“psst … what do you think?” the relationship between advice prestige, type of advice, and...

14
‘‘Psst ... What Do You Think?’’ The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance Rachel A. Smith & Brittany L. Peterson This study investigates the relationship between classmates seeking out a student for advice (advice prestige) and that student’s academic performance. Students’ conversa- tions could inhibit or encourage their academic performance depending on the conversation’s topic. Specifically, it is hypothesized that as more classmates report asking a student for general advice, then the student would perform less well. In contrast, it is hypothesized that as more classmates report asking a student for class advice, then the student would perform better. Hypotheses (n 139) were supported. Even after controlling for sex and GPA, less general-advice prestige and higher class-advice prestige relates to higher academic performance. Keywords: Networks; Academic Performance; Advice; College Student Interaction; Student Information-Seeking For 20 years, researchers have acknowledged that students’ interactions influence their achievement, independent of course work or instruction (Johnson & Johnson, 1993). Knowledge is not constructed in an individual vacuum, but in the communication and the exchanges embedded in social networks (Haythornthwaite, 2002; Lave & Wenger, 1991). Numerous studies consistently demonstrate that improving students’ ability and motivation to process information leads to greater recall (e.g., Norris & Colman, 1992). It also leads to meaningful cognitive engagement (e.g., Walker, Greene, & Mansell, 2006) in specific disciplines, such as mathematics Rachel Smith (Ph.D., Michigan State, 2003) is an assistant professor in the Department of Communication Arts & Sciences at the Pennsylvania State University. Brittany Peterson (M.A., University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, 2005) is a doctoral student in the Department of Communication Studies at the University of Texas, Austin. The authors would like to thank Wendi Miller for her insights and Dr. Patricia Kearney for her constructive suggestions. Rachel Smith can be contacted at [email protected] ISSN 0363-4523 (print)/ISSN 1479-5795 (online) # 2007 National Communication Association DOI: 10.1080/03634520701364890 Communication Education Vol. 56, No. 3, July 2007, pp. 278 291

Upload: brittany-l

Post on 22-Mar-2017

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

‘‘Psst . . . What Do You Think?’’ TheRelationship between Advice Prestige,Type of Advice, and AcademicPerformanceRachel A. Smith & Brittany L. Peterson

This study investigates the relationship between classmates seeking out a student for

advice (advice prestige) and that student’s academic performance. Students’ conversa-

tions could inhibit or encourage their academic performance depending on the

conversation’s topic. Specifically, it is hypothesized that as more classmates report asking

a student for general advice, then the student would perform less well. In contrast, it is

hypothesized that as more classmates report asking a student for class advice, then the

student would perform better. Hypotheses (n�139) were supported. Even after

controlling for sex and GPA, less general-advice prestige and higher class-advice prestige

relates to higher academic performance.

Keywords: Networks; Academic Performance; Advice; College Student Interaction;

Student Information-Seeking

For 20 years, researchers have acknowledged that students’ interactions influence

their achievement, independent of course work or instruction (Johnson & Johnson,

1993). Knowledge is not constructed in an individual vacuum, but in the

communication and the exchanges embedded in social networks (Haythornthwaite,

2002; Lave & Wenger, 1991). Numerous studies consistently demonstrate that

improving students’ ability and motivation to process information leads to greater

recall (e.g., Norris & Colman, 1992). It also leads to meaningful cognitive engagement

(e.g., Walker, Greene, & Mansell, 2006) in specific disciplines, such as mathematics

Rachel Smith (Ph.D., Michigan State, 2003) is an assistant professor in the Department of Communication Arts

& Sciences at the Pennsylvania State University. Brittany Peterson (M.A., University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee,

2005) is a doctoral student in the Department of Communication Studies at the University of Texas, Austin. The

authors would like to thank Wendi Miller for her insights and Dr. Patricia Kearney for her constructive

suggestions. Rachel Smith can be contacted at [email protected]

ISSN 0363-4523 (print)/ISSN 1479-5795 (online) # 2007 National Communication Association

DOI: 10.1080/03634520701364890

Communication Education

Vol. 56, No. 3, July 2007, pp. 278�291

Page 2: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

(Stevens, Olivarez, & Hamman, 2006), and in overall academic performance (Ortiz,

Hoyos, & Lopez, 2004). Classmates may ask a student for advice about a class because

that student is performing well (e.g., Klein, Lim, Saltz, & Mayer, 2004). It is also

possible that being sought out for class advice helps the student to continue to

process the course material, advantaging that student’s performance.

