prediction of drive for muscularity by body...

106
1 PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY COMPOSITION AND PSYCHOLOGICAL FACTORS By NICKLES I. CHITTESTER A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 2007

Upload: others

Post on 29-Sep-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

1

PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY COMPOSITION AND PSYCHOLOGICAL FACTORS

By

NICKLES I. CHITTESTER

A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT

OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

2007

Page 2: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

2

© 2007 Nickles I. Chittester

Page 3: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

3

Dedicated to my parents, Tom and Leanne Chittester.

Page 4: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

4

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

Several people provided their assistance on this project. I would like to give special thanks

to Dr. Heather Hausenblas, not only for serving as the chair for this committee, but also for her

guidance, assistance, and encouragement on this project and others I have undertaken. I wish to

thank the members of my committee—Dr. Pete Giacobbi, Dr. Chris Janelle, and Dr. Sam

Sears—for their suggestions at various stages of the study. In addition, I would like to thank the

numerous class instructors who enabled me to recruit participants for this study, as well as fellow

graduate students Brian Cook, Jessica Doughty, and Anna Campbell for their friendship and

support.

My fiancée, Mindy Mansour, has been a steady source of support during this project, and I

wish to express my heartfelt thanks to her for her love and understanding. Finally, I would like to

express the sincerest of appreciation for my parents, Tom and Leanne Chittester, for their

steadfast and unconditional love, support, and encouragement throughout my life.

Page 5: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

5

TABLE OF CONTENTS page

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS ...............................................................................................................4

LIST OF TABLES...........................................................................................................................7

LIST OF FIGURES .........................................................................................................................8

ABSTRACT.....................................................................................................................................9

CHAPTER

1 INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................................11

2 MATERIALS AND METHODS ...........................................................................................22

Subjects...................................................................................................................................22 Measures .................................................................................................................................22

Body Fat Percentage........................................................................................................23 FFMI................................................................................................................................23 BMI..................................................................................................................................23 Demographic Information ...............................................................................................24 Exercise Behavior............................................................................................................24 Eating Pathology .............................................................................................................25 Supplement Use...............................................................................................................25 Sociocultural Pressure .....................................................................................................26 Drive for Muscularity ......................................................................................................26 Exercise Dependence.......................................................................................................27 Self-esteem ......................................................................................................................27

Procedure ................................................................................................................................28 Data Analysis..........................................................................................................................29

3 LITERATURE REVIEW .......................................................................................................31

What Is It?...............................................................................................................................31 Initial Identification ................................................................................................................33 Measurement...........................................................................................................................33 Heuristic Model of Male Body Change Strategies .................................................................34

Biological Factors............................................................................................................35 Body composition/BMI............................................................................................35 Pubertal growth ........................................................................................................37 Pubertal timing .........................................................................................................38

Psychological Functioning ..............................................................................................39 Negative affect .........................................................................................................39 Self-esteem ...............................................................................................................39

Societal Factors ...............................................................................................................40

Page 6: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

6

Media influence........................................................................................................40 Peer and parental influence ......................................................................................41 Teasing .....................................................................................................................42 Peer popularity .........................................................................................................43

Social Body Comparison.................................................................................................43 Body-image Dissatisfaction.............................................................................................43

Muscularity...............................................................................................................44 Body fat ....................................................................................................................45

Health Risk Behaviors.....................................................................................................46 Steroids.....................................................................................................................46 Steroid precursors.....................................................................................................47 Ephedrine .................................................................................................................49 Dieting to lose weight ..............................................................................................50 Dieting to gain weight/increase muscularity ............................................................50

Sports...............................................................................................................................52 Organized and informal team sports ........................................................................53 Weightlifting ............................................................................................................53

Other Factors Related to Male Body-Image Disturbance.......................................................54 Eating Disorders ..............................................................................................................54

Anorexia nervosa......................................................................................................56 Bulimia nervosa........................................................................................................56 Binge-eating disorder ...............................................................................................57

Exercise Dependence.......................................................................................................57 Romantic Partners ...........................................................................................................59

Summary.................................................................................................................................60

4 RESULTS...............................................................................................................................63

Descriptive Statistics ..............................................................................................................63 Multiple Regression Analyses ................................................................................................63

5 DISCUSSION.........................................................................................................................69

Limitations..............................................................................................................................75 Future Research ......................................................................................................................78

APPENDIX

A LIST OF MEASURES ...........................................................................................................84

B RECRUITMENT FLYER ......................................................................................................92

LIST OF REFERENCES...............................................................................................................93

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH .......................................................................................................106

Page 7: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

7

LIST OF TABLES

Table page 4–1 Descriptive statistics for outcome variables .........................................................................65

4–2 Correlation matrix of outcome variables ..............................................................................66

4–3 Stepwise regression predicting drive for muscularity using subjective BMI .......................67

4–4 Stepwise regression predicting drive for muscularity using objective BMI.........................68

Page 8: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

8

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure page 3–1 A heuristic model of male body change behavior; solid arrows indicate relationships

with greater support than broken arrows ...........................................................................62

5–1 Proposed continua for male body-image disturbance........................................................83

Page 9: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

9

Abstract of Dissertation Presented to the Graduate School of the University of Florida in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY COMPOSITION AND

PSYCHOLOGICAL FACTORS

By

Nickles I. Chittester

May 2007

Chair: Heather A. Hausenblas Major: Health and Human Performance

The ideal physique for men that is portrayed in the media is a lean and muscular

physique, particularly upper body muscularity. The desire to obtain this ideal physique has

resulted in increased body dissatisfaction within men. High levels of body dissatisfaction may

result in a specific drive for muscularity wherein a man holds attitudes that muscularity is crucial

to attain; this attitude is often accompanied by extreme body change behaviors aimed at

increasing muscularity. Drive for muscularity is associated with low self-esteem, exercise

dependence, eating pathology, and substance abuse (e.g., anabolic steroids, dietary supplements).

Information on the psychological risk and maintenance factors of drive for muscularity is sparse.

Furthermore, body composition is believed to be an important factor in the drive for muscularity;

body mass index (BMI), a simple height-to-weight ratio is most often used, followed by fat-free

mass index (FFMI; e.g., the amount of body weight attributable to muscle), and finally body fat

percentage. BMI has been associated with general body-image disturbance in men, but this

measure of body composition is limited because it does not account for weight attributable to

muscle—a key factor in the drive for muscularity. Thus, it is unclear which measure of body

composition (e.g., BMI, FFMI, body fat percentage) is most useful in understanding the

Page 10: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

10

physique-related aspect of drive for muscularity. This is critical to understand because whether

drive for muscularity is related to actual or perceived degree of muscularity (similar to the

question of actual vs. perceived thinness as it relates to the drive for thinness in women) remains

equivocal.

This study’s objective was to identify the psychological and body composition predictors

of drive for muscularity. To achieve this, 113 men completed psychological (e.g., self-esteem,

exercise dependence, eating pathology, substance abuse) and body composition (e.g., BMI, body

fat percentage, FFMI) measures. Multiple regression analysis was conducted to determine the

psychological and body composition measures that were most predictive of drive for

muscularity. The results indicated that drive for muscularity is predicted by weightlifting,

supplement use, exercise dependence, and self-esteem; however, none of the body composition

measures predicted drive for muscularity. Future research efforts should focus on clarifying the

role of body composition in drive for muscularity and on developing interventions that target

behaviors (e.g., exercise, supplement use) that are associated with drive for muscularity.

Page 11: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

11

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION

Kostanski, Fisher, and Gullone (2004) argued that body-image disturbance is so common

that it is a normal part of a young woman’s life. Is it really that common? Consider the three

following findings (Spitzer, Henderson, & Zivian, 1999):

• the body mass index (BMI) of Playboy centerfolds decreased from 18.12 in 1977 to 18.03 in 1996 (18.50 is considered the low end of the “normal” range; Willett, Dietz, & Colditz, 1999);

• the BMI of Miss America beauty pageant winners decreased from 19.35 in 1953 to 18.06

in 1985; • the BMI of American women has increased from 22.20 in the 1950s to 24.50 in 1990

(24.90 is considered the high end of the “normal” range; Willett et al., 1999).

Based on these findings it is clear that the slimmer female ideal promoted through the

media is being viewed by American women who are becoming progressively larger.

Consequently, several researchers believe that this disparity between media representation of the

female “ideal” and the female reality is a main factor in body-image disturbance (e.g., Cattarin,

Thompson, Thomas, & Williams, 2000; Spitzer et al., 1999). Recent research, however, indicates

that women have less body dissatisfaction than their cohorts from the mid-1990s (Cash, Morrow,

Hrabosky, & Perry, 2004). While encouraging, the following caveat exists: Cash et al. (2004)

drew these cohorts from the same university, therefore limiting the generalizability of this

finding.

In contrast to recent body-image research on women, body-image disturbance among men

is on the rise, in part because of the now-prevalent portrayal of the male “ideal” in the mass

media (Pope, Phillips, & Olivardia, 2000). For example, Spitzer et al. (1999) analyzed the body

size of Playgirl centerfolds and they found a sharp increase in BMI from 1986 to 1996, while

Page 12: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

12

simultaneously observing an increase in the average American man’s BMI. At first this may

seem congruent, however the increase in body size of the Playgirl centerfolds is attributable to

muscle mass, whereas the increase in BMI for American men is attributable to fat. Indeed, Leit,

Pope, and Gray (2001) confirmed this in a similar study of Playgirl centerfolds. Furthermore, the

action figures young boys play with are becoming more muscular. For example, assuming the

1998 Batman action figure stood 5’10”, Pope, Olivardia, Gruber, and Borowiecki (1999)

calculated that, given his physical proportions, he would have a 30.3” waist, a 57.2” chest, and

26.8” biceps. These measurements do not represent the typical man, for if Batman were to enter

the Mr. Olympia bodybuilding competition he would pose a serious threat to usurping the

reigning champion (arguably the most muscular man on earth), who at a height of 5’11” has a

58” chest and 24” biceps. Unfortunately, by playing with extremely muscular action toys, boys

are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger age.

Therefore, despite some limited evidence to the contrary (e.g., Rozin, Trachtenberg, &

Cohen, 2001), it appears that a disparity between the “ideal” physique and reality has also

emerged for men. Identifying the time at which men began looking at their bodies with more

dissatisfaction is difficult, but Chung (2001) argued that the rise to stardom in the 1980s of

bodybuilders such as Arnold Schwarzenegger, who eventually became President George H.W.

Bush’s Fitness Council head, was one factor. According to Chung (2001), Schwarzenegger’s

prominent role as the pinnacle of fitness inadvertently set the bar higher for what acceptable

muscularity is.

Indeed, this “drive for muscularity,” along with a simultaneous dissatisfaction with degree

of body fat, is the main source of body-image disturbance in men (Pope, Phillips, et al., 2000).

Moreover, some men who possess the “ideal” physique nevertheless view themselves as either

Page 13: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

13

small or puny (Pope, Katz, & Hudson, 1993). For example, the individual may adopt a strict diet,

forego social engagements and other activities in favor of spending more time in the gym, and in

some instances use anabolic steroids to add more muscle mass. In addition, some highly

dissatisfied men may engage in other compulsive muscle-related behaviors such as mirror-

checking and weigh-ins several times a day (Pope, Gruber, Choi, Olivardia, & Phillips, 1997).

This unique form of male body-image dissatisfaction, termed muscle dysmorphia, is also

associated with mood disorders, anxiety, and disturbed eating practices (Pope, Phillips, et al.,

2000).

As with women, body-image disturbance in men is a strong risk factor for eating disorders

(Stice, 2002), and it is associated with low self-esteem in boys as young as 8 years old (Grilo &

Masheb, 2005; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2003; McCreary & Sasse, 2000). Also, male body-image

disturbance is positively associated with depression (Kostanski & Gullone, 1998; McCreary &

Sasse, 2000; Olivardia, Pope, & Hudson, 2000), which is a strong predictor of body

dissatisfaction in high school boys (Presnell, Bearman, & Stice, 2004). Finally, some men may

exercise excessively to achieve the “ideal” physique, which may lead to exercise dependence

(Hausenblas & Symons Downs, 2002a, 2002b; Smith & Hale, 2004; Smith, Hale, & Collins,

1998); which is associated with physical and psychological difficulties (e.g., withdrawal

symptoms, decreased time spent with family or friends, overuse injuries; Andersen, Cohn, &

Holbrook, 2000; Pope, Phillips et al., 2000).

Because male body-image disturbance is centered on a preoccupation with muscularity, a

key issue is that this preoccupation will lead to the adoption of unhealthy behaviors to gain

muscle and decrease fat. For example, disturbed eating practices are often observed in men with

body-image disturbance (Cafri, Thompson, Ricciardelli, McCabe, Smolak, & Yesalis, 2005), and

Page 14: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

14

these eating practices have the following two goals: 1) to add muscle mass by eating high

amounts of protein, and 2) to restrict foods high in fat content to decrease overall adiposity.

However, one limitation to understanding these eating behaviors is that they are typically

assessed by the use of instruments designed to assess eating disorder pathology (e.g., Eating

Disorder Inventory—2). These measures do not capture the unique eating behaviors undertaken

by individuals whose goal it is to increase muscle mass and shred body fat (e.g., paying close

attention to the macronutrient breakdown of each meal consumed). Furthermore, these

instruments have been validated in female eating disordered samples for whom eating pathology

is qualitatively different (i.e., restricting food intake to decrease body size) when compared to a

man who has a high drive for muscularity (i.e., eating large amounts of food to increase body

size in the form of muscle). Therefore, a measure validated in men would add more

understanding as to the specific nature of the eating pathology seen in men attempting to gain

muscle.

The use of dietary supplements is common in men high in drive for muscularity

(Kanayama, Pope, & Hudson, 2001). Although popular and expensive (Saper, Eisenberg, &

Phillips, 2004), they generally show little impact on muscle mass (Kreider, 1999), and they may

promote dependence (Kanayama et al., 2001), and have harmful side effects (Haller & Benowitz,

2000). The use of anabolic steroids is also a practice adopted by many men in their pursuit for

muscularity (Chng & Moore, 1990; Cole, Smith, Halford, & Wagstaff, 2003; Wroblewska,

1997). This is alarming because of the health risks associated with anabolic steroid use (e.g.,

hypertension, disturbed lipid profiles, increased irritability, increased aggression, body-image

disturbance, and mood disturbances; Hartgens & Kuipers, 2004).

Page 15: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

15

Surprisingly, little research has examined body composition in relation to general body-

image disturbance or drive for muscularity. Indeed, satisfaction with body composition is critical

in determining whether body-image disturbance will develop in men. For example, BMI is

related to body-image disturbance in men (Kostanski et al., 2004; Kostanski & Gullone, 1998;

Presnell et al., 2004), and this relationship is either positive or negative depending on whether

the body-image disturbance reflects (1) a self-perception that one is too thin, which results in a

drive for muscularity; or (2) a self-perception that one is too heavy, which results in

simultaneous drives to lose body fat and add muscle. Two studies (McCabe, Ricciardelli, &

Banfield, 2001; McCreary, Karvinen, & Davis, 2006) have found that BMI was negatively

correlated with body satisfaction in boys and men respectively. However, McCabe et al. (2001)

also found that no correlation between BMI and a desire to increase muscle tone existed. This

latter finding is consistent with the findings of McCreary and Sasse (2000) that BMI was

uncorrelated with drive for muscularity. Taken together, these null findings indicate a need to

clarify the relationship between BMI and drive for muscularity.

Because BMI does not yield precise estimates of body fat percentage and muscle mass,

direct assessment of these latter two measures of body composition is preferable when

conducting male body-image research. Upon reading the muscle dysmorphia literature one

recognizes the importance of these assessments, yet only McCreary et al. (2006) used BMI, body

fat percentage, and muscle mass (expressed as fat-free mass index [FFMI]) to predict drive for

muscularity. They found that BMI was significantly correlated with both body fat percentage (r =

.68) and FFMI (r = .93), and that a moderate yet significant correlation existed between body fat

and FFMI (r = .41; McCreary et al., 2006). However, the only anthropometric measure that

significantly predicted behaviors related to drive for muscularity was flexed bicep circumference

Page 16: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

16

(McCreary et al., 2006). Thus, there exists a need to clarify which measure of the three main

body composition measures is most informative when conducting drive for muscularity research.

Cafri et al. (2005) attributed the lack of body composition assessment to factors such as

time constraints on researchers, the need for personnel trained in body fat assessment, and cost

associated with techniques such as hydrostatic weighing and Dual Energy X-ray Absorptiometry.

The result is an absence of information pertaining to the body fat percentages and extent of

muscle mass possessed by persons with a high drive for muscularity. Identifying and clarifying

such relationships would be helpful in developing a precise model of the drive for muscularity.

The purpose of this study was to determine the psychological (e.g., self-esteem, exercise

behavior, exercise dependence, eating pathology) and body composition (e.g, BMI, body fat, fat-

free mass index [FFMI]) predictors of drive for muscularity in college-aged men. In accordance

with this purpose, the following hypotheses were advanced:

Self-esteem. Similar to research in women (Stice, 2002), self-esteem is a predictor of

negative body-image in men (Kostanski & Gullone, 1998). Self-esteem is also negatively

correlated with body and muscle dissatisfaction in men (Cafri, Strauss, & Thompson, 2002;

Kostanski & Gullone, 1998; McCreary & Sasse, 2000). However, in contrast to women, men

who are thin do not report greater self-esteem than normal weight men (Mazzeo, Slof, Tozzi,

Kendler, & Bulik, 2004); it is believed that this is because a thin woman is closer to the female

“ideal” body whereas a thin man is farther from the “ideal” muscular male body. In addition, low

self-esteem predicts problematic eating behavior, increased dieting, and use of binge-purge

cycles (McGee & Williams, 2000; Neumark-Sztainer & Hannan, 2000; Stice, 2002). In light of

these findings, I hypothesized that self-esteem would be a negative predictor of drive for

muscularity.

Page 17: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

17

Exercise. Although some studies indicate that exercise is associated with greater body

satisfaction in men (e.g., Davis & Cowles, 1991; Hausenblas & Fallon, 2002; Williams & Cash,

2001), other studies with men have found exercise behavior to be associated with body

dissatisfaction (Tiggemann & Williamson, 2000; Varnado-Sullivan, Horton, Savoy, 2006).

Specifically, some studies have found that weightlifting is related to the drive for muscularity,

greater physique dissatisfaction, and muscle dysmorphia (Lantz, Rhea, & Cornelius, 2002;

McCreary & Sasse, 2000; Pope et al., 1997), but other research has found weightlifting to

increase body satisfaction (Fisher & Thompson, 1994; Williams & Cash, 2001). Part of the

discrepancy in study findings may be related to the measure of exercise. That is, some exercise

measures were general and assessed aerobic and anerobic exercise in a nonspecific context (e.g.,

Hausenblas & Fallon, 2002), whereas other studies have focused on weightlifting (e.g., Williams

& Cash, 2001). It is conceivable that exercise measures that focus on weightlifting may be more

strongly related to drive for muscularity than general exercise measures because theoretically

weightlifting should be related to drive for muscularity. Thus, I assessed both general exercise as

well as weightlifting measures to determine if there was a difference. I hypothesized that

weightlifting would be a stronger predictor of drive for muscularity than general exercise

behavior.

Dietary supplements. A method many men use to achieve greater muscularity while

decreasing body fat is taking dietary supplements (Kanayama et al., 2001). Men who have body

dissatisfaction often use dietary supplements to increase muscularity (Varnado-Sullivan et al.,

2006). Of importance, adolescent boys who report supplement use have lower body esteem than

nonsupplement using adolescent boys (Smolak, Murnen, & Thompson, 2005). The popularity of

supplements for these purposes has led Kanayama et al. (2001) to refer to these supplements as

Page 18: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

18

“body-image drugs.” Therefore, I hypothesized that the use of dietary supplements would be a

significant predictor of drive for muscularity.

