neurobiology of muscle fatigue - freee.guigon.free.fr/rsc/article/enokastuart92.pdfneurobiology of...

18
Neurobiology of muscle fatigue ROGER M. ENOKA AND DOUGLAS G. STUART Departments of Physiology and Exercise and Sport Sciences, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721 ENOKA, ROGER M., AND DOUGLAS G. STUART.Neurobiology of muscle fatigue. J. Appl. Physiol. 72(5): 1631-1648, 1992.-Muscle fatigue encom- passes a class of acute effects that impair motor performance. The mecha- nisms that can produce fatigue involve all elements of the motor system, from a failure of the formulation of the descending drive provided by supra- segmental centers to a reduction in the activity of the contractile proteins. We propose four themes that provide a basis for the systematic evaluation of the neural and neuromuscular fatigue mechanisms: 1) task dependency to identify the conditions that activate the various mechanisms; 2) force- fatigability relationship to explore the interaction between the mechanisms that results in a hyperbolic relationship between force and endurance time; 3) muscle wisdom to examine the association among a concurrent decline in force, relaxation rate, and motor neuron discharge that results in an optimization of force; and 4) sense of effort to determine the role of per- ceived effort in the impairment of performance. On the basis of this per- spective with an emphasis on neural mechanisms, we suggest a number of experiments to advance our understanding of the neurobiology of muscle fatigue. task; force-fatigability relationship; muscle wisdom; sense of effort ONEPROMINENTCHARACTERISTIC oftheneuromuscular system is its adaptability. When it is subjected to a chronic stimulus, such as immobilization, training, or ag- ing, it can adapt to the altered demands of usage. These adaptations can be extensive and have been shown to affect most aspects of the system, both morphological and functional. Similarly, the neuromuscular system can adapt to more acute challenges, such as those associated with sustained activity. One of the best known acute ad- aptations is a phenomenon that is referred to as muscle fatigue (hereafter referred to as fatigue). To advance our understanding of the adaptive responses that we call fa- tigue, it is important that we identify a set of principles, including a definition, that can provide the basis for sub- sequent studies. However, although the literature on fa- tigue is extensive, its interpretation is confounded by the diversity of definitions and by the variety of paradigms that have been used to study it. Our perspective is to consider fatigue as a phenomenon that had its origin in the study of human performance (for historical prece- dents see Refs. 5,l7l,l77,l87,190,220). In this context, fatigue is a general concept intended to denote an acute impairment of performance that includes both an in- crease in the perceived effort necessary to exert a desired force and an eventual inability to produce this force (79; for other definitions see Refs. 3,16,69,90). Because of its link to human performance and the multidisciplinary in- terest in the study of fatigue, it does not seem appro- priate or necessary to attribute more precision to the defi- nition of fatigue. Rather, the study of fatigue should focus on the mechanisms that can contribute to an acute impairment of performance. Not surprisingly, the notion that the neuromuscular system can be stressed to the point of exhaustion has intrigued numerous investigators, particularly in recent years. From these studies on fatigue have emerged at least four themes (task dependency, force-fatigability re- lationship, muscle wisdom, and sense of effort) that seem to provide a reasonable basis for future neural and neuro- muscular work in this area, especially studies for which the requisite techniques are already available. Although many studies have been undertaken in an attempt to identify a single fatigue factor, there seems to be little doubt that the concept of fatigue refers to a class of acute effects that can impair motor performance and not to a single mechanism that can, under all conditions, account for the decline in performance. These observations have led to the notion that the mechanisms underlying fatigue are task dependent. Furthermore the fatigue mecha- nisms appear to cascade so as to exhibit a hyperbolic 0161-7567/92 $2.00 Copyright 0 1992 the American Physiological Society 1631

Upload: dangdieu

Post on 14-Mar-2018

219 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Neurobiology of muscle fatigue

ROGER M. ENOKA AND DOUGLAS G. STUART Departments of Physiology and Exercise and Sport Sciences, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721

ENOKA, ROGER M., AND DOUGLAS G. STUART. Neurobiology of muscle fatigue. J. Appl. Physiol. 72(5): 1631-1648, 1992.-Muscle fatigue encom- passes a class of acute effects that impair motor performance. The mecha- nisms that can produce fatigue involve all elements of the motor system, from a failure of the formulation of the descending drive provided by supra- segmental centers to a reduction in the activity of the contractile proteins. We propose four themes that provide a basis for the systematic evaluation of the neural and neuromuscular fatigue mechanisms: 1) task dependency to identify the conditions that activate the various mechanisms; 2) force- fatigability relationship to explore the interaction between the mechanisms that results in a hyperbolic relationship between force and endurance time; 3) muscle wisdom to examine the association among a concurrent decline in force, relaxation rate, and motor neuron discharge that results in an optimization of force; and 4) sense of effort to determine the role of per- ceived effort in the impairment of performance. On the basis of this per- spective with an emphasis on neural mechanisms, we suggest a number of experiments to advance our understanding of the neurobiology of muscle fatigue.

task; force-fatigability relationship; muscle wisdom; sense of effort

ONEPROMINENTCHARACTERISTIC oftheneuromuscular system is its adaptability. When it is subjected to a chronic stimulus, such as immobilization, training, or ag- ing, it can adapt to the altered demands of usage. These adaptations can be extensive and have been shown to affect most aspects of the system, both morphological and functional. Similarly, the neuromuscular system can adapt to more acute challenges, such as those associated with sustained activity. One of the best known acute ad- aptations is a phenomenon that is referred to as muscle fatigue (hereafter referred to as fatigue). To advance our understanding of the adaptive responses that we call fa- tigue, it is important that we identify a set of principles, including a definition, that can provide the basis for sub- sequent studies. However, although the literature on fa- tigue is extensive, its interpretation is confounded by the diversity of definitions and by the variety of paradigms that have been used to study it. Our perspective is to consider fatigue as a phenomenon that had its origin in the study of human performance (for historical prece- dents see Refs. 5,l7l,l77,l87,190,220). In this context, fatigue is a general concept intended to denote an acute impairment of performance that includes both an in- crease in the perceived effort necessary to exert a desired force and an eventual inability to produce this force (79;

for other definitions see Refs. 3,16,69,90). Because of its link to human performance and the multidisciplinary in- terest in the study of fatigue, it does not seem appro- priate or necessary to attribute more precision to the defi- nition of fatigue. Rather, the study of fatigue should focus on the mechanisms that can contribute to an acute impairment of performance.

Not surprisingly, the notion that the neuromuscular system can be stressed to the point of exhaustion has intrigued numerous investigators, particularly in recent years. From these studies on fatigue have emerged at least four themes (task dependency, force-fatigability re- lationship, muscle wisdom, and sense of effort) that seem to provide a reasonable basis for future neural and neuro- muscular work in this area, especially studies for which the requisite techniques are already available. Although many studies have been undertaken in an attempt to identify a single fatigue factor, there seems to be little doubt that the concept of fatigue refers to a class of acute effects that can impair motor performance and not to a single mechanism that can, under all conditions, account for the decline in performance. These observations have led to the notion that the mechanisms underlying fatigue are task dependent. Furthermore the fatigue mecha- nisms appear to cascade so as to exhibit a hyperbolic

0161-7567/92 $2.00 Copyright 0 1992 the American Physiological Society 1631

1632 BRIEF REVIEW

relationship between force and endurance time (force-fa- tigability relationship) and to exert interactive effects on each other. One prominent interaction, which is referred to as muscle wisdom, concerns the parallel changes (slowin g) in the rate of muscle relaxation charge rate of m .otor neu .rons th at accompa

and .nies

the dis- a loss of

force during sustained maximum or strong isometric con- tractions. Another significant, but less appreciated, in- teraction that influences motor performance involves the psychophysical phenomenon of the sense of effort and the force that is exerted during a sustained task. These issues can provide a conceptual basis for many studies on muscle fatigue. The purpose of this review is to describe these four themes (task dependency, force- fatigability relationship, muscle wisdom, and sense of ef- fort) and their util .ity as a focus for subsequent studies on muscle fatigue. A preliminary account of some of these themes has appeared previously (213).

Task Dependency

This theme embodies the concept that fatigue is not the consequence of a single omnipresent mechanism but, rather, that it can be induced by a variety of mechanisms. The term “task dependency” accounts for the role of the details of the task in determining the underlying mecha- nism(s) and sites associated with fatigue. When the de- tails of the task vary significantly, it appears that the mechanism s underlyin g fatigue also vary. Furthermore, for a given task, it seem s that the m echanisms contribut- ing to fatigue can vary as the task proceeds. Task vari- ables that can be manipulated by an investigator and influence the prevailing mechanism include the level of subject motivation, the neural strategy (pattern of mus- cle activation and motor command), the intensity and duration of the activity, the speed of a contraction, and the extent to which the activity is continuously sus- tained. These features of the task influence such fatigue mechanisms as the central nervous system EN S) drive to motor neu rons, the muscles and motor u nits that are activated (neural strategy), neuromuscular propagation, excitation-contraction coupling, the availability of meta- bolic substrates, the intracellular milieu, the contractile apparatus, and muscle blood flow. The following discus- sion of the task dependency theme is i .ntended to demon- strate that these diverse mechanisms can, under specific conditions, induce fatigue; the purpose is neither to pro- vide a comprehensive review of each topic nor to identify the boundary conditions associated with various tasks.

Central driue. One popular technique that has been used to identify the role of various mechanisms in fatigue has been to compare the force that can be elicited by supramaximal electrical stimulation (imposed activation by the experimenter) with the force that the human sub- ject can exert by voluntary activation. This approach typ- ically involves applying single shocks or a brief train of shocks mine w

to the nerve supplying the test muscle to deter- hether the force that is exerted v ,oluntarily can be

supplemented by the imposed stimulation (76,113). Dur- ing a 60-s maximum voluntary contraction (MVC), Big- land-Ritchie et al. (21,24) found that the force exerted by the adductor pollicis decreased by -3050% but that this decline in force could not be supplemented by the im-

posed electrical stimulation. Similarly, when subjects performed an intermittent (6-s contraction, 4-s rest) task with the quadriceps femoris muscle for which the target force was 50% MVC, Bigland-Ritchie et al. (19) found that the maximal voluntary and electrically elicited force declined in parallel so that at the endurance limit (mean time 4.4 min) both the MVC and the electrically elicited force were equal to the 50% MVC target force. These observations suggest that the decline in force (i.e., fa- tigue) under these conditions was not due to an inability to provide the necessary motor neuron activation.

However, there appear to be at least four exceptions to the general notion that humans are able to generate an adequate central drive during fatiguing activity. First, to observe a parallel decline in the voluntary and imposed electrically elicited force, it is necessary that the subjects be well motivated and practiced at the task (19,21,22,24, 31,97,110,169). A lack of motivation or practice presum- ably results in an inadequate CNS drive to the appro- priate motor neurons. Second, it seems that it is difficult, even in motivated subjects, to maintain a maximal cen- tral drive for some muscles, even in an unfatigued state (69, 76, 124, 176). For example, Belanger and McComas (6) found that subjects could maximally activate the tib- ialis anterior, as assessed by the twitch superimposition test, but that 10 of 17 male and 4 of 11 female subjects could not maximally activate the plantarflexor muscles. This limitation seems to be enhanced during sustained activity. When Bigland-Ritchie et al. (19) had subjects perform the intermittent (6-s contraction, 4-s rest) task to the 50% MVC target force with the soleus, they found at the endurance limit (mean time 35 min) that the MVC force had declined to 50% of the initial value but that imposed electrical stimulation elicited a force that was 77% of the initial value. Similarly, Thomas et al. (217) reported that well-motivated subjects had difficulty maintaining an MVC for 5 min with the tibialis anterior, even though the subjects could momentarily increase the force with voluntary activation when they were asked to do so. Despite recent electrophysiological advances, it has not been possible to extend the observations of the 1960s (reviewed in Ref. 186) that revealed a differential strength of corticospinal connections to motor neurons innervating different muscles. For this reason, in part, it is unclear why some muscles, and not others, are prone to this type of failure. Third, subjects appear to have greater difficulty activating all the motor units innervat- ing a muscle during repeated maximal concentric (short- ening) contractions than during eccentric (lengthening) contractions (75, 215, 225). Furthermore, activation fail- ure appears to vary with contraction speed and is great- est for low-speed concentric contractions (159a, 180a). This effect may be due to changes in central drive and neuromuscular propagation. Fourth, during a simulated 4O-day ascent of Mt. Everest in a hypobaric chamber, some subjects were unable to generate a maximum CNS drive, as assessed by twitch superimposition, during a sustained MVC (104). These observations on unmoti- vated subjects, muscle-specific effects, eccentric contrac- tions, and altitude suggest that fatigue can be caused by an inability to generate the necessary CNS drive.

Neural strategy. Related to the observation that an in-

BRIEF REVIEW 1633

adequate CNS drive may contribute to fatigue is the pos- sibility that some tasks may permit a subject to alter the neural strategy (i.e., muscle activation patterns, motor commands) and hence influence the time course of fa- tigue (66, 82a, 130, 196). Clearly, changes in the muscle activation patterns are possible only with tasks that in- volve submaximal forces. Such was the case when Sjergaard et al. (203) had subjects sustain an isometric knee extensor force at a target of 5% MVC for 1 h. On the basis of measurements of intramuscular pressure and electromyogram (EMG) for rectus femoris and vastus lateralis, they found that the subjects often switched the activity among the muscles that comprise the quadriceps femoris group while maintaining the target force for 1 h. Sjogaard and colleagues (203,204) suggested that the fa- tigue (12% decline in MVC force and increase in effort) associated with this task was due to a decrease in muscle cell excitability that was caused by a loss of K+ from the cells. It is possible, however, that these effects were mini- mized by the option the task afforded the subjects to vary the activity among the quadriceps femoris muscles. Al- though it has never been examined, the switching of activ- ity during fatigue may occur within parts of a muscle, because we know that some muscles (e.g., biceps brachii, biceps femoris, sartorius, semimembranosus) consist of discrete compartments (219, 228). This seems most likely in muscles with distributed attachments, in which changes in the direction of the force vector can be asso- ciated with changes in motor unit activity (81).

Another, more speculative, example of a change in the muscle activation pattern is the possible rotation of mo- tor units during fatiguing activity. Motor unit rotation would provide periods of inactivity that could be used for the replenishment of metabolic needs. The concept of orderly recruitment implies that once a motor unit has been recruited it will remain active as long as the force exerted by the muscle exceeds the threshold force of re- cruitment for the unit. Although most investigators ac- cept this assumption, there were early suggestions that a muscle could minimize fatigue by rotating some of the active motor units (85,201). Some evidence of this possi- bility was provided by Person (184) among the motor units of rectus femoris during an isometric knee extensor task. Furthermore, Enoka et al. (78) examined motor unit behavior during a ramp-and-hold task that was per- formed before and after a fatiguing contraction and found that some low-threshold motor units were not ac- tive after the fatiguing activity although the task was identical to that performed before the fatigue task. This variation in motor unit recruitment was interpreted as reflecting some degree of history-dependent flexibility in recruitment order among these motor units (see also Ref. 184), which is consistent with the concept of motor unit rotation. This possibility adds a new dimension to the strategies available to the motor system to accommodate demanding activity.

