neural circuitry of upper airway respiratory plasticity

172
Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity: Identifying the Neural Circuitry Underlying Long-Term Facilitation of Inspiratory Genioglossus Motor Output by Simon Kent Chow Lui A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Department of Cell and Systems Biology University of Toronto © Copyright by Simon Kent Chow Lui (2019)

Upload: others

Post on 02-Dec-2021

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity:

Identifying the Neural Circuitry Underlying Long-Term Facilitation of

Inspiratory Genioglossus Motor Output

by

Simon Kent Chow Lui

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Department of Cell and Systems Biology

University of Toronto

© Copyright by Simon Kent Chow Lui (2019)

Page 2: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

ii

Abstract

Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity: Identifying the

Neural Circuitry Underlying Long-Term Facilitation of Inspiratory

Genioglossus Motor Output

Simon Kent Chow Lui

Doctor of Philosophy

Department of Cell and Systems Biology

University of Toronto

2019

The respiratory system is highly adaptive and can change its behaviour to provide protective

responses in face of repeated respiratory challenges. Elucidating the role of how neurons and

neural circuits mediate these responses can open new treatments for those that cannot adapt.

Respiratory long-term facilitation (LTF) is one form of adaptation that can increase genioglossus

motor output, potentially providing a means to mitigate respiratory disorders such as

obstructive sleep apnea. I examined the neural circuitry that underlies this form of respiratory

motor plasticity and I identified: (1) the neural circuit that mediates respiratory LTF of

inspiratory genioglossus motor output, (2) a novel trigger that elicits LTF without directly or

indirectly modulating the respiratory feedback systems, and (3) the neurotransmitter(s) that

are essential to the manifestation of respiratory motor plasticity of genioglossus motor output.

Using tract-tracing, immunohistochemical and pharmacological approaches, I have identified a

Page 3: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

iii

tripartite circuit connecting the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS), the locus coeruleus (LC), and

the hypoglossal motor nuclei (XII) that are individually critical to the elicitation of LTF. I also

employed a series of optogenetic approaches to identify the LC as a trigger that can elicit LTF

following intermittent stimulation of the LC alone. Lastly, I identified noradrenaline, released by

the LC and acting on α1-adrenergic receptors at the hypoglossal motor nuclei, to be the key

neurotransmitter-receptor system that gates the expression of respiratory motor plasticity.

These findings provide direction and novel therapeutic targets to treat respiratory disorders

such as obstructive sleep apnea.

Page 4: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

iv

Acknowledgements

Deciding to pursue a doctoral degree was easy, but completing this task was something that I

never could have been capable of without the help of many wonderful people.

The most important and influential person in this journey is undoubtedly my doctoral

supervisor, Dr. John Peever. I consider myself extremely lucky to have found such a truly

patient, kind, and brilliant mentor to take me under his wing. I have never met anyone who is

so dedicated to guiding and molding his students to become the best scientists they can be.

John’s dedication to his students and his brilliant mentorship has allowed me to become not

only a better scientist, but also a better person. The difference he has made in my life is

immeasurable and with everything in my heart, I want to thank you for everything you have

done for me. Thank you. Thank you. Thank you.

In addition to John, I would like to thank the members of my advisory committee, Dr. Melanie

Woodin, Dr. Richard Horner, and Dr. Leslie Buck. Your help and guidance throughout my PhD

career shaped me into a better scientist and a better person. I would like to thank Dr. David

Lovejoy for participating in my final oral examination, and Dr. Deborah Sloboda, from McMaster

University for serving as my external examiner.

I also want to thank many members of the Peever lab. Dr. Jennifer Lapierre has been my friend,

my manager, my mentor, and most importantly, my lab wife. Jenn has helped me through

sickness and in health, and I owe her more than I can say to have succeeded this far. Dr. Jimmy

Fraigne has been a friend and mentor as well. Jimmy has been the one I turn to on more than

Page 5: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

v

one occasion to troubleshoot the realm of science. He has been the calm voice of reason when I

needed it and is truly a gift to the Peever lab.

I want to thank the staff of the Department of Cell & Systems Biology, especially Ian Buglass,

Tamar Mamourian, and Peggy Salmon for all their help. I want to thank the staff of the

Bioscience Facility, especially Christine McCaul with all her help throughout the years.

My family and friends have been overwhelmingly supportive throughout my 7 year journey.

Without my parent’s support, my brother Leo’s help, and the love my dearest Kevin Dyal, I

could not have completed this journey. Dr. Stephanie Hughes and Justin Cooke have been my

best friends and often times my statisticians and programmers. Their support has been vital to

my success.

Lastly, I would like to thank Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada

(NSERC) for my Ph.D. funding. The work presented in this thesis was supported by grants held

by Dr. John Peever.

Page 6: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

vi

Preface

My journey into the scientific community began with a desire to understand what it means to

have a healthy brain. I’ve been told from a young age to wear a helmet to protect my head, to

eat foods that are nutritious for my brain, and to avoid drugs and alcohol because it will harm

my body and my mind. But what defines a healthy brain? A generic definition of health can be

translated to being fit for survival. Therefore, a healthy brain must facilitate the organism’s

survival, and the best way to survive is to adapt. By this definition, neural adaptation or neural

plasticity defines a healthy brain. A brain must be adaptable, plastic, and capable of responding

to acute and persistent stimuli. If the brain cannot adapt, and the organism will likely perish.

The pursuit of science has allowed means to prolong a healthy brain – introduce plasticity

where there was none or augment plasticity when it is insufficient. However, plasticity cannot

occur if the organism cannot breathe. Breathing is critical to survival, and plasticity of breathing

is therefore of utmost importance. Long-term facilitation is a form of plasticity within the

respiratory system. My encounter with this form of plasticity has led me to desire

understanding how it works. I want to know how it works so I can induce it, augment it, deliver

it to those who cannot adapt, and have as many people breathing for as long as possible.

Page 7: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

vii

Table of Contents

Abstract ......................................................................................................................................................... ii

Acknowledgements ...................................................................................................................................... iv

Preface ......................................................................................................................................................... vi

Table of Contents ........................................................................................................................................ vii

List of Figures and Tables ............................................................................................................................. xi

List of Abbreviations ...................................................................................................................................xiii

Chapter One – Introduction .......................................................................................................................... 1

Overview ................................................................................................................................................... 1

Neural Circuits ........................................................................................................................................... 5

Neural Circuit Underlying Respiratory Control ......................................................................................... 7

Plasticity .................................................................................................................................................. 10

Respiratory Plasticity .............................................................................................................................. 11

Phrenic LTF .......................................................................................................................................... 13

Hypoglossal LTF ................................................................................................................................... 16

How Noradrenaline Induces Plasticity .................................................................................................... 20

Noradrenaline and Hypoglossal LTF........................................................................................................ 23

Neural Circuits Underlying Hypoglossal LTF ........................................................................................... 25

Experimental Objectives ......................................................................................................................... 27

Chapter Two – Materials and Methods ...................................................................................................... 29

Animals .................................................................................................................................................... 29

Drug and Tracer Preparation .................................................................................................................. 29

Surgical Procedures ................................................................................................................................. 30

Stereotaxic Injection (Virus / Tracer) .................................................................................................. 31

Drug Delivery....................................................................................................................................... 32

Optogenetic Manipulations ................................................................................................................ 32

Electrophysiology Recordings ................................................................................................................. 33

Measurement of ET-CO2 and O2 Saturation ............................................................................................ 34

Experimental Protocol ............................................................................................................................ 34

Objective 1 – To determine the brainstem structures activated alongside apnea-induced hLTF ...... 35

Page 8: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

viii

Objective 2 – To determine the anatomical connections between the noradrenergic locus coeruleus

neurons and the hypoglossal motor pool ........................................................................................... 36

Objective 3 – To determine whether the locus coeruleus is a critical component of the neural circuit

mediating apnea-induced hLTF ........................................................................................................... 36

Objective 4 – To determine whether optogenetic manipulation of ChR2-expressing locus coeruleus

neurons alone can elicit hLTF .............................................................................................................. 37

Objective 5 – To determine whether optical silencing of eNpHR-expressing locus coeruleus neurons

prevent apnea-induced hLTF .............................................................................................................. 38

Objective 6 – To determine whether noradrenaline released specifically from the locus coeruleus is

the underlying mechanism that mediates hLTF ................................................................................. 39

Data Analysis ........................................................................................................................................... 40

Histology ................................................................................................................................................. 40

Cell Quantification .................................................................................................................................. 42

Statistical Analysis ................................................................................................................................... 42

Chapter Three – A Tripartite Circuit Mediates Respiratory Motor Plasticity ............................................. 45

Summary ................................................................................................................................................. 45

Introduction ............................................................................................................................................ 45

Locus Coeruleus and its Role in Breathing and Plasticity .................................................................... 47

Projections of Locus Coeruleus Neurons ............................................................................................ 48

Results ..................................................................................................................................................... 48

Repeated Obstructive Apneas Trigger LTF of Inspiratory Genioglossus Muscle Activity ................... 48

Activation of Noradrenergic LC Neurons Correlates with hLTF .......................................................... 50

Activation of Noradrenergic Cells in the LC is Specific to hLTF Responders ....................................... 51

Noradrenergic LC Neurons Have Direct Projections to the Hypoglossal Motor Pool ......................... 55

Bilateral Inactivation of the LC Prevents Apnea-Induced hLTF ........................................................... 57

Discussion................................................................................................................................................ 59

Noradrenergic Cells in the LC are Active During hLTF ........................................................................ 59

Noradrenergic LC Neurons Project to the Hypoglossal Motor Pool ................................................... 61

LC Activation is Independent of Hypoxia or Hypercapnia Associated with Repeated Apneas ........... 61

The LC is a Required Component of the Neural Circuit Underlying Apnea-Induced hLTF .................. 62

Brainstem regions associated with apnea-induced hLTF .................................................................... 63

Methodological Considerations .......................................................................................................... 65

Scientific Importance and Clinical Significance ................................................................................... 67

Page 9: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

ix

Chapter Four – Optical LC Stimulation Triggers for LTF of Inspiratory Genioglossus Motor Output ......... 69

Summary ................................................................................................................................................. 69

Introduction ............................................................................................................................................ 69

Triggers of LTF That Act Through the Chemosensory or Broncho-Pulmonary Feedback System ...... 70

Triggers of LTF Independent of the Chemosensory and Broncho-Pulmonary Feedback Systems ..... 70

The LC and its Potential Role in hLTF .................................................................................................. 71

Results ..................................................................................................................................................... 71

LC Cells Equally Infected by Viral Vectors Across All Groups .............................................................. 71

Baseline Genioglossus Motor Activity is Decreased During Optical Inactivation of eNpHR-Expressing

LC Neurons .......................................................................................................................................... 74

LTF of Genioglossus Motor Activity is Elicited After Intermittent Stimulation of ChR2-Expressing LC

Neurons ............................................................................................................................................... 76

Intermittent Light Exposure on Non-ChR2-Expressing mCherry LC Neurons Does Not Trigger hLTF 80

hLTF Expression Requires an Intermittent Pattern of LC Stimulation ................................................ 81

Intermittent LC stimulation elicits hLTF at the same frequency as repeated apneas ........................ 84

hLTF Requires a Minimum Threshold Activation of LC Cells ............................................................... 86

Inactivation of the LC Abolishes Apnea-Induced hLTF ........................................................................ 89

Discussion................................................................................................................................................ 94

LC Provides an Endogenous Noradrenergic Drive to Hypoglossal Motor Neurons ............................ 95

Baseline Genioglossus Motor Activity is Unaffected During Stimulation of ChR2-Expressing LC

Neurons ............................................................................................................................................... 96

Stimulation of ChR2-Expressing LC Neurons Trigger LTF of Inspiratory Genioglossus Motor Output 97

LC is Critical for hLTF ........................................................................................................................... 98

A Minimum Threshold of LC Stimulation is Required for hLTF Expression ......................................... 98

Plasticity Occurs at the Level of the LC and at the Level of the Hypoglossal Motor Neuron ............. 99

Methodological Considerations ........................................................................................................ 100

Scientific Importance and Clinical Significance ................................................................................. 102

Chapter Five - α1-Adrenergic Receptor Binding at the Hypoglossal Motor Pool Is Required for LC-Induced

hLTF ........................................................................................................................................................... 103

Summary ............................................................................................................................................... 103

Introduction .......................................................................................................................................... 103

Results ................................................................................................................................................... 108

Intermittent Stimulation of LC Axons at the Hypoglossal Motor Pool Did Not Trigger hLTF ........... 108

Page 10: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

x

Noradrenaline Released from the LC is Critical for hLTF .................................................................. 110

Saline Perfusion into the Hypoglossal Motor Pool Does Not Influence hLTF Expression ................. 112

α1-Adrenergic Receptor Blockade Abolishes hLTF Expression ......................................................... 113

Discussion.............................................................................................................................................. 115

Direct Stimulation of LC Axons Projecting to the Hypoglossal Motor Pool Did Not Elicit hLTF ........ 115

The LC Co-Releases Neurotransmitters That Can Induce Plasticity and/or Modulate Respiratory

Output ............................................................................................................................................... 117

The Same Intracellular Machinery is Involved in Mediating hLTF Expression Across Multiple Triggers

.......................................................................................................................................................... 118

Methodological Considerations ........................................................................................................ 119

Scientific Importance and Clinical Significance ................................................................................. 120

Chapter Six – General Discussion .............................................................................................................. 122

Hypoglossal / Genioglossus LTF is Mechanistically Distinct From Phrenic / Diaphragm LTF ................ 123

The Brainstem Circuit Mediating hLTF: NTS → LC → XII ...................................................................... 126

The Site of Plasticity in the hLTF Tripartite Circuit ................................................................................ 128

Plasticity at the NTS .......................................................................................................................... 128

Plasticity at the LC ............................................................................................................................. 129

Plasticity at the Hypoglossal Motor Neuron ..................................................................................... 131

Summary of Proposed Mechanism Underlying hLTF ............................................................................ 132

Methodological Considerations ............................................................................................................ 134

Significance of Findings ......................................................................................................................... 137

Future Directions .................................................................................................................................. 139

References ............................................................................................................................................ 140

Page 11: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

xi

List of Figures and Tables

Figure 1.1. A schematic showing the cyclic sequence of events leading to the development of

obstructive sleep apnea and the events that restore patency in the upper airways ............................... 3

Figure 1.2. Representative trace of integrated phrenic nerve activity before, during and after acute

intermittent hypoxia or no hypoxia in rats, and the subsequent increase in phrenic nerve amplitude

(i.e. pLTF) ................................................................................................................................................... 4

Figure 1.3. A schematic of the respiratory control network ................................................................... 10

Figure 1.4. Working model of acute intermittent hypoxia-induced pLTF............................................... 15

Figure 1.5. Schematic diagram of human tongue and the muscles innervated by the hypoglossal nerve

................................................................................................................................................................ 17

Figure 1.6. LTF of genioglossus motor output induced by intermittent apneas .................................... 20

Figure 1.7. Schematic of the signalling cascade following noradrenaline-binding to α1- or α2-

adrenergic receptors ............................................................................................................................... 23

Figure 1.8. Hypothesized circuit responsible for hypoglossal LTF .......................................................... 27

Figure 2.1. Protocol for the delivery of repeated apneas in Objective 1 ................................................ 35

Figure 2.2. Protocol for clonidine intervention followed by repeated apneas in Objective 3 ............... 37

Figure 2.3. Protocol for intermittent LC stimulation in Objective 4 ....................................................... 38

Figure 2.4. Protocol for continuous LC stimulation in Objective 4 ......................................................... 38

Figure 2.5. Protocol for continuous inhibition of the LC with repeated apneas in Objective 5 ............. 39

Figure 2.6. Protocol for terazosin perfusion with intermittent LC stimulation in Objective 5 ............... 39

Figure 3.1. Repeated obstructive apneas elicit LTF of the genioglossus motor activity ......................... 50

Figure 3.2. LC activation correlates with apnea-induced hLTF ............................................................... 53

Figure 3.3. Levels of anesthesia, expired CO2, O2 saturation and blood pressure do not correlate with

hLTF ......................................................................................................................................................... 54

Figure 3.4. Noradrenergic LC neurons have direct projections to the hypoglossal motor pool ............ 56

Figure 3.5. Inactivation of the LC prevents apnea-induced hLTF............................................................ 58

Figure 3.6. Hypothesized circuitry responsible for hLTF. ........................................................................ 65

Figure 4.1. Increased or decreased c-Fos expression following light-induced manipulation to ChR2- or

eNpHR-expressing LC cells ...................................................................................................................... 73

Figure 4.2. Optical inactivation of LC cells decreased genioglossus motor output ................................ 76

Figure 4.3. Intermittent optical stimulation of the LC elicits LTF of genioglossus motor activity .......... 79

Page 12: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

xii

Figure 4.4. Intermittent LC stimulation activates ChR2-expressing LC neurons more than mCherry-

expressing LC cells or continuous LC stimulation ................................................................................... 83

Figure 4.5. Probability of LTF expression was increased following intermittent stimulation of the LC . 85

Figure 4.6. LTF requires a minimum LC stimulation threshold ............................................................... 88

Figure 4.7. Optical inactivation of the LC prevents apnea-induced LTF ................................................. 91

Figure 4.8. LTF did not manifest following LC inactivation ..................................................................... 94

Figure 5.1. Protocol for intermittent LC stimulation with and without terazosin perfusion ................ 107

Figure 5.2. Intermittent stimulation of LC axons did not trigger LTF .................................................... 109

Figure 5.3. LTF is mediated by α1-adrenergic receptor binding of noradrenaline released from the LC

.............................................................................................................................................................. 111

Figure 5.4. Saline perfusion at hypoglossal motor pool does not affect LTF ........................................ 113

Figure 5.5. Probability of LTF was reduced following α1-adrenergic receptor blockade at the

hypoglossal motor pool ........................................................................................................................ 114

Figure 6.1. Hypothesized neural circuit underlying hLTF ...................................................................... 132

Table 3.1. LC is the only noradrenergic cell group implicated in the LTF circuit .................................... 50

Table 6.1. Comparison of mechanisms underlying pLTF vs GG LTF ...................................................... 125

Page 13: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

xiii

List of Abbreviations

°C Degree Celcius

< Less than

> Greater than

± Plus or minus

µL Microliter

µM Micromolar

µm Micrometer (micron)

5-HT 5-hydroxytryptamine (i.e., serotonin)

5-HT2A 5-hydroxytryptamine receptor subtype 2A

AIH Acute intermittent hypoxia

AMPA α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid

ANOVA Analysis of variance

AP Anterior posterior

ATP Adenosine triphosphate

Aug-E Augmenting expiratory

BDNF Brain-derived neurotrophic factor

BötC Bötzinger complex

ChR2 Channelrhodopsin2

CIH Chronic intermittent hypoxia

CLO Clonidine

cm Centimeter

CO2 Carbon dioxide

CSN Carotid sinus nerve

Page 14: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

xiv

CtB Cholera toxin subunit B

cVRG Caudal ventral respiratory group

DAG Diacylglycerol

Dia Diaphragm

DRG Dorsal respiratory group

Early-I Early-inspiratory

EMG Electromyogram

eNpHR3.0 Halorhodopsin

ERK Extracellular signal-regulated kinases

ET-CO2 End-tidal carbon dioxide

fB Breath frequency

FiCO2 Fraction of inspired carbon dioxide

FiO2 Fraction of inspired oxygen

FW Formula weight

GABA Gamma-aminobutyric acid

GG Genioglossus

Gq/Gi G protein-mediated activation / inhibition

hLTF Hypoglossal / genioglossus long-term facilitation

kHz Kilohertz

Hz Hertz

IP3 Inositol triphosphate

IPSC Inhibitory post-synaptic current

Late-E Late-expiratory

LC Locus coeruleus

Page 15: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

xv

LTF Long-term facilitation

LTP Long-term potentiation

MAPK Mitogen-activated protein kinases

Min Minute

mg Milligram

mm Millimeter

mw Milliwatts

N2 Nitrogen

NA Noradrenaline

NMDA N-methyl-D-aspartate

NTS Nucleus tractus solitarius

O2 Oxygen

OLS Ordinary least square

OSA Obstructive sleep apnea

PI3K Phosphoinositol 3-kinase

PKC Protein kinase C

PLC Phospholipase C

pLTF Phrenic / diaphragm long-term facilitation

Post-I Post-inspiratory

preBötC PreBötzinger complex

Pre-I/I Pre-inspiratory/inspiratory

PRG Pontine respiratory group

PSR Pulmonary stretch receptors

Ramp-I Ramping-inspiratory

Page 16: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

xvi

REM Rapid Eye Movement

RM Repeated measures

RNA Ribonucleic acid

RTN Retrotrapezoid nucleus

RRG Respiratory rhythm generator

rVRG Rostral ventral respiratory group

SEM Standard error of the mean

SubC Subcoeruleus

TrkB Tropomyosin receptor kinase B

vLTF Ventilatory long-term facilitation

VRG Ventral respiratory group

XII Hypoglossal

Page 17: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

1

Chapter One – Introduction

1.1 Overview

Respiration is a natural function that is critical for survival. It involves the intake of oxygen and

expulsion of carbon dioxide and requires the activation of respiratory pump muscles such as the

diaphragm and secondary muscles that include upper airway dilator muscles such as the

genioglossus. The activity of these muscles is controlled by the respiratory network situated

within the brainstem (Fitzgerald, 1995, Smith et al., 2007). Under normal circumstances, there

are peripheral and central chemosensors that detect levels of dissolved gases in the blood and

adjusts respiratory output to maintain homeostatic balance within the body (Duffin, 2005, Kline

and Mendelowitz, 2012, Moreira et al., 2011, Yokhana et al., 2012). For example, peripheral

chemosensors such as the carotid bodies sense O2, CO2/H+, and glucose in arterial blood and

signal the respiratory network to adjust respiratory output accordingly (Mohan and Duffin, 1997,

Nakayama et al., 2003, Peng et al., 2010). Central chemosensors, such as the retrotrapezoid nucleus

(RTN) act by detecting pH/H+ ions in cerebral spinal fluid (Basting et al., 2015, Takakura et al., 2006)

and signal the respiratory network to adjust respiratory output by changing tidal volume and/or

respiratory frequency (Abbott et al., 2013, Abbott et al., 2009, Holloway et al., 2015). However,

during sleep, there is a reduction in muscle tone and a reduced response from the genioglossus

to hypercapnia (Fung and Chase, 2015, Horner et al., 2002). Under abnormal conditions, as

observed in patients with obstructive sleep apnea (OSA), the reduction of upper airway muscle

tone can increase the collapsibility of the upper airways leading to an apnea which causes

hypoxia and hypercapnia. Due to the physical obstruction, hypoxia and hypercapnia will build,

thus homeostatic balance cannot be reached. The reflexive increase in ventilatory effort to

correct the hypoxia and hypercapnia can wake the patient, ending the apnea, only to repeat the

cycle when the patient resumes sleep (Fig. 1.1) (Syed et al., 2013). This is OSA and these

repetitive airway occlusions throughout the night can cause adverse effects such as sleep loss,

day time fatigue, and increase the risk of other adverse health effects such as hypertension,

stroke, or congestive heart failure (Hung et al., 1990, Nieto et al., 2000, Shahar et al., 2001).

Currently, the prevalence of OSA in North America is at 2% in middle-aged women and 4% in

Page 18: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

2

middle-aged men (Young et al., 1993, Young et al., 2002). However, the prevalence of OSA

increases further East, with reports showing 4-6% in patients of East-Asian descent (Ip et al.,

2001, Ip et al., 2004), despite having an average lower body-mass index compared to

Caucasians; this difference can be attributed to craniofacial physiology and size of the upper

airways that may influence collapsibility (Lam et al., 2005, Pham et al., 2018). Optimistically,

studies have showed that humans exposed to intermittent episodes of hypoxia can trigger

respiratory motor plasticity (Aboubakr et al., 2001, Harris et al., 2006), which may mitigate the

reduction of upper airway muscle tone during sleep. For example, repeated bouts of hypoxia

can lead to a decrease in upper airway resistance (Aboubakr et al., 2001, Chowdhuri et al.,

2008), which suggests that the respiratory network can use previous experiences or stimuli (i.e.

intermittent hypoxia) to modulate factors that control respiratory output. This type of motor

plasticity is known as long-term facilitation (LTF) (Fig. 1.2) and may serve to improve effective

lung ventilation (Harris et al., 2006).

Page 19: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

3

Figure. 1.1. A schematic showing the cyclic sequence of events leading to the development of obstructive sleep apnea and the events that restore patency in the upper airways. Sleep onset is accompanied by a reduction in muscle tone, pharyngeal dilator muscle reflex, as well as increase the hypercapnia recruitment threshold for genioglossus (GG) muscle activity. This can lead to an increase in the propensity for upper airway collapse, effectively producing an obstructive apnea. The resultant increase in hypoxia and hypercapnia reflexively triggers an increase in ventilatory effort and eventual arousal from sleep to restore upper airway patency to correct hypoxia and hypercapnia. This cycle repeats, leading to the disorder known as obstructive sleep apnea (OSA). Long-term facilitation (LTF) may mitigate the reduction in upper airway muscle tone (Adapted from Mateika and Syed, 2013).

My work addresses the neural circuits and mechanisms underlying a form of plasticity that

augments respiratory motor output. Over the last three decades, it has been established that

repeated modulation of chemosensory feedback (e.g. intermittent episodes of hypoxia) can

trigger respiratory motor plasticity (Devinney et al., 2015, Dodig et al., 2012), and the

neurochemical mechanism that facilitates this process is hypothesized to be serotonin released

from the medullary raphe to act on the respiratory control network (Bocchiaro and Feldman,

Page 20: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

4

2004, Dodig et al., 2012, McGuire et al., 2004). My work addresses a novel form of respiratory

motor plasticity that is independent of hypoxia but is instead triggered by repeated apneas or

repeated stimulation of the locus coeruleus (LC). This form of respiratory motor plasticity is

mediated by the noradrenergic system and I will provide evidence demonstrating noradrenaline

released from the LC to be the critical component mediating LTF of inspiratory genioglossus

motor output. I will also introduce a novel neural circuit that I hypothesize outlines the critical

brainstem structures required for respiratory motor plasticity to occur. The goal of this thesis is

to determine the mechanisms and circuits that mediate LTF to provide a better understanding

of LTF. Since this is a form of neural plasticity that naturally exists in humans and other animals

(Cao et al., 1992, Chowdhuri et al., 2008, Tadjalli et al., 2010, Terada et al., 2008, Turner and

Mitchell, 1997), elucidating the neural circuits and mechanisms that mediate LTF may help

provide new therapeutic targets to treat OSA and mitigate symptoms in some respiratory

disorders.

Figure 1.2 Representative trace of integrated phrenic nerve activity before, during and after acute intermittent hypoxia or no hypoxia in rats, and the subsequent increase in phrenic nerve amplitude (i.e. pLTF). Dotted line indicates baseline amplitude. Gray represents increases from baseline. (Adapted from Devinney et al., 2015).

To this end, I will first review the known neural networks underlying respiratory control

followed by an overview of plasticity within the respiratory control network. Next, I will focus

on studies that demonstrate that noradrenaline plays a significant role in LTF, then outline the

gaps in knowledge that this thesis focused on. I will then identify the main objectives of this

Page 21: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

5

thesis, and in the subsequent chapters, I describe the experiments I performed to elucidate the

role of noradrenergic neurons in the LC in respiratory motor plasticity.

1.2 Neural circuits

A fundamental concept of neuroscience is that the ability of the brain to produce complex

behaviours, such as motor control, arises from a network of interconnected neurons (Getting,

1989). A network can be simplified into an anatomical and functional organization. An

anatomical organization refers to the afferent and efferent projections, and the synaptic

connectivity between neurons within the network (Getting, 1989). This can be expanded to

include the number of connections between interconnected neurons and the density of

dendritic spines at each connection (Lendvai et al., 2000, Segal, 2005). The anatomical

organization defines the structural limits of the network and identifies which neurons are

communicating with each other. It does not provide information regarding the function of

these neurons and the behaviour that it generates. The functional organization refers to how

the network processes information and generates an output pattern to produce a behaviour.

The ability for a network to produce a behaviour depends on the state of the network at the

time it is activated. Specifically, it depends on factors at the network level (i.e. which neurons

are being activated), at the synapse (i.e. amount of neurotransmitter release and receptors

expressed), and at the cellular level (i.e. the intrinsic excitability of the post-synaptic cell and its

firing properties). These factors can be modulated and the anatomical network can be

reconfigured to produce various behaviours. For example, in rats with T5 spinal cord

transection, elevated levels of nerve growth factor (NGF), a neurotrophin that supports survival

and differentiation of neurons, was observed (Lujan et al., 2010). This correlated with an

increase in innervation and arborization of sympathetic preganglionic neurons, suggesting

neurons can undergo plasticity and initiate the process for making new connections. The most

intuitive modulator is activity (i.e. activity-dependent plasticity) (Cline, 1993, Hawkins et al.,

1993).

Page 22: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

6

Traditionally, activity-dependent changes to a neural circuit were first described in Hebbian

synaptic plasticity, where activity between pre- and post- synaptic neurons led to an increase in

synaptic strength between the connected neurons (Hebb, 1949). This later led to the famous

quote “cells that fire together wire together” (Shatz, 1992), which suggests that activity guides

the formation of a neural circuit. Although activity is not the sole contributor to the formation

of neuronal connections (Goodman and Shatz, 1993), activity is nonetheless a potent stimulus

that modulates the strength of neuronal connections. Through repeated use or activation, the

connection is strengthened and the circuit is reinforced. However, these neural circuits are

subject to modulation (e.g. through repeated use or disuse), and modulation of a neural circuit

is what allows for adaptation to match ongoing needs. In other words, activity reinforces the

anatomical organization of a neural circuit, and the functional organization can undergo

plasticity to match it.

The mechanism that mediates anatomical reorganization and functional plasticity is

hypothesized to be attributed (but not limited) to the neurotrophin brain-derived neurotrophic

factor (BDNF), and its receptor tropomyosin kinase receptor B (TrkB), also known as tyrosine

kinase B (Schaser et al., 2012, Schjetnan and Escobar, 2012, Wilkerson and Mitchell, 2009). The

production and release of BDNF is regulated by activity (Isackson et al., 1991, Wetmore et al.,

1994, Zafra et al., 1991); however BDNF itself can also regulate activity (Rutherford et al., 1998).

For example, a decrease in TrkB receptor activation reduced pyramidal neuron firing rates,

while an increase in BDNF levels increase the activity of interneurons (Rutherford et al., 1998).

