theory in the pacific, the pacific in theory

38
1 NOTE: this paper was uploaded to arxiv by the author prior to discussion with the proposed publisher and book editor. To appear in: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory: Archaeological Perspectives Editor: Timothy Thomas Routledge Press Chapter Title: Evolutionary Theory in Pacific Island Archaeology Ethan E. Cochrane, The University of Auckland Introduction The Pacific Islands have been a crucible for the development of evolutionary concepts in archaeology and anthropology that are now used world-wide. Pacific researchers’ experimentation with evolution (e.g., Sahlins, 1960) was originally based upon the cultural evolution of nineteenth century scholars, and their ladder of evolutionary progress. Since the 1970s, however, and under some influence from Americanist New Archaeology (O'brien, Lyman and Schiffer, 2005), evolutionary research in the region dismissed the more strident, progressivist Victorian ideas and focused increasingly on adaptive processes that might explain the functioning of past societies (e.g., Kirch, 1980). Later still, another evolutionary framework in the Pacific developed from a mixture of mathematical models of cultural trait

Upload: others

Post on 23-May-2022

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

1

NOTE: this paper was uploaded to arxiv by the author prior to discussion with the

proposed publisher and book editor.

To appear in:

Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory: Archaeological

Perspectives

Editor: Timothy Thomas

Routledge Press

Chapter Title:

Evolutionary Theory in Pacific Island Archaeology

Ethan E. Cochrane, The University of Auckland

Introduction

The Pacific Islands have been a crucible for the development of evolutionary concepts in

archaeology and anthropology that are now used world-wide. Pacific researchers’

experimentation with evolution (e.g., Sahlins, 1960) was originally based upon the cultural

evolution of nineteenth century scholars, and their ladder of evolutionary progress. Since the

1970s, however, and under some influence from Americanist New Archaeology (O'brien,

Lyman and Schiffer, 2005), evolutionary research in the region dismissed the more strident,

progressivist Victorian ideas and focused increasingly on adaptive processes that might

explain the functioning of past societies (e.g., Kirch, 1980). Later still, another evolutionary

framework in the Pacific developed from a mixture of mathematical models of cultural trait

Page 2: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

2

transmission (e.g., Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman, 1981; Boyd and Richerson, 1985) and a

generalisation of evolutionary concepts beyond biology (e.g., Dunnell, 1978).

This chapter outlines the use of evolutionary theory in Pacific Island archaeology, noting the

points of commonality and divergences of thought in the most prominent evolutionary

frameworks. The next section reviews these frameworks in the Pacific and focuses on their

most important theoretical contributions. The earliest framework, the Ladder of Cultural

Evolution, while tinged with dubious notions of progress, emphasised the importance of

phylogenetic history or cultural relatedness, and encouraged theorizing on processes of

societal change. A framework focused on phylogenetic history and adaptation emerged from

ladder-like approaches, but developed and expanded both a range of adaptive processes to

account for the similarities and differences amongst populations and sophisticated techniques

to generate hypotheses of cultural or phylogenetic relatedness. A third framework, focused on

selection, drift and related artefact classifications privileges explanations that invoke

evolutionary processes, complementary to proximate processes. Proximate processes refer to

the socio-natural dynamics that generate the costs and benefits associated with variants of a

behavioural type, while evolutionary processes refer to the sorting mechanisms, such as

selection, that act on those costs and benefits to generate behavioural distributions through

time and space (Winterhalder and Smith, 2000, pp. 52-54).

The subsequent section compares two frameworks’ explanation of a pivotal event, the Lapita

migration from Near to Remote Oceania. This comparison demonstrates the application of

proximate and evolutionary mechanisms and the recognition of phylogeny, but also proposes

that robust explanations should employ descriptions of the archaeological record using units,

artefact types or other classes, that are explicitly linked to evolutionary theory.

Page 3: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

3

Evolutionary Theories in the Pacific

Like many attempts at intellectual categorisation, the following division of evolutionary

frameworks in Pacific Island archaeology is meant to highlight distinguishing features, but of

course these frameworks are sometimes combined in single analyses or bodies of work.

The Ladder of Cultural Evolution: Stages and Adaptive Advances

The evolutionary concepts first used to explain variation in Pacific Island material culture and

practices derive from a ladder-like view of cultural evolution. In this view populations

occupy different rungs on a ladder of increasing complexity or progress. For example,

d’Urville’s (1832) widely-used division of the Pacific into Melanesia, Micronesia, and

Polynesia included an assumption that the people in these geographic regions were of

differing social and political complexities, with Polynesians the most advanced and

Melanesians the least (Clark, 2003, pp. 157-158). However, the idea that populations of

different complexity were evidence of processes of evolutionary development was not

elaborated for several more decades until Spencer (1855, 1857, 1873) prominently argued

that human social development, from lower forms to higher forms, was caused by the cultural

inheritance of human inventions and a force leading to perfection or progress (Freeman,

1974). Rungs on the ladder of progress were also given labels including Morgan’s (1877)

savagery, barbarism, and civilization that fit with the age-system of European archaeology

popular at the time (Dunnell, 1988).

The ladder-like view of cultural evolution is distinct from Darwinian evolution (Figure 1) and

is not built upon the latter’s theoretical assumptions of variation, transmission and selection

(Blute, 1979; Dunnell, 1980; Mesoudi, 2016). Instead, and despite the use of phrases such as

Page 4: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

4

‘survival of the fittest’ by 19th century cultural evolutionists, the ladder-like view of cultural

evolution is transformational, and not based on a Darwinian selection process (contra

Carneiro, 2003, pp. 68-73). In the transformational view explanations state the processes by

which one societal type transforms to another. Tylor (1870), as an example of the time,

suggested technology transfer might explain the transformation of a tribe, a low rung on the

ladder, to a higher civilization. Even with more rungs or finer-grained types—incipient and

simple chiefdoms (Steponaitis, 1978; Earle, 1987), archaic and modern states (Marcus and

Feinman, 1998)—the explanatory focus remains the processes whereby one finer-grained

type transforms to another. In the transformational view, societal types exist in the sense that

explanations account for how types originate.

<Figure 1 about here>

This contrasts with some explanation in Darwinian evolution where types do not exist

independent of the analyst, but are conceptualized and constructed as measurement units

(Mayr, 1959), the artificiality of which is obvious if you imagine a type in the human lineage,

an H. erectus, birthing an H. sapiens. Whit a similar approach in archaeology, types are

constructed and used to characterise variant distributions in the archaeological record

(Cochrane, 2001) and it is these variant distributions that are the focus of explanation (Figure

1). As types are a product of archaeological classification procedures, their origin is a non-

question.

The influence of 19th century cultural evolutionism on Americanist archaeology and

ethnology was dimmed by Boas’s focus (e.g., 1911) on the particularistic aspects of cultures1.

1 Late 19th and early 20th century British anthropologists in the Pacific were influenced by evolutionary thought (see e.g., Kuklick, 1996) but their influence on modern archaeology (post-WWII) in the region has been more limited.

Page 5: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

5

By the mid-20th century, however, ladder-like cultural evolution was re-kindled by Leslie

White who linked Spencer’s nebulously defined force of progress to specific empirical

variation across populations. White (1959, pp. 144-145) clearly associated cultural evolution

with the amount of energy extracted from the environment by populations:

Social systems are but the social form of expression of technological control over the

forces of nature. Social evolution is therefore a function of technological

development. Social systems evolve as the amount of energy harnessed per capita per

year increases, other factors remaining constant. This is to say, they become more

differentiated structurally, more specialized functionally, and as a consequence of

differentiation and specialization, special mechanisms of integration and regulation

are developed (see also White, 1943).

