representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships...

20
This article was downloaded by: [Universitat Politècnica de València] On: 17 October 2014, At: 01:45 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Attachment & Human Development Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rahd20 Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool Ann-Margret Rydell , Gunilla Bohlin & Lisa B Thorell a Uppsala University , Sweden b Department of Psychology , PO Box 1225, S-751 42, Uppsala, Sweden E-mail: Published online: 02 Feb 2007. To cite this article: Ann-Margret Rydell , Gunilla Bohlin & Lisa B Thorell (2005) Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool, Attachment & Human Development, 7:2, 187-204, DOI: 10.1080/14616730500134282 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14616730500134282 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &

Upload: lisa-b

Post on 09-Feb-2017

213 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

This article was downloaded by: [Universitat Politècnica de València]On: 17 October 2014, At: 01:45Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Attachment & Human DevelopmentPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/rahd20

Representations of attachment toparents and shyness as predictors ofchildren's relationships with teachersand peer competence in preschoolAnn-Margret Rydell , Gunilla Bohlin & Lisa B Thorella Uppsala University , Swedenb Department of Psychology , PO Box 1225, S-751 42, Uppsala,Sweden E-mail:Published online: 02 Feb 2007.

To cite this article: Ann-Margret Rydell , Gunilla Bohlin & Lisa B Thorell (2005) Representationsof attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachersand peer competence in preschool, Attachment & Human Development, 7:2, 187-204, DOI:10.1080/14616730500134282

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14616730500134282

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to orarising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &

Page 2: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 3: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

Representations of attachment to parents and shyness aspredictors of children’s relationships with teachers andpeer competence in preschool

ANN-MARGRET RYDELL, GUNILLA BOHLIN, & LISA B. THORELL

Uppsala University, Sweden

AbstractIn a group of 112 children (46% boys), representations of attachment to parents and shyness at age 5were used as predictors of social relationships in preschool at age 6. A Story Completion task was usedto assess attachment representations and shyness was assessed through parent ratings andobservations. Preschool teachers rated the child – teacher relationship and the child’s peer competence.Children with avoidant representations had more conflictual and less close teacher relationships, andshowed less prosocial orientation with peers than did children with secure attachment representations.Children with bizarre-ambivalent representations had somewhat less intimate teacher relationships andless social initiative with peers than did children with secure representations. Shy children had lessclose and less conflictual teacher relationships and somewhat less social initiative with peers than didnon-shy children. There was one marginally significant interaction effect of the quality of attachmentrepresentations and shyness on social relationships.

Keywords: Preschool, attachment, shyness, child – teacher relationships, peer competence

Introduction

The present study investigated representations of attachment to parents and shyness as

predictors of children’s relationships with teachers and their competence with peers in

preschool. It is a commonly held view that, when children enter preschool and school,

harmonious interpersonal relationships in the new context become a marker of healthy

development. The short- and long-term importance of peer acceptance and positive peer

relations has been amply demonstrated through the years (e.g., Asher, Parkhurst, Hymel, &

Williams, 1990; Ladd, Kochenderfer, & Coleman, 1997; Parker & Asher, 1987). During the

precedent 5 – 10 years, researchers have recognised that, in addition to peer relations, the

relationships children form with out-of-home caregivers and teachers are formative for

adaptation (e.g., Birch & Ladd, 1998; Pianta, 1997; Pianta & Steinberg, 1992). Teachers

and caregivers of young children take on responsibilities (e.g., comforting, disciplining, and

assisting in self-help activities) with clearly relationship building qualities (Hamilton &

Howes, 1992). Not surprisingly, the teacher/caregiver – child relationship has been assessed

in terms of attachment quality, or using measures based in attachment theory (e.g.,

Correspondence: Ann-Margret Rydell, Department of Psychology, PO Box 1225, S-751 42 Uppsala, Sweden.

E-mail: [email protected]

Attachment & Human Development,

June 2005; 7(2): 187 – 204

ISSN 1461-6734 print/ISSN 1469-2988 online ª 2005 Taylor & Francis Group Ltd

DOI: 10.1080/14616730500134282

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 4: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

DeMulder, Denham, Schmidt, & Mitchell, 2000; Howes & Hamilton, 1992; Howes,

Matheson, & Hamilton, 1994; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1992; Pianta, 1996).

Two characteristics of the child’s relationship with out-of-home caregivers/teachers point

to its importance. First, it is fairly stable with the same caregiver/teacher and consistency in

relationship quality has also been found across teachers (Birch & Ladd, 1998; Howes &

Hamilton, 1992; Howes, Phillipsen, & Peisner-Feinberg, 2000; Pianta, Steinberg, & Rollins,

1995; Vondra, Shaw, Swearingen, Cohen, & Owens, 1999). Second, relationship quality

seems to be related to the child’s school career and social functioning. Teacher attachment

has been found to predict functioning in preschool both concurrently and prospectively

(Howes, Hamilton, & Matheson, 1994; Howes, Matheson, et al. 1994; Howes & Ritchie,

1999; Mitchell-Copeland, Denham, & DeMulder, 1997). Negative teacher relationships in

particular have been found to be associated with a variety of poor school outcomes (e.g.,

Birch & Ladd 1997, 1998; Hamre & Pianta, 2001; Howes, 2000; Howes et al., 2000; Pianta

et al., 1995). Thus, factors that may predict the quality of the child – teacher relationship are

of considerable interest. Following peer relationship research, we have focused on two

factors: the child’s relational experiences with parents and the child’s shyness.

Attachment to parents and social relationships

The child’s attachment to parents, assessed directly through observations of the child’s

behaviour in standardized lab procedures (e.g., Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978;

Main & Cassidy, 1987) or using representational methods to explore the child’s internal

working models of the attachment relationship (e.g., Bretherton, Ridgeway, & Cassidy,

1990) supposedly has an impact on development. A central prediction from attachment

theory is that the quality of the child’s attachment to parents will be related to the quality of

relationships with other people. With regard to peer relations, these claims have received at

least moderate support (see Schneider, Atkinson, & Tardif, 2001, for a review). In

longitudinal studies relating infant attachment to preschool/school functioning as well as in

concurrent investigations, insecurely attached infants and children, and children with

insecure attachment representations, have shown less competence with peers and been less

accepted by peers than have secure children (Bohlin, Hagekull, & Rydell, 2000; Cohn, 1990

(boys only); Granot & Mayseless, 2001; Sroufe, 1983; Suess, Grossman, & Sroufe, 1992;

Turner, 1991; Verschueren & Marcoen, 1999).

Social initiative and prosocial orientation are two aspects of peer competence found to be

of considerable importance for peer acceptance (Newcomb, Bukowski, & Pattee, 1993;

Rydell, Hagekull, & Bohlin, 1997). Prosocial orientation refers to a propensity to be helpful,

empathic, and generous towards others, and social initiative denotes behaviours that

promote active participation in peer interactions. According to attachment theory, secure

attachment should be associated with both these aspects. Secure children should feel that

the social world is a safer place to explore and also be more prone to empathic and helping

behaviour compared with insecure children (Elicker, Egeland, & Sroufe, 1992). Support

seems somewhat more substantial for links to social initiative than to prosocial orientation

(e.g., Bohlin, Hagekull et al., 2000), but the issue has not been sufficiently studied, the

dimensions have often been included in the same competence measures (e.g., Cohn, 1990;

Verschueren & Marcoen, 1999). Further, in previous studies, associations between types of

insecure attachment or attachment representations and deficits in specific competence or

skills aspects have, in contrast to problem aspects, received little attention.

Turning to teacher relationships, aspects of family relations (e.g., communication patterns

and a sense of relatedness) have been related to child – teacher relations (Lynch & Cicchetti,

188 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 5: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

1992; Page & Bretherton, 2001; Pianta & Nimetz, 1991; Pianta, Nimetz, & Bennett, 1997).