Associated with the fatigue-related flexibility in motor unit behavior (i.e., recruitment order, discharge rate, and discharge pattern), there is emerging evidence that the activation of a motor unit pool may vary with the relative magnitude of the muscle and load torques. When the muscle torque is less than the load torque, the active

muscle lengthens in a so-called eccentric contraction. Conversely, when the muscle torque is greater than the load torque, the active muscle shortens in what is re- ferred to as a concentric contraction. It seems that it is difficult to generate a maximal CNS drive to the motor unit pool under eccentric conditions, at least in compari- son to that achieved in concentric conditions (75, 215, 225). Furthermore, when a contraction changes from the shortening (concentric) of active muscle to lengthening (eccentric), there can be a change in the motor units that contribute to the muscle force (178). More work is needed to determine the generality of these observations, the mechanisms underlying the variability in motor unit recruitment, and the susceptibility of these mechanisms to the effects of fatigue.

In addition to changes in muscle and motor unit acti- vation patterns, fatigue-related changes in the neural strategy can include alterations in the motor command. These changes can affect both the quantity and the qual- ity of the motor command. Clearly, when a subject is required to sustain a maximal force for a given duration, the subject does not have the option of increasing the magnitude of the motor command as the force declines. In contrast, when the task involves a submaximal con- traction, the subject is able to increase the motor com- mand to counteract the reduction in force due to periph- eral (e.g., neuromuscular propagation, contractile appa- ratus) mechanisms. This strategy is used frequently during submaximal fatiguing contractions and is evident as an increase in the discharge rate of active motor units and the recruitment of additional units (17, 161). The actual expression of the increase in motor command, however, will vary among muscles because of differences in the upper limit of motor unit recruitment (144).

Although much less is known about the qualitative features of a motor command, it does appear that rela- tively minor changes in a task are accompanied by signifi- cantly different motor commands. For example, the mo- tor command that elicits a one-legged isometric MVC (concurrent knee and hip extension) appears to be differ- ent from the command associated with a two-legged MVC. Rube and Secher (197) examined the fatigability of subjects during the performance of one- and two- legged MVCs before and after they completed a 5-wk training program with either the one- or two-legged task. The training resulted in a reduction in fatigability, but the effect was specific to the training task; that is, those subjects who trained with the two-legged MVC became less fatigable with this task and not with the one-legged task. Although both legs were trained with the two- legged task, the subjects were less fatigable only during the two-legged MVC and not when either leg was acti- vated by itself. This observation suggests that the motor command for the two-legged MVC is sufficiently differ- ent from that for the one-legged MVC. Clearly, more work is needed on this issue to determine the specificity and adaptability of motor commands.

Neuromuscular propagation. In studies that have com- pared the voluntary and imposed electrically elicited force, the protocol has also often included measurements to determine whether neuromuscular propagation failure is associated with the decline in force. The most common

1634 BRIEF REVIEW

approach has been to measure the electrical response in - - the muscle (M tion. The M w

wave) to the imposed electrical stimu ‘ave consi sts of the sy nchronous sum

many muscle fiber action potentials th at are elicited the electrical stimulation. Because the M waves are ways initi .ated by action potentials tha t begin in the mo- tor axons at the I .evel of mixed nerves or muscle nerves, changes in the M wave indicate alterations in neuromus- cular propagation between the site of initiation (nerves) and the site of recording (muscle fibers). Despite the sim- plicity of this scheme, there remains a controversy over whether the M wave s change with fatigue. Alth .ough some studies have reported that M waves do not decrease during a 60-s MVC (21, 24, 141, 145, 217), others have shown that the M waves do decrease with sustained activ- ity (8, 158, 175, 209, 232; A. J. Fuglevand, K. Zackowski, K. Huey, and R. M. Enoka, unpublished observations). This discrepancy may be partially due to differences in the tasks performed to induce fatigue. Low-force long- duration contractions have been shown to induce greater M-wave depression than hi gh-force contractions (Fugle- vand et al., unpublished observations).

On .e possible explanation for the decline in M waves

la- of by al-

could be a reduction in the excitability of the muscle fiber membranes. This could be accomplished by fatigue-in- duced accumulation of K+ and depletion of Na+ from the extracellular spaces. With electrical stimulation of iso- lated mouse muscle, Jones and colleagues (132) demon- strated that it is possible to mimic the rapid force decline with high-frequency imposed stimulation by reducing the Na+ concentration in the bathing medium. Similarly, muscle action potentials were diminished by increasing the extracellular concentration of K+ (131). Further- more, Bezanilla et al. (13) demonstrated that manipula- tion of Na+ in the medium bathing single muscle fibers could prevent myofibrillar contraction, which they attrib- uted to a failure of action potential propagation in the t-tubules. This effect, however, does not seem to be ited to high-frequency conditions, becau se although

. lim- hu-

man subjects can sustain a 5% MVC for 1 h, this is accom- panied by an extracellular accumulation of K+ (202) and fatigue-related significant increases in EMG amplitude and decreases in the mean frequency of the EMG power spectrum (135).

Excitation-contraction coupling. When the task is such that performance is not impaired by subject motivation, changes in the neural strategy, or reduction in the M waves, then the decline in force must be caused by other mechanisms. Bigland-Ritchie et al. (17) examined this condition with intermittent (6-s contraction, 4-s rest) submaximal (30% MVC) isometric contractions of the quadriceps femoris. During the first 30 min of the task, the MVC force and electrically elicited force declined in parallel to 50% of the initial value, yet there were no significant changes in muscle lactate, ATP, or phospho- creatine and the glycogen depletion was minimal and confined to the type I and IIa fibers. The decline in MVC force could not be explained by an inadequate central drive (M waves), acidosis, or lack of metabolic sub- strates. However, there was a disproportionate decrease in the electrically elicited twitch compared with the te- tanic (50-Hz) responses and MVC, which Bigland-Rit-

chic et al. interpreted as evidence of impaired excitation- contraction coupling. On the basis of this rationale, the decline in MVC force under these conditions was proba- bly caused by a disruption of the link between activation of the muscle fiber membrane and the force exerted by the fibers.

Another widely accepted example of excitation-con- traction coupling failure involves a standard test of mo- tor unit fatigability (35). In this protocol, isolated single motor units are activated with 330-ms trains of stimuli (40 Hz) at a rate of once per second for 2 min (120 trains). When subjected to this regimen, some motor units [fast- twitch fatiguable (type FF)] fatigue and exhibit a marked decline in force while other motor units [fast-twitch fa- tigue resistant and slow-twitch (types FR and S)] do not fatigue. Although the type FF units can exert little force after 2 min, the EMG (compound muscle action poten- tial) measured during the test shows little decrease below initial values (80, 115, 129). Because it has not yet been shown that 2 min of this stimulation can deplete the muscle fibers of metabolic substrates, cause marked al- terations in the concentrations of various metabolites, or impair the contractile machinery, the fatigue is generally attributed to a mechanism related to excitation-contrac- tion coupling (114, 115, 129).

Although a failure of excitation-contraction coupling has also been implicated in other experimental protocols (62, 65, 73, 129, 174), little is known about the relative contribution of the specific mechanisms. In a thorough review of the possible excitation-contraction coupling mechanisms, Fitts and Metzger (84) summarized the transformation of an action potential into cross-bridge activity as involving seven steps: 1) the sarcolemmal ac- tion potential, 2) t-tubular charge movement, 3) cou- pling of t-tubular charge movement with Ca2+ release from the sarcoplasmic reticulum, 4) Ca2+ release from the sarcoplasmic reticulum, 5) reuptake of Ca2+ by the sarcoplasmic reticulum, 6) Ca2+ binding to troponin, and 7) actomyosin hydrolysis of ATP and cross-bridge cy- cling. Changes in the intracellular milieu with fatigue seem to reduce the magnitude of the Ca2’ transient (step 4) (1, 28) and to impair the Ca2+-adenosinetriphospha- tase-mediated Ca2+ uptake into the sarcoplasmic reticu- lum (step 5) (1, 57, 148, 224). The decline in the magni- tude of the Ca2+ transient could be due to a decrease in the t-tubular charge movement (but this is unlikely) (15), inhibition of the coupling step, or impairment of the re- lease process. Some evidence favors the latter mecha- nism and may involve an increased inactivation of the Ca2+ release channel and depletion of Ca2+ stores in the sarcoplasmic reticulum (28, 84, 222). Furthermore the observed decrease in the rate of Ca2+ reuptake into the sarcoplasmic reticulum may combine with an increased intracellular binding of Ca2+ to parvalbumin, troponin C, and the sarcoplasmic reticulum pump (224) to reduce the Ca2+ concentration gradient across the sarcoplasmic re- ticulum. The net effect of a reduction in the Ca2+ concen- tration gradient would be to diminish the flux of Ca2+ across the sarcoplasmic reticulum in response to t-tubu- lar charge movement. However, the contribution of these mechanisms to muscle fatigue in vivo remains to be de- termined (84).

BRIEF REVIEW 1635

Despite the uncertainty concerning the relative contri- butions made by the specific mechanisms to the decline in force, the process of excitation-contraction coupling has been implicated in several reports on fatigue. A com- mon strategy for assessing the impairment of excitation- contraction coupling has been to monitor the recovery from fatigue. In these experiments, the force loss due to impaired excitation-contraction coupling appears to be significant, to be most evident after contractions of long duration, and to recover slowly (30-60 min) (73,146,147, 172). Some evidence suggests that the depression in te- tanic force after contractile activity may be due to an impairment in the coupling of the t-tubular charge move- ment to sarcoplasmic release of Ca2+ (222). In compari- son, recovery of force loss due to metabolite-induced im- pairment of cross-bridge function seems to occur rapidly (-2 min) (147,174,198), as does recovery from impaired neuromuscular propagation (4-6 min) (Fuglevand et al., unpublished observations; 174). Furthermore the mecha- nisms underlying the reduction in force appear to depend on the details of the task and can involve an impairment of several processes, including excitation-contraction coupling (Fuglevand et al., unpublished observations; 147, 174, 188). Experimental studies are required to de- fine the task-dependent boundaries that activate the various mechanisms that cause a reduction in force, in- cluding an evaluation of the extent to which they are activated sequentially.

IMetabolic substrates. As described by Edwards and Gibson (71), muscle force will decline when energy de- mands cannot be met by the rate of supply of ATP and the metabolites generated by contractile activity (e.g., ADP, Pi, H+) influence cross-bridge activity or the sup- ply of energy. Although the decline in force is not always associated with a depletion of metabolic substrates (17, 202), the extent of the dissociation seems to depend on the intensity of activity. Hermansen et al. (120) found that when subjects exercised (cycle ergometer) at a rate of 70-80% of maximal aerobic power, exhaustion coin- cided with glycogen depletion in the muscle fibers of lat- eral quadriceps femoris. In this context, “exhaustion” represented the point in time when the subjects could not exert the forces necessary to perform the task. Under similar conditions, Coyle and colleagues (49,53) reported that when exercising subjects are fed glucose or have it infused intravenously, they are able to exercise longer. These observations suggest that the availability of car- bohydrates is an important factor in how long motivated subjects can ride a cycle ergometer at 6585% of maximal aerobic power (34, 125).

In addition to this task-dependent reliance on carbo- hydrates, it seems that the supply of energy can have a differential effect on the decline in force with continuous and intermittent activation of muscle. In these studies, muscle has been activated with electrical stimulation for various durations for the purpose of varying the number of cycles (activation-relaxation cycles) within a given force-time integral. For example, Hultman and col- leagues (11, 45) stimulated the ischemic quadriceps fe- moris at a frequency of 20 Hz and with an intensity suffi- cient to elicit a 25% MVC. The stimulation was either applied continuously for 52 s or intermittently (stimula-

tion-rest durations of 0.8:0.8 s, 1.6:1.6 s, or 3.2:3.2 s) for ~54 s of stimulation. The decline in force was least for the continuous stimulation and greatest for the intermit- tent condition with the most activation-relaxation cycles (0.8:0.8 s). Measurements made on biopsies from vastus lateralis indicated that intermittent stimulation was as- sociated with an increase in ATP utilization. Bergstrom and Hultman (11) estimated that -40% of the energy cost of a l-s tetanus was attributable to the development and relaxation of force. These results indicate that under anaerobic conditions the force elicited with continuous stimulation is more economical than that produced with intermittent stimulation. However, when the test muscle (adductor pollicis) was not ischemic, Duchateau and Hainaut (65) found that the decline in the electrically elicited (30-Hz) force was similar with continuous and intermittent (l-s activation, l-s rest) stimulation. From other experiments involving aerobic conditions and the use of reduced anesthetized animal preparations, there is evidence of a lower rate of cross-bridge formation during the steady-state phase of the tetanus than during its ris- ing phase (33,51,153,221). Therefore it appears that the energy cost of a task is influenced by whether the exerted force is intermittent or continuous and that this effect is modulated to some extent when the blood flow to the muscle is occluded.

As a complement to the study of energy supply and its role in fatigue, many investigators have examined the effects of the products of ATP hydrolysis on the declin? in force. One early candidate for a prominent role in fa- tigue was lactic acid. However, the consensus now ap- pears to be that an elevation of H+ concentration is more critical than lactate or the undissociated lactic acid (84). Furthermore, although H+ can inhibit glycolysis, this in- teraction does not appear to be a major mechanism un- derlying the decline in force (84). One fruitful approach to identifying the role of H+ in fatigue has been to reduce the intracellular pH in single intact muscle fibers by in- creasing the CO, in the extracellular medium; the extent of the reduction in pH (7.0 to 6.6) is similar to that ob- served in humans during fatiguing contractions (147). On the basis of such studies, it appears that intracellular acidification results in a moderate decline in the number of attached cross bridges and a decrease in the force ex- erted by each cross bridge (68,147). Qualitatively similar observations have been reported in studies on skinned muscle fibers (44, 50); however, there may be some dif- ferences between the two preparations in the magnitude of the H+-induced decline in force (147).

Edman and Lou (68) suggested that although intracel- lular acidification is responsible for much of the altered mechanical performance of muscle with fatigue, the ele- vated H+ concentration does not provide a complete ex- planation of the changes. This reservation is based on the discrepancy in the stiffness of muscle fibers after intracellular acidification and fatiguing contractile activ- ity. Probably, the other products of ATP hydrolysis (Mg- ADP and Pi) are able to modulate cross-bridge behavior. For example, an increase in the concentration of Pi has been shown to reduce maximum isometric force but not to affect the maximum speed of shortening (44,50). Con- versely, an increased concentration of Mg-ADP causes a

1636 BRIEF REVIEW

small increase in maximum isometric force and a modest decline in the maximum speed of shortening (50). Subse- quent studies will undoubtedly address the interactive effects of changes in these products.