Eating pathology. McCreary and Sasse (2000) found that drive for muscularity is

significantly higher in adolescent boys trying to gain weight (via unhealthy eating practices) than

those who are not; and it is well established that body dissatisfaction and eating pathology are

related (Olivardia et al., 2000; Olivardia, Pope, Mangweth, & Hudson, 1995). For example, in

their study of 18–25-year-old men, Heywood and McCabe (2006) found a significant correlation

between dietary restraint and body dissatisfaction related to body parts such as the shoulders,

chest, and arms. Furthermore, in their study of 83 bodybuilders, weightlifters, and athletically

active controls, Hallsworth, Wade, and Tiggemann (2005) found that, after controlling for BMI,

drive for muscularity was significantly related to the Bulimia subscale of the Eating Disorders

Inventory. Therefore, I hypothesized that eating pathology would be a significant predictor of

drive for muscularity.

Sociocultural pressure. The etiology of drive for muscularity is multifactorial, and it is

believed to include sociocultural sources such as peers, parents, and romantic partners. For

example, during adolescence boys who mature early (and thus move closer towards the male

“ideal” physique) enjoy high popularity, but also are more likely than boys who have not yet

reached puberty to engage in body change strategies (e.g., weightlifting, dieting to gain weight,

supplement use; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004a). Other research indicates that experiencing

teasing from peers is associated with decreased body satisfaction (Paxton, Eisenberg, &

Neumark-Sztainer, 2006); furthermore, encouragement from parents to lose weight has been

associated with both decreased body satisfaction and use of muscle building strategies (e.g.,

steroid and supplement use; Smolak et al., 2005; Wertheim, Martin, Prior, Sanson, & Smart,

Page 19: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

19

2002). In addition, there is limited evidence that men told by a female dating partner to gain

weight (presumably in the form of muscle) report low relationship satisfaction (Sheets &

Ajmere, 2005), although other research indicates that men may be dissatisfied with their bodies

despite a female dating partner’s satisfaction with it (Ogden & Taylor, 2000). Taken together,

there is sufficient reason to believe that sociocultural pressure does impact drive for muscularity;

therefore, I hypothesized that sociocultural pressure would be a significant predictor of drive for

muscularity.

Exercise dependence. Exercise dependence is believed to be a key aspect of male body-

image disturbance, especially muscle dysmorphia (Rhea et al., 2004). While moderate amounts

of exercise are associated with greater body satisfaction (Williams & Cash, 2001), excessive

amounts of exercise are associated with greater body-image disturbance (Pope et al., 1997; Rhea

et al., 2004). Furthermore, exercise dependence is predicted by weight loss strategies (which is

associated with male body-image disturbance) in adolescent boys (McCabe & Ricciardelli,

2004a). Therefore, I hypothesized that exercise dependence would be a significant predictor of

drive for muscularity.

BMI. Although there exists a relationship between BMI and male body-image disturbance

wherein both high and low BMI are associated with greater dissatisfaction (Frederick, Peplau, &

Lever, 2006; Gila, Castro, Cesena, & Toro, 2005), McCreary et al. (2006) recently found that

BMI was not a predictor of drive for muscularity. A possible explanation for this is in the nature

of the BMI itself: because it is a height-to-weight ratio, it does not distinguish between weight

attributable to body fat versus weight attributable to muscle. This distinction is critical because,

by definition, the drive for muscularity is specifically associated with a desire to have larger

muscles. Therefore, in light of both the finding of McCreary et al. (2006) and the nonspecificity

Page 20: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

20

inherent in the BMI, I hypothesized that BMI would not be a significant predictor of drive for

muscularity.

FFMI. While FFMI is significantly higher in men with muscle dysmorphia than normal

comparison men (Olivardia et al., 2000), a recent study found that FFMI did not emerge as a

predictor of drive for muscularity (McCreary et al., 2006). The authors speculated that perhaps

self-assessment of muscularity is compromised because a layer of body fat “hides” the true

extent of a man’s muscularity, and that the actual degree of muscularity one possesses is most

easily appraised in men who either have low body fat or have very well-developed muscles.

Therefore, because the men in this study were expected to have normal levels of body fat

(thereby “concealing” the degree of muscularity the men actually possessed), I hypothesized that

FFMI would not be a significant predictor of drive for muscularity.

Body fat percentage. Male body-image disturbance is centered around degree of

muscularity; when given the opportunity to indicate ideal body fat percentage, men generally do

not report desiring body fat percentage that is a large departure from what they currently have

(Cafri et al., 2002). Pickett, Lewis, and Cash (2005) found that although noncompetitive weight

trainers score significantly higher than athletically active controls on measures of body image

such as appearance orientation, appearance evaluation, and satisfaction with muscle tone, they

have similar body fat percentage. Furthermore, Olivardia et al. (2000) found similar levels of

body fat percentage between men with muscle dysmorphia and normal comparison men.

Although some research has found body-image satisfaction and body fat percentage to be

inversely related (e.g., Huddy, Johnson, Stone, Proulx, & Pierce, 1997), the trend of most

research on the topic is that body fat percentage is not the primary concern in male body-image,

a point reinforced by McCreary et al. (2006) when they found that body fat percentage did not

Page 21: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

21

emerge as a predictor of drive for muscularity. Therefore, I hypothesized that body fat

percentage would not be a significant predictor of drive for muscularity.

Self-report BMI vs. measured BMI. Although many studies rely on self-reported (as

opposed to measured) values of height and weight to derive participants’ BMI, there is question

as to how accurate such self-reported values are. Specifically, several studies have found that

“subjective” BMI is significantly lower than “objective” BMI (Brener, McManus, Galuska,

Lowry, & Wechsler, 2003; Elgar, Roberts, Tudor-Smith, & Moore, 2005; Hill & Roberts, 1998).

Therefore, I hypothesized that “subjective” BMI (resulting from self-reported height and weight

at pre-screening) would be significantly lower than “objective” BMI (resulting from objective

measurement of height and weight as assessed at the testing session), as evidenced by a paired-

samples t-test.

Page 22: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

22

CHAPTER 2 MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Because multiple regression was to be used to examine the study’s purposes, the power

tables developed by Green (1991) for multiple regression analyses were used to determine

sample size. Based on the maximum number of predictors for the multiple regression analysis

(9), to detect a medium effect with α = .05, a sample of 113 was required for a power of .80

(Green, 1991); therefore, the target sample size for this study was N = 113. To be eligible for

inclusion, participants had to be men between the ages of 18–24; this age range was selected not

only because other studies have used a similar age range (e.g., Heywood & McCabe, 2006), but

also to remove age as a potential confounding variable in light of evidence that men of this age

have different motivations for exercising than do older men (e.g., mid-30s and older; Davis &

Cowles, 1991; Tiggemann & Williamson, 2000). The mean age of the 113 men in this study was

20.34 years (SD = 1.52, Range = 18–24 years); most described their ethnicity as White (n = 77),

followed by Hispanic (n = 14), Asian (n = 9), Black (n = 6) or Middle Eastern (n = 2). In

addition, each of the following descriptions of ethnicity was provided once: American, East

Indian, Indian, Jewish, and Pacific Islander. Most of the men reported their sexual orientation as

heterosexual (n = 105). With respect to academic standing, juniors were most frequent (n = 40),

followed by seniors (n = 33), sophomores (n = 21), freshmen (n = 18), and completion of a

masters degree (n = 1). Two participants reported current use of anabolic steroids, while one

reported past but not current use of anabolic steroids.

Measures

The following variables were assessed in the study (see Appendix A).

Page 23: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

23

Body Fat Percentage

Body fat percentage was assessed by the researcher using the 3-site (chest, abdomen,

thigh) skinfold method for men (American College of Sports Medicine, 2000). This method

correlates strongly with the hydrostatic weighing method, and it has an error of ± 3.5% (ACSM,

2000).

FFMI

Each participant’s FFMI was determined to quantify degree of fat-free mass. The equation

for FFMI (Kouri, Pope, Katz, & Oliva, 1995; Pope, Gruber, et al., 2000) is:

{Wt x (100–BF%) / Ht2 x 100} + 6.1 (1.8–Ht)

where “Wt” is weight in kilograms, BF% is body fat percentage, and “Ht” is height in meters.

The FFMI of nonweightlifting men typically ranges from 18–21; nonsteroid using bodybuilders

typically have a FFMI between 21–25, and steroid using bodybuilders typically have a FFMI

from 25 to the low 30s (Olivardia et al., 2000). The importance of ascertaining FFMI in studies

of body image is well-noted (e.g., Eston, 2002).

BMI

BMI assesses weight (kg) relative to height (m2). While BMI correlates significantly with

body fat percentage (EPIETOOA, 1998), it is used mainly to determine overweight or obese

status (Wei et al., 1999). People with a BMI between 18.5 to 24.9 kg/m2 are classified as normal;

people with a BMI of 25.0 to 29.9 kg/m2 are classified as overweight, and people with a BMI of

30.0 kg/m2 or greater are classified as obese (Willett et al., 1999). BMI was calculated in two

ways: a) subjectively via self-reported values of height and weight at pre-screening, and b)

objectively via measurements of height and weight as assessed upon arrival at the Exercise

Psychology laboratory. BMI was derived in these two ways because, while many

epidemiological studies calculate BMI based on self-reported height and weight, there is

Page 24: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

24

mounting evidence that BMI based on objectively measured height and weight is more accurate

(e.g., Brener et al., 2003; England et al., 1998).

Demographic Information

The following demographic information was obtained from each participant: age, ethnicity,

sexual orientation, academic standing (if applicable), past/current anabolic steroid use, and

current duration/frequency of cardiovascular and weightlifting sessions (detailed in the next

section). Author developed questions related to anabolic steroid use were the following: (1) “Are

you currently using anabolic steroids in order to build muscle mass?”, which required either a

“yes” or “no” response; (2) “If you are not currently using anabolic steroids to build muscle

mass, have you in the past?”, which required either a “yes,” “no,” or “not applicable” response;

similar methods have been employed to ascertain anabolic steroid usage (e.g., Neumark-Sztainer,

Story, Falkner, Beuhring, & Resnick, 1999).

Exercise Behavior

Typical exercise behavior was assessed with the Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire

(LTEQ; Godin & Shephard, 1985). The LTEQ asks participants to indicate how frequently

during a typical week they engage in mild, moderate, and strenuous exercise for at least 15

minutes. An overall weekly exercise index is then derived from the following formula:

3(frequency of participation in mild activities during the past week) + 5(frequency of moderate

activities) +9(frequencies of strenuous activities). The LTEQ has adequate reliability and validity

(Godin, Jobin, & Bouillon, 1986), and correlates moderately with VO2max, an index of

cardiorespiratory fitness (Jacobs, Ainsworth, Hartman, & Leon, 1993).

Because the LTEQ does not specify the mode of exercise, participants were asked the

following author-developed questions on the demographic survey: (1) “How many sessions of

cardiovascular activity do you engage in per week, and how long does a typical session run?”

Page 25: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

25

and (2) “How many sessions of weight training do you engage in per week, and how long does a

typical session run?” For this latter question, because no standardized measures of weightlifting

frequency or session duration exist, a weightlifting index (frequency x duration) was calculated.

This was done because the LTEQ does not specify how much overall activity is due to

weightlifting; this form of exercise is of critical interest in the present study because

weightlifting is the primary exercise behavior that is responsible for muscular hypertrophy,

which is what a man with high drive for muscularity strives to achieve.

Eating Pathology

Eating behavior specific to gaining muscle was assessed with the Diet subscale of the

Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory (MDI; Lantz et al., 2002; Rhea et al., 2004). This subscale asks

participants to rate the extent (1 = Never to 6 = Always) to which certain statements (e.g., “I

regulate my caloric intake to maximize muscular development”) apply to them. This subscale has

good reliability in powerlifters (α = .84), bodybuilders (α = .87–.94), and recreational weight

trainers (α = .88; Lantz et al., 2002; Rhea et al., 2004), and has good construct validity (Rhea et

al., 2004). A high score indicates greater eating pathology. The internal consistency of the MDI-

Diet subscale in the present study was good (α = .82).

Supplement Use

Supplement use was assessed by the Supplement subscale of the MDI (Lantz et al., 2002;

Rhea et al., 2004). This subscale asks participants to rate the extent (1 = Never to 6 = Always) to

which certain statements (e.g., “Before a workout, I consume energy supplements.”) apply to

them. This subscale has excellent reliability in powerlifters (α = .91), good to excellent reliability

in bodybuilders (α = .80–.94), and adequate reliability in recreational weight lifters (α = .75;

Lantz et al., 2002; Rhea et al., 2004). Furthermore, it has good construct validity (Rhea et al.,

Page 26: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

26

2004). A high score indicates greater use of supplements. The internal consistency of the MDI-

Supplement subscale in the present study was good (α = .86).

Sociocultural Pressure

The Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale (Stice, Ziemba, Margolis, & Flick, 1996) was

used to assess the extent (1 = Never to 5 = Always) to which participants perceived pressure

from friends, family, dating partners, and the media to be lean and muscular. Because the scale

was originally developed for use in eating disorder populations, the items were adapted to reflect

the nature of body image of relevance to this study (the original scale is first in the appendix,

followed by the adapted version used in this study). For example, the item, “I’ve felt pressure

from my friends to lose weight” was changed to read as follows: “I’ve felt pressure from my

friends to lose body fat.” An additional example would be the item, “I’ve noticed a strong

message from my friends to have a thin body”, which was changed to read as follows: “I’ve

noticed a strong message from my friends to have a muscular body.” The adaptation of the items

was derived after consulting with graduate students and a professor, all of whom have extensive

experience in body image research. A high score indicates greater perceived sociocultural

pressure to conform to the male “ideal.” The internal consistency of the Perceived Sociocultural

Pressure Scale in the present study was acceptable (α = .75).

Drive for Muscularity

Drive for muscularity was assessed by the Drive for Muscularity Scale (DMS; McCreary

& Sasse, 2000), which is considered to be the best available scale for assessing muscularity-

related concerns (Cafri & Thompson, 2004). Specifically, the DMS is a 15-item likert-type scale

with two subscales that assesses the extent (1 = Always to 6 = Never) to which the respondent

holds attitudes (Muscle-oriented body image; MBI) and engages in behaviors (Muscularity-

related behavior; MB) indicative of the pursuit of a muscular physique. The DMS has good

Page 27: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

27

reliability (α = .81–.91), and has been shown to have good face, convergent, and discriminant

validity in men (Chittester, 2003; McCreary et al., 2006; McCreary & Sasse, 2000; McCreary et

al., 2004). In accordance with the recommendation by McCreary et al. (2004), one item (“I think

about taking anabolic steroids”) was omitted from the survey because it does not load on either

subscale of the DMS. All items are reverse coded so that high scores on the DMS indicate a high

drive for muscularity. The internal consistency of the DMS-MBI (α = .88) and DMS-MB (α =

.83) in the present study was good.

Exercise Dependence

Exercise dependence was assessed with the Exercise Dependence Scale (EDS; Hausenblas

& Symons Downs, 2002b). The EDS is a 21-item likert-type scale that consists of seven

subscales: withdrawal effects (“I exercise to avoid feeling stressed.”), tolerance (“I feel less of an

effect/benefit with my current exercise.”), continuance (“I exercise despite recurring physical

problems.”), lack of control (“I am unable to reduce how long I exercise.”), reduction in other

activities (“My exercise interferes with work/school responsibilities.”), time (“I organize my life

around exercise.”), and intention effects (“I often exercise longer than I intend.”). The EDS has

acceptable reliability (Hausenblas & Fallon, 2002; Hausenblas & Symons Downs, 2002b), and

preliminary data indicate the EDS is valid (Hausenblas & Symons Downs, 2002b; Symons

Downs, Hausenblas, & Nigg, 2004). Higher EDS scores indicate greater exercise dependence.

The internal consistency of the EDS in the present study was excellent (α = .91).

Self-esteem

The Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (Rosenberg, 1989) was used to assess global self-esteem.

This 10-item scale asks participants to rate the extent (1 = Strongly agree to 4 = Strongly

disagree) to which they agree with each question (e.g., “At times I think I am no good at all”).

This scale is reliable and valid; recent research indicates it may be superior to other measures of

Page 28: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

28

self-esteem (Griffiths et al., 1999). Some items are reverse coded; low scores on the Rosenberg

Self-esteem Scale indicate poor self-esteem. The internal consistency of the Rosenberg Self-

esteem Scale in the present study was good (α = .83).

Procedure

Participants were recruited with advertisements to participate in a study on body image that

would include body fat assessment (see Appendix B). These advertisements were disseminated

by either being read aloud in college classes or by being posted in various locations. The

advertisement was read aloud in select courses between August 2006 and January 2007 within

the departments of Applied Physiology & Kinesiology and Psychology at the University of

Florida, and the department of Social and Behavioral Sciences at Santa Fe Community College.

In addition, the advertisement was posted from late-June to December 2006 in the following

three ways:

• The announcement was placed on a website, http://www.my.ufl.edu, which is a secure website accessible only to university students;

• The announcement was placed in prominent areas within selected university parking

structures; • The announcement was placed in several private gyms and fitness centers within the

Gainesville area.

The advertisement directed interested men to an email address to write to if they wanted to

participate. The advertisement asked interested men to include the following demographic

information in their email: age, height, weight, and contact information. The researcher then

contacted each respondent (via email, or if unsuccessful, by phone call) to schedule an individual

testing session (Note: a reminder email was sent to each participant the day before his

appointment) with the primary researcher at the Exercise Psychology laboratory. Out of 159 men

who either contacted the lab to indicate they were interested in participating or signed up when

Page 29: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

29

the study announcement was made in their classes, 113 men actually enrolled in the study. Once

at the testing center, after providing informed consent, the participant’s height and weight was

measured by using a Healthometer scale (Chicago, IL). Height without shoes was measured to

the nearest 0.25 inch, and weight was measured to the nearest 0.25 lb. Each participant was then

asked to complete several surveys (detailed in the previous section). After the participant had

completed the surveys, body fat percentage was assessed via skinfold measurement. After all

data had been collected, the participant was debriefed and excused from the testing center.

Testing sessions were between 20–30 minutes in length. The protocol for this study was

reviewed and approved by the University of Florida Institutional Review Board prior to

participant recruitment.

Data Analysis

First, descriptive statistics for all variables were calculated, and the internal consistency

of all surveys was ascertained. In addition, once data were entered, a search for outliers (e.g., 3

or more standard deviations from the mean) and missing data was initiated. When data were

missing, the mean of the particular variable was inserted when less than 5% of the values for the

variable were missing (Cohen & Cohen, 1983). In addition, a correlation matrix was derived that

showed the correlations between the following variables: objective BMI, self-report BMI, body

fat percentage, FFMI, weightlifting index, exercise behavior (as measured by the LTEQ), eating

habits (as measured by the MDI—Diet subscale), supplement use (as measured by the MDI—

Supplement subscale), perceived sociocultural pressure, muscle-oriented body-image (as

measured by the DMS—MBI) subscale), muscle-related behaviors (as measured by the DMS—

MB subscale), drive for muscularity, exercise dependence, and self-esteem. A paired-samples t-

test was used to determine whether differences between subjective and objective BMI were

significant.

Page 30: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

30

Finally, to examine the main purposes, one stepwise multiple regression analysis was

conducted. Stepwise regression was used because there exists no empirical data indicating which

factor would be the best predictor of drive for muscularity; were such data available hierarchical

regression, where the researcher specifies the ordering of predictors, would have been conducted.

The stepwise regression analysis determined which measures of body composition (Independent

variables = BMI, body fat percentage, FFMI) and psychological factors (Independent variables =

exercise behavior, weightlifting index, eating pathology, supplement use, perceived sociocultural

pressure, exercise dependence, and self-esteem) best predicted drive for muscularity (Dependent

variable). Because multicollinearity (e.g., high correlations between independent variables) was a

potential problem in this analysis, the tolerance values for the regression equation were

determined.

