the effects of standardized and personalized hypnotic induction techniques on depth of trance

98
Western Michigan University ScholarWorks at WMU Dissertations Graduate College 8-1979 e Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance William C. Schirado Western Michigan University Follow this and additional works at: hps://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations Part of the Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy Commons is Dissertation-Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Recommended Citation Schirado, William C., "e Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance" (1979). Dissertations. 2703. hps://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations/2703

Upload: others

Post on 11-Sep-2021

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Western Michigan UniversityScholarWorks at WMU

Dissertations Graduate College

8-1979

The Effects of Standardized and PersonalizedHypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth ofTranceWilliam C. SchiradoWestern Michigan University

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations

Part of the Psychoanalysis and Psychotherapy Commons

This Dissertation-Open Access is brought to you for free and open accessby the Graduate College at ScholarWorks at WMU. It has been accepted forinclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorksat WMU. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Recommended CitationSchirado, William C., "The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance" (1979).Dissertations. 2703.https://scholarworks.wmich.edu/dissertations/2703

Page 2: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

THE EFFECTS OF STANDARDIZED AND PERSONALIZED HYPNOTIC INDUCTION TECHNIQUES ON DEPTH

OF TRANCE

byWilliam C. Schirado

Dissertation Submitted to the

Faculty of The Graduate College in partial fulfillment

of theDegree of Doctor of Education

Western Michigan University Kalamazoo, Michigan

August 1979

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 3: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

ACKNOWLE DGEMENTS

A special note of appreciation is extended to Dr. Robert F. Hopkins, who directed this research; and to Drs. William A. Carlson, Frederick P. Gault, and George P. Sidney, for the valuable and varied perspectives they offered throughout this research. My deepest thanks go to Drs. Hopkins and Carlson for their generous contribution of time.

William C. Schirado

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 4: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

INFORMATION TO USERS

This was produced from a copy of a document sent to us for microfilming. Wliile the most advanced technological means to photograph and reproduce this document have been used, the quality is heavily dependent upon the quality of the material submitted.

The following explanation of techniques is provided to help you understand markings or notations which may appear on this reproduction.

1. The sign or “ target” for pages apparently lacking from the document photographed is “Missing Page(s)”. If it was possible to obtain the missing page(s) or section, they are spliced into the film along with adjacent pages. This may have necessitated cutting through an image and duplicating adjacent pages to assure you of complete continuity.

2. When an image on the film is obliterated with a round black mark it is an indication that the film inspector noticed either blurred copy because of movement during exposure, or duplicate copy. Unless we meant to delete copyrighted materials that should not have been filmed, you will find a good image of the page in the adjacent frame.

3. Wlien a map, drawing or chart, etc., is part of the material being photo­graphed the photographer has followed a definite method in “sectioning” the material. It is customary to begin filming at the upper left hand corner of a large sheet and to continue from left to right in equal sections with small overlaps. I f necessary, sectioning is continued again-beginning below the first row and continuing on until complete.

4. For any illustrations that cannot be reproduced satisfactorily by xerography, photographic prints can be purchased at additional cost and tipped into your xerographic copy. Requests can be made to our Dissertations Customer Services Department.

5. Some pages in any document may have indistinct print. In all cases we have filmed the best available copy.

UniversityMicrofilms

International

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 5: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

792 66 36SCH IR ADÜ , W IL LI A M CHA RL ES

THE E FF EC T S OF S T A N D A R D IZ E D AND PE R S O N A L IZ E D H YP NO T I C IND UC T I O N T EC HN I QU E S ON DEPTH OF TRANCE,W ES TE RN MIC HI G AN UNI VE RSI TY , ED,0,, 1979

COPR, 1979 SCH IRA DO, WIL LI AM C H A RL ESInternational 300 n. zeed road, ann arbor, mi 48ioc

0 1979

W IL LIA M CHARLES SCHIRADO

ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 6: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

TABLE OF CONTENTS

CHAPTER PAGEI The P r o b l e m ..................................... 1

II The L i t e r a t u r e ................................ 9Personalized Techniques .................... 9Standardized Techniques and Scales . . . . 12Personalized and Standardized Techniques

C o m p a r e d ................................. 19Research Problems ........................... 25

III The Method, Subjects, Procedure .............. 29M e t h o d ...................................... 29S u b j e c t s .................................... 34Procedure.................................... 35

IV R e s u l t s ......................................... 37V Discussion, Conclusions, Summary, Recommenda­

tions ........................................ 47D i s c u s s i o n ................................. 47Conclusions................................. 51S u m m a r y ...................................... 54Recommendations ............................. 55

APPENDIXA Experienced Clinical Hypnotic Operator

Defxned . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 58B Standard Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale Scor­

ing Blank, Form A, and Interrogatory Blank 60C Equivalent Scoring Criteria .................. 64

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 7: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

D D e b r i e f i n g .................................... 66E Preliminary Data-Gathering .................. 68F Standard Explanation to Potential Volunteers

Describing Involvement in Research Concern­ing H y p n o s i s ............................... 70

G Informed Consent Form ......................... 72H Debriefing Handout ........................... 74I Research D a t a .................................. 76

REFERENCES.......................................... 78

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 8: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

LIST OF TABLES

TABLE PAGE1. Two-Way Analysis of Variance: Sex and Induction

Type » • • • • • • • • • • • ■ • • • • • • « • • 382. Two-Way Analysis of Variance: Experienced

Clinical Hypnotic Operator and Induction Type 393. Two-Way Analysis of Variance: Age and Induction

T y p e ................................................. 404. Weighted Means Analysis of Variance : Age and

Induction T y p e ..................................... 415. Two-Way Analysis of Variance: Department and

Induction T y p e ...................................... 426. Weighted Means Analysis of Variance : Department

and Induction T y p e ................................. 437. One-Way Analysis of Variance: Total Sample . . 448. Differences Between Means ........................ 45

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 9: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

CHAPTER I

The Problem

The area of hypnosis received increased attention in the past decade. However, many aspects of hypnosis were in need of further investigation. One such area, regarding induction procedures, received limited attention and needs further study. This research addresses induction procedures.

The scientific study of hypnotic behavior was initially hindered by the lack of systematic, standardized methods of induction and measurement necessary for control of variables and replication of research. The Barber Suggestibility Scale (BSS) and the Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS) addressed this problem by providing standardized formats for induction and measurement of depth of trance. However, with the introduction of standardized material, the personalized clinical practice of structuring the hypnotic induction and behavior to individuals became less suitable to laboratory settings.

Hypnosis has been viewed, in general, as a highly indi­vidualized experience (Shor & Orne, 1965; Weitzenhoffer,1953) . Personalized (e.g., individualized, permissive) clinical hypnotic techniques address idiosyncratic needs, and it may be contended that such personalized techniques are a major factor in determining the resultant hypnotic behavior

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 10: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

2and depth of trance (Erickson, 1958, 1962, 1964b; Fromm &Shor, 1972; Hull, 1933; Weitzenhoffer, 1963). If personal­ized techniques are of major consequence in the induction and depth of trance, standardized inductions and suggestibility scales may not offer a suitable comparison to the traditional, personalized clinical setting (Handler & Gridner, 1975; Erickson, 1954a, 1960, 1965, 1966a; Gindes, 1951; Greene,1970; Hartland, 1966; Spiegel, 1959; Wilson, 1967). The question arises as to whether or not laboratory research in the area of hypnosis, using standardized induction techniques, is comparable to hypnosis as practiced in the clinical set­ting, using personalized induction techniques.

In looking for possible differences between these two general methods of induction, it is noted that a "high" inci­dence of nonresponsive subjects has been reported in stand­ardized laboratory research, as compared to reported person­alized clinical experience (Dana & Cooper, 1964 ; Davis & Husband, 19 31 ; Deckert & West, 196 3; Dermen & London, 1965 ;Faw & Wilcox, 1958; Furneaux & Gibson, 1961; Hilgard, Weit- zenhoffer, Landes, & Moore, 1961; Hilgard & Bentler, 1963; Melie & Hilgard, 1964; Rosenhan & London, 1963; Shor, Orne,& O'Connell, 1966; Tart & Hilgard, 1966). No mutual criteria have been applied to both settings in an attempt to determine the possible existence of significant differences in success rates in terms of depth of trance (Friedlander & Sarbin,1938; Shor, 1962; Tart, 1966 ; Von Dedenroth, 1962). This

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 11: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

3research used the same criteria for measuring depth of trance across induction techniques, techniques which represent a continuum in the range from standardized to personalized. Advocates of the pure, personalized approach may assert that even the most personalized technique used in this research does not represent their approach. They will be correct, for the setting down of mutual criteria, which is not determined by any one individual's current needs, violates the defini-

... tion of the personalized approach. Yet, such a defense of the personalized technique will represent circular reasoning until such a time that the technique is objectively defined, evaluated, and supported. No implication is intended that the personalized technique is haphazard, chaotic, or arbi­trary in nature. On the contrary, the technique is most demanding in terms of planning, organization, and implemen­tation. Personalized, for the purpose of this research, refers to the fact that the technique relies on the subject's needs to define the organization and content of the induction and the behavior used to judge the depth of trance.

The clinical setting represents the use of personalized techniques, while the laboratory setting represents the use of standardized techniques. Part of the rationale for using the personalized technique comes from the nature of the clinical population. The clinical population presents a demand factor, that is, the resolution of a problem (Bowers, 1966; Jackson, 1975; Orne, 1962, 1965). Laboratory subjects.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 12: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

on the other hand, usually are not included in research for the purpose of resolving some acute problem or distressing situation. Such a discrepancy in the populations may also be viewed in terms of task motivation (Barber, 1970, 1976a; Chaves & Barber, 1976; Ferenczi, 1950; Freud, 1953; Hilgard, 1965). Within this conceptual framework, the clincial popu­lation represents high task motivation due to their wish to resolve psychic and/or physical pain. The laboratory popula­tion represents low task motivation, low in that it may result, for example, from wanting to do well or wanting to help or please the experimenter. Low motivation may be per­ceived as "high" by the individual subject. However, task motivation is present because of participation in the experi­ment. In fact, the experiment presents or imposes a motivat­ing situation upon the subject. Assuming that people seeking clinical help are more highly motivated toward problem reso­lution than people wanting to do well or please others in an experiment, the demand factor may account for greater depth of trance in clinical settings, perhaps regardless of tech­nique. In this research, efforts were made to exclude sub­jects with clinical motivation. That is, this research addressed standardized and personalized techniques and did not address demand factor. Demand factor was limited to motivation for wanting to do well, help, and so forth, and not extreme demands (i.e., client problems). Results repre­senting differences between the induction techniques would

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 13: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

5indicate the significance of the standardized/personalized variables. Results showing no difference between the induc­tion techniques would indicate the significance of other factors, for example, demand.

Within the context of the general problem of standard­ized versus personalized techniques, an internal problem exists as to what techniques adequately represent these approaches. The SHSS represents a uniform, standardized technique, complete with scoring criteria and norms. Within the framework of this research, presenting a tape-recorded version of the SHSS represented the most standardized tech­nique, eliminating such potentially confounding variables as variation in tone of voice and rate of presentation. Pre­senting the SHSS orally represented the next technique along the continuum approaching personalized techniques. Present­ing an oral, personalized induction followed by an oral presentation of the measurement of suggestibility portion of the SHSS represented a more personalized approach. The most personalized approach allowed for oral, personalized presen­tation both of the induction and suggestibility scale or equivalent scale items.

Personalized technique, for the purpose of this research, was given the widest possible latitude in defini­tion, to allow for the clinical expertise of the experi­menters. Any technique, not involving physiological inter­vention, which did not follow the content and guidelines of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 14: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

standardized techniques (e.g., the SHSS, the BSS) was con­sidered acceptable. Experimenters were free to draw from their range of experience, adhering only to the overall time limit of the sessions and the scoring criteria required by the SHSS or equivalent criteria. The personalized techniques, therefore, were idiosyncratic to the experimenters and highly individualized in response to the subjects. The enforcement of minimal constraints on the experimenters when using per­sonalized techniques offered, at best, the opportunity for maximum contrast between the standardized and personalized approach to hypnotic induction. All subjects received iden­tical information regarding the research prior to their participation (As, O'Hara, & Munger, 1962; Cronin, Spanos,& Barber, 1971).

