the referent of children's early songs

19
This article was downloaded by: [Ams/Girona*barri Lib] On: 27 November 2014, At: 00:26 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Music Education Research Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cmue20 The referent of children's early songs Esther Mang a Hong Kong Baptist University , Hong Kong Published online: 23 Jan 2007. To cite this article: Esther Mang (2005) The referent of children's early songs, Music Education Research, 7:1, 3-20, DOI: 10.1080/14613800500041796 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14613800500041796 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions

Upload: esther

Post on 30-Mar-2017

218 views

Category:

Documents


4 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The referent of children's early songs

This article was downloaded by: [Ams/Girona*barri Lib]On: 27 November 2014, At: 00:26Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Music Education ResearchPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cmue20

The referent of children's early songsEsther Manga Hong Kong Baptist University , Hong KongPublished online: 23 Jan 2007.

To cite this article: Esther Mang (2005) The referent of children's early songs, Music EducationResearch, 7:1, 3-20, DOI: 10.1080/14613800500041796

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14613800500041796

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to orarising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: The referent of children's early songs

The referent of children’s early songs

Esther Mang*Hong Kong Baptist University, Hong Kong

Musical creativity during early childhood is readily exemplified in vocal behaviours. This paper is a

discussion of observations on children’s performance of learned songs and self-generated songs.

Longitudinal observations suggest that self-generated songs may be seen as referent-guided

improvisation using source materials derived from learned songs. It is hypothesised that

interactions with the environment, such as a need for the child to communicate emotions in

play settings, could autonomously abstract novel sensory input into existing categories or distort

aspects of existing ones to assume novel parametric combinations. Hence, using learned songs as

referent, a child possesses a repertoire of learned musical features that allows her to create and

improvise as a reaction to environmental stimuli. The resultant early songs, therefore, exhibit a

distinctive sense of ownership endowed with rich creative instincts.

Introduction

Young children are often found actively engaged in vocalising, singing and moving to

musical stimuli. As early as 18 months, children may begin singing spontaneously,

and at around the age of 2 years, they may gradually incorporate snatches of learned

songs into their self-generated songs. Early songs of children are marked by an

improvisatory character and resemblance to variants of songs modelled after those

taught to them. Since the array of configurations in early songs is abundant, analyses

of the relationships between the self-generated songs and those that were taught to a

child could provide adults a glimpse of her musical world. Hence, observations of a

young child’s early songs may help deepen understanding of early childhood musical

creativity encoded in vocal behaviours. The present paper is an exploratory

discussion of some observed affiliation between young children’s learned song

achievement and the multiplicity of their self-generated song performance.

Speech, songs and vocalisations

Researchers have described different strains of early songs according to their musical

characteristics and the context in which they were observed. The pioneering study

conducted by Moorhead and Pond in 1941 reported spontaneous vocalisations that

were ‘unfettered and free rhythmically, like plainsong’ and another type of ‘chant’,

*Department of Music and Fine Arts, Hong Kong Baptist University, Kowloon Tong, Hong Kong.

Email: [email protected]

ISSN 1461-3808 (print)/ISSN 1469-9893 (online)/05/010003-18

# 2005 Taylor & Francis Group Ltd

DOI: 10.1080/14613800500041796

Music Education ResearchVol. 7, No. 1, March 2005, pp. 3�/20

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 3: The referent of children's early songs

which was ‘rhythmic, of limited compass and repetitious’ (1978, p. 8). According to

the researchers, those spontaneous songs of young children were often narrow in

range and not diatonic; they were usually uttered in undulating contours without a

definite tonal centre; and had flexible but occasionally patterned rhythm. Because

those songs are not confined to the Western musical idiom, Moorhead and Pond

described them as unpredictable and unmemorable. It should be noted that the

researchers excluded ‘fragmentary songs’ in their description of spontaneous songs

(Moorhead & Pond, 1978, p. 9).

A similar emphasis in rhythmic interest was reported by Fujita (1990), who

identified a prevalent form of spontaneous vocalisation as ‘intermediate performance

between talking and singing’, in which the phrase or words were uttered metrically,

or melodically with some fixed pitch (p. 146). As found in previous studies,

rhythmical talking was observed during free play, produced when children wished

to communicate with others (ages 4 or 5), while 2�3-year-olds talked rhythmically

for their own satisfaction rather than for communication (p. 143). Fujita pointed out

that much of the rhythmical talking was accompanied with movement and bore a

close affiliation to the inflection of the Japanese language and its culture as a whole.

Fujita maintained that ‘the children refined their musical performance by them-

selves, conforming to the form of musical expression, and never created a musical

performance which exceed their cultural context’ (p. 142).

Contrary to the emphasis on rhythmic interest as reported in the literature, Ries

(1987) reported that the tonal centre was a prominent feature in the spontaneous

songs that she observed and collected. Ries regarded spontaneous songs as ‘bits of

singing that stood out from the surrounding material because of their cohesiveness

and usually slightly greater length. These renditions were consistent in mood and

always maintained a tonal center, even at 7 months. They seemed to be more than

just a reply to the stimulus or prompt and appear to be a special musical effort’

(p. 15).

Moog (1976) provides an extensive analysis of spontaneous songs as they emerge

at different age level during childhood. At ages 2�4, spontaneous singing dominated

the musical activities of children. Moog identified three different forms*/

‘imaginative’, ‘narrative’ and ‘pot-pourri’ songs. Imaginative songs form only a small

repertoire of spontaneous songs ; they bore no resemblance to a known song, and were

often hummed or sung to a single syllable. Narrative songs seemed not to be intended

for an audience, although they often told stories. They were sung monologues

composed mainly of nonsense snatches of words and tunes, and occasionally

included excerpts of learned songs. Pot-pourri songs were combinations of learned

songs with mixed up words and melodies, and they might contain original

improvisations.