Little concordance has been shown, however, between attachment to mothers and

attachment to caregivers/teachers (DeMulder et al., 2000; Goossens & van IJzendorn,

1990; Howes & Hamilton, 1992), which has been taken to indicate that attachment quality is

a product of the interaction history within a given dyad (Goossens & van IJzendorn, 1990).

When child – teacher relationships have been evaluated with regard to aspects such as

conflicts, dependency, or openness, parent attachment has seemed to play a role. Sroufe,

Fox and Pancake (1983) and Sroufe (1989) performed the first study to assess links between

infant attachment to mothers and relations with preschool and elementary school teachers.

Insecure children had more dependent relationships with preschool teachers and less

positive relationships with elementary school teachers than did secure children. In the

preschool setting, the dependence of insecure/avoidant and insecure/ambivalent children

took somewhat different forms. Ambivalent children seemed to be constantly and openly

contact seeking, while avoidant children were more indirect in their strategies to get the

attention from their teacher. In school, insecure-ambivalent children evoked control but also

nurturing behaviours, whereas insecure-avoidant children received less warmth than did

others and even anger at times. In another prospective study, insecure, particularly avoidant

infants, were found, later on, to be more negative and dependent in relation to teachers, and

less likely to turn to teachers for help than were secure infants (Vondra et al., 1999). Besides

these longitudinal relations, concurrent relations have also been demonstrated between

parent attachment and teacher relationships. Cohn (1990) found that insecure 6-year-old

boys were less liked by teachers than were secure boys of the same age. Turner (1993) found

that, at 4 years of age, insecure children were more help-seeking than were secure children,

but that insecure boys had the most negative teacher relationships, exemplified by less

positive teacher interactions and more opposition to and strong control from teachers. To

date, however, studies relating parent attachment to teacher relationships have been

relatively few.

Shyness and social relationships

A second strong tradition addressing individual differences in children’s social functioning is

found in temperament research. In the present study, we focused on shyness or behavioural

inhibition. Behavioural inhibition is regarded as a biologically based, fairly stable disposition

entailing wariness of the unknown, and leading to withdrawn and non-exploratory

behaviour in the face of novel situations and new people (Fordham & Stevenson-Hinde,

1999; Kagan, Reznick, & Gibbons, 1989; Reznick, Kagan, Snidman, Gersten, Baak, &

Rosenberg, 1986). Shy children have been found to be less involved, active, prosocial, and

accepted in peer interactions than are uninhibited children (e.g., Bohlin, Bengtsgard, &

Andersson, 2000; Fordham & Stevenson-Hinde, 1999; Phillipsen, Bridges, McLemore, &

Saponaro, 1999; Reznick et al., 1986; Rubin, 1993).

Shyness is also relevant in interactions with new adults in that inhibited preschoolers have

been shown to demonstrate wariness with an examiner in a lab situation (Reznick et al.,

1986). Shy children may be ill-prepared to get close to and develop warm, trusting

relationships with teachers. Alternatively, a shy child may take shelter from anxiety-

provoking situations in the new context and perhaps become overly dependent on the

teacher. Withdrawal has been shown to be both concurrently and prospectively associated

with dependent relationships with teachers, although withdrawn children have also been

found to develop healthy teacher relationships over time (Birch & Ladd, 1998; Ladd &

Burgess, 1999). However, the extent to which truly inhibited, shy children participated in

Predictors of children’s preschool relationships 189

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 6: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

these studies is uncertain, therefore very little is known about shy children’s teacher

relations.

The literature has primarily regarded shyness as a negative factor for development, but

shyness could also serve a protective role. Because fear and anxiety are conceived of as

regulators of the motivational approach system, they may play a role in constraining

aggressive behaviours (Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997). Kagan (1994) has suggested that

behavioural inhibition may decrease the risk for antisocial behaviour. Support for this notion

has been found in lower adolescent delinquency rates among boys who were inhibited in

middle childhood compared to those who were non-inhibited (Kerr, Tremblay, Pagani, &

Vitaro, 1997). Similar suggestions have been made by Wangby and coworkers who studied

childhood shyness and adult criminality among women (Wangby, Bergman, & Magnusson,

1999). Following this thinking, shy children should be less likely than their non-shy peers to

get into conflicts with teachers.

The interaction of attachment and shyness

There is an ongoing debate about the relative importance of attachment and temperament

for adaptation (e.g., Vaughn & Bost, 1999). In some models, temperament and family

experiences have been hypothesized to interact in forming development. Interaction effects

of inhibition, or fearfulness, and attachment security have been demonstrated in a few

studies. Especially strong stress reactions were found among insecure inhibited toddlers,

and little internalization of rules was prominent among insecure, fearless toddlers

(Kochanska, 1995; Nachmias, Gunnar, Mangelsdorf, Hornik Parritz, & Buss, 1996). In a

laboratory assessment procedure among 4-year-olds, an interaction between attachment

quality and inhibition was also found regarding heart period (Stevenson-Hinde &

Marshall, 1999). A recent study of the predictive relations from attachment security and

behavioural inhibition/shyness in infancy to peer social competence at school-age showed

an interactive effect, meaning that the effect of attachment was larger for children who had

been inhibited as infants (Bohlin, Hagekull, & Andersson, 2005). Thus, there is some,

although so far limited support for the idea that shyness and attachment security interact

in forming the child’s social relationships. To our knowledge, however, teacher relations

have not been studied from this angle. One might speculate that children who are shy as

well as insecure should be particularly at risk for negative relations with people outside the

family circle.

The present study

The aim of the study was to investigate representations of attachment to parents and

shyness as predictors of child – teacher relationships and competence with peers in

preschool. We also wanted to investigate the interaction between attachment representa-

tions and shyness as predictors of social relationships in preschool. Children with

representations of the relationship as insecure were expected to have less positive teacher

relationships and show less peer competence than were children with secure

representations. Current knowledge does not allow for predictions of associations

between type of insecurity in the representations and specific aspects of teacher relations/

peer competence. Shy children were expected to have less positive relationships in terms

of closeness and intimacy with teachers than were uninhibited children, and shy children,

especially those with insecure attachment representations, were expected to show less

initiative with peers.

190 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 7: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

Method

Participants

Participants were 112 children (46% boys) living in, or in the vicinity of, Uppsala, a Swedish

university town and the country’s fourth largest city. The children are participants in an

ongoing prospective study beginning at age 5 and following 151 children for 4 years. The

sample of 112 children comprises all children with a complete set of the data used in the

present paper. At the time of recruitment, maternal age was M=35 years (SD=5) and

paternal age was M=37 years (SD=7). As might be expected in a university town, parental

education level was high. Fifty-three percent of the mothers and 50% of the fathers held a

college or a university degree, 45% of the mothers and fathers had completed secondary

school or had vocational training, and 2% of the mothers and 5% of the fathers had the 9-

year compulsory school as their only schooling. Most children, 63%, had one sibling, while

9% were the only child in the family, and 28% had two or more siblings.

The recruitment base was parent ratings (79% by mothers, 6% by fathers, and 15% by

both parents jointly) of socio-emotional functioning, including behavioural inhibition for

705 children, 70.5% of a random sample of 1000 children born between January and

September 1993 (M=4 years 10 months, SD=2 months). A sample of 151 children was

drawn and, in order to secure variation in inhibition, we over-sampled at the tails of the

distribution, balancing for sex. Thus, one-third of the sample (50 children) was randomly

recruited from the top 20% ratings and one third was recruited from the bottom 20%. There

was no mean difference in inhibition between the subsample and the present study sample of

112 children, t(135.5) = 7 .47, ns. The present sample was normally distributed, tests for

skewness and curtosis being non-significant, p4 .05 (Tabachnik & Fidell, 1996). The large

sample was positively skewed (more children at the uninhibited tail), p5 .001. There were

no differences in sex distribution, parental age, parental education, or number of siblings

between the study sample and the large sample, all p4 .10.