Page 31: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

31

CHAPTER 3 LITERATURE REVIEW

Although body-image disturbance has historically been viewed as occurring mostly in

women (Pasman & Thompson, 1988; Rand & Wright, 2000), recent research has determined that

men also experience body-image disturbance (Salusso-Deonier & Schwarzkopf, 1991; Pope et

al., 1993; Pope, Gruber, et al., 2000). While in women body-image disturbance arises when one

compares her body to the thin and toned female “ideal” physique often portrayed in the media,

body-image disturbance in men arises when one compares his body to the lean and muscular

male “ideal” physique often portrayed in the media (e.g., Leit et al., 2001). Body-image

disturbance is a risk factor for eating disorders (Phelps, Johnston, & Augustyniak, 1999; Stice,

2002), and the DSM–IV–TR lists preoccupation with body shape as a criterion for both anorexia

and bulimia (APA, 2000). In addition, male body dissatisfaction is accompanied by a host of

problematic behaviors (e.g., social avoidance, steroid use, disturbed eating; Brower, Blow, &

Hill, 1994; Olivardia et al, 2000; Cafri et al., 2005), poor self-esteem (Cafri et al., 2005), and

negative affect (Olivardia, 2001; Presnell et al., 2004). Because of the myriad problems

associated with body-image disturbance, and the fact it is being observed more frequently in

boys (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2003; Cohane & Pope, 2001), it is imperative that predisposing

factors be identified. This review focuses on the following male body-image issues: 1) What

body-image disturbance is, 2) initial identification of body-image disturbance, 3) measurement

of body-image disturbance, and 4) the Cafri et al. (2005) model of male body change strategies.

What Is It?

Body-image disturbance is a “subjective negative evaluation of one’s figure or body

parts” (Presnell et al., 2004, p. 389). While body-image disturbance occurs in both men and

women, the aspect of the body from which this disturbance stems varies between the sexes. In

Page 32: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

32

women, body-image dissatisfaction arises from evaluation of one’s overall weight (i.e., the belief

that one is too fat), and this is coupled with concern specific to the shape of the lower torso (e.g.,

hips, thighs, and buttocks; Cash et al., 2004). However, the source of men’s body-image

disturbance arises from an evaluation of muscularity (Gokee-LaRose, Dunn, & Tantleff-Dunn,

2004). Knowledge of this concern was inadvertently gained during a study on the psychiatric

effects of anabolic steroids in bodybuilders (Pope et al., 1993). The researchers observed that, of

108 bodybuilders, two (both of whom had a history of anorexia) reported such pronounced

feelings of “puniness” and weakness that they would frequently decline social invitations and

even wear heavy clothes on hot days to conceal their “small” size. Pope et al. (1993) coined the

phrase “reverse anorexia” to explain this unique body-image disturbance, which is now referred

to as muscle dysmorphia (Pope et al., 1997).

Although not explicitly stated in the diagnostic criteria advanced by Pope et al. (1997), a

man with muscle dysmorphia would have to be muscular to the casual observer to warrant

diagnosis. Because degree of muscularity can be subjective, Olivardia et al. (2000) proposed that

one’s fat-free mass index (FFMI), a function of height, weight, and body fat percentage, be

calculated to determine muscularity sufficient enough for diagnosis. The FFMI of

nonweightlifting men typically ranges from 18–21; nonsteroid using bodybuilders typically have

a FFMI between 21–25, and steroid using bodybuilders typically have a FFMI from 25 to the

low 30s (Olivardia et al., 2000).

Given the muscularity requirement, one could argue that only very muscular men would

even qualify for muscle dysmorphia. However, many non weightlifting men experience body-

image disturbance, which usually focuses on a lack of muscularity. If these men do not have

muscle dysmorphia (i.e., they do not possess the muscularity required for diagnosis), what do

Page 33: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

33

they have? One way to describe this body-image disturbance is in relation to one’s “drive for

muscularity” and its accompanying behaviors and attitudes (McCreary & Sasse, 2000). Because

this is the most salient form of body-image disturbance in men, Cafri and Thompson (2004)

advocated that its measurement be the cornerstone of future male body-image research.

Initial Identification

The first mention of body-image disturbance is Arkoff and Weaver’s (1966) article

detailing the body-image of Japanese-Americans; ironic because body-image disturbance is more

prevalent in Western society than in others (Yang, Gray, & Pope, 2005). Other early body-image

research primarily dealt with dissatisfaction in relation to facial surgery (e.g., Peterson &

Topazian, 1976; Macgregor, 1981) and obesity (Guggenheim, Poznanski, & Kaufmann, 1977).

However, by the mid-1980s researchers began to focus on the link between body-image

disturbance and eating disorders (e.g., Freeman, Beach, Davis, & Solyom, 1985); this is likely

the reason why most body-image research up to the early 1990s focused on women.

Measurement

Several measures have been developed to assess body-image disturbance in men (see

Stewart & Williamson, 2004, for a review), and the focus of each measure varies depending on

what aspect of body-image disturbance (i.e., concerns surrounding degree of muscularity or

adiposity) is of primary interest. The most widely used measures for assessing male body-image

disturbance are the Drive for Muscularity Scale (DMS; McCreary & Sasse, 2000), the

Multidimensional Body Self-Relations Questionnaire (MBSRQ; Brown, Cash, & Mikulka, 1990)

and the body dissatisfaction subscale of the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI—BD; Garner,

Olmstead, & Polivy, 1983).

Over the past five years several new indices of male body-image disturbance have been

advanced, including the Muscle Appearance Satisfaction Scale (Mayville, Williamson, White,

Page 34: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

34

Netemeyer, & Drab, 2002), Muscle Dysmorphia Survey (Chittester, 2003), Muscle Dysmorphia

Inventory (Rhea et al., 2004), and the Male Eating Behavior and Body Image Evaluation

(Kaminski, Chapman, Haynes, & Own, 2005). These measures recognize that a major

component of male body-image disturbance focuses on one’s simultaneous desire for more

muscle mass and less body fat. Unfortunately, while these measures effectively assess

preoccupations and behaviors characteristic of male body-image disturbance related to

muscularity, they do not assess specific nutritional strategies employed to gain muscle mass

while simultaneously stripping away body fat. This information is critical in completely

understanding a multidimensional construct (i.e., how dietary practices, substance use, body

composition, body-image dissatisfaction, and other psychological factors interact) such as male

body-image disturbance.

Heuristic Model of Male Body Change Strategies

In an effort to organize the constructs associated with male body-image disturbance,

Cafri et al. (2005) advanced a heuristic model that maps these constructs and provides an

illustration of the strength of the various relationships that exist between the constructs (Figure

3–1). The model consists of seven main constructs, with six constructs consisting of

subcomponents. The seven main constructs (and their subcomponents) are:

• Biological Factors (body composition/BMI, pubertal growth, pubertal timing);

• Psychological Functioning (negative affect, self-esteem);

• Societal Factors (media influence, peer and parental influence, teasing, peer popularity);

• Social Body Comparison;

• Body Image Dissatisfaction (muscularity, body fat);

• Health Risk Behaviors (steroids, steroid precursors, ephedrine, dieting to lose weight, dieting to gain weight, dieting to increase muscularity);

Page 35: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

35

• Sports (organized team sports, informal team sports, weightlifting).

The present study focuses on the following components: Biological Factors, Psychological

Functioning, Societal Factors, Body Image Dissatisfaction, and Health Risk Behaviors.

Accordingly, each of these constructs will be discussed in subsequent sections of this document.

Because two constructs (Social Body Comparison and Sports) of the Cafri et al. (2005) model

are not included in the present study, the reader is referred directly to the aforementioned article

for a more in-depth review of those constructs.

Biological Factors

Body composition/BMI

BMI is a ratio of body weight (kg) to height (m2) widely employed to ascertain overweight

or obese status. According to the Expert Panel on the Identification, Evaluation, and Treatment

of Overweight and Obesity in Adults (EPIETOOA; 1998), BMI correlates significantly with

body fat percentage; it is for this reason that it is advisable to assess BMI as a routine procedure

of a patient’s visit to see a physician (Manson, Skerrett, Greenland, & VanItallie, 2004).

While BMI can be conveniently ascertained from self-reported height and weight, there is

some evidence suggesting that BMI derived from self-report data is inaccurate. For example, in

their study of 6000 16–64-year-old males and females in southwestern England, Hill and Roberts

(1998) compared BMI based on self-reported height and weight to BMI based on measured

height and weight. They found that, based on self-reported BMI, 49.7% of the males were

classified as “normal,” whereas 37.3% were classified as “overweight.” However, based on

objectively measured BMI, they found that the exact opposite was true: only 36.1% of the males

had a “normal” BMI, while 46.7% were classified as “overweight.” Furthermore, similar patterns

in the underestimation of BMI based on self-report data have been observed in 11th grade Welsh

boys (Elgar et al., 2005) as well as high school-aged boys in the United States (Brener et al.,

Page 36: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

36

2003). This underestimation of BMI based on self-report data is attributed to an overestimation

of height and an underestimation of weight, although some research suggests that only at-risk

status (e.g., restrained eaters) significantly predicts weight underestimation (Shapiro &

Anderson, 2003). Taken together, these findings suggest that BMI derived from self-reported

height and weight is inaccurate, and this poses a noteworthy problem for researchers who have

large sample sizes yet need this information from their participants.

A common practice of studies on male body-image disturbance involves the calculation

of BMI to serve as an objective measure of body size, which could then be correlated with

degree of body dissatisfaction. Results of such studies have consistently shown a strong

curvilinear relationship between BMI and body dissatisfaction in males (Frederick et al., 2006;

Gila et al., 2005; Grilo & Masheb, 2005; Kostanski et al., 2004; Kostanski & Gullone, 1998;

Presnell et al., 2004). However, a clear link between BMI and either the drive for muscularity or

muscle dysmorphia, constructs that represent specific descriptions of male body-image

disturbance, has yet to be established. In fact, only two studies are known to have even

investigated the link between BMI and any variables related to either drive for muscularity or

muscle dysmorphia (Cafri, van den Berg, & Thompson, 2006; McCreary et al., 2006). In their

study of 13-18-year old boys, Cafri et al. (2006) observed small but significant positive

correlations between BMI and dieting to gain weight (r = .16) and the use of performance

enhancing drugs (e.g., anabolic steroids, ephedrine, prohormones; r = .13), but did not observe

correlations between BMI and either body dissatisfaction or muscle dymorphia symptoms (Cafri

et al., 2006). This latter finding is a surprising departure from the general male body-image

disturbance literature. In addition, McCreary et al. (2006) found that BMI was not related to

Page 37: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

37

attitudes or behaviors related to drive for muscularity among college-aged men. The findings of

these studies indicate a need to clarify the role of BMI in drive for muscularity.

The most often cited reason why BMI is not used more often in drive for muscularity or

muscle dysmorphia research is the recognition that BMI is a poor measure of body composition

for persons who have a high degree of muscularity (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004b). For persons

with a high amount of muscle mass, BMI could easily indicate a man was obese when he

actually carried a significant amount of muscle. As a result, BMI is rarely (if ever) used as a

primary outcome variable in research on either drive for muscularity or muscle dysmorphia.

However, as has been established in this section, the decision whether to use objectively

measured BMI vs. self-reported BMI should not be taken lightly because the available data

suggest objectively measured BMI is superior to BMI based on self-report.

Pubertal growth

Puberty is a time in which a male’s body changes from that of a child to a mature young

adult. For males this means an increase in body mass; specifically, a marked increase in

muscularity. Thus, this is the stage at which boys begin to acquire the physique of a man and

subsequently move closer towards the male “ideal,” which is often accompanied by increased

body satisfaction (e.g., Rodriguez-Tomẻ, Bariaud, Cohen Zardi, Delmas, Jeanvoine, & Szylagyi,

1993). However, because puberty marks a period of body change it is possible that some

adolescent males may seek even greater levels of muscularity through a variety of means;

research on this topic is mixed. For example, O’Dea and Abraham (1999) found that trying to

lose weight is significantly more likely in boys who have already reached puberty than in

prepubescent boys, while McCabe et al. (2001) observed that pubertal growth was associated

with behavior aimed at increasing muscle tone and with increased use of food supplements

designed to add muscle mass. These findings illustrate a pattern of behavior consistent with the

Page 38: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

38

simultaneous desire to decrease body fat and increase muscle mass. However, in their study of

269 adolescent boys, Cafri et al. (2006) found no relationship between pubertal development and

substance use (steroids and food supplements), muscle dysmorphia symptoms, or dieting to gain

weight. Taken together, the role of pubertal growth on body change strategies and body-image

disturbance is unclear and would benefit from further study.

Pubertal timing

Although the precise role of puberty in male body-image disturbance is unclear, there is

more certainty in how the timing of puberty impacts body-image disturbance and body change

strategies. Specifically, the available research indicates that early maturation is associated with

greater body-image satisfaction amongst adolescent boys, and that late maturation is associated

with greater body-image disturbance (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004a; Siegel, Yancey,

Aneshensel, & Schuler, 1999). While this may suggest an advantage to early maturation, a closer

look at the longitudinal data of McCabe and Ricciardelli (2004a) reveals that early-maturating

males are not devoid of problematic behaviors associated with their early jump into adulthood.

Consistent with the findings of O’Dea and Abraham (1999), McCabe and Ricciardelli (2004a)

found that early maturing males were more likely than on-time or late-maturing males to adopt

weight loss strategies. Furthermore, weight loss strategies predicted exercise dependence and use

of food supplements in these males eight months later. This troublesome pattern of behavior was

not exclusive to early maturing males: common findings across early, on-time, and late-maturing

males indicated that: 1) strategies to decrease weight predicted exercise dependence eight months

later, and 2) use of food supplements predicted disordered eating (McCabe & Ricciardelli,

2004a).

Indeed, with respect to body change strategies, it seems that the early-maturing males are

happy to have their increasingly masculine bodies, but they also engage in behaviors to make it

Page 39: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

39

even more masculine. Needless to say, late-maturing males eventually “catch-up” to early and

on-time maturers in regards to the onset of body change behaviors, but there is a final caveat: the

only group for whom steroid use was not part of the final structural model at 8 months was the

late-maturing males (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004a). Perhaps there is nothing wrong with

maturing a little later after all.

Psychological Functioning

Negative affect

Negative affect or depression is positively correlated with body dissatisfaction in boys

(Kostanski & Gullone, 1998; McCreary & Sasse, 2000), and has been identified as a strong

predictor of body-image disturbance in the same population (Presnell et al., 2004). In addition,

the model proposed by Cafri et al. (2005) hypothesizes that negative affect is related to unhealthy

behaviors (e.g., use of anabolic steroids, ephedrine, special dieting techniques to minimize body

fat and maximize muscularity) often engaged in by men with body-image disturbance.

Furthermore, Olivardia et al. (2000) found that men with muscle dysmorphia had significantly

higher rates of past mood disorders (58%) than normal weightlifters (20%). With respect to body

change behaviors, a recent study found negative affect in men to be significantly positively

related to both efforts to increase muscle and dietary restraint (Heywood & McCabe, 2006). It is

of note that some studies have assessed depression in anabolic steroid users, a subgroup of

weightlifting enthusiasts and bodybuilders that may present with body-image disturbance

(Blouin & Goldfield, 1995).

Self-esteem

The relationship between self-esteem and body-image disturbance has received a fair

amount of research. In an overview of the risk factors for drive for muscularity, Cafri et al.

(2005) proposed a model in which self-esteem and the adoption of unhealthy behaviors (e.g., use

Page 40: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

40

of anabolic steroids, ephedrine, special dieting techniques to minimize body fat and maximize

muscularity) are related. Simultaneously, Cafri et al. (2005) stated that body dissatisfaction leads

one to adopt these unhealthy behaviors. What the model does not propose, however, is a direct

link between self-esteem and body dissatisfaction, which is believed to be a stable relationship

(Cohane & Pope, 2001). Indeed, poor self-esteem is often associated with body-image

disturbance, and this has been found when measuring the drive for muscularity in adolescent

boys (McCreary & Sasse, 2000). In addition, Irving, Wall, Neumark-Sztainer, and Story (2002)

found that adolescent boys who used anabolic steroids had significantly lower self-esteem than

non-users, and tentative support for a relationship between low self-esteem and use of anabolic

steroids in weightlifters has been identified (Kanayama, Pope, Cohane, & Hudson, 2003).

Findings such as these lend support to the sentiments of researchers who speculate that some

men may enter bodybuilding, in part, to offset low self-esteem (Oliosi, Dalle Grave, & Burlini,

1999).

Societal Factors

Media influence

One reason for the increase in male body-image disturbance is the portrayal of an “ideal”

male body in the mass media (e.g., men’s magazines; Labre, 2005), and there is evidence that

viewing this ideal is negatively impacting high school boys (Presnell et al., 2004) and leading

them to take measures to increase their muscularity (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2005).

Furthermore, not only are men from Playgirl centerfolds (Leit et al., 2001) to professional

football players (Kraemer et al., 2005) becoming more muscular, the action figures preferred by

many preadolescent boys are taking on superhuman physiques (Pope et al., 1999). To better

understand the impact of viewing the ideal male body (as portrayed in the media) on body-image

disturbance, Leit, Gray, and Pope (2002) showed slides depicting the ideal male physique to

Page 41: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

41

undergraduate men in a laboratory setting and then asked them to indicate via the somatomorphic

matrix: a) their perceived body type, b) the ideal body type, c) the average man’s body type, and

d) the body type women preferred. Compared to men who had been shown neutral slides that did

not depict the ideal male physique, men who saw slides of the ideal physique not only selected

an ideal body type with a significantly higher FFMI (Leit et al., 2002), but also indicated they

believed the average man had a significantly higher FFMI.

The drive for muscularity is not only pervasive in American culture—studies in other

Westernized cultures such as Australia, France, The Netherlands, Great Britain, Austria, and

South Africa have shown a similar pattern of preference for increasingly muscular physiques.

For example, in a study comparing Austrian, French, and American men, Pope, Gruber et al.

(2000) found no difference in how much more muscle each group desired: the preference was for

an additional 27 lbs. of muscle. Contrasting these findings with research in Taiwanese men,

which shows that they prefer to have a physique with only about 4.5 lbs. more muscle (Yang et

al., 2005), illustrates the disparities between West and East in male body-image ideals.

Peer and parental influence

Studies have examined the impact of both peers and parents on body-image and body

change strategies in males. For example, body dissatisfaction in boys is significantly related to

the amount of weight-loss encouragement a parent provides (Wertheim et al., 2002). In light of

this finding, some argue that there may be a difference in what mode of weight loss mothers

(dieting) and fathers (exercise) consider acceptable (Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2004). Furthermore,

Smolak et al. (2005) found that both peer and parental comments were predictive of muscle

building techniques; specifically, boys who used either steroids or supplements reported

significantly higher levels of parental comments regarding body size than did non-using boys.

Page 42: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

42

Collectively, these studies suggest that peers and parents are quite influential in whether a boy

decides to adopt body change behaviors aimed at increasing muscularity.

Teasing

One hallmark of muscle dysmorphia is that a very muscular male perceives that others will

mock or ridicule his self-perceived small or wimpy physique. This belief can then lead to

problems such as social avoidance and anxiety. In a similar vein, several studies have

investigated the impact of teasing on the body-image of males. For example, Cafri et al. (2006)

found teasing to be significantly negatively correlated with dieting to gain weight and body

dissatisfaction in adolescent males; the latter finding was believed to have occurred because of

an indirect relationship between the two variables that is mediated via negative affect. In

addition, teasing was significantly positively correlated with muscle dysmorphia

symptomatology, but did not emerge as a significant predictor of muscle dysmorphia symptoms.

Again, Cafri et al. (2006) explained this latter finding as indicative of an indirect relationship

between the two.

Paxton et al. (2006) recently conducted a longitudinal study on adolescent boys over a 5-

year span in an effort to predict body dissatisfaction during middle adolescence. They found that

Time 1 weight-related teasing was a significant predictor of Time 2 body dissatisfaction, but

once the regression model was reduced and depression was included, weight-related teasing

dropped out as a significant predictor. This suggests, as did Cafri et al. (2006), a relationship

early in adolescence between teasing and body dissatisfaction that is mediated by depression.

Although the relationship between teasing and body dissatisfaction must be teased apart by

future research, what is already known of the relationship between the two has prompted the

development of interventions aimed at decreasing teasing in the elementary school setting (e.g.,

Haines, Neumark-Sztainer, Perry, Hannan, & Levine, 2006).