It is noteworthy that Moog observed that children adapted the same formal design

from their learned songs into songs that they created. Interestingly, for children who

did not demonstrate singing any learned songs in the study, no original song was

observed.

4 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 4: The referent of children's early songs

Young (2002) documented children singing during free-play and analysed

spontaneous vocalisation according to its context and acoustical characteristics.

Young observed that some known songs resurfaced in child’s free-flowing singing as

discrete fragments in dramatic play. This form of vocalisation was labelled as

reworkings of known songs, which among her six categories of spontaneous vocalisa-

tion, relates most closely to Moog’s conception of spontaneous songs. Similarly, the

contexts of the spontaneous vocalisations collected by Young were dominated by

vocal play triggered by verbalisations in role-play situations.

Spontaneous songs are also regarded as a key building block in song acquisition.

Davidson et al . (1981) proposed that a spontaneous song transforms musical events

from the environment (assimilation) to fit into a scheme, made up of the properties

of songs in the culture, of which the child has already developed (accommodation)

(p. 303). As the researchers explained:

In many child-versions of standard songs, those bits that most closely approximate the

model appear at the same time as identical melodic structures in the child’s spontaneous

repertoire . . . Now the child can pick and choose from among his repertoire of fragments

and bit those most appropriate to a certain spot in a certain song. This ability to handle

one’s own repertoire with increasing flexibility so that it can lead to the production a

number of standard tunes leads us to characterize the child’s competence by the age of

three or so*/as the mastery of the outline song. The capacity to master a number of basic

fragments, and then to compose them into a reasonable approximation of a target

model, is by no means restricted to the child’s progress in the musical sphere. Indeed,

this kind of ‘outline knowledge’ of a symbolic domain seems quite characteristics of the

child around the age of three. (p. 306)

The abundant documentation of young children’s early songs brings about an array

of diverse viewpoints. In summary, researchers have consistently raised issues such as

singing versus speaking, and performance of learned songs versus self-generated,

improvisatory songs.

Data collected form longitudinal observations suggest that young children’s

speech and song acquisition are developmental behaviours that exhibit phases of

mutations. Mang (2001) proposed that young children undergo a three-stage

development, whereby children under 2 years old demonstrate acoustical intermediate

vocalisations , which facilitate their initial acquisition of vocal control. By the age of

2 years, children are capable of making productive distinctions between singing and

speaking, and by 3½ years, children may use novel forms of vocalisations freely to

facilitate expression. This form of vocalisation is observed after children had

mastered the skill needed to perform songs accurately and communicate effectively

in speech but purposefully alternate easily between singing and speaking to

communicate in novel forms of contextual intermediate vocalisation (Mang, 2002).

Referent in improvisation

The present paper is a discussion of the forms of early songs collected longitudinally

between the ages of 2 and 4 when children already possess a conception of song and

The referent of children’s early songs 5

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 5: The referent of children's early songs

begin to acquire fundamental singing skill. Published findings concur that some early

songs at this age level appear to be distant mutations of song material familiar to the

child; others are noticeably reproductions of learned songs with impetuous

adjustments. Following this line of inquiry, it seems pertinent to delineate possible

relationship between young children’s learned and self-generated songs. One possible

approach to examining early songs is to contemplate the notion of ‘referent’ in

improvisation. Pressing (1984) explains that a ‘referent’ has direct ramifications for

the resultant improvisation.

Central to improvisation is the notion of the ‘referent’. The referent is an underlying

formal scheme or guiding image specific to a given piece, used by the improviser to

facilitate the generation and editing of improvised behaviour on an intermediate time

scale. The generation of behaviour on a fast time scale is primarily determined by

previous training and is not very piece-specific. If no referent is present, or if it is devised

in real-time, we speak of ‘free’ or ‘absolute’ improvisation. This is much rarer than

referent-guided, or ‘relative’ improvisation. (p. 346)

In the case of children’s self-generated songs, their referent is often learned songs.

Key features of a learned song, such as repeated lyrics or melodic motif, are often

reconfigured and integrated into another song. As Pressing (1984) explains, ‘the

referent typically functions either as a source for material, which is then repeated,

transformed, varied or developed, or as a focus for the production and organization

of material from other sources’ (p. 347).

Hence, children’s self-generated songs could be seen as referent-guided improvisa-

tions because the source materials are often derived from learned songs. According to

the Pressing (1988) model, the source materials (i.e. in Pressing’s terminology,

object, features and process array types) are basically fixed, but innate ecological

capacity could autonomously abstract novel sensory input into existing categories or

distort aspects of existing ones to assume novel parametric combinations. Hence,

interaction with the environment could result in aggregation of memory constituents

to extract or create new cognitive assemblages. In the case of children’s early songs,

a strong affiliation between learned songs and self-generated songs might be

inferred from Pressing’s model (Figure 1). Since a child could manipulate the

source material by freely combining selected aspects of different learned songs, early

songs display different levels of novelty. Hence, researchers use labels such as

‘narrative song’, ‘musical monologue’, ‘pot-pourri song’, ‘imaginative song’ and

‘private song’, to depict the multiplicity of children’s early songs (Moog, 1976;

Papousek & Papousek, 1981). Applying the principals of the Pressing (1988) model,

the array of self-generated songs might well illustrate the richness of novel sensory

inputs.

The purpose of this article is to report and discuss data concerning children’s early

songs collected in a longitudinal study on young children’s vocal development.

Specifically, it explores the notion of children’s use of learned songs as the referent

for their self-generated songs. Since self-generated, improvisatory singing responses

are most prominently observed between ages 2 and 4, findings and discussions are

6 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 6: The referent of children's early songs

limited to this age phase, although more comprehensive investigation was conducted

in the original study. This article focuses on the examination of musical and

singing achievement exhibited in the performance of learned songs and analysis of

how children adapted this to self-generated songs. It is therefore inferred that

analysing the performance of learned and self-generated songs will facilitate

understanding of early childhood musical creativity as it is encoded in vocal

behaviours.