From these studies on the role of metabolism in fa- tigue, it seems reasonable to conclude, as with the other factors considered with thi s theme, that gi .ven the appro- priate conditions an impairment of metabolism-related processes can contribute to the decline in force during fatiguing activity. Furthermore it appears that factors related to both the supply of energy and the accumula- tion of metabolites can contribute to this force reduction.

di Summary. When the results from these va

gms and protocols are consid .ered together, riou . . 1t 1s

s para- appar-

ent that the decline in force associated with fatigue can be caused by many different mechanisms. In fact, it aP- pears that it is possible to design an experimental proto- col for which any of the identified mechanisms can con- tribute to fatigue. Furthermore the data suggest that it is the details of the task that will largely determine the relative contribution of the different mechanisms to fa- tigue. done

There on this

remains, however, theme to identify

a great deal of the boundary

work to be conditions

for each mechanism and to determine whether various red .uced preparations appropriately mi mic the function of an intact organism. Studies are needed to 1) identify the association between task variables and fatigue, 2) characterize the time course and interaction between the fatigue mechanisms within tasks (Fuglevand et al., un- published observations; 147,1X,174), 3) assess the gen- erality of the observations for different motor systems (e.g., limb, respiratory, visual, speech), and 4) determine which mechanisms are important in human perfor- mance.

Force-Fatigability Relationship

Although some work has suggested that it is possible to sustain low-level isometric contractions indefinitely (193), most evidence indicates that activation of the neu- romuscular system at any intensity will eventually elicit fatigue (203,218). The greater the force exerted during a task, the more rapidly the muscle fatigues (9, 47, 151, 193). Although such a relationship might be anticipated, it does raise at least two interesting issues about the mechan isms underlyin .g fatigue. First, on the basis of the rationale associated with task dependency, the observa- tion of a generalized force-fatigability relationship sug- gests that the mechanisms causing fatigue interact in such nism

a way that t ‘hey scale with force. The multi-mecha- basis of the task dependen cy observations suggests

that the force-fatigability relationship is not a simple ex- trapolation of motor unit force-fatigability relationships. Second, the tests that characterize motor unit fatigabil- ity (35, 107, 199) generally stress one, or perhaps two, fatigue mechanisms but not the complete set that span the force-fatigability relationship. Studies related to this theme need to address the role of the various fatigue mechanisms in defining the force-fatigability relation- ship.

Classical tests of muscle endurance, which involve a sustained isometric contraction, have typically revealed

a hyperbolic relationship between force and endurance time. For example, when the jaw-closing muscles sustain an isometric force at various levels, the associated endur- ance times are inversely related to the magnitude of the force (47,48,143). Similarly, Bellemare and Grassino (9) found an inverse relationship between the magnitude of the transdiaphragmatic pressure during inspiration and the fatigue time constant (i.e., endurance time). Further- more, when the duty cycle was increased (9, l50), which amounts to an increase in the work performed or the force-time integral for isometric contractions, the rate of fatigue increased (i.e., a decrease in endurance time). In a similar vein, when the duty cycle was held constant and the frequency of imposed stimulation was varied (15 or 30 Hz), Garland et al. (100) found that there was more fatigue when the force-time integral was greater. The rate of fatigue has also been reported to increase when the duty cycle was increased (65) or the duration of the cycle decreased (ll), which suggests that the rate of en- ergy utilization for intermittent contractions also influ- ences the decline in force. Such observations suggest that the greater the force exerted, work done, or rate of work performance during a task, the greater the fatigue experi- ence d by the muscle.

A central feature of the force-fatigability relationship seems to be its dependence on the absolute force exerted during the task. McKenzie and Gandevia (168) had sub- jects perform intermittent isometric contractions with either the elbow flexor or inspiratory muscles at two dif- ferent muscle lengths: the optimal length and a shorter length that reduced the MVC force by 25%. Although the maximal absolute force was different at the two lengths, the subjects performed the intermittent task at the max- imum voluntary force for each length. Fatigability was assessed as the decline in peak force after a series of 18 MVCs. Although the inspiratory muscles were less fa- tigued at the optimal length (final force: short length = 81% of initial; optimal length = 87%), the elbow flexor muscles were less fatigued at the shorter length (final force: short length = 61%; optimal length = 55%). Fitch and McComas (82) similarly found a greater fatigability (decline in electrically elicited torque) for tibialis ante- rior at its optimum length. These observations indicate for the limb muscles that although the relative force was the same at each length (i.e., for an MVC) and the mus- cles exhibited different amounts of fatigability, the abso- lute force was different and fatigability varied in direct proportion to the absolute force, as predicted by the force-fatigability relationship.

Although the force-fatigability relationship has ap- peared robust in a variety of experimental conditions, it appears that it is possible to alter the relationship by varying the rate and pattern of the imposed electrical stimulation. Although the rate of muscle activation has long been known to influence fatigability, it is now appar- ent that the pattern of activation can also have a signifi- cant effect on the decline in force. As expected for the generalized force-fatigability relationship, the rate and amount of force decline both increase with the frequency of electrical stimulation, both for whole muscles (23,132) and for single motor units (199). This effect presumably involves a force-dependent sequential activation of the

BRIEF REVIEW 1637

mechanisms underlying fatigue (e.g., failure of neuromus- cular propagation, excitation-contraction coupling) (23, 73, 199). However, when the imposed stimulation fre- quency is systematically reduced, there is less of a decline in whole muscle force (26, 130, 132). By simply changing the activation pattern, it is possible to significantly alter the force-fatigability relationship, so that from the same initial force there can be different amounts of fatigabil- ity. Presumably this effect is the result of a change in the dominant fatigue mechanism, such as a failure of action potential propagation with high-frequency stimulation and a failure of excitation-contraction coupling with low- frequency stimulation. The frequency-dependent change in the fatigue mechanism results in a change in the force- frequency relationship with fatigue, such as a decline in the force elicited with low but not high frequencies of stimulation (52, 73, 129, 199).

In addition to these whole muscle experiments, a pat- tern effect has been demonstrated at the level of the mo- tor unit with only subtle changes in the activation pat- tern (12). In these experiments, which are an extension of work on the catchlike property of single motor units (36, 206, 231), Stuart and colleagues (12) compared the fatigability of type FF motor units with two stimulus regi- mens: one that comprised a constant submaximal fre- quency and another that rearranged the stimuli to opti- mize the force but retain the same average frequency. Repetitive activation with the optimized stimulus regi- men resulted in a greater initial force and less fatigabil- ity, which contradicts the expectation based on the gener- alized force-fatigability relationship. It remains to be shown whether this observation reflects a change in the dominant fatigue mechanism or less stress on the prevail- ing mechanism(s).

Although chronic adaptations associated with alter- ations of the normal patterns of usage can change the fatigability of muscle, there are no systematic studies on the effects on the force-fatigability relationship. While endurance training is known to decrease fatigability (67, 136), it appears that the muscle atrophy associated with various reduced-use regimens does not cause the muscle to become more fatigable, but instead the muscle may become less fatigable (89, l11,122a, l73,178a, 192,212). This reciprocal change in force and fatigability is consis- tent with the force-fatigability relationship. However, the dissociation between changes in maximum force and fatigability may be due to the selective impairment of high-threshold fatigable motor units following a re- duced-use protocol (65a). A potentially informative ex- perimental strategy would be to characterize the force- fatigability relationship and the time course of changes in muscles that had undergone various chronic perturba- tions with the intent of identifying the adaptability of the various mechanisms that underlie fatigability.

Summary. A generalized force-fatigability relationship seems to be valid for most, but not all, experimental con- ditions that have been examined. According to this rela- tionship, the greater the force exerted by a muscle, or a motor unit, during a given task, the more the muscle will fatigue. Presumably the mechanisms underlying fatigue vary with the level of the force that is exerted. Studies are needed that 1) systematically explore the force-fatigabil-

A B

60 3 80- .- g ."

45 60-

8 *' b

5 30 ;;; 40- I I 0

0-J I 0 20 40 60

Time

o--r-

(s1°

100 200 300 1 I 1

400 500 600

I I I I r t I 1 I I

60 38 30 20 605045 40 35 30

Stimulus Frequency (Hz)

FIG. 1. Muscle wisdom. A: decline in maximum voluntary contrac- tion (MVC) (less smoot,h line) of adductor pollicis can be mimicked by electrical stimulation of muscle (smooth line) when stimulus frequency declined (from 60 to 20 Hz) over course of task. When stimulus fre- quency was held constant, force declined more rapidly. [Adapted from Jones et al. (132).] B: submaximal force elicited in quadriceps femoris muscle declined less when stimulus frequency was reduced during the test. l , Force elicited with variable-frequency stimulation; 0, force due to constant-frequency stimulation. These data indicate that with vari- able-frequency stimulation it is possible to mimic fatigue-related de- cline in MVC and optimize submaximal force. [Adapted from Binder- Macleod and Guerin (26).]

ity domain for a variety of muscles and tasks to deter- mine the global nature ize local regions of the the rel

of the force

relationship, 2) character- -fatigability domain where

ationihip is nonmonotonic, 3 that predominate at various

) identify the mecha- levels of force, 4) as- nisms

sess the interactions between the fatigue mechanisms that produce the hyperbolic relationship between force and endurance time, and 5) use the force-fatigability re- lationship as a tor system tha

probe to examine adaptations in the mo- t are associated with short-term changes

in the activation pattern and chronic perturbations such as long-term activation patterns, immobilization, exer- cise training, and aging.

Muscle Wisdom

As fatigue proceeds in conscious humans during both MVCs and imposed contractions (electrical stimulation with a selected pattern), there is a progressive decrease in the rate of relaxation in whole muscle (21,72,126), single motor units (35, 64, 107, 188, 216), and single muscle fibers (1, 147); a reduction in motor neuron discharge rate and in the frequency of activation necessary to elicit the maximum force (20,61); and, for a given submaximal frequency, a greater degree of fusion in the force (110, 132, 158). For example, the original work (Fig. 1A) dem- onstrated that the decline in force during a 60-s MVC of adductor pollicis could be mimicked by a reduction in the stimulus frequency (60 to 20 Hz) used for the electrically elicited force (132). Furthermore, Binder-Macleod and colleagues (25,26) showed that the force elicited in quad- riceps femoris with imposed 30-Hz) electrical stimulation

variable-frequency (60- to . decreased less than that

with a constant-frequency (60-Hz) protocol, which sug- gests that reduction of the stimulus frequency serves to optimize the submaximal force (Fig. 1B). Similarly, Bot- terman and Cope (32) found that to sustain a submaxi- mal force (25% of maximum) it was necessary to reduce the stimul ciation of

.ation rate of fast-twitch motor a concurrent decline in force,

units. This relaxation

asso- rate,

and motor neuron discharge rate has been referred to as

1638 BRIEF REVIEW

A - 100 E

E' 80 .-

2 60 E

9 40 0

8 20 8

LL 0

tions; 103, 108, 129, 142, 179, 189), Thomas et al. (216) make the reasonable suggestion that the differences among motor units in the shift of the stimulus fre- quency-force relationship depend on the balance be- tween the time courses of fatigability and potentiation for each unit. Because whole muscle responses represent the weighted average of the constituent motor unit re- sponses, this variability among motor units raises the

200 300 0 20 40

Stimulus Frequency (H > Z issue of how various tasks influence the direction of the shift in the stimulus frequency-force relationship for the whole muscle.

0

FIG. 2. Muscle wisdom-associated shifts in stimulus frequency- force relationship. A: fatigue results in a leftward shift of stimulus frequency-force relationship for frog semitendinosus muscle (0, before fatigue; a, after fatigue). Functional significance of this adaptation is that although maximum force declines with fatigue, a submaximal stim- ulus frequency will elicit a greater proportion of current (normalized) maximal force when relationship is shifted to the left. [Adapted from Fitts et al. (83).] B: stimulus frequency-force relationship is not shifted to the left for human quadriceps femoris, and at some frequencies of stimulation relationship is shifted to the right, exhibiting low-fre- quency fatigue (Cl, before fatigue; n , after fatigue). These data repre- sent shift in relationship when fatigue was induced by electrical stimula- tion of quadriceps femoris. The same result was found when fatigue was induced by voluntary activation of muscle (27). These data indicate that effect of fatigue on stimulus frequency-force relationship is unre- solved. [Adapted from Binder-Macleod and McDermond (27).]

“muscle wisdom” (157, 158). The functional significance of muscle wisdom is that it optimizes the force and en- sures an economical activation of fatiguing muscle by the CNS.

The ability to replicate the decline in MVC force by reducing the stimulus frequency has been hypothesized to result from a leftward shift of the stimulus frequency- force relationship (Fig. 2A). The basis of this rationale is that if the time course of the twitch increases, then the activation rate can decline and it is still possible to achieve the same degree of fusion of the twitch re- sponses. Bigland-Ritchie et al. (21) proposed, albeit on the basis of limited data (their Fig. 4), that fatigue causes a leftward shift in the normalized stimulus frequency- force relationship because of changes in the time course of the twitch with fatigue. Accordingly, Fitts et al. (83) reported that the optimal frequency for the frog semiten- dinosus muscle decreased from 150 to 60 Hz after 5 min of electrical stimulation (Fig. 2A). However, other experi- mental observations on both motor units (199, 216) and whole muscles (10, 27) showed that the stimulus fre- quency-force relationship normalized to the maximum force does not always move to the left with fatigue (Fig. 2B; see also Ref. 52). Rather, depending on the fatigue regimen, the relationship may move to the right for some stimulus frequencies (Fig. 2B).

At the motor unit level, in both conscious humans (216) and experimental animals (188), the stimulus fre- quency-force relationship seems to move in opposite di- rections as a function of motor unit type. Powers and Binder (188) found that the stimulus frequency-force re- lationship for type FR motor units moved to the left im- mediately after a period of stimulation and then to the right after -30 mi n of recovery. In contrast, the sti mulus frequency-force relationship for type FF motor units sim- ply moved to the right. Because the activation of muscle is known to influence processes that both potentiate and diminish force (Fuglevand et al., unpublished observa-

Most of the evidence on the activation patterns asso- ciated with muscle wisdom has been derived from studies of whole muscle and populations of motor units (18,105). Largely because of technical limitations, much less is known about the contribution of single motor units to this phenomenon. Because muscle wisdom has been mim- icke d by reducing the stimulus frequency, it was antici- pated that evidence of a fatigue-related decline in motor unit discharge would be forthcoming. Although some in- vestigators (30, 94, 110, 158) reported an expected de- cline in the discharge rate of single motor units during maximal and submaximal sustained contractions, others have observed either no reduction in discharge rate (161) or a variety of discharge patterns in single motor units, including an increase in discharge rate (31, 63, 102, 105, 161, 182) during sustained submaximal contractions. Consequently, although the net discharge ( based on mea- surements for populations of motor units) appears to de- cline with sustained activity, there can be substantial variability at the level of individual motor units.

On the basis of the population responses, therefore, it seems that the muscle wisdom theme is associated with a concurrent decline in force, relaxation rate, and motor neuron discharge rate that optimizes muscle force and ensures the economical activation of fatiguing muscle by the CNS. Although the prolongation of the twitch and the increased sluggishness of the muscle may be appro- priate for isometric conditions, they do not seem optimal for rhythmic activities, such as locomotion. The slower relaxation of muscle will increase the energy consumed by a muscle as it works against antagonist muscles. This requisite coactivation will increase the energy cost of a task and will resul t in a decrease in the net torque ex- erted about a joint for a given leve 1 of muscle activation. Clearly the muscle wisdom theme needs to be extended to dynamic conditions to de termine whether the adapta- tions parallel those observed under isometric conditions.