Page 43: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

43

Peer popularity

Little research has examined the relationship between popularity and male body change

strategies, but it is known that a curvilinear relationship exists between popularity and physique

in adolescent males. Specifically, in comparison to either thin or heavy adolescent males,

adolescent males who have a muscular physique have greater body satisfaction and are more

popular, but they are also more likely to be dieting (Graham, Eich, Kephart, & Peterson, 2000;

McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004a ; Wang, Houshyar, & Prinstein, 2006). Furthermore, there is

limited evidence of a weak association between popularity and efforts to increase muscularity

amongst adolescent males (McCabe, Ricciardelli, & Finemore, 2002).

Social Body Comparison

Social comparison is the process by which an individual evaluates his or her attributes,

characteristics, or outcomes based on how they compare to the attributes, characteristics, or

outcomes of another person. For example, if an individual scored a 75% on an exam he may be

discouraged; however, after learning the class mean was 65% he may view his score more

positively in light of what “could have been.” Similar processes are believed to be at work when

individuals evaluate their bodies; opportunities for a male to compare his body to another male’s

body are numerous, especially considering how much television adolescent boys watch (e.g.,

music videos) and how many other media outlets (e.g., magazines) routinely produce content that

emphasizes a lean and muscular physique (Labre, 2005; Tiggemann, 2005). Specifically, studies

have found that males report greater body dissatisfaction after being exposed to images of the

male “ideal” body (e.g., Leit et al., 2002; Strong, 2005).

Body-image Dissatisfaction

Unlike body-image disturbance in women, body-image disturbance in men arises because

men perceive themselves as either too heavy or too thin (Kostanski et al., 2004; Kostanski &

Page 44: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

44

Gullone, 1998; Presnell et al., 2004). This distinguishes the nature of body-image disturbance in

men from that of women because it identifies a “double-edged sword” of risk in men not present

in women. What has arisen from these findings is twofold: (1) when a male reports

dissatisfaction with being too small, the desire is to gain weight in the form of muscle mass, and

(2) when a male reports dissatisfaction with being too large, the desire is to lose weight in the

form of body fat.

Muscularity

Recognizing the central role of muscularity in male body-image disturbance, several

studies have derived FFMI as the objective measure of degree of muscularity in participants

(Pope, Gruber, et al., 2000). For example, Olivardia et al. (2000) found that men with muscle

dysmorphia had significantly higher FFMI than did normal comparison weightlifters; this lends

support to the argument that a key element of muscle dysmorphia is that a quite muscular man

perceives himself to be small or weak. Furthermore, body composition is a hypothesized (yet

poorly understood) precursor to body-image disturbance in the Cafri et al. (2005) model,

although some evidence indicates minimal musculature is a significant predictor of muscle

dysmorphia symptoms (Chittester, 2003). There has been only one study to date that has directly

correlated FFMI with drive for muscularity (McCreary et al., 2006), and it found no correlation

between the two. Further research is clearly required because gaining better insight into this

relationship would greatly advance our knowledge of the complexity of male body-image

disturbance and actual muscularity; to this point such research has been a notable oversight in the

literature because of the critical role actual (or perceived) muscularity plays in male body-image

disturbance.

Page 45: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

45

Body fat

Because the BMI is unable to partial out weight attributable to body fat from weight

attributable to muscle, estimations of body fat are preferred when assessing body composition.

Not only is this practice more scientifically precise, it is also reflective of the recognition that

body fat is one of two (degree of muscularity is the other) physique-related factors that males

evaluate when asked their degree of body satisfaction. For example, Olivardia et al. (2000) noted

that men with muscle dysmorphia had a similar amount of body fat than normal comparison

weightlifters; this lends support to the argument that one aspect of muscle dysmorphia is a

disturbance in how one appraises his body composition. Most research, though, indicates that

between body fat and muscularity, body fat is the least consequential; perhaps this is why

surprisingly little research has directly correlated body fat percentage with body-image

disturbance in men.

One method of assessing satisfaction with body fat percentage is by asking the participant

to ascertain his body fat percentage by using the somatomorphic matrix. This is a computer-

based test in which the silhouette of a man is presented on the screen. The participant’s task is to

simultaneously manipulate the degree of muscularity and body fat of the silhouette in order to

arrive at (1) the participant’s perceived actual body shape, (2) the participant’s ideal body shape,

and (3) the body shape he believes that most women find ideal. For example, using the

somatomorphic matrix, Leit et al. (2002) found that undergraduate men perceived average men

to have about 4% less body fat than the participants perceived themselves as having; this

suggests a social comparison in which these participants believed themselves to be chubby

relative to an average man.

Unfortunately, recent reliability assessment by Cafri, Roehrig, and Thompson (2004) has

raised legitimate concerns that the somatomorphic matrix is not reliable; clearly this assessment

Page 46: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

46

tool should be re-examined and properly adjusted before being included in future body-image

studies. This measurement problem underscores the need for direct assessment of body fat

whenever it is an outcome of interest. Cafri et al. (2005) hypothesize that body composition is a

key precursor to body dissatisfaction, yet there is little in the way of empirical data that can

either confirm or falsify this proposed link. Therefore, there is a need to better understand the

relationship between body fat percentage and body-image disturbance in men; such information

could strengthen descriptive models such as those presented by Cafri et al. (2004) and Lantz et

al. (2002).

Health Risk Behaviors

The pursuit of muscularity may potentially lead to the use of illegal substances or dietary

supplements and products, most of which are marketed as “weight-gainer” or weight loss

supplements. These substances are attractive to someone with high body-image disturbance

because of the belief that they offer a “quick fix” to one’s body dissatisfaction that will require

minimal lifestyle change on the part of the user. In addition, many men adopt rigid eating habits

in an attempt to lose weight, gain weight, or add muscle mass. This section will detail these

behaviors that increase the risk to health.

Steroids

Anabolic steroids, despite being illegal in the United States, are attractive to persons who

desire greater muscularity because these substances significantly enhance muscle mass in a

relatively short period of time (Hartgens & Kuipers, 2004). While steroids offer these desirable

effects, they also pose numerous risks to both physical and psychological health. Adverse

physical symptoms include destabilization of lipid levels (which may increase risk of

cardiovascular events), disturbances in liver function, testicular atrophy, and acne (Hartgens &

Kuipers, 2004; Pope & Katz, 1994; Tricker, O’Neill, & Cook, 1989). In addition, psychological

Page 47: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

47

ramifications of steroid use include aggressive behavior, mood disorders, and psychotic

symptoms (Lefavi, Reeve, & Newland, 1990; Pope & Katz, 1987; 1988; 1994; Tricker et al.,

1989). Finally, beyond physical and psychological ramifications, steroid use is both a financial (a

user could expect to pay between $100–$275 for a 100 count bottle; Kouri, Pope, & Katz, 1994)

and legal risk (attempting to acquire steroids on the black market may be the pathway to jail for

some users; e.g., Pope et al., 1993).

Perhaps the most troublesome aspect of body-image disturbance in men is its relationship

to anabolic steroid use. Several studies have concluded that body dissatisfaction is a primary

predictor of steroid use in men (Blouin & Goldfield, 1995; Brower et al., 1994; Chng & Moore,

1990; Kanayama et al., 2003; Cole et al., 2003). Furthermore, in a summation of interviews with

10 former steroid users, Olrich and Ewing (1999) alluded to body dissatisfaction as a

precipitating factor. Specifically, these men reported steroid use, in part, because of (1) upward

social comparisons (i.e., comparing oneself with more muscular men in the gym), and (2)

perceived lack of gains in size and strength. In recognition of this unequivocal link, and the dire

consequences of steroid use, it is prudent to develop interventions aimed at increasing body

satisfaction in young men.

Steroid precursors

Adrenal hormones are substances designed to increase muscle mass because they are

testosterone precursors. Some examples include growth hormones (e.g., levodopa or L-DOPA)

and testosterone stimulants such as HCG. Perhaps the best-known adrenal hormone is

androstenedione, the substance admittedly used by former major league baseball star Mark

McGwire during his quest for the single season home run record in 1998. In a review of these

types of hormones, Ziegenfuss, Berardi, Lowery, and Antonio (2002) concluded that while

physiological effects such as decreases in high density lipoprotein (“good” cholesterol) and

Page 48: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

48

increases in estrogen and testosterone levels are seen while taking these supplements, clear

evidence that they positively increase either muscle mass or performance (the primary claim

made when they are marketed) is lacking. In fact, Ziegenfuss et al. (2002) argued that the

theoretical risks of using these adrenal hormones outweigh any potential benefits they have to

offer, while others (Kreider, 1999) have gone further and said the use of these hormones should

not be encouraged.

Men with body-image disturbance often use a variety of performance-enhancing

substances (ranging from supplements found in health food stores to anabolic steroids obtained

on the black market) in an attempt to attain the “ideal” physique. In addition, adolescent boys

frequently consume these substances; this is alarming in light of McCabe and Ricciardelli’s

(2004a) finding that disordered eating in adolescent boys is predicted by the use of food

supplements. There are literally scores of different kinds of substances that are used by men who

want to simultaneously shred fat and gain muscle (Pope, Phillips, et al., 2000). Because of their

use primarily to alter body composition, which the user believes will facilitate greater body

satisfaction, some researchers have begun referring to these as “body image drugs” (Kanayama

et al., 2001). Two additional classes of supplements are described below.

Protein supplements are designed to add muscularity; they typically come in the form of

snack bars, shakes, or powder, and are often consumed pre-workout, post-workout, or between

meals. For example, protein supplements such as “Met-Rx” and “Power Bar” are high in protein

content, although there is little evidence that such supplements are more effective at promoting

muscle growth than foods such as lean meat or skim milk (Pope, Phillips, et al., 2000).

Creatine is a very popular supplement, and studies have shown it to increase muscle mass

(e.g., Chilibeck, Stride, Farthing, & Burke, 2004; Kreider, 1999), but it is believed that a large

Page 49: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

49

portion of this gain may be attributable to water (Pope, Phillips, et. al, 2000). Creatine is

regarded as relatively safe; a recent review of its side effects noted that the greatest harm it poses

is likely restricted to issues regarding the purity of the supplement itself as it is manufactured

(Bizzarini & De Angelis, 2004). Perhaps concerns over water retention explain the use of

diuretics in some users (to eliminate water weight and increase the appearance of muscle mass).

In addition, hydroxycitrate acid is a supplement frequently used as a compliment to muscle-

promoting supplements because of its reported ability to inhibit lipogenesis and thus promote

weight loss and a leaner physique. These claims, however, appear unfounded, as neither weight

loss nor increased energy expenditure are facilitated by this supplement (Heymsfield, Allison,

Vasselli, Pietrobelli, Greenfield, & Nunez, 1998; van Loon, van Rooijen, Niesen, Verhagen,

Saris, & Wagenmakers, 2000).

Ephedrine

Ephedrine (ma huang) is a central nervous system stimulant often used in conjunction with

caffeine to produce weight loss. The user will frequently combine the use of this supplement

with exercise and strict dietary practices aimed at shredding fat and gaining muscle. In recent

years there have been several reports of adverse effects associated with ephedrine, including

mania-like episodes, acute myocardial infarction, stroke, and in especially vulnerable persons,

death (Emmanuel, Jones, & Lydiard, 1998; Haller & Benowitz, 2000; Saper et al., 2004; Traub,

Hoyek, & Hoffman, 2001). While Hutchins (2001) argued that “the implication of ephedrine-

type alkaloids in deaths from a wide variety of conditions that occur in the general population is

no more than idle speculation” (p. 1096), the Food and Drug Administration found sufficient

reason to ban in April 2004 the sale of ephedrine in the United States.

Page 50: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

50

Dieting to lose weight

Although females are more likely to try to lose weight than males (e.g., McCabe et al.,

2002), males also adopt weight loss strategies. It is troublesome to note that this starts early in

life: evidence indicates that boys as young as 8–years old already have thoughts of losing weight,

or have already engaged in weight loss behavior (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2003). When males

desire to lose weight the efforts are primarily aimed at decreasing body fat (as opposed to overall

body size reduction, as is the goal when females attempt to lose weight). In some men the drive

to lose weight may be so severe that it leads to problems such as exercise dependence or eating

disorders (O’Dea & Abraham, 2002; Sharp, Clark, Dunan, Blackwood, & Shapiro, 1994; Stice,

2002).

It is well established that bodybuilders, especially those who compete, closely monitor

their dietary intake (Lantz et al., 2002) and display significantly greater eating disturbed attitudes

than athletically active controls (Goldfield, Blouin, & Woodside, 2006; Pickett et al., 2005);

however, it has been argued that careful attention to dietary intake is part of the competitive

nature of the sport of bodybuilding, and, provided it harmed neither the health nor performance

of the individual, could easily be viewed as “drive” or motivation (Chung, 2001). Of course, the

problem arises when harm is being done to the individual when disturbed eating behaviors are

taken in context of an overall pattern of behavior designed to achieve maximum muscularity.

Dieting to gain weight/increase muscularity

It is interesting to note that one of the first articles that observed a desire on the part of

boys to increase weight attributed this desire to the tendency of boys to “value change in shape

or tallness” (Andersen & Holman, 1997, p. 392); in retrospect, although they alluded to a desire

to add muscle, it appears this did not occur to the researchers at the time, and rightly so because

there was little knowledge of or research on this relatively new phenomenon. Whereas for

Page 51: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

51

women the issue of dieting usually refers to weight loss efforts, a unique form of eating behavior

is being observed more frequently in men. Specifically, males with body-image disturbance not

only restrict foods high in fat, they also consume large amounts of food that are high in protein.

Indeed, a study from Northern Ireland found that the amount of protein consumed by males

significantly increased between the ages of 15 and 22 (Boreham, Robson, Gallagher, Cran,

Savage, & Murray, 2004), although the extent to which this was an intentional dietary practice

was not determined. This dieting behavior is aimed at maximizing gains in muscle mass while

simultaneously limiting calories from fat. This may result in continuous cycles of weight gain

and loss of over 11 lbs. in the case of competitive bodybuilders (Oliosi et al., 1999). If extreme

eating practices produce such drastic weight fluctuations in competitive bodybuilders, it is

disturbing to note that adolescent boys are increasingly turning to such strategies to achieve the

lean, muscular look. This is especially concerning due to the knowledge that body-image

disturbance is an identified risk factor for such unhealthy eating behavior (Cafri et al., 2005;

Muris, Meesters, van de Blom, & Mayer, 2005).

A major limitation of understanding the role of dietary practices in the pursuit of

muscularity has been the lack of a psychometrically sound instrument. Previous studies have

used measures that have been validated for use with eating disordered populations (e.g.,

Tiggemann, 2005), but even these are not without limitation when being used in men. Recently a

Muscle-Oriented behavior subscale has been identified on the Drive for Muscularity Scale

(McCreary, Sasse, Saucier, & Dorsch, 2004); this contains some items that more precisely

measure dietary practices aimed at building muscle (e.g., “I try to consume as many calories as I

can in a day”). In addition, Kaminski et al. (2005) have developed the MEBBIE, a scale

specifically designed to assess eating, exercise, and body-image related attitudes and behaviors; a

Page 52: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

52

recent confirmatory factor analysis has determined the factor structure to be strong (Kaminski,

personal communication, 2005), but it does not appear to contain questions specific to eating

practices undertaken to add muscle. However, the Diet subscale of the Muscle Dysmorphia

Inventory (MDI; Lantz et al., 2002; Rhea et al., 2004), with very specific items (e.g., “I control

the intake of proteins, carbohydrates, and fats to maximize my muscular development”) and

established psychometric properties, has emerged as a potential gold standard for assessing

muscle-building dietary practices.

Sports

Millions of males worldwide participate in sports ranging from little league baseball and

youth soccer to professional sports teams. In addition to the pure enjoyment of the specific sport,

males become involved in sports for a variety of reasons, including moral development, self-

esteem enhancement, development of leadership qualities, and social interactions (e.g., making

friends, functioning within a group setting; Coakley, 2004; Tritschler, 2000). Considerable

research over the past 30 years has been conducted to determine precisely how sport involvement

impacts males on these variables and a host of others, and the consensus is that sport

involvement is generally a positive experience. However, for some males, sport involvement can

be accompanied by hazardous behaviors aimed at increasing performance, such as disturbed

eating practices, supplement use, and steroid use (Hausenblas & Carron, 1999; Irving et al.,

2002; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004a). Interestingly, qualitative research suggests that adolescent

boys involved in sports report engaging in such behaviors to increase athletic performance, but

nonetheless are fixated on their physiques for purely aesthetic reasons (Ricciardelli, McCabe, &

Ridge, 2006). To further understand the role of sports in the adoption of body change strategies,

Cafri et al. (2005) have drawn a distinction between organized and informal team sports; because

no operational definitions were provided for what constitutes an “organized” or “informal” sport,

Page 53: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

53

the following section is a general discussion about sport involvement regardless of organization

status.

Organized and informal team sports

Sports teams could theoretically include YMCA/YWCA and other locally organized

recreational leagues, in addition to professional, intercollegiate, or high school varsity teams.

Within the context of drive for muscularity and its correlates the most studied population of

competitive athletes is bodybuilders; indeed, success in this sport is completely reliant on unique

diet and exercise behavior. Furthermore, many of these athletes use substances to help achieve

maximal muscularity. Beyond bodybuilding (which will be discussed in further detail below), the

sports that are most associated with unhealthy behavior related to diet, exercise, or substance use

are those that are either (1) power or strength oriented (e.g., football, field events such as shot

put), or (2) weight oriented (e.g., wrestling, martial arts, horse jockey; Jonnalagadda,

Rosenbloom, & Skinner, 2001; Moore, Timperio, Crawford, Burns, & Cameron-Smith, 2002).

However, competitive sport involvement (e.g., football, basketball, tennis, cricket) has been

found to be a significant predictor of disordered eating and steroid use in early maturing

adolescent boys, but not in on-time or late-maturing boys (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004a). This

is consistent with the qualitative findings of Ricciardelli et al. (2006) in that part of the

explanation for the increased likelihood of early maturing boys to engage in these behaviors lies

in aesthetic concerns.

Weightlifting

The literature is equivocal when attempting to understand which groups of exercisers are

most susceptible to experiencing body-image disturbance. For example, athletically active

exercisers have lower levels of body-image disturbance than non-exercisers (Schwerin et al.,

1996), Also, exercise behavior is a strong predictor of body satisfaction in physically active men

Page 54: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

54

(Hausenblas & Fallon, 2002), and Davis and Cowles (1991) found body dissatisfaction to be

negatively correlated with physical activity. Furthermore, there is evidence that men who

regularly lift weights are satisfied with their bodies (Pickett et al., 2005), and that weightlifting

interventions designed to improve body satisfaction are efficacious (Williams & Cash, 2001).

However, it is clear that for some men weightlifting is taken to unhealthy levels to achieve the

ultimate high muscularity/low body fat look (Hildebrandt, Schlundt, Langenbucher, & Chung,

2006). Collectively, these findings suggest that exercise may have a protective effect against

body-image disturbance for most men, but may be associated with problematic psychopathology

and behavior in others.

Bodybuilders in particular report greater body-image disturbance than control men

(Mangweth et al., 2001), martial artists (Blouin & Goldfield, 1995), and power lifters (Lantz et

al., 2002). However, Boroughs and Thompson (2002) and Pickett et al. (2005) have recently

found that bodybuilders are quite satisfied with their bodies. These conflicting findings suggest

that some persons may initiate weightlifting as a result of pre-existing body dissatisfaction

(Kanayama et al., 2003), while others become dissatisfied with their bodies only after initiating a

weightlifting program; further research would help better understand this association.

Other Factors Related to Male Body-Image Disturbance

There are three additional factors that are notable when attempting to understand male

body-image disturbance; what follows is a brief review of eating disorders, exercise dependence,

and the impact of romantic partners.

Eating Disorders

Eating disorders are often viewed as one of the consequences of body-image disturbance,

and the interplay between these constructs in women has received a great deal of interest; indeed,

body-image disturbance is an extremely salient and stable risk factor for eating pathology

Page 55: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

55

(Cooley & Toray, 2001; Stice, 2002). Although there is no difference between men and women

in eating disorder presentation (Braun, Sunday, Huang, & Halmi, 1999), the relationship between

eating disorders and body-image disturbance in men has received little attention. College men

with eating disorders have greater levels of body-image disturbance than non-eating disordered

college men (Olivardia et al., 1995); this suggests that more research should examine the

relationship between these variables in men. It is worthwhile to note, however, that body-image

disturbance in men with eating disorders would likely center on dissatisfaction with overall

weight, whereas body-image disturbance in non-eating disordered men would likely center

around dissatisfaction with muscularity. That being said, Mangweth et al. (2001) demonstrated

the complexity of male body-image disturbance when they found that men with eating disorders

and male bodybuilders were virtually similar in their degree of dissatisfaction.