Method

The sample and subjects

Subjects were recruited through already established personal contacts. The sample

consisted of only girls because no parent with a male child expressed interest in the

study. Seven of the eight girl participants were aged 2�4 years at the start of the

study. (See Table 1 for more information about the subjects.) Six of the subjects had

had some informal musical training with the investigator before the study

commenced. Throughout the 42 months of the study, contact was maintained

with the parents to update them on progress made by each child and to obtain more

in-depth views about consistency and changes their child’s vocal behaviours.

Ongoing confirmation and synthesis of data allowed the investigator to compare

and corroborate observations of each child.

To obtain information about the type of songs the children had acquired, they were

asked to sing their favourite songs and games. It seemed that all children had

acquired traditional nursery rhymes and popular North American children’s songs

such as: Eency Weency Spider; The Wheels on the Bus; Bingo; It’s Raining, It’s Pouring;

Pop Goes the Weasel; and This Old Man . It was not clear whether children had

acquired these songs at their daycare centres or elsewhere; nevertheless, their song

performances could provide an overview of their repertoires.

“Referent”

Decompose andreconfigure

key features oflearned songs

Learned Songs

Songs recallingpreviouslyintroduced songs

Referent-guided Self-generated Songs

(e.g., “narrative song”,“musical monologue”,“pot-pourri song”,“imaginative song”,“private song”)

Novel sensory input

Novel sensory input

Figure 1. Affiliation between learned songs and self-generated songs

The referent of children’s early songs 7

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 7: The referent of children's early songs

The role of the researcher

Six of the subjects had attended a music programme offered by the investigator for

eight or 16 45-minute sessions before the study commenced. These children had

joined the programme at some time between the ages of 18 and 20 months. They

were invited to participate in the study only after the child had left the music

programme. The types of musical experiences those children had, before the study

commenced, included movement, listening and playing instruments.

For the two other subjects who did not attend the music programme, the

investigator was a family friend. For the present study, the investigator assumed the

role of an observer-participant and was treated as a visiting family friend.

Data collection

Each child was visited every 4�6 months over 42 months. A typical home visit

consisted of approximately 45 minutes to tape the child speaking and singing, and at

least 15 minutes of an informal conversation interview with the parents.

All data collection took place at each subject’s home. Whenever possible, the child

was asked to sit on the carpet in her bedroom where she was surrounded by familiar

things to ensure that interaction could take place in a comfortable manner.

Sometimes, data were collected in the living room, where the child would be asked

to bring along some favourite toys and books.

The parents sometimes sat beside or within several feet of the child to show

support. The mothers would typically assume the role of an observer at the beginning

of the recording session, but would soon join as a participant. Other parents

participated actively throughout the recording session, encouraging their children to

sing and speak. Explanations and reminders were given to parents to avoid

prompting the child to sing.

Table 1. Some basic information about the subjects

Name of subject Age (at start of the study) First spoken language Place of birth

Heidi 18 months Mandarin (English) Taiwan

Clare 25 months English Canada

Vicki 27 months Cantonese (English) Hong Kong

Jodi 29 months Mandarin (English) Canada

Wendy 31 months Cantonese (English) Hong Kong

Amber 33 months English Canada

Gina 34 months English Canada

Polly 38 months English (Spanish)a Peru

The language given in parentheses is the secondary language spoken by a child at home.aBoth of her parents are native speakers of Spanish from Peru. English is the primary language

spoken at home, as the mother explained, because the child became more reluctant to

communicate in Spanish after they moved to Canada when she was 2 years old.

8 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 8: The referent of children's early songs

Vocal responses were recorded during child�adult interactions and in various

play settings. During each visit, the children were asked to sing favourite songs,

regardless of the language they preferred. Seldom would a child refuse to sing, but

would at times find it difficult to choose a song without assistance. It appeared more

natural to first engage the child in a conversation, then have the parents gradually

encourage the child to ‘teach me’ her favourite song by performing it. All vocal

responses were recorded with a portable professional standard cassette recorder

(Marantz PMD 201) using a unidirectional professional microphone (Shure, SM48-

LC). ‘Digital accuracy’ cassette tapes (TDK SA-X) were used to store the vocal

signals.

Descriptive notes were written at the end of each visit to record the setting and the

observed activities. The following observations were made on the context of the

children’s vocalisations: (i) the source of stimulus (a storybook, an invitation to sing,

etc.) versus the type of vocal response; (ii) other corresponding behaviours (body

movements, use of toys, etc.) observed when the vocalisations occurred; (iii) the

child’s communication intention as inferred from the scenario when the vocalisation

occurred (items i and ii) or verification with the child; and (iv) the parents’

interpretation of the vocal responses.

I usually chatted with the parents after each recording session to facilitate data

interpretation. Since a more literal description would provide a relatively precise

account of the observations, some of the conversations were also taped to enable

citing the exact wording used by parents.

Data analyses

Three judges, who were graduates of music and had vocal training, analysed the

singing responses. Copies of the recorded singing responses were given to each judge,

allowing the tapes to be replayed as many times as needed. The judges were asked to

categorise singing responses into learned song performance, self-generated songs, or

other novel forms of vocalisations.

The judges then provided perceptual analyses of the learned songs to evaluate the

child’s musical and singing competence. Analysis of children’s achievement in

learned song is important because memory serves as a repository for the referent.

Hence, skill achievement of the child as she demonstrated in recalling familiar songs,

could be taken as the readily available resources that were at the disposal of the child

for self-generated songs.

The judges were also asked to examine how the child used learned songs as

referent to organise elemental structures of her early songs. Hence, musical elements

such as cadential material, melodic and rhythmic motifs, repetition and contrast, and

arrangement of words in early songs were the key features under scrutiny. The

following is a discussion of the best examples that captured the typical behaviours

observed in the age levels concerned.

