hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (suricata suricatta)

10

Click here to load reader

Upload: anne-a-carlson

Post on 31-Oct-2016

216 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

www.elsevier.com/locate/yhbeh

Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150

Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

Anne A. Carlson,a,* Andrew J. Young,a Andrew F. Russell,a Nigel C. Bennett,b

Alan S. McNeilly,c and Tim Clutton-Brocka

aLarge Animal Research Group, Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 3EJ, UKbMammal Research Institute, Department of Zoology and Entomology, University of Pretoria 0002, South Africa

cMRC HRSU, Centre for Reproductive Biology, The University of Edinburgh, Academic Centre, Edinburgh EH16 4SB, UK

Received 27 January 2003; revised 22 April 2003; accepted 19 January 2004

Available online 18 May 2004

Abstract

In cooperatively breeding meerkats (Suricata suricatta), individuals typically live in extended family groups in which the dominant male

and female are the primary reproductives, while their offspring delay dispersal, seldom breed, and contribute to the care of subsequent litters.

Here we investigate hormonal differences between dominants and subordinates by comparing plasma levels of luteinizing hormone (LH),

estradiol and cortisol in females, and testosterone and cortisol in males, while controlling for potential confounding factors. In both sexes,

hormone levels are correlated with age. In females, levels of sex hormone also vary with body weight and access to unrelated breeding

partners in the same group: subordinates in groups containing unrelated males have higher levels of LH and estradiol than those in groups

containing related males only. When these effects are controlled, there are no rank-related differences in circulating levels of LH among

females or testosterone among males. However, dominant females show higher levels of circulating estradiol than subordinates. Dominant

males and females also have significantly higher cortisol levels than subordinates. Hence, we found no evidence that the lower levels of

plasma estradiol in subordinate females were associated with high levels of glucocorticoids. These results indicate that future studies need to

control for the potentially confounding effects of age, body weight, and access to unrelated breeding partners before concluding that there are

fundamental physiological differences between dominant and subordinate group members.

D 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Suricata suricatta; Luteinizing hormone; Reproductive suppression; Steroid; Dominance; Glucocorticoid; Stress

do not reproduce because unrelated breeding partners are

Introduction

In cooperatively breeding vertebrates, individuals typical-

ly live in extended family groups in which all members

participate in the care of the young, though only the dominant

male and female tend to reproduce (Emlen, 1995; Solomon

and French, 1997; Stacey and Koenig, 1990). As a result,

researchers have attempted to understand why subordinate

males and females fail to breed within these cooperative

groups (e.g., Abbott et al., 1988; Creel et al., 1997; French,

1997; Moehlman and Hofer, 1997). Alternative hypotheses

include the possibility that (1) subordinate individuals are

reproductively suppressed and physiologically incapable of

breeding (reviewed by Creel and Creel, 1991; French, 1997;

Schoech et al., 1996), and (2) subordinate males and females

0018-506X/$ - see front matter D 2004 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.01.009

* Corresponding author. CRES, Zoological Society of San Diego, PO

Box 120551, San Diego, CA 92112-0551. Fax: +1-619-557-3959.

E-mail address: [email protected] (A.A. Carlson).

lacking in their natal group (e.g., Clutton-Brock et al., 2001;

O’Riain et al., 2000; Reyer et al., 1986; Rickard and Bennett,

1997; Schoech et al., 1996).

Attempts to understand the proximate causes of the

reduced reproductive rates of subordinates are commonly

based on comparisons of the hormonal profiles of dominants

and subordinates. A number of studies have documented

significantly increased concentrations of luteinizing hor-

mone (LH) or sex steroids in dominants compared with

subordinates (Bennett et al., 1996; Creel et al., 1992, 1997;

Keane et al., 1994; Poiani and Fletcher, 1994; Schoech et

al., 1991; Ziegler et al., 1987), though others have found no

significant differences (Bennett et al., 1997; French et al.,

1989; Ginther et al., 2001; Khan et al., 2001; Mays et al.,

1991; Smith et al., 1997). In addition, studies have com-

monly compared levels of cortisol and other stress hor-

mones between dominants and subordinates due to

hypothesized links between glucocorticoid levels and repro-

Page 2: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150142

ductive suppression (reviewed by Creel, 2001). Here, too,

results vary, with dominants showing similar levels to

subordinates in male Alpine marmots and male dwarf

mongooses, but higher levels of cortisol in female wild

dogs, male Alpine marmots, female dwarf mongooses, and

common marmosets (Creel, 2001).

Though differences in hormonal status between domi-

nants and subordinates are relatively common and may play

an important role in the control of reproduction, it is not yet

clear whether they are a consequence of contrasts in

dominance per se or of correlated variation in other traits

related to hormone levels. For example, dominant individ-

uals are often older and heavier than subordinates (Clutton-

Brock et al., 2001; Creel and Creel, 1991, 2002; O’Riain et

al., 2000). In addition, dominant individuals typically have

access to an unrelated opposite-sexed individual whereas

this may not be the case for subordinates (O’Riain et al.,

2000; Schoech et al., 1996). As a result, apparent differ-

ences in the hormonal levels of dominant and subordinate

individuals in cooperatively breeding species may be due to

the co-variance of other variables with dominance status

rather than an effect of dominance per se. Consequently,

controlling for age, weight, and access to unrelated breeding

partners is likely to be particularly important in any attempt

to disentangle the effects of social rank from those of

associated variables.

In this study, we investigate contrasts in sex hormones and

cortisol between dominant and subordinate meerkats, and

attempt to discriminate between the effects of social status

and those of associated variables, including age, weight, and

access to unrelated breeding partners. Meerkats are desert-

adapted mongooses (<1 kg) that live in groups of 3–45

members in Southern Africa (Russell et al., in press) in which

reproduction is usually limited to one dominant male and one

dominant female. However, subordinates of both sexes do

sometimes breed (Griffin et al., 2003; Russell et al., 2003a),

and their probability of doing so increases in the presence of

unrelated breeding partners within the group (Clutton-Brock

et al., 2001; O’Riain et al., 2000). The probability that

subordinate females breed also increases with their age and

body weight, though after age and weight effects are con-

trolled, dominant females still breed more frequently than

subordinates (Clutton-Brock et al., 2001). Subordinates of

both sexes contribute to pup care by babysitting pups at the

breeding burrow during their first month of life, and feeding

them with small prey items as they travel with the group

during their subsequent 2 months of dependence (Brotherton

et al., 2001; Clutton-Brock et al., 1998, 2002).