This suggests that manipulations to BDNF levels changes both the activity of cortical neural

circuits and how they interact. Another example where a neural circuit can be modulated by

BDNF is the circuit that mediates respiratory motor plasticity (Baker-Herman et al., 2004,

Wilkerson and Mitchell, 2009). Respiratory motor output is constantly changing to match its

environment. For example, respiratory output changes during phonation, exercise, or eating

(Dobbins and Feldman, 1995, Fregosi and Fuller, 1997, Tangel et al., 1995). Characterizing the

neural circuitry underlying respiratory control can help us understand how this circuit can

Page 23: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

7

exhibit plasticity. In this thesis, I will discuss the neural circuit that underlies a respiratory motor

behaviour and the circuit that underlies upper airway respiratory motor plasticity.

1.3 Neural circuit underlying respiratory control

To understand the neural circuit underlying respiratory motor plasticity, it is necessary to

understand the circuit that controls respiratory output. This is important because the metric for

respiratory plasticity varies across studies. For example, respiratory plasticity can be measured

as an increase in respiratory nerve activity (e.g. hypoglossal or phrenic nerve activity) (Blitz and

Ramirez, 2002, Bocchiaro and Feldman, 2004, Neverova et al., 2007, Schwartz et al., 2012), an

increase in respiratory muscle activity (Cao and Ling, 2010, Ryan and Nolan, 2009, Tadjalli et al.,

2010), a decrease in the resistance of airflow in upper airways (Chowdhuri et al., 2008, Wirth et

al., 2013), or a change in tidal volume or breath frequency (Edge and O'Halloran, 2015, Gerst et

al., 2011, Griffin et al., 2012). Respiratory plasticity could, therefore, be occurring at the level of

the motor neuron or within the respiratory network that control breathing. To understand how

respiratory plasticity could interact with the respiratory network, it is necessary to define the

structures that control breathing.

Respiratory output involves the coordinated contraction of thoracic respiratory muscles (e.g.

diaphragm) and muscles of the upper airways (e.g. the genioglossus). The contraction of these

muscles is controlled by the structures situated within the brainstem (Fitzgerald, 1995, Molkov

et al., 2017, Smith et al., 2007). Historically, the region of the brainstem that control breathing

was found when sections were “extracted” from rabbits until breathing stopped (Legallois,

1813). Over the next century and a half, it was found that respiratory premotor neurons within

the medulla are predominantly found bilaterally in two distinct columns of cells termed the

dorsal respiratory group (DRG) and the ventral respiratory group (VRG) (Duffin, 2004, Feldman

et al., 1985). Respiratory premotor neurons have also been identified in the pons, termed the

pontine respiratory group (PRG) (Duffin, 2004). These respiratory groups coordinate with each

other to generate a continuous breathing rhythm (Fig. 1.3) (Duffin, 2004, Smith et al., 2007).

Page 24: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

8

The VRG can be divided into the rostral and caudal regions within the medulla. The caudal VRG

primarily contains expiratory premotor neurons that provide the drive onto respiratory muscles

to contract during expiration (Feldman et al., 1985, Shen and Duffin, 2002). The rostral VRG

(rVRG) can be further subdivided into the rVRG, the Bötzinger complex (BötC) and pre-

Bötzinger complex (preBötC). Neurons in the rVRG are premotor inspiratory neurons with

augmenting activity pattern (ramp-inspiratory neuron) that shape phrenic motor output (Smith

et al., 2007). BötC neurons are primarily inhibitory and project to the VRG and spinal motor

neurons where they are hypothesized to play a critical role in forming the phases of breathing

by acting to initiate expiration by inhibiting premotor and motor neurons (Jiang and Lipski,

1990, Merrill and Fedorko, 1984). The neurons in the preBötC are considered to be the critical

structure for respiratory rhythm as they are both necessary and sufficient for its generation

(Bacak et al., 2016, Gray et al., 1999, Guyenet and Wang, 2001, Johnson et al., 2001, Koshiya

and Smith, 1999, Tan et al., 2008). When preBötC neurons are isolated in vitro, preBötC

neurons continue to fire in a coordinated pattern demonstrating pacemaker-like properties (Del

Negro et al., 2002). Studies in vivo have shown ablation of preBötC neurons produces ataxic

breathing (Tan et al., 2008). This led to the theory that the preBötC is the source of rhythm

generation with neurons showing pre-inspiratory and early-inspiration patterns of activity

(Guyenet and Wang, 2001). However, because animals with the preBötC ablated could still

breathe, in addition to studies showing retrotrapezoid nucleus (RTN) neurons activated prior to

preBötC neurons in an inspiratory pattern (Mellen et al., 2003, Onimaru and Homma, 2003),

other sources may contribute or control the generation of breathing rhythm. The current

hypothesis is that there exist two respiratory rhythm generators; the preBötC generates

inspiratory rhythm and the RTN generates active expiratory rhythm (Janczewski and Feldman,

2006).

The DRG is situated dorsal to the VRG and contains neurons whose activity increases

progressively during inspiration (de Castro et al., 1994). The DRG is involved in sensory afferent

processing and contains the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) (de Castro et al., 1994, Ezure and

Page 25: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

9

Tanaka, 2000), which is responsible for integration and transmission of signals received from

arterial baroreceptors (i.e. receptors that sense pressure changes in the arterial wall)

(Andresen, 1994), pulmonary stretch receptors (i.e. receptors that detect the physical

distension of the lungs) (Bonham and McCrimmon, 1990), and chemoreceptors (i.e. receptors

that detect changes in the blood such as pH and dissolved CO2 and O2) (Mifflin, 1992). The cells

in this region are critical to the respiratory reflex to terminate inspiration upon sufficient lung

inflation, known as the Hering-Breuer reflex, as inactivation or silencing of cells in this region

abolishes the reflex (Torontali, 2012, Widdicombe, 2001). The NTS is also the first structure to

receive chemoreceptor signals (Mifflin, 1992), acting as a gateway to integrate chemosensory

information (Andresen, 1994, Dampney, 1994).

The PRG is situated in the lateral pons and contains the parabrachial and Kolliker-Fuse nuclei.

The PRG controls the transition from inspiration to expiration and expiration to inspiration as

stimulation of this region switched the phase from inspiration to expiration (Cohen, 1971,

Okazaki et al., 2002). The PRG may also be involved in prolonging inspiration as lesioning these

two nuclei or blockade of neuronal activity extended inspiratory duration (Berger et al., 1978,

Caille et al., 1981). This region may, therefore, act to control inspiratory activity, likely through

its connections with medullary respiratory groups (Duffin, 2004).

The respiratory network coordinates respiratory activity to generate a continuous breathing

rhythm. The network provides continuous adjustments to respiratory output to compensate for

changes or perturbations in the environment that alter breathing rhythm, such as changes in

arousal state, health status associated with disease, posture and phonation (Feldman et al.,

2003). The respiratory network reflexively responds to these perturbations but the respiratory

network can also adapt and exhibit plasticity.

Page 26: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

10

Figure 1.3. A schematic of the respiratory control network. Pulmonary stretch receptors (PSRs) provide mechanical feedback to pump cells in the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS). Excitatory pump cells P(e) activate post-inspiratory (post-I) neurons in the Bötzinger complex (BötC) to inhibit pre-inspiratory/inspiratory neurons (pre-I/I) in the pre-Bötzinger complex (pre-BötC). The inihibition of these neurons decrease ramp-inspiratory neuron (ramp-I) activity in the rostral ventral respiratory group (rVRG) and disfacilitate phrenic nerve (PN) activity and diaphragm muscle contraction. PSRs also provide feedback to inhibitory pump cells P(i) to inhibit early-inspiratory neurons (early-I) in the pre-BötC, which in turn inhibit ramp-I neurons in the rVRG, disfacilitating PN and diaphragm activity. The retrotrapezoid nucleus (RTN) receives chemical feedback to provide a tonic drive onto late expiratory (late-E) neurons within the RTN, which act to activate expiratory neurons in the caudal VRG (cVRG) to activate the abdominal nerve (AbN) and abdominal muscles. The RTN also provides tonic drive onto augmenting-expiratory neurons (aug-E) in the BötC, which inhibit early-I neurons in the preBötC to inhibit inspiration via ramp-I neurons. (Adapted from Molkov et al. 2017).

1.4 Plasticity

Before discussing respiratory plasticity, it is necessary to first establish neuroplasticity in

general. The term “plasticity” originated in 1906 by Italian psychiatrist, Ernesto Lugaro, which

was translated from Italian into English in 1909 (Berlucchi, 2002). In both Italian and English, the

index states “psychic plasticity; plasticity of the neurons; plasticity of the neurofibrils”, and in

later text discusses compensation following brain lesions. Lugaro proposed that prenatal

Page 27: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

11

organization of the nervous system can continue throughout life in order to adapt anatomical-

functional connections between neurons (Berlucchi, 2002). The concept that learning and

memory involved changes in the connections between neurons was proposed by Cajal in 1911

(Cajal, 1911), but it was not until 1949 when Donald Hebb refined the concept into a model to

illustrate that synaptic strength could be augmented through repeated use. He postulated that

synaptic modifications occur as a consequence of coincidence between pre- and post- synaptic

activity (Hebb, 1949). It was in 1966-1973 that LTP was discovered and created the basis or

model that underlie learning and memory and launched the field of LTP (Bliss and Lomo, 1973,

Lomo, 1966). Over time, the term “plasticity” has broadened to include various short and long-

term changes, as well as changes occurring in the cell, the synapse, or at the molecular level.

For example, GAP43 is a protein associated with plasticity as it has been linked to the formation

of new synapses (Benowitz and Routtenberg, 1997, Collingridge et al., 1983, Strittmatter et al.,

1992). At the level of the cell, parvalbumin basket cells have been suggested to regulate

plasticity (Karunakaran et al., 2016, Mendez and Bacci, 2011).

Other types of plasticity are also included, such as long-term depression (LTD) induced by

prolonged periods of low-frequency stimulation, opposed to short, high-frequency bursts

required for LTP (Dunwiddie and Lynch, 1978, Kemp et al., 2000, Lee et al., 1998). In this thesis,

I will discuss a type of plasticity within the respiratory control network known as long-term

facilitation (LTF), where repeated respiratory stimuli (e.g. intermittent hypoxia) can induce a

prolonged increase in respiratory nerve or muscle output, such as the phrenic nerve and

diaphragm muscle, respectively.

1.5 Respiratory plasticity

The ability to undergo plasticity and adjust respiratory output is a fundamental characteristic of

the respiratory system. Plasticity within the respiratory system was first noticed when Millhorn

and colleagues (Millhorn et al., 1980a), where they showed that intermittent electrical

stimulation of the carotid sinus nerve (CSN) triggered a prolonged (50-90 minutes) increase in

Page 28: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

12

phrenic nerve activity in an anaesthetized cat preparation. At the time, this prolonged increase

in phrenic nerve activity was termed “afterdischarge”, presumably referring to the persistent

discharge in phrenic nerve activity after the stimulus. This was later termed “long-lasting

facilitation” in their subsequent findings (Millhorn et al., 1980b).

Since then, long-lasting facilitation, now referred to as long-term facilitation (LTF), has been

identified in multiple mammals including cats, dogs, goats, rats, mice, and humans (Cao et al.,

1992, Chowdhuri et al., 2008, Griffin et al., 2012, Harris et al., 2006, Hickner et al., 2014,

Millhorn et al., 1980b, Song and Poon, 2017, Tadjalli et al., 2010, Terada et al., 2008, Turner and

Mitchell, 1997), suggesting this form of plasticity is conserved across mammals. It is important

to note that the trigger for respiratory plasticity varied in each animal model. To date, multiple

triggers to induce respiratory motor plasticity have been discovered. The most commonly used

trigger to elicit LTF mimics the original CSN stimulation by delivering 3 episodes of hypoxia (10%

O2), each lasting 5-minutes in duration separated by 5-minutes of normoxic breathing (Bach

and Mitchell, 1996, Fuller et al., 2000, Kinkead et al., 2001). Other triggers for LTF include

variations in the number and/or duration of hypoxic episodes (Cao et al., 1992, Turner and

Mitchell, 1997), repeated loss or suppression of vagus activity (Tadjalli et al., 2010, Zhang et al.,

2003), repeated application of serotonin (Bocchiaro and Feldman, 2004) or noradrenaline

(Neverova et al., 2007) in vitro, or episodic loss of respiratory activity such as that experienced

in neural apneas (Baertsch and Baker-Herman, 2013, Mahamed et al., 2011). The consistent

theme in all triggers is the requirement for an intermittent stimulus (Baker et al., 2001, Baker

and Mitchell, 2000), similar to triggers in forms of plasticity observed in other systems (i.e. LTP)

(Bliss and Lomo, 1973, Huang and Kandel, 1997)

Changes or variations to the intervention can alter aspects of how respiratory motor plasticity

will manifest. For example, the time course of the plasticity (i.e. short-term lasting seconds vs

long-term persisting for hours) can differ depending on the stimulus. Continuous CSN

stimulation triggers short-term facilitation (>3 minutes) post-stimulation (Wagner and Eldridge,

1990) whereas repeated CSN stimulation elicits LTF that persists for more than 60 minutes

Page 29: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

13

(Baker and Mitchell, 2000, Olson et al., 2001). Variations to the trigger can also affect the

direction of respiratory motor plasticity (i.e. facilitation or depression). For example, instead of

using repeated bouts of hypoxia, it was demonstrated that 3 episodes of hyperoxic hypercapnia

(i.e., 50% O2, 10% CO2), each 5 minutes in duration separated by 5 minutes of hyperoxic

normocapnia, elicited long-term depression of phrenic nerve activity (Bach and Mitchell, 1998).

The differing results suggest that the respiratory system naturally adapts under various

environmental conditions and exhibits distinct forms of plasticity in response. This thesis will

focus only on one form of respiratory motor plasticity: long-term (>60 minutes) facilitation of

inspiratory genioglossus motor output.

1.5.1 Phrenic LTF

To understand the neural circuit and mechanisms that underlie LTF, it is necessary to first

understand what is known about LTF. To date, the majority of LTF studies focus on phrenic

nerve activity and/or the diaphragm muscle that it innervates. LTF of the phrenic nerve (pLTF)

or diaphragm muscle increases the strength of diaphragm muscle contractions, increasing

airflow into the lungs and therefore ventilation. The mechanisms that underlie pLTF is a

serotonin-dependent mechanism (Fuller et al., 2001, Millhorn et al., 1980b). Systemic delivery

of the serotonergic antagonist, ketaserin, prevents CSN-stimulated induced facilitation of

phrenic nerve activity, but was unaffected by dopaminergic or noradrenergic antagonists,

suggesting serotonin plays a critical role in mediating LTF. The importance of serotonin was

further supported when pLTF was elicited following repeated bouts of hypoxia (3 episodes of

hypoxia at 11% FIO2, each 5 minutes in duration separated by 5 minutes of hyperoxia at 50%

FIO2), and subsequently abolished by systemic pre-treatment with 5-HT2A antagonist (Fuller et

al., 2001) or 5-HT2B antagonist, methysergide (Bach and Mitchell, 1996). This suggests that

intermittent 5-HT2 receptor activation is critical for hypoxia-induced pLTF.

The circuits that cause serotonin release to initiate pLTF have been well elucidated. LTF was

first demonstrated in an anaesthetized cat preparation that stimulation of the raphe obscurus

Page 30: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

14

can trigger pLTF (Millhorn, 1986). Specifically, continuous stimulation of the raphe obscurus

triggered pLTF manifesting an increase in tidal volume and breath frequency. This approach

used a continuous stimulus which differed from the intermittent triggers used to induce LTP or

LTF. However, the role of the raphe obscurus was supported when it was demonstrated that

hypoxia activates chemosensory brainstem nuclei that include the ventral medulla which

encompass the medullary raphe (Teppema et al., 1997). In addition, the medullary raphe also

has direct projections to the hypoglossal and phrenic motor nuclei (Dobbins and Feldman,

1994), making the medullary raphe anatomically and functionally positioned to mediate

hypoxia-induced LTF. Most importantly, it was demonstrated that pLTF expression correlated

with an increase in raphe obscurus neuron firing (Morris et al., 2001), again reinforcing the

hypothesis that the raphe to be the source of serotonin mediating pLTF. An increase in activity

was also observed in the inspiratory-augmenting neurons of the rVRG (Morris et al., 2001),

suggesting plasticity within the respiratory network also contribute to pLTF.

In addition to this, pLTF requires new protein synthesis following an intermittent hypoxia

intervention (Baker-Herman et al., 2004, Baker-Herman and Mitchell, 2002, Satriotomo et al.,

2012). Earlier, I mentioned that the production and release of BDNF is regulated by activity

(Isackson et al., 1991, Wetmore et al., 1994, Zafra et al., 1991); BDNF also plays an important

role in mediating plasticity and in this case, pLTF. For example, BDNF activates TrkB receptors

which initiate a signaling cascade through the mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) and

phosphoinositide 3-kinase (PI3K) pathways (Gottschalk et al., 1999). BDNF-induced activation of

MAPK can lead to phosphorylation of synapsin I, which primes synaptic vesicles for release on

the pre and/or post synaptic cell (Valente et al., 2012), allowing for immediate (<15 minutes)

changes at the synapse. Alternatively, BDNF modulates plasticity via BDNF-induced protein

synthesis as LTP was blocked when protein synthesis was inhibited (Scharf et al., 2002).

In the context of LTF, increased levels of BDNF synthesis was observed in the region containing

the phrenic motor pool following intermittent hypoxia and in proportion to the magnitude of

pLTF (Baker-Herman et al., 2004). The importance of BDNF was further supported when

Page 31: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

15

blockade of BDNF with interfering RNA or blockade of its receptor (TrkB) prevented hypoxia-

induced pLTF (Baker-Herman et al., 2004). The involvement of BDNF led to further

investigations into the intracellular cascades following TrkB receptor binding, where it can

either initiate a downstream signalling to AKT (i.e. protein kinase B) or extracellular signal-

regulated kinases (ERK), which is also referred to as MAPK. It was determined that pLTF could

be prevented following ERK inhibition but not AKT inhibition (Hoffman et al., 2012), suggesting

that pLTF requires BDNF-TrkB receptor binding to initiate the ERK signalling pathway. How ERK

activation subsequently mediates pLTF is not known, but may involve MAPK-mediated

phosphorylation of synapse I mentioned earlier (see Figure 1.4).

Figure 1.4. Working model of acute intermittent hypoxia-induced pLTF. Acute intermittent hypoxia (AIH) induces release of serotonin from the medullary raphe to activate 5HT2 receptors on phrenic motor neurons, increasing protein kinase C-θ (PKCθ) activity and initiating new BDNF synthesis. TrkB activation by BDNF is necessary for pLTF, and that the relevant TrkB (red) is localized within phrenic motor neurons. Subsequent ERK/MAP kinase activation is hypothesized to facilitate descending respiratory drive through unknown mechanisms that enhance glutamate-mediated excitation. (Adapted from Dale et al. 2017).

Page 32: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

16

1.5.2 Hypoglossal LTF

LTF of respiratory motor output occurs in muscles other than the diaphragm. The other most

studied form of LTF is hypoglossal LTF (hLTF), which innervates the genioglossus, hyoglossus

and styloglossus muscles. The genioglossus is of particular importance as it is the largest muscle

in the upper airway that acts to maintain upper airway patency (Sauerland and Mitchell, 1970).

This is particularly important during sleep where there is a reduction in upper airway muscle

tone, which can result in the narrowing of the upper airways and increase the propensity for

upper airway collapse and obstruct the airways (Horner, 1996). This obstruction causes an

apnea which results in a decrease in blood oxygen saturation (hypoxia) and carbon dioxide

buildup (hypercapnia), leading to a reflexive response to increase ventilatory effort (Blanco et

al., 1984, Duffin, 1990, Hirakawa et al., 1997). This eventually leads to arousal to restore muscle

tone and upper airway patency to correct the hypoxia and hypercapnia, only to repeat the cycle

when the patient resumes sleep (Fig. 1.1), as seen in patients with OSA. LTF of the hypoglossal

or genioglossus muscle activity may, therefore, be important in mitigating the reduction in

upper airway muscle tone during sleep. For example, during non-rapid eye movement sleep in

humans, repeated episodes of hypoxia triggered an increase in ventilatory output (vLTF)

(Shkoukani et al., 2002), a decrease in upper airway resistance (Aboubakr et al., 2001,

Chowdhuri et al., 2008, Shkoukani et al., 2002) or an increase in genioglossus muscle activity

(Chowdhuri et al., 2008). In awake humans, intermittent hypoxia triggered LTF of genioglossus

muscle activity and vLTF (Harris et al., 2006). Together, it suggests that hLTF increases the

strength of genioglossus muscle contractions, which can reduce upper airway resistance and

facilitate airflow into the lungs during inspiration. As such, understanding the neural circuits

that underlie plasticity of hypoglossal motor neuron activity could provide an increase in

genioglossus muscle tone that may mitigate the reduction in airway muscle tone and aid

patients with OSA.

Page 33: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

17

Figure 1.5. Schematic diagram of human tongue and the muscles innervated by the hypoglossal nerve. The hypoglossal nerve innervates the medial genioglossus (M. Geniogloggus), the largest muscle of the tongue involved in inspiration. The hypoglossal nerve also innervates the medial styloglossus (M. Styloglossus) and medial hyoglossus (M. Hyoglossus), which are involved with swallowing. The vectors represented by solid lines show the direction of the tongue movement produced by contraction of the respective muscle. The dashed vectors are estimates based on anatomical attachment of the tongue muscle fibres (Adapted from Fregosi and Fuller 1997).

The circuit and mechanisms underlying hLTF have not been established. To date, triggers of

hLTF include repeated bouts of hypoxia (Fuller, 2005, Harris et al., 2006), but also hypoxia-

independent stimuli. For example, repeated modulation of vagal feedback by intermittently

cooling the vagus nerve elicits LTF of genioglossus motor output (Tadjalli et al., 2010).

Alternatively, studies using an in vitro preparation of brainstem slices showed repeated

application of serotonergic 5-HT2- (Bocchiaro and Feldman, 2004) or noradrenergic α1-

(Neverova et al., 2007) receptor agonists can induce hLTF. Moreover, hLTF can be elicited by

Page 34: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

18

repeated optical stimulation of the A5 or A7 noradrenergic cell groups (Song and Poon, 2017).

Unique to these findings is that the triggers used elicited LTF solely in the hypoglossal /

genioglossus, with no long-term effects on diaphragm activity. Even triggers that induce mild

hypoxia, such as repeated obstructive apneas, can also trigger LTF of genioglossus activity

without long-term effects on diaphragm activity in anaesthetized rats (Ryan and Nolan, 2009,

Song and Poon, 2017, Tadjalli et al., 2010). This suggests that the trigger mechanism underlying

hLTF differs from pLTF as it either operates at different sensitivities or can be elicited with a

hypoxia-independent trigger.

The neural mechanism separating hLTF and pLTF may be due to a difference in the

neurotransmitter released. LTF of the phrenic nerve / diaphragm muscle is serotonin-

dependent (Bach and Mitchell, 1996, Fuller et al., 2001), whereas LTF of the hypoglossal nerve /

genioglossus muscle is noradrenaline-dependent (Huxtable et al., 2014, Tadjalli et al., 2010).

Although hLTF can be elicited by 5HT application under in vitro conditions (Bocchiaro and

Feldman, 2004), it is possible that hLTF was elicited due to the similar mechanisms between 5-

HT receptor activation and noradrenergic receptor activation. Both 5-HT2 and α1-adrenergic

receptors are Gq protein-coupled receptors and their activation have similar intracellular

cascades such as activation of inosotol 1,4,5-triphosphate (IP3) and protein kinase C (PKC). In

fact, α1-adrenergic receptor activation at the phrenic motor pool can be used to elicit pLTF, but

systemic blockade of α1-adrenergic receptors does not prevent hypoxia-induced pLTF (Huxtable

et al., 2014). In comparison, systemic 5-HT2A receptor blockade prevented apnea-induced hLTF

(Huxtable et al., 2014, Tadjalli, 2012), but blockade of 5-HT2A receptors at the level of the

hypoglossal motor pool did not prevent apnea-induced hLTF (Tadjalli et al., 2010). Although

there may be some degree of cross-talk between the noradrenergic and serotonergic

neurotransmitters systems in mediating LTF, there is a clear distinction between the

neurotransmitter mechanisms that are essential for the elicitation of pLTF versus hLTF. Direct

stimulation of noradrenergic cell groups that project to the hypoglossal motor pool (i.e. A5 or

A7) alone can also trigger hLTF (Song and Poon, 2017), further supporting the concept that

noradrenaline is the key neurotransmitter mediating hLTF (Fig. 1.6). This separation from pLTF

Page 35: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

19

is further emphasized by the fact that there is little to no noradrenergic input from the

brainstem to phrenic motor neurons (Dobbins and Feldman, 1994). However, this does not

nullify the role of serotonin on hypoglossal motor neurons nor noradrenaline on phrenic motor

neurons; both modulate motor neuron activity, respectively.

The source of noradrenaline involved in hLTF has not been fully elucidated, although the source

of serotonin acting on phrenic motor neurons to elicit pLTF has been suggested to originate

from the medullary raphe (Bach and Mitchell, 1996, Fuller et al., 2001, Millhorn, 1986). The

sources of noradrenaline acting on hypoglossal motor neurons have been suggested to arise

from the A1 (18.5% of the noradrenergic input to hypoglossal motor neurons arise from the

A1), A5 (43.5%), A6 or LC (1.7%), A7 (15.0%), and the subcoeruleus (21.0%) (Fig. 1.6) (Aldes et

al., 1992, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). The release of noradrenaline, presumably from these cell

groups, provide the tonic noradrenergic drive on hypoglossal motor activity as blockade of α1-

adrenergic receptors at the level of the hypoglossal motor pool reduce the amplitude of

inspiratory genioglossus motor activity (Chan et al., 2006). The LC is of particular interest as it

considered the largest source of noradrenaline in the brain (Moore, 1979). Although the LC was

reported to provide only 1.7% of the noradrenergic input to the hypoglossal motor pool

(Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007), the LC is dorsal to the subcoeruleus and together is referred to as

the “dorsal noradrenergic bundle” (Stanton and Sarvey, 1985). Studies of plasticity have shown

noradrenaline to play a critical role in the manifestation of LTP. Specifically, ablation of the

dorsal noradrenergic bundle prevented LTP in the dentate gyrus following intermittent

stimulation (Stanton and Sarvey, 1985). This further supports the concept that noradrenaline is

a neuromodulator that can promote or induce plasticity.

Page 36: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

20

Figure 1.6. LTF of genioglossus motor output induced by intermittent apneas. (A) Example trace showing repeated obstructive apneas lasting 10–15 seconds in a urethane-anesthetized, vagi-intact, and mechanically ventilated rat eliciting a reflexive increase in the amplitude of integrated genioglossus motor activity (∫GG EMG) during each apnea (denoted by dots above the ∫GG EMG recording). Following 10 apneas, a sustained facilitation of ∫GG EMG amplitude was observed above baseline denoted by dashed red line, evidence of long-term facilitation (LTF). Integrated diaphragm motor activity (∫Dia EMG) was unaffected. (B) Brain map showing the approximate location of the A1, A5, locus coeruleus (LC), A7, nucleus tractus solitaris (NTS) and hypoglossal motor pool (XII), highlighting afferents and efferents (red arrows) to the hypoglossal and LC. Insert provides expanded view of the brainstem nuclei and noradrenergic structures potentially involved in mediating hLTF (Adapted from Song and Poon 2017).

1.6 How noradrenaline induces plasticity

LTF has been considered to be a serotonin-dependent form of plasticity due to numerous

studies showing pLTF to require serotonin. However, unlike pLTF, noradrenaline is required for

LTF of the upper airways (Huxtable et al., 2014, Tadjalli et al., 2010). This distinction is

supported anatomically as noradrenergic cells innervate the hypoglossal motor pool (Aldes et

Page 37: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

21

al., 1992, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007) but few if any project to the phrenic motor pool (Dobbins

and Feldman, 1994). Since α1-adrenergic receptor activation is necessary for apnea-induced

hLTF (Tadjalli et al., 2010), it is necessary to understand how noradrenaline can induce

plasticity.

Noradrenaline can mediate three types of plasticity: developmental (which will not be

addressed in this thesis), intrinsic (modulating the excitability of a neuron), or synaptic

(modulating activity at the level of the synapse). Noradrenaline mediates these changes via

ligand-binding to two primary receptor subtypes: α and β. Within hypoglossal motor neurons

only α receptors are expressed, specifically excitatory α1-adrenergic receptor and inhibitory α2-

adrenergic receptors (Volgin et al., 2003, Volgin et al., 2001). This thesis will only focus on α

receptor binding on hypoglossal motor neurons.

Noradrenergic α receptors are primarily divided into two classes: α1 and α2. α1-adrenergic

receptor activation can induce plasticity synaptically or intrinsically on the post-synaptic neuron

(Jones et al., 1985). Changes at the synapse are induced when noradrenaline acts on α1-

adrenergic receptors. For example, noradrenaline potentiates the excitatory actions of

glutamate on motor neurons (Katakura and Chandler, 1990, Kiehn et al., 1999). The mechanism

underlying this potentiation is hypothesized to involve Gq protein-mediated activation of

phospholipase C β (PLC β) (Jiao et al., 2002). This in turn can generate the second messengers

inositol-(1,4,5)-trisphosphate (IP3) and diacyl-glycerol (DAG). IP3 causes the release of

intracellular Ca2+, which together with DAG activates protein kinase C (PKC) (Zhong and

Minneman, 1999). The activation of PKC causes AMPA receptor phosphorylation and is the

hypothesized mechanism that potentiates glutamate-evoked cell firing (Feldman et al., 2005,

Neverova et al., 2007). The activation of DAG and subsequently PKC can also increase protein

synthesis of BDNF (Juric et al., 2008), which in turn can lead to the insertion of new ion

channels at the synapse (Itami et al., 2003) (Fig. 1.7).

Page 38: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

22

Intrinsic changes to the post-synaptic cell also occur following α1-adrenergic receptor

activation. Activation of α1-adrenergic receptors inhibit TASK-1 potassium leak channel to

increase motor neuron excitability (Talley et al., 2000), and in hypoglossal motor neurons, α1-

adrenergic receptor stimulation can increase motor neuron excitability by increasing the input

resistance. This may be mediated by a reduction in a resting potassium current and activation

of a barium-insensitive inward current (Parkis et al., 1995). Together with changes at the

synapse, these mechanisms may increase the excitability of the post-synaptic cell (Fig. 1.7).