To this point, the problem was one of comparison of techniques. Data exist concerning the nature of standardized and personalized techniques. Yet no suitable comparison could be made because of the lack of mutual criteria. Once mutual criteria were defined, such as the SHSS, the problem of comparing depth of trance could be addressed. A number of assumptions were required, first, that standardized and per­sonalized techniques could be mixed, forming a continuum from standardized to personalized techniques. A few limitations will be outlined here and considered in detail in the dis­cussion. Generalization of results was limited necessarily, due to (1) the highly specific population used in this

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 15: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

7research (graduate volunteers who had not been hypnotized previously), and (2) the limited size of the sample (N = 24,6 subjects per treatment group). A theoretical limitation existed in that the factors considered in this research may not be crucial in defining hypnotic induction as it relates to depth of trance. Researchers are attempting to provide various, alternative conceptual models for viewing hypnosis. For example. Barber (1974, 1975, 1977), Spanos and Barber (1976) , and Weitzenhoffer and Sjoberg (1961) excluded the need for the traditional induction process as a preliminary for hypnotic behavior. However, this research attempted to provide information relevant to established induction tech­niques and their relationship to depth of trance.

The greatest threat to internal validity came from the possible differential selection of subjects (Isaac, 1971) .To lessen the possibility that one experimental group reflected a preexperimental difference rather than showing the effects of the treatment procedure, (1) only subjects with no previous hypnotic experience were used (Boucher & Hilgard, 1962; Cooper, 1972; London, 1961; London, Cooper,& Johnson, 1962; Lubin, Brady, & Leavitt, 1962); (2) subjects were assigned to experienced clinical hypnotic operators (CHO's) on an alternating basis ; and (3) all possible treat­ment groups, for male or female subjects, were ordered ran­domly (i.e., subjects were assigned to treatment groups in the order in which they volunteered). One factor

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 16: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

jeopardizing external validity came from the interaction effects of selection bias and the treatment (Isaac, 1971). This factor limited generalization because of the possibility that the subjects selected for participation in the experi­ment differed significantly from other graduate volunteers on such uncontrolled variables as intelligence, socioeconomic status, and so forth. In addition, the reactive effects of experimental procedures were considered. The experimental procedures might have produced effects that would limit the generalization of the findings. This was considered when describing the different levels of motivation found in per­sonalized clinical and standardized laboratory settings. In the strictest sense, this factor limits any claim that the effect of the treatment for the sample population was the same for subjects who are exposed to such hypnotic techniques in nonexperimental situations.

Historically, research has focused on variables within the confines of the standardized technique. Despite an ever growing body of laboratory research, few researchers have questioned the validity of standardized induction techniques as the basis for the abundant hypotheses and conclusions con­cerning hypnotic behavior generated by such techniques. This research made a comparison between standardized inductions and personalized inductions, using depth of trance as a means of measurement and comparison.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 17: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

CHAPTER II

The Literature

Personalized TechniquesPersonalized hypnotic induction techniques were the only

form of induction during the early days of hypnotism. Induc­tions were individualized because research had not categor­ized similarities and differences in the process (Hull, 1933), let alone anything approaching linguistic analysis (Handler & Gridner, 1975). An early position regarding the nature of hypnosis held to the theory that hypnosis was a special state (Hilgard, 1978). Explanations of this special state tended to provide indirect support for the use of personalized tech­niques. One such view of hypnosis referred to "the art of induction" (Spiegel, 1959, p. 634) where the subject's behav­ior was considered consistent with psychoanalytic theory.Cues came from the subject, providing the therapist with information which guided the therapist's "signals" to the subject. Implicit in this approach was the importance of the relationship (i.e., transference). Behavior on the part of the subject was viewed as a subjective experience (As & Ost- vold, 1968), resulting from the fantasy life and unresolved conflicts of the subject. In the strictest sense, hypnosis was to provide the subject with a secure reality (Erickson, 1962) which promoted the basic ingredients of therapy, that

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 18: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

10is, trust, respect, and acceptance, all to the resolution of guilt and anxiety.

Just as conventional therapeutic responses were guided by the client's verbal and nonverbal behaviors, hypnosis was seen as naturalistic in that the hypnotist tailored his behavior in response to the subject's behavior (Erickson,1958). Personalized techniques were given further support by the use of indirect techniques which circumvented resistance on the part of the client (Erickson, 1954a). The use of indirect hypnotic techniques was analogous to the interpreta­tion of latent language in psychoanalysis. Even the use of the client's symptoms was incorporated into hypnotic induc­tion to facilitate client cooperation and by-pass unconscious resistance through naturalistic, personalized techniques (Erickson, 1965). The personalized approach was carried to other areas of the therapist-client relationship, including interpretation of regressive behavior (Scheflen, 1960) and developmental phases (Hilgard & Hilgard, 1962). Regardless of the problem area, the issue was how to use words (Frankl, 1960; Greene, 1970), and the consensus of the hypnotic state theorists (Erickson, 1955, 1964b; Haley, 1973; Hilgard, 1978) implicitly supported the therapist's adaptability to, and anticipation of, the client's behavior to assure continuing successes by the client (Weitzenhoffer, 1957).

Gradually, personalized techniques were identified as such and given direct support. A most ardent supporter of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 19: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

11personalized techniques described the approach as "meaning­ful communication," as opposed to direct suggestion and authoritarian techniques (Sandier & Gridner, 1975). Authors could go only so far in describing the personalized approach, indicating that results were dependent on the interpersonal relationship and interaction between the hypnotist and the subject (Bentler, 1963; Haley, 1958). Generally, the person­alized technique was viewed as requiring a slow pace and per­sistence to develop a trusting relationship and good prognosis due to the individualized nature of the relationship (Kaplan, 1977). Outright support accompanied such rules as that the therapist's words must relate closely to the subject's actions (Gindes, 1951). Research stated that responsiveness to test suggestions increases when suggestions are linked with natu­rally occurring events (Wilson, 1967). Hypnotists were encouraged to couple the effect they wanted to produce with one that the subject was actually experiencing at that moment (Hartland, 1966). To meet the subject at his own level and to establish the necessary rapport, flexibility and observa­tional skills were encouraged for the hypnotist (Beahrs,1977), To state that the therapist's reactions must match the client's behaviors was to suggest that the client control the therapeutic process (Erickson, 1977). In fact, the use of personalized hypnotic techniques gained increased support and further refinement in terms of definition (Shibata, 1976), leading to the use of this approach in more specific and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 20: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

12specialized areas. Articles and texts applied personalized techniques to medical (Erickson, 1957), dental (Erickson, Hershman, & Secter, 1961), and psychiatric (Erickson, 1939a, 1939b, 1961) situations. Brief psychotherapy (Erickson, 1954b, 1959; Sanders, 1977), symptom and pain control (Erick­son, 1966c), and obesity were all the focus for the useful­ness of personalized techniques. The use of phonograph recordings was either totally rejected (Erickson, 1966a) or qualified with statements as to how the recordings must be familiar to the subject in order to facilitate a deeper trance (Caldironi, 1975). Even the most ardent proponents of the nonhypnotic state theory listed the coupling of sugges­tions with naturally occurring events as one variable that could be delineated in hypnotic induction procedures (Barber & DeMoor, 19 72).

Standardized Techniques and ScalesBefore the development of standardized scales, the use

of unstated and ambiguous criteria in the measurement of hypnotic behavior was common (Arnold, 1946; Field, 1965;Mears, 1957; Tinterow, 1970). The nineteenth-century scales of Liebeault and Bernheim were some of the first attempts to standardize the measurement of depth of hypnosis (Hilgard, 1965). While some measurement and comparison were possible, scoring criteria and the construction of the measurement scale remained major obstacles to refined investigation. The Davis-Husband (1931) Scale went into more detail, considered

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 21: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

13greater variations in depth, and used more refined scoring than previous scales. The scale used the follov/ing classifi­cations for depth; insusceptible, hypnoidal, light trance, medium trance, and somnambulistic trance. Each classifica­tion starting with the hypnoidal classification used from four to eight objective symptoms for scoring (Hilgard, 1965). However, researchers found that each measurement tended to alter subsequent measurements by decreasing the discriminat­ing power of scoring items (Watkins, 19 34). Even with revi­sions, the Davis-Husband Scale showed little evidence that accepted scale construction methods were used, the scoring criteria remained ambiguous and no information regarding validity or reliability was present (LeCron, 1953).

Although the Harvard Discrete and the Harvard Continuous scales provide for deliberate answers from subjects, the scales defined only awake and deep states, precluding further, discrete definition (Fromm & Shor, 1972). Spiegel and Spiegel (1978) developed the Hypnotic Induction Profile (HIP). The scale contains three parts: biological measurement (eyeroll), subjective discovery experience, and an ideomotor item (hand levitation). The HIP is intended to measure hypnotiz- ability and provide information relevant to clinical issues. Major research effort has been focused on the relationship of the HIP to personality traits and degree of psychopathology. Findings relating the HIP to the Stanford Hypnotic Suscept­ibility Scale (SHSS) as yet are unpublished.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 22: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

14The Friedlander-Sarbin Scale used four items in an

attempt to classify hypnotic susceptibility, including eyelid closure, negative suggestion tests, posthypnotic voice hallu­cinations, and amnesia. The scale was validated by a test of internal consistency, and the scale was built around stand­ardized procedures. However, use of the scale revealed that it covered a narrow range of suggestibility, especially on the side of deeper trance states. Subjects who appeared deeply hypnotized were, at times, unable to respond to the more difficult test suggestions (Friedlander & Sarbin, 1938). The Friedlander-Sarbin Scale provided the basis for the development of the SHSS.

The SHSS refined the Friedlander-Sarbin scales, using comparative inductions and items. In addition, the two forms (A and B) permit before-and-after studies. Further revision consisted of greater permissiveness in the challenge items and a simplified scoring basis, that is, pass-fail (Hilgard, 1965). The scale consisted of 12 items, each item followed by the criteria for passing; the pass-fail score; space for notes on amount of response, effort, rate of movement, etc.; and an interrogatory scoring section in which to record recall in the test for amnesia. Form C of the SHSS was developed to accommodate a greater variety of hypnotic behav­iors and was not considered equivalent to Forms A and B (Weitzenhoffer & Hilgard, 1959). All three forms include a 15-minute standardized induction and 12 standardized test

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 23: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

15suggestions.

The Barber Suggestibility Scale (BSS) was developed in response to two major problems with the SHSS. First, the SHSS was not suitable for a comparison group that was not exposed to hypnotic induction. Second, subjective responses were not considered when the SHSS was used. The BSS con­sisted of eight standardized test suggestions, including arm- lowering, arm levitation, hand lock, thirst hallucination, verbal inhibition, body immobility, posthypnotic response, and selective amnesia. Both objective and subjective scoring criteria were specified (Barber, 1976b, 1977). Concerning the BSS, Hilgard (1965, p. 92) pointed out that normative data were gathered "without prior attempted induction of hypnosis." Furthermore, a "considerable fraction of the responses to suggestion that are associated with hypnosis can be obtained from susceptible subjects who have not gone through the usual induction procedures. While the percent passing each item tends to be less than following a usual hypnotic induction, some fourteen to fifteen percent of the subjects" passed difficult items.

Development of standardized methods was extrapolated from the laboratory to the clinical setting. Problem areas already delineated in clinical practice were approached anew through the use of hypnosis, resulting in texts which attempted to specify how specific hypnotic procedures could be applied to specific clinical problems (Crasilneck & Hall,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 24: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

161975; Kroger, 1963). Additional variables which affect hypnotic performance were sought in an attempt to account for variations in subject response. Rather than question the nature of the standardized technique, researchers worked with the framework of standardized scales, focusing on such vari­ables as the effect of the experimenter's tone of voice on hypnotic behavior and performance (Barber & Calverley, 1964b). Results in this area were generally positive (Fisher, 1962) and tended to further a reliance on standardized techniques.

