slowness of movement in melancholic...

10
Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depression Perminder Sachdev and Ahmad M. Aniss We studied 10 subjects each with melancholic depression evidencing significant motor retarda- tion (RM), Parkinson's disease (PD) with bradykinesia, and normal healthy controls (NC), matched closely for age and gender, on measurements of motor function and depression, and their performance of simple and complex ballistic movements. The simple movements involved the execution of 10% 20 °, and 40 ° angular movements using a methodology adapted from Hallett and Khoshbin (1980). The complex movements involved the performance by the right arm and hand of a squeeze and a flexion movement, both sequentially and simultaneously, using a methodology adopted from Benecke et al (1986, 1987). The RM and PD groups demonstrated a smaller increase in the angular velocity as the angle of the movement increased from 10 ° to 40 ° than did the NC group. Many PD and RM subjects showed multiple electromyographic (EMG) bursts during the ballistic movements. The RM and PD subjects tended to take longer to perform the simultaneous and sequential movements, but nonsignificantly so. The RM group performed the squeeze movement slower when executed as part of the simultaneous movement than when performed as a simple movement. The pause time between the movements when performed sequentially was longer (nonsignificantly) for the RM subjects. Our study demon- strated a disturbance in the execution of simple and complex movements by RM subjects that resembled the disturbance seen in PD. This argues for a common pathophysiological basis for at least some aspects of motor retardation in the two disorders. Reduced dopamine function is one common abnormality that may partially account for these findings. Key Words: Psychomotor retardation, bradykinesia, melancholia, Parkinson's disease, ballistic movements Introduction In the long-standing debate on the classification of depres- sive disorders (Kendell 1968), the syndrome variously called "endogenous," "biological," or "melancholic" de- pression has most consistently been regarded as a categori- cal entity with an imputed biological basis (Mendels and Cochrane 1968; Nelson and Charney 1981). Investigators have attempted to characterize the typical features of this From the Neuropsychiatric Institute. The Prince Henry Hospital. Little Bay. Sydney, Australia. Address reprint requests to Dr. PS Sachdev. Neuropsychiatric Institute. The Prince Henry Hospital. P.O. Box 233. Matraville. NSW 2036. Australia. Received March 29.1993; revised September 22,1993. entity, which we will refer to as melancholia (Davidson et al 1984), but a number of old controversies remain unre- solved. Of the various strategies available to validate the diagnosis, the demonstration of an associated alteration in the physiological state has been considered to be one of the most powerful (Cowen and Wood 199 I). In this paper we attempt to demonstrate a physiological disturbance in me- lancholic patients with motor retardation. The justification for this approach stems from two major observations re- ported in the literature: the importance of psychomotor change in melancholia, and the strong parallel this has with the bradykinesia of Parkinson's disease. The defining features of melancholia are not consistently accepted by all researchers, and concordance between dif- © 1994 Society of Biological Psychiatry 0006-32231941507.00

Upload: others

Post on 14-Aug-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depression

Perminder Sachdev and Ahmad M. Aniss

We studied 10 subjects each with melancholic depression evidencing significant motor retarda- tion (RM), Parkinson's disease (PD) with bradykinesia, and normal healthy controls (NC), matched closely for age and gender, on measurements of motor function and depression, and their performance of simple and complex ballistic movements. The simple movements involved the execution of 10% 20 °, and 40 ° angular movements using a methodology adapted from Hallett and Khoshbin (1980). The complex movements involved the performance by the right arm and hand of a squeeze and a flexion movement, both sequentially and simultaneously, using a methodology adopted from Benecke et al (1986, 1987). The RM and PD groups demonstrated a smaller increase in the angular velocity as the angle of the movement increased from 10 ° to 40 ° than did the NC group. Many PD and RM subjects showed multiple electromyographic (EMG) bursts during the ballistic movements. The RM and PD subjects tended to take longer to perform the simultaneous and sequential movements, but nonsignificantly so. The RM group performed the squeeze movement slower when executed as part of the simultaneous movement than when performed as a simple movement. The pause time between the movements when performed sequentially was longer (nonsignificantly) for the RM subjects. Our study demon- strated a disturbance in the execution of simple and complex movements by RM subjects that resembled the disturbance seen in PD. This argues for a common pathophysiological basis for at least some aspects of motor retardation in the two disorders. Reduced dopamine function is one common abnormality that may partially account for these findings.

Key Words: Psychomotor retardation, bradykinesia, melancholia, Parkinson's disease, ballistic movements

Introduction In the long-standing debate on the classification of depres- sive disorders (Kendell 1968), the syndrome variously called "endogenous," "biological," or "melancholic" de- pression has most consistently been regarded as a categori- cal entity with an imputed biological basis (Mendels and Cochrane 1968; Nelson and Charney 1981). Investigators have attempted to characterize the typical features of this

From the Neuropsychiatric Institute. The Prince Henry Hospital. Little Bay. Sydney, Australia.

Address reprint requests to Dr. PS Sachdev. Neuropsychiatric Institute. The Prince Henry Hospital. P.O. Box 233. Matraville. NSW 2036. Australia.

Received March 29.1993; revised September 22,1993.

entity, which we will refer to as melancholia (Davidson et al 1984), but a number of old controversies remain unre- solved. Of the various strategies available to validate the diagnosis, the demonstration of an associated alteration in the physiological state has been considered to be one of the most powerful (Cowen and Wood 199 I). In this paper we attempt to demonstrate a physiological disturbance in me- lancholic patients with motor retardation. The justification for this approach stems from two major observations re- ported in the literature: the importance of psychomotor change in melancholia, and the strong parallel this has with the bradykinesia of Parkinson's disease.

The defining features of melancholia are not consistently accepted by all researchers, and concordance between dif-

© 1994 Society of Biological Psychiatry 0006-32231941507.00

Page 2: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

254 BIOLPSYCHIATRY P. Sachdev and A.M. Aniss 1994;35:253-262

ferent systems such as DSM-III (American Psychiatric As- sociation 1980), Newcastle (Carney et al 1985) and Michi- gan (Feinberg and Carroll 1982) discriminant indices is modest at best (Davidson et al 1984). Some influential reviews have held "psychomotor retardation" to an impor- tant discriminant variable between the categories of melan- cholic and nonmelancholic depressions (Mendels and Cochnme 1968; Nelson and Chamey 1981). Widl~her (1983) considered retardation to be the core behavioral pat- tern in affective disorders. Recent work by Parker et al (1990) places emphasis on psychomotor change as a core feature of melancholic depression. In spite of this increasing recent interest, descriptions of motor dysfunction in depres- sion have been generally restricted to impressionistic, sub- jective observations, with perhaps a few exceptions (Greden and Carroll 1981; WidlOcher 1983; Parker et al 1990).