Because most current male body-image research addresses dissatisfaction specific to

muscularity, and because past body-image research has typically been done in women, there is

little information available on the relationship between male body-image disturbance and eating

disorders. In fact, preliminary evidence suggests that a history of eating disorders is associated

with muscle dysmorphia. For example, Olivardia et al. (2000) found a significantly higher

incidence of past eating disorders in men with muscle dysmorphia (29%) than in normal

comparison weightlifters (0%). Furthermore, Hitzeroth, Wessels, Zungu-Dirwayi, Oosthuizen,

and Stein (2001) found that 6% (1 of 15) of amateur South African competitive bodybuilders

with muscle dysmorphia had a past eating disorder. What follows is a brief overview of the

research examining the relationship between male body-image disturbance and each eating

disorder.

Page 56: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

56

Anorexia nervosa

Although anorexia is seen less frequently in men than in women, evidence indicates that

the disease presents with similar pathology for women and men (Woodside et al., 2001).

Because body-image disturbance is a key component of anorexia, it is essential to understand

body-image disturbance in men. For example, Gila et al. (2005) found that adolescent boys with

anorexia had significantly higher body dissatisfaction than non-anorexic adolescent boys; this

clearly indicates the presence of a relationship between the two, and suggests that if body-image

disturbance is alleviated it would be accompanied by an alleviation of other symptoms of

anorexia.

In some men anorexia may be a precursor to the development of muscle dysmorphia. For

example, Pope et al. (1993) found that two out of three bodybuilders with muscle dysmorphia

reported a history of anorexia. While the sample of muscle dysmorphic bodybuilders was small,

this unique pairing of opposite diagnoses at different times within the same individual suggests a

complex clinical picture of male body-image disturbance.

Bulimia nervosa

The clinical presentation of bulimia in men closely resembles that of bulimic women

(Pope, Hudson, & Jonas, 1986). Furthermore, adolescent boys with eating patterns similar to

bulimia and binge-eating disorder have expressed greater body dissatisfaction than age-matched,

non-eating disturbed controls (Keel, Klump, Leon, & Fulkerson, 1998). In addition, Blouin and

Goldfield (1995) identified a subgroup of bodybuilders whose psychological make-up consisted

of a disturbing mixture of body dissatisfaction, bulimic-like eating patterns, and steroid use.

Unfortunately, bulimic tendencies appear frequently in bodybuilders, a population that, despite

recent findings of Pickett et al. (2005), seems predisposed to body dissatisfaction (Lantz et al.,

2002).

Page 57: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

57

Binge-eating disorder

People with binge-eating disorder are typically obese, and previous sections of this

manuscript have established the link between bodyweight and body-image disturbance in men.

Specifically, several studies have found that body-image disturbance is significantly higher in

women with binge-eating disorder than in men (Barry, Grilo, & Masheb, 2002; Grilo & Masheb,

2005; Grilo, Masheb, Brody, Burke-Martindale, & Rothschild, 2005), and that body-image

disturbance is correlated with body mass index (Grilo & Masheb, 2005) in men with binge-

eating disorder. This is congruent with the finding that men often report body dissatisfaction

when they perceive themselves as either not muscular enough or too fat.

Exercise Dependence

Regular exercise has positive physical and psychological benefits (Berger & Motl, 2000;

Landers & Arent, 2001), a point underscored by a growing body of research that indicates

exercise is an effective intervention for treating body-image disturbance (e.g., Fisher &

Thompson, 1994; Williams & Cash, 2001). However, if taken to an unhealthy level exercise may

be detrimental to health. The “craving for leisure-time physical activity, resulting in

uncontrollable excessive exercise behavior, that manifests in physiological (e.g.,

tolerance/withdrawal) and/or psychological (e.g., anxiety, depression) symptoms” (Hausenblas &

Symons Downs, 2002a, p. 90) is called exercise dependence. Based on one’s motives for

excessive exercise behavior, exercise dependence can be described as either primary or

secondary. Primary exercise dependence is when the exercise behavior is the end in itself,

whereas secondary exercise dependence is when the exercise behavior is undertaken specifically

to alter or control the desired body composition (Carron, Hausenblas, & Estabrooks, 2003).

There is a subset of males who take exercise to unhealthy levels to attain the “ideal”

physique. Indeed, a person with exercise dependence may experience withdrawal effects, and

Page 58: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

58

this may be reflected in the finding of O’Dea and Abraham (2002) that 34% of surveyed college

men indicated they are distressed if unable to exercise as much as they desired, and that 20%

displayed characteristics of disordered eating. This unhealthy overemphasis on exercise, and the

simultaneous focus on disordered eating practices, is concerning in light of McCabe and

Ricciardelli’s (2004a) recent finding that exercise dependence in adolescent boys is predicted by

weight loss strategies over an 8–month period. Taken together, these findings suggest that, as

disruptive and harmful as exercise dependence may be, disordered eating habits may be a

precursor to unhealthy exercise behavior.

Research on drive for muscularity does not usually assess exercise dependence per se, but

instead assesses “body change strategies” in which exercise (e.g., McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004a;

Ricciardelli & McCabe, 2002) or, specifically, weightlifting (e.g., Smolak et al., 2005) is a

component. Contrast this approach with that undertaken in the muscle dysmorphia literature.

According to the diagnostic criteria proposed by Pope et al. (1997), exercise continuation that

results in significant social, occupational, or interpersonal impairment is a component of muscle

dysmorphia. Therefore, because the exercise behavior itself is implicit in the diagnosis, and

because it is implied that weightlifting is the mode of exercise, most assessment of exercise

behavior is gained as part of a diagnostic interview (e.g., Hitzeroth et al., 2001; Olivardia et al.,

2000). Attempts to circumvent an interview have led to the creation of measures such as the

Bodybuilding Dependence Scale (Smith, et al., 1998) and the Muscle Appearance Satisfaction

Scale (Mayville et al., 2002). However, these scales do not assess the actual mode, duration, and

frequency of exercise; instead, their focus is on the respondent’s degree of persistence in

exercising despite barriers (e.g., illness, injury, demands of daily activities). Therefore, the above

Page 59: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

59

findings indicate a need to quantify the mode, duration, and frequency of exercise behavior when

conducting future research on male body-image disturbance.

Romantic Partners

If there is only one certainty about how romantic partners (male-female dyads) view both

their own and their partner’s body it is that the female’s body is more scrutinized than the male’s

(Ogden & Taylor, 2000). In females, the scrutiny is related to the belief that the female should

either lose (because she is seen as too heavy) or gain (because she is seen as too thin) weight. For

example, Sheets and Ajmere (2005) found that an equal number (7%) of college women in their

study had been told within the previous 3 months by a dating partner to either lose or gain

weight; however, women who were told to gain weight had significantly higher relationship

satisfaction than women told to lose weight. This finding implies that thinner women perceive

themselves as closer to the “ideal” female physique. In addition, Weller and Dziegielewski’s

(2004) study of 117 college-aged women found that romantic partner support was significantly

and inversely related to body-image disturbance, which is consistent with the finding of Befort,

Robinson Kurpius, Hull-Blanks, Foley Nicpon, Huser, and Sollenberger (2001) that college-aged

women who receive weight-related criticism from romantic partners report greater body shame.

Although Ogden and Taylor (2000) have argued that female body dissatisfaction stems from

sources other than their romantic partner’s reactions to their bodies, the aforementioned findings

make it difficult to dismiss the impact males can have on the body-image of their female

romantic partners.

Can females have a similar impact on the body-image of their male romantic partners?

There has been little research done on this topic, but there is limited evidence, both qualitative

(e.g., Adams, Turner, & Bucks, 2005) and empirical, that the female partner’s evaluation of the

male’s body does impact the male’s body satisfaction. For example, in their study of college

Page 60: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

60

men, Sheets and Ajmere (2005) found that 24.4% of them had been told by a dating partner

within the previous 3 months to either lose or gain weight. However, a disproportionate number

of these men (19.7%) were told to gain weight; because the mean BMI of the sample men (M =

24.70) was higher than the BMI of 88% of these men, their female partners presumably were

expressing their desire for a male partner with more bulk on his frame. The message was not lost

on these males, as relationship satisfaction in men told to gain weight was significantly lower

than in other men (Sheets & Ajmere, 2005). Consistent with this, Tantleff-Dunn and Thompson

(1995) found that the only factors that significantly predicted appearance evaluation in men was

what the men perceived their female partners thought of their bodies and what their female

partners rated as their actual male body size preference. At the same time, other research

indicates that although females view their male partners’ bodies, as well as specific body parts

(e.g., shoulders, chest, arms), as closer to the “ideal” than the males believe they are, males still

display a significant discrepancy between what they consider the actual and the “ideal” sizes

(Ogden & Taylor, 2000). While the above findings indicate female romantic partners can

influence male body-image, further research is required to clarify this relationship.

Summary

This chapter has described the main factors relevant to the study of male body-image

disturbance. Males do experience body-image disturbance, although the rates of disturbance are

lower than in women. Another distinguishing feature that separates male body-image disturbance

from that experienced by women is the aspect of appearance that is dissatisfying: women

primarily are dissatisfied with self-perceived heaviness, whereas men may be equally dissatisfied

with either self-perceived heaviness or thinness. The Cafri et al. (2005) heuristic model of male

body change strategies serves as a useful tool in attempting to understand the complexity of male

body-image disturbance, especially because body-image disturbance is a known risk factor for

Page 61: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

61

eating disorders, a health concern no longer restricted entirely to females, and is associated with

low self-esteem and negative affect. Finally, it is believed that the drive for muscularity is also

associated with (1) use of anabolic steroids to gain muscle mass, (2) misuse of dietary

supplements designed to add muscle and strip away fat, and (3) the adoption of strict eating

behaviors that could lead to rapid weight fluctuation.

In conclusion, the model offered by Cafri et al. (2005) serves as the inspiration for the

current study. While there are seven main constructs in the model, for brevity the present study

focuses primarily on the testing of hypothesized relationships that either have limited empirical

support (e.g., biological factors and body-image dissatisfaction) or no proposed relationship

(e.g., self-esteem and body-image dissatisfaction) where one should be. Notable exclusions from

this study are social body comparison and the impact of sport. Social body comparison was

omitted from the study because of the strength of support already existing for its inclusion;

although weightlifting was included in the present study, the impact of organized and informal

sports was omitted because the primary investigator elected to focus on general exercise

behavior instead of sport involvement per se. Additional research on model constructs not

included in this study is warranted to further our knowledge of drive for muscularity and

associated body change behaviors.

Page 62: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

62

Health Risk Behaviors 1. Steroids 2. Steroid precursors 3. Ephedrine 4. Dieting to lose weight 5. Dieting to gain weight 6. Dieting to increase muscularity

Body-image

Dissatisfaction

Psychological

Functioning

Social Body

Comparison

Societal Factors 1. Media influence 2. Peer/parental influence 3. Teasing 4. Peer popularity

Biological Factors 1. Body composition/BMI

2. Pubertal growth

3. Pubertal timing

Sports 1. Organized team sports 2. Informal team sports 3. Weightlifting

Figure 3–1 A heuristic model of male body change behavior; solid arrows indicate relationships with greater support than broken

arrows. Reprinted from Clinical Psychology Review, Vol. 25, Cafri et al., Pursuit of the muscular ideal: Physical and psychological consequences and putative risk factors, pp. 215-239, Copyright (2005), with permission from Elsevier.

Page 63: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

63

CHAPTER 4 RESULTS

Descriptive Statistics

Body fat percentage was within the normal range for men of this age, and the FFMI

indicated muscularity of nonsteroid using recreational weightlifters (Table 1–1 contains

descriptive statistics). In addition, the mean objective BMI indicated “overweight” status; it was

also found that BMI based on objective measures of height and weight was significantly higher

than BMI based on self-reported height and weight (t(112) = 13.83, p < .001). The correlation

matrix for study variables is located in Table 1–2. Among the more noteworthy correlations are

the following: (1) Objective BMI and FFMI (r = .77, p < .01), (2) self-report BMI and FFMI (r =

.75, p < .01), and (3) perceived sociocultural pressure and the measures of body composition

(objective BMI: r = .32, p < .01; self-report BMI: r = .29, p < .01; body fat: r = .24, p < .01;

FFMI: r = .21, p < .05).

Multiple Regression Analyses

Because the correlation between objective BMI and self-reported BMI was so high (r =

.97), two separate regression analyses were run: one with objective BMI as an independent

variable and the other with self-report BMI as an independent variable. Because the results

across the analyses were consistent, only the regression analysis for the objective BMI is

reported here (Table 1–3 contains the regression analysis using subjective BMI); the decision to

report the data for the objective BMI was due to the fact that this method of BMI calculation is

more accurate than subjective BMI (Elgar et al., 2005). Examination of the tolerance values

revealed that I did not have multicolinearity among the independent variables (Tolerance values

range = .02 to .92; Mertler & Vannatta, 2002). The regression equation explained 51% of the

variance in drive for muscularity, F(10,102) = 12.68, p < .001, with weightlifting index (β = .17,

Page 64: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

64

p = .05), supplement use (β = .37, p < .001), exercise dependence (β = .24, p < .01), and self-

esteem (β = -.23, p < .01) emerging as significant predictors (Table 1–4).

Page 65: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

65

Table 4–1 Descriptive statistics for outcome variables ______________________________________________________________________________

Variable M SD Range BMI – Obj 25.05 3.63 17.79–42.08 BMI – SR 23.82 3.31 17.01–39.14 Body fat percentage 15.96 7.01 4.70–32.70 FFMI 21.20 2.26 16.59–29.32 Cardio frequency 2.63 1.79 0.00–10.00 Cardio duration (mins.) 41.04 34.66 .00–180.00 Weightlifting frequency 2.24 1.80 .00–6.00 Weightlifting duration (mins.) 38.41 31.36 .00–150.00 Weightlifting index 127.57 134.90 .00–750.00 LTEQ 55.83 41.91 .00–333.00 MDI – Diet 13.10 5.48 5.00–25.00 MDI – Supplement 8.31 4.65 4.00–22.00 PSPS 16.63 5.07 8.00–33.00 DMS – MBI 3.79 1.22 1.43–6.00 DMS – MB 2.53 1.01 1.00–5.00 DMS Total 3.16 .94 1.36–5.07 EDS 45.51 14.33 21.00–86.00 RSES 33.73 4.37 23.00–40.00 ______________________________________________________________________________Note: Obj = Measured; SR = Self-report; FFMI = Fat-free mass index; LTEQ = Leisure Time Exercise Questionnaire; MDI—Diet = Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory—Diet subscale; MDI—Supplement = Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory—Supplement subscale; PSPS = Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale; DMS—MBI = Drive for Muscularity—Muscle-oriented body image subscale; DMS—MB = Drive for Muscularity—Muscularity-related behaviors subscale; EDS = Exercise Dependence Scale; RSES = Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale.

Page 66: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

66

Table 4–2 Correlation matrix of outcome variables Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 1. BMI – OBJ - .97** .65** .77** .06 -.02 .19* .01 .32** -.21* -.02 -.14 .02 -.08 2. BMI – SR - .61** .75** .09 .00 .23* .01 .29** -.19* .00 -.12 .01 -.07 3. Body fat - .03 -.23* -.05 .08 -.03 .24** -.17 -.17 -.20* -.16 -.10 4. FFMI - .27** .04 .18 .02 .21* -.13 .11 -.03 .18 -.04 5. Weightlifting index - .24* .35** .45** -.03 .11 .62** .40** .36** .11 6. LTEQ - .10 .07 .02 -.01 .14 .06 .14 .06 7. MDI – Diet - .53** .08 .06 .55** .33** .33** .05 8. MDI -- Supplement - .12 .28** .77** .59** .40** -.02 9. PSPS - .22* .14 .21* .20* -.34** 10. DMS – MBI - .44** .88** .35** -.37** 11. DMS -- MB - .81** .57** -.09 12. DMS Total - -.53** -.29** 13. EDS - -.20* 14. RSES - ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Note: Obj = Measured; SR = Self-report; FFMI = Fat-free mass index; LTEQ = Leisure Time Exercise Questionnaire; MDI—Diet = Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory—Diet subscale; MDI—Supplement = Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory—Supplement subscale; PSPS = Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale; DMS—MBI = Drive for Muscularity—Muscle-oriented body image subscale; DMS—MB = Drive for Muscularity—Muscularity-related behaviors subscale; EDS = Exercise Dependence Scale; RSES = Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale. *p < .05. **p < .01.

Page 67: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

67

Table 4–3 Stepwise regression predicting drive for muscularity using subjective BMI Predictor β SE 95% Confidence interval Tolerance Subjective BMI .20 .07 -.07–.19 .08 Body fat -.28 .02 -.08–.00 .19 FFMI -.30 .08 -.28–.02 .13 Weightlifting index .17* .00 .00 .61 LTEQ -.03 .00 .00 .93 MDI – Diet .02 .01 -.03–.03 .62 MDI – Supplement .38*** .02 .04–.11 .57 PSPS .12 .01 -.01–.05 .77 EDS .25** .05 .01–.03 .68 RSES -.24** .02 -.08–(-.02) .83 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Note: FFMI = Fat-free mass index; LTEQ = Leisure Time Exercise Questionnaire; MDI—Diet = Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory—Diet subscale; MDI—Supplement = Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory—Supplement subscale; PSPS = Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale; EDS = Exercise Dependence Scale; RSES = Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale. F(10,102) = 12.85, p < .001, Adj R2 = .51; *p = .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Page 68: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

68

Table 4–4 Stepwise regression predicting drive for muscularity using objective BMI Predictor β SE 95% Confidence interval Tolerance Objective BMI .06 .12 -.22–.25 .02 Body fat -.20 .04 -.11–.05 .05 FFMI -.20 .15 -.38–.21 .04 Weightlifting index .17* .00 .00 .61 LTEQ -.03 .00 .00 .92 MDI – Diet .04 .01 -.02–.04 .65 MDI – Supplement .37*** .02 .04–.11 .56 PSPS .12 .01 -.01–.05 .76 EDS .24** .01 .01–.03 .69 RSES -.24** .02 -.08–(-.02) .81 ____________________________________________________________________________________________________________ Note: Obj = Measured; FFMI = Fat-free mass index; LTEQ = Leisure Time Exercise Questionnaire; MDI—Diet = Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory—Diet subscale; MDI—Supplement = Muscle Dysmorphia Inventory—Supplement subscale; PSPS = Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale; EDS = Exercise Dependence Scale; RSES = Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale. *p = .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

Page 69: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

69

CHAPTER 5 DISCUSSION

Drive for muscularity represents a form of male body dissatisfaction that is associated with

substance use, eating pathology, and exercise dependence (McCreary et al., 2004). However,

knowledge of the predictors of drive for muscularity, including the role of body composition, is

limited. Therefore, the purpose of my study was to identify the body composition and

psychological predictors of drive for muscularity. Implications of my results, study limitations,

and future research directions are discussed below.

First, consistent with my hypotheses and the research of McCreary et al. (2006), the body

composition measures of BMI, body fat percentage, and FFMI did not predict drive for

muscularity. It is possible that no effect emerged because the men in my sample had average

levels of body fat and FFMI, and were only slightly overweight; if more men in the sample

deviated from average values (e.g., had either higher body fat or FFMI) different results may

have been attained. However, the fact that FFMI did not emerge as a predictor of drive for

muscularity is noteworthy because theoretically one would expect to see higher drive for

muscularity in less muscular men. In explaining their findings, McCreary et al. (2006) speculated

that perhaps body fat “hides” the musculature that lies beneath, which would result in a man’s

inability to gauge his true level of muscularity – a similar effect may be at work here.

