children on the margins: the effect of social background on mathematics learning in early childhood

11
Children on the margins: The effect of social background on Mathematics learning in early childhood Bronwyn Ewing School of Early Childhood, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Road, Kelvin Grove, Qld. 4059 [email protected] Contributed presentations and papers Research from the Queensland Preparing for School Trials (2003/2004) indicates that children from socially disadvantaged backgrounds progressed less in numeracy measures compared to children from socially advantaged backgrounds, particularly in Year 1 (p. 177). This finding highlights two key questions for early childhood educators. Why are children from disadvantaged groups not making adequate progress in their Mathematics learning? And, how can these children be supported in their Mathematical learning and understanding to ensure their success and progress in the later years of schooling? This presentation puts forward several propositions for why particular groups of children may not be engaging in Mathematics. In particular, it will discuss the literature on the effects of poverty on children’s mathematical learning and understanding, the teaching practices utilised in early childhood contexts for developing this understanding, and the language of Mathematics. This discussion will provide a foundation for further discussion of how to enhance Mathematics learning for all groups of children in early childhood contexts. The teaching and learning of Mathematics has been strongly influenced by elements of Behaviourist and other cognate theories (See for example, Cobb & Yackel, 1998; Merrett & Wheldall, 1987) and therefore, as Lerman (2000) has shown, maintains particular viewpoints of knowledge, teaching and learning. Borasi (1996) argues that within a traditional perspective on teaching and learning, Mathematics is treated as a body of established facts and techniques broken down and transmitted by the expert (teacher) to the novice (learner), who is expected to accumulate isolated pieces of information by listening, observing, memorising and practising. Enacted in this model are particular assumptions about knowledge, teaching, and learning. Knowledge is considered to be a body of established mathematical rules and formulas. These rules and formulas are transmitted by the teacher using set rules and procedures. Learning entails memorising and recalling these same processes. This approach has been substantially critiqued by a number of researchers in the field because of the inappropriate and less than effective ways in which students are expected to learn Mathematics. For example, in their studies of epistemologies of Mathematics classrooms, Sierpinska (1998) argues that learning Mathematics should not be reduced to the memorising of technical facts that are abstract and removed from children’s everyday experiences. These authors argue that adopting a more social approach to the teaching and learning of Mathematics generates other ways of understanding knowledge, teaching and learning in Mathematics. This paper reviews the literature with a focus on particular practices that act as barriers to learners and learning in the early years, instead of offering opportunities for engagement and participation in the subject.

Upload: qut

Post on 11-Dec-2023

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Children on the margins: The effect of social background on Mathematics learning in

early childhood

Bronwyn Ewing

School of Early Childhood, Queensland University of Technology, Victoria Park Road,

Kelvin Grove, Qld. 4059

[email protected]

Contributed presentations and papers

Research from the Queensland Preparing for School Trials (2003/2004) indicates

that children from socially disadvantaged backgrounds progressed less in

numeracy measures compared to children from socially advantaged backgrounds,

particularly in Year 1 (p. 177). This finding highlights two key questions for

early childhood educators. Why are children from disadvantaged groups not

making adequate progress in their Mathematics learning? And, how can these

children be supported in their Mathematical learning and understanding to ensure

their success and progress in the later years of schooling? This presentation puts

forward several propositions for why particular groups of children may not be

engaging in Mathematics. In particular, it will discuss the literature on the effects

of poverty on children’s mathematical learning and understanding, the teaching

practices utilised in early childhood contexts for developing this understanding,

and the language of Mathematics. This discussion will provide a foundation for

further discussion of how to enhance Mathematics learning for all groups of

children in early childhood contexts.

The teaching and learning of Mathematics has been strongly influenced by elements of

Behaviourist and other cognate theories (See for example, Cobb & Yackel, 1998; Merrett &

Wheldall, 1987) and therefore, as Lerman (2000) has shown, maintains particular viewpoints

of knowledge, teaching and learning. Borasi (1996) argues that within a traditional

perspective on teaching and learning, Mathematics is treated as a body of established facts

and techniques broken down and transmitted by the expert (teacher) to the novice (learner),

who is expected to accumulate isolated pieces of information by listening, observing,

memorising and practising. Enacted in this model are particular assumptions about

knowledge, teaching, and learning. Knowledge is considered to be a body of established

mathematical rules and formulas. These rules and formulas are transmitted by the teacher

using set rules and procedures. Learning entails memorising and recalling these same

processes.