The referent of children’s early songs 9

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 9: The referent of children's early songs

Findings

Ages 2�3

Performance of learned songs. At the 2�3-year level, about one-third of the children’s

song repertoire seemed to be modelled after songs previously introduced to them.

However, those songs were seldom rendered in their complete form and omission of

parts was frequent. The words of the songs were also simplified; some key words of

the song remained intact but other words were freely replaced with nonsense

syllables or words that appeared to fit the sound. Frequently, only short segments of

melody were intact, while other parts of the song were, according to the child,

forgotten. However, it appeared that the child had forgotten only the words but not

the overall musical structure of the song.

The relationships between performance of words and melody were particularly

interesting. When a child said that she had forgotten how to sing a song, she was

often able to sing the learned song accurately after being prompted with the words.

However, a child often failed to ‘remember’ a song even after being prompted with

the melody alone on monosyllables such as ‘doo’, or simply hummed the melody.

This suggests that the words (or specifically, the sounds that made up the words

because they could understand very little about the meaning of the words) appeared

to operate as an ‘anchor’ that secured the musical elements of a song. Hence, when

words were forgotten, some children considered that the song as a whole was

forgotten. Nevertheless, some children at this age were able to render a learned song

without perfect recall of all the words. For example, when Wendy performed a

learned song, she managed to accurately render all the words in the first verse, but

failed to sing the melody accurately. After singing the first verse, she paused briefly

before continuing with the second verse; that is, she appeared to have forgotten the

latter part of the words in the second verse. However, without noticeable

interruption, she substituted nonsense syllables, ‘da-dee-da-dee’ for the forgotten

portion, and continued to perform the song. Evidence suggests that although a child

at the 2�3 level possesses the ability to perform a learned song without accurate recall

of all the words, she may sometimes consider the entire song forgotten when only

some of the words are forgotten.

The word meanings of a learned song often appeared to be more attractive to a

child when sung with body movement. For example, when Clare was invited to sing

an action song, Teddy Bear, she acted out all the body movements corresponding to

the correct sequence of the song but did not attempt to sing the song at any time.

Since the whole song was performed as if it was a ‘silent movie’, the body movements

were acted out in their correct sequence, possibly because Clare was mentally

rehearsing the song from the beginning to the end. When Clare performed Teddy

Bear , I also noticed that her movements were acted out in accordance with the

appropriate melodic rhythm of the words. Hence, Clare did not simply remember the

content of the song, she acted it out with body movements; that is, she ‘expressed’

the song without actually singing it aloud. However, this is a complex example of

dissociation, the ramifications of which are not explored in this paper.

10 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 10: The referent of children's early songs

The learned song was sometimes expressed through tactile experience. For

example, just shortly after Clare began singing Mary had a Little Lamb , she stopped

abruptly and walked away to get her toys. She appeared to be anxious and asked her

mother to help her find ‘Mary’ and ‘Mary’s lamb’. With the mother’s assistance,

Clare found these two characters and returned to the sofa with her toys. She

completed singing the first verse of the song accurately and then attempted to sing

the second verse. However, she was soon absorbed in manipulating ‘Mary’ and her

‘lamb’ to act out the rest of the song and never completed singing the second verse. It

therefore appears possible that for a young child, singing may not be an isolated form

of experience that involves only vocal production; singing is sometimes integrated

with, or even contrasted from other forms of expression such as body movement and

imaginative play.

Performance of self-generated songs. Early songs by children at the 2�3 level took on a

range of different configurations such as ‘pot-pourri songs’ and ‘vocal babbling’ as

defined by Moog (1976). The children appeared to take pleasure in using their voices

to manipulate learned materials and explore new resources for sounds. For a ‘pot-

pourri’ song, it was often difficult to differentiate whether a child was intentionally

manipulating the musical materials from more than one song or if it was simply a

case of confusion over two songs. This difficulty is illustrated in the following

scenarios.

Scenario 1: At age 2 years 3 months, Clare sang the first phrase of Bingo but

shifted to sing the first phrase of Old McDonald without a pause. After that, she

hesitated for a while as though she had forgotten something or made a mistake. Clare

paused for a moment but did not resume singing either of the two songs. It is possible

that there was some confusion over the two songs, i.e. she ambiguously connected

the final word ‘Oh’, from the first phrase of Bingo to the first word ‘Old’, as in Old

McDonald . This interesting scenario suggests that words often serve as an anchor

securing the musical elements of a learned song.

Scenario 2: At age 2 years 8 months, Wendy sang the first three phrases of Happy

Birthday in English. She was later distracted from the words when pretending to

blow out candles, consequently she did not sing the last phrase of the song. However,

since all judges agreed that the first three phrases were sung in tune, it appeared that

Wendy knew the song well. Wendy then continued to recite a Cantonese nursery

rhyme and sang two Cantonese songs. While Wendy sang the second song, It’s a

Small World (a version with words translated into Cantonese), she paused after

singing the first phrase when she noticed her father had just returned home. Wendy

subsequently continued to sing but modified the words to fit into the melody of

Happy Birthday. She then gradually returned to the melody of It’s a Small World and

continued to sing the refrain. Thereafter, although the tonality was successfully

maintained, fragments of the two songs were chained together. It is not clear whether

Wendy was confused over the melodies of the two songs, whether she was

intentionally mixing the two learned songs , or whether she had forgotten some of

the words of both songs. This ‘pot-pourri’ song demonstrates how a child might

The referent of children’s early songs 11

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 11: The referent of children's early songs

combine melodic material from two learned songs. Since earlier in the same

recording session, Wendy sang almost the entire Happy Birthday song accurately and

in tune, it is unlikely that she was confusing the two songs. Because the two songs

were sung in two different languages (Happy Birthday in English and It’s a Small

World in Cantonese), I speculate that it is unlikely that Wendy confused the two

songs as one.