The closest that research has come to comparing stand­ardized and personalized induction techniques has been a focus on taped versus spoken presentations. Barber (1976b) noted the importance of the spoken and tape-recorded methods when considering independent (antecedent) variables "subsumed under the topic hypnosis" (p. 12). An early work by Hosko- vec, Svorad, and Lane (196 3) used taped and spoken sugges­tions. The results indicated that the initial suggestions for both groups were equally effective. Both groups showed increased body sway when compared to basal levels. Spoken suggestions followed by recorded suggestions showed spoken suggestions to be more effective, with the reverse order showing no difference. The research results conclude a justification for the use of recorded (taped) suggestions in experimental and clinical work, although no extensive ration­ale was presented to justify the inclusion of clinical set­tings. The research also shov/ed that Group B (N = 20, base

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 25: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

17response, spoken followed by recorded suggestions) indicated the equality of spoken and taped suggestions, while Group A (N = 20, base response, recorded followed by spoken sugges­tions) yielded a statistically significant difference between spoken and taped suggestions, making the results equivocal at best.

Barber and Calverley (1964a) addressed the question of recorded and spoken suggestions, stating that recordings were an important research aspect to standardized presentations and that it had not been demonstrated that recorded and spoken suggestions produce comparable effects. Their purpose was to "determine whether recorded suggestions are as effec­tive as spoken suggestions in eliciting 'hypnotic-like' behaviors" (p. 1) . In their first experiment, 84 volunteerfemale student nurses, ages 19-22 and not used in previous experiments, were included. Subjects were assigned randomly to Groups A (recorded) and B (spoken), with 42 subjects in each group. The BSS was administered to all subjects. The experimenter present used his own taped voice. The objective and subjective scores (dependent variables) did not differ significantly on seven test suggestions. To help generalize the results and to see if similar results came if a hypnotic induction procedure was administered prior to the test sug­gestions, a second experiment was run. A total of 66 male volunteer dental students, ages 20-25 and who were not pre­vious hypnotic subjects, were used. All subjects were

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 26: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

18assigned randomly to Groups 1 (recorded) and 2 (spoken).All subjects received a 15-minute standardized induction pro­cedure. The objective and subjective scores did not differ significantly on six test suggestions. The recorded and spoken presentations yielded nonsignificant differences of subjective and objective scores on the total scale. The results indicated that recorded suggestions are comparable to spoken suggestions, and researchers are recommended to use recorded suggestions to increase standardization and reduce confounding variables (Barber, 1976b; Barber & Calverley, 1964a).

Research demonstrating comparable results with spoken and recorded suggestions has come out of the laboratory set­ting and is finding support in clinical areas (Mclnelly,196 7). Extended research is now focusing on the comparison of live versus video-taped hypnotic inductions. Ulett, Akpinar, and Itil (1972) used video tape and the BSS with induction, to find that 25 percent of the subjects demon­strated deep trance, 20 percent a light trance, and 55 per­cent in-between, and to recommend the use of video tape as an acceptable method of hypnotic induction. It is noted that the study made no comparisons with other induction methods. Bean and Duff (1975) corroborated the findings of Ulett et al. (1972), concluding that video-taped inductions are as effective as live inductions.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 27: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

19Personalized and Standardized Techniques Compared

Questions remain concerning the generalization of results. Research to date has not made a strict comparison between standardized and personalized techniques. Studies using spoken and recorded variables have taken one possible aspect of the personalized technique (i.e., spoken presenta­tion) , standardized the spoken presentation into a consistent format, and then compared it with an even more standardized presentation (i.e., taped presentation). What is being studied is the comparison of two different standardized tech­niques, not the comparison of standardized and personalized techniques. Wilson and Barber (1977) attempted to address this question as part of their study. Stating that existing scales which measure responsiveness to suggestions were too authoritarian, implied control by the experimenter, usually required an induction procedure, and were not easily adminis­tered to groups and individuals, the researchers developed the Creative Imagination Scale (CIS). The CIS was intended to be non-authoritarian, promote the subject's production of hypnotic phenomena themselves, to be used with or without induction, and to be administered to groups and individuals. This "permissive" scale was intended to measure responsive­ness to 10 itemized test suggestions, including arm heaviness, hand levitation, finger anesthesia, water "hallucination," temperature "hallucination," time distortion, age regression, and mind-body relaxation. Norms were established using 217

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 28: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

20subjects, with test-retest, split-half, and factoral reli­ability being established. The researchers claimed the use­fulness of the CIS in clinical and laboratory settings. However, a review of the CIS reveals that the instructions were read verbatim (i.e., structured and standardized) and not drawn from subject behavior or needs. What was actually presented was an updated version of antiquated, standardized verbal instructions, similar to revising intelligence tests in accordance with current cultural language usage. This revision made the instructions more understandable to the subjects and a greater responsiveness was expected from the intended age group. However, the basic meaning of permis­siveness or non-authoritarian techniques was not a part of the Wilson/Barber study, for to issue identical instructions to all subjects— no matter how liberal, updated, or "permis­sive" the instructions— contradicts the essential definition of the personalized approach.

A number of studies have attempted to focus on the prob­lems of hypnotic susceptibility (Erickson, 1932, 1948; Pattie, 1956; Sacerdote, 1970). Yet conclusions and recommendations, while commenting on standardized techniques and individual differences, have remained within the framework of highly standardized techniques. Ricks (1969) was unable to find support for the following hypothesis: Subjects with highinternal locus of control are more suggestible with communi­cations oriented toward internal control. After using the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 29: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

21BSS and tape-recorded presentations, Ricks suggested that further studies would do well to account more for individual differences in the subjects. In comparing alpha feedback, progressive relaxation, and hypnotic practice. Duff (1977) concluded that there was no significant difference between his groups and that hypnotic susceptibility was stable regardless of outside influence. Hewitt (1965) found that previously unsusceptible subjects did not increase in sus­ceptibility following a "communication" designed to achieve this purpose. Both the Duff (1977) and Hewitt (1965) studies used standardized instruments (e.g., the SHSS), but did not question their findings in terms of individual differences as was done by Ricks (1969). Studies by Moore (1961) and Fried- land (1976) were also characteristic of working within the standardized framework, yet drawing conclusions outside of the studies' parameters. They concluded, respectively, that hypnotic susceptibility is independent of susceptibility to social influences, and there are no significant mean or vari­ance differences among three induction methods (i.e., sleep talk, blackboard visualization, and chiasson). All of the above-mentioned studies had N's ranging from 41 to 180, and used volunteer college students. Conceptually, the studies cited previously may be categorized as belonging to the "non­state" theory. That is, the hypnotic subject is not funda­mentally different from normal individuals cooperating in a social situation. Conversely, "hypnotic state" theory

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 30: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

22implies that a quantitative and qualitative difference exists between the hypnotic state and the waking state.Spanos and Barber (1974) described the hypnotic state theo­rist as tending toward vagueness, with a "lack of amenability to operational definitions" (p. 508).

Spanos and Barber's conclusions are consistent with a variety of studies conducted by researchers who may be placed under the general hypnotic state theory (Bernheim, 1895; Erickson, 1941, 1952, 1964b, 1966b; Eysenck & Furneaux, 1945; Roberts, 1964). Young (1927) implicitly supported the con­tention that the subject's perceptions are of prime impor­tance in determining subject response to hypnotic suggestions. Weitzenhoffer (1957) was more specific when pointing out the importance of setting, attitude, and cooperation in effecting suggestibility and listing the limitations of standardized scale measurements. Haley (1973) was clear in his recommen­dation that the therapist intervene actively and directly through observing the subject, the subject's methods of com­munication, and factors which motivate the subject. Discuss­ing the differences between clinical (personalized) and laboratory (standardized) settings, Mears (1954) concluded that standardized tools of suggestion are not suitable in the clinical setting because of their "show" quality and authori­tarian attitude. Erickson, Rossi, and Rossi (1976) concluded that "theorists [i.e., Sarbin, Coe, Barber] who have identi­fied hypnosis and trance with suggestibility . . . [have

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 31: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

23downgraded] the autonomous and involuntary aspects of hypnotic experience" (p. 269). They identified these theo­rists as advocating (or, at a minimum, reliant on) direct suggestions which tell the subject how to respond. Standard­ized, structured presentations remain the clearest example of direct suggestion. Despite support for both theoretical per­spectives, as indicated previously, Spanos and Barber's (19 74) observations regarding the vagueness and lack of opera­tional definitions by hypnotic state theorists generally remains accurate. Erickson (1967b) stated that laboratory and clinical hypnosis may yield the same observable results (here the author used personalized techniques exclusively) but seem different to the subject, that is, have different personal meanings to the subjects.

Attempts have been made to reconcile the differences between the personalized, clinical, state theories and the standardized, laboratory, non-state theories. Although Holden (1977) presented a factual account of differences, avoiding conclusions, Kroger (1963) stated that the standard­ized authoritarian approach is best when "success is expected" and the personalized permissive approach is best when dealing with resistance. Spanos and Barber (1974) were more specific in pointing out two areas of agreement: (1) that the sub­ject's willingness to cooperate is important, and (2) that subjects respond to suggestions when they become involved in "imaginings that are consistent with the aims of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 32: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

24suggestions" (p. 508). They went on to predict that state theorists are likely to pursue "research that might indicate that hypnotic performance involves unique or highly unusual changes in perceptual functioning or in cognitive function­ing" (p. 508). Non-state theorists, on the other hand, "may be expected to probe further into the effects of such vari­ables as how the situation is defined to the subject [view­ing] hypnotic performance as a set of socially influenced cognitive skills or abilities" (p. 509). To date, state theorists have not agreed upon a consistent method of study­ing or substantiating their contentions. Communication pat­terns and linguistic analysis provide a potential structure, but are far from universally accepted (Erickson, 1952, 1967a; Henle, 1962; Kursh, 1971; Pearson, 1966; Watslawick, Bravin,& Jackson, 1967). The study of logic and problem-solving patterns has emerged as another contender in the struggle to provide objective support for longstanding naturalistic techniques (Bateson, 1972; Bronowske, 1966; Sarbin & Coe,1972; Watslawick, Weakland, & Fisch, 1974). Traditional interpretations remain vague but supported by researchers (Jackson & Haley, 1963; Milgram, 1963, 1964, 1965 ; White,1937), while newer, more objective approaches such as com­munication patterns already are vying with subgroupings and specialties (Deikman, 1972; Erickson, 1964a, 1964c; Young, 1931).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 33: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Research ProblemsA number of authors have highlighted potential problems

in research (O'Connor, 1976; Rosenthal, 1966; Slotnick & London, 1965; Troffer & Tart, 1964). Barber (1973) has delineated nine possible investigator and experimenter effects which may complicate research; those effects will follow and be considered separately. By Barber's definition, the inves­tigator is responsible for the experimental design and the overall conduct of the study, while the experimenter conducts the study.

(1) The investigator paradigm effect is concerned with basic assumptions and the way of conceptualizing the area of inquiry. Paradigms and associated theories are considered necessary to research and are therefore best made explicit.The literature supports the presence of two contrasting the­ories regarding hypnotic induction. Because most research to date deals with structured techniques, to the exclusion of support or even consideration of unstructured techniques, assumptions are made concerning the validity of unstructured techniques and their suitability for accounting for individ­ual differences. It is further assumed that theories which support the use of personalized techniques are not so invalid as underdeveloped.

(2) The investigator loose-protocol effect refers to the lack of step-by-step details of how the experiment is to be conducted. To avoid such a problem, this research defined

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 34: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

26such aspects as the materials, subjects, raters, and experi­menters throughout the method, subjects, and procedure chap­ter, along with their place in the research, to promote understanding of the design/procedure and replication of the research.

(3) The investigator analysis effect is evident when the investigator chooses and focuses on data after conducting the research. The obvious drawback is the possibility that only data which support the investigator's bias will be considered. In the statement of the problem and again in the data analy­sis section, all data to be used in the final analysis were specified along with the method of analysis.

(4) The investigator fudging effect arises when the investigator reports results that are not the results actu­ally obtained. A preliminary step taken to avoid such a pit­fall was the use of raters to collect data. To avoid the problem of establishing a level of significant differences after concluding the research, the .05 level was chosen prior to the start of the research. Lastly, all data gathered were kept and will be made available to other researchers for analysis.