This approach has been substantially critiqued by a number of researchers in the field

because of the inappropriate and less than effective ways in which students are expected to

learn Mathematics. For example, in their studies of epistemologies of Mathematics

classrooms, Sierpinska (1998) argues that learning Mathematics should not be reduced to the

memorising of technical facts that are abstract and removed from children’s everyday

experiences. These authors argue that adopting a more social approach to the teaching and

learning of Mathematics generates other ways of understanding knowledge, teaching and

learning in Mathematics. This paper reviews the literature with a focus on particular practices

that act as barriers to learners and learning in the early years, instead of offering opportunities

for engagement and participation in the subject.

Survival of the Quickest1 or Cracking the Code

2 inside a traditional Mathematics

context

A number of practices are utilised in traditional mathematics classrooms, for example,

memorisation of procedures and methods, and working repetitively through drill and practice

exercises and completing worksheets and textbooks. These practices are exemplified in two

pejorative comments – Zevenbergen’s (2000) metaphor of “cracking the code” of the

language of mathematics classrooms and Boaler’s (2002) provocative summation of ability

grouping in Mathematics classrooms as “survival of the quickest”. The former can be applied

to the subject positions3 set up in a Discourse and how in these positions, teachers and

students are allowed to say or not say within that discourse type, while the latter suggests that

those students who managed to get through their work were more likely to occupy a subject

position higher in status than their counterparts who did not get through their work.

The practices associated with the Discourse of traditional Mathematics, for example, didactic

communication, textbooks, worksheets, and teaching content in discrete steps, will have

substantial implications for the ways in which learners shape, or fail to shape, an identity of

participation in Mathematics classrooms. In consequence, the Mathematics classroom

becomes a potential site for contestation of, struggle with, resistance to, or withdrawal from

its practices, assumptions and expectations. In the following discussion, the practices found in

a traditional Mathematics classroom are presented and discussed.

Teacher and Student Interactions and Relationships

Research indicates that the foundations of student failure have been found to be rooted in their

earliest school experiences with teachers. Alexander, Entwisle and Horsey (1997), for

example, report that poor and unsatisfactory relationships and interactions between teachers

and students, and a negative school environment, both contribute substantially to such failure.

In a study that was instrumental to school reform in Australia, Hill and Rowe (1998) found

that, over any other variable, including school type, teachers make the most impact on student

learning. Their study highlights and emphasises the importance of the role of teachers as

central to enhancing learning.

Although not specifically related to Mathematics education, other significant studies

also stress the effects of teachers on students. For example, Anyon’s (1997) study of how

schools exclude young people reported that teachers frequently held very stereotypical views

of students. Consequently, teachers developed practices that worked against the success of

certain students, particularly students from disadvantaged groups, such as indigenous students

and those living in poverty.

In further studies of teachers and students, the seminal work of Rosenthal and

Jacobsen (1968) emphasised that where teachers held preconceived views of students, they

1 Survival of the quickest is drawn from the work of Boaler (2002).

2 Cracking the code is drawn from the work of Zevenbergen (2000).

3 Within a preferred Discourse such as traditional Mathematics, subject positions, as Fairclough (2001, p. 31) prefers to call them, are set up.

In a sense, through occupying these positions, teachers and students are what they do – they become teachers and students (Fairclough,

2001). Thus, occupying a subject position becomes a matter of doing (or not doing) certain things, in line with the discoursal rights and

obligations of teachers and pupils – what each is allowed and required to say, and not allowed or required to say, within that particular

discourse type. (Fairclough, 2001, p. 31)Here the constraints of such a Discourse include the content of what is said and done, the social

relations that the teacher and students enter into in that Discourse, and their contrasting subject positions. These constraints “derive from the

conventions of the discourse type which is being drawn upon” (Fairclough, 1992, p. 47), in the case of this study, the preferred Discourse of

traditional Mathematics.

were more likely to interact in ways to produce behaviours that reinforced their viewpoint of

students. To illustrate, they claimed that the more that “lower-track” (p. 179) students

improved academically, the more they were viewed negatively by their teacher. This was

because the students’ “slow track status made it unlikely in their teachers’ eyes that they

would behave in an intellectually competent manner” (p. 179) in the future. Arguably, this

finding heightens the concern regarding the powerful effects that teacher and student

interactions and relationships enacted in Mathematics classrooms have on participation and

non-participation in Mathematics learning communities.