Nevertheless, my findings suggest that drive for muscularity occurs independently of body

composition.

Further similarities with McCreary et al. (2006) emerged, as evidenced by the following

significant correlations: (1) objective BMI and subjective BMI, (2) objective BMI and body fat

percentage, (3) subjective BMI and body fat percentage, (4) objective BMI and FFMI, and (5)

subjective BMI and FFMI. However, in a departure from McCreary et al. (2006), the correlation

Page 70: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

70

between body fat percentage and FFMI in my study was nonexistent. That these two measures of

body composition are uncorrelated is to be expected because of the distinct difference between

fat and muscle. Furthermore, all measures of body composition were weakly yet significantly

related to perceived sociocultural pressure; although the direction of causation is unclear, it is

possible that either (1) men misinterpret the messages coming from others and infer that their

body does not fit the “ideal” male physique, or (2) the body composition of the men truly doesn’t

reflect the “ideal” male physique, which increases the likelihood that a man would hear messages

confirming this.

Among the measures of body composition, only objective and subjective BMI were

significantly related to dietary habits consistent with drive for muscularity. It is possible that this

is due to the belief men have that eating more food, especially foods high in protein, will add

muscle; while this may help, the extra calories gained from these foods will lead to more fat

deposition as well, thus increasing BMI. However, while BMI was unrelated to muscularity-

related behaviors, it was inversely related to muscle-oriented body image; this is an unexpected

finding because it indicates that men with higher BMI’s have less concern about their

muscularity, although this may be clarified by the fact that BMI correlated significantly with

FFMI.

Second, in support of my hypotheses I found that supplement use, exercise dependence,

and self-esteem significantly predicted drive for muscularity. The findings regarding supplement

use indicate that it is predictive of drive for muscularity, as evidenced not only by the regression

analysis but also by significant correlations with weightlifting, dietary practices, muscle-oriented

body-image, and muscularity-related behaviors. Furthermore, supplement use was positively

related to exercise dependence. Thus, the negative health behaviors of pathological exercise and

Page 71: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

71

supplement use are related. Future research is needed to determine the temporal course of these

two behaviors because this information will aid in determining men who are at-risk for unhealthy

exercise and eating. The collective picture these findings paint is one of an interconnected set of

strategies simultaneously undertaken to increase muscularity.

The results of my study also revealed that exercise dependence predicts drive for

muscularity. It is possible that, because men view exercise as a critical means by which to

increase muscularity, some men experience severe frustration, negative mood, or anxiety when

they are prevented from exercising or are unable to exercise. Further evidence of the

ramifications of exercise dependence is found in its significant inverse relationship with self-

esteem; this indicates that for some men, feelings of self-worth are closely tied to their ability to

engage in exercise. Exercise dependence was significantly related to perceived sociocultural

pressure to be lean and muscular; thus it is possible that some men derive self-worth based on

whether they possess a physique that is deemed “ideal” by society, and exercise represents the

best way to achieve that ideal. Exercise dependence was also significantly related to

weightlifting, dietary practices, muscle-oriented body-image, and muscularity-related behaviors.

This indicates that some men, because of their high drive for muscularity, become dependent on

weightlifting as a specific form of exercise, and that this dependence is accompanied by

unhealthy eating aimed at increasing muscle. These conclusions are consistent with a host of

research in weightlifters and bodybuilders showing that exercise dependence and dietary

practices are related (Lantz et al., 2002; Rhea et al., 2004); indeed the Bodybuilding Dependence

Scale (Smith & Hale, 2004) was developed specifically to assess this unhealthy exercise

behavior.

Page 72: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

72

Self-esteem was a significant negative predictor of drive for muscularity, further

supporting the notion that for some men, self-esteem is closely connected to possessing the

“ideal” physique. This is consistent with research that has found an inverse relationship between

self-esteem and both satisfaction with muscularity and problematic eating behaviors in

preadolescent boys (McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2003; McGee & Williams, 2000). The results also

indicate that self-esteem is inversely related to both muscle-oriented body-image and perceived

sociocultural pressure to be lean and muscular. These findings not only underscore the impact of

the media on self-esteem when it comes to how men evaluate their bodies, but also how

physique-related comments from friends, parents, and dating partners impact self-esteem in

general. Therefore, these persons could provide a protective effect against body dissatisfaction

by building self-esteem in males via interpersonal support and refusal to themselves internalize a

body-image “ideal.”

Third, consistent with my hypothesis, objectively measured BMI was significantly higher

than BMI derived from self-reported height and weight. Upon inspection of the data, this

discrepancy in BMI was due to the tendency of men to overestimate their height. Thus, men

appear to be accurate in self-reporting their weight. However, they may overestimate their height

because of the self-presentational and social benefits of being tall. That is, researchers have

found that taller men typically attain high social status, are able to exert greater social

dominance, and enjoy the psychological upper hand in threatening situations (Cassidy, 1991;

Bailey, Caffrey, & Hartnett, 1976). My data support the recommendations of Eston (2002) and

suggest that when BMI is calculated in men, researchers should proceed cautiously when

interpreting their results, especially if the BMI is for diagnostic purposes (e.g., obesity, anorexia

nervosa). Furthermore, my findings are consistent with those of others (e.g., Brener et al., 2003;

Page 73: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

73

Hill & Roberts, 1998) who have found that self-reported height is overestimated relative to

measured height.

Fourth, consistent with my hypothesis, weightlifting behavior was a significant predictor of

drive for muscularity. For the man who desires greater muscularity weightlifting is a necessity

because it results in strength and muscle gains. This is not to say that weightlifting is inherently

pathological, maladaptive, or unhealthy; rather, my findings suggest that weightlifting may be a

tool misused by those men highly fixated on attaining a more muscular physique. It is a paradox,

then, that while weightlifting was significantly related to dietary practices, supplement use, and

muscularity-related behaviors, it was not significantly related to muscle-oriented body-image.

Collectively, these results suggest that men with high drive for muscularity rely heavily upon

weightlifting and other behaviors (e.g., supplement use) in pursuit of the “ideal” physique,

whereas men with moderate or low levels of drive for muscularity adopt these same behaviors

independent of body-image concerns (e.g., for perceived health benefits that supplements and

high protein diets offer).

Fifth, consistent with my hypothesis, general exercise behavior (as measured by the

LTEQ) did not predict drive for muscularity. It is possible that this is a reflection of the lack of

specificity regarding exercise mode inherent in the LTEQ; this study has established

weightlifting specifically as a predictor of drive for muscularity. Although there was a weak yet

significant relationship between the LTEQ and weightlifting, my results indicate that because the

LTEQ does not specifically assess weightlifting it may not be the best measure of exercise

behavior for use in men. Instead, the LTEQ assesses general leisure-time activity, and therefore

is more appropriate for body-image research in women, where the primary emphasis on body-

image is on weight reduction as opposed to muscular hypertrophy (McCabe et al., 2002).

Page 74: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

74

Contrary to my hypotheses, eating pathology and perceived sociocultural pressure did not

predict drive for muscularity. First, dietary behaviors were not predictive of drive for

muscularity. This is interesting in light of the finding that supplement use was predictive of drive

for muscularity; this suggests that normal college-aged men do not adopt eating behaviors aimed

at maximizing muscular growth (perhaps because it is too laborious), but they instead will

readily turn to supplements to achieve greater muscularity. This is consistent with the larger

societal trend towards “quick fixes” such as pills, powders, snack bars, exercise equipment, etc.,

that promise fast results with minimal effort. It is likely that dietary behaviors would have been

predictive of drive for muscularity had the sample been comprised of advanced weightlifters or

bodybuilders as such athletes are known to adopt strict dietary practices to achieve their goals of

muscular hypertrophy (Lantz et al., 2002).

Second, perceived sociocultural pressure did not predict drive for muscularity. The

Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale consists of eight items that ask the respondent how

messages about body fat and muscularity from friends, family, dating partners, and the media

impact them. It is possible that if these different influences were isolated then a more specific

prediction would be available. For example, friends and family may serve as protective factors

against developing drive for muscularity, whereas dating partners and the media may contribute

to drive for muscularity. It is noteworthy that fellow gym-goers or weightlifting partners were

not included in the Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale; it is possible that pressure to attain

greater muscularity may stem from others in the gym environment. Further research would aid in

clarifying this issue.

My findings have several implications for the Cafri et al. (2005) model. Specifically, my

results support their model of male body change strategies because weightlifting predicted drive

Page 75: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

75

for muscularity (body-image dissatisfaction in the model). My findings also suggest that the link

between health risk behaviors and body-image dissatisfaction should be bi-directional because

use of supplements predicted drive for muscularity. However, my findings do not support the

hypothesized link between body composition (BMI, body fat percentage, FFMI) and body

dissatisfaction; replication of this finding would suggest that this hypothesized link be removed.

Furthermore, my findings also indicate the need for two additions to the model: 1) self-esteem

predicted drive for muscularity (body-image dissatisfaction in the model), therefore providing

support for a directional arrow between psychological functioning and body-image

dissatisfaction, and 2) exercise dependence predicted drive for muscularity, therefore providing

support for its inclusion as a health risk behavior. Implementing these changes in the model

would help refine the relationships between these constructs and male body-image

dissatisfaction.

Limitations

Although this study advances our knowledge of how body composition and selected

psychological factors relate to drive for muscularity, there are several limitations. First, the

participants were a convenient sample of college-aged men, which limits the generalizability of

the results. Future researchers are encouraged to examine predictors of drive for muscularity in

other populations, including adolescent boys, older men, men who are not attending college, and

athletically active women (e.g., bodybuilders). For example, establishing predictors of drive for

muscularity in adolescent boys would aid in the identification of boys who are at highest risk of

drive for muscularity and its associated features. This is especially important because increasing

numbers of boys as young as 8-years old report body-image dissatisfaction and disordered eating

(Cohane & Pope, 2001; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2003). The generalizablility of the results is also

limited to college-aged men in Florida; because Florida is a peninsula its citizens are able to visit

Page 76: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

76

beaches (where a greater proportion of one’s body will be exposed to others) frequently. It is

therefore possible that men in Florida are more invested in their appearance than men who live in

northern states where such bodily exposure occurs less frequently. Thus, further research

examining drive for muscularity in other regions is needed.

Second, while demographic variables such as sexual orientation and ethnicity were

assessed, their low frequency precluded moderator analysis. It is possible that these variables, as

well as other demographic variables that were not assessed (e.g., socioeconomic status),

moderate drive for muscularity. With respect to sexual orientation, one participant reported

homosexual orientation, but seven men reported “Male” as their sexual orientation. Because the

question of sexual orientation was open-ended on the demographic questionnaire, it is unclear

whether these participants meant to communicate their gender or if they were indeed

homosexual. Most research indicates that homosexual men have greater body dissatisfaction and

drive for muscularity than heterosexual men (Kaminski et al., 2005; Yelland & Tiggemann,

2003), however other research has not supported this (e.g., Boroughs & Thompson, 2002).

Therefore, it would be informative if future research on drive for muscularity included sexual

orientation as a moderator.

Ethnicity has been shown to moderate body dissatisfaction in women, whereby Whites

report more body dissatisfaction than non Whites (e.g., Wildes, Emery, & Simon, 2001). For

example, when compared to Mexican women, American women have greater levels of concern

about significant weight gain and fear of becoming fat (Crandall & Martinez, 1996). Although

the topic has received little research attention in men, preliminary evidence suggests that culture

and ethnicity may also moderate male body dissatisfaction as well as the likelihood of adopting

body change strategies (Ricciardelli, McCabe, Williams, & Thompson, 2007). For example,

Page 77: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

77

there is evidence that Taiwanese men are more satisfied with their bodies than American and

European men (Yang et al., 2005), and other research indicates that nonwhite men are less likely

than white men to desire the V-shaped taper that is associated with a muscular physique (Fallon,

DeBraganza, Chittester, & Hausenblas, 2005). However, some research indicates that nonwhite

adolescent boys have greater rates of disordered eating than white adolescent boys (Neumark-

Sztainer & Hannan, 2000). Because this is counter to the available research in men with eating

disorders more research is needed.

Third, although current and past steroid use was ascertained, there were insufficient

numbers of participants who answered “yes” to these questions to conduct any meaningful

analyses (two participants indicated current steroid use, and one participant indicated past but not

current steroid use). However, four men who were not current steroid users indicated that the

question, “If you are not currently using anabolic steroids to build muscle mass, have you in the

past?” did not apply to them. This response technically would indicate current usage; whether

this was the case, or the response options provided were confusing to the participant, cannot be

determined.

This also illustrates the final limitation of the study, which is the use of self-report

measures. Although all self-report measures contained herein have adequate psychometric

properties, it is possible that some information collected from some participants on these types of

measures contain inaccuracies because of (1) Normal memory deficits, (2) inaccurate recall, or

(3) intentionally providing inaccurate information. For example, the measurement of exercise

behavior by the LTEQ was limited because the respondent was asked to indicate the number of

strenuous, moderate, and mild exercise sessions engaged in per week. In an attempt to help the

respondent answer the items, the LTEQ provides examples of activities that would qualify as

Page 78: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

78

strenuous, moderate, or mild in intensity, but the only reference to weightlifting is under the

strenuous exercise heading. This begs the question: Can a person have a “light” weightlifting

session? Most people would say yes, but because such a session must be subjectively described

as moderate or mild there is reason to suspect that the ability to accurately delineate between

these intensities is diminished: one man’s moderate intensity weightlifting session may be

another man’s mild intensity weightlifting session. In addition, while reliable and valid, the

skinfold method of body fat estimation has a ± 3.5% margin of error (ACSM, 2000); another

method of body fat estimation (e.g., DEXA) may yield a more precise estimate and thus

strengthen the findings of subsequent research.

Finally, out of 159 men who indicated they were interested in participating in the study by

signing up when the study announcement was made in their classes, 113 men actually enrolled in

the study. It is possible that the lack of follow through on the part of the 46 men who did not

participate was attributable to factors such as lack of time, insufficient incentive (e.g., knowing

their body fat percentage or earning extra credit toward their classes was not enticing enough to

participate), reluctance to have their body fat estimated (perhaps due to body-image concerns), or

simply lack of interest. However, all men who reported to the lab at their scheduled appointment

time to participate in the study completed all aspects of the study.

Future Research

Because body dissatisfaction in men has severe psychological and physical ramifications

further research is needed to examine its causes and consequences.

First, there is a need for a psychometrically sound measure that assesses eating behaviors

specific to adding muscle mass and decreasing body fat. Certain items of the DMS (McCreary &

Sasse, 2000) and MEBBIE (Kaminski et al., 2005) assess such eating patterns in a general sense,

but more precise items are required to fully understand the specific eating patterns of persons

Page 79: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

79

attempting to add muscle mass while simultaneously limiting body fat accumulation. The MDI

(Lantz et al., 2002; Rhea et al., 2004) shows promise.

Second, while Cafri et al. (2005) have proposed a model of the risk factors for male body-

image disturbance, they do not indicate a direct link between self-esteem and body image

dissatisfaction—why not? This is puzzling because there is sufficient evidence that there is a

direct inverse relationship between body-image disturbance and self-esteem, and this study has

found that self-esteem is a negative predictor of drive for muscularity in particular.

Third, most male body dissatisfaction research has been conducted in adolescent boys; the

only line of research that is a consistent exception to this is muscle dysmorphia. Indeed, it is

logical to focus research on a population that may be a target for intervention, but there is ample

evidence indicating there is a need for better understanding of body-image disturbance in men.

Specifically, information on drive for muscularity across the life span would contribute to

understanding how it is similar to, or different than, other men’s health issues (e.g., male pattern

baldness, weight gain, impotence). For example, it is possible that drive for muscularity is

especially salient in adolescence and young adulthood because this is a time when boys and

young men gain a sense of what it means to be masculine, and they therefore derive self-identity

from having a lean and muscular physique. In fact, some researchers have argued that, as

traditional gender roles become increasingly blurred and there exists greater parity between the

sexes, men may focus on developing their physiques in an effort to assert their masculinity

(Pope, Phillips et al., 2000). In light of this it would be informative to conduct prospective

research to determine the course of drive for muscularity as men age; at what point in a man’s

life does possessing a muscular physique lose its appeal or perceived benefits? For example, how

do significant life events such as marriage and fatherhood impact a man’s lifestyle, as well as his

Page 80: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

80

concept of what makes him masculine? Future research that addresses these questions would be

most enlightening.

Fourth, the conceptualization of male body-image disturbance as two parallel continua may

be beneficial (Figure 5–1). Together, these continua are able to encapsulate the finding that

males are dissatisfied with being too small (with respect to muscularity), not large enough (with

respect to muscularity), or too large (with respect to body fat). At opposite ends of the body fat

dimension is the dissatisfaction due to perceiving oneself to be either too skinny or too fat; this

reflects body-image disturbance relative to degree of body fat (e.g., the higher the body fat the

higher the body-image dissatisfaction). However, because body fat is only half of the body-

image equation for men, there is need for a muscle mass continuum that addresses dissatisfaction

with degree of muscle; this reflects the fact that body-image disturbance can also be a function of

dissatisfaction with minimal muscularity (which would increase the chances of a drive for

muscularity) or dissatisfaction that one’s actual high muscularity is not muscular enough (which

would indicate muscle dysmorphia). Therefore, these two criteria may be used together to

ascertain the likelihood of male body-image dissatisfaction if both body fat and muscularity

(expressed as FFMI) are known. For example, a man with little body fat and minimal

musculature would have a higher chance of experiencing body-image disturbance than a man

with little body fat but optimal musculature. Conversely, a man with high musculature and low

body fat would likely have minimal body-image dissatisfaction; the exception would be if,

despite his considerable muscle mass, the man perceived himself as having a “puny” or

“scrawny” body with minimal musculature – this would be indicative of muscle dysmorphia.

Fifth, more research is needed to determine the efficaciousness and safety of the myriad

nutritional supplements that men with body-image disturbance frequently use, especially given

Page 81: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

81

the findings of this study that supplement use is predictive of drive for muscularity. Current

knowledge indicates that some supplements (e.g., creatine) are relatively inert, whereas others

(e.g., ephedrine) pose potentially life-threatening side effects if abused or used by at-risk

persons.

Sixth, weightlifting as a specific mode of exercise was found to predict drive for

muscularity in this study, but the role of exercise in male body-image disturbance is still

relatively unclear. Many studies report a positive correlation between exercise behavior and body

satisfaction in men, yet some studies, especially in the muscle dysmorphia literature, indicate

bodybuilders, whose preferred mode of exercise is weightlifting, are more likely to experience

body-image disturbance than other groups of physically active men. This suggests that some men

involved in bodybuilding are more invested than others in their appearance and are therefore

more likely to be dissatisfied with perceived physique flaws; more research using samples of

male weightlifters would clarify this relationship.

Seventh, although the findings of this study and McCreary et al. (2006) indicate that there

is no relationship between body composition and drive for muscularity, there remains a lack of

research describing the correlation between body composition (as assessed by BMI, body fat,

and FFMI) and body-image disturbance in men. Most relevant would be further information

correlating these measures with drive for muscularity and muscle dysmorphia – does a person

need to have a certain combination of objective muscle mass and fat mass before becoming

driven to attain muscularity? The findings presented herein indicate no, however establishing a

dose-response-type relationship may improve understanding of how actual body composition is

related to body-image disturbance; in turn, this information could be integrated into interventions

(e.g., Cash & Hrabosky, 2004) aimed at decreasing body-image disturbance in males, or may aid

Page 82: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

82

researchers in determining which persons are most susceptible to experiencing body-image

disturbance (this could facilitate the identification of “at-risk” persons based on body

composition).

In conclusion, this study has established additional support for the Cafri et al. (2005)

model in that weightlifting was found to predict drive for muscularity (e.g., body-image

dissatisfaction in the model). Furthermore, whereas the model indicates that body-image

dissatisfaction leads to health risk behaviors (e.g., supplement use), this study found that

supplement use predicts drive for muscularity. In addition, this study also suggests two important

components be considered for inclusion in the model: (1) Self-esteem as a negative predictor of

drive for muscularity, and (2) exercise dependence, possibly under the “Health Risk Behavior”

or “Sports” constructs, as a predictor of body-image dissatisfaction. Finally, the finding that

measures of body composition do not predict drive for muscularity call into question the

proposed link between biological factors and body-image dissatisfaction in the model. Future

research is required to determine if body-image dissatisfaction in men truly is independent from

actual body composition. Collectively, this study’s findings add to the understanding of drive for

muscularity in men, and may potentially contribute to interventions designed to decrease drive

for muscularity and its associated features.

