7 years 7 < 8 years 8 < 9 years 9 < 10 years 10 < 15 years Over 15 years

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with < 1 month 1 < 3 months 3 < 6 months 6 < 9 months 9 months < 1 year

1 < 1.5 years 1.5 < 2 years 2 < 3 years 3 < 4 years 4 < 5 years 5 < 6 years 6 < 7 years 7 < 8 years 8 < 9 years 9 < 10 years 10 < 15 years Over 15 years

24. Prior to working with each of the co-workers on the matter or firm-related project

you shared, how close was your working relationship? Person I WORKED BEST with No Prior Contact Distant Somewhat

Distant Somewhat Close Close Very Close

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with No Prior Contact Distant Somewhat Distant Somewhat Close Close Very Close

25. Prior to working with each of the co-workers on the matter or firm-related project

how often did you communicate? Person I WORKED BEST with Never Once every 3 months or less

Once every 2nd month Once a month Twice a month Once a week Twice a week Daily

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Never Once every 3 months or less Once every 2nd month Once a month Twice a month Once a week Twice a week Daily

26. Prior to working with each of the co-workers on the matter or firm-related project to

what extent did you typically interact? Person I WORKED BEST with To No Extent To Little Extent To

Some Extent To a Great Extent To a Very Great Extent

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with To No Extent To Little Extent To Some Extent To a Great Extent To a Very Great Extent

Page 297: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  285  

27. My relationship with each of the co-workers I mentally selected was a very intense, strong relationship prior to working on the matter or firm related project we shared.

Person I WORKED BEST with 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

28. While working on the matter or firm-related project you shared, how close was your

working relationship with each of the co-workers you mentally selected? Person I WORKED BEST with Distant Somewhat Distant

Somewhat Close Close Very Close Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Distant Somewhat Distant

Somewhat Close Close Very Close 29. While working on the matter or firm-related project you shared, how often did you

communicate with each of the co-workers you mentally selected? Person I WORKED BEST with Once every 3 months or less Once every

2nd month Once a month Twice a month Once a week Twice a week Daily

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with Once every 3 months or less Once every 2nd month Once a month Twice a month Once a week Twice a week Daily

30. While working on the matter or firm-related project you shared, to what extent did

you typically interact with each of the co-workers you mentally selected? Person I WORKED BEST with To No Extent To Little Extent To

Some Extent To a Great Extent To a Very Great Extent

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with To No Extent To Little Extent To Some Extent To a Great Extent To a Very Great Extent

31. My relationship with each of the co-workers I mentally selected was a very intense,

strong relationship while working on the matter or firm related project we shared. Person I WORKED BEST with 1 2 3 4 5

Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with 1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Page 298: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  286  

32. Please indicate your level of agreement with each of the statements below. 1 2 3 4 5

Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Person I WORKED BEST with

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with

- I believed that this person approached his or her job with professionalism and dedication. - Given his or her track record, I saw no reason to doubt this person’s competence and preparation. - This person is very capable of performing his/her job. - This person is known to be successful at the things he/she tries to do. - This person has much knowledge about the work that needs done. - I feel confident about this person’s skills. - This person has specialized capabilities that can increase performance. - This person is well qualified. - I can rely on this person not to make my job more difficult by careless work. - This person is very concerned about my welfare. - My needs and desires are very important to this person. - This person would not knowingly do anything to hurt me.

33. Please indicate your level of agreement with each of the statements below.

1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Person I WORKED BEST with

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with

- This person really looks out for what is important to me. - This person would go out of his or her way to help me. - This person would go out of his or her way to make sure I am not damaged or harmed in this relationship. - I feel like this person cares what happens to me.

Page 299: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  287  

- This person would always look out for my interests. - I feel like this person is on my side. - This person has a strong sense of justice. - I never have to wonder whether this person will stick to his/her word. - This person tries hard to be fair in dealings with others. - This person’s actions and behaviors are not very consistent. - I like this person’s values. - Sound principles seem to guide this person’s behavior. 34. Please indicate your level of agreement with each of the statements below.