At the age of 5 years 3 months (SD=1 month) the 151 children visited the department

laboratory. At 6 years (SD=3 months), 124 of the children’s preschool teacher or day-care

provider receiving children in her home (16% of the respondents) completed questionnaires

regarding social functioning and the teacher – child relationship. Day-care providers

receiving children in their home usually care for several children, thus the child is provided

with a group setting roughly comparable to preschool, although the group is generally

smaller in size. Reasons for attrition for the teacher ratings were that the family had moved/

the child did not have out-of-home care (4 children), parents did not consent to contacts

with the child’s preschool (13 children), and questionnaires were not returned despite two

reminders (10 children). At 6 years 6 months (M=6 years 6 months, SD=1 month) 133

children came for a second visit to the laboratory. Six families had moved, four children or

families declined participation, and eight parents claimed having a time shortage.

Procedures

Parent questionnaire. A few months before the child’s fifth birthday, parents received a mailed

questionnaire covering several aspects of the child’s socio-emotional functioning including

shyness. Initial non-responders received two reminders.

Laboratory visit. At age 5, the children made a first visit to the department laboratory, during

which shyness and attachment to parents were assessed. The visit lasted about 1 hour.

During most of the visit, child and tester were alone. Before starting the procedure, the

Predictors of children’s preschool relationships 191

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 8: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

parent and child were informed that the session was going to be videotaped and a signed

consent form was obtained from all parents. The camera was placed in a corner of the room

behind a drapery. At the end of the procedure, the child received a small gift as an

appreciation of his/her participation.

The visit included an Attachment Story Completion Task designed to measure the child’s

representations of attachment to parents; this was an adaptation of the Verschueren,

Marcoen, and Schoefs (1996) procedure. The story stems used by Verschueren and

coworkers build on stories developed by Bretherton and coworkers (1990). In the present

procedure, the child was asked to use a doll family (Mom, Dad, male/female child) and props

(car, toy furniture, lawn) to complete the stories. There was one warm-up story, four stories

from Verschueren and Marcoen (1994), and one separation and one reunion story. The

stories from Verschueren and Marcoen were: child’s bike is stolen by an unfamiliar child;

child gives Mom a present; child, having done something forbidden, says ‘‘I’m sorry Mom’’;

and child wakes up seeing a monster in the bedroom. The monster story is also part of the

Bretherton and coworkers procedure. Because separations and reunions are central in

activating the attachment system, the separation-reunion sequence from Bretherton and

coworkers (1990) was added. In this sequence, which was introduced as the last two stories,

parents were depicted as going away for a few days, the child would remain with grandmother

and, after a couple of days, the parents would return. The procedure was introduced about

halfway through the lab visit, so the child would have time to warm up to the tester.

The idea behind story stems is that the child, by completing attachment-related story

stems, will reveal his/her internal representations of the relationship with the parent(s). The

material is designed to elucidate the extent to which (a) the child sees him/herself as

participating in a secure relationship (feels safe, protected, and valued), (b) parents are

depicted as accessible and supportive, and (c) conflictual and stressful situations are

acknowledged and solved (Cassidy, 1988). The validity of the story stems that were first

developed has been demonstrated through associations with Strange Situation security

classifications, made at 18 months, and through concurrent security assessments, made at

age 3, of observed separations and reunions (Bretherton et al., 1990). There was also

agreement between doll-play classifications in a procedure including a separation-reunion

sequence, and attachment classifications based on behaviour in a reunion episode among 6-

year-olds (Solomon, George, & de Jong, 1995). The security score derived from the

Verschueren and coworkers procedure has been related to security scores based on the

Slough and Greenberg 1990 version of the Separation Anxiety Test, which is another

representational attachment measure, (Verschueren & Marcoen, 1996), and a secure

classification was associated with the child’s positive self representation (Verschueren et al.,

1996). According to attachment theory, the secure child’s internal working model should

include a perception of the self as valuable and competent.

The lab procedure also included a Stranger Encounter Situation (Klein, Plunkett, &

Meisels, 1988; Plunkett, Klein, & Meisels, 1988). Towards the end of the visit, while child

and parent were together in the room, a stranger, blind to the child’s inhibition classification

(see description above), entered the room and tried to make contact with the child in five

consecutive episodes with duration of 30 – 60 seconds. In the first episode, the stranger

entered the room ignoring the child and his/her parent. Over the episodes, the stranger

became more active in trying to relate to the child; before leaving the room, the stranger had

become intrusive.

Teacher questionnaire and second laboratory visit. At age 6, the child’s preschool teacher/day-

care provider completed a mailed questionnaire concerning the child’s social functioning in

192 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 9: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

the preschool setting and the child – teacher relationship. The ratings were made during the

last months of the school year. Approximately 6 months later, the children visited the

department laboratory a second time. This time, an assessment of the child’s verbal ability

was included in the procedure.

Measures

Representations of attachment. The doll story stem completions were transcribed verbatim

from the videotapes and used as the data source for the classification. The four story stems

from Verschueren and Marcoen (1994) were coded by two people not otherwise involved in

the study. One person coded all the data and one person coded the data for 24 children so

that interrater reliability could be computed for 17% of the 145 children who completed the

story stem task. As described above, the 112 children for whom there was a complete set of

the data relevant to the present paper were included in further analyses. The coding was

done in accordance with the coding manual, which had been translated from Dutch to

Swedish. Each story was rated on a 5-point scale for attachment security. Stories receiving a

score of 3 – 5 were considered to reflect representations of attachment as secure and stories

receiving a score of 1 or 2 were considered as reflecting insecure representations. Secure

answers were those that reflected a responsive parent, open interaction, and little hesitation

in responding. There were two types of insecure responses. An ‘‘avoidant’’ score was based

on minimal child – parent interactions, and reluctance to complete the story or to answer

probes. A ‘‘bizarre-ambivalent’’ score reflected negative, hostile, or chaotic interactions with

parents, sometimes alternating with harmonic episodes. Stories receiving a score of 1

contained the most avoidant (only ‘‘don’t know’’ answers or no parent – child interactions at

all) or bizarre-ambivalent (severely disorganized or ending in hostility, injury, destruction, or

death) narratives. Stories with elements of parent – child interactions or less catastrophic and

more coherent content received scores of 2 for avoidant or bizarre-ambivalent representa-

tions, respectively. Reliability amounted to r (24) = .93 for attachment security score across

situations.

The separation and reunion stories were coded by two of the authors (G.B. and A.M.R.).

Reliability estimates were computed on independent codings of 39 children, 27% of the

children who completed the story stem task. We used the 5-point scale from the

Verschueren and Marcoen (1994) system but were inspired also by the Slough, Goyette, and

Greenberg (1988) system for coding the Separation Anxiety Test. In the separation

sequence, secure (3 – 5) narratives contained acknowledgement of the separation, openness

about feelings, for instance sadness or anger, adequate motivation of feelings (should be due

to the separation), and at least some element of coping with the situation, such as doing

things with grandmother. A coding of the narrative as avoidant (1 or 2) was based on lack of

acknowledgement of the separation psychologically or physically, no emotions or

inadequately motivated feelings, and reluctance or refusal to respond. A bizarre-ambivalent

narrative (1 or 2) contained incoherence, negative interactions, or bizarre/catastrophic

events (e.g., accidents, deaths), although positive episodes could exist in the narrative.