Sometimes, however, studies find that students’ conversations correspond to

greater satisfaction in a class, but not to their performance (e.g., Baldwin, Bedell, &

Johnson, 1997; Thomas, 2000). One might hope that students seek class-related

advice from their classmates; however, that is not always the case. The effects of being

sought for general advice (i.e., not about the class itself) have not been investigated.

One could argue that serving as a source of general advice may be related to poorer

academic performance. For example, if a student is fielding many questions from

their classmates, then they may not have a chance to concentrate during class. The

purpose of this study is to investigate the relationship between general and class

advice prestige and academic performance in a large classroom. Scholars note that

large classrooms often set up a distance between instructors and students (Kuh,

Schuh, & Whitt, 1991), which should provide a relevant context for advice-seeking

between students.

Literature Review

Scholars suggest that students’ interactions can improve performance in at least two

ways: (a) improving cognitive processing, and (b) creating a favorable climate for

learning (Baldwin et al., 1997). For example, by telling another classmate what the

professor covered in class, the student has an opportunity to further process the

information, even restructuring it within their thoughts (Baldwin et al., 1997).

Repetition and restructuring both improve learning (e.g., Bandura, 1986; Ebbin-

ghaus, 1913; Petty & Cacioppo, 1986).

A favorable climate may also promote learning. For example, a student may

reassure their classmates that they will do well on a quiz, thereby lowering a

classmate’s anxiety enough to allow for better performance (e.g., Webb, 1982).

Previous studies have shown that undergraduates’ networks positively influence their

persistence in undergraduate courses (Thomas, 2000) and their reception of social

support through school (Bogat, Caldwell, Rogosch, & Kriegler, 1985). One form of

social support is sharing information, such as one sees in groups of people facing a

common dilemma or challenge (e.g., Arnold, 2005; Finfgeld, 2000; Muncer, Burrows,

Pleace, Loader, & Nettleton, 2000; Selwyn, 2000; Tardy & Hale, 1998). Social support

may include sharing and supporting emotions as much as providing task-related

information. In fact, research shows that more task and emotionally related

conversations within groups are lia are nked to higher final grades (Yuan, Gay, &

Hembrooke, 2006). One may learn about information sharing and social support in

network research.

Advice Prestige 279

Page 3: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

Networks

Network scholars describe a phenomenon known as the strength of weak ties

(Granovetter, 1973). Granovetter (1973) describes two types of connections in a

network: strong ties, such as those found in close relationships or between friends,

and weak ties associated with distant acquaintances. Granovetter argues (1973) that

strong ties are more integrated, developed, and perceived as closer than weak ties.

Through closeness and frequent interaction, strong ties reinforce beliefs, provide sup-

port required to face life’s challenges, and carry an assumption of reciprocity. During

times of crises, strong ties provide more psychosocial and tangible support than weak

ties (e.g, Granovetter, 1973; Krackhardt, 1992). Granovetter (1973, 1982) explains

that in some circumstances, strong ties may not be as effective as weak ties in achi-

eving a desired outcome. For example, strong ties can dampen exposure to new ideas.

In contrast, weak ties can provide people access to new ideas; weak ties tend to be

more open to new ideas and lack a norm of reciprocity (Granovetter, 1973). Scholars

describe advice networks as connections through which people share information,

tips, ideas, and guidance (e.g., Sparrowe, Liden, Wayne, & Kraimer, 2001). As

students ask each other for advice, they may provide themselves the opportunity to

access new ideas. For these reasons, advice networks, generally made of weak ties, are

provided as examples of the strength of weak ties (Granovetter, 1982). Weak and

strong connections may explain how much and what type of information a person

may access. Another key concept is that of centrality.

Centrality. Scholars suggest that centrality provides people with greater access to a

variety of resources which facilitate success (Brass, 1981; Sparrowe et al., 2001). For

example, students with more sources of advice have less dependency on any single

person (Cook & Emerson, 1978). This affords them a better ability to validate

information, such as their class notes on a lecture, to learn the quirks of different

professors, and generally to stay well informed about the details of a class. According

to Baldwin et al. (1997), students with more extensive advice networks also may be

exposed to a broader range of perspectives, which provide greater access to a larger

quantity and better quality of information. As predicted, Baldwin et al. (1997) found

that students who are more embedded in advice networks report more class

satisfaction and exhibit better individual performance.

Identifying key actors in social networks leads to the development of measures of

centrality (e.g., Wasserman & Faust, 1994). One of those measures is degree centrality,

which refers to the number of links a person has to others within the network

(Freeman, 1979).1 Previous studies find that a higher degree of centrality relates

positively with performance, such as higher assignment and test scores (Baldwin et

al., 1997), as well as task mastery and role clarity (Morrison, 2002).