Sahlins described White’s proposal as general evolution, the focus of which is “the

determination and explanation of the successive transformations of culture through its several

stages of overall progress” (1960, p. 29). General evolution, Sahlins also argued, was distinct

from specific evolution, which was concerned with adaptation within cultural lineages, for

example, how the diversification of production systems in related Polynesian chiefdoms

show adaptive advances (Sahlins, 1958). Specific evolution was also related to Steward’s

(1955) multilinear cultural evolution that investigated similarities between cultural traditions

to identify adaptive regularities (e.g., patrilineal bands), particularly those associated with

certain environments. In contrast to Steward, however, Sahlins considered a complete

explanation of culture to include both specific and general evolution as “the former is a

connected, historic sequence of forms, the latter a sequence of stages exemplified by forms of

a given order of development” (Sahlins, 1960, p. 33).

Page 6: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

6

Nevertheless, Sahlins (1958) focused on specific evolution in his study of social stratification

in Polynesia where he identified adaptive regularities, in large part, by the number of status

levels present, and grouped them into three types (from greater to fewer status levels):

ramage systems, descent-line systems, and atoll systems. Since it was not possible to generate

data on productivity or per capita energy extraction in these societies (as White’s proposal

required), Sahlins argued that variation in surplus food production, measured by the spatial

scale and frequency of its distribution, is a valid proxy for per capita energy extraction

(Sahlins, 1958, pp. 107-110, Table 1). He concluded that societies with greater social

stratification, with ramage systems for example, had greater surplus, and therefore greater per

capita energy extraction, confirming White’s general evolutionary thesis.

White’s notion that more evolved societies should also exhibit relatively greater structural

differentiation and functional specialisation was also confirmed by Sahlins and others (e.g.,

Service, 1962) working in the Pacific. For example, Sahlins described how more highly

developed chiefdoms should have a greater number of cultural sub-systems and more

specialised sub-systems than less developed tribes (Sahlins, 1960, pp. 21-22 and 35-37;

1972).

While mid-century cultural evolutionists correlated societal stages with productivity

measured in various ways, the processes that cause societies to become more productive and

transform from one rung on the ladder to the next have been continuously, if somewhat

infrequently, debated (Carneiro, 2003, pp. 155-156). These processes, such as Goldman’s

(1955) inherent status rivalry, Friedman’s (1981) structuralist transformation, Kirch’s (1984)

competition in limited environments, Earle’s (1997) political economy, and the culturally

specific rules or social logic of Flannery and Marcus (2012), all include individuals, groups

Page 7: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

7

or societies adapting to new socio-natural environments. Current archaeological research that

privileges these processes relies, at least in part, upon emic perspectives or human

motivations to explain cultural evolution and changes in cultural complexity (e.g., Kirch,

2010; Lepofsky and Kahn, 2011; Clark, 2017; Quintus and Cochrane, 2018).

Ladder-like cultural evolution has influenced archaeological thinking on the processes that

may explain changes in cultural complexity, but Ladder-like cultural evolution is also widely

employed in Pacific archaeology in another way. The general characteristics of a rung on the

ladder, a societal type, are used to culturally reconstruct a time-space portion of the

archaeological record as representing, for example, a tribe or chiefdom. The association of

some characteristic of the archaeological record with a particular type – monumental

architecture with chiefdoms, for example – is then used to aid in the reconstruction of other

aspects of ancient society (e.g., Stevenson, 2002; Wallin and Solsvik, 2010). Clark et al.

(2008) for example, suggest that, the cultural chronology of monumental tomb constructions

and habour modifications at Lapaha, Tonga, supports a pre-AD 1450 date for marine

transport associated with ‘inasi, the first-fruits ceremony found in many Pacific chiefdoms.

Tongan chiefs “may have been using the ‘inasi and other ceremonies to promote social

cohesion among local and non-local groups in the newly constituted and geographically

dispersed chiefdom” (Clark, Burley and Murray, 2008, p. 1006). Other aspects of chiefdom

society have been reconstructed at various times and places from the Pacific island

archaeological record including alliances between New Caledonia chiefdoms (Sand, Bolé and

Ouetcho, 2003), the possible dual sacred and secular leadership roles in ancient Samoa

(Wallin and Martinsson-Wallin, 2007), and attached specialist carvers in Rapa Nui (Simpson,

Van Tilburg and Dussubieux, 2018)

Page 8: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

8

Leonard and Jones (1987) pointed out a potential problem with this cultural reconstruction

approach over 30 years ago after analysing the data within Murdock’s (1967) Ethnographic

Atlas, and focusing on three variables: community organisation, settlement pattern, and class

stratification. They demonstrated that the values within these variables that typically define

cultural evolutionary stages – for example the segmented communities, complex settlements,

and dual stratification of chiefdoms – do not often appear as a package within particular

ethnographically-described societies (Leonard and Jones, 1987, Table 1). In other words,

using an archaeologically visible trait, such as monumental architecture, to infer additional,

less easily observed traits of particular societal types, can lead to unrecognised errors, a

problem occasionally recognized in the Pacific (Sand, 2002) and elsewhere (Wengrow and

Graeber, 2015; Hildebrand et al., 2018).

Phylogenetic History and Adaptation: the Legacy of Islands as Laboratories

In his discussion of specific and general evolution, Sahlins described the study of specific

evolution of cultures as the examination of cultural traits “arising through adaptation”

(Sahlins, 1960, p. 25) within phylogenetically related societies. According to Sahlins when a

group enters a new environment, traits arising through adaptation are synthesized from the

group’s previous traits, or are diffused from other groups, or both (Sahlins, 1960, p. 24).

These ideas were developed in the Pacific most prominently by Kirch (1984) in his seminal

book The Evolution of the Polynesian Chiefdoms (see also Kirch and Green, 1987; Kirch and

Green, 2001). In particular Kirch argued that the culturally-related societies of Polynesia

provide an opportunity to investigate how variation in environments, interaction, and

demography have influenced traits arising through adaptation in separate, but related

societies.

Page 9: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

9

The focus on adaptation is a defining feature of this approach. Adaptation is typically

conceptualised as a proximate, ecological process whereby entities, such as human groups,

continuously achieve a better fit with their changing environment (cf., Steward, 1955; Kirch,

1980). This privileges systems-like explanations, which are a component of evolutionary

explanations in general, and identify how the values within a set of interrelated variables

change (e.g., Kirch et al., 2012). These explanations are compatible with selection processes

as they focus on the adaptive function of an evolved system, but they do not demonstrate why

one system evolved relative to another (Dunnell, 1980). To illustrate the latter question,

consider several behaviours explained as adaptive systems that minimize lethal competition

between groups over limited resources: Rapa Nui moai construction and costly signalling

(Dinapoli et al., 2017); settlements locations in Fiji and economic defendability (Field, 2004);

subsistence and flexible territories in the Channel Islands (Kennett and Clifford, 2004). To

determine the evolutionary processes that resulted in a particular system – costly signalling

and not economic defendability, for example – requires identifying the chronological and

spatial history of behavioural alternatives and their relative fitnesses. Although such research

has rarely occurred, Allen’s (2011) study of different political systems provides a structure

for this kind of analysis.

Various conceptual frameworks are used to generate explanations of societies as adaptive

systems with Political Economy and Human Behavioural Ecology frameworks particularly

prevalent in Pacific research. Political Economy frameworks focus on types of individuals,

such as elites, and their variable ability to draw on different sources of power “to strategically

direct (and resist) the actions of social groups” (Earle, 1997, p. 208). Earle argues that three

sources of power – economic, military, and ideological – are most important for explaining

changes in cultural complexity and control in societies. Natural environments and historical

Page 10: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

10

circumstances will also shape the use of particular power sources (Earle, 1997). Reviewing

the history of Political Economy frameworks, Earle (1997, pp. 68-70) distinguishes those that

are “voluntarist, adaptationist theories” from “coercive, political” ones. The former describe

societies where elites and non-elites perform different, integrated roles that create an adaptive

system, for example chiefly management of collective water-rights for commoner farmers.