The practices employed in mathematics learning contexts will strongly influence the

relations between a learning community of teachers and students. They will also largely

determine if a student is included, excluded, or marginalised from this community (Ewing,

2005). These practices are more likely to discriminate against the needs and interests of

certain groups of students making them appear “naturally smart or dumb depending on their

familiar experiences” (Bredo, 2000, p. 133). Those over-represented in such groups are more

likely to be males, indigenous students, low achievers, and students from disadvantaged

backgrounds. These same students are likely to find adapting to a teacher’s teaching style

difficult.

Teaching Style

Recent International, (National Council for Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM), 2000;

Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD), 2004), National,

(Department of Education, Training and Youth Affairs (DETYA), 2000, 2001) and;

Ministerial Council for Education, Employment, Training and Youth Affairs (MCEETYA)

(1998) studies have emphasised the need to improve Mathematics teaching with a focus on

learning and participation. The Mathematics results from the Program for International

Student Assessment (PISA) 2003 (OECD, 2004) indicate that students’ socio-economic

background combined with the practices utilised in learning contexts have a “substantial” (p.

26) effect on student performance in Mathematics. Similarly, the National Goals for

Schooling (MCEETYA, 1998) highlight that need for teachers to develop in learners “the

capacity for, and skills in analysis and problem solving and the ability to communicate ideas

and information, to plan and organise activities and to collaborate with others” (p. 3).

Arguably, this emphasis can be construed as a call for a learner-centred curriculum. Implicit

in such a call is the need for teachers to alter or change the practices of the preferred

Discourse of Mathematics.

Askew, Brown, Rhodes, Johnson and Wiliam (1997) assert that a didactic style of

teaching views the teaching and learning process as the transmission of information from

teacher to student, with little scope for interactive two-way communication, negotiation and

discussion of mathematical ideas. Consequently, the preferred Discourse of Mathematics sets

up contrasting subject positions for teachers and students. In this framework, teachers are

positioned as having the legitimate authority to determine the practices, the content and

curriculum constitutive of Mathematics classrooms. Learners are positioned as the passive

recipients of knowledge. They are expected to occupy the subject position constructed by the

discourse type of the classroom. As Askew (2001) argues, learners are expected to absorb

facts and solve routine tasks through drill and practice exercises from textbooks which lack

connections and relevance to their lives. In this context, meaningful social interaction, where

learners engage in the enterprise of a Mathematics community, is almost non-existent. Rather,

what does exist is a strong emphasis on getting through the content and on constraining

student input and participation through the practices inherent in the preferred Discourse of

Mathematics.

Mathematics Learning Contexts

In studies of Mathematics classrooms, Jones (1997) and Cobb and Yackel (1998) argue that

Mathematics learning contexts or conditions for learning have become an important

consideration for developing a learner’s mathematical understanding. These authors state that

a learning context includes the pedagogical approach employed by the teacher, their

instructional intentions, their conception of Mathematics, and the classroom environment. In

this context, working quietly through pen and paper tests is considered to show what students

really know, with students classified and labelled by their mathematical ability. The research

literature within and outside the field of Mathematics education has powerfully indicated that

these classifications influence and impact negatively on student learning, participation,

achievement and social identity.4 Thus, Boaler and Wiliam (2001a) argue that particular

groups of students, such as those who are disadvantaged, are less likely to participate and

consequently be excluded or marginalised because they are seen as failures who did not make

adequate progress. The traditional or preferred Discourse of Mathematics has the potential to

conflict (in values, ways of acting, thinking, and talking about Mathematics) with particular

students’ home or community-based language. More importantly, this approach may be

hostile to, or define itself in opposition to, particular groups of students, such as those who are

socially and economically disadvangated.

In their studies of Mathematical contexts, Wood and Turner-Vorbeck, (1999) and

Jones (1997) reported that teachers’ conceptions of Mathematics have been shown to be

highly influential in learning contexts. Jones suggests those who believe that Mathematics is

about pre-existing truths that are straightforward with readily identifiable correct answers,

will tend to focus on computations and algorithmic procedures. In this framework, he argues,

these truths have been created, discovered, or outlined by someone else foreign to the

Mathematics classroom. The students’ task is to receive, accept, absorb and reproduce, in that

order, what the teacher delivers (or what the teacher wants) (Wood & Turner-Vorbeck, 1999,

p. 184). When students are treated as receivers and reproducers of this form of knowledge,

they are not likely to make meaningful connections and understandings in Mathematics.

