mechanisms of the contextual interference effect in individuals poststroke

10
Mechanisms of the contextual interference effect in individuals poststroke Nicolas Schweighofer, 1 * Jeong-Yoon Lee, 2 * Hui-Ting Goh, 1 Youggeun Choi, 2,5 Sung Shin Kim, 3 Jill Campbell Stewart, 1 Rebecca Lewthwaite, 1,4 and Carolee J. Winstein 1 1 Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, 2 Computer Science, and 3 Neuroscience, University of Southern California, Los Angeles; 4 Rancho Los Amigos National Rehabilitation Center, Downey, California; and 5 Applied Computer Engineering, Dankook University, Yongin, South Korea Submitted 2 May 2011; accepted in final form 9 August 2011 Schweighofer N, Lee JY, Goh HT, Choi Y, Kim S, Stewart JC, Lewthwaite R, Winstein CJ. Mechanisms of the contextual interfer- ence effect in individuals poststroke. J Neurophysiol 106: 2632–2641, 2011. First published August 10, 2011; doi:10.1152/jn.00399.2011.— Although intermixing different motor learning tasks via random schedules enhances long-term retention compared with “blocked” schedules, the mechanism underlying this contextual interference effect has been unclear. Furthermore, previous studies have reported inconclusive results in individuals poststroke. We instructed partici- pants to learn to produce three grip force patterns in either random or blocked schedules and measured the contextual interference effect by long-term forgetting: the change in performance between immediate and 24-h posttests. Nondisabled participants exhibited the contextual interference effect: no forgetting in the random condition but forget- ting in the blocked condition. Participants at least 3 mo poststroke exhibited no forgetting in the random condition but marginal forget- ting in the blocked condition. However, in participants poststroke, the integrity of visuospatial working memory modulated long-term reten- tion after blocked schedule training: participants with poor visuospa- tial working memory exhibited little forgetting at 24 h. These coun- terintuitive results were predicted by a computational model of motor memory that contains a common fast process and multiple slow processes, which are competitively updated by motor errors. In blocked schedules, the fast process quickly improved performance, therefore reducing error-driven update of the slow processes and thus poor long-term retention. In random schedules, interferences in the fast process led to slower change in performance, therefore increasing error-driven update of slow processes and thus good long-term reten- tion. Increased forgetting rates in the fast process, as would be expected in individuals with visuospatial working memory deficits, led to small updates of the fast process during blocked schedules and thus better long-term retention. stroke; neurorehabilitation; motor learning; computational neurosci- ence DURING NEUROREHABILITATION after brain injury, but also in activities such as sports, technical training, and music, one must often learn, or relearn, multiple motor tasks within a given period. Intermixing the learning of different tasks via random schedules reduces performance during training but enhances long-term retention compared with blocked sched- ules, (e.g., Schmidt and Lee 1999; Shea and Morgan 1979; Tsuitsui et al. 1998). This phenomenon is known as the contextual interference (CI) effect. Despite close to a century of research (Pyle 1919), however, the mechanism underlying the CI effect are unclear. According to the “forgetting-reconstruction” hypothesis of the CI effect, short-term forgetting between successive presentations of the same task during random training requires the learner to “reconstruct the action plan at each presentation,” resulting in stronger memory representations (Lee and Magill 1983; Lee et al. 1985). Recent computational models similarly suggest a crucial role of working memory in the CI effect. It has notably been proposed that motor adaptation occurs via simultaneous update of a fast process that contributes to fast initial learning but forgets quickly and a slow process that contributes to long-term retention but learns slowly (Joiner and Smith 2008; Smith et al. 2006). We recently extended this model to account for multiple task adaptation (Lee and Schweighofer 2009). In our model, a single fast process is arranged in parallel with multiple independent slow processes switched via contextual cues. During adaptation, motor errors simultaneously update fast and slow processes in a competitive manner. In a situation where tasks are intermixed during training, our model predicts that the decay in the fast process due to both time and interference from other tasks leads to greater update of the slow process. Thus random schedule training should lead to less long-term forgetting than blocked schedule training, as in the CI effect. Individuals with unilateral stroke-related damage in the sensorimotor areas often exhibit deficits in visuospatial work- ing memory (Winstein et al. 1999). Accordingly, the integrity of visuospatial working memory may play a role in the CI effect in individuals poststroke. The two previous attempts at testing the CI effect in stroke individuals with motor impair- ments were inconclusive. Specifically, Hanlon (1996) reported a CI effect; the effect of schedules could not be unequivocally determined in this study, however, because the practice ses- sions were of variable lengths. On the contrary, Cauraugh and Kim (2003) reported no CI effect; the tasks used in this study were strengthening tasks (such as wrist/finger extension), how- ever, not goal-directed tasks that required acquisition of new skills. Thus it is unclear whether these contrasted results are due to either, or both, the experimental design or the hetero- geneous grouping of all stroke individuals poststroke with potential differences in working memory, thus masking the CI effect. The goals of this study are thus to provide a mechanistic explanation for the CI effect using our previous model (Lee and Schweighofer 2009), notably to study the role of the fast process in the CI effect and to test the CI effect in individuals poststroke with visuospatial working memory deficits, provid- ing behavioral support for the model. For this purpose, we designed an experiment in which young nondisabled partici- * N. Schweighofer and J.-Y. Lee contributed equally to this work. Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: N. Schweighofer, Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, Univ. of Southern California, 1540 E. Alcazar St., CHP 155, Los Angeles, CA 90089 (e-mail: [email protected]). J Neurophysiol 106: 2632–2641, 2011. First published August 10, 2011; doi:10.1152/jn.00399.2011. 2632 0022-3077/11 Copyright © 2011 the American Physiological Society www.jn.org

Upload: northwestern

Post on 27-Nov-2023

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Mechanisms of the contextual interference effect in individuals poststroke

Nicolas Schweighofer,1* Jeong-Yoon Lee,2* Hui-Ting Goh,1 Youggeun Choi,2,5 Sung Shin Kim,3

Jill Campbell Stewart,1 Rebecca Lewthwaite,1,4 and Carolee J. Winstein1

1Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, 2Computer Science, and 3Neuroscience, University of Southern California,Los Angeles; 4Rancho Los Amigos National Rehabilitation Center, Downey, California; and 5Applied Computer Engineering,Dankook University, Yongin, South Korea