In summary, inspirations for most of the self-generated songs recorded from

children at the 2�3 level appeared to stem from fragments of learned songs. Children

usually began singing a learned song with repetition of a single syllable or nonsense

words before they sang the rest of the song. Reference to the diatonic system and

metrical rhythm would often loosen when a spontaneous early song began.

Ages 3�4

Performance of learned songs. There was a marked increase in the number of learned

songs collected from children at the 3�4 level. Roughly half of the singing responses

collected in this age level were attempts to reproduce familiar songs that the children

had acquired. Some of the favourite learned songs collected from all children in this

age level were: Bingo; Teddy Bear; Twinkle, Twinkle, Little Star; Old McDonald; I Love

You; Jingle Bells; and The Alphabet Song . When compared with data collected earlier,

most children at this age level were able to sing phrases of learned songs with greater

accuracy in melodic and rhythmic patterns. The general contours of the repetitive

patterns in the songs, as well as most of the narrower melodic intervals were often

sung correctly. Judges were therefore able to identify successfully almost all learned

songs sung by the children, even when they were incomplete.

Maintaining tonality of a song was a problem for all children. Even when able to

demonstrate a sense of tonality at the beginning of a song they often changed the

tonality several times within a song. In traditional Western music, most melodies

contain a clear reference to a defined tonal centre. That children modulated during

the course of singing a song even after they had established a tonal centre, suggests

that they had yet to learn how to retain the primacy of an established key as a

function of musical memory.

The words of learned songs appeared to be a challenge for children at the 3�4

level. They sometimes expressed frustration about forgotten words, which made it

difficult for them to complete a song. Even when a child could successfully recall a

melody but not the words, she would frequently give up singing the song. At this age

level, only occasionally would a child substitute forgotten words with nonsense words

or syllables, and continue singing. Children at this age level often failed to produce

the precise structure of learned songs, thus the order of verse and chorus was

frequently confused.

At age 3 years 4 months, Gina wanted to sing The Whale Song . She began by

humming some melodic fragments of the song. Unfortunately, she failed to recall any

of the words and soon gave up singing the song. Similarly, at age 3 years 6 months,

12 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 12: The referent of children's early songs

Wendy sang the first phrase of a learned song but forgot the rest of the words. She

then repeated the first phrase three times before she eventually gave up singing.

When a child claimed that she forgot the words of a learned song, melodic

accuracy suffered. At 3 years 10 months, Gina successfully sang the first verse of

Baby Beluga in tune. However, she appeared to have forgotten some of the words in

the second verse, from which she sang fragments, and then hummed the melody as

she tried to recall the words she was missing. The humming later faded away as she

tried harder to recall the words. Eventually, she chanted fragments of the words

without attempting to sing the melody. It appeared that Gina had shifted her

attention to recalling the words to such an extent that the melody could no longer be

recalled accurately. This issue is addressed in studies on text�melody integration in

melody recall (see, e.g., Serafine et al ., 1984, 1986).

Performance of self-generated songs. Scenario 1: Heidi’s favourite activity was

drawing. She had an easel, newsprint paper and crayons in her room, so that she

could draw whenever she wanted.

After Heidi’s mother showed me some of her daughter’s drawings displayed on the

walls, Heidi was enthusiastic and said she could draw some eggs for me. She walked

over to the easel, picked up a crayon and announced a title for her drawing. With a

loud and clearly projected voice, Heidi enunciated ‘EGGS. There is a nest . . .’ She

then drew an egg in the centre of the newsprint paper, and sang, ‘and inside the nest,

there is an egg.’ Heidi chose another crayon and drew a nest around the egg. She then

sang the final two phrases of a learned song, The Green Grass Grew All Around .

Heidi moved on to draw a second egg, using another colour. As she drew, she said,

‘There was (were) two eggs, sitting in a nest’. Repeating what she had done before,

she then sang the final two phrases of The Green Grass Grew All Around . The ‘drawing

narrative’ now took on a strophic form exemplified by a recurring pattern of

organisation in Heidi’s speaking and singing. Although Heidi spoke and sang as she

drew, she appeared to be fully absorbed in the drawing activity. Hence, speaking and

singing were observed to be fully integrated during the ‘drawing narrative’.

More detailed descriptions of the activities observed in Heidi’s ‘drawing narrative’

may further illustrate how speaking and singing were integrated. (i) Heidi was

constantly making choices about what she wanted to draw, and how she would

achieve her objectives. Not only did she pause for a while in between the verses to

painstakingly choose different colours for her eggs, she expressed frustration when

she could not find the specific colour she wanted and asked her mother for

assistance. She complained that she could not find the right hue among several

crayons within the same colour range, and even asked for a felt tip pen in order to get

the exact colour she had in mind. (ii) Heidi was telling a story as she drew. She

paused occasionally to look at her mother and me, as if she was trying to establish

some eye contact with her audience. Heidi also raised the volume of her voice and

spoke slowly in a clear manner. Heidi’s mother explained that her daughter

frequently made up stories for her drawings. Even when Heidi was drawing alone

in the room, she would raise her voice and tell a story as she drew. (iii) Heidi was

The referent of children’s early songs 13

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 13: The referent of children's early songs

singing as she drew. It is interesting to see how a 3½-year-old child adapted a learned

song, The Green Grass Grew All Around, as the basis for her ‘drawing narrative’.

Heidi used only the beginning and the ending phrases of the song, omitting the

more complicated middle section. To simplify even further, Heidi spoke the first

phrase but sang the ending. Each verse was given a title sentence, ‘There was (were)

___ eggs’, and followed the same recurring pattern. Heidi ended the ‘drawing

narrative’ after she drew five eggs and could not find any room for more eggs in

the nest.