The latter describe societies where different types of individuals variously compel others, or

resist, through use of power. In fact, both kinds of Political Economy frameworks are

adaptationist in the sense that the explanations generated consider the behaviour of types of

individuals as interrelated with the variables of changing natural and social environment. In

Hawaii for example, the political economy of 600 or more years ago was based on control of

staple finance (crops) in an area where agricultural productivity was high, and could be

increased through means of landesque capital. Contrastingly, in the Thy region of Denmark

during the Neolithic-Bronze Age transition, the political economy was based on control of

wealth finance (cattle) that could be easily moved and exchanged in a natural environment

with little possibility for control of crop-based staple finance. Political Economy frameworks

are also adaptationist in that they typical produce system-like explanations and conceptualize

culture as comprised of sets of interrelated variables, such that change in one variable may

affect others (Figure 2).

<Figure s about here>

Human Behavioural Ecology (HBE) frameworks may also form the foundation for adaptive

explanations in the Pacific. In archaeology, HBE frameworks have long-assumed that

relevant human variation is patterned by natural selection acting upon the distribution of

fitness-related behaviours in particular social-ecological contexts (Irons, 1979; Smith and

Winterhalder, 1992; Bird and O’connell, 2006). Two additional assumptions are important:

first, behaviours in a population are assumed to be a product of inheritance, but the type of

Page 11: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

11

inheritance, genetic or cultural, is not relevant (the phenotypic gambit; Smith and

Winterhalder, 1992; Codding and Bird, 2015); second, HBE explanations are typically based

on either optimisation (Macarthur and Pianka, 1966) or game-theory methods (Lewontin,

1961). When using optimisation methods, the distribution of individuals’ behaviours is

predicted to optimize some currency, such as net caloric return of hunted animals. The Prey

Choice Model, for example, can be used to explain variable frequencies of hunted animals in

a diachronic deposit by calculating the weights of different taxa (e.g., Morrison and

Cochrane, 2008). Game-theory methods are used to predict the distribution of behaviours in a

population when the fitness of a behaviour is influenced by the probabilities of interactions

between different behaviour-types in a population. The Hawk-Dove model, for example, can

be used to explain the distribution of aggressive and acquiescent behaviours in a population

when the chances of aggressive-aggressive, aggressive-acquiescent, and acquiescent-

acquiescent interactions vary. Hayman (2019) has used this model to investigate the changing

numbers of competitors for high-status titles in ancient Tonga relative to those who acquiesce

at less potential cost, but receive lower ranks (cf. Aswani and Graves, 1998)

HBE, Political Economy and other adaptationist evolutionary research in Pacific archaeology

often examines different systems that share phylogenetic history or cultural relatedness (e.g.,

Kirch, 1990; Allen, 2011; Dinapoli et al., 2017). Phylogenetic history may distinguish

analogous or convergent similarity from homologous similarity or similarity that is a product

of relatedness (Binford, 1968; Kirch and Green, 1987). Phylogenetic history is typically

reconstructed either quantitatively using linguistic (e.g., Gray, Drummond and Greenhill,

2009) or, less commonly, artefact data (e.g., Cochrane, 2015), or comparatively using

historical linguistic methods (e.g., Blust, 1995) or artefact types (e.g., Emory, 1946).

Page 12: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

12

For archaeologists, phylogenetic relationships, irrespective of how they are generated, are

typically a valuable aid in archaeological interpretations. Gray and Jordan (2000), for

example, constructed quantitative linguistic phylogenies to evaluate competing hypotheses

concerning Lapita migration while Pawley (2018) used comparative linguistic methods to

propose a particular settlement history for Fiji-West Polynesia. While archaeological

interpretation of various phylogenies has proven popular (e.g., Green, 1966; Kirch and Green,

1987; Pawley, 1996; Sheppard and Walter, 2006; Burley, 2013) there are two frequently

voiced concerns (Dewar, 1995; Terrell, Hunt and Gosden, 1997; Moore, 2001). One is the

degree of isomorphism between language phylogenies and the phylogenetic relationships

defined by human biology or artefact traditions. This amounts to the significant argument that

language, culture, and biology might not travel together down the same evolutionary

pathways (see comments in Bedford et al., 2018). The other concern is that the branching

relationships required by many phylogenetic methods might be inappropriate models to

investigate cultural change (Collard, Shennan and Tehrani, 2006). One remedy for the first

concern is to construct phylogenies based on different realms of human variation, languages

and artefacts for example, and identify mismatches that require explanation (Tehrani, Collard

and Shennan, 2010). The concern that branching models of relatedness are inappropriate can

be approached as an empirical problem with branching and reticulate models applied to the

same data (e.g., Cochrane and Lipo, 2010; Shennan, Crema and Kerig, 2015).

Phylogenetic relationships are also investigated to determine if similarities in different

populations are homologous, such as shared ancestral traits, or if similarities are analogous,

arising due to independent invention, among other possible processes. Distinguishing

between the two is known as Galton’s problem (Mace and Pagel, 1994) and has been

investigated in the Pacific with reference to agriculture techniques, as well as fishhooks (e.g.,

Page 13: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

13

Allen, 1992; Pfeffer, 2001), and ceramics (e.g., Winter et al., 2012). Considering agricultural

techniques, the processes that explain similarities in the origins and distribution of raised

beds (Kirch and Lepofsky, 1993), simple flooding (Kuhlken, 1994, p. 364; Mccoy and

Graves, 2010:94), windbreak farming (Mccoy and Graves, 2010:94), and lithic mulch

cultivation (Barber, 2010, p. 76) are all debated with specific proposals based variably on

reconstructed proto-lexemes, archaeological chronologies and distances between island

groups.

Reconstructing the cultural characteristics of ancestral societies at the branch points or nodes

of cultural phylogenies is another focus of phylogenetic history and adaptation research. This

has been pursued by Kirch and Green (1987, 2001) using a triangulation approach whereby

controlled comparison across different domains of human variation – ethnography,

linguistics, material culture, and biology – is used to develop cultural reconstructions.

Beginning with a comparative linguistic phylogeny, they propose that Ancestral Polynesian

Society, the population from which East Polynesian groups radiated, was likely situated in

Tonga, Samoa, and some of their nearby islands. Using both lexical reconstruction of proto-

Polynesian language, and comparative ethnography, Kirch and Green reconstruct, for

example, the spatial segregation of domestic activities and argue that separate cooking and

dwelling structures existed in the region and time period (i.e., from about 2300 – 1000 BP),

although there is currently no archaeological evidence of this (Kirch and Green, 2001, p.

196). A related research program using quantitative approaches to phylogenetic

reconstruction has recently emerged. This research uses linguistic trees produced within a

Bayesian framework to estimate the likelihood of particular proto-language stages, and

concomitantly, the likelihood that certain concepts or social forms represented by

Page 14: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

14

reconstructed lexemes were present (e.g., Currie et al., 2010; Kushnick, Gray and Jordan,

2014).

Selection, Drift and Other Evolutionary Mechanisms

The most recent evolutionary approach to be applied in the Pacific focuses on mechanisms

such as selection, drift, and other cultural transmission processes to explain the distribution of

archaeological (e.g., Graves and Cachola-Abad, 1996; O'connor, White and Hunt, 2017) and

ethnographic traits (e.g., Rogers and Ehrlich, 2008). This framework, typically called

evolutionary archaeology (Shennan, 2008), and sometimes cultural evolution, a label

confusingly similar to mid-20th century work, derives from two intellectual lineages. The first

is quantitative cultural transmission research beginning about the 1970s (e.g., Cavalli-Sforza

and Feldman, 1981; Lumsden and Wilson, 1981; Boyd and Richerson, 1985), a programme

later termed dual-inheritance theory or gene-culture co-evolution. The second lineage

includes research focused on drift and selection applied to explicitly constructed artefact

classes (e.g., Dunnell, 1978, 1992). Both lineages are also inter-woven with human

behavioural ecology (Broughton and O'connell, 1999; O'brien and Lyman, 2002).