Submitted 2 May 2011; accepted in final form 9 August 2011

Schweighofer N, Lee JY, Goh HT, Choi Y, Kim S, Stewart JC,Lewthwaite R, Winstein CJ. Mechanisms of the contextual interfer-ence effect in individuals poststroke. J Neurophysiol 106: 2632–2641,2011. First published August 10, 2011; doi:10.1152/jn.00399.2011.—Although intermixing different motor learning tasks via randomschedules enhances long-term retention compared with “blocked”schedules, the mechanism underlying this contextual interferenceeffect has been unclear. Furthermore, previous studies have reportedinconclusive results in individuals poststroke. We instructed partici-pants to learn to produce three grip force patterns in either random orblocked schedules and measured the contextual interference effect bylong-term forgetting: the change in performance between immediateand 24-h posttests. Nondisabled participants exhibited the contextualinterference effect: no forgetting in the random condition but forget-ting in the blocked condition. Participants at least 3 mo poststrokeexhibited no forgetting in the random condition but marginal forget-ting in the blocked condition. However, in participants poststroke, theintegrity of visuospatial working memory modulated long-term reten-tion after blocked schedule training: participants with poor visuospa-tial working memory exhibited little forgetting at 24 h. These coun-terintuitive results were predicted by a computational model of motormemory that contains a common fast process and multiple slowprocesses, which are competitively updated by motor errors. Inblocked schedules, the fast process quickly improved performance,therefore reducing error-driven update of the slow processes and thuspoor long-term retention. In random schedules, interferences in thefast process led to slower change in performance, therefore increasingerror-driven update of slow processes and thus good long-term reten-tion. Increased forgetting rates in the fast process, as would beexpected in individuals with visuospatial working memory deficits,led to small updates of the fast process during blocked schedules andthus better long-term retention.

stroke; neurorehabilitation; motor learning; computational neurosci-ence

DURING NEUROREHABILITATION after brain injury, but also inactivities such as sports, technical training, and music, onemust often learn, or relearn, multiple motor tasks within agiven period. Intermixing the learning of different tasks viarandom schedules reduces performance during training butenhances long-term retention compared with blocked sched-ules, (e.g., Schmidt and Lee 1999; Shea and Morgan 1979;Tsuitsui et al. 1998). This phenomenon is known as thecontextual interference (CI) effect.

Despite close to a century of research (Pyle 1919), however,the mechanism underlying the CI effect are unclear. According

to the “forgetting-reconstruction” hypothesis of the CI effect,short-term forgetting between successive presentations of thesame task during random training requires the learner to“reconstruct the action plan at each presentation,” resulting instronger memory representations (Lee and Magill 1983; Lee etal. 1985). Recent computational models similarly suggest acrucial role of working memory in the CI effect. It has notablybeen proposed that motor adaptation occurs via simultaneousupdate of a fast process that contributes to fast initial learningbut forgets quickly and a slow process that contributes tolong-term retention but learns slowly (Joiner and Smith 2008;Smith et al. 2006). We recently extended this model to accountfor multiple task adaptation (Lee and Schweighofer 2009). Inour model, a single fast process is arranged in parallel withmultiple independent slow processes switched via contextualcues. During adaptation, motor errors simultaneously updatefast and slow processes in a competitive manner. In a situationwhere tasks are intermixed during training, our model predictsthat the decay in the fast process due to both time andinterference from other tasks leads to greater update of the slowprocess. Thus random schedule training should lead to lesslong-term forgetting than blocked schedule training, as in theCI effect.

Individuals with unilateral stroke-related damage in thesensorimotor areas often exhibit deficits in visuospatial work-ing memory (Winstein et al. 1999). Accordingly, the integrityof visuospatial working memory may play a role in the CIeffect in individuals poststroke. The two previous attempts attesting the CI effect in stroke individuals with motor impair-ments were inconclusive. Specifically, Hanlon (1996) reporteda CI effect; the effect of schedules could not be unequivocallydetermined in this study, however, because the practice ses-sions were of variable lengths. On the contrary, Cauraugh andKim (2003) reported no CI effect; the tasks used in this studywere strengthening tasks (such as wrist/finger extension), how-ever, not goal-directed tasks that required acquisition of newskills. Thus it is unclear whether these contrasted results aredue to either, or both, the experimental design or the hetero-geneous grouping of all stroke individuals poststroke withpotential differences in working memory, thus masking the CIeffect.

The goals of this study are thus to provide a mechanisticexplanation for the CI effect using our previous model (Leeand Schweighofer 2009), notably to study the role of the fastprocess in the CI effect and to test the CI effect in individualspoststroke with visuospatial working memory deficits, provid-ing behavioral support for the model. For this purpose, wedesigned an experiment in which young nondisabled partici-

* N. Schweighofer and J.-Y. Lee contributed equally to this work.Address for reprint requests and other correspondence: N. Schweighofer,

Biokinesiology and Physical Therapy, Univ. of Southern California, 1540 E.Alcazar St., CHP 155, Los Angeles, CA 90089 (e-mail: [email protected]).

J Neurophysiol 106: 2632–2641, 2011.First published August 10, 2011; doi:10.1152/jn.00399.2011.

2632 0022-3077/11 Copyright © 2011 the American Physiological Society www.jn.org

pants and individuals at least 3 mo poststroke learned how toproduce three specific force patterns in either a random or ablocked schedule practice condition. The CI effect was mea-sured by long-term forgetting, the change in performancebetween two retention tests given immediately and 24 h fol-lowing training, respectively.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

Nondisabled participants. Twenty-four young participants (12 fe-males) with no reported neurological deficits were randomly assignedto either a blocked training schedule or a pseudorandom trainingschedule (n � 12 in each group; because of problems with therecording device with 1 participant, we analyzed data for 11 partici-pants in the random group and 12 in the blocked group for theretention results). The participants met the following inclusion crite-ria: �18 yr of age and right hand dominant. A summary of thedemographic data for nondisabled individuals is reported in Table 1.

Individuals poststroke. Twenty-five participants (8 females) withstroke at least 3 mo from onset were randomly assigned to either ablocked training schedule or a pseudorandom training schedule (n �12 in blocked group, n � 13 in random group). To be included in thestudy, the participants needed to fulfill the following inclusion criteria:1) �18 years of age, 2) at least 3 mo poststroke, 3) stable medicalcondition, 4) an upper extremity (UE) Fugl-Meyer score no less than33 (Fugl-Meyer et al. 1975); 5) the ability to produce at least 10 N offorce in power and 2 N in precision grasp, 6) a score at least 10 outof 17 on the Functional Test of the Hemiparetic Upper Extremity(FTHUE; Wilson 1984), and 7) a score of no less than 25 on the mini

mental state exam (MMSE). The participants were excluded from thestudy if they 1) demonstrated excessive pain in any joint of the moreaffected extremity that could limit ability to participate in the graspingtasks or 2) had any previous history of surgery of fracture in theaffected extremity that might impair their ability to perform the task.A summary of the demographic data for individuals poststroke isreported in Table 2.

All participants (nondisabled individuals and individuals post-stroke) signed an informed consent to participate in this study, whichwas approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University ofSouthern California.

Experimental Design and Procedures

A critical impairment after stroke is the inability to generate preciseforce output at moderate levels with sufficiently rapid force rise. Wethus designed a learning experiment that required learning to exertthree force patterns with unique magnitude and timing in response tothree visually distinct target force profiles. Participants were pseudo-randomly assigned to either a blocked schedule condition or a randomschedule condition.