Although the slowness of movement in retarded melan- cholia (RM) has been largely ignored, its counterpart (bra- dyldnesia) in Parkinson's disease (PD) has been extensively investigated. The parallel between the two has attracted the attention of neurologists and psychiatrists since the time of Charcot (Marsden 1989a), The RM patient with a stooped posture, expressionless face, reduced spontaneous motor activity, and slowed movement looks uncannily like the parkinsonian patient. Although PD is characterized by bra- dyldnesia, rigidity, tremor, and postural instability, the slowness or loss of ability to move is considered to be the most characteristic and fundamental motor deficit (Marsden 1989b). Van Praag and colleagues (1975) have argued that similar behavioral syndromes in neuropsychiatry may have the same pathophysiology, Neurophysiological techniques that have proved useful in analyzing bradyldnesia in PD may, therefore, be rewarding in RM,

Some disturbances thought to underlie the bradykinesia of PD are demonstrable in the execution of fast simple and complex limb movements (Marsden 1989b). When patients with PD attempt a self-paced, fast movement at one joint, not only is the reaction time increased, but the movement time is increased as well. PD patients do not increase the velocity when they make larger movements as much as normal subjects (Hailett and Khoshbin 1980), PD patients have an added difficulty when they try to execute two movements simultaneously (Benecke et a11986) or sequen- tially (Benecke et al 1987). We argue in this paper that, if there are similarities in the pathophysiological basis of the motor disorder of RM and PD, RM patients should have similar disturbances in the execution of fast limb move- ments.

Methods

Subjects

The subjects comprised patients with retarded melancholia

matched for gender and, as closely as possible, for age. All were right-handed individuals (score > +9 on the modified Annett's hand preference questionnaire (Briggs and Nebes 1975). The following inclusion criteria were used:

1. RM (Retarded Melancholic patients): (1) Satisfy DSM-III-R (American Psychiatric Association 1987) and Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) (Spitzer et al 1978) for melancholic/endogenous major depression. (2) A mood Disorders Unit (MDU) Core Score >16 (indicating high endogeneity). This rating scale of psychomotor function in depression has been devel- oped by Parker et al (I 990) at the Prince Henry Hos- pital, Sydney, and has been demonstrated to have high interrater reliability. The 24-item version was used. (3) A MDU Core Sign rating of ~e2 (i.e,, moder- ate or severe abnormality) on at least four of the following items: facial immobility, postuml slump- ing, delay in cognitive processing, immobility, slowed movements, delay in motor activity, and slowing of speech rate (indicating moderate to severe retardation). (4) Presence of retarda~i.Jn ~1 week. (5) No other neurological or musculoskeletal abnor- malities on examination (as judged by a neurologist) and a normal computed tomography (CT) scan of head. (6) Absence of a medium frequency (4-6 Hz) tremor at rest as demonstrated on accelerometry. (7) On no neuroleptic medication for a minimum of 6 weeks (3 months in case of depot preparation), or lithium carbonate for 2 weeks. (8) Patients free of all medication were preferentially included, but antide- pressants or benzodiazepines therapy did not lead to exclusion. (9) No history of parkinsonism, severe head injury, dementia, epilepsy, alcohol dependence, peripheral neuropathy, neuromuscular disorder, or other significant neurological disease. (10) No first- degree family history of Parkinson's disease, Pro- gressive Supranuclear Palsy, Multiple System Atro- phy, Huntington's disease. (11) Age <70 years. (12) Ability to give informed consent and complete the study.

2. PD (Parkinson's disease patients): (1) Patients diag- nosed as having idiopathic PD by two independent neurologists, and who fulfilled the UK Parldnson's Disease Society Brain Bank Diagnostic Criteria (Rogers et al 1986). (2) Do not meet DSM-III-R or RDC criteria for major depression, dysthymia, or minor depression. (3) Zung Depression Scale (Zung 1965) score ~40. (4) A rating of 2 on at least two of the following Unified Parldnson's Disease Rating Scale (UPDRS) (Falm et al 1987) items: facial ex- pression, posture, gait, body bradykinesia, rapid al- ternating hand movements. (5) Absence of significant

Page 3: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

Slowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262

with the study. (6) Absence of dementia (clinically judged) and no more than mild memory impairment, and Mini-Mental State (Folstein et al 1975) Score -> 25 (if there was doubt, a detailed neuropsychologi- cal evaluation was performed to rule out dementia). (7) Taken no medication on the day of assessment. (8) Ability to give informed consent.

3. NC (normal control subjects): (1) No current diagno- sis on axes I and III of DSM-IH-R and on RDC. (2) No significant current medical or neurological illness, in particular PD. (3) Zung score < 40. (4) No past his- tory of affective disorder that required treatment. (5) Score of "zero" on all items of section Ill (motor examination) of the UPDRS. (6) On no medication.

Patients were administered section I (mentation, behav- ior, and mood) and IU (motor examination) of the UPDRS, and a modified Hoehn and Yahr (1967) staging was per- formed on the PD group.

An MDU core signs rating was undertaken for each sub- ject prior to the experiment. The following tests were also administered:

1. Finger Tapping Test (F i r ) , three 10-sec trials, using the right index finger (Reitan and Davidson 1974). The mean score for the 3 trials was obtained;

2. Purdue Pegboard test, three 30-sec trials using the right hand (Purdue Research Foundation 1948). The mean number of pegs placed correctly was obtained;

3. a Global rating of bradykinesia on a 100-mm ana- logue scale designed for this study.

Investigations were carried out in the morning, after a light breakfast and with no medication on that morning.