Further, as Schoenfeld (1994) and Boaler (2002) argue, they are not likely to identify and

participate in the shared enterprise of that learning community because the Mathematics

activities are seen by many students as irrelevant and disconnected from their lives outside the

classroom.

To account for the ways context is important for learning Mathematics, Jones (1997)

suggests that attention must be paid to the different interpretations that “teachers, students,

and communities may have for what is mathematical” (p. 145). These contrasting and often

competing views are consequent on the various and often contesting Discourses of such

groups. Tension emerges when, according to Wood and Turner-Vorbeck (1999), teachers are

more inclined to employ teaching practices that are taken-for-granted and seen as “natural” (p.

184) but which are inappropriate for those they teach rather than the “counterintuitive forms

of teaching for promoting necessary changes in student learning” (p. 184). These tensions and

the competing Discourses from which they rise have implications for teacher and student

interactions and relations.

4 See for example Hill & Rowe (1998).

Learning Mathematics from a Textbook

In his studies of textbook use in classrooms, Shield (2000) found that learning Mathematics is

still largely based on the assumption that it is learned from a textbook. Askew (2001) and

Nickson (2002) argue that learning this way suggests it is a highly individual process, and

separated from other curriculum areas and activities. In this context, Nickson (2002) reported

that students are restricted to solving routine problems that are broken into discrete steps and

isolated from their real world experiences. Learners are tested on their knowledge and

understanding which has largely been acquired, if at all, through repetitive drill and practice

exercises from Mathematics textbooks and worksheets (Nickson, 2002).

Work by researchers indicates that teachers who rely on teaching Mathematics from a

textbook also learned Mathematics this way (Lubinski & Jaberg, 1997; Romberg & Kaput,

1997). This claim is not surprising, given that the theoretical framework that has largely

informed the teaching and learning of Mathematics is largely framed around the transmission

of knowledge through step-by-step instructional programs. Lubinski and Jaberg (1997) argue

that despite the commitment of recent reports to reforms in Mathematics classrooms,

changing this tradition has met with strong resistance. One reason for this resistance, Harries

and Sutherland (1999) suggest, may be that teachers relinquish their responsibility for lesson

planning to textbooks which provide a routine and time saving approach to teaching and

learning Mathematics. Consequently, teachers were found to be dependent on textbook

schemes to inform what happens in Mathematics lessons; talking about Mathematics in

relation to exercises or chapters in textbooks with little or no conceptual framework for the

subject (Harries & Sutherland, 1999). Lubinski and Jaberg (1997) found that content selection

was framed largely around suggestions in textbooks and “restricted to topics and numbers that

the textbook recommended” (p. 234), rather than around student learning and understanding.

When Mathematics is taught in this manner, meaningful learning about Mathematics and

shaping an identity of participation is ignored; in turn, discounting how learning transforms a

learner’s ability to participate in the activities of a Mathematics community.

The use of textbooks raises further concerns about the learning activities in the texts

with which students are expected to engage. Research indicates the activities are often poorly

thought out and written, thus focusing more on repetition and review with topics covered

superficially (Shield, 2000). Shield (2000), for example, observes that “textbooks do not

convey the intent of recent reports and syllabuses, even though they were written in response

to these documents” (p. 521). He suggests that whilst it is not possible to replicate everything

in syllabus documents, it “should be possible to develop textbook presentations which come

much closer than at present” (p. 516). Findings by Remillard (2000) suggest that textbooks

need to be designed to speak to teachers, not merely through them. Whilst textbook authors

do not have complete authority over how textbooks are used in classrooms, she concludes by

suggesting that writers of textbooks need to talk to teachers about the Mathematics and

pedagogical ideas underpinning the texts, and make their agendas and perspectives more

accessible to teachers and learners.