The participants came to our laboratory for two sessions on 2consecutive days. The first session consisted of physical and cognitivetests, a training session, and an immediate retention test (see below).A delayed retention tests was given 24 h following the first (imme-diate) retention test. During the training session, all participants weretrained on the 3 motor tasks, with 50 trials per task. In the blockedschedule condition, there were 3 blocks of 50 consecutive trials, witha single task within a block. Task order was counterbalanced acrossparticipants. In the random schedule condition, there were 50 blocksof 3 trials, with each task occurring once per block at a random order

Table 1. Characteristics of nondisabled individuals

Characteristics Blocked Random P Value

No. of individuals 12 11No. of men 6 6 �0.1Age, yr 25.5 � 1.86 26.7 � 2.65 �0.1Power force, N 863 � 99 794 � 111 �0.1Wechsler figural score (max 10) 8.58 � 0.29 8.33 � 0.33 �0.1

Values are means � SE. Max value indicates the maximum score for the Wechsler figural test.

Table 2. Characteristics of individuals poststroke

Characteristics Blocked Random P Value

No. of individuals 12 13No. of men 8 9 �0.1Age, yr 61.25 � 13.92 54.58 � 13.39 �0.1No. with left hemiparesis 6 4 �0.1No. with concordance 7 5 �0.1Time postonset, mo 39.25 � 16.45 25.83 � 22.32 �0.1Power force, N 462 � 67 326 � 29 0.082UE Fugl-Meyer score

ROM (max 24) 22.58 � 1.83 22.25 � 1.76 �0.1Pain (max 24) 24.00 � 0.00 23.25 � 2.30 �0.1Sensory (max 12) 12.00 � 0.00 11.00 � 2.23 �0.1Motor (max 66) 55.00 � 8.98 54.00 � 7.01 �0.1Wrist (max 10) 9.10 � 2.81 8.00 � 3.28 �0.1Hand (max 14) 12.25 � 1.54 11.58 � 2.31 �0.1

FTHUE 14.50 � 3.53 14.91 � 2.94 �0.1MMSE (max 30) 29.25 � 1.21 29.41 � 0.90 �0.1Wechsler digital score (max 24) 16.83 � 4.71 16.08 � 3.17 �0.1Wechsler figural score (max 10) 6.16 � 1.69 6.67 � 1.97 �0.1

Values are means � SE. Max value indicates the maximum score for the indicated test. UE, upper extremity; ROM, range of motion; FTHUE, Functional Testof the Hemiparetic Upper Extremity; MMSE, mini mental state exam.

2633CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org

within the block. The initial task order was counterbalanced acrossparticipants.

The participants were instructed to reach and grasp a plasticcylinder and to exert a force profile with a power grasp that matchedone of three target profiles displayed on a computer screen in bothmagnitude and timing (Fig. 1). Participant poststroke were instructedto use their affected hand. Nondisabled participants were instructed touse their dominant hand. Force data was acquired through a sensorembedded in the cylinder sampling at a rate of 100 Hz.

At the beginning of each trial, one of the three target force profileswas shown. Two seconds later, the target profile disappeared, a “GO”signal was displayed, and the participants were instructed to producea force trajectory that matched the target force profile. If the partici-pant did not grasp the cylinder within the 2 s of the “GO” signal, themessage “Next time move faster” was displayed.

In training trials, feedback was provided 4 s after the end of thetarget display. Specifically, the actual force trajectory was shown

superimposed on the desired trajectory (Fig. 1). An error value wasalso displayed indicating the total root mean squared error (RMSE)between the desired and actual force trajectory.

To assess performance after training, retention tests were given im-mediately and 24 h following training. Each test consisted of five trialsper task, with each trial similar to training trials but without feedback. Inthe immediate test for the blocked schedule group, the test for each taskwas given immediately following the 50 training trials. In the immediateretention tests for the random schedule group, as well in the delayed testsfor both groups, the order of the three tasks was randomized.

For each participant, maximal force was recorded with the appa-ratus in three separate trials and averaged before training. The max-imum magnitude of each target force profile was set at 40% ofmaximum force for each participant. Figure 2 shows examples of theforce profiles and the actual force trajectories for one force pattern fora representative participant in each group (stroke blocked, strokerandom, nondisabled blocked, nondisabled random) for the first trialof training, for the first trial of the immediate test, and for the first trialof the delayed retention test.

Visuospatial Working Memory Test

In our task, participants exerted grip forces based on line-drawingcues visually displayed 2 s previously and not available during forcemodulation. Furthermore, visual feedback was provided 4 s followingforce modulation. Thus visuospatial working memory was needed tolink the visual information provided as cue and feedback to the actualmovement generated. We assessed visuospatial working memory foreach participant with the figural memory subtest of the Wechslermemory test revised (Wechsler 1987). In the figural memory test, atarget geometrical pattern is presented to the participant. After a 6-sdelay, both the target pattern and alternative patterns are shown. Theparticipant is asked to identify the correct pattern. The maximumscore for the figural test is 10. The figural test has been used inprevious research studies with various clinical populations to assessvisual-visuospatial working memory (e.g., Hawkins et al. 1997; Nixonet al. 1987; Winstein et al. 1999). The digital memory subtest of theWechsler memory score was also given for comparison purpose.

Fig. 1. Motor task. At each trial, 1 of 3 target force profiles was shown duringthe “ready” period. The specific force profile was selected according to apredetermined schedule (random or blocked). Two seconds later, a “GO”signal appeared and the participant was instructed to reach and grasp theapparatus and modulate the power grip force to approximate the target profilethat lasted 2 s. The computer screen then became blank for 4 s, and feedbackwas shown for 4 s. Feedback included the actual force profile superimposedwith the desired profile, the root mean squared error (RMSE) between the 2profiles, as a well as a “best score,” which reflected the smallest error so far andwas included for motivational purposes.

Fig. 2. Examples of force trajectories for 4 par-ticipants for 1 task for the first trial of practice(left), the first trial of the immediate retention test(middle), and the first trial of the delayed test(right). Representative data are from a participantpoststroke in blocked schedule (stroke-blocked), aparticipant poststroke in random schedule (stroke-random), a nondisabled participant in blockedschedule (healthy-blocked), and a nondisabled par-ticipant in random schedule (healthy-random). Thegray line indicates the desired force profile; theblack line indicates the actual force (in N) exertedby the participant during 2 s.

2634 CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org

Data Analysis

To compare performance across two schedules and across youngnondisabled and stroke individuals, we computed performance at eachtrial as 1 minus a normalized root mean square error (nRMSE)averaged across the tasks. We normalized the RMSE for each task: wedivided the RMSE at each trial by the difference between the maximumRMSE (often but not always on the first trial) and the minimum RMSEgenerated during training. The nRMSE for subject i at trial n is thusgiven as nRMSEi(n) � [RMSEi(n) � min(RMSEi)]/[max(RMSEi) �min(RMSEi)], where min(RMSEi) and max(RMSEi) are calculated forsubject i across training trials for all three tasks.