Previous research on hormonal variation in meerkats

shows that dominant females have higher levels of both

plasma LH (O’Riain et al., 2000) and fecal estrogen

metabolites (Clutton-Brock et al., 2001) than subordinate

females, though dominants and subordinates show similar

LH responses to a challenge with gonadotropin-releasing

hormone (GnRh) (O’Riain et al., 2000). Among subordinate

females, those with access to unrelated males in the same

group show higher levels of fecal estrogen metabolites than

those living in groups containing related males only (Clut-

ton-Brock et al., 2001). In contrast, dominant males do not

differ from subordinates in levels of plasma LH (O’Riain et

al., 2000). However, none of these analyses controlled for

variation in the age and weight of sampled individuals, and

no hormonal comparisons between dominant and subordi-

nate meerkats have ever controlled for whether these indi-

viduals had access to unrelated breeding partners in their

group. Here, we investigate whether there are consistent

differences in plasma levels of sex hormones and cortisol

between dominants and subordinates when the potential

confounding effects of age, weight, and access to unrelated

breeding partners are controlled for.

Methods

Animals and sample collection

We collected hormonal data between November 1997

and April 2002 on a total of 157 animals (69 females, 88

males) from 13 different meerkat groups in our study area

near Van Zyl’s Rus in the South African Kalahari Desert

(26j59VS, 21j50VE). Meerkats in our study population have

been monitored since 1993, are individually marked, and

have been habituated to close proximity and handling.

Long-term study of the groups allowed the origins of each

individual (immigrant versus natal) to be ascertained for

analyses dealing with dispersal status and access to unrelat-

ed breeding partners. All research protocols were approved

by the Research Ethics Committee at the University of

Cambridge and complied with regulations stipulated in the

Guidelines for the Use of Animals in Research.

Each blood sample was obtained by placing an animal

inside a capture bag and immobilizing it with an intramus-

cular injection of ketamine and dormitor at a 2:1 ratio and

administered as a weight-dependent dose (100 mg/ml ket-

amine hydrochloride concentration and 1 mg/ml meditomi-

dine hydrochloride) of 0.06 ml ketamine and 0.04 ml

dormitor (O’Riain et al., 2000). After the animal was

completely anesthetized, 0.3–1.5 ml of blood was taken

from the jugular vein using a 24 1/2-G needle and 1-ml

syringes. After blood collection, each animal received an

intramuscular injection of 0.05 ml antisedan (at 5 mg/ml

atipamezole hydrochloride/kg) to reverse the effects of the

dormitor. Blood samples were placed on ice until centri-

fuged for 30 min at 500 � g. Plasma samples were stored

frozen at �40jC until they were assayed. To minimize

potential diurnal variation in hormonal levels, blood sam-

ples were collected between 17 h and 20 h 30 min.

Hormonal assays

Hormonal assays for the determination of LH concen-

trations were completed at the University of Pretoria in

Page 3: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150 143

South Africa. All hormonal assays for the determination of

estradiol, testosterone, and cortisol were carried out at the

Medical Research Council Human Reproductive Sciences

Unit laboratories at the University of Edinburgh in Scotland.

LH bioassay

LH concentrations were measured in an in vitro bioassay

based on the production of testosterone by dispersed mouse

Leydig cells (Van Damme et al., 1974). Harlow et al. (1984)

and Hodges et al. (1987) describe details of the assay, and

O’Riain et al. (2000) report on the validation of the bioassay

for meerkat plasma samples. Plasma samples were assayed

in duplicate at a dilution of 1:20 and 1:40 as a routine check

for parallelism and compared to the human LH pituitary

preparation (2nd International Standard 1988, code no. 80/

552, NIBSC, UK) over the range 400–1.4 miu/ml. The

testosterone produced was measured by radioimmunoassay

as described by Bennett (1994). Checks for parallelism to

the standard curve were carried out to validate the LH

bioassay for plasma taken before and after GnRh adminis-

tration. The curves were parallel and not significantly

different from the reference preparation. Sensitivity of the

assay was 12.7 miu/tube or 2.5 miu/ml. Intra- and inter-

assay coefficients of variation for repeated measurement of a

quality control were 7% and 12%, respectively.

Estradiol

Samples were assayed using Coat-a-Count Estradiol kits

(Diagnostic Products Corporation, Los Angeles, CA). Serial

dilutions of meerkat plasma were parallel to the standard

curve (n = 4, t test = �0.35, P > 0.05). Accuracy, calculated

as the percent recovery of known amounts of estradiol added

to each standard curve point, was 100.4% (n = 5). Assay

sensitivity was 10 pg/ml. Cross-reactivity of the Coat-a-

Count estradiol assay was 10.0% with estrone, and <5%

with estriol, estrone-h�D-glucuronide, estrone-3-sulfate, d-

equilenin, 17h-estradiol-3-monosulfate, testosterone, and

androsterone. Briefly, 25 Al meerkat plasma and 75 Al assaydiluent were added to the antibody-coated test tubes, the

assay was incubated for 3 h at room temperature after adding

1 ml 125I-estradiol to each tube, and then the assay was

decanted before counting each tube for 1 min in a gamma

counter. Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation for a

meerkat plasma pool were 5.2% and 6.7%, respectively (n = 9

assays). In repeated blood sampling (2–4 samples/animal) of

seven subordinate females 3–38 min after capture and

anesthesia, estradiol concentrations did not change signifi-

cantly with increasing capture-to-bleed time (Spearman rank

correlation = 0.106, n = 22, P = 0.64).