For many of the reasons discussed above, internationally, teachers are being asked to

transform their Mathematics teaching (Franke, Fennema & Carpenter, 1997). Such

transformations, Franke et al. (1997) argue, require teachers to examine the assumptions

underpinning the way they believe Mathematics should be taught and learned. These

assumptions, Ellerton and Clements (1998) report, are more likely to have evolved from a

traditional perspective which sees learners as working individually on content delivered in

discrete steps and isolated from other aspects of Mathematics. This process may serve the

agenda of textbook authors and teachers rather than the learner of Mathematics. Students who

struggle to keep pace with content delivery, and the way textbooks are written and applied in

Mathematics classrooms, are more likely to withdraw. Furthermore, the Mathematics is

demonstrated in ways that do not provide students with an understanding of its coherence as a

body of theory and practice, and the connections to other subjects and their world beyond the

classroom walls. Consequently, they are more likely to see Mathematics as simply finding the

right answer to routine exercises.

Streaming Students by Ability

In studies conducted in the United States (US) (Linchevski & Kutscher, 1998) , Britain (UK)

(Boaler & Wiliam, 2001) and Australia (Zevenbergen, 2001) the streaming of students by

ability has been found to negatively affect student success and achievement in Mathematics

classrooms. There is a range of terms used to describe streaming, for example, the US

describes it as tracking and differentiation, while the UK uses the term, sets. In Australia, the

most frequently used terms are streaming and ability grouping. In this study the term

streaming will be used. However the authors cited use the term appropriate to their context.

Whatever the terminology, these authors argue that such practices influence and shape

students’ social identity and role within Mathematics classrooms, their interaction with

teachers, and their attitudes towards school and schoolwork.

In studies of streaming in Australian schools, Zevenbergen (2001) found ability

grouping of students is widely adopted in schools today despite negative reports in the

research literature. She suggests that these practices position students in a manner that

negatively influences their views and experiences of their learning of Mathematics in school.

These findings are further reinforced from the findings of the PISA 2003 report (OECD

2004), which indicates that in “highly differentiated systems it is easier to move students not

meeting certain performance standards to other schools, tracks or streams with lower

performance expectations than investing the effort to raise their performance” (p. 29).

In studies of differentiation in schools, Gamoran and Weinstein (1998) report that in

Mathematics more than any other subject teachers held rigidly to the view that Mathematics

“must be taught sequentially, with certain concepts and skills mastered before others can be

introduced” (p. 398). They found that the Mathematics teachers’ reluctance to detrack was

tied to their conceptions of the subject. They reported that these teachers believed that

students could be taught more effectively when they were divided into groups of similar

ability rather than heterogeneously grouped. Oakes, Gamoran and Page (1992) report that

heterogeneous classrooms, that is, mixed ability classrooms, are more likely to be found in the

higher streams than the low streams, again disadvantaging students in the low streamed

classes.

Such practices continue despite research that indicates that streaming students is poor

pedagogical practice and disproportionately harms students from disadvantaged and non-

white backgrounds (Oakes, Gamoran & Page, 1992). McLaren (1998) argues along similar

lines (1992), suggesting that the tracking (streaming) of students into ability groups locks

particular groups of students into positions of limited opportunity, whereby they adjust their

aspirations and self-worth upwards or downwards. Consequently, some students come to see

school as a burden, and knowledge unrelated to their lives and instruction as a waste of their

time. Leinhardt and Watson (1994) and Gates (2002) propose a similar argument,

emphasising that ability grouping and discrimination do not raise standards in classrooms.

Rather, Gates (2002) claims that ability grouping enables teachers to apparently better match

classroom “work to the ability levels of students since the spread of ability is narrower than

would otherwise be the case in all attainment groups” (Gates, 2002, p. 221). Whilst this claim

sounds reasonable, Boaler, Wiliam and Brown (2001) found that, on closer inspection, the

teachers actually spent less time responding to individual student’s needs as compared to

teaching a mixed ability group.

Welner and Oakes (1996) highlight the legal implications for educators of the harmful

effects of streaming or tracking. They claim that tracking is “pedagogically ineffective and

subject to abuse by those who would racially discriminate” (p. 452). In US schools, they

found that race and intelligence were used to provide a solid foundation to support streaming

or tracking of students. Their study revealed that Latinos and African Americans were

deemed less intelligent than White Americans. Consequently, they were considered less

motivated to learn. Hence, disproportionate numbers of students from non-white backgrounds

and low-income families were allocated to classrooms prescribed for so-called less capable

students, whilst white middle class students were over-represented in more advanced classes.

In short, this practice has the potential to deny students access to higher streamed

classes, tertiary education, or future employment opportunities. This argument also applies to

the assessment of student learning.