Our main dependent measure was “long-term forgetting.” Long-term forgetting was computed as the difference in task performancebetween the immediate and the 24-h test. As a measure of retentionperformance in the immediate and delayed retention tests, we used theaverage performance in the first trial for the three tasks. We did notuse all five trials of each task in the retention test because we notedthat performance largely improved during the test even withoutfeedback in the nondisabled participant group (e.g., immediate group,average of 3 tasks, repeated ANOVA for the 5 test trials: P � 0.001in blocked condition, P � 0.04 in random condition).

All data (and residuals for regression models) used were tested fornormality with the Shapiro-Wilks’ test and for equality of variancewith the Levene’s test. For group comparisons, when the data werenormally distributed and when the sample size was �10 in eachgroup, we used t-tests for independent measures and paired t-tests forrepeated measures. Otherwise, we used the Mann-Whitney test forindependent measures. In repeated ANOVA, nonspherical data werecorrected using Greenhouse-Geisser correction. Differences betweengroups in participants’ baseline characteristics were tested with t-testswhen the data were numerical (e.g., age) and with �2 tests when thedata were categorical (e.g., sex). We classified the individuals withstroke as “high” and “low” function in visuospatial working memory,based on a cutoff of 7 on the figural Wechsler score (the cutoff valuewas chosen because it yielded nearly equal group sizes in our data set)and compared the effect of practice schedules for high- and low-levelgroups. Our significance level in all tests was set at P � 0.05.

Computer Modeling

It has been proposed that motor adaptation occurs via simultaneousupdate of a fast process that contributes to fast initial learning butforgets quickly and a slow process that contributes to long-termretention but learns slowly (Joiner and Smith 2008; Smith et al. 2006).We recently extended this model to account for multiple task adap-tation (Lee and Schweighofer 2009). In our model, a single fastprocess is arranged in parallel with multiple independent slow processesswitched via contextual cues. During adaptation, motor errors simultane-ously update the fast and the selected slow processes in a competitivemanner. Because the model was specifically developed to account formultiple task-adaptation, it can be used to simulate the changes inperformance resulting from different task schedules. We therefore testedin what conditions, if any, the model could reproduce the CI effect.

The model contains one fast process and N slow processes, with Nbeing the number of tasks, all organized in parallel (the model is thuscalled a 1-fast N-slow parallel model). The model output and the stateupdate rules are given by Eqs. 1 and 2, respectively:

y(n) � xf(n) � xs(n)Tc(n) (1)

xf(n � 1) � Afxf(n) � Bfe(n)

xs(n � 1) � Asxs(n) � Bse(n)c(n)(2)

where n is the trial number. The motor error determined by thedifference between an external perturbation f(n) and the motor output y(n)at time step n is e(n) � f(n) � y(n), xf is the state of the fast process, xs

is the state vector of the slow process, and c is the contextual cue vector,

with both xs and c vectors having a length equal to the number of tasks,N. Because we assumed no interference and perfect switching amongstates in the slow process in the model, c is a unit vector with a singlenon-zero element. Af is the forgetting rate of the fast process, As is theforgetting rate of the slow processes, Bf is the learning rate of the fastprocess, and Bs is the learning rate of the slow processes. Defaultparameters in the model in the current simulations were Af � 0.8, Bf �0.2, As � 0.995, and Bs � 0.04, and they were hand-tuned to qualitativelyreproduce the learning curve of the nondisabled subjects in the blockedand random schedule.

At trial n, the motor output is generated by the sum of the fast processand the corresponding slow process, according to Eq. 1. After a task ispresented, if the motor error is not zero, both the fast state and thecorresponding slow state are updated according to Eq. 2. Because of thegating by the contextual input, the slow states for other (nonpresented)tasks are not updated after this trial but instead decay with forgetting rateAs. When no tasks are presented, such as after training, for instance, allfast and slow processes decay toward zero. Forgetting in the fast pro-cesses is rapid (in the order of tens of seconds), but forgetting in the slowprocesses is more long-lasting (in the order of tens of minutes). Althoughwe do not model long-term retention (in the order of days), it has beenshown that for adaptation to one task, the amount of long-term retentionis predicted as the level of activity in the slow process at the end oftraining (Joiner and Smith 2008). Thus, to compare computer simulationswith in our experimental data, performance for the first task at the end oftraining was taken as immediate retention performance; level of the slowprocess for the first task at the end of training was taken as long-termretention at 24 h.

In the present work, the competition between the fast and slowprocesses for errors is crucial to explain the differential effects of blockedand random schedules. Such competition stems from the model’s parallelarchitecture. In our previous work (Lee and Schweighofer 2009), weshowed that only the parallel 1-fast N-slow architecture, and not the serial1-fast N-slow architecture, could account for adaptation data in a randomschedule. Because of this parallel architecture of the model, the fast andslow processes compete for motor errors at each trial. Thus, in a blockedtask presentation, there is no interference in the fast process, which is onlyupdated by the errors of the task presented in the block. Because the fastprocess as a high learning rate (compare the values of the parameters Bf

and Bs and above), errors will be quickly reduced. In a random schedule,however, there are large interferences in the fast process; if the twoperturbations have equal magnitude but opposite signs, for instance, thefast process will have activity oscillating around zero. As a result, thechanges in performance during training will be driven mostly by the slowprocess. See Lee and Schweighofer (2009) for additional details of themodel.

In addition, we made the simplifying assumption that, in the model,the integrity of visuospatial working memory is linked to the rate ofdecay of the fast process, with larger rates of decay linked to poorervisuospatial working memory. We thus modeled the integrity of visu-ospatial working memory by modulating the time constant �f in the fastprocess, with �f � 1/(1 � Af)T, where Af is the fast process forgetting rategiven in Eq. 2 and T corresponds to a simulated trial length, which was12 s as in the experiment. The default “normal” forgetting rate Af � 0.8gives �f � 60 s. A small time constant, which indicates fast decay in thefast process (small value of forgetting rate Af), is used to model poorworking memory. The default “poor” working memory forgetting ratewas Af � 0.4, which gives �f � 20 s.

RESULTS

The CI Effect in the Computational Model

Our computational model reproduced the CI effect: slowerimprovements in performance during training (Fig. 3, A and C,black lines) and less long-term forgetting following training inrandom schedules than in blocked schedules (Fig. 3, B and D).

2635CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org

During blocked presentations, the fast process exhibited highactivity levels (Fig. 3A, dotted line) because there was littlelong-term forgetting during the short intervals between presen-tations of the same task. Because fast and slow processescompete for errors, the high activity levels of the fast processleads to relatively little update of the slow process (Fig. 3A,gray line). As a result, there was fast adaptation during trainingbut large long-term forgetting in delayed retention (Fig. 3B). Incontrast, during random presentations, the fast process exhib-ited low and jittered activity levels (Fig. 3C, dotted line); thisoccurred because interferences between tasks was high and thepassage of time between presentations of the same task wasrelatively long. This led to relatively large update of the slowprocess (Fig. 3C, gray line). As a result, there was relativelyslow adaptation during training but little long-term forgettingin delayed retention (Fig. 3D, compare with B). Specifically, atthe end of training and for task 1 for the blocked schedule,performance was 0.79 and the slow process was 0.57 (perfor-mance is equal to the slow process at long-term retention; seeMATERIALS AND METHODS); for the random schedule, perfor-mance was 0.73 and the slow process was 0.76.