Brief Description of the Sample The principal clinical characteristics of the subjects are presented in Table 1. Three RM subjects, who met the criteria for inclusion, were subsequently excluded because of their inability to complete the investigations. A fourth RM subject was excluded because of her inability to gener- ate ballistic movements. One RM subject failed to complete the study in the first instance, but was reexamined after 2 weeks of treatment with imipramine, with successful com- pletion. The RM subjects had been ill for 3 months to 2 years. Four RM subjects were currently (at least 2 weeks) on no psychotropic medication, but one of these had re- ceived electroconvulsive therapy 2 days previously; the remaining six were on the following psychotropic drugs: desipramine 2, imipramine 2, nortriptyline 1, temazepam 1, diazepam 2. No subject had received a neuroleptic drug in the previous 3 months.

The PD subjects had been ill for 3 to 9 years, and were being treated with the following drugs: levodopa 9/10, bro- . ~ , v .~4 . t ; . ~ ~11 fl arrt~rst~tllrt~ 911 fl ho .nThexn l I / 1 0 . tricvclic

antidepressant (small dose) 3110, benzodiazepine 4/10. They rated as stages 2, 2.5, 3, or 4 on the Hoehn and Yahr (1967) scale.

S t u d y I

Simple Ballistic Movements Ten matched retarded melancholic patients, Parkinson's disease patients, and normal controls were studied. These patients were assessed as above by the first investigator (PS). The measurements were performed by AMA who was not informed about the diagnostic category of the sub- ject, although the nature of the disorders precluded com- plete blinding of the investigator.

The technique used was adapted from that previously described by Hallett and Khoshbin (1980). The subject sat in a chair facing an oscilloscope. Movements were made in the horizontal plane by abducting the shoulder to 90 ° and securing it firmly on a manipuladum constructed for this study. This consisted of a steel ann mounted on a wooden frame. The arm rotated freely at a hinge at the level of the elbow into which was incorporated a potentiometer to con- vert the rotation of the elbow into a variable voltage. The other end of the metal arm had a U-shaped metal bar for the subject to grip, which rotated the forearm into supination, so that the biceps remained the chief flexor of the elbow. The oscilloscope screen facing the subject displayed the voltage from the potentiometer as the height of the single beam, which ran rapidly to appear as a line. The graticule was illuminated, with the lowest line corresponding to an elbow angle of 120 °, with further lines above at intervals of 10 °. Subjects were asked to make rapid ("as quickly as possi- ble"), accurate elbow flexion movements beginning at 120 ° , with the line under their control superimposed on the bottom of the graticule. Each movement was made in re- sponse to an auditory signal, which also triggered the recording device. Three movements of differing angular distance were studied (10 °, 20 °, 40°). Only well-practiced movements were studied, and during the course of the ex- periment the subjects were constantly urged to move as rapidly as possible. Electromyographs (EMG) with surface electrodes were recorded from biceps and triceps. The audio-trigger, the EMG activity, and the change in elbow angle were stored on magnetic tape for later analysis.

ANALYSIS OF DATA. The angular velocities of the movements were calculated from the angle and the time taken from onset of movement. The reaction times from the auditory signal to the start of the movements were calcu- lated for the different groups. The reaction times and move- ment velocities for small and large amplitude movements were compared between the patient (RM and PD) groups and NC, using analyses of variance (ANOVA) with Tukey test for multiple comparisons. A repeated measures analysis

Page 4: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

BIOL PSYCHIATRY P. Sachdev and A.M. Aniss 1994:35:253-262

256

Table 1. Some Clinical Characteristics o f the Three Groups: Retarded Melanchol ia (RM),

Parkinson 's Disease (PD), Normal Controls (NC)"

RM PD NC (n = 10) (n = 10) (n = 10)

Age mean (SD) b (years) 61.5 ± 9.3 61.5 -4- 5.6 61.5 +- 7.2 Sex M/F b 515 515 515

Duration of illness (range) 3 mths-2 yrs 3-9 yrs Bradykinesia rating 31.4 4- 15.7 f 41.1 4- 22.21 0 ~ MDU Core score 22.1 4- 11.4/ 12.1 4- 10.2 h 0 ~ F I T - - R 11,22 4- 3.0~S 45.9 4- 9.7 47.4 4- 5.9 ¢

Purdue Pegboard score--R 1 !.7 -+ 2.3 9.3 4" 3,08 13.4 4- 0.9 c UPDR Scale score 6.4 + 3,4 17,5 - 11.9 j 0.5 4- 0,8 d Zung Depression score 55.6 - 10.1~J 33.5 . . . . . . . . . . _ 4,2 h 26.3 4- 5.1"

R = right hand; FIT ffi Finger Tapping Test; MDU [] Mood Disorders Unit Core Scale score (Parker et al 1990); UPDR = Unified

Parklnson's Disease Rating Scale, "See text for explanation of scales and references, a'Mutched variable,

Contrasts using ANOVA with Tukey test for multiple comparisons. (I) RM and PD versus NC "P < 0.05;'lP < 0,01~ 'P < 0,001. (2} RM versus NC /p < 0,001, (3) PD versus NC #p < 0,05; hp < 0,01; Jp < 0,001, (4) RM versus PD sp < 0,001,

of variance was performed between NC and the two patient groups (RM and PD), with velocity at 10 °, 20 °, and 40 ° as the repeated measures factor, using the Wilks' lambda test. The EMG burst patterns for the RM, PD, and NC groups were examined visually. Rank-order correlations were cal- culated between the MDU, UPDRS, Bradykinesia ratings, FTr, and Pegboard scores and the movement times and velocities for the different ballistic movements.

Study 2 Simultaneous and Sequential Limb Movements in Melancholia Ten subjects each of RM, PD, and NC were studied. The experimental procedure was adapted from Benecke et al (1986, 1987), and only a summary is presented here. Sub- jects were seated as in Study I and the angular movement was similarly monitored. A U-shaped metal bar at the end of the manipulandum, with a strain gauge attached to it, was grasped between the thumb and fingers. The different movements to be studied were as follows:

i. single flexion,.: of the elbow through an angle of 20 ° from a starting angle of 120 ° ("flex");

2. single squeezes of the strain gauge bar to exert a force 20% of the maximum voluntary contraction previously determined ("squeeze");

3. both these tasks at the same time ("squeeze and flex");

4. both tasks in sequence ("squeeze then flex").

The subjects were instructed to perform each movement as rapidly as possible, and this instruction was repeated periodically. For the sequential movements (squeeze then flex), the subjects were instructed to perform the squeeze and immediately on its completion, flex the elbow.