Narratives regarding the reunion episode were coded as secure (3 – 5) if a psychological

reunion and at least some positive interaction between child and parents took place. A

coding of the narrative as avoidant (1 or 2) was based on either absent reunion, that is, no

greetings, no interactions, or hesitance (many ‘‘don’t know’’ answers) or refusal to answer

probes. A coding of the narrative as bizarre-ambivalent (1 or 2) indicated negative

interactions or emotions (e.g., ‘‘too bad they came home’’), or bizarre, irrelevant, or

incoherent elements. As was the case with the first four stories, a score of 1 indicated the

Predictors of children’s preschool relationships 193

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 10: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

most insecure narratives. Interrater reliability for the 5-point security rating was r (39) = .91

and r (39) = .88 for the separation and the reunion stories, respectively. Kappa for the

separation story (classification of the narrative as secure, avoidant or bizarre) was .77, and

for the reunion story kappa was .87.

The classification of children followed Verschueren and Marcoen (1994). Scores 4 3

were considered as indicating secure representations, and scores 5 2 were considered as

indicating insecure representations. The classification of each child as having secure,

insecure-avoidant, or insecure-bizarre/ambivalent representations of the attachment

relationship was based on five stories. ‘‘Child gives Mom a present’’ showed little variation

in this sample (72% secure) and was excluded. Because separation-reunion themes are

thought of as particularly potent in activating the child’s attachment system, these two

stories were given most weight (cf., George & Solomon, 2000). Between these two, we gave

the reunion story most weight. The other three stories were used to reach a scoring decision

for disparate separation and reunion sequences. To be scored as having secure

representations, both the separation and reunion had to be scored 5 3. However, one

child whose narrative was secure only in the reunion story was scored as having secure

representations due to secure scores on two of the three other stories. Eighty-eight percent of

the scorings according to these principles were confirmed in the remaining three stories. To

establish type of insecure representations, the separation-reunion stories were again used as

guidelines. In case of disagreement between the two, the scoring followed that of the

majority of the narratives. Type of insecure representation scored from the separation-

reunion stories was confirmed by other stories for 79% of the children with insecure

representations.

Shyness. Shyness was assessed in two ways. Rated shyness was measured with 7 items,

a= .90, from the questionnaire at recruitment. The shyness measure consisted of four items

from the Buss and Plomin (1984) EAS inventory and three items constructed by the

research group (e.g., ‘‘my child becomes reserved upon entering a room full of strangers’’;

see Bohlin, Bengtsgard et al., 2000).

An experienced coder, not involved in the study and blind to the child’s initial inhibition

classification, coded observed shyness from the videotapes of the stranger encounter

situation. A second coder who was not involved in the study independently coded 27

children, 18% of the sample, in order to establish reliability. A global scale based on the

child’s contacts with the stranger, spontaneous utterances, responses, and general

demeanour was used. The scale ranged from 1= ‘‘child is not at all uncomfortable with

the stranger or with the situation’’ to 5= ‘‘child actively avoids all contacts with stranger

(e.g., turns away when spoken to, turns to mother)’’. Intercoder reliability was high, r

(27) = .92. To obtain a multi informant-based shyness measure, the rated shyness scale

score and the observed shyness scale score were standardized and aggregated to form a

shyness scale, a= .65. The correlation between the two scales was r (151) = .49.

The child – teacher relationship. The child – teacher relationship was assessed with a Swedish

version and adaptation of the Student –Teacher Relationship Scale (STRS; Pianta, 1996).

The STRS has well-established psychometric properties and reflects ‘‘teachers’ internal

working models of the relationships they share with students’’ (Pianta & Nimetz, 1991, p.

381). Most of the 5-step items (1= ‘‘does not apply at all’’; 5 = ‘‘applies very well’’) refer to

children’s relationship-relevant behaviours towards teachers. The STRS has three

dimensions: closeness (e.g., ‘‘I share an affectionate, warm relationship with this child’’),

dependency (e.g., ‘‘this child reacts strongly to separations from me’’), and conflict (e.g.,

194 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 11: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

‘‘this child and I always seem to be struggling with each other’’). The STRS was translated

to Swedish, but to reduce the length of the questionnaire, six items that were deemed less

ecologically valid for preschoolers were dropped (e.g., ‘‘I’ve noticed this child copying my

behaviour or ways of doing things’’). Scales were computed as the mean of items. The

closeness scale had eight items, a= .75 and the conflict scale had 10 items, a= .89. Because

of a very low alpha, a= .41, the 4-item dependency scale was dropped from the analyses.

From the closeness scale, we extracted four items, which seemed to reflect security and

closeness on the part of the child and were reminiscent of secure attachment-related

behaviours, and computed a secure behaviour scale. The items were ‘‘if upset, this child will

seek comfort with me’’, ‘‘this child spontaneously shares information about him/herself’’, ‘‘

it is easy to be in tune with what this child feels’’, and ‘‘this child openly shares his/her

feelings and experiences with me’’, a= .69.

Peer competence. Peer competence was assessed with the Social Competence Inventory (SCI;

Rydell et al., 1997). A 5-step response format was used with scale endpoints stated for each

item (1= ‘‘doesn’t apply at all’’; 5 = ‘‘applies very well to my child’’), and scale scores were

computed as the mean of items. The prosocial orientation scale captures the ability to

actively engage in positive peer interactions (e.g., ‘‘usually shares/lends his/her belongings’’,

‘‘often able to find solutions or compromises when involved in a conflict’’). Three items

were added to the original social initiative scale, which taps the ability to initiate and take

part in social interactions (e.g., ‘‘suggests activities and games to play’’, ‘‘leader in play

activities’’). Two items capturing competence with adults were excluded, resulting in a 16-

item prosocial orientation measure, a= .92, and a 9-item social initiative measure, a= .83.

High scores indicate high degrees of prosocial orientation and social initiative, respectively.

Verbal ability. As a measure of the child’s verbal ability, a Swedish translation of the Peabody

Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT; Dunn & Dunn, 1981) was used as a control variable.

There are no Swedish norms, so the child’s raw score, reflecting his/her ceiling, was used.

Statistical analyses

The SAS package was used. Relations between attachment representations, shyness and

outcome variables were tested in an overall two-way GLM MANOVA and in two-way

GLM-ANCOVAs controlling for sex and verbal ability. Planned comparisons between each

of the insecure attachment representation groups and the group with secure attachment

representations were conducted to test differences in outcome variables. Regression analyses

were used to test interaction effects of shyness, using the continuous measure, and

attachment representation security. w2 tests, t-tests, one-way GLM ANOVAs and Pearson

product moment correlations were used to test sex differences and relations between verbal

ability and other variables.

Results

The attachment classification resulted in 60 (54%) children with secure representations, 32

(29%) children with avoidant representations, and 20 (18%) children with bizarre-

ambivalent representations. The distribution of attachment representations did not differ,

w2 = .25, ns, from the distribution for those children (n=33) assessed during the lab

procedure but not included in the present data set, nor was there a difference in shyness, t

(149) = .09. The children with different kinds of attachment representations did not differ in

Predictors of children’s preschool relationships 195

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 12: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

shyness, F (2, 109) = 1.47, ns, or in verbal ability, F (2, 109)= 2.14, ns, but shyness was

negatively related to verbal ability, r (112) = 7 .20, p5 .05. Verbal ability was unrelated to

all other variables, all r 5 .10, ns. The shyness scale was used to form shyness groups. The

shy group comprised children scoring in the upper third of the shyness scale distribution in

the sample of 151 children and the remaining children comprised the non-shy group.

As seen in Table I, there were a few sex differences in the sample. There was a trend

towards a significant sex by attachment representation interaction, such that more girls than

boys had secure representations, w2 = 4.66. The difference was due to more boys having

avoidant representations while the proportion of girls and boys with bizarre/ambivalent

representations was rather similar. There was no sex difference in shyness score or in terms

of classification into shyness groups. Girls were more prosocial in preschool and showed

somewhat more secure behaviour towards the teacher than did boys, while boys had

somewhat higher verbal ability than did girls.