α2-adrenergic receptors are located on both pre- and post- synaptic sites and can modulate cell

activity at either site intrinsically (Aoki et al., 1994). Binding of α2-adrenergic receptor initiates a

Gi/o protein-mediated signalling cascade that can hyperpolarize the cell through the opening of

inwardly rectifying potassium channels (Surprenant and North, 1988), the activation ATP-

dependent potassium channels (Zhao et al., 2008), or through the inhibition of voltage-sensitive

calcium channels by (DeBock et al., 2003). These mechanisms all contribute to the reduction of

neurotransmitter release following α2-adrenergic receptors activation (Fig. 1.7).

Page 39: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

23

Figure 1.7. Schematic of the signalling cascade following noradrenaline-binding to α1- or α 2- adrenergic receptors. Binding of noradrenaline (NA) to α1-adrenergic receptors (top) can depolarize the cell membrane by triggering the G protein-mediated activation (Gq) to inhibit TASK-1 potassium leak channels, as well as activate barium-insensitive inward currents. Activation of the Gq pathway also triggers the activation of phospholipase C β (PLC β). This activates diacylglycerol (DAG) and produces inositol triphosphate (IP3) which causes release of calcium from intracellular stores. Both contribute to the activation of protein kinase C (PKC) which can phosphorylate AMPA-receptor subunits to potentiate glutamate-evoked action potentials. PKC activation can also increase the synthesis of brain-derived neurotrophic factors (BDNF), which bind to TrkB receptors where it in turn can lead to the insertion of new ion channels. Binding of NA to α2-adrenergic receptor (bottom) activates the G protein-mediated inhibition (Gi/o) to increase potassium conductance via ATP-dependent potassium channels, as well as inwardly rectifying potassium channels. Initiation of the Gi/o signalling cascade also suppresses voltage-activated calcium channels. (Modified from Marzo et al.2009).

1.7 Noradrenaline and hypoglossal LTF

To understand how noradrenaline acts on hypoglossal motor neurons to induce plasticity, it is

necessary to understand the physiology of hypoglossal motor neurons themselves. The

hypoglossal nuclei are bilateral structures situated along the midline in the caudal brainstem

Page 40: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

24

directly below the fourth ventricle and central canal. In rats, the hypoglossal nuclei extend

approximately 2 mm rostral-caudal and send descending axons to form the twelfth (XII) cranial

nerve. Currently, there are two known distinct types of neurons within the hypoglossal motor

pool: the motor neurons which drive contraction of respiratory muscles such as the

genioglossus, and the interneurons that modulate its activity, likely via inhibitory inputs (Boone

and Aldes, 1984, Peever et al., 2002). Estimations have placed 95% of the population to be

motor neurons and 5% to be interneurons (Sawczuk and Mosier, 2001). The activity of these

neurons control the dilation of upper airways muscles during inspiration in eupneic breathing,

as well as under an anaesthetized, tracheostomized, spontaneously breathing condition

(Fregosi and Fuller, 1997). The inspiratory drive on hypoglossal motor neurons is glutamatergic,

originating from the lateral tegmental field (Peever et al., 2002) and the rhythm generator (i.e.

preBötC) (Li et al., 2003), and act primarily through AMPA and NMDA receptors, both of which

are expressed on hypoglossal motor neurons (G.D. et al., 1993). Noradrenergic input onto the

hypoglossal motor neurons likely act through α1-adrenergic receptors as they are the most

highly expressed (Rukhadze et al., 2010, Volgin et al., 2001).

In the context of respiratory motor plasticity, direct evidence demonstrating noradrenaline to

be critical in LTF of hypoglossal/genioglossus activity have been observed in vitro and in vivo.

Repeated application of phenylephrine to brainstem slices elicits LTF of the hypoglossal nerve

(Neverova et al., 2007). Specifically, they found that episodic, not continuous activation of α1-

adrenergic receptors must act through protein kinase C to elicit a prolonged increase in

hypoglossal nerve activity. Alternatively, in an in vivo approach, repeated apneas delivered to

an anaesthetized, spontaneously breathing rat model can induce LTF of genioglossus motor

output but was completely abolished upon α1-adrenergic receptor blockade at the level of the

hypoglossal motor pool (Tadjalli et al., 2010). Taken together, it suggests that noradrenaline can

induce hLTF. However, neither study provides any insight into how noradrenaline may be

mediating hLTF.

Page 41: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

25

Noradrenaline may be mediating hLTF through α1-adrenergic receptor activation. Hypoglossal

motor neurons primarily express α1-adrenergic receptors (Rukhadze et al., 2010, Volgin et al.,

2001) and α1-adrenergic receptor activation can induce plasticity in other systems (Mouradian

et al., 1991). Furthermore, in direct context of respiratory plasticity, α1-adrenergic receptors

increase expression at the hypoglossal motor pool following chronic intermittent hypoxia,

increasing the endogenous excitatory drive onto hypoglossal motor neurons (Rukhadze et al.,

2010, Stettner et al., 2012). Direct application of phenylephrine induces LTF in vitro by

potentiating AMPA-mediated currents on hypoglossal motor neurons (Feldman et al., 2005).

This suggests that noradrenaline-induced plasticity may be occurring via changes on the

synapse at the level of the hypoglossal motor neuron through AMPAR-mediated changes.

However, it is also possible that the intrinsic excitability of the hypoglossal motor neuron is

mediating hLTF. For example, input resistance was increased on hypoglossal motor neurons

following application of noradrenaline (Parkis et al., 1995), requiring less current to induce

repetitive firing. This mechanism is hypothesized to act through reduction in Ba-sensitive

potassium channels and activating Ba-insensitive inward current carried by sodium ions. hLTF

could, therefore, be a result of noradrenaline inducing both intrinsic and synaptic plasticity at

the level of the hypoglossal motor pool.

1.8 Neural circuit underlying hypoglossal LTF

To date, the neural circuit underlying hypoglossal LTF has not been elucidated. The known

neural circuit involves the NTS (Torontali, 2012) and the activation of α1-adrenergic receptors

on hypoglossal motor neurons (Tadjalli et al., 2010). The hypoglossal motor neuron must in turn

trigger muscle contraction of the genioglossus muscle. The hypoglossal motor pool is,

therefore, a fundamental part of the hLTF circuit. The source of noradrenaline however,

remains unknown. Endogenous noradrenergic drive play a defined role in facilitating motor

output from hypoglossal motor neurons during wakefulness, sleep and during anaesthesia

(Chan et al., 2006). The source of noradrenaline could arise from one or more noradrenergic

cell groups that project to it. For example, the source of noradrenaline that mediates hLTF may

Page 42: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

26

originate from the A5 and/or A7 (Song and Poon, 2017). Specifically, intermittent apneas

increase c-Fos expression at the A5 and A7, and intermittent optical stimulation of these

regions triggered an increase in inspiratory genioglossus motor output persisting for 20 mins. In

addition, carbachol-induced “REM sleep” decreased A5 and A7 activity which correlated with a

decrease in hypoglossal nerve activity (Fenik et al., 2002, Fenik et al., 2008), suggesting the A5

and A7 may be positioned to provide the noradrenergic drive onto hypoglossal motor neurons

to trigger hLTF. However, the A5 may not act on hypoglossal motor neurons as the cells that

exhibited a reduction in activity did not project to hypoglossal motor nucleus as determined by

antidromic mapping (Fenik et al., 2002).

Alternatively, the LC also has direct projections to the hypoglossal motor pool and can

modulate respiratory activity (Aldes et al., 1992, Cedarbaum and Aghajanian, 1978, Hakuno et

al., 2004, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). For example, an increase in inspired CO2 increases LC

activity, suggesting the LC to be involved in chemoreception (Coates et al., 1993, Gargaglioni et

al., 2010, Haxhiu et al., 1996, Oyamada et al., 1999, Teppema et al., 1997). In fact, lesioning of

the LC decreases the hypercapnic ventilatory response (Biancardi et al., 2008, Li and Nattie,

2006), and prevents noradrenaline-induced increases in respiratory activity (Hilaire et al., 2004,

Oyamada et al., 1999). This suggests that the LC is capable of modulating respiratory output.

This was further supported when a reduction in LC activity correlated with REM sleep, and REM

sleep correlated with a reduction in upper airway muscle tone (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981,

Nitz and Siegel, 1997), suggesting LC activity may be involved in the reduction of muscle tone

during REM sleep. This connection was again supported when it was shown that LC inactivation

with tetrodotoxin reduced hypoglossal nerve firing rate in an isolated brainstem-spinal cord

preparation, and electrical stimulation of the LC under the same preparation increased the

firing rate (Hakuno et al., 2004). Together, it suggests that the LC may contribute to the

reduction of upper airway muscle tone during REM sleep. Although this correlation does not

directly implicate the LC, markers for cell injury in LC neurons following prolonged exposure to

intermittent hypoxia has been observed (Zhu et al., 2007), suggesting hypoxia experienced by

OSA patients may have added comorbidities that may circumvent the LC’s role in facilitating

Page 43: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

27

upper airway motor plasticity. Taken together, this suggests that the LC has the potential to

induce or promote respiratory motor plasticity.

Aside from the noradrenergic input required for hLTF, which could originate from the A5, A7, or

LC, there is evidence to suggest other structures are involved in the neural circuit mediating

hLTF. The triggers to induce hLTF (e.g. repeated bouts of hypoxia, repeated apneas, or repeated

modulation to vagal feedback) activate the chemosensory feedback (by hypoxia or apneas)

and/or the broncho-pulmonary feedback (by apneas or vagal modulation) systems. The latter is

of particular importance since the vagal afferents that deliver broncho-pulmonary feedback

have been shown to terminate in the NTS (Kalia and Sullivan, 1982), and pharmacological

inactivation of NTS cells prevented the expression of hLTF following repeated apneas (Torontali,

2012). In addition, LTF of genioglossus motor output can be triggered by repeated modulation

to vagal feedback alone (Tadjalli et al., 2010). Together, this suggests that the NTS is a critical

component of the hLTF circuit, providing a basic layout of the hypothesized neural circuit

underlying hLTF (Fig. 1.8).

Figure 1.8. Hypothesized circuit responsible for hypoglossal LTF. Repeated obstructive apneas modulate vagal afferent activity, which terminates in the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS). Cells in the NTS send projections to and activate noradrenergic cell group(s), which in turn extend axons directly to the hypoglossal (XII) motor pool to modulate hypoglossal (and therefore genioglossus) activity, effectively triggering hypoglossal long-term facilitation (hLTF). I hypothesize that this is the neural circuit underlying LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor output.

Page 44: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

28

1.9 Experimental objectives

The above evidence supports the working hypothesis that noradrenaline, the NTS, and the

hypoglossal motor pool are all critically important for the manifestation of hLTF. However, what

remains unknown is the source of noradrenaline, the circuit that mediates hLTF, and how these

structures work together within this proposed circuit. The goal of this thesis is to, therefore,

determine the neural circuit and mechanism underlying LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor

output. I propose that the current hypothesized circuit involves recurrent apneas acting

through vagal afferents terminating in the NTS, which in turn activates noradrenergic

structure(s) to episodically release noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons to induce

plasticity. I will test this hypothesis in the following research objectives:

Objective 1 (Chapter 3): To determine the brainstem structures activated by apnea-induced

hLTF.

Objective 2 (Chapter 3): To determine anatomical connections between the noradrenergic

neurons of the locus coeruleus and the hypoglossal motor pool.

Objective 3 (Chapter 3): To determine if the locus coeruleus is a critical component of the

neural circuit mediating apnea-induced hLTF.

Objective 4 (Chapter 4): To determine whether optogenetic manipulation of channelrhodopsin

(ChR2) -expressing locus coeruleus neurons alone can elicit hLTF.

Objective 5 (Chapter 4): To determine whether optical silencing of halorhodopsin (eNpHR) -

expressing locus coeruleus neurons prevent apnea-induced hLTF.

Objective 6 (Chapter 5): To determine whether noradrenaline released specifically from the

locus coeruleus is the underlying mechanism that mediates hLTF.

Page 45: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

29

Chapter Two – Materials and Methods

2.1 Animals

Experiments were performed on anaesthetized, spontaneously breathing adult male Sprague-

Dawley rats. A total of 107 rats, 447 ± 58 g, aged 8-12 weeks, were included in this study. Rats

were shipped from Charles River Laboratories (Wilmington, MA) and housed at the University

of Toronto Cell and Systems Biology Animal Bioscience Facility. Rats were housed in pairs with

unlimited access to food and water in room temperature on a 12:12 hour light-dark cycle.

Animals were given minimum 1 week to acclimatize to housing conditions upon arrival before

any experimental procedures were performed. All experimental procedures in this study were

performed in accordance with both the Canadian Council on Animal Care and University of

Toronto Animal Care Committee.

2.2 Drug and tracer preparation

All drugs were made on the day of experiments; drugs were dissolved in lactated Ringer’s then

filtered (0.2 μm nylon; Thermo Fisher Scientific). Clonidine (clonidine hydrochloride; 266.55 FW;

Sigma-Aldrich), an α-2 noradrenergic auto-receptor agonist, was delivered through a 28-gauge

stainless steel cannula via microinjection of 200 nL over 2 minutes at 4.8 µg/mL. This

concentration has been shown to be effective at reducing cortical noradrenaline levels by more

than 60% (Sakamoto et al., 2013), which is sufficient at affecting LC-dependent behaviours

(Mair et al., 2005). Terazosin (terazosin hydrochloride; 423.80 FW; Sigma-Aldrich), an α1

noradrenergic receptor antagonist, was delivered through reverse-microdialysis at 1 μM

perfusing at 2 μL per min at the LC. This concentration was also chosen as it was used

previously in the lab to show efficacy at preventing apnea-induced hLTF (Tadjalli et al., 2010)

without affecting baseline genioglossus activity (Tadjalli, 2012).

Cholera toxin B (CtB) is a neuroanatomical tracer that is taken up at the site of injection and

retrogradely labels cells that project to the target site. Here, CtB was used to determine the

projections to the hypoglossal motor pool. CtB was chosen because it is currently available

Page 46: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

30

conjugated with Alexa Fluor 488, which is a fluorescent tag that has shown to be bright and

more photostable than other fluorescent dyes conjugated to other neuroanatomical tracers

(Panuchuk-Voloshina et al., 1999). CtB was reconstituted in sterile phosphate buffered saline

the day of use (Conte et al., 2009).

In contrast, an adeno-associated viral vector (AAV-hSyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry) was used to

trace anterograde projections from the LC to the hypoglossal motor pool to verify CtB results.

Although an AAV viral vector is not inherently an anterograde tracer, it provides clear

visualizations of axonal projections (Hunanyan et al., 2013, Muzerelle et al., 2016).

2.3 Surgical procedures

Animal were weighed before each surgery. Anaesthesia was introduced by placing rats into an

induction chamber with 3.5% isoflurane in a 50/50 oxygen nitrogen mix and maintained via a

nose cone at 3% isoflurane. In all experiments, rectal temperature was monitored and

maintained at 37.5 ± 0.5°C via a servo-controlled heating pad (09585; FHC, Bowdoinham, ME,

or TC-1000; CWE Inc.) throughout surgeries and experimental recordings. After complete

absence of corneal and foot-withdrawal reflexes, a tracheostomy was performed where a

midline ventral incision was made to expose the trachea and a custom-made silicone T-tube

cannula was inserted just below the larynx. Anaesthesia was maintained through the T-tube for

the remainder of the experiments at 2-2.5% isoflurane set to a flow rate of 1 L per min. To

prevent the accumulation of mucosal secretions that may occlude the tracheal T-tube, a

subcutaneous injection of atropine sulfate (0.4 mg/kg) was administered. Airway obstructions

were introduced by occluding the custom T-tube cannula with hemostats. The jugular vein was

cannulated for administration of fluids (lactated Ringer’s solution) at a rate of 1.5 ml per hour.

Lactated ringer’s solution was administered by a way of pump driver (Hive Syringe Pump

Controller, MD-1020, BASi). To record upper airway respiratory motor activity, two needle

electrodes (F-E2; Grass Technologies) were inserted into the genioglossus muscle, with one

electrode on either side of the muscle. To record diaphragm EMG activity, a 1-2 cm midline

Page 47: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

31

abdominal incision was made and a custom-made bipolar electrode was fastened onto the

fascia of the right diaphragm. All incisions were closed with 9mm wound clips (Becton

Dickinson) to prevent tissue desiccation.

2.3.1. Stereotaxic injection (virus / tracer)

In some experiments, stereotaxic surgery was performed on animals to introduce an adeno-

associated virus (AAV) or a neuronal retrograde tracer into the locus coeruleus (LC) or

hypoglossal motor pool. Animals were placed in a stereotaxic setup (David Kopf Instruments)

with their heads secured with ear bars and a snout clamp. Animals were draped and had their

eyes covered with an ophthalmic ointment to prevent drying. All stainless surgical instruments

are autoclaved prior to surgery. When performing multiple surgeries, tools are sterilized

between surgeries with a dry bead sterilizer for minimum 15 seconds, 1 minute for items larger

than forceps/scissors. A 1:16 dilution of Accel is used as a surface disinfectant. A 2 cm midline

incision was made onto the skin on the dorsal surface of skull. Hydrogen peroxide and saline

was used to remove overlying connective tissue and to expose bregma and lambda. To level the

skull, a digital reader (David Kopf Instruments) was used to ensure ±0.01 mm accuracy between

bregma and lambda. Burr holes were drilled (TX Series, Foredom Electric Co.) at the surface of

the skull to expose the dura, bilaterally above the LC at coordinates (relative to bregma) 10.0

mm posterior, 1.4 mm lateral, 7.5 mm ventral, or unilaterally above the hypoglossal motor pool

(AP 14.5 mm, ML 0.2 mm, DV 9.0 mm). Coordinates were guided by the stereotaxic brain atlas

by Paxinos and Watson (1998). The dura was then punctured using a 25-gauge sterile

hypodermic needle. Bleeding caused by puncturing the dura ceased after applying pressure

with a sterile cotton swab on the burr hole. A stainless steel 28-gauge cannula connected to a

digital microinjection syringe pump (Pump 11 Elite; Harvard Apparatus) was then lowered to

the target region to deliver 600 nL of either AAV5-hsyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry (4.1x10^12

vg/mL), AAV5-hsyn-eNpHR3.0-mCherry (6.7x10^12 vg/mL), or AAV5-hsyn-mCherry (3.4x10^12

vg/mL), purchased from the University of North Carolina Vectorcore. For retrograde tracing,

200 nL of CtB was injected into the hypoglossal motor pool. After microinjection, the incision

Page 48: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

32

was sutured and post-operative care was given. Animals injected with a viral vector given for 3-

4 weeks for recovery and to allow for gene expression before experiments began. Animals

injected with CtB were killed after 10 days with an overdose of isoflurane and perfused with 4%

paraformaldehyde.

2.3.2 Drug delivery

In some experiments, a drug was delivered to the LC or hypoglossal motor pool through

microinjection or perfusion through reverse microdialysis. To do this, animals were surgically

instrumented for stereotaxic injections (See section 2.3.1). Microinjection was performed using

a stainless steel 28-gauge cannulas connected to a 1 mL gastight syringe (MD00500 Gastight

Syringes, BASi) with FEP Teflon tubing (inner diameter of 0.12 mm; Eicom). The cannula was

lowered into the LC to deliver the drug or vehicle solution at a rate of 0.1 μL per min.

Alternatively, reverse microdialysis was performed using a microdialysis probe (6000 Da cut-off

membrane: 1 mm long × 250 μm wide; CMA) connected to a 1 mL gastight syringe (MD00500

Gastight Syringes, BASi) with FEP Teflon tubing (inner diameter of 0.12 mm; Eicom). The probe

was lowered into the hypoglossal motor pool to perfuse Terazosin (1 µM) at a rate of 2 µL per

min for 20 minutes prior to any intervention. In both cases, drug delivery was controlled via a

syringe pump driver and controller (Hive Syringe Pump Controllers, MD-1020, BASi) and

cannula placements was verified by post-mortem histology.

2.3.3 Optogenetic manipulations

Optogenetic manipulations involved the use of optic fibres to deliver light to targeted brain

regions expressing ChR2 or eNpHR, such as millisecond control of neuronal activity could be

manipulated. In some experiments, optogenetic stimulation or inhibition was performed using

473 nm wavelength laser system (LRS-0473, Laserglow) or a 532 nm wavelength laser system

(LRS-0532, Laserglow), respectively. Each laser system was connected to a mono fiberoptic

patchcord (MFP-200/230/900-0.37-2m-FC-ZF1.25(F), Doric Lenses), which was connected to a

custom made optic implant that consisted of an optic fibre (200 nm diameter, ThorLabs). Only

Page 49: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

33

optic implants that provided a circular light path with no visible light diffractions were accepted

for experimental use. Custom optic implants were made by connecting optic fibre to a ceramic

ferrule (MM-FER2007C-2300, Precision Fibre Products) using epoxy. The length of the optic

implant was measured to reach 7.5 mm ventral to bregma such that the optic probe tip would

presumably be at the top of the target site (i.e., locus coeruleus). Optic probe tip would be

inserted past the target site by approximately 0.1-0.2 mm to verify probe tip location in post-

mortem histology then retracted. Only light output that was greater than 25 mW and less than

50 mW measured at the optic fibre tip using a power meter (PM100A, ThorLabs) were used for

experiments. This power output is higher compared to recent reports on optical LC stimulation

in mice which used 20 mW (Carter et al., 2010) or 10-12 mW (Wang et al., 2014). At 25-50 mW,

there is potential for heat-induced cell damage (Qian and Gu, 2005), heat-induced cell firing

(Reig et al., 2010, Stujenske et al., 2015), and photodilation of blood vessels (Rungta et al.,

2017). Despite these potential side effects, I chose this power output to ensure sufficient ChR2

activation. The amount of heat produced is dependent on the size of the optic fibre, the

duration of light-exposure, and the wavelength of light (Gysbrechts et al., 2016, Stujenske et al.,

2015). To reduce potential side effects caused by heat production, I used a 200 µm optic fibre

to deliver light in 5 ms pulses at 5 Hz for 15 seconds, which has been reported to produce a

0.005ᵒC increase in temperature (Anikeeva et al., 2011). This value is within the range of the

natural fluctuations observed in the awake rat brain (Shirey et al., 2015). Unfortunately, the

effects of light in naïve mice on cerebral blood flow has been reported to occur at levels as low

as 1 mW (Rungta et al., 2017) and was, therefore, not avoided in these experiments. For

optogenetic stimulation and inhibition protocols, see sections 2.6.4 and 2.6.5.

2.4 Electrophysiology recordings

Genioglossus and diaphragm EMG signals were amplified between 500-2000 Hz using a Super-Z

High Impedance Head Stage (cat# 10-02010, CWE Inc.) and a BMA-400 AC/DC Bioamplifier

(cat#09-03010, CWE Inc.). Signals were filtered with a bandpass between 1-3000 Hz for EMG

signals sampled at 1000 Hz. End tidal CO2 and temperature measurements were sampled at

Page 50: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

34

40Hz (Spike2 software, 1401 Interface; CED) and digitized (1 kHz; Micro1401; Cambridge

Electronic Design). Integrated respiratory EMG activities were quantified using Spike2 software

(Cambridge Electronic Design). All signals were stored on a computer for offline analysis.

2.5 Measurement of ET-CO2 and O2 saturation

End-tidal CO2 (ET-CO2) was monitored in real-time using a calibrated fast response CO2 analyzer

(Model 17630; VacuMed or MicroCapster Endtidal CO2 analyzer, CWE Inc.) connected to the

tracheal T-tube. End-tidal CO2 monitor was calibrated using carbagen (5% CO2 in 95% O2) before

every experiment. Arterial O2 saturation was measured using a pulse oximeter designed for

rodents, connected to the hind-paw of the animal (MouseOx Pulse Oximeter; STARR Life

Sciences Corp.) WINDAQ Waveform Browser software (Dataq Instruments) was used to digitize

and analyze O2 saturation signals, which were then recorded using Spike2 software. Both

variables were analyzed and quantified offline using Spike2 software.

2.6 Experimental protocol

After each experimental paradigm’s surgical intervention, animals were left to stabilize for 60

minutes to establish a baseline activity for the genioglossus and diaphragm EMG amplitude, ET-

CO2, and O2 saturation. After a stable baseline is established, specific experimental protocols

were executed (e.g. repeated obstructive apneas). After each intervention, physiological

variables were recorded for an additional 60-90 minutes. Control experiments underwent an

identical procedure with vehicle solutions or control viral vectors. Furthermore, an absence of

an intervention was included as well to control for time-dependent fluctuations in variables

measured.

Page 51: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

35

2.6.1 Objective 1 – To determine the brainstem structures activated alongside apnea-induced

hLTF

To reaffirm that repeated obstructive apneas can trigger long-term facilitation of genioglossus

motor activity (hLTF), genioglossus and diaphragm EMG activity was recorded from

anaesthetized, spontaneously breathing rats (n=14). After a baseline was established, a

repeated apnea protocol was performed (Fig. 2.1). This included 10 apneas, each 15-seconds in

duration separated by 1-minute recovery. Apneas were triggered during end-expiration in order

to mimic the obstruction pattern experienced in OSA patients (Sanders and Moore, 1983,

Sanders et al., 1985). Following repeated apneas, genioglossus and diaphragm EMG, ET-CO2 and

O2 saturation was recorded over 60-90 minutes. Time matched control group of animals

without exposure to repeated apneas (n=10) were used to account for the effects of the

anaesthetic and surgical intervention over the duration of the recording period. At the end of

the recording period, the animal was killed by isoflurane overdose and transcardial perfusion.

All animals perfused were used for analysis.

Figure 2.1. Protocol for the delivery of repeated apneas in Objective 1.

To determine whether brainstem structures (noradrenergic or otherwise) were activated by

apnea-induced hLTF, rats were divided into 3 groups: rats that exhibited apnea-induced hLTF

(n=5), non-responders (i.e., rats that did not exhibit hLTF following the repeated apnea

protocol) (n=5), and time matched controls (i.e., without apneas) (n=5). All brains were

sectioned and stained for c-Fos, a neuronal marker for cell activation (Haxhiu et al., 1996,

Kaliszewska et al., 2012, Lim and Veasey, 2010, Teppema et al., 1997), and tyrosine hydroxylase

expression to identify noradrenergic neurons.

Page 52: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

36

2.6.2 Objective 2 – To determine the anatomical connection between the noradrenergic locus

coeruleus neurons and the hypoglossal motor pool

Previous studies investigating the anatomical connection between the LC and the hypoglossal

motor pool initially reported that cells in the LC (noradrenergic or otherwise) do not project to

the hypoglossal motor pool (Aldes, 1990, Aldes et al., 1992, Aston-Jones et al., 1970, Fritschy

and Grzanna, 1990, Levitt and Moore, 1979). However, more recent studies suggest

approximately 1.7% of LC neurons project to the region of the hypoglossal motor pool

(Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). Furthermore, a decrease in LC activity was observed during REM

sleep (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981), and a decrease in genioglossus activity was also observed

during REM sleep [23]. Taken together, I hypothesize that LC activity contributes to

genioglossus activity, and that there is an anatomical connection between the LC and

hypoglossal motor pool. To determine whether LC neurons have direct axonal projections to

the hypoglossal motor pool, rats (n=6) were unilaterally injected with a retrograde tracer, CtB,

into the hypoglossal motor pool. A control injection (n=7) was performed by injecting CtB

ventral to the hypoglossal motor pool. Ten days were given for CtB to retrogradely label cells

before the animal was killed by isoflurane overdose and transcardial perfusion. Tissue was then

stained for tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) to determine whether noradrenergic cells of the LC are

colocalized with CtB. Cells were counted manually using an upright fluorescent microscope

(AxioImager Z1; Zeiss). To further verify the anatomical connection between the LC and the

hypoglossal motor nucleus, an AAV was injected into the LC and allowed to express over 3-4

weeks post injections. The brains were later imaged to determine the presence of axonal

projections at the level of the hypoglossal motor.

2.6.3 Objective 3 – To determine whether the locus coeruleus is a critical component of the

neural circuit mediating apnea-induced hLTF

To determine whether the LC is critical for the manifestation of hLTF following a repeated

apnea protocol, I bilaterally inactivated the LC to prevent apnea-induced hLTF. Rats (n=14) were

injected with 200 nL of clonidine (4.8 µg/mL) (Mair et al., 2005, Sakamoto et al., 2013) or

Page 53: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

37

vehicle solution (i.e., lactated Ringer’s) into the LC at 0.1 µL/min over 2 minutes, followed by

repeated obstructive apnea protocol (Fig. 2.2). Genioglossus and diaphragm EMG, ET-CO2 and

O2 saturation was recorded over 60 minutes prior to clonidine application or vehicle solution to

establish a baseline. After administration of clonidine, another 30 minutes was recorded for a

second baseline under the influence of clonidine before performing repeated apneas, and

subsequently recorded for another 60-90 minutes before the animal was killed by isoflurane

overdose and transcardial perfusion.

Figure 2.2. Protocol for clonidine intervention followed by repeated apneas in Objective 3.

2.6.4 Objective 4 – To determine whether optogenetic manipulation of ChR2-expressing locus

coeruleus neurons alone can elicit hLTF

To determine whether intermittent optical stimulation of the LC can trigger hLTF in lieu of

repeated apneas, rats (n=26) were infected with either AAV5-hSyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry or

AAV5-hSyn-mCherry. After a minimum of 3 weeks for viral expression, optic implants were

inserted bilaterally into the LC and I again recorded genioglossus and diaphragm EMG, ET-CO2

and O2 saturation over 60 minutes to establish a baseline. Unfortunately, the firing rates of LC

neurons have never been recorded during or after repeated apneas and thus, the frequency in

which LC neurons fire during apneas is not known. However, studies that have recorded LC

activity has reported that the LC fires at 0.5 Hz under anaesthesia (Seager et al., 2004, Vazey

and Aston-Jones, 2014), 1-2 Hz tonically (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981, Dremencov et al.,

2007), and up to 5 Hz when activated (Kogan et al., 1992). Therefore, I chose to repeatedly

stimulate LC neurons with 473 nm light at 5 Hz for 15 seconds separated by 1 minute of no

stimulation, repeated 10 times (Fig. 2.3). This intervention mimics the repeated apnea protocol

Page 54: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

38

in Objective 1. Following this intervention, recordings continued for another 60-90 minutes

before the animal was killed by isoflurane overdose and transcardial perfusion. Three controls

were included: an off-target control (n=6) to determine the effect of stimulation outside the LC,

a continuous stimulation control (n=5) to determine the requirement for the intermittent

nature of the stimulus (Fig. 2.4), and a viral vector control (n=6) that is absent the light-sensitive

ion channel, channelrhodopsin2 (Chr2), to determine whether viral infection alone influenced

the manifestation of hLTF.