Sensory feedback hypothesis. In principle, the muscle wisdom-associated change in motor neuron discharge with sustained activation should be mediated by some combination of afferent feedback from peripheral sources, adaptation in the discharge rate of segmental interneurons and motor n .eurons, and than ges in de- scending commands from suprasegmental centers. Sey- ffarth proposed that “in fatigue the active units usually decrease in frequency. . . [which] might be due to affer- ent impulses sent from the fatigued muscle to the spinal medulla . . . This view appears to be supported by the results of the ischaemia experiments” (Ref. 201, p. 46). Bigland-Ritchie et al. similarly suggested that “during fatigue, motoneurone firing rates may be regulated by a

BRIEF REVIEW 1639

peripheral reflex originating in response to fatigue-in- duced changes within the muscle” (Ref. 18, p. 451), which has been referred to as the sensory feedback hypothe- sis (213).

Although some indirect evidence has implicated small- diameter afferents (group III/IV) in the reflex inhibition of motor neurons during fatigue (99, lOl), the discharge of many slowly conducting group III/IV mechanorecep- tor afferents declines during the initial phase of fatigue (117, 118). Alternatively, because motor units do not ex- hibit a decline in discharge rate during sustained activity until recovery from anesthetic block is complete, which suggests that small-diameter axons are involved, Bon- giovanni and Hagbarth (30) proposed that the decline in discharge rate from a high initial value depends on disfa- cilitation (i.e., a reduction in fusimotor-driven feedback from muscle spindles; see also Ref. 229). Subsequently, direct evidence in favor of this possibility was reported by Macefield et al. (156), who found that the discharge of most muscle spindle afferents decreased by -50% dur- ing sustained submaximal isometric contraction of the human dorsiflexor muscles. These observations suggest that the fatigue-related decline in motor unit discharge does include a peripheral component (94), but the rela- tive roles and time courses of reflex inhibition mediated by small-diameter afferents and disfacilitation of the motor neuron pool are unresolved. One possibility is that “any decline in motoneurone discharge rate during the first 5-10 s of a maximal voluntary contraction may re- flect the reduction in muscle spindle input and increase in presynaptic inhibition, while metabolic effects exerted reflexly through small-diameter afferents contribute more later in the contraction” (90).

At present, there are conflicting observations on the effects of fatigue on the sensitivity of large-diameter pro- prioceptive afferents and their spinal reflex efficacy. Ini- tial results on the sensitivity of muscle spindle afferents in anesthetized reduced animals are generally in agree- ment, with fatigue shown to enhance the responsiveness of group Ia and II afferents to single motor unit contrac- tions (46, 77, 233; cf. 180). However, evidence from conscious humans indicates that fatigue results in a de- cline of fusimotor drive to muscle spindles (30) as well as other mechanisms that may contribute to a reduction in spindle discharge during sustained isometric contrac- tions (156). The effects of fatigue on the group Ib tendon organ afferent are also uncertain. In studies on reduced animals, there is evidence of little (208) or no (109) change in tendon organ responsiveness, whereas there appears to be a reduction in group Ib responsiveness to whole muscle stretch (128) and a reduction in contrac- tion-induced group Ib autogenic inhibition in motor neu- rons (233). There is similar uncertainty concerning the spinal reflex efficacy of this sensory input. Some evi- dence suggests that fatigue enhances the efficacy of short- and long-latency reflex EMG and motor neuron responses to brief muscle perturbations (55, 139, 227), but these results appear to be contrary to other reports (54, 112, 127). Studies are needed that examine the ef- fects of type-identified motor units on afferent fibers during fatigue in reduced animals (77) and conscious hu- mans (226); the interactions among proprioceptive feed-

back, recurrent inhibition, and motor neuron adaptation in experimental animals (229); and analysis of spinal re- flex transmission in interneuronal pathways in the conscious human, with use of recently developed tech- niques (86).

In the formulation of the sensory feedback hypothesis, it was also emphasized that “The decline in motoneurone firing rates seen during fatigue of a sustained m.v.c. may result primarily from changes in central motoneurone excitability; the time course of frequency changes is quite similar to that reported by Kernel1 and colleagues for changes in the discharge rates of cat single motoneu- rones in response to constant current injection” (Ref. 18, p. 458). Kernel1 and Monster (137, 138) certainly pro- vided convincing evidence that the intracellular injection of a sustained depolarizing current elicits, in the pres- ence of an after-hyperpolarization in the action poten- tial, a progressive reduction in the discharge rate of mo- tor neurons in a deeply anesthetized adult cat. This adap- tation, which has been termed late adaptation, is more prominent in larger motor neurons and, for the same cell, it is more pronounced at higher initial discharge rates (137, 183, 205). It is important to emphasize, however, that these results were obtained in deeply anesthetized cats, whereas the sensory feedback hypothesis is based on the voluntary contractions of humans. It has been argued that the afterhyerpolarization and its obligatory late adaptation are unlikely features of motor neuron discharge during voluntary contractions (205, 206). On the basis of these various results, there is a need for sub- stantially more work on discharge rate adaptation and its association to the sensory feedback hypothesis. The change in motor neuron discharge during fatigue un- doubtedly involves an interaction between intrinsic mo- tor neuron properties and synaptically mediated effects associated with muscle activation (i.e., as attributable to descending command signals, interneuronal pattern gen- eration, and sensory feedback).

Despite the uncertainty of the mechanisms underlying the change in motor unit discharge during fatigue, they undoubtedly exert a powerful effect on the activity of the motor neuron pool. When a human subject sustains a submaximal force to exhaustion, the whole muscle EMG increases, yet motor unit discharge appears to decline (18,102,185). Consequently, the increase in EMG is gen- erally attributed to an increase in the recruitment of mo- tor units. This means that, for a given excitatory drive to the motor neuron pool during fatigue, there is a dissocia- tion between the recruitment and discharge rate of motor units. In contrast, increases in muscle force during non- fatigue conditions are accomplished by concurrent in- creases in motor unit recruitment and discharge rate (214). Fatigue-inducing activity, therefore, must activate some influential processes, either related to intrinsic mo- tor neuron properties or synaptically mediated effects, that permit an excitatory drive to recruit motor units yet for discharge rate to decline. Furthermore, these effects seem to be capable of influencing nonfatigued synergist muscles (116).

Although most attention has focused on potential roles of intrinsic motor neuron properties and synaptically mediated effects in the decline of motor unit discharge, a

1640 BRIEF REVIEW

contribution from a reduction in central drive cannot be force rather than the absolute magnitude of the force excluded. Formulation of the sensory feedback hypothe- that is exerted. This judgment is referred to as the sense sis was based on observations of a decline in the dis- of effort and is distinct from the force sensation asso- charge of motor unit populations in biceps brachii during ciated with a contraction. Because increased effort and sustained MVCs (18, 201, 230). Because the subjects force failure are associated with an impairment of motor were highly motivated and practiced at the task and on the b asis of the twitch superimposition tech .nique, it was assumed that central drive was maximal throughout the task. Subsequently, Garland and McComas (101) demon- strated a reduction in the excitability of soleus motor

perform ante, th .ey are both regarded as essen tial fea- tures of fatigue. Accordingly 9 any physiological process that contributes to either of these features can be de- scribed as a fatigue mechanism.

Corollary discharge. Three lines of experimental evi- neurons (H reflex) in response to an imposed (electrical) dence suggest that the sense of effort appears to be intermittent stimulation regimen that induced fatigue. strongly influenced, if not totally dependent, on centrally Although this evidence implicates a peripheral reflex mechanis m in the diminution of motor unit discharge, it does not eliminate a role for a decline in the central drive. Along these lines, Maton (160) recorded from cells in

generated corticofugal motor commands that give rise to corollary discharges (2, 39, 123, 167). Corollary dis- charges are defined here as “the internal actions of motor commands’ ’ (167) and possibly of the high-level neuronal

area 4 of the motor cortex of monkeys as they exerted processing associated with the formulation of such com- repetitive isometric elbow-flexion torques (duration 2-10 mands (S. Gandevia, personal communication; 152). s). On the basis of spike-triggered averaging, the cortical From this perspective, the sense of effort is a sensation cells were shown to be associated with the EMG of the muscles th .at cross the elbow joint . Maton examined the

derived from the component of the corticopetal compo- nent of the coral .lary discharge that projects to the pri-

change in cortical cell discharge and the EMG power mary somatosensory cortex. spectrum from the 1st to the 20th repetition. As with the As reviewed by McCloskey et al. (167), the evidence for motor unit studies, cortical cell discharge was found to a role for corollary discharge in the sense of effort is decrease, remain constant, or increase as the EMG based on an analysis of perturbations to the system at power spectrum changed. This suggests that motor neu- three different levels of the neuraxis. First, when the ron discharge may also be modulated by descending sig- force that a muscle can exert is experimentally reduced nals during fatiguing contractions. (e.g., by fatigue, with curarization, by cutaneous-derived

Summary. Although the adaptability of motor unit dis- inhibition of motor neurons, or by their reciprocal inhibi- charge seems to be well established at the level of whole tion elicited by vibration of the antagonist muscle), the muscle, there is less certainty about whether all motor units contribute to this effect, and the relative signifi- cance of the mechanisms (sensory feedback, spinal neu- ron adaptations, descending modulation) that control

perceived effort for a task in crea ses in association with the more substantial motor corn mand that the su bject must generate to achieve the target force (42,95,96,164, 166). Second, when the excitability of a motor neuron

the change in motor unit behavior remains to be deter- pool is experimentally heightened (e.g., vibration of a mined. Studies are needed to 1) examine the discharge muscle to elicit an excitatory spindle discharge), there behavior of single motor units in various fatiguing tasks, can be a decrease in the necessary motor command and including dynamic conditions, 2) characterize fatigue-re- in the perceived effort (166). Other investigators, how- lated changes in the efficacy of spinal reflex pathways, 3) ever, reported the converse observation that vibration of quantify fatigue-induced changes in the transducing the muscle tendon consistently increases force sensation properties of various afferent species, 4) determine the during a muscle contraction (38, 134), which suggests functional Si .gnific ante of spinal neuron adaptation and its potenti al relati on to m ore pe ripheral mechanisms of

that intramuscular receptor S are able to influence the perception of muscl .e tension ( 37,39). T hird, intracranial

discharge rate adaptation, and 5) assess the role of de- lesions due to simple motor strokes can result in muscle scending modulation of motor neuron discharge during weakness that requires an increase in centrally gener- fatiguing activity. ated commands to achieve normal activation of spinal

motor neurons. Even when these lesions produce only

Sense of Effort motor deficits with no clinically detectable loss of periph- eral sensation, there is an increase in perceived effort

Whenever a human subject is asked to sustain a sub- (91, 92, 96). maximal force for an extended period of time, such as Psychophysical experiments have demonstrated the holding a heavy briefcase while waiting for a bus, the first independence of perceived effort and force failure during hint that this task cannot be accomplished indefinitely is not an inability to exert the necessary force but, rather, a

sustained activity. When s ubjects were asked to support a weight with one h .and for 10 min, they were able to do so

perception that it is necessary to increase the effort asso- but, with a matching contraction performed by the con- ciated with the task (93, 133, 162, 165, 166). It is even tralateral hand, indicated that the perceived effort in- possible, especially with some clinical conditions, for an creased during the task (166). Similar observations have individual to report an effort-related fatigue but with no been reported for intermittent (brief rest periods) sub- impairment of the ability to exert force (152, 211). The maximal and maximal contractions (39). In these stud- effort associated with performing a task is assessed by ies, subjects are required to estimate the force during the requiring subjects to match forces. It seems that subjects’ fatiguing contraction with the contralateral limb, and in- judgments are based on the effort required to generate a vestigators infer, on the basis of the increase in the per-

BRIEF REVIEW 1641

A 500 7

z 3001

a 2 0 200 LL I

100 1 0-L

0

I 0

I 0

1 I I 1 I

20 40 60 80 100

Perceived Effort (% maximum)

B

r 80-

2 .- z 60-

E E 40-

8 5 20- LL 0 I I I1 1

0 20 40 60 80 100

Excitation (% maximum)

FIG. 3. Relationship between muscle force and perceived effort. A: mean values for 5 subjects and 2 muscles (0, adductor pollicis; 0, quad- riceps femoris). Perceived effort increases as an exponential function of muscle force. Decreased steepness of curve indicates reduced discrim- inative capability for sense of effort. [Adapted from Banister (4).] R: modeling studies (88, 119) suggested a sigmoidal relationship between excitatory drive to a motor neuron pool (input) and force (output). This comparison suggests that sense of effort is not a simple linear interpre- t,ation of excitatory drive t.o a motor neuron pool.

ceived amplitude of the matching force, that the subjects were estimating effort. Some studies have found that during a constant-force task the perceived effort in- creases as a power function of duration and that for dif- ferent tasks the perceived effort increases as a power function of the target force (4, 34a, 91, 167, 177a, 210). Similarly, when the effort associated with a handgrip con- traction was held constant, the decline in force was char- acterized by an exponential fu nction ML 1 34a). Others, however, have found a linear relatio nship between the matching force and time during constant-force contrac- tions (93, 133, 166).

The shape of the perceived effort-force relationship (Fig. 3A) implies a reduced discriminative capability for the perceived effort at low forces. Interestingly, the rela- tionship between perceived effort and force is opposite to that predicted from modeling studies (88, 119). These models have produced a sigmoidal relationship between the excitatory drive to the motor neuron pool and the force exerted by muscle (Fig. 3B). According to this rela- tionship, the requisite increase in excitatory drive neces- sary to increase force by 10% is greater at high force levels. In contrast, the perceived effort increases more for a 10% increase in force at low forces (Fig. 3A). This difference suggests a nonlinear transformation, as can be achieved with neuronal networks, between the excitatory drive to a motor neuron pool and perceived effort.

Although the sense of effort appears to be derived from corollary discharge, this association is probably in- fluenced by humoral factors circulating in the cerebrospi- nal fluid. This expectation is based on observations of the effect of various pharmacological agents on endur- ante capability. For example, the administration of am- phetamine increases the time of swimming rats to reach exha ustion (1 4). Furthermore the pretreatment of run- ning rats wi th 6- hydroxydopamine , a neurotoxin that de- stroys catecholaminergi .C fibers, decreased the time to reach exhaustion (121). S uch observations have led in- vestigators to conclude that epinephrine (122) and the neurotransmitter Ei-hydroxy tryptamine (29, 43) may

play a role in central fatigue. If endurance time is in- fluenced by such agents, then it seems likely that the sense of effort would also be affected. This possibility awaits investigation.

Systems issues. Despite the significance of perceived effort as it relates to fatigue experienced by humans, lit- tle is known about the sense of effort and fatigue. There are at least four avenues that invite systematic investiga- tion at the systems level of inquiry. First, the relationship between the sense of effort and the magnitude of force reduction during fatiguing contractions should be quan- tified for different muscle groups and motor tasks. It is unclear whether this relationship would be affected by the motor system (e.g., respiratory vs. jaw vs. limb), mus- cle mass (hand vs. leg), muscle function (flexors vs. ex- tensors, single joint vs. multi-joint), or degree of direct cortical control (hand and forearm vs. leg). Furthermore, because force failure is known to be task dependent, it is necessary to determine the influence of motor task on the relationship between the sense of effort and force reduction. This would involve an evaluation of the effects of subject motivation, the intensity and duration of the activity, and the extent to which the activity is continu- ously sustained.