Scenario 2: ‘Pot-pourri’ songs were abundant among children’s self-generated

singing. For example, at age 3 years 5 months, Heidi sang Jingle Bells spontaneously

but also attempted to fit in fragments of Old McDonald . Nevertheless, she managed

to make her way back to Jingle Bells and rendered the song in an arrangement of

verse�chorus�verse. In common practice, Jingle Bells is sung with the chorus section

as the conclusion of the song. It appeared that Heidi deliberately ended the song with

a verse section because she added a sprightly ‘Hey!’ to conclude the song. As

mentioned earlier in the discussion of ‘pot-pourri’ song, it was often difficult to

discriminate a ‘pot-pourri’ song from a mix of two melodies. Heidi clearly

demonstrated a thorough understanding of the structure of Jingle Bells because she

was able to perform both the verse and the chorus sections and achieve a sense of

unity and contrast. Hence, her attempt to integrate Old McDonald into Jingle Bells

appeared to be her experiment with manipulating learned song materials.

Songs collected at the 3�4 level showed a strong emphasis on the manipulation of

musical elements such as rhythmic patterns, scalic structure and melodic contour

(ascending and descending, and parallel and contrary motion). Songs at this age level

might be seen as musical exercises, which appear to play an important role in

assimilating culturally relevant musical practices.

Some game-like songs were collected from Amber when she was 3 years 1 month.

About 20 minutes into the recording session, she showed me her toy phone and then

shifted her attention to a toy xylophone. Using a mallet, she first played several notes

and then sang ‘doh-re-mi-fah-soh’ while playing the xylophone. Although she sang

the scale pattern one beat per syllable, her singing did not follow the pitches she

played on the instrument; i.e. the pitch movement of Amber’s singing moved in a

different direction from the scalic passage she was playing on the xylophone. From an

adult’s perspective, the choice of a parallel motion would appear to be the initial step

in an attempt to match the singing with an instrument, but to play and sing scalic

passages in contrary motion would appear to require much more sophisticated

perceptual and performance skills.

After that initial attempt to sing while playing the toy xylophone, Amber then

moved on to develop more rhythmic variety in her game-like song. At this point, the

xylophone playing and the singing were no longer synchronised. The song was

largely sung with nonsense words, which included a repetitive phrase, ‘a la-dy Oh!’,

while the earlier ascending and descending scalic pattern recurred only in the

xylophone part.

14 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 14: The referent of children's early songs

There was a brief diversion when Amber suddenly went to pick up her toy phone

for an imaginative play. She talked to her ‘sister’ on the phone, and said, ‘Hello

sister, . . . I’ll meet you at the ball game.’ Immediately after she hung up the phone,

Amber returned to the spontaneous song she created just a short moment ago. The ‘a

la-dy Oh!’ motive was performed in a chant-like manner with strong accent and a

clearly metrical pattern. Although the spontaneous song retained the ‘a la-dy Oh’

motive, the xylophone no longer accompanied it. The song then took on a playful

character. Amber took my hands and led me in a dance while we swung our arms and

skipped around in circular motion. At this point, she extended the repetitive ‘a la-dy

Oh!’ sequence by adding ‘and all fall down’. This game reminded me of the singing

game Ring A-Ring A Rosy. (Amber was familiar with this singing game, which she

had learned at the daycare centre.) The chanting soon became faster and faster, and

Amber even pretended to fall down onto the floor from time to time. Amber became

more excited as she sang, and the game went on until Amber was exhausted. Since

The Sound of Music was one of Amber’s favourite movies, it is speculated that the

source of this ‘a la-dy Oh!’ motive was from the song The Lonely Goatherd . In other

words, Amber was possibly yodelling.

Conclusions

To summarise, the children at age level 2�3 mostly performed incomplete renditions

of learned songs with words freely substituted and nonsense syllables in abundance.

When the words of a learned song were forgotten, a child failed to recall the melody.

In a typical learned song performed by a child at this age level, only fragments of a

melody were intact, other parts of the song had melodic and rhythmic patterns

incorrectly sung. Some children in the study recalled a song through body

movements or imaginative play rather than singing it aloud. Many self-generated

songs were observed, especially ‘pot-pourri’ songs combining fragments of learned

songs and improvised songs, sometimes regardless of different languages. Children

were also observed to make less reference to diatonic system and metrical rhythm in

their self-generated songs than in their learned songs.

A marked increase of learned songs was found in all children at age level 3�4; the

melodic and rhythmic patterns were sung mostly correctly. Although children

demonstrated a growing sense of tonality at the beginning of a song, they frequently

moved through several modulations within a song. However, the entire song was

often considered forgotten when only the words were forgotten. In some cases,

nonsense syllables were used to fill in missing words but the precise structure of

learned songs was not recalled accurately even when all other aspects of the song

were sung accurately. For self-generated singing, ‘pot-pourri’ songs and other novel

form of singing were abundant. These self-generated songs consisted of integrated

narration and adaptation of learned song, and were sometimes improvised with

movements and musical instruments, or resembled singing games and other play

activity such as drawing.

The referent of children’s early songs 15

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 15: The referent of children's early songs

Observations from this study confirm that words present the greatest challenge to

younger children when they are at the initial stage of learning to sing. The sounds of

words often operate as an anchor to the musical elements, especially the melody and

the formal structure of the song. Thus, a large proportion of configurations of

learned songs might be, in fact, a spin-off of efforts to recall the words of learned

songs. The meaning of words, on the other hand, contribute as resources for

imaginative play, whereby children often move into activities such as role play and

movement to express the storyline of the song. Maintenance of tonality still presents

a great deal of challenge, even after the children had acquired a superior level of

melodic accuracy.

A comparison between performance of learned and self-generated songs suggests

that some melodic features were produced more abundantly in songs learned from

adult models, whereas others were produced more abundantly in self-generated

songs. Figure 2 illustrates some melodic features observed in children’s early songs.

The following is a discussion of some possible explanations of the differences.