Figure 2.3. Protocol for intermittent LC stimulation in Objective 4.

Figure 2.4. Protocol for continuous LC stimulation in Objective 4.

2.6.5 Objective 5 – To determine whether optical silencing of eNpHR-expressing locus

coeruleus neurons prevent apnea-induced hLTF

To determine whether optical inactivation of the LC prevents apnea-induced hLTF, rats (n=22)

were infected with either AAV5-hSyn-eNpHR3.0-mCherry or AAV5-hSyn-mCherry. After a

minimum of 3 weeks for viral expression, optic implants were inserted bilaterally into the LC

and I again recorded genioglossus and diaphragm EMG, ET-CO2 and O2 saturation over 60

minutes to establish a baseline. LC neurons were then exposed to 532 nm light continuously

while simultaneously delivering obstructive apneas for 15 seconds separated by 1 minute

recovery, repeated 10 times (Fig. 2.5). Following the intervention, recordings continued for

Page 55: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

39

another 60-90 minutes before the animal was killed by isoflurane overdose and transcardial

perfusion. A viral vector absent the light-sensitive ion channel, halorhodopsin (eNpHR), was

used to determine whether viral infection itself influenced the manifestation of hLTF.

Figure 2.5. Protocol for continuous inhibition of the LC with repeated apneas in Objective 5.

2.6.6 Objective 6 – To determine whether noradrenaline released specifically from the locus

coeruleus is the underlying mechanism that mediates hLTF

To determine whether hLTF elicited by intermittent LC stimulation is truly mediated by

noradrenaline, rats (n=9) were infected with AAV5-hSyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry. After a

minimum of 3 weeks for viral expression, optic implants were inserted bilaterally into the LC

and I again recorded genioglossus and diaphragm EMG, ET-CO2 and O2 saturation over 60

minutes to establish a baseline. I then perfused 1 µM Terazosin over 20 minutes at rate of 0.1

μL/min (Tadjalli, 2012) to antagonize α1-adrenergic receptors at the level of the hypoglossal

motor pool using reverse microdialysis. LC neurons were then repeatedly stimulated with 473

nm light at 5 Hz for 15 seconds separated by 1 minute of no stimulation, repeated 10 times (Fig.

2.6). Following the intervention, recordings continued for another 60-90 minutes before the

animal was killed by isoflurane overdose and transcardial perfusion.

Figure 2.6. Protocol for Terazosin perfusion with intermittent LC stimulation in Objective 5.

Page 56: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

40

2.7 Data analysis

Peak integrated inspiratory genioglossus and diaphragm EMG amplitudes as well as respiratory

frequency were quantified on a breath-by-breath basis in 60 second intervals during all

experiments. Inspiratory amplitude and respiratory frequency were expressed as a percent

change from baseline ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Baseline values for inspiratory

amplitude and respiratory frequency were acquired during the 240 seconds prior to each

experimental intervention. Data were quantified and expressed before (i.e., baseline) and at 15,

30, 45 and 60 minutes after experimental interventions. Equivalent time points were quantified

and expressed in experiments serving as controls without particular interventions. Animals

were determined to exhibit hLTF if they met two specific criteria: (1) genioglossus inspiratory

amplitude was two standard deviations above baseline levels 60-min after recurrent apneas;

and, (2) summated genioglossus inspiratory amplitude averaged over 60-min was two standard

deviations above baseline levels. If an animal failed either criterion, they were considered not

to exhibit hLTF and were placed in a separate group henceforth known as “non-responders”.

This allowed for a clear separation between animals that exhibited hLTF and non-responders.

Although setting a standard to exclude non-responders is traditionally used in the field of LTP

research (Abraham et al., 1993, Watanabe et al., 2002), this selection approach can skew

statistical comparisons that only compare hLTF-expressing animals. To address this, further

analysis appropriately included one or both groups for statistical comparisons (see section

2.10). Arterial O2 saturation and end-tidal CO2 values were also expressed as a percentage

change from baseline ± SEM and presented at 15, 30, 45 and 60 minutes after each

experimental intervention or at equivalent time points in control experiments. Each presented

data point was an average over 60 seconds.

2.8 Histology

Immunohistochemical staining was performed on all rats to verify (1) probe tract locations, (2)

cell activity as determined by c-Fos expression, and (3) cell phenotype as determined by

tyrosine hydroxylase staining to identify noradrenergic neurons. At the end of each experiment,

Page 57: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

41

rats were overdosed with isoflurane (5%) until ventilation ceased, followed by transcardial

perfusion with 4% paraformaldehyde (in 0.1M phosphate buffer). Brains were extracted and

stored in 4% paraformaldehyde overnight, followed by a cryoprotection step by submerging

brains into 30% sucrose in 0.1M PB solution over several days until brains were saturated.

Brains were then immersed in Tissue-Tek OCT Compound (Electron Microscope Sciences) and

frozen on dry ice. Frozen brains were then sliced in a cryostat (CM3050 S, Leica Microsystems)

at 40 μm coronal sections. To determine whether noradrenergic cells were activated alongside

apnea-induced hLTF, immunohistochemistry was used to identify colocalized expression of c-

Fos and tyrosine hydroxylase. Primary antibody rabbit anti-c-Fos (1:5000 dilution, cat# 26209,

lot# 113018B, Immunostar) was used in conjunction with mouse anti-TH (1:1000 dilution, cat#

22941, lot# 907001, Immunostar). After 48 hours of incubation at 4°C, biotinylated secondary

antibodies, biotinylated goat anti-rabbit IgG (1:800 dilution, cat# BA-1000, lot# Z0619, Vector

Laboratories) and biotinylated goat anti-mouse IgG (1:600 dilution, cat# BA-9200, lot# W2206,

Vector Laboratories) were used. To visualize, an avidin biotin complex (ABC) kit (VECTASTAIN

Elite ABC HRP Kit, PK-6100, Vector Laboratories) used in conjunction with a 3,3’-

diaminobenzidine (DAB) peroxidase kit (DAB Kit, VECTSK4100, Vector Laboratories) to oxidize

DAB, providing a brown-black colour in the nuclei of c-Fos positive cells, and NovaRed (NovaRed

Kit, VECTSK4800, Vector Laboratories) was used to provide a contrasting red colour to identify

noradrenergic cells. Stained tissue was then imaged using Cellsens Slide Scanner (Olympus,

FSX100) under bright field at 4x magnification. The location of lesion tracts were plotted on

standardized brain maps (Paxinos and Watson, 1998). In experiments where virally infected LC

neurons expressed mCherry, tissue was incubated in primary rabbit anti-mCherry (1:500

dilution, cat# NBP2-25157, lot# 12016, Novus Biologicals) and secondary goat anti-rabbit Cy3

antibodies (1:500, cat# 111-167-003, lot#78034, Jackson ImmunoResearch). To identify

noradrenergic cells under fluorescence, tissue was incubated again in primary mouse anti-TH

with fluorescent secondary goat anti-mouse Alexa Fluor 488 (1:500, cat# 111-167-003, lot#

130258, Jackson ImmunoResearch). Tissue was then counterstained with DAPI. Sections were

imaged with the upright fluorescent (AxioImager Z1; Zeiss), confocal microscope (AxioObserver

Page 58: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

42

Z1; Zeiss), or under bright-field through a slide scanner (FSX-100 Inverted Microscope,

Olympus).

2.9 Cell quantification

In initial studies, sections were first non-quantitatively analyzed to identify regions with notable

changes in c-Fos expression. A cell was considered c-Fos+ if a cell expressed a black nucleus and

excluded cells that expressed nuclei that were light/medium brown (which may or may not be

c-Fos+). This level of stringency ensured that we only identified c-Fos+ cells and thereby

excluded the possibility of identifying false positive cells. Noradrenergic cells were identified by

a red-brown colour in the whole cell obtained with NovaRed staining. Noradrenergic regions

and areas with unambiguous c-Fos expression were then manually counted with observers

blinded to the treatment. An automatic counting process was not possible for the LC due to the

irregular shape of LC neurons relative to standard automatic cell counting parameters available.

Regions of interest were identified using the rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson, 1998) and

counted using ImageJ. Three images representative of slices across the rostral/caudal axis were

taken for each region per animal, and this sampling strategy is based on a recent study that

showed that the distribution of LC projections had no specific organization across

anterior/posterior or medial/lateral axes (Schwarz et al., 2015). Each image was 0.5 x 0.5 mm

and encompassed the structure of interest. In later studies, fluorescent staining was used to

identify mCherry and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) positive cells. In these studies, cells were

manually counted for each image with the experimenter blinded to the treatment.

2.10 Statistical analysis

The specific statistical tests used for each experiment are stated within the results section. All

datasets passed normality. In all groups, values were compared as percent change from

baseline. Comparisons for each respiratory variable within a treatment across time (e.g.,

repeated apneas on genioglossus amplitude at baseline, 15, 30, 45, and 60 minute time points)

were made using a one-way repeated measure analysis of variance (one-way RM ANOVA) and

Page 59: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

43

post hoc comparisons were performed using the Dunnett test. Comparisons between

treatments for each respiratory variable were made using a two-way RM ANOVA with post hoc

Bonferroni test to infer statistical significance. One caveat with this approach is the exclusion of

animals that do not exhibit hLTF when comparing genioglossus amplitude between groups. To

determine whether an intervention influenced hLTF expression, a chi-square comparison was

performed to determine whether my intervention was significantly correlated with hLTF

expression. A chi-square test can help determine whether my observed values fit within an

expected distribution. In other words, the chi-square tests the null hypothesis that the variables

are independent. The test compares the observed data to a model that expects that the

variables are independent. Where the observed data do not fit provides the likelihood that the

variables are dependent. In addition to this, I also performed a Firth logistic regression to

measure the relationship between one or more independent variable that determines an

outcome. In other words, a Firth logistic regression allows me to determine whether my

predictors (e.g. an intervention such as repeated apneas) influenced my dependent outcome

(i.e., hLTF expression). This differs from a chi-square test because a chi-square test is not a

modelling technique. By providing the probability of hLTF expression based on my predictors

(i.e., intervention such as repeated apneas), a logistic regression can model whether my

intervention can predict an outcome. In both calculations (chi-square test and Firth logistic

regression), I included all animals (i.e., hLTF-expressing animals and non-responders) to

determine whether an intervention influenced the probability of hLTF expression. Lastly,

comparisons of respiratory muscle activity between all groups were also made using an

ordinary least square (OLS) linear regression. An OLS allowed me to identify whether my

intervention had a statistically significant effect compared to controls and determine the

strength and direction of the effect. This approach allowed me to compare the muscle activity

of all animals (i.e., hLTF-expressing animals and non-responders), in all treatment groups (i.e.,

intervention and controls), at a specific time point to infer statistical significance. Cell counts

between groups were compared using a one-way ANOVA and post hoc comparisons were

performed using the Bonferroni test, or a student t-test where applicable. All statistical

Page 60: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

44

analyses used GraphPad Prism (Prism v5.0, GraphPad), STATA (v5.10 and R Studio (v3.4.3). Data

are presented as a mean + standard error of the mean.

Page 61: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

45

Chapter Three – A Tripartite Circuit Mediates Respiratory Motor Plasticity

(Data in Chapter 3 is published; Lui, S. et al., 2018. The data generated by the co-authors were

not included in this thesis. Only data generated by myself is presented in Chapter 3).

3.1 Summary

The respiratory network takes in various stimuli and adjusts respiratory output accordingly. The

respiratory network can make long-term adjustments to its output by undergoing plasticity. For

example, following repeated obstructive apneas the respiratory network can augment

respiratory motor output and strengthen genioglossus muscle contractions for a prolonged

period, implicating it to have potential in mitigating obstructive sleep apnea. This form of

plasticity is known as hypoglossal (XII) long-term facilitation (hLTF). The neural circuit

underlying hLTF is unknown but was suggested to require activation of the nucleus tractus

solitaris (NTS) and required the activation of α1-adrenergic receptors on hypoglossal motor

neurons. Here I propose the locus coeruleus (LC) to be the source of noradrenaline acting on

α1-adrenergic receptors to mediate hLTF. First, I elicited hLTF with repeated apneas to identify

which cell groups were activated alongside hLTF. Next, I identified the LC to be the only

noradrenergic cell group to display an increase in neural activity following hLTF, then traced its

axonal projections from the LC to the hypoglossal motor pool. Lastly, I show that inactivation of

the LC prevented apnea-induced hLTF. Taken together, I have identified a three-part circuit

within the brainstem (NTS → LC → XII) that underlies respiratory motor plasticity.

3.2 Introduction

Understanding motor neuron physiology is important because respiratory motor neurons are

critical in triggering effective breathing movements. Respiratory motor neurons (e.g.,

hypoglossal) are sensitive to and modulated by repeated perturbations in central respiratory

drive. For example, intermittent episodes of hypoxia or airway obstruction induce a form of

respiratory motor plasticity known as long-term facilitation (LTF) (Hickner et al., 2014, Hoffman

Page 62: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

46

et al., 2012, Ryan and Nolan, 2009, Song and Poon, 2017, Tadjalli et al., 2010). LTF results in a

long-lasting increase in inspiratory motor outflow to inspiratory muscles (e.g., genioglossus),

which may function to facilitate ventilation. Previously, it was demonstrated that repeated

airway obstructions trigger LTF of hypoglossal motor outflow (i.e., apnea-induced hLTF) and

that this form of respiratory plasticity is mediated by a noradrenergic mechanism (Tadjalli et al.,

2010). Specifically, the blockade of α1-noradrenergic receptors at the level of hypoglossal

motor pool prevented hLTF, suggesting that noradrenaline release likely underlies hLTF (Tadjalli

et al., 2010). Here, I reaffirm that repeated apneas can consistently trigger hLTF and

investigated the brainstem regions that were activated during apnea-induced hLTF.

Currently, the source(s) of the noradrenaline that drives respiratory motor plasticity remains

unidentified. What is known is that the hypoglossal motor pool receives noradrenergic input

from several noradrenergic cell groups within the brainstem including the A1, A5, A6 (i.e. LC),

A7, and subcoeruleus (Aldes et al., 1992, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007), with the proportions of

noradrenergic input to the hypoglossal motor pool being 18.5%, 43.5%, 1.7%, 15.0%, and

21.0%, respectively (Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). The A5 and A7 are of particular interest as

they have been previously shown to mediate a form of hLTF (Song and Poon, 2017). Specifically,

an increase in c-Fos expression was observed at the A5 and A7 following apnea-induced hLTF,

and intermittent optical stimulation of these regions triggered a transient increase in

inspiratory genioglossus motor output persisting for up to 20 minutes (Song and Poon, 2017).

This suggests that the A5 and A7 are likely candidates in mediating hLTF. Alternatively, although

the A1 and subcoeruleus has not been directly shown to mediate respiratory motor plasticity,

studies of the A1 have suggested that it is involved in mediating structural plasticity on

oxytocinergic neurons (Michaloudi et al., 1997), and its inactivation can reduce phrenic burst

activity (Hilaire, 2006, Zanella et al., 2006). Studies of the subcoeruleus’s role in plasticity were

often integrated with the LC, making the role of the subcoeruleus difficult to isolate (Stanton

and Sarvey, 1985). However, some studies have suggested the subcoeruleus to be involved in

learning and memory (Siwek et al., 2014), suggesting the subcoeruleus is involved in mediating

some forms of plasticity. In the context of respiration, the subcoeruleus responds to hypoxia

Page 63: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

47

(Berquin et al., 2000, Bodineau and Larnicol, 2001, Joubert et al., 2016, Teppema et al., 1997).

This suggests that the A1 and the subcoeruleus may also be involved in the neural circuit

mediating hLTF. Lastly, the LC has been intensively studied and well established to be involved

in both breathing and plasticity.

3.2.1 Locus coeruleus and its role in breathing and plasticity

The locus coeruleus is the largest source of noradrenaline within the central nervous system

(Moore, 1979), with diffuse projections throughout the entire neuroaxis (Schwarz et al., 2015).

The LC has shown very little topographical organization within itself, suggesting cells across the

neuroaxis within the LC have equal probability to project rostrally or caudally (Schwarz et al.,

2015). The most prominent role of the LC in the context of breathing has been as a central

chemoreceptor. Many studies have shown that an increase in inspired CO2 correlates with an

increase in LC activity, determined by an increase in c-Fos expression in the LC (Coates et al.,

1993, Gargaglioni et al., 2010, Haxhiu et al., 1996, Oyamada et al., 1999, Teppema et al., 1997).

In fact, lesioning of the LC decreased the hypercapnic ventilatory response (Biancardi et al.,

2008, Li and Nattie, 2006). The LC also has direct effects on hypoglossal nerve activity because

LC inactivation with tetrodotoxin reduced hypoglossal nerve firing rate in an isolated brainstem-

spinal cord preparation, and electrical stimulation of the LC under the same preparation

increased the firing rate (Hakuno et al., 2004).

In the context of plasticity, the LC has long been considered to be a mediator for plasticity, such

as LTP (Jedrzejewska-Szmek et al., 2017, Stanton and Sarvey, 1985). The ablation of

noradrenergic LC neurons prevents the manifestation of some forms of plasticity (Stanton and

Sarvey, 1985), suggesting that the LC is capable of promoting or inducing plasticity. However,

the role of the LC in mediating respiratory motor plasticity was not as readily studied. Studies

have linked a decreased in locus coeruleus activity during REM sleep (Aston-Jones and Bloom,

1981), and REM sleep with a decrease in upper airway muscle tone (Nitz and Siegel, 1997).

Together, it may suggest that a reduction in LC activity may be involved in the reduction of

Page 64: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

48

upper airway muscle tone during REM sleep. This is supported by data showing LC inactivation

with tetrodotoxin reduced hypoglossal nerve firing, while electrical LC stimulation increased

hypoglossal firing rates (Hakuno et al., 2004). Considering the LC is involved in influencing

hypoglossal nerve activity and plasticity in other systems, this places the LC in a prime position

to mediate noradrenaline-dependent respiratory plasticity.

3.2.2 Projections of locus coeruleus neurons

Earlier I mention that the hypoglossal motor pool receives dense noradrenergic input from the

A1, A5, LC, A7 and subcoeruleus. However, despite the dense noradrenergic innervations to the

hypoglossal motor pool, previous studies have claimed that the LC does not project to the

hypoglossal motor pool (Aldes, 1990, Aldes et al., 1992, Aston-Jones et al., 1970, Fritschy and

Grzanna, 1990, Levitt and Moore, 1979), or at most, provide 1.7% of the total noradrenergic

input at the hypoglossal motor pool (Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). Here, I provide evidence

showing a higher proportion of LC neurons possess a direct anatomical connection between the

LC and the hypoglossal motor pool using a retrograde tracer, cholera toxin B (CtB).

3.3 Results

3.3.1 Repeated obstructive apneas trigger LTF of inspiratory genioglossus muscle activity

First, I wanted to reaffirm that repeated obstructive apneas can trigger LTF of inspiratory

genioglossus motor output. I found that recurrent airway occlusions (10, 15-s apneas each

separated by 1-min) triggered a robust and sustained increase in inspiratory genioglossus

muscle activity that peaked at 96 ± 17% above baseline levels by 60 min min (2-way RM

ANOVA, F=11.22, p<0.0001; Fig. 3.1A,B,C). Apneas, which were confirmed by a total loss of

expired CO2 and a drop in O2 saturation (Fig. 3.1D,E), only amplified the magnitude of

inspiratory genioglossus activity; they had no long-term effect on respiratory frequency or

diaphragm inspiratory activity (2-way RM ANOVA, F=2.113, p=0.0880 and F=2.433, p=0.0557,

breath frequency and inspiratory diaphragm amplitude, respectively; Fig. 3.1F,G). LTF of

Page 65: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

49

inspiratory genioglossus amplitude was not attributable to changes in end-tidal CO2 nor

changes to O2 saturation as both variables remained consistent between groups over the 60-

min recording period (2-way RM ANOVA, F=0.4221, p=0.7921 and F=1.043, p=0.4160, for end-

tidal-CO2 and O2 saturation, respectively; Figure 3.1H,I).

It is important to note that within the group of animals given repeated apneas, 35% did not

exhibit hLTF and were labelled as non-responders. The lack of hLTF in these animals were not a

result of incomplete apneas, which were again confirmed by a total loss of expired CO2 and a

drop in O2 saturation (Fig. 3.1D,E). Non-responders are further discussed in section 3.3.3.

To demonstrate that respiratory activity remained stable throughout the recording period,

genioglossus and diaphragm activity were recorded in a control group of rats that experienced

no recurrent apneas. I found that genioglossus amplitude, breath frequency, end-tidal CO2 and

O2 saturation remained stabled during the 60 minute time-window (RM ANOVA, genioglossus:

F=1.33, p=0.2757; breath frequency: F=1.988, p=0.1148; ET-CO2: F=1.485, p=0.2272; O2

saturation: F=1.546, p=0.2776; Figure 3.1), indicating that genioglossus activity does not change

over the 60 minute recording period and that LTF of genioglossus muscle activity is attributable

to recurrent apneas per se. A decrease in diaphragm amplitude over time was observed (RM

ANOVA, F=4.996, p=0.0026), but was in line with animals given recurrent apneas (RM ANOVA,

F=6.408, p=0.0007). Taken together, this reaffirms that repeated apneas can successfully trigger

apnea-induced LTF of inspiratory genioglossus muscle activity.

Page 66: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

50

Figure 3.1. Repeated obstructive apneas elicit LTF of the genioglossus motor activity. (A) Integrated inspiratory genioglossus motor output (∫GG EMG) recorded from an anaesthetized spontaneously breathing rat, depicting baseline genioglossus amplitude and the subsequent increase in EMG amplitude following repeated apneas (i.e., hLTF). (B) High-temporal resolution EMG traces showing genioglossus (top) and diaphragm (bottom) activity at baseline, 15, 30, 45, and 60 min after repeated apneas. (C) Group data showing inspiratory genioglossus activity time matched control (i.e. no apneas; black bars) and intermittent apnea groups (white bars) at 15, 30, 45, and 60 min. Dotted line represents baseline activity. Intermittent apneas induced an increase in inspiratory genioglossus amplitude peaking at 96 ± 17%. A raw trace showing end-tidal CO2 (D) and arterial O2 saturation (E) before, during, and after an apnea (left), and group data (right) showing undetectable end-tidal CO2 levels during apnea indicating complete occlusion, and arterial O2 saturation levels reduced by 20 ± 4% following an apnea. Although intermittent apneas triggered a robust increase in inspiratory genioglossus activity (i.e., hLTF), this same intervention had no significant effect on either (F) inspiratory diaphragm (∫Dia EMG) amplitude or breath frequency. (H) Both end-tidal CO2 and (I) O2 saturation levels remained constant across the 60 min recording period. Data is presented as mean + SEM.*Denotes a significant difference (p < 0.05) from baseline.

3.3.2 Activation of noradrenergic LC neurons correlates with hLTF

To determine the source of noradrenaline mediating apnea-induced hLTF, I investigated the

activity of all noradrenergic cell groups with known projections to the hypoglossal motor pool

Page 67: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

51

following apnea-induced hLTF. I did this by quantifying c-Fos expression within noradrenergic

cell groups (i.e., A1, A2, A5, A6 [LC], and A7) following induction of hLTF and compared

expression levels with a control group (i.e., animals not exposed to recurrent airway

occlusions). Specifically, noradrenergic cells that were c-Fos positive were counted in each

region. Compared to the time control group, I found that apnea-induced hLTF increased c-Fos

expression in the LC by 176 ± 7% (unpaired t-test, t(51) = 3.334, p = 0.0016; Figure 3.2); however,

I found no evidence for changes in c-Fos expression in the other noradrenergic cells groups (i.e.,

A1, A2, A5 and A7; Table 3.1), suggesting that apnea-induced hLTF only activates noradrenergic

cells in the LC.

3.3.3 Activation of noradrenergic cells in the LC is specific to hLTF responders

While the preceding experiments suggest that hLTF activates LC cells, LC activity is also

increased by hypoxic, hypercapnia and airway occlusion (Berquin et al., 2000, Haxhiu et al.,

2001, Haxhiu et al., 1996, Teppema et al., 1997), which makes it difficult to link the observed

increases in LC activity with hLTF induction. Therefore, I wanted to determine if increased LC

activity is attributable to hLTF per se, so I examined LC c-Fos expression levels in which

Page 68: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

52

recurrent apneas triggered hLTF and compared them with cases in which recurrent apneas did

not trigger hLTF (i.e. non-responders) (Figure 3.2A). I was able to make such comparisons

because hLTF does not always occur following repeated apneas. Specifically, I found that

recurrent apneas only triggered hLTF 65% of the time (i.e., 35% of the time repeated apneas did

not induce hLTF), and this rate of plasticity induction is in line with classic long-term

potentiation (LTP) studies which report that LTP only occurs 50 to 90% of the time (i.e. LTP does

not occur 10 to 50% of the time) (Abraham et al., 1993, Watanabe et al., 2002). Nonetheless, I

first wanted to ensure that the lack of hLTF expression in the non-responder group was not

influenced by changes in anaesthesia depth. Therefore, in addition to corneal and toe pinch

reflexes used throughout the experiment, I quantified the percentage of inhaled isoflurane at

the beginning and end of each experiment and recorded respiratory frequency as an index of

anaesthesia depth in the hLTF and No hLTF groups. In both cases, animals were induced at 3.5%

isoflurane and reduced after tracheostomy to maintain anaesthesia. Maintenance varied

between animals but averaged around 2.5% isoflurane (Fig. 3.3A). Similarly, no difference in

breath frequency at 60-min post apneas and presumably anaesthesia depth was observed

(unpaired t-test, t(12)=0.3227, p=0.7525) (Fig. 3.3B). Next, I compared end-tidal CO2 and oxygen

saturation levels between groups to ensure blood gases were not confounding factors in hLTF

expression. Neither ET-CO2 nor oxygen saturation levels were significantly different between

groups (2-way RM ANOVA, responders vs non-responders, ET-CO2: F = 0.7049, p=0.5933. O2

saturation: F = 0.5439, p=0.7054) (Fig. 3.3C,D). I also compared mean arterial blood pressure

between groups and found no observable difference between animals that developed hLTF and

those that did not (Fig. 3.3E). Limited numbers of non-responders with blood pressure

recordings prevented a statistical comparison. I then compared c-Fos expression between

animals that exhibited apnea-induced hLTF and those that did not (non-responders). Compared

to non-responders, we found that apnea-induced hLTF increased c-Fos expression in the LC by

186 ± 7% (apnea-induced hLTF vs non-responders: unpaired t-test, t(56) = 2.103, p=0.0399; Fig.

3.2B,C). This observation suggests that activation of noradrenergic cells in the LC is hLTF-

dependent and is not associated with the hypoxia and hypercapnia associated with apneic

stimuli.

Page 69: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

53

Figure 3.2. LC activation correlates with apnea-induced hLTF. (A) Group data showing inspiratory genioglossus amplitude after recurrent apneas in responders (i.e., apnea-induced hLTF; n = 9, white bars) and non-responders (i.e. apnea without hLTF; n = 5, grey bars) at 15, 30, 45, and 60 min. Dotted line represents percent baseline activity. Intermittent apneas produced hLTF with inspiratory genioglossus amplitude peaking at 96 ± 17% but non-responders did not exhibit hLTF following the repeated apnea intervention. (B) Group data showing LC activity in the no apnea (time control, n = 5, black bar), apneas without hLTF (“non-responders”, n = 5), and apnea-induced hLTF (n = 5) groups. Animals that exhibited hLTF had LC activity increased by 176 ± 7%. This increase is correlated with hLTF and not the apneas themselves as an identical intervention but absent hLTF did not display an increase in double-labelled cells. In fact, animals exhibiting hLTF had LC activity increase by 186 ± 7% compared to animals given an identical protocol but did not exhibit hLTF. No difference was observed between the time control and non-responder group. (C) An example of LC activity represented by c-Fos expression in the time control, non-responders, and apnea-induced hLTF groups. c-Fos expression was identified by cell nuclei stained black with DAB and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) expression was identified by red-brown NOVA-Red stain to phenotype noradrenergic cells. Magnified examples of LC neurons (black arrows) that are c-Fos negative, TH-positive (time control and non-responders) and LC neurons that are double-positive (last panel). Data is presented as mean + SEM. *Denotes a significant difference (p < 0.05).

Page 70: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

54

Figure 3.3. Levels of anesthesia, expired CO2, O2 saturation and blood pressure do not correlate with hLTF. (A) A comparison of the percentage of isoflurane used to maintain anesthesia in animals that exhibited hLTF after repeated apneas (i.e., “apnea-induced hLTF”; n = 9, white diamonds) and animals that did not exhibit hLTF after apneas (i.e., “non-responders”; n = 5, black diamonds). (B) Breath frequency of each animal expressed as a percent change from baseline did not differ between animals exhibiting apnea-induced hLTF and non-responders. No difference was observed in levels of anaesthesia, ET-CO2, oxygen saturation, and mean arterial blood pressure (ABP) in the apnea-induced hLTF and non-responder groups (A–E) suggest these factors are unlikely contributors to the lack of hLTF in the non-responder group. Dotted line represents percent baseline. Data is presented as mean + SEM.

Page 71: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

55

3.3.4 Noradrenergic LC neurons have direct projections to the hypoglossal motor pool

Having shown that noradrenergic LC cell activity is associated with apnea-induced hLTF, my

next goal was to verify that these cells project to and synapse on neurons within the

hypoglossal motor pool. Available anatomical tracing data has shown minimal to no projections

between the LC the hypoglossal motor pool (Aldes et al., 1992, Fritschy and Grzanna, 1990,

Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). By injecting CtB conjugated with AlexaFluor-488 into the

hypoglossal motor pool, I aimed to determine if noradrenergic cells (i.e., TH+ noradrenergic

cells) project to the hypoglossal motor pool. Following CtB injection into the hypoglossal motor

pool, I quantified the total number of TH positive, CtB positive, and double-labelled cells and

found that CtB injection within the hypoglossal motor pool labeled 71 ± 9% of noradrenergic

neurons in the LC (Figure 3.4A-C,G-J). However, I found that injections that were 0.2-0.8 mm

ventral to the hypoglossal motor pool did not result in CtB labeling of noradrenergic LC cells

(Figure 3.4D-F), demonstrating that noradrenergic LC cell labeling is selective to CtB injections

in the hypoglossal motor pool. This suggests that the CtB labeling observed arose from a direct

projection from the LC to the hypoglossal motor pool.