Second, there is a need to examine the effects of fa- tigue in experimental paradigms that have been used pre- viously to demonstrate the importance of sensory feed- back in seemingly simple low-force tasks: jaw control in intact and deafferented monkeys (106); postural control in intact humans after ischemic deafferentation of the legs (60, 163); and thumb, wrist, and arm movements of humans with selected sensory neuropathies (159, 195, 200). The relationship of this literature to the perception of force during fatiguing contractions has not yet been considered. For example, sensory updating may still be required for such perceptions, even with the availability of the sense of effort.

Third, the role of corollary discharge and kinesthesis has recently been extended to include judgments about the timing of muscle contractions (167) and the control of fine manipulation (98). The perception that is formed is presumably task dependent; it could be critical in tasks that require the cooperative activation of separate mus- cle groups for high-speed movements in which sensory feedback is not available to the control system. Studies on the effect of fatigue on timing judgments could pro- vide insight into both the types of motor performance most sensitive to the adaptations that underlie fatigue and the mechanisms by which corollary discharge may mediate this perception. The implication of a role for corollary discharge in fine manipulative tasks (98) under- scores the precision of perceptions based on corollary discharge. Indeed, Gandevia and Rothwell were able to conclude that “motor commands can be precisely moni- tored and fractionated for individual intrinsic muscles of the human hand without recourse to afferent feedback” (Ref. 98, p. 1117). It would be of interest to determine whether this discriminative capability was impaired by fatigue.

Finally, virtually no information is available on the relationship between the sense of effort and the readi- ness potential (Bereitschaftspotential) (140), which is a

1642 BRIEF REVIEW

resonance understan

have all contributed ding of the mechani

to sm

recent advan s underlying

ces in our clinically

relevant fatigue. These mechan sms include adaptations in neuro muscular transmission muscle membrane exci- tation, excitation-contraction coupling, intracellular met- abolic changes, and substrate depletion (70, 149, 176, 181,194). For the role of CNS pathophysiology in fatigue, however, progress has been far slower due to problems of definition, inattention to the potential value of psycho- physical tests, and lack of control data (56). Nonetheless, well-established techniques that are now in regular clini- cal use (74, 176) can be used to study suprasegmental aspects of clinical fatigue. For example, Stokes, Edwards and colleagues (211) used these techniques to examine patients with “effort syndromes,” a term loosely used to describe a variety of syndromes that include an increased sense of effort during activities of daily living. Their pa- tient sample was shown to have central, rather than pe- ripheral, neuromuscular impairments. Similarly, in pa-

the “chronic fatigue .ce that the abnorma

syndrome,” .lity was ex-

negative-going signal that is averaged from scalp electro- encephalogram recordings and thought to originate in cerebral cortical structures during the planning of volun- tary (particularly novel) movement. However, a prelimi- nary report has shown that muscle fatigue is accompa- nied by an increased readiness potential, suggesting that “isometric hand contractions with fatigued muscles in- volve a greater degree of cortical activation or in other terms, the engagement must be higher when working with fatigued muscles” (87). An extension of this finding might lead to establishing electrophysiological markers of suprasegmental factors in fatigue. For example, it would be valuable to determine whether fatigue has a greater effect on the early bilateral component of the readiness potential nent. This would be

than on the later unilateral of interest, because the early

compo- compo-

nent has recently been shown to be primarily generated by the supplementary motor area and the latter by the primary motor area (58). This assessment was based the concurrent measurement of the magnetoelectroencephalogram,

readiness potential, the and regional blood flow.

This combination of measurements could potentially provide a noninvasive method of determining the role of

from eviden

level of the motor cortex” and “the for movement” (152). These studies

tients there

suffering is recent

66 pressed executiv ‘e

above the pathways

underscore the need for advanced psychophysical testing and therapy for such patients and suggest that the sense of effort may entail at least two componen ciated with the impairment of performance

ts: an

one asso- .d another

of “higher” pathophysi .ological origin, wherein t ceived effort does not relate directly to motor

he per- perfor-

various cerebral structures in fatigue. CelLuLar issues. It may be possible to explore the cellu-

lar basis of the sense of effort. A substantial database exists on the impulse traffic between suprasegmental and segmental centers during the elaboration of move- ment, , as recorded form av ariety of

in conscious motor tasks.

prim The

ates trained to per- data consist of the

from a number of supra- related to the kinematic

mance. changes in the perceived effort represent an important compo-

little is known about the basis of this sensation.

behavioral and However, the

psychophysical and neurophysiological techniques to study the sense of effort are currently available, and

recorded and then

movement. This approach 1960s has provided a mea

, pio- ns to

determine the relative roles of the cerebral cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum in the control of movement. This work includes the demonstration of corticopetal input from primary sensory afferents during movement, but not of corollary discharges from lower spinal centers, as

Summary. .ssociated w

Although ith a task a

nent of fatigue, neurobiological

activity of segmental

single cells structures

and kinetic details of the neered by Evarts in the

has nal

been achieved in primate visual centers of other species [electric

now POPU

seems lation

centers and the spi- fish, cat (7, 154)]. It

extend Evarts’ technique to in the study of suprasegmen-

appropriate to analyses (104a)

following discharge

there is a demonstrated clinical need for systematic stud- ies in this area. Studies are needed to I) quantify the relationship between the sense of effort and the decline in force for different muscle groups and tasks, 2) exam- ine the effects of fatigue on tasks for which sensory feed- back is fatigue

kn on

.own to timing

be important, 3 judgments and

) assess the the means

corollary discharge may mediate this percep acterize the relationship between the sense

effects of by which

tion,h) char- of effort and

the . log1

readiness potential, 5) use .cal population-analysis tee

established electrophysio- hniques to explore the cel-

lular basis of the sense of effort, and 6) exten physical procedures to study the sense of effort

.d psycho- in clinical

tal aspects questions. 1

of muscle ) Can a neu

fatigue to ronal basis

address for coral

the .lary

at various levels of the neuraxis be established in the conscious primate? 2) What is the temporal relationship between changes in corollary discharge and force failure

influence the association en by corollary discharge

e contracti influenced

ons? 4) Is corollary by peripheral affer-

feedback? Studies that address such issues shou tribute to our unde rstanding of the cellular basis

Id of

during fatigue? 3) Does fatigue between corticopetal input driv populations. and the timing o discharge during

f muscl fatigue Conclusions

Fatigue is effects that

a concept that encompasses a class of acute impair motor performance. We have identi-

that provide a basis for the systematic themes fatigue.

I) Task dependency. The specific mechanisms that cause fatigue in a given condition are determined by the details of the task performed by the individual; these mechanisms can include the CNS drive to motor neu- rons, the muscles and motor units that are activated,

ent con

fied four study of

th .e sense Clinical

of effort. issues. Al thou gh muscle fatigue is a prominent

symptom in a wide variety of diseases of the CNS and the peripheral neuromuscular system, there are no recent systematic accounts of its prevalence in the clinical litera- ture. This situation is changing at the peripheral neuro- muscular level of clinical inquiry, where electrop hysiol- ogy, muscle biopsy biochemistry, and nuclear magnetic neuromuscular propagation, excitation-contraction cou-

BRIEF REVIEW 1643

pling, the availability of metabolic substrates, the intra- cellular milieu, the contractile apparatus, and muscle blood flow. Task variables that can influence the prevail- ing mechanism include the level of motivation of the sub- ject, the intensity and duration of the activity, and the extent to which the activity is continuously sustained (isometric vs. dynamic). Studies are needed that identify the boundary conditions and interactions between these mechanisms during human performance.

2) Force-fatigability relationship. For most, but not all, experimental conditions, the greater the force exerted by a muscle or motor unit for a given task, the more the muscle will fatigue. This suggests that the mechanisms underlying fatigue scale with the force that is exerted. There is a need to systematically explore the force-fati- gability domain for a variety of muscles and tasks to de- termine the global nature of the relationship and to iden- tify local regions where the relationship is nonmono- tonic.

3) Muscle wisdom. One set of adaptations that has been associated with sustained activity includes a concur- rent decline in force, relaxation rate, and motor neuron discharge. This association has been termed “muscle wisdom,” because it optimizes the force and ensures an economical activation of fatiguing muscle by the CNS. The mechanisms underlying muscle wisdom remain to be identified.

4) Sense of effort. The perceived effort associated with a task, which represents an important component of fa- tigue, is derived from centrally generated motor com- mands (and perhaps their formulation) that give rise to corollary discharges. Little is known about the interac- tion between changes in the sense of effort and force failure in fatigue, although the techniques to study the sense of effort are available and a clinical need for infor- mation on this topic has been demonstrated.

The authors are grateful to Drs. Enzo Cafarelli, Andrew Fuglevand, Simon Gandevia, Lynette Jones, and David McKenzie for comments on a draft of the manuscript.

This work was supported, in part, by National Institutes of Health Grants AG-09000, GM-08400, NS-25077, HL-07249, NS-07309, and RR-05675 and National Science Foundation Grant INT-852083.

Address for reprint requests: R. M. Enoka, Dept. of Exercise and Sport Sciences, Gittings Bldg., University of Arizona, Tucson, AZ 85721.

REFERENCES

1. ALLEN, D. G., J. A. LEE, AND H. WESTERBLAD. Intracellular cal- cium and tension during fatigue in isolated single muscle fibres from Xenopus laevis. J. Physiol. Lond. 415: 433-458, 1989.

2. ANISS, A. M., S. C. GANDEVIA, AND R. J. MILNE. Changes in per- ceived heaviness and motor commands produced by cutaneous reflexes in man. J. Physiol. Lond. 397: 113-126, 1988.

3. ASMUSSEN, E. Muscle fatigue. Med. Sci. Sports 11: 313-321, 1979. 4. BANISTER, E. W. The perception of effort: an inductive approach.

Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 41: 141-150, 1979. 5. BARTLEY, S. H., AND E. CHUTE. Fatigue and Impairment in Man.

New York: McGraw-Hill, 1947. 6. BELANGER, A. Y., AND A. J. MCCOMAS. Extent of motor unit acti-

vation during effort. J. Appl. Physiol. 51: 1131-1135, 1981. 7. BELL, C. C. Effects of motor commands on sensory inflow, with

examples from electric fish. In: Comparative Physiology of Sensory Systems, edited by L. Bolis, R. D. Keynes, and S. H. P. Maddress. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1984, p. 637-646.

8. BELLEMARE, F., AND N. GARZANITI. Failure of neuromuscular

9.

10.

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

16.

17.

18.

19.

20.

21.

22.

23.

24.

25.

26.

27.

28.

29.

30.

propagation during human maximal voluntary contraction. J. Appl. Physiol. 64: 1084-1093, 1988. BELLEMARE, F., AND A. GRASSINO. Evaluation of human dia- phragm fatigue. J. Appl. Physiol. 53: 1196-1206, 1982. BERGSTROM, M., AND E. HULTMAN. Contraction characteristics of the human quadriceps muscle during percutaneous electrical stimulation. Pfluegers Arch. 417: 136-141, 1990. BERGSTROM, M., AND E. HULTMAN. Energy cost and fatigue dur- ing intermittent electrical stimulation of human skeletal muscle. J. Appl. Physiol. 65: 1500-1505, 1988. BEVAN, L., Y. LAOURIS, R. M. REINKING, AND D. G. STUART. The effect of the stimulation pattern on the fatigue of single motor units. J. Physiol. Lond. In press. BEZANILLA, F., C. CAPUTO, H. GONZALEZ-SERRATOS, AND R. A. VENOSA. Sodium dependence of the inward spread of activation in isolated twitch muscle fibres of the frog. J. Physiol. Lond. 223: 507-523, 1972. BHAGAT, B., AND N. WHEELER. Effect of amphetamine on the swimming endurance of rats. Neuropharmacology 12: 711-713, 1973. BIANCHI, C. P., AND S. NARAYAN. Possible role of the transverse tubules in accumulating calcium released from the terminal cis- ternae by stimulation and drugs. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 60: 503-507,1982. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B. Muscle fatigue and the influence of chang- ing neural drive. Clin. Chest Med. 5: 21-34, 1984. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B., E. CAFARELLI, AND N. K. VBLLESTAD. Fa- tigue of submaximal static contractions. Acta Physiol. Stand. Suppl. 556: 137-148, 1986. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B., N. J. DAWSON, R. S. JOHANSSON, AND 0. C. J. LIPPOLD. Reflex origin for the slowing of motoneurone firing rates in fatigue of human voluntary contractions. J. Physiol. Lond. 379: 451-459, 1986. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B., F. FURBUSH, AND J. J. WOODS. Fatigue of intermittent submaximal voluntary contractions: central and pe- ripheral factors. J. Appl. Physiol. 61: 421-429, 1986. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B., R. JOHANSSON, 0. C. J. LIPPOLD, S. SMITH, AND J. J. WOODS. Changes in motoneurone firing rates during sustained maximal voluntary contractions. J. Physiol. Lond. 340: 335-346, 1983. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B., R. JOHANSSON, 0. C. J. LIPPOLD, AND J. J. WOODS. Contractile speed and EMG changes during fatigue of sustained maximal voluntary contractions. J. Neurophysiol. 50: 313-324, 1983. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B., D. A. JONES, G. P. HOSKING, AND R. H. T. EDWARDS. Central and peripheral fatigue in sustained maximum voluntary contractions of human quadriceps muscle. Clin. Sci. Mol. Med. 54: 609-614, 1978. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B., D. A. JONES, AND J. J. WOODS. Excitation frequency and muscle fatigue: electrical responses during human voluntary and stimulated contractions. Exp. Neurol. 64: 414-427, 1979. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, B., C. G. KUKULKA, 0. C. J. LIPPOLD, AND J. J. WOODS. The absence of neuromuscular transmission failure in sustained maximal voluntary contractions. J. Physiol. Lond. 330: 265-278, 1982. BINDER-MACLEOD, S. A., AND C. B. BARKER. Use of a catchlike property of human skeletal muscle to reduce fatigue. Muscle Nerve 14: 850-857, 1991. BINDER-MACLEOD, S. A., AND T. GUERIN. Preservation of force output through progressive reduction of stimulation frequency in human quadriceps femoris muscle. Phys. Ther. 70: 619-625,199O. BINDER-MACLEOD, S. A., AND L. R. MCDERMOND. Changes in the force-frequency relationship of the human quadriceps femoris muscle following electrically and voluntarily induced fatigue. Phys. Ther. 72: 95-104, 1992. BLINKS, J. R., R. RUDEL, AND S. R. TAYLOR. Calcium transients in isolated amphibian skeletal muscle fibres: detection with ae- quorin. J. Physiol. Lond. 277: 291-323, 1978. BLOMSTRAND, E., F. CELSING, AND E. A. NEWSHOLME. Changes in plasma concentrations of aromatic and branched-chain amino acids during sustained exercise in man and their possible role in fatigue. Acta Physiol. Stand. 133: 115-121, 1988. BONGIOVANNI, L. G., AND K.-E. HAGBARTH. Tonic vibration re-

1644 BRIEF REVIEW

flexes elicited during fatigue from maximal voluntary contrac- tions in man. J. Physiol. Lond. 423: l-14, 1990.