It seems likely that the cognitive load involved in performing a learned song will be

different from that for a self-generated song. Performing a learned song requires a

child to recall musical material according to a previously introduced model, which

would place a demand on good self-monitoring skills. Such learned vocal behaviours

might also be bounded by cultural conventions, which a child would be expected to

have mastered in order to reproduce the song. Self-generated songs, on the other

hand, are initiated by a child. When a child is not required to reproduce according to

a model, it is speculated that the self-monitoring mechanism could be comparatively

relaxed because the musical features become source material rather than reproduc-

tion target; the child’s aesthetic judgment would seem to be a more important

component when singing spontaneously. Pressing (1998) proposes that, with referent

as material for variation, less processing capacity (attention) is needed in the

improvised performance. Hence, it is speculated that a processing reduction might

Learned Songs Self-generated SongsMelodic features observedin children’s singing Age levels (in years)

2–3 3–4 2–3 3–4Repetitive melodic patternsMelodic contourDescending contour N/A N/AMelodic phrasingDiatonic scale structureMaintenance of tonalityAccurate intervalsTransposition of melody

Abilities not observed in any of the childrenAbilities demonstrated by some of the childrenAbilities demonstrated by all the children

N/A Not applicable (learned songs contain essentially both ascending and descending contour)

Figure 2. Melodic features observed in children’s learned songs and self-generated songs

16 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 16: The referent of children's early songs

result in a relatively inferior melodic performance in self-generated songs when

compared to performance of learned songs. However, Pressing points out that a

processing reduction could free up cognitive load for a child to engage in a higher

degree of integration among different modes of activities (such as the singing and

drawing activity observed in Heidi at age 3) and to enhance the expression of the

communication intent (such as the self-generated singing game observed in Amber at

age 3). Observations from the present investigation suggest that through using

learned songs as referent, a child would vary the musical features of a familiar song to

create and improvise according to their imagination. Hence, the resultant early songs

exhibit a distinctive sense of ownership and rich creative instinct.

Implications

Based on the preceding discussion on observations of young children’s early songs,

the following suggestions aim to enhance singing experience in childhood.

(a) Vocal model. Because children assimilate familiar acoustic structures from their

auditory environment, a good vocal model may facilitate the development of singing

(Goetze et al ., 1990; Green, 1990; Mang, 1997). It is hypothesised specifically that

vocal models, which demonstrate proper vocal control over such parameters as pitch,

volume, timbre, breath and articulations, are advantageous for children learning to

sing.

(b) Singing in monosyllables. Although the words of a song often appear to attract

the initial interest of younger children, evidence from research findings suggests that

young children sing more accurately in monosyllables (Levinowitz, 1989; Goetze et

al ., 1990). To assist older children to acquire more sophisticated melodic features,

songs could sometimes be presented in monosyllables. Some music educators

advocate the use of ‘chinning’ to heighten the musical characteristics of a song and to

facilitate learning musical elements while singing (Richards, 1980; Richards &

Langness, 1982; Bennett & Bartholomew, 1997).

(c) Preserving tonality. Children master the maintenance of tonality in singing at a

relatively later age. In fact, it was reported to be a problem even in school age

children (Goetze et al ., 1990; Green, 1990). At the initial stage of learning to

establish a sense of tonality, children often demonstrate the ability in familiar songs

but not in unfamiliar songs. For this reason, experience with singing a familiar song

using different tonal centres may assist children to conserve the relative tonal

relationships in a song. It may also provide children with abundant stimuli generated

from the same archetype of melodic patterns in order that they may advance

progressively and learn to maintain tonality in less familiar songs. Singing a familiar

song in different tonal centres also provides opportunities to extend the vocal

registers of young children.

The referent of children’s early songs 17

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 17: The referent of children's early songs

(d) Singing as a movement experience. Children were sometimes observed to

voluntarily integrate singing with body movements. A child usually begins by singing

a song, gradually adding body movements. Since movements appear to elevate a

child’s involvement in sensory experience, she may become too absorbed in moving

to continue singing. In other words, my data suggests that body movements may

become the performance mode of a song, whilst singing the melody and the words

may assume a secondary role. Bruner (1960, 1966) describes the initial mode of

knowledge representation as ‘enactive’. Such a mode is found when a child

demonstrates concept acquisition by engaging in overt behaviours in response to a

stimulus. Here, externalising a song in body movement depicts how musical

experience for a child may be in a ‘movement’ or ‘enactive’ mode, which holds the

musical elements of a song together even when it is not sung aloud (Bennett &

Bartholomew, 1997). In Pressing’s model of improvisation, the integration of singing

and movement is seen as novel parametric combinations (Pressing, 1988).

Connecting movements with singing can therefore be beneficial for young children,

especially during the developmental stage when they connect more easily with tactile

experience. Some music teaching approaches advocate movement activities. For

example, the Orff�Schulwerk approach encourages ‘translating body movement into

percussion instrument’ and the Kodaly approach combines vocal and movement

activities. Both approaches are widely used in elementary classrooms and in music

therapy settings.

(e) Singing as playing. Playing is an important component in a child’s singing

activities. Children in the present study displayed a rich spectrum of musical and

linguistic behaviours when they played. In fact, all self-generated songs were

collected when they were immersed in playing. Evidence suggests that the high level

of social interaction in a play context may have induced the need to express and

communicate. Again, with reference to Pressing’s model of improvisation (1988), a

high level of novel sensory input during play could facilitate the extraction or creation

of new cognitive assembles.

How can a singing activity be initiated as playing? Singing games are particularly

effective in engaging children and thus provide excellent settings for learning. A body

of research has been accumulated on the singing games of children in different

cultures (Prim, 1989; Riddell, 1990; Addo, 1995; Blacking, 1995; Marsh, 1997).

Hence, singing games can be employed as a valuable foundation for musical

experience in childhood.