To further demonstrate that LC cells project to hypoglossal motor neurons, I also injected an

adeno-associated virus (AAV) carrying reporter protein mCherry to trace LC axonal projections

to the hypoglossal motor pool. Specifically, when AAV5-hSyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry was

injected into the LC, I found clear evidence of mCherry labelled axon terminals at the level of

the hypoglossal motor pool (Figure 3.4K). Together, these results indicate that noradrenergic LC

cells project to the hypoglossal motor pool, presumably on hypoglossal motor neurons.

Page 72: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

56

Page 73: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

57

Figure 3.4. Noradrenergic LC neurons have direct projections to the hypoglossal motor pool. A histological example of the LC 10 days after injection of cholera toxin B (CtB) conjugated with AlexaFluor-488 into the hypoglossal motor pool (n=6). (A) Cells double-labelled for cholera toxin B (CtB, green) (B) and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH, red) (C) can be observed, identifying noradrenergic LC neurons that project to the hypoglossal motor pool. Blue hue in overlay represents DAPI staining. CtB injected ventral to the hypoglossal motor pool (n=7) did not double-label cells in the LC (D) as no CtB positive cells were observed (E), but TH positive cells are still present (F). Magnified images of cells positive for CtB (H), TH (I), DAPI (J), or triple-labelled cells (G). Triple-labelled neurons are indicated by the white arrows, suggesting noradrenergic cells in the LC directly project to the hypoglossal motor pool. To verify this connection, AAV5-hsyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry injected into the LC showed visible axon terminals at the level of the hypoglossal motor pool (K). Differences in hue in the images above are attributed to different filters used on different microscopes. CC, central canal.

3.3.5 Bilateral inactivation of the LC prevented apnea-induced hLTF

Having demonstrated that noradrenergic LC cells were activated during hLTF and that they

project to the hypoglossal motor pool, my next step was to determine if noradrenergic LC cells

mediate LTF of genioglossus muscle activity. To do this, I pharmacologically inactivated LC cells

by focally injecting clonidine into the left and right LC nuclei. However, before this I wanted to

verify that bolus fluid injections alone did not influence the expression of apnea-induced hLTF.

Therefore, I injected an equal volume of vehicle (Ringer’s) into the left and right LC nuclei 30

min before recurrent airway occlusions. I found that vehicle injections had no effect on

expression of apnea-induced hLTF (RM ANOVA, F=6.476, p=0.0008) with inspiratory

genioglossus activity increasing up to 40 ± 3% above baseline levels (Fig. 3.5D). However,

compared to vehicle injections, I found that inactivating noradrenergic LC cells with bilateral

clonidine microinjection prevented apnea-induced hLTF (RM ANOVA, F=0.5975, p=0.6686; Fig.

3.5C,D), with inspiratory genioglossus activity remaining with baseline levels during the 60 min

period following injection. This observation suggests that noradrenergic LC cells are required

for driving the expression of apnea-induced hLTF. Post-mortem histology was used to confirm

that injections were located within the left and right LC nuclei (Fig. 3.5A).

To ensure that clonidine applied to the LC did not suppress baseline genioglossus activity we

compared baseline genioglossus activity before and after clonidine application, and found no

significant effect of clonidine on genioglossus activity (unpaired t-test, t(10) = 0.5490, p=0.5950;

Page 74: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

58

Fig. 3.5B). This suggests that the absence of hLTF after clonidine treatment does not stem from

suppressed genioglossus activity.

Page 75: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

59

Figure 3.5. Inactivation of the LC prevented apnea-induced hLTF. (A) Probe tract locations in the LC following bilateral microinjection of clonidine. Green circles represent clonidine-treated animals and blue circles represent vehicle controls. (B) Group data showing inspiratory genioglossus (GG) activity before (BL) is not significantly different after clonidine application at the LC (BL w/CLO). (C) A representative EMG traces of integrated inspiratory genioglossus (GG) activity after LC inactivation at baseline, 15, 30, 45, and 60 min after repeated apneas. (D) Group data showing inspiratory genioglossus activity in the vehicle-treated (n=6, white bars) and clonidine-treated (n=8, black bars) animals at 15, 30, 45, and 60 min following repeated apneas. Dotted line represents average baseline activity. Intermittent apneas induced a peak of 40 ± 13% increase in inspiratory genioglossus amplitude (i.e., hLTF) in vehicle-treated animals, but in the presence of clonidine, LC cells were inactivated effectively abolishing apnea-induced hLTF. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. * denotes a significant difference (p<0.05).

3.5 Discussion

This study is scientifically important because it contributes to our understanding of circuit and

transmitter mechanisms underlying respiratory motor plasticity. Here, I identified a tripartite

circuit that underlies apnea-induced LTF of genioglossus motor activity. It has already been

shown that hLTF requires noradrenaline (Tadjalli, 2012), the activation of NTS neurons

(Torontali, 2012), and the activation of α1-adrenergic receptors on hypoglossal motor neurons

(Tadjalli et al., 2010). Here, I reaffirm that repeated apneas can elicit hLTF, which has allowed

me to further investigate the brainstem regions that may be involved in mediating hLTF. Using

c-Fos expression as an index of cell activity, I found that noradrenergic cells in the LC were

activated during apnea-induced hLTF, suggesting that they could be the neural substrate that

triggers noradrenaline release onto hypoglossal motor neurons. I then show that hypoglossal

motor neurons receive noradrenergic inputs from the LC, indicating that hLTF could be

triggered by noradrenaline released from the LC onto hypoglossal motor neurons. Lastly, I

inactivated the LC and found repeated apneas could no longer elicit hLTF. These results suggest

that a tripartite circuit in the brainstem (i.e., NTS ➔ LC ➔ XII) is responsible for triggering

apnea-induced LTF of genioglossus motor output.

3.5.1 Noradrenergic cells in the LC are active during hLTF

Hypoglossal motor neurons receive noradrenergic inputs from several different cells groups,

including the A1, A5, A7, LC, and subcoeruleus (Aldes et al., 1992, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007),

indicating that hLTF could be triggered by noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons from

Page 76: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

60

one or more of these noradrenergic nuclei. Using c-Fos expression as an index of noradrenergic

activity, I found that noradrenergic cells in the LC were activated by apnea-induced hLTF,

suggesting that they could be the neural substrate that triggers noradrenaline release onto

hypoglossal motor neurons.

However, unlike a previous report (Song and Poon, 2017), I found no evidence to indicate that

noradrenergic cells in either the A5 or A7 nuclei were activated following hLTF induction.

Methodological differences are the likely reason for the differences between our observations.

Here, I probed c-Fos expression levels 90 min after hLTF induction, whereas, Song and Poon

examined expression levels 20 min after hLTF induction. I quantified c-Fos expression 90 min

following hLTF induction because c-Fos levels are maximal 60-90 min following stimulus-

induced neuronal activation (Bullitt, 1990, Morgan et al., 1987). My choice to identify c-Fos

protein expression matched the time frame for hLTF persisting for >60 minutes post

intervention. This is in contrast to other biomarkers for neuronal activation such as ERK/pERK

(expression window within 2-10 minutes) (Gao and Ji, 2009), Arc (expression window within 5-

30 minutes from activation) (Guzowski et al., 1999), or Zif268 (expression window within 15-60

minutes from activation) (Guzowski et al., 1999). As such, it may be possible my protocol

captured the brain regions required to sustain hLTF, while the A5 and A7 cell groups may be

involved in the trigger. Differences between my results and those of Song and Poon could also

stem from the use of different anaesthetics, which are known to influence c-Fos expression

(Dragunow et al., 1990, Roda et al., 2004). Alternatively, it may be possible that the initial

increase in noradrenaline release at the hypoglossal motor pool recruited the A5 and A7, but

persistent hLTF (i.e. hLTF persisting for more than 20 minutes) requires the LC. This synergy

between noradrenergic cell groups was observed before. For example, neonatal rats receive

stronger inhibitory inputs from the A5 on the respiratory rhythm generator (RRG), while the

RRG is simultaneously being modulated by excitatory noradrenergic input from the LC in

adulthood (Dobbins and Feldman, 1994, Viemari et al., 2004a, Viemari et al., 2004b).

Page 77: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

61

3.5.2 Noradrenergic LC neurons project to the hypoglossal motor pool

Current literature reported limited to no anatomical connections between the LC neurons and

the hypoglossal motor neurons (Aldes, 1990, Aldes et al., 1992, Aston-Jones et al., 1970,

Fritschy and Grzanna, 1990, Levitt and Moore, 1979, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). At most, it

was reported that only 1.7% of the noradrenergic input to the hypoglossal motor pool

originates from the LC (Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). However, I suspected that the percent of

noradrenergic LC neurons to be larger than previous reports as LC activity correlates with

genioglossus activity (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981, Chan et al., 2006, Hakuno et al., 2004). It

may be possible that previous tracing studies missed aspects of the hypoglossal motor pool that

may be innervated by the LC. I found that the CtB injected into the hypoglossal motor pool

labelled 71% ± 9.23% of neurons in the LC. These values were higher than previously reported

so to ensure accuracy of these findings, I injected CtB ventral or rostral to the hypoglossal

motor pool to act as off-target controls. When CtB was injected ventral to the hypoglossal

motor pool, I found no double-labelled LC neurons (Fig. 3.4). To further support the premise

that the LC has direct projections to the hypoglossal motor pool, LC neurons were infected with

a virus carrying a fluorescent reporter protein, mCherry. In these animals, 44% of noradrenergic

LC neurons were infected and axon terminals could easily be visualized at the level of the

hypoglossal motor pool. This is in direct contradiction with anterograde tracing studies that

claim the LC does not project to brainstem motor pools (Fritschy and Grzanna, 1990). However,

their approach with PHA-L was limited by time. PHA-L was reported to provide limited

expression in anterogradely labelled cells following 2-4 weeks post injections, with no labelled

cells by 9 weeks (Kott et al., 1991). Fritschy and Grzanna claimed no direct projections from the

LC to brainstem motor pools, but sacrificed the animal at the 2-3 week mark, suggesting the LC

to hypoglossal connections may have been missed with this approach.

3.5.3 LC activation is independent of hypoxia or hypercapnia associated with repeated apneas

My results indicate that noradrenergic LC cell activity is associated with hLTF expression, but

because LC cells are responsive to obstructive apnea, hypercapnia and hypoxia (Berquin et al.,

Page 78: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

62

2000, Haxhiu et al., 1996, Teppema et al., 1997), it is possible that changes in LC activity

following recurrent apneas could result for apneic and/or hypercapnia/hypoxic stimuli rather

than hLTF itself. To indirectly test this possibility, I examined c-Fos in cases where recurrent

apneas did not elicit LTF of inspiratory genioglossus activity (i.e., 5 of 14 cases). I found no

evidence for increased c-Fos expression in LC cells when recurrent apneas did not trigger hLTF,

which contrasts with the robust increase in c-Fos expression when recurrent apneas triggered

hLTF (Fig. 3.2B,C). This is important because the increased activity in the LC was strictly limited

to animals that exhibited hLTF suggesting that activation of noradrenergic LC cells is not caused

by chemical stimuli associated with repeated apneas; instead, LC activation is linked to hLTF

induction per se. I interpret these findings to indicate that increased LC activity is the causal

mechanism that triggers LTF of genioglossus activity because: (1) noradrenergic cells are only

active when hLTF is triggered by apneas (i.e., increased c-Fos in animals that exhibit hLTF; Fig.

3.2B,C); (2) that noradrenergic LC neurons project to hypoglossal motor neurons (Fig. 3.4); and,

(3) inactivation of LC cells during apneas prevented hLTF (Fig. 3.5). Based on these pieces of

evidence and the fact that apneas act through vagal afferents and NTS neurons, which in turn

project to LC neurons (Lopes et al., 2016, Tadjalli et al., 2010), I claim that recurrent apneas

activate LC neurons to trigger apnea-induced hLTF. As presented and interpreted, my data are

consistent with previously published studies showing that increases in c-Fos expression can be

used to identify cells associated with inducing other forms of plasticity, such as granule cells

and their role in mediating long-term potentiation (LTP) in the cerebellum (Gandolfi et al.,

2017), or parvalbumin basket cells and their role in regulating plasticity (Karunakaran et al.,

2016).

3.5.4 The LC is a required component of the neural circuit underlying apnea-induced hLTF

Apnea-induced hLTF is is mediated by a noradrenergic-dependent mechanism (Lui et al., 2018,

Tadjalli et al., 2010). However, the source of noradrenaline responsible for mediating hLTF

remained, until now, unidentified. Here, I provide evidence indicating that LC cells are the likely

source of noradrenaline release onto hypoglossal motor neurons that ultimately underlies LTF

Page 79: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

63

of inspiratory genioglossus activity. First, I provide correlative evidence showing that LC cells

are recruited during hLTF (Fig. 3.2B,C). I also show that noradrenergic LC cells project to and

innervate hypoglossal motor neurons (Fig. 3.4), confirming previous reports that LC neurons

innervate hypoglossal motor neurons (Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). Taken together, these

observations suggest that recurrent obstructive apneas activate LC cells, which in turn release

noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons thereby triggering hLTF. Most importantly, I

showed that pharmacologically inactivating LC cells prevented the expression of apnea-induced

hLTF. This observation provides support for the causal link between apnea-induced LC

activation and its contribution to triggering LTF of inspiratory genioglossus activity. LC neurons

are anatomically and functionally poised to mediate hLTF and are, therefore, likely the source

of noradrenaline acting on hypoglossal motor neurons.

Although focal inactivation of the LC blocked hLTF expression, it did not suppress genioglossus

activity as LC inactivation did not reduce baseline inspiratory genioglossus activity, suggesting

that LC cells release negligible amounts of noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons

during anaesthetized conditions. Because previous studies show that inspiratory hypoglossal

motor outflow is facilitated by an endogenous noradrenergic drive during both natural

behaviors and anaesthesia, it is likely that other noradrenergic nuclei (e.g., A5 and/or A7 cell

groups) provide this drive (Fenik et al., 2008, Song and Poon, 2017).

3.5.5 Brainstem regions associated with apnea-induced hLTF

To date, most studies have examined the molecular pathways that trigger respiratory hLTF

within motor neurons (Devinney et al., 2015, Huxtable et al., 2014, Kinkead et al., 2001,

Neverova et al., 2007, Song and Poon, 2017), but my current study is important because it

identifies an intact circuit underlying apnea-induced respiratory motor plasticity. I propose that

hLTF is mediated by a tripartite circuit (NTS → LC → XII) and I base this on 5 lines of evidence:

1) apnea-induced hLTF is noradrenaline-dependent (Tadjalli, 2012); 2) inactivation of the NTS

prevents apnea-induced hLTF (Torontali, 2012); 3) noradrenergic LC cells were only active when

Page 80: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

64

hLTF is triggered by apneas (i.e., increased c-Fos in animals that exhibit hLTF; Fig. 3.2); 4)

noradrenergic LC neurons project to hypoglossal motor neurons (Fig. 3.4); and, 5) inactivation

of LC cells during apneas prevented hLTF (Fig. 3.5D). Based on these results I propose that

recurrent obstructive apneas – similar to those experienced in obstructive sleep apnea –

modulate vagal feedback, which activates NTS cells. These cells in turn manipulate LC cells

(Aston-Jones et al., 1970, Lopes et al., 2016, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007), which episodically

releases noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons thereby triggering motor neuron

plasticity and hence LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor activity (Fig. 3.6).

Page 81: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

65

Figure 3.6. Hypothesized circuitry responsible for hLTF. (A) Brain map showing the potential sources of noradrenaline mediating hLTF (red arrows). The locus coeruleus (LC) was activated alongside hLTF and the hypothesized circuit is outlined in purple (inset) and expanded in (B). Repeated obstructive apneas modulate vagal afferent activity, which terminates in the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS). Cells in the NTS send excitatory projections to the noradrenergic cells of the locus coeruleus (LC), which in turn extend axons directly to the hypoglossal (XII) motor pool to modulate hypoglossal (and therefore genioglossus) activity, effectively triggering hypoglossal long-term facilitation (hLTF). I hypothesize that this is the neural circuit underlying LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor output.

3.5.6 Methodological considerations

The c-Fos protein, though useful in identifying neural networks (Lu et al., 2006), undoubtedly

has limitations. The most notable is the inability to detect cells that have been inactivated or

inhibited (Chan and Sawchenko, 1994). This is important because hypercapnia can

hyperpolarize neurons (Ritucci et al., 1997); my intervention introduced a low hypercapnic load

but it is possible for some cell groups to have been hyperpolarized or inactivated. These

Page 82: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

66

neurons would not have been visualized by my approach. Furthermore, not all activated

neurons express c-Fos. For example, hypercapnic load increases respiratory output without

changing c-Fos expression in phrenic or hypoglossal motor neurons (Haxhiu et al., 1996). It is

therefore possible for our proposed neural circuit to exclude regions that also facilitate the

elicitation of apnea-induced hLTF. For example, the A5 and A7 are noradrenergic structures

with known reciprocal connections to the LC, as well as direct connections to the hypoglossal

motor pool (Aldes et al., 1992, Byrum, 1987, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007). It may therefore be

possible the involvement of these structures, as well as others, were not captured by my

approach but may still play a role in mediating hLTF. Nonetheless, my results suggest the LC to

be involved in apnea-induced hLTF.

Other methodological considerations include my choice in clonidine. In this study, I chose

clonidine due to its specificity to agonizing α2-adrenergic receptors. Other candidate drugs such

as yohimbine, simultaneously activates α1-adrenergic receptors and 5HT receptors, both of

which are present on LC neurons (Nakamura et al., 1988, Szabo et al., 2000). In addition, I chose

a dose that was found effective at decreasing overall noradrenaline levels in the brain to 30% of

baseline levels (Mair et al., 2005, Sakamoto et al., 2013), while maintaining a small enough

volume to confidently deliver the drug to a confined region. Nonetheless, perfusion of CLO can

spread beyond the targeted region of interest. It is possible that regions outside the LC were

affected, which could have influenced the manifestation of hLTF. For example, the Kolliker-Fuse

is a respiratory group that is rostral to the LC and may have been influenced by CLO perfusion,

thereby affecting the manifestation of apnea-induced hLTF. As such, it is critical that future

steps manipulate the LC with more precision, such as that of an optogenetic approach.

One caveat to this study is that LC inhibition can increase blood pressure by ~10 mmHg

(Bhaskaran and Freed, 1988), potentially influencing the absence of hLTF observed. However,

hLTF can still be generated even when blood pressure is elevated by 10–15 mmHg (Tadjalli,

2012), suggesting that increased blood pressure does not prevent apnea-induced hLTF

expression. My results are therefore physiologically important because they suggest that the

Page 83: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

67

absence of hLTF following LC inactivation is not due to blood pressure changes but is due to

reduced noradrenergic drive to hypoglossal motor neurons.

Lastly, the largest caveat in this study is the comparison of clonidine-treated animals to hLTF-

expressing animals following repeated obstructive apneas. The field of LTF has always selected

animals that express LTF and intentionally separated them from non-responders. However, in

the study using focal inactivation of LC cells with clonidine, all animals were included in

statistical comparisons. This is problematic as I had reported that 35% of animals do not exhibit

hLTF following repeated apneas, which implies that the same percentage of animals would not

have exhibited hLTF regardless of the clonidine intervention. One potential solution would be

to identify animals that presumably would exhibit hLTF. For example, I showed that hLTF was

correlated with increased c-Fos expression at the LC, indicative of LC activation. It may be

possible to only include animals that show high c-Fos expression in the LC in the clonidine-

treated group. Unfortunately, my intervention involved focal inactivation of LC cells and, thus,

the increase in c-Fos expression observed in the LC of hLTF-expressing animals cannot be used

as a marker to distinguish responders from non-responders. In the next chapter, I introduce an

alternative statistical approach to address this issue.

3.5.7 Scientific importance and clinical significance

LTF is a form of neural plasticity that directly affects respiratory output. Apnea-induced hLTF is

unique in that its effects are localized to the upper airway (genioglossus), can be triggered in

vivo, uses a physiologically relevant trigger (repeated obstructive apneas), and occurs naturally.

hLTF is a form of respiratory plasticity that requires a trigger, where the plasticity itself occurs

on its own. Understanding the neural circuit underlying this plasticity is therefore critical to

opening a vast number of pharmacological approaches to augmenting genioglossus activity.

This is particularly relevant during sleep where muscle tone is decreased, potentially leading to

obstructive sleep apneas in humans (OSA) (Aboubakr et al., 2001, Fung and Chase, 2015,

Horner, 2008). These patients either do not exhibit hLTF, or their manifestation of hLTF is

Page 84: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

68

insufficient at overcoming the apnea. In fact, LC cells are largely inactive during sleep (Aston-

Jones and Bloom, 1981, Fung et al., 1991, Szymusiak and McGinty, 2008), suggesting that the

circuits required for hLTF are dysfunctional during sleep. OSA has also been linked to a loss of

LC cells (Lim and Veasey, 2010), further supporting our findings that the LC is vital in providing

the noradrenergic drive onto hypoglossal motor neurons to mediate hLTF. Although respiratory

LTF was identified in humans, its potential role in mitigating reduced genioglossus muscle tone

in OSA remains to be determined. A recent study demonstrated LTF in humans could not be

elicited using a chemical trigger (hypercapnic/hypoxia) (Deacon et al., 2017). My results are

particularly important in this context because I have defined the potential neural circuitry that

mediates hLTF, providing novel therapeutic targets that could elicit hLTF. Here I show that hLTF

depends on noradrenergic LC cell activity, independent of hypercapnia/hypoxia, suggesting this

mechanism could be targeted to treat OSA.

Page 85: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

69

Chapter Four – Optical LC Stimulation Triggers LTF of Inspiratory Genioglossus Motor Output

4.1 Summary

Respiratory long-term facilitation of genioglossus motor output (hLTF) is a form of respiratory

motor plasticity that can be elicited by repeated apneas or repeated bouts of hypoxia. Here, I

demonstrate that hLTF can be triggered in the absence of repeated apneas or hypoxia by

intermittently stimulating locus coeruleus (LC) neurons. In cells expressing channelrhodopsin2

(ChR2), I show that repeated optical stimulation of the LC in the same pattern as repeated

obstructive apneas can elicit hLTF. I also show that the LC is critical for hLTF as optical

inactivation of LC cells expressing halorhodopsin (eNpHR) prevented apnea-induced hLTF.

These results implicate the LC as being a critical component mediating hLTF. I then go on to use

c-Fos expression as an indicator of cell activity and found evidence to suggest that a minimum

stimulation threshold is required to elicit hLTF. I, therefore, propose that hLTF requires the

activation of the LC to release noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons to trigger hLTF.

4.2 Introduction

Respiratory LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor output can be elicited by intermittent bouts

of hypoxia (Baker and Mitchell, 2000, Chowdhuri et al., 2015, Dale et al., 2017). However, it has

shown that LTF of hypoglossal/genioglossus activity can be elicited solely through the broncho-

pulmonary feedback system, such as through intermittent cooling of vagal afferents (Tadjalli et

al., 2010). More recently, hLTF was elicited transiently for 20 minutes while bypassing both the

broncho-pulmonary and chemosensory feedback altogether by directly stimulating the A5 or A7

noradrenergic cell group (Song and Poon, 2017). Here, I demonstrate that hLTF can be elicited

in the absence of repeated apneas or repeated bouts of hypoxia by using intermittent optical

stimulation of the LC, eliciting hLTF that persists for 60 min post intervention.

Page 86: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

70

4.2.1 Triggers of LTF that act through the chemosensory or broncho-pulmonary feedback

system

To understand the mechanism underlying the trigger for hLTF, it is necessary to distinguish the

various factors that induce it. As mentioned earlier, the most common trigger involves

intermittent bouts of hypoxia to elicit LTF. This approach, 3 bouts of hypoxia (i.e., FiO2 10%)

separated by 5 minutes of normoxic breathing, is the most robust at eliciting phrenic LTF (pLTF)

(Dale et al., 2017, Devinney et al., 2015, Dodig et al., 2012) but is less consistent at eliciting

hypoglossal/genioglossus LTF (hLTF) (Baker-Herman and Strey, 2011, Janssen and Fregosi,

2000), though still possible (Fuller, 2005, Harris et al., 2006). This contrasts triggers used for

hLTF; repeated apneas or intermittent vagal cooling can elicit LTF of genioglossus muscle

activity but do not trigger pLTF (Tadjalli et al., 2010). This suggests that pLTF largely interacts

with the chemosensory feedback system whereas hLTF interacts with both chemosensory and

broncho-pulmonary feedback systems. Here I demonstrate a trigger that does not interact with

either feedback systems to trigger LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor activity.

4.2.2 Triggers of LTF independent of the chemosensory and broncho-pulmonary feedback

systems

To date, only one study has shown a trigger that does not interact with the respiratory

feedback systems in an in vivo model (Song and Poon, 2017). Song and Poon (2017)

demonstrated a form of hypoglossal/genioglossus LTF persisting for 20 minutes following

intermittent optical stimulation of the A5 or A7 noradrenergic cell group in anaesthetized,

spontaneously breathing rats. Although the duration of hLTF persisted for 20 minutes, differing

from the most commonly reported form of LTF that lasts 60 minutes, this was the first

demonstration of hLTF that was elicited in the absence of chemosensory or broncho-pulmonary

feedback in vivo. Here I demonstrate that prolonged hLTF persisting for 60 minutes can be

triggered by intermittent optical stimulation of the LC.

Page 87: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

71

4.2.3 The LC and its potential role in hLTF

In Chapter 3, I showed that c-Fos expression is associated with apnea-induced hLTF expression,

suggesting that apnea-induced hLTF may be induced by activation of noradrenergic LC cells,

which release noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons. I also demonstrated that the LC

is critical for the manifestation of hLTF as its inactivation prevented apnea-induced hLTF. The

role of the LC in mediating hLTF has not been tested directly, therefore my next goal was to

determine whether activation of noradrenergic LC cells alone can trigger hLTF. Specifically, LC

neurons expressing ChR2 via infection with a viral vector (AAV5-hSyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry)

were activated using intermittent light pulses.

It is important to note that the activity of LC cells before, during, or after hLTF expression has

not been reported. As a result, it is not possible to mimic the exact pattern of LC activity using

light. However, LC cells have been reported to be maximally active at 5 Hz (Kogan et al., 1992).

Therefore, I aimed to optically stimulate ChR2-expressing LC cells at 5 Hz. In addition, since

repeated apneas can trigger hLTF, I aimed to optically stimulate the LC in the same pattern as

repeated obstructive apneas (i.e., 10 episodes of stimulation, each 15 seconds in duration

separated by 1 minute).

4.3 Results

4.3.1 LC cells equally infected by viral vectors across all groups

Before investigating whether optical manipulation of LC neurons influences hLTF, I first wanted

to determine whether there was an equal viral infection rate in animals that received the empty

viral vector (AAV5-hSyn-mCherry), or the viral vector containing stimulating opsin (AAV5-hSyn-

ChR2(H134R)-mCherry) or inhibitory opsin (AAV5-hSyn-eNpHR3.0-mCherry). This is important

as it addresses the quality of the viral vectors used across all treatment groups. To determine

whether LC neurons were equally infected, I quantified the total number of cells expressing the

Page 88: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

72

mCherry reporter protein in the LC across all groups. Specifically, all mCherry positive cells were

manually counted for each experimental group with the experimenter blinded to the treatment

(see Chapter 2, section 2.9 for additional details). My cell counts revealed no difference in

mCherry expression across all groups (1-way ANOVA, F=1.153, p=0.3333 Figure 4.1A,B),

suggesting viral infection rates were equal for all animals used in this study.

Next, I aimed to determine the percentage of virally infected noradrenergic neurons. The viral

vector used here non-specifically targets all cells, but my goal is to determine whether

noradrenergic LC neurons mediate hLTF. Therefore, I quantified the number of virally infected

(i.e., mCherry expressing) cells co-expressing tyrosine hydroxylase and found that

approximately 61 ± 14% of noradrenergic LC cells were infected.

Lastly, I aimed to verify that virally infected cells expressing ChR2 or eNpHR were functional

(i.e., responded to light exposure). To determine whether the virally-expressed ion channel,

ChR2, was functional within LC neurons, I optically stimulated LC neurons (432nm, 40mW, 15s

at 5 Hz separated by 1 minute of no stimulation repeated 10 times) and found c-Fos expression

in LC cells to be increased by 49 ± 13% compared to animals absent ChR2 (unpaired t-test, LC

Stim. vs Non-ChR2-expressing mCherry, t(7)=4.326, p=0.0035, Fig. 4.1C,D), suggesting light

exposure on LC neurons did increase LC activity. To determine whether the virally-expressed

ion channel, eNpHR, was functional within LC neurons, I optically inhibited LC neurons

expressing (532nm, 40mW, continuously) while simultaneously attempted to activate LC cells

using apnea-induced hLTF (15s apneas separated by 1 minute repeated 10 times). Following

this intervention, I found fewer c-Fos positive neurons in the LC of animals with the LC

inactivated compared to the LC of animals absent the inhibitory opsin and exhibiting hLTF

(unpaired t-test, LC inactivation + apneas vs Non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry + apneas,

t(8)=4.305, p=0.0026, Fig. 4.1E), suggesting light exposure on LC neurons inhibited their activity.