31. BORG, J., L. GRIMBY, AND J. HANNERZ. The fatigue of voluntary contraction and the peripheral electrical propagation of single motor units in man. J. Physiol. Lond. 340: 435-444, 1983.

32. BOTTERMAN, B. R., AND T. C. COPE. Motor-unit stimulation pat- terns during fatiguing contractions of constant tension. J. Neuro- physiol. 60: 1198-1214, 1988.

33. BRIDGES, C. R., B. J. CLARK, R. L. HAMMOND, AND L. STEPHEN- SON. Skeletal muscle bioenergetics during frequency-dependent fatigue. Am. J. Physiol. 260 (Cell Physiol. 29): C643-C651, 1991.

34. BROBERG, S., AND K. SAHLIN. Adenine nucleotide degradation in human skeletal muscle during prolonged exercise. J. Appl. Phys- iol. 67: 116-122, 1989.

%a.BuRDON, J. G. W., K. J. KILLIAN, D. G. STUBBING, AND E. J. M. CAMPBELL. Effect of background loads on the perception of added loads to breathing. J. Appl. Physiol. 54: 1222-1228, 1983.

35. BURKE, R. E., D. N. LEVINE, P. TSAIRIS, AND F. E. ZAJAC III. Physiological types and histochemical profiles in motor units of the cat gastrocnemius. J. Physiol. Lond. 234: 723-748, 1973.

36. BURKE, R. E., P. RUDOMIN, AND F. E. ZAJAC III. The effect of activation history on tension production by individual muscle units. Brain Res. 109: 515-529, 1976.

37. CAFARELLI, E. Force sensation in fresh and fatigued human skele- tal muscle. In: Exercise and Sport Sciences Reviews, edited by K. B. Pandolf. New York: Macmillan, 1988, vol. 16, p. 139-168.

38. CAFARELLI, E., AND C. E. KOSTKA. Effect of vibration on static force sensation in man. Exp. Neurol. 74: 331-340, 1981.

39. CAFARELLI, E., AND J. LAYTON-WOOD. Effect of vibration on force sensation in fatigued muscle. Med. Sci. Sports Exercise 18: 516-521, 1986.

40. CAIN, W. S. Nature of perceived effort and fatigue: roles of strength and blood flow in muscle contractions. J. Motor Behav. 5: 33-47, 1973.

41. CAIN, W. S., AND J. C. STEVENS. Effort in sustained and phasic handgrip contractions. Am. J. Psychol. 84: 52-65, 1971.

42. CAMPBELL, E. J. M., S. C. GANDEVIA, K. J. KILLIAN, C. K. MA- HUTTE, AND J. R. A. RIGG. Changes in the perception of inspira- tory resistive loads during partial curarization. J. Physiol. Lond. 309: 93-100, 1980.

43. CHAOULOFF, F., G. A. KENNETT, B. SERRURIER, D. MERINO, AND G. CURZON. Amino acid analysis demonstrates that increased plasma free tryptophan causes the increase of brain tryptophan during exercise in the rat. J. Neurochem. 46: 1647-1650, 1986.

44. CHASE, P. B., AND M. J. KUSHMERICK. Effects of pH on contrac- tion of rabbit fast muscle and slow skeletal muscle fibers. Biophys. J. 53: 935-946, 1988.

45. CHASIOTIS, D., M. BERGSTROM, AND E. HULTMAN. ATP utiliza- tion and force during intermittent and continuous muscle contrac- tions. J. Appl. Physiol. 63: 167-174, 1987.

46. CHRISTAKOS, C. N., AND U. WINDHORST. Spindle gain increase during muscle unit fatigue. Brain Res. 365: 388-392, 1986.

47. CLARK, G. T., AND M. C. CARTER. Electromyographic study of human jaw-closing muscle endurance, fatigue and recovery at various isometric force levels. Arch. Oral Biol. 30: 563-569, 1985.

48. CLARK, G. T., M. C. CARTER, AND P. L. BEEMSTERBOER. Analysis of electromyographic signals in human jaw closing muscles at various isometric force levels. Arch. Oral BioL. 33: 833-837, 1988.

49. COGGAN, A. R., AND E. F. COYLE. Reversal of fatigue during pro- longed exercise by carbohydrate infusion or ingestion. J. Appl. Physiol. 63: 2388-2395, 1987.

50. COOKE, R., K. FRANKS, G. B. LUCIANI, AND E. PATE. The inhibi- tion of rabbit skeletal muscle contraction by hydrogen ions and phosphate. J. Physiol. Lond. 395: 77-97, 1988.

51. COOKE, R., AND E. PATE. The inhibition of muscle contraction by the products of ATP hydrolysis. In: Biochemistry of Exercise VII, edited by A. W. Taylor, P. D. Gollnick, H. J. Green, C. D. Ianuzzo, E. G. Noble, G. Metivier, and J. R. Sutton. Champaign, IL: Hu- man Kinetics, 1990, p. 59-72.

52. COOPER, R. G., R. H. T. EDWARDS, H. GIBSON, AND M. J. STOKES. Human muscle fatigue: frequency dependence of excitation and force generation. J. Physiol. Lond. 397: 585-599, 1988.

53. COYLE, E. F., A. R. COGGAN, M. K. HEMMERT, AND J. L. IVY. Muscle glycogen utilization during prolonged strenuous exercise when fed carbohydrate. J. Appl. Physiol. 61: 165-172, 1986.

54. CRAGO, P. E., AND S. V. ZACHARKIW. Lack of stretch reflex com- pensation for fatigue induced changes in the stiffness of the flexor pollicis longus. Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 11: 213, 1985.

55. DARLING, W. G., AND K. C. HAYES. Human servo responses to load disturbances in fatigued muscle. Brain Res. 267: 345-351, 1983.

56. DAVID, A. S., S. WESSELY, AND A. PELOSI. Postviral fatigue syn- drome: time for a new approach. Br. Med. J. 296: 696-699, 1988.

57. DAWSON, M. J., D. G. GADIAN, AND D. R. WILKIE. Mechanical relaxation rate and metabolism studied in fatiguing muscle by phosphorus nuclear magnetic resonance. J. Physiol. Lond. 299: 465-484,198O.

58. DEECKE, L. Experiments in voluntary movement physiology. 6th Int. Symp. Motor Control Albenia 1989, p. 63.

59. DELITTO, A., M. BROWN, M. J. STRUBE, S. J. ROSE, AND R. C. LEHMAN. Electrical stimulation of quadriceps femoris in an elite weight lifter: a single subject experiment. Int. J. Sports Med. 10: 187-191, 1989.

60. DENIER, H. C., J. DICHGANS, B. GUSCHLBAUER, AND H. MAU. The significance of proprioception on postural stabilization as as- sessed by ischemia. Brain Res. 296: 103-109, 1984.

61. DIETZ, V. Analysis of the electrical muscle activity during maxi- mal contraction and the influence of ischaemia. J. NeuroZ. Sci. 37: 187-197, 1978.

62. DONALDSON, S. K. Fatigue of sarcoplasmic reticulum: failure of excitation-contraction coupling in skeletal muscle. In: Biochemis- try of Exercise VII, edited by A. W. Taylor, P. D. Gollnick, H. J. Green, C. D. Ianuzzo, E. G. Noble, G. Metivier, and J. R. Sutton. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1990, p. 49-57.

63. DORFMAN, L. J., J. E. HOWARD, AND K. C. MCGILL. Triphasic behavioral response of motor units to submaximal fatiguing exer- cise. MuscZe Nerve 13: 621-628, 1990.

64. DUBOSE, L., T. B. SCHELHORN, AND H. P. CLAMANN. Changes in contractile speed of cat motor units during activity. Muscle Nerve 10: 744-752, 1987.

65. DUCHATEAU, J., AND K. HAINAUT. Electrical and mechanical fail- ures during sustained and intermittent contractions in humans. J. Appl. Physiol. 58: 942-947, 1985.

65a.DUCHATEAU, J., AND K. HAINAUT. Effects of immobilization on electromyogram power spectrum changes during fatigue. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 63: 458-462, 1991.

66. DUCHENE, J., AND F. GOUBEL. EMG spectral shift as an indicator of fatigability in an heterogeneous muscle group. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 61: 81-87, 1990.

67. DUDLEY, G. A., AND R. DJAMIL. Incompatibility of endurance- and strength-training modes of exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 59: 1446-1451, 1985.

68. EDMAN, K. A. P., AND F. LOU. Changes in force and stiffness induced by fatigue and intracellular acidification in frog muscle fibres. J. Physiol. Lond. 424: 133-149, 1990.

69. EDWARDS, R. H. T. Human muscle function and fatigue. In: Hu- man Muscle Fatigue: Physiological Mechanisms, edited by R. Porter and J. Whelan. London: Pitman, 1981, p. 1-18.

70. EDWARDS, R. H. T. Weakness and fatigue of skeletal muscles. Adv. Med. 18: 100-119, 1982.

71. EDWARDS, R. H. T., AND H. GIBSON. Perspectives in the study of normal and pathological skeletal muscle. In: Muscle Fatigue: Bio- chemical and Physiological Aspects, edited by G. Atlan, L. Be- liveau, and P. Bouissou. Paris: Masson, 1991, p. 3-15.

72. EDWARDS, R. H. T., D. K. HILL, AND D. A. JONES. Metabolic changes associated with the slowing of relaxation in fatigued mouse muscle. J. Physiol. Lond. 251: 287-301, 1975.

73. EDWARDS, R. H. T., D. K. HILL, D. A. JONES, AND P. A. MERTON. Fatigue of long duration in human skeletal muscle after exercise. J. Physiol. Lond. 272: 769-778, 1977.

74. EDWARDS, R. H. T., A. YOUNG, G. P. HOSKING, AND D. A. JONES. Human skeletal muscle function: description of tests and normal values. Clin. Sci. Mol. Med. 52: 283-290, 1977.

75. ELORANTA, V., AND P. V. KOMI. Function of the quadriceps fe- moris muscle under maximal concentric and eccentric contrac- tions. Electromyogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 20: 159-174, 1980.

76. ENOKA, R. M., AND A. J. FUGLEVAND. Neuromuscular basis of the maximum voluntary force capacity of muscle. In: Current Issues in Biomechanics: A Prospectus, edited by M. D. Grabiner. Cham- paign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1992.

BRIEF REVIEW 1645

77. ENOKA, R. M., M. A. NORDSTROM, R. M. REINKING, AND D. G. STUART. Motor unit-muscle spindle interactions during fatigue. Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 16: 886, 1990.

78. ENOKA, R. M., G. A. ROBINSON, AND A. R. KOSSEV. Task and fatigue effects on low-threshold motor units in human hand mus- cle. J. Neurophysiol. 62: 1344-1359, 1989.

79. ENOKA, R. M., AND D. G. STUART. The contribution of neuro- science to exercise studies. Federation Proc. 44: 2279-2285, 1985.

80. ENOKA, R. M., N. TRAYANOVA, Y. LAOURIS, L. BEVAN, R. M. REINKING, AND D. G. STUART. Fatigue-related changes in motor unit action potentials of adult cats. Muscle Nerve 14: 138-150, 1992.

81. ERIKSSON, P.-O., E. STALBERG, AND L. ANTONI. Flexibility in mo- tor-unit firing pattern in the human temporal and masseter mus- cles related to type of activation and location. Arch. Oral Biol. 29: 707-712, 1984.

82. FITCH, S., AND A. J. MCCOMAS. Influence of human muscle length on fatigue. J. Physiol. Lond. 362: 205-213, 1985.

82a.FITTING, J. W., T. D. BRADLEY, P. A. EASTON, M. J. LINCOLN, M. D. GOLDMAN, AND A. GRASSINO. Dissociation between dia- phragmatic and rib cage muscle fatigue. J. Appl. Physiol. 64: 959- 965, 1988.

83. FITTS, R. H., E. M. BALOG, AND L. V. THOMPSON. Fatigue-in- duced alterations in contractile function: the role of excitation- contraction coupling. In: Muscle Fatigue: Biochemical and Physio- logical Aspects, edited by G. Atlan, L. Beliveau, and P. Bouissou. Paris: Masson, 1991, p. 40-48.

84. FITTS, R. H., AND J. M. METZGER. Mechanisms of muscular fa- tigue. In: Principles of Exercise Biochemistry, edited by J. R. Poort- mans. Basel: Karger, 1988, p. 212-229.

85. FORBES, A. The interpretation of spinal reflexes in terms of pres- ent knowledge of nerve condition. Physiol. Reu. 2: 361-414, 1922.

86. FOURNIER, E., AND E. PIERROT-DESEILLIGNY. Changes in trans- mission in some reflex pathways during movement in humans. News Physiol. Sci. 4: 29-32, 1989.

87. FREUDE, G., P. ULLSPERGER, AND M. PIETSCHMANN. Are self- paced repetitive fatiguing hand contractions accompanied by changes in movement related brain potentials? In: Motor Control, edited by G. N. Gantchev, B. Dimitrov, and P. Gatev. New York: Plenum, 1987, p. 99-103.

88. FUGLEVAND, A. J. A Motor Unit Pool Model: Relationship of Neural Control Parameters to Isometric Muscle Tension and the Electrom.yogram (PhD thesis). Waterloo, Ontario: Univ. of Water- loo, 1989.

89. FUGLEVAND, A. J., AND R. M. ENOKA. Effects of immobilization on motor function in a human hand muscle. Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 16: 421, 1990.

90. GANDEVIA, S. C. Neuromuscular fatigue mechanisms: central and peripheral factors affecting motoneuronal output in fatigue. In: Fatigue in Sports and Exercise. Footscray, Australia: FIT Victoria University of’ Technology, 1990, p. 9-15.

91. GANDEVIA, S. C. Roles for perceived voluntary motor commands in motor control. Trends Neurosci. 10: 81-85, 1987.

92. GANDEVIA, S. C. The perception of motor commands or effort during muscular paralysis. Brain 105: 151-159, 1982.

93. GANDEVIA, S. C., K. J. KILLIAN, AND E. J. M. CAMPBELL. The effect of respiratory muscle fatigue on respiratory sensations. Clin. Sci. Lond. 60: 463-466, 1981.

94. GANDEVIA, S. C., G. MACEFIELD, D. BURKE, AND D. K. MCKEN- ZIE. Voluntary activation of human motor axons in the absence of muscle afferent feedback. Brain 113: 1563-1581, 1990.

95. GANDEVIA, S. C., AND D. I. MCCLOSKEY. Effects of related sen- sory inputs on motor performances in man studied through changes in perceived heaviness. J. Physiol. Lond. 272: 653-672, 1977.

96. GANDEVIA, S. C., AND D. I. MCCLOSKEY. Changes in motor com- mands, as shown by changes in perceived heaviness, during par- tial curarization and peripheral anaesthesia in man. J. Physiol. Lond. 272: 673-689, 1977.

97. GANDEVIA, S. C., AND D. K. MCKENZIE. Activation of the human diaphragm during maximal static efforts. J. Physiol. Lond. 367: 45-56, 1985.

98. GANDEVIA, S. C., AND J. C. ROTHWELL. Knowledge of motor com- mands and the recruitment of human motoneurons. Brain 110: 1117-1130, 1987.

99. GARLAND, S. J. Role of small diameter afferents in reflex inhibi- tion during human muscle fatigue. J. Physiol. Lond. 435: 547-558, 1991.