(5) The experimenter attributes effect takes place when personal attributes such as age, sex, race, prestige, social status, warmth, friendliness, and dominance result in differ­ent data from different experimenters. Experimenters in this research were matched for approximate age, sex, race.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 35: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

27prestige, and social status, being from the same university department, general theoretical orientation, and professional affiliations.

(6) Another possible effect surfaces when experimenters fail to follow the established protocol. In addition to having verbatim instructions concerning the presentation of the SHSS and the debriefing, all sessions were observed by raters who monitored the presence or absence of established scoring criteria and time limits.

(7) To avoid the possibility of experimenters mis- recording subjects' responses, raters were trained to follow the SHSS criteria, observing all sessions in full and rating subject responses independent of experimenter opinion. Rater training continued until rater scores correlated positively at the .90 level or above.

(8) Experimenter fudging of data was most limited in that experimenters (a) did not record any data, and (b) knew what was being observed. Data obtained by raters were not open to question or change by experimenters.

(9) We were left with the possibility that the experi­menters would expect and/or desire certain results, transmit­ting that desire to the subjects by means of unintentional para-linguistic cues (e.g., tone of voice, posture, facial expression), resulting in the subjects meeting the experi­menter's expectations. A complex chain of events must occur before the experimenter unintentional-expectancy effect

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 36: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

28occurs, including transmission of expected results from investigator to experimenter, transmission of expectancies from experimenter to subject, and the subject complying with the unintentional cues. Because the unintentional expectancy effect remained a possibility, the best check will be repli­cation studies which systematically isolate communication of expectancies, perhaps video-taping the sessions for later nonverbal and linguistic analysis of possible unintentional expectancy effects.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 37: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

CHAPTER III

The Method, Subjects, Procedure

MethodThe sample population was comprised of 24 volunteer sub­

jects who previously had not been hypnotized. The subjects consisted of the first 12 males and the first 12 females to volunteer. Each treatment group consisted of three males and three females. Each experienced clinical hypnotic operator (CHO) was responsible for 12 inductions, that is, 3 inductions in each of the four treatment groups. Each male subject was assigned to one of the 12 possible combinations of experi­enced CHO treatment groups, using the table of random numbers. The term experienced clinical hypnotic operator has been defined in Appendix A. The same procedure was used for female subjects. The Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS) Form A was used throughout the research. The four treatment groups were defined as follows:

(1) Taped induction. Each experienced CHO recorded the SHSS verbatim on audio tape for presentation to the assigned subject. The experienced CHO who recorded the tape and was assigned to the treatment was present throughout the entire session. The following six sentences from the SHSS Rapport section were deleted from the taped induction and read induc­tion to allow for standardized presentation, uninterrupted

29

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 38: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

by questions and answers:Let's talk a little while before we start. I want you to be quite at ease, and it may help if I answer a few questions first. I am assuming that this is the first time you are experiencing hypno­tism. Am I right about this? Have you any ques­tions? Have you any other questions or comments before we go ahead?(2) Read induction. Each experienced CHO read the SHSS

verbatim (excluding the six sentences mentioned previously) to the subjects assigned to this treatment group.

(3) Own induction/standard scale. Each experienced CHO used his own personalized induction technique, followed by an attempt to elicit the SHSS scoring criteria behavior in order.

(4) Own induction/own scale. Each experienced CHO used his own personalized induction technique followed by his own personalized presentation of the suggestibility scale. The experienced CHO's attempted to elicit, in any order, either the SHSS scoring criteria behavior or equivalent behavior.

The subjects were grouped as follows:CHO #1 CHO #2

Taped induction female femalemale femalemale male

Read induction femalefemale

female maleOwn induction/ male malestandard scale female male

female femaleOwn induction/ male femaleown scale female

female

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 39: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

31Independent variables consisted of the standardized and

personalized induction techniques. The dependent variable was depth of trance. The research hypothesis stated: Theuse of personalized induction techniques will result in a greater depth of trance than those inductions using standard­ized induction techniques. The null hypothesis stated:There is no difference between standardized and personalized induction techniques as measured by depth of trance. Depth of trance was measured by the SHSS scoring criteria (Appendix B) and/or equivalent scoring criteria (Appendix C). Factors held constant across treatment groups included the following :

(1) All treatment sessions were followed by the subject receiving a 15-minute debriefing from the experienced CHO responsible for the treatment (Appendix D).

(2) The total maximum length of treatment time for all sessions was 66 minutes (which included a 41-minute time limit for establishing rapport, induction, and testing; a 10-minute time limit for the final inquiry ; and a 15-minute time limit for debriefing). Time limits were established from results obtained from preliminary data-gathering by the researcher as summarized in Appendix E. The Rapport and Induction/Testing components were grouped together in order to maintain consistency between induction types and with the notion that rapport may significantly influence and/or be a part of the induction process. The mean time of these two components was used as the basis for selecting the 41-minute

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 40: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

32time limit. Preliminary data indicated that 10 minutes was sufficient for final inquiry. The debriefing component was given the upper limit of 15 minutes in order to allow for a variety of possible situations, recognizing that this compo­nent would not influence scoring, as induction and scoring had been completed.

(3) The "laboratory experiments" section of the SHSS was used rather than the "subjects coming for therapy" section.

(4) All sessions were conducted in the morning (i.e., 9:00, 10:00, or 11:00) in an attempt to minimize possible differences which might occur in comparing A.M. and P.M. inductions (e.g., before- and after-meal effects and/or fatigue). Each session was viewed either through a one-way mirror by a rater of the actual hypnotic session or video­taped for rating within 24 hours. Two raters of the actual hypnosis (a master's student and the researcher) were trained in the use of the SHSS standardized scoring forms and the equivalent scoring criteria. Training consisted of each rater of the actual hypnosis scoring four video-taped presen­tations of the SHSS, at which point their scoring (a) agreed with the SHSS criteria and equivalent scoring criteria, and (b) correlated at the .92 level. The last three video-taped sessions in the research were rated by both raters of the actual hypnosis to recheck reliability and correlated at the .95, .96, and .95 levels, respectively. In addition, the following films (from the 1976-77 Video Tape Catalogue,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 41: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Division of Institution Communication y Television Services, Western Michigan University) were viewed by both raters prior to the research, to develop familiarity with various induc­tion techniques and scoring criteria:

Hypnosis, Barber and Group (Restricted)44:56 Barber WMU-TVDr. Barber demonstrates subject's ability to perform human plank feat without the induction of hypnosis. Then demonstrates group response to suggestibility using some items from the Barber Suggestibility Scale. Discussion and demonstration of tolerance of pain. (12-14-70)

Hypnosis, Deep (Restricted)48:00 Carlson, Cudney, Reid WMU-TV Demonstrates the rapid induction of somnambu­listic trance. Subject passes all items on Barber Suggestibility Scale. Subject enters somnambulistic state when operator says a key word as a signal for hypnosis. Hypnotic oper­ator and subject demonstrate a struggle of wills to see if the subject can be kept in a trance against her will. (6-70)

Hypnosis Number Two (Restricted)60:20 Carlson, Betz, Kotarski WMU-TV Brief discussion of hypnosis followed by an attempt at a rapid induction of deep trance using confusion and surprise, which fails.Somewhat long eye fixation results in a trance with the subject passing most but not all of the items on the Barber Suggestibility Scale.Subtle interpersonal and intrapersonal dynamics may be observed. (7-70)

Initial Induction— Beverly (Restricted)59:12 Carlson, Fogarty, Martinson WMU-TV Discussion of hypnosis and hypnotic techniques which merges into the application of the con­fusion and surprise techniques of hypnotic induction, resulting in a somnambulistic state with the subject believing that she is a thou­sand miles away approaching Denver and viewing the Rocky Mountains. Later, subject passes all items on the Barber Suggestibility Scale.(8-6-70)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 42: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

34Subjects

Subjects were graduate, master's level volunteers recruited from the Counseling and Personnel, Psychology, and Social Work departments at Western Michigan University. A standard explanation was made to all potential volunteers, describing their involvement in research concerning hypnosis (Appendix F). Prior to a volunteer's participation in the research, the subject was given an informed consent form (Appendix G). After reviewing the form, with opportunity for questions, all volunteers wishing to participate in the research signed the informed consent form. During the final 15-minute debriefing period of the experiment, each subject was informed, orally and in writing (Appendix H), that he/she could contact the researcher and/or the experienced CHO after participation in the research if he/she had further questions. The same written statement detailed instructions for referral to the University Counseling Center if further concerns, arising out of participation in the research, remained unre­solved. Each experienced CHO was instructed to assist any subject in contacting the University Counseling Center if, in the clinical judgment of the experienced CHO, the subject was in need of additional follow-up services. While each experi­enced CHO could terminate the experiment at any time with a subject, the 15-minute debriefing period remained mandatory for each subject involved in the research. Individual experi­ments could be terminated at the request of the subject. No

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 43: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

35subjects were referred to the University Counseling Center.In addition, no sessions were terminated by the experienced CHO or subjects.

All volunteers were screened by the researcher and the assigned experienced CHO. The procedure was established whereby the researcher could tell any volunteer deemed unsuit­able for the research, based on clinical grounds and judgment, that participants would be selected at random from all volun­teers and that if the person was selected he/she would be contacted. Further contact would not be established. No volunteers were deemed unsuitable by the researcher. Either experienced CHO could deem a volunteer unsuitable for the research, based on clinical grounds and judgment. When con­fronted with such a volunteer, the experienced CHO would talk to the volunteer about hypnosis without proceeding with the research, dismissing the volunteer after a time thought suit­able by the experienced CHO. No volunteers were deemed unsuitable by the experienced CHO's.

ProcedureA total of 24 SHSS scoring blanks were filled in to

indicate the experienced CHO and the induction type. Com­pleted scoring blanks indicated the subject's sex, appoint­ment time, the rater of the actual hypnosis, the elapsed time through Item 11, and the total elapsed time excluding the debriefing. Terminated sessions were to be indicated as such on the scoring blank by the rater of the actual hypnosis. No

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 44: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

36sessions were terminated. All subjects were informed that the session might be video-taped and erased after review.The raters of the actual hypnosis observed each treatment session— either live or video-taped— in its entirety, com­pleting the scoring blank as the session progressed. Com­pleted scoring blanks were presented to the researcher for analysis.

Each experienced CHO was physically present during all treatment sessions, including the taped presentation. Taped sessions were accompanied by the experienced CHO who recorded the tape. All sessions were conducted in an 11' x 15' room equipped with a one-way mirror for observation and scoring by the rater of the actual hypnosis. The experienced CHO and volunteer, when seated, were at a distance of 6 feet. The tape recorder was a Bell and Howell Educational Series Cas­sette, model number 3085, placed next to and manipulated by the experienced CHO. Tape speed was 1-7/8 inches per second, using Sony C-120, Plus 2 cassette tape, with the entire SHSS recorded on one side to assure uninterrupted presentation.The data collection period of the research project covered a time period of 12 continuous weeks.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 45: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

CHAPTER IV

Results

The results of this study are presented in terms of (1) a tV70-way analysis of variance to identify the influence of the subject's sex, the two different experienced clinical hypnotic operators (CHO's), the subject's age, and the sub­ject's university department; (2) a one-way analysis of vari­ance of the total sample to determine the difference(s) between induction types; (3) a one-way analysis of variance to identify the difference(s) between induction types when males and females are considered separately; (4) a one-way analysis of variance to identify differences between induc­tion types when the experienced CHO's are considered sepa­rately ; and (5) an analysis of the significant F ratio (obtained from the one-way analysis of variance of the total sample), using the least significant difference two-sample t analysis, to identify the number and location of signifi­cant differences between induction types. A summary of the raw data is presented in Appendix I.

Table 1 summarizes the two-way analysis of variance for sex and induction type. Specifically, Table 1 provides a summary of the means and standard deviations for each of the eight possible combinations of sex and induction type, along with the resulting F ratios and probabilities. The F ratios

37

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 46: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Two-Way Analysis of Variance: Sex andInduction Type

SexInduction

Type Male FemaleN M SD N M SD

1) Tape 3 3. 33 1. 155 .3 4.33 1. 5282) Read 3 7.00 1. 732 3 6.33 2. 0823) OISS 3 9,.67 2. 082 3 9. 33 .5774) OIOS 3 12,.00 000 3 11. 33 5. 77

Least Squares ANOVASource MS F E

Cells 7 211.83Sex eliminating induction type 1 .17 .17 .08 . 7765Induction type eliminating sex 3 208.83 69. 61 34.81 .0000Sex by induction type 3 2.83 . 94 .47 . 7059Within 16 32. 00 2.00Total 23 2 4 3 . 8 3

and probabilities support the contention that the subject's sex is not a significant factor in determining the induction type scores.

Table 2 summarizes the two-way analysis of variance for experienced CHO and induction type. In addition to the means and standard deviations for the eight possible combinations of experienced CHO and induction type, the F ratios and prob­abilities presented in Table 2 indicate that the use of two

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 47: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Two-Way Anlaysis of Variance: Experienced ClinicalHypnotic Operator and Induction Type

Experienced CHOInduction

Type CHO #1 CHO #2N M SD N M SD

1) Tape 3 3. 00 1. 000 3 4. 67 1.1552) Read 3 6. 33 2.517 3 7.00 1.0003) OISS 3 10. 33 1. 528 3 8. 67 . 5774) OIOS 3 11. 33 . 577 3 12. 00 .000

Least Squares ANOVASource SS ^ F PCells 7 218.50ECHO eliminating induction type 1 . 67 .67 . 42 . 5256Induction type eliminating ECHO 3 208.83 69.61 43. 96 . 0000ECHO by induction type 3 9.00 3.00 1.89 .1712Within 16 25. 33 1. 58Total 23 243.83

experienced CHO's does not adversely affect the induction type scores.

To determine the effect that age has on the induction type scores, subjects were placed into two groups. Group 1 includes all ages 25 years and below. Group 2 includes all ages 26 years and above. Table 3 lists the means, standard deviations, preliminary analysis of variance, and least squares analysis of variance resulting from the two-way

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 48: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Two-Way Analysis of Variance: Age andInduction Type

AgeInduction

Type 25 and Below 26 and AboveN M SD N M

1) Tape 5 3,80 1.483 1 4. 00 . 0002) Read 2 6.00 .000 4 7.00 2.1603) OISS 4 9.25 .500 2 10. 00 2.8284) OIOS 3 11 .33 .577 3 12. 00 . 000

Preliminary ANOVASource âË SS m F 2Cells 7 211.62 30.23 15. 01 . 0000Age ignoring induction type 1 13. 38Induction type ignoring age 3 208.83Within 16 32.22 2.01Total 23 243.83

Least Squares ANOVASource âÆ SS MS F 2Cells 1 211.62Age eliminating induction type 1 2.45 2.45 1.22 . 2863Induction type eliminating age 3 197.91 65.97 32.76 . 0000Age by induction type 3 .33 .11 .06 . 9823Within 16 32.22 2.01Total 23 243.83

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 49: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

41analysis of variance which considered age and induction type. The weighted means analysis of variance (Table 4) is used to account for the unequal number of subjects in each of the eight groups representing combinations of age and induction type. The resulting F ratios and probabilities in Table 4 indicate that age does not significantly influence the induc­tion type scores.

Table 4Weighted Means Analysis of Variance:

Age and Induction Type

Source âÆ SS F PCells 7 211..62 30., 23Age 1 2., 03 2.,03 1.01 . 3299Induction type 3 158., 73 52., 91 26.28 . 0000Age by induction type 3 33 .11 . 06 . 9823Within 16 32. 22 2. 01Total 23 243. 83

Subjects used in the present study came from the Counsel­ing and Personnel, Psychology, and Social Work departments of Western Michigan University. Because most of the subjects were from the Counseling and Personnel Department, the sub­jects were placed in one of two groups to determine the effect of the subject's department of origin on the induction type scores. Group 1 represents subjects from the Counseling and Personnel Department, and Group 2 represents subjects from the Psychology and Social Work departments. Table 5

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 50: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Two-Way Analysis of Variance: Department andInduction Type

DepartmentInduction

Type Group 1 Group 2N M SD N M SD

1) Tape 4 3.25 957 2 5. 00 1.4142) Read 4 6. 75 2. 217 2 6.50 . 7073) OISS 4 9. 75 1. 708 2 9 . 0 0 .0004) OIOS 3 11 .67 577 2 11. 67 . 577

Preliminary ANOVASource SS F P

Cells 7 2 1 3 . 7 5 30.54 1 6 . 2 4 . 0000Dept, ignoring induction type 1 4.01Induction type ignoring dept. 3 208.83Within 16 30. 08 1 . 8 8

Total 23 2 4 3 . 8 3

Least Squares .ANOVASource ÊÈ S^ F P

Cells 7 2 1 3 . 7 5

Dept, eliminating induction type 1 .18 .18 .10 . 7598Induction type eliminating dept. 3 2 0 5 . 0 0 68. 33 3 6 . 3 4 .0000Dept, by induction type 3 4 . 7 3 1.58 .84 . 4920Within 16 3 0 . 0 8 1 . 8 8

Total 23 243.83

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 51: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

43summarizes the means, standard deviations, F ratios, and probabilities for the eight groups obtained by combining department and induction type. Table 6 accounts for the unequal number of subjects in the eight groups through the use of weighted means analysis of variance. The F ratios and probabilities summarized in Table 6 indicate that the subject's department of origin does not adversely affect the resulting induction type scores.

Table 6Weighted Means Analysis of Variance:

Department and Induction Type

Source F £Cells 1 218.. 75 30,. 54Department 1 ,19 .19 .10 .7529Induction type 3 180., 84 60..28 32.06 . 0000Department by induction type 3 4., 73 1., 58 . 84 .4920Within 16 30. 08 1. 88Total 23 243. 83

In summary, two one-way analyses of variance, computed separately for subject's age, department affiliation, and the use of two different experienced CHO's are not significant. The results indicate that the above-stated factors do not significantly affect the induction type scores. Therefore, a one-way analysis of variance is used to identify the pos­sible presence of significance between induction types.Table 7 summarizes the means and standard deviations from

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 52: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

One-Way Analysis of Variance : Total Sample

Induction Type N M SD1) Tape 6 3.83 1.3292) Read 6 6. 67 1.7513) OISS 6 9.50 1.3784) OIOS 6 11.67 .516

ANOVASource F PBetween 3 208.83 69 . 61 39.78 .0000Within 20 35.00 1. 75

Total 23 243.83

the one-way analysis of variance. The F ratio indicates the presence of significant difference(s), F(3, 23) = 39.78,p < .000.

Noting the significant F in Table 7, the two-sample tanalysis (least significant difference) is used to identifythe number and location of significant differences betweeninduction types.

Least significant difference (LSD) computational formula (Snedocor & Cochran, 1968, p. 272):

V MS (I)

with degrees of freedom = k(n-l)where X = treatment means

MS = errorn = number of subjects in each treatment group k = number of experimental groups.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 53: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Two-sample t analysis;

= 2.086 V 1.75(1/6 + 1/6)

= 2.086 \/ 1.75/3= 2.086 (.76) = 1.593

The means for the induction types are as follows:Taped ^1 ~ 3.83Read ^2 “ 6.6 7Own induction/standard scale = 9.50Own induction/own scale = 11.67

The differences between all means are shown in Table 8.

Table 8 Differences Between Means

2 3 4

Mean 1 2.834 5.667 7.8372 2.833 5.0033 2.170

Comparing difference with the results of the two-samplet analysis (1.593) leads to the conclusion that all means are significantly different at the «X = .05 level.

In summary, all induction types are found to differ sig­nificantly from each other, with the most personalized methods achieving the greatest depth of trance, and the other

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 54: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

46methods rank-ordered, with the standardized procedure achiev­ing the least depth of trance. Potentially confounding vari­ables such as sex, the influence of the individual hypnotic operator, age, and academic affiliation do not contribute to the effects noted above.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 55: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Discussion, Conclusions, Summary, Recommendations

DiscussionConsistent with the original research design, intended

to limit the possible differential selection of subjects, all subjects indicated that they had no previous hypnotic experi­ence, were assigned to the experienced clinical hypnotic operators (CHO's) on an alternating basis, and were assigned to treatment groups using the table of random numbers. The problem of possible interaction effects of selection bias and the treatment remained throughout the research, as the popu­lation was relatively select and homogeneous. Of particular note is the possibility that the experimental procedures may have produced effects that limit the generalization of the findings. By using the range of induction techniques struc­tured into this particular research, a variety of procedures may be compared. The results of such comparisons raise clear questions regarding the reactive effects introduced by the more structured induction techniques. One critical component left to other research is the question of motivation, in par­ticular, the motivation evident in clinical populations.

In rating the experimental sessions, a master's level student rated 14 sessions while the researcher rated 10

47

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 56: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

sessions. As mentioned previously, inter-rater reliability was checked both before the research and during the final sessions of the research. While training of raters presented no particular difficulty, there were a number of initial problems securing raters who were able to devote adequate, extended time to such research. The films used in training were valuable particularly in the recognition of equivalent scoring items. Scheduling remained a difficulty throughout the research, making the use of video tape essential. The use of equivalent scoring criteria did not present untoward difficulties, as most were easily recognizable as equivalent items. Rather than find an inclusion of questionable equiva­lent items, the major differences appeared to be in such areas as wording, phrasing, and so forth. The 15-minute debriefing period was adequate for all questioning. Both the 41-minute time limit for establishing rapport, induction, and testing and the 10-minute limit for final inquiry were adequate.

The notes kept by both raters on the scoring blank revealed one consistent item during the taped and read induc­tions. Subjects experienced consistent confusion in attempt­ing to respond to the instructions for Item 4, immobilization (left arm). The instructions refer to both the hand and the arm, thereby confusing subjects as to the appropriate or desired response.

The data analysis rested upon four underlying

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 57: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

assumptions (Glass & Stanley, 1970; Kerlinger, 1964):1. That the contributions to variance in the

total sample were additive.2. That the observations within sets were

mutually independent.3. That the variances within experimentally

homogeneous sets were approximately equal.4. That the variations within experimentally

homogeneous sets were from normally dis­tributed populations.

After the overall hypothesis of equality of means was rejected using analysis of variance, the problem of selecting an appropriate multiple comparison technique was addressed. While the significant analysis of variance F (supported by the two-way ANOVA) results) indicated that something happened in the experiment which had a small probability (alpha) of happening by chance, a method was needed to determine which groups were different from each other. The least significant difference procedure was used because it met the required criteria of three or more groups comprising the experiment and the analysis of variance F being significant. Moreover, given that the analysis of variance F is significant, the least significant difference test is a more powerful multiple comparison procedure than the Tukey or Scheffe' methods (Ker- linger, 1964; Stoline, 1979).

A review of the information provided by Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard (1959) reveals consistencies with the research herein described. The Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale (SHSS) was designed primarily for those being tested for the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 58: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

50first time. A thumbtack was used as the fixation point (target) and was placed in the ceiling about 6 feet from the subject. The room contained the required seating arrange­ments and was free of distractions.

The treatment groups tended to maximize the possibility of measuring the differences between the "inflexibility of a standardized method" (p. 4), which Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard cautioned against, and personalized presentations of the induction.

The standardized information provided by Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard was derived from a sample of 124 students with no demonstrable sex differences in the sample. Their raw scores were grouped into three categories; high, 12 through 8; medium, 7 through 5; and low, 4 through 0. The number of cases in each of their categories is as follows : high, 31(24 percent); medium, 34 (31 percent); low, 54 (45 percent), with standard scores having a mean of 50 and a standard devi­ation of 10. The range of raw scores obtained during the current research was as follows :

Induction Type Raw Score RangeTaped 2 - 6Read 4 - 9Own induction/standard scale 8 - 1 2Own induction/own scale 11 - 12

It is noted that while the highest score in this current research, from either the taped or read induction, was 9,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 59: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

5185.5 percent of the raw scores reported by Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard fell in the 0-9 range, indicating surface consistency between the data gathered from both samples. One would expect a greater range if a larger sample were used in this research, a range that perhaps approaches the results obtained by Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard. However, the continued differ­entiation between means would also be expected as indicated by this current research.

Two additional factors were present throughout the research. First, the master's level rater of the actual hyp­nosis was not given information regarding the research hypoth­esis until alter the research was completed. Second, it was expected that both experienced CHO's would attempt to do their best in all possible inductions in order to demonstrate their overall skill in hypnotic induction. Therefore, one would expect that the standardized inductions would yield results equivalent to the personalized inductions if these two inductions were, in fact, equivalent.

ConclusionsData analysis reveals that all means are significantly

different at the aC .05 level.A consistent pattern emerged in comparing the results

obtained for each induction type. That is, the depth of trance increased as the induction type became more person­alized. In addition, the results were not significantly influenced by subject sex, age, university department, the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 60: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

52use of two experienced CHO's, or the use of two raters of the actual hypnosis.

At this time, care must be taken to remember that this research deals with a limited sample size (N = 24). More­over, the subjects were selected on the basis of number of specified criteria, including being master's level university students who had not been hypnotized previously. However, in the face of these limiting factors, real differences appear to be materializing between the standardized induction tech­niques traditionally used in laboratory settings and the per­sonalized induction techniques traditionally found in clinical settings. While the importance of motivation is not to be excluded, the consistency of the results strongly supports the notion that technique plays a significant role in hypnotic results.

The emergence and growing use of standardized techniques has followed a consistent pattern in attempting both to add a note of authenticity and to identify important factors in research concerning hypnosis. Yet research which indicates a significant difference between standardized and personalized techniques, and demonstrates increased effectiveness (in terms of depth of trance) with personalized techniques, sup­ports the contention that there remains considerable doubt as to the appropriateness of using standardized techniques in research concerning clinical hypnosis. Laboratory research which relies on the use of standardized techniques may not

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 61: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

53yield data which are applicable to the understanding of clinical hypnosis. This current research suggests consider­able differences between laboratory and clinical hypnosis.One may wonder, legitimately, whether research concerning hypnosis has proceeded in a most backward fashion. That is, instead of focusing on the structure and dynamics of the individual personality, research has delimited the importance of the individual and his relationships in favor of metholog- ically impeccable research which contributes little to our understanding of man qua man. The results obtained in this current research suggest the importance of focusing on indi­vidual, personality dynamics.

Standardized, structured hypnotic induction presenta­tions remain the clearest example of direct suggestion. Such techniques are consistent with theoretical views which state that the hypnotic subject is not fundamentally different from normal individuals cooperating in a social situation. As a result, the autonomous and involuntary aspects of hypnosis are downgraded or ignored. Consistent with this "non-state" theory of hypnosis, researchers attempted to operationalize the theory in the form of standardized techniques. However, the results of this current research suggest that the sound possibility exists that hypnosis is indeed a special-state.The results to be derived from this research are far from conclusive regarding the question of "state" versus "non­state" theory. However, at best, direct support is provided

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 62: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

54for the hypnotic-state theory. At the minimum, the results bring into serious question the validity of using research results obtained from the use of standardized techniques as evidence in support of the "non-state" theory. The sugges­tion remains that hypnosis is a special state "where the skill of the therapist together with the needs of the patient interacts to produce the striking discontinuities between trance and the normal state of consciousness" (Erickson, Rossi, & Rossi, 1976, p. 300).

SummaryFour treatment groups were formed to represent induction

techniques ranging from standardized to personalized. Each group contained three male and three female subjects who had not been hypnotized previously. All subjects were seen indi­vidually. Individuals in the first group were presented with an audio-taped version of the SHSS. Individuals in the second group were read the SHSS. Individuals in the third group were presented with an induction of the experienced CHO's own choosing and were given the standard scoring cri­teria from the SHSS. Individuals in the fourth group were presented with an induction of the experienced CHO's own choosing and were given either the standard scoring criteria from the SHSS, in any order, or equivalent scoring criteria. All 24 treatment sessions were observed and scored, using the SHSS scoring criteria or equivalent scoring criteria, by a rater of the actual hypnosis. Mean scores from each of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 63: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

55four treatment groups were compared and found to differ sig­nificantly at the c< = .05 level. The differences were not confounded significantly by the subjects' age, sex, univer­sity department, the use of two experienced CHO's, or the use of two raters of the actual hypnosis. Results indicate the presence of real differences between standardized and person­alized induction techniques with implications regarding the direction of research in the area of hypnosis.

RecommendationsReplication of this research is recommended and would

prove informative. However, expansion of this research is suggested in accordance with the following considerations.A number of practical recommendations emerged during the course of the research. The use of bachelor level students would have allowed for both a larger sample and more flexi­bility and convenience in setting appointments. Raters were difficult to find on an ongoing basis, suggesting that some type of payment to raters would help alter this problem and also allow exclusion of the researcher from the rating proc­ess, thereby assuring greater reliability and integrity of design. Also, equal balancing of raters in terms of number of subjects, sex of subjects, and treatment groups would solidify the design.

Video-taping of all sessions is recommended to allow for a more careful check of rater reliability, storage of research results for training and replication, and for more

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 64: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

56in-depth analysis of other factors influencing hypnotic induction. Commercial quality audio equipment is recommended to help assure more realistic taped presentations without the inherent mechanical noise and difficulty found in manipulat­ing educational quality audio equipment. Overhead fluorescent lights used in this research were noted as distracting by some subjects. Table lamps would be more suitable. The use of chairs which allow subjects to place their heads on a headrest is recommended. Excluding the time devoted to the researchby the subjects, the raters of the actual hypnosis, and theexperienced CHO's, the data resulting from the participation of one subject required 12 hours of time on the part of the researcher. This amount of time is considered minimal in order to insure overall quality. The value of using experi­enced CHO's cannot be overemphasized. Such experience remains a critical factor in such research, where clinical skill, creativity, and spontaneous innovation play an important role in responding to a subject's individual needs.

A number of scheduling problems arose, as the room usedthroughout this research was available to a number of groups. Aside from coordination problems with room scheduling, equip­ment and furniture required rearrangement before each session due to previous use of the room by other individuals. Four scheduled appointment times had to be rescheduled due to room conflicts. The use of a specified research room could eliminate many of these problems, including the possible

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 65: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

57complicating factor of obtaining differential results from subjects faced with the frustration of rescheduling their appointment times.

In addition to the replication and/or expansion of this research, there remain specific suggestions for extending the research to other clinical questions. In particular, clinical motivation (on the part of the subject) can be studied for its possible significance and influence on the hypnotic expe­rience. Further research may match a clinical and nonclini- cal population, using only personalized induction techniques, to measure the demand factor (i.e., motivation). The most critical factor, which has been emphasized throughout this research, is the necessity to provide a research design which allows the experimenter to use his entire range of clinical expertise in responding to the individual needs presented by each subject. While such a design may be more cumbersome and time-consuming than traditional laboratory designs, the results obtained appear to be substantially significant in terms of both their theoretical and practical implications.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 66: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX A

Experienced Clinical Hypnotic Operator Defined

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 67: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Experienced Clinical Hypnotic Operator Defined

Both experienced clinical hypnotic operators (CHO's)have received their doctorates and are certified by the Stateof Michigan Department of Licensing and Regulation at theCertified Consulting Psychologist level. In addition., eachexperienced clinical hypnotic operator possesses a minimumof 10 years' clinical experience in the use of hypnosis.

One experienced clinical hypnotic operator is a memberof the following professional organizations:

— American Psychological Association (member)— Society of Clinical and Experimental HypnosisThe other experienced clinical hypnotic operator is a

member of the following professional organizations:— American Psychological Association (member),

Division 30— American Society of Clinical Hypnosis (fellow)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 68: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX B

Standard Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale Scoring Blanks Form A, and Interrogatory Blank

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 69: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Scoring Blank: Form A

To be used in connection with Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard's Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Consulting Psychol­ogists Press, Inc., Palo Alto, California.Grad/Undergrad__ Appt. time_

DateSubject No.__Sex_____ University Dept._Rater (initials)_

Age_

AM/PM Terminated

Total ScoreHypnotist (initials)_

Induction Type _TapeOISS

_ReadOIOS

ItemNotes on Amount

Criterion of Passing Score of Response, (Yielding score of +)(+ or -) Effort, Rate of

Movement, etc.1) Postural

swayFalls without forcing

2) Eyeclosure

Closes eyes without forcing

3) Handlov/ering(left)

Lowers at least 6 inches by end of 10 seconds

4) Immobili- (right arm;

Arm rises less than 1 inch in 10 seconds

)5) Finger

lockIncomplete separation of fingers at end of 10 seconds

6) Arm rigid­ity (left arm)

Less than 2 inches of arm bending in 10 seconds

7) Handsmovingtogether

Hands at least as close as 6 inches after 10 seconds

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 70: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

62Notes on Amount

Criterion of Passing Score of Response, (Yielding score of +)(+ or -) Effort, Rate of

Movement, etc.8) Verbal

inhibition(name)

Name unspoken in 10 seconds

9) Hallucina­tion (fly)

Any movement, grimac­ing, acknowledgement of effect

10) Eyecatalepsy

Eyes remain closed at end of 10 seconds

11) Post­hypnotic(changeschairs)

Any partial movement response

E.T. :12) Amnesia

testThree or fewer items recalled (see below)

Total (+) score . . .

Record of Recall in Test for Amnesia (from Interrogatory)

Order of MentionHand lowering (left) _________________Arm immobilization (right) _________________Finger lock _________________Arm rigidity (left) _________________Hands together _________________Verbal inhibition (name) _________________Hallucination (fly) _________________Eye catalepsy _________________Post-hypnotic suggestion (changing chairs) _Total number of items recalled

Total time:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 71: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Interrogatory Blank

To be used in connection with Weitzenhoffer and Hilgard's Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale, Consulting Psychol­ogists Press, Inc., Palo J^lto, California.

Subject _______________________Hypnotist (initials) Date

1) Please tell me now in your own words everything that has happened since you began looking at the target.

List items in order of mention. If blocked, ask "Any­thing else?" until subject reaches a further impasse.

Anything else?

2) Listen carefully to my words. Now you can remember everything. Anything else now?

List in order of mention.

3) About the fly (or mosquito)— How real was it to you?Transfer information of item recall to the scoring blank, and score for amnesia. Score + if three or fewer items recalled before "Now you can remember everything."

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 72: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX C

Equivalent Scoring Criteria

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 73: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Equivalent Scoring Criteria

Item1) Postural sway2) Eye closure3) Hand lowering

4) Immobilization

5) Finger lock

6) Arm rigidity

7) Hands moving together

8) Verbal inhibition9) Hallucination (fly)

10) Eye catalepsy11) Posthypnotic (changes

chairs)

12) Amnesia test

Equivalent ItemsSame as SHSSSame as SHSSRight arm lowering Right hand lowering Left arm lowering Leg lowering Head loweringArm immobilization Leg immobilization Body immobilizationPalms together locked Arms locked Legs lockedLeg rigidity Body rigidityArms raising Legs raisingAny verbal inhibitionAny sensory hallucinationAny catalepsyAny posthypnotic sugges­tion (e.g., thirst, smoking)Same as SHSS

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 74: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX D

Debriefing

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 75: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Debriefing

To be presented to the volunteer verbatim:"Now that we have concluded the experiment, tell me if

you have any questions or concerns regarding your experience." (Wait for response.) "If questions or concerns arise after you leave today, feel free to contact the researcher or myself at any time. Our telephone numbers are listed on this paper." (Present subject with copy of names and telephone numbers [Appendix H].) "Although it is very rare for people to experience unwanted side effects after hypnosis, informa­tion is provided on the paper you have that tells you how to contact the University Counseling Center, if you would like to talk to someone else." (Time, 5 minutes.)

The experienced clinical hypnotic operator now proceeds with his own clinical debriefing. (Time, 10 minutes.)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 76: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX E

Preliminary Data-Gathering

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 77: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Preliminary Data-Gathering: StanfordHypnotic Susceptibility Scale

Method (Time Indicated in Minutes) TI/TS RI/RS OI/RS OI/OS

Rapport 5 8 10 7 34 3337 44

Induction/testing 30 30 30 30 13 11

Final inquiry 11 10 10 12 10 10 13 11

Debriefing 7 10 10 15 13 12 15 15

Total time: 53 58 60 64 70 66 65 70

TI/TS = Taped induction/taped scale RI/RS = Read induction/read scale OI/RS = Own induction/read scale OI/OS = Own induction/own scale

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 78: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX F

Standard Explanation to Potential Volunteers Describing Involvement in Research

Concerning Hypnosis

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 79: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

standard Explanation to Potential Volunteers Describing Involvement in Research

Concerning Hypnosis

I am looking for volunteer master's level students to par­ticipate in research concerning hypnosis. The only criterion for participation is that each student must not have been hypnotized previously.The actual research will be carried out by persons highly trained and experienced past the doctoral level. You will not be asked to do anything that will make you look silly or stupid, or that will prove embarrassing to you. You will be there for serious scientific purposes. The clinicians will not probe into your personal affairs, so that there will be nothing personal about what you are to do or say.There are no expected risks due to participation in the research. Furthermore, the clinician and the researcher in charge will be available to answer any questions you may have following your participation. Only a limited number of stu­dents will be included in the research. If you have ques­tions and/or would like to schedule an appointment for participation, call :

(Name)(Phone)

If you are unable to reach me, call:(Alternate name)__________(Alternate phone)_________

and leave your name and a phone number where you can be reached.Thank you for your help.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 80: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX G

Informed Consent Form

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 81: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Informed Consent Form

Purpose of the research; The purpose of this research is to study basic questions regarding the nature of hypnosis. Spe­cifically, the research focuses on different types of induc­tions. The research does not go beyond the induction of hypnosis, i.e., into the actual use of hypnosis.Procedures : Volunteers will be exposed to one of a varietyof hypnotic inductions. All of the inductions are either well established in the research literature or based on the expertise of the highly trained experimenters. You will not be asked to do anything that will make you look silly or stupid, or that will prove embarrassing to you. The experi­menters will not probe into your personal affairs. There are no expected risks due to participation in the research. It is considered most rare for people to experience unwanted side effects or concerns after hypnosis. The experimenter, the researcher, and other resource people will be available to answer any questions you may have following your partici­pation. You will be provided with their names and telephone numbers following your participation in the research.Confidentiality : All data gathered in this research will bepaired only with the university department, level, age, and sex of the volunteer. No names will be used.Participation : Participation is voluntary. You may, at anytime, cease participation and/or have all data regarding your participation destroyed.

I have had an opportunity to review and ask questions regarding the above statements and wish to participate voluntarily in the research as described.Signed:__________________________________________Witness :Male_____ Female_____ University level_University department_______________ Age_Date :

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 82: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX H

Debriefing Handout

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 83: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Debriefing Handout

Thank you for your help and participation in this research. If questions or concerns arise after you leave today, feel free to contact the researcher,

(Name)__________ ___(Phone)

or the person you worked with today, (Name)_____________________(Phone)

It is very rare for people to experience unwanted side effects or concerns after hypnosis. However, if for any reason you would like to talk with someone on an ongoing basis regarding personal concerns you may have, contact the person listed below to make such arrangements.

(Name)_______ ___University Counseling Center Western Michigan University (Phone)____________________

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 84: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

APPENDIX I

Research Data

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 85: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Ilil

+J£^2

«I

f r o r v i ' ^ j ' H o o H C N o o ' ^ c N r - I C ' i H r O H f ^ c N ' ^

r-) fN CN rH CN H r s J C N C N C M C M C N C N J C N C N C N H C

rHCN(NrHHrHHCNCNJCNCN<NHrvlCNHi-ICN<>JrHHHCMH

H H H H C N r H H n H f O r O H H H r n H H H C N C N J H H C N r

rHHHrHCNHHCNHCMtNp

' l (N(Nr4CXr4(Nr\)r4r4r4CNr4p

> i r^ CNl ' ^ CT» '^V£ ) (T i CN ' ï I ' ' 3 'OO VJ< cr iCN O'^ r

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 86: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

REFERENCES

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 87: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

REFERENCES

Arnold, M. B. On the mechanism of suggestion and hypnosis. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1946, 41, 107-108. —

As, A., O'Hara, J. W . , & Hunger, M. P. The measurement of subjective experience presumably related to hypnotic susceptibility. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1962, 3, 47-64.

As, A., & Ostvold, S. Hypnosis as subjective experience. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 1968, 9, 33-38.

Handler, R., & Gridner, J. Patterns of the hypnotic tech­niques of Milton H. Erickson, M.D. (Vol. 1). Cupertino, Calif.: Meta Publications, 1975.

Barber, T. X. LSD, marihuana. Yoga, and hypnosis. Chicago: Aldine, 1970.

Barber, T. X. Pitfalls in research: Nine investigator andexperimenter effects. In R. M. W. Travers (Ed.), Second handbook of research on education. Chicago: RandMcNally, 1973.

Barber, T. X . Implications for human capabilities and poten­tialities. In T. X. Barber, N. F. Spanos, & J. F.Chaves (Eds.), "Hypnosis," imagining, and human poten­tialities. Elmsford, N.Y.: Pergamon, 1974.

Barber, T. X. Responding to "hypnotic" suggestions : Anintrospective report. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1975, ]^(1), 6-22.

Barber, T. X. Biofeedback and self-control 1975/76.Chicago : Aldine, 1976. (a)

Barber, T. X. Hypnosis, a scientific approach. New York: Psychological DimensTons, 1976. (b)

Barber, T. X. "Hypnosis," suggestions, and psychosomatic phenomena : A new look from the standpoint of recentexperimental studies. Medfield, Mass.: MedfieldFoundation, 1977.

Barber, T. X., & Calverley, D. S. The comparative effects on "hypnotic-like" suggestibility of recorded and spoken suggestions. Harding, Mass.: Medfield Foundation, 1964.(a)

79

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 88: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Barber, T. X., & Calverley, D. S. Effects of E's tone of voice on "hypnotic-like" suggestibility. Psychology Reports, 1964, ]^, 139-144. (b)

Barber, T. X., & DeMoor, W. A theory of hypnotic induction procedures. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis,1972, ^(2), 112-135.

Bateson, G. Steps to an ecology of mind. New York : Ballan-tine, 1972.

Beahrs, J. O. Integrating Erickson's approach. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1977, ^(1), 55-68.

Bean, B ., & Duff, J. The effects of video tape, and of situ­ational and generalized locus of control, upon hypnotic susceptibility. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1975, 2^(1), 2 8 - 3 3 .

Bentler, P. M. Interpersonal orientation in relation to hyp­notic susceptibility. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1963, 4 2 6 -4 3 1 .

Bernheim, H. Suggestive therapeutics : A treatise on thenature and uses of hypnotism. New York: Putnam, 1895.

Boucher, R. G., & Hilgard, E. R. Volunteer bias in hypnotic experimentation. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1962, 5, 45-51.

Bowers, K. Hypnotic behavior: The differentiation of tranceand demand characteristic variables. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1966, J71, 42-51.

Bronowske, J. The logic of the mind. American Scientist, 1966, 1-14.

Caldironi, B. Elaboration on hypnotherapeutic techniques. Medicina Psicosomatica, 1975, 20(4), 381.

Chaves, J. F ., & Barber, T. X. Hypnotic procedures andsurgery: A critical analysis with applications to "acu­puncture analgesia." American Journal of Clinical Hyp­nosis, 1976, ^(4), 217-236.

Cooper, L. M. Expectations about hypnosis of college stu­dents who have not been hypnotized. In E. Fromm &R. Shor (Eds.), Hypi^sis: Research development andperspectives. Chicago : Aldine, 1972.

Crasilneck, H. B ., & Hall, J. A. Clinical hypnosis: Princi­ples and applications. New York: Grune & Strattonl "1975.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 89: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

81Cronin, D. M., Spanos, N. P., & Barber, T. X. Augmenting

hypnotic suggestibility by providing favorable informa­tion about hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hyp- nosis, 1971, 259-264.

Dana, R. H., & Cooper, G. W., Jr. Prediction of suscepti­bility to hypnosis. Psychology Reports, 1964, 14, 251-265. —

Davis, L. W ., & Husband, R. W. A study of hypnotic suscepti­bility in relation to personality traits. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 1931, 2^, 175-182.

Deckert, G. H., & West, L. J. The problem of hypnotiza-bility: A review. International Journal of ClinicalExperimental Hypnosis, 1963, M , 205-235.

Deikman, A. J. Deautomatization in the mystic experience.In C. T. Tart (Ed.), Altered states of consciousness.New York: Doubleday, 1972.

Dermen, D., & London, P. Correlates of hypnotic suscepti­bility. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 1965, 29, 537-545. —

Duff, J. L. Comparison of procedures for enhancing hypnotic susceptibility. Unpublished doctoral dissertation^ University of Kansas, 1977.

Erickson, M. H. Possible detrimental effects of experimental hypnosis. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1932, 321-32 7.

Erickson, M. H. The application of hypnosis to psychiatry. Medical Record, 1939, ] ^ , 60-65. (a)

Erickson, M. H. Demonstration of mental mechanisms by hyp­nosis. Arch. Neurological and Psychiatric, 1939, 42, 367-370. (b) —

Erickson, M. H. Hypnosis: A general review. Diseases ofthe Nervous System, 1941, 2, 13-18.

Erickson, M. H. Hypnotic psychotherapy. Medical Clinics of North America, 1948, 9, 571-583.

Erickson, M. H. Deep hypnosis and its induction. In L. M. LeCron (Ed.), Experimental hypnosis. New York: Mac­millan, 1952.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 90: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

82Erickson, M. H. A clinical note on indirect hypnotic therapy.

Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 1954, 2, T 7 Ï - 1 7 4 ." T a l --------------------------------------------------------------------------

Erickson, M. H. Special techniques of brief hypnotherapy.Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 1954, 2, 1 0 9 -1 2 9 . (b l

Erickson, M. H. Self-exploration in the hypnotic state.Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 1955, 3, 49-57.

Erickson, M. H. Hypnosis in general practice. State of Mind, 1957, 1, 6-10.

Erickson, M. H. Naturalistic techniques of hypnosis. Amer­ican Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1958, 1, 3-8.

Erickson, M. H. Further techniques of hypnosis— Utilization techniques. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis,1959, 2, 3-21:

Erickson, M. H. The utilization of patient behavior in the hypnotherapy of obesity : Three case reports. AmericanJournal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1960, 112-116.

Erickson, M. H. Historical note on the hand levitation and other ideomotor techniques. American Journal of Clini­cal Hypnosis, 1961, 3, 196-199.

Erickson, M. H. The identification of a secure reality.Family Process, 1962, 1, 294-303.

Erickson, M. H. A hypnotic technique for resistant patients. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1964, 1, 8-32. (a)

Erickson, M. H. Initial experiments investigating the nature of hypnosis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis,1964, 7, 152-162. (b)

Erickson, M. H. Pantomime techniques in hypnosis and the implications. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis,1964, 65-70. (c)

Erickson, M. H. The use of symptoms as an integral part of therapy. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1965,8, 57-65.

Erickson, M. H. Advanced techniques of hypnosis and therapy (J. Haley, compiler). New York: Grune & Stratton:1966. (a)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 91: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Erickson, M. H. Experimental knowledge of hypnotic phenom­ena employed for hypnotherapy. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1966, 8, 299-309. (b)

Erickson, M. H . The interpersonal hypnotic technique forsymptom correction and pain control. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1966, £, 198-209. (c)

Erickson, M. H. Further experimental investigation of hyp­nosis: Hypnotic and non-hypnotic realities. AmericanJournal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1967, 1^, 87-135. (a)

Erickson, M. H. Laboratory and clinical hypnosis: The sameor different phenomena? American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1967, 9, 166-170. (b)

Erickson, M. H. Hypnotic approaches to therapy. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1977, ^(1), 20-35.

Erickson, M.H., Hershman, S., & Secter, I. The practicalapplication of medical and dental hypnosis. New York: Julian Press, 1961.

Erickson, M. H., Rossi, E. L., & Rossi, S. I. Hypnoticrealities : The induction of clinical hypnosis and formsof indirect suggestion. New York: Irvinqton Publishers,1976.

Eysenck, H. J., & Furneaux, W. D. Primary and secondary sug­gestibility : An experimental and statistical study.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1945, 35., 485-503.

Faw, V., & Wilcox, W. W. Personality characteristics of sus­ceptible and unsusceptible hypnotic subjects. Journal of Clinical Experimental Hypnosis, 1958, 83-94.

Ferenczi, S. Sex and psychoanalysis. New York: Brunner,1950.

Field, P. B. An inventory scale of hypnotic depth. Inter­national Journal of Clinical Experimental Hypnosis,1965, 238-249.

Fisher, X. Problems of interpretation and controls in hyp­notic research. In G. H. Estabrooks (Ed.), Hypnosis: Current problems. New York: Harper and Row, 1962.

Frankl, V. E. Paradoxical intention. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 1960, ]A, 520-535.

Freud, S. Three essays on the theory of sexuality (Standard ed., vol. 7). London: Hogarth, 1953.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 92: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

84Friedland, M. R. A comparison of three methods for inducing

hypnosis. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Oregon, 1976.

Friedlander, J. W . , & Sarbin, T. R. The depth of hypnosis. Journal of Social Psychology, 1938, 281-294.

Fromm, E ., & Shor, R. E. Hypnosis, research developments, and perspectives. Chicago: Aldine, 1972.

Furneaux, W. D., & Gibson, H. G. The Maudsley PersonalityInventory as a predictor of susceptibility to hypnosis. International Journal of Clinical Experimental Hypnosis, 1961, 9, 167-176.

Gindes, B. C. New concepts of hypnosis. New York: JulianPress, 1951.

Glass, G., & Stanley, J. Statistical methods in education and psychology. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: ’ Prentice- Hall, 1970.

Greene, G. How to get people to do things with words. In Papers from the 8th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago : UniversTty of ChicagoPress, 1970.

Haley, J. An interactional explanation of hypnosis. Amer- ican Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1958, 1, 41-57%

Haley, J. Uncommon therapy: The psychiatric techniques ofMilton H. Erickson, M. D. New York: W. W. Norton,1973. “ "

Hartland, J. Medical and dental hypnosis and its clinicalapplications. Baltimore: Williams and Williams, 1966.

Henle, M. On the relation between logic and thinking. Psy­chological Review, 1962, 366-398.

Hewitt, J. L. A communications approach to the predictionand alteration of hypnotic susceptibility. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Kansas, 1965.

Hilgard, E. R. Hypnotic susceptibility. New York: Har-court. Brace, and World, 1965.

Hilgard, E. R. Hypnosis and consciousness. Human Nature, January 1978, pp. 42-49.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 93: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

85Hilgard, E. R., & Bentler, P. M. Predicting hypnotizability

from the Maudsley Personality Inventory. Britain Jour­nal of Psychology, 1963, 63-69.

Hilgard, E. R., Weitzenhoffer, A. M., Landes, J., & Moore,R. K. The distribution of susceptibility to hypnosis in a student population: A study using the StanfordHypnotic Susceptibility Scale. Psychological Monographs, 1961, 75(8, Whole No. 512).

Hilgard, J. R., & Hilgard, E. R. Developmental interactive aspects of hypnosis: Some illustrative cases. GeneticPsychological Monographs, 1962, 143-178.

Holden, C. Going beyond pain. Psychology, April 1977 (Trial issue), pp. 10-15.

Hsokovec, J., Svorad, D., & Lane, 0. The comparative effec­tiveness of spoken and tape-recorded suggestions of body sway. International Journal of Clinical Experimental Hypnosis, 1963, 163-166.

Hull, C. L. Hypnosis and suggestibility : An experimentalapproach. New York: Appleton-Century, 1933.

Isaac, S. Handbook in Research and Education (W. Michael, collaborator). San Diego: Edits Publishers, 1971.

Jackson, D. D., & Haley, J. Transference revisited. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1963, 137, 363-3711

Jackson, J. A. Hypnotherapy for whom? Analysis of referred patients. Australian Family Physician, 1975, _4, 451-453.

Kaplan, S. J. Hysterical states. Hypnosis Quarterly, 1977, 20(2), 16-19, 36, 39.

Kerlinger, F. Foundations of behavioral research (2nd ed.). New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1964.

Kroger, W. S. Clinical and experimental hypnosis in medicine, dentistry and psychology. Philadelphia: Lippincott,1 9 6 3 .

Kursh, C. D. The benefits of poor communication. Psycho- analytic Review, 1971, 198-208.

LeCron, L. M. A method of measuring the depth of hypnosis. Journal of Experimental Hypnosis, 1953, 1, 4-7.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 94: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

86London, P. Subject characteristics in hypnosis research.

I. A survey of experience, interest, and opinion. International Journal of Clinical Experimental Hypnosis,1961, 9, 151-161:

London, P., Cooper, 0. M., & Johnson, H. J. Subject charac­teristics in hypnosis research. II. Attitudes towards hypnosis, volunteer status, and personality measures. III. Some correlates of hypnotic susceptibility. International Journal of Clinical Experimental Hypnosis,1962, 13-21.

Lubin, B., Brady, J. P., & Levitt, E. E. A comparison of personality characteristics of volunteers and non­volunteers for hypnotic experiments. Journal of Clini­cal Psychology, 1962, 1^, 341-343.

Mclnelly, W. A. Teaching self-hypnosis through tape record­ings for reducing test anxiety. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wyoming, 1967.

Mears, A. Non-verbal and extra-verbal suggestions in theinduction of hypnosis. Parts I and II. Britain Journal of Medical Hypnotism, 1954, 5, 26; 1954, 6, 51-54.

Mears, A. A working hypothesis as to the nature of hypnosis. American Medical Association Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 1957, 77, 549-555.

Melie, J. P., & Hilgard, E. R. Attitudes toward hypnosis, self-predictions, and hypnotic susceptibility. Inter­national Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 1964, 99-108.

Milgram, S. Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 1963, 371-378.

Milgram, S. Issues in the study of obedience: A reply to Baumrind. American Psychology, 1964, 848-852.

Milgram, S. Some conditions of obedience and disobedience to authority. Human Relations, 1965, ]^, 57-76.

Moore, R. K. Susceptibility to hypnosis and susceptibilityto social influence. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1961.

O'Connor, R. E . Influence of pre-induction instructions, induction format, sex, and locus-of-control expectan­cies upon hypnotizability. Unpublished doctoral dis­sertation, Loyola University of Chicago, 1976.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 95: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

87Orne, M. T. On the social psychology of the psychological

experiment: With particular reference to demand charac­teristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 1962, 776-783.

Orne, M. T. Demand characteristics and their implicationsfor real life : The importance of quasi-controls. Paperpresented at the convention of the American Psychologi­cal Association, Chicago, September 1965.

Pattie, F. A., Jr. Methods of inductions, susceptibility of subjects, and criteria of hypnosis. In R. M. Dorcus (Ed.), Hypno sis and its therapeutic application. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1956.

Pearson, R. Communication and motivation. Part I. A fable. Part II. The brick— A personal experience. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1966, 18-23.

Ricks, J. M. Internal-external control and hypnotic suggest- ibility. UnpublTshed doctoral dissertation, University of Kansas, 1969.

Roberts, M. J. Attention and cognitive controls as related to individual differences in hypnotic susceptTbility. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University,1964.

Rosenhan, D., & London, P. Hypnosis in the unhypnotizable:A study in rote learning. Journal of Experimental Psy­chology, 1963, 30-34.

Rosenthal, R. Experimenter effect in behavioral research.New York : Appleton-Century, 1966.

Sacerdote, P. An analysis of induction procedures in hypno­sis. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1970, 12, 236-253. ------ —

Sanders, S. An exploration of utilization techniques inshort-term hypnotherapy. American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1977, ^(1), 76-79.

Sarbin, T., & Coe, W. Hypnosis: A social-psychologicalanalysis of influence communications. New York:Holt, 1972.

Scheflen, A. E. Regressive one-to-one relationships. Psy­chiatric Quarterly, 1960, 692-709.

Shibata, I. New trend— Hypnotherapy. Japanese Journal of Psychotherapy, 1976, 2(2), 164-165.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 96: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Shor, R. E. Three dimensions of hypnotic depth. Inter­national Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 1962, 23-38.

Shor, R. E., & Orne, M. T. (Eds.). The nature of hypnosis ;Selected basic readings. New York: Holt, Rinehart andWinston, 1965.

Shor, R. E., Orne, M. T., & O'Connell, D. N. Psychological correlates of plateau hypnotizability in a special volunteer sample. Journal of Personal Social Psychol­ogy, 1966, 3, 80-95.

Slotnick, R., & London, P. Influence of instructions on hyp­notic and non-hypnotic performance. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1965, 7_0, 38-46.

Snedocor, G. W., & Cochran, W. G. Statistical methods (6th ed.). Ames: Iowa State University Press, 1968.

Spanos, N. P., & Barber, T. X. Toward a convergence in hyp­nosis research. American Psychologist, 1974, 29(7), 500-510.

Spanos, N. P., & Barber, T. X. Behavior modification and hypnosis. In M. Hersen et al. (Eds.), Progress in behavioral modification (Vol. 3). New York : AcademicPress, 1976.

Spiegel, H. Hypnosis and transference. Archives of General Psychiatry, 1959, 1, 634-639.

Spiegel, H., & Spiegel, D. Trance and treatment. New York: Basic Books, 1978.

Stoline, M. R. Recent developments in multiple comparisons. Handout prepared for the Second Annual Midwest Biophar- maceutical Workshop, Ball State University, Muncie, Indiana, May 22-23, 1979.

Tart, C. T. Types of hypnotic dreams and their relation to hypnotic depth. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 1966,71, 377-382.

Tart, C. T., & Hilgard, E. R. Responsiveness to suggestions under "hypnosis" and "waking-imagination" conditions :A methodological observation. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 1966, , 247-256.

Tinterow, M. M. Foundations of hypnosis. Springfield, 111.: C. C. Thomas, 1970.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 97: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Troffer, S. A., & Tart, C. T. Experimenter bias in hypno­tist performance. Science, 1964, 1 ^ , 1330-1331.

Ulett, G. A., Akpinar, S., & Itil, T. A. Hypnosis by video tape. International Journal of Clinical and Experi­m e n t ^ Hypnosis, 1972, 46-51.

Von Dedenroth, T. E. A. Trance depth: An independent vari­able in therapeutic results. American Journal of Clini- cal Hypnosis, 1962, £, 174-176.

Watkins, J. G. Hypnotherapy of war neurosis. New York: Houghton Mifflin, 1934.

Watzlawick, P., Bravin, J. H., & Jackson, D. D. Pragmatics of human communication : A study of interactional pat­tern^, p^athologies, and paradoxes. New York: W. WT“Norton, 1967.

Watzlawick, P., Weakland, J. H., & Fisch, R. Change ; Prin­ciples of problem formation and problem resolution.New York : Norton, 1974.

Weitzenhoffer, A. M. Hypnotism : An objective study in sug-gestibility. New York: Wiley, 1953.

Weitzenhoffer, A. M. General techniques of hypnotism. New York : Grune & Stratton, 1957.

Weitzenhoffer, A. M. The nature of hypnosis : Part II.American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 1963, 6, 40-72.

Weitzenhoffer, A. M . , & Hilgard, E. R. Stanford Hypnotic Susceptibility Scale: Forms A and B. Palo Alto,Calif.: Consulting Psychologists Press, 1959.

Weitzenhoffer, A, M . , & Sjoberg, B. M., Jr. Suggestibility with and without "induction of hypnosis." Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 1961, 132, 204-220.

White, R. W. Prediction of hypnotic susceptibility from a knowledge of subject's attitudes. Journal of Psychol­ogy, 1937, 3, 265-277.

Wilson, D. L. The role o f confirmation of expectancies inhypnotic induction. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of North Carolina, 1967.

Wilson, S. C., & Barber, T. X. The Creative ImaginationScale as a measure of hypnotic responsiveness: Appli-cations to experimental and clinical hypnosis (Rev. ed.). Medfield, Mass.: Medfield Foundation, 1977.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Page 98: The Effects of Standardized and Personalized Hypnotic Induction Techniques on Depth of Trance

Young, P. C. Is rapport an essential characteristic ofhypnosis? Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1927, 130:T39T

Young, P. C. Suggestion as indirection. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1931, 2^, 69-90.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.