Were children in the present study representative of others at their age level? This

question can perhaps, be more aptly answered through further experimental studies,

which exercise physical or statistical control of variables and measurement to

maximise the validity of findings. My investigation sought to document and interpret

broad observable patterns in early songs of young children, the complexity of which

could not be captured through unmediated knowledge about the contexts.

Qualitative inquirers maintain that ethnography aims to study specific cases rather

18 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 18: The referent of children's early songs

than to develop theoretical knowledge. Hence, ethnography is a training of the

capacity for perception to enhance practical wisdom (Schwandt, 1997, pp. 57�60).

Thus, for the purpose of the present study, it may be more practical to select a small

group of children for a more in depth ethnographical investigation.

Notes on contributor

Dr Esther Mang received her doctorate at University of British Columbia, Canada,

where she also taught music education courses and early childhood music

programmes. Her research interests are inter-disciplinary, encompassing child

development, speech science and choral music education. She is currently

Assistant Professor of the Hong Kong Baptist University and lectures in Choral

and Instrumental Conducting, Choral Pedagogy, Principles of Music Educa-

tion, and Music in Early Childhood.

References

Addo, A. O. (1995) Ghanaian children’s music cultures: a video ethnography of selected singing games .

Doctoral dissertation, University of British Columbia, Vancouver.

Bennett, P. D. & Bartholomew, D. R. (1997) Song works 1: singing in the education of children

(Belmont, CA, Wadsworth Publishing).

Blacking, J. (1995) Music, culture, and experience: selected papers of John Blacking . Reginald Byron

(Ed.) (Chicago, IL, The University of Chicago).

Bruner, J. (1960) The process of education (Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press).

Bruner, J. (1966) Toward a theory of instruction (Cambridge, MA, Belknap Press of Harvard

University).

Davidson, L., McKernon P. Gardner, H. (1981) The acquisition of song: a developmental

approach. Documentary report of Ann Arbor Symposium: applications of psychology to the teaching

and learning of music (Reston, VA, Music Educators National Conference), 301�315.

Fujita, F. (1990) The intermediate performance between talking and singing*/from an observa-

tional study of Japanese children’s music activities in nursery schools, in: J. Dobbs (Ed.)

Music education: facing the future (Christchurch, New Zealand, ISME), 140�146.

Goetze, M., Cooper, N. & Brown, C. J. (1990) Recent research on singing in the general music

classroom, Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 104(4), 17�37.

Green, G. A. (1990) The effect of vocal modeling on pitch-matching accuracy of elementary

schoolchildren, Journal of Research in Music Education, 38(3), 225�231.

Levinowitz, L. M. (1989) An investigation of preschool children’s comparative capability to sing

songs with and without words, Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 100,

14�19.

Mang, E. (1997) The effects of training with male and female vocal modeling on the singing

achievement of grade one children, Canadian Journal of Research in Music Education, 38(4),

25�33.

Mang, E. (2001) Intermediate vocalizations: an investigation of the boundary between speech and

songs in young children’s vocalizations, Bulletin for the Council of Research in Music Education,

147, 116�121.

Mang, E. (2002) Vocal play as agent of musical creativity, Proceedings of the 7th International

Conference on Music Perception and Cognition , Sydney (Adelaide, Causal Productions), 1�4.

The referent of children’s early songs 19

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14

Page 19: The referent of children's early songs

Marsh, K. M. (1997) Variation and transmission processes in children’s singing games in an

Australian playground. Doctoral dissertation, University of Sydney.

Moog, H. (1976) The musical experience of the pre-school child (C. Clarke, Trans.) (London, Schott

& Co. Ltd).

Moorhead, G. E. & Pond, D. (1978) Music of young children: Pillsbury Foundation Studies (Santa

Barbara, CA, Pillsbury Foundation for Advancement of Music Education).

Papousek, H. & Papousek, M (1981) Musical elements in infant’s vocalization: their significance

for communication, cognition, and creativity, in: L. P. Lipsitt & C. K. Rovee-Collie (Eds)

Advances in infancy research (Vol. 1) (Norwood, NJ, Ablex).

Pressing, J. (1984) Cognitive processes in improvisation, in: W. R. Crozier & A. J. Chapman (Eds)

Cognitive processes in perception of art (North Holland, Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.),

345�363.

Pressing, J. (1988) Improvisation: methods and models, in: J. Sloboda (Ed.) Generative processes in

music (Oxford, Clarendon), 129�178.

Pressing, J. (1998) Psychological constraints on improvisational expertise and skill, in: B. Nettl

(Ed.) In the course of performance (Chicago, IL, University of Chicago Press), 47�67.

Prim, F. M. (1989) The importance of girl’s singing games in music and motor education,

Canadian Journal of Research in Music Education, 32, 115�123.

Richards, M. H. (1980) Aesthetic foundations for thinking, rethought, part 1: Experience (Portola

Valley, CA, Richards Institute).

Richards, M. H. & Langness, A. (1982) Music language, Section 2 (Portola Valley, CA, Richards

Institute).

Riddell, C. (1990) Traditional singing games of elementary school children in Los Angeles . Unpublished

doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles, CA.

Ries, N. L. (1987) An analysis of the characteristics of infant-child singing expressions: replication

reports, Canadian Music Educator, 29(1), 5�20.

Schwandt, T. A. (1997) Qualitative inquiry: a dictionary of terms (Newbury Park, CA, Sage

Publications).

Serafine, M. L., Crowder, R. G. & Repp, B. H. (1984) Integration of melody and text in memory

for songs, Cognition, 16, 285�303.

Serafine, M. L., Crowder, R. G., Davidson, J. & Repp, B. H. (1986) On the nature of melody-text

integration in memory for songs, Journal of Memory and Language, 25, 123�135.

Young, S. (2002) Young children’s spontaneous vocalizations in free-play: observations of two- to

three-year-olds in a day-care setting, Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education ,

43�53.

20 E. Mang

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Am

s/G

iron

a*ba

rri L

ib]

at 0

0:26

27

Nov

embe

r 20

14