As seen in Table I, the preschool teachers described the children’s relationship with them

as generally close, secure, and non-conflictual, and the children as competent in the

preschool setting. Regarding relations between preschool variables, there were no relations

between closeness or its subscale secure behaviour and conflict, r 5 .10, ns. Prosocial

orientation and social initiative with peers were modestly related, r= .40, p5 .001, and both

these variables were positively related to closeness and secure behaviour, r= .34 to r= .44,

p5 .01. Prosocial orientation was negatively related to conflicts, r= 7 .56, p5 .001,

whereas social initiative was completely unrelated to this variable, r= 7 .00.

Attachment representations and shyness as predictors of social relationships in preschool

The overall MANOVA revealed significant main effects of attachment representation

category F (10, 222) = 1.92, p5 .05, and of shyness, F (5, 111) = 3.47, p5 .01, on the three

teacher relationship variables and the two peer competence variables. The overall interaction

Table I. Descriptive data for all variables (n=112).

PossibleBoys Girls

p for boy/girl

Variable range M(SD) Freq (%) M(SD) Freq (%) difference

Attachment representations 5 .10

Secure 24 (47) 36 (60)

Avoidant 20 (38) 12 (20)

Bizarre/ambivalent 8 (15) 12 (20)

Shyness a 70.01 (.87) 0.02 (.81) ns

Groups: Shy 35 (67) 38 (.63) ns

Non-shy 17 (33) 22 (.37)

Child – teacher relationship

Closeness 1 – 5 3.99 (.71) 4.19 (.66) ns

Secure behaviour 1 – 5 3.99 (.88) 4.26 (.75) 5 .10

Conflicts 1 – 5 1.49 (.65) 1.35 (.69) ns

Peer competence

Prosocial orientation 1 – 5 3.42 (.82) 3.95 (.81) 5 .001

Social initiative 1 – 5 3.97 (.69) 3.82 (.90) ns

Peabody score 76.3 (12.9) 81.4 (15.4) 5 .10

a standardized values

196 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 13: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

effect was not significant, F (10, 222)= 1.26, ns. Consequently, we performed two-way

GLM ANOVAs in order to explore these relationships in detail. Sex and verbal ability were

included as covariates in all analyses.

The child-teacher relationship. There were main effects of type of attachment representation

on all three child – teacher relationship scales. The planned comparisons (see Table II, top)

showed that both groups with insecure representations had lower closeness and secure

behaviour scores, but the difference in closeness reached significance only for the children

with avoidant representations. Children with bizarre-ambivalent representations tended to

behave less securely with the teacher than did children with secure representations. Children

with insecure-avoidant representations also had more conflicts with teachers than did

children with secure representations. There were also main effects of shyness on secure

behaviour and on conflicts (see Table III, top). Shy children showed less secure behaviours

with teachers than did non-shy children, and they also had fewer conflicts. There were no

interaction effects of type of attachment representations and shyness on any aspect of the

child-teacher relationship, t 5 1.20, ns. However, to pursue the issue of interaction effects

further, we performed two regression analyses, one including children with insecure/

avoidant representations and children with secure representations, and one including

children with insecure bizarre/ambivalent representations and children with secure

representations on each of the three child-teacher relationship scales. In these analyses we

used the continuous shyness scale, type of attachment representations, and an interaction

term as predictors. In no case was the interaction significant, b 5 .10.

Peer competence. As seen at the bottom of Table II, the planned contrasts revealed that

children with both types of insecure attachment representations differed from the group with

secure representations in peer competence, although the pattern of associations was

somewhat different. Children with avoidant representations had significantly lower prosocial

orientation scores, and children with bizarre/ambivalent representations had significantly

lower social initiative scores. There was also a close to significant main effect of shyness on

one peer competence aspect. In line with predictions, shy children were somewhat lower in

social initiative than were non-shy children (see Table III, bottom), while there was no

difference in prosocial orientation. As above, there were no significant interaction effects in

the ANOVAs, t 5 .99. However, in the regression analyses using the continuous shyness

measure and including the two groups with insecure representations in separate analyses,

Table II. Least square means and standard error of child – teacher relationship and peer competence and planned

comparisons of children with secure (S, n=60), children with insecure-avoidant (Ins/A, n=32) and children with

insecure-bizarre/ambivalent attachment representations (Ins/BA, n=20).

S Ins/A Ins/BA S 4 Ins/A S 4 Ins/BA

Variable M(SE) M(SE) M(SE) t t

Child – teacher relationship

Closeness 4.20 (.10) 3.87 (.12) 3.94 (.15) 2.10** 1.44

Secure behaviour 4.29 (.11) 3.81 (.14) 3.94 (.18) 2.64*** 1.69*

Conflicts 1.27 (.10) 1.61 (.12) 1.31 (.15) 2.16** 0.22

Peer competence

Prosocial orientation 3.87 (.12) 3.40 (.15) 3.71 (.19) 2.44** 0.72

Social initiative 4.03 (.11) 3.82 (.14) 3.52 (.18) 1.13 2.43**

* p5 .10 **p5 .05 ***p5 .01

Predictors of children’s preschool relationships 197

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 14: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

the interaction effect on social initiative of the secure representations group versus the

insecure avoidant representations group and shyness was marginally significant, b= 7 .23,

p5 .10. The interaction indicated that children with avoidant representations who were

high in shyness had the lowest social initiative scores.

Discussion

The present study confirmed predictions from theory and prior research that representations

of attachment to parents and shyness predict children’s interpersonal relationships in

preschool. Children with insecure representations had less positive teacher relationships and

showed less peer competence than did children with secure representations. Shy children

had less secure teacher relationships and somewhat lower peer competence in terms of social

initiative compared to non-shy children, but they also had less teacher conflicts. There was

only one marginally significant effect of the interaction between the type of attachment

representations and shyness.

Attachment representations and social relationships in preschool

First, the codings of attachment representations need to be commented on. Story stem

measures presumably tap aspects of the child’s internal working models of the relationship

with parents, and thus reflect the child’s attachment organization. Separation-reunion

stories, which were given most weight in the classifications, have been validated in relations to

behavioural assessments of attachment (Bretherton et al., 1990; Solomon et al., 1995), and

the coding scheme we adopted has shown relations to children’s representations of self and to

other representational measures (see Methods). Thus, we feel there is sufficient ground to

consider the children’s responses as reflections of their thinking about attachment themes.

The separation and reunion stories were added to the doll procedure we used. For these

two stories we developed a coding system that relied heavily on Verschueren and Marcoen

(1994). Our adaptation of this coding system has not been validated. However, psychometric

data give credibility to the coding system as a reliable instrument. Intercoder agreement was

good. Further, using this new system, we obtained a distribution of attachment

representation categories (54% with secure representations, 29% with avoidant, and 18%

with bizarre-ambivalent representations) that is similar to those obtained earlier. Verschueren

and Marcoen (1999) and Verschueren and coworkers (1996) found 44 – 53% children with

Table III. Least square means and standard error of child – teacher relationship and peer competence of non-shy

(n=73) and shy children (n=39).