The elbow position and force of grip were displayed as two horizontal bars on an oscilloscope screen in front of the patient. After a series of practice trials, I0 single trials of each task were collected. Trials were excluded from analy- sis if they were not simultaneous or sequential, as the case may be, or if the movements were performed in the wrong order. EMG activity was monitored in the biceps and tri- ceps muscles using surface electrodes. The velocity of the elbow movement (electronically derived from the position signal), the force of hand squeeze, and rectified EMG were recorded. Reaction times were measured from the go signal to movement onset, for both "flex and squeeze. For both the simultaneous and sequential tasks, the interval between the onset of the first and second movements (the interonset latency, IOL) was measured. The movement time of the first movement was subtracted from the IOL of the sequential task to give the pause time between the two movements. The times taken for flexion and squeeze movements to be per- formed, independently and as part of the complex simulta- neous movement, were calculated.

ANALYSIS OF DATA. T h e m o v e m e n t times for the "flex," "squeeze," "flex and squeeze," "squeeze then flex," the IOL for both tasks, and the pause time were compared between patient (RM and PD) groups and NC subjects using analysis of variance with Tukey test for mul-

Page 5: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

Slowness of Movement inMelancholia BIOL PSYCHIATRY 257 1994;35:253-262

tiple comparisons. Spearman's test of rank correlations were used to test the relation between scores on FTT, Peg- board test, MDU, UPDRS, Zullg, 8 ~ l e Bradykinesia scores with the parameters of simultaneous and sequential movements.

Results

For the entire sample, the correlations between the different measures of bradykinesia and depression were calculated, along with one-tailed lewds of significance. These results are presented in Table 2. The global rating of bradykinesia had a highly significant positive correlation with the MDU Core score and the UPDRS Section 3 score, and a negative correlation with FTF and Pegboard scores. The MDU Core score also correlated significantly with the Zung depression score and the FTT and Pegboard scores. When the following scores: MDU Core, UPDRS section 3, global bradykinesia, Zung depression, FIT and Pegboard were subjected to a factor analysis to determine if they measured similar or different constructs, two factors emerged. The rotated factor matrix revealed high factor ioadings for Zung (0.93) and MDU Core (0.79) scores on Factor I (interpreted as the Depression factor), and UPDRS (0.92) and global bradykinesia (0.72) scores on Factor 2 (interpreted as the bradykinesia factor). There was a high correlation be- tween the two factors (r = 0.61) indicating a great overlap between the two constrncts.

Study 1 The movement and reaction times, and the angular veloci- ties for the three movements are given in Table 3. The NC subjects significantly increased the angular velocity as the movement angle increased from 10 ° to 20 ° to 40 ° (repeated measures ANOVA, n = 10, df = 2, F = 121.23, p < 0.001), as has been demonstrated previously (Hallett and Marsden 1979). The PD (n --- 10, df = 2, F = 52.86,p <

0.001) and RM (n = 10, df = 2, F = 24.38, p = 0.003) subjects similarly increased the velocity, but not to the same extent. For the 10 ° movement, the mean angular velocity was nonsignificantly lower for the PD group, but no differ- ent for the RM group, when compared with controls. There was no significant difference for the 20 ° angular velocity between the patient groups and the controls, but for the 40 ° angular velocity, RM and PD was significantly slower than the controls (p < 0.05), and the difference between RM and PD was not significant. When the RM group was compared with the NC group using repeated measures ANOVA with angular velocities at 10 °, 20 ° and 40 ° as the repeated mea- sure, the group by angle effect was highly significant (SS = 1410.00, df = 1,2, MS --- 705.00, F = 6.44, p = 0.004). The PD group also differed significantly from the controls (SS = 589.05, df = 1,2, MS = 294.13, F = 3.29, p = .049). The conclusion drawn was that both patient groups failed to show as great an increase in angular velocity as did the NC group, and this was most evident when the angle of movement increased to 40 °. Figure I is a graphic represen- tation of the angular velocities for the three groups for different angles.

NC subjects performed the flexion movements with a biphasic or tdphasic pattern of EMG activity in the agonist and antagonist muscles (Figure 2). Many of the PD subjects showed 3-5 bursts of EMG activity during the movements. The RM subjects showed a similar abnormality in some movements (Figure 2). Because of the poor definition of the onset and offset of the first agonist burst in many cases, enough measurements of its duration could not be obtained for a valid analysis.

Study 2 The results of the simultaneous (squeeze and flex) and se- quential (squeeze then flex) movements are presented in Table 4.

The RM and PD groups were slower to complete the

Table 2. Spearman's Correlation Coefficients Matrix of Scores for the Entire Sample (n - 30) d

MDU UPDRS FTr Pegboard Bradykin Zung Variable score score score score score score

Reaction Simultaneous Sequential

Velocity time Complex excess Movements 10 ° 20 ° 400 (40 °) IOL squeeze, flexion IOL Pause

MDU Core - - 0.32 -0.636 -0.40 a 0.76 c

SCOre

UPDRS 0.32 m -0.02 --0.54 b 0.62 b

(Factor 3) Bradykinesia 0.76 c 0.62 b -'0.50 ~ -0.52 ° - - Zung score 0.69 b 0.04 -0.78 a -0.12 0.48 a

0.69 b 15 -0.00 --0.33 0.35 a 0.30 0.11 0.49 a 0.38 a 0.17

0.04 --0.05 -0.23 -0.20 0.45 a 0.49 a 0.27 0.83 b 0.65 a 0.39

0.48 a -0.06 0.05 -0.16 0.40 ° 0.24 0.22 0.61 a 0.5(P 0.30 0.02 0.01 -0.30 -0.06 0.01 0.01 0.10 0.26 0.17

One-tailed significance, °p < 0.05 bp < 0.01 "p < 0.001. aSome data missing in 3 cases. • The difference in time for complex and simple squeeze and flexion movements, MDU = Mood Disorders Unit Core Score; UPDRS = Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale; ~ : Finger Tapping Test; Bradykin = Bradykinesia Analogue Score: Zung = Zung Depression Rating Scale; IOL = lnteronset Latency; Complex excess = Difference between movement times for complex and simple execution.