Our model makes the prediction that the integrity of visu-ospatial working memory differentially affects delayed reten-tion in blocked and random schedules. In Fig. 4, a simulatedparticipant with poor visuospatial working memory (parameterAf � 0.4) showed little long-term forgetting following eitherblocked or random schedule training (B and D). Duringblocked schedule training (Fig. 4A), compared with the simu-lated participant with normal visuospatial working memory inFig. 3A, there was reduced update of the fast process becauseof large long-term forgetting from one trial to the next. How-ever, because of the competition between fast and slow pro-cess, there was an enhanced update of the slow process (Fig.4A, gray line). Long-term forgetting following blocked trainingwas then minimal (Fig. 4B). During random schedule training

(Fig. 4C), the update of the slow process was nearly identicalto that of the normal simulated participant (Fig. 3C). Specifi-cally, at the end of training and for task 1 for the blockedschedule, performance was 0.78 and the slow process was 0.71(performance is equal to the slow process at long-term reten-tion, see MATERIALS AND METHODS); for the random schedule,performance was 0.72 and the slow process was 0.75.

In the model, long-term forgetting following blocked sched-ule training positively correlates with the time constant ofvisuospatial working memory: for small time constants (i.e.,poor visuospatial working memory), there was little long-termforgetting in the delayed test (Fig. 5A, left), because muchshort-term forgetting happened between presentations duringtraining (as in Fig. 4A). For larger time constants (i.e., goodvisuospatial working memory), there was large long-term for-getting in the delayed test (Fig. 5A, right), because littleshort-term forgetting happened between presentations duringtraining (as in Fig. 3A). In contrast, long-term forgettingfollowing random schedule training did not correlate with thistime constant (Fig. 5B): because of interference between tasksduring training, there was little buildup of fast process.

The CI Effect in Nondisabled Individuals

We first examined whether the typical CI effect was repli-cated with our tasks in our young nondisabled sample. Therewas no difference in baseline characteristics between the twonondisabled groups for sex, age, maximal power grip force,and Wechsler figural score in nondisabled participants (seeTable 1).

Long-term forgetting was positive following blocked train-ing (0.183 � 0.063; P � 0.011, 1-sample t-test) but notfollowing random training (�0.0004 � 0.041; P � 0.99,1-sample t-test). Furthermore, long-term forgetting was greater

Fig. 3. Computer simulations: contextual interference (CI)effect in the nondisabled model. Performance (black line),fast process (dotted line), and slow process (gray line) areshown during blocked (A) and random schedule training(C). Immediate and long-term retention are shown follow-ing blocked (B) and random schedule training (D). Imm,immediate test; Del, 24-h delayed test. Notice the largeforgetting following blocked training and slower perfor-mance improvement during random than during blockedtraining. The jitter in the fast process memory (reflected inperformance) during random training in C was due to bothdecay and interferences between the tasks.

2636 CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org

following blocked schedule than following random training(P � 0.023, t-test) (see Fig. 6, B and D).

The performance of participants in both groups improvedduring training in both conditions (repeated-measuresANOVA, 10 blocks of 5 trials: P � 0.0001 for both blockedand random groups) (Fig. 6, A and C). There was nodifference in performance in the first block of five trialsbetween groups (P � 0.129, t-test), no difference in the lastblock of five trials (P � 0.233, t-test), and no difference inthe overall change in performance between the first and thelast block of five trials (P � 0.763, t-test). However,performance continued to improve in the random group afterthe first five trials, but not in the blocked group. To showthis, we regressed the performance from trials 6 to 50 as afunction of trial number for each participant: the slopes werepositive in the random group (mean 0.0051 � 0.0006trial�1) but near zero in the blocked group (mean 0. 0015 �0.0011 trial�1) and larger in the random group comparedwith the blocked group (P � 0.008, t-test).

The CI Effect in Individuals Poststroke

There was no difference in baseline characteristics betweenthe two groups for sex, age, side of paresis, concordance of

stroke (i.e., whether the stroke affected the dominant hand),time postonset, maximal force, any of the UE Fugl-Meyersubscale scores [range of motion (ROM), pain, sensory, armmotor, hand, wrist], FTHUE, MMSE, and Wechsler figuralscore (all values are given in Table 2). The only marginallynon-zero difference between groups was maximal power gripforce (blocked: 462 � 67 N; random: 326 � 29 N; P � 0.082,t-test).

We first examined the CI effect for all individuals poststroke(i.e., without dividing the participants into subgroups based ontheir figural memory test scores). Long-term forgetting wasmarginally positive following blocked training but not follow-ing random training (blocked: 0.076 � 0.040, P � 0.085;random: 0.0085 � 0.063, P � 0.89, 1-sample t-tests) (see Fig. 7,B and D). Furthermore, long-term forgetting was not differentfollowing blocked and random training (P � 0.342, t-test). Weverified that there was no correlation between maximal powergrip force and long-term forgetting in either group (blocked:P � 0.97; random: P � 0.75, Pearson).

The performance of participants in both groups improvedduring training in both conditions (repeated-measures ANOVA,10 blocks of 5 trials: P � 0.0001 for both groups, Greenhouse-Geisser corrections) (Fig. 7, A and C). There was no difference in

Fig. 4. Computer simulations: reduced CI effect in amodel with poor visuospatial working memory. Perfor-mance (black line), fast process (dotted line), and slowprocess (gray line) are shown during blocked (A) andrandom schedule training (C). Immediate and long-termretention are shown following blocked (B) and randomschedule training (D). In blocked schedules, there waslittle forgetting in the delayed retention test (B; comparewith Fig. 3B) because of relatively higher buildup ofslow process and lower buildup of fast process duringtraining (A; compare with Fig. 3A). In random sched-ules, there was little difference during training (C) andtesting (D) compared with the normal model (comparewith Fig. 3, C and D).

Fig. 5. Computer simulations: forgetting measured atthe delayed retention test as a function of the timeconstant of decay of fast process after either blocked (A)or random schedule (B) for 2 tasks. Overall, and notablyfor larger time constants, there was less forgettingfollowing random schedule than blocked schedule.However, for smaller time constants, forgetting wascomparable following random and blocked schedule.

2637CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org

performance in the first trial between groups (P � 0.12, t-test).Furthermore, there was no difference in performance in the firstblock of five trials between groups (P � 0.13, t-test), no differencein the last block of five trials (P � 0.67, t-test), and no differencein the overall change in performance between the first and the lastblock of five trials (P � 0.19, t-test). There was no difference inslopes between group in the regression model of performance vs.trials 6–50 (P � 0.63, t-test; see above for details of analysis).