Non-shy children Shy children

Variable M(SE) M(SE) t

Child – teacher relationship

Closeness 4.11 (.10) 3.90 (.11) 1.45

Secure behaviour 4.24 (.10) 3.79 (.13) 2.68***

Conflicts 1.54 (.09) 1.25 (.11) 2.01**

Peer competence

Prosocial orientation 3.61 (.11) 3.71 (.14) 0.55

Social initiative 3.93 (.11) 3.65 (.14) 1.67*

* p5 .10 **p5 .05 ***p5 .01

198 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 15: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

secure, 20 – 30% children with avoidant, and 26 – 29% children with bizarre-ambivalent

representations. Neither is the sex difference found here (more boys with insecure/avoidant

representations) unique. Verschueren and coworkers (1996) also reported more boys with

insecure representations, and more boys than girls with avoidant representations have been

found in middle childhood (Granot & Mayseless, 2001). Sex differences are not found in

infant attachment (e.g., Bohlin, Hagekull, et al., 2000); perhaps boys are at a disadvantage

when verbal assessments of representations are used, especially when doll play is involved.

However, our analyses of group differences controlled for sex and for verbal ability, and thus

the associations found are not artefacts of the proportion of boys and girls with different types

of attachment representations, nor are they an effect of verbal ability.

Children with avoidant representations had more non-close, conflictual teacher relation-

ships and also low levels of prosocial orientation in preschool, compared with children with

secure representations. These results confirm those of other studies in that children who

were avoidant as infants have been shown to have more negative relationships with teachers,

and to be unlikely to turn to teachers for support (Sroufe, 1983, 1989; Vondra et al., 1999).

Avoidant children have also been found to be low in social participation and initiative (e.g.

Bohlin, Hagekull, et al., 2000; Sroufe, 1983; Suess et al., 1992), but particularly

characteristic have been low levels of empathy, prosocial behaviour and cooperation (Suess

et al., 1992; Vondra et al., 1999). In a preschool sample, children with avoidant

representations of the relationship with mother (but not children with avoidant

representations of the relationship with father) had low peer competence using a measure

including prosocial behaviour, but they had not higher levels of withdrawn behaviour

(Verschueren & Marcoen, 1999). Thus, avoidant attachment and avoidant attachment

representations in children have been related to a low social profile as well as to somewhat

antisocial relations and to aggression (e.g., Renken, Egeland, Marvinney, Mangelsdorf, &

Sroufe, 1989) a complicated pattern that parallels reports of both internalizing and

externalizing problem behaviours among children with avoidant representations (Goldberg,

Gotowiec, & Simmons, 1995; Granot & Mayseless, 2001).

Children with bizarre-ambivalent attachment representations did not have more teacher

conflicts or lower levels of prosocial orientation, but bizarre-ambivalent representations were

marginally associated with low levels of secure behaviour with teachers, and significantly

associated with low initiative with peers. Children with bizarre-ambivalent representations

with regard to father but not to mother were rated high in withdrawn/anxious behaviour and

low in a composite measure of peer competence in another study on preschool children

(Verschueren & Marcoen, 1999). Intriguingly, this kind of insecure attachment representa-

tions, which is characterized by hostile, violent, and chaotic interactions mixed with

harmonious episodes, has associations with outcomes also found for children with resistant/

ambivalent attachment assessed with behavioural methods. According to the theory,

resistant-ambivalent attachment could lead to withdrawal and other internalizing behaviours

(e.g., Renken et al., 1989), and resistant-ambivalent infants have been reported to show a

mixture of moodiness, unhappiness, clingy behaviour, and anger/frustration (e.g., Cassidy &

Berlin, 1994; Sroufe, 1983). Compared to secure children, they have been rated less

prosocial but above all lower in social initiative/participation and as socially inept in middle

childhood (Bohlin, Hagekull, et al., 2000; LaFreniere & Sroufe, 1985). In concurrent

assessments in middle childhood, children with ambivalent representations have also been

found to be socially less competent, in measures that included participation and leadership,

than were children with secure representations (Granot & Mayseless, 2001). Speculatively,

children with bizarre-ambivalent representations and the resistant-ambivalent attachment

group, although topographically not particularly similar, may have anxiety, poor behavioural

Predictors of children’s preschool relationships 199

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 16: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

organization and emotional instability in common, making these children prone to

withdrawal. These are merely speculations, but they do point to the importance of trying

to understand the theoretical meaning of new methods to study attachment and attachment

representations in relation to more well-researched attachment categories.

Interestingly, the new subscale from the STRS (see Measurement), designed to capture

attachment-like behaviours of a child who is very confident with the teacher, proved the

most sensitive with respect to discriminating between children with insecure and secure

attachment representations. This result fits well with data that link attachment security more

clearly to child friendships, relationships that are characterized by intimacy and trust, than to

popularity and peer status (Schneider et al., 2001). Perhaps working models of early

relationships will prove more relevant to explaining intimate personal bonds than to

explaining less emotionally charged social relations.

In summary, our results regarding representations of attachment to parents and preschool

relationships support the claim of attachment theory that internal representations of

relationships with primary care givers may set the stage for relationships in other contexts.

The present study also gives some support to speculations (e.g., Cohn, 1990) that avoidant

children are most at risk for poor relationships. The children with avoidant representations

had a wider range of non-optimal teacher relationships and competence deficits than did

children with bizarre-ambivalent representations, and these results are in line with findings

from middle childhood that peer rejection was more frequent among children with avoidant

than with ambivalent representations (Granot & Mayseless, 2001). Our data as well as

results from some prior studies (e.g., Suess et al., 1992; Vondra et al., 1999) suggest that

avoidant attachment may be most strongly associated with conflicts and weak propensities

for prosocial behaviour, while ambivalent-bizarre (and ambivalent-resistant) attachment and

attachment representations may be mainly associated with withdrawal and low initiative.

However, one shortcoming of the present study is that the coding system we used does not

include disorganized representations. The above speculations concerning relations from

specific types of insecurity to specific social competence deficits need to be studied in

designs allowing the coding of disorganized representations, especially since there has often

been cross-classification of avoidant and disorganized infants (Lyons-Ruth, Easterbrooks, &

Davidson Cibelli, 1997). It may also be mentioned that George and Solomon (2000)

consider ‘‘don’t know’’ answers, which in the coding system presently used indicate

avoidant representations, as signs of mental constriction typical of disorganized attachment

representations. Separating out children with disorganized representations would probably

further clarify the picture of how attachment representations and social relationships are

connected.

Shyness and social relationships in preschool

Shy children showed somewhat less social initiative with peers than did non-shy children,

again (e.g., Bohlin, Bengtsgard, et al., 2000; Rubin, 1993) pointing to shy children’s lower

involvement with peers. Shy children also had less intimate and close relationships with

teachers. This is a new finding, since shy children’s teacher relationships have not been

studied before, and demonstrates that these children’s low-keyed social style is not

confined to relationships with peers. Because of the low consistency of our version of the

dependency scale (one item was missing), we did not include dependency in our analyses,

thus a relationship between shyness and dependency, which could be expected (e.g., Birch

& Ladd, 1998; Ladd & Burgess, 1999), remains to be tested. A positive result was that shy

children had less conflictual relationships with teachers, thus confirming suggestions (e.g.,

200 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 17: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

Kagan, 1994) that an inhibited disposition may work as a protective factor against

aggression and conflicts. In sum, shy children seemed to show lower social involvement in

the preschool context than did non-shy children. It should be pointed out that this can not

be understood as a warm-up effect in a new environment, since the ratings were made at

the end of the school year, when the children had acquired at least 7 – 8 months’

experience with peers and teacher. Rather, the effects indicate that shy children may, as

proposed by Rubin (1993), enter a circle of low involvement that could be self-perpetual.

In these analyses, we used categorical data, as advocated by Kagan (1994). This policy

proved fruitful, and our results1 support findings from other studies (e.g., Stevenson-

Hinde & Glover, 1996) that comparing extremely shy children with non-shy children can

reveal effects of shyness that might not be seen in analyses that build on assumptions of a

linear relationship.