Page 89: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

73

Page 90: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

74

Figure 4.1. Increased or decreased c-Fos expression following light-induced manipulation to ChR2- or eNpHR-expressing LC cells. Following microinjection of a viral construct AAV5-hSyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry (n=13), AAV5-hSyn-eNpHR3.0-mCherry (n=6), or AAV5-hSyn-mCherry (n=6), expression of mCherry, tyrosine hydroxylase (TH), and c-Fos was quantified with experimenters blinded to the treatment. All infected animals expressed an equal number of mCherry positive cells (A,B). (C) Histological example showing LC neurons that are mCherry positive (red) to identify virally infected cells, TH positive (green) to identify noradrenergic cells, the overlay (third panel), and c-Fos (black) to identified activated cells. Visualization of c-Fos was under bright field and therefore could not be overlaid with fluorescent images in earlier panels. White arrows point to infected noradrenergic cells that were activated (i.e., c-Fos positive). Yellow arrow points to infected noradrenergic cell that was considered c-Fos negative (i.e., not activated). (D) Cells infected with the excitatory opsin, ChR2 (n=6, blue), displayed a significant increase in c-Fos expression following intermittent light stimulation compared to animals absent any opsin (non-ChR2-expressing mCherry, n=3, white). Approximate location of c-Fos positive cells in the LC (dotted outline) are shown on the right for animals absent any opsin (non-ChR2-expressing mCherry) and animals expressing ChR2. (E) Cells infected with the inhibitory opsin, eNpHR (n=6, green), displayed a significant decrease in c-Fos expression following light exposure and repeated obstructive apneas compared to animals absent any opsin (non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry + Apneas, n=4, white) but received light exposure and repeated apneas. Approximate location of c-Fos positive cells are shown on the right for animals absent any opsin (non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry) and animals expressing eNpHR following repeated apneas. *denotes significance (p<0.05) between groups. Data presented as mean + SEM.

4.3.2. Baseline genioglossus motor activity is decreased during optical inactivation of eNpHR-

expressing LC neurons

My next goal was to determine if LC stimulation or inhibition affected respiratory output.

Therefore, to address this, I quantified respiratory output directly after optical manipulation of

LC cells. I hypothesized that hypoglossal motor neurons would increase in activity following LC

stimulation because hypoglossal motor neurons had been previously demonstrated to increase

in activity following application of phenylephrine or norepineprine (Chan et al., 2006, Neverova

et al., 2007, Parkis et al., 1995). However, when LC neurons were exposed to intermittent light

stimulation, I found no change in genioglossus amplitude, or any other respiratory variable

measured (paired t-test, genioglossus: t(8)=0.8608, p=0.4144, diaphragm: t(3)=1.511, p=0.2279,

respiratory frequency: t(3)=1.965, p=0.1442, O2 saturation: t(3)=0.1060, p=0.9223, end-tidal CO2:

t(2)=0.3183, p=0.7804. Fig. 4.2A,B,E). The lack of increase in genioglossus output during LC

stimulation may be due to insufficient noradrenaline release to augment genioglossus motor

output. Similarly, no change in genioglossus output was observed in a study that optically

stimulated noradrenergic groups (A5 and A7), which has greater noradrenergic innervations to

Page 91: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

75

the hypoglossal motor pool (Song and Poon, 2017). In contrast, when eNpHR-expressing LC

neurons were inactivated with a continuous pulse of light, I found genioglossus amplitude

decreased by 19 ± 7% (paired t-test, t(8)=2.506, p=0.0366, Fig. 4.2C,D), but no change was

observed in any other respiratory variable measured (paired t-test, diaphragm: t(3)=0.5763,

p=0.6048, respiratory frequency: t(3)=1.708, p=0.1862, O2 saturation: t(3)=0.2496, p=0.8190,

end-tidal CO2: t(3)=0.3068, p=0.3068. Fig. 4.2E). The decrease in genioglossus motor output

following optical inhibition of eNpHR-expressing LC neurons suggest that LC neurons provide an

endogenous noradrenergic drive to hypoglossal motor neurons.

Page 92: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

76

Page 93: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

77

Figure 4.2. Genioglossus motor output was decreased during optical LC inactivation. (A) A raw trace of integrated genioglossus amplitude (∫GG EMG) recorded from an anaesthetized spontaneously breathing rat, before (baseline) and during LC stimulation (dotted blue line). Bottom panel depicts a higher temporal resolution of genioglossus amplitude during the switch from light off to light on. (B) Group data (n=11) comparing genioglossus amplitude at baseline (black bar) and during intermittent LC stimulation (blue bar), where no significant difference was observed. (C) A raw trace showing genioglossus activity before (baseline) and during LC inactivation (green line). Bottom panel shows higher temporal resolution genioglossus activity before and after the laser was turned on and the subsequent decrease in genioglossus amplitude. (D) In the group data (n=9), baseline genioglossus amplitude (black bar) was significantly decreased following LC inactivation (green bar) by 19 ± 7% from baseline. (E) In both groups (LC activation or inactivation), integrated diaphragm amplitude, respiratory frequency, oxygen saturation levels and end-tidal CO2 levels were unaffected. Data presented as mean + SEM. *denotes significance (p<0.05) between groups.

4.3.3 LTF of genioglossus motor output is elicited after intermittent stimulation of ChR2-

expressing LC neurons

My next goal was to investigate whether intermittent stimulation of LC neurons could trigger

hLTF. I hypothesized that the activation of the LC would cause intermittent release of

noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons to trigger hLTF. To test this hypothesis, I

modelled a stimulation protocol that is similar to repeated obstructive apneas by optically

stimulating LC neurons in the same pattern (i.e., stimulation at 5 Hz for 15 seconds separated

by 1 minute of no stimulation, repeated 10 times). Following this optical stimulation protocol, I

found that repeated optical stimulation of ChR2-expressing LC neurons increased genioglossus

amplitude by 41 ± 10% at 60 min post-stimulation (1-way RM ANOVA, F=3.629, p=0.0167, Fig.

4.3A-C), suggesting intermittent LC stimulation can trigger hLTF.

The viral vector used was non-specific and therefore infected all neurons. As a result, the

location of the optic probe tip is critically important in determining whether the LC is truly being

stimulated. To address the non-specific nature of the viral vector used, I stimulated cells 0.2-0.4

mm lateral to the LC in an identical fashion (Fig. 4.3G). I found that repeated stimulation of cells

outside the LC had no effect on genioglossus activity (1-way RM ANOVA, F=3.898, p=0.8289,

Figure 4.3C), or any effect on other respiratory variables measured (2-way RM ANOVA,

diaphragm: F=0.5053, p=0.7320, respiratory frequency: F=1.329, p=0.2726, O2 saturation:

Page 94: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

78

F=0.5756, p=0.6820, Figure 4.3D-F). This suggests that the manifestation of hLTF was due to the

stimulation of LC cells, and not the cells surrounding the LC.

Page 95: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

79

Page 96: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

80

Figure 4.3. LTF of genioglossus motor activity is elicited after intermittent optical stimulation of LC neurons. (A) Integrated inspiratory genioglossus motor output (∫GG EMG) recorded from an anaesthetized spontaneously breathing rat, depicting baseline genioglossus amplitude and the subsequent increase in EMG amplitude after intermittent LC stimulation (i.e., hLTF). (B) High-temporal resolution EMG traces showing genioglossus activity at baseline, 15, 30, 45, and 60 min after intermittent LC stimulation. (C) Group data showing an increase in inspiratory genioglossus activity following intermittent LC stimulation (n=8, blue bars) compared to animals given the same intervention but outside of the LC nuclei (i.e., Off-Target Stimulation, n=6, black bars) at 15, 30, 45, and 60 min. Dotted line represents percent baseline activity. Intermittent LC stimulation induced an increase in inspiratory genioglossus amplitude of 41 ± 10% at 60 minutes. In both groups, inspiratory diaphragm amplitude (∫Dia EMG), respiratory frequency, and oxygen saturation levels were unaffected over time (D-F). (G) Optic probe tract locations for animals that received on-target LC stimulation (blue circles), or off-target stimulation (grey circles). Dotted outline represents the LC. Each shade represents an individual animal. Data is presented as mean + SEM. * denotes a significant difference (p<0.05) from baseline.

4.3.4 Intermittent light exposure on non-ChR2-expressing mCherry LC neurons does not

trigger hLTF

Recent reports have suggested that light exposure in naïve mice (i.e., mice absent any opsin)

can cause heat-induced cell damage (Qian and Gu, 2005), heat-induced cell firing (Reig et al.,

2010, Stujenske et al., 2015), and photodilation of blood vessels (Rungta et al., 2017). To

control for the potential adverse effects caused by light exposure, I performed an identical

stimulation protocol in animals infected with a control viral vector (i.e., AAV5-hSyn-mCherry).

This viral vector is identical to the viral vector used in the treatment group but absent the light-

sensitive ion channel, ChR2. Repeated light exposure should, therefore, have no effect on LC

cells and thus should not trigger hLTF.

Here, I demonstrated that optical exposure of non-ChR2-expressing mCherry LC neurons did

not affect respiratory activity throughout the recording period and that light exposure had no

effect on genioglossus activity throughout the 60-minute time period (1-way RM ANOVA, Non-

ChR2-expressing mCherry, F=0.7763, p=0.5535, Fig. 4.4 C,D). Furthermore, no changes were

observed in any other respiratory variables measured (2-way RM ANOVA, Int. LC Stim. vs Non-

ChR2-expressing mCherry, diaphragm amplitude: F=0.2242, p=0.9233, respiratory frequency:

F=0.8425, p=0.5058, O2 saturation: F=0.5823, p=0.6772. Fig. 4.4E-G). These data indicate that

Page 97: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

81

light per se is not responsible for hLTF and as demonstrated in section 4.3.3, hLTF is induced by

intermittent activation of LC neurons themselves.

4.3.5 hLTF expression requires an intermittent pattern of LC stimulation

Previous studies have demonstrated that an intermittent stimulus is required to elicit plasticity

(Baker and Mitchell, 2000, Bliss and Lomo, 1973). In the context of LTF, repeated episodes of

hypoxia were required whereas a single continuous episode of hypoxia did not elicit pLTF

(Baker and Mitchell, 2000). This was again demonstrated when intermittent cooling of vagal

afferents triggered hLTF, but a single cooling episode of equal duration did not trigger hLTF

(Tadjalli, 2012). I wanted to determine whether the intermittent nature of optical stimulation

was required to trigger hLTF. Here, I show that continuous light exposure encompassing the

duration of the optical stimulation protocol did not trigger hLTF. To do this, I continuously

stimulated ChR2-expressing LC cells for the duration equal to the total time taken for the

intermittent optical stimulation protocol (i.e., 5 Hz for 12.5 minutes continuously). With

continuous LC stimulation, I found that genioglossus activity decreased to 70 ± 19% of baseline

levels following initial exposure to light (paired t-test, genioglossus at baseline vs during

continuous stimulation: t(4)=3.418, p=0.0268. Fig. 4.4A) but had returned to baseline levels by

the 15-minute time point, where it remained consistent for the rest of the recording period (1-

way RM ANOVA, F=0.4031, p=0.8037. Fig. 4.4C,D). Following continuous LC stimulation, I again

did not observe any change over time in other respiratory variables measured (2-way RM

ANOVA, Int. LC Stim. vs Cont. Stim., Diaphragm: F=0.5053, p=0.7320, respiratory frequency:

F=0.0381, p=0.9971, O2 saturation: F=1.077, p=0.3806. Fig. 4.4E-G), suggesting continuous LC

stimulation had minimal effect on respiratory output.

The decrease in GG activity following continuous LC stimulation may be due to insufficient

noradrenaline release. A previous study has shown that intermittent electrical stimulation of

the LC caused more release of noradrenaline than continuous stimulation (Florin-Lechner et al.,

Page 98: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

82

1996). Since no increase in genioglossus activity was observed following intermittent LC

stimulation, as demonstrated in section 4.3.2., it is not surprising that no increase in

genioglossus activity was observed when the LC was stimulated continuously. The decrease in

GG motor output may suggest that continuous stimulation caused auto-inhibition of LC cells or

potentially induced depolarization block following prolonged LC stimulation (Adams and Foote,

1988). This is supported by histological c-Fos expression in the LC following continuous LC

stimulation. Specifically, continuous LC stimulation had fewer activated cells (i.e., c-Fos positive

cells) than intermittent LC stimulation (unpaired t-test, Int. LC Stim. vs Continuous LC Stim.,

t(8)=5.163, p=0.0009. Fig. 4.4B), suggesting LC cells were less activated following continuous

stimulation.

Page 99: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

83

Page 100: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

84

Figure 4.4. Intermittent LC stimulation activates ChR2-expressing LC neurons more than non-ChR2-expressing mCherry LC neurons or continuous LC stimulation. (A) Group data showing integrated genioglossus activity (∫GG EMG) decreased following continuous light exposure (purple bars, n=5), but had no effect in animals with no opsins (i.e., non-ChR2-expressing mCherry, white bars, n=6) relative to percent baseline (black bars). (B) Group data showing more c-Fos positive cells in ChR2-expressing animals that received intermittent LC stimulation (blue bars, n=6) and exhibited hLTF compared to non-ChR2-expressing mCherry (white bars, n=3) or animals given continuous LC stimulation (purple bars, n=4). (C) Representative EMG traces of integrated inspiratory genioglossus activity recorded from an anesthetized spontaneously breathing rat after intermittent light pulses in an animal with no opsin (i.e., non-ChR2-expressing mCherry, top trace) or continuous light exposure in ChR2-expressing LC cells (i.e., Continuous LC Stimulation, bottom trace). (D) Group data showing no change in inspiratory genioglossus activity in non-ChR2-expressing mCherry and continuous LC stimulation groups at 15, 30, 45, and 60 min compared to ChR2-expressing animals that received intermittent LC stimulation. Dotted line represents percent baseline activity. In all groups, diaphragm amplitude (∫Dia EMG), respiratory frequency, and oxygen saturation (E-G) were unaffected over time. Data is presented as mean + SEM. * denotes a significant difference (p<0.05) from baseline.

4.3.6 Intermittent LC stimulation elicits hLTF at the same frequency as repeated apneas

It is important to note that hLTF does not always manifest following repeated apneas or

repeated LC stimulation. In studies of long-term potentiation, intermittent stimulation triggered

plasticity 50-90% of the time (Abraham et al., 1993, Watanabe et al., 2002). In the field of LTF,

animals that expressed LTF were intentionally separated from non-responders but the percent

of animals that did not exhibit LTF was never reported. In Chapter 3, 35% of the animals did not

exhibit hLTF following repeated obstructive apneas. Here, I found that intermittent LC

stimulation triggered hLTF 73% of the time (i.e., 27% of animals did not exhibit hLTF following

intermittent LC stimulation). However, in animals where no intervention was performed (i.e.,

time control, non-ChR2-expressing mCherry and off-target groups), hLTF spontaneously

occurred in 25% of animals. To determine whether intermittent LC stimulation had an influence

on hLTF expression, and that it was not a result of random chance, I performed a chi-square

test to determine whether my observed probability fit within an expected distribution.

Intermittent LC stimulation significantly correlated with hLTF expression (chi-square test, Int. LC

Stimulation vs controls, chi2=10.8107, p<0.001), suggesting it was not due to random chance.

To further emphasize that intermittent LC stimulation increases the probability of hLTF

expression, I also performed a Firth logistic regression to determine whether LC stimulation can

Page 101: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

85

predict the probability of hLTF expression. This differs from a chi-square test because a Firth

logistic regression provides a model to predict the likelihood of an outcome following an

intervention, whereas a chi-square test measures the strength of the relationship, akin to a

correlation. Specifically, using a Firth logistic regression, I found that hLTF was more likely to

manifest than controls (Firth logistic regression, Int. LC stimulation vs controls, p=0.004. Fig.

4.5). This shows that hLTF expression following intermittent LC stimulation is not due to

random chance but can be triggered through LC stimulation.

Figure 4.5. Probability of hLTF expression was increased following intermittent stimulation of the LC. Following intermittent LC stimulation (n=8, blue), hLTF expression was more likely to manifest compared to other groups, which include animals given intermittent stimulation with no opsin (i.e., non-ChR2-expressing mCherry, n=6, orange), given continuous LC stimulation (n=4, purple), or given intermittent stimulation outside of the LC (i.e., off-target stimulation, n=6, black). Each line represents an animal that received one of the interventions. Intermittent LC stimulation is significantly more likely to augment genioglossus motor output above baseline levels at 60 minutes post-intervention.

Page 102: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

86

Lastly, I compared the frequency of hLTF expression following repeated apneas to the

frequency of hLTF expression following intermittent LC stimulation to determine whether

intermittent LC stimulation had a higher probability of eliciting hLTF than repeated apneas. I

hypothesized that direct modulation of the source of noradrenaline in the proposed hLTF

pathway would increase the probability for hLTF expression compared to repeated apneas.

Using a chi-square test, I found that the occurrence of hLTF was not different whether the

trigger was repeated apneas or intermittent LC stimulation (chi-square test, Repeated Apneas

vs Int. LC Stim., chi2=0.2017, p=0.653). When I compared the same groups with a Firth logistic

regression analysis, I again found no difference in the probability of hLTF expression between

either trigger (Firth logistic regression, Repeated Apneas vs Int. LC Stim., p=0.654). These results

support my finding that intermittent LC stimulation increases the probability for hLTF

expression and it elicit hLTF in an equally consistent manner as the repeated apnea trigger.

4.3.7 hLTF requires a minimum threshold activation of LC cells

Following LC stimulation, 27% of animals did not exhibit hLTF. My next goal was to examine

why hLTF was not expressed in these animals. One possibility could be due to fewer infected

noradrenergic LC neurons. Therefore, I quantified the total number of double-labelled mCherry

and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) positive cells in animals that did and did not exhibit hLTF

following LC stimulation. What I found was that the number of cells expressing both mCherry

and TH were not significantly different between animals that exhibited hLTF and those that did

not (unpaired t-test, ChR2 hLTF vs ChR2 No hLTF, t(7)=1.086, p=0.3134. Fig. 4.6A). This suggests

that the number of infected noradrenergic LC cells was not the reason for a lack of hLTF

observed following intermittent LC stimulation in these animals.

Next, I aimed to determine whether the animals that did not exhibit hLTF was a result of

insufficient LC stimulation. Although there were similar levels of mCherry and TH expression,

this may not reflect the same level of LC activity during light exposure. I, therefore, examined

Page 103: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

87

the c-Fos expression in noradrenergic LC cells of animals that exhibited hLTF and those that did

not. What I found was that there were a greater number of double-labelled c-Fos and TH

positive cells in animals that exhibited hLTF compared to animals that did not (unpaired t-test,

ChR2 hLTF vs ChR2 No hLTF, t(7)=2.499, p=0.0411. Fig. 4.6B), suggesting the lack of hLTF in these

animals may be a result of insufficient LC stimulation. This is supported by my results in Chapter

3 that showed higher c-Fos expression in animals that exhibited hLTF compared to animals that

did not following repeated apneas (Lui et al., 2018). In addition, this is further supported by

studies that showed noradrenergic LC neurons naturally fire synchronously when sufficiently

stimulated (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981, Christie et al., 1989, Ishimatsu and Williams, 1996). It

may therefore be possible that insufficient LC stimulation did not meet the threshold required

to trigger a synchronous response in LC activity and therefore did not elicit hLTF.

Page 104: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

88

Figure 4.6. Hypoglossal LTF requires a minimum LC stimulation threshold. (A) Group data showing equal amounts of mCherry and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) expression in animals that expressed hLTF (Int. LC Stim. (hLTF), n=6, blue bar) compared to animals that did not express hLTF (Int. LC Stim. (No hLTF), n=3, grey bars). (B) Group data showing animals that exhibited hLTF had c-Fos expression increased by 43 ± 11% in noradrenergic cells compared to animals that did not express hLTF. (C) An example of LC activity represented by c-Fos expression (black nuclei) in animals given intermittent LC stimulation and exhibiting hLTF (left) or intermittent LC stimulation but did not exhibit hLTF (right). Dotted outline identifies the LC. Data is presented as mean + SEM. * denotes a significant difference (p<0.05).

Page 105: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

89

4.3.8 Inactivation of the LC abolishes apnea-induced hLTF

Lastly, I aimed to determine whether the LC is critical for apnea-induced hLTF. In Chapter 3, I

demonstrated that pharmacological inactivation of the LC with clonidine prevented apnea-

induced hLTF. However, pharmacological approaches have limitations including the spread of

the drug and the timing of the drug’s effect persisting after initial application. For example,

application of clonidine could inhibit nearby cells such as the subCoeruleus (subC), which may

provide a tonic drive on hypoglossal motor neurons (Chan et al., 2006); the unintended

inactivation of the subC by clonidine may persist after the intervention. An inhibitory

optogenetic approach could mitigate these issues. In addition, it may also be possible that

repeated stimulation of the LC activates the hLTF circuit without the LC being directly involved

in the circuit itself. To determine if the LC is part of the hLTF circuit, I optically silenced LC

activity during repeated obstructive apneas. Specifically, LC neurons expressing halorhodopsin

(eNpHR) via infection with a viral vector (AAV5-hSyn-eNpHR3.0-mCherry) were inhibited using a

continuous light source. Since repeated apneas can trigger hLTF, I aimed to optically inhibit

eNpHR-expressing LC cells during this trigger. If the LC is indeed mediating hLTF, then I

hypothesized that optical inactivation of the LC would prevent apnea-induced hLTF.

Similar to my results using pharmacological inactivation of the LC, optical inactivation of

eNpHR-expressing LC neurons prevented apnea-induced hLTF. In fact, a significant decrease in

genioglossus motor output at 60 minutes was observed (1-way RM ANOVA, F=4.874, p=0.0041,

Figure 4.7A,B) but other respiratory variables were unaffected (2-way RM ANOVA, LC

Inactivation + apneas vs non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry + apneas, diaphragm amplitude:

F=1.128, p=0.3535, respiratory frequency: F=0.4403, p=0.7789, O2 saturation: F=1.032,

p=0.3998, end-tidal CO2: F=1.552, p=0.2023. Fig. 4.6C-F). These findings support my results

from Chapter 3 demonstrating that LC activity is critical for apnea-induced hLTF.

To ensure that light itself did not influence hLTF expression, or that virally infected cells did not

compromise LC function, I used a control viral vector that is absent the inhibitory opsin (i.e.,

Page 106: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

90

AAV5-hSyn-mCherry). In these animals, I performed the same intervention exposing the LC to

continuous beam of green light while simultaneously delivering repeated apneas. I hypothesize

that LC cells would not be inhibited due to the absence of the light-sensitive inhibitory opsin

(eNpHR) and repeated apneas should therefore elicit hLTF. Congruent with earlier uses of the

control viral vector, repeated apneas were able to elicit hLTF with genioglossus amplitude

reaching 120 ± 8% above baseline (1-way RM ANOVA, F=2.771, p=0.0466, Figure 4.7A,B). This

suggests that LC inactivation prevents apnea-induced hLTF and the LC is a critical component of

the neural circuit underlying apnea-induced hLTF.

Page 107: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

91

Page 108: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

92

Figure 4.7. Optical inactivation of the LC prevents apnea-induced hypoglossal LTF. (A) Representative EMG traces of integrated inspiratory genioglossus activity (∫GG EMG) recorded from an anaesthetized, spontaneously breathing rat after LC inactivation (top) or non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry control (bottom) at baseline, 15, 30, 45, and 60 min following repeated apneas. (B) Group data showing inspiratory GG activity in animals that did not express the inhibitory opsin (non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry, n=6, white bars) increased by 120 ± 8% following repeated apneas (i.e., hLTF). However, when the LC was inactivated (eNpHR, n=9, green bars), repeated apneas produced a decrease in GG amplitude over time. In both groups, diaphragm activity, respiratory frequency, oxygen saturation, and end-tidal CO2 levels (C-F) were unaffected over time. Dotted line represents percent baseline. Data are presented as mean + SEM. * denotes a significant difference (p<0.05).

Although the above findings suggest LC inactivation prevents apnea-induced hLTF, there is one

caveat in the statistical analysis that requires additional examination. Specifically, the analysis

above compares animals that exhibited apnea-induced hLTF to animals that did not exhibit hLTF

following LC inactivation. However, as mention in section 4.3.6, about 27% of animals do not

exhibit hLTF following intermittent LC stimulation, and about 35% of animals do not exhibit

hLTF following repeated obstructive apneas. This may skew my earlier statistical analysis

because it implies that the 35% of animals would not have exhibited hLTF regardless of my

intervention at the LC.

There is no precedent on how to address this caveat as previous LTF studies follow the same

statistical analysis applied above (Bach and Mitchell, 1996, Baker-Herman and Mitchell, 2002,

Dale et al., 2017, Fuller et al., 2001, McGuire et al., 2005). Here, I introduce a statistical

approach that addresses this problem by including all animals (i.e., animals that exhibit hLTF

and non-responders) in the statistical analysis. In addition to the chi-square test and Firth

logistic regression that provides insight into whether my intervention prevented apnea-induced

hLTF (i.e., not due to random chance), I also perform an ordinary least squares linear regression

(OLS). This differs from the chi-square test and Firth logistic regression in that it uses the

measured genioglossus EMG values across all groups and includes all animals (responders and

non-responders). A chi-square test and Firth logistic regression are useful when determining

whether a categorical outcome variable (i.e., did hLTF occur, yes or no). An OLS analysis is more

Page 109: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

93

stringent in that measured genioglossus EMG values are taken into account to determine

whether the intervention affected genioglossus amplitude at the 60-minute time point.

Following a chi-square test, I found that LC inactivation significantly correlated with an absence

of hLTF expression (chi-square test, LC Inactivation + Apneas vs non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry

+ Apneas, chi2=9.6923, p=0.002). Similarly, upon performing a Firth logistic regression analysis,

I found a significant difference in the probability of hLTF expression when the LC is inactivated

(Firth logistic regression, LC inactivation + Apneas vs non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry + Apneas,

p=0.022. Fig. 4.8). Lastly, upon performing OLS analysis, I found that LC inactivation prevented

apnea-induced hLTF (OLS, LC inactivation + apneas vs non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry + apneas,

R2=0.3629, F(19)=10.82, β=-0.40125, p=0.004). Taken together, this suggests that the LC is critical

for apnea-induced hLTF.

Page 110: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

94

Figure 4.8. hLTF did not manifest following LC inactivation. Following LC inactivation (eNpHR, n=9, green), repeated apneas did not elicit hLTF. This is in contrast to the results observed in animals that were given the same intervention but had no opsin (non-eNpHR-expressing mCherry, n=6, blue), where repeated apneas continued to elicit hLTF in 65% of animals. Repeated apneas had a significantly greater probability at inducing an increase in genioglossus motor output above baseline at 60 minutes than when the LC was inactivated and hLTF never occurred.

4.4 Discussion

Long-term facilitation of inspiratory genioglossus motor output can be elicited by various

triggers. Here I presented a novel trigger that can elicit hLTF without interacting with the

chemosensory or broncho-pulmonary feedback systems. By intermittently stimulating LC

neurons in the same pattern as repeated apneas, I was able to increase inspiratory genioglossus

motor output for 60 minutes post-intervention, with no effects on other respiratory variables

measured (i.e. diaphragm amplitude, respiratory frequency, and blood oxygen saturation).

Furthermore, I showed that hLTF was not elicited when LC neurons were optically inactivated.

Page 111: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

95

These findings suggest that the LC is essential for hLTF as manipulation of the LC alone can

trigger hLTF and inactivation of LC prevents apnea-induced hLTF.

4.4.1 LC provides an endogenous noradrenergic drive to hypoglossal motor neurons

Genioglossus motor output is under noradrenergic state-dependent modulation (Chan et al.,

2006). Here I provide evidence demonstrating the presence of an endogenous noradrenergic

drive arising from the LC onto hypoglossal motor neurons. In chapter three, I showed that the

hypoglossal motor pool was directly innervated by noradrenergic LC neurons. I also showed

that pharmacological inactivation of the LC had no effect on baseline genioglossus activity

(Chapter 3), which would suggest that the LC does not provide a tonic drive to hypoglossal

motor neurons. However, it is possible that my pharmacological approach did not adequately

inhibit LC neurons. The concentration of clonidine used was shown to be effective at reducing

cortical noradrenaline levels by approximately 72% (Sakamoto et al., 2013). The LC is the sole

contributor of noradrenaline to the cortex; the remaining active noradrenergic LC neurons may

therefore have been sufficient at providing the tonic drive necessary to maintain genioglossus

motor output at baseline levels, but insufficient at mediating hLTF. By using an optogenetic

approach, I could inactivate cells using light to penetrate cells within a small cone (Gradinaru et

al., 2010, Yizhar et al., 2011) possibly inhibiting more cells than with a pharmacological

approach. Following LC inactivation, basal genioglossus amplitude decreased by 19 ± 7% of

baseline, suggesting the presence of an endogenous noradrenergic drive arising from the LC

that acts on hypoglossal motor neurons. Despite reports of limited innervation (Aldes et al.,

1992, Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007), these findings suggest that a portion of the tonic

noradrenergic drive at the hypoglossal motor pool stems from the LC. My findings also showed

that continuous LC stimulation did not increase genioglossus muscle activity nor trigger hLTF. In

fact, a decrease in genioglossus amplitude was observed upon continuous LC stimulation,

suggesting a decrease in LC activity with prolonged excitation. The reason for the decrease in

genioglossus motor output observed here is not known, but was likely due to auto-inhibition or

Page 112: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

96

depolarization block where excessive activation of LC cells can lead to a decrease in LC activity

(Adams and Foote, 1988).

4.4.2. Baseline genioglossus motor activity is unaffected during stimulation of ChR2-

expressing LC neurons

We observed genioglossus motor output at baseline and found that during intermittent light

stimulation of ChR2-expressing LC neurons, genioglossus motor output did not change. Similar

results were observed in other studies showing baseline genioglossus activity to remain

unchanged during optical stimulation of ChR2-expressing neurons in the A5 or A7 noradrenergic

cell groups (Song and Poon, 2017). This suggests that the immediate effect of intermittent

optical stimulation of noradrenergic cell groups likely does not release enough noradrenaline to

directly augment genioglossus motor output, but instead may be changing the pattern of LC

activity and thereby noradrenaline release.

In addition, studies that showed an increase in genioglossus or hypoglossal activity following

application of noradrenaline or phenylephrine use concentrations that are at minimum, 1000

fold higher than endogenous noradrenaline levels. For example, microdialysis in the rat

prefrontal cortex show that electrical LC stimulation increases noradrenaline levels to

approximately 0.0006 µM (Florin-Lechner et al., 1996), but studies that directly applied

noradrenaline or phenylephrine to hypoglossal motor neurons had concentrations ranging from

10 µM (Neverova et al., 2007), 50 µM (Parkis et al., 1995), or 1000 µM (Chan et al., 2006,

Schwarz et al., 2014). The concentrations of noradrenaline at the hypoglossal motor pool during

LC stimulation (intermittent or continuous) may therefore be too low to induce an immediate

response in genioglossus amplitude.