Prestige centrality. Let us consider an example of an advice-seeking network. One

student, John, reports that he has obtained advice from Sarah, but Sarah may not

report that she has received advice from John. These connections are called

280 R. A. Smith & B. L. Peterson

Page 4: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

directional (Wasserman & Faust, 1994). Directional ties allow for the calculation of

two different types of degree centrality. A student’s prestige (in-degree, Knoke & Burt,

1983) indicates the number of classmates who claimed a connection to them, whereas

reach (out-degree) indicates the number of classmates with whom the student

claimed connections.

Directional estimates allow one to distinguish between (a) influence, a student

generating connections to others, and (b) support, students receiving connections

from others (Batagelj, 1993). In contrast, without this directional information, one

may estimate centrality but not know who generated the relationship or if it is

symmetrical. Scholars argue that reach centrality suffers more than prestige centrality

from the limitations of self-reports (Sparrowe et al., 2001). For this reason, this study

focuses only on prestige centrality.

Prestige derives from an assumption that prominent actors are generally the object

of relational ties (Knoke & Burt, 1983), but they may reach out to few people. As

more classmates may ask a student for advice, they provide them high prestige

centrality in the advice network. When a classmate seeks out students for advice, the

students gain a chance to reconsider, reframe, or elaborate on their existing

knowledge as they provide advice. A student may learn simply by verbalizing. For

example, in problem-solving activities, the act of verbalizing one’s thoughts aloud

facilitated students’ learning of thinking skills (Bandura, 1986, 1997). It is possible,

then, that discussion in and of itself facilitates academic performance.

Advice and Academic Performance

Although most scholarship on peer interactions is focused on its ability to improve

performance, empirical findings are mixed. In previous research, greater prestige

accounted for students earning more points in a course (Russo & Koesten, 2005). In

other studies, prestige centrality corresponded to persistence to stay in classes, but did

not relate to students’ year-end grade point average (GPA; Thomas, 2000). In a

related context, prestige centrality in advice networks has been related to better job

performance (Sparrowe et al., 2001).

Types of advice. Classmates may ask each other for advice about a class, which should

improve their academic performance. It is also possible that classmates may ask each

other for advice about things that have nothing to do with the class. For example,

friends often share values, habits, and other characteristics, and bond through mutual

reinforcement of those similarities (Wasserman & Faust, 1994). Friendships can

suppress successful learning, because the friendships lock students away from

developing new relationships and being exposed to new ideas (Cho, Lee, Stefanone,

& Gay, 2005; Granovetter, 1973). Too much intimacy can create barriers to new ideas

and constructive critiques (Wenger, McDermott, & Snyder, 2002). Limiting access to

different views and critiques may inhibit academic performance; indeed, some

scholars believe that students must struggle with a variety of opposing viewpoints on

the same idea in order to construct knowledge (Perkins, 1991). Researchers propose

Advice Prestige 281

Page 5: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

that in-class discussions may compensate for the rigid homogeneity in friendship

networks (McDevitt & Kiousis, 2006).

Friendships take time and energy, and people have limits as to how many strong,

interconnected relationships they can maintain (Burt, 1992; Scott, 1991). The time

and energy needs of friendships and memberships in social groups may compete with

the effort it takes to perform academically.

Friends may talk about issues pertaining to bonding, such as ‘‘did you see what

Samantha was wearing?’’ Classmates may seek general advice, especially from friends,

about issues pertaining to relationships instead of the class. Studies show that advice

sought from acquaintances more than from friends led to the reception of useful

knowledge and contributed more to project outcomes (Levin & Cross, 2004). In

addition, although centrality in friendship networks corresponded to greater

satisfaction in a class, centrality was uncorrelated with individual performance

(Baldwin et al., 1997).

The type of advice sought from classmates, then, may explain why sometimes scho-

lars find that having advice prestige leads to higher performance and sometimes it does

not. When classmates seek advice about the class from a student, that student should

show higher academic performance. When classmates seek general advice from a

student, these questions may serve to distract the student from learning, so they would

show lower academic performance. Therefore, the following hypotheses are offered:

H1: As students’ prestige centrality due to class advice rises, they should exhibithigher academic performance.

H2: As students’ prestige centrality due to general advice rises, they should exhibitlower academic performance.

Method

Participants

The sample consisted of 139 students enrolled in an upper-level, large lecture course

at a large southwestern university. The students (70% women and 30% men) were

21 years old (M�21.11, SD�3.56). Most students self-identified as White (70%),

followed by Hispanic (13%), Asian (12%), African American (3%), and Middle

Eastern (2%). On average, students had a 3.26 grade point average (SD�.40) on a

4-point scale.