Evolutionary archaeology research in the Pacific (and elsewhere) regularly uses artefact

classes specifically created to investigate mechanisms defined in evolution theory (Graves

and Ladefoged, 1995; Cochrane, 2002; Morrison, 2012). These artefact classes are the types

by which variation in the archaeological record is quantified to produce variant distributions.

Archaeological variant distributions are then compared with the distributional expectations of

evolutionary mechanisms to determine if a specific mechanism, such as drift, is an adequate

explanation. The comparison of empirical distributions and theoretical expectations facilitates

evaluation of artefact classification as the arbitrary decisions in classification must be

Page 15: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

15

justified relative to the proposed evolutionary mechanisms (Lipo, 2001; Glatz, Kandler and

Steele, 2011). Cochrane (2009), for example, evaluated variously defined Fijian ceramic

classes for their ability to track drift processes by examining class criteria against theoretical

expectations of continuous distributions and others.

Allen’s (1996) work on ancient fishhooks is an excellent example of creating specific artefact

classes to investigate evolutionary mechanisms. By using hook types whose definitional

criteria are hypothesized to largely measure the results of cultural transmission, Allen was

able to show stochastic (i.e., drift) change over time in hook-type abundances from particular

islands. After classifying the same hooks with criteria hypothesized to track variation in

fishing environments, she demonstrated that the distribution of these hook types in different

populations is likely explained by selection and adaptation to different prey types. Applying a

similar classification-focused approach to ethnographic variation in canoes, Rogers and

Ehrlich (2008) argued that decorative canoe traits such as prow carvings change rapidly, as

predicted by drift, and more than functional traits such as outrigger attachment types whose

distribution is explained by selection and adaptation.

As artefact classes in evolutionary archaeology are designed to investigate evolutionary

mechanisms they will not necessarily match emic or ethnohistoric classes. Allen’s (1996)

fishhook types, for example, might not be recognised as such by ancient fishers. While these

artefact types may or may not have emic reality, their usefulness is evaluated by both

justifying the assumptions that underlie their construction, why for example might fishhook

head morphology track transmission, and by their ability to produce empirical patterns that

can be compared to theoretical expectations. In comparison, other evolutionary frameworks

sometimes use observational units proposed to map onto past emically meaningful variation.

Page 16: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

16

For example, Kahn and Kirch’s (2011; 2015) Political Economy explanations of variation in

Society Islands temple architecture employs culturally relevant types drawn from

ethnohistory such as shrines, marae, community-level marae, particular kinds of priestly

dwellings, and structures in which sacred objects were stored. They argue that greater

architectural complexity over time and increasing spatial differentiation of these emic

architectural types resulted from increasing elite manipulation of ideology and control of

staple finance. Cochrane (2015) also examined temple architecture across the Society Islands

and Polynesia, but his approach to archaeological classification was based on expectations for

the architectural variables whose distribution would be patterned predominantly by

transmission and drift. Briefly, these expectations include that frequencies of different values

in variables should be independent, they should not be correlated with different

environments, and they should vary spatially and temporally. Cochrane’s resulting

architectural classes do not match emic categories, but the analysis based on them indicates

wide-spread transmission of ideas across East Polynesia, a finding supported by other recent

evolutionary archaeology and artefact sourcing studies (e.g. Weisler et al., 2016; O'connor,

White and Hunt, 2017).

A Case Study of Two Approaches

A case study comparing different evolutionary frameworks applied to the Pacific

archaeological record will serve, to highlight points of similarity and divergence, and suggest

how multiple frameworks might be used to produce better explanations. The archaeological

record of initial migration from Near to Remote Oceania has long been a significant research

issue in Pacific Island archaeology and two recent evolutionary studies have attempted to

explain this population movement: Earle and Spriggs’ (2015) Political Economy explanation

and Cochrane’s (2018) selection-based approach.

Page 17: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

17

Evidence for the initial migration of populations from Near to Remote Oceania (Figure 3) is

based on the distribution of Lapita ceramics, an intricately decorated earthenware. These

ceramics appear in the Bismarck Archipelago, without local precedent, most likely between

3535-3234 cal BP (95% probability, Rieth and Athens, 2017). After a pause, the ceramics

appear with the first human colonists in southwestern Remote Oceania between 3000 cal BP

(Sheppard, 2011) for islands in the west like Vanuatu and New Caledonia , and 2750 cal BP

for Samoa (Petchey, 2001), the farthest eastern island colonized at the time. These colonists

crossed the Near-Remote Oceanic biogeographic boundary that, in part, kept humans and

other species confined to the west for approximately 50,000 years (Green, 1991).

<Figure 3 about here>

Before Lapita pottery appears in Near Oceania, populations there had a long history of

hunting and gathering, mid-Holocene agriculture and arboriculture, and animal management,

as well as tuber and aroid use beginning in the Pleistocene. After the appearance of Lapita

ceramics and associated material culture in Near Oceania these subsistence practices

continued at new Lapita occupations and at locations with both pre-Lapita and Lapita

deposits (Kirch, 1997, pp. 203-205; Lentfer and Torrence, 2007). A suite of domesticated

Asian animals also appear in Near Oceania during and after the advent of Lapita pottery.

After voyaging to Remote Oceania, the Lapita ceramic producing populations encountered

new plants, animals, and landscapes and the colonists targeted these rich resources (Nagaoka,

1988; Steadman, Plourde and Burley, 2002; Valentin et al., 2010). They also brought crops

such as bananas, and taro, that had a multi-millennia history of human use in Near Oceania

(Horrocks and Bedford, 2005; Horrocks and Nunn, 2007; Fall, 2010). Some domesticated or

commensal Asian animals also accompanied the colonists (Matisoo-Smith, 2007). There is a

Page 18: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

18

substantially greater number of Lapita deposits in Remote Oceania compared to Near

Oceania (Anderson et al., 2001).

Those groups who left for Remote Oceania initially maintained contact with Near Oceanic

populations as evidenced by obsidian transport and the similar, intricate Lapita pottery

decorations repeated on locally-made pottery (Dickinson, 2006) in both regions. By

approximately 2700 cal BP this movement between populations had largely stopped, both

within Remote Oceania and between the regions, as evidenced by the lack of artefact

transport, and the replacement of commonly decorated Lapita pottery with different,

archipelago-specific styles in Remote Oceania.

A Political Economy Explanation of Lapita Movement

Earle and Spriggs (2015) develop a Political Economy explanation of Lapita movement to

Remote Oceania focusing on differential access to power within society. They propose that

differential access begins with bottlenecks or the “constriction points in commodity chains

that offer an aspiring leader the opportunity to limit access, thus creating ownership over

resources, technologies, or knowledge” (Earle and Spriggs, 2015, p. 517). Once an aspiring

leader has restricted other’s access to a resource she or he can more easily extract surplus

from the resource to underwrite control of economic, military, or ideological power sources.

Earle and Spriggs note that the archaeological application of Political Economy requires the

identification of important resources and their constriction points by focusing on the objects

which move through ancient economies.

Earle and Spriggs apply this strategy to the Lapita case through an emic approach to unit

formation, in particular relying on Pacific ethnography and cross-cultural comparison to

Page 19: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

19

determine what archaeological types would correspond to prestige items, Lapita pots, shell

ornaments, and bird feathers for example. They argue, however, that the nature of Lapita

prestige items, including the widespread Lapita pots, limited their control, with a similar lack

of constriction points and opportunities for control of staple finance. In contrast, they point

out that knowledge of long-distance voyaging and the ability to colonize new lands, and with

new opportunities for trade, was a knowledge-resource that was not available to all and

whose distribution and use might be controlled by a minority. If use of this knowledge was a

somewhat competitive arena, this, in part, explains Lapita migration “to search out

undiscovered islands that could offer easy subsistence, exchangeable products, and a basis for

leaders to establish themselves independently” (Earle and Spriggs, 2015, p. 518).