Our simulations predict that visuospatial working memoryintegrity influences the degree of long-term forgetting afterblocked schedules but not after random schedules. Figure 8Aillustrates the strong dependency of the figural Wechsler scoreon long-term forgetting in the blocked schedule (compare Figs.5A and 8A). A repeated ANOVA model with test as repeatedfactor and figural Wechsler as covariate showed significantlong-term forgetting in the blocked schedule (P � 0.025) and

Fig. 6. Data: CI effect in nondisabled participants.Performance (mean and SE) is shown during blocked(A) and random training (C). Immediate and long-termretention are shown following blocked (B) and randomtraining (D).

Fig. 7. Data: CI effect in participants poststroke. Per-formance (mean and SE) is shown during blocked (A)and random training (C). Immediate and long-termretention are shown following blocked (B) and randomtraining (D).

2638 CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org

a significant effect of the figural Wechsler on long-term for-getting (P � 0.0067). A similar analysis show no long-termforgetting in the random schedule (P � 0.55) and no effect ofthe figural Wechsler score on long-term forgetting (P � 0.56),again as predicted by our computer simulations (compare Figs.5B and 8B).

The influence of visuospatial working memory on long-term forgetting is well illustrated by classifying strokeindividuals between high and low visuospatial workingmemory based on a cutoff of 7 on the figural Wechslermemory score. The difference between long-term forgettingin blocked and random schedules was significant in the highspatial working memory subgroups (n � 5 high spatialworking memory in blocked, n � 8 high spatial workingmemory in random; P � 0.042, Mann-Whitney test). How-ever, there was no difference in long-term forgetting in thelow spatial working memory subgroups (P � 0.40, Mann-Whitney test).

Finally, we verified that in the individuals poststroke en-rolled in the study, there was no correlation between the figuralWechsler memory score and any motor and sensory variables(maximum power force, P � 0.482; UE Fugl-Meyer ROM,P � 0.480; UE Fugl-Meyer pain, P � 0.825; UE Fugl-Meyersensory, P � 0.316; UE Fugl-Meyer motor, P � 0.839; UEFugl-Meyer wrist, P � 0.330; UE Fugl-Meyer hand, P �0.615; FTHUE, P � 0.839). There was no correlation of thefigural memory score with the digital Wechsler memory score(P � 0.87). The only significant correlation between the figuralscore was with the MMSE (r � 0.70, P � 0.0005). Further-more, there was no correlation between long-term forgettingand any motor and sensory variables (maximum power force,P � 0.66; UE Fugl-Meyer ROM, P � 0.34; UE Fugl-Meyerpain, P � 0.98; UE Fugl-Meyer sensory, P � 0.79; UEFugl-Meyer motor, P � 0.2; UE Fugl-Meyer wrist, P � 0.31;UE Fugl-Meyer hand, P � 0.44; FTHUE, P � 0.56). Since theMMSE contains working memory components, this validatesthe use of the Wechsler figural memory score as a visuospatialworking memory test in our study. These results indicate thatit is the integrity of figural visuospatial working memory, notany motor or sensory impairment after stroke, that leads to theobserved changes in long-term forgetting following blockedtraining.

DISCUSSION

In this study, we have replicated the CI effect in nondisabledparticipants and present novel evidence that the CI effect can hold

in participants with stroke affecting the motor system but ismodulated by the integrity of visuospatial working memory. Ourresults indicate that individuals with stroke with normal visuospa-tial working memory, like nondisabled individuals, exhibit lesslong-term forgetting of visuomotor skills acquired in randomtraining schedules than in blocked training schedules. In contrast,individuals with stroke with low visuospatial working memoryexhibit little long-term forgetting after either random or blockedschedules. Our results may explain why previous studies reportedconflicting results on the CI effect in individuals with stroke(Cauraugh and Kim 2003; Hanlon 1996), since participants werenot separated by low and high working memory capabilities.

The rather counterintuitive effect of the integrity of visu-ospatial working memory on long-term retention followingblocked schedules (the better the working memory, the worsethe long-term retention!) was predicted on a theoretical basiswith our previous computational model of motor memory (Leeand Schweighofer 2009). Furthermore, we found no othermotor or sensory variables that correlated with long-termforgetting after training in the blocked schedule or with theintegrity of visuospatial working memory. Thus our combinedcomputational and experimental results suggest that the integ-rity of visuospatial working memory is a crucial factor under-lying the modulation of the CI effect in participants poststrokein our experiment.

Although the CI effect has led to a considerable amount ofresearch over almost a century (Magill and Hall 1990), its under-lying mechanism has remained unclear. Three nonexclusive hy-potheses have been proposed to explain the CI effect in motorlearning. First, according to the “elaboration-distinctiveness” hy-pothesis, random training schedules allow intertask comparisonsduring the planning stage that lead to distinctive memory repre-sentations (e.g., Cross et al. 2009; Immink and Wright 2001; Linet al. 2008; Shea and Morgan 1979). Second, according to the“deficient processing” hypothesis, blocked repetitions lead toreduced rehearsal or attention in later presentations (e.g., Callanand Schweighofer 2010; Hintzman et al. 1973). Third, accordingto the “forgetting-reconstruction” hypothesis of the CI effect,forgetting between successive presentations of the same taskduring random training results in stronger memory representations(Lee and Magill 1983; Lee et al. 1985).

In this report, we propose a novel mechanistic account of the CIeffect in motor learning. Like in the forgetting-reconstructionhypothesis, our account of the CI effect relies on forgettingbetween presentations of the same task during training. Thespecific mechanisms underlying the enhancement of long-term

Fig. 8. Data: forgetting in individuals poststroke in a24-h posttraining period is shown as a function ofWechsler visual memory score (figural) following train-ing in either blocked (A) or random schedule (B) in theindividuals poststroke who participated in the study.Black lines are regression lines; the gray line in B is arobust regression line (with default weighting functionin Matlab robustfit function); the robust regression lineis superimposed on the regression line in A. Comparewith the computer simulations of Fig. 5.

2639CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org

memory differ in the forgetting-reconstruction hypothesis and inour model, however. According to the forgetting-reconstructionhypothesis, forgetting in working memory between spaced pre-sentations necessitates retrieval from long-term memory, whichincreases long-term retention. In our model, forgetting in visu-ospatial working memory between spaced presentations duringtraining leads to slower improvements in performance in randomschedule than in blocked schedules. The resulting greater errorsduring training benefit the update of the slow process, leading tobetter long-term retention. Note that our account of the CI effectdoes not exclude additional explanations such as the elaboration-distinctiveness and the deficient processing theories; further stud-ies are needed to dissociate the possible contributions of suchmechanisms. Moreover, further studies are needed to determinethe putative neural substrates for the slow and fast processes. Forexample, we recently showed that the neural substrates of mem-ory consolidation depend on practice structure (Kantak et al.2010).