Assessing for Absorption or Learning?

Assessment in Mathematics is widely recognised as a practice which is biased.5 That is, it

sorts groups of students along the lines of gender, race and class. Yet, it remains a powerful

tool in Mathematics classrooms today despite reforms in assessment and calls for authentic

assessment of learning. In their studies of culture, class and realistic tests, Cooper and Dunne

(2000) found that working-class students were less likely to understand the difference

between esoteric and realistic Mathematics items on tests. An item was considered realistic if

it “contains[ed] either persons or non-mathematical objects from everyday settings. Otherwise

it was coded as ‘esoteric’” (p. 84). A key finding was that compared to middle class students,

working class students performed less well on realistic items compared to esoteric items.

Their findings suggest that assessment reflects the life and experiences of some students over

others, such as working-class students. They argue that these students are less likely to

succeed in school Mathematics because of the differences between school and what they bring

with them from home. Their claim follows Gee’s (1996) argument that students from

disadvantaged groups are less likely to adopt the preferred Discourse of the classroom

because it conflicts with their home or community-based Discourse. Consequently, students

who are not able to adapt to the Discourse of Mathematics classrooms, which includes

assessment, are more likely to be marginalised from that community. Further, Cooper and

Dunne (2000) suggest that the difference in performance in Mathematics has the potential to

influence a student’s future opportunities.

This point is emphasised in Walkerdine’s (1998) study of gender in Mathematics. She

found poor achievement and participation in Mathematics of the same social groups of

students reinforces the idea of Mathematics assessment as a tool for sorting different groups.

This point is heightened further by a teacher’s response from Walkerdine’s (1998) study:

‘Poor’ children have ‘low ability’, are ‘unable to grasp mathematical ideas’, ‘unsure of

themselves’ and ‘nervy’, cannot remember what was learnt before, and apply it to the

present. They fail to make connections. (p. 140)

5 See for example, Linchevski & Kutscher (1998) and, Zevenbergen (2001).

Clearly, from this account, the problem is seen to be the student’s and one that neither school

nor the teacher is likely to change. Walkerdine (1998) found that when students are viewed as

having the problem, they are precluded from the very things needed for their success in

Mathematics, that is, “an interest in, and curiosity about their surroundings, perseverance, and

enthusiasm” (p. 140).

Research by Cooper and Dunne (2000) and Ruthven (2002) suggests that the

assumptions teachers make about students are more part of the hidden agenda of the

Mathematics classroom than the rhetoric of educational reform. Ruthven (2002), for example,

reports that teachers used student performance on assessment items to justify their pervasive

idea of ability. This practice results in the differential treatment of students, with teachers

perceiving ability as a permanent trait restricting the capacity for learning of many students.

Therefore, teachers are seen to “protect” (p. 182) students by “increasing the predictability of

mathematical tasks and correspondingly reducing the degree of active interpretation and

critical engagement expected” (p. 182).

Ruthven (2002) claims that particularly with interpreting formal assessment, the idea

of ability relates strongly to comparisons made between and within groups of students.

Comparisons that focused on recognisable understanding in relation to peers tended to

undermine many students’ motivation for learning, particularly those considered less

successful in Mathematics such as those mentioned previously. Unfortunately, what such

students do learn is that they cannot do Mathematics. Watson (2001; 2002) suggests such

methods are an over-simplification of how learning happens. The repetitive drill exercises

found in assessment items are not reasonable grounds for grouping students by ability. Nor

does this approach mean that students attach meaning to what is being taught. Watson argues

understanding requires more than rote learning, drill and practice or following procedures

seen as correct by the teacher, although she claims these could form the basis for future

development of understanding.

Conclusion

Indeed, much of formal schooling today still stems from treating school instruction as an

efficient factory (Rogoff, Bartlett & Turkanis Goodman, 2002). This legacy has led to the

widespread assumption that learning is the result of adults teaching and transmitting

knowledge to learners as if they are empty receptacles. In such a context, the focus is

primarily on the teacher to deliver what should be learned at the expense of acknowledging,

providing and supporting the more complex ways that students may learn. Such practices, and

the assumptions on which they are founded, further and in particular disadvantage an already

disadvantaged group of learners.

References

Alexander, K., Entwisle, D., & Horsey, C. (1997). From First Grade forward: Early

foundations of High School Dropout. Sociology of Education, 70, 87-107.