100. GARLAND, S. J., S. H. GARNER, AND A. J. MCCOMAS. Relationship between numbers and frequencies of stimuli in human muscle fatigue. J. Appl. Physiol. 65: 89-93, 1988.

101. GARLAND, S. J., AND A. J. MCCOMAS. Reflex inhibition of human soleus muscle during fatigue. J. Physiol. Lond. 429: 17-27, 1990.

102. GARLAND, S. J., L. P. SERRANO, G. A. ROBINSON, AND R. M. EN- OKA. Behavior of motor units in human biceps brachii muscle during fatigue. Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 15: 396, 1989.

103. GARNER, S. H., A. L. HICKS, AND A. J. MCCOMAS. Prolongation of twitch potentiating mechanisms throughout muscle fatigue and recovery. Exp. Neurol. 103: 277-281, 1989.

104. GARNER, S. H., J. R. SUTTON, R. L. BURSE, A. J. MCCOMAS, A. CYMERMAN, AND C. S. HOUSTON. Operation Everest II: neuromus- cular performance under conditions of extreme simulated alti- tude. J. Appl. Physiol. 68: 1167-1172, 1990.

~04a.GEORGOPOuLOS, A. P., A. B. SCHWARTZ, AND R. E. KETTNER. Neuronal population coding of movement direction. Science Wash. DC 233: 1416-1419, 1986.

105. GOOCH, J. L., B. Y. NEWTON, AND J. H. PETAJAN. Motor unit spike counts before and after maximal voluntary contraction. Muscle Nerve 13: 1146-1151, 1990.

106. GOODWIN, G. M., D. HOFFMAN, AND E. S. LUSCHEI. The strength of the reflex response to sinusoidal stretch of monkey jaw closing muscles during voluntary contraction. J. Physiol. Lond. 279: 81- 111, 1978.

107. GORDON, D. A., R. M. ENOKA, G. M. KARST, AND D. G. STUART. Force development and relaxation in single motor units of adult cats during a standard fatigue test. J. Physiol. Lond. 421: 583-594, 1990.

108. GRANGE, R. W., AND M. E. HOUSTON. Simultaneous potentiation and fatigue in quadriceps after a 60-second maximal voluntary isometric contraction. J. Appl. Physiol. 70: 726-731, 1991.

109. GREGORY, J. E. Relations between identified tendon organs and motor units in the medial gastrocnemius muscle of the cat. Exp. Brain Res. 81: 602-608, 1990.

110. GRIMBY, L., J. HANNERZ, AND B. HEDMAN. The fatigue and volun- tary discharge properties of single motor units in man. J. Physiol. Lond. 316: 545-554, 1981.

111. HAIDA, N., W. M. FOWLER, R. T. ABRESCH, D. B. LARSON, R. B. SHARMAN, R. G. TAYLOR, AND R. K. ENTRIKIN. Effect of hind- limb suspension on young and adult. skeletal muscle. I. Normal mice. Exp. Neurol. 103: 68-76, 1989.

112. H;~;KKINEN, K., AND P. V. KOMI. Electromyographic and mechan- ical characteristics of human skeletal muscle during fatigue under voluntary and reflex conditions. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neuro- physiol. 55: 436-444, 1983.

113. HALES, J. P., AND S. C. GANDEVIA. Assessment of maximal volun- tary contraction with twitch interpolation: an instrument to mea- sure twitch responses. J. Neurosci. Methods 25: 97-102, 1988.

114. HAMM, T. M., P. M. NEMETH, L. SOLANKI, D. A. GORDON, R. M. REINKING, AND D. G. STUART. Association between biochemical and physiological properties in single motor units. Muscle Nerve 11: 245-254, 1988.

115. HAMM, T. M., R. M. REINKING, AND D. G. STUART. Electromyo- graphic responses of mammalian motor units to a fatigue test. Electromyogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 29: 485-494, 1989.

116. HAYWARD, L., D. BREITBACH, AND 2. RYMER. Increased inhibi- tory effects on close synergists during muscle fatigue in the decer- ebrate cat. Brain Res. 440: 199-203, 1988.

117. HAYWARD, L., U. WESSELMANN, AND W. 2. RYMER. The effects of muscle fatigue on small diameter muscle afferents in the anesthe- tized cat. Sot. Neurosci. Abstr. 14: 794, 1988.

118. HAYWARD, L., U. WESSELMANN, AND W. Z. RYMER. Effects of muscle fatigue on mechanically sensitive afferents of slow con- duction velocity in the cat triceps surae. J. Neurophysiol. 65: 360- 370, 1991.

119. HECKMAN, C. J., AND M. D. BINDER. Computer simulation of the steady-state input-output function of the cat medial gastrocne- mius motoneuron pool. J. Neurophysiol. 65: 952-967, 1991.

120. HERMANSEN, L., E. HULTMAN, AND B. SALTIN. Muscle glycogen during prolonged severe exercise. Acta Physiol. Stand. 71: 129- 139, 1967.

1646 BRIEF REVIEW

121. HEYES, M. P., E. S. GARNETT, AND G. COATES. Central dopami- nergic activity influences rats’ ability to exercise. Life Sci. 36: 671- 677, 1985.

122. HEYES, M. P., E. S. GARNETT, AND G. COATES. Nigrostriatal dopa- minergic activity is increased during exhaustive exercise stress in rats. Life Sci. 42: 1537-1542, 1988.

122a.HICKS, A. L., C. M. CUPIDO, J. MARTIN, AND J. DENT. Muscle

123.

124.

125.

126.

127.

128.

129.

130.

131

132

133.

134.

excitation in elderly adults: the effects of training. Muscle Nerve 15: 87-93,1992. HOBBS, S. F., AND S. C. GANDEVIA. Cardiovascular responses and the sense of effort during attempts to contract paralysed muscles: role of the spinal cord. Neurosci. Lett. 57: 85-90, 1985. HOWARD, J. D., AND R. M. ENOKA. Maximum bilateral contrac- tions are modified by neurally mediated interlimb effects. J. Appl. Physiol. 70: 306-316, 1991. HULTMAN, E., M. BERGSTROM, L. L. SPRIET, AND K. S~DERLUND. Energy metabolism and fatigue. In: Biochemistry of Exercise VII, edited by A. W. Taylor, P. D. Gollnick, H. J. Green, C. D. Ianuzzo, E. G. Noble, G. M&ivier, and J. R. Sutton. Cham- paign, IL: Human Kinetics, 1990, p. 73-92. HULTMAN, E., AND H. SJ~HOLM. Electromyogram, force and re- laxation time during and after continuous electrical stimulation of human skeletal muscle in situ. J. Physiol. Lond. 339: 33-40, 1983. HUNTER, I. W., AND R. E. KEARNEY. Invariance of ankle dynamic stiffness during fatiguing muscle contractions. J. Biomech. 16: 985-991, 1983. HUTTON, R. S., AND D. L. NELSON. Stretch sensitivity of Golgi tendon organs in fatigued gastrocnemius muscle. Med. Sci. Sports Exercise 18: 69-74, 1986. JAMI, L., K. S. K. MURTHY, J. PETIT, AND D. ZYTNICKI. After-ef- fects of repetitive stimulation at low frequency on fast-contract- ing motor units of cat muscle. J. Physiol. Lond. 340: 129~143,1983. JAMMES, Y., P. COLLETT, P. LENOIR, A. LAMA, F. BERTHELIN, AND C. ROUSSOS. Diaphragmatic fatigue produced by constant or modulated electric currents. lMuscLe Nerve 14: 27-34, 1991. JONES, D. A. Muscle fatigue due to changes beyond the neuromus- cular junction. In: Human Muscle Fatigue: Physiological Mechu- nisms, edited by R. Porter and J. Whelan. London: Pitman, 1981, p. 178-196. JONES, D. A., B. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, AND R. H. T. EDWARDS. Exci- tation frequency and muscle fatigue: mechanical responses during voluntary and stimulated contractions. Exp. NeuroL. 64: 401-413, 1979. JONES, L. A., AND I. W. HUNTER. Effect of fatigue on force sensa- tion. Ex~. Neurol. 81: 640-650, 1983. (JONES, L. A., AND I. W. HUNTER. Effect of muscle tendon vibra- tion on the perception of force. Exp. Neurol. 87: 35-45, 1985.

135.

136.

137.

l%a.JONES, L. A., AND I. W. HUNTER. Force and EMG correlates of constant effort contractions. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 51: 75-83,1983. JBRGENSEN, K., N. FALLENTIN, C. KROGH-LUND, AND B. JEN- SEN. Electromyography and fatigue during prolonged low-level static contractions. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 57: 316- 321, 1988. KERNELL, D., Y. DONSELAAR, AND 0. EERBEEK. Effects of physio- logical amounts of high- and low-rate chronic stimulation on fast- twitch muscle of the cat hindlimb. II. Endurance-related proper- ties. J. Neurophysiol. 58: 614-627, 1987. KERNELL, D., AND A. W. MONSTER. Time course and properties of late adaptation in spinal motoneurones of the cat. Exp. Bruin Res. 46: 191-196, 1982. KERNELL, D., AND A. W. MONSTER. Motoneurone properties and motor fatigue. An intracellular study of gastrocnemius motoneur- ones of the cat. Exp. Bruin Res. 46: 197-204, 1982. KIRSCH, R. F., AND W. 2. RYMER. Neural compensation for muscu- lar fatigue: evidence for significant force regulation in man. J. Neurophysiol. 57: 1893-1910, 1987. KORNHUBER, H. H., AND L. DEECKE (Editors). Motiuation, Motor and Sensory Processes of the Brain: Electrical Potentials, Behavior and CLinical Use. New York: Elsevier, 1980, vol. 54. (Prog. Brain Res.) KRANZ, H., A. M. WILLIAMS, J. CASSELL, D. J. CADDY, AND R. B. SILBERSTEIN. Factors determining the frequency content of the electromyogram. J, Appl. PhysioZ. 55: 392-399, 1983.

138.

139.

140.

141.

142.

143.

144.

145.

146.

147.

148.

149.

150.

151.

152.

153.

154.

155.

156.

157.

158.

159.

KRARUP, C. Enhancement and diminution of mechanical tension evoked by staircase and by tetani in rat muscle. J. Physiol. Lond. 311: 355-372, 1981. KROON, G. W., M. NAEIJE, AND T. L. HANSSON. Electromyo- graphic power-spectral changes during repeated fatiguingcontrac- tions of the human masseter muscle. Arch. Oral Biol. 31: 603-608, 1986. KUKULKA, C. G., AND H. P. CLAMANN. Comparison of the recruit- ment and discharge properties of motor units in human brachial biceps and adductor pollicis during isometric contractions. Brain Res. 219: 45-55, 1981. KUKULKA, C. G., A. G. RUSSELL, AND M. A. MOORE. Electrical and mechanical changes in human soleus muscle during sustained maximum isometric contractions. Brain Res. 362: 47-54, 1986. LANNERGREN, J., L. LAR~SON, AND H. WESTERBLAD. A novel type of delayed tension reduction observed in rat motor units after intense activity. J. Physiol. Lond. 412: 267-276, 1989. LANNERGREN, J., AND H. WESTERBLAD. Force decline due to fa- tigue and intracellular acidification in isolated fibres from mouse skeletal muscle. J. Physiol. Lond. 434: 307-322, 1991. LANNERGREN, J., AND H. WESTERBLAD. Maximum tension and force-velocity properties of fatigued, single Xenopus muscle fibres studied by caffeine and high K+. J. Physiol. Lond. 409: 473-490, 1989. LENMAN, A. J. R., F. M. TULLEY, G. VRBOVA, M. R. DIMITRIJE- VIC, AND J. A. TOWLE. Muscle fatigue in some neurological dis- orders. Muscle Nerve 12: 938-942, 1989. LIND, A. R. Muscle fatigue and recovery from fatigue induced by sustained contractions. J. Physiol. Lond. 127: 162-171, 1959. LIND, A. R., AND J. S. PETROFSKY. Amplitude of the surface elec- tromyogram during fatiguing isometric contractions. Muscle Nerve 2: 257-264, 1979. LLOYD, A. R., S. C. GANDEVIA, AND J. P. HALES. Muscle perfor- mance, voluntary activation, twitch properties and perceived ef- fort in normal subjects and patients with chronic fatigue syn- drome. Bruin 114: 85-98, 1991. LOISELLE, D. S., AND B. WALMSLEY. Cost of force development as a function of stimulus rate in rat soleus muscle. Am. J. Physiol. 243 (Cell Physiol. 12): C242-C246, 1982, LUNDBERG, A. Function of the ventral spinocerebellar tract. A new hypothesis. Exp. Brain Res. 12: 317-330, 1971. LUTTGAU, H. C. The effect of metabolic inhibitors on the fatigue of the action potential in single muscle fibres. J. Physiol. Lond. 178: 45-67, 1965. MACEFIELD, G., K.-E. HAGBARTH, R. GORMAN, S. C. GANDEVIA, AND D. BURKE. Decline in spindle support to ar-motoneurones during sustained voluntary contractions. J. Physiol. Lond. 440: 497-512,1991. MARSDEN,~. D.,J.C. MEADOWS,AND P. A. MERTON. Muscular wisdom (abstract). J. Physiol. Lond. 200: 15P, 1969. MARSDEN, C. D., J. C. MEADOWS, AND P. A. MERTON. “Muscular wisdom” that minimized fatigue during prolonged effort in man: peak rates of motoneuron discharge and slowing of discharge dur- ing fatigue. In: Motor Control Mechanisms in Health and Disease, edited by J. E. Desmedt. New York: Raven, 1983, p. 169-211. MARSDEN, C. D., J. C. ROTHWELL, AND B. L. DAY. The use of peripheral feedback in the control of movement. Trends Neurosci. 7: 253-257, 1984.

l!%.MATHIASSEN, S. E. Influence of angular velocity and movement frequency on the development of fatigue in repeated isokinetic knee extensions. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 59: 80-88, 1989.

160. MATON, B. Central nervous changes in fatigue induced by local work. In: Muscle Fatigue: Biochemical and Physiological Aspects, edited by G. Atlan, L. Beliveau, and P. Bouissou. Paris: Masson, 1991, p. 207-221.

161. MATON, B., AND D. GAMET. The fatigability of two agonistic mus- cles in human isometric voluntary submaximal contraction: an EMG study. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 58: 369-374, 1989.

162. MATTHEWS, P. B. C. Where does Sherrington’s “muscular sense” originate? Muscles, joints, corollary discharges? Annu. Rev. Neur- osci. 5: 189-218, 1982.

163. MAURITZ, H.-K., AND V. DIETZ. Characteristics of postural insta-

BRIEF REVIEW 1647

164.

165.

166.

167.

168.

169.

171.

172.

173.

174.

176.