Testosterone

Samples were assayed using Coat-a-Count Testosterone

kits (Diagnostic Products Corporation). Serial dilutions of

meerkat plasma were parallel to the standard curve (n = 4, t

test = �0.10, P > 0.05). Accuracy was 101% (n = 7). Assay

sensitivity was 20 ng/dl. Cross-reactivity of the Coat-a-

Count testosterone antibody was 16% with 11-ketotestoster-

one, <5% with dihydrotestosterone and 19-hydroxyandros-

tendione, and <1% with aldosterone, androstendione,

cortisol, corticosterone, estrone, methyltestosterone, and

progesterone. Assay procedures were similar to those used

for estradiol, but with 10 Al meerkat plasma and 90 Al assaydiluent per tube and an incubation period of 3 h at 37jC.Intra- and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 4.1%

and 6.2%, respectively (n = 10 assays). In repeated blood

sampling (2–4 samples/animal) of 10 subordinate males 3–

38 min after capture and anesthesia, testosterone levels did

not change significantly with increasing capture-to-bleed

time (Spearman rank correlation = �0.007, n = 38, P =

0.97).

Cortisol

Samples were assayed using Coat-a-Count Cortisol kits

(Diagnostic Products Corporation). Serial dilutions of meer-

kat plasma were parallel to the standard curve (n = 3, t test =

1.36, P > 0.05). Accuracy was 103.4% (n = 5). Assay

sensitivity was 0.25 Ag/dl. Cross-reactivity with other hor-

mones was 76.0% with prednisolene, 11.4% with 11-deox-

ycortisol, 2.3% with prednisone, and <1% with aldosterone,

corticosterone, cortisone, estriol, estrone, and pregnenolone.

Assay procedures were similar to those used for estradiol,

but with 25 Al meerkat plasma (and no diluent) added to

each tube and a 45-min incubation period at 37jC. Intra-and inter-assay coefficients of variation were 5.2% and

8.7%, respectively (n = 7 assays).

To assess the effects of the stress response on glucocor-

ticoid levels, we first determined that cortisol concentrations

did not vary according to sex (t test, n = 43 subordinate

females, n = 40 subordinate males, T = 0.19, P = 0.85).

Next, we combined different data from 10 subordinate

males and seven females during repeated blood sampling

(2–4 samples/animal) 3–38 min after capture and anesthe-

sia in an analysis that showed that cortisol levels increased

significantly with increasing capture-to-bleed time (Spear-

man rank correlation, n = 58, P < 0.001). Though we

factorize cortisol levels for final analysis, we still control

for capture-to-bleed time in the analyses.

Statistical analyses

The effects of individual, group, and environmental

characteristics on hormonal concentrations were investigat-

ed using linear models (GLM) in GENSTAT 4.2 (2000).

All terms were initially entered into these models and then

sequentially dropped until the model only included terms

whose elimination would have significantly reduced the

explanatory power of the model. The process was repeated

by the additive method (sequential adding of terms with

Page 4: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150144

the retention of terms only if their addition significantly

increased explained variance) to confirm the structure of

the minimal model (Russell et al., 2002). All two-way

interactions between terms were tested but none was found

to be significant.

The primary term of interest in each analysis was a three-

level factor used to tease apart the effects of dominance

from those of access to unrelated breeding partners, classi-

fying individuals as either (i) dominant, (ii) subordinate

without access to unrelated breeding partners, or (iii) sub-

ordinate with access to unrelated breeding partners. All

sampled dominants had access to unrelated breeding part-

ners within their group. This was because dominant males

were typically immigrants, so dominants of both sexes

generally had access to unrelated breeding partners, and

rare samples collected from dominants whose mates had

died were excluded. In contrast, subordinate individuals of

both sexes often did not have access to unrelated breeding

partners in their group (i.e., 83 of 166 subordinate females in

11 study groups lived with related males only). Because

females do not transfer between established groups, but

males do, subordinate females were deemed to have access

to unrelated males if either (a) males had immigrated into

their group after the female was born or (b) the females had

founded a new group with foreign males. Subordinate males

were deemed to have access to unrelated breeding partners if

they had dispersed into or founded their current group

(‘immigrant subordinate’), and not to have access to unre-

lated breeding partners if they were born in their current

group (‘natal subordinate’).

Females designated as ‘dominant’ were the primary

breeders in the group (Clutton-Brock et al., 2001); their ages

ranged from 19 to 68 months. ‘Subordinate’ females were

behaviorally submissive to the dominant female in their

group (O’Riain et al., 2000) and bred at significantly lower

rates than the dominant female (Clutton-Brock et al., 2001);

their ages ranged from 12 to 47 months. Subordinate males

were behaviorally submissive to the dominant male in their

group (O’Riain et al., 2000). Dominant males had high rates

of anal marking and were the fathers of most litters born in

the group (Griffin et al., 2003). Dominant, subordinate natal,

and subordinate immigrant males ranged in age from 26 to

80, 12 to 44, and 15 to 53 months, respectively. Dominance

status is usually inherited by the oldest same-sex subordinate

after the dominant animal dies; as a result, dominant animals

can differ dramatically in age (14–68 months) as they need

only be the oldest of their sex class in the group at the time of

the breeding vacancy (Clutton-Brock et al., 2001).

We included a substantial number of additional variables

in our statistical models, for which definitions and details

follow. The age of each sampled individual was fitted as a

variate and calculated in days since their date of birth, all of

which were known. Rainfall and temperature were recorded

daily at the site; we fitted average maximum monthly

temperature and total monthly rainfall as variables. To

investigate the importance of seasonality, samples were

classified as either being taken during the conceptive season

(the period between June and January inclusive) or not

(Russell et al., 2003a). To control for a possible effect of

lactation on female hormone levels, samples were classified

as having been collected during lactation or not. Group size

was fitted as a variate and calculated as the number of

individuals in each group, excluding pups (i.e., those less

than 3 months of age) (Clutton-Brock et al., 2002). To

control for a possible effect of the breeding context of the

group, samples were classified as having been collected in

one of three contexts: (i) babysitting period, (ii) pup feeding

period, or (iii) currently without pups. Many of these

variables do not appear in the table detailing the minimal

models resulting from our analyses, as they did not have a

significant effect on the circulating levels of the hormone in

question (P > 0.05) and thus were dropped from the

statistical models (see above).

We excluded samples from all pregnant females from the

cortisol data set to control for reproductive state but used

samples from the first 2 weeks of pregnancy for the estradiol

analysis after confirming that estrogen levels in early

pregnancy samples did not differ from nonpregnant samples

(Moss et al., 2001; this study). Gestation in female meerkats

can be detected visually by the end of the first month, and

lasts approximately 70 days (Doolan and Macdonald, 1997),

so dates of conception could thus be backdated from the day

of parturition.

Finally, we compared the testes size for dominant and

subordinate (>1 year old) male meerkats. Measurements of

testes size (length and breadth) were obtained with cali-

pers during captures (see above), and we calculated a

testes size index by multiplying length by breadth. Testes

size index was fitted as a response term in a general linear

mixed model in which we fitted dominance status, age,

and body weight as fixed effects, and individual identity

as a random term, with the random term accounting for

repeated measures within males (1–11 measures per

individual). In this analysis, we were only able to compare

testes sizes of dominant and natal subordinates because

we were only able to collect testes size data from three

immigrant subordinates.

Sample sizes

We used a total of 63 plasma samples from 11 groups (9

dominant females and 36 subordinate females) to assess the

effects of dominance status on female LH levels after a

natural logarithm transformation (Kolmogorov–Smirnov

normality tests: raw data, P = 0.021, transformed data, P >

0.2). We used a total of 73 plasma samples from 11 groups

(7 dominant females and 50 subordinate females) to assess

the effects of dominance status on estradiol levels. As 48 of

the 73 samples had undetectable levels of plasma estradiol,

statistical models based on continuous error structures were

inappropriate (normality test, P = 0.001). Estradiol levels

were factorized into values of 0 or 1 signifying undetectable

Page 5: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150 145

and detectable levels of plasma estradiol, respectively,

allowing the use of a generalized linear model with a

binomial error structure. We used a total of 114 plasma

samples from 13 groups (10 dominant males and 72

subordinate males) to assess the effects of dominance status

on testosterone levels (normality test, P = 0.20), and

obtained 55 measures of testes size from 21 dominants

and 170 measures from 98 subordinate natal males.

We conducted separate cortisol analyses for male and

female meerkats. For females, we used a total of 48 plasma

samples from 10 groups (from 8 dominant females and 36

subordinate females) to assess the effects of dominance

status on cortisol levels. For males, we used a total of 100

plasma samples from 12 groups (from 7 dominant males and

60 subordinate males). As the distributions of plasma

cortisol concentrations were skewed, it was not possible to

use a continuous error structure for statistical modeling

(normality tests for males and females: P = 0.001). Cortisol

levels were factorized into values of 0 or 1 signifying

undetectable and detectable levels of plasma cortisol, re-

spectively, allowing the use of a generalized linear model

with a binomial error structure.

Results

Females

In female meerkats, plasma levels of LH were significant-

ly influenced by age and the presence or absence of unrelated

males in the group (Table 1). After controlling for the effects

of age, we found that dominant females had comparable

levels of plasma LH to subordinate females with access to

unrelated males (P = 0.30, Fig. 1a), but significantly higher

levels than subordinate females without access to unrelated

males (P = 0.002, Fig. 1a). In contrast, plasma levels of

Table 1

Factors affecting hormone levels in female and male meerkats

Hormone analysis Model terms Estimate

Female LH Female type See Fig. 1a

Age (days) �0.00057

Constant 2.13

Female estradiol Female type See Fig. 1b

Body weight (g) 0.0014

Constant �0.7

Female cortisol Female type See Fig. 1c

Male testosterone Age (days) 0.39

Conceptive season (yes > no) 291.2

Constant 397.4

Male cortisol Male type See Fig. 2c

Age (days) 0.0020

Capture-to-bleed time (min) 0.93

Constant �4.07

Minimal models for each of the five multivariate hormonal analyses are presente

significant terms have been presented; for all other terms (see Methods) P > 0.05. ‘

with access to unrelated males, and subordinates without access to unrelated male

natal, and subordinate immigrant. All status effects are presented in Fig. 1 (fema

estradiol were significantly and positively influenced by

body weight and by the presence or absence of unrelated

males. After controlling for the effects of body weight, we

found that dominant females were significantly more likely

to have detectable levels of plasma estradiol than subordinate

females irrespective of whether unrelated males were present

(P < 0.05 in both cases; Fig. 1b). Means and ranges of raw

estradiol were 1202 pg/ml (4.5–4139), 160.7 pg/ml (0–

1300), and 28.7 pg/ml (0–377.4), respectively, for dominant

females, subordinate females with access to unrelated males,

and subordinate females without access to unrelated males.

The probability of having detectable levels of plasma

cortisol was unaffected by female age or body weight (P

values > 0.2). However, the probability of having detectable

cortisol levels differed significantly according to female

status (Table 1). Dominant females were significantly more

likely to have detectable cortisol levels than subordinate

females regardless of whether subordinates had access to

unrelated males in the same group or not (P < 0.05; Fig. 1c).

Means and ranges of raw cortisol were 1.6 Ag/dl (0.5–4.2),0.85 Ag/dl (0–5.8), and 1.2 Ag/dl (0–8.8), respectively, fordominant females, subordinate females with access to un-

related males, and subordinate females without access to

unrelated males.

Males

Plasma testosterone levels were positively correlated

with male age and were higher during the conceptive

season (Table 1). After controlling for these effects,

dominant males did not have higher levels of circulating

testosterone than either subordinate natal males (P = 0.37)

or subordinate immigrant males (P = 0.36) (Fig. 2a).

Additionally, subordinate immigrant males did not have

higher levels of testosterone than subordinate natal males

(P = 0.85).

S.E. F statistic df P value

7.76 2,61 0.001

0.00020 7.89 1,60 0.007

0.18

7.91 2,59 <0.001

0.0066 5.27 1,58 0.022

21.3

6.80 2,47 0.001

0.095 16.62 1,112 <0.001

69.5 17.55 1,112 <0.001

83.5

5.65 2,97 0.004

0.0010 4.14 1,96 0.042

0.29 14.31 1,96 <0.001

2.01

d above. Estimates indicate the direction and strength of each effect. Only

Female type’ refers to three types of adult females: dominants, subordinates

s. ‘Male type’ refers to three types of adult males: dominants, subordinate

les) and 2 (males).

Page 6: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

Fig. 2. Endocrine and anatomical correlates of dominance status in male

meerkats. In each case means (F SE) are presented. (a) Dominant (white

bars), subordinate natal (hatched bars), and subordinate immigrant males

(black bars) had equivalent concentrations of plasma testosterone. (b)

Dominant (white bars) and subordinate natal (hatched bars) males had

similarly sized testes. (c) Dominant males (white bars) were significantly

more likely to have detectable levels of plasma cortisol than immigrant

males (black bars), while the cortisol levels of dominant and subordinate

natal males (hatched bars) did not differ. Subordinate natal males were

significantly more likely to have detectable levels of plasma cortisol than

subordinate immigrant males.

Fig. 1. Endocrine correlates of female status in meerkats. In each case

means (F SE) are presented. (a) After controlling for significant effects of

age, we found that dominant female meerkats (white bars) had similar

levels of plasma LH to subordinate females with access to unrelated males

(hatched bars), but significantly higher levels than subordinate females

without access to unrelated males (black bars). (b) Dominant female

meerkats (white bars) were significantly more likely to have detectable

levels of estradiol than both types of subordinate female. Subordinate

females with access to unrelated males (hatched bars) were significantly

more likely to have detectable levels of plasma estradiol than subordinate

females without access to unrelated males (black bars). (c) Dominant

female meerkats (white bars) were significantly more likely to have

detectable levels of plasma cortisol than subordinate female meerkats with

(hatched bars) and without (black bars) access to unrelated males.

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150146

Testosterone levels alone may not yield a complete

picture of a male’s reproductive potential as dominant

males may have larger testes, and hence larger ejaculates,

than subordinates. A crude comparison by dominance

class alone suggested that dominant males had larger

testes than subordinate males (v2 = 4.64, df = 1, P =

0.031). However, testes size increased with male age (v2 =6.16, df = 1, P = 0.013) and weight (v2 = 16.14, df = 1,

P < 0.001). After these effects were controlled, the

significant difference between the testes size of dominant

Page 7: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150 147

and subordinate males disappeared (v2 = 0.79, df = 1, P =

0.37; Fig. 2b).

Like dominant females, cortisol levels were not influ-

enced by age (P > 0.2) and dominant males usually had

higher cortisol levels than subordinate males. However, in

this case, the difference was largely due to differences

between dominant males and subordinate immigrant

males: dominant males were significantly more likely to

have detectable levels of cortisol than subordinate immi-

grant males (P = 0.014) but not natal subordinates (P =

0.17; Fig. 2c). Subordinate natal males had a significantly

higher probability of having detectable levels of cortisol

than immigrant subordinate males (P = 0.014). Means

and ranges of raw cortisol were 1.1 Ag/dl (0–2.2), 1.0 Ag/dl (0–7.1), and 0.3 Ag/dl (0–1.8), respectively, for dom-

inant males, subordinate natal males, and subordinate

immigrant males.

Discussion

Previous studies of meerkats have shown that dominant

females have higher levels of both plasma LH (O’Riain et

al., 2000) and fecal estrogen metabolites (Clutton-Brock et

al., 2001) than subordinates. This study significantly

extends these findings, because neither of the previous

studies controlled for female age, weight, or access to

unrelated males. Here, we show that after controlling for

these confounding effects, rank-related differences in LH

levels disappear, whereas those for estradiol remain. In

female meerkats, circulating levels of LH and estradiol are

positively affected by the presence of unrelated males and

are also influenced by female age and weight, respectively.

These results suggest that although the hypothalamic and

pituitary activity of dominant and subordinate female meer-

kats may be equivalent, subordinate females appear to

experience downregulation of their ovarian activity, depress-

ing circulating levels of estradiol below those of dominants.

This rank-related disparity in ovarian activity may result

either from suppression of subordinate female fertility by

dominants (e.g., through aggression, Young, 2003) or phys-

iological restraint by subordinates, due to the high proba-

bility of their litters being killed by the dominant (Clutton-

Brock et al., 2001; Russell et al., 2003b). Either way, the

reduced ovarian activity of subordinate females offers a

physiological explanation for their lower breeding rates

(even when they have access to unrelated males), relative

to dominants (Clutton-Brock et al., 2001; O’Riain et al.,

2000). In addition, the fact that heavier females are more

likely to have detectable levels of plasma estradiol offers a

physiological explanation for the higher breeding rates of

heavier females (Clutton-Brock et al., 2001).

Though there are rank-related differences in ovarian

activity, the primary determinant of sex hormone levels in

female meerkats is the presence or absence of unrelated

males in their group. The results presented here confirm

previous findings that access to unrelated males increases

both female LH and estrogen levels (Clutton-Brock et al.,

2001; O’Riain et al., 2000), and extends them by demon-

strating that this difference remains after controlling for

the potentially confounding effects of age and body

weight. The effect of the presence or absence of unrelated

males is presumably related to mechanisms that inhibit

reproduction with close relatives, and provides a physio-

logical explanation for the dramatic effect of unrelated

males on the breeding rates of subordinate females (Clut-

ton-Brock et al., 2001). Schoech et al. (1996) have rightly

emphasized the importance of distinguishing the absence

of a necessary stimulus from the presence of an inhibitor

when investigating causes of breeding suppression. Our

results indicate that, for female meerkats, both factors may

play a role: a stimulatory effect of unrelated males is

apparently necessary for the attainment of physiological

fertility, but an inhibitory effect of subordination appears

to suppress estradiol levels which in turn may compromise

breeding rates.

Dominant female meerkats were significantly more

likely to have detectable levels of cortisol than both types

of subordinate females, and similar differences between

dominants and subordinates have been found in several

other studies of social mammals (reviewed by Creel,

2001). Because dominant female meerkats breed signifi-

cantly more often than subordinate females (Clutton-Brock

et al., 2001; Griffin et al., 2003) and heightened levels of

reproductive hormones have a stimulatory effect on adre-

nal steroidogenesis (reviewed by Saltzman et al., 2000), it

is possible that frequent pregnancies permanently alter the

basal adrenal steroidogenesis patterns of dominant females.

Alternatively, increased levels of glucocorticoids in dom-

inant animals may be related to the stress of dominating

other group members (Creel, 2001). Either way, we found

no evidence that the reduced levels of plasma estradiol in

subordinate female meerkats were associated with high

levels of circulating glucocorticoids.

Male testosterone levels varied seasonally and increased

with age, whereas testes sizes increased with both male

weight and age. After controlling for these effects, dominant

and subordinate males were found to have comparable

levels of testosterone and similarly sized testes, irrespective

of their dispersal status (whether they were in the presence

of unrelated females). Furthermore, O’Riain et al. (2000)

showed that dominant and subordinate males have similar

levels of LH. Though there is no physiological disparity

been dominant and subordinate male meerkats, subordinate

males never breed in their natal group, and even after

dispersal they breed at lower rates than dominants (Griffin

et al., 2003). Consequently, the low breeding rates of

subordinate male meerkats likely result from inbreeding

avoidance in the presence of related females only (while

males remain in their natal group) and behavioral interfer-

ence by dominants when in the presence of unrelated

females (after males have dispersed).

Page 8: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150148

A lack of physiological suppression among males fits

the pattern emerging for males of other cooperative

species, where dominants and subordinates commonly

have comparable hormonal profiles (e.g., Bennett, 1994;

Bennett et al., 1993; Creel et al., 1992), but see naked

mole rats (Heterocephalus glaber) for an exception

(Faulkes et al., 1991, 1994). Selection may have favored

behavioral rather than physiological suppression in male

cooperative mammals, for several reasons. If dominants

can monopolize reproduction through mate-guarding alone,

selection may not favor more complex means of suppres-

sion. Alternatively, subordinate males may be selected to

maintain their fertility if they are able to mate outside their

group by conducting extraterritorial prospecting forays

(e.g., meerkat males: Doolan and Macdonald, 1996; Grif-

fin et al., 2003).

Cortisol levels of dominant males and natal subordinate

males did not differ from one another, but both dominant

and natal subordinate males were significantly more likely

to have detectable levels of cortisol than immigrant subor-

dinate males. In a review of the effects of dominance status

on stress hormones in cooperatively breeding species, Creel

(2001) found that the glucocorticoid levels of dominant

males were either significantly higher than or equal to the

glucocorticoid levels of subordinate males. While our

results are generally consistent with the findings from the

review of Creel (2001), this is the first time to our knowl-

edge that immigrant males have been found to have signif-

icantly lower levels of cortisol than dominant and

subordinate natal males in a cooperative breeder. While

one might expect increased levels of glucocorticoids in

dominant males relative to subordinate males if intragroup

dominance hierarchies were unstable (Sapolsky, 1992) or

dominant males fought more frequently than subordinate

males (Creel, 1996), we have no reason to believe that either

of these factors accounts for the observed patterns of cortisol

secretion in our study groups. Instead, plasma cortisol levels

of subordinate immigrant males appear reduced in compar-

ison with cortisol levels of dominant and subordinate natal

males, offering a curious parallel to the discovery of Saltz-

man et al. (2000) that the plasma cortisol concentrations of

female common marmosets dropped when females became

subordinate in a new group and then remained chronically

suppressed (compared with the basal cortisol levels of

dominant females) (see also Ziegler et al., 1995). However,

while Saltzman et al. (2000) believe that these patterns are

caused by a combination of the effects of social subordina-

tion and the absence of reproductive hormones in female

common marmosets, there is no evidence that subordinate

male meerkats experience chronic reproductive suppression.

As yet, we are unable to explain the observed variation in

cortisol secretion among male meerkats of different status.

Our comparisons of the hormonal levels of dominant

and subordinate meerkats have implications for all

attempts to understand the proximate causes of rank-

related differences in breeding rates in cooperative socie-

ties. At present, it is difficult to tell whether these

reproductive disparities are determined physiologically or

behaviorally, despite the considerable number of studies on

the topic. This difficulty arises in part because few studies

have controlled for factors that may confound physiolog-

ical comparisons of dominant and subordinate group

members. Here we show that circulating levels of repro-

ductive hormones may vary significantly with individual

age (LH and testosterone), weight (estradiol), and access to

unrelated breeding partners (LH and estradiol), all of

which are likely to confound rank-related comparisons in

most cooperative species. It is consequently important that

future correlational studies in both birds and mammals

employ multivariate analyses to control for such effects,

allowing the physiological characteristics associated with

dominance per se to be identified.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to members of the Mammal Research

Institute at the University of Pretoria, without whom this

study would not have been possible, including Professors

John Skinner and Johan du Toit, as well as Martin Haupt.

We thank Mr. and Mrs. H. Kotze for generously allowing us

to work on their land at Vanzyl’s Rus in the southern

Kalahari, and the Northern Cape Conservation Trust for

granting us permission to conduct research in South Africa.

We are grateful to R. Carlson and G. McIlrath for collecting

blood samples. D. Abbott, T. Coulson, J. McNeilly, and W.

Saltzman provided much-appreciated discussion, and two

anonymous reviewers made valuable suggestions for

improvements to the manuscript. The National Institute of

Biological Standard and Controls (2nd International Stan-

dard 1988 Code no. 80/552, Hertfordshire, England) is

acknowledged for the donation of the LH reference

preparation. This research was funded by grants from the

National Research Foundation, South Africa, and the

University of Pretoria awarded to N.C. Bennett and the

Natural Environmental Research Council and the Biotech-

nology and Biological Sciences Research Council awarded

to T. H. Clutton-Brock.