One issue was whether shyness and attachment representations interacted in affecting

preschool functioning. There was no interaction effect in the overall MANOVA, but one

marginally significant effect in the expected direction appeared in the regression analyses

using a continuous shyness measure. Shy children with avoidant attachment representations

had the lowest levels of social initiative with peers, which is in line with results form the

recent Bohlin and coworkers study (2005) of stronger effects of attachment on peer

competence for children who were inhibited in infancy. Still, in the present study,

attachment representations and shyness mainly came forward as two independently working

factors affecting children’s interpersonal relationships.

Conclusions

The quality of a child’s attachment representations and shyness stand out as two largely

independent pathways to children’s social relationships in preschool. First, we may

conclude that the same predictors appear relevant for child – teacher relationships as have

previously been identified in peer relation’s research. Second, there seem to be

differences in the patterns of associations between types of insecure attachment

representations and types of relationship problems, an issue that should be pursued

further. Lastly, we found only a marginal interaction between shyness and the quality of

attachment representations, but the effect of the interplay of temperament and

relationship experiences on social functioning should certainly remain on the agenda

for developmental researchers.

Endnote

1 Relations between shyness and the five outcome variables were also tested using the continuous shyness measure.

Controlling for verbal ability, one association was marginally significant, that between shyness and secure

behaviour with teacher, r= 7 18, p5 .10.

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by a grant from the Axel and Margret Ax:son Johnson

Foundation and by the Swedish Mayflower Charitable Foundation for Children

References

Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlsbaum.

Predictors of children’s preschool relationships 201

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 18: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

Asher, S. R., Parkhurst, J. T., Hymel, S., & Williams, G. (1990). Peer rejection and loneliness in childhood. In S. R.

Asher & J. D. Coie (Eds.), Peer rejection in childhood. Cambridge studies in social & emotional development

(pp. 253 – 273). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). The teacher – child relationship and children’s early school adjustment. Journal

of School Psychology, 35, 61 – 79.

Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1998). Children’s interpersonal behaviours and the teacher – child relationship.

Developmental Psychology, 34, 934 – 946.

Bohlin, G., Bengtsgard, K., & Andersson, K. (2000). Social inhibition and overfriendliness as related to

socioemotional functioning in 7 and 8 year old children. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 29, 414 – 423.

Bohlin, G., Hagekull, B., & Andersson, K. (2003). Behavioral inhibition as a precursor of peer social competence in

early school age: The interplay with attachment and non-parental care. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 51, 1 – 19.

Bohlin, G., Hagekull, B., & Rydell, A.-M. (2000). Attachment and social functioning: A longitudinal study from

infancy to middle childhood. Social Development, 9, 24 – 39.

Bretherton, I., Ridgeway, D., & Cassidy, J. (1990). Assessing internal working models of attachment relationships:

An attachment story completion task for 3-year olds. In M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, & E. M. Cummings

(Eds.), Attachment in the preschool years: Theory, research and intervention (pp. 273 – 308). Chicago: University of

Chicago Press.

Buss, A., & Plomin, R. (1984). A temperament theory of personality development. New York: Wiley.

Cassidy, J. (1988). Child –mother attachment and self in six-year-olds. Child Development, 59, 121 – 134.

Cassidy, J., & Berlin, L. (1994). The insecure/ambivalent pattern of attachment: Theory and research. Child

Development, 65, 971 – 991.

Cohn, D. A. (1990). Child –mother attachment of six-year-olds and social competence at school. Child

Development, 61, 152 – 162.

DeMulder, E. K., Denham, S., Schmidt, M., & Mitchell, J. (2000). Q-sort assessment of attachment security

during the preschool years: Links from home to school. Developmental Psychology, 36, 274 – 282.

Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament.

Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633 – 652.

Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test—revised. Manual for forms L and M. Circle

Pines, MN: American Guidance Services.

Elicker, J., Egeland, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992). Predicting peer competence and peer relationships in childhood

from early parent – child relationships. In R. Parke & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Family and peer relationships: Modes of

linkage (pp. 77 – 106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Fordham, K., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1999). Shyness, friendship quality and adjustment during middle childhood.

Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 757 – 768.

Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (1993). Pathways to aggression and social withdrawal: Interactions among

temperament, attachment and regulation. In H. K. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition

and shyness in childhood (pp. 81 – 99). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

George, C., & Solomon, J. (2000). Six-year attachment doll play classification system. Unpublished classification

manual, Mills College, Oakland, CA.

Goldberg, S., Gotowiec, A., & Simmons, R.J. (1995). Infant –mother attachment and behaviour problems in

healthy and chronically ill preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 267 – 282.

Goossens, F. A., & van IJzendorn, M. H. (1990). Quality of infants’ attachments to professional caregivers: Relation

to infant – parent attachment and day-care characteristics. Child Development, 61, 832 – 837.

Granot, D., & Mayseless, O. (2001). Attachment security and adjustment to school in middle childhood.

International Journal of Behavioural Development, 25, 530 – 541.

Hamilton, C. E., & Howes, C. (1992). A comparison of young children’s relationships with mothers and teachers.

In R.C. Pianta (Ed.), Beyond the parent: The role of other adults in children’s lives. New Directions for Child

Development, 57 (pp. 41 – 59). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.

Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacher – child relationships and the trajectory of children’s school

outcomes through eighth grade. Child Development, 72, 625 – 638.

Howes, C. (2000). Socio-emotional classroom climate in child care, child – teacher relationships and children’s

second grade peer relations. Social Development, 9, 191 – 204.

Howes, C., & Hamilton, C. E. (1992). Children’s relationships with child care teachers: Stability and concordance

with parental attachment. Child Development, 63, 867 – 878.

Howes, C., Hamilton, C. E., & Matheson, C. (1994). Children’s relationships with peers: Differential associations

with aspects of the teacher – child relationship. Child Development, 65, 253 – 263.

Howes, C., Matheson, C. C., & Hamilton, C. (1994). Maternal, teacher and child care history correlates of

children’s relationships with peers. Child Development, 65, 264 – 273.

202 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 19: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

Howes, C., Phillipsen, L. C., & Peisner-Feinberg, E. (2000). The consistency of perceived teacher – child

relationships between preschool and kindergarten. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 113 – 132.

Howes, C., & Ritchie, S. (1999). Attachment organizations in children with difficult life circumstances. Development

and Psychopathology, 11, 251 – 268.

Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in human nature. New York: Basic Books.

Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Gibbons, J. (1989). Inhibited and uninhibited types of children. Child Development, 60,

838 – 845.

Kerr, M., Tremblay, R. E., Pagani, L., & Vitaro, F. (1997). Boys’ behavioural inhibition and the risk of later

delinquency. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54, 809 – 816.

Klein, T., Plunkett, J. W., & Meisels, S. J. (1988). Training guide and scoring procedures for the stranger sociability

procedure. Ann Arbor, MI: Center for Human Growth and Development, University of Michigan.

Kochanska, G. (1995). Children’s temperament, mothers’ discipline, and security of attachment: Multiple

pathways to emerging internalization. Child Development, 66, 597 – 615.

Ladd, G. W., & Burgess, K. B. (1999). Charting the relationship trajectories of aggressive, withdrawn, and

aggressive/withdrawn children during early grade school. Child Development, 70, 910 – 929.

Ladd, G. W., Kochenderfer, B. J., & Coleman, C. (1997). Classroom peer acceptance, friendship and victimization:

Distinct relational systems that contribute uniquely to children’s school adjustment? Child Development, 68,

1181 – 1189.

LaFreniere, P. J., & Sroufe, L. A. (1985). Profiles of peer competence in the preschool: Interrelations between

measures, influences of social ecology, and relation to attachment history. Developmental Psychology, 21, 56 – 69.

Lyons-Ruth, K., Easterbrooks, M. A., & Davidson Cibelli, C. (1997). Infant attachment strategies, infant mental

lag, and maternal depressive symptoms: Predictors of internalizing and externalizing problems at age 7.