Our results have implications for rehabilitation of patients withstroke. Although there is good evidence that task training iseffective for improving upper extremity function after stroke(Butefisch 1995; Kwakkel et al. 1999; Wolf et al. 2002, 2006),none of these studies addresses the “microscheduling” of individ-ual tasks. Thus, although the use of random task schedule duringtraining has been advocated (Krakauer 2006), physical and occu-pational therapists rely on guidelines that simply suggest inclusionof extensive and variable training without weighing heavily onindividual capability (e.g., Bach-y-Rita and Balliet 1987; Lee et al.1991). Our results suggest that patients with stroke with normalvisuospatial working memory should receive training schedulesthat mix tasks randomly. Because such training is hard to imple-ment in normal therapeutic situations and places high demand onthe therapists, robots that can present functional tasks and that caneasily switch between these tasks during training could be devel-oped; see our preliminary work in this direction (Choi et al. 2009).On the contrary, and in a rather counterintuitive manner, ourresults suggest that individuals with stroke with poor visuospatialworking memory can receive blocked training schedules with nocompromise to long-term retention.

Our experiment has a number of limitations that need to beaddressed in future work. First, our study included only a rela-tively small number of participants in each group and needsreplication with a larger sample size and different populations,such as the elderly. In the current study, we chose to study younghealthy subjects as a control population and not (older) age-matched controls because the elderly show decreased workingmemory capability, which might have led to a reduced CI effectin this population (see Anguera et al. 2011). Two CI studies havefound a CI effect in motor tasks in older adults (Lin et al. 2007,2010). Lin et al. (2010) directly compared young and elderlysubjects on a serial reaction time task and found the CI effect inboth groups. Lin et al. (2007) found a CI effect in a subjectpopulation of ages similar to those of our participants poststrokewith motor tasks similar to ours in that subjects were to producedifferent force patterns as a result of specific visuomotor patterns.Second, although the young participants in our study were in-structed to use their dominant hand, the participants poststrokewere instructed to use their most affected hand, which was or wasnot their affected hand before stroke. There is therefore the slightpossibility that our results are due to not-controlled handedness,although concordance was balanced across groups (see Table 2).

Third, a prediction of the model is that poor visuospatial workingmemory will reduce the rate of performance improvement duringacquisition in blocked practice. We found no correlation betweenthe figural memory and the rate of learning in the stroke group,however. This can be due to the large variability between andwithin participants at each trial during training in the stroke groupcompared with the nondisabled group (compare the shaded areasof Figs. 6A and 7A). Increasing the sample size in motor tasks thatlead to less trial-by-trial variability may be desirable. Fourth,because visuospatial working memory is thought to have a limitedcapacity (e.g., Cowan 2001), it is possible that in addition to, or inlieu of, shortened time span, limited visuospatial working memoryin our participants is due to a deficit in the number of items thatcan be stored in memory (our current model of motor memorycannot account for such limited storage mechanism). Fifth, wewere only able to obtain the MRI scans for a small subset of ourparticipants. In future work, it would be most interesting to relatestroke characteristics, such as locations and volumes, to theamount of CI effect that can be induced.

Similarly, our model, because of its simplicity, has a number ofimportant limitations. First, like others (e.g., Kording et al. 2007;Smith et al. 2006), we only attempted to model the commonneuronal mechanism of error-driven motor adaptation or learning,not the mechanism for specific types of motor adaptation orlearning. As such, the model accounts for dual-adaptation data ineye movement, visuomotor rotation, and force field adaptation(see Lee and Schweighofer 2009). However, our model does notaccount for the effect of physiological factors, such as musclemechanics, etc., of our specific experimental tasks. Second, al-though our computation model is a model of motor adaptation(where “adaptation” is the change in motor performance thatallows the motor system to regain its former capabilities in alteredcircumstances), we used it in this study to account for visuomotorlearning of novel tasks. Such extension from adaptation to motorlearning requires us to make two assumptions about motor learn-ing in our experiment. The first assumption is that learning in ourexperiment is error driven. Since we provided an error measure inthe form of the actual force trajectory superimposed on the desiredtrajectory (Fig. 1), this appears reasonable. In addition, there isalso a large body of evidence that supports the fact that skilllearning is at least in part driven via errors (reviewed for instancein Hikosaka et al. 2002). The second assumption is that skilllearning, like motor adaptation, results from a combination of fastand slow memory processes; there is a large body of evidence thatsupports this view (see for review Anguera et al. 2009, 2010,2011). The final and perhaps most crucial limitation of our modelin the current context is that our model assumes that there is nointerference/generalization between tasks in the slow processes.Thus our model cannot reproduce any transfer of learning effectacross tasks. This is clearly an oversimplification in light of thesimilarities between the three tasks in our experiment (see Fig. 2),and this prevented us from using the participant data to estimatethe model parameters (which had to be hand-tuned instead).However, our simulation results show that our model well ac-counts for the CI effect when performance across tasks areaveraged. Further work is needed to determine whether the CIeffect occurs when several unique tasks, such as a force task, adigit manipulation task, and a bimanual task, etc., are given.

In summary, our combined theoretical and experimental resultssuggest a relationship between the integrity of visuospatial work-ing memory and motor learning. Such a relationship between

2640 CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org

working memory and motor learning has been previously reportedin a number of recent studies (e.g., Anguera et al. 2010; Boyd andWinstein 2004; Keisler and Shadmehr 2010); our results add toand extend this body of work by showing that working memory isinvolved in the CI effect. Causal evidence for a role of visuospa-tial working memory in the CI effect in motor learning may beobtained via virtual lesion of the areas involved in visuospatialworking memory via repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation(see related, Tanaka et al. 2010).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank Matt Sandusky for helping to build the apparatus, ShaileshKantak for helping to pilot the experiment, Jihye Lee and Wesley McGeahy forhelping to conduct the experiment, and Drs. Tsuyoshi Ikegami, James Gordon,and Steven Cen for helpful comments.

GRANTS

This work was funded by National Institute of Child Health and HumanDevelopment Grant R03 HD050591-02 and National Science FoundationGrants IIS 0535282 and PAC 1031899 to N. Schweighofer.

DISCLOSURES

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the author(s).

REFERENCES

Anguera JA, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Willingham DT, Seidler RD. Contribu-tions of spatial working memory to visuomotor learning. J Cogn Neurosci22: 1917–1930, 2010.

Anguera JA, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Willingham DT, Seidler RD. Failure toengage spatial working memory contributes to age-related declines invisuomotor learning. J Cogn Neurosci 23: 11–25, 2011.

Anguera JA, Seidler RD, Gehring WJ. Changes in performance monitoringduring sensorimotor adaptation. J Neurophysiol 102: 1868–1879, 2009.

Bach-y-Rita P, Balliet R. (editors). Recovery from stroke. In: Stroke Reha-bilitation: the Recovery of Motor Control. Chicago, IL: Year Book Medical,1987, p. 79–107.