Page 113: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

97

4.4.3 Stimulation of ChR2-expressing LC neurons trigger LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor

output

In Chapter 3, I showed an increase in c-Fos expression at the LC correlated with apnea-induced

hLTF. Here, I showed that intermittent stimulation of the LC in the same pattern as repeated

apneas elicits hLTF. My controls support this claim as intermittent light pulses did not elicit hLTF

when absent the excitatory opsin ChR2. More importantly, I also showed that intermittent

stimulation lateral to the LC does not trigger hLTF. These findings suggest that stimulation of

the LC itself is required for hLTF, although it may be possible that stimulation of cells in the LC

that are not noradrenergic in nature may have contributed to hLTF. These studies used a non-

specific viral vector to target cells in the region of the LC in non-transgenic rats. It would be

ideal to repeat these studies using either a viral vector specific to LC neurons, or a cre-

dependent viral vector in TH-cre rats (i.e. transgenic rats expressing cre in tyrosine hydroxylase

positive cells). These approaches would allow me to determine the role of the noradrenergic LC

neurons in the manifestation of hLTF. However, this will be addressed in my next chapter

(Chapter 5).

Previously, it was shown that repeated application of noradrenaline on hypoglossal motor

neurons in vitro can potentiate their response to excitatory inputs (Neverova et al., 2007). My

stimulation protocol forced intermittent release of noradrenaline from the LC onto the

hypoglossal motor pool, thereby triggering hLTF. This intervention could, therefore, not be

reflective of what happens during the natural trigger (i.e., repeated apneas), but simply an

observation of an already known phenomenon (i.e., that repeated exposure to noradrenaline

can trigger hLTF). However, a recent study has shown in vivo that repeated exposure of

noradrenaline from non-LC sources does not trigger sustainable hLTF (Song and Poon, 2017).

Specifically, repeated optical stimulation of hypoglossal-projecting noradrenergic cell groups

(A5 and A7) increases genioglossus motor output transiently, persisting for approximately 20

minutes. Here, I show that the same intervention targeting LC neurons triggers hLTF that

Page 114: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

98

persists for a minimum of 60 minutes, suggesting that repeated stimulation of the LC in

particular elicits hLTF that is reflective of the plasticity observed with repeated apneas.

4.4.4 LC is critical for hLTF

My hypothesis was that optically inactivating the LC would prevent apnea-induced hLTF. My

results directly supported this hypothesis as apnea-induced hLTF was abolished when the LC

was inactivated. This suggests that LC activity is critical for hLTF expression triggered by

repeated apneas and is therefore a required component of the hLTF circuit. Experiments in

control (non-ChR2-expressing mCherry) animals supported these results, as repeated apneas in

the presence of continuous light exposure continued to trigger hLTF. Taken together, it suggests

that the repeated apnea trigger halts at the inactivated LC within the hLTF circuit, preventing

the intermittent release of noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons to elicit hLTF.

4.4.5 A minimum threshold of LC stimulation is required for hLTF expression

Here, I demonstrated that repeated stimulation of the LC triggered respiratory plasticity of

inspiratory genioglossus motor output. However, some animals did not exhibit hLTF following

intermittent LC stimulation. These animals may have had a different response to the fixed

stimulus delivered (i.e. 10 periods of LC stimulation at 5 Hz for 15 seconds, separated by 1

minute) as it does not account for the variability between each animal. Despite receiving the

same intervention, some animals may not have received the same level of stimulation. This is

supported in my results as c-Fos expression in the LC was lower in animals that did not exhibit

hLTF compared to animals that exhibited hLTF. Considering LC neurons have been suggested to

fire synchronously (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981, Christie et al., 1989, Ishimatsu and Williams,

1996), it may be possible that an insufficient number of cells were stimulated to trigger

plasticity. This implies that a minimum threshold of LC stimulation is required to trigger

synchronous firing in order for hLTF to manifest.

Page 115: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

99

4.4.6 Plasticity occurs at the level of the LC and at the level of the hypoglossal motor neuron

A previous study showed terazosin perfused onto hypoglossal motor neurons after repeated

apneas can reverse signs of hLTF (Tadjalli, 2012), suggesting on-going activation of α1-

adrenergic receptor signalling at the hypoglossal motor pool is necessary to maintain hLTF. An

alternative interpretation of my histological results may support this finding. c-Fos expression is

maximally active between 30-90 minutes. My results showed an increase in c-Fos expression at

the LC following intermittent LC stimulation (i.e., 90 minutes from the trigger) is indicative of LC

cells being activated by light. However, the increase in c-Fos expression could reflect hLTF

expression from the 60-minute time point (i.e., 30 minutes after maximal hLTF expression),

potentially representing an increase in LC activity persisting after the trigger. It may be possible

that repeated LC stimulation has altered the firing pattern of LC neurons (i.e., the LC has

undergone plasticity).

The implication above contrasts what is known regarding phrenic LTF, which has shown

plasticity to occur at the level of the motor neuron (Dale et al., 2017, Devinney et al., 2015,

Hoffman et al., 2012, Satriotomo et al., 2012). However, plasticity could also be happening at

the hypoglossal motor neuron by changing its intrinsic plasticity. I base this on two lines of

evidence: (1) the binding of neuromodulators such as noradrenaline can initiate a signalling

cascade that acts through TrkB receptors to trigger plasticity (Andero et al., 2014, Baker-

Herman et al., 2004, Minichiello et al., 2002, Wilkerson and Mitchell, 2009, Yoshii and

Constantine-Paton, 2010). When TrkB receptors are antagonized at the level of the hypoglossal

motor neuron, repeated apneas did not elicit hLTF (Tadjalli, 2012), suggesting noradrenaline-

induced changes within the hypoglossal motor neuron following repeated apneas could be

mediating plasticity. (2), TrkB receptor activation alone induced hLTF (Tadjalli, 2012), suggesting

hLTF expression may initiate and/or require activation of the TrkB signalling pathway.

The above data has led me to postulate that plasticity may be happening at two fronts:

plasticity at the LC alters its firing pattern to provide on-going release of noradrenaline onto

Page 116: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

100

hypoglossal motor neurons, and the hypoglossal motor neuron itself undergoes plasticity

following noradrenaline-mediated activation of the TrkB signalling cascade to induce plasticity

at the level of the hypoglossal motor neuron.

4.4.7 Methodological considerations

As with any approach, there were limitations to my methodology. First and foremost, during LC

inhibition, it may be possible for LC neurons to fired spontaneously due to the inhibitory post-

synaptic current (IPSC) reversal potential (Mahn et al., 2016). Although this may have occurred

during my intervention, the use of halorhodopsin (eNpHR3.0) is arguably the most effective at

maintaining prolonged inhibition compared the other inhibitory opsins such as archaerhodopsin

which has been shown to increase spontaneous neurotransmitter release (Mahn et al., 2016).

In addition, my histology showed fewer c-Fos positive cells in the LC following LC inactivation,

which suggest that LC neurons were largely inactivated during my intervention.

Another limitation involves potential drawbacks with the use of optogenetics. For example, the

use of light is accompanied by heat produced at the tip of the optic fibre (Han, 2012, Stujenske

et al., 2015, Tye and Deisseroth, 2012), which can lead to heat-induced cell damage (Qian and

Gu, 2005), heat-induced cell firing (Reig et al., 2010, Stujenske et al., 2015), and photodilation

of blood vessels (Rungta et al., 2017). The amount of heat produced depends on the power

output, the distance from the optic fibre tip, and duration of light exposure. The power output

used in my experiments (25-50 mW) was greater than in recent reports on optical LC

stimulation in mice which used 20 mW (Carter et al., 2010) or 10-12 mW (Wang et al., 2014). To

reduce heat production, I used a 200 µm optic fibre to deliver light in 5 ms pulses at 5 Hz for 15

seconds, which has been reported to produce a 0.005ᵒC increase in temperature (Gysbrechts et

al., 2016).

Page 117: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

101

The next limitation involves the use of c-Fos as a marker for cell activity. As stated in chapter

three, c-Fos does not provide insight into cells that have been inactivated or inhibited (Chan

and Sawchenko, 1994). Furthermore, not all activated neurons express c-Fos. As a result, my

claims on LC activity following LC stimulation or inhibition may not truly reflect the real-time

activities of the cell before, during, or after hLTF induction. It can only provide a snapshot of

what may be occurring at the LC during my intervention. In addition, noradrenaline levels were

not measured in these studies following stimulation or inhibition of LC cells. Going forward, it

would be ideal to record the firing pattern of LC cells before, during, and after hLTF induction,

as well as direct measurements of noradrenaline levels at the hypoglossal motor pool to

determine how LC neurons contribute to hLTF expression.

In chapter three, I discussed how LC manipulation also affects blood pressure. In this chapter, I

showed that intermittent LC stimulation had no effect on diaphragm amplitude, respiratory

frequency, oxygen saturation, or end-tidal CO2, but did not measure other variables such as

blood pressure and heart rate. LC stimulation can decrease blood pressure and heart rate

(Hakuno et al., 2004, Sved and Felsten, 1987). It may therefore be possible that repeated

modulation of these factors contributed to the manifestation of hLTF.

Lastly, one concerning limitation is in the exclusion of non-responders for statistical analyses. In

the field of LTF, animals that do not exhibit LTF are not reported and excluded in the statistical

comparisons. By omitting these animals, there is a degree of sampling bias by selecting for

responders. In this chapter, I discussed the use of several statistical analyses to address this

concern. My solution is to use a chi-square test, Firth logistic regression, and an OLS to include

all animals, responders and non-responders, to objectively determine whether hLTF was

expressed. I believe is the start to addressing this concern and I encourage future studies to

report how non-responders are dealt with statistically.

Page 118: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

102

4.4.8 Scientific importance and clinical significance

My results provide evidence that the LC contributes to the tonic noradrenergic drive onto

hypoglossal motor neurons and there by genioglossus motor output. Furthermore, I show that

LC stimulation alone can increase genioglossus motor output. This is significant as a reduction in

LC activity correlated with rapid-eye movement (REM) sleep, and REM sleep correlated with a

reduction in upper airway muscle tone (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981, Nitz and Siegel, 1997),

suggesting LC activity may be involved in the reduction of muscle tone during REM sleep. My

results provide evidence that manipulation of LC activity could potentially aid in maintaining

upper airway muscle tone during sleep. By mitigating the reduction in muscle tone observed

during sleep, it may reduce the number of apneic events in obstructive sleep apnea patients.

Lastly, I propose two sites at which plasticity of genioglossus motor output can be manipulated:

at the LC and at the hypoglossal motor pool as both are required to mediate hLTF. The

identification of these regions could provide a new therapeutic target to induce plasticity and

treat OSA.

Page 119: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

103

Chapter Five - α1-Adrenergic Receptor Binding at the Hypoglossal Motor Pool is Required for

LC-Induced Hypoglossal LTF

5.1 Summary

My previous chapters have provided evidence that the locus coeruleus (LC) is critical for and

can drive hypoglossal long-term facilitation (hLTF) expression. It is reasonable to hypothesize

that the LC releases noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons to trigger hLTF. However,

the LC co-releases multiple neurotransmitters (e.g., glutamate) that may influence the

manifestation of hLTF. To determine whether noradrenaline released onto the hypoglossal

motor pool, specifically from the LC, is the critical neurotransmitter for the manifestation of

hLTF, I employed two approaches. First, I used intermittent stimulation of LC axons at the level

of the hypoglossal motor pool to determine whether stimulation of this connection can trigger

hLTF. I then elicited hLTF with intermittent stimulation of the LC while simultaneously blocking

α1-receptors at the hypoglossal motor pool. Here, I demonstrated that stimulation of LC axons

could not trigger hLTF, but hLTF elicited by intermittent LC stimulation was abolished following

α1-receptor blockade at the hypoglossal motor pool. This suggests that LC-induced hLTF

requires α1-adrenergic receptor activation on hypoglossal motor neurons, and that the source

of noradrenaline acting on α1 receptors may originate from the LC.

5.2 Introduction

My findings to this point have identified the LC as being a critical component of the circuit

underlying hLTF. The first aim of this chapter was to determine whether hLTF was mediated by

a direct LC to hypoglossal connection. In chapter 3, I showed that LC neurons send projections

to the hypoglossal motor pool, creating the neural circuit (NTS→LC→XII) that I hypothesize

underlies hLTF. The first aim in this chapter was therefore to determine if stimulation of LC

axons at the level of the hypoglossal motor pool can elicit hLTF. My hypothesis is that

intermittent stimulation of LC axons would cause episodic release of noradrenaline onto

hypoglossal motor neurons to trigger hLTF. This would address whether LC-induced hLTF is

acting through a direct connection between LC neurons and hypoglossal motor neurons.

Page 120: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

104

To test this hypothesis, I recorded respiratory activity in anaesthetized, spontaneously

breathing, male Sprague-Dawley rats expressing channelrhodopsin2 (ChR2) in the region of the

LC. At four weeks post-injection (AAV5-hSyn-ChR2(H134R)-mCherry) into the LC region, I

stimulated the axon fibres originating from the region of the LC at the level of the hypoglossal

motor pool. I stimulated ChR2-expressing LC axons in same fashion as my previous studies (i.e.,

15 second pulses at 5 Hz, separated by 1 minute no stimulation, repeated 10 times, Fig. 5.1A). I

recorded genioglossus and diaphragm activity, as well as respiratory frequency and oxygen

saturation for 60-90 minutes post stimulation.

The second aim of this chapter was to determine whether the LC is the source of noradrenaline

mediating hLTF. In chapter 4, I demonstrated that intermittent stimulation of the LC alone can

elicit hLTF that persists for 60 minutes, suggesting intermittent noradrenaline released from a

specific source (i.e., the LC) is driving hLTF. Direct application of noradrenaline onto hypoglossal

motor neurons in vitro has been shown to trigger hLTF (Feldman et al., 2005, Neverova et al.,

2007), and that blockade of α1-receptors at the hypoglossal motor pool prevents apnea-

induced hLTF (Tadjalli et al., 2010), suggesting that noradrenaline acting on α1-receptors is

required for hLTF to manifest. However, when similar experiments were performed in vivo,

intermittent noradrenaline release induced by stimulation of noradrenergic cell group A5 or A7

elicited a transient form of hLTF, persisting for 10-20 minutes (Song and Poon, 2017). This

suggests that another mechanism may be involved in mediating hLTF that persists for more

than 20 minutes.

Several studies have shown LC neurons to co-release multiple neurotransmitters including

glutamate (Fung et al., 1994, Trudeau, 2004), neuropeptide Y (Everitt et al., 1984, Holets et al.,

1988, Tsuda et al., 1989), galanin (Holets et al., 1988, Tsuda et al., 1989), enkephalin (Van

Bockstaele et al., 2000), and dopamine (Devoto and Flore, 2006, Kempadoo et al., 2016, Smith

and Greene, 2012). This suggests that other neurotransmitters may be involved following hLTF

induced by intermittent LC stimulation. In fact, the neurotransmitters co-released by the LC can

Page 121: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

105

modulate respiratory output and/or plasticity (Bocchiaro and Feldman, 2004, Feldman et al.,

2005, Kempadoo et al., 2016, Ling et al., 2001, McGuire et al., 2005, Rukhadze et al., 2010,

Sharifullina et al., 2004, Steenland et al., 2006, Tadjalli et al., 2010). For example, the LC co-

releases glutamate which can act on NMDA receptors found on hypoglossal motor neurons to

augment respiratory output (Steenland et al., 2006). In fact, phrenic LTF was abolished upon

NMDA receptor blockade at the level of this motor pool (McGuire et al., 2005). Similarly, LTF of

hypoglossal nerve activity elicited in vitro was abolished upon metabotropic glutamate receptor

blockade (Feldman et al., 2005, Sharifullina et al., 2004). This suggests that hLTF elicited by

intermittent LC stimulation may have been influenced or dependent on the co-release of

glutamate acting on NMDA and/or metabotropic glutamate receptors on the hypoglossal motor

neuron to induce post-synaptic changes.

In the field of LTF research, it was well demonstrated that multiple neurotransmitters can

influence the manifestation of LTF, and the trigger dictates which neurotransmitter is released.

For example, LTF of phrenic/diaphragm activity was induced by repeated application of

adrenaline (Huxtable et al., 2014) but is dependent on serotonin when elicited by an

intermittent hypoxia trigger (Fuller et al., 2001, Kinkead et al., 2001, Ling et al., 2001, Niebert et

al., 2011). Similarly, repeated activation of 5-HT2 can elicit hLTF in vitro (Bocchiaro and

Feldman, 2004, Feldman et al., 2005) but serotonin is not critical for hLTF elicited by a repeated

apnea trigger (Tadjalli et al., 2010). This overlap is likely due to similar intracellular mechanisms

following 5HT or α1-adrenergic receptor binding. Both receptor subtypes are Gq protein-

coupled metabotropic receptors whose signalling cascade involve inositol 1,4,5-triphosphate

(IP3) and protein kinase C (PKC) (Hannon and Hoyer, 2008).

To determine whether hLTF is induced by noradrenaline released from the LC, I elicited hLTF

using intermittent stimulation of LC cells expressing channelrhodopsin2 (ChR2) while

simultaneously blocking α1-adrenergic receptors at the level of the hypoglossal motor pool

with 1 µM Terazosin, an α1-adrenergic receptor antagonist (Fig. 5.1B). This concentration is

found effective at preventing apnea-induced hLTF without reducing basal genioglossus activity

Page 122: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

106

(Tadjalli, 2012). If hLTF requires noradrenaline released from the LC, then intermittent LC

stimulation should no longer elicit hLTF. Alternatively, if hLTF is critically-dependent by other

neurotransmitters co-released from the LC, then blockade of α1-adrenergic receptors at the

level of the hypoglossal motor pool should have no effect on the manifestation of hLTF

triggered by LC stimulation.

Together, these experiments would show that the projections arising from the region of the LC

are directly acting on hypoglossal motor neurons to trigger noradrenaline release to elicit hLTF,

providing a functional and anatomical circuit underlying hLTF.

Page 123: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

107

Figure 5.1 – Protocols for intermittent LC stimulation with and without terazosin perfusion. (A) Protocol for recording genioglossus (GG) activity in an anaesthetized, spontaneously breathing rat at baseline, followed by intermittent stimulation of LC axons at the hypoglossal motor pool (XII) with 10 light pulses at 5 Hz for 15 seconds (black bars), separated by 1 minute no stimulation, and subsequent recording for an additional 90 minutes. (B) Protocol for recording genioglossus activity followed by intermittent LC stimulation with simultaneous perfusion of saline at the hypoglossal motor pool, switch to terazosin (1 µM, dotted line), and washout with saline for the remainder of the experiment. (C) Protocol for recording genioglossus activity followed by saline perfusion at the hypoglossal motor pool with simultaneous repeated obstructive apneas (10 apneas for 15 seconds separated by 1-minute recovery) and subsequent 90-minute recording.

Page 124: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

108

5.3 Results

5.3.1 Intermittent stimulation of LC axons at the hypoglossal motor pool did not trigger hLTF

In chapter four, I showed that intermittent stimulation of the LC triggered hLTF, presumably

through the release of noradrenaline from the LC onto hypoglossal motor neurons.

Furthermore, my own findings, in addition to another study (Rukhadze and Kubin, 2007), report

the existence of an anatomical connection between LC neurons and the hypoglossal motor

pool. Based on these findings, I hypothesized that intermittent optical stimulation of ChR2-

expressing LC axons at the hypoglossal motor pool would induce repeated release of

noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons and trigger hLTF of inspiratory genioglossus

motor activity. I found that repeated stimulation of LC axons did not change genioglossus motor

activity over time (1-way RM-ANOVA, F=0.2538, p=0.9031, Fig. 5.2). This could imply that LC-

induced hLTF is not mediated by a direct connection (i.e., LC cells do not directly act on

hypoglossal motor neurons to trigger hLTF). Alternatively, it could also imply that axon terminal

stimulation did not cause sufficient release of noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons to

trigger hLTF.

Measurements of diaphragm amplitude and oxygen saturation showed a minor decrease

compared to baseline (1-way RM ANOVA, diaphragm amplitude: F=6.816, p=0.0021. O2

saturation: F=3.709, p=0.0255 Fig. 5.2C,E), but this change was within the range of other

experimental groups as no change can be observed when compared to animals given

intermittent LC stimulation (2-way RM ANOVA, Int. Stim of LC axons vs Int. LC Stim, Diaphragm

amplitude: F=0.08737, p=0.9859. O2 saturation: F=2.199, p=0.0864). Respiratory frequency was

unaffected following intermittent stimulation of LC axons (1-way RM ANOVA, F=0.3989,

p=0.8066. Fig. 5.2D). Taken together, it suggests that intermittent stimulation of LC axons could

not elicit hLTF, and did not influence other respiratory variables.

Page 125: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

109

Figure 5.2. Intermittent stimulation of LC axons did not trigger hLTF. (A) Histological example of the LC expressing tyrosine hydroxylase (TH, green), channelrhodopsin2 (mCherry, red), and the overlay. Last panel depicts the presence of non-ChR2-expressing mCherry positive axons at the hypoglossal motor pool, and optic probe tract location. CC, central canal. (B) Group data (n=5) showing integrated genioglossus amplitude (∫GG EMG) following intermittent stimulation of LC axons at the level of the hypoglossal motor pool. No change in genioglossus amplitude was observed across all time points. A significant decrease in Integrated diaphragm amplitude (∫Dia EMG) was observed at 30, 45, and 60 minutes, and in oxygen saturation at 60 minutes (C,E). Respiratory frequency was unaffected (D). Data presented as mean + SEM. *denotes significance (p<0.05) between groups.

Page 126: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

110

5.3.2 Noradrenaline released from the LC is critical for hLTF

Although intermittent stimulation of LC axons did not trigger hLTF suggesting it is not a direct

connection, my next step indirectly readdresses this connection by directly stimulating LC

neurons while simultaneously blocking its downstream effects. Specifically, I performed

repeated optical stimulation of ChR2-expressing LC cells (5 Hz for 15 seconds separated by 1

minute recovery, repeated 10 times) while perfusing 1 µM terazosin, an α1-adrenergic receptor

antagonist, at the hypoglossal motor pool (Fig. 5.1B). When α1-adrenergic receptors on the

hypoglossal motor pool are blocked, intermittent LC stimulation could not trigger hLTF. In fact,

a significant decrease in genioglossus motor output was observed (1-way RM ANOVA, F=3.737,

p=0.0166, Fig. 5.3). The absence of hLTF suggests that the release of noradrenaline from the LC

acts on α1-adrenergic receptors at the hypoglossal motor pool for hLTF to manifest.

This finding supports earlier studies that show α1-adrenergic receptors at the hypoglossal

motor pool prevented apnea-induced hLTF (Tadjalli et al., 2010), suggesting α1-adrenergic

receptor activation is a critical step in initiating the mechanism underlying hLTF of inspiratory

genioglossus motor output.

To ensure the α1-adrenergic receptor blockade did not influence other respiratory variables, I

quantified diaphragm amplitude, respiratory frequency, and oxygen saturation. Intermittent LC

stimulation with α1-adrenergic receptor blockade did not significantly influence these

respiratory variables over time (2-way RM ANOVA, Intermittent LC stimulation vs Intermittent

LC stimulation + Terazosin: diaphragm: F=0.0888, p=0.9856; respiratory frequency: F=0.4509,

p=0.7712; oxygen saturation: F=1.967, p=0.1133, Fig. 5.3E-G), suggesting neither intermittent

LC stimulation nor the dose of terazosin used had any impact on respiratory output.

A minor increase end-tidal CO2 was observed at the 45 and 60 minute time points (1-way RM

ANOVA, F=3.707, p=0.0152, Fig. 5.3H), however this increase was not physiologically relevant as

ET-CO2 at 60 minutes was at 5.25 ± 0.28% and was not accompanied by changes in respiratory

frequency or diaphragm amplitude.

Page 127: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

111

Page 128: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

112

Figure 5.3. hLTF is mediated by α1-adrenergic receptor binding of noradrenaline released from the LC. (A) Raw trace recorded from an anaesthetized, spontaneously breathing rat depicting spike in genioglossus motor activity following successful targeting of the hypoglossal motor pool with microdialysis probe. (B) A representative EMG trace of integrated inspiratory genioglossus (GG) activity following intermittent LC stimulation with simultaneous α1-adrenergic receptor blockade at the level of the hypoglossal motor pool at 15, 30, 45, and 60 min. (C) Probe tract locations in the LC (dotted outline, left panels) and in the hypoglossal motor pool (dotted outline, right panels) following LC stimulation and terazosin perfusion. Matching shade of pink/purple represents probe locations in the LC and hypoglossal motor pool within each animal. (D) Group data showing inspiratory genioglossus activity of animals perfused with the α1-adrenergic blocker (n=8, black bars) could not exhibit hLTF following intermittent LC stimulation. Diaphragm activity, respiratory frequency, and oxygen saturation was unaffected compared to animals that received the same intervention absent terazosin (n=8, E-G). A significant but physiologically irrelevant increase was observed in end-tidal CO2 levels at 45 and 60 min (H). Dotted line represents percent baseline activity. Data are presented as mean + SEM. * denotes a significant difference (p<0.05).

5.3.3 Saline perfusion into the hypoglossal motor pool does not influence hLTF expression

Perfusion of α1-adrenergic receptor antagonist, terazosin, prevents both apnea-induced and

LC-induced hLTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor output. However, it is important to ensure

that the absence of hLTF observed is a result of α1-adrenergic receptor blockade thereby

preventing LC-induced hLTF, and not a result of damage caused by probe insertion into the

hypoglossal motor pool. However, experiments that show saline perfusion into the hypoglossal

motor pool do not affect hLTF expression has already been performed in earlier experiments

(chapter 3), as well as in past studies (Tadjalli, 2012, Tadjalli et al., 2010). As such, I deliberately

did not perform experiments with repeated stimulation of LC neurons while perfusing saline

into the hypoglossal motor pool. However, to provide a direct comparison showing LC-induced

hLTF with and without α1-adrenergic receptor blockade, here I show genioglossus amplitude

following perfusion of saline or terazosin after exposure to an hLTF trigger (i.e., repeated

apneas or repeated LC stimulation). When saline was perfused at the hypoglossal motor pool,

the manifestation of hLTF was uninterrupted compared to when α1-adrenergic receptors were

blocked (2-way RM-ANOVA, Int. LC Stim + Teraz vs Saline perfusion at hypoglossal, F=9.653,

p<0.0001. Fig. 5.4A). In both groups, diaphragm amplitude and respiratory frequency were

unaffected (2-way ANOVA, Int. LC Stim + Teraz vs Saline, Diaphragm amplitude: F=0.7625,

p=0.5549. Respiratory frequency: F=1.901, p=0.1243. Fig. 5.4B,C). This reinforces that saline

perfusion into the hypoglossal motor pool does not negatively influence the expression of hLTF

following a trigger.

Page 129: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

113

Figure 5.4. Saline perfusion at hypoglossal motor pool does not affect hLTF. (A) Group data showing apnea-induced hLTF of integrated genioglossus activity (∫GG EMG) was unaffected following saline perfusion into the hypoglossal motor pool (n=6, white bars) compared to terazosin perfused animals following intermittent LC stimulation (n=8, black bars). In both groups, integrated diaphragm activity (∫Dia EMG) and respiratory frequency (B,C) were unaffected. Dotted line represents percent baseline activity. Data are presented as mean + SEM. * denotes a significant difference (p<0.05).

5.3.4 α1-Adrenergic receptor blockade abolishes hLTF expression

As discussed in chapter 4, this statistical analysis used in these experiments compares animals

that exhibited hLTF following intermittent LC stimulation to animals that did not exhibit hLTF

following terazosin perfusion. However, approximately 27% of animals do not exhibit hLTF

following intermittent LC stimulation and the exclusion of these animals may skew the

statistical analysis used. Here, I use a chi-square test, a Firth logistic regression, and an ordinary

least square linear regression (OLS) to statistically analyze whether my intervention influenced

the probability of hLTF expression. These statistical approaches remove the confound of

excluding non-responders by including all animals (i.e., animals that exhibit hLTF and non-

responders) in my analysis.

Page 130: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

114

Following a chi-square test, I found that terazosin perfusion prevented LC-induced hLTF and this

result was significantly correlated with my intervention (chi-square test, Int. LC Stim. vs Int. LC

Stim. + Teraz, chi2=10.05, p=0.002). Similarly, upon performing a Firth logistic regression, I

found a significant difference in the probability of hLTF expression when terazosin is perfused

at the hypoglossal motor pool compared to intermittent stimulation alone (Firth logistic

regression, Int. LC Stim. vs Int. LC Stim. + Teraz, p=0.019. Fig. 5.5). Lastly, upon performing an

OLS analysis, I showed that terazosin perfusion at the hypoglossal motor pool prevented LC-

induced hLTF and that it was not due to random chance (OLS, Int. LC Stim. vs Int. LC Stim. +

Teraz, R2=0.3272, F(32)=7.782, β=0.9541, p=0.002). Taken together, all three statistical analyses

support the same finding, suggesting that the LC-induced hLTF is mediated through

noradrenergic mechanism with noradrenaline release from the LC onto α1-adrenergic receptors

on the hypoglossal motor neurons.

Figure 5.5. Probability of hLTF was reduced following α1-adrenergic receptor blockade at the hypoglossal motor pool. Following intermittent LC stimulation (n=8, blue), hLTF expression was observed in 73% of all attempts. However, intermittent LC stimulation could not elicit hLTF when α1-adrenergic receptors were blocked at the hypoglossal motor pool (pink). Each line represents an animal that received one of the interventions. α1-adrenergic receptor binding is required for hLTF expression.

Page 131: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

115

5.4 Discussion

The first aim of this chapter was to determine whether the LC directly influences hypoglossal

motor neuron activity to trigger hLTF. Since the LC was reported to be the largest source of

noradrenaline in the brain (Moore, 1979) and its stimulation elicits hLTF (chapter 4),

noradrenaline is presumably the critical neurotransmitter mediating hLTF. However, since the

LC has diverse projections to other noradrenergic brainstem structures that innervate

hypoglossal motor neurons such as the A5 and subcoeruleus (Byrum, 1987, Sakai et al., 1977), it

became necessary to determine whether hLTF elicited by intermittent LC stimulation was

mediated by a direct connection. Here I show that intermittent stimulation of ChR2-expressing

LC axons at the hypoglossal motor pool did not elicit hLTF.

The second aim of this chapter was to determine whether noradrenaline released from the LC

is the underlying mechanism mediating hLTF. The LC was reported to co-release multiple

neurotransmitters. Here I show that noradrenaline acting on α1-adrenergic receptors is

required for hLTF expression, suggesting noradrenaline is the critical neurotransmitter

mediating hLTF.

5.4.1 Direct stimulation of LC axons projecting to the hypoglossal motor pool did not elicit

hLTF

In Chapter 3, I provided evidence to show that the LC has direct noradrenergic projections to

the hypoglossal neurons, with clearly visible axon terminals at the hypoglossal motor pool. In

this chapter, I aimed to stimulate these axon terminals in an attempt to elicit hLTF. However,

upon intermittent stimulation of LC axons expressing ChR2, hLTF was not expressed. Although

these experiments were performed in a small group of animals (n=5), this finding suggests that

hLTF may not be mediated by a direct connection originating from the region of the LC to the

hypoglossal motor pool, but could be mediated by the LC to another noradrenergic structure

such as the A5 or subcoeruleus (SubC) (Byrum, 1987, Sakai et al., 1977). If hLTF is mediated by

an indirect connection stemming from the LC, I theorize it would act through these

Page 132: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

116

noradrenergic structures to activate α1-adrenergic receptors on the hypoglossal motor neurons

to elicit hLTF. It may therefore be possible for LC-induced hLTF to be mediated through an

indirect pathway (i.e., LC → A5/SubC → XII).

Alternatively, the absence of hLTF observed following stimulation of LC axons could be a failure

in the technique. Prior to my findings in chapter 3, the LC was reported to possess 1.7% of the

noradrenergic input to the hypoglossal motor pool. The limited noradrenergic input from the LC

may result in insufficient stimulation of LC axons and therefore insufficient noradrenaline

release to induce hLTF.

In chapter 4, I provide evidence that a minimum threshold of LC stimulation is required to

trigger hLTF. LC neurons have been reported to fire synchronously upon stimulation (Aston-

Jones, 1981, Ishimatsu and Williams, 1996), and any stimulation below the threshold may not

trigger a synchronous response. Stimulation of LC axons may have been insufficient at

triggering a synchronous response in LC cells. Furthermore, the synchronicity of LC firing was

hypothesized to be mediated by gap junctions (Travagli et al., 1995) and axon stimulation may

not trigger a synchronous response that may only be achievable through stimulation at the cell

body.

Lastly, the absence of hLTF observed could be due to the use of a non-specific viral vector. The

axons observed at the level of the hypoglossal motor pool may not be noradrenergic. As such,

stimulation of these axons may be causing the release of neurotransmitters that do not elicit

hLTF. Further experiments would be required to verify the phenotype of these axon terminals.

Due to the difficulties associated with axon stimulation listed above, I decided to focus on the

mechanism underlying hLTF and determine whether noradrenaline released from the LC is the

critical neurotransmitter mediating hLTF.

Page 133: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

117

5.4.2 The LC co-releases neurotransmitters that can induce plasticity and/or modulate

respiratory output

The LC was reported to co-release multiple neurotransmitters including glutamate (Fung et al.,

1994, Trudeau, 2004), galanin (Holets et al., 1988, Tsuda et al., 1989), neuropeptide y (Everitt et

al., 1984, Holets et al., 1988, Tsuda et al., 1989), enkephalin (Van Bockstaele et al., 2000), and

dopamine (Devoto and Flore, 2006, Kempadoo et al., 2016, Smith and Greene, 2012). These

neurotransmitters can induce plasticity and/or influence respiratory activity. The most studied

co-released neurotransmitter is glutamate, which can act on NMDA receptors found on

hypoglossal motor neurons to augment respiratory output (Steenland et al., 2006). In the

context of LTF, NMDA receptor blockade abolished hypoxia-induced phrenic LTF (McGuire et

al., 2005). Similarly, LTF of hypoglossal nerve activity was abolished upon metabotropic

glutamate receptor blockade (Feldman et al., 2005, Sharifullina et al., 2004). Together, this

suggests that glutamate may play an important role in mediating hLTF. The mechanism

mediating hLTF was hypothesized to involved post-synaptic changes to AMPA receptor function

(Feldman et al., 2005, Wang et al., 2005) or AMPA receptor recruitment (Itami et al., 2003). In

this study, I showed that noradrenaline is the critical neurotransmitter required for hLTF

expression; co-released glutamate acting on AMPA receptors may still be involved.

Other neurotransmitters co-released by LC neurons include galanin which can attenuate the

chemosensory reflex by reducing phrenic nerve activity during hypoxia (Abbott and Pilowsky,

2009). Galanin can inhibit other forms of plasticity such as long-term potentiation (Coumis and

Davies, 2002), presumably through the inhibition of the CREB signalling cascade (Badie-Mahdavi

et al., 2005). Neuropeptide Y is another co-released neurotransmitter that can reduce

respiratory rate and tidal volume when applied to the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) (Barraco

et al., 1990) as well as reduce presynaptic glutamate release (Furtinger et al., 2001), suggesting

neuropeptide Y may interact with the tripartite circuit (NTS → LC → XII) hypothesized to be

mediating respiratory motor plasticity. Enkephalin has also been shown to depress the activity

of respiratory neurons in the NTS (Sessle and Henry, 1985), and potentially plays a crucial role

in regulating the efficiency of neurotransmission at the synapse by disinhibiting nearby

Page 134: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

118

GABAergic transmission (Morris and Johnston, 1995), although this interaction requires further

investigation. Lastly, dopamine can be co-released by activating LC neurons. Electrical

stimulation of the LC has increased dopamine levels in the prefrontal cortex in a frequency-

dependent manner (Choi et al., 2015), and optical stimulation of the LC increases dopamine

levels in the dorsal hippocampus (Kempadoo et al., 2016). In the context of respiration,

dopamine can modulate the chemosensory response (Hedner et al., 1982, Hsiao et al., 1989),

and dopamine receptor agonists can increase the hypercapnic ventilatory response (Lalley,

2008). In the context of plasticity, dopamine promotes structural changes at the synapse

(Yagishita et al., 2014). Neuromodulators such as noradrenaline and dopamine can initiate

rapid activation of neurotrophic downstream signalling (Natarajan and Berk, 2006), which in

turn can enhance synaptic signalling by acting on phospholipase C (PLC) or protein kinase

pathways (Blum and Konnerth, 2005). However, there is little evidence to suggest dopamine is

playing an active role in hLTF. Instead, noradrenaline is the critical neurotransmitter required

for hLTF expression.

5.4.3 The same intracellular machinery is involved in mediating hLTF expression across

multiple triggers

Upon intermittent stimulation of LC neurons while simultaneously blocking α1-adrenergic

receptors at the hypoglossal motor pool, hLTF could not be elicited. This supports previous

findings that show hLTF elicited by repeated apneas was abolished upon α1-adrenergic

receptor blockade at the hypoglossal motor pool (Tadjalli et al., 2010). In fact, hLTF elicited by

repeated episodes of hypoxia was also abolished upon systemic α1-receptor blockade

(Neverova et al., 2007). Taken together, this suggests that noradrenaline, and specifically α1-

adrenergic receptor activation, is critical for hLTF regardless of the trigger (i.e., repeated

apneas, repeated episodes of hypoxia, or repeated LC stimulation).

In my results, I showed that the probability of hLTF was greatly reduced upon LC inactivation or

α1-adrenergic receptor blockade. This suggests that the trigger (repeated apneas or optical LC

stimulation) must act through the LC which in turn intermittently releases noradrenaline to

Page 135: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

119

bind α1-adrenergic receptors at the hypoglossal motor pool to initiate hLTF. This is important

because it suggests that the same intracellular machinery is involved in mediating hLTF

regardless of trigger. These findings could also provide direction for pharmaceutical companies

to direct their focus towards activating α1-adrenergic machinery to trigger hLTF.

5.4.4 Methodological Considerations

One consideration in these experiments was discussed in earlier chapters which identified the

viral vector used in these studies to be non-specific to noradrenergic cells but instead infected

all neurons. As such, it is possible that stimulation of cells in the LC that are non-noradrenergic

in nature may have contributed to hLTF. However, the findings in this chapter still identify

noradrenaline to be the key player in mediating hLTF. It does not however imply that other

neurotransmitters co-released are uninvolved as my approach does not address the role of

other cells and neurotransmitters and their effects on hLTF. For example, astrocytes produce

and release L-lactate which can trigger noradrenaline release from the LC (Tang et al., 2014). It

could, therefore, be possible for intermittent stimulation to cause astrocytic release of L-lactate

to induce and/or enhance noradrenaline release from the LC. Nonetheless, noradrenaline

acting on α1-adrenergic receptors on hypoglossal motor neurons is critical to the manifestation

of hLTF.

Another consideration includes my approach which involved the perfusion of 1 µM terazosin

into the hypoglossal motor pool via reverse microdialysis. It is possible that terazosin perfusion

affected α1-adrenergic receptors found on NTS neurons (Zhang and Mifflin, 2007), and that this

interaction influenced the expression of hLTF. However, α1-adrenergic receptors on NTS

neurons are not tonically active (Zhang and Mifflin, 2007) and its antagonism should therefore

have no influence on hLTF expression.

Following terazosin perfusion, intermittent stimulation of ChR2-expressing LC cells induced a

decrease in genioglossus amplitude. This was initially surprising considering the dose of

terazosin (1 µM) delivered through reverse-microdialysis was used previously without affecting

Page 136: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

120

baseline genioglossus motor output (Tadjalli, 2012). The decrease in genioglossus amplitude

observed may be attributed to the effects of the co-released neurotransmitter galanin. Galanin

co-released from LC terminals has been shown to be inhibitory, reducing LC firing rate and

inducing hyperpolarization presumably via an increase in K+ conductance (Xu et al., 2005). Its

release is also dependent on the depletion of noradrenaline following high phasic bursting of LC

neurons (Consolo et al., 1994, Weinshenker and Holmes, 2016). The decrease in genioglossus

amplitude observed may therefore be a result of decreased LC activity caused by galanin, but

further studies are needed to verify this interaction.

Alternatively, the decrease in genioglossus amplitude could also be due to the intended release

of noradrenaline from the LC. Hypoglossal motor neurons have been reported to express both

α1 and α2 adrenergic receptors (Volgin et al., 2001). Although α1-adrenergic receptor

activation has been reported to produce excitatory effects on the post-synaptic cell, α2-

adrenergic receptor activation is traditionally considered to inhibit activity (DeBock et al., 2003).

In fact, activation of α2-adrenergic receptors with hypercapnia can cause long-term depression

of hypoglossal motor activity (Bach and Mitchell, 1996). Here, I block α1-adrenergic receptor

activation but continue to stimulate LC cells. It may, therefore, be possible that noradrenaline

could not bind to α1-adrenergic receptors on the hypoglossal motor neuron and bound instead

to open α2-adrenergic receptors causing the decrease in genioglossus amplitude observed.

Further studies would be required to confirm this interaction.

5.4.5 Scientific importance and clinical significance

Sleep onset is accompanied by a reduction in upper airway muscle tone and a sudden decrease

in LC activity (Aston-Jones and Bloom, 1981, Chan et al., 2006, Fenik et al., 2013, Fung and

Chase, 2015), which may suggest that noradrenaline released specifically from the LC is

involved in maintaining upper airway muscle tone during sleep. The results of this chapter

provide more definitive evidence that α1-adrenergic receptor activation, presumably by

noradrenaline released from the LC, to be the key required drive hLTF expression. The

Page 137: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

121

involvement of the LC is important because forced noradrenaline release from other

noradrenergic cell groups such as the A5 and A7 only augments inspiratory genioglossus motor

output for 20 minutes (Song and Poon, 2017). It is possible that persistent release of

noradrenaline from the LC is required to sustain hLTF for longer durations, implying that

plasticity at the LC is necessary for persistent hLTF.

The fact that α1-adrenergic receptors activation is consistently involved in gate-keeping hLTF

expression regardless of the trigger suggests that direct manipulation of α1 receptor

intracellular machinery could be the next steps towards a pharmaceutical treatment to induce

LTF of genioglossus motor output. These findings could provide a direction for pharmaceutical

companies to focus towards a treatment for respiratory disorders such as obstructive sleep

apnea.

Page 138: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

122

Chapter Six – General Discussion

Prior to the experiments discussed in this thesis, the mechanism and circuit underlying

hypoglossal/genioglossus long-term facilitation (hLTF) were not clearly elucidated. What was

known included the requirement for an intermittent stimulus (Tadjalli, 2012), activation of α1-

adrenergic receptors on the hypoglossal motor pool (Tadjalli et al., 2010), and activation of the

nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) (Torontali, 2012), the primary terminate site for vagal afferents.

However, (1) the source of noradrenaline was unknown, (2) the complete neural circuit

underlying hLTF was unknown, and (3) the site of plasticity was unknown.

Over the last 6 years, I have addressed all three unknowns. I have identified the source of

noradrenaline to be the LC as it is the only noradrenergic structure to increase in c-Fos

expression, and presumably increase activity, following apnea-induced hLTF (chapter 3). I have

also shown that the LC has direct projections to the hypoglossal motor pool (chapter 3),

suggesting a neural circuit (NTS → LC → XII) may underlie hLTF. I also showed that intermittent

stimulation of the LC alone can trigger hLTF (chapter 4), while inactivation of the LC reduced the

probability for hLTF to manifest (chapter 3 and 4). In addition, intermittent stimulation of the LC

while blocking α1-adrenergic receptors at the hypoglossal motor pool prevented hLTF (chapter

5), suggesting the LC releases noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons to drive hLTF.

Furthermore, I showed a greater increase in c-Fos expression in the LC of animals that exhibited

hLTF compared to animals that did not, suggesting an increase in LC activity (i.e., plasticity at

the LC) is what drives hLTF (chapter 4). Taken together, my data has added to the field of

respiratory motor plasticity research by identifying the neural circuit that mediates hLTF (i.e.,

NTS → LC → XII), in addition to identifying a new trigger for hLTF that bypasses respiratory

feedback by directly stimulating the LC. Lastly, this research provides further evidence to

support the role of noradrenaline in mediating plasticity of the upper airways as hLTF requires

sustained noradrenaline release from the LC onto α1-adrenergic receptors on the hypoglossal

motor pool. In this section, I discuss the significance of my experimental findings, the

methodological considerations, and the future directions of LTF research.

Page 139: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

123

6.1. Hypoglossal / genioglossus LTF is mechanistically distinct from phrenic LTF

The field of LTF research has largely been focused on the diaphragm or phrenic nerve (pLTF).

However, LTF can occur in other respiratory muscles such as the genioglossus. Although the

duration of LTF (and thereby its definition) varies across studies, ranging from 20 minutes

(Harris et al., 2006, Song and Poon, 2017) to the standard experimental duration of 60 minutes

(i.e., the standard for pLTF), I chose to abide by the time frame of 60 minutes to define hLTF to

provide a better comparison to the most commonly studied form of LTF. The goal of my PhD is

to elucidate the mechanisms and neural circuit underlying hLTF. By choosing the 60-minute

time frame, it allowed for comparisons with pLTF, which comes with a rich foundation of

research, providing insight into what could be happening at the level of the neuron during LTF

of genioglossus muscle activity.

The triggers and mechanisms underlying pLTF have been extensively studied, with current

evidence identifying the most common trigger to be repeated bouts of hypoxia (Bach and

Mitchell, 1996, Baker and Mitchell, 2000, Bocchiaro and Feldman, 2004, Fuller et al., 2001). The

mechanism which underlies hypoxia-induced pLTF is hypothesized to be serotonin (5HT)

dependent, correlating an increase in phrenic nerve activity with an increase serotonergic

raphe firing rates in cats (Morris et al., 2001). This led to studies in rats showing hypoxia-

induced pLTF required the binding of serotonin to 5HT2 receptors at the level of the phrenic

motor pool (Bocchiaro and Feldman, 2004, Fuller et al., 2001, Kinkead et al., 2001, McGuire et

al., 2004), and that inhibition of caudal raphe neurons prevented hypoxia-induced pLTF (Dodig

et al., 2012, Valic et al., 2010), suggesting caudal raphe neurons are the source of serotonin

mediating pLTF. The intracellular cascade at the phrenic motor neuron has also been

investigated, identifying the requirement of TrkB receptor activation (Dale et al., 2017), as well

as identifying ERK/MAPK activation at the level of the motor neuron that ultimately determine

drives and maintains pLTF (Hoffman et al., 2012, Wilkerson and Mitchell, 2009).

By direct comparison, the triggers and mechanisms underlying hLTF have been less extensively

studied. Current evidence show various triggers for hLTF, including repeated apneas (Lui et al.,

Page 140: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

124

2018, Song and Poon, 2017, Tadjalli et al., 2010), repeated modulation of vagal afferents

(Tadjalli et al., 2010), or repeated stimulation of the LC (chapter 4). The mechanism which

underlies hLTF is hypothesized to be noradrenaline-dependent, correlating an increase in

genioglossus motor activity with an increase in c-Fos expression in LC neurons (Lui et al., 2018).

In addition, studies have shown apnea-induced hLTF required the binding of noradrenaline to

α1-adrenergic receptors at the level of the hypoglossal motor pool (Tadjalli et al., 2010; chapter

five), and that inhibition of LC neurons prevented apnea-induced hLTF (chapter 3 and 4),

suggesting LC neurons are the source of noradrenaline mediating hLTF. The intracellular

cascade at the hypoglossal motor neuron has also been investigated, identifying the

requirement of TrkB receptor activation (Tadjalli, 2012). Taken together, this suggests that the

mechanism underlying hLTF was investigated in the same manner as pLTF but was found to be

mechanistically distinct from each other (i.e., serotonin vs. noradrenaline) (Table 6.1).

Page 141: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

125

Table 6.1. Comparison of mechanisms underlying pLTF vs hLTF

Animal Model

Intervention Used Primary Finding Reference

Ph

ren

ic /

Dia

ph

ragm

LTF

Rats Repeated bouts of hypoxia

Hypoxia-induced pLTF occurs in rats and requires serotonin

(Bach and Mitchell, 1996)

Cats Repeated bouts of hypoxia

Hypoxia-induced pLTF correlates with an increase in serotonergic raphe neurons firing

(Morris et al., 2001)

Rats Repeated bouts of hypoxia + 5HT2 receptor antagonism

Hypoxia-induced pLTF requires serotonin binding to 5HT2 receptors

(Fuller et al., 2001, McGuire et al., 2004)

Rats Repeated bouts of hypoxia + inhibition of caudal raphe

Hypoxia-induced pLTF requires activation of caudal raphe neurons

(Dodig et al., 2012, Valic et al., 2010)

Rats Repeated bouts of hypoxia + TrkB receptor blockade at phrenic motor pool

Hypoxia-induced pLTF requires TrkB receptor activation at the phrenic motor pool

(Dale et al., 2017)

Rats Repeated bouts of hypoxia + ERK/MAPK antagonism

Hypoxia-induced pLTF requires ERK/MAPK activation at the phrenic motor pool

(Hoffman et al., 2012, Wilkerson and Mitchell, 2009)

Hyp

ogl

oss

al /

Gen

iogl

oss

us

LTF

Rats Repeated apneas Apnea-induced hLTF occurs in rats and requires noradrenaline

(Tadjalli et al., 2010)

Rats Repeated apneas Apnea-induced hLTF correlates with an increase in c-Fos expression in LC neurons

(Lui et al., 2018)

Rats Repeated apneas + α1-receptor blockade at hypoglossal

Apnea-induced hLTF requires noradrenaline binding to α1-receptor at hypoglossal motor pool

(Tadjalli et al., 2010)

Rats Repeated apneas + LC inactivation

Apnea-induced hLTF requires activation of LC neurons

Chapter 3 Chapter 4

Rats Intermittent LC stimulation

LC stimulation triggers hLTF Chapter 4

Rats Intermittent LC stimulation + α1-receptor blockade at hypoglossal

LC-induced hLTF requires α1-receptor activation at hypoglossal

Chapter 5

Rats Repeated apneas + TrkB receptor blockade at hypoglossal

Apnea-induced hLTF requires TrkB activation at the hypoglossal motor pool

(Tadjalli, 2012)

Rats TrkB agonist at hypoglossal

hLTF can be induced by activating TrkB signalling alone

(Tadjalli, 2012)

Page 142: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

126

6.2. The brainstem circuit mediating hLTF: NTS → LC → XII

Prior to the current experiments outlined in this thesis, the hypothesized neural circuit

underlying hLTF was not clearly elucidated. hLTF could be elicited by modulation of vagal

afferents alone by repeatedly cooling the vagus nerve to inhibit vagal neurotransmission

(Tadjalli et al., 2010). This suggested that vagal neurotransmission (or the absence of it) is

required for hLTF expression, and was further supported when apnea-induced hLTF was

prevented by vagotomy or continuous cooling of vagal afferents (Tadjalli et al., 2010). Since the

nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) is the primary termination site for vagal afferents, these

experiments suggested that the NTS is critical for apnea-induced hLTF (Tadjalli et al., 2010). This

was supported when the inactivation of NTS neurons also prevented apnea-induced hLTF (Lui,

S., et al. 2018), solidifying the NTS as a critical component of the hLTF circuit.

In chapter 3, I showed that apnea-induced hLTF correlated with an increase in c-Fos expression

in LC neurons. In fact, it was the only noradrenergic brainstem region to display a greater

increase in c-Fos expression compared to animals without intervention (i.e., repeated apneas),

and animals exposed to the intervention but were absent hLTF. The latter is important as it

suggests that the increase in c-Fos expression in LC cells is not attributed to the hypoxia or

hypercapnia associated with repeated obstructive apneas. Instead, it suggested that the LC is

the source of noradrenaline mediating hLTF. The LC also receives direct projections from the

NTS (Lopes et al., 2016, van Bockstaele et al., 1999). These experiments were followed by a

tracing study to determine whether the LC has direct projections to the hypoglossal motor pool.

Current literature claims that LC neurons have limited, if any, direct innervations to hypoglossal

motor neurons (Aldes, 1990, Fritschy and Grzanna, 1990, Levitt and Moore, 1979, Rukhadze

and Kubin, 2007), suggesting the link between an increase in LC activity and LTF of genioglossus

motor output may, therefore, not be a direct interaction. However, contrary to published

studies, a large proportion of LC neurons were found to project to the hypoglossal motor pool,

setting LC neurons to be anatomically positioned to drive hypoglossal motor activity. To verify

that the LC provides a functional role in mediating hLTF, ChR2-expressing LC cells were

stimulated in the same pattern as the repeated apnea protocol and was found to trigger hLTF of

Page 143: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

127

genioglossus motor output. These results are important because it identifies a novel trigger of

respiratory motor plasticity; it suggests that respiratory motor plasticity of genioglossus muscle

activity can be elicited without directly manipulating respiratory activity. In addition, LC

neurons were pharmacologically inactivated, whereupon its inactivation prevented apnea-

induced hLTF. To account for the limitations of a pharmacological approach and to increase

precision, this study was repeated using optical inactivation of the LC and yielded identical

results (chapter 4).

Furthermore, I verified that LC manipulation (i.e., stimulation or inhibition) influenced the

probability for hLTF to manifest compared to controls. This is important as few studies to date

in the field of LTF have reported the percentage of responders and non-responders following an

intervention. Repeated bouts of hypoxia can be effective at eliciting pLTF, but some studies

have reported no pLTF following repeated bouts of hypoxia (Chowdhuri et al., 2015, Janssen

and Fregosi, 2000, McGuire et al., 2002). However, the exact percentage of responders was not

reported. This suggests that LTF, although not directly reported, does not always occur

following an intervention. Here, I showed that repeated apneas consistently elicit hLTF in 65%

of animals exposed to the intervention, while intermittent LC stimulation elicit hLTF in 73% of

animals, both of which are comparable to the probability of plasticity occurring in other

systems, such as long-term potentiation in hippocampal neurons (Abraham and Huggett, 1997,

Karunakaran et al., 2016, Watanabe et al., 2002). Taken together, this suggests that the LC is a

critical component of the hLTF circuit, and its manipulation influences hLTF expression.

Lastly, it is unsurprising that the hypoglossal motor pool is involved in mediating hLTF to drive

genioglossus activity, completing the tripartite circuit (NTS → LC → XII) that mediates hLTF.

Page 144: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

128

6.3 The site of plasticity in the hLTF tripartite circuit

Within the neural circuit mediating hLTF, there remains the question of where plasticity is

occurring. This question can be addressed by investigating the wealth of data within the

histology. Specifically, using c-Fos as an indicator for cell activity, it is possible to deduce where

plasticity may be occurring. The neural circuit (i.e., NTS → LC → XII) identifies the NTS as the

first structure critical to apnea-induced hLTF with direct projections to the LC (Lopes et al.,

2016, van Bockstaele et al., 1999). The LC is the second critical structure in the hLTF circuit and

was shown to not only possess direct projections to the hypoglossal motor pool but is critical in

apnea-induced hLTF and its stimulation can trigger hLTF. Plasticity could therefore potentially

be occurring at one or more structures within the neural circuit.

6.3.1 Plasticity at the NTS

Plasticity in the NTS has been observed before (Kline, 2008, Yamamoto et al., 2015). For

example, in hypertensive rats, NTS neurons displayed an increase in AMPA receptors and

expression of larger dendritic spines (Aicher et al., 2003, Chan et al., 2000, Saha et al., 2004). In

the context of respiration and LTF, chronic intermittent hypoxia attenuated glutamate-induced

inhibition of phrenic nerve activity (Costa-Silva et al., 2011), and acute intermittent optical

stimulation of NTS neurons triggered pLTF (Yamamoto et al., 2015). This suggests that the NTS

alone is capable of inducing LTF of phrenic/diaphragm activity. It may, therefore, be possible

that since the LC projects to the NTS (Lopes et al., 2016), intermittent LC stimulation resulted in

NTS stimulation, thereby eliciting hLTF through plasticity induced within NTS neurons. This

outcome may be unlikely since pLTF was not observed in my results, but it nonetheless suggests

that NTS neurons are capable of inducing hLTF, and a subset of NTS neurons may, therefore, be

undergoing plasticity to induce hLTF. In addition to these studies, hLTF requires NTS activity

(Torontali, 2012), and the NTS has projections to the hypoglossal motor pool (Rukhadze and

Kubin, 2007), suggesting the potential for direct modulation of hypoglossal motor neuron

activity. Although my results do not support this possibility (i.e., no increase in c-Fos activity

was observed in the NTS), c-Fos is not definitive and only provides an index of cell activity. This

Page 145: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

129

approach naturally has limitations and cannot account for all cell activation (or inactivation)

that may have occurred. Some neurons do not express c-Fos and may not have been detected

(Teppema et al., 1997). The NTS could, therefore, be a potential site of plasticity in the hLTF

circuit.

6.3.2 Plasticity at the LC

The LC is a stronger candidate to be the site of plasticity. Majority of the studies on pLTF has

suggested that plasticity is occurring at the level of the phrenic motor neuron. Serotonin and

noradrenaline are neuromodulators that can alter the synaptic efficacy and intrinsic membrane

properties of the post-synaptic neuron. However, it is possible that the site of plasticity

mediating LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor output is not at the hypoglossal motor neuron.

Although this differs from pLTF where plastic changes have been reported at the level of the

motor neuron (Baker-Herman et al., 2004, Dale et al., 2017, Hoffman et al., 2012, Wilkerson

and Mitchell, 2009), some experimental evidence suggest plasticity could be occurring

elsewhere. Specifically, pLTF observed in cats reported an increase in serotonergic raphe firing

rates during pLTF (Morris 2001), suggesting serotonergic raphe neurons altered their firing

pattern after repeated bouts of hypoxia (i.e., serotonergic raphe neurons exhibited plasticity).

In the context of hLTF, blockade of α1-adrenergic receptors after repeated apneas prevented

hLTF (Tadjalli, 2012). The lack of hLTF observed upon α1-receptor antagonism after hLTF

induction suggests that plasticity may not have occurred at the level of the hypoglossal motor

neuron but is responding to an increased and persistent release of noradrenaline. Plasticity is

therefore likely upstream of the hypoglossal motor pool, occurring potentially at the LC.

In this thesis, I provided evidence showing an increase in c-Fos expression (and presumably LC

activity) correlated with my intervention. For example, I showed an increase in c-Fos expression

in the LC following intermittent LC stimulation and fewer c-Fos positive cells following LC

inactivation. However, c-Fos expression represents a time window of cell activation that range

from 30 to 90 minutes. Therefore, the c-Fos expression could reflect cell activity at the 90-

Page 146: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

130

minute time point when it was reported to be maximally activated (Bullitt, 1990, Morgan et al.,

1987), or the c-Fos expression could reflect activity at the 30-minute time window. At 30

minutes, the c-Fos profile observed would not reflect the intervention but instead reflect LC

activity during hLTF expression at the 60-minute time point. The possibility that the c-Fos

profile observed is reflective of hLTF (i.e., the 60-minute time point) was supported when I

compared c-Fos profiles in animals that exhibited hLTF to non-responders. Animals that

exhibited hLTF had more c-Fos positive cells than non-responders which could be reflective of

persistent LC firing, indicating a prolonged change in LC firing pattern (i.e., the LC has

undergone plasticity), and implicating the LC as the site of plasticity.

LC neurons also fire synchronously when activated (Christie et al., 1989, Ishimatsu and Williams,

1996a) and release more noradrenaline following repeated stimulation compared to

continuous stimulation (Florin-Lechner et al., 1996), suggesting the LC is capable of increasing

the activity to release more noradrenaline. This too, was reflected in the histology as the

percentage or proportion of c-Fos positive cells was greater than the total number of non-

ChR2-expressing mCherry positive cells, suggesting that LC activation likely recruited uninfected

cells during LC stimulation (i.e., synchronous firing). This is further supported by the fact that

stimulation of any part of the LC (i.e., rostral, medial, or caudal) can still trigger hLTF. Taken

together, the LC is likely a site for plasticity to occur within the hLTF circuit.

Further evidence that suggests plasticity to be occurring at the LC involves a recent study that

showed a persistent form of hLTF lasting 60 minutes could not be elicited following stimulation

of non-LC noradrenergic cell groups (i.e., A5 and A7) (Song and Poon, 2017). This implies that

noradrenaline release caused by in vivo stimulation of non-LC noradrenergic cell groups is

insufficient at triggering a persistent form of hLTF. Plasticity may, therefore, be occurring at the

LC itself to deliver persistent release of noradrenaline. Over the last several decades, studies

showed that the LC can alter its firing patterns and adapt under context-specific conditions,

suggesting that the LC itself readily undergoes plastic changes to its firing pattern (Aston-Jones

and Bloom, 1981, Fazlali et al., 2016, Rajkowski et al., 2004, Vankov et al., 1995), further placing

Page 147: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

131

the LC as the likely site of plasticity in mediating hLTF. However, it is important to note that this

does not mean hypoglossal motor neurons did not undