sources of self-efficacy, self-efficacy for self … · learning, and student engagement in...

176
SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF-REGULATED LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD by Ashley Major A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development Ontario Institute for Studies in Education University of Toronto © Copyright by Ashley Major 2016

Upload: others

Post on 18-Jun-2020

12 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF-REGULATED

LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD

by

Ashley Major

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements

for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Graduate Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development

Ontario Institute for Studies in Education

University of Toronto

© Copyright by Ashley Major 2016

Page 2: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

ii

SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF-REGULATED

LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD

Doctor of Philosophy 2016

Ashley Major

Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development

University of Toronto

Abstract

In the study of motivation and achievement, self-efficacy for self-regulated

learning (SESRL) beliefs have been identified as a driving force behind student

engagement in learning. Self-efficacy perceptions are thought to be shaped by four key

sources of information: mastery experiences, vicarious learning, verbal encouragement,

and emotional states. Although adolescents with ADHD exhibit deficits in motivation and

self-regulation, few researchers have examined SESRL beliefs in this population,

including the sources of self-efficacy and their potential influence on student

engagement. Therefore, the objectives of this dissertation were to: (1) examine group

(ADHD versus comparison) and gender differences in the sources of self-efficacy,

SESRL, and student engagement, and (2) investigate the relations between these

variables and symptoms of inattention. Adolescents completed measures of the sources of

self-efficacy, SESRL, and engagement, while parents rated their children’s inattention

symptoms. To address objective one, participants were 98 adolescents (age 14 to 16),

with 47 (30 males, 17 females) classified as having ADHD and 51 (19 males, 32 females)

forming a typically functioning comparison group. To address objective two, the ADHD

and comparison groups were combined to form a larger sample of 105 adolescents (53

males, 52 females). Results revealed that, relative to their non-ADHD peers, adolescents

with ADHD rated themselves as having fewer mastery experiences, less positive

Page 3: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

iii

encouragement from others, lower SESRL beliefs, and lower levels of engagement.

Gender differences also emerged in the sources of self-efficacy, with male adolescents

rating themselves lower than females in mastery experiences, vicarious learning, and

verbal encouragement. Path analysis in the full sample showed that the sources of self-

efficacy and SESRL mediated the relationship between inattention and student

engagement. These results contribute to our understanding of motivation in adolescents

with ADHD and shed light on how they perceive themselves and their school

experiences. Clinical and educational implications involve designing interventions that

not only focus on building knowledge and skills, but also on supporting adolescents in

developing the confidence to succeed.

Page 4: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

iv

Acknowledgements

I wish to take this opportunity to express my sincerest gratitude to a number of

people whose constant guidance and support helped me to accomplish this research.

Firstly, I am grateful to my supervisor, Dr. Rhonda Martinussen, who has been an

incredible source of support, encouragement, and mentorship throughout my graduate

training. Rhonda, thank you for taking me on as a graduate student and putting me on this

path towards a rewarding career in clinical psychology. Your constant enthusiasm and

belief in my work has been invaluable throughout my completion of this dissertation.

Thank you for motivating me to seek answers to questions that will move our

understanding of ADHD forward and touch the lives of families and children affected by

the disorder.

I also wish to thank my committee members, Dr. Judy Wiener and Dr. Ruth

Childs. Your direction, insightful comments, and challenging questions have added depth

to this research project. Without your feedback and unique expertise, my dissertation

would not have reached its full potential. Thank you to Dr. Linda Mason for your

willingness to take on the role of my external examiner and to Dr. Esther Geva for

participating in my defense.

I am also grateful to the parents and adolescents who participated in this research.

Without their involvement, I would not have had the pleasure of learning about the self-

efficacy beliefs of adolescents with ADHD. I would also like to thank my laboratory

colleagues and the research assistants who gave up hours of their time to collect and

manage data related to this work.

Lastly, thanks to my family and friends who have always been so proud of the

work that I have accomplished and have provided me with unrelenting support

throughout this journey. I am grateful for the friendships that I forged at OISE and for

how much these relationships enriched my experience in graduate school. Thank you to

my parents for always trusting my judgment, motiving me to take on new challenges, and

believing in my ability to succeed. I am also grateful to my personal cheerleading squad:

my sister, Stephanie, and my best friends, Nicole and Ashley. Thank you for your

constant support and positivity, for helping me manage the ups and downs of graduate

studies, and for always making me feel like there is nothing that I cannot achieve.

Page 5: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

v

Table of Contents

Abstract…………………….….…………………….…………………………………...ii

Acknowledgements…………..…………………….…………………………………....iv

List of Tables…………………………..………….………..……………………….......vii

List of Figures………………………………………………………………………….viii

Chapter 1. Introduction…………………………………………………………………1

Chapter 2. Literature Review…………………………………………………………...6

ADHD……………………………………………………………………………..6

History, Definition, Prevalence, and Etiology………………………………...6

ADHD as a Disorder of Self-Regulation and Executive Functioning……….13

ADHD as a Deficit in Motivation…………………………………………....15

ADHD and Academic Impairment………………………………………..…17

Developmental Considerations in Adolescence……………………………..19

ADHD and Gender………………………………………......………………23

Student Engagement……………………………………………………………..26

Definition…………………………………………………………………….26

Engagement in Adolescents with ADHD…………………………………....29

Self-Efficacy……………………………………………………………………..33

Theory and Research………………………………………………………...33

Self-Efficacy and Student Engagement……………………………………...39

Self-Efficacy and ADHD…………………………………………………….41

Sources of Self-Efficacy…………………………………………………………45

Theory and Research………………………………………………………...45

The Sources of Self-Efficacy and ADHD…………………………………...51

Rationale and Objectives of the Current Study………………………………….56

Chapter 3. Method……………………………………………………………………...61

Participants……………………………………………………………………….61

Measures…………………………………………………………………………66

Procedure………………………………………………………………………...72

Data Analysis………………………………………………………………….....73

Chapter 4. Results………………………………………………………………………76 Research Objective 1…………………………………………………………….76

Research Objective 2…………………………………………………………….82

Preliminary Analyses………………………………………………………....82

Assessment of the Measurement Model……………………………………...85

Assessment of the Structural Model………………………………………….85

Supplemental Analyses…………………………………………………….....89

Chapter 5. Discussion…………………………………………………………………..90

Group Differences in the Sources, SESRL, and Engagement…………………...91

Relations Among Inattention, the Sources, SESRL, and Engagement…………101

Page 6: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

vi

Limitations and Future Directions……………….……………………..……....107

Implications for Clinicians and Educators……………………………………...112

Conclusion………………………………………………………………….......117

References……………………………………………………………….…………..…118

Page 7: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

vii

List of Tables

Table 1. Sample Characteristics of the ADHD Group, Comparison Group, and Full

Sample………………………………………………………….……………..64

Table 2. Sample Characteristics of Male and Female Adolescents in the ADHD and

Comparison Groups….………………………………………………………..77

Table 3. Group Differences in the Sources of Self-Efficacy, SESRL, and Student

Engagement………….……………………………………..…………………80

Table 4. Bivariate Correlations among Relevant Sample Characteristics, Parent-

Reported Inattention, the Sources of Self-Efficacy, SESRL, and Student

Engagement in the Full Sample………….……………….………………….84

Table 5. Standardized Direct, Indirect, and Total Effects from Parent-Rated Inattention

to Student Engagement…………………..…………………………………..88

Page 8: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

viii

List of Figures

Figure 1. Hypothesized Model Depicting the Relations among Parent-Rated Inattention,

the Sources of Self-Efficacy, SESRL, and Student Engagement…………….60

Figure 2. Structural Model Depicting the Relations among Parent-Rated Inattention, the

Sources of Self-Efficacy, SESRL, and Student Engagement ………………………..87

Page 9: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

1

SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF-REGULATED

LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental

disorder characterized by persistent difficulties with sustaining attention, inhibiting

behaviour, and regulating activity levels (U.S. Department of Education, 2003). These

core symptoms create a host of challenges for individuals with ADHD, including

functional impairment across home, school, and social settings (American Psychiatric

Association, 2013). ADHD is typically diagnosed in childhood, with approximately 5 to

7 percent of children affected (Polanczyk, Willcutt, Salum, Kieling, & Rohde, 2014;

Willcutt, 2012). Although prevalence rates decline with age (Biederman, Mick, &

Faraone, 2000; Biederman, Petty, Evans, Smally, & Faraone, 2010), many adolescents

who were diagnosed as children will continue to experience subthreshold levels of

symptoms as well as clinically significant impairment across multiple domains of

functioning (Hinshaw et al., 2006; Lahey et al., 2004; Lee, Lahey, Owens, & Hinshaw,

2008; Mannuzza et al., 1998; Sibley et al., 2012). Relative to children, however, there is a

small research base in adolescents with ADHD, which limits our understanding of

ADHD across the life-span (Faraone, 2013). Of particular concern in adolescents with

ADHD is poor educational outcomes, as academic underachievement has been associated

with high school dropout and significant occupational and socioeconomic disadvantage

(Barbaresi, Katusic, Colligan, Weaver, & Jacobsen, 2007; Barkley, Fischer, Edelbrock, &

Smallish, 1990; Biederman et al., 2006; Faraone et al., 2000; Kent et al., 2011; Murphy &

Page 10: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

2

Barkley, 1996). Therefore, it is a priority that researchers continue to identify those

factors that promote academic success in individuals with ADHD, particularly during

adolescence, a period of development that is marked by a number of transitions and

increasing academic and social demands (Holmbeck, Friedman, Abad, & Jandasek, 2006;

Litner, 2003).

Low academic motivation is particularly salient during adolescence and is a major

factor that negatively influences the academic functioning of adolescents (Schunk &

Mullen, 2012). Motivation is often defined as the process whereby goal-directed

activities are energized, directed, and sustained (Schunk, Pintrich, & Meece, 2008). It is a

complex and multi-faceted construct that comprises cognitive (e.g., self-perceptions,

beliefs), affective (e.g., interest), and behavioural (e.g., engagement, effort and

persistence) components (Schunk et al., 2008; Schunk & Mullen, 2012). In typically

developing children and adolescents, student engagement is one of the most valuable

motivational variables to examine when exploring student behaviours that are linked to

improved educational functioning (Archambault, Janosz, Morizot, & Pagani, 2009;

Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 1996; Greenwood, 1996; Whitlock, 2006).

Student engagement is often viewed as the manifestation of motivation, or how one’s

cognitions, behaviors, and emotional reactions are energized, directed, and sustained

during learning and other academic activities (Skinner, Kindermann, Connell, &

Wellborn, 2009). Students who are more engaged at school experience more frequent

learning opportunities, achieve higher grades, and are less likely to drop out (e.g.,

Archambault et al., 2009; Bandura et al., 1996; Finn & Zimmer, 2012; Marks, 2000;

Wang & Holcombe, 2010; Whitlock, 2006). Although there is ample evidence to support

Page 11: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

3

a motivational dysfunction in adolescents with ADHD (Barkley, 1997; Carlson, Booth,

Shin, & Canu, 2002; Hoza, Waschbusch, Owens, Pelham, & Kipp, 2001), very few

researchers have directly investigated their engagement levels, despite the increased risk

for underachievement and dropout in this population (e.g., Loe & Feldman, 2007;

Wehmeier, Schacht, & Barkley, 2010). Given that educators can foster engagement

behaviours in students to enhance educational outcomes (e.g., Christenson, 2009;

Christenson et al., 2008; Reschly & Christenson, 2006), there is considerable value to

understanding student engagement in adolescents with ADHD.

Although there are different theoretical approaches used to explain student

motivation and engagement, the present study draws on Bandura’s (1986, 1997, 2001)

social cognitive theory of psychological functioning. This model emphasizes that

individuals are active agents who are engaged in shaping their own development and

outcomes. Bandura proposed that self-efficacy beliefs—or one’s perceived capabilities

for learning or performing actions at designated levels—are a key cognitive variable that

influence motivation, self-regulation and, in particular, student engagement (Bandura,

1986, 1997; Schunk & Mullen, 2012). Self-efficacy for self-regulated learning (SESRL) is

of particular interest in the current study and refers to the beliefs that students hold about

their ability to use self-regulated learning strategies to cope with difficult learning

conditions (Bandura, 2006). There is a substantial amount of literature demonstrating that

SESRL beliefs affect the degree to which students are engaged in their learning; in turn,

this engagement promotes their competence as learners and their academic achievement

(Bandura, 1997; Caraway, Tucker, Reinke, & Hall, 2003; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2003;

Pajares, 1996; Schunk & Pajares, 2009; Usher & Pajares, 2006; Walker, Greene, &

Page 12: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

4

Mansell, 2006). Unfortunately, students who have low levels of confidence in their

ability to self-regulate their learning are less likely to rely on adaptive strategies when

faced with challenging tasks and are more likely to give up in the face of difficulty

(Pajares, 1996). Despite the solid foundation of self-efficacy research in typically

developing children and adolescents, fewer scholars have assessed its relevance for

adolescents with ADHD. This is of critical importance, as understanding the self-efficacy

beliefs of adolescents with ADHD and their role in student engagement may serve to

inform existing academic, pharmacological, and behavioural interventions. Furthermore,

adolescents with ADHD may be particularly at risk for low SESRL given that self-

regulation weaknesses characterize many children and adolescents with the disorder (e.g.,

Johnson & Reid, 2011; Toplak, Bucciarelli, Jain, & Tannock, 2009). Prominent

theoretical models of ADHD suggest that individuals with ADHD have difficulties using

higher-order cognitive skills, referred to as executive functions (EFs; e.g., planning, goal-

setting, self-monitoring, task initiation, self-motivation, working memory, emotion

regulation), to self-regulate and attain their goals (Barkley, 2012). While deficits in EFs

and self-regulation in adolescents with ADHD are well-documented (e.g., Barkley,

Murphy, & Bush, 2001; Nigg, Hinshaw, Carte, & Treuting, 1998; Toplak et al., 2009;

Valera, Faraone, Murray, & Seidman, 2007; Willcutt et al., 2010; Willcutt et al., 2005),

little is known regarding their beliefs about their ability to self-regulate their learning.

This study’s findings will provide a unique contribution to this literature and will expand

our knowledge of the academic functioning of these adolescents.

Although it has been widely established that self-efficacy beliefs can powerfully

influence academic outcomes (see Pajares, 1996 for a review), less is known about how

Page 13: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

5

these beliefs develop, particularly in adolescents with ADHD. Embedded in his social

cognitive theory, Bandura (1997, 2000) suggested that self-efficacy beliefs are acquired

through four major sources: previous experiences of success (mastery experiences);

exposure to and identification with efficacious models (vicarious experiences); verbal

encouragement and support from others (verbal/social persuasion); and emotional

reactions in the context of task performance (physiological responses/arousal). Consistent

with this theory, research on typically developing adolescents indicates that these four

sources account for a significant proportion of variance in academic self-efficacy and

SESRL (e.g., Anderson & Betz, 2001; Usher & Pajares, 2006, 2008). There is evidence

to suggest that adolescents with disabilities, including those with ADHD, may have less

access to these sources and that this reduced access may influence the development of

self-efficacy in these individuals (Hampton, 1998; Hampton & Mason, 2003). However,

there is no known research examining the sources of self-efficacy in adolescents with

ADHD. Understanding how the sources of self-efficacy relate to enhanced self-efficacy

beliefs is of particular interest to those working with adolescents with ADHD in

secondary schools, as this represents a time when self-efficacy beliefs are of critical

importance to academic success (Caprara et al., 2008; Zimmerman, 2000).

The overall objective of this dissertation is to expand the literature on motivation

in adolescents with ADHD by examining important social cognitive and motivational

variables including sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and student engagement, as well as

the relations among these variables, in a sample of adolescents with and without ADHD.

Page 14: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

6

CHAPTER 2

LITERATURE REVIEW

The present chapter will provide an overview of relevant constructs that are the

focus of this dissertation, including definitions of key variables and a review of existing

research. This chapter will also briefly describe the history of ADHD and current trends

in the ADHD literature, as well as relevant research in the areas of self-efficacy, the

sources of self-efficacy, and student engagement. It will conclude with a description of

the primary objectives and rationale for the present research. I will also explain how this

research intends to contribute to our understanding of ADHD. The methods for this

research study are outlined in Chapter 3, and Chapter 4 will describe the study results.

This dissertation will end with Chapter 6, in which I provide a discussion of the results,

relevant clinical and practical implications, the study limitations, and directions for future

research.

ADHD

History, Definition, Prevalence, and Etiology

The conceptualization of ADHD has evolved considerably throughout its history.

Symptoms consistent with ADHD were first described in the medical literature in 1775

by German physician, Melchior Adam Weikard (Barkley & Peters, 2012). By the early

20th century, observations of ADHD-like behaviours were increasingly documented;

however, it was not until 1968 that an early definition of the disorder, termed

“Hyperkinetic Reaction of Childhood,” was officially recorded in the Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-II; Barkley, 2006). By the 1970s, the

growth of systematic research led to a reconceptualization of the condition, including a

Page 15: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

7

recognition that many children displayed symptoms of inattention without significant

levels of hyperactivity (Barkley, 2006). With the third edition of the DSM published in

1980 came the first reliable operational diagnostic criteria for “Attention Deficit Disorder

(ADD), with or without hyperactivity.” The criteria included symptom lists for

inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity, cutoff scores for symptoms, guidelines for age

of onset and duration, and exclusionary criteria (Barkley, 2006; Lange, Reichi, Lange,

Tucha, & Tucha, 2010). The disorder was renamed to its current label “Attention Deficit-

Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)” in a revised version of the DSM-III. With the

publication of the DSM-IV in 1994, factor analytic research supported the existence of

three subtypes (a predominately inattentive type; a predominately hyperactive-impulsive

type; and a combined type with symptoms of both dimensions) along with increasing

recognition of the disorder beyond childhood (American Psychiatric Association 1994;

Barkley, 2006; Lahey et al. 1994; Lange et al., 2010).

The most recent update to ADHD nosology came in 2013 with the release of the

DSM-5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; Epstein & Loren, 2013). This update

included several modifications to previous definitions of the disorder including a greater

consideration regarding the manifestation of symptoms of ADHD in adolescents and

adults as well as the elimination of subtypes with the inclusion of specifiers depicting

current presentation (Epstein & Loren, 2013). This latter shift from ‘subtypes’ to

‘presentations’ reflected research demonstrating that the DSM-IV subtypes do not

accurately represent discrete subgroups with adequate long-term stability, but that

symptoms are fluid states that change across an individual’s lifespan (Lemiere et al.,

2010; Willcutt et al., 2012). Another notable shift in the DSM-5 from previous editions is

Page 16: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

8

the conceptualization of ADHD as a ‘neurodevelopmental disorder’ rather than a

‘disruptive behaviour disorder.’ This shift not only captures the increasing evidence for

brain abnormalities in ADHD, but it also has important implications for how ADHD is

understood and treated by parents, clinicians, and educators (Epstein & Loren, 2013).

According to the definition of ADHD in the DSM-5, Criterion A requires

individuals to demonstrate six or more symptoms of inattention and/or

hyperactivity/impulsivity for children up to age 16, or five or more for individuals 17 and

older. Criterion B indicates that symptoms must be present before the age of 12, and

Criterion C states that these symptoms must occur in two or more settings (e.g., home,

school, work, social settings). According to Criterion D, there must be clear evidence that

the symptoms interfere with, or reduce the quality of an individual’s functioning. Lastly,

Criterion E states that the symptoms must not be better explained by another psychiatric

disorder (e.g., Schizophrenia, Mood Disorder, Anxiety Disorder). Based on the

combination of symptoms, three ‘presentations’ of ADHD can occur: a predominately

inattentive presentation, a predominately hyperactive-impulsive presentation, and a

combined presentation. Presentations are identified based on an individual’s current

symptomatology and can change over time (American Psychological Association, 2013;

Epstein & Loren, 2013). Furthermore, modifiers (e.g., mild, moderate, and severe) can be

used to indicate symptom severity (Epstein & Loren, 2013).

Like other psychological and mental health conditions, there is no single,

objective test used to diagnose ADHD. In children and adolescents, current practice

guidelines indicate that ADHD parent and teacher reports of diagnostic criteria should be

used to evaluate ADHD symptoms and functional impairment (American Academy of

Page 17: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

9

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 2007; American Academy of Pediatrics, 2001).

Information can be collected via diagnostic interviews or validated ratings scales,

although most clinicians combine informant reports to examine symptom patterns across

settings (De Los Reyes & Kazdin, 2005; Offord et al., 1996; Pelham, Fabiano, &

Massetti, 2005; Piacentini, Cohen, & Cohen, 1992; Rubio-Stipec, Fitzmaurice, Murphy,

& Walker, 2003; Seixas, Weiss, & Muller, 2012; Wright, Waschbusch, & Frankland,

2007). Clinicians gather information regarding symptom presentation, age of onset, and

functional impairment, and determine whether symptoms are more persistent, severe, and

impairing than what is expected based on the child’s developmental level (American

Psychiatric Association, 2015; Faraone et al., 2015; Seixas et al., 2012).

ADHD is one of the most common neuropsychological disorders affecting

children (Faraone et al., 2015). In 2012, Willcutt completed a meta-analytic review of 86

studies and found that 5.9% to 7.1% of children and adolescents worldwide met DSM-IV

diagnostic criteria for ADHD. Although epidemiological studies exclusively in Canadian

samples are less common, a study by Brault and Lacourse (2012) revealed that, between

2006 and 2007, 4.1% of Canadian school-age children had an ADHD diagnosis. Despite

growing concerns among professionals and the public that ADHD is on the rise, when

standardized diagnostic procedures are followed, there is no empirical evidence to

support a genuine increase in the number of ADHD diagnoses over the last three decades

(Polanczyk et al., 2014; Singh, 2008; Timimi, 2004).

Prevalence rates for ADHD have been examined in relation to other

sociodemographic variables, including country of origin, gender, and socioeconomic

status. Although estimates vary widely across studies, researchers have found that, after

Page 18: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

10

controlling for variability in how ADHD is defined, there are no significant differences in

the number of ADHD case across countries in Europe, Africa, Australia, Asia, and the

Americas (Polanczyk et al., 2014; Willcutt, 2012). With respect to gender differences,

ADHD more commonly affects males in childhood and adolescence, with a male-to-

female ratio of 4:1 in clinical samples and 2.4:1 in community samples (Polanczyk, de

Lima, Horta, Biederman, & Rohde, 2007). In adulthood, these differences nearly

disappear (e.g., Matte et al., 2015), leading some researchers to question the accuracy of

gender ratios in childhood. Some have suggested a possible referral bias in males due to

their increased tendency to show disruptive behaviours (e.g., Gershon & Gershon, 2002;

Solden, 1995), while others have speculated that the DSM criteria do not adequately

capture symptoms in females (e.g., Nadeau & Quinn, 2002; Staller & Faraone, 2006);

however, longitudinal research is still needed to clarify this debate (Nussbaum, 2012).

Lastly, some studies have connected low household income to increased rates of ADHD

(e.g., Larsson, Sariaslan, Langstrom, D’Onofrio, & Lichtenstein, 2014). Because ADHD

runs in families and often leads to academic and occupational underachievement, high

rates of unemployment in this group may contribute to an overestimation of

socioeconomic disadvantage (Biederman et al., 2008; Faraone et al., 2015).

The etiology of ADHD is complex and multifactorial. Genetic studies have

revealed that ADHD is a highly heritable condition. First-degree relatives of those with

ADHD are two to eight times more likely to develop the disorder than the relatives of

non-affected individuals (Faraone, et al., 2005). Moreover, twin studies report heritability

rates ranging from 71 to 90 percent (Faraone et al., 2005; Nikolas & Burt, 2010; Thapar,

Holmes, Poulton, & Harrington, 1999). Current research suggests that genetic risk is

Page 19: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

11

likely caused by multiple common genetic variants of small effect size, rather than by one

or two genes per se (Neale, et al., 2010; Steinhausen, 2009). Moreover, it is thought that

genetic factors influence the onset, persistence, and expression of ADHD symptoms

across the lifespan (Willcutt et al., 2012; Swanson et al., 2001).

Brain imaging studies have found abnormalities in the structure and functioning

of the brain. Structurally, ADHD is associated with a smaller overall brain volume (e.g.,

Castellanos et al., 2002; Durston, et al., 2004; Faraone et al., 2015; Greven, et al., 2015),

as well as smaller volumes in specific regions of the brain, including the pre-frontal

cortex and cerebellum (Faraone et al., 2015; Frodl & Skokauskas, 2012; Stoodley &

Schmahmann, 2009). With respect to brain functioning, functional MRI studies have

provided support for underactivation across multiple neural pathways, including the

frontostriatal, frontoparietal, and ventral attentional networks. There is also evidence for

overactivation of the somatomotor and visual systems in individuals with ADHD relative

to controls. These neural pathways are implicated in cognitive processes such as

attentional control, response to reward, inhibitory control, salience thresholds, and motor

behaviour (Cortese, et al., 2012; Doehnert, Brandeis, Imhof, Drechsler, & Steinhausen,

2010; Faraone et al., 2015; Plichta & Scheres, 2014). In addition to dysregulation of

several brain regions and pathways, a recent meta-analysis (Scassellati,

Bonvicini, Faraone, & Gennarelli, 2012) identified certain biochemical markers (e.g.,

norepinephrine, 3-methoxy-4-hydroxyphenylethylene glycol, monoamine oxidase, Zinc,

and cortisol) that help differentiate individuals with ADHD from non-affected individuals

(Scassellati et al., 2012). Despite the increases in genetic and neuroimaging studies in

ADHD, the link between underlying brain abnormalities and symptom expression across

Page 20: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

12

development remains unclear. At the present time, evidence supports a lag in the

development of ADHD-related brain structures and functions, and this lag does not seem

to completely disappear in adulthood (Almeida et al., 2010; Shaw et al., 2013;

Steinhausen, 2009).

ADHD has been associated with a wide range of environmental risk factors

including pre- and perinatal factors, such as maternal smoking, alcohol, and substance

use, low birth weight, prematurity, maternal stress; environmental toxins, such as lead,

polychlorinated biphenyls, and organophosphate pesticides; dietary influences, such as

zinc deficiencies. There is also evidence of an association between ADHD and

psychosocial factors, such as parental hostility and severe early deprivation

(Banerjee, Middleton, & Faraone, 2007; Faraone et al., 2015; Harold et al., 2013;

Milberger, Biederman, Faraone, Chen, & Jones, 1996; Skoglund, Chen,

D’Onofrio, Lichtenstein, & Larsson, 2014; Stevens, et al., 2008; Thapar, Coopoer, Eyre,

& Langley, 2013). Research examining environmental risk factors for ADHD is plagued

by challenges such as how to identify which factors are causal versus simply correlational

(Lahey, D’Onofrio, & Waldman, 2009; Rutter, 2007; Thapar & Rutter, 2009). It is likely

that some observed relationships may be due to an unmeasured third variable while

others may be due to child and/or parental behaviours that exert influence on the

environment (Faraone et al., 2015; Thapar et al., 2013). Nonetheless, results from

etiological studies strongly suggest that, in most cases, no single risk factor is necessary

or sufficient to cause ADHD (Faraone et al., 2015). Rather, both neurobiological and

environmental variables are implicated in the disorder and they continuously and

dynamically interact over time to influence its phenotypical presentation. This

Page 21: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

13

presentation includes a heterogeneous profile of brain abnormalities, neurocognitive

deficits, psychopathology, and impairment across the life span (Faraone et al., 2015;

Thapar et al., 2013).

ADHD as a Disorder of Self-Regulation and Executive Functioning

Several theoretical models of ADHD exist in the literature. The most influential

are neuro-cognitive in nature and consider EF deficits as central to the etiology of ADHD

(e.g., Barkley, 1997, 2006; Castellanos & Tannock, 2002; Sonuga-Barke, 2002).

Although there is no agreed upon definition of executive functioning, the term is often

used to describe a broad set of higher-order cognitive processes (e.g., set shifting,

inhibition, working memory, planning) that allows an individual to engage in flexible,

goal-directed behaviour (Castellanos, Sonuga-Barke, Milham, & Tannock, 2006).

According to an influential model proposed by Barkley (1997, 2006), ADHD and its

associated difficulties arise from a developmental delay in or acquired impairment of the

behavioural inhibition networks of the brain that disrupt self-regulation. Self-regulation

involves any self-directed action to change one’s own behaviour in order to alter the

probability of a future consequence (e.g., Barkley, 1997, 2006). In this model,

behavioural inhibition is critical to the performance of four key EFs, which in turn

influence motor control and self-regulation. These four EFs include: 1) nonverbal

working memory; 2) verbal working memory (internalized, self-directed speech); 3) self-

regulation of affect, motivation, and arousal (self-directed emotion); 4) planning or

reconstitution (self-directed play; Barkley, 1997, 2006). When inhibition is

compromised, as hypothesized in individuals with ADHD, the efficient performance of

Page 22: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

14

these EFs and, therefore, self-regulation and adaptability, are adversely affected (Barkley,

1997, 2006).

Some aspects of Barkley’s model are well supported empirically. For instance,

results of a meta-analysis examining EFs in ADHD reveal that ADHD is associated with

weaknesses on measures of most EF domains using performance measures (Willcutt,

Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). There is also evidence of weaknesses when

rating scale assessments of EFs are used (e.g., Toplak et al., 2009). This association

between ADHD and EF impairments remains significant even when group differences in

intelligence, reading ability, or comorbid conditions are controlled (e.g., Barkley et al.,

2001; Nigg et al., 1998; Willcutt et al., 2010; Willcutt et al., 2005). Furthermore, brain-

imaging studies, which have found that the underlying brain structures and functions

associated with EFs are compromised in adolescents with ADHD (Valera et al., 2007),

provide further support for the EF challenges facing many individuals with the disorder.

Although EF impairments appear to be associated with ADHD, the claim that EF deficits

are universal in the disorder is not well supported. For instance, overall effect sizes show

that each specific EF deficit (including behavioural inhibition) rarely accounts for more

than 10% of the variance in ADHD symptoms (Castellanos et al., 2006; Thissen et al.,

2014). Studies have found that the proportion of children and adolescents with ADHD

who have an EF deficit ranges from 30 to 50 percent, suggesting that weaknesses in EF

are not necessary or sufficient to cause ADHD (e.g., Biederman et al., 2004; Loo et al.,

2007; Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga-Barke, 2005). In addition, because behavioural

inhibition and EFs are linked to motor control, Barkley’s (1997, 2006) theoretical model

only applies to cases in which hyperactivity-impulsivity are observed (Barkley, 1997).

Page 23: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

15

Thus, executive and self-regulatory deficits may be one independent path to ADHD;

however, given the heterogeneity of the disorder, it is likely that other pathways are

implicated (Wicks-Nelson & Israel, 2003).

ADHD as a Deficit in Motivation

Other influential models of ADHD represent a departure from the dominant

neuro-cognitive paradigms and emphasize a motivational dysfunction as an alternative

pathway to ADHD (e.g., Douglas, 1989; Haenlein & Caul, 1987; Sergeant, Oosterlaan, &

VanderMeere, 1999; Sonuga-Barke, 2002). For instance, Sonuga-Barke (1994) proposed

a delay aversion hypothesis, which is based on the assumption that ADHD

symptomatology and behaviours are manifestations of an underlying motivational style

(delay aversion) associated with fundamental alterations in reward mechanisms. More

specifically, this model suggests that individuals with ADHD are motivated to escape or

avoid delay. Thus, when faced with a choice between immediacy and delay, individuals

with ADHD will most often choose immediacy. When no choice is available, they will

act in a way to reduce their perception of time by either creating or attending to non-

temporal features of the environment. Therefore, symptoms of inattention, impulsivity,

and hyperactivity are functional expressions of this delay aversion (Sonuga-Barke, 1994,

1998, 2003; Swanson, Sergeant, Taylor, Sonuga-Barke, Jensen, & Cantwell, 1998). In

this model, cognitive deficits, such as working memory and planning, arise as secondary

effects of delay aversion associated with patterns of reduced task engagement (Sonuga-

Barke, Williams, Hall, Saxton, 1996).

There is evidence to support a motivational dysfunction and altered sensitivity to

reward in individuals with ADHD. Children and adolescents with ADHD demonstrate

Page 24: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

16

better performance under conditions of reward relative to controls (Luman, Oosterlaan, &

Sergeant, 2005; Rosch & Hawk, 2013), and they prefer immediate over large delayed

rewards under many circumstances, supporting the delay aversion hypothesis (Carlson &

Tamm, 2000; Kuntsi, Oosterlaan, Stevenson, 2001; McInerny & Kerns, 2003; Sonuga-

Barke, 2002, 2003; Sonuga-Barke et al., 1992). In addition, psychophysiological studies

have found evidence for reduced physiological responding (i.e., differences in heart rate

and skin conductance) under conditions of reward versus nonreward (Crone et al., 2003;

Iaboni et al., 1997), while data from brain imaging studies highlights abnormalities in the

neural circuits and dopamine receptors involved in motivation and reward processing

(Cubillo, Halari, Smith, Taylor, & Rubia, 2012; Schultz, Tremblay, & Hollerman, 2000;

Sonuga-Barke, Dalen, & Remington, 2003; Sonuga-Barke & Sergeant, 2005; Volkow et

al., 2011). Objectively, lower rates of persistence and effort on academic tasks, higher

levels of frustration when faced with a challenge, and a greater preference for easy work

compared to their non-ADHD peers seem to reflect motivational impairments in children

and adolescents with ADHD (Barkley, 1997; Carlson et al., 2002; Hoza et al., 2001).

Children with ADHD also benefit from behaviour modification programs in which

reinforcement contingencies are central (Barkley, 2002), with improvements in

performance documented in school, home, and extracurricular settings (Hupp, Reitman,

Northup, O’Callaghan, & LeBlanc, 2002; Kelly & McCain, 1995; Luman et al., 2005;

Pelham et al., 1993; Pelham & Hinshaw, 1992; Rapport, Murphy, & Bailey, 1982). In

addition, there is evidence to suggest that behaviours consistent with low motivation in

adolescents with ADHD are more strongly linked to symptoms of inattention than

symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity (Langberg et al., 2010; Power, Werba, Watkins,

Page 25: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

17

Angelucci, & Eiraldi, 2006; Sasser et al., 2015), highlighting the importance of

considering the role of inattention in relation to motivational variables. In a recent study

by Plamondon and Martinussen (2015), researchers found that academic achievement

scores in mathematics, reading, and writing were predicted by variance shared among

inattention, intrinsic motivation, and behavioural engagement, rather than the unique

variance explained by each of the three predictors. These findings highlight the

importance of examining the shared impact of inattention and motivational variables on

achievement-related outcomes.

Although theoretical models of ADHD are still evolving, the existing evidence

suggests that neither the executive nor motivational models alone can fully account for

the heterogeneous nature of the disorder. In recent years, more comprehensive models

have been proposed, emphasizing multiple pathways to ADHD (e.g., Castellanos et al.,

2006; Lopez-Vergara & Colder, 2013; Nigg, Goldsmith, & Sachek, 2004; Sonuga-Barke,

2002, 2005). These models, however, generally focus on cognitive and behavioural

responses, rarely taking into account beliefs or attitudes. Thus, a comprehensive model of

ADHD would be one that identifies and explains dysfunction in multiple processes,

including EF, motivation, and self-perceptions, while demonstrating links to underlying

neural networks in the brain (Barkley, 2014).

ADHD and Academic Impairment

Impairment in the academic domain is one of the most significant challenges

faced by children and adolescents with ADHD (DuPaul & Stoner, 2003). Academic

impairment in this population is of principal concern given that chronic

underachievement in childhood is associated with reduced occupational attainment in

Page 26: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

18

adulthood, as well as higher unemployment rates and lower socioeconomic status

(Biederman et al., 2006; Faraone et al., 2000; Murphy & Barkley, 1996).

Poor academic outcomes are well documented in children and adolescents with

ADHD. Specifically, they obtain lower scores on standardized achievement measures,

have lower grade point averages, and are more likely to repeat a grade compared to their

non-ADHD peers (Barbaresi et al., 2007; Bauermeister et al., 2007; Ek, Westerlund,

Holmberg, & Fernell, 2011; Frazier, Youngstrom, Glutting, & Watkins, 2007; Lahey et

al., 2004; Rogers, Hwang, Toplak, Weiss, & Tannock, 2011). They are also more likely

to be suspended or expelled and have higher rates of absenteeism than students without

ADHD (Kent et al., 2011; LeFever, Willers, Morrow, & Vaughn, 2002). A large

proportion of children with ADHD experience learning difficulties, with up to 30%

meeting criteria for a specific learning disability (LD; Biederman, Newcorn, &

Sprich, 1991; Hinshaw, 1992). Although their IQ scores can span the spectrum of

intelligence, as a group, children with ADHD tend to score slightly lower on intelligence

tests than children without ADHD (Biederman et al., 1991; Jepsen, Fagerlund,

Mortensen, 2009). It is not surprising given their academic challenges that children with

ADHD use special education services more often than their non-ADHD peers (Jensen et

al., 2004; LeFever et al., 2002). For instance, they are often placed in special education

classrooms and are more likely than their peers to use ancillary services, such as remedial

services, tutoring, curriculum accommodations, and after-school programs (Loe &

Feldman, 2007).

A number of researchers have examined the link between the core symptoms of

ADHD and underachievement in both community and clinical samples. Results from

Page 27: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

19

community samples have consistently demonstrated that inattention predicts poor

performance on standardized achievement measures as well as functional outcomes such

as grade failure and high school dropout (Duncan et al., 2007; Fergusson & Horwood,

1995; Fergusson, Lynskey, & Horwood, 1997; Galera, Melchior, Chastang, Bouvard, &

Fombonne, 2009; Gray, Martinussen, Rogers, & Tannock, 2015). In a clinical sample,

Massetti and colleagues (2008) found that children with ADHD-Inattentive Type were

more likely to obtain lower scores on achievement tests in adolescence than those who

were diagnosed with ADHD-Combined Type. Moreover, the relationship between

inattention and underachievement is not explained by co-occurring behaviors such as

hyperactivity/impulsivity, conduct problems, or anxiety (Duncan et al., 2007; Massetti et

al., 2008; Miller, Nevado-Montenegro, & Hinshaw, 2012; Polderman, Boomsma, Bartels,

Verhulst, & Huizink, 2010), suggesting that inattention may be a unique predictor of

underachievement in individuals with ADHD (Langberg et al., 2011).

Developmental Considerations in Adolescence

ADHD is a persistent and debilitating disorder. Although most cases are identified

in childhood, studies have shown that between 70 and 80 percent of children with the

diagnosis will continue to meet criteria for ADHD in adolescence (Babinski et al., 2011;

Biederman et al., 2000; Biederman et al., 2010; Bussing, Mason, Bell, Porter, & Garvan,

2010; Lee et al., 2008; Sibley et al., 2012). Of those adolescents who do not qualify for a

diagnosis, many will still experience subthreshold levels of symptoms as well as

clinically significant impairment across multiple domains of functioning (Hinshaw et al.,

2006; Lahey et al., 2004; Lee et al., 2008; Mannuzza et al., 1998; Sibley et al., 2012).

Adolescence is a particularly challenging period of development for all youth, as it is

Page 28: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

20

marked by physical changes, increasing social and academic demands, and the search for

autonomy and a healthy sense of self (Holmbeck et al., 2006; Litner, 2003). Effectively

achieving these adolescent milestones relies on a number of skills, many of which are

inherently limited by ADHD (e.g., social cognition, EF, self-awareness, affect

processing, motivation; Barkley, 1997; Brim & Whitaker, 2000; Da Fonseca, Seguier,

Santos, Poinso, & Deruelle, 2009; Litner, 2003; Luman et al., 2005; McQuade et al.,

2011; Marton, Weiner, Rogers, Moore, & Tannock, 2008; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996;

Uekermann et al., 2010). Thus, like their peers, adolescents with ADHD must effectively

navigate the transition from childhood to adolescence; however, unlike their peers, they

are doing so with the added challenges that result from the symptoms, cognitive deficits,

and functional impairments that accompany ADHD. It is not surprising, therefore, that

very few young children diagnosed with ADHD at school entry are well-adjusted once

they reach adolescence (Lee et al., 2008).

The educational impairments associated with ADHD are persistent, as studies

have shown that in approximately 75% of children with ADHD, poor academic outcomes

continue to adolescence (Hechtman, 2000). ADHD can create a host of academic

challenges for adolescents in post-secondary education. In addition to many of the

academic difficulties noted in children, teens with ADHD are more likely to fail their

courses, receive lower grades across subject areas, and select classes with less

academically rigorous requirements. They are also more likely to be late or absent during

the academic year, and are less likely to complete and turn in assignments compared to

their peers without ADHD (Barbaresi et al., 2007; Barkley et al., 1990; Kent et al., 2011).

Furthermore, LDs that may have been previously overlooked in the more supportive

Page 29: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

21

elementary school environment can become evident with the increased demands in high

school (Brook & Boaz, 2005; Robin, 1997).

Adolescents with ADHD are also at an elevated risk for high school dropout (e.g.,

Barbaresi et al., 2007; Barkley et al., 2006; Frazier et al., 2007). One study demonstrated

that adolescents with ADHD are 2.7 times more likely to drop out before high school

graduation and that they are less likely to pursue postsecondary education compared to

their non-ADHD counterparts (Barbaresi et al., 2007). Furthermore, Fried and colleagues

(2013) found that ADHD status predicted high school dropout in adolescents,

independent of confounds such as IQ, social class, and LD status. Even in community

samples of children, students with attention problems are more likely to drop out of high

school than students without attention problems, suggesting that inattention may be a

unique risk factor for high school dropout (Pagani et al., 2008). High school dropout is of

particular concern in adolescents with ADHD, given their pre-existing cognitive and

functional limitations. Moreover, it places them at high risk for the many disadvantages

faced by those who do not graduate with a high school diploma, including fewer

employment opportunities, lower annual incomes, and higher rates of unemployment,

incarceration, poverty, and long-term dependency on social services (Appleton,

Christenson, & Furlong, 2008; Barton, 2004; Trampush, Miller, Newcorn, Halperin,

2009; Christenson, Sinclair, Lehr, & Hurley, 2000).

The EF and motivational deficits that are commonly found in children with

ADHD may become particularly salient during the adolescent years. Upon entering high

school, the demands of the environment increase dramatically, as students are expected to

be more self-directed in their learning, master more material, and juggle multiple classes

Page 30: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

22

with different teachers (Robin, 1997). Therefore, it is not surprising that EF weaknesses

in individuals with ADHD remain robust in adolescence (Toplack, Bucciarelli, Jain, &

Tannock, 2009; Wasserstein, 2005), and these weaknesses are not accounted for by age,

intellectual functioning, comorbid disorders, or gender differences (Martel et al., 2007).

Similarly, critical learning skills that rely on EFs, such as studying, note-taking, test-

taking, summarizing, planning, organizing, and time management, are also significantly

impaired in adolescents with ADHD and have been linked to reduced academic

performance (Demaray & Jenkins, 2011; Gureasko-Moore, Dupaul, & White, 2006;

Meyer & Kelley, 2007; Reaser et al., 2007; Volpe at al., 2006; Zwart & Kallemeyn,

2001). Few studies have examined motivation in adolescents with ADHD longitudinally;

however, research in typically developing children has found evidence for a decline in

several aspects of motivation from elementary to high school (e.g., Anderman & Maehr,

1994; Eccles et al., 1998; Gottfried, Fleming, & Gottfried, 2001; Harter, 1981; Lepper,

Sethi, Dialdin, & Drake, 1997; Wigfield et al., 1997). Given that underlying motivational

deficits are common in ADHD (e.g. Carlson & Tamm, 2000; Kuntsi et al., 2001; Luman

et al., 2005; Sonuga-Barke, 2002, 2003; Sonuga-Barke et al., 1992), the decline in

motivation may be particularly pronounced in this population.

Lastly, it is important to note that ADHD symptoms manifest themselves

differently from childhood to adolescence. Specifically, symptom trajectory studies have

found that symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity tend to subside in adolescence,

whereas symptoms of inattention remain constant (Biederman et al., 2000; Hart, Lahey,

Loeber, Applegate, & Frick, 1995; Hurtig et al., 2007; Mick, Faraone, & Biederman,

2004). Among adolescents with ADHD, common manifestations of inattention include

Page 31: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

23

procrastination, daydreaming, and indecisiveness (Robin, 1997). Although the classic

hyperactive behaviours of young children tend to decrease in adolescence, classroom

observers continue to document subtle forms of restlessness and minor motor behaviour

in adolescents during sedentary activities (Barkley et al., 1990; Clarke, Heussler, &

Kohn, 2005; Hechtman, 1989; Willoughby, 2003). Furthermore, adolescents with ADHD

often report subjective or internal feelings of restlessness and feeling confined when

expected to stay in one place (Barkley et al., 1990; Clarke et al., 2005; Hechtman, 1989;

Robin, 1997; Wender, 1995; Weyandt et al., 2003; Willoughby, 2003). Although there

may be an age-dependent decline in hyperactivity/impulsivity in individuals with ADHD,

subtle manifestations of these symptoms are still visible when adolescents with ADHD

are compared to their non-ADHD peers (Wender, 1995). Of note, researchers have found

that EF deficits in ADHD may be more closely related to inattention symptoms than

hyperactive-impulsive symptoms (e.g., Stavro, Ettenhofer, & Nigg, 2007), which are

more stable across development.

ADHD and Gender Differences

There has been a growing body of research that has demonstrated subtle but

important gender differences in the symptom presentation and psychosocial functioning

of individuals with ADHD (Nussbaum, 2012; Rucklidge, 2010; Rucklidge & Tannock,

2001). For instance, researchers have consistently demonstrated that females most often

present with symptoms of inattention, whereas males tend to show symptoms of

hyperactivity/impulsivity (Biederman et al., 2005; Rucklidge, 2010; Weiss, Worling,

Wasdell, 2003; Seidman et al., 2005). These differences in symptom presentation have

lead some theorists to argue that ADHD is under-identified in girls, as inattention

Page 32: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

24

symptoms are more likely to go unnoticed than the disruptive behaviours associated with

hyperactivity/impulsivity (Gershon et al., 2005; Hinshaw & Blachman, 2005; Staller &

Faraone, 2006). Despite differences in symptom presentation, males and females with

ADHD appear to struggle equally with deficits in EF and self-regulation (Gross-Tsur et

al., 2006; Hartung et al., 2002; Rucklidge, 2010; Seidman et al., 2005). Seidman and

colleagues (2005) examined age and gender differences in EF in a large sample of

preteens and teens (ages 9 to 17 years). Females and males with ADHD had very similar

profiles of executive dysfunction, and this held true for both the younger and older age

cohorts. Similarly, males and females with ADHD do not differ with respect to their

scores on achievement tests (Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001).

Adolescent females with ADHD may be at a higher risk for internalizing and

psychosocial problems relative to their male counterparts (Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001).

Specifically, females with ADHD tend to report higher levels of distress, anxiety, and

low mood compared to males with ADHD and females without ADHD (Biederman et al.,

1994; Gershon & Gershon, 2002; Hinshaw, 2002; Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001; Staller &

Faraone, 2006). In addition, adolescent females with ADHD rate themselves as being less

accomplished, having lower self-esteem, and being more affected by negative life events

compared to adolescent males with ADHD (Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001). In contrast,

males with ADHD typically show more externalizing difficulties, as they tend to be more

disruptive, engage in more rule-breaking, and are more likely to have comorbid

Oppositional Defiant Disorder/Conduct Disorder diagnoses relative to females with

ADHD (Abikoff et al., 2002; Biederman et al., 1994; Gaub & Carlson, 1997; Hartung et

al., 2002). Therefore, the combination of having ADHD and being female may place

Page 33: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

25

individuals at higher risk for reporting more psychological distress and psychosocial

challenges (Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001), emphasizing the importance of considering

gender differences in studies examining clinical outcomes in this population.

In summary, ADHD is a chronic and debilitating disorder characterized by

symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity/impulsivity. Although EF/self-regulatory

deficits have received considerable attention in theoretical models of the disorder, there is

an increasing interest in exploring alternative pathways to ADHD, including those that

emphasize a motivational dysfunction. The core symptoms of ADHD and associated self-

regulatory and motivational deficits create a host of challenges for individuals with

ADHD across school, home, and social settings. Academic impairment is of particular

concern, especially during the adolescent years when students must cope with novel

developmental demands including the need to make decisions about their future

educational and career paths. However, relative to children with ADHD, adolescents have

been understudied; therefore, there remains a need to pinpoint those factors that may

enhance the school functioning of adolescents with ADHD in particular. Researchers

investigating the connection between ADHD and achievement have isolated inattention

as a key variable in accounting for poor scholastic outcomes. Given the widespread

motivational deficits in individuals with ADHD, it is important to also identify prominent

motivational variables, including student perceptions and behaviours, that may be of

importance when considering the school functioning of this population. In addition,

subtle but important gender differences in many psychosocial factors are evident in

ADHD, which suggests the need to consider possible gender differences when examining

important clinical outcomes.

Page 34: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

26

Student Engagement

Definition

The construct of student engagement has a long history in the literature, primarily

driven by an interest in how to promote student learning and enhance academic

performance (Christenson et al., 2012). Despite its lengthy history, there is no agreed-

upon definition of engagement among theorists (Fredricks et al., 2004), with two-, three-,

and four-subtype models proposed in the literature (e.g., Appleton et al., 2006;

Christenson et al., 2008; Finn, 1989; Martin, 2007; Skinner, Furrer, Marchand, &

Kindermann, 2008; Skinner et al., 2009). Most researchers agree, however, that

engagement is a multidimensional construct that is, at minimum, comprised of a

behavioural and affective component (Christenson et al., 2012). Behavioural engagement

is typically defined as the degree to which a student shows positive conduct at school

(e.g., adhering to rules, the absence of disruptive behaviour), is involved in learning and

academic activities (task persistence, on-task behaviour, participating in class discussion),

and participates in school-based social activities, such as athletics or school committees

(Fredricks et al., 2004). Emotional/affective engagement involves a student’s emotional

reactions in the classroom, including interest, boredom, anxiety, a sense of belonging,

and a positive attitude towards learning (Stipek, 2002). Despite being conceptually

distinct, behavioural and emotional engagement are positively correlated (Fredricks et al.,

2004). Some researchers have recently proposed a cognitive component to engagement

that includes factors such as attention, mental effort, and self-regulation of learning

(Fredericks et al., 2004; Wang, Willett, & Eccles, 2011). However, this aspect of

engagement will not be considered in the present study as there are disputes in the

Page 35: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

27

literature about how this concept should be defined and measured (Fredricks et al., 2004).

Furthermore, because many aspects of cognitive engagement (e.g., focusing attention,

resisting distractions) overlap with symptoms of inattention (Corno & Mandinach, 1983;

Skinner et al., 2008), this component of engagement was not considered in the present

study in order to reduce any shared variance between inattention and engagement.

Although closely related, researchers typically view student engagement and

motivation as distinct constructs. Common perspectives describe motivation as the

psychological processes that underlie the energy, purpose, and durability of one’s

engagement with a task (Skinner et al., 2009), and address the question of “why am I

doing this?” for a particular behaviour (Maehr & Meyer, 1997). Engagement is often seen

as the outward manifestation of motivation, or one’s active involvement in completing a

task (Appleton et al., 2008; Reeve, Jang, Carrell, Jeon, & Barch, 2004; Skinner et al.,

2009). Therefore, motivation is necessary for a student to be engaged, but it is not

sufficient. As such, one may be motivated to complete a task, but not actively engaged in

it (Connell & Wellborn, 1991; Furrer & Skinner, 2003). Recent research has supported

the hypothesis that motivation predicts engagement, which in turn, predicts achievement

outcomes (Green et al., 2012; Strand et al., 2012).

When students are engaged in lessons and academic tasks, they experience

increased opportunities to respond to the material, and this enhances the rate and depth of

their learning (Di Perna, Volpe, & Elliot, 2002; Fisher et al., 1980; Leach & Dolan,

1985). Student engagement, particularly behavioural engagement, is a strong predictor of

a range of academic outcomes (e.g., Archambault et al., 2009; Archambault &

Vandenbossche-Makombo, 2014; Bandura et al., 1996; Finn & Rock, 1997; Finn &

Page 36: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

28

Zimmer, 2012; Marks, 2000; Guo, Connor, Tompkins, & Morrison, 2011; Wang &

Holcombe, 2010; Whitlock, 2006). It is considered one of the most important variables

to examine when studying the effect of student behaviours on academic performance

(Greenwood, 1996; Greenwood, Horton, & Utley, 2002). For instance, Johnson and

colleagues (2006) used data from the population-based Minnesota Twin and Family

Study to examine the relation between student engagement and later achievement,

controlling for confounding variables. The authors found that only engagement affected

grades from age 11 to age 17, beyond the contribution of IQ, externalizing behaviour, and

family risk. Student engagement is also central in understanding school dropout, as there

is an extensive literature demonstrating that students who are more behaviourally

engaged are more likely to stay in school and aspire to higher education (Alexander,

Entwisle, & Horsey, 1997; Stewart, 2008; Wang & Eccles, 2012; Wang & Holcombe,

2010). Enhancing student engagement is a key component of prevention programs

designed to improve educational outcomes for at-risk adolescents and reduce high school

dropout (Christenson, 2009; Christenson et al., 2008; Finn, 1989; Sinclair et al., 1998,

2005).

Students who do not fully engage in school not only miss important opportunities

for academic growth and development, but they are also more likely to be involved in

risky activities or experience adjustment problems. For instance, low student engagement

is linked to a range of negative outcomes including substance use, delinquency,

externalizing behaviour challenges, and higher depressive symptomatology (Catalano et

al., 2004; Hughes, Luo, Kwok, & Loyd, 2008; Henry, Knight, & Thornberry, 2012; Li &

Lerner, 2011; Loukas et al., 2009; Resnick et al., 1997; Shochet, Dadds, Ham, &

Page 37: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

29

Montague, 2006). Students who report low engagement also report more dissatisfaction

with school, a lower sense of belonging, and higher rates of family conflict (Christenson,

Reschly, & Wylie, 2012; Corville-Smith, Ryan, Adams, & Dalicandro, 1998). Thus, the

importance of understanding student engagement extends beyond its relationship with

academics to social-emotional functioning and well-being.

Student engagement varies as a function of factors such as age and gender. With

respect to age-related differences, Eccles and colleagues (1993) found that student

engagement declines from elementary to junior high school. The authors hypothesized

that these changes in engagement may be a function of a poor fit between the individual’s

needs and the demands of the environment as well as having fewer opportunities for

autonomy and relatedness during this critical developmental period (Eccles et al., 1993).

During later adolescence, student engagement tends to stabilize, although it still remains

lower than in the elementary school years (Janosz, Archambault, Morizot, & Pagani,

2008). In terms of gender differences, there is some evidence, albeit limited, to suggest

that male students tend to be less engaged than female students (Elliott, DiPerna, Mroch,

& Lang, 2004; Li & Lerner, 2011; Whitlock, 2006), highlighting the importance of

considering the role of gender in studies of student engagement.

Engagement in Adolescents with ADHD

Researchers have found that students who struggle academically receive fewer

opportunities to engage with academic material than their typically achieving peers

(Greenwood et al., 1984; Li & Lerner, 2011; Thurlow, Yesseldyke, Graden, & Algozzine,

1984). Unfortunately, very little is known about student engagement in adolescents with

ADHD, even though this population is at considerable risk for academic

Page 38: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

30

underachievement, school dropout, and poor social-emotional adjustment (e.g., Loe &

Feldman, 2007; Wehmeier et al., 2010). Given that engagement behaviours can be

fostered to enhance educational outcomes (e.g., Christenson, 2009; Christenson et al.,

2008; Reschly & Christenson, 2006), there is considerable value to understanding the role

of engagement in adolescents with ADHD.

Given the symptoms, neurocognitive deficits, and functional impairments that

characterize ADHD, one would expect children and adolescents with ADHD to struggle

with several aspects of engagement in the classroom. Research generally supports this

hypothesis. For instance, relative to their peers, students with ADHD are more likely to

demonstrate challenges with on-task behaviour (Abikoff et al., 2002; Daley &

Birchwood, 2010; Vile Junod, DuPaul, Jitendra, Volpe, & Cleary, 2006), and they are

less likely to participate in class lessons (e.g., Steiner, Sheldrick, Frenette, Rene, &

Perrin, 2014) and follow teacher instructions (e.g., Nolan, Gadow, & Sprafkin, 2001), all

of which are components of behavioural engagement. Underlying motivational and EF

deficits may further interfere with behavioural engagement by making it challenging for

adolescents with ADHD to initiate tasks (McCandless & O'Laughlin, 2007; Willcutt et

al., 2005), persist over time (Dovis, Van der Oord, Wiers, & Prins, 2012; Hoza et al.,

2001), and complete homework assignments (Langberg et al., 2010; Power et al., 2006).

Adhering to classroom rules and demonstrating appropriate classroom conduct may be

challenging for those adolescents with comorbid externalizing difficulties (Biederman et

al., 1998; Spencer et al., 2007). Furthermore, given their widespread academic and

motivational challenges, it is not surprising that students with ADHD are more likely than

their peers to report feeling less connected to their teachers, have a negative attitude

Page 39: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

31

towards school, and report boredom and low levels of curiosity, interest, and enjoyment

of learning (Carlson et al., 2002; Castens & Overbey, 2009; DuPaul & Stoner, 2003;

Pfiffner, Barkley, & DuPaul, 1998; Rogers & Tannock, 2013). This pattern of findings

suggests that they may also be at risk for lower levels of emotional engagement relative

to their non-ADHD peers.

Although limited, there is some evidence to suggest that children with ADHD

have difficulties with student engagement (e.g., Demaray & Jenkins, 2011; Martin, 2012;

Vile Junod et al., 2006). For instance, Vile Junod and colleagues (2006) found that, after

controlling for differences in achievement test scores and SES, children with ADHD in

grades 1 to 4 had significantly higher rates of off-task behaviour and significantly lower

rates of engagement relative to their non-ADHD peers. Other researchers have explored

the role of student engagement as a mediating variable between symptoms of inattention

and academic outcomes (Demaray & Jenkins, 2011; DuPaul et al., 2004; Langberg et al.,

2011; Plamondon & Martinussen, 2015; Rapport, Scanlan, & Denney, 1999; Volpe et al.,

2006). Rapport and colleagues (1991) proposed a dual-pathway model, which describes

two parallel pathways—one cognitive and the other behavioural—that mediate the

relationship between attention problems and achievement. The cognitive pathway

implicates higher-order cognitive functions, such as memory and vigilance, whereas the

behavioural pathway highlights the role of daily classroom performance, which includes

the role of student engagement (Rapport et al., 1999). Rapport and colleagues tested their

hypothesized model and found that teacher-rated classroom performance significantly

mediated the relationship between inattention and academic achievement, such that the

direct relationship was rendered nonsignificant. The overall model, including attention

Page 40: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

32

problems, classroom performance, and select cognitive abilities, accounted for 77% of

the variance in achievement on standardized tests.

Since Rapport and colleagues proposed their dual-pathway model, several

additional studies have supported the finding that certain classroom behaviours, including

school engagement, are important in explaining the relationship between ADHD

symptoms and achievement (DuPaul et al., 2004; Langberg et al., 2011). For instance,

two studies, Volpe et al. (2006) and Demaray and Jenkins (2011), examined the

mediating role of academic enablers, which are the “attitudes and behaviours that allow a

student to participate in and ultimately benefit from academic instruction in the

classroom” (DiPerna & Elliott, 2002, p. 294). The enablers include student engagement

as well as other behaviours and attitudes, such as interpersonal skills, motivation, and

study skills. Both these studies revealed that certain academic enablers, including student

engagement, mediated the relationship between ADHD symptoms and achievement in a

sample of elementary school students. Similarly, given the specificity and malleability of

engagement behaviours, promoting student engagement has been a target of interventions

designed to enhance the educational functioning of adolescents with attention problems

(e.g., Clarfield & Stoner, 2005; Martin, 2012, 2013; Pfiffner, Villodas, Kaiser, Rooney, &

McBurnett, 2013).

Taken together, the above research suggests that student engagement is an

important motivational construct to study when seeking to uncover those behaviours that

promote positive academic outcomes. There is evidence to suggest that individuals with

ADHD may struggle with several aspects of student engagement; however, only a

handful of researchers have directly studied this variable in adolescents with the disorder.

Page 41: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

33

This research is important given that adolescents with ADHD are at risk for poor

academic functioning, including school failure and dropout, and that student engagement

is a key variable in reducing the risk of these outcomes. When considering student

engagement as a target of intervention, it may be particularly valuable to identify those

factors that foster engagement behaviours. One such factor is self-efficacy, which will be

discussed below, as these self-beliefs have been consistently linked to enhanced

performance, well-being, and engagement in the classroom.

Self-Efficacy

Theory and Research

In his book Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive

Theory, Bandura (1986) put forth a theory of human agency which proposes that

individuals are agents proactively engaged in shaping their own development and

outcomes and that "what people think, believe, and feel affects how they behave"

(Bandura, 1986, p. 25). Thus, according to this model, individuals are proactive, self-

reflecting, and self-regulating beings, rather than reactive organisms who are shaped

solely by environmental forces or subconscious inner drives. From Bandura’s

perspective, human thought and action are the result of a dynamic interaction of personal,

behavioural, and environmental factors. For example, how people interpret the results of

their own behaviour informs and alters their environment and the personal factors they

possess which, in turn, inform and alter subsequent behaviour.

Bandura also argued that “among the mechanisms of personal agency, none is

more central or pervasive than peoples’ beliefs about their capabilities to exercise control

over events that affect their lives” (Bandura, 1986, p. 1175). Bandura was referring to

Page 42: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

34

self-efficacy beliefs, which are at the centre of his social cognitive theory. Bandura

defined self-efficacy beliefs as subjective judgments of one’s ability to organize and

execute courses of action to attain designated goals (Bandura, 1986, 1993). He proposed

that self-efficacy beliefs can be measured based on their level, generality, and strength

across various activities and contexts (Bandura, 1997). Level of self-efficacy beliefs are

dependent on the difficulty level of a particular task, such as completing math problems

of increasing difficulty; generality refers to the transferability of self-efficacy beliefs

across activities, such as from addition to division problems; strength refers to the

amount of certainty about performance on a particular task (Bandura, 1997; Zimmerman,

2000, Zimmerman, 1995). Bandura proposed that these characteristics of self-efficacy

perceptions can be measured quantitatively by questionnaires with items that are task

specific, vary in difficulty, and capture degrees of confidence (e.g., 0 to 100% confident)

(Bandura, 1997; Pajares, 1996; Zimmerman, 2000).

Although closely related to other self-perceptions, such as self-concept, the

construct of self-efficacy has several defining features that set it apart (Bandura, 1997;

Zimmerman, 2000; Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006). Firstly, unlike self-concept, self-

efficacy beliefs focus on performance capabilities (i.e., what a person thinks he/she can

do), rather than judgments about personal qualities, such as one’s physical or

psychological attributes (e.g., “I am confident that I can complete this math test” versus

“I am good at math”). Secondly, whereas self-concept is a more general self-descriptive

construct, self-efficacy beliefs are domain-, task, and context-specific. For instance,

although an individual may express higher self-efficacy for math than science, within

mathematics, he/she may feel more capable of completing algebra than statistics. Even

Page 43: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

35

still, he/she might feel more capable of completing algebra in a quiet library than in a

noisy classroom. Thirdly, self-efficacy beliefs are dependent upon a mastery criterion of

performance, rather than normative data or other criteria. For example, individuals judge

their certainty in completing a math task of a certain difficulty level, rather than how well

they expect to perform relative to others. Lastly, self-efficacy beliefs are future-oriented

in that they are judgments made prior to performing a given task. Thus, they involve

beliefs about capability of accomplishing a task, versus whether they have done so in the

past (Bandura, 1997; Pajares, 1996; Zimmerman, 2000; Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006).

According to Bandura, self-efficacy beliefs are the driving force behind

motivation, well-being, and personal accomplishment (Bandura, 1986, 1997). He argued

that people’s beliefs about their capabilities are better predictors of performance than

their existing knowledge and skills, as people would have little incentive to persist in the

face of difficulty unless they believe that their actions can produce a desired outcome

(Pajares, 1996; Bandura, 1986, 1997; Zimmerman, 2000). Although Bandura

acknowledged that no amount of confidence can lead to success when requisite skills and

knowledge are absent, he argued that self-efficacy beliefs are powerful in that they dictate

how individuals use the knowledge and skills that they possess (Pajares, 1996; Bandura,

1986, 1997; Zimmerman, 2000). Bandura theorized that self-efficacy beliefs affect the

choices people make, how much effort they put forth, and their emotional reactions when

faced with difficulty (Bandura, 1997; Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 2001;

Bandura, Pastorelli, Barbaranelli, & Caprara, 1999; Muris, 2002). He also proposed that

these beliefs are a critical determinant of self-regulation, particularly in the context of

learning (Bandura, 1997, 2001; Pajares, 1996; Zimmerman, 2000).

Page 44: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

36

Self-efficacy beliefs provide students with a sense of agency to motivate their

learning through self-regulated learning strategies (Zimmerman, 2000). Self-regulated

learners can effectively use strategies, such as organizing information, planning,

reflecting on one’s performance, and rehearsing or using memory aids, in a manner that is

directed at successfully acquiring information or academic skills (Zimmerman, 2002;

Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1992). Consistent with Bandura’s social cognitive theory,

how confident students are in their ability to self-regulate their learning can determine

how well they are able to manage their learning environment and use self-regulated

learning strategies effectively (Bandura, 1993; Pajares & Urdan, 2006; Zimmerman,

2000). Thus, self-efficacy for self-regulated learning (SESRL) refers to the beliefs that

students hold about their ability to use self-regulated learning strategies to cope with

difficult learning conditions (Bandura, 2006).

Researchers have studied SESRL beliefs extensively in the context of student

achievement and learning, with results demonstrating that these beliefs predict a range of

academic outcomes (see Bandura, 1997; Pajares, 2007). Specifically, SESRL beliefs

predict students’ grades, standardized achievement scores, as well as the quality and

quantity of homework completion (e.g., Bandura, 1993; Kitsantas & Zimmerman, 2008;

Klobas, Renxi, & Nigrelli, 2007; Lent, Brown, & Larkin, 1987; Linnenbrink & Pintrich,

2002; Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Graham, 1999; Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Usher &

Pajares, 2008; Zimmerman 1994; Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994; Zimmerman &

Kitsantas, 2005, 2007; Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1990). Interestingly, Zuffianὸ and

colleagues (2013) found that SESRL beliefs at the beginning of eighth grade predicted

academic achievement (students’ grades obtained from school records) at the end of the

Page 45: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

37

school year, beyond the effects of previous academic achievement, socioeconomic status,

intelligence, personality traits, and self-esteem. These results provide support for

Bandura’s social cognitive theory and his contention that SESRL beliefs are one of the

most powerful predictors of performance. There has also been a growing interest in

examining the role of SESRL beliefs in high school dropout (e.g., Caprara et al., 2008;

Kitsantas & Zimmerman, 2009; Schunk, 1985; Zimmerman, Bandura, & Martinez-Pons,

1992; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2005; 2007). Caprara and colleagues (2008) studied this

relationship longitudinally and found that students’ SESRL beliefs at the junior high level

contributed to their academic achievement in high school and their likelihood of

completing their high school education, even after controlling for variations in prior

academic performance and socioeconomic status.

Gender differences in SESRL beliefs are also evident, with girls exhibiting higher

SESRL beliefs than boys (e.g., Pajares, 2002; Pajares & Valiante, 2001; Vecchio,

Gerbino, Pastorelli, Del Bove, & Caprara, 2007). For example, Zimmerman and

Martinez-Pons (1990) examined gender differences in self-efficacy and strategy use in a

sample of 90 boys and girls in grades 5, 8, and 11. They found that girls not only

exhibited more goal-setting, planning, self-monitoring, and record-keeping behaviours

compared to boys, but they also felt more confident in their ability to do so.

In addition to gender differences, there are developmental variations in SESRL

beliefs. In adolescence, a strong sense of SESRL is especially important, as adolescents

face a major shift in the difficulty of academic work and must also become more

independent and assume responsibility for their own learning (Wigfield, Eccles, &

Pintrich, 1996; Zimmerman & Cleary, 2006). They are also expected to develop and

Page 46: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

38

apply a number of general learning skills such as note-taking, reading, essay-writing,

studying, and test-taking (Zimmerman, Bonner, & Kovach, 1996). However, as they

enter the teenage years, adolescents also develop more self-awareness as well as the

increased ability to self-reflect, integrate evaluative feedback from others, and make

social comparisons (Eccles, Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998; Richman, Hope, & Mihalas,

2010; Wigfield, Eccles, & Rodriguez, 1998). Consequently, they are able to make more

accurate self-appraisals of their abilities and skills than they would have as children

(Harter, 1999; Stipek, 1998). Self-efficacy theorists have suggested that the beliefs that

young people hold about their abilities can help them navigate the challenges involved in

applying self-regulation skills and coping with the many learning demands of high school

(Bandura, 2006; Pajares & Urdan, 2006; Zimmerman, 2000; Zimmerman & Cleary,

2006). Unfortunately, existing evidence suggests that, with the transition from elementary

to high school, adolescents experience a decline in self-efficacy, including their SESRL

beliefs (Caprara et al., 2008; Pajares & Valiante, 2002; Usher & Pajares, 2008; Vecchio

et al., 2007). For example, Caprara and colleagues (2008) examined the developmental

progression of SESRL beliefs in a group of 412 Italian students between the ages of 12 to

22 years. They found that adolescents experienced a gradual decrease in their SESRL

beliefs from junior to senior high school and that this decline was greater for male

students than female students. In addition, the authors found that, after controlling for

socioeconomic status, students who experienced less of a decline in their SESRL beliefs

were more likely to achieve higher grades and less likely to drop out of high school.

Thus, adolescence is a period during which students feel less confident in their self-

regulatory abilities. Unfortunately, this comes at a time during which academic

Page 47: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

39

achievement is key in determining the academic and vocational opportunities that

become available to adolescents (Bandura, 2006; Pajares & Urdan, 2006). This may be

particularly problematic for adolescents with ADHD, who are already at a greater risk for

poor academic and vocational outcomes compared to their typically developing peers.

Furthermore, relative to children with ADHD, there is meagre research on adolescents,

which limits our understanding of ADHD across the life-span (Faraone, 2013). By

exploring self-efficacy and student engagement in adolescents with ADHD, this study

will add to the current adolescent literature.

Self-Efficacy and Student Engagement

The negative consequences of student disengagement, including academic failure,

delinquency, and dropout, have prompted many researchers to explore whether it is

possible to foster the psychological factors of individuals that enhance engagement

(Caraway et al., 2003). Bandura’s social cognitive theory states that self-efficacy beliefs

are a key cognitive variable that influences motivation and, in particular, student

engagement (Bandura, 1986, 1997; Schunk & Mullen, 2012). Indeed, there is a

substantial amount of literature demonstrating that general self-efficacy and SESRL

beliefs affect several aspects of student engagement in the classroom. For instance,

students who feel more confident in their self-regulation skills are able to use these skills

more effectively. They set challenging goals, monitor and evaluate their progress, and

employ a range of learning strategies across academic domains (Bandura et al., 1996,

2001; Bandura, Caprara, Barbaranelli, Gerbino, & Pastorelli, 2003; Bong, 2001;

Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994; Zimmerman, Bandura, & Martinez-Pons, 1992;

Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1990). They also focus their attention on the task at hand,

Page 48: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

40

put forth a more consistent effort, and strive to avoid distraction (Linnenbrink & Pintrich,

2003; Schunk, 1995; Schunk & Mullen, 2012; Zimmerman, 2000). When self-efficacious

students perceive that their progress is inadequate, they take the appropriate steps to

enhance their learning, such as changing a strategy, seeking assistance, or modifying

aspects of the environment (Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2003; Schunk & Mullen, 2012).

Consistent with social cognitive theory, Bouffard-Bouchard and colleagues (1991) found

that self-efficacy exerted a significant influence on various aspects of self-regulation in a

population of junior and senior high school students who were asked to complete

problems of varying difficulty. Specifically, students with higher self-efficacy beliefs

were more likely to monitor their work time, persist on the task, and use effective

problem-solving methods than students with lower self-efficacy beliefs, irrespective of

differences in grade level and cognitive ability.

Students who feel more confident in their ability to self-regulate their learning are

also more likely to hold positive outcome expectations, value the learning process, set

mastery goals, and take responsibility for their academic outcomes (e.g., Bandura, 1993;

Lent et al., 1987; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2002; Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Graham, 1999;

Pintrich & De Groot, 1990; Usher & Pajares, 2008; Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994;

Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2007; Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1990). These are all

important aspects of student engagement. Furthermore, students who feel more

efficacious about using self-regulated learning strategies are also less likely to hold

negative perceptions about school, set performance (versus mastery) goals, procrastinate,

or become anxious in threatening academic situations relative to their peers with lower

SESRL beliefs (e.g., Joo, Bong, & Choi, 2000; Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Graham, 1999;

Page 49: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

41

Pajares, Miller, & Johnson, 1999; Pajares & Valiante, 1999, 2002; Tan et al., 2008; Usher

& Pajares, 2006; Zimmerman & Bandura, 1994; Zimmerman et al., 1992; Zimmerman &

Martinez-Pons, 1990).

Some researchers have directly examined the relationship between self-efficacy

beliefs and measures of student engagement (Caraway et al., 2003; Walker et al., 2006).

For instance, Caraway and colleagues (2003) sought to identify psychological variables

that facilitate or hinder engagement in a sample of 123 adolescents ages 13 to 19. Results

indicated that adolescents with higher levels of self-efficacy were more likely to be

engaged in various aspects of school and achieve higher grades. Similarly, using path

analysis, Walker et al. (2006) found that self-efficacy beliefs, along with other

psychological variables such as intrinsic motivation and academic identification, uniquely

contributed to the predication of cognitive engagement. Taken together, the above

research suggests that SESRL beliefs affect several aspects of engagement, including

student behaviours and their emotional responses towards learning.

Self-Efficacy and ADHD

Research examining self-efficacy beliefs in individuals with ADHD is limited to a

few studies in clinical and community samples of children and adolescents, yet this

research is of critical importance given the vital role that self-efficacy beliefs play in

student engagement and learning. Of the existing studies, there is some evidence to

suggest that adolescents with ADHD have lower self-efficacy beliefs than their non-

ADHD peers (Luzzo, Hitchings, Retish, & Shoemaker, 1999; Major, Martinussen, &

Wiener, 2013; Norvilitis, Sun, & Zhang, 2010; Norwalk, Norvilitis, & MacLean, 2009;

Young, Heptinstall, Sonuga-Barke, Chadwick, & Taylor, 2005). For example, Young and

Page 50: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

42

colleagues (2005) followed a community sample of girls showing pervasive hyperactivity

and conduct problems across an 8-year span beginning from age 7. Of note, Young et

al.’s measure of hyperactivity also included items that assessed symptoms of inattention,

such as distractibility and sustaining attention. They found that childhood symptoms of

ADHD, not conduct problems, were a risk factor for low confidence related to school

performance in adolescence. Similar results have been found in college students with

respect to their decision-making about their future career (Tomevi, 2013; Norvilitis et al.,

2010; Norwalk et al., 2009). Specifically, students with higher levels of self-reported

ADHD symptoms also felt less confident in their ability to effectively make decisions

about, and plan for, their careers and education. Furthermore, results of the regression

equation predicting career decision-making self-efficacy revealed that, even after

controlling for depression and gender, inattention emerged as a significant predictor of

career decision-making self-efficacy, whereas hyperactivity did not (Norwalk et al.,

2009).

Similar results have emerged in clinical samples. Tabassam and Grainger (2002)

reported that the academic self-efficacy beliefs of children with LDs with and without co-

morbid ADHD were lower than those of the children in the control group. Furthermore,

Major et al. (2013) found that adolescents with ADHD, particularly females, reported the

lowest levels of confidence in their ability to self-regulate their learning relative to their

non-ADHD counterparts. Similar to the results found from Norwalk et al. (2009), Major

and colleagues (2013) found that symptoms of inattention, but not symptoms of

hyperactivity-impulsivity, were related to SESRL beliefs. This finding adds to the

growing body of literature suggesting that inattention is more strongly associated with

Page 51: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

43

negative outcomes than hyperactivity. Norwalk and colleagues reasoned that the greater

cognitive and EF deficits found in inattentive students may make it difficult for them to

plan ahead for their future, self-reflect, divide their attention, and demonstrate the self-

control necessary to make decisions about their future career goals. In addition, poorer

academic adjustment found in students with inattentive symptoms may also make these

students feel less confident in their ability to achieve their educational and vocational

goals (Norwalk et al., 2009). Although Norwalk et al.’s (2009) study focused on career

decision-making self-efficacy, their approach can be applied to understanding the SESRL

beliefs in adolescents with ADHD, as these two forms of self-efficacy rely on similar

strategies and skills associated with self-regulation (i.e., planning, goal setting, self-

reflecting, etc.).

Additional support for the hypothesis that adolescents with ADHD may

demonstrate lower SESRL beliefs than their peers comes from a recent qualitative study

by Wiener and Daniels (2015) in which 12 adolescents with ADHD (ages 14 to 16)

described their school experiences. In this study, the researchers found that students with

ADHD demonstrated low levels of personal agency, and that this emerged as one of the

most striking themes across interviews. Participating adolescents readily identified their

performance deficits related to academics and self-regulation of learning (e.g., inadequate

study skills, procrastination, limited planning and goal-setting) and attributed these

difficulties to limited personal agency—or what adolescents described as “laziness.”

Participants with ADHD also acknowledged the increased demands for independence and

autonomy in adolescence and secondary school, and they perceived themselves as being

unable to fulfill these demands due, in part, to low levels of personal agency.

Page 52: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

44

The above research provides support for the hypothesis that, as a group,

adolescents with ADHD may perceive themselves as being less capable than their non-

ADHD peers in managing the demands of secondary school and utilising the appropriate

self-regulation skill to support their learning. In addition, research by Major and

colleagues (2013) highlights the importance of considering possible gender differences in

the self-efficacy perceptions of adolescents with ADHD. However, this research is still in

its infancy and the latter study’s findings must be interpreted carefully due to the small

sample size of adolescent females with ADHD. It is therefore important to determine

whether we can replicate these findings in future studies. Establishing whether there are

group and gender differences in SESRL beliefs would add to our growing knowledge of

how adolescents with ADHD perceive themselves and their abilities, an essential

component of adolescent development and well-being. Although self-perceptions, such as

self-esteem and self-concept, have been studied extensively in children with ADHD (see

Owens, Goldfine, Evangelista, Hoza, & Kaiser, 2007 for a review), few researchers have

examined these self-beliefs during the adolescent years—a time when even typically

developing adolescents tend to make more negative judgments about their capabilities

(Caprara et al., 2008; Harter, 1999; Pajares & Valiante, 2002; Stipek, 1998; Usher &

Pajares, 2008; Vecchio et al., 2007). Existing literature regarding general self-perceptions

(i.e., self-concept and self-esteem) supports the hypothesis that adolescents with ADHD

may have lower self-efficacy beliefs than their peers (e.g., Anderson, Williams, McGee,

& Silva, 1989; Edbom, Granlund, Lichtenstien, & Larssonn, 2008; Hinshaw et al., 2006;

Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001; Young et al., 2005). For example, Slomkowski, Klein, and

Mannuzza (1995) found that after controlling for the presence of a current mental

Page 53: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

45

disorder, adolescents and young adults ages 16 to 23 who had ADHD symptoms in

childhood displayed lower self-esteem than adolescents who did not have ADHD

symptoms in childhood. Moreover, researchers found that in the hyperactive and control

groups, adolescents with higher self-esteem rated themselves as having fewer ADHD

symptoms and were judged by clinicians to have better psychosocial functioning. In

terms of gender differences, female adolescents with ADHD have been found to exhibit

lower self-esteem compared to male adolescents with ADHD, in addition to other

psychosocial difficulties such as higher levels of anxiety and depression, increased

feeling of distress, and poorer coping skills (Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001; Rucklidge,

2010; Young et al., 2005). Although self-esteem and self-concept are conceptually

different from self-efficacy, the above findings from studies in adolescents, college

students, and young adults with ADHD symptoms suggest that adolescents with ADHD

may have more negative views of themselves and their abilities compared to their non-

ADHD peers. If this is indeed the case, it may be of added value to uncover those

variables that foster a healthy sense of self-efficacy in adolescents, as these can serve to

inform academic interventions for adolescents with ADHD.

Sources of Self-Efficacy

Theory and Research

Embedded in his social cognitive theory, Bandura (1997) proposed that

individuals form their self-efficacy beliefs by interpreting information from four main

sources: (1) mastery experiences; (2) vicarious experiences (modeling); (3) verbal

(social) persuasion; and (4) physiological and affective states. The most powerful of the

four sources of self-efficacy are mastery experiences. They are defined as one’s reflective

Page 54: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

46

assessment of past successes or failures with a task. Thus, after completing a particular

task, individuals will naturally interpret and evaluate the results obtained, and judgments

of competence with the task are subsequently formed based on these reflections. When

individuals perceive their efforts to be successful, their self-efficacy related to

accomplishing a similar task in the future is increased. When individuals believe that

their efforts did not produce the desired effect, their confidence to succeed with a similar

task in the future is reduced (Bandura, 1997; Usher & Pajares, 2006). Self-efficacy

beliefs are informed when one’s actions or performance are cognitively appraised.

Therefore, perceptions of mastery experiences are often better predictors of self-efficacy

than objective results (Bandura, 1986, 1997; Usher & Pajares, 2006).

In addition to interpreting the results of their own performance, individuals form

their self-efficacy beliefs through the vicarious experience of observing and interpreting

the performance of others (Usher & Pajares, 2006). In other words, modeling can be a

very influential in the development of self-efficacy beliefs. Moreover, the more closely

the observer identifies with the model, the more likely the efficacy of the observer will

change (Bandura, 1986, 1997; Schunk, 1987). Vicarious information can also be very

potent when students are uncertain about their own abilities or have limited experience

with the task. Thus, observing a model successfully complete a difficult task may

convince people that they can also succeed at the task. Similarly, watching a model fail

with such a task can decrease the observer’s efficacy (Bandura, 1986, 1997; Usher &

Pajares, 2006). Vicarious experiences in academic settings are also relevant to appraising

the performance of one’s peers relative to their own performance. For instance, self-

Page 55: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

47

efficacy with a task may increase if one judges his/her performance as superior to his/her

peers or decrease if they feel they are performing worse (Usher & Pajares, 2006).

The third source of self-efficacy according to Bandura (1986, 1997) involves the

verbal (social) persuasions that individuals receive from significant others. Verbal/social

persuasion involves the feedback a student receives from others, such as parents,

teachers, coaches, and peers. As students are developing their sense of competence in an

area, they often rely on evaluative feedback from others. The effectiveness of this

feedback depends on the perceived expertise, trustworthiness, and credibility of the

persuader (Bandura, 1986). Bandura also argued, however, that verbal persuasions are

limited in their ability to create long-lasting increases in self-efficacy and that it may be

more common that verbal persuasions undermine self-efficacy than boost it (Bandura,

1986). Nonetheless, supportive messages and positive encouragement can serve to

increase students’ efforts and self-confidence, particularly when accompanied by other

conditions that breed success (Usher & Pajares, 2006).

Lastly, emotional and physiological states inform self-efficacy beliefs. These

states include arousal levels, fatigue, anxiety, and stress. Students can attend to and

interpret these physiological cues and use this information to provide insight into how

well they may accomplish a task. For instance, when a task incites feelings of stress or

anxiety, these cues can undermine students’ self-efficacy beliefs because students’ may

view them as signals of impending failure. Furthermore, individuals with low self-

efficacy are more likely to negatively interpret past experiences than those with high self-

efficacy, which may heighten their anxiety about completing such tasks in the future and

lead to avoidance behaviour. Conversely, students with higher self-efficacy beliefs may

Page 56: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

48

not be as affected by negative physiological states or previous failed experiences

(Bandura, 1997). In general, increasing an individual’s physical and psychological well-

being and reducing negative emotional states can strengthen self-efficacy and expected

success with a task (Bandura, 1986, 1997; Usher & Pajares, 2006).

Although there is significant support for the predictive power of self-efficacy

beliefs, fewer researchers have examined the sources of these beliefs, and the existing

evidence is mixed. Most research is consistent with Bandura’s (1986, 1997) theory and

has found a relationship between each of the four sources of self-efficacy and both

academic and self-regulatory efficacy (Anderson & Betz, 2001; Chen & Usher, 2013;

Joët, Usher, & Bressoux, 2011; Kıran & Sungur, 2012; Klassen, 2004; Lent et al., 1991;

Lent et al., 1996; Lopez et al., 1997; Usher & Pajares, 2006; van Dinther, Dochy, &

Segers, 2011). However, other researchers have found that not all four sources predict

self-efficacy beliefs, and that some show stronger relationships with self-efficacy than

others (e.g., Bandura, 1997; Butz & Usher, 2015; Hampton, 1998; Lopez & Lent, 1992;

Usher & Pajares, 2008). Mastery experiences are theorized as the most influential source

of self-efficacy (Bandura, 1986, 1997), and this is indeed consistent with most research

findings (e.g., Anderson & Betz, 2001; Chen & Usher, 2013; Joët et al., 2011; Kıran &

Sungur, 2012; Klassen, 2004; Lent et al., 1991; Lent et al., 1996; Lopez et al., 1997;

Usher & Pajares, 2006; van Dinther et al., 2011). Researchers have attributed the

differences among the findings of studies investigating the sources of self-efficacy and

self-efficacy beliefs to subtle differences in the research methodologies used to

investigate the source variables as well as individual differences related to the

participants (Usher & Pajares, 2008). Although limited, there is some evidence that the

Page 57: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

49

effects of the sources may differ as a function of gender. For instance, studies among

high school and college students have found that females tend to report more experiences

of verbal persuasion and vicarious learning than do males (Anderson & Betz, 2001; Butz

& Usher, 2015; Lent et al., 1996; Usher & Pajares, 2006; Zeldin & Pajares, 2000). Some

researchers have concluded that the feedback female students receive from others may be

a more important contributor to their self-efficacy beliefs than their actual experiences of

success or failure with the task (Zeldin & Pajares, 2000). Males students tend to weigh

mastery experiences more heavily in comparison (Usher & Pajares, 2006; Zeldin &

Pajares, 2000). In addition, the four sources of self-efficacy may differ as a function of

ability level. Studies of students with LDs have revealed that these students tend to report

fewer mastery experiences in school, less frequent exposure to efficacious models, less

positive feedback from others, and higher levels of negative physiological arousal than

their non-LD peers (Hampton, 1998; Hampton & Mason, 2003; Usher & Pajares, 2006).

Similarly, Usher and Pajares (2006) found that academic and self-regulatory efficacy in

students with below average reading abilities was related to the four sources of self-

efficacy.

Bandura’s model of self-efficacy emphasizes the mediating role of self-efficacy

beliefs between sources of information and achievement-related outcomes (Bandura,

1986, 1997). Consistent with Bandura’s full model, a number of research studies (Bong,

2001; Fenollar et al., 2007; Liem et al., 2008; Pajares, 1996; Pajares & Graham, 1999;

Pajares & Miller, 1994; Pajares & Valiante, 1997; Phan, 2010; Prat-Sala & Redford,

2010; Sins et al., 2008) have identified the central mechanism of personal self-efficacy

between antecedents and student learning and achievement. For instance, Phan (2012)

Page 58: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

50

tested this model longitudinally and explored the interrelations between antecedents (i.e.,

four sources of self-efficacy), self-efficacy beliefs, and academic achievement in a

sample of 332 elementary school students. Structural equation modeling revealed that

mastery and vicarious experiences exerted a positive effect on self-efficacy beliefs, which

in turn, contributed to the prediction of academic achievement. The author concluded that

self-efficacy does not act in isolation, but rather operates in tandem with other

antecedents to influence academic performance. Furthermore, because direct, indirect,

and total effects were examined, a priori and a posteriori, support for self-efficacy as a

mediator between the sources and achievement-related outcomes was provided.

Moreover, Hampton and Mason (2003) tested Bandura’s full self-efficacy model in a

sample of 278 high school students with and without LDs. By using structural equation

modelling, the researchers examined the relations among the sources of self-efficacy,

academic self-efficacy beliefs, and academic achievement, with gender and LD status as

moderating variables. Results indicated that LD status had an indirect effect on self-

efficacy beliefs through an aggregated score representing the four sources of self-

efficacy. In addition, the sources of self-efficacy had a direct effect on academic self-

efficacy beliefs, which, in turn, affected academic performance. In this study, gender did

not have a direct or indirect influence on self-efficacy beliefs. The structural model fit the

data well, explaining 55% of the variance in academic achievement. Taken together,

these studies provide support for Bandura’s self-efficacy model and the mediating role of

self-efficacy between the sources and subsequent performance. Whether this model holds

true in a sample of adolescents with attention difficulties has yet to be determined.

Page 59: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

51

The Sources of Self-Efficacy and ADHD

Lent and Hackett (1987) first suggested that students with disabilities may have

less access to sources of self-efficacy and that this reduced access may influence the

development of self-efficacy in these individuals. Although there is research investigating

the sources in adolescents with LDs (Hampton, 1998; Hampton & Mason, 2003), there is

no known research examining the sources of self-efficacy beliefs in adolescents with

ADHD. Similar to students with LD, many adolescents with ADHD experience a host of

challenges in school, and these challenges may affect their access to mastery experiences,

vicarious learning, and positive feedback from others, and may heighten their levels of

negative physiological arousal.

Adolescents with ADHD are likely to experience fewer mastery experiences in

school due to their widespread academic difficulties. Educational impairments are well

documented in students with ADHD, with prevalence rates for learning and/or

achievement problems in adolescents between 50 to 80 percent (DuPaul & Stoner, 2014).

Adolescents with ADHD are more likely to fail a grade, have lower grade point averages,

and score lower on standardized achievement tests than their non-ADHD peers

(Barbaresi et al., 2007; Barkley et al., 2006; Ek et al., 2011; Frazier et al., 2007; Rogers et

al., 2011). In addition, critical self-regulatory skills that rely on EFs, such as studying,

note-taking, test-taking, summarizing, planning, organization, and time management are

also significantly impaired in students with ADHD, and have been linked to reduced

academic performance (Demaray & Jenkins, 2011; Gureasko-Moore et al., 2006; Meyer

& Kelley, 2007; Reaser et al., 2007; Volpe at al., 2006; Zwart & Kallemeyn, 2001).

Because of their academic and self-regulatory impairments, adolescents with ADHD may

Page 60: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

52

be less likely to master such tasks and more likely to experience repeated failure relative

to their non-ADHD peers. These repeated failure experiences may become internalized

over time, and this may weaken their beliefs about their abilities. The impact of skill

deficits on the self-perceptions of adolescents with ADHD was recently highlighted in a

qualitative study by Wiener and Daniels (2015). In this study, participating adolescents

described their performance deficits related to note-taking, study skills, goal-setting,

planning, etc., and attributed these to low levels of personal agency. The adolescents in

this study understood that these difficulties had a negative impact on their academic

performance and they perceived themselves as being unable to meet the increased

academic and social demands of high school. Thus, adolescents with ADHD may come

to expect a challenge on academic and/or self-regulatory tasks and may be less likely to

attempt such tasks in the future or to persist in the face of difficulty. For a population of

youth who already struggle significantly with academic tasks and self-motivation, the

impact of low self-efficacy is likely even more detrimental to their learning.

Adolescents with ADHD may have less access to efficacious models, which may

also have a deleterious effect on their self-efficacy beliefs. According to Bandura (1986,

1997), modelling, or vicarious learning, is most effective when the observer and the

model share similar qualities. Studies have found that only half of students diagnosed

with ADHD will access appropriate services (Hoagwood, Kellher, Feil, & Comer, 2000),

with stigma and embarrassment being the main barriers (Bussing et al., 2012). Therefore,

high school students with ADHD may be spending more time in mainstream classrooms

where they are surrounded by peers who are likely having more success in school than

they are. Although their typically developing peers may indeed be modelling success, this

Page 61: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

53

may not have the bolstering effect on their self-efficacy beliefs given that adolescents

with ADHD may be sensitive to their academic differences and may not identify with

their typically developing peers in this realm. For youth with ADHD, it is likely that

being in the presence of students for whom success comes more easily may have a

particularly detrimental effect on their self-efficacy beliefs. Even if adolescents with

ADHD are among peers of similar ability levels, given their shared academic and

cognitive weaknesses, both groups may struggle to succeed at academic or self-regulatory

tasks.

Teachers play an important role in providing students with the positive

encouragement they need to build their sense of security, confidence, and well-being at

school (Bandura, 1997; Hamre & Pianta, 2001). There is evidence to suggest that, in

students with emotional or behavioural challenges, the quality of the teacher-student

relationship may be compromised (Murray & Zvoch, 2011), and this may lead to

negative patterns of interaction, including less verbal encouragement towards these

students. For instance, Rogers and colleagues (2015) found that children with symptoms

of ADHD (particularly females) and their teachers reported a weaker emotional bond and

less collaboration in their relationship with each other. Furthermore, among those with

ADHD symptoms, a strong bond was associated with more internal motivation,

highlighting the potential link between positive teacher-student interactions and self-

efficacy beliefs. In addition, the authors argued that, because they measured ADHD

symptoms rather than a clinical diagnosis of the disorder, barriers in the teacher-student

bond were likely attributable to the core symptoms of ADHD, rather than the label itself.

Additional research in the area of ADHD and teacher perceptions has highlighted other

Page 62: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

54

possible barriers in the student-teacher relationship that may lead to less verbal

persuasions towards adolescents with ADHD. For instance, many teachers report having

limited knowledge about ADHD (Arcia et al., 2000), lower confidence in teaching these

students (Ohan et al, 2011; Taylor & Larson, 1998), and that they find children with

ADHD more effortful and stressful to teach (Atkinson, Robinson, & Shute, 1997; Greene

et al., 2002). Teachers are also more likely to perceive a child with ADHD less favorably

than a child without ADHD with regards to intelligence, personality, and behaviour

(Batzle et al., 2010). Thus, lack of appropriate knowledge and negative perceptions

towards ADHD may foster a negative interaction style towards adolescents with ADHD,

thereby exposing them to less positive verbal encouragement relative to their non-ADHD

peers. Less verbal encouragement from teachers can further compromise the self-efficacy

beliefs of adolescents with ADHD.

Adolescents with ADHD may also be more prone to experiencing negative

emotional states, and this may affect their sense of self-efficacy. Emotional disturbances

such as depression, anxiety, and low self-esteem are common in individuals with ADHD

(see Barkley, 1998, for a discussion) as well as in those with symptoms of the disorder

(e.g., Herman, Lambert, Ialongo, & Ostrander, 2007; MacPhee & Andrews, 2006). In a

recent longitudinal study by Lee and colleagues (2008), researchers found that 37 % of

adolescents who were diagnosed with ADHD in preschool experienced internalizing

difficulties in adolescence, compared to 10% of adolescents in the comparison group. In

addition, some researchers have connected higher levels of internalizing symptoms in

adolescents with ADHD with lower levels of self-esteem (e.g., Treuting & Hinshaw,

2001), emphasizing the potential link between negative emotional states and reduced

Page 63: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

55

feelings of competence in this population. Furthermore, ADHD is commonly

conceptualized as a disorder of self-regulation, with increasing attention being given to

the regulation of emotion (Barkley & Fischer, 2010; Melnick & Hinshaw, 2000;

Wehmeier et al., 2010). More recently, Musser and colleagues (2011) sought to extend

this research by examining the physiological mechanisms related to emotion regulation in

ADHD. Researchers found that, unlike typically developing children who showed more

variation in parasympathetic activity according to task demands and emotion valence,

children with ADHD demonstrated a stable pattern of elevated parasympathetic activity

across all task conditions. The authors concluded that ADHD in childhood is associated

with abnormal parasympathetic mechanisms involved in emotion regulation, making it

more difficult for them to respond to their emotions in a flexible and effective manner.

Taken together, the above research suggests that adolescents with ADHD may be more

prone to experiencing negative emotional states, such as anxiety and depression, as well

as heightened physiological arousal and difficulties regulating their emotions. Therefore,

when attempting novel, challenging, or previously unsuccessful tasks, adolescents with

ADHD may be more prone to experiencing intense and stable emotional reactions, which

may send signals about expected success or failure. Feeling upset, anxious, or hopeless

combined with a limited ability to moderate such feelings, may lead adolescents with

ADHD to believe that they are not capable of performing that task, thereby decreasing

their motivation and persistence.

In summary, Bandura’s social cognitive theory (1986, 1997) provides a

conceptual framework for understanding self-efficacy beliefs, including how these beliefs

are formed and how they relate to subsequent learning and achievement. Although this

Page 64: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

56

framework has been studied in typically developing children and adolescents as well as in

adolescents with LDs, we know very little about how this model might apply to

adolescents with ADHD. Evidence to date suggests that, as a result of the characteristics

inherent in the disorder, adolescents with ADHD may be limited in their access to the

sources of self-efficacy, which may subsequently hinder the development of their SESRL

beliefs. This may have a detrimental effect on the learning and academic engagement of

adolescents with ADHD, who already struggle considerably with many aspects of their

school functioning.

Rationale and Objectives of the Current Study

The above literature review emphasizes the need to further our knowledge

regarding the adolescent experience of individuals with ADHD, including how engaged

they judge themselves to be in school and whether they perceive themselves as capable

learners. To date, much of the research examining predictors of academic functioning in

children and adolescents with ADHD has focused on cognitive factors (e.g., EF,

processing speed, intelligence). It is well known that adolescents with ADHD also

demonstrate deficits in motivation (e.g., Barkley, 1997; Carlson et al., 2002; Hoza et al.,

2001), yet very few researchers have isolated and examined key motivational variables,

such as self-efficacy and student engagement, in adolescents with ADHD. This research

is of critical importance, as student engagement and self-efficacy beliefs are considered

to be powerful predictors of academic outcomes, such as higher levels of academic

achievement and the likelihood that adolescents will stay in school and aspire to higher

education (Alexander et al., 1997; Pajares, 1996; Stewart, 2008; Wang & Eccles, 2012;

Wang & Holcombe, 2010). Given that adolescents with ADHD are at a significant risk

Page 65: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

57

for negative academic outcomes, including underachievement and high school dropout

(e.g., Barbaresi et al., 2007; Barkley et al., 1990; Biederman et al., 2006; Faraone et al.,

2000; Murphy & Barkley, 1996; Kent et al., 2011), identifying those variables that may

reduce the likelihood of such outcomes is an important research goal. Low SESRL beliefs

may limit the effort that adolescents with ADHD put forth on self-regulated learning

tasks, and the combination of academic skill deficits and less optimistic beliefs related to

self-regulation may be particularly detrimental to the educational success of these

students. By extending this research further and examining the sources of self-efficacy in

adolescents with ADHD, this study may also provide insight into those factors that may

support or hinder the development of self-efficacy in this population.

Given that self-efficacy beliefs and student engagement are key aspects of

motivation (Bandura, 1986, 1997; Schunk & Mullen, 2012), this research would also add

to our understanding of the motivational dysfunction that is commonly found in

ADHD—a topic that is of growing interest in the literature and is central to many

theoretical models of the disorder. Furthermore, because a majority of the research base

in ADHD focuses on children, understanding the adolescent experience of individuals

with ADHD can help fill this gap in the literature and provide researchers with a better

understanding of ADHD across different periods of development. The study of self-

efficacy, particularly as it pertains to self-regulation, is especially important in

adolescence because of the shift toward more self-directed learning that occurs during

this period of development. In high school, adolescents must assume more responsibility

for their learning and education and develop diverse self-regulatory skills to cope with

the increasing demands of school (Holmbeck et al., 2006; Litner, 2003; Zimmerman,

Page 66: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

58

2002). A strong sense of self-efficacy supports students’ use of self-regulation skills and

their ability to engage more fully in school (Bandura, 1997, 2000; Zimmerman, 2002).

This dissertation aims to (1) determine whether male and female adolescents with

and without ADHD differ in terms of the sources of self-efficacy, their SESRL beliefs,

and their levels of engagement at school, and (2) examine the relations among

inattention, the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and student engagement. To address the

first objective of this study, group and gender differences across all relevant dependent

variables will be examined. Based on the existing research in community and clinical

samples of adolescents with attention difficulties, I hypothesize that adolescents with

ADHD will rate their SESRL beliefs and student engagement as lower than their non-

ADHD peers. Studies in adolescents with LDs provide support for the hypothesis that

adolescents with ADHD will also rate themselves as having less access to the four

sources of self-efficacy. Because gender differences are evident in the sources of self-

efficacy, SESRL, and student engagement, gender will be included as an independent

variable in the analyses.

Path analysis will be used to address the second objective of this study, which is

to examine the relations among inattention, the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and

school engagement. Based on Bandura’s theory and the existing literature, a hypothesized

model of the relations among the variables will be established and tested in the full

sample (see Figure 1). Previous research has identified symptoms of inattention as more

problematic with respect to academic functioning and self-efficacy beliefs, which

justified including inattention symptoms in the analysis and exploring the role of

inattention as it relates to Bandura’s model. Therefore, I hypothesize that SESRL beliefs

Page 67: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

59

will play a mediating role in the relationship between the sources and student

engagement. I also predict that the relationship between inattention and subsequent

student engagement will be mediated by the sources of self-efficacy and SESRL.

Page 68: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

60

Figure 1

Hypothesized Model Depicting the Relations among Parent-Rated Inattention, the Sources of Self-Efficacy, SESRL, and Student

Engagement

PR Inatten.

Sources

SESRL Engage.

Mastery Emotional Persuasion Vicarious

Page 69: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

61

CHAPTER 3

METHOD

Participants

The sample comprised 98 14 to 16 year-old adolescents: 47 (30 males, 17

females) were classified as having ADHD, and 51 (19 males, and 32 females) were a

typically functioning comparison group. To be included in the study, adolescents in the

ADHD sample were required to have: (a) a previous diagnosis of ADHD from a

physician or mental health professional (as reported by parents/guardians); (b) evidence

for ongoing ADHD symptomatology as confirmed by clinically significant scores (T-

score ≥70) on at least one of the core ADHD indices (i.e., DSM-IV Hyperactive

Impulsive, DSM-IV Inattentive, and DSM-IV Global) of the Conners 3 Parent Rating

Scale, Long Form (Conners 3-P; Conners, 2008); (c) a Full-Scale IQ of 70 or greater1 on

the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999); (d) the ability

to fluently speak, understand, and read in English, as the assessment questionnaires and

tasks were validated and completed in English; and (e) no evidence of a significant

neurological, psychiatric, sensory, or physical disability (e.g., traumatic brain injury,

pervasive developmental disorder, psychosis, blindness) that would preclude their ability

to complete the assessment battery (according to parent report). Despite efforts to recruit

females with ADHD, we were only able to recruit 17 within the study period.

Analogous inclusion criteria were used for the adolescents in the comparison

sample: (a) no parent-reported previous or current diagnosis of ADHD or other

1 Two of the participants with ADHD had a Full-Scale IQ below 70 on the WASI; however, both were

included in the final sample because they obtained a standard score of 70 or above on either the verbal or

nonverbal subtests.

Page 70: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

62

behavioural disorder; (b) scores in the normal range of functioning (T-scores ≤ 65) on the

DSM-IV scales of the Conners 3-P; (c) a Full-Scale IQ of 70 or above on the WASI; and

(d) proficiency in reading, speaking, and writing in English.

Adolescents with and without ADHD were recruited through posters and ads in

the community, including local newspapers, agencies, clinics, and community centers.

Participants in the present study were part of a larger study being conducted at the

Ontario Institute for Studies in Education of the University of Toronto (OISE/UT) by Dr.

Rhonda Martinussen, examining listening, reading comprehension, and self-perceptions

of adolescents with and without ADHD. A total of 109 participants initially consented to

participate in the study. Of the initial 56 consenting participants with ADHD, nine

participants were excluded for the following reasons: parent Conners data were not

available (n = 2); they failed to meet criteria for exhibiting ongoing symptoms of ADHD

(n = 2); parents were unclear about whether their child had a confirmed diagnosis (n =

3); they obtained IQ scores below the cut off T-score of 70 (n = 2). In the comparison

group, 53 participants initially consented to the study; however, two were excluded

because they demonstrated clinically significant levels of ADHD symptoms on the parent

Conners. Thus, the remaining sample included 47 adolescents with ADHD and 51

comparison adolescents, for a total of 98 adolescents.

In the present study, participants were first examined categorically (ADHD versus

comparison group) to address objective one, which focused on examining group

differences across the variables of interest (source of self-efficacy, SESRL, and school

engagement). Participants with ADHD were treated as one diagnostic group, rather than

examining differences among symptom presentations (e.g., primarily inattentive versus

Page 71: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

63

primarily hyperactive/impulsive). This decision was based on mounting evidence that

ADHD symptoms exist along a continuum, and that symptom presentations are less

distinct than previously thought and are likely to shift over time (Baeyens, Roeyers, &

Walle, 2006; Hurtig, Ebeling, Taanila et al., 2007; Lubke, Hudziak, Derks, van

Bijsterveldt, & Boomsma, 2009; Willcutt et al., 2012). To address the second objective of

this study, which examined the relations among the sources, SESRL, and school

engagement, the ADHD and comparison adolescents were combined into one group and

ADHD symptoms were examined dimensionally. Thus, adolescents who failed to meet

criteria for inclusion in the ADHD or comparison group due to their scores on the

Conners 3-P (n = 4) were added back into the sample to create a larger group of 105

participants (53 males, 52 females).

The age at which the adolescents with ADHD were first diagnosed ranged from 4

to 15 years, with a mean age of 9 years, 1 month. Of the adolescents with ADHD, 75%

were taking psychostimulant medication for their symptoms (e.g., Ritalin, Concerta).

Because participants were completing a broad range of measures in a larger study, some

of which are known to be sensitive to medication, adolescents with ADHD were asked to

refrain from taking their medication on the testing day. Demographic information for

each of the ADHD and comparison groups as well as the full sample is provided in Table

1. Results of the chi-square analyses indicated that adolescents with and without ADHD

did not differ significantly from each other on parent marital status or parent education

levels. However, significantly more adolescents with ADHD in this study were born in

Canada in comparison to the adolescents without ADHD. The ADHD and comparison

groups also differed in the number of languages other than English spoken within the

Page 72: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

64

Table 1

Sample Characteristics of the ADHD Group, Comparison Group, and the Full Sample

ADHD

(n = 47)

Comparison

(n = 51)

Full Sample

(n = 105)

Sample Characteristic % (n) % (n) X2(df) % (n)

Parent marital status 4.15(5)

Married 68(32)

2(1)

13(6)

13(6)

4(2)

55(28)

2(1)

18(9)

18(9)

0(0)

62(65)

Common Law 2(2)

Single 15(16)

Separated or divorced 15(16)

Widowed 2(2)

Highest level of parent educationa 13.55(8)

Less than high school degree 4(2)

30(14)

47(22)

6(3)

0(0)

12(6)

20(10)

37(19)

20(10)

0(0)

8(8)

High school 7(7)

College or university 64(61)

Graduate 13(14)

Post graduate 0(0)

Born in Canada 92(43)

21(10)

63(32)

49(25)

7.98(1)** 74(78)

Second language spoken at home 10.24(1)** 38(40)

Current grade level 1.74 (4)

Grade 8 4(2) 6(3) 6(6)

Page 73: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

65

aDefined by the highest score across education ratings for parent 1 and parent 2; bDetermined by a standard score of 85 or less on any of the reading, writing, and mathematics

composites from the Woodcock Johnson Tests of Achievement—Third Edition; IEP = Individual Education Plan; cDetermined by a T score equal to or greater than 60 on the

DSM-IV ODD and/or the DSM-IV CD subscales of the Conners 3-P; dDetermined by a score of 2 or higher on the impairment questions from the Conners 3-P *p < .05, ** p < .01, ***p < .001

Grade 9 36(17)

32(15)

26(12)

2(1)

39(20)

35(18)

20(10)

0(0)

36(38)

Grade 10 35(37)

Grade 11 22(23)

Grade 12 1(1)

Low achievementb 36(17)

81(38)

16(8)

4(2)

5.40(1)* 26(27)

IEP support 61.88(20)*** 41(43)

Behaviour difficulties 75(35) 20(10) 29.64(1)*** 47(49)

Academic impairmentd 89(42)

64(30)

68(32)

10(5)

4(2)

6(3)

62.03(1)*** 50(52)

Social impairmentd 39.92(1)*** 31(32)

Home impairmentd

41.22(1)*** 36(38)

Page 74: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

66

home. Specifically, fewer participants with ADHD spoke a language other than English

relative to participants in the comparison group. There were no significant differences

between the ADHD and comparison groups in their current grade level completed.

However, as is typical with an ADHD sample, many of the adolescents were

experiencing co-occurring difficulties. That is, adolescents with ADHD were

significantly more likely to be classified as low achievers (i.e., standard score below 85

on each the reading, writing, and mathematics composites of the Woodcock Johnson Test

of Achievement—Third Edition) and to be receiving some form of special education

support through an Individual Education Plan (IEP) at the time of the study. Adolescents

with ADHD were significantly more likely than those in the comparison group to display

behaviour challenges, as determined by a score in the clinical or borderline range (T-

score ≥ 60) on the DSM IV Oppositional Defiant Disorder and/or the DSM IV Conduct

Disorders subscales of the parent Conners 3. In addition, a greater number of adolescents

with ADHD demonstrated impairments in the academic, social, and home settings than

their non-ADHD peers. This pattern of results was identical even when the analyses were

stratified by gender.

Measures

Demographic information. Demographic and background information was obtained via

a brief questionnaire given to the participants’ mother or father. The information

collected included mothers’ and fathers’ highest level of education, parent marital status,

language(s) spoken at home, and the participants’ country of birth. Parental education

was chosen to represent social class because it has been identified in previous literature as

one of the most stable components of a family’s social status (Featherman, Spenner, &

Page 75: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

67

Tsunematsu, 1988). Thus, the highest level of education obtained by each parent was

averaged as a broad indicator of socioeconomic status (SES).

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999). The WASI is a

standardized abbreviated measure of intellectual functioning that consists of four

subtests: Block Design, Similarities, Vocabulary, and Matrix Reasoning. It can be

administered in a four- or two-subtest form. In the current study, the two-subtest version

was used (Vocabulary and Matrix Reasoning) to yield a Full-Scale IQ score, which is

represented as a T-score with a mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15. The WASI

has been found to correlate highly with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (r

=.83 to .89), a widely used measure of intelligence (Wechsler, 1999). There is also

evidence for high reliability as well as good concurrent and construct validity (Sattler,

2001; Wechsler, 2003).

Woodcock Johnson Test of Achievement—Third Edition (WJ-III; Mather &

Woodcock, 2001). Academic achievement was estimated using selected subtests from

the WJ-III, which is a widely-used, norm-referenced test of achievement. The WJ-III

includes 22 achievement subtests organized into reading, math, written language, and oral

language clusters. Each achievement subtest and cluster yields a standard score with a

mean of 100 and a standard deviation of 15 (Mather & Woodcock, 2001). In the current

study, estimates of reading achievement were obtained by averaging a participant’s score

across the Letter-Word Identification, Word Attack, and Passage Comprehension

subtests. Written expression was estimated using the Writing Samples subtest, and

mathematics achievement was estimated using an average score across the Calculation

and Math Fluency subtests. An overall achievement score was calculated for each

Page 76: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

68

participant by obtaining his/her average score across each of the aforementioned subtests.

The percentage of adolescents with and without ADHD who obtained a standard score

below 85 (< 16th percentile) on each of the academic areas (reading, writing, and math)

was also calculated to provide descriptive data regarding the number of participants who

could be categorized as low achievers.

The WJ-III has good evidence for internal consistency with subtest correlations

exceeding .80 and .90 for the clusters (Mather & Woodcock, 2001).

Conners Rating Scales-Third Edition (Conners 3; Conners, 2008). The Conners 3

ratings scales were used to measure ongoing symptoms of ADHD. This measure is

widely used for the clinical assessment of ADHD in children and adolescents. In the

current study, parents of adolescents with and without ADHD completed the parent

version of the Conners (Conners 3-P) to screen for participation in the study. Adolescents

were administered a parallel self-report version (Conners 3-SR) as a measure of their

perceptions of their ADHD symptoms. The parent- and self-report scales consist of 110

and 99 items, respectively. The 11 subscales of the parent version and the nine subscales

of the self-report version are designed to measure various symptoms and behaviours

associated with ADHD, including inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity, learning

problems, EF, peer and family relations, aggression, conduct disturbances, and

oppositional behaviour. Ratings of ADHD symptoms and associated behaviours are

provided on a 4-point Likert-type-type scale: 0 (not true at all), 1 (just a little true), 2

(pretty much true), and 3 (very much true). For each of the subscales, a T-score is derived

with higher T-scores suggesting more problematic functioning. Internal consistency of

Page 77: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

69

the Conners is high (coefficient alphas range from .79 to .96 across subscales; Conners,

Sitarenios, Parker, & Epstein, 1998).

The Sources of Self-Efficacy Scale (Usher & Pajares, 2006). Originally adapted from

Lent et al. (1991), the Sources of Academic Self-Efficacy Scale was modified by Usher

and Pajares (2006) in order to investigate sources of academic and self-regulatory

efficacy beliefs of entering middle school students. The Sources of Self-Efficacy Scale

consists of 24 items designed to measure the four theorized sources of self-efficacy based

on Bandura’s model. Six items assessed mastery experiences (e.g., “I get good grades in

school”), 6 assessed vicarious experiences (e.g., “People I admire are good at academic

work”), 5 assessed verbal persuasion (e.g., “My friends tell me that I am a good

student”), and 7 assessed physiological/affective factors (e.g., “I’m nervous about doing

school work”). Participants provided their responses on a 6-point Likert-type scale, from

which 4 subscale scores and a total score can be derived. In the present study,

Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were .84 for mastery experiences, .66 for vicarious

learning, .88 for social persuasion, and .85 for physiological arousal. Cronbach’s alpha

reliability for the total score was .90. Because this study linked self-efficacy and its

sources to a general, non-domain-specific measure of school engagement, we did not

specify a domain for the sources of self-efficacy scale, despite the enhanced predictive

validity found in subject-specific measures (Bandura, 1994; Schunk & Pajaras, 2009).

Self-Efficacy for Learning Form (SELF; Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2005). The SELF

is a 57-item measure that assesses students’ beliefs about their ability to use specific self-

regulatory processes to cope with difficult learning conditions that involve reading, note-

taking, test-taking, writing, and studying. Items on this scale include questions such as

Page 78: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

70

“When you don’t understand a paragraph you have just read, can you clarify it by

carefully rereading?” and “When you feel very anxious before taking a test, can you

remember all the material you studied?” Students rate items such as these on a scale that

ranges from 0 to 100 in 10-unit increments. Written descriptors are provided alongside

the following data points: 0 (definitely cannot do it), 30 (probably cannot do it), 50

(maybe), 70 (probably can), and 100 (definitely can do it). Higher scores on the SELF

reflect more positive SESRL beliefs.

For the purposes of the current study, adaptations were made to the SELF so that

it was presented on computer rather than the traditional paper and pencil format. Each

item appeared individually on the computer screen with the 0 to 100 scale (in increments

of 10) and the corresponding descriptions. To reduce any differences based on individual

reading levels, items were also read aloud to students through the computer. Once the

item was read, students were asked to enter their response using the numbers on the

keyboard in a response box that appeared on the screen.

In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha reliability for the SELF was .97.

Zimmerman and Kitsantas (2005) reported a reliability of .99. In addition, they reported a

correlation between the SELF and teacher ratings of students’ actual self-regulatory

behaviour of .72, providing evidence for this measure’s strong predictive validity.

Zimmerman and Kitsantas (2005, 2007) also found that scores on the SELF reflect a

single underlying self-regulatory factor. Unlike previous measures of SESRL (i.e.,

Bandura, 1989; Schunk, 1996), the predictive validity of the SELF is optimized because

the items extend beyond students’ beliefs about their procedural knowledge and skill

Page 79: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

71

(e.g., setting goals, monitoring progress) to include their self-perceptions of confidence

regarding their ability to cope with particular academic problems and contexts.

School Engagement Questionnaire. Participants completed an adaptation of a measure

reported by Archambault, Janosz, Fallu, and Pagani (2009) to evaluate their levels of

engagement at school. Although the original measure was designed to assess the three

theoretical components of engagement (cognitive, behavioural, and affective), only items

from the behaviour and affective subscales were of interest in the present study. Items

assessing the cognitive aspect of engagement were not used because it was believed that

they would correlate highly with measures of attention. In addition, items were used from

the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA; Organisation for Economic

Co-operation and Development, 2009) to further support the measurement of behavioural

and affective engagement. On the adapted School Engagement Questionnaire, 9 items

were used to assess behavioural engagement. On 6 of these 9 items, participants were

asked to rate their attendance, compliance, and involvement with school teams on a 4-

point Likert-type scale from ‘never’ to ‘always.’ Sample items include, “How often have

you skipped class without a valid reason?” and “How often have you disrupted your class

on purpose?” Three of the 9 items asked participants to rate how much time they spent on

various academic activities on a 4-point Likert-type scale from ‘no time’ to ‘3 hours or

more.’ Seven items were used to assess affective engagement, which queried

participant’s enjoyment of school and their level of interest with school-related

challenges and activities. Sample items from this scale include, “I like school” and “What

we learn in school is interesting.” Participants rated these items 4-point Likert-type scale

that ranged from ‘never’ to ‘always.’ An overall score of school engagement was created

Page 80: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

72

by averaging the responses to the total 16 items. After considering reverse-scored items,

higher scored were taken to reflect higher levels of school engagement.

In the current study, Cronbach’s alpha reliability for the school engagement

measure was .79. In addition, an exploratory factor analysis was completed, and results

indicated that scores on the School Engagement measure reflect a single underlying

factor.

Procedure

The testing sessions were conducted in a laboratory at the Ontario Institute for

Studies in Education of the University of Toronto. Parents of the adolescents who were

interested in participating were given a brief intake screen and asked to complete the

Conners 3-P over the telephone to determine whether their child met the inclusion

criteria. Those families who were eligible to participate were scheduled for an assessment

session and were mailed consent forms. When participants arrived at the lab, consent

forms were collected and adolescents were given a verbal overview of the procedure by

the research assistant. Adolescents worked individually with a research assistant for

approximately 4 to 5 hours, during which they completed a battery of self-report

measures and standardized tests that were included as part of a larger study being

conducted by Dr. Rhonda Martinussen. Measures that were relevant to the present study

were located approximately one third of the way through the testing battery. Adolescents

were provided with a break halfway through the session, and they were also encouraged

to take small breaks in between tasks when needed. Research assistants were graduate

students in school and clinical child psychology who were well-trained in psychological

test administration. As an incentive for their participation in the study, all parents and

Page 81: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

73

adolescents received an educational report describing their child’s functioning on relevant

cognitive and academic measures. In addition to the report, adolescents were given the

choice of receiving $30.00 cash or community service hours (necessary to receive a high

school diploma in Ontario) to compensate for their time and travel expenses.

Data Analysis

The first objective of this study was to determine whether male and female

adolescents with and without ADHD differ in the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and

school engagement. Prior to addressing this objective, the univariate analyses of variance

(ANOVAs) with group status (ADHD, comparison) and gender as the between-subject

factors were analyzed to examine whether there were ADHD and/or gender differences

on the demographic, behavioural, cognitive, and academic achievement measures. Each

of the continuous variables was examined for normality. Except for the WASI Nonverbal

IQ score, all other variables were normally distributed within each subgroup and across

the full sample. The analyses were nonetheless conducted on the WASI Nonverbal IQ, as

the F-test is considered robust to normality violations (see Lindman, 1974 for a

summary). Chi-square analyses were used to examine ADHD group differences on the

categorical variables (e.g., marital status, parent employment status). Six two (ADHD,

comparison) by two (male, female) ANOVAs were then conducted to examine group and

gender differences across the dependent variables. These included the total score from the

SELF, the total score from the School Engagement Questionnaire, and the four subscale

scores from the Sources of Self-Efficacy Scale (Mastery Experiences, Vicarious

Experiences, Social Persuasion, and Physiological Responses).

Page 82: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

74

The second objective of this study was to examine the relations among parent-

rated inattention, the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and school engagement in a

sample of 14 to 16 year-old adolescents. Based on Bandura’s theory and the existing

literature, a hypothesized model of the relationship among the variables was established

and the data were subjected to structural equation modeling (SEM) using Mplus 7.0

(Muthén & Muthén, 2009). All variables were normally distributed in the full sample.

Unlike other multivariate statistical procedures, SEM is beneficial because of its ability to

deconstruct structural relations into direct, indirect, and total effects. It also requires

strong theoretical underpinnings and empirical evidence, rather than being exploratory in

nature. The testing of an a priori model may involve a comparison with another

alternative or a posteriori model. Acceptance of a final model can be made with an

evaluation of various goodness-of-fit index values, such as the chi-square statistics, the

Bentler comparative fit index (CFI; Bentler, 1990), the Steiger-Lind root mean square

error of approximation (RMSEA; Steiger, 1990) with its 90% confidence interval (CI),

and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). In the present study, adequate

fit was suggested by CFI > 0.90, RMSEA < 0.05 with its 90% CI, and SRMR < .08 (Hu

& Bentler, 1999; Kline, 2011; MacCallum, Browne, & Sugawara, 1996). Once the full

model was specified, the parameters estimates were interpreted.

The hypothesized model is depicted in Figure 1. According to the model, school

engagement is hypothesized to show direct effects from SESRL beliefs, which in turn, is

directly affected by the sources of self-efficacy. Parent-rated inattention would be

indirectly related to school engagement through the sources of self-efficacy and SESRL.

Preliminary analyses were first conducted to explore the bivariate correlations among the

Page 83: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

75

variables and determine which, if any, of the control variables (i.e., age, SES, Full Scale

IQ, and achievement), were relevant to include in the subsequent analyses. Control

variables that were significantly associated with school engagement were subjected to a

subsequent regression analysis to determine their predictive power relative to inattention,

the sources, and SESRL.

Prior to testing the fit of the structural model, it was necessary to establish the

measurement model. The measurement model determines the connection between the

constructs in the model and the underlying data that define them (Kline, 2011).

Individuals who did not have complete data at all time points (i.e., missing data) were

still included in analyses, as Mplus uses a Full Information Maximum Likelihood (FIML)

estimation of the mean, variance, and covariance parameters. FIML is a helpful technique

which addresses missingness and provides unbiased and efficient estimates, assuming the

data are missing at random and conditional on all of the observable data (Enders &

Bandalos, 2001; Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002). Further explorations of the data revealed

that the multivariate normality assumption was violated. Consequently, in contrast to ML

estimation procedure that is based on multivariate normal distribution, Restricted ML

(RML) estimation methods were used instead. The Sobel Test was used to test for

mediation (Geiser, 2010); that is, to determine whether the sources of self-efficacy and

SESRL mediated the relationship between parent-rated inattention and engagement.

Page 84: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

76

CHAPTER 4

RESULTS

Research Objective 1: Do male and female adolescents with and without ADHD

differ in the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL beliefs, and school engagement?

Table 2 presents means, standard deviations, and F values for additional sample

characteristics. The results of these preliminary univariate ANOVAs revealed that there

were no significant group, gender, or interaction (group by gender) effects for age or

SES. However, there was an effect of group status on estimated Verbal IQ and Full Scale

IQ, with adolescents in the ADHD group scoring significantly lower than adolescents in

the comparison group. There were no significant gender or interaction effects for these

variables, nor were there significant group, gender, or interaction effects for Nonverbal

IQ. In terms of academic achievement, there was a significant main effect of group status

for the mathematics, writing, and overall achievement scores. That is, adolescents with

ADHD scored significantly lower than their typically developing peers on overall

academic achievement and achievement in the core academic areas of mathematics and

written expression. The main effect for gender and the group by gender interactions were

not significant for these variables. With respect to reading achievement, the effect of

group status was not significant, nor was the interaction effect; however, there was a

significant main effect for gender, with males scoring significantly lower on this

composite measure than females.

ANOVAs were conducted on the parent- and self-report versions of the Conners

3, with Learning Problems, DSM-IV ADHD Inattentive, and DSM-IV ADHD

Hyperactive-Impulsive subscales as the dependent variables. Significant group

Page 85: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

77

Table 2

Sample Characteristics of Male and Female Adolescents in the ADHD and Comparison Groups

ADHD Comparison ANOVA Results

Females

(n = 17 )

Males

(n = 30)

Total

(n = 47)

Females

(n = 32)

Males

(n = 19 )

Total

(n = 51)

Group Gender

Group x

Gender

Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) F(df ) F(df ) F(df )

Age (years) 14.87 (.95) 15.26 (.88) 15.12(.92) 15.12 (.86) 14.84 (.97) 15.01(.90) .21 (1,94) .10 (1,94) 3.13(1,94)

SES 7.68 (2.54) 8.03 (1.64) 7.90(1.99) 8.15 (1.84) 7.97 (2.09) 8.09(1.91) .23 (1,90) .05 (1,90) .39 (1,90)

Cognitive abilities (WASI)

Verbal 55.47 (11.10) 53.07 (8.19) 53.94(9.30) 57.81 (6.66) 57.74 (6.90) 57.78(6.69) 4.27(1,94)* .53(1,94) .47(1,94)

Nonverbal 52.24 (6.97) 52.10(10.09) 52.15(9.01) 54.50 (5.77) 55.37 (6.19) 54.82(5.89) 3.00(1,94) .05(1,94) .10(1,94)

Full Scale 106.88 (12.85) 104.83 (12.87) 105.57(12.77) 110.66 (9.37) 111.42 (9.31) 110.94(9.26) 4.89(1,94)* .08(1,94) .36(1,94)

Achievement (WJ-III ACH)

Overall achievement 97.04 (9.33) 95.08 (11.38) 95.79(10.63) 103.85 (7.84) 103.32 (8.78) 103.65(8.12) 14.33(1,94)*** .39(1,94) .13(1,94)

Math composite 85.24 (16.22) 88.53 (17.93) 87.34(17.23) 101.38 (14.43) 104.13 (14.51) 102.40(14.38) 22.62(1,94)*** .82(1,94) .01(1,94)

Page 86: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

78

Writing composite 102.00 (11.29) 101.03 (13.68) 101.38(12.75) 111.50 (10.84) 109.58 (11.22) 110.78(10.91) 13.00(1,94)** .33(1,94) .04(1,94)

Reading composite 102.92 (8.47) 97.43 (9.60) 99.42(9.50) 102.95 (7.97) 100.68 (7.17) 102.10(7.69) .85(1,94) 4.79(1,94)* .82(1,94)

ADHD Symptoms (Conners 3)

PR Learning problems 73.59 (11.00) 72.87 (11.29) 73.13(11.07) 49.41 (7.35) 51.74 (8.89) 50.27(7.95) 125.63(1,94)*** .16(1,94) .57 (1,94)

PR Inattentive 83.00 (7.46) 81.83 (8.97) 82.26(8.39) 51.16 (6.13) 50.42 (6.58) 50.88(6.24) 413.67 (1,94)*** .37 (1,94) .02 (1,94)

PR Hyperactive-Impulsive 81.00 (11.60) 83.87 (8.60) 82.83(9.77) 50.81 (7.71) 48.95 (6.70) 50.12(7.34) 325.71 (1,94)*** .08 (1,94) 1.72(1,94)

SR Learning problems 62.12 (10.97) 58.63 (12.57) 59.89(12.01) 50.50 (9.05) 50.32 (9.70) 50.43(9.20) 19.77 (1,94)*** .67 (1,94) .54 (1,94)

SR Inattentive 65.94 (12.53) 60.37 (10.07) 62.38(11.22) 54.53 (7.66) 52.11 (9.55) 53.63(8.40) 23.19(1,94)*** 3.84 (1,94) .59 (1,94)

SR Hyperactive-Impulsive 66.94(15.21) 63.03 (13.00) 64.45(13.81) 52.28 (9.64) 53.11 (9.26) 52.59(9.41) 24.79 (1,94)*** .39(1,94) .92(1,94)

SES = Socioeconomic status; WASI = Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence; WJ-III ACH = Woodcock Johnston Test of Achievement—Third Edition; SR = Self-Report;

PR = Parent-Report.

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001

Page 87: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

79

differences emerged across both versions on ratings of learning problems, inattention,

and hyperactivity/impulsivity. As expected, parent- and self-reported learning challenges

and symptoms of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity were significantly higher in

the ADHD group than in the comparison group2. There were no gender differences in

parent- or self-reported learning problems or symptoms of inattention or hyperactivity-

impulsivity, nor were there significant interaction effects.

Although there were several group differences that emerged from the preliminary

ANOVAs (i.e., IQ, achievement, parent- and self-reported learning problems, inattention,

and hyperactivity-impulsivity), these variables were not considered as potential

covariates because they reflect relationships that are highly associated with ADHD. One

of the assumptions of analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) is that the covariates must be

statistically independent from the grouping variable (Miller & Chapman, 2001).

Therefore, a series of two (ADHD, comparison) by two (female, male) ANOVAs were

performed to evaluate group and gender differences across the four sources of self-

efficacy (mastery experiences, vicarious experiences, verbal encouragement,

physiological responses), SESRL, and school engagement (see Table 3).

With respect to the sources of self-efficacy, there was a significant main effect of

group status on mastery experience and social persuasion, with moderate effect sizes

(partial η2 = .08 for both mastery and social persuasion). Specifically, participants in the

ADHD group perceived themselves to have fewer mastery experiences and less positive

encouragement from others relative to adolescents in the comparison group. The group

effects for vicarious experiences and physiological arousal were not significant.

2 These findings were noted in Chan & Martinussen (2015), which examined the positive illusory bias

(PIB) in adolescents with ADHD using the same sample as the present study.

Page 88: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

80

Table 3

Group Differences in the Sources of Self-Efficacy, SESRL, and Student Engagement

ADHD Comparison ANOVA Results

Females

(n = 17 )

Males

(n = 30)

Total

(n = 47)

Females

(n = 32)

Males

(n = 19)

Total

(n = 51) Group Gender Group x Gender

Mean (SD)

Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

Mean (SD)

Mean (SD) Mean (SD)

F(df ) F(df ) F(df )

Mastery 3.74(.85) 3.11(.82) 3.35(.88) 3.97 (.70) 3.78(.71) 3.90(.70) 6.07(1,73)* 4.99(1,73)* 1.39(1,73)

Vicarious 3.62(.81) 3.29(.84) 3.41(.84) 3.67(.52) 3.19(.84) 3.49(.70) .01(1,73) 5.00(1,73)* .19(1,73)

Social Persuasion 3.71(1.15) 3.27(1.09) 3.44(1.12) 4.30(.56) 3.80(.71) 4.11(.66) 6.36(1,73)* 4.51(1,73)* .02(1,73)

Physiological 2.83(1.20) 3.04(.94) 2.96(1.03) 3.18(.86) 3.40(1.12) 3.26(.96) 2.17(1,73) .83(1,73) .00(1,73)

SELF 57.59(18.19) 58.63(16.27) 58.25(16.80) 71.77(10.42) 69.34(14.25) 70.87(11.91) 16.56(1,94)*** .05(1,94) .32(1,94)

Engagement 1.84(.42) 1.63(.41) 1.71(.42) 1.88(.37) 1.98(.37) 1.92(.36) 4.27(1,73)* .31(1,73) 2.61(1,73)

SELF = Self-Efficacy for Learning Form

*p < .05, **p < .01, ***p < .001

Page 89: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

81

As seen in Table 3, there was a significant effect of gender status on mastery experiences

(partial η2 = .06), vicarious experiences (partial η2 = .06), and social persuasion (partial η2

= .06), with the effect sizes being in the moderate range. Specifically, males obtained

lower scores than females across all three measures. There were no significant

interaction effects across any of the four sources of self-efficacy. Despite the lack of

significant interaction effects, an inspection of the means suggests that males with ADHD

were at a particular disadvantage for mastery experiences as their score was the lowest of

the four groups.

In terms of SESRL beliefs, there was a significant main effect of group status

(partial η2 = .15, indicating a large effect size), with participants in the ADHD group

rating their self-efficacy beliefs significantly lower than comparison adolescents.

Specifically, adolescents with ADHD provided ratings that corresponded with the

“maybe-probably can” range on the SELF, whereas the comparison group provided

ratings that fell closer to the “probably can” value. In other words, adolescents with

ADHD felt less competent in dealing with challenging learning situations related to

reading, writing, note-taking, studying, and test-taking compared to their non-ADHD

peers (see Table 3). The main effect for gender status and the interaction effect were not

statistically significant. However, the non-significant interaction effect may be a result of

limited power due to the small sample size in some cells (e.g., n = 17 females with

ADHD; n = 19 males without ADHD). Lastly, a supplemental two (ADHD, comparison)

by two (female, male) ANOVA was performed to evaluate group and gender differences

in SESRL after excluding adolescents who were categorized as low achievers. The

pattern of results was identical (partial η2 = .12), indicating that low achievement status

Page 90: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

82

did not account for the observed differences in SESRL between adolescents with and

without ADHD.

As illustrated in Table 3, a two (ADHD, comparison) by two (female, male)

ANOVA with student engagement as the dependent variable revealed a significant main

effect of group status (partial η2 = .06, indicating a moderate effect size), with

participants in the ADHD group rating themselves as significantly less engaged at school

than adolescents in the comparison group. That is, adolescents with ADHD perceived

themselves as being less interested and involved in school, and as spending less time on

school-related activities than their non-ADHD peers. However, an inspection of the

means shows that the group with the lowest ratings of engagement was that comprised of

males with ADHD.

Research Objective 2: How do inattention, the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and

school engagement relate?

Preliminary Analyses

Bivariate correlations among the variables of interest (predictors, mediators, and

outcome), as well as potential covariates (e.g., age, SES, cognitive abilities, achievement)

were examined first (see Table 4). Parent-rated inattention was significantly negatively

associated with school engagement, the sources of self-efficacy, and SESRL beliefs.

Thus, adolescents with higher levels of attention difficulties were more likely to rate

themselves as being less engaged at school, less confident in their ability to self-regulate

their learning, and less likely to have access to the four sources of self-efficacy. In

addition, the sources total score was significantly positively correlated with SESRL and

school engagement, such that higher ratings on the sources total score were related to

Page 91: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

83

more positive self-efficacy beliefs and higher levels of engagement. Self-efficacy for self-

regulated learning beliefs were also positively correlated with school engagement. Thus,

adolescents who felt more confident in their self-regulated learning abilities also rated

themselves as being more engaged at school. Full scale IQ scores and overall

achievement were related to the variable of interest (i.e., school engagement) in the

expected direction, suggesting that it was important to consider their influence further.

This was achieved by conducting a hierarchical regression analysis predicting school

engagement, with the control variables in the first step (i.e., WASI Full Scale IQ and WJ-

III Overall Achievement scores) and the theoretically relevant predictor variables in the

second step (i.e., parent-rated inattention, total scores from the SELF and Sources scales).

Results indicated that only achievement predicted a significant proportion of the variance

in school engagement (R2= .112, F(2,81) = 5.00, p < .01, ß = .423, p < .05) before

inattention, the sources, and SESRL were included in the model. When these variables

were added in the second step, additional variance was predicted (Δ R2= .491, F(5,81) =

23.09, p < .001) and overall achievement was no longer a significant predictor (ß = .105,

p = .419). In this final step, only the sources and SESRL were unique predictors of school

engagement (ß = .573, p < .001 and ß = .229, p < .05). This pattern of results suggests

that IQ and achievement do not share a unique relationship with school engagement;

rather, they share variance with inattention symptoms, the sources of self-efficacy beliefs,

and with SESRL, which are, in turn, unique predictors of student engagement (see Table

4). Given the sample size, the decision was made to exclude overall achievement and

full-scale IQ from the path analysis. In future studies, it would be important to examine

Page 92: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

84

Table 4

Bivariate Correlations among Relevant Sample Characteristics, Parent-Reported Inattention, the

Sources of Self-Efficacy, SESRL, and Student Engagement in the Full Sample

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. Age 1.0

2. SES -.05 1.0

3. Cognitive abilities -.24* .30** 1.0

4. Achievement -.10 .37** .78** 1.0

5. PR Inattention .13 -.09 -.24* -.35** 1.0

6. Sources -.02 .28* .20 .29** -.38** 1.0

7. SESRL .03 .28* .32** .43** -.39** .66** 1.0

8. Engagement .06 .17 .23* .33** -.34** .75** .63** 1.0

PR = Parent-Rated

∗p <05, **p <.01, ***p

Page 93: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

85

whether achievement contributes to the development of SESRL beliefs or is an outcome

of adolescents’ beliefs and levels of engagement.

Assessment of the Measurement Model

The Sources of Self-Efficacy Scale includes four factors (mastery experiences,

vicarious learning, verbal persuasion, and physiological arousal). Confirmatory factors

analysis was done to determine the adequacy of factor loadings and model fit of the

Sources of Self-Efficacy Scale. All factors loadings for the latent source variable were

statistically significant and in the expected direction (see Figure 2). As indicated by the

following fit statistics, the four-factor model fit the data exceptionally well. Specifically,

RMSEA = .00, 90% CI (.00, .15); CFI = 1.00; and SRMR = .01.

Assessment of the Structural Model

To evaluate the second objective of this study, the associations among parent-

rated inattention, the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and school engagement were

examined by the structural equation model (see Figure 1). In this model, Source is a

latent variable specified by four factors: mastery experiences, vicarious learning, verbal

persuasion, and physiological arousal. Inattention was specified by parent ratings on the

DSM-IV ADHD-Inattentive subscale of the Conners 3; SESRL was specified by the total

SELF score; school engagement was specified by the total score on the School

Engagement Questionnaire.

The results indicated that the model fit was adequate [RMSEA = 0.123, 90% CI

(.05, .16); CFI = .93; SRMR = .06]. The structural parameter estimates are presented in

3 The RMSEA value exceeded the cut-off of .05; however, that this cut-off value was within the 90% CI was

considered (Kenny, 2015). A model with fewer parameters was tested to examine the influence on the goodness-of-fit indices. See Supplemental Analyses.

Page 94: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

86

Figure 2. Direct, indirect, and total effects are presented in Table 5. The following results

were obtained: 1) the total effect from inattention to student engagement was significant;

2) the direct effect of inattention on student engagement was not significant; 3) the

indirect relationship from inattention to student engagement via SESRL was not

significant; 4) there was a significant indirect relationship from inattention to student

engagement via the sources of self-efficacy; 5) inattention was indirectly related to school

engagement through both the sources of self-efficacy and SESRL. An examination of the

total, direct, and indirect effects provides evidence for mediation (see Table 5). That is,

the sources of self-efficacy and SESRL mediated the relationship between inattention and

student engagement.

Page 95: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

87

Figure 2

Structural Model Depicting the Relations among Parent-Rated Inattention, the Sources of Self-Efficacy, SESRL, and Student

Engagement

Mastery

.52***

.06

-.14

-.47***

.40*** .86***

Emotional

.59*** .92***

PR Inatten.

Sources

SESRL

Persuasion

Engage.

Vicarious

.27**

.62***

Page 96: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

88

Table 5

Standardized Direct, Indirect, and Total Effects from Parent-Rated Inattention to Student

Engagement

Estimate SE p-value

Total -.35 -3.43 <.01

Direct .06 .66 .51

Total Indirect -.40 -4.95 <.01

Specific Indirect

PR Inattention, to SESRL, to engagement -.04 -1.19 .24

PR Inattention, sources, engagement -.30 -3.57 .00

PR Inattention, sources, SESRL, engagement -.07 -2.82 .01

SESRL = Self-Efficacy for Self-Regulated Learning; PR = Parent-Rated

Page 97: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

89

Supplemental Analyses

The RMSEA value for the structural model exceeded the cut-off of 0.05;

however, this cut-off fell within the 90% confidence interval, which takes into account

sampling error (Kenny, 2015). Of note, models with small df and low N (< 200), as with

the present study, are known to have artificially large values of the RMSEA (Kenny,

2015). In fact, some researchers (e.g., Kenny, Kaniskan, & McCoach, 2014) have argued

that the RMSEA value not be computed for low df and low N models. To determine

whether the RMSEA value may have been inflated due to the small sample size and

relatively large number of parameters in the model, a supplemental analysis of the

structural model was completed without the inclusion of the four factors (mastery

experiences, vicarious learning, verbal persuasion, and physiological arousal) comprising

the latent Sources variable. Thus, rather than treating the Sources as a latent variable, the

composite (total) score from the Sources of Self-Efficacy Scale in the supplemental

structural equation model was used. Results indicated that the RMSEA value was

improved [RMSEA = .00, 90% CI (.00, .00)], as were the other fit statistics (CFI = 1.00,

and SRMR = .00). This improvement in model fit with fewer factors suggests that the

small sample size and large number of parameters in the first structural model may have

contributed to inflated fit statistics.

Page 98: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

90

CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION

The overall objective of this dissertation was to advance our understanding of

motivation in adolescents with ADHD by examining key motivational constructs

including self-efficacy beliefs, the sources of self-efficacy, and student engagement, as

well as how these variables relate to each other and to symptoms of inattention. Although

different theoretical models explain student motivation, the present study utilized

Bandura’s (1986, 1997, 2001) social cognitive theory of psychological functioning,

which emphasizes that self-efficacy beliefs are a driving force behind student behaviour,

including their interest and engagement at school (Bandura, 1986, 1997; Schunk &

Mullen, 2012). Bandura (1997, 2000) also identified four sources of self-efficacy through

which individuals interpret and draw from to form their self-efficacy beliefs: previous

experiences of success, exposure to and identification with efficacious models, verbal

encouragement and support from others, and emotional reactions in the context of task

performance. Thus, self-efficacy beliefs play a mediating role between the sources of

self-efficacy and student engagement. Although researchers have examined Bandura’s

model in typically developing adolescents and adolescents with LDs, very little is known

about the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and student engagement in adolescents with

ADHD or how symptoms of inattention may influence these key motivational variables.

Thus, this dissertation aimed to (1) determine whether male and female adolescents with

and without ADHD differ in terms of the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and their

levels of engagement at school, and (2) examine the relations among inattention, the

sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and student engagement. This chapter will provide a

Page 99: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

91

discussion of the study results, practical implications, limitations, and directions for

future research.

Group Differences in the Sources, SESRL, and Engagement

The first objective of this study was to determine whether male and female

adolescents with and without ADHD differ in terms of the sources of self-efficacy,

SESRL, and student engagement. With respect to group differences in SESRL beliefs,

results indicated that adolescents in the ADHD group rated their SESRL beliefs

significantly lower than comparison adolescents, and the degree of this effect was large.

Specifically, adolescents with ADHD provided ratings that corresponded with the

“maybe-probably can” range (58% confident) on the SELF, whereas ratings of the

comparison group fell closer to the “probably can” value (70% confident). In other

words, compared to their non-ADHD peers, adolescents with ADHD felt less confident in

their ability to use self-regulation strategies to overcome challenging learning tasks

related to reading, writing, note-taking, studying, and test-taking. These results are

consistent with the study hypotheses as well as previous research examining self-efficacy

in community and clinical samples of participants with ADHD (Luzzo et al., 1999; Major

et al., 2013; Norvilitis et al., 2010; Norwalk et al., 2009; Tabassam & Grainger, 2002;

Tomevi, 2013; Young et al., 2005). These results also fit well with recent qualitative

research (Wiener & Daniels, 2015) in which adolescents with ADHD identified

themselves as having challenges with self-regulation and personal agency. Relative to

their peers, it appears that adolescents with ADHD are less sure of their ability to

successfully manage the self-regulatory demands of secondary school. As there is a

substantial amount of empirical evidence for deficits in EFs and self-regulation in

Page 100: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

92

individuals with ADHD (e.g., Barkley et al., 2001; Nigg et al, 1998; Toplak et al., 2009;

Valera et al., 2007; Willcutt et al., 2010; Willcutt et al., 2005), this finding provides a

unique contribution to the literature by shedding light on adolescents’ beliefs regarding

their ability to self-regulate their learning effectively. This is important given evidence

that student beliefs and expectations can powerfully influence academic outcomes, at

times, beyond existing knowledge and skill (e.g., Zuffianὸ et al., 2013).

This study failed to find a gender difference in SESRL beliefs nor a significant

interaction between gender and ADHD. . That is, both males and females in the present

study provided similar ratings of confidence in their ability to self-regulate their

learning—a finding that is inconsistent with the existing literature. For instance, in

typically developing children and adolescents, females tend to rate themselves higher on

measures of SESRL relative to males (e.g., Pajares, 2002; Pajares & Valiante, 2001;

Vecchio et al., 2007). Conversely, in a clinical sample, Major and colleagues (2013)

found that female adolescents with ADHD provided the lowest ratings of SESRL relative

to female adolescents without ADHD and male adolescents with and without ADHD. Of

note, this latter study was limited by a small sample of female adolescents with ADHD

(n=13), which may have led to spurious results (Major et al., 2013). Unfortunately, the

present study is also limited by a small sample size of female adolescents with ADHD,

making it difficult to provide clarification regarding potential gender differences in

SESRL. There is a growing body of research that has demonstrated subtle but important

vulnerabilities in the emotional functioning of females with ADHD (Nussbaum, 2012;

Rucklidge, 2010; Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001), as they are at a higher risk for

internalizing and psychosocial problems relative to their male counterparts (Rucklidge &

Page 101: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

93

Tannock, 2001). Given this risk combined with the present study’s results regarding

gender differences in SESRL, future research should continue to include gender in their

analyses of the self-perceptions of adolescents with ADHD.

With respect to the sources of self-efficacy, the hypotheses were partially

supported. As predicted, adolescents with and without ADHD differed significantly in

their ratings of mastery experiences and verbal persuasion. Thus, like their peers with

LDs (Hampton, 1998; Hampton & Mason, 2003; Usher & Pajares, 2006), adolescents

with ADHD perceived themselves as having less success on academic tasks and receiving

less positive encouragement from others. However, contrary to the study hypotheses and

previous findings in the LD literature, there were no group differences in reported

vicarious experiences or levels of physiological arousal. In other words, adolescents with

ADHD and typically developing adolescents reported comparable levels of exposure to

efficacious models and emotional responsivity on academic tasks.

The finding that adolescents with ADHD reported having less academic success

than their non-ADHD peers is not surprising given that widespread academic

impairments have been well-documented in ADHD (e.g., Barbaresi et al., 2007;

Bauermeister et al., 2007; DuPaul & Stoner, 2014; Frazier et al., 2007; Lahey et al., 2004;

Rogers et al., 2011). However, this finding adds to the literature showing that adolescents

with ADHD are indeed aware of these difficulties. This is important for several reasons.

Firstly, existing evidence suggests that children with ADHD tend to be positively biased

in their judgments about their capabilities, particularly in the domain with which they

experience the most difficulty (e.g., Hoza et al., 2002, 2004; Milich & Okazaki, 1991;

Owens & Hoza, 2003). Although there is evidence to suggest that this positive illusory

Page 102: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

94

bias (PIB) continues into adolescence (Hoza et al., 2010; McQuade, et al., 2011),

research has demonstrated that adolescents with ADHD do not lack complete awareness

of their challenges (Chan & Martinussen, 2015; Varma, 2013). In spite of the PIB in

ADHD, most studies comparing children with and without ADHD on academic self-

concept have found lower self-concept in children with ADHD (Owens et al., 2007). This

is consistent with the present findings demonstrating that adolescents with ADHD report

lower ratings of mastery experiences in adolescents with ADHD relative to comparison

adolescents. Secondly, being aware of one’s difficulties is considered an important

component of learning and self-enhancement (Milich & Okazaki, 1991). Therefore,

having an awareness of their challenges relating to mastering academic tasks may be a

starting point for adolescents with ADHD in accepting feedback from others, seeking

help when needed, and taking the appropriate steps towards building skills. However, the

opposite effect may also be true, and repeated failure may foster learned helplessness in

adolescents with ADHD, decreasing their motivation on similar tasks in the future.

Indeed, there is evidence to support this contention, as children with ADHD are more

likely than children without ADHD to demonstrate behaviours consistent with learned

helplessness when faced with repeated failure (Milich, 1994; Milich & Okazaki, 1991;

Firmin, Hwang, Copella, & Clark, 2004). Consistent with Bandura’s social cognitive

theory (1986, 1997), if adolescents with ADHD perceive themselves as having fewer

mastery experiences, it is feasible that this may impact their confidence levels on

academic tasks and how much effort and persistence they put forth.

In addition to reporting fewer mastery experiences, adolescents with ADHD also

perceived themselves as receiving less verbal encouragement about their abilities in the

Page 103: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

95

classroom than typically developing adolescents. This finding fits well with existing

evidence of a compromised teacher-student relationship in students with ADHD—one

that is characterized by more negativity, a weaker emotional bond, and less collaboration

(Arcia et al., 2000; Atkinson et al., 1997; Batzle et al., 2010; Greene et al., 2002; Murray

& Zvoch, 2011; Ohan et al, 2011; Rogers et al., 2015; Taylor & Larson, 1998). Several

possible factors may interfere with the teacher-student relationship and lead to reduced

verbal persuasion toward adolescents with ADHD. For instance, teachers of students with

ADHD report limited knowledge regarding the characteristics of the disorder (Arcia et

al., 2000), lower confidence in teaching these students (Ohan et al, 2011; Taylor &

Larson, 1998), and greater levels of effort and stress (Atkinson et al., 1997; Greene et al.,

2002). Rogers and colleagues (2015) found that, by measuring ADHD symptoms rather

than a clinical diagnosis of the disorder, barriers in the teacher-student bond were likely

attributable to the core symptoms of ADHD, rather than the label itself. The influence of

inattention on the sources of self-efficacy was explored as part of the second objective of

this study, with results demonstrating that higher levels of inattention predicted lower

scores on the sources of self-efficacy measure, including verbal encouragement. In

addition, given weaker academic skills and reports of fewer mastery experiences in

adolescents with ADHD, it may be that there are simply fewer opportunities for teachers,

as well as parents and peers, to provide adolescents with ADHD with positive feedback

about their performance. However, that adolescents with ADHD are not receiving

comparable levels of encouragement as their peers raises additional concerns regarding

what information parents and teachers are attending to and how success is defined in the

classroom. That is, there may be fewer opportunities for encouragement if educators are

Page 104: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

96

focusing on outcomes, such as grades, rather than identifying adolescents with ADHD’s

unique strengths, small successes, and the effort they put forth in the process.

Nevertheless, the present study suggests that adolescents with ADHD likely do not feel

encouraged at school, and this is problematic given the importance of a positive and

supportive classroom environment to learning, motivation, and achievement (Bandura,

1997; Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Rogers et al., 2015).

Contrary to expectations, adolescents with ADHD reported having exposure to as

many efficacious models as their non-ADHD peers. This finding is inconsistent with

previous research conducted with adolescents with LDs, who rate themselves as having

fewer role models to identify with relative to typically developing adolescents (Hampton,

1998; Hampton & Mason, 2003). According to Bandura (1986, 1997), modelling, or

vicarious learning, occurs when the observer and the model share similar qualities.

Because most adolescents with ADHD are educated in mainstream classrooms among

their typically developing peers (e.g., Bussing et al., 2012; Hoagwood et al., 2000), it was

presumed that they would have less exposure to other adolescents with ADHD who have

similar characteristics and academic challenges. Even if adolescents with ADHD are

among peers with ADHD whom they identify with, these adolescents may be less likely

to model success relative to typically developing adolescents given that they may also

have neurocognitive and functional impairments. Having positive peer and adult role

models can have a bolstering effect on achievement (e.g., Bempechat, 1992; Brechwald

& Prinstein, 2011; Murphey & Arao, 2001; Parsons et al., 1982), and this may be

particularly true for individuals with ADHD (e.g., Carbone, 2001; Deault, 2010; Hurt,

Hoza, & Pelhem, 2007). Therefore, the finding that adolescents with ADHD perceive

Page 105: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

97

themselves as having positive role models for learning and achievement is a positive one,

insofar as they interpret and utilize this information to drive the efforts they put forth in

their learning. It is also easy to conceive, however, that being around others for whom

academic success comes more readily can have a detrimental affect on one’s self-efficacy

beliefs and motivation. Although it was not the focus of the present study to determine

the unique impact of vicarious learning on SESRL beliefs, this is a question for future

research.

Also contrary to expectations, adolescents with ADHD reported comparable

levels of emotional responsivity on academic tasks as their non-ADHD peers. This

finding is inconsistent with the LD literature, as adolescents with LDs report high levels

of anxiety on source measures relative to typically developing youth (Hampton, 1998;

Hampton & Mason, 2003). These results are also inconsistent with what would be

expected given the high rates of internalizing difficulties in children and adolescents with

ADHD, including depression, anxiety, and low self-esteem (see Barkley, 1998 for a

discussion; Herman et al., 2007; MacPhee & Andrews, 2006), as well as their challenges

with emotion regulation (Barkley & Fischer, 2010; Melnick & Hinshaw, 2000; Musser et

al., 2011; Wehmeier et al., 2010). It is unclear as to why adolescents with ADHD are

reporting comparable levels of anxiety on academic tasks as their non-ADHD peers given

these emotional vulnerabilities. One theory is that adolescents with ADHD in the present

study have lower rates of pre-existing internalizing difficulties than what is typically

found in the population, making them less prone to experiencing anxiety in the context of

academic task performance than adolescents with ADHD who have comorbid

internalizing symptoms. Internalizing symptoms were not measured in the present study;

Page 106: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

98

therefore, clarification regarding their influence on the sources of self-efficacy and, in

particular, physiological arousal, may be an interesting line of inquiry for future research.

It may also be that the comparable ratings of emotional reactivity in adolescents with and

without ADHD reflect the supports that may be put in place for those with ADHD. That

is, 81% of students with ADHD in the present study reported having access to special

education support, which may take the form of accommodations (e.g., more time, access

to the resource room) on tests and other high-stakes academic tasks. It is plausible that,

with these supports, adolescents with ADHD may feel more at ease when completing

demanding academic work, thereby buffering their emotional reactions. Alternatively,

adolescents with ADHD may be less aware of their emotional responsivity when asked

on a rating scale measure, whereas they may be more likely to report it in the moment

when the feelings are more obvious to them.

Gender differences emerged on the sources measure. Specifically, female

adolescents, regardless of whether or not they had ADHD, were more likely to perceive

themselves as having higher rates of mastery and vicarious experiences, and more

positive verbal encouragement from others relative to male adolescents. Males and

females did not differ in terms of self-reported emotional reactivity. Previous research

provides support for gender differences in the sources of self-efficacy. More specifically,

researchers have consistently found that females report higher rates of verbal

encouragement from others and more vicarious learning opportunities than males

(Anderson & Betz, 2001; Butz & Usher, 2015; Lent et al., 1996; Usher & Pajares, 2006;

Zeldin & Pajares, 2000), which was indeed observed in the present study. There is

evidence to suggest that females tend to demonstrate more appropriate classroom

Page 107: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

99

behaviour and are more self-regulated in their learning (Pajares, 2002; Pajares &

Valiante, 2001; Vecchio et al., 2007; Zimmerman & Martinez-Pons, 1990). Thus, they

may be more likely than males to receive positive feedback from their teachers and they

may have more exposure to similar peers who model their efforts and successes in the

classroom. It is also plausible that male and female adolescents do not objectively differ

in their exposure to verbal encouragement and efficacious models, but that they perceive

themselves to differ, suggesting that there may be gender differences regarding how

males and females attend to and reflect upon various aspects of their environment. In line

with this reasoning is evidence from previous research indicating that girls tend to notice

social information more readily than boys, and they are more likely to draw on this

information when forming their self-perceptions (Usher & Pajares, 2006; Zeldin &

Pajares, 2000). Evidence for gender differences in self-judgments may account for

differences between males and females in their self-reported mastery experiences. This is

because male and female adolescents did not differ on standardized achievement tests in

the present study—suggesting that their pre-existing academic skills are comparable—yet

female adolescents reported having more mastery experiences than males. Of note,

gender differences in mastery experiences have not been demonstrated in previous

research (Pajares & Schunk, 2001; Usher & Pajares, 2008), suggesting that a replication

of the present results are needed to draw firmer conclusions. Nonetheless, males seem to

be at an overall disadvantage in their access to three of the four sources of self-efficacy.

Despite this disadvantage, male adolescents did not report lower levels of SESRL beliefs,

although given the small sample size, this may be an issue of statistical power. Males

Page 108: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

100

may draw upon other sources of information not explored in the current study when

forming their self-efficacy beliefs.

Lastly, adolescents with ADHD also rated their engagement at school as

significantly lower than adolescents in the comparison group. That is, adolescents with

ADHD reported less enjoyment and interest in school, being less engaged in the

classroom, and less involved in school-based activities (e.g., school clubs, homework,

class discussions) compared their non-ADHD peers. Although limited, previous research

in clinical and community samples has linked inattention to lower levels of behavioural

engagement (e.g., Demaray & Jenkins, 2011; Martin, 2012; Vile Junod et al., 2006), and

the present results fit well with these findings. It is easy to speculate why adolescents

with ADHD may have lower levels of student engagement. Challenges with aspects of

behavioural engagement, including task initiation, persistence, and follow-through may

be reflective of underlying neurocognitive impairments commonly associated with

ADHD, namely deficits in EF and motivation. In addition, by virtue of demonstrating

symptoms of inattention, one would expect adolescents with ADHD to be less engaged

(i.e., more off-task) on academic tasks. However, given that the correlation between

inattention and engagement in the present study was only modest (r = -.34), this suggests

that the engagement measure is capturing more than just symptoms of inattention and off-

task behaviour. Indeed, items on the School Engagement Questionnaire not only assess

aspects of behavioural engagement (e.g., participation in class, work completion,

following the rules, involvement in school clubs), but also aspects of affective

engagement, including how much adolescents value learning, and their general interest

and enjoyment of school. Studies have found that adolescents with ADHD are more

Page 109: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

101

likely to report boredom, feeling less connected to their teachers, a negative attitude

towards school, and low levels enjoyment in learning relative to their peers (Carlson,

Booth et al. 2002; Castens & Overbey, 2009; DuPaul & Stoner, 2003; Pfiffner et al.,

1998; Rogers & Tannock, 2013). This low level of affective engagement is of particular

concern given the strong link between low student engagement and negative academic

outcomes, including lower grades and high school dropout (e.g., Archambault et al.,

2009; Bandura et al., 1996; Finn & Zimmer, 2012; Marks, 2000; Wang & Holcombe,

2010; Whitlock, 2006).

Relations Among Inattention, the Sources, SESRL, and Engagement

The second objective of this study was to explore the relations among inattention

symptom severity, the sources of self-efficacy, SESRL, and student engagement in the

full sample of adolescents with and without ADHD. This objective was achieved by

using structural equation modeling to test a hypothesized model based on Bandura’s self-

efficacy theory and research examining the relations among inattention symptoms and

self-efficacy beliefs (Luzzo et al., 1999; Major et al., 2013; Norvilitis et al., 2010;

Norwalk et al., 2009; Young et al., 2005). Of note, parent ratings of inattention were used

rather than self-reports to provide a more objective estimate of inattentive symptoms and

to reduce shared variance due to informant overlap between inattention and engagement.

Preliminary correlations revealed that adolescents with higher scores on the sources of

self-efficacy measure also had higher levels of SESRL beliefs, which in turn, were

related to higher levels of engagement. In addition, adolescents with higher levels of

parent-rated inattention perceived themselves to have less access to the sources of self-

efficacy, lower SESRL beliefs, and lower levels of engagement at school. Path analysis

Page 110: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

102

revealed that the sources of self-efficacy and SESRL mediated the relationship between

parent ratings of inattention and student engagement. The structural model fit the data

reasonably well, accounting for 44% of the variance in engagement. Potential

confounding variables, such as full-scale IQ and overall achievement were ruled out in a

preliminary regression analysis as unique predictors of student engagement. In future

studies, it would be helpful to examine how achievement contributes to and is a function

of self-efficacy beliefs and the sources of self-efficacy. A longitudinal perspective would

be most appropriate to best understand the developmental trajectory of SESRL beliefs

from middle school to high school and their relationship with achievement.

Adolescents who reported more access to the sources of self-efficacy also

reported higher levels of SESRL, which in turn, influenced student engagement. This

finding supports the core tenants of Bandura’s social cognitive theory (1986, 1997),

which states that self-efficacy beliefs play a mediating role between four key sources of

information and academic outcomes such as student engagement. Much of the previous

research examining Bandura’s social cognitive theory has examined the antecedents and

outcomes of self-efficacy separately (e.g., Anderson & Betz, 2001; Caraway et al., 2003;

Klassen, 2004; Lent et al., 1991; Lent et al., 1996; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2003; Lopez

et al., 1997; Usher & Pajares, 2006). By including both antecedents (sources of self-

efficacy) and outcomes (student engagement) in one study and employing structural

equation modelling, the present study unites these two lines of inquiry and provides a

closer approximation of Bandura’s theoretical model and the temporal relations among

these variables. Thus, this study adds to a small but growing research base that has

identified self-efficacy as a central mechanism between source variables and student

Page 111: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

103

learning and achievement (Bong, 2001; Fenollar et al., 2007; Liem et al., 2008; Pajares,

1996; Pajares & Graham, 1999; Pajares & Miller, 1994; Pajares & Valiante, 1997; Phan,

2010; Prat-Sala & Redford, 2010; Sins et al., 2008). The connection between SESRL

beliefs and higher levels of student engagement fits well with the existing literature

describing the characteristics of self-efficacious learners. That is, students who feel more

confident in their learning are likely to set challenging goals, monitor and evaluate their

progress, focus their attention, put forth a consistent effort, and employ a range of

learning strategies across academic domains (e.g., Bandura et al., 1996, 2001; Bandura et

al., 2003; Bong, 2001; Linnenbrink & Pintrich, 2003; Schunk, 1995; Zimmerman et al.,

1992). In other words, self-efficacious learners are also more engaged in the learning

processes.

The present study provides an extension of Bandura’s social cognitive theory by

examining the role of parent-rated inattention symptoms. This research also sheds light

on the mechanisms through which behavioral inattention exerts its influence on

engagement. That is, the direct relationship between inattention and student engagement

was rendered non-significant once the influence of the sources of self-efficacy and

SESRL were considered. Symptoms of inattention have been consistently identified as a

risk factor for negative academic outcomes relative to symptoms of

hyperactivity/impulsivity (Nigg et al., 2005; Todd et al., 2002). For example, researchers

have found that students with higher levels of inattention score lower on cognitive and

achievement tests, have worse grades, and have more frequent placement in special

education compared to students with higher levels of hyperactivity (e.g., Todd et al.,

2002). Researchers have also previously found a link between inattention and self-

Page 112: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

104

efficacy beliefs (Luzzo et al., 1999; Major et al., 2013; Norvilitis et al., 2010; Norwalk et

al., 2009; Young et al., 2005). Findings from the current study add to this literature by

demonstrating that adolescents with attention problems also have less access to the

sources of self-efficacy and feel less efficacious about their ability to overcome learning

challenges, both of which influence their engagement and interest in school. A number of

studies have demonstrated that the relationship between inattention and academic

outcomes is indirect through important “academic enablers” such as attitudes, beliefs, and

engagement (Demaray & Jenkins, 2011; DuPaul et al., 2004; Langberg et al., 2011;

Plamondon & Martinussen, 2015; Volpe et al., 2006). For instance, Rapport and

colleagues (1999) found that teacher-rated classroom performance, which included

aspects of behavioural engagement, significantly mediated the relationship between

inattention and academic achievement. Taken together, these results suggest that

inattention is a cognitive risk factor for a host of challenges in the classroom, and that

inattention may exert its influence on academic outcomes through multiple indirect

pathways, one of which may involve the sources of self-efficacy and SESRL beliefs. The

impact of cognitive vulnerabilities on the sources of self-efficacy, self-efficacy beliefs,

and academic outcomes is also reflected in the LD literature. Hampton and Mason (2003)

found that LD status had an indirect effect on academic outcomes (i.e., grades) through

the four sources of self-efficacy and academic self-efficacy beliefs. These findings

support the notion that cognitive impairments can impact the development of self-

efficacy beliefs by limiting one’s access to the sources of self-efficacy. Furthermore, the

relations among these variables are likely cyclical in nature. For instance, lower levels of

engagement may both result from and contribute to less access to the sources and

Page 113: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

105

SESRL, such that the less engaged students are in the classroom, the less likely they are

to master tasks, elicit verbal encouragement, or identify with positive role models.

As evidenced from the present study, adolescents with attention difficulties have

lower levels of SESRL due, in part, to less access to the sources of self-efficacy.

Although not directly examined, it may be that underlying deficits in EF also contribute

to the relationship between inattention and SESRL. There is indeed evidence linking

inattention to deficits in EF, as adolescents with more attention problems may have

particular difficulties with tasks such as goal setting, planning ahead, self-control, and

focusing attention (e.g., Stavro et al., 2007; Wasserstein, 2005) than adolescents with

lower levels of attention difficulties. In addition, the high school learning environment is

far more demanding and less structured than in elementary school and students are

expected to be more self-directed in their learning (Pajares & Urdan, 2006). Students who

are more inattentive and have EF deficits may be particularly disadvantaged in managing

these diverse requirements and may consequently feel less confident in their self-

regulatory skills than students with inattention symptoms alone. Therefore, EF skills may

co-vary with symptoms of inattention, both of which exert their influence on the sources

of self-efficacy and subsequent SESRL beliefs.

Taken together, the above research findings make a novel contribution to our

understanding of Bandura’s social cognitive theory and to the motivational dysfunction in

ADHD. Motivation is a complex and multi-faceted construct that is comprised of

cognitive (e.g., self-perceptions, beliefs), affective (e.g., interest), and behavioural (e.g.,

engagement, effort and persistence) components (Schunk et al., 2008; Schunk & Mullen,

2012). Both self-efficacy beliefs and student engagement are valuable motivational

Page 114: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

106

variables to examine when exploring student behaviours that are linked to improved

educational functioning (Archambault et al., 2009; Bandura et al., 1996; Greenwood,

1996; Pajares & Urdan, 2006; Pajares, 1996; Whitlock, 2006). Existing theories have

attributed ADHD symptomatology and behaviours to an underlying motivational style

(delay aversion) associated with fundamental alterations in reward mechanisms,

including a motivation to escape or avoid delay (Sonuga-Barke, 1994, 1998, 2003;

Swanson et al., 1998). Objectively, motivational impairments in children and adolescents

with ADHD are reflected in their lower rates of persistence and effort on academic tasks,

higher levels of frustration when faced with a challenge, and a greater preference for easy

work compared to their non-ADHD peers (Barkley, 1997; Carlson et al., 2002; Hoza et

al., 2001). The present study suggests that low self-efficacy beliefs may, in part,

contribute to these objective deficits in motivation in ADHD. In typically developing

children and adolescents, those who perceive themselves as confident in their abilities are

indeed more likely to choose challenging tasks, put forth a consistent effort, effectively

use self-regulation strategies, and modulate their emotional responses when faced with

difficulty (Bandura, 1997; Bandura et al., 2001; Bandura et al., 1999; Muris, 2002;

Pajares, 1996; Zimmerman, 2000). Furthermore, low motivation is more often associated

with inattention than hyperactivity/impulsivity (Langberg et al., 2010; Power et al., 2006;

Sasser et al., 2015). Therefore, by examining self-efficacy and engagement in adolescents

with ADHD while also exploring how symptoms of inattention relate to the sources,

SESRL, and engagement, this dissertation has expanded our knowledge of the factors that

characterize low motivation in ADHD.

Page 115: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

107

Limitations and Future Directions

Findings from the current study must be considered in light of several limitations.

First, parent reports of a previous ADHD diagnosis with current levels of

symptomatology confirmed via the use of ADHD parent ratings scales was used to

identify the sample of youth with ADHD. Although previous psychological and/or

psychiatric assessments were requested to confirm ADHD diagnoses, very few

participants provided this information. In future studies, the validity of ADHD diagnoses

can be improved by including a full ADHD diagnostic inventory as part of the

recruitment process.

Second, the sources of self-efficacy were measured using youths’ self-reports on a

Likert-type scale. Bandura (1997, 2000) argued in his self-efficacy theory that the sources

are complex and multifaceted variables, suggesting that an in-depth understanding of

each source of self-efficacy may require additional data sources (Phan, 2012).

Unfortunately, most studies examining the sources of self-efficacy have also relied on

self-report measures (e.g., Matsui et al., 1990; Phan, 2012), which highlights the need for

further advancement in this area. Researchers may consider qualitative methods, such as

in-depth interviews, to gather additional information regarding adolescents’ individual

experiences with the sources that may not be captured using Likert-type instruments,

including the unique impact of each source as well as how they are integrated and

weighed (Phan, 2012).

Third, this study is limited by a small sample size, particularly of female

adolescents with ADHD. Although this is in part a reflection of gender ratios in ADHD,

future studies should strive to examine female adolescents with ADHD, as they are often

Page 116: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

108

understudied and there is a need to understand the potentially unique vulnerabilities of

this group. Previous research by Major et al. (2013) suggested that there may be gender

differences in the SESRL beliefs of adolescents with ADHD. Although the present study

did not replicate this finding, both of these studies were limited by a small sample size of

females, making it challenging to draw any firm conclusions. Furthermore, the relatively

small overall sample size also limited the number of factors that could be included into

the structural model. Although IQ and achievement were ruled out via a regression

analysis as significant predictors of school engagement, these and other confounding

variables (e.g., anxiety diagnosis) are important to consider in future research. In the

current study, the examination of inattention symptoms and the sources of self-efficacy

sheds light on the potential factors that may account for the lower SESRL beliefs of

adolescents with ADHD (e.g., symptoms of inattention, mastery experiences, social

persuasion), However, given that the structural model only accounted for 44% of the

variance in engagement, there may be other variables that require investigation in order to

further our understanding of why adolescents with ADHD view themselves as less

capable and less engaged than their non-ADHD peers. It is widely accepted that ADHD

is a heterogeneous disorder with high rates of comorbidity and associated difficulties

(American Psychiatric Association 2000; Barkley 1990). Research has demonstrated that

the presence of co-occuring difficulties, such as low mood or aggression, can impact the

self-perceptions of students with ADHD (see Owens et al., 2007 for a review). For

instance, boys with ADHD with high levels of aggression tend to provide overly positive

estimates of their abilities relative to ADHD boys with low levels of aggression,

particularly in the social and behavioural domains (Hoza et al., 2002). As previously

Page 117: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

109

noted, both self-efficacy and school engagement may also be impacted by adolescents’

EF skills, such that adolescents with EF impairments may have less access to the sources

of self-efficacy, which may lead to lower SESRL beliefs and disengagement at school.

Given evidence for a PIB in ADHD (Hoza et al., 2010; McQuade et al., 2011), future

research may benefit from including objective measures of self-regulation and/or EF, not

only to examine their influence on self-efficacy and engagement, but also to assess the

accuracy with which adolescents rate their SESRL beliefs. Other potential antecedents

that may be of value to explore include how much adolescents value and are interested in

the task, the relevance of the task for their future goals, or their levels of self-esteem.

There is evidence, for example, that high self-esteem and self-concept exert a positive

influence on self-efficacy (Ahmed & Bruinsma, 2006; Dodgson & Wood, 1998; Lane,

Jones, & Stevens, 2002; Phan, 2010). Given that lower levels of domain-specific self-

concepts have been found in children with ADHD (e.g., Anderson et al., 1989; Hinshaw

et al., 2006; Owens et al., 2007; Rucklidge & Tannock, 2001), it may be of value to

examine the relationship between these two self-constructs.

Furthermore, given the complexities of the classroom environment, there are no

doubt several other relevant psychosocial or contextual antecedents not explored in the

present study that may exert their influence on SESRL beliefs. For instance, there is

evidence to suggest that youths’ perceptions of their teacher’s attitude toward students

and their learning (e.g., mastery oriented; caring and warmth towards students)

contributes to the prediction of self-efficacy beliefs (Fast et al., 2010). Other contextual

factors, such as access to academic supports, peer group influences, parent involvement,

or broader social-cultural factors may also be valuable to investigate. How does, for

Page 118: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

110

example, the level of parent involvement in their children’s academics shape their

children’s self-efficacy beliefs? Do adolescents who have access to special education

services and other learning supports fare better than those who have little or no access to

such resources, and are some of these supports more influential than others in fostering

self-efficacy? To what extent do peer influences, such as the degree to which peers value

school, play a role? Adolescents who are at risk for learning challenges, such as those

with ADHD, may be particularly susceptible to the impact of these environmental

influences. Furthermore, there is evidence for cross-cultural differences in ratings of

general self-efficacy (e.g., Scholz, Dona, Sud, & Schwarzer, 2002; Schwarzer, Bäßler,

Kwiatek, Schröder, & Zhang, 1997), indicating that the role of language and cultural

influences would be valuable to explore in future research. The present study provided a

preliminary investigation of the factors that contribute to low SESRL and student

engagement in ADHD. With a larger sample size, researchers may have the advantage of

testing Bandura’s model in each of the ADHD and comparison groups and/or across

males and females. This would allow for the addition of other relevant variables noted

above as well as permit researchers to compare the model fit across groups.

Fourth, due to the small sample size limiting the number of parameter estimates

that could be entered into the structural model, the sources were represented as a single

latent variable rather than examining the independent effects of each source. Previous

research has found that there are differences in the relative impact of the sources of self-

efficacy on self-efficacy beliefs. Specifically, mastery experiences tend to consistently

emerge as the most powerful predictor of self-efficacy beliefs, whereas the other sources

may only demonstrate marginal effects on self-efficacy (Hampton, 1998; Lopez & Lent,

Page 119: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

111

1992; Pajares et al., 2007; Matsui, Matsui, & Ohnishi, 1990). For example, in a recent

study, Phan (2012) used structural equation modeling and found that only mastery

experiences and vicarious experiences influenced self-efficacy beliefs in typically

developing children. Adolescents with ADHD in the present study differed from their

non-ADHD peers in reported mastery experiences and verbal encouragement; however,

they reported similar levels of vicarious experiences and physiological arousal as

comparison adolescents. Thus, some sources may exert a greater influence on self-

efficacy beliefs and these relationships may differ as a function of group status (ADHD

versus non-ADHD). Future research in this domain may offer a more fruitful examination

of Bandura’s self-efficacy model by separating each of the four sources of self-efficacy

and assessing their relative impact on SESRL in each the ADHD and comparison groups.

Fifth, although the present study used SEM to provide stronger statistical

grounding regarding the relations among the variables of interest, it is still limited by a

cross-sectional design. Combining SEM with a longitudinal approach would allow

researchers to make stronger conclusions regarding potential cause-and-effect

relationships among inattention, the sources, SESRL, and engagement. Additionally,

there is evidence for a decrease in the predictive power of the sources on self-efficacy

with the passing of time (Phan, 2012), as well as temporal changes in the impact of self-

efficacy beliefs on future academic performance and achievement-related outcomes (e.g.,

Pajares & Kranzler, 1995; Pajares & Valiante, 1999; Skaalvik & Rankin, 1998). Thus, a

longitudinal design would also allow for an investigation of how the sources and SESRL

function and change over time.

Page 120: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

112

Sixth, the present study relied on student engagement as an outcome variable due

to its role in predicting high school dropout and achievement. However, future research,

particularly longitudinal studies, may consider directly measuring school dropout and

grades and determining the extent to which SESRL beliefs predict these outcomes. This

is especially important given that adolescents with ADHD are at a considerable risk for

low achievement and dropout (Barbaresi et al., 2007; Bauermeister et al., 2007; Ek et al.,

2011; Frazier et al., 2007; Lahey et al., 2004; Rogers et al., 2011), putting their future

educational and vocational opportunities at risk. Thus, understanding the relative impact

of SESRL on achievement and school dropout in adolescents with ADHD can help

inform existing programs designed to support school retention in this vulnerable

population of youth.

Implications for Clinicians and Educators

Findings from the present study have several important implications for

professionals who work with adolescents with ADHD. In clinical and educational

settings, there tends to be a focus on pre-existing ability and skill when seeking to

understand and evaluate the academic functioning of adolescents with ADHD. However,

this study suggests that clinicians and educators should also attend to adolescents’

confidence levels, as this information may help us develop a greater understanding of the

factors that may be contributing to the low classroom engagement, poor self-regulation,

and negative academic outcomes faced by adolescents with attention difficulties and a

clinical diagnosis of ADHD. Although no amount of confidence can lead to success when

requisite skills and knowledge are absent, self-efficacy beliefs are powerful in that they

dictate how individuals use the knowledge and skills that they possess—there may be

Page 121: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

113

little incentive for adolescents to persist in the face of difficulty unless they believe that

their actions can produce a desired outcome (Pajares, 1996; Bandura 1986, 1997;

Zimmerman, 2000). This may be especially true for adolescents with ADHD whose

motivational style is characterized by an altered sensitivity to reward, such that they may

have difficulties sustaining motivation if the task is not immediately gratifying (Carlson

& Tamm, 2000; Kuntsi et al., 2001; Luman et al., 2005; Rosch & Hawk, 2013). It is

important that clinicians and educators are mindful of this connection, given evidence

that self-efficacy beliefs can predict performance beyond knowledge and skill (e.g.,

Zuffianὸ et al., 2013).

Clinicians or educators can gather information about adolescents’ self-efficacy

beliefs in several possible ways. One approach would involve using formal measures,

such as what was utilized in the present study. For instance, the SELF has an abbreviated

version that may be effective for those seeking a more formal assessment of SESRL. The

SELF-A is comprised of 19 items taken from the original form and assesses adolescents’

judgments about their ability to use self-regulated learning skills to overcome challenges

related to note-taking, studying, test-taking, and reading. The SELF-A has demonstrated

good psychometric properties and has the practical advantage of requiring approximately

one-third of the time necessary for completion (Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 2007).

Alternatively, for a less formal assessment, the clinician can simply ask adolescents with

ADHD how confident they are in their ability to complete a particular task involving self-

regulation. Over time, adults can begin to gauge how self-efficacious adolescents with

ADHD perceive themselves to be. By gathering this information directly from

adolescents themselves, important insights about their beliefs can be acquired in a way

Page 122: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

114

that may not be easily attained via observation or through other informants (e.g., parent-

ratings).

The study results also have important implications for interventions designed to

enhance the academic functioning of adolescents with ADHD. More specifically, the

findings suggest that existing interventions that focus on building academic and/or self-

regulation skills in ADHD may benefit from incorporating a component that addresses

adolescents’ self-efficacy beliefs. Moreover, given that the present study demonstrated a

positive relationship between the sources of self-efficacy and SESRL, this study sheds

light on potential targets for enhancing SESRL beliefs in adolescents with attention

difficulties. In adolescents with a clinical diagnosis of ADHD, educators and clinicians

may want to pay particular attention to increasing mastery experiences and verbal

persuasion given the lower ratings by adolescents with ADHD in these domains.

Fostering mastery experiences may involve direct teaching of pre-requisite skills needed

to complete academic and self-regulatory tasks (e.g., Evans, Langberg, Egan, & Molitor,

2014). It may also involve ensuring that adolescents with ADHD are provided with the

appropriate level of scaffolding and accommodations to support their learning. For

instance, there is evidence to suggest that explicit training in the use of strategies that

support goal-setting, project planning, note-taking, time management, and other learning

skills may increase adolescents with ADHD’s successful completion of academic tasks

(e.g., Jacobson & Reid, 2012; Mason & Shriner, 2008) and this may enhance their

SESRL beliefs and subsequent classroom engagement. There is also a need to consider

how we define mastery experiences or a student’s ‘success’ with a task. Adolescents with

ADHD have more difficulty learning and they have to work harder in order to achieve

Page 123: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

115

academic success that is comparable to their peers. Thus, in devising programs that

support the academic development of adolescents with ADHD, it is important to consider

personal success in learning. Educators, clinicians, and parents should cultivate an

attitude that recognizes and appreciates individual growth and supports adolescents in

valuing their own learning experiences and making the connection between their efforts

and improvements in their learning. Research has indicated that students who experience

academic success as a result of their efforts and strategy use, regardless of having

learning challenges, are more likely to put forth a consistent effort and strive for success

(Meltzer, Katzir, Miller, Reddy, & Roditi, 2004). These are the aspects of student

learning and achievement that should be acknowledged when providing adolescents with

verbal persuasion. Thus, what grade students’ receive should not be of primary concern;

rather, educators and parents alike should attend to students’ efforts and the strategies

they used in the processes. Adults should support adolescents in self-reflecting upon their

efforts and use of self-regulated learning strategies and in applying this information

constructively to facilitate future learning and strengthen their self-efficacy beliefs. In

addition, because adolescents with ADHD have more challenges relative to their peers

with academic tasks and self-regulation, they may require more frequent and salient

verbal encouragement. This may be especially true given evidence for altered reward

sensitivity in ADHD (Douglas, 1989; Haenlein & Caul, 1987; Sergeant et al., 1999;

Sonuga-Barke, 2002). Thus, verbal feedback may need to be more explicit and immediate

in order for adolescents with ADHD to incorporate and interpret this information in a

manner than can be used to foster their self-efficacy beliefs.

Page 124: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

116

Boosting self-efficacy beliefs may also involve supporting adolescents in

understanding the challenges that accompany their disability and their unique strengths

and difficulties. Programs aimed at increasing self-awareness and self-help skills are

important so that individuals can become aware of the types of accommodations and

supports available to them. It is also important that adolescents with ADHD feel

empowered to strive in the classroom despite their learning and attention difficulties.

Thus, rather than focusing on deficits or areas of difficulty, educators and clinicians

should focus on enhancing individual growth, while emphasizing adolescents’

competencies, interests, and adaptive potential. School psychologists can offer valuable

support in helping adolescents with ADHD develop a greater understanding of their

disorder, including how they can identify their own strengths to compensate for areas of

difficulty.

As results from this study demonstrate, the effect of symptoms of inattention on

SESRL beliefs and engagement at school was indirect through the influence of the

sources of self-efficacy. This finding suggests that helping inattentive students develop

skills and strategies to improve their focus may indirectly boost their self-efficacy beliefs

and engagement. For instance, there is evidence to suggest that children with ADHD

show improved engagement and performance on a task when the characteristics of the

task are modified to be more novel or interesting, thereby improving attention (Beike &

Zentall, 2012). Given that both SESRL and student engagement are predictive of

important outcomes such as grades and school retention, there is substantial value for the

ongoing study of these motivational variables in ADHD.

Page 125: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

117

Conclusion

This research study has made a novel contribution to our understanding of

motivation in ADHD and provides researchers and clinicians with greater awareness of

the way in which adolescents with ADHD perceive themselves and their school

experiences. Although this investigation is not without its limitations, it has provided

preliminary evidence that adolescents with ADHD are less confident than their peers in

their ability to self-regulate their learning and that they perceive themselves as having

less success at school, less positive encouragement from others, and lower levels of

interest and engagement in school. This research has also highlighted the indirect

relationship between inattention and engagement through the sources of self-efficacy and

SESRL beliefs. Thus, adolescence appears to be a time when individuals with ADHD do

not hold positive perceptions of their capabilities related to self-regulation, and future

research should continue to explore the antecedents of self-efficacy as well as the

influence of negative self-perceptions on outcomes such as academic performance,

school retention, and vocational opportunities. Given that a strong sense of self-efficacy

supports student learning and achievement, academic interventions for adolescents with

ADHD should not only focus on building skills, but should also address the attitudes and

beliefs that adolescents with ADHD hold about themselves and their abilities.

Page 126: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

118

References

Abikoff, H.B., Jensen, P.S., Arnold, L.E., et al. (2002). Observed classroom behavior of

children with ADHD: relationship to gender and comorbidity. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 4, 349–359.

Alexander, K. L., Entwisle, D. R., & Horsey, C. S. (1997). From first grade forward:

Early foundations of high school dropout. Sociology of education, 87-107.

Almeida, L. G., Ricardo-Garcell, J., Prado, H., Barajas, L., Fernández-Bouzas, A., Ávila,

D., & Martínez, R. B. (2010). Reduced right frontal cortical thickness in children,

adolescents and adults with ADHD and its correlation to clinical variables: a

cross-sectional study. Journal of psychiatric research, 44(16), 1214-1223.

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. (2007). Practice parameter for the

assessment and treatment of children and adolescents with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 894–921.

American Academy of Pediatrics. (2001). Clinical practice guideline: Treatment of the

school-aged child with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Pediatrics, 108,

1033–1044. doi:10.1542/peds.108.4.1033

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders. (Fourth Edition-Text Revision). Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric

Association.

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders, 5th Edition. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association.

Page 127: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

119

Anderman, E. M., & Maehr, M. L. (1994). Motivation and schooling in the middle grades.

Review of Educational Research, 64, 287-309. doi:10.2307/1170696

Anderson, A. R., Christenson, S. L., Sinclair, M. F., & Lehr, C. A. (2004). Check &

Connect: The importance of relationships for promoting engagement with school.

Journal of School Psychology, 42(2), 95-113.

Anderson, S. L., & Betz, N. E. (2001). Sources of social self-efficacy expectations: Their

measurement and relation to career development. Journal of Vocational Behavior,

58(1), 98-117.

Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., & Furlong, M. J. (2008). Student engagement with

school: Critical conceptual and methodological issues of the construct. Psychology

in the Schools, 45(5), 369-386

Appleton, J. J., Christenson, S. L., Kim, D., & Reschly, A. L. (2006). Measuring cognitive

and psychological engagement: Validation of the Student Engagement Instrument.

Journal of School Psychology, 44(5), 427-445.

Archambault, I., & Vandenbossche-Makombo, J. (2014). Validation de l’échelle des

dimensions de l’engagement scolaire (ÉDES) chez les élèves du primaire.

Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du

comportement, 46(2), 275.

Archambault, I., Janosz, M., Morizot, J., & Pagani, L. (2009). Adolescent behavioral,

affective, and cognitive engagement in school: Relationship to dropout. Journal of

School Health, 79(9), 408-415.

Babinski, D. E., Pelham Jr, W. E., Molina, B. S., Gnagy, E. M., Waschbusch, D. A., Yu,

J., ... & Karch, K. M. (2011). Late adolescent and young adult outcomes of girls

Page 128: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

120

diagnosed with ADHD in childhood: An exploratory investigation. Journal of

Attention Disorders, 15, 204-214.

Baeyens, D., Roeyers, H., & Walle, J.V. (2006) Subtypes of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): Distinct or related disorders across

measurement levels? Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 36, 403–417.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.

New York: Prentice-Hall, Inc.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Macmillan.

Bandura, A. (2000). Self-efficacy. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological

Association.

Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Multifaceted

impact of self-efficacy beliefs on academic functioning. Child development, 1206-

1222.

Bandura, A., Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Gerbino, M., & Pastorelli, C. (2003). Role

of affective self‐regulatory efficacy in diverse spheres of psychosocial

functioning. Child development, 74(3), 769-782.

Banerjee, T. D., Middleton, F., & Faraone, S. V. (2007). Environmental risk factors for

attention‐deficit hyperactivity disorder. Acta Paediatrica, 96(9), 1269-1274.

Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., & Jacobsen, S. J. (2007).

Long-term school outcomes for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder: a population-based perspective. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral

Pediatrics, 28(4), 265-273.

Page 129: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

121

Barkley, R.A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:

constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 65–94

Barkley, R.A. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis

and Treatment. New York: Guilford Publications.

Barkley, R. A. (2012). Executive Functioning and Self-Regulation: Extended Phenotype,

Synthesis, and Clinical Implications. New York: Guilford Publications.

Barkley, R. A. (2014). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis

and treatment. New York: Guilford Publications.

Barkley, R. A., & Fischer, M. (2010). The unique contribution of emotional

impulsiveness to impairment in major life activities in hyperactive children as

adults. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

49(5), 503-513.

Barkley, R. A., & Peters, H. (2012). The earliest reference to ADHD in the medical

literature? Melchior Adam Weikard’s description in 1775 of “attention

deficit”(Mangel der Aufmerksamkeit, Attentio Volubilis). Journal of attention

disorders, 1087054711432309.

Barkley, R. A., Fischer, M., Edelbrock, C. S., & Smallish, L. (1990). The adolescent

outcome of hyperactive children diagnosed by research criteria: I. An 8-year

prospective follow-up study. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 29(4), 546-557.

Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K. R., & Bush, T. (2001). Time perception and reproduction in

young adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology,

15(3), 351.

Page 130: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

122

Bempechat, J. (1992). The role of parent involvement in children’s academic

achievement. The School Community Journal, 2(2), 31-41.

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological

bulletin, 107(2), 238.

Biederman, J., Faraone, S., Milberger, S., Curtis, S., Chen, L., Marrs, A., ... & Spencer,

T. (1996). Predictors of persistence and remission of ADHD into adolescence:

results from a four-year prospective follow-up study. Journal of the American

Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(3), 343-351.

Biederman, J., Kwon, A., Aleardi, M., Chouinard, V. A., Marino, T., Cole, H., ... &

Faraone, S. V. (2005). Absence of gender effects on attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder: findings in nonreferred subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry,

162(6), 1083-1089.

Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and

symptom type. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 816-818.

doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816

Biederman, J., Mick, E., & Faraone, S. V. (2000). Age-dependent decline of symptoms of

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Impact of remission definition and

symptom type. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 816-818.

doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.5.816

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferrero, F.,

... & Faraone, S. V. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-

Page 131: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

123

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal

of consulting and clinical psychology, 72(5), 757.

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Mick, E., Spencer, T., Wilens, T. E., Klein, K. L., ... &

Faraone, S. V. (2006). Psychopathology in females with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a controlled, five-year prospective study. Biological

psychiatry, 60(10), 1098-1105.

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Spencer, T., Wilens, T. E., MacPherson, H. A., &

Faraone, S. V. (2008). Stimulant therapy and risk for subsequent substance use

disorders in male adults with ADHD: a naturalistic controlled 10-year follow-up

study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(5), 597-603.

Biederman, J., Newcorn, J., & Sprich, S. (1991). Comorbidity of attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry, 148(5), 564-577.

Biederman, J., Petty, C. R., Evans, M., Small, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2010). How persistent

is ADHD? A controlled 10-year follow-up study of boys with ADHD. Psychiatry

Research, 177, 299-304. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.12.010

Bong, M. (2001). Between-and within-domain relations of academic motivation among

middle and high school students: Self-efficacy, task value, and achievement goals.

Journal of educational psychology, 93(1), 23.

Bouffard-Bouchard, T., Parent, S., & Larivee, S. (1991). Influence of self-efficacy on

self-regulation and performance among junior and senior high-school age

students. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 14(2), 153-164.

Brault, M. C., & Lacourse, É. (2012). Prevalence of prescribed attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder medications and diagnosis among Canadian preschoolers

Page 132: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

124

and school-age children: 1994-2007. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 57(2), 93-

101.

Brechwald, W. A., & Prinstein, M. J. (2011). Beyond homophily: A decade of advances

in understanding peer influence processes. Journal of Research on Adolescence,

21(1), 166-179.

Brim, S. A., & Whitaker, D. P. (2000). Motivation and students with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Preventing School Failure: Alternative Education for

Children and Youth, 44(2), 57-60.

Brook, U., & Boaz, M. (2005). Attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and

learning disabilities (LD): adolescents perspective. Patient Education and

Counseling, 58(2), 187-191.

Bussing, R., Koro-Ljungberg, M., Noguchi, K., Mason, D., Mayerson, G., & Garvan, C.

W. (2012). Willingness to use ADHD treatments: a mixed methods study of

perceptions by adolescents, parents, health professionals and teachers. Social

science & medicine, 74(1), 92-100.

Bussing, R., Mason, D. M., Bell, L., Porter, P., & Garvan, C. (2010). Adolescent

outcomes of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a diverse

community sample. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 49(6), 595-605.

Butz, A. R., & Usher, E. L. (2015). Salient sources of early adolescents' self-efficacy in

two domains. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 42, 49-61.

Page 133: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

125

Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS, EQS, and LISREL:

Comparative approaches to testing for the factorial validity of a measuring

instrument. International journal of testing, 1(1), 55-86.

Caprara, G. V., Fida, R., Vecchione, M., Del Bove, G., Vecchio, G. M., Barbaranelli, C.,

& Bandura, A. (2008). Longitudinal analysis of the role of perceived self-efficacy

for self-regulated learning in academic continuance and achievement. Journal of

Educational

Caraway, K., Tucker, C. M., Reinke, W. M., & Hall, C. (2003). Self‐efficacy, goal

orientation, and fear of failure as predictors of school engagement in high school

students. Psychology in the Schools, 40(4), 417-427.

Carbone, E. (2001). Arranging the classroom with an eye (and ear) to students with

ADHD. Teaching Exceptional Children, 34(2), 72.

Carlson, C. L., & Tamm, L. (2000). Responsiveness of children with attention deficit–

hyperactivity disorder to reward and response cost: Differential impact on

performance and motivation. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology,

68(1), 73.

Carlson, C. L., Booth, J. E., Shin, M., & Canu, W. H. (2002). Parent-, teacher-, and self-

rated motivational styles in ADHD subtypes. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35,

104-113. doi:10.1177/002221940203500202

Carlson, C. L., Tamm, L., & Gaub, M. (1997). Gender differences in children with

ADHD, ODD, and co-occurring ADHD/ODD identified in a school population.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(12),

1706-1714.

Page 134: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

126

Castellanos, F. X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(8), 617-

628.

Castellanos, F. X., Lee, P. P., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N. O., Greenstein, D. K., Clasen, L. S.,

... & Zijdenbos, A. (2002). Developmental trajectories of brain volume

abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Jama, 288(14), 1740-1748.

Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Milham, M. P., & Tannock, R. (2006).

Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in

cognitive sciences, 10(3), 117-123.

Castens, A. R., & Overbey, G. A. (2009). ADHD, Boredom, Sleep Disturbance, Self-

Esteem, and Academic Achievement in College Students. Psi Chi Journal of

Undergraduate Research, 14(2).

Chen, J. A., & Usher, E. L. (2013). Profiles of the sources of science self-efficacy.

Learning and Individual Differences, 24, 11-21.

Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., & Wylie, C. (Eds.). (2012). Handbook of research on

student engagement. Springer Science & Business Media.

Christenson, S. L., Reschly, A. L., Appleton, J. J., Berman, S., Spanjers, D., & Varro, P.

(2008). Best practices in fostering student engagement. Best practices in school

psychology, 5, 1099-1120.

Clarfield, J., & Stoner, G. (2005). The effects of computerized reading instruction on the

academic performance of students identified with ADHD. School Psychology

Review, 34(2), 246.

Page 135: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

127

Clarke, S., Heussler, H., & Kohn, M. R. (2005). Attention deficit disorder: not just for

children. Internal medicine journal, 35(12), 721-725.

Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, autonomy, and relatedness: A

motivational analysis of self-system processes.

Conners, C. K. (2008). Conners 3rd Edition Manual. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems

Inc.

Conners, C. K., Sitarenios, G., Parker, J. D. A., & Epstein, J. N. (1998a). The revised

conners' parent rating scale (CPRS-R): Factor structure, reliability, and criterion

validity. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An Official Publication of the

International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 26,

257-268. doi:10.1023/A:1022602400621

Corno, L., & Mandinach, E. B. (1983). The role of cognitive engagement in classroom

learning and motivation. Educational psychologist, 18(2), 88-108.

Cortese, S., Kelly, C., Chabernaud, C., Proal, E., Di Martino, A., Milham, M. P., &

Castellanos, F. X. (2012). Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-

analysis of 55 fMRI studies. American Journal of Psychiatry, 69, 1038- 055

Crone, E. A., Vendel, I., & van der Molen, M. W. (2003). Decision-making in

disinhibited adolescents and adults: insensitivity to future consequences or driven

by immediate reward?. Personality and Individual Differences, 35(7), 1625-1641.

Cubillo, A., Halari, R., Smith, A., Taylor, E., & Rubia, K. (2012). A review of fronto-

striatal and fronto-cortical brain abnormalities in children and adults with

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and new evidence for

Page 136: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

128

dysfunction in adults with ADHD during motivation and attention. Cortex, 48(2),

194-215.

Da Fonseca, D., Seguier, V., Santos, A., Poinso, F., & Deruelle, C. (2009). Emotion

understanding in children with ADHD. Child psychiatry and human development,

40(1), 111-121.

Daley, D., & Birchwood, J. (2010). ADHD and academic performance: why does ADHD

impact on academic performance and what can be done to support ADHD

children in the classroom?. Child: care, health and development, 36(4), 455-464.

De Los Reyes, A., & Kazdin, A. E. (2005). Informant discrepancies in the assessment of

childhood psychopathology: A critical review, theoretical framework, and

recommendations for further study. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 483–509.

doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483

Deault, L. C. (2010). A systematic review of parenting in relation to the development of

comorbidities and functional impairments in children with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Psychiatry & Human Development,

41(2), 168-192.

Demaray, M. K., & Jenkins, L. N. (2011). Relations among academic enablers and

academic achievement in children with and without high levels of parent‐rated

symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity. Psychology in the

Schools, 48(6), 573-586.

DiPerna, J. C., & Elliott, S. N. (2002). Promoting academic enablers to improve student

achievenment: An introdution to the mini-series. School Psychology Review,

31(3), 293.

Page 137: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

129

Dovis, S., Van der Oord, S., Wiers, R. W., & Prins, P. J. (2012). Can motivation

normalize working memory and task persistence in children with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder? The effects of money and computer-gaming.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(5), 669-681.

Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P.,

... & Sexton, H. (2007). School readiness and later achievement. Developmental

psychology, 43(6), 1428.

Dunn, P.B., & Shapiro, S.K. (1999). Gender differences in the achievement goal

orientations of ADHD children. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 23, 327-344.

doi:10.1023/A:1018747716137

DuPaul, G. J. (2007). School-based interventions for students with attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder: Current status and future directions. School Psychology

Review, 36(2), 183.

DuPaul, G. J., Volpe, R. J., Jitendra, A. K., Lutz, J. G., Lorah, K. S., & Gruber, R.

(2004). Elementary school students with AD/HD: Predictors of academic

achievement. Journal of School Psychology, 42(4), 285-301.

Durston, S., Pol, H. E. H., Schnack, H. G., Buitelaar, J. K., Steenhuis, M. P., Minderaa,

R. B., & Kahn, R. S. (2004). Magnetic resonance imaging of boys with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(3), 332-340.

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon,

& N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, 5th ed.: Vol 3. social,

Page 138: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

130

emotional, and personality development. (pp. 1017-1095). Hoboken, NJ, US:

John Wiley & Sons Inc.

Edbom, T., Granlund, M., Lichtenstein, P., & Larsson, J. (2008). ADHD symptoms related

to profiles of self-esteem in a longitudinal study of twins: A person-oriented

approach. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Nursing, 21, 228-237.

Retrieved from www.csa.com

Ek, U., Westerlund, J., Holmberg, K., & Fernell, E. (2011). Academic performance of

adolescents with ADHD and other behavioural and learning problems—a

population‐based longitudinal study. Acta Paediatrica, 100(3), 402-406.

Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full information

maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation models.

Structural Equation Modeling, 8(3), 430-457.

Epstein, J. N., & Loren, R. E. (2013). Changes in the definition of ADHD in DSM-5:

subtle but important. Neuropsychiatry, 3(5), 455-458.

Evans, S. W., Langberg, J. M., Egan, T., & Molitor, S. J. (2014). Middle School–based

and High School–based Interventions for Adolescents with ADHD. Child and

adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America, 23(4), 699-715.

Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., & Mick, E. (2006). The age-dependent decline of attention

deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies. Psychological

medicine, 36(02), 159-165.

Faraone, S. V., Biederman, J., Spencer, T., Wilens, T., Seidman, L. J., Mick, E., & Doyle,

A. E. (2000). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: an overview.

Biological psychiatry, 48(1), 9-20.

Page 139: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

131

Faraone, S.V., Perlis, R.H., Doyle, A.E., Smoller, J.W., Goral- nick, J.J., Holmgren,

M.A., ... & Sklar, P. (2005). Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Biologi- cal Psychiatry, 57, 1313–1323.

Featherman, D. L., Spenner, K. I., & Tsunematsu, N. (1988). Class and the socialization of

children: Constancy, change, or irrelevance? In E. M. Hetherington, R. M. Lerner

& M. Perlmutter (Eds.), Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Retrieved from www.csa.com

Fenollar, P., Román, S., & Cuestas, P. J. (2007). University students' academic

performance: An integrative conceptual framework and empirical analysis. British

Journal of Educational Psychology, 77(4), 873-891.

Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. (1995). Early disruptive behavior, IQ, and later school

achievement and delinquent behavior. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,

23(2), 183-199.

Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of educational research, 59(2), 117-

142.

Finn, J. D., & Rock, D. A. (1997). Academic success among students at risk for school

failure. Journal of applied psychology, 82(2), 221.

Finn, J. D., & Zimmer, K. S. (2012). Student engagement: What is it? Why does it matter?.

In Handbook of research on student engagement (pp. 97-131). Springer US.

Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2007). Hyperactive children as

young adults: Driving abilities, safe driving behavior, and adverse driving

outcomes. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 39(1), 94-105.

Page 140: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

132

Frazier, T. W., Youngstrom, E. A., Glutting, J. J., & Watkins, M. W. (2007). ADHD and

achievement: Meta-analysis of the child, adolescent, and adult literatures and a

concomitant study with college students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 49-

65. doi:10.1177/00222194070400010401

Fredricks, J. A., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of

the concept, state of the evidence. Review of educational research, 74(1), 59-109.

Fried, R., Petty, C., Faraone, S. V., Hyder, L. L., Day, H., & Biederman, J. (2013). Is

ADHD a risk factor for high school dropout? A controlled study. Journal of

attention disorders, 1087054712473180.

Frodl, T., & Skokauskas, N. (2012). Meta‐analysis of structural MRI studies in children

and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects.

Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(2), 114-126.

Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in children's academic

engagement and performance. Journal of educational psychology, 95(1), 148.

Galéra, C., Melchior, M., Chastang, J. F., Bouvard, M. P., & Fombonne, E. (2009).

Childhood and adolescent hyperactivity-inattention symptoms and academic

achievement 8 years later: the GAZEL Youth study. Psychological medicine,

39(11), 1895-1906

Gaub, M., & Carlson, C. L. (1997). Gender differences in ADHD: a meta-analysis and

critical review. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent

Psychiatry, 36(8), 1036-1045.

Gershon, J., & Gershon, J. (2002). A meta-analytic review of gender differences in ADHD.

Journal of attention disorders, 5(3), 143-154

Page 141: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

133

Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Continuity of academic

intrinsic motivation from childhood through late adolescence: A longitudinal study.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(1), 3-13.

doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.93.1.3

Gray, S. A., Rogers, M., Martinussen, R., & Tannock, R. (2015). Longitudinal relations

among inattention, working memory, and academic achievement: testing mediation

and the moderating role of gender. PeerJ, 3, e939.

Green, C. T., Long, D. L., Green, D., Iosif, A. M., Dixon, J. F., Miller, M. R., ... &

Schweitzer, J. B. (2012). Will working memory training generalize to improve off-

task behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder?.

Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 639-648.

Green, J., Liem, G. A. D., Martin, A. J., Colmar, S., Marsh, H. W., & McInerney, D.

(2012). Academic motivation, self-concept, engagement, and performance in high

school: Key processes from a longitudinal perspective. Journal of adolescence,

35(5), 1111-1122.

Greenwood, C. R., Horton, B. T., & Utley, C. A. (2002). Academic engagement: Current

perspectives on research and practice. School Psychology Review, 31(3), 328.

Greenwood, T. A., Joo, E. J., Shekhtman, T., Sadovnick, A. D., Remick, R. A., Keck, P.

E., ... & Kelsoe, J. R. (2013). Association of dopamine transporter gene variants

with childhood ADHD features in bipolar disorder. American Journal of Medical

Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 162(2), 137-145.

Greven, C. U., Bralten, J., Mennes, M., O’Dwyer, L., van Hulzen, K. J., Rommelse, N., ...

& Oosterlaan, J. (2015). Developmentally stable whole-brain volume reductions

Page 142: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

134

and developmentally sensitive caudate and putamen volume alterations in those

with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and their unaffected siblings. JAMA

psychiatry, 72(5), 490-499.

Gross-Tsur, V., Goldzweig, G., Landau, Y. E., Berger, I., Shmueli, D., & Shalev, R. S.

(2006). The impact of sex and subtypes on cognitive and psychosocial aspects of

ADHD. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 48(11), 901-905.

Guo, Y., Connor, C. M., Tompkins, V., & Morrison, F. J. (2011). Classroom quality and

student engagement: contributions to third-grade reading skills. Frontiers in

psychology, 2

Gureasko-Moore, S., Dupaul, G. J., & White, G. P. (2006). The effects of self-management

in general education classrooms on the organizational skills of adolescents with

ADHD. Behavior modification, 30(2), 159-183.

Haenlein, M., & Caul, W. F. (1987). Attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity: A

specific hypothesis of reward dysfunction. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 26(3), 356-362.

Hampton, N. Z. (1998). Sources of Academic Self-Efficacy Scale: An assessment tool for

rehabilitation counselors. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin.

Hampton, N. Z., & Mason, E. (2003). Learning disabilities, gender, sources of efficacy,

self-efficacy beliefs, and academic achievement in high school students. Journal of

School Psychology, 41(2), 101-112.

Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectory

of children's school outcomes through eighth grade. Child development, 72(2), 625-

638.

Page 143: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

135

Harold, G. T., Leve, L. D., Barrett, D., Elam, K., Neiderhiser, J. M., Natsuaki, M. N., ... &

Thapar, A. (2013). Biological and rearing mother influences on child ADHD

symptoms: revisiting the developmental interface between nature and nurture.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(10), 1038-1046.

Hart, E. L., Lahey, B. B., Loeber, R., Applegate, B., & Frick, P. J. (1995). Developmental

change in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in boys: a four-year longitudinal

study. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 23(6), 729-749.

Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the

classroom: Motivational and informational components. Developmental

psychology, 17(3), 300.

Harter, S. (1999). The construction of the self: A developmental perspective. New York,

NY, US: Guilford Press. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Hartung, C. M., Willcutt, E. G., Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney, J., Stein, M. A., &

Keenan, K. (2002). Sex differences in young children who meet criteria for

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent

Psychology, 31(4), 453-464.

Hechtman, L. (2000). Assessment and diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Child and adolescent psychiatric clinics of North America, 9(3), 481-498.

Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Academic underachievement, attention deficits, and aggression:

comorbidity and implications for intervention. Journal of consulting and clinical

psychology, 60(6), 893.

Page 144: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

136

Hinshaw, S. P. (1992). Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement

in childhood and adolescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms.

Psychological bulletin, 111(1), 127.

Hinshaw, S. P., & Blachman, D. R. (2005). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in girls.

In Handbook of behavioral and emotional problems in girls (pp. 117-147). Springer

US.

Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., Fan, C., Jassy, J. S., & Owens, E. B. (2007).

Neuropsychological functioning of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder followed prospectively into adolescence: Evidence for continuing deficits?

Neuropsychology, 21, 263-273. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.263

Hinshaw, S. P., Owens, E. B., Sami, N., & Fargeon, S. (2006). Prospective follow-up of

girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adolescence: Evidence for

continuing cross-domain impairment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 74, 489-499. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.489

Hinshaw, S. P., Owens, E. B., Zalecki, C., Huggins, S. P., Montenegro-Nevado, A. J.,

Schrodek, E., & Swanson, E. N. (2012). Prospective follow-up of girls with

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into early adulthood: continuing impairment

includes elevated risk for suicide attempts and self-injury. Journal of consulting

and clinical psychology, 80(6), 1041.

Hoagwood, K., Kelleher, K. J., Feil, M., & Comer, D. M. (2000). Treatment services for

children with ADHD: a national perspective. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(2), 198-206.

Page 145: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

137

Holmbeck, G. N., Friedman, D., Abad, M., & Jandasek, B. (2006). Development and

Psychopathology in Adolescence.

Hoza, B., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, L. E., Pelham Jr, W. E., Molina, B. S., ...

& Wigal, T. (2004). Self-perceptions of competence in children with ADHD and

comparison children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(3), 382.

Hoza, B., Mrug, S., Gerdes, A. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Bukowski, W. M., Gold, J. A., ... &

Arnold, L. E. (2005). What aspects of peer relationships are impaired in children

with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder?. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 73(3), 411.

Hoza, B., Pelham, W. E., Milich, R., & Pillow, D. (1993). The self-perceptions and

attributions of attention deficit hyperactivity disordered and nonreferred boys.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An Official Publication of the

International Society for Research in Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 21,

271-286. doi:10.1007/BF00917535

Hu, L. T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure

analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural equation

modeling: a multidisciplinary journal, 6(1), 1-55.

Huang, C. (2013). Gender differences in academic self-efficacy: a meta-analysis. European

Journal of Psychology of Education, 28(1), 1-35

Hupp, S. D., Reitman, D., Northup, J., O’Callaghan, P., & LeBlanc, M. (2002). The effects

of delayed rewards, tokens, and stimulant medication on sportsmanlike behavior

with ADHD-diagnosed children. Behavior Modification, 26(2), 148-162.

Page 146: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

138

Hurt, E. A., Hoza, B., & Pelham Jr, W. E. (2007). Parenting, family loneliness, and peer

functioning in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of

Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(4), 543-555.

Hurtig, T., Ebeling, H., Taanila, A., Miettunen, J., Smalley, S. L., McGough, J. J., ... &

Moilanen, I. K. (2007). ADHD symptoms and subtypes: relationship between

childhood and adolescent symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(12), 1605-1613

Iaboni, F., Douglas, V. I., & Ditto, B. (1997). Psychophysiological response of ADHD

children to reward and extinction. Psychophysiology, 34(1), 116-123

Jacobson, L. T., & Reid, R. (2012). Improving the writing performance of high school

students with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and writing difficulties.

Exceptionality, 20(4), 218-234.

Jepsen, J. R. M., Fagerlund, B., & Mortensen, E. L. (2008). Do attention deficits influence

IQ assessment in children and adolescents with ADHD?. Journal of Attention

Disorders.

Joët, G., Usher, E. L., & Bressoux, P. (2011). Sources of self-efficacy: An investigation of

elementary school students in France. Journal of educational psychology, 103(3),

649.

Johnson, W., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. G. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences

on academic achievement trajectories during adolescence. Developmental

psychology, 42(3), 514.

Page 147: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

139

Junod, R. E. V., DuPaul, G. J., Jitendra, A. K., Volpe, R. J., & Cleary, K. S. (2006).

Classroom observations of students with and without ADHD: Differences across

types of engagement. Journal of School Psychology, 44(2), 87-104.

Kelley, M. L., & McCain, A. P. (1995). Promoting Academic Performance in Inattentive

Children The Relative Efficacy of School-Home Notes with and without

Response Cost. Behavior Modification, 19(3), 357-375.

Kenny, D. A., Kaniskan, B., & McCoach, D. B. (2014). The performance of RMSEA in

models with small degrees of freedom. Sociological Methods & Research,

0049124114543236.

Kiran, D., & Sungur, S. (2012). Middle school students’ science self-efficacy and its

sources: Examination of gender difference. Journal of Science Education and

Technology, 21(5), 619-630.

Kitsantas, A., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2009). College students’ homework and academic

achievement: The mediating role of self-regulatory beliefs. Metacognition and

Learning, 4, 97-110. doi:10.1007/s11409-008-9028-y

Klassen, R. M. (2004). Optimism and realism: a review of self‐efficacy from a cross‐

cultural perspective. International Journal of Psychology, 39(3), 205-230.

Klobas, J. E., Renzi, S., & Nigrelli, M. L. (2007). A scale for the measurement of self-

efficacy for learning (SEL) at university.

Kuntsi, J., Oosterlaan, J., & Stevenson, J. (2001). Psychological mechanisms in

hyperactivity: I response inhibition deficit, working memory impairment, delay

aversion, or something else?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

42(02), 199-210.

Page 148: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

140

Lahey, B. B., Applegate, B., McBurnett, K., Biederman, J., Greenhill, L., Hynd, G. W., ...

& Garfinkel, B. (1994). DMS-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder in children and adolescents. The American Journal of Psychiatry.

Langberg, J. M., Arnold, L. E., Flowers, A. M., Epstein, J. N., Altaye, M., Hinshaw, S.

P., ... & Jensen, P. S. (2010). Parent-reported homework problems in the MTA

study: Evidence for sustained improvement with behavioral treatment. Journal of

Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 39(2), 220-233.

Langberg, J. M., Becker, S. P., Epstein, J. N., Vaughn, A. J., & Girio-Herrera, E. (2013).

Predictors of response and mechanisms of change in an organizational skills

intervention for students with ADHD. Journal of child and family studies, 22(7),

1000-1012.

Lange, K. W., Reichl, S., Lange, K. M., Tucha, L., & Tucha, O. (2010). The history of

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. ADHD Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity

Disorders, 2(4), 241-255.

Larsson, H., Sariaslan, A., Langstrom, N., D’Onofrio, B. & Lichtenstein, P. Family

income in early childhood and subsequent attention deficit/ hyperactivity

disorder: a quasi-experimental study. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 428–435

(2014).

Lee, S. S., Lahey, B. B., Owens, E. B., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2008). Few preschool boys and

girls with ADHD are well-adjusted during adolescence. Journal of abnormal

child psychology, 36(3), 373-383.

LeFever, G. B., Villers, M. S., Morrow, A. L., & Vaughn, E. S. (2002). Parental

perceptions of adverse educational outcomes among children diagnosed and

Page 149: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

141

treated for ADHD: A call for improved school/provider collaboration. Psychology

in the Schools, 39(1), 63-71.

Lemiere, J., Wouters, H., Sterken, C., Lagae, L., Sonuga-Barke, E., & Danckaerts, M.

(2010). Are children with ADHD predominantly inattentive and combined

subtypes different in terms of aspects of everyday attention?. European child &

adolescent psychiatry, 19(8), 679-685.

Lent, R. W., & Hackett, G. (1987). Career self-efficacy: Empirical status and future

directions. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 30, 347-382. doi:10.1016/0001-

8791(87)90010-8

Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Larkin, K. C. (1987). Comparison of three theoretically

derived variables in predicting career and academic behavior: Self-efficacy, interest

congruence, and consequence thinking. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 34, 293-

298. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.34.3.293

Lent, R. W., Lopez, F. G., & Bieschke, K. J. (1991). Mathematics self-efficacy: Sources

and relation to science-based career choice. Journal of counseling psychology,

38(4), 424.

Lent, R. W., Lopez, F. G., Brown, S. D., & Gore Jr, P. A. (1996). Latent structure of the

sources of mathematics self-efficacy. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 49(3), 292-

308.

Lepper, M. R., Sethi, S., Dialdin, D., & Drake, M. (1997). Intrinsic and extrinsic

motivation: A developmental perspective. Developmental psychopathology:

Perspectives on adjustment, risk, and disorder, 23-50.

Page 150: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

142

Liem, A. D., Lau, S., & Nie, Y. (2008). The role of self-efficacy, task value, and

achievement goals in predicting learning strategies, task disengagement, peer

relationship, and achievement outcome. Contemporary Educational Psychology,

33(4), 486-512.

Lindman, H. R. (1974). Analysis of variance in complex experimental designs. Oxford,

England: W. H. Freeman & Co.

Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2002). Motivation as an enabler for academic

success. School Psychology Review, 31, 313-327. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Linnenbrink, E. A., & Pintrich, P. R. (2003). The role of self-efficacy beliefs in student

engagement and learning in the classroom. Reading &Writing Quarterly, 19(2),

119-137

Litner, B. (2003, June). Teens with ADHD: The challenge of high school. In Child and

Youth Care Forum (Vol. 32, No. 3, pp. 137-158). Kluwer Academic Publishers-

Plenum Publishers.

Loe, I. M., & Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children

with ADHD. Journal of pediatric psychology, 32(6), 643-654.

Lopez, F. G., & Lent, R. W. (1992). Sources of mathematics self‐efficacy in high school

students. The Career Development Quarterly, 41(1), 3-12.

Lopez, F. G., Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Gore, P. A. (1997). Role of social–cognitive

expectations in high school students' mathematics-related interest and performance.

Journal of Counseling Psychology, 44(1), 44.\

Lopez-Vergara, H., & Colder, C. R. (2013). An examination of the specificity of

motivation and executive functioning in ADHD symptom-clusters in adolescence.

Page 151: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

143

Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38(10), 1081-1090.

doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jst050

Lubke, G. H., Hudziak, J. J., Derks, E. M., van Bijsterveldt, T. C., & Boomsma, D. I.

(2009). Maternal ratings of attention problems in ADHD: evidence for the

existence of a continuum. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(11), 1085-1093.

Luman, M., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The impact of reinforcement

contingencies on AD/HD: a review and theoretical appraisal. Clinical psychology

review, 25(2), 183-213.

Luzzo, A.D., Hitchings, W.E., Retish, P., & Shoemaker, A. (1999). Evaluating

differences in college students’ career decision making on the basis of disability

status. The Career Development Quarterly, 48, 142-156.

MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and

determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological

methods, 1(2), 130.

Maehr, M. L., & Meyer, H. A. (1997). Understanding motivation and schooling: Where

we've been, where we are, and where we need to go. Educational Psychology

Review, 9(4), 371-409.

Major, A., Martinussen, R., & Wiener, J. (2013). Self-efficacy for self-regulated learning

in adolescents with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 149-156.

Mannuzza, S., Klein, R. G., Bessler, A., Malloy, P., & LaPadula, M. (1998). Adult

psychiatric status of hyperactive boys grown up. American journal of psychiatry.

Page 152: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

144

Marks, H. M. (2000). Student engagement in instructional activity: Patterns in the

elementary, middle, and high school years. American educational research

journal, 37(1), 153-184.

Martel, M., Nikolas, M., & Nigg, J. T. (2007). Executive function in adolescents with

ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

46(11), 1437-1444.

Martin, A. J. (2012). The role of personal best (PB) goals in the achievement and

behavioral engagement of students with ADHD and students without ADHD.

Contemporary Educational Psychology, 37(2), 91-105.

Martin, A. J. (2013). Improving the achievement, motivation, and engagement of students

with ADHD: The role of personal best goals and other growth-based approaches.

Australian Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 23(01), 143-155.

Marton, I., Wiener, J., Rogers, M., Moore, C., & Tannock, R. (2009). Empathy and social

perspective taking in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Journal of abnormal child psychology, 37(1), 107-118.

Mason, L. H., & Shriner, J. G. (2008). Self-regulated strategy development instruction for

writing an opinion essay: Effects for six students with emotional/behavior

disorders. Reading and Writing, 21(1-2), 71-93.

Massetti, G. M., Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney, J., Ehrhardt, A., Lee, S. S., & Kipp,

H. (2008). Academic achievement over 8 years among children who met modified

criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder at 4–6 years of age. Journal of

abnormal child psychology, 36(3), 399-410.

Page 153: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

145

Massetti, G. M., Lahey, B. B., Pelham, W. E., Loney, J., Ehrhardt, A., Lee, S. S., & Kipp,

H. (2008). Academic achievement over 8 years among children who met modified

criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder at 4–6 years of age. Journal of

abnormal child psychology, 36(3), 399-410.

Mather, N., & Woodcock, R. W. (2001). Application of the woodcock-johnson tests of

cognitive Ability—Revised to the diagnosis of learning disabilities. In A. S.

Kaufman, & N. L. Kaufman (Eds.), Specific learning disabilities and difficulties

in children and adolescents: Psychological assessment and evaluation. (pp. 55-

96). New York, NY, US: Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from

www.csa.com

Matte, B., Anselmi, L., Salum, G. A., Kieling, C., Gonçalves, H., Menezes, A., ... &

Rohde, L. A. (2015). ADHD in DSM-5: a field trial in a large, representative

sample of 18-to 19-year-old adults. Psychological medicine, 45(02), 361-373.

McCandless, S., & O'Laughlin, L. (2007). The clinical utility of the Behavior Rating

Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) in the diagnosis of ADHD. Journal of

Attention Disorders, 10(4), 381-389.

McInerney, R. J., & Kerns, K. A. (2003). Time reproduction in children with ADHD:

motivation matters. Child Neuropsychology, 9(2), 91-108.

McQuade, J. D., Tomb, M., Hoza, B., Waschbusch, D. A., Hurt, E. A., & Vaughn, A. J.

(2011). Cognitive deficits and positively biased self-perceptions in children with

ADHD. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 39(2), 307-319.

Page 154: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

146

Melnick, S. M., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2000). Emotion regulation and parenting in AD/HD

and comparison boys: Linkages with social behaviors and peer preference.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 28(1), 73-86.

Meltzer, L., Katzir, T., Miller, L., Reddy, R., & Roditi, B. (2004). Academic self‐

perceptions, effort, and strategy use in students with learning disabilities: Changes

over time. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19(2), 99-108.

Meyer, K., & Kelley, M. L. (2007). Improving homework in adolescents with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Self vs. parent monitoring of homework behavior

and study skills. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 29(4), 25-42.

Mick, E., Faraone, S. V., & Biederman, J. (2004). Age-dependent expression of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Psychiatric Clinics of North America,

27(2), 215-224

Milberger, S., Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., Chen, L., & Jones, J. (1996). Is maternal

smoking during pregnancy a risk factor for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

in children?. The American journal of psychiatry, 153(9), 1138.

Milich, R. (1994). The response of children with ADHD to failure: If at first you don't

succeed, do you try, try, again? School Psychology Review, 23, 11-28. Retrieved

from www.csa.com

Milich, R., & Okazaki, M. (1991). An examination of learned helplessness among

attention-deficit hyperactivity disordered boys. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 19, 607-623. doi: 10.1007/BF00925823.

Miller, G. M., & Chapman, J. P. (2001). Misunderstanding analysis of covariance. Journal

of Abnormal Psychology, 110(1), 40-48. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.40

Page 155: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

147

Miller, M., Nevado-Montenegro, A. J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2012). Childhood executive

function continues to predict outcomes in young adult females with and without

childhood-diagnosed ADHD. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 40(5), 657-

668.

Molina, B. S., & Pelham Jr, W. E. (2003). Childhood predictors of adolescent substance

use in a longitudinal study of children with ADHD. Journal of abnormal

psychology, 112(3), 497.

Muris, P. (2002). Relationships between self-efficacy and symptoms of anxiety disorders

and depression in a normal adolescent sample. Personality and individual

differences, 32(2), 337-348.

Murphey, T., & Arao, H. (2001). Reported belief changes through near peer role

modeling. TESL-EJ, 5(3), 1-15.

Murphy, K., & Barkley, R. A. (1996). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder adults:

comorbidities and adaptive impairments. Comprehensive psychiatry, 37(6), 393-

401.

Murray, C., & Zvoch, K. (2011). Teacher—student relationships among behaviorally at-

risk African American youth from low-income backgrounds: student perceptions,

teacher perceptions, and socioemotional adjustment correlates. Journal of

Emotional and Behavioral Disorders, 19(1), 41-54.

Musser, E. D., Backs, R. W., Schmitt, C. F., Ablow, J. C., Measelle, J. R., & Nigg, J. T.

(2011). Emotion regulation via the autonomic nervous system in children with

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of abnormal child

psychology, 39(6), 841-852.

Page 156: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

148

Musser, E. D., Backs, R. W., Schmitt, C. F., Ablow, J. C., Measelle, J. R., & Nigg, J. T.

(2011). Emotion regulation via the autonomic nervous system in children with

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of abnormal child

psychology, 39(6), 841-852.

Muthén, B., & Muthén, B. O. (2009). Statistical analysis with latent variables. Wiley.

Neale, B. M., Medland, S. E., Ripke, S., Asherson, P., Franke, B., Lesch, K. P., ... &

Daly, M. (2010). Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(9), 884-897.

Nigg, J. T., Hinshaw, S. P., Carte, E. T., & Treuting, J. J. (1998). Neuropsychological

correlates of childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Explainable by

comorbid disruptive behavior or reading problems?. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 107(3), 468.

Nikolas, M. A., & Burt, S. A. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on ADHD

symptom dimensions of inattention and hyperactivity: a meta-analysis. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 119(1), 1.

Nolan, E. E., Gadow, K. D., & Sprafkin, J. (2001). Teacher reports of DSM-IV ADHD,

ODD, and CD symptoms in schoolchildren. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(2), 241-249.

Norvilitis, J. M., Sun, L., & Zhang, J. (2010). ADHD symptomatology and adjustment to

college in China and the United States. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 43(1),

86-94.

Page 157: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

149

Norwalk, K., Norvilitis, J. M., & MacLean, M. G. (2009). ADHD symptomatology and

its relationship to factors associated with college adjustment. Journal of Attention

Disorders, 13(3), 251-258.

Nussbaum, N. L. (2012). ADHD and Female Specific Concerns A Review of the

Literature and Clinical Implications. Journal of attention disorders, 16(2), 87-

100.

Offord, D. R., Boyle, M. H., Racine, Y., Szatmari, P., Fleming, J. E., Sanford, M., &

Lipman, E. L. (1996). Integrating assessment data from multiple informants.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 35(8), 1078-

1085.

Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development, 2009

Owens, J. S., Goldfine, M. E., Evangelista, N. M., Hoza, B., & Kaiser, N. M. (2007). A

critical review of self-perceptions and the positive illusory bias in children with

ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 10, 335-351.

doi:10.1007/s10567-007-0027-3

Pagani, L. S., Vitaro, F., Tremblay, R. E., McDuff, P., Japel, C., & Larose, S. (2008).

When predictions fail: The case of unexpected pathways toward high school

dropout. Journal of Social Issues, 64, 175-193. doi:10.1111/j.1540-

4560.2008.00554.x

Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in academic settings. Review of educational

research, 66(4), 543-578.

Page 158: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

150

Pajares, F. (2007). Empirical properties of a scale to assess writing self-efficacy in school

contexts. Measurement and Evaluation in Counseling and Development, 39(4),

239.

Pajares, F., & Graham, L. (1999). Self-efficacy, motivation constructs, and mathematics

performance of entering middle school students. Contemporary Educational

Psychology, 24), 124-139. doi:10.1006/ceps.1998.0991

Pajares, F., & Miller, M. D. (1994). Role of self-efficacy and self-concept beliefs in

mathematical problem solving: A path analysis. Journal of educational

psychology, 86(2), 193.

Pajares, F., & Urdan, T. (Eds.). (2006). Adolescence and education: Vol. 5.Self-efficacy

beliefs of adolescents. Greenwich, CT: Information Age.

Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (1997). Influence of self-efficacy on elementary students'

writing. The Journal of Educational Research, 90(6), 353-360

Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (2001). Gender differences in writing motivation and

achievement of middle school students: A function of gender orientation?

Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26, 366-381.

doi:10.1006/ceps.2000.1069

Pajares, F., & Valiante, G. (2002). Students' self-efficacy in their self-regulated learning

strategies: A developmental perspective. Psychologia: An International Journal

of Psychology in the Orient, 45, 211-221. doi:10.2117/psysoc.2002.211

Pajares, F., Miller, M. D., & Johnson, M. J. (1999). Gender differences in writing self-

beliefs of elementary school students. Journal of educational Psychology, 91(1),

50.

Page 159: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

151

Parsons, J. E., Adler, T. F., & Kaczala, C. M. (1982). Socialization of achievement

attitudes and beliefs: Parental influences. Child development, 310-321.

Pelham, W. E., Carlson, C., Sams, S. E., Vallano, G., Dixon, M. J., & Hoza, B. (1993).

Separate and combined effects of methylphenidate and behavior modification on

boys with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder in the classroom. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61(3), 506.

Pelham, W. E., Fabiano, G. A., & Massetti, G. M. (2005). Evidence-based assessment of

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Journal of

Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 449–476.

doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp3403_5

Pennington, B.F., Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental

psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–78.

Pfiffner, L. J., Barkley, R. A., & DuPaul, G. J. (1998). Treatment of ADHD in school

settings. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and

treatment, 2, 458-490.

Pfiffner, L. J., Villodas, M., Kaiser, N., Rooney, M., & McBurnett, K. (2013).

Educational outcomes of a collaborative school–home behavioral intervention for

ADHD. School Psychology Quarterly, 28(1), 25.

Phan, H. P. (2011). Interrelations between self‐efficacy and learning approaches: a

developmental approach. Educational Psychology, 31(2), 225-246.

Piacentini, J. C., Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (1992). Combining discrepant diagnostic

information from multiple sources: are complex algorithms better than simple

ones?. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 20(1), 51-63.

Page 160: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

152

Pintrich, P. R., & de Groot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and self-regulated learning

components of classroom academic performance. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 82, 33-40. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.33

Plamondon, A., & Martinussen, R. (2015). Inattention Symptoms Are Associated With

Academic Achievement Mostly Through Variance Shared With Intrinsic

Motivation and Behavioral Engagement. Journal of attention disorders,

1087054715587098.

Plichta, M. M., & Scheres, A. (2014). Ventral–striatal responsiveness during reward

anticipation in ADHD and its relation to trait impulsivity in the healthy

population: A meta-analytic review of the fMRI literature. Neuroscience &

Biobehavioral Reviews, 38, 125-134.

Polanczyk, G. V., Willcutt, E. G., Salum, G. A., Kieling, C., & Rohde, L. A. (2014).

ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: An updated systematic review

and meta-regression analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology, 43(2), 434-

442. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/ije/dyt261

Polanczyk, G., de Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The

worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression

analysis. American journal of psychiatry.

Polderman, T. J. C., Boomsma, D. I., Bartels, M., Verhulst, F. C., & Huizink, A. C.

(2010). A systematic review of prospective studies on attention problems and

academic achievement. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 122(4), 271-284.

Page 161: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

153

Power, T. J., Werba, B. E., Watkins, M. W., Angelucci, J. G., & Eiraldi, R. B. (2006).

Patterns of parent-reported homework problems among ADHD-referred and non-

referred children. School Psychology Quarterly, 21(1), 13.

Prat‐Sala, M., & Redford, P. (2010). The interplay between motivation, self‐efficacy, and

approaches to studying. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(2), 283-

305

Rapport, M. D., Murphy, H. A., & Bailey, J. S. (1982). Ritalin vs. Response cost in the

control of hyperactive children: a within‐subject comparison. Journal of Applied

Behavior Analysis, 15(2), 205-216.

Rapport, M. D., Scanlan, S. W., & Denney, C. B. (1999). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder and scholastic achievement: A model of dual developmental pathways.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(08), 1169-1183.

Reaser, A., Prevatt, F., Petscher, Y., & Proctor, B. (2007). The learning and study

strategies of college students with ADHD. Psychology in the Schools, 44, 627-

638. doi:10.1002/pits.20252

Reeve, J., Jang, H., Carrell, D., Jeon, S., & Barch, J. (2004). Enhancing students'

engagement by increasing teachers' autonomy support. Motivation and emotion,

28(2), 147-169.

Reschly, A. L., & Christenson, S. L. (2006). Prediction of Dropout Among Students With

Mild Disabilities A Case for the Inclusion of Student Engagement Variables.

Remedial and Special Education, 27(5), 276-292.

Page 162: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

154

Resnick, R. J. (2005). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in teens and adults: They

don't all outgrow it. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 61(5), 529-533.

doi:10.1002/jclp.20117

Robin, A. L. (1998). ADHD in adolescents: Diagnosis and treatment. Guilford Press.

Rogers, M., & Tannock, R. (2013). Are classrooms meeting the basic psychological

needs of children with ADHD symptoms? A self-determination theory

perspective. Journal of attention disorders, 20(10), 1-7.

Rogers, M., Hwang, H., Toplak, M., Weiss, M., & Tannock, R. (2011). Inattention,

working memory, and academic achievement in adolescents referred for attention

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychology, 17(5), 444-458.

Rolls, E. T. (2004). The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain and cognition, 55(1),

11-29.

Rosch, K. S., & Hawk, L. W. (2013). The effects of performance‐based rewards on

neurophysiological correlates of stimulus, error, and feedback processing in

children with ADHD. Psychophysiology, 50(11), 1157-1173.

Rubio-Stipec, M., Fitzmaurice, G., Murphy, J., & Walker, A. (2003). The use of multiple

informants in identifying the risk factors of depressive and disruptive disorders:

Are they interchangeable? Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 38,

51–58. doi:10.1007/s00127-0030600-0

Rucklidge, J. J. (2010). Gender differences in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 33, 357-373. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2010.01.006

Page 163: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

155

Rucklidge, J. J., & Tannock, R. (2001). Psychiatric, psychosocial, and cognitive

functioning of female adolescents with ADHD. Journal of the American Academy

of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40(5), 530-540.

Sasser, T. R., Bierman, K. L., & Heinrichs, B. (2015). Executive functioning and school

adjustment: The mediational role of pre-kindergarten learning-related behaviors.

Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 30, 70-79.

Sattler, J. M. (2001). Assessment of children: Cognitive applications (4th ed.). La Mesa,

CA, US: Jerome M Sattler Publisher. Retrieved from www.csa.com

Scassellati, C., Bonvicini, C., Faraone, S. V., & Gennarelli, M. (2012). Biomarkers and

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a systematic review and meta-analyses.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 51(10),

1003-1019.

Scholz, U., Doña, B. G., Sud, S., & Schwarzer, R. (2002). Is general self-efficacy a

universal construct? Psychometric findings from 25 countries. European journal

of psychological assessment, 18(3), 242.

Schultz, W., Tremblay, L., & Hollerman, J. R. (2000). Reward processing in primate

orbitofrontal cortex and basal ganglia. Cerebral Cortex, 10(3), 272-283.

Schunk, D. H. (1987). Peer models and children’s behavioral change. Review of

educational research, 57(2), 149-174.

Schunk, D. H., & Mullen, C. A. (2012). Self-efficacy as an engaged learner. In Handbook

of research on student engagement (pp. 219-235). Springer US.

Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2009). Self-efficacy theory. Handbook of motivation at

school, 35-53.

Page 164: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

156

Schwarzer, R., Bäßler, J., Kwiatek, P., Schröder, K., & Zhang, J. X. (1997). The

assessment of optimistic self‐beliefs: comparison of the German, Spanish, and

Chinese versions of the general self‐efficacy scale. Applied Psychology, 46(1),

69-88.

Seidman, L. J., Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Valera, E., Doyle, A. E., & Faraone, S.

V. (2005). Impact of gender and age on executive functioning: do girls and boys

with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder differ

neuropsychologically in preteen and teenage years?. Developmental

neuropsychology, 27(1), 79-105.

Seixas, M., Weiss, M., & Müller, U. (2011). Systematic review of national and

international guidelines on attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Journal of

Psychopharmacology, 0269881111412095.

Sergeant, J. A., Oosterlaan, J., & van der Meere, J. (1999). Information processing and

energetic factors in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In Handbook of

disruptive behavior disorders (pp. 75-104). Springer US.

Shaw, P., Malek, M., Watson, B., Greenstein, D., de Rossi, P., & Sharp, W. (2013).

Trajectories of cerebral cortical development in childhood and adolescence and

adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 74(8), 599-

606.

Sibley, M. H., Pelham Jr, W. E., Molina, B. S., Gnagy, E. M., Waschbusch, D. A.,

Garefino, A. C., ... & Karch, K. M. (2012). Diagnosing ADHD in adolescence.

Journal of consulting and clinical psychology, 80(1), 139.

Page 165: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

157

Sins, P. H., van Joolingen, W. R., Savelsbergh, E. R., & van Hout-Wolters, B. (2008).

Motivation and performance within a collaborative computer-based modeling

task: Relations between students’ achievement goal orientation, self-efficacy,

cognitive processing, and achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology,

33(1), 58-77.

Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (2009). Engagement

and disaffection as organizational constructs in the dynamics of motivational

development. Handbook of motivation at school, 223-245.

Skinner, E., Furrer, C., Marchand, G., & Kindermann, T. (2008). Engagement and

disaffection in the classroom: Part of a larger motivational dynamic?. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 100(4), 765.

Skoglund, C., Chen, Q., D’Onofrio, B. M., Lichtenstein, P. & Larsson, H. Familial

confounding of the association between maternal smoking during pregnancy and

ADHD in offspring. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 61–68 (2014). 52.

Slomkowski, C., Klein, R. G., & Mannuzza, S. (1995). Is self-esteem an important

outcome in hyperactive children? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology: An

Official Publication of the International Society for Research in Child and

Adolescent Psychopathology, 23, 303-315. doi:10.1007/BF01447559

Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. (1994). Annotation: On dysfunction and function in psychological

theories of childhood disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

35(5), 801-815.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2002). Psychological heterogeneity in AD/HD—a dual pathway

model of behaviour and cognition. Behavioural brain research, 130(1), 29-36.

Page 166: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

158

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2003). The dual pathway model of AD/HD: an elaboration of neuro-

developmental characteristics. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 27(7),

593-604.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:

from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biological

psychiatry, 57(11), 1231-1238.

Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. S., Houlberg, K., & Hall, M. (1994). When is “impulsiveness” not

impulsive? The case of hyperactive children's cognitive style. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(7), 1247-1253.

Sonuga‐Barke, E. J., & Sergeant, J. (2005). The neuroscience of ADHD:

multidisciplinary perspectives on a complex developmental disorder.

Developmental Science, 8(2), 103-104.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Dalen, L., & Remington, B. (2003). Do executive deficits and delay

aversion make independent contributions to preschool attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms?. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(11), 1335-1342.

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Saxton, T., & Hall, M. (1998). The role of interval underestimation

in hyperactive children's failure to suppress responses over time. Behavioural

Brain Research, 94(1), 45-50.

Sonuga‐Barke, E. J., Williams, E., Hall, M., & Saxton, T. (1996). Hyperactivity and

delay aversion III: The effect on cognitive style of imposing delay after errors.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(2), 189-194.

Page 167: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

159

Staller, J., & Faraone, S. V. (2006). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in girls. CNS

drugs, 20(2), 107-123.

Stavro, G. M., Ettenhofer, M. L., & Nigg, J. T. (2007). Executive functions and adaptive

functioning in young adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the

International Neuropsychological Society, 13(02), 324-334.

Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval

estimation approach. Multivariate behavioral research, 25(2), 173-180.

Steinhausen, H. (2009). The heterogeneity of causes and courses of attention

deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 120(5), 392-399

Stevens, S. E., Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Kreppner, J. M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Colvert, E.,

... & Rutter, M. (2008). Inattention/overactivity following early severe

institutional deprivation: presentation and associations in early adolescence.

Journal of abnormal child psychology, 36(3), 385-398.

Stewart, T. (2008). Community service self-efficacy and first-year undergraduate honors

service-learning. Scholarship for sustaining service-learning and civic

engagement: Research and the k-20 continuum, 29-53.

Stipek, D. (1998). Differences between americans and chinese in the circumstances

evoking pride, shame, and guilt. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 616-

629. doi:10.1177/0022022198295002

Stipek, D. (2002). Good instruction is motivating. Retrieved from www.psycnet.apa.org

Stoodley, C. J., & Schmahmann, J. D. (2009). Functional topography in the human

cerebellum: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Neuroimage, 44(2), 489-

501.

Page 168: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

160

Stout, K. E., & Christenson, S. L. (2009). Staying on track for high school graduation:

Promoting student engagement. The prevention researcher, 16(3), 17-21.

Strand, M. T., Hawk Jr, L. W., Bubnik, M., Shiels, K., Pelham Jr, W. E., & Waxmonsky,

J. G. (2012). Improving working memory in children with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the separate and combined effects of incentives and

stimulant medication. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 40(7), 1193-1207.

Swanson, J. M., Kraemer, H. C., Hinshaw, S. P., Arnold, L. E., Conners, C. K., Abikoff,

H. B., ... & Hechtman, L. (2001). Clinical relevance of the primary findings of the

MTA: success rates based on severity of ADHD and ODD symptoms at the end of

treatment. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

40(2), 168-179.

Swanson, J. M., Sergeant, J. A., Taylor, E., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Jensen, P. S., &

Cantwell, D. P. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and hyperkinetic

disorder. The Lancet, 351(9100), 429-433.

Tabassam, W., & Grainger, J. (2002). Self-concept, attributional style and self-efficacy

beliefs of students with learning disabilities with and without attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder. Learning Disability Quarterly, 25(2), 141-151.

Tan, C. X., Ang, R. P., Klassen, R. M., See Yeo, L., Wong, I. Y. F., Huan, V. S., &

Chong, W. H. (2008). Correlates of academic procrastination and students’ grade

goals. Current Psychology, 27, 135-144. doi: 10.1007/s12144-008-9028-8

Taylor, H. E., & Larson, S. (1998). Teaching Children with ADHD–: What Do

Elementary and Middle School Social Studies Teachers Need to Know?. The

Social Studies, 89(4), 161-164.

Page 169: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

161

Thapar, A., Cooper, M., Eyre, O., & Langley, K. (2013). Practitioner review: what have we

learnt about the causes of ADHD?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

54(1), 3-16.

Thapar, A., Holmes, J., Poulton, K., & Harrington, R. (1999). Genetic basis of attention

deficit and hyperactivity. British Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 105–111.

Thissen, A. J. A. M., Rommelse, N. N. J., Hoekstra, P. J., Hartman, C., Heslenfeld, D.,

Luman, M., . . . Buitelaar, J. K. (2014). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD) and executive functioning in affected and unaffected adolescents and their

parents: Challenging the endophenotype construct. Psychological Medicine, 44(4),

881-92. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713001153

Timimi, S. (2004). A critique of the international consensus statement on ADHD. Clinical

child and family psychology review, 7(1), 59-63

Tomevi, C. (2013). Career-decision self-efficacy among college students with symptoms of

Attention Deficit Disorder. McNair Scholars Research Journal, 9(1), 13.

Toplack, M., E., Bucciarelli, S. M., Jain, U, & Tannock, R. (2009). Executive functions:

Performance-based measures and the behaviour rating inventory of executive

function (BRIEF) in adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder

(ADHD). Child Neuropsyhcology, 15, 53-72. doi: 10.1080/09297040802070929

Trampush, J. W., Miller, C. J., Newcorn, J. H., & Halperin, J. M. (2009). The impact of

childhood ADHD on dropping out of high school in urban adolescents/young

adults. Journal of attention Disorders, 13(2), 127-136.

Treuting, J. J., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2001). Depression and self-esteem in boys with attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Associations with comorbid aggression and

Page 170: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

162

explanatory attributional mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,

29(1), 23-39.

U.S. Department of Education, 2003

Uekermann, J., Kraemer, M., Abdel-Hamid, M., Schimmelmann, B. G., Hebebrand, J.,

Daum, I., ... & Kis, B. (2010). Social cognition in attention-deficit hyperactivity

disorder (ADHD). Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34(5), 734-743.

Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2006). Sources of academic and self-regulatory efficacy beliefs

of entering middle school students. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31,

125-141. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.03.002

Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008). Self-efficacy for self-regulated learning: A validation

study. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 68, 443-463.

doi:10.1177/0013164407308475

Valera, E. M., Faraone, S. V., Murray, K. E., & Seidman, L. J. (2007). Meta-analysis of

structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological

psychiatry, 61(12), 1361-1369.

van Dinther, M., Dochy, F., & Segers, M. (2011). Factors affecting students’ self-efficacy

in higher education. Educational Research Review, 6(2), 95-108.

Vecchio, G. M., Gerbino, M., Pastorelli, C., Del Bove, G., & Caprara, G. V. (2007). Multi-

faceted self-efficacy beliefs as predictors of life satisfaction in late adolescence.

Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1807-1818.

doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.05.018

Page 171: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

163

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Newcorn, J. H., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Telang, F., ... &

Wong, C. (2011). Motivation deficit in ADHD is associated with dysfunction of the

dopamine reward pathway. Molecular psychiatry, 16(11), 1147-1154.

Volpe, R. J., DuPaul, G. J., DiPerna, J. C., Jitendra, A. K., Lutz, J. G., Tresco, K., & Junod,

R. V. (2006). Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and scholastic achievement: A

model of mediation via academic enablers. School Psychology Review, 35(1), 47.

Walker, C. O., Greene, B. A., & Mansell, R. A. (2006). Identification with academics,

intrinsic/extrinsic motivation, and self-efficacy as predictors of cognitive

engagement. Learning and Individual Differences, 16(1), 1-12.

Wang, M. T., & Holcombe, R. (2010). Adolescents’ perceptions of school environment,

engagement, and academic achievement in middle school. American Educational

Research Journal, 47(3), 633-662.

Wang, M. T., Willett, J. B., & Eccles, J. S. (2011). The assessment of school

engagement: Examining dimensionality and measurement invariance by gender

and race/ethnicity. Journal of School Psychology, 49(4), 465-480.

Wasserstein, J. (2005). Diagnostic issues for adolescents and adults with ADHD. Journal

of Clinical Psychology, 61, 535-547. doi:10.1002/jclp.20118

Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. San Antonio, TX:

Psychological Corporation.

Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV. San Antonio, TX:

Psychological Corporation.

Page 172: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

164

Wehmeier, P. M., Schacht, A., & Barkley, R. A. (2010). Social and emotional

impairment in children and adolescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of

life. Journal of Adolescent Health, 46(3), 209-217.

Weiss, M., Worling, D., & Wasdell, M. (2003). A chart review study of the inattentive

and combined types of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 7(1), 1-9.

Wiener, J., & Daniels, L. (2015). School Experiences of Adolescents With Attention-

Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Journal of learning disabilities,

0022219415576973.

Wender, E. H. (1995). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorders in adolescence. Journal

of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 16(3), 192-195.

Weyandt, L. L., Iwaszuk, W., Fulton, K., Ollerton, M., Beatty, N., Fouts, H., ... &

Greenlaw, C. (2003). The internal restlessness scale performance of college

students with and without ADHD. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(4), 382-

389.

Whitlock, J. L. (2006). Youth perceptions of life at school: Contextual correlates of

school connectedness in adolescence. Applied Developmental Science, 10(1), 13-

29.

Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). Development between the ages of 11

and 25. In D. C. Berliner, & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), (pp. 148-185). New York, NY,

US: Macmillan Library Reference USA; London, England: Prentice Hall

International.

Willcutt, E. G. (2012). The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:

a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics, 9(3), 490-499.

Page 173: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

165

Willcutt, E. G., Betjemann, R. S., McGrath, L. M., Chhabildas, N. A., Olson, R. K.,

DeFries, J. C., & Pennington, B. F. (2010). Etiology and neuropsychology of

comorbidity between RD and ADHD: The case for multiple-deficit models. Cortex,

46(10), 1345-1361

Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005).

Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:

a meta-analytic review. Biological psychiatry, 57(11), 1336-1346.

Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005).

Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder:

a meta-analytic review. Biological psychiatry, 57(11), 1336-1346.

Willcutt, E. G., Nigg, J. T., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R.,

... & Lahey, B. B. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity

disorder symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of abnormal psychology,

121(4), 991.

Willcutt, E. G., Pennington, B. F., Olson, R. K., Chhabildas, N., & Hulslander, J. (2005).

Neuropsychological analyses of comorbidity between reading disability and

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: In search of the common deficit.

Developmental neuropsychology, 27(1), 35-78.

Willcutt, E.G., Nigg, J., Pennington, B.F., Solanto, M.V., Rohde, L.A., Tannock, R., ... &

Lahey, B.B. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder

symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. In press

Page 174: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

166

Willoughby, M. T. (2003). Developmental course of ADHD symptomatology during the

transition from childhood to adolescence: a review with recommendations. Journal

of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(1), 88-106.

Wright, K. D., Waschbusch, D. A., & Frankland, B. W. (2007). Combining data from

parent ratings and parent interview when assessing ADHD. Journal of

Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29, 141–148. doi:10.1007/s10862-

006-9039-4

Young, S., Heptinstall, E., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Chadwick, O., & Taylor, E. (2005). The

adolescent outcome of hyperactive girls: Self-report of psychosocial status. Journal

of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 255-262. doi:10.1111/j.1469-

7610.2004.00350.x

Zeldin, A. L., & Pajares, F. (2000). Against the odds: Self-efficacy beliefs of women in

mathematical, scientific, and technological careers. American Educational

Research Journal, 37(1), 215-246.

Zimmerman, B. J. (1986). Becoming a self-regulated learner: Which are the key

subprocesses? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 11, 307-313.

doi:10.1016/0361-476X(86)90027-5

Zimmerman, B. J. (1995). Self-efficacy and educational development. Self-efficacy in

changing societies, 202-231.

Zimmerman, B. J. (1998). Academic studing and the development of personal skill: A self-

regulatory perspective. Educational psychologist, 33(2-3), 73-86.

Zimmerman, B. J. (1998). Developing self-fulfilling cycles of academic regulation: An

analysis of exemplary instructional models. In D. H. Schunk, & B. J. Zimmerman

Page 175: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

167

(Eds.), Self-regulated learning: From teaching to self-reflective practice. (pp. 1-

19). New York, NY, US: Guilford Publications.

Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Achieving academic excellence: A self-regulatory perspective.

In M. Ferrari (Ed.), The pursuit of excellence through education. (pp. 85-110).

Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Retrieved from

www.csa.com

Zimmerman, B. J. (Eds.). (2000). Self-efficacy: An essential motive to learn. [Special

issue]. Contemporary Educational Psychology: Motivation

Zimmerman, B. J., & Bandura, A. (1994). Impact of self-regulatory influences on writing

course attainment. American Educational Research Journal, 31, 845-862.

doi:10.2307/1163397

Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. J. (2006). Adolescents’ development of personal agency:

The role of self-efficacy beliefs and self-regulatory skill. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan

(Eds.), Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 45–69). Greenwich, CT: Information

Age.

Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (2005). Homework practices and academic

achievement: The mediating role of self-efficacy and perceived responsibility

beliefs. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 397-417.

doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2005.05.003

Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1990). Student differences in self-regulated

learning: Relating grade, sex, and giftedness to self-efficacy and strategy use.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 51-59. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.51

Page 176: SOURCES OF SELF-EFFICACY, SELF-EFFICACY FOR SELF … · LEARNING, AND STUDENT ENGAGEMENT IN ADOLESCENTS WITH ADHD Doctor of Philosophy 2016 Ashley Major Department of Applied Psychology

168

Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Perceptions of efficacy and strategy use

in the self-regulation of learning. In D. H. Schunk, & J. L. Meece (Eds.), Student

perceptions in the classroom. (pp. 185-207). Hillsdale, NJ, England: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self-motivation for

academic attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting.

American Educational Research Journal, 29, 663-676. doi:10.2307/1163261

Zimmerman, B. J., Bonner, S., & Kovach, R. (1996). Developing self-regulated learners:

Beyond achievement to self-efficacy. Washington, DC, US: American

Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/10213-000

Zimmerman, B., & Kitsantas, A. (Eds.). (2007). Reliability and validity of self-efficacy for

learning form (SELF) scores of college students. [Special issue]. Zeitschrift Für

Psychologie/Journal of Psychology: Self-Regulation, 215, 157-163.

doi:10.1027/0044-3409.215.3.157

Zuffianò, A., Alessandri, G., Gerbino, M., Kanacri, B. P. L., Di Giunta, L., Milioni, M., &

Caprara, G. V. (2013). Academic achievement: The unique contribution of self-

efficacy beliefs in self-regulated learning beyond intelligence, personality traits,

and self-esteem. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 158-162.

Zwart, L. M., & Kallemeyn, L. M. (2001). Peer-based coaching for college students with

ADHD and learning disabilities. Journal of Postsecondary Education and

Disability, 15, 1-15. doi: 10.1037/10213-000