A Selection-Based Explanation of Lapita Movement

Cochrane’s (2018) explanation of the Lapita migration is different from Earle and Spriggs

(2015). Instead of assuming a relevant explanatory process is individuals’ use of power to

direct and resist groups, Cochrane divides explanations into ultimate-evolutionary and

proximate-adaptive types. In the former the relative advantage of cultural traits causes their

differential transmission (i.e., replication) over time and across space. That is, trait

distributions are explained by selection. The Remote Oceanic Lapita trait or trait-suite

includes a focus on hunting and gathering, and long-distance maritime movement, while the

Near Oceanic trait-suite include less focus on both hunting and gathering, and long-distance

movement. If there is no differential transmission of these trait-suites, then selection is

probably not implicated in Lapita movement into Remote Oceania. For proximate-adaptive

explanations, adaptation or the changing correlations of traits and environments is a system-

level explanation. These correlations can be explained by physiological responses, learned

and flexible behaviours or some combination. And such correlations are triggered by other

Page 20: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

20

changes in the system, such as environmental change, technological change, or social

changes.

To apply this framework, Cochrane develops a method to identify the archaeological

signatures of different movement types, such as range-expansion or range shift, with the

distribution of these types predicted by both evolutionary and adaptive processes in different

environments. Cochrane argues that the Lapita migration is a range-expansion and the greater

rate of replication of Lapita trait-suites in Remote Oceania compared to Near Oceania

indicates selection. The differential replication of Lapita trait-suites in Near and Remote

Oceania can also be explained as a proximate system-like process with changes in both

regional weather patterns and canoe technology that changed the costs and benefits of long-

distance voyaging. In short, “While climate change and innovation in voyaging technology

lowered the cost of movement for Lapita populations, selection processes related to

environmental variation and demography explain the relatively greater rate of Lapita

deposition in Remote Oceania” (Cochrane, 2018, p. 543). Importantly, there are several

empirical observations that would refute or challenge this explanation. If for example, there is

not a greater rate of Lapita trait-suite replication in Remote Oceania compared to Near

Oceania, then selection is not a valid explanation.

Conclusion

The selection-based approach and the Political Economy approach to Lapita migration

encapsulate many of the salient characteristics of various evolutionary approaches in the

Pacific, highlight the differences, and suggest where they might be combined for mutual

benefit. Evolutionary approaches in the Pacific can be broadly divided into adaptationist,

system-focused frameworks and selectionist, transmission-focused frameworks. As has been

Page 21: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

21

recognized for decades (Mayr, 1961), a complete evolutionary explanation of behaviour, in

this case past behaviour fossilised in the archaeological record, should ideally include both

kinds of explanations. To exemplify the possibilities, Cochrane’s explanation of Lapita

migration does not delve into the proximate social triggers that may have kick-started the

movement. It is here that a Political Economy and cross-cultural approach might identify

more social competitive processes and associated archaeological units by which to investigate

the evolution of range-expansion and other movement types. A selection-based approach

might identify the evolutionary mechanisms that explain the probable phylogenetic

relatedness of populations such as the pre-Lapita ceramic-using groups in Island Southeast

Asia, Lapita populations, and the descendant populations of the Pacific whose hierarchical

social systems are often the focus of Political Economy research. Such a division of labour

mirrors Mesoudi et al.’s (2006) unified approach to the evolutionary study of culture. Here,

Political Economy is cast in the role of micro-evolutionary studies with selection-based

approaches investigating macro-evolution.

All evolutionary approaches often espouse a concern with scientific explanations, but what

this means differs between frameworks. Minimally science uses theory or ideas, to explain

the distribution of empirical phenomena, in this case the archaeological record, and potential

scientific explanations should be empirically testable. That is, explanations should be

constructed in a way that one can envision an empirical observation that would falsify them.

All evolutionary approaches should strive for this, lest we construct unverifiable stories.

Acknowledgements

Page 22: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

22

This research was partly funded by the Royal Society Te Apārangi Marsden Fund (contract

UOA1709). The comments of Thegn Ladefoged, Timothy Thomas, and an anonymous

reviewer improved the manuscript.

References

Allen, M., S 1996. Style and function in East-Polynesian fishhooks. Antiquity, 70, 97-116.

Allen, M. S. 1992. Temporal Variation in Polynesian Fishing Strategies: The Southern Cook

Islands in Regional Perspective. Asian Perspectives, 31, 183-204.

Allen, M. S. 2011. Oscillating climate and socio-political process: the case of the Marquesan

Chiefdom, Polynesia. Antiquity, 84, 86-102.

Anderson, A., Bedford, S., Clark, G. R., Lilley, I., Sand, C., Summerhayes, G. R. &

Torrence, R. 2001. An Inventory of Lapita Sites Containing Dentate-Stamped Pottery.

In: CLARK, G. A., ANDERSON, A. J. & VUNIDILO, T. S. (eds.) The Archaeology

of Lapita Dispersal in Oceania. Canberra: Pandanus Books.

Aswani, S. & Graves, M. W. 1998. Tongan Sociopolitical Evolution. Asian Perspectives, 37,

135-164.

Barber, I. 2010. Diffusion or innovation? Explaining lithic agronomy on the southern

Polynesian margins. World Archaeology, 42, 74-89.

Bedford, S., Blust, R., Burley, D. V., Cox, M., Kirch, P. V., Matisoo-Smith, E., Næss, Å.,

Pawley, A., Sand, C. & Sheppard, P. 2018. Ancient DNA and its contribution to

understanding the human history of the Pacific Islands. Archaeology in Oceania, 53,

205-219.

Binford, L. R. 1968. Archaeological Perspectives. In: BINFORD, S. R. & BINFORD, L. R.

(eds.) New Perspectives in Archaeology. Chicago: Aldine Publishing Company.

Page 23: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

23

Bird, D. & O’Connell, J. 2006. Behavioral Ecology and Archaeology. Journal of

Archaeological Research, 14, 143-188.

Blust, R. 1995. The prehistory of the Austronesian-speaking peoples: A view from language.

Journal of World Prehistory, 453-510.

Blute, M. 1979. Sociocultural evolutionism: An untried theory. Behavioral Science, 24, 46-

59.

Boas, F. 1911. The Mind of Primitive Man, New York, The Macmillian Company.

Boyd, R. & Richerson, P. J. 1985. Culture and the Evolutionary Process, Chicago, University

of Chicago Press.

Broughton, J. M. & O'Connell, J. F. 1999. On Evolutionary Ecology, Selectionist

Archaeology, and Behavioral Archaeology. American Antiquity, 64, 153-165.

Burley, D. V. 2013. Fijian Polygenesis and the Melanesian/Polynesian Divide. Current

Anthropology, 54, 436-462.

Carneiro, R. L. 2003. Evolutionism in cultural anthropology a critical history, Boulder,

Colo., Boulder, Colo. : Westview Press 2003.

Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. & Feldman, M. W. 1981. Cultural Transmission and Evolution: A

Quantitative Approach, Princeton, Princeton University Press.

Clark, G. 2017. The Tongan Maritime State. In: HODOS, T. (ed.) The Routledge Handbook

of Archaeology and Globalization. New York: Routledge.

Clark, G. R. 2003. Shards of Meaning: Archaeology and the Melanesia–Polynesia

Divide Journal of Pacific History, 38, 197-215.