Developmental Psychology, 33, 681 – 692.

Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1992). Maltreated children’s reports of relatedness to their teachers. In R. C. Pianta

(Ed.), Beyond the parent: The role of other adults in children’s lives. New Directions for Child Development, 57

(pp. 81 – 107). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.

Main, M., & Cassidy, J. (1987). Assessment of child – parent attachment at six years of age. Unpublished scoring

manual, Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkley.

Mitchell-Copeland, J., Denham, S. A., & DeMulder, E. K. (1997). Q-sort assessment of child – teacher attachment

relationships and social competence in preschool. Early Education and Development, 8, 27 – 39.

Nachmias, M., Gunnar, M., Mangelsdorf, S., Hornik Parritz, R., & Buss, K. (1996). Behavioural inhibition and

stress reactivity: The moderating role of attachment security. Child Development, 67, 508 – 522.

Newcomb, A. F., Bukowski, W. M., & Pattee, L. (1993). Children’s peer relations: A meta-analytic review of

popular, rejected, neglected, controversial and average social status. Psychological Bulletin, 113, 99 – 128.

Page, T., & Bretherton, I. (2001). Mother – and father – child attachment themes in the story completions of

preschoolers from post-divorce families: Do they predict relationships with peers and teachers? Attachment and

Human Development, 3, 1 – 29.

Parker, J. G., & Asher, S. R. (1987). Peer relations and later personal adjustment: Are low-accepted children at risk?

Psychological Bulletin, 102, 375 – 389.

Phillipsen, L. C., Bridges, S. K., McLemore, T. G., & Saponaro, L. A. (1999). Perceptions of social behaviour and

peer acceptance in kindergarten. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 14, 68 – 77.

Pianta, R. C. (1996). Manual and scoring guide for the Student –Teacher Relationship Scale. Charlottesville: University

of Virginia.

Pianta, R. C. (1997). Adult – child relationship processes and early schooling. Early Education and Development, 8,

11 – 26.

Pianta, R. C., & Nimetz, S. L. (1991). Relationships between children and teachers: Associations with classroom

and home behaviour. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 12, 379 – 393.

Pianta, R. C., Nimetz, S. L., & Bennett, E. (1997). Mother – child relationships, teacher-child relationships and

school outcomes in preschool and kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 263 – 280.

Pianta, R. C., & Steinberg, M. S. (1992). Teacher – child relationships and the process of adjusting to school. In R.

C. Pianta (Ed.), Beyond the parent: The Role of other adults in children’s lives. New Directions for Child Development,

57 (pp. 61 – 80). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Publishers.

Pianta, R. C., Steinberg, M. S., & Rollins, B. (1995). The first two years of school: Teacher – child relationships and

deflections in children’s classroom adjustment. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 295 – 312.

Plunkett, J. W., Klein, T., & Meisels, S. J. (1988). The relationship of preterm infant –mother attachment to

stranger sociability at 3 years. Infant Behaviour and Development, 11, 83 – 96.

Renken, B., Egeland, B., Marvinney, D., Mangelsdorf, S., & Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Early childhood antecedents of

aggression and passive-withdrawal in early elementary school. Journal of Personality, 57, 257 – 281.

Predictors of children’s preschool relationships 203

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 20: Representations of attachment to parents and shyness as predictors of children's relationships with teachers and peer competence in preschool

Reznick, J. S., Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Gersten, M., Baak, K., & Rosenberg, A. (1986). Inhibited and uninhibited

children: A follow-up study. Child Development, 57, 660 – 680.

Rubin, K. H. (1993). The Waterloo longitudinal project: Correlates and consequences of withdrawal from

childhood to adolescence. In H. K. Rubin & J. B. Asendorpf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, inhibition and shyness in

childhood (pp. 291 – 310). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Rydell, A.-M., Hagekull, B., & Bohlin, G. (1997). Measurement of two social competence aspects in middle

childhood. Developmental Psychology, 33, 824 – 833.

Schneider, B. H., Atkinson, L., & Tardif, C. (2001). Child – parent attachment and children’s peer relations: A

quantitative review. Developmental Psychology, 37, 86 – 100.

Slough, N. M., Goyette, M., & Greenberg, M. T. (1988). Scoring indices for the Seattle version of the Separation

Anxiety Test. University of Washington.

Solomon, J., George, C., & de Jong, A. (1995). Children classified as controlling at age six: Evidence of

disorganized representational strategies and aggression at home and school. Development and Psychopathology, 7,

447 – 463.

Sroufe, L. A. (1983). Infant – caregiver attachment and patterns of adaptation in preschool: The roots of

maladaptation and competence. In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium in Child Psychology, 16, 41 – 81.

Sroufe, L. A. (1989). Relationships, self, and individual adaptation. In A. J. Sameroff & R. N. Emde (Eds.),

Relationship disturbances in early childhood. A developmental approach (pp. 70 – 93). New York: Basic Books.

Sroufe, L. A., Fox, N. E., & Pancake, V. R. (1983). Attachment and dependency in developmental perspective.

Child Development, 54, 1615 – 1627.

Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Glover, A. (1996). Shy girls and boys: A new look. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,

37, 181 – 187.

Stevenson-Hinde, J., & Marshall, P. J. (1999). Behavioural inhibition, heart period and sinus arrhythmia: An

attachment perspective. Child Development, 70, 805 – 816.

Suess, G. J., Grossman, K. E., & Sroufe, L. A. (1992). Effects of infant attachment to mother and father on quality

of adaptation in preschool: From dyadic to individual organization of self. International Journal of Behavioural

Development, 15, 43 – 65.

Tabachnik, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (1996). Using multivariate statistics, (3rd ed.). NY: HarperCollins College

Publishers.

Turner, P. (1991). Relations between attachment, gender, and behaviour with peers in preschool. Child

Development, 62, 1475 – 1488.

Turner, P. (1993). Attachment to mother and behaviour with adults in preschool. British Journal of Developmental

Psychology, 11, 75 – 89.

Vaughn, B. E., & Bost, K. K. (1999). Attachment and temperament. Redundant independent or interacting

influences on interpersonal adaptation and personality development? In J. Cassidy and P. R. Shaver (Eds.),

Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications (pp. 198 – 225). New York: Guilford.

Verschueren, K., & Marcoen, A. (1994). Test med gehechtheids verhalen. classificatiesystem. Aanpassing van der Doll

Stories Completion Task van Cassidy (1986) voor gebruik bih 4.5 tot 6-jarigen. Center for Developmental

Psychology, University of Louvain.

Verschueren, K., & Marcoen, A. (1996, August). Correlates of the overall attachment representation in five-year-

olds. In U. Ziegenhain & T. Jacobsen (Chair), Assessing and validating the inner working models of attachment in

young school children. Symposium conducted at the XIVth Biennial ISSBD Conference, Quebec City, Canada.

Verschueren, K., & Marcoen, A. (1999). Representation of self and socio-emotional competence in kindergartners:

Differential and combined effects of attachment to mother and to father. Child Development, 70, 183 – 201.

Verschueren, K., Marcoen, A., & Schoefs, V. (1996). The internal working model of the self, attachment and

competence in five-year olds. Child Development, 67, 2493 – 2511.

Vondra, J. I., Shaw, D. S., Swearingen, L., Cohen, M., & Owens, E. B. (1999). Early relationship quality from

home to school: A longitudinal study. Early Education and Development, 10, 163 – 190.

Wangby, M., Bergman, L. R., & Magnusson, D. (1999). Development of adjustment problems in girls: What

syndromes emerge? Child Development, 70, 678 – 699.

204 A.-M. Rydell et al.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

itat P

olitè

cnic

a de

Val

ènci

a] a

t 01:

45 1

7 O

ctob

er 2

014