References

Abbott, D.H., Hodges, J.K., George, L.M., 1988. Social status controls LH

secretion and ovulation in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus).

J. Endocrinol. 117, 329–339.

Bennett, N.C., 1994. Reproductive suppression in eusocial Cryptomys

damarensis colonies: a lifetime of socially-induced sterility in males

and females. J. Zool. (London) 234, 25–39.

Bennett, N.C., Jarvis, J.U.M., Faulkes, C.G., Millar, R.P., 1993. LH

responses to single doses of exogenous GnRH by freshly captured

Damaraland mole-rats, Cryptomys damarensis. J. Reprod. Fertil. 99,

81–86.

Bennett, N.C., Faulkes, C.G., Molteno, A.J., 1996. Reproductive suppres-

sion in subordinate, non-breeding female Damaraland mole-rats: two

Page 9: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150 149

components to a lifetime of socially-induced fertility. Proc. R. Soc.

London, Ser. B 263, 1599–1603.

Bennett, N.C., Faulkes, C.G., Spinks, A.C., 1997. LH responses to single

doses of exogenous GnRh by social Mashona mole-rats: a continuum of

socially-induced infertility in the family Bathyergidae. Proc. R. Soc.

London, Ser. B 264, 1001–1006.

Brotherton, P.N.M., Clutton-Brock, T.H., O’Riain, M.J., Gaynor, D.,

Sharpe, L.L., Kansky, R., McIlrath, G.M., 2001. Offspring food alloca-

tion by parents and helpers in a cooperative mammal. Behav. Ecol. 12,

590–599.

Clutton-Brock, T.H, Gaynor, D., Kansky, R., MacColl, A.D.C., McIlrath,

G., Chadwick, P., Brotherton, P.N.M., O’Riain, J.M., Manser, M., Skin-

ner, J.D., 1998. Costs of cooperative behaviour in suricates (Suricata

suricatta). Proc. R. Soc. London, Ser. B 265, 185–190.

Clutton-Brock, T.H., Brotherton, P.N.M., Russell, A.F., O’Riain, M.J.,

Gaynor, D., Kansky, R., Griffin, A., Manser, M., Sharpe, L., McIlrath,

G.M., Small, T., Monfort, S., 2001. Cooperation, control, and conces-

sion in meerkat groups. Science 291, 478–481.

Clutton-Brock, T.H., Russell, A.F., Sharpe, L.L., Young, A.J., Balmforth,

Z., McIlrath, G.M., 2002. Evolution and development of sex differences

in cooperative behavior in meerkats. Science 297, 253–256.

Creel, S., 1996. Social stress and dominance. Nature 379, 212.

Creel, S., 2001. Social dominance and stress hormones. Trends Ecol. Evol.

16, 491–497.

Creel, S.R., Creel, N.M., 1991. Energetics, reproductive success and obli-

gate communal breeding in carnivores. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 28,

263–270.

Creel, S.R., Creel, N.M., 2002. The African Wild Dog: Behavior, Ecology

and Conservation. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton.

Creel, S., Creel, N., Wildt, D.E., Monfort, S.L., 1992. Behavioural and

endocrine mechanisms of reproductive suppression in Serengeti dwarf

mongooses. Anim. Behav. 43, 231–245.

Creel, S., Creel, N.M., Mills, M.G.L., Monfort, S.L., 1997. Rank and

reproduction in cooperatively breeding African wild dogs: behavioral

and endocrine correlates. Behav. Ecol. 8, 298–306.

Doolan, S.P., Macdonald, D.W., 1996. Dispersal and extra-territorial

prospecting by slender-tailed mongooses (Suricata suricatta) in the

south-western Kalahari. J. Zool. (London) 240, 59–73.

Doolan, S.P., Macdonald, D.W., 1997. Breeding and juvenile survival

among slender-tailed meerkats (Suricata suricatta) in the south-west-

ern Kalahari: ecological and social influences. J. Zool. (London) 242,

309–327.

Emlen, S.T., 1995. An evolutionary theory of the family. Proc. Natl. Acad.

Sci. U. S. A. 92, 8092–8099.

Faulkes, C.G., Abbott, D.H., Jarvis, J.U.M., 1991. Social suppression

of reproduction in male naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber).

J. Reprod. Fertil. 91, 593–604.

Faulkes, C.G., Trowell, S.N., Jarvis, J.U.M., Bennett, N.C., 1994. Investi-

gation of sperm numbers and motility in reproductively active and

socially suppressed males of two eusocial African mole-rats, the naked

mole-rat (Heteropcephalus glaber), and the Damaraland mole-rat

(Cryptomys damarensis). J. Reprod. Fertil. 100, 411–416.

French, J.A., 1997. Proximate regulation of singular breeding in Callitri-

chid primates. In: Solomon, N.G., French, J.A. (Eds.), Cooperative

Breeding in Mammals. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, England,

pp. 34–75.

French, J.A., Inglett, B.J., Dethlefs, T.M., 1989. The reproductive status of

nonbreeding group members in captive golden lion tamarin social

groups. Am. J. Primatol. 18, 73–86.

Ginther, A.J., Ziegler, T.E., Snowdon, C.T., 2001. Reproductive biology of

captive male cotton-top tamarin monkeys as a function of social envi-

ronment. Anim. Behav. 61, 65–78.

Griffin, A.S., Pemberton, J.M., Brotherton, P.N.M., McIlrath, G., Gaynor,

D., Kansky, R., Clutton-Brock, T.H., 2003. A genetic analysis of breed-

ing success in the cooperative meerkat (Suricata suricatta). Behav.

Ecol. 14, 472–480.

Harlow, C.R., Gems, S., Hodges, J.K., Hearn, J.P., 1984. The relationship

between plasma testosterone and the timing of ovulation and early

embryonic development in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus).