Clark, G. R., Burley, D. V. & Murray, T. 2008. Monumentality and the development of the

Tongan maritime chiefdom. Antiquity, 82, 994-1008.

Cochrane, E. E. 2001. Style, Function, and Systematic Empiricism: The Conflation of

Process and Pattern. In: HURT, T. D. & RAKITA, G. F. M. (eds.) Style and

Page 24: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

24

Function: Conceptual Issues in Evolutionary Archaeology. Westport, Conn.: Bergin

and Garvey.

Cochrane, E. E. 2002. Separating Time and Space in Archaeological Landscapes: An

Example from Windward Society Islands Ceremonial Architecture. In:

LADEFOGED, T. N. & GRAVES, M. W. (eds.) Pacific Landscapes: Archaeological

Approaches. Los Osos, CA: Easter Island Foundation Press.

Cochrane, E. E. 2009. The Evolutionary Archaeology of Ceramic Diversity in Ancient Fiji,

Oxford, Archaeopress.

Cochrane, E. E. 2015. Phylogenetic Analysis of Polynesian Ritual Architecture Suggests

Extensive Cultural Sharing and Innovation. The Journal of the Polynesian Society,

124, 7-46.

Cochrane, E. E. 2018. The Evolution of Migration: the Case of Lapita in the Southwest

Pacific. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 25, 520-558.

Cochrane, E. E. & Lipo, C. P. 2010. Phylogenetic analyses of Lapita decoration do not

support branching evolution or regional population structure during colonization of

Remote Oceania. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological

Sciences, 365, 3889-3902.

Codding, B. F. & Bird, D. W. 2015. Behavioral ecology and the future of archaeological

science. Journal of Archaeological Science, 56, 9-20.

Collard, M., Shennan, S. J. & Tehrani, J. J. 2006. Branching, blending, and the evolution of

cultural similarities and differences among human populations. Evolution and Human

Behavior, 27, 169-184.

Currie, T. E., Greenhill, S. J., Gray, R. D., Hasegawa, T. & Mace, R. 2010. Rise and fall of

political complexity in island South-East Asia and the Pacific. Nature, 467, 801-804.

Page 25: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

25

d'Urville, D. 1832. Notice sur les îles du Grand Océan et sur l'origine des peuples quie les

habitent. Société de Géographie Bulletin, 17.

Dewar, R. E. 1995. Of Nets and Trees: Untangling the Reticualte and Dendritic in

Madagascar's Prehistory. World Archaeology, 26, 301-318.

Dickinson, W. R. 2006. Temper Sands in Prehistoric Oceanian Pottery: Geotectonics,

Sedimentology, Petrography, and Provenance, Boulder, Colorado, Geological Society

of America.

DiNapoli, R. J., Morrison, A. E., Lipo, C. P., Hunt, T. L. & Lane, B. G. 2017. East

Polynesian Islands as Models of Cultural Divergence: The Case of Rapa Nui and

Rapa Iti. The Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology, 1-18.

Dunnell, R. C. 1978. Style and Function: A Fundamental Dichotomy. American Antiquity,

43, 192-202.

Dunnell, R. C. 1980. Evolutionary Theory and Archaeology. In: SCHIFFER, M. B. (ed.)

Advances in Archaeological Method and Theory. New York: Academic Press.

Dunnell, R. C. 1988. The Concept of Progress in Cultural Evolution. In: NITECKI, M. H.

(ed.) Evolutionary Progress. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Dunnell, R. C. 1992. Archaeology and Evolutionary Science. In: WANDSNIDER, L. (ed.)

Quandaries and Quests: Visions of Archaeology's Future. Carbondale: Southern

Illinois University Press.

Earle, T. 1997. How Chiefs Come to Power: The Political Economy in Prehistory, Stanford,

Stanford University Press.

Earle, T. & Spriggs, M. 2015. Political Economy in Prehistory: A Marxist Approach to

Pacific Sequences. Current Anthropology, 56, 515-544.

Earle, T. K. 1987. Chiefdoms in archaeological and ethnohistorical perspective. Annual

review of Anthropology, 16, 279-308.

Page 26: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

26

Emory, K. P. 1946. Eastern Polynesia, Its Cultural Relationships. Dissertation, Yale.

Fall, P. 2010. Pollen evidence for plant introductions in a Polynesian tropical island

ecosystem, Kingdom of Tonga. In: HABERLE, S. G., STEVENSON, J. &

PREBBLE, M. (eds.) Altered Ecologies: Fire, climate and human influence on

terrestrial landscapes. Canberra: Australia National University.

Field, J. 2004. Environmental and climatic considerations: a hypothesis for conflict and the

emergence of social complexity in Fijian prehistory. Journal of Anthropological

Archaeology, 23, 79-99.

Flannery, K. V. & Marcus, J. 2012. The creation of inequality : how our prehistoric

ancestors set the stage for monarchy, slavery, and empire, Cambridge, Mass.,

Cambridge, Mass. : Harvard University Press 2012.

Freeman, D. 1974. The Evolutionary Theories of Charles Darwin and Herbert Spencer.

Current Anthropology, 15, 211-237.

Friedman, J. 1981. Notes on structure and history in Oceania. Folk. Dansk Ethnografisk

Tidsskrift Kobenhavn, 23, 275-295.

Glatz, C., Kandler, A. & Steele, J. 2011. Cultural Selection, Drift, and Ceramic Diversity at

Boğazköy-Hattusa. In: COCHRANE, E. E. & GARDNER, A. (eds.) Evolutionary

and Interpretive Archaeologies. Walnut Creek, California: Left Coast Press.

Goldman, I. 1955. Status Rivalry and Cultural Evolution in Polynesia. American

Anthropologist, 57, 680-697.

Graves, M. W. & Cachola-Abad, C. K. 1996. Seriation as a method of chronologically

ordering architectural design traits: an example from Hawai`i. Archaeology in

Oceania, 31, 19-32.

Page 27: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

27

Graves, M. W. & Ladefoged, T. N. 1995. The Evolutionary Significance of Ceremonial

Architecture. In: TELTSER, P. A. (ed.) Evolutionary Archaeology: Methodological

Issues. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

Gray, R. D., Drummond, A. J. & Greenhill, S. J. 2009. Language Phylogenies Reveal

Expansion Pulses and Pauses in Pacific Settlement. Science, 323, 479-483.

Gray, R. D. & Jordan, F. M. 2000. Language Trees Support the Express-Train Sequence of

Austronesian Expansion. Nature, 405, 1052-1055.

Green, R. C. 1966. Linguistic Subgrouping within Polynesia: The Implications for

Prehistoric Settlement. Journal of the Polynesian Society, 75, 6-38.

Green, R. C. 1991. Near and Remote Oceania: Disestablishing 'Melanesia' in Culture

History. In: PAWLEY, A. K. (ed.) Man and a Half: Essays in Pacific Anthropology

and Ethnobiology in Honour of Ralph Bulmer. Auckland: Polynesian Society.

Hayman, J. M. H. 2019. Islands in an Empire: The Tongan Maritime Chiefdom in

Evolutionary Perspective. Master of Arts Master’s Thesis, The University of

Auckland.

Hildebrand, E. A., Grillo, K. M., Sawchuk, E. A., Pfeiffer, S. K., Conyers, L. B., Goldstein,

S. T., Hill, A. C., Janzen, A., Klehm, C. E., Helper, M., Kiura, P., Ndiema, E., Ngugi,

C., Shea, J. J. & Wang, H. 2018. A monumental cemetery built by eastern Africa’s

first herders near Lake Turkana, Kenya. Proceedings of the National Academy of

Sciences, 115, 8942-8947.

Horrocks, M. & Bedford, S. 2005. Microfossil Analysis of Lapita Deposits in Vanuatu

Reveals Introduced Araceae (Aroids). Archaeology in Oceania, 40, 67-74.