Page 6: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

258 BtOL PSYCHIATRY P. Sachdev and A.M. Aniss 1994;35:253-262

Table 3. Some Characteristics of the Simple Angular Movements for the Three Groups: Retarded Melancholia (RM), Parldnson's Disease (PD) and Normal Controls (NC)

RM (n = 10) PD (n = 10) NC (n = 10)

Flexion 10 ° RT (msec) 338.8 + 91.6 352.2.4- 59.7 292.3 _ 139.3 MT (msec) 361.9 - 105.5 423.9 ± 193.5 353.7 ± 75.5

AV (°/see) 30.0 --- 9.5 26.7 + 8,2 29.6 ± 6.8

Flexion 20 ° RT (msec) 331,4 ± 105.7 345.4 ± 119.4 294.8 ± 127.8 MT (msec) 483,3 ± 211.0 449.1 ± 158.1 427.8 ± 98.9

AV (°/see) 48,0 ± 18.1 49.0 ± 14.5 49.4 ± 12.9

Flexion 40 ° RT (msec) 367 ± 156 346.0 ± 112.4 290.3 ± 98.8 MT (msec) 721,9 ± 386.5 c 587.6 ± 190.1 • 470.7 ± 92.9 =

AV (°lsec) 67,6 ± 27,7 ¢ 73.8 ± 20.3 88.7 ± 20.4 b

20°-100 MT (msec) 121,4 ± 163,0 25,2 - 137,2 71,4 ± 76.2 AV (°lsec) 18,0 --. !4,4 22,2 - 12,0 19.8 ± 9.5

40°-100 MT 360,0 ± 329,2 d,! 163,7 ± 81,8 117 ± 67.9 a AV (°/sec) 37,5.4. 22,3 d 47,0 ± 14,5' 59.1 "*" 15.9 ~'

RT - reaction time; MT = movement time; AV = average velocity (degrees per see), Contro~ts using ANOVA with 'rukey test for multiple comparisons. (I) itlVl and PD venus NC ap < 0.1 ; bp < 0,05, (2) ItM venus NC "p < 0,1; dp < 0,05, (3) PD venus NC 'p < 0,1, (4) BUM venus PD sp < 0.01.

simultaneous movements, but the differences from the con- trois were not significant. All subjects tended to initiate the squeeze a little before the flexion movement even when instructed to perform them simultaneously, and the interon- set latencies did not differ between the three groups.

The time taken to perform the flexion and squeeze move. ments as part of the simultaneous movements (complex flexion and squeeze) were compared with the times for the movements when performed alone (simple flexion and

100,

, , | - ~- , -

,~ 40,

• -,4)-,- NO

m , • ,

1'0 Io 40

Anglo (Oegren)

Figure i. Mean angular velocity (degrees per sec) for the 10 °, 20 °, and 40" movements by the Retarded Melancholic (RM), Parkinson's disease (PD) and Normal Control (NC) subjects.

squeeze). The complex squeeze was slower than the simple squeeze in all three groups, but the difference was signifi- cant for only the RM group 07 < 0.05). When the RM group was compared to the NC group by repeated measures ANOVA, using simple and complex squeeze time as the repeated measure, the difference was not significant ((If --- 1, F - 2,26, p -- 0.15). The complex flexion was signifi- cantly slower than simple flexion for all three groups, but RM and PD again did not differ from the controls on re- peated measures multivariate analysis of variance (MAN- OVA).

The movement times for the sequential task were again greater for the RM and PD groups, tending toward a signifi- cant level (ANOVA, p ffi 0.12 and 0.08, respectively) of difference from the NC group. The IOL was marginally greater for the RM 07 < 0.1) and PD 07 < 0.1) groups, with the RM and PD subjects showing a longer pause time that tended to approach statistical significance.

Discussion

The pathophysiology of the slowness of movement in both PD and RM is incompletely understood. As stated in the Introduction, there are similarities in the slowness seen in the two disorders. It is important to decide whether these similarities are superficial, or whether they can withstand a careful analysis of movement in the two disorders. If the latter is true, it has implications for the understanding of the pathogenesis of either disorder. Our PD subjects, who were judged by us to be free of depression, received a mean score

Page 7: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

Slowness o f M o v e m e n t in Melanchol ia BIOL PSYCHIATRY 259 1994;35:253-262

RM PD NC

" f " ~ t " t -

Figure 2. Electromyographi¢ activity (raw and integrated) in agonist (Ag) and antagonist (Ant) muscles during a typical 20 ° fast-flexion movement by an RM, PD, and NC subject.

of 12.1 on the MDU Core rating scale (Parker et al 1990)---a scale constructed to measure the retardation, and to a lesser extent agitation, seen in melancholia--which was lower than that for RM but much higher than the score of zero for the NC group. Conversely, RM subjects rated a mean 6.4 on section 3 of UPDRS, significantly different from the con- trois but not from PD (oneway ANOVA, p = 0.07). A complete concordance of ratings for PD and RM on the two scales was unlikely, as melancholia does not include rigid- ity, resting tremor, or postured instability as its features, and sustained mood change, loss of emotional reactivity, and guilt feelings are not defining features of PD. The parallels between the two disorders must, therefore, be restricted to those features that explain bradykinesia, in so far as this can

be distinguished from the other cardinal features of either disorder.

In our study we have demonstrated that the motor dis- order of RM bears a number of similarities with that of PD. The RM subjects showed an increased movement time for the larger ballistic movements, and did not show the same increase in velocity when increasing the angle of movement from 10 ° to 40 ° as normal subjects. Their ballistic move- ments were often characterised by multiple agonist and antagonist bursts, and some were unable to generate ballis- tic movements at the nadir of the depression. These difficul- ties in ballistic movements cannot be explained simply on the basis of volitional disturbance in RM. The subjects made a satisfactory effort to complete the tasks; the reaction

Table 4. Some Characteristics of the Simultaneous (Squeeze and Flex) and Sequential (Squeeze then Flex) Movements of the Three Groups; Retarded Melancholia (RM), Parkinson's disease (PD), Normal Controls (NC)

i

RM (n = i0) PD (n = 10) HC (n = 10)

Squeeze RT (msee) 236.2,4. 62.0 257.2.4. 122.4 215.8 - 47.2 MT (msex) 386.1 ± 327.0 351.2 "*" 157.3 d 251.7 - 84.8