177.

bility induced by ischemic blocking of leg afferents. Exp. Bruin Res. 38: 117-l 19, 1980. MCCLOSKEY, D. I. Differences between the senses of movement and position shown by the effects of loading and vibration of mus- cles in man. Brain Res. 63: 119-131, 1973. MCCLOSKEY, D. I. Kinesthetic sensibility. Physiol. Reu. 58: 763- 820, 1978. MCCLOSKEY, D. I., P. EBELING, AND G. M. GOODWIN. Estimation of weights and tensions and apparent involvement of a “sense of effort.” Exp. Neurol. 42: 220-232, 1974. MCCLOSKEY, D. I., S. GANDEVIA, E. K. POTTER, AND J. G. COLE- BATCH. Muscle sense and effort: motor commands and judgments about muscular contractions. In: Motor Control Mechanisms in Health and Disease, edited by J. E. Desmedt. New York: Raven, 1983, p. 151-167. MCKENZIE, D. K., AND S. C. GANDEVIA. Influence of muscle length on human inspiratory and limb muscle endurance. Respir. Physiol. 67: 171-182, 1987. MCKENZIE, D., AND S. C. GANDEVIA. Recovery from fatigue of human diaphragm and limb muscles. Respir. Physiol. 84: 49-60, 1991. MCKENZIE, D. K., B. L. PLASSMAN, AND S. C. GANDEVIA. Maxi- mal activation of the human diaphragm but not inspiratory mus- cles during static inspiratory efforts. Neurosci. Lett. 89: 63-68, 1988. MERTON, P. A. Voluntary strength and fatigue. J. Physiol. Lond. 123: 553-564, 1954. METZGER, J. M., AND R. H. FITTS. Fatigue from high- and low- frequency muscle stimulation: contractile and biochemical alter- ations. J. Appl. Physiol. 62: 2075-2082, 1987. MICHEL, R. N., AND P. F. GARDINER. To what extent is hindlimb suspension a model of disuse? Muscle Nerve 13: 646-653, 1990. MILLER, R. G., D. GIANNINI, H. S. MILNER-BROWN, R. B. LAYZER, A. P. KORETSKY, D. HOOPER, AND M. W. WEINER. Ef- fects of fatiguing exercise on high-energy phosphates, force, and EMG: evidence for three phases of recovery. Muscle Nerve 10: 810-821, 1987. MILNER-BROWN, H. S., AND R. G. MILLER. Muscle membrane excitation and impulse propagation velocity are reduced during muscle fatigue. Muscle Nerve 9: 367-374, 1986. MILNER-BROWN, H. S., AND R. G. MILLER. Increased muscular fatigue in patients with neurogenic muscle weakness: quantifica- tion and pathophysiology. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehubil. 70: 361-366, 1989. MOSSO, A. Die Ermudung. Leipzig: Horzel, 1892.

185.

186.

187.

188.

189.

190.

191.

192.

193.

194.

195.

196.

197.

198.

199 177a.MuzA, S. R., AND F. W. ZECHMAN. Scaling of added loads to ---’

breathing: magnitude estimation vs. handgrip matching. J. Appl. Physiol. 57: 888-891, 1984.

178. NARDONE, A., C. ROMANO, AND M. SCHIEPPATI. Selective recruit- 200.

ment of high-threshold human motor units during voluntary iso- tonic lengthening of active muscles. J. Physiol. Lond. 409: 451- 201. 471,1989.

178a.NARIC1, M. V., M. BORDINI, AND P. CERRETELLI. Effect of aging on human adductor pollicis muscle function. J. Appl. Physiol. 71: 202. 1277-1281, 1991.

179. NASSAR-GENTINA, V., J. V. PASSONNEAU, J. L. VERGARA, AND S. I. RAPOPORT. Metabolic correlates of fatigue and recovery from 203. fatigue in single muscle fibers. J. Gen. Physiol. 72: 593-606, 1978.

180. NELSON, D. L., AND R. S. HUTTON. Dynamic and static stretch responses in muscle spindle receptors in fatigued muscle. Med. Sci. Sports Exercise 17: 445-450, 1985. 204.

180a.NEWHAM, D. J., T. MCCARTY, AND J. TURNER. Voluntary activa- tion of human quadriceps during and after isokinetic exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 71: 2122-2126, 1991. 205.

181. NEWMAN, R. J., P. J. BORE, AND L. CHAN. Nuclear magnetic reso- nance studies of forearm muscle in Duchenne dystrophy. Br. Med. J. 284: 1072, 1982. 206.

182. NORDSTROM, M. A., AND T. S. MILES. Fatigue of single motor units in human masseter. J. Appl. Physiol. 68: 26-34, 1990.

183. NORDSTROM, M. A., J. M. SPIELMANN, Y. LAOURIS, R. M. REINK- ING, AND D. G. STUART. Motor-neuron adaptation to intermittent 207. stimulation. 3rd IBRO World Congr. Neurosci. Montreal 1991, p. 407. 208.

184. PERSON, R. S. Rhythmic activity of a group of human motoneur-

ones during voluntary contraction of a muscle. Electroencephu- logr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 36: 585-595, 1974. PERSON, R. S., AND L. P. KUDINA. Discharge frequency and dis- charge pattern of human motor units during voluntary contrac- tion of muscle. Electroencephulogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 32: 471- 483,1972. PHILLIPS, C. G., AND R. PORTER. Corticospinul Neurones, Their Role in Movement. London: Academic, 1977. PORTER, R., AND J. WHELAN (Editors). Human Muscle Fatigue: Physiological Mechanisms. London: Pitman, 1981. POWERS, R. K., AND M. D. BINDER. Effects of low-frequency stim- ulation on the tension-frequency relations of fast-twitch motor units in the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 66: 905-918, 1991. RANKIN, L. L., R. M. ENOKA, K. A. VOLZ, AND D. G. STUART. Coexistence of twitch potentiation and tetanic force decline in rat hindlimb muscle. J. Appl. Physiol. 65: 2687-2695, 1988. REID, C. The mechanism of voluntary muscle fatigue. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. 19: 17-42, 1928. RESPIRATORY MUSCLE FATIGUE WORKSHOP GROUP. NHLBI workshop summary: respiratory muscle fatigue. Am. Reu. Respir. Dis. 142: 474-480, 1990. ROBINSON, G. A., R. M. ENOKA, AND D. G. STUART. Immobiliza- tion-induced changes in motor unit force and fatigability in the cat. Muscle Nerve 14: 563-573, 1991. ROHMERT, W. Ermittung von Erholung-spausen fur statische Ar- beit des Menschen. Int. Angew. Physiol. 18: 123,196O. [Cited in B. Bigland-Ritchie and J. J. Woods. Changes in muscle contractile properties and neural control during human muscular fatigue. Muscle Nerve 7: 691-699, 1984.1 Ross, B. D., G. K. RADDA, D. G. GADIAN, G. ROCKER, M. ESIRI, AND J. FALCONER-SMITH. Examination of a case of suspected McArdle’s syndrome by 31P nuclear magnetic resonance. N. Engl. J. Med. 304: 1338-1342, 1981. ROTHWELL, J. C., M. M. TRAUB, B. L. DAY, J. A. OBESO, P. K. THOMAS, AND C. D. MARSDEN. Manual motor performance in a deafferented man. Bruin 105: 515-542, 1982. ROUSSOS, C., M. FIXLEY, D. GROSS, AND P. T. MACKELM. Fatigue of inspiratory muscles and their synergic behavior. J. Appl. Phys- iol. 46: 897-904, 1979. RUBE, N., AND N. H. SECHER. Effect of training on central factors in fatigue following two- and one-leg static exercise in man. Actu Physiol. Scund. 141: 87-95, 1990. SAHLIN, K., AND J. M. REN. Relationship of contraction capacity to metabolic changes during recovery from a fatiguing contrac- tion. J. Appl. Physiol. 67: 648-654, 1989. SANDERCOCK, T. G., J. A. FAULKNER, J. W. ALBERS, AND P. H. ABBRECHT. Single motor unit and fiber action potentials during fatigue. J. Appl. Physiol. 58: 1073-1079, 1985. SANES, J. N., K.-H. MAURITZ, M. C. DALAKAS, AND E. V. EVARTS. Motor control in humans with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. Hum. Neurobiol. 4: 101-114, 1985. SEYFFARTH, H. The behaviour of motor-units in voluntary con- tractions. Avhundlinger Utgitt Norske Videnskup-Akud Oslo. I. Mutemutisk-Nuturvidenskupelig Klusse 4: l-63, 1940. SJBGAARD, G. Muscle energy metabolism and electrolyte shifts during low-level prolonged static contraction in man. Actu Phys- iol. Scund. 134: 181-187, 1988. SJBGAARD, G., B. KIENS, K. JORGENSEN, AND B. SALTIN. Intra- muscular pressure, EMG and blood flow during low-level pro- longed static contraction in man. Actu Physiol. Scund. 128: 475- 484, 1986. SJBGAARD, G., G. SAVARD, AND C. JUEL. Muscle blood flow during isometric activity and its relation to muscle fatigue. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 57: 327-335, 1988. SPIELMANN, J. M. Quantification of Motor-Neuron Adaptation to Sustained and Intermittent Stimulation (PhD thesis). Tucson, AZ: Univ. of Arizona, 1991. SPIELMANN, J. M., Y. LAOURIS, M. A. NORDSTROM, R. M. REINK- ING, AND D. G. STUART. The relation between motor-neuron adap- tation and muscle fatigue. 3rd IBRO World Congr. Neurosci. Mon- treal 1991, p. 470. STEIN, R. B., AND F. PARMIGGIANI. Optimal motor patterns for activating mammalian muscle. Bruin Res. 175: 372-376, 1979. STEPHENS, J. A., R. M. REINKING, AND D. G. STUART. Tendon organs of cat medial gastrocnemius: responses to active and pas-

1648 BRIEF REVIEW

209.

210.

211.

212.

213.

214.

215.

216.

217.

218.

219.

220.

221.

sive forces as a function of muscle length. J. Neurophysiol. 38: 1217-1231, 1975. STEPHENS, J. A., AND A. TAYLOR. Fatigue of maintained volun- tary muscle contraction in man. J. Physiol. Land. 220: 1-18, 1972. STEVENS, J. C., AND W. S. CAIN. Effort in isometric muscular contractions related to force level and duration. Percept. Psy- chophys. 8: 240-244, 1970. STOKES, M. J., R. G. COOPER, AND R. H. T. EDWARDS. Normal muscle strength and fatigability in patients with effort syn- dromes. Br. Med. J. 297: 1014-1017, 1988. ST. PIERRE, D. M. M., D. LEONARD, R. HOULE, AND P. F. GAR- DINER. Recovery of muscle from tetrodotoxin-induced disuse and the influence of daily exercise. Exp. Neurol. 101: 327-346, 1988. STUART, D. G. New openings in the study of muscle fatigue. In: Symposium Proceedings- Perspectives in Motor Control: Brain- storming on the State of Aff airs and Future Developments, edited by H.-D. Henatsch, Y. Laouris, U. Windhorst, and J. Meyer-Loh- man. Gottingen, FRG: AIM Verlag, 1989, p. 89-95. STUART, D. G., AND R. M. ENOKA. Motoneurons, motor units, and the size principle. In: The Clinical Neurosciences, edited by R. N. Rosenberg. New York: Churchill Livingstone, 1983, p. V:471- v:517. TESCH, P. A., G. A. DUDLEY, M. R. DUVOISIN, B. M. HATHER, AND R. T. HARRIS. Force and EMG signal patterns during re- peated bouts of concentric or eccentric muscle actions. Acta Phys- iol. Stand. 138: 263-271, 1990. THOMAS, C. K., B. BIGLAND-RITCHIE, AND R. S. JOHANSSON. Force-frequency relationships of human thenar motor units. J. Neurophysiol. 65: 1509-1516, 1991. THOMAS, C. K., J. J. WOODS, AND B. BIGLAND-RITCHIE. Impulse propagation and muscle activation in long maximal voluntary con- tractions. J. Appl. Physiol. 67: 1835-1842, 1989. ULMER, H.-V., W. KNIERIEMEN, T. WARLO, AND B. ZECH. Interin- dividual variability of isometric endurance with regard to the en- durance performance limit for static work. Biomed. Biochim. Acta 48: S504-S508, 1989. VAN ZUYLEN, E. J., C. C. A. M. GIELEN, AND J. J. DENIER VAN DER CON. Coordination and inhomogeneous activation of human arm muscles during isometric torques. J. Neurophysiol. 60: 1523- 1548, 1988. WALLER, A. D. The sense of effort: an objective study. Brain 14: 179-249, 1891 WALMSLEY, B., AND U. PROSKE. Comparison of the stiffness of the soleus and medial gastrocnemius muscles in cats. J. Neuro- physiol. 46: 250-259, 1981.

222.

223.

224.

225.

226.

227.

228.

229.

230.

231.

232.

233.

WEBB, M. R., M. G. HIBBERD, Y. E. GOLMAN, AND D. R. TRENTHAM. Oxygen exchange between Pi in the medium and water during ATP hydrolysis mediated by skinned fibers from rabbit skeletal muscle. J. Biol. Chem. 261: 15557-15564, 1986. WESTERBLAD, H., AND J. LANNERGREN. Recovery of fatigued Xenopus muscle fibres is markedly affected by the extracellular tonicity. J. Muscle Res. Cell Motil. 11: 147-153, 1990. WESTERBLAD, H., AND J. LANNERGREN. Slowing of relaxation during fatigue in single mouse muscle fibres. J. Physiol. Land. 434: 323-336,199l. WESTING, S. H., A. G. CRESSWELL, AND A. THORSTENSSON. MUS- cle activation during maximal voluntary eccentric and concentric knee extension. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 62: 104-108, 1991. WESTLING, G., R. S. JOHANSSON, C. K. THOMAS, AND B. BIG- LAND-RITCHIE. Measurement of contractile and electrical proper- ties of single human thenar motor units in response to intraneural motor-axon stimulation. J. Neurophysiol. 64: 1331-1338, 1990. WINDHORST, U., C. N. CHRISTAKOS, W. KOEHLER, T. M. HAMM, R. M. ENOKA, AND D. G. STUART. Amplitude reduction of motor unit twitches during repetitive activation is accompanied by rela- tive increase of hyperpolarizing membrane potential trajectories in homonymous a-motoneurons. Brain Res. 398: 181-184, 1986. WINDHORST, U., T. M. HAMM, AND D. G. STUART. On the func- tion of muscle and reflex partitioning. Behav. Brain Sci. 12: 629- 682, 1989. WINDHORST, U., AND T. KOKKOROYIANNIS. Interactions of recur- rent inhibitory and muscle spindle afferent feedback during mus- cle fatigue. Neuroscience 43: 249-259, 1991. WOODS, J. J., F. FURBUSH, AND B. BIGLAND-RITCHIE. Evidence for a fatigue-induced reflex inhibition of motoneuron firing rates. J. Neurophysiol. 58: 125-137, 1987. ZAJAC, F. E., AND J. L. YOUNG. Properties of stimulus trains pro- ducing maximum tension-time area per pulse from single motor units in medial gastrocnemius muscle of the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 43: 1206-1220, 1980. ZIJDEWIND, C., W. BOSCH, L. GOESSENS, T. W. A. KANDOU, AND D. KERNELL. Electromyogram and force during stimulated fa- tigue tests of muscles in dominant and non-dominant hands. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 60: 127-132, 1990. ZYTNICKI, D., J. LAFLEUR, G. HORCHOLLE-BOSSAVIT, F. LAMY, AND L. JAMI. Reduction of Ib autogenetic inhibition in motoneu- rons during contractions of an ankle extensor muscle in the cat. J. Neurophysiol. 64: 1380-1389, 1990.