Horrocks, M. & Nunn, P. D. 2007. Evidence for introduced taro (Colocasia esculenta) and

lesser yam (Dioscorea esculenta) in Lapita-era (c. 3050-2500 cal. yr BP) deposits

Page 28: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

28

from Bourewa, southwest Viti Levu Island, Fiji. Journal of Archaeological Science,

34, 739-748.

Irons, W. 1979. Natural Selection, Adaptation, and Human Social Behavior. In: CHAGNON,

N. A. & IRONS, W. (eds.) Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior. North

Scituate, Mass.: Duxbury Press.

Kahn, J. G. 2015. Identifying residences of ritual practitioners in the archaeological record as

a proxy for social complexity. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 40, 59-81.

Kahn, J. G. & Kirch, P. V. 2011. Monumentality and the materialization of ideology in

Central Eastern Polynesia. Archaeology in Oceania, 46, 93-104.

Kennett, D. J. & Clifford, R. A. 2004. Flexible strategies for resource defense on the

Northern Channel Islands of California: an agent-based model. In: FITZPATRICK, S.

M. & ANDERSON, A. (eds.) Voyages of Discovery: The Archaeology of Islands.

Westport, CT: Praeger.

Kirch, P. V. 1980. The Archaeological Study of Adaptation: Theoretical and Methodological

Issues. In: SCHIFFER, M. B. (ed.) Advances in Archaeological Method and Theory.

New York: Academic Press.

Kirch, P. V. 1984. The Evolution of the Polynesian Chiefdoms, Cambridge, Cambridge

University Press.

Kirch, P. V. 1990. Monumental Architecture and Power in Polynesian Chiefdoms: A

Comparison of Tonga and Hawaii. World Archaeology, 22, 206-222.

Kirch, P. V. 1997. The Lapita Peoples, Cambridge, Blackwell.

Kirch, P. V. 2010. How Chiefs Became Kings: Divine Kingship and the Rise of Archaic

States in Ancient Hawai‘i, Berkley, University of California Press.

Kirch, P. V., Asner, G., Chadwick, O. A., Field, J., Ladefoged, T., Lee, C., Puleston, C.,

Tuljapurkar, S. & Vitousek, P. M. 2012. Building and testing models of long-term

Page 29: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

29

agricultural intensification and population dynamics: A case study from the Leeward

Kohala Field System, Hawai’i. Ecological Modelling, 227, 18-28.

Kirch, P. V. & Green, R. C. 1987. History, Phylogeny, and Evolution in Polynesia. Current

Anthropology, 28, 431-456.

Kirch, P. V. & Green, R. C. 2001. Hawaiki, Ancestral Polynesia: an Essay in Historical

Anthropology, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

Kirch, P. V. & Lepofsky, D. 1993. Polynesian Irrigation: Archaeological and Linguistic

Evidence for Origins and Deveiopment. Asian Perspectives, 32, 183-204.

Kuhlken, R. T. 1994. Agricultural Terracing in the Fiji Islands. PhD dissertation, Louisiana

State University and Agicultural and Mechanical College.

Kuklick, H. 1996. Islands in the Pacific: Darwinian Biogeography and British Anthropology.

American Ethnologist, 23, 611-638.

Kushnick, G., Gray, R. D. & Jordan, F. M. 2014. The sequential evolution of land tenure

norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35, 309-318.

Lentfer, C. J. & Torrence, R. 2007. Holocene volcanic activity, vegetation succession, and

ancient human land use: Unraveling the interactions on Garua Island, Papua New

Guinea. Review of Palaeobotany and Palynology, 143, 83-105.

Leonard, R. D. & Jones, G. T. 1987. Elements of an Inclusive Evolutionary Model for

Archaeology. Journal of Anthropological Archaeology, 6, 199-219.

Lepofsky, D. & Kahn, J. 2011. Cultivating an Ecological and Social Balance: Elite Demands

and Commoner Knowledge in Ancient Ma‘ohi Agriculture, Society Islands. American

Anthropologist, 113, 319-335.

Lewontin, R. C. 1961. Evolution and the theory of games. Journal of theoretical biology, 1,

382-403.

Page 30: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

30

Lipo, C. P. 2001. Science, Style, and the Study of Community Structure: An Example from the

Central Mississippi River Valley, Oxford, Hadrian Books Ltd.

Lumsden, C. & Wilson, E. O. 1981. Genes, Mind, and Culture, Cambridge, Harvard

University Press.

MacArthur, R. H. & Pianka, E. R. 1966. On optimal use of a patchy environment. The

American Naturalist, 100, 603-609.

Mace, R. & Pagel, M. 1994. The Comparative Method in Anthropology. Current

Anthropology, 35, 549-564.

Marcus, J. & Feinman, G. M. 1998. Introduction. In: FEINMAN, G. M. & MARCUS, J.

(eds.) Archaic states. Santa Fe: School for Advanced Research.

Matisoo-Smith, E. A. 2007. Animal translocations, genetic variation and the human

settlement of the Pacific. In: FRIEDLAENDER, J. S. (ed.) Genes, Language and

Culture History in the Southwest Pacific. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Mayr, E. 1959. Darwin and Evolutionary Theory in Biology. In: MEGGERS, B. J. (ed.)

Evolution and Anthropology: A Centennial Appraisal. Washington, D.C.:

Anthropological Society of Washington.

Mayr, E. 1961. Cause and Effect in Biology. Science, 134, 1501-1506.

McCoy, M. & Graves, M. W. 2010. The role of agricultural innovation on Pacific Islands: a

case study from Hawai`i Island. World Archaeology, 42, 90-107.

Mesoudi, A. 2016. Cultural Evolution: A Review of Theory, Findings and Controversies.

Evolutionary Biology, 43, 481-497.

Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A. & Laland, K. N. 2006. Towards a unified science of cultural

evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 329-383.

Page 31: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

31

Moore, J. H. 2001. Ethnogenetic patterns in native North America. In: TERRELL, J. E. (ed.)

Archaeology, language and history: Essays on culture and ethnicity. Westport:

Bergin and Garvey.

Morgan, L. H. 1877. Ancient Society, New York, Henry Holt.

Morrison, A. E. 2012. An Archaeological Analysis of Rapa Nui Settlement Structure: a Multi-

Scalar Approach. PhD, University of Hawaii.

Morrison, A. E. & Cochrane, E. E. 2008. Investigating shellfish deposition and landscape

history at the Natia Beach site, Fiji. Journal of Archaeological Science, 35, 2387-

2399.

Murdock, G. P. 1967. Ethnographic Atlas, Pittsburgh, University of Pittsburgh Press.

Nagaoka, L. 1988. Lapita Subsistence: The Evidence of Non-fish Archaeofaunal Remains.

In: KIRCH, P. V. & HUNT, T. L. (eds.) Archaeology of the Lapita Cultural Complex:

A Critical Review. Seattle: Thomas Burke Memorial Washington State Museum.

O'Brien, M. J. & Lyman, R. L. 2002. Evolutionary Archaeology: Current Status and Future

Prospects. Evolutionary Anthropology, 11, 26-36.

O'Brien, M. J., Lyman, R. L. & Schiffer, M. B. 2005. Archaeology as a process:

processualism and its progeny, University of Utah Press.

O'Connor, J. T., White, F. J. & Hunt, T. L. 2017. Fishhook variability and cultural

transmission in East Polynesia. Archaeology in Oceania, 52, 32-44.

Pawley, A. K. 2018. Linguistic Evidence as a Window into the Prehistory of Oceania. In:

COCHRANE, E. E. & HUNT, T. L. (eds.) The Oxford Handbook of Prehistoric

Oceania. Oxford University Press.

Pawley, A. K. 1996. On the Polynesian Subgroup as a Problem for Irwin's Continuous

Settlement Hypothesis. In: DAVIDSON, J., IRWIN, G., LEACH, F., PAWLEY, A.