Anyon, J. (1997). Ghetto schooling: A political economy of urban educational reform. Wilson

VT: Teachers College Press.

Askew, M. (2001). Policy, practice and principles in teaching numeracy: What makes a

difference? In P. Gates (Ed.), Issues in Mathematics Teaching (pp. 105-120). London:

Routledge/Falmer.

Askew, M., Brown, M., Rhodes, V., Johnson, D., & William, D. (1997). Effective teachers of

numeracy. London: Kings College, School of Education.

Boaler, J., & Wiliam, D. (2001). "We've still got to learn!" Students' perspectives on ability

grouping and mathematics achievement. In P. Gates (Ed.), Issues in mathematics

teaching (pp. 77-92). London: RoutledgeFalmer.

Boaler, J., Wiliam, D., & Brown, M. (2001). Students' experiences of ability grouping -

disaffection, polarisation and the construction of failure. British Educational Research

Journal, 27.

Borasi, R. (1996). Reconceiving Mathematics Instruction: A Focus on Errors. New Jersey:

Ablex Publishing.

Bredo, E. (2000). Reconsidering social constructivism: The relevance of George Herbert

Mead's interactionism. In D. Phillips (Ed.), Constructivism in education: Opinions and

second opinions on controversial issues. Chicago: The national society for the study of

education.

Cobb, P., & Yackel, E. (1998). A Constructivist Perspective on the Culture of the

Mathematics Classroom. In F. Seeger, J., Voigt, & U., Waschescio, (Ed.), The Culture

of the Mathematics Classroom (pp. 158-190). Cambridge: Cambridge University

Press.

Cooper, B., & Dunne, M. (2000). Assessing children's mathematics knowledge: social class,

sex and problem-solving. Buckingham: Open University Press.

Ellerton, N., & Clements, M. A. (1998). Transforming the International Mathematics

Education Research Agenda. In A. Sierpinska, & J. Kilpatrick (Ed.), Mathematics

Education as a Research Domain: A Search for Identity (Vol. 1, pp. 153-176).

Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Ewing, B. F. (2005). Distrupting tradition: Identity, participation and membership in

mathematics classrooms. Doctoral these work in progress. Nathan: Griffith

University.

Fairclough, N. (1992). Discourse and social change. Cambridge: Polity Press.

Franke, M., Fennema, E., & Carpenter, T. (1997). Teachers creating change: Examining

evolving beliefs and classroom practice. In E. Fennema, & B. Nelson Scott (Ed.),

Mathematics teachers in transition (pp. 255-282). Mahwah New Jersey: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.

Gamoran, A. W., M. (1998). Differentiation and opportunity in restructured schools.

American Journal of Education, 106(May), 385-415.

Gates, P. (2002). Issues of equity in mathematics education: defining the problem, seeking

solutions. In L. Haggarty (Ed.), Teaching mathematics in secondary schools (pp. 211-

228). London: The Open University.

Gee, J. (1996). Social linguistics and literacies (2nd ed.). London: Tayler & Francis Ltd.

Harries, T., & Sutherland, R. (1999). Primary school mathematics textbooks: an international

comparison. In I. Thompson (Ed.), Issues in teaching numeracy in primary schools

(pp. 49-50). Buckingham: Open university press.

Hill, P. W., & Rowe, K. J. (1998). Multilevel modelling in school effectiveness research.

School effectiveness and school achievement, 7, 1-34.

Jones, D. (1997). A conceptual framework for studying the relevance of context to

mathematics teachers' change. In E. Fennema, & B., Nelson Scott, (Ed.), Mathematics

teachers in transition (pp. 131-154). Mahwah, New jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum

Associates.

Leinhardt, G., Watson. (1994). Comments on Thomas Romberg's chapter. In A. Schoenfeld

(Ed.), Mathematical thinking and problem solving (pp. 305-311). Hillsdale, New

Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Lerman, S. (2000). The Social Turn in Mathematics Education Research. In J. Boaler (Ed.),

Multiple Perspectives on Mathematics Teaching and Learning (pp. 19-44). London:

Ablex Publishing.

Linchevski, L. K., B. (1998). Tell me with whom you're learning, and I'll tell you how much

you've learned: Mixed ability versus same-ability grouping in mathematics. Journal

for Research in Mathematics Education, 29(5), 533-545.