Procedures

The course met for 15 weeks. During the sixth week, students were asked to

participate in an online survey for another study. Within this online survey, students

were shown the roster of classmates and asked to mark anyone from whom they

sought class advice and from whom they sought general advice as well as other

information.2 After the course concluded, information about students’ academic

performance was gathered from the course’s instructor.

282 R. A. Smith & B. L. Peterson

Page 6: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

Measurement

Classroom performance. In this course, students completed two quizzes and three

short papers. These scores were summed into one score to index their academic

performance (M�86.15, SD�5.37, a�.61).

Network indicators. In order to gather network indicators, students were shown the

entire roster of students (e.g., Erickson, Nosanchuk, & Lee, 1981 who find that rosters

of 150 names or less function well). Students were then asked to indicate what type of

advice, if any, they solicited from each classmate listed. Specifically, they could mark

next to a classmate’s name if they agreed that, yes, ‘‘I have gotten advice about this

class from this person.’’ Additionally, students could mark in a different place next to

a classmate’s name if they agreed that, yes, ‘‘I have gotten general advice (not about

the class) from this person.’’ The roster of names appeared in the rows, and the two

questions appeared at the top of each column. Students could place a check mark in

each column for any given classmate, indicating that they received class advice and/or

general advice from the same classmate.

A student’s prestige (in-degree) centrality indicates the number of classmates who

claimed a connection to them. On average, students reported seeking class advice

from one student (M� .92, SD�1.26, Maximum�6). They reported seeking

general advice from two students (M�2.44, SD�3.76, Maximum�18). These

nominations are used to calculate a given student’s prestige in each network. On

average, students were nominated by one classmate as a source of class advice (M�.92, SD�1.08, Maximum�4). Students were nominated by two classmates as a

source of general advice (M�2.44, SD�2.90, Maximum�12).

Two estimates were calculated for each student’s prestige in class advice and general

advice networks using Freeman’s (1979) definition in UCINET 6.0 for Windows

spreadsheet (Borgatti, Everett, & Freeman, 2002). This program allows one to

calculate estimates that have been standardized for the maximum possible number of

connections (Wasserman & Faust, 1994).3 Descriptive statistics and correlations

among all variables can be seen in Table 1.

Table 1 Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among Variables

Variable M SD 1 2 3 4

1. Sex 1.70 .462. GPA 3.26 .40 .063. Class-advice Prestige .92 1.08 .02 .004. General-advice Prestige 2.43 2.90 �.06 �.05 .73*5. Academic Performance 86.15 5.34 .17* .38* .11 �.06

Note : N�139.*pB .05.

Advice Prestige 283

Page 7: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

Results

Descriptive Statistics

On average, students earned 86.15 out of 100 points (SD�5.37) on the class

assignments, equivalent to a ‘‘B’’ on the course’s grading scale. Of 139 students, half

had at least one classmate report that they sought them out for general advice (51%,

n�71). The other half (49%, n�68) had not been reported as sources of general

advice (49%, n�68). Slightly more than half (55%, n�76) had at least one classmate

report that they sought them out for advice specifically pertaining to the class. The

other students had not been reported as sources of class advice (45%, n�63).

The largest proportion of students received nominations as sources of general

advice and class advice (44%, n�63) followed by those who were not nominated at

all (38%, n�53). Smaller proportions of students were nominated as a source of class

advice, but not a source of general advice (11%, n�15) or a source of general but not

class advice (7%, n�10).

Hypothesis Testing

H1 proposed that serving as a source of class advice would correspond to a higher

academic performance. H2 proposed that serving as a source of general advice would

correspond to a lower academic performance. The independent effects of student sex,

GPA, class-advice prestige, and general-advice prestige were regressed onto students’

academic performance. The regression was statistically significant, F(4, 134)�8.82,

p B.05, R2�.21. Table 2 provides a summary of the regression analysis.3 Upon

inspection of the beta weights, all variables except for sex were significant at p B.05.

Students with higher GPAs, more class-advice prestige, and less general-advice

prestige showed better performance. H1 and H2 received support.

We checked for a possible interaction between serving as a source of class advice

and general advice and academic performance. Dummy variables were created for

both types of advice and run in an ANOVA after controlling for students’ sex and

GPA. The entire model was statistically significant, F(5, 133)�8.23, p B.001, R2�.24. Figure 1 shows the estimated means of academic performance based on serving as

Table 2 Summary of Regression Analysis for Variables Predicting Academic

Performance

Variable B SE B b

Student Sex 1.51 .90 .13$

GPA 4.81 1.02 .36*Class-advice Prestige 1.42 .55 .29*General-advice Prestige �.45 .21 �.24*

Note : F(4, 134)�8.82, pB .05, R2�21.*p B.05.$p B.10.