Page 32: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

32

K. & BROWN, D. (eds.) Oceanic Culture History: Essays in Honour of Roger

Green. Dunedin: New Zealand Archaeological Association.

Petchey, F. J. 2001. Radiocarbon determinations from the Mulifanua Lapita site, Upolu,

western Samoa. Radiocarbon 43, 63-68.

Pfeffer, M. T. 2001. The Engineering and Evolution of Hawaiian Fishhooks. In: HUNT, T.

L., LIPO, C. P. & STERLING, S. L. (eds.) Posing Questions for a Scientific

Archaeology. Westport, Conn.: Bergin and Garvey.

Quintus, S. & Cochrane, E. E. 2018. The Prevelance and Importance of Niche in Agricultural

Development in Polynesia. Journal of Anthropologial Archaeology, 51, 173-186.

Rieth, T. M. & Athens, J. S. 2017. Late Holocene Human Expansion into Near and Remote

Oceania: A Bayesian Model of the Chronologies of the Mariana Islands and Bismarck

Archipelago. The Journal of Island and Coastal Archaeology, 1-12.

Rogers, D. S. & Ehrlich, P. R. 2008. Natural selection and cultural rates of change.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 3416-3420.

Sahlins, M. D. 1958. Social Stratification in Polynesia, Seattle, The University of

Washington Press.

Sahlins, M. D. 1972. Stone Age Economics, Chicago, Aldine.

Sahlins, M., D 1960. Evolution: Specific and General. In: SAHLINS, M. D. & SERVCIE, E.

R. (eds.) Evolution and Culture. Ann Arbor: The University of Michigan Press.

Sand, C. 2002. Melanesian Tribes vs. Polynesian Chiefdoms: Recent Archaeological

Assessment of a Classic Model of Sociopolitical Types in Oceania. Asian

Perspectives, 41, 284-296.

Sand, C., Bolé, J. & Ouetcho, A. 2003. Prehistory and its perception in a Melanesian

Archipelago: the New Caledonia example. Antiquity, 77, 505-519.

Service, E. R. 1962. Primitive Social Organization, New York, Random House.

Page 33: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

33

Shennan, S. 2008. Evolution in Archaeology. Annual Review Of Anthropology, 37, 75-91.

Shennan, S. J., Crema, E. R. & Kerig, T. 2015. Isolation-by-distance, homophily, and “core”

vs. “package” cultural evolution models in Neolithic Europe. Evolution and Human

Behavior, 36, 103-109.

Sheppard, P. J. 2011. Lapita Colonization across the Near/Remote Oceania Boundary.

Current Anthropology, 52, 799-840.

Sheppard, P. J. & Walter, R. 2006. A Revised Model of Solomon Islands Culture History.

Journal of the Polynesian Society, 115, 47-76.

Simpson, D. F. J., Van Tilburg, J. A. & Dussubieux, L. 2018. Geochemical and radiometric

analyses of archaeological remains from Easter Island’s moai (statue) quarry reveal

prehistoric timing, provenance, and use of fine–grain basaltic resources. Journal of

Pacific Archaeology, 9, 12-34.

Smith, E. A. & Winterhalder, B. 1992. Natural Selection and Decision Making: Some

Fundamental Principles. In: SMITH, E. A. & WINTERHALDER, B. (eds.)

Evolutionary Ecology and Human Behavior. New York: Aldine De Gruyter.

Spencer, H. 1855. The Principles of Psychology, London, Longman, Brown, Green and

Longmans.

Spencer, H. 1857. Progress: its law and cause. Westminster Review, 67, 445-485.

Spencer, H. 1873. The study of sociology. No. XVI: Conclusion. Contemporary Review, 22,

663-677.

Steadman, D. W., Plourde, A. & Burley, D. V. 2002. Prehistoric Butchery and Consumption

of Birds in the Kingdom of Tonga, South Pacific. Journal of Archaeological Science,

29, 571-584.

Steponaitis, V. 1978. Location Theory and Complex Chiefdoms. In: SMITH, B. (ed.)

Mississippian Settlement Patterns. New York: Academic Press.

Page 34: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

34

Stevenson, C. 2002. Territorial Divisions on Easter Island in the 16th Century: Evidence

from the Distribution of Ceremonial Architecture. In: LADEFOGED, T. N. &

GRAVES, M. W. (eds.) Pacific Landscapes: Archaeological Approaches. Los Osos,

CA: Easter Island Foundation Press.

Steward, J. H. 1955. Theory of Cultural Change, Chicago, University of Illinois Press.

Tehrani, J. J., Collard, M. & Shennan, S. J. 2010. The cophylogeny of populations and

cultures: reconstructing the evolution of Iranian tribal craft traditions using trees and

jungles. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365,

3865-3874.

Terrell, J., Hunt, T. L. & Gosden, C. 1997. The Dimensions of Social Life in the Pacific:

Human Diversity and the Myth of the Primitive Isolate. Current Anthropology, 38,

155-195.

Tylor, E. B. 1870. Researches into the Early History of Mankind and the Development of

Civilizations, London, John Murray.

Valentin, F., Buckley, H. R., Herrscher, E., Kinaston, R., Bedford, S., Spriggs, M., Hawkins,

S. & Neal, K. 2010. Lapita subsistence strategies and food consumption patterns in

the community of Teouma (Efate, Vanuatu). Journal of Archaeological Science, 37,

1820-1829.

Wallin, P. & Martinsson-Wallin, H. 2007. Settlement Patterns - Social and ritual space in

prehistoric Samoa. Archaeology in Oceania, 42 (Supplement), 83-89.

Wallin, P. & Solsvik, R. 2010. Marae reflections: On the evolution of stratified chiefdoms in

the Leeward Society Islands. Archaeology in Oceania, 45.

Weisler, M. I., Bolhar, R., Ma, J., St Pierre, E., Sheppard, P. J., Walter, R. K., Feng, Y.,

Zhao, J.-x. & Kirch, P. V. 2016. Cook Island artifact geochemistry demonstrates

Page 35: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

35

spatial and temporal extent of pre-European interarchipelago voyaging in East

Polynesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 8150-8155.

Wengrow, D. & Graeber, D. 2015. Farewell to the ‘childhood of man’: ritual, seasonality,

and the origins of inequality. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 21, 597-

619.

White, L. A. 1959. The Evolution of Culture, New York, McGraw-Hill.

White, L. A. 1943. Energy and the Evolution of Culture. Amerian Anthropologist, 45, 335-

356.

Winter, O., Clark, G. R., Anderson, A. & Lindahl, A. 2012. Austronesian sailing to the

northern Marianas, a comment on Hung et al. (2011). Antiquity, 86, 898-914.

Winterhalder, B. & Smith, E. A. 2000. Analyzing adaptive strategies: Human behavioral

ecology at twenty-five. Evolutionary Anthropology, 9, 51-72.

Page 36: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

36

Figures and captions:

Figure 1. Comparison of Darwinian Evolutionary and Ladder-like Cultural Evolutionary

perspectives on change. Top figure, redrafted from Mesoudi (2011, Figure 2.1), demonstrates

the transformational approach to variation within types compared to the Darwinian approach

to variation within types. Bottom figure redrafted from Cochrane (2001, Figure 10.2)

demonstrates how distributions (exponential on left, monotonic on right) produced by

archaeologist-constructed cultural trait classes (“marae” and the finer-grained “marae class

1”) are the focus of explanation in evolutionary archaeology

Page 37: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

37

Figure 2. Political Economy explanations as systems of interrelated variables, redrafted from

Earle (1997, Figure 6.1).

Page 38: Theory in the Pacific, the Pacific in Theory

38

Figure 3: Near and Remote Oceania separated by the dashed line. Australia shown, but not

considered Near Oceania.