Page 83: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

83

Body fat dimension

Low Fat Optimal Excessive Fat

Muscle mass dimension

Minimal Optimal Excessive Muscularity Muscularity (Actual) (Actual)

Figure 5–1. Proposed continua for male body-image disturbance.

Page 84: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

84

APPENDIX A LIST OF MEASURES

General Information

Age: _____ Ethnicity: ______________ Sexual orientation: ____________ (e.g., hetero-, homo-, bi-sexual) Academic standing: _____ Fr. _____ So. _____ Jr. _____ Sr. Are you currently using anabolic steroids in order to build muscle mass? _____ Yes _____ No

If you are not currently using anabolic steroids to build muscle mass, have you in the past?

_____ Yes _____ No _____ Not applicable How many sessions of cardiovascular activity do you participate in per week? _____ How long (in minutes) does a typical session run? _______ How many weightlifting sessions do you participate in per week? _____ How long (in minutes) does a typical session run? _______

Page 85: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

85

Leisure-Time Exercise Questionnaire Instructions. This is a scale which measures your leisure-time exercise (i.e., exercise that was done during your free time such as intramural sports–NOT your physical education class). Considering a typical week, please indicate how often (on average) you have engaged in strenuous, moderate, and mild exercise more than 15 minutes during your free time? 1. Strenuous exercise: heart beats rapidly (e.g., running, basketball, jogging, hockey, squash, judo, roller skating, vigorous swimming, vigorous long distance bicycling, vigorous aerobic dance classes, heavy weight training) How many times per typical week do you perform strenuous exercise for 15 minutes or longer? ______ 2. Moderate exercise: not exhausting, light sweating (e.g., fast walking, baseball, tennis, easy bicycling, volleyball, badminton, easy swimming, popular and folk dancing) How many times per typical week do you perform moderate exercise for 15 minutes or longer? _______ 3. Mild exercise: minimal effort, no sweating (e.g., easy walking, yoga, archery, fishing, bowling, lawn bowling, shuffleboard, horseshoes, golf) How many times per typical week do you perform mild exercise for 15 minutes or longer?

_______

Page 86: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

86

MDI—Diet and Supplement Subscales

Instructions

Read each item (1-9) carefully and then indicate the degree to which the item is characteristic or true of you by circling the appropriate number corresponding to each statement. There are no right or wrong answers so please respond as honestly as possible. The anonymity of your responses is guaranteed.

1. I regulate my caloric intake to maximize muscle development. 1 2 3 4 5 6

2. Before a workout, I consume energy supplements. 1 2 3 4 5 6

3. I monitor my diet closely to limit my fat intake. 1 2 3 4 5 6

4. I use supplements to help me recuperate from strenuous workouts. 1 2 3 4 5 6

5. I control the intake of proteins, carbohydrates, and fats to maximize my muscular development. 1 2 3 4 5 6

6. I use supplements to increase my lifting performance. 1 2 3 4 5 6

7. I use nutritional supplements to help me train through injuries. 1 2 3 4 5 6

8. My diet is regimented to the point that I eat the same foods several days in a row. 1 2 3 4 5 6

9. I avoid foods high in sodium. 1 2 3 4 5 6

1 2 3 4 5 6

Never Rarely Sometimes Often Usually Always

Page 87: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

87

Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale—Original

Using the following scale, please indicate the response that best captures your own experience.

1 = Never 2 = Rarely 3 = Sometimes 4 = Often 5 = Always

1. I’ve felt pressure from my friends to lose weight.______ 2. I’ve noticed a strong message from my friends to have a thin body._____ 3. I’ve felt pressure from my family to lose weight.__ 4. I’ve noticed a strong message from my family to have a thin body._____ 5. I’ve felt pressure from people I’ve dated to lose weight.______ 6. I’ve noticed a strong message from people I have dated to have a thin body.____ 7. I’ve felt pressure from the media (e.g., TV, magazines) to lose weight._____ 8. I’ve noticed a strong message from the media to have a thin body.___

Page 88: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

88

Perceived Sociocultural Pressure Scale—Adapted Using the following scale, please indicate the response that best captures your own experience.

1 = Never 2 = Rarely 3 = Sometimes 4 = Often 5 = Always

1. I’ve felt pressure from my friends to lose body fat.______ 2. I’ve noticed a strong message from my friends to have a muscular body._____ 3. I’ve felt pressure from my family to lose body fat.__ 4. I’ve noticed a strong message from my family to have a muscular body._____ 5. I’ve felt pressure from people I’ve dated to lose body fat.______ 6. I’ve noticed a strong message from people I have dated to have a muscular body.____ 7. I’ve felt pressure from the media (e.g., TV, magazines) to lose body fat._____ 8. I’ve noticed a strong message from the media to have a muscular body.___

Page 89: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

89

The Drive for Muscularity Scale

Please read each item carefully, then, for each one, circle the number that best applies to you. 1. I wish that I were more muscular. 1 2 3 4 5 6 2. I lift weights to build up muscle. 1 2 3 4 5 6 3. I use protein or energy supplements. 1 2 3 4 5 6 4. I drink weight gain or protein shakes. 1 2 3 4 5 6 5. I try to consume as many calories as I can in a day. 1 2 3 4 5 6 6. I feel guilty if I miss a weight training session. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7. I think I would feel more confident if I had more muscle mass. 1 2 3 4 5 6 8. Other people think I work out with weights too often. 1 2 3 4 5 6 9. I think that I would look better if I gained 10 pounds in bulk. 1 2 3 4 5 6 10. I think that I would feel stronger if I gained a little more muscle mass. 1 2 3 4 5 6 11. I think that my weight training schedule interferes with other aspects of my life. 1 2 3 4 5 6 12. I think that my arms are not muscular enough. 1 2 3 4 5 6 13. I think that my chest is not muscular enough. 1 2 3 4 5 6 14. I think that my legs are not muscular enough. 1 2 3 4 5 6

1 2 3 4 5 6

Never Rarely Sometimes Often Very often Always

Page 90: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

90

Exercise Dependence Scale Instructions. Using the scale provided below, please complete the following questions as honestly as possible. The questions refer to current exercise beliefs and behaviors that have occurred in the past 3 months. Please place your answer in the blank space provided after each statement.

1 2 3 4 5 6 Never Always

1. I exercise to avoid feeling irritable._____ 2. I exercise despite recurring physical problems._____ 3. I continually increase my exercise intensity to achieve the desired effects/benefits._____ 4. I am unable to reduce how long I exercise._____ 5. I would rather exercise than spend time with family/friends._____ 6. I spend a lot of time exercising._____ 7. I exercise longer than I intend._____ 8. I exercise to avoid feeling anxious._____ 9. I exercise when injured._____ 10. I continually increase my exercise frequency to achieve the desired effects/benefits._____ 11. I am unable to reduce how often I exercise._____ 12. I think about exercise when I should be concentrating on school/work._____ 13. I spend most of my free time exercising._____ 14. I exercise longer than I expect._____ 15. I exercise to avoid feeling tense._____ 16. I exercise despite persistent physical problems._____ 17. I continually increase my exercise duration to achieve the desired effects/benefits._____ 18. I am unable to reduce how intense I exercise._____ 19. I choose to exercise so that I can get out of spending time with family/friends._____ 20. A great deal of my time is spent exercising.____ 21. I exercise longer than I plan._____

Page 91: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

91

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale

Directions: For each question, please indicate the degree to which you strongly agree, agree, disagree, or strongly disagree with each statement. Strongly Agree Agree Disagree Strongly Disagree 1 2 3 4 1. On the whole, I am satisfied with myself. 1 2 3 4 2. At times I think I am no good at all. 1 2 3 4 3. I feel that I have a number of good qualities. 1 2 3 4 4. I am able to do things as well as most other people. 1 2 3 4 5. I feel I do not have much to be proud of. 1 2 3 4 6. I certainly feel useless at times. 1 2 3 4 7. I feel that I am a person of worth, at least on an equal plane with others 1 2 3 4 8. I wish I could have more respect for myself. 1 2 3 4 9. All in all, I am inclined to feel that I am a failure. 1 2 3 4 10. I take a positive attitude toward myself. 1 2 3 4

Page 92: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

92

APPENDIX B RECRUITMENT FLYER

ATTENTION MEN

Have you ever wondered what your body fat percentage is? A study in the Department of Applied Physiology and Kinesiology is currently exploring the relationship between exercise habits, eating, body image, and body composition. All you need to do to qualify for your FREE body fat estimation is to simply fill out a few surveys (which will take about 20 minutes) – it’s that simple! To make an appointment please email the Exercise Psychology Laboratory (located in FLG 143) at [email protected] and include the following information:

• Name • Phone number • Age • Height • Weight

Page 93: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

93

LIST OF REFERENCES

Adams, G., Turner, H., & Bucks, R. (2005). The experience of body dissatisfaction in men. Body Image, 2, 271–283.

American College of Sports Medicine (2000). Guidelines for exercise testing and prescription (6th Ed.). Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th Ed.)—Text revision. Washington, D.C.: Author.

Andersen, A., Cohn, L., & Holbrook, T. (2000). Making weight: Men’s conflicts with food, weight, shape & appearance. Carlsbad, CA: Gurze Books.

Andersen, A. E., & Holman, J. E. (1997). Males with eating disorders: Challenges for treatment and research. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 33, 391–397.

Arkoff, A., & Weaver, H. B. (1966). Body image and body dissatisfaction in Japanese Americans. Journal of Social Psychology, 68, 323–330.

Barry, D. T., Grilo, C. M., & Masheb, R. M. (2002). Gender differences in patients with binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 63–70.

Befort, C., Robinson Kurpius, S. E., Hull-Blanks, E., Foley Nicpon, M., Huser, L., & Sollenberger, S. (2001). Body image, self-esteem, and weight-related criticism from romantic partners. Journal of College Student Development, 42, 407–419.

Berger, B.G., & Motl, R. W. (2000). Exercise and mood: A subjective review and synthesis of research employing the Profile of Mood States. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 12, 69–92.

Bizzarini, E., & De Angelis, L. (2004). Is the use of oral creatine supplementation safe? Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 44, 411–416.

Blouin, A. G., & Goldfield, G. S. (1995). Body image and steroid use in male bodybuilders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 18, 159–165.

Boreham, C., Robson, P. J., Gallagher, A. M., Cran, G. W., Savage, J. M., & Murray, L. J. (2004). Tracking of physical activity, fitness, body composition and diet from adolescence to young adulthood: The young hearts project, Northern Ireland. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 1, 14.

Boroughs, M., & Thompson, J. K. (2002). Exercise status and sexual orientation as moderators of body image disturbance and eating disorders in males. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 307–311.

Braun, D. L., Sunday, S. R., Huang, A., & Halmi, K. A. (1999). More males seek treatment for eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 415–424.

Page 94: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

94

Brener, N. D., McManus, T., Galuska, D. A., Lowry, R., & Wechsler, H. (2003). Reliability and validity of self-reported height and weight among high school students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 32, 281–287.

Brewerton, T. D., Stellefson, E. J., Hibbs, N., Hodges, E. L., & Cochrane, C. E. (1995). Comparison of eating disorder patients with and without compulsive exercising. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 413–416.

Brower, K. J., Blow, F. C., & Hill, E. M. (1994). Risk factors for anabolic-androgenic steroid use in men. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 28, 369–380.

Brown, T.A., Cash, T.F., & Mikulka, P.J. (1990). Attitudinal body-image assessment: Factor analysis of the body self relations questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 135–144.

Cafri, G., Roehrig, M., & Thompson, J. K. (2004). Reliability assessment of the somatomorphic matrix. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 35, 597–600.

Cafri, G., Strauss, J., & Thompson, J. K. (2002). Male body image: Satisfaction and its relationship to well-being using the somatomorphic matrix. International Journal of Men’s Health, 1, 215–231.

Cafri, G., & Thompson, J. K. (2004). Measuring male body image: A review of the current methodology. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 5, 18–29.

Cafri, G., Thompson, J. K., Ricciardelli, L., McCabe, M., Smolak, L., & Yesalis, C. (2005). Pursuit of the muscular ideal: Physical and psychological consequences and putative risk factors. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 215–239.

Cafri, G., van den Berg, P., & Thompson, J. K. (2006). Pursuit of muscularity in adolescent boys: Relations among biopsychosocial variables and clinical outcomes. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 35, 283–291.

Carron, A. V., Hausenblas, H. A., & Estabrooks, P. A. (2003). The psychology of physical activity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Cash, T. F., & Hrabosky, J. I. (2004). Treatment of body image disturbances. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Handbook of Eating Disorders and Obesity (pp. 515-541). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Cash, T. F., Morrow, J. A., Hrabosky, J. I., & Perry, A. A. (2004). How has body image changed? A cross-sectional investigation of college women and men from 1983 to 2001. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 1081–1089.

Cattarin, J. A., Thompson, J. K., Thomas, C., & Williams, R. (2000). Body image, mood, and televised images of attractiveness: The role of social comparison. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 220–239.

Page 95: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

95

Chilibeck, P. D., Stride, D., Farthing, J. P., & Burke, D. G. (2004). Effect of creatine ingestion after exercise on muscle thickness in males and females. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 36, 1781–1788.

Chittester, N. I. (2003). Development and validation of the muscle dysmorphia survey (MDS). Unpublished masters thesis, Washington State University.

Chng, C. L., & Moore, A. (1990). A study of steroid use among athletes: Knowledge, attitude and use. Health Education, 21, 11–17.

Chung, B. (2001). Muscle dysmorphia: A critical review of the proposed criteria. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 44, 565–574.

Coakley, J. (2004). Sports in society: Issues & controversies (8th ed). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Cohane, G. H., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (2001). Body image in boys: A review of the literature. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 373–379.

Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Cole, J. C., Smith, R., Halford, J. C. G., & Wagstaff, G. F. (2003). A preliminary investigation into the relationship between anabolic-androgenic steroid use and the symptoms of reverse anorexia in both current and ex-users. Psychopharmacology, 166, 424–429.

Cooley, E., & Toray, T. (2001). Body image and personality predictors of eating disorder symptoms during the college years. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 30, 28–36.

Davis, C., & Cowles, M. (1991). Body image and exercise: A study of relationships and comparisons between physically active men and women. Sex Roles, 25, 33–44.

Elgar, F. J., Roberts, C., Tudor-Smith, C., & Moore, L. (2005). Validity of self-reported height and weight and predictors of bias in adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health, 37, 371–375.

Emmanuel, N. P., Jones, C., & Lydiard, R. B. (1998). Use of herbal products and symptoms of bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 1627.

Eston, R. G. (2002). Use of the body mass index (BMI) for individual counseling: The new section editor for kinanthropometry is “Grade 1 obese, overweight” (BMI 27.3), but dense and “distinctly muscular” (FFMI 23.1)! Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 515–518.

Expert Panel on the Identification, Evalation, and Treatment of Overweight and Obesity in Adults (1998). Executive summary of the clinical guidelines on the identification, evaluation, and treatment of overweight and obesity in adults. Archives of Internal Medicine, 158, 1855–1867.

Page 96: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

96

Fisher, E., & Thompson, J. K. (1994). A comparative evaluation of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) versus exercise therapy (ET) for the treatment of body image disturbance. Behavior Modification, 18, 171–185.

Frederick, D. A., Peplau, L. A., & Lever, J. (2006). The swimsuit issue: Correlates of body image in a sample of 52,677 heterosexual adults. Body Image, 3, 413–419.

Freeman, R. J., Beach, B., Davis, R., & Solyom, L. (1985). The prediction of relapse in bulimia nervosa. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 19, 349–353.

Garner, D. M., Olmstead, M. P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorder inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 2, 15–34.

Gila, A., Castro, J., Cesena, J., & Toro, J. (2005). Anorexia nervosa in male adolescents: Body image, eating attitudes and psychological traits. Journal of Adolescent Health, 36, 221–226.

Godin, G., Jobin, J., & Bouillon, J. (1986). Assessment of leisure time exercise behavior by self-report: A concurrent validity study. Canadian Journal of Public Health, 77, 359–362.

Godin, G., & Shephard, R. J. (1985). A simple method to assess exercise behavior in the community. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Science, 10, 141–146.

Gokee-LaRose, J., Dunn, M. E., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. (2004). An investigation of the cognitive organization of body comparison sites in relation to physical appearance related anxiety and drive for thinness. Eating Behaviors, 5, 133–145.

Goldfield, G. S., Blouin, A. G., & Woodside, D. B. (2006). Body image, binge eating, and bulimia nervosa in male bodybuilders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 51, 160–168.

Graham, M. A., Eich, C., Kephart, B., & Peterson, D. (2000). Relationship among body image, sex, and popularity of high school students. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 90, 1187–1193.

Green, S. B. (1991). How many subjects does it take to do a regression analysis? Multivariate Behavioral Research, 20, 499–510.

Griffiths, R. A., Beumont, P. J. V., Giannakopoulos, E., Russell, J., Schotte, D., Thornton, C., et al. (1999). Measuring self-esteem in dieting disordered patients: The validity of the Rosenberg and Coopersmith contrasted. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 25, 227–231.

Grilo, C. M., & Masheb, R. M. (2005). Correlates of body image dissatisfaction in treatment-seeking men and women with binge eating disorder. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 38, 162–166.

Page 97: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

97

Grilo, C. M., Masheb, R. M., Brody, M., Burke-Martindale, C. H., & Rothschild, B. S. (2005). Binge eating and self-esteem predict body image dissatisfaction among obese men and women seeking bariatric surgery. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 37, 347–351.

Guggenheim, K., Poznanski, R., & Kaufmann, N. A. (1977). Attitudes of adolescents to their body build and the problem of juvenile obesity. International Journal of Obesity, 1, 135–149.

Haines, J., Neumark-Sztainer, D., Perry, C. L., Hannan, P. J., & Levine, M. P. (2006). V.I.K. (very important kids): A school-based program designed to reduce teasing and unhealthy weight-control behaviors. Health Education Research, 21, 884–895.

Haller, C. A., & Benowitz, N. L. (2000). Adverse cardiovascular and central nervous system events associated with dietary supplements containing ephedra alkaloids. New England Journal of Medicine, 343, 1833–1838.

Hallsworth, L., Wade, T., & Tiggemann, M. (2005). Individual differences in male body-image: An examination of self-objectification in recreational body builders. British Journal of Health Psychology, 10, 453–465.

Hartgens, F., & Kuipers, H. (2004). Effects of androgenic-anabolic steroids in athletes. Sports Medicine, 34, 513–554.

Hausenblas, H. A., & Carron, A. V. (1999). Eating disorder indices and athletes: An integration. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 21, 230–258.

Hausenblas, H. A., & Fallon, E. A. (2002). Relationship among body image, exercise behavior, and exercise dependence symptoms. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 32, 179–185.

Hausenblas, H. A., & Symons Downs, D. (2002a). Exercise dependence: A systematic review. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 3, 89–123.

Hausenblas, H. A., & Symons Downs, D. (2002b). How much is too much? The development and validation of the exercise dependence scale. Psychology and Health, 17, 387–404.

Heymsfield, S. B., Allison, D. B., Vasselli, J. R., Pietrobelli, A., Greenfield, D., & Nunez, C. (1998). Garcinia cambogia (hydroxycitric acid) as a potential antiobesity agent: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 280, 1596–1600.

Heywood, S., & McCabe, M. P. (2006). Negative affect as a mediator between body dissatisfaction and extreme weight loss and muscle gain behaviors. Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 833–844.

Hildebrandt, T., Schlundt, D., Langenbucher, J., & Chung, T. (2006). Presence of muscle dysmorphia symptomology among male weightlifters. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 47, 127–135.

Page 98: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

98

Hill, A., & Roberts, J. (1998). Body mass index: a comparison between self-reported and measured height and weight. Journal of Public Health Medicine, 20, 206–210.