Flexion 20 o RT (reset) 331.4 ± 105.7 345,4 ± 119.4 294.8 ± 127,8 MT (msee) 483.3 ± 211.0 449.1 --. 158.1 427.8 "¢" 98.9

Squeeze and flex MT (msec) 1129,4 ± 919.9 1049.3 - 615.6 d 710.5 ± 190,3 IOL (msee) 211,3 ± 169.1 163,1 ± 139.7 143.3 ± 88,5

Squeeze then flex MT (msee) 1625 ± 1315.1 1562 ± 857,8 d 1044.3 - 204.7 IOL (msec) 1044,4 4,_ 954.1 c 714.3 - 331.1 d 510.7,4- 186,9

Pause Dt (reset) 562,2 -,+ 668,7 326,4 + 191,8 d 226,2 ± 153,0

Complex a squeeze MT (msex) 470,2 +- 306.1 388.0 ± ! 90,2 305.8 +- 119.8 Complex a flexion MT (msec) 714,9 - 380.9 847.9 - 561.3 d 530.9 ± 100.6 b_

R T : reaction time; MT = movement time; IOL = interonset latency. °Movements when part of simultaneous movement. Contrasts using ANOVA with Tukey test for multiple comparisons, (I) RM and PD versus NC ~p < 0.1, (2) RM versus NC cp < 0.1, (3) PD versus NC dp< 0.01. (4) RM versus PD NS.

Page 8: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

260 BIOL PSYCHIATRY P. Sachdev and A.M. Aniss 1994;35:253-262

times, which may be seen as partial indicators of volition and cooperativeness, even though increased, were not sta- tistically different from the controls; and the movement times for the smaller movements (10 °, and less so 20 °) were not much larger than the NC group. The difficulty was, therefore, in the inability to increase the velocity of move- ment as the angle increased, a disturbance similar to that shown by the PD group.

The RM and PD groups tended to show similar distur- bances in the execution of complex movements. Because the patient groups showed greater variation, the power of the study to detect significant differences was reduced, but the trends were in the predicted direction. The flexion and squeeze movements were slower when performed jointly with another movement, although the RM group differed significantly from the NC only for the difference between complex and simple squeeze times. In performing two movements sequentially, the RM and PD subjects again demonstrated an added difficulty, and a longer pause time, which tended toward significance at the 0.05 level.

We would like to emphasize the great care taken in the selection of subjects, which took over 2 years. The RM subjects were neuroleptic-free, and drug-induced parkin- sonism cannot therefore be considered the basis for the disturbance. No RM subject had muscular rigidity, tremor, or loss ofpostural reflexes, and there was no suspicion of PD in them. Some subjects failed to complete the study or had an inability to generate ballistic movements, that is, the EMG agonist burst did not cease before the completion of the movement. These subjects were excluded, but the in- ability to execute a ballistic movement in RM is worthy of note as it may reflect a disturbance in the execution of a motor plan. The criticism that is often raised against such studies is that the data reflect a lack of attention, or that of interest and motivation, in the RM group. We counter this with the data that the disturbances we report here are spe- cific and not across the board. A future strategy would be to compare depressives with similar motivational difficulties but different degrees of retardation in order to substantiate our findings. Our subject numbers were too small to exam- ine correlations between ratings of bradykinesia and the movement measurements for each group, but when the en- tire group was examined, there were significant correlations with some measures as detailed in Table 2. We do not have an adequate explanation for some of our observations:

i. The RM subjects performed very poorly on the FTT relative to their performance on the Pegboard and their overall bradykinesia ratings. This may relate to the nature of the FIT task.

2. The NC group took longer to perform complex flex- ion and squeeze movements compared to the simple movements (Table 4). This is unlike the previous

One possible explanation is that our subjects were older, and age may be a factor. Our RM subjects demonstrated an added difficulty in switching from simple to complex movements.

We do not intend to argue in this paper that the abnormal- ities we report explain entirely the slowness of movement seen in RM. Like PD, there are many aspects to the slowness of a depressed patient. What we intend to put forward is that the bradykinesia of RM is more than what Wilmanns viewed as "intrapsychic inhibition," or the outward expres- sion of an "unpleasant mood-tone" (Lewis 1934). We tend to agree with the French psychiatrist Widl~her (1983) that motor change in melancholia should not be viewed as sec- ondary to mood change, with retardation being construed as the patient's unwillingness to move because of loss of inter- est or a paralysis of will. Instead, it should be seen as a "core behavioral pattern, not merely a symptom." If this is ac- cepted, then one can move from a global and impressionistic description of motor change in depression toward a more detailed and fine analysis of the patterns of motor distur- bance. Such an approach has been advocated by WidlOcher (1983) and more recently by Parker et al (1990).

At the neurochemical level, the motor disorder in PD correlates best with deficits in DA. Dopamine function in PD is disturbed in both the nigrostriatal and the meso-cor- tico-limbic regions, but the precise pathoanatomic correlate with bradykinesia is uncertain. Early pathological data tended to correlate bradykinesia with cell loss in the nigros- triatal DA system (Hornykiewicz 1979). Bradykinesia is produced in humans and lower primates with lesions of the substantia nigra induced by N-methyl-4-phenyi-1,2,3,6-te- trahydropyridine (MPTP) (Langston et al 1983; Burns et al 1983). It has been suggested by some authors (Javoy-Agid and Agid 1980) that DA deficiency in the mesocortical projections may be the basis of at least some aspects of bradykinesia, for example, defect in initiation and strategy of movement. Still other investigators suggest that DA may, in fact, have a more general homeostatic and regulatory role in the brain. Le Moal and Simon (1991) argue, "it is not yet possible to define the fundamental deficit(s) that character- izes the absence of dopaminergic neurons in terms other than total behavioral disorganization and inability to initiate and adapt: incapacity to evaluate changes in environments, inability to purposefully initiate a motor response, and inca- pacity to perceive goals and organize responses, all distur- bances that can be labelled, broadly speaking, as revealing incentive and motivational disturbances. Paradoxically all these capacities are virtually present and latent, although not expressed anymore after dopaminergic lesions" (p 209).