1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Person I WORKED BEST with

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with

- I could understand completely what this person meant when he or she was talking. - I was familiar with the jargon/terminology that he or she used. - It felt like we could communicate on the same "wavelength”. - I felt like this person and I were working toward completely different goals. - I assumed that this person and I cared about the same issues. - I believed that this person and I shared a commitment to a common purpose. - I believed that this person and I shared the same ambitions and vision. - I believed that this person and I shared enthusiasm about pursuing the collective goals and mission of the whole organization.

Page 300: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  288  

35. Please indicate your level of agreement with each of the statements below. 1 2 3 4 5

Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Person I WORKED BEST with

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with

- I would take the initiative to provide this individual with useful tools I have developed (e.g. precedents, memos, client information, industry information). - I would allow this individual to spend significant time observing and collaborating with me in order for him/her to better understand and learn from my work. - I would willingly share with this person rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other insights into the work of my office and that of the organization I have learned. - I would willingly share my new ideas with this individual. - I would willingly share with this individual the latest organizational rumors, if significant.

36. Please indicate your level of agreement with each of the statements below.

1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

Person I WORKED BEST with

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with

- I would eagerly receive and use tools developed by this person including precedents, memos, client information and industry information. - I would welcome the opportunity to spend significant time observing and collaborating with this individual in order for me to better understand and learn from his/her work. - I would welcome and use any rules of thumb, tricks of the trade, and other insights they have learned. - I would eagerly receive and consider any new ideas this individual might have. - I would tend to believe organizational rumors shared by this individual and would use such knowledge as appropriate.

Page 301: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  289  

37. The information I received from each of the co-workers made (or is likely to make) the following contribution to:

1 2 3 4 5 Very Very Negative Positive

Person I WORKED BEST with

Person I DID NOT WORK WELL with

- Client satisfaction with the matter / project. - The matter / projects quality. - The project team's overall performance. - The overall success of FIRM NAME. - The cost and/or the time it took to complete the portion of the matter/project I am responsible for. - My individual performance on the matter/project.

Page 302: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  290  

38. Please indicate your level of agreement with each of the following statements about the general work environment at FIRM NAME68.

1 2 3 4 5 Strongly Strongly Disagree Agree

- I feel safe to openly say what I am really thinking to anyone else in FIRM NAME, regardless of their position. - I feel safe to question others' decisions without fear of negative consequences. - My colleagues regularly ask me for my opinion on client matters that they are working on. - I regularly ask my colleagues for their opinions on client matters that I am working on. - The knowledge that I need to work on files is readily available to me. - The precedents that I need to work on files are readily available to me. - Information flows freely within my regional practice group. - Information flows freely within my national practice group. - Colleagues freely share legal information that would be helpful on client matters. - Colleagues freely share industry information that would be helpful on client matters. - Colleagues freely share client information that would be helpful on client matters. - Client contact partners freely share client information with others within the firm. - I know where to go to access the information that I need to perform my work on client matters. - Information flows freely between those in management and those who are not in management. - I feel that I have access to all of the information that I need to be an effective professional.

                                                                                                               68 Item was included at the request of the firm. However, the responses for the item were not analyzed in the present study.

Page 303: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  291  

A.3 Firm-wide Invitation Email  Subject: Knowledge Sharing within FIRM NAME - Survey of FIRM NAME professionals/Sondage sur le partage des connaissances chez FIRM NAME Importance: High

La version française de ce message suit la version anglaise.

You are invited to complete a survey that is going to all FIRM NAME professionals and law clerks. The survey is meant to measure how we use information collaboratively within the firm. The survey is being undertaken by Max Evans in the Faculty of Information Studies at the U. of T. with the consent and support of FIRM NAME. The survey will form part of Max's doctoral dissertation.