284 R. A. Smith & B. L. Peterson

Page 8: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

a source of general advice and/or class advice, after controlling for sex and GPA. The

interaction was not statistically significant, F(1, 133)�.90, ns , h2�.01.4 This

nonsignificant interaction suggests that the prestige effects for class advice and

general advice are additive, rather than interactive.

Discussion

Previous studies have shown both positive and negative effects of interstudent advice-

seeking on academic performance. This study attempted to explain the conflicting

results by examining types of advice. As predicted, when students were sought by

more of their classmates for advice about the class, the students exhibited a higher

academic performance. In contrast, students who were sought by more classmates for

general advice exhibited a lower performance. The effects appeared after controlling

for students’ GPAs. In addition, the effects seem to be additive, instead of interactive.

These results coincide with a phenomenon known as the strength of weak ties

(Granovetter, 1973). That is, in some circumstances, strong ties may not be as

effective for some types of outcomes as weak ties. Strong ties, because they dampen

new ideas and focus on relational functions, tend to inhibit performance. Baldwin

and colleagues (1997) suggested that friendships should improve a student’s direct

access to late-breaking information about a class because friends would seek each

80

81

82

83

84

85

86

87

88

89

90

No Yes

Sought out for Class-Advice

Est

imat

ed M

argi

nal M

eans

No

Yes

Sought outfor General-

Advice

Figure 1. Estimated marginal means of academic performance based on class and general

advice prestige.

Advice Prestige 285

Page 9: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

other out to transmit such important information. However, class friendships have

not shown positive effects on performance (Baldwin et al., 1997). Taking pre-existing

friendships into the classroom and maintaining many memberships in campus

groups may provide constructive social support to survive college, but they also may

be an academic liability (Cho et al., 2005).

In contrast, weak ties facilitate performance, because they provide greater access to

new ideas and fewer distracting social obligations. In previous research, advice sought

from acquaintances, in contrast to friends, has been shown to contribute to better

knowledge and project outcomes (Levin & Cross, 2004). Moreover, as students feel

more willingness to communicate with classroom acquaintances, they also explore

additional social ties (Cho, Gay, Davidson, & Ingraffea, in press).

This study, however, did not ask students to qualify if they sought advice from

acquaintances or friends. Advice and friendship networks may blur expressive and

instrumental goals, and although both ties predicted information-seeking in previous

studies (Chia, Foo, & Fang, 2006), this study did have the information to discriminate

between the tie’s function (expressive) and the tie’s label (friendship). Future research

should include both types of networks to further understand the dialectical tension

between garnering social support and new ideas and their impacts on academic

performance and remaining in school.

The explanations provided so far focus primarily on how networks may influence

performance. It is also possible that classmates select students who perform well for

class advice, and those with street smarts for general advice. Prior studies have shown

that people who want to improve their competence in course-related tasks pursue

opportunities to acquire knowledge and to perfect skills (Yi & Hwang, 2003). Social

exchange theory (e.g., Blau, 1964; Molm & Cook, 1995) suggests that people seek

relationships out of self-interest. There is evidence that people may select advisors

who exhibit particular qualities. For example, people with a higher activity preference

(or strong work ethic) hold more central positions in advice networks (Klein, Lim,

Saltz, & Mayer, 2004). These relationships deserve further theoretical explanation and

empirical inquiry.

Limitations

A few issues limit this study’s results, including sampling, content, timing, and

personal characteristics. This network analysis relies on the dynamics within one

class. It is possible that the network and its characteristics developed uniquely within

this class, perhaps due to their teacher. Previous research (e.g., Myers & Knox, 2001)

shows that students’ reported information-seeking practices vary by perceptions of

their teachers. The class studied herein covered communication content. Although we

predict that the relationships will hold in classes covering other material with other

teachers, and only the mean levels of network size and/or advice seeking practice will

vary, this remains an empirical question. Future research may benefit from testing

these predictions with students majoring in other topics, in courses covering other

issues.

286 R. A. Smith & B. L. Peterson

Page 10: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

Next, this study looked at students’ networks at one point in the class, which was

late enough into the semester for networks to have developed. Networks, however,

can play different roles in different stages (Adler & Kwon, 2002; Earley & Mosakowski,

2000). Future research would benefit from longitudinal tracking of these networks

and their influence on classroom performance. In addition, individual attributes

impact evolution of structural relationships (e.g., Salancik, 1995), and deserve further

inquiry. Last, and most critically, this study did not gather information on study

practices, which may very well (as one might hope) mediate or moderate the

relationships between communication networks and educational performance. It is

possible that class and general advice prestige served as proxies for time on task.5

Future research into advice networks and academic performance would benefit from

including distraction and time on task to further understand these relationships.