What is the evidence that DA and basal ganglia function is disturbed in melancholia? Although the neurochemical research in depression has largely focused op ~orepineph-

Page 9: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

Slowness of Movement in Melancholia BIOL PSYCHIATRY 261 1994;35:253-262

portant neurotransmitter in the neuronal circuits associated with mood modulation (Holcomb 1985). Much of the evi- dence comes from the behavioral effects of drugs such as amphetamine, L-dopa, apomorphine, bromocriptinc, alpha- methyl-para-tyrosine, reserpine, and the neumleptics. Many effective antidepressants, for example, nomifensine maprotiline, butriptyline, Irimipramine, iprindole, mianser- ine, etc., are potent inhibitors of 3H-DA uptake and this may indeed be related to their antidepressant property (Randmp and Braestrup 1977). The investigations of the behavioral effects of L-dopa have produced diverse results (Holcomb 1985) with one study from the National Institute of Mental Health (Goodwin et al 1970) reporting improvement mainly in the psychomotor retardation rather than the mood state. Direct DA agonists, such as pifibedil (Post et al 1978) and bromocriptine (Nordin et al 1981), have been reported to have measurable antidepressant effects in some studies. Direct evidence for the role of DA comes from the studies of Van Praag and colleagues, which demonstrated decreased turnover of DA in the cerebrospinal fluid in patients with melancholia and retardation, and this was comparable in magnitude to that reported in parkinsonian patients (Olsson and Roos 1968). DA deficiency is possibly only one of the many neurotransmitter disturbances that occur in depres- sion, but it may be of particular importance in melancholia, especially in the presence of retardation.

Other evidence of basal ganglia dysfimction in depres- sion comes from recent brain imaging studies. Structural imaging studies using magnetic resonance imaging have reported a higher prevalence of white matter and basal ganglia hyperintensities in depressives (Krishnan et al 1988; Coffey et al 1990; Brown et al 1992). Positron emis- sion tomographic studies report reduced metabolism and blood flow to the prefrontal cortex and the caudate nucleus in depression (Baxter et al 1989; Martinot et al 1990; Buchsbaum et al 1986; Hagman et al 1990). Affective dis-

References Albin RL, Young AB, Penny JB (1989): The functional anatomy

of basal ganglia disorders. Trends Neurosci 12:366-375. Ali-Cherif A, Royere ML, Gosset A, Poncet M, Salamon G, Khalil

R (1984): Troubles du comportement et de I'activite mentale apres intoxication oxycarbonee lesions pallidales bilaterales. Rev Neuw1140:401--405.

American Psychiatric Association (1980): Diagnostic and Statisti- cal Manual for Mental Disorders, 3rd ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press.

American Psychiatric Association (1987): Diagnostic andStatisti- cai Manual for Mentai Disorders, 3rd ed. rev. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Press.

Baxter LR, Schwartz JM, Phelps ME, et al (1989): Reduction of prefrontal cortex glucose metabolism common to three types of depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 46:243-250.

Benecke R, Rothweli JC, Dick JPR, Day BL, Marsden CD (1986): Performance of simultaneous movements in patients with Per-

turbance has often been reported in patients with basal ganglia lesions (Ali-Cherif et al 1984; Laplane et al 1989). All these reports suggest that disturbances in the basal gang- lia-frontal lobe circuits are important in the pathogenesis of depression. Laplane et al (1989), in their description of eight patients with bilateral basal ganglia lesions, concluded: "Similarities existed between some symptoms in our pa- tients and certain features of major psychiatric illnesses such as severe depression.., this raises the hypothesis that some aspects of these psychiatric disorders could be related to structural disturbances in the systems linking the frontal associative cortex and the basal ganglia" (p 699). Striatal- thalamocortical circuits are the focus of attention in PD (Albin et al 1989), and commonality of disturbances be- tween PD and RM is therefore not unexpected. DA defi- ciency may well be the common link, transient and revers- ible in the case of RM, and progressive and irreversible in PD.

We conclude, therefore, from our study that the slowness of melancholia possibly has similar pathogenetic mecha- nisms to that of bradykinesia in PD. It would be important to explore further the motor abnormalities in depression with strategies that have been fruitful in PD (Marsden 1989b). Functional neuroimaging and neuropsychological studies of both depression and PD will contribute to our under- standing of the mood, behavioral, and cognitive features that these disorders share. Our study also suggests that dopaminergic mechanisms deserve further investigation in depressive disorders.

The study was supported in part by the Ramaciotti Foundations, the Re. becca Cooper Foundation and the Prince Henry Hospital Centenary Re- search Fund. We are grateful to Professors Gordon Parker, Henry Brodaty and David Burke for comment and Dr Anthony Kuk and Mr Dusan Hadzi. Pavlovi¢ for statistical advice.

Benecke R, Rothwell JC, Dick JPR, Day BL, Marsden CD (1987): Disturbance of sequential movements in patients with Parkin- son's disease. Brain 110:361-379.

Briggs GG, Nebes RD (1975): Patterns of hand preference in a student population. Cortex 11:230--238.

Brown FW, Lewine ILl, Hndgins PA, Risch SC (1992): White matter hyperintensity signals in psychiatric and nonpsychiatric subjects. Am J Psychiatry 149:620-625.

Buchsbaum MS, Wu J, DeLisi LE, et al (1986): Frontal cortex and basal ganglia metabolic rates assessed by positron emission tomography with [tsF]2-deoxyglucose in affective illness. J Affective Disord 10:137-152.

Burns RS, Chiveh CC, Markey SP, Ebert MH, Jacobowitz DM, Kopin IJ (1983): A primate model of parkinsonism: selective destruction of dopaminergic neurons in the pars compacta of the substantia nigra by n-me~Y/~phe_nyl-l;_2,.3,6-tetrahydro-

Page 10: Slowness of Movement in Melancholic Depressione.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/SachdevAniss94.pdfSlowness of Movement in Melancholia ~1ot. PSYCHIATRY 255 ~;35:253-262 with the study. (6)

262 BIOL PSYCHIATRY P. Sachdev and A.M. Aniss 1994:35:253-262

Carney MWP, Roth M, Garside RF (1985): The diagnosis of depressive syndromes and the prediction of ECT response. BrJ Psychiatry 111:659--674.

Coffey CE, Figiel GS, Djang WT (1990): Subcortical hyperinten- sity on magnetic resonance imaging: A comparison of normal and depressed elderly. Am J Psychiatry 147:187-190.