The survey has three parts. The first is an individual section, which asks you to answer questions about yourself and your background; the second asks you to answer questions about two coworkers: someone you worked best with on a matter or law firm related project and someone you did not work well with on a matter or law firm related project. The final section asks a few questions about the general work environment at FIRM NAME.

The individual survey responses are anonymous and confidential. Max will only share with us aggregate results.

We feel that this will be an excellent baseline study for us to see how well we collaborate. Perhaps a similar survey will be in order after we complete our SharePoint based portal, which will provide for greater collaboration tools for our professionals.

Note that there is a $5 reward in the form of a Starbucks coffee certificate for completing the survey. All you will have to do to redeem the certificate is print out the last page of the survey (see sample below) and bring it to the receptionist in your office. (Toronto participants should go to the reception desk on the 47th floor, behind the opaque glass doors.) They will be able to exchange the printout for a physical card. Please note one giftcard per participant.

We estimate that the survey will take between 15-20 minutes to complete. For most questions you simply select a numbered response that best matches your opinion.

To begin the survey, please click on the following link:

http://survey.qualtrics.com/SE/?SID=SV_6ukUUwsHo9Aqlne

Thank you in advance for your participation.

Veuillez remplir le sondage (voir le lien ci-dessous) qui est envoyé à tous les professionnels et stagiaires de FIRM NAME. L’objectif du sondage est de mesurer de

Page 304: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  292  

quelle manière , au cabinet, nous utilisons l’information en collaboration. Max Evans, de la Faculté des études en information de l’Université de Toronto, se charge du sondage, avec le consentement des cadres supérieurs de FIRM NAME et le soutien de l’équipe de la Gestion du savoir du cabinet. Le sondage fera partie de la thèse de doctorat de Max.

Le sondage compte trois parties. Dans la première, on vous demande de répondre à des questions personnelles et à des questions sur vos antécédents; dans la deuxième, on vous demande de répondre à des questions au sujet de deux collègues : 1) un collègue avec lequel vous avez travaillé de façon très satisfaisante, dans le cadre d’un dossier ou d’un projet relié au cabinet, et 2) un collègue avec lequel vous n’avez pas eu de relations de travail heureuses, également dans le cadre d’un dossier ou d’un projet relié au cabinet. Dans la troisième partie, on vous pose quelques questions au sujet du milieu de travail en général, chez FIRM NAME.

Les réponses que vous donnez au sondage sont anonymes et confidentielles. Max ne nous fera part que des résultats globaux.

À notre avis, il s’agit là d’une excellente étude de base qui reflétera bien la collaboration qui règne au cabinet.

Veuillez prendre note qu’une récompense de 5 $, à savoir une carte Starbucks, est remise à chaque personne qui répond au sondage. Pour obtenir la carte, imprimez simplement la dernière page du sondage et remettez-la à la réception principale de votre bureau (dans le cas du bureau de Toronto, veuillez vous rendre à la réception du 47e étage, derrière les portes de verre opaque, du côté ouest). On échangera alors votre page imprimée contre une carte Starbucks. Veuillez noter que chaque personne qui répond au sondage ne peut recevoir qu’une seule carte.

Nous croyons qu’il faudra de 15 à 20 minutes pour répondre au sondage. Dans la plupart des cas, vous n’avez qu’à sélectionner le numéro qui correspond le mieux à la réponse que vous souhaitez donner.

Pour commencer à répondre au sondage, veuillez cliquer sur le lien suivant :

http://survey.qualtrics.com/SE/?SID=SV_6ukUUwsHo9Aqlne

Je vous remercie à l’avance de votre participation.

Page 305: Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of …choo.ischool.utoronto.ca/evans_thesis.pdf! ii! Knowledge Sharing: An empirical study of the role of trust and other social-cognitive

  293  

A.4 University of Toronto Ethics Approval Letter