The findings do not seem to be limited by culture; however, there is reason to

suspect cultural differences. For example, Asian American students are less likely to

seek support from friends in times of stress (Oliver, Reed, Katz, & Haugh, 1999).

Although our findings did not show differences by ethnicity, sex differences existed.

Future research should continue to monitor how culture and/or sex may mediate or

moderate these relationships.

The implications for understanding the connection between networks and

education extend outside the classroom. In a study of deliberative learning, students

with greater participation in in-class discussions and debates about politics had more

discussions with parents and friends about politics and larger discussion networks

outside the classroom (McDevitt & Kiousis, 2006). These discussions had impacts on

students’ learning. In-class networks may affect students’ participation in class and

the diffusion of such involvement to friendship and family networks afterwards.

Future research should investigate these findings with different networks before

implementing new programs. Scholars (e.g., Easton, 2003; Vess, 2005) question

whether online course tools can replace in-class participation. Even before these

programs can be evaluated, other extensions are already underway. For example,

some schools, like the University of Alabama, are looking to extend the impact of

students’ online opportunities to create an online networking site for parents of first-

year students (‘‘Alabama creates parents’ facebook,’’ 2006). Unfortunately, studies find

that parent networks, that is, parents spending more time with other students’

parents, negatively influenced student performance (e.g., John, 2005). This is a

concern because web-based portals, such as UPeers , already provide parents of college

students’ opportunities to network. Future research also should consider the impact

of adversarial networks, which have shown negative relationships to course

satisfaction (Baldwin et al., 1997).

This work carries implications for the design of online courses, where technology

may provide the opportunity to seek advice from other classmates. Vess (2005) found

that asynchronous online discussions enhanced students’ involvement and elabora-

tion on course materials. Just because the technology can allow for seeking, sharing,

and distributing advice does not mean it automatically occurs (Yuan et al., 2006). In a

discussion board, for example, a few students may seek advice and receive advice

Advice Prestige 287

Page 11: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

from a few classmates while most other students lurk *read messages sent between

those two actors without ever contributing themselves. This type of communication

behavior may lead to the network’s collapse (Kollock & Smith, 1999). It also may

complicate the analysis of networks, as those lurkers may not list that they are

receiving advice. Many of these considerations for generating an effective online

environment have been raised (Sandars, 2005; Vess, 2005) and deserve research

efforts.

Finally, this research may provide further import for investigating the role of cell

phones within college classrooms. Cell phones provide another method to maintain

social networks. Cell phones in class are no longer uncommon and could benefit

students by providing them access to support and resources or distract them (for a

review, see Campbell, 2006). Campbell (2006) found that college students and their

professors strongly felt that cell phones ringing within a class were distracting.

Policies to ban cell phones from class rooms altogether are under development

(Campbell, 2006), but perhaps further research should distinguish between the

contact and content before blanket decisions are made.

Conclusion

These findings present a cautionary tale. When students receive advice requests from

another classmate, they may do well to consider if the advice pertains to the class or

not. On the other hand, students who make themselves available to answer a

classmate’s questions about the class may perform better than if they avoided such

conversations. The concern has been raised, and deserves further elaboration, on

when, and if, communities and the communication within them become an ‘‘ideal

structure for avoiding learning’’ (Wenger et al., 2002, p. 141).

Notes

[1] Although other estimates for centrality, such as betweenness and closeness (Wasserman &

Faust, 1994), are available, degree centrality is robust to isolates in a network, those people

who have no ties to anyone. In addition, it represents a stress of more people.

[2] The course’s instructor did not see information about the students’ networks.

[3] Students’ advice prestige estimates were highly correlated, r (137)�.73, p B.05. According to

Kline (1998), multicolinearity is present when the correlation between two independent

variables is greater than .85 so both estimates were retained in this analysis.

[4] No differences appeared by race, F B1, or age, r (137)�.02, ns . In addition, a second

ANOVA was computed with sex as a factor to test for interactions between sex and the advice

effects. The interactions between sex and serving as a source of advice were not statistically

significant.

[5] Thank you to an anonymous reviewer for this insight.

References

Adler, P. S., & Kwon, S. (2002). Social capital: Prospects for a new concept. Academy of Management

Review, 27 , 17�40.

288 R. A. Smith & B. L. Peterson

Page 12: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

Alabama creates parents’ facebook to boost freshman persistence. (2006). Recruitment & Retention

in Higher Education , 20 , 3.

Arnold, L. B. (2005). Don’t you know what causes that? Advice, celebration, and justification in a

large families bulletin board. Communication Studies , 56 , 331�351.