J. Zool. (London) 201, 272–282.

Hodges, J.K., Cottingham, P., Summers, P.M., Yingnan, L., 1987. Con-

trolled ovulation in the marmoset monkey (Callithrix jacchus), with

human chorionic gonadotropin following prostaglandin induced luteal

regression. Fertil. Steril. 48, 299–305.

Keane, B., Waser, P.M., Creel, S., Creel, N.M., Elliott, L.F., Minchella,

D.J., 1994. Subordinate reproduction in dwarf mongooses. Anim.

Behav. 47, 65–75.

Khan, M.Z., McNabb, F.M.A., Walters, J.R., Sharp, P.J., 2001. Patterns of

testosterone and prolactin concentrations and reproductive behavior of

helpers and breeders in the cooperatively breeding red-cockaded wood-

pecker (Picoides borealis). Horm. Behav. 40, 1–13.

Mays, N.A., Masters Vleck, C., Dawson, J., 1991. Plasma luteinizing

hormone, steroid hormones, behavioral role, and nest stage in coop-

eratively breeding Harris’ hawks (Parabuteo unicinctus). Auk 108,

619–637.

Moehlman, P.D., Hofer, H., 1997. Cooperative breeding, reproductive sup-

pression and body mass in canids. In: Solomon, N.G., French, J.A.

(Eds.), Cooperative Breeding in Mammals. Cambridge Univ. Press,

Cambridge, England, pp. 76–128.

Moss, A.M., Clutton-Brock, T.H., Monfort, S.L., 2001. Longitudinal go-

nadal steroid excretion in free-living male and female meerkats (Sur-

ricata suricatta). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 122, 158–171.

O’Riain, M.J., Bennett, N.C., Brotherton, P.N.M., McIlrath, G., Clutton-

Brock, T.H., 2000. Reproductive suppression and inbreeding avoidance

in wild populations of co-operatively breeding meerkats (Suricata sur-

icatta). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 48, 471–477.

Poiani, A., Fletcher, T., 1994. Plasma levels of androgens and gonadal

development of breeders and helpers in the bell miner (Manorina mel-

anophrys). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 34, 31–41.

Reyer, H.-U., Dittami, J.P., Hall, M.R., 1986. Avian helpers at the nest: are

they psychologically castrated? Ethology 71, 216–228.

Rickard, C.A., Bennett, N.C., 1997. Recrudescence of sexual activity in a

reproductively quiescent colony of the Damaraland mole-rat, by the

introduction of a genetically unrelated male—A case of incest avoid-

ance in ‘queenless’ colonies. J. Zool. (London) 241, 185–202.

Russell, A.F., Clutton-Brock, T.H., Brotherton, P.N.M., Sharpe, L.L., McIl-

rath, G.M., Dalerum, F.D., Cameron, E.Z., Barnard, J.A., 2002. Factors

affecting pup growth and survival in co-operatively breeding meerkats

Suricata suricatta. J. Anim. Ecol. 71, 700–709.

Russell, A.F., Brotherton, P.N.M., McIlrath, G.M., Sharpe, L.L., Clutton-

Brock, T.H., 2003a. Breeding success in cooperative meerkats: effects

of helper number and maternal state. Behav. Ecol. 14, 486–492.

Russell, A.F., Brotherton, P.N.M., McIlrath, G.M., Sharpe, L.L., Clutton-

Brock, T.H., 2003b. Cost minimization by helpers in cooperative verte-

brates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 100, 3333–3338.

Russell, A.F., Carlson, A.A., McIlrath, G.M., Jordan, N.R. and Clutton-

Brock, T.H., in press. Adaptive modification by dominant female meer-

kats. Evol.

Saltzman, W., Prudom, S.L., Schultz-Darken, N.J., Abbott, D.H., 2000.

Reduced adrenocortical responsiveness to adrenocorticotropic hormone

(ACTH) in socially subordinate female marmoset monkeys. Psycho-

neuroendocrinol 25, 463–477.

Sapolsky, R.M., 1992. Neuroendocrinology of the stress response. In:

Becker, J.B., et al. (Eds.), Behavioral Endocrinology. Massachusetts

Institute of Technology Press, Massachusetts, pp. 287–324.

Schoech, S.J., Mumme, R.L., Moore, M.C., 1991. Reproductive endocri-

nology and mechanisms of breeding inhibition in cooperatively breed-

ing Florida scrub jays (Aphelocoma c. coerulescens). Condor 93,

234–244.

Schoech, S.J., Mumme, R.L., Wingfield, J.C., 1996. Delayed breeding in

the cooperatively breeding Florida scrub-jay (Aphelocoma coerules-

cens): inhibition or the absence of stimulation? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

39, 77–90.

Smith, T.E., Schaffner, C.M., French, J.A., 1997. Social and developmental

Page 10: Hormonal correlates of dominance in meerkats (Suricata suricatta)

A.A. Carlson et al. / Hormones and Behavior 46 (2004) 141–150150

influences on reproductive function in female Wied’s black tufted-ear

marmosets (Callithrix kuhli). Horm. Behav. 31, 159–168.

Solomon, N.G., French, J.A., 1997. Cooperative Breeding in Mammals

Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, England.

Stacey, P.B., Koenig, W.D., 1990. Cooperative Breeding in Birds:

Long-term Studies of Ecology and Behavior. Cambridge Univ.

Press, Cambridge.

Van Damme, M.P., Robertson, D.M., Diczfalusy, E., 1974. An improved in

vitro bioassay method for measuring luteinising hormone (LH) activity

using mouse Leydig cell preparations. Acta Endocrinol. 77, 655–671.

Young, A.J., 2003. Subordinate tactics in cooperative meerkats: helping,

breeding and dispersal. PhD thesis, Cambridge University, England.

Ziegler, T.E., Savage, A., Scheffler, G., Snowdon, C.T., 1987. The endo-

crinology of puberty and reproductive functioning in female cotton-top

tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) under varying social conditions. Biol.

Reprod. 37, 618–627.

Ziegler, T.E., Scheffler, G., Snowdon, C.T., 1995. The relationship of

cortisol levels to social environment and reproductive functioning in

female cotton-top tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Horm. Behav. 29,

407–424.