Boyd LA, Winstein CJ. Providing explicit information disrupts implicit motorlearning after basal ganglia stroke. Learn Mem 11: 388–396, 2004.

Butefisch C, Hummelsheim H, Denzler P, Mauritz KH. Repetitive trainingof isolated movements improves the outcome of motor rehabilitation of thecentrally paretic hand. J Neurol Sci 130: 59–68, 1995.

Callan DE, Schweighofer N. Neural correlates of the spacing effect in explicitverbal semantic encoding support the deficient-processing theory. HumBrain Mapp 31: 645–659, 2010.

Cauraugh JH, Kim SB. Stroke motor recovery: active neuromuscular stim-ulation and repetitive practice schedules. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 74:1562–1566, 2003.

Choi Y, Gordon J, Kim D, Schweighofer N. An adaptive automated robotictask-practice system for rehabilitation of arm functions after stroke. IEEETrans Robot 25: 556–568, 2009.

Cowan N. The magical number 4 in short-term memory: a reconsideration ofmental storage capacity. Behav Brain Sci 24: 87–114, 2001.

Cross ES, Kraemer DJ, Hamilton AF, Kelley WM, Grafton ST. Sensitivityof the action observation network to physical and observational learning.Cereb Cortex 19: 315–326, 2009.

Fugl-Meyer AR, Jaasko L, Leyman I, Olsson S, Steglind S. The post-strokehemiplegic patient. 1. A method for evaluation of physical performance.Scand J Rehabil Med 7: 13–31, 1975.

Hanlon RE. Motor learning following unilateral stroke. Arch Phys MedRehabil 77: 811–815, 1996.

Hawkins KA, Sullivan TE, Choi EJ. Memory deficits in schizophrenia:inadequate assimilation or true amnesia? Findings from the WechslerMemory Scale—revised. J Psychiatry Neurosci 22: 169–179, 1997.

Hikosaka O, Nakamura K, Sakai K, Nakahara H. Central mechanisms ofmotor skill learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 12: 217–222, 2002.

Hintzman DL, Block RA, Summers JJ. Contextual associations and memoryfor serial position. J Exp Psychol 97: 220–229, 1973.

Immink MA, Wright DL. Motor programming during practice conditionshigh and low in contextual interference. J Exp Psychol Hum PerceptPerform 27: 423–437, 2001.

Joiner WM, Smith MA. Long-term retention explained by a model ofshort-term learning in the adaptive control of reaching. J Neurophysiol 100:2948–2955, 2008.

Kantak SS, Sullivan KJ, Fisher BE, Knowlton BJ, Winstein CJ. Neuralsubstrates of motor memory consolidation depend on practice structure. NatNeurosci 13: 923–925, 2010.

Keisler A, Shadmehr R. A shared resource between declarative memory andmotor memory. J Neurosci 30: 14817–14823, 2010.

Kording KP, Tenenbaum JB, Shadmehr R. The dynamics of memory as aconsequence of optimal adaptation to a changing body. Nat Neurosci 10:779–786, 2007.

Krakauer JW. Motor learning: its relevance to stroke recovery and neurore-habilitation. Curr Opin Neurol 19: 84–90, 2006.

Kwakkel G, Wagenaar RC, Twisk JW, Lankhorst GJ, Koetsier JC.Intensity of leg and arm training after primary middle-cerebral-artery stroke:a randomised trial. Lancet 354: 191–196, 1999.

Lee JY, Schweighofer N. Dual adaptation supports a parallel architecture ofmotor memory. J Neurosci 29: 10396–10404, 2009.

Lee TD, Magill RA. The locus of contextual interference in motor-skillacquisition. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 9: 730–746, 1983.

Lee TD, Magill RA, Weeks DJ. Influence of practice schedule on testingschema theory predictions in adults. J Mot Behav 17: 283–299, 1985.

Lee TD, Swanson LR, Hall AL. What is repeated in a repetition? Effects ofpractice conditions on motor skill acquisition. Phys Ther 71: 150–156, 1991.

Lin CH, Fisher BE, Winstein CJ, Wu AD, Gordon J. Contextual interfer-ence effect: elaborative processing or forgetting-reconstruction? A post hocanalysis of transcranial magnetic stimulation-induced effects on motorlearning. J Mot Behav 40: 578–586, 2008.

Lin CH, Sullivan KJ, Wu AD, Kantak S, Winstein CJ. Effect of taskpractice order on motor skill learning in adults with Parkinson disease: apilot study. Phys Ther 87: 1120–1131, 2007.

Lin CH, Wu AD, Udompholkul P, Knowlton BJ. Contextual interferenceeffects in sequence learning for young and older adults. Psychol Aging 25:929–939, 2010.

Magill RA, Hall KG. A review of the contextual interference effect in motorskill acquisition. Hum Mov Sci 9: 241–289, 1990.

Nixon SJ, Kujawski A, Parsons OA, Yohman JR. Semantic (verbal) andfigural memory impairment in alcoholics. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 9:311–322, 1987.

Pyle WH. Transfer and interference in card-distributing. J Educ Psychol 10:107–110, 1919.

Schmidt RA, Lee TD. Motor Control and Learning: a Behavioral Emphasis.Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1999.

Shea JB, Morgan R. Contextual interference effects on the acquisition,retention, and transfer of a motor skill. J Exp Psychol Hum Learn Mem 5:179–187, 1979.

Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R. Interacting adaptive processes withdifferent timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol 4: e179,2006.

Tanaka S, Honda M, Hanakawa T, Cohen LG. Differential contribution of thesupplementary motor area to stabilization of a procedural motor skill acquiredthrough different practice schedules. Cereb Cortex 20: 2114–2121, 2010.

Tsuitsui S, Lee TD, Hodges NJ. Contextual interferences in learning newpatterns of bimanual coordination. J Mot Behav 30: 151–157, 1998.

Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale—Revised. New York: Psychological, 1987.Wilson D, Baker LL, Craddock JA. Functional test for the hemiparetic upper

extremity. Am J Occup Ther 38: 159–164, 1984.Winstein CJ, Merians AS, Sullivan KJ. Motor learning after unilateral brain

damage. Neuropsychologia 37: 975–987, 1999.Wolf SL, Blanton S, Baer H, Breshears J, Butler AJ. Repetitive task

practice: a critical review of constraint-induced movement therapy in stroke.Neurologist 8: 325–338, 2002.

Wolf SL, Winstein CJ, Miller JP, Taub E, Uswatte G, Morris D, GiulianiC, Light KE, Nichols-Larsen D. Effect of constraint-induced movementtherapy on upper extremity function 3 to 9 months after stroke: the EXCITErandomized clinical trial. JAMA 296: 2095–2104, 2006.

2641CONTEXTUAL INTERFERENCE EFFECT IN STROKE

J Neurophysiol • VOL 106 • NOVEMBER 2011 • www.jn.org