Lubinski, C., & Jaberg, P. (1997). Teacher change and mathematics K-4: Developing a

theoretical perspective. In E. Fennema, & B. Nelson Scott (Ed.), Mathematics teachers

in transition (pp. 223-254). Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

McLaren, P. (1998). Life in schools: An introduction to critical pedagogy in the foundations

of education (3rd ed.). New York: Longman.

Merrett, F., & Wheldall, K. (1987). British Teachers and the Behavioural Approach to

Teaching. In K. Wheldall (Ed.), The Behaviourist in the Classroom (pp. 18-49).

London: Allen & Unwin.

National Council for Teachers of Mathematics (NCTM). (2000). Principles and standards for

school mathematics. Reston VA: National Council for teachers of mathematics.

Nickson, M. (2002). Social and critical mathematics education. In L. Haggarty (Ed.),

Teaching mathematics in secondary schools (pp. 229-242). London: Routledge/falmer.

Oakes, J., Gamoran, A., & Page, N. R. (1992). Curriculum differentiation: Opportunities,

outcomes, and meanings. In P. W. Jackson (Ed.), Handbook of research on

curriculum: A project of the American Educational Research Association (pp. 570-

608). New York: Macmillan.

Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development (OECD). (2004). First results from

PISA 2003 - Executive summary. Retrieved 9/12/2004, from http://www.pisa.oecd.org

Rogoff, B., Bartlett, L., & Turkanis Goodman, C. (2002). Lessons about learning as a

community. In B. Rogoff, Turkanis Goodman, C., & Bartlett, L. (Ed.), Learning

together (pp. 3-21). New York: Oxford University Press.

Romberg, T., & Kaput, J. (1997). Mathematics in context: Impact on teachers. In E. Fennema,

& B., Nelson Scott, (Ed.), Mathematics teachers in transition (pp. 357-380). Mahwah,

New jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Rosenthal, R., & Jacobson, L.,. (1968). Pygmalion in the classroom. New York: Irvington

Publishers, Inc.

Ruthven, K. (2002). Assessment in mathematics education. In L. Haggarty (Ed.), Teaching

mathematics in secondary schools (pp. 176-191). London: The Open University.

Shield, M. (2000). Mathematics textbooks: Messages to students and teachers. In J. Bana, &

A. Chapman (Ed.), Mathematics education beyond 2000 (Proceedings of the 23rd

Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia,

Fremantle, pp. 516-522). Sydney: MERGA.

Sierpinska, A. (1998). Three Epistemologies, Three Views of Classroom Communication:

Constructivism, Sociocultural Approaches, Interactionism. In H. Steinbring, M. G.,

Bartolini Bussi, & A., Sierpinska, (Ed.), Language and Communication in the

Mathematics Classroom (pp. 30-64). Reston: National Council of Teachers of

Mathematics.

Walkerdine, V. (1998). Counting girls out: Girls and mathematics. London: Falmer Press.

Watson, A. (2001). Making judgements about pupils' mathematics. In P. Gates (Ed.), Issues in

mathematics teaching (pp. 217-231). London: Routledge/Falmer.

Watson, A. (2002). What does it mean to understand something and how do we know when it

has happened? In L. Haggarty (Ed.), Teaching mathematics in secondary schools (pp.

161-175). London: The Open University.

Welner, K., & Oakes, J. (1996). (Li) Ability grouping: The new susceptibility of school

tracking systems to legal challenges. Harvard Educational Review, 66(3), 451-470.

Wood, T., & Turner-Vorbeck, T. (1999). Developing teaching of mathematics: Making

connections in practice. In L. Burton (Ed.), Learning mathematics: From hierarchies

to networks (pp. 173-186). London: Falmer Press.

Zevenbergen, R. (2000). Cracking the Code of Mathematics Classrooms. In J. Boaler (Ed.),

Multiple Perspectives on Mathematics Teaching and Learning (pp. 201-224). London:

Ablex Publishing.

Zevenbergen, R. (2001). Is streaming an equitable practice? Students' experiences of

streaming in the middle years of schooling. In J. Bobis, B., Perry, & M., Mitchelmore,

(Ed.), Numeracy and beyond: Proceedings of the 24th annual conference of the

Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia (Vol. 2, pp. 563-570). Sydney:

Mathematics Education Research Group of Australia.