Baldwin, T. T., Bedell, M. D., & Johnson, J. L. (1997). The social fabric of a team-based M.B.A.

program: Network effects on student satisfaction and performance. Academy of Management

Journal , 40 , 1369�1397.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood

Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control . New York: Freeman.

Batagelj, V. (1993). Centrality in social networks. In A. Ferligoj & A. Kramberger (Eds.),

Developments in statistics and methodology (pp. 129�138). Ljubljana, Slovenia: University

of Ljubljana.

Blau, P. M. (1964). Exchange and power in social life . New York: Wiley.

Bogat, G., Caldwell, R. A., Rogosch, F. A., & Kriegler, J. A. (1985). Differentiating specialists and

generalists within college students’ support networks. Journal of Youth and Adolescence , 14 ,

23�35.

Borgatti, S. P., Everett, M. G., & Freeman, L. C. (2002). UCINET for Windows 6: Software for social

network analysis . Harvard, MA: Analytic Technologies.

Brass, D. J. (1981). Structural relationships, job characteristics, and worker satisfaction and

performance. Administrative Science Quarterly, 26 , 331�348.

Burt, R. S. (1992). Structural holes: The social structure of competition . Cambridge, MA: Harvard

Press.

Campbell, S. W. (2006). Perceptions of mobile phones in college classrooms: Ringing, cheating, and

classroom policies. Communication Education , 55 , 280�294.

Chia, H. B., Foo, M. D., & Fang, R. L. (2006). Workplaces as communities: The role of social

networks in who seeks, gives, and accepts information on justice issues. Journal of

Community Psychology, 34 , 363�377.

Cho, H., Lee, J. S., Stefanone, M., & Gay, G. (2005). Development of computer-supported

collaborative social networks in a distributed learning community. Behavior & Information

Technology, 24 , 435�447.

Cho, H. C., Gay, G., Davidson, B. D., & Ingraffea, A. (2007). Communication styles, social

networks, and learning performance in a CSCL community. Journal of Computers and

Education , 49 , 309�329.

Cook, K. S., & Emerson, R. M. (1978). Power, equity and commitment in exchange networks.

American Sociological Review, 43 , 721�739.

Earley, P. C., & Mosakowski, E. (2000). Creating hybrid team cultures: An empirical test of

transnational team functioning. Academy of Management Journal , 43 , 26�49.

Easton, S. S. (2003). Clarifying the instructor’s role in online distance learning. Communication

Education , 52 , 87�105.

Ebbinghaus, H. (1913). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology . (Columbia University,

Trans.) New York: Columbia University. (Original work published in 1885).

Erickson, B. H., Nosanchuk, T. A., & Lee, E. (1981). Network sampling in practice: Some second

steps. Social Networks , 3 , 127�136.

Finfgeld, D. L. (2000). Therapeutic groups online: The good, the bad, and the unknown. Issues in

Mental Health Nursing , 21 , 241�255.

Freeman, L. C. (1979). Centrality in social networks: Conceptual clarification. Social Networks , 1 ,

215�239.

Granovetter, M. S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology, 78 , 1360�1380.

Granovetter, M. S. (1982). The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. In P. W.

Marsden & N. Lin (Eds.), Social structure and network analysis (pp. 106�130). Beverly Hills,

CA: Sage.

Advice Prestige 289

Page 13: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

Haythornthwaite, C. (2002). Strong, weak, and latent ties and the impact of new media. Information

Society, 18 , 385�401.

John, P. (2005). The contribution of volunteering, trust, and networks to educational performance.

The Policy Studies Journal , 33 , 635�656.

Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1993). Structuring groups for cooperative learning. In C. M.

Vance (Ed.), Mastering management education: Innovations in teaching effectiveness (pp. 187�198). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Klein, K. J., Lim, B-C, Saltz, J. L., & Mayer, D. M. (2004). How do they get there? An examination

of the antecedents of centrality in team networks. Academy of Management Journal , 47 ,

952�963.

Kline, R. B. (1998). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling . New York: Guilford Press.

Knoke, D., & Burt, R. S. (1983). Prominence. In R. S. Burt & M. J. Minor (Eds.), Applied network

analysis (pp. 195�222). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Kollock, P., & Smith, M. (1999). Communities in cyberspace. In M. Smith & P. Kollock (Eds.),

Communities in cyberspace (pp. 3�25). London: Routledge.

Krackhardt, D. (1992). The strength of strong ties: The importance of philos in organizations. In N.

Nohria & R. Eccles (Eds.), Networks and organizations: Structure, form and action (pp. 216�239). Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

Kuh, G. D., Schuh, J. H., & Whitt, E. J. (1991). Involving colleges: Successful approaches to fostering

student learning and development outside the classroom . San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation . Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Levin, D. Z., & Cross, R. (2004). The strength of weak ties you can trust: The mediating role of trust

in effective knowledge transfer. Management Science , 50 , 1477�1490.