Cowen PJ, Wood AJ (1991): Biological markers of depression. Psychol Med 21:831-836.

Davidson J, Turnbull C, Strickland R, Belyea M (1984): Compara- tive diagnostic criteria for melancholia and endogenous de- pression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 41:506-511.

Fahn S, Elton RL, and members of the UPDRS Development Committee (1987): Unified Parkinson's Disease Rating Scale. In Fahn S, Marsden CD, Goldstein M, Calne CD (eds), Recent Developments in Parkinson's Disease, Vol. II. Florham Park, NJ: Macmillan, pp 153-163.

Feinberg M, Carroll BJ (1982): Separation of subtypes of depres- sion using discriminant analyses. Br J Psychiatry 140:384- 391.

Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975): Mini-Mental State: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatry Res 12:189-198.

Goodwin i~4~, Murphy DL, Brodie HK (1970): L-dopa, catechole- mines, and behavior: A clinical and biochemical study in de- pressed patients. Biol Psychiatry 2:341-366.

Greden .IF, Carroll BJ (1981): Psychomotor function in affective disorders: An overview of new monitoring techniques. Am J Psychiatry 138:1441-1448.

Hagman JO, Buchsbaum MS, Wu JC, Rao S J, Reynolds CA, Blinder BJ (1990): Comparison of regional brain metabolism in bulimia nervosa and affective disorder assessed with posi- tron emission tomography. J Affective Disord 19:153-162.

Hallett M, Marsden CD (1979): Ballistic flexion movements of the human thumb. J Physiol (Lend) 29:33-50.

Hallett M, Khoshbin S (1980): A physiological mechanism of bradykinesia. Brain 103:310-.314.

Hoehn MM, Yahr MD (1967): Parkinsonism: Onset, progression and mortality. Neurology 17:427-432.

Holcomb HH (1985): Perkinsonism and depression: Dopaminer- 8ic mediation of neuropathologic processes in human beings. In Lake CR, Ziegler MG (eds), The Catecholamines in Psychi. atric and Neurologic Disorders. Boston: Butterworths, pp 269-282.

Homykiewicz O (1979): Brain dopamine in Parkinson's disease and other neurological disturbances. In Horn AS, Koff J, Wes- terink BHC (eds), The Neurobiology of Dopamine. New York: Academic Press, pp 633-643.

Javoy-Agid F, Agid Y (1980): Is the mesocortical dopaminergic system involved in Parkinson disease? Neurology 30:1326- 1330.

Kendell RE (1968): The Classification of Depressive Illness. Lon- don: Oxford University Press.

Krishnan KRR, Goli V, Ellinwood EH, France RD, Blazer DG, Nemeroff CB (1988): Leucoencephalopathy in patients diag- nosed as major depression. Biol Psychiatry 23:519-522.

Laplane D, Levasseur M, Pillon B, Dubois B, Baulac M, Mazoyer B, Tran Dinh S, Sette G, Danze F, Baron JC (1989): Obsessive- compuls,'.ve and other behavioural changes with bilateral basal ~an~iia i~inn,m /Im,l;. I I 9.~OO_"/9~

Langston JW, Ballard P, Tetrud JW, Irwin I (1983): Chronic par- kinsonism in humans due to a product of meperidine analogue synthesis. Science 219:979-980.

Le Meal M, Simon H (1991): Mesocorticolimbic dopaminergic network: Functional and regulatory roles. Physiol Rev 71:155- 234.

Lewis A (1934): Melancholia: A clinical survey of depressive states. J Ment Sci 80:277-378.

Marsden CD (1989a): Movement disorders in neuropsychiatry. In Reynolds EH, Trimble MR (eds), The Bridge between Neurol- ogy and Psychiatry. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone, pp 155-156.

Marsden CD (1989b): Slowness of movement in Parkinson's dis- ease. Mov Disord 4(suppl 1):$26-$37.

Martinet J-L, Hardy P, Feline A, et al (1990): Left prefrontal glucose hypometabolism in the depressed state. A confirma. tion. Am J Psychiatry 147:1313-1317.

Mendels J, Cochrane C (1968): The nosology of depression: The endogenous-reactive concept. Am J Psychiatry i 24(suppi): 1- 11.

Nelson JC, Charney DS (1981): The symptoms of major depres. sire illness. Am J Psychiatry ! 38: I-I 3,

Nordin C, Siwers B, Bertilsson L (198 !): Bromocriptine treatment of depressive disorders: Clinical and biochemical effects. Acta Psychiatr Scand 64:25-33.

Olsson R, Ross B-E (1968): Concentrations of 5HIAA and HVA in the cerebrospinal fluid after treatment with probenecid in patients with Parkinson's disease, Nature 219:502-503.

Parker G, Hadzi-Pavlovic D, Boyce P, et al (1990): Classifying depression by mental state signs. BrJ Psychiatry 157:55-65.

Post RM, Gemer RKH, Carmen JS, et al (1978): Effects of a dopamine agonist piribedii in depressed patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry 35:609-615,

Purdue Research Foundation (1948): Examiner's Manual for the Purdue Pegboard. Chicago: Science Research Associates.

Randrup A, Braestrup C (1977): Uptake inhibition of biogenic amines by newer antidepressant drugs: Relevance to the dopa. mine hypothesis of depression. Psychophannacology 53:309- 314.

Reitan RM, Davidson LA (1974): Clinical Neuropsychology: Current Status and Application. New York: Hemisphere.

Rogers D, Lees AJ, Trimble M, Stern GM (1986): Concept of bradyphrenia: A neuropsychological approach. Adv Neurol 45:447-450,

Spitzer RL, Endicott J, Robins E (1978): Research Diagnostic Criteria for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders. New York: New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Van Praag HM, Koff J, Lakke JPWF, Schut T (1975): Dopamine metabolism in depressions, psychoses, and Parkinson's dis- ease: The problem of the specificity of biological variables in behaviour disorders. Psychol Med 5:138-146.

Widl~her DJ (1983): Psychomotor retardation: Clinical, theoreti- cal and psychometric aspects. Psychiatr Clin North Am 6:27- 40.

Zung WKK (1965): A self-rating depression scale. Arch Gen Psy. chiatry 12:63-70.