Hitzeroth, V., Wessels, C., Zungu-Dirwayi, N., Oosthuizen, P., & Stein, D. J. (2001). Muscle dysmorphia: A South African sample. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 55, 521–523.

Huddy, D. C., Johnson, R. L., Stone, M. H., Proulx, C. M., & Pierce, K. A. (1997). Relationship between body image and percent body fat among male and female college students enrolled in an introductory 14-week weight-training course. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 85, 1075–1078.

Hutchins, G. M. (2001). Letter to the editor. New England Journal of Medicine, 344, 1095–1096.

Irving, L. M., Wall, M., Neumark-Sztainer, D., & Story, M. (2002). Steroid use among adolescents: Findings from project eat. Journal of Adolescent Health, 30, 243–252.

Jacobs, D. R., Jr., Ainsworth, B. E., Hartman, T. J., & Leon, A. S. (1993). A simultaneous evaluation of 10 commonly used physical activity questionnaires. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 25, 81–91.

Jonnalagadda, S. S., Rosenbloom, C. A., & Skinner, R. (2001). Dietary practices, attitudes, and physiological status of collegiate freshman football players. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 15, 507–513.

Kaminski, P. L., Chapman, B. P., Haynes, S. D., & Own, L. (2005). Body image, eating behaviors, and attitudes toward exercise among gay and straight men. Eating Behaviors, 6, 179–187.

Kanayama, G., Pope, H. G., Jr., Cohane, G., & Hudson, J. I. (2003). Risk factors for anabolic-androgenic steroid use among weightlifters: A case-control study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 71, 77–86.

Kanayama, G., Pope, H. G., Jr., & Hudson, J. I. (2001). “Body image” drugs: A growing psychosomatic problem. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70, 61–65.

Keel, P. K., Klump, K. L., Leon, G. R., & Fulkerson, J. A. (1998). Disordered eating in adolescent males from a school-based sample. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 125–132.

Kostanski, M., Fisher, A., & Gullone, E. (2004). Current conceptualization of body image dissatisfaction: Have we got it wrong? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 1317–1325.

Kostanski, M., & Gullone, E. (1998). Adolescent body image dissatisfaction: Relationships with self-esteem, anxiety, and depression controlling for body mass. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 255–262.

Page 99: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

99

Kouri, E. M., Pope, H. G., Jr., & Katz, D. L. (1994). Letter to the editor. JAMA, 271, 347.

Kouri, E. M., Pope, H. G., Jr., Katz, D. L., & Oliva, P. (1995). Fat-free mass index in users and nonusers of anabolic-androgenic steroids. Clinical Journal of Sport Medicine, 5, 223–228.

Kraemer, W. J., Torine, J. C., Silvestre, R., French, D. N., Ratamess, N. A., Spiering, B. A., et al. (2005). Body size and composition of national football league players. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 19, 485–489.

Kreider, R. B. (1999). Dietary supplements and the promotion of muscle growth with resistance exercise. Sports Medicine, 27, 97–110.

Labre, M. P. (2005). Burn fat, build muscle: A content analysis of Men’s Health and Men’s Fitness. International Journal of Men’s Health, 4, 187–200.

Landers, D. M., & Arent, S. M. (2001). Physical activity and mental health. In R. N. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, & C. M. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (2nd Ed., pp 740-765). New York, NY: Wiley.

Lantz, C. D., Rhea, D. J., & Cornelius, A. E. (2002). Muscle dysmorphia in elite-level power lifters and bodybuilders: A test of differences within a conceptual model. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 16, 649–655.

Lefavi, R. G., Reeve, T. G., & Newland, M. C. (1990). Relationship between anabolic steroid use and selected psychological parameters in male bodybuilders. Journal of Sport Behavior, 13, 157–166.

Leit, R. A., Gray, J. J., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (2002). The media’s representation of the ideal male body: A cause for muscle dysmorphia? International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 334–338.

Leit, R. A., Pope, H. G., Jr., & Gray, J. J. (2001). Cultural expectations of muscularity in men: The evolution of playgirl centerfolds. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 90–93.

Macgregor, F. C. (1981). Patient dissatisfaction with results of technically satisfactory surgery. Aesthetic Plastic Surgery, 5, 27–32.

Mangweth, B., Pope, H. G., Jr., Kemmler, G., Ebenbichler, C., Hausmann, A., De Col, C., et al. (2001). Body image and psychopathology in male bodybuilders. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70, 38–43.

Manson, J. E., Skerrett, P. J., Greenland, P., & VanItallie, T. B. (2004). The escalating pandemics of obesity and sedentary lifestyle: A call to action for clinicians. Archives of Internal Medicine, 164, 249–258.

Page 100: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

100

Mayville, S. B., Williamson, D. A., White, M. A., Netemeyer, R. G., & Drab, D. L. (2002). Development of the muscle appearance satisfaction scale: A self-report measure for the assessment of muscle dysmorphia symptoms. Assessment, 9, 351–360.

Mazzeo, S. E., Slof, R. M., Tozzi, F., Kendler, K. S., & Bulik, C. M. (2004). Characteristics of men with persistent thinness. Obesity Research, 12, 1367–1369.

McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2003). Body image and strategies to lose weight and increase muscle among boys and girls. Health Psychology, 22, 39–46.

McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2004a). A longitudinal study of pubertal timing and extreme body change behaviors among adolescent boys and girls. Adolescence, 39, 145–166.

McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2004b). Body image dissatisfaction among males across the lifespan: A review of past literature. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 56, 675–685.

McCabe, M. P., & Ricciardelli, L. A. (2005). A prospective study of pressures from parents, peers, and the media on extreme weight change behaviors among adolescent boys and girls. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43, 653–668.

McCabe, M. P., Ricciardelli, L. A., & Banfield, S. (2001). Body image, strategies to change muscles and weight, and puberty: Do they impact on positive and negative affect among adolescent boys and girls? Eating Behaviours, 2, 129–149.

McCabe, M. P., Ricciardelli, L. A., & Finemore, J. (2002). The role of puberty, media, and popularity with peers as strategies to increase weight, decrease weight and increase muscle tone among adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 52, 145–153.

McCreary, D. R., Karvinen, K., & Davis, C. (2006). The relationship between the drive for muscularity and anthropometric measures of muscularity and adiposity. Body Image, 3, 145–152.

McCreary, D. R., & Sasse, D. K. (2000). An exploration of the drive for muscularity in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of American College Health, 48, 297–304.

McCreary, D. R., Sasse, D. K., Saucier, D. M., & Dorsch, K. D. (2004). Measuring the drive for muscularity: Factorial validity of the drive for muscularity scale in men and women. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 5, 49–58.

McGee, R., & Williams, S. (2000). Does low self-esteem predict health compromising behaviours among adolescents? Journal of Adolescence, 23, 569–582.

Moore, J. M., Timperio, A. F., Crawford, D. A., Burns, C. M., & Cameron-Smith, D. (2002). Weight management and weight loss strategies of professional jockeys. International Journal of Sport Nutrition and Exercise Metabolism, 12, 1–13.

Page 101: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

101

Muris, P., Meesters, C., van de Blom, W., & Mayer, B. (2005). Biological, psychological, and sociocultural correlates of body change strategies and eating problems in adolescent boys and girls. Eating Behaviors, 6, 11–22.

Neumark-Sztainer, D., & Hannan, P. J. (2000). Weight-related behaviors among adolescent girls and boys. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 154, 569–577.

Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., Falkner, N. H., Beuhring, T., & Resnick, M. D. (1999). Sociodemographic and personal characteristics of adolescents engaged in weight loss and weight/muscle gain behaviors: Who is doing what? Preventive Medicine, 28, 40–50.

O’Dea, J. A., & Abraham, S. (1999). Onset of disordered eating attitudes and behaviors in early adolescence: Interplay of pubertal status, gender, weight, and age. Adolescence, 34, 671–679.

O’Dea, J. A., & Abraham, S. (2002). Eating and exercise disorders in young college men. Journal of American College of Health, 50, 273–278.

Ogden, J., & Taylor, C. (2000). Body dissatisfaction within couples: Adding the social context to perceptions of self. Journal of Health Psychology, 5, 25–32.

Oliosi, M., Dalle Grave, R., & Burlini, S. (1999). Eating attitudes in noncompetitive male body builders. Eating Disorders, 7, 227–233.

Olivardia, R. (2001). Mirror, mirror on the wall, who’s the largest of them all? The features and phenomenology of muscle dysmorphia. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 9, 254–259.

Olivardia, R., Pope, H. G., Jr., & Hudson, J. I. (2000). Muscle dysmorphia in male weightlifters: A case-control study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1291–1296.

Olivardia, R., Pope, H. G., Jr., Mangweth, B., & Hudson, J. I. (1995). Eating disorders in college men. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1279–1285.

Olrich, T. W., & Ewing, M. E. (1999). Life on steroids: Bodybuilders describe their perceptions of the anabolic-androgenic steroid use period. The Sport Psychologist, 13, 299–312.

Pasman, L., & Thompson, J. K. (1988). Body image and eating disturbance in obligatory runners, obligatory weightlifters, and sedentary individuals. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 7, 759–769.

Paxton, S. J., Eisenberg, M. E., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2006). Prospective predictors of body dissatisfaction in adolescent girls and boys: A five-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 42, 888–899.

Peterson, L. J., & Topazian, R. G. (1976). Psychological considerations in corrective maxillary and midfacial surgery. Journal of Oral Surgery, 34, 157–164.

Page 102: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

102

Phelps, L., Johnston, L. S., & Augustyniak, K. (1999). Prevention of eating disorders: Identification of predictor variables. Eating Disorders, 7, 99–108.

Phillips, K. A., O’Sullivan, R. L., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (1997). Muscle dysmorphia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 58, 361.

Pickett, T. C., Lewis, R. J., & Cash, T. F. (2005). Men, muscles, and body image: Comparisons of competitive bodybuilders, weight trainers, and athletically active controls. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 39, 217–222.

Pope, H. G., Jr., Gruber, A. J., Choi, P., Olivardia, R., & Phillips, K. A. (1997). Muscle dysmorphia: An underrecognized form of body dysmorphic disorder. Psychosomatics, 38, 548–557.

Pope, H. G., Jr., Gruber, A. J., Mangweth, B., Bureau, B., DeCol, C., Jouvent, R., et al. (2000). Body image perception among men in three countries. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 1297–1301.

Pope, H. G., Jr., Hudson, J. I., & Jonas, J. M. (1986). Bulimia in men: A series of fifteen cases. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174, 117–119.

Pope, H. G., Jr., & Katz, D. L. (1987). Bodybuilder’s psychosis. The Lancet, 1, 863.

Pope, H. G., Jr., & Katz, D. L. (1988). Affective and psychotic symptoms associated with anabolic steroid use. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145, 487–490.

Pope, H. G., Jr., & Katz, D. L. (1994). Psychiatric and medical effects of anabolic-androgenic steroid use. Archives of General Psychiatry, 51, 375–382.

Pope, H. G., Jr., Katz, D. L., & Hudson, J. I. (1993). Anorexia nervosa and “reverse anorexia” among 108 male bodybuilders. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 34, 406–409.

Pope, H. G., Jr., Olivardia, R., Gruber, A., & Borowiecki, J. (1999). Evolving ideals of male body image as seen through action toys. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 65–72.

Pope, H. G., Jr., Phillips, K. A., & Olivardia, R. (2000). The adonis complex: The secret crisis of male body obsession. New York, NY: The Free Press.

Presnell, K., Bearman, S. K., & Stice, E. (2004). Risk factors for body dissatisfaction in adolescent boys and girls: A prospective study. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, 389–401.

Rand, C. S. W., & Wright, B. A. (2000). Thinner females and heavier males: Who says? A comparison of female to male ideal body sizes across a wide age span. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 29, 45–50.

Page 103: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

103

Rhea, D. J., Lantz, C. D., & Cornelius, A. E. (2004). Development of the muscle dysmorphia inventory (MDI). Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fitness, 44, 428–435.

Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2002). Psychometric evaluation of the body change inventory: An assessment instrument for adolescent boys and girls. Eating Behaviors, 3, 45–59.

Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2004). A biopsychosocial model of disordered eating and the pursuit of muscularity in adolescent boys. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 179–205.

Ricciardelli, L. A., McCabe, M. P., & Ridge, D. (2006). The construction of the adolescent male body through sport. Journal of Health Psychology, 11, 577–587.

Rodriguez-Tomẻ, H., Bariaud, F., Cohen Zardi, M. F., Delmas, C., Jeanvoine, B., & Szylagyi, P. (1993). The effects of pubertal changes on body image and relations with peers of the opposite sex in adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 16, 421–438.

Rosenberg, M. (1989). Society and the adolescent self-image (Rev. ed). Middletown, CT: Wesleyan University Press.

Rozin, P., Trachtenberg, S., & Cohen, A. B. (2001). Stability of body image and body image dissatisfaction in American college students over about the last 15 years. Appetite, 37, 245–248.

Salusso-Deonier, C. J., & Schwarzkopf, R. J. (1991). Sex differences in body-cathexis associated with exercise involvement. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 73, 139–145.

Saper, R. B., Eisenberg, D. M., & Phillips, R. S. (2004). Common dietary supplements for weight loss. American Family Physician, 70, 1731–1738.

Schwerin, M. J., Corcoran, K. J., Fisher, L., Patterson, D., Askew, W., Olrich, T., et al. (1996). Social physique anxiety, body esteem, and social anxiety in bodybuilders and self-reported anabolic steroid users. Addictive Behaviors, 21, 1–8.

Shapiro, J. R., & Anderson, D. A. (2003). The effects of restraint, gender, and body mass index on the accuracy of self-reported weight. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 34, 177–180.

Sharp, C. W., Clark, S. A., Dunan, J. R., Blackwood, D. H., & Shapiro, C. M. (1994). Clinical presentation of anorexia nervosa in males: 24 new cases. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 125–134.

Sheets, V., & Ajmere, K. (2005). Are romantic partners a source of college students’ weight concern? Eating Behaviors, 6, 1–9.

Siegel, J. M., Yancey, A. K., Aneshensel, C. S., & Schuler, R. (1999). Body image, perceived pubertal timing, and adolescent mental health. Journal of Adolescent Health, 25, 155–165.

Page 104: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

104

Smith, D., & Hale, B. (2004). Validity and factor structure of the bodybuilding dependence scale. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 38, 177–181.

Smith, D. K., Hale, B. D., & Collins, D. (1998). Measurement of exercise dependence in bodybuilders. Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Activity, 38, 66–74.

Smolak, L., Murnen, S. K., & Thompson, J. K. (2005). Sociocultural influences and muscle building in adolescent boys. Psychology of Men & Masculinity, 6, 227–239.

Spitzer, B. L., Henderson, K. A., & Zivian, M. T. (1999). Gender differences in population versus media body sizes: A comparison over four decades. Sex Roles, 40, 545–565.

Stewart, T. M., & Williamson, D. A. (2004). Assessment of body image disturbances. In J. K. Thompson (Ed.), Handbook of Eating Disorders and Obesity (pp. 495-514). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825–848.

Strong, S. M. (2005). The role of exposure to media-idealized male physiques on men’s body image (Doctoral dissertation, University of Texas-Austin, 2005). Dissertation Abstracts International, 65, 4306.

Symons Downs, D., Hausenblas, H. A., & Nigg, C. R. (2004). Factorial validity and psychometric examination of the exercise dependence scale-revised. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Science, 8, 183–201.

Tantleff-Dunn, S., & Thompson, J. K. (1995). Romantic partners and body image disturbance: Further evidence for the role of perceived-actual disparities. Sex Roles, 33, 589–605.

Tiggemann, M. (2005). Television and adolescent body image: The role of program content and viewing motivation. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 361–381.

Tiggemann, M., & Williamson, S. (2000). The effect of exercise on body satisfaction and self-esteem as a function of gender and age. Sex Roles, 43, 119–127.

Traub, S. J., Hoyek, W., & Hoffman, R. S. (2001). Letter to the editor. New England Journal of Medicine, 344, 1096.

Tricker, R., O’Neill, M. R., & Cook, D. (1989). The incidence of anabolic steroid use among competitive bodybuilders. Journal of Drug Education, 19, 313–325.

Tritschler, K. (2000). Practical measurement and assessment (5th ed). Baltimore, MD: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

Page 105: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

105

van Loon, L. J. C., van Rooijen, J. J. M., Niesen, B., Verhagen, H., Saris, W. H. M., & Wagenmakers, A. J. M. (2000). Effects of acute (-)-hydroxycitrate supplementation on substrate metabolism at rest and during exercise in humans. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 72, 1445–1450.

Varnado-Sullivan, P. J., Horton, R., & Savoy, S. (2006). Differences for gender, weight and exercise in body image disturbance and eating disorder symptoms. Eating and Weight Disorders, 11, 118–125.

Wang, S. S., Houshyar, S., & Prinstein, M. J. (2006). Adolescent girls’ and boys weight-related health behaviors and cognitions: Associations with reputation- and preference-based peer status. Health Psychology, 25, 658–663.

Wei, M., Kampert, J. B., Barlow, C. E., Nichaman, M. Z., Gibbons, L. W., Paffenbarger, R. S., et al. (1999). Relationship between low cardiorespiratory fitness and mortality in normal-weight, overweight, and obese men. JAMA, 282, 1547–1553.

Weller, J. E., & Dziegielewski, S. F. (2004). The relationship between romantic partner support styles and body image disturbance. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 10, 71–92.

Wertheim, E. H., Martin, G., Prior, M., Sanson, A., & Smart, D. (2002). Parent influences in the transmission of eating and weight related values and behaviors. Eating Disorders, 10, 321–334.

Willett, W. C., Dietz, W. H., & Colditz, G. A. (1999). Guidelines for healthy weight. New England Journal of Medicine, 341, 427–434.

Williams, P. A., & Cash, T. F. (2001). Effects of a circuit weight training program on the body images of college students. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 30, 75–82.

Woodside, D. B., Garfinkel, P. E., Lin, E., Goering, P., Kaplan, A. S., Goldbloom, D. S., et al. (2001). Comparisons of men with full or partial eating disorders, men without eating disorders, and women with eating disorders in the community. American Journal of Psychatry, 158, 570–574.

Wroblewska, A.-M. (1997). Androgenic-anabolic steroids and body dysmorphia in young men. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 42, 225–234.

Yang, C. F. J., Gray, P., & Pope, H. G., Jr. (2005). Male body image in Taiwan versus the west: Yanggang zhiqi meets the adonix complex. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 263–269.

Ziegenfuss, T. N., Berardi, J. M., Lowery, L. M., & Antonio, J. (2002). Effects of prohormone supplementation in humans: A review. Canadian Journal of Applied Physiology, 27, 628–645.

Page 106: PREDICTION OF DRIVE FOR MUSCULARITY BY BODY …ufdcimages.uflib.ufl.edu/UF/E0/02/01/25/00001/chittester_n.pdf · are exposed to the muscular “ideal” at an increasingly younger

106

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Nickles Irvin Chittester was born on 20 November 1976 in Mason City, Iowa. His parents

moved him and his younger sister to Phoenix, Arizona, in 1980, and this is where he was raised.

After graduating from Deer Valley High School in Glendale, Arizona, in June of 1995, Nick

enrolled at Glendale Community College, where he earned an A.A. (major: psychology) in May

of 1998. He enrolled at Arizona State University’s West campus in Phoenix in June of 1997,

where he graduated with honors with a B.A. (major: psychology) in May of 1999. In August of

1999, he moved to Pullman, Washington, to pursue a Ph.D. in experimental psychology at

Washington State University (WSU). However, in late April of 2001 his advisor, Dr. Lori Irving,

passed away unexpectedly from a previously unknown heart condition. He earned an M.S. in

experimental psychology from WSU in May of 2003; in August of 2003 he moved to

Gainesville, Florida, to attend the University of Florida, where in May of 2007 he earned a Ph.D.

in Health and Human Performance with an emphasis in Sport and Exercise Psychology.

Upon completion of his Ph.D. he married Mindy Mansour on 12 May 2007 in

Gainesville, Florida. In August of 2007 he began his appointment as an assistant professor of

psychology at Concordia University at Austin, Texas.