McDevitt, M., & Kiousis, S. (2006). Deliberative learning: An evaluative approach to interactive

civic education. Communication Education , 55 , 247�264.

Molm, L. D., & Cook, K. S. (1995). Social exchange and exchange networks. In K. S. Cook, G. A.

Fine, & J. S. House (Eds.), Sociological perspectives on social psychology (pp. 209�235).

Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Morrison, E. W. (2002). Newcomers’ relationships: The role of social network ties during

socialization. Academy of Management Journal , 45 , 1149�1160.

Muncer, S., Burrows, R., Pleace, N., Loader, B., & Nettleton, S. (2000). Births, deaths, sex and

marriage . . . but very few presents? A case study of social support in cyberspace. Critical

Public Health , 10 , 1�18.

Myers, S. A., & Knox, R. L. (2001). The relationship between college student information-seeking

behaviors and perceived instructor verbal behaviors. Communication Education , 50 , 343�356.

Norris, C. E., & Colman, A. M. (1992). Context effects on recall and recognition of magazine

advertisements. Journal of Advertising , 21 , 37�47.

Oliver, J. M., Reed, C. K. S., Katz, B. M., & Haugh, J. A. (1999). Students’ self-reports of help-

seeking: The impact of psychological problems, stress, and demographic variables on the

utilization of formal and informal support. Social Behavior and Personality, 27 , 109�128.

Ortiz, M. G. R., Hoyos, J. R. C., & Lopez, M. G. R. (2004). The social networks of academic

performance in a student context of poverty in Mexico. Social Networks , 26 , 175�188.

Perkins, D. N. (1991). What constructivism demands of the learner. Educational Technology, 31 ,

19�21.

Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In L.

Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, (vol. 19, pp. 123�205). New

York: Academic Press.

Russo, T. C., & Koesten, J. (2005). Prestige, centrality, and learning: A social network analysis of an

online class. Communication Education , 54 , 254�261.

290 R. A. Smith & B. L. Peterson

Page 14: “Psst … What Do You Think?” The Relationship between Advice Prestige, Type of Advice, and Academic Performance

Salancik, G. R. (1995). Wanted: A good network theory of organization. Administrative Science

Quarterly, 40 , 345�349.

Sandars, J. (2005). Work based learning: A social network perspective. Work based Learning in

Primary Care , 3 , 4�12.

Scott, J. (1991). Social network analysis: A handbook . London: Sage.

Selwyn, N. (2000). Creating a ‘‘connected’’ community? Teachers’ use of an electronic discussion

group. Teachers College Record , 102 , 750�778.

Sparrowe, R. T., Liden, R. C., Wayne, S. J., & Kraimer, M. L. (2001). Social networks and the

performance of individuals and groups. Academy of Management Journal , 44 , 316�425.

Stevens, T., Olivarez, A. Jr., & Hamman, D. (2006). The role of cognition, motivation, and emotion

in explaining the mathematics achievement gap between Hispanic and White students.

Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences , 28 , 161�186.

Tardy, R. W., & Hale, C. L. (1998). Bonding and cracking: The role of informal, interpersonal

networks in health care decision making. Health Communication , 10 , 151�173.

Thomas, S. L. (2000). Ties that bind: A social network approach to understanding student

integration and persistence. Journal of Higher Education , 71 , 591�615.

Vess, D. L. (2005). Asynchronous discussion and communication patterns in online and hybrid

history courses. Communication Education , 54 , 355�364.

Walker, C. O., Greene, B. A., & Mansell, R. A. (2006). Identification with academics, intrinsic/

extrinsic motivations, and self-efficacy as predictors of cognitive engagement. Learning and

Individual Differences , 16 , 1�12.

Wasserman, S., & Faust, K. (1994). Social network analysis: Methods and applications . Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Webb, N. M. (1982). Student interactions and learning in small groups. Review of Educational

Research , 52 , 421�445.

Wenger, E. C., McDermott, R., & Snyder, W. C. (2002). Cultivating communities of practice: A guide

to managing knowledge . Boston: Harvard Business School Press.

Yi, M. Y., & Hwang, Y. (2003). Predicting the use of web-based information systems: Self-efficacy,

enjoyment, learning goal orientation, and the technology acceptance model. International

Journal of Human�Computer Studies , 59 , 431�449.

Yuan, Y. C., Gay, G., & Hembrooke, H. (2006). Focused activities and the development of social

capital in a distributed learning ‘‘community’’. The Information Society, 22 , 25�39.

Received January 23, 2007

Accepted March 25, 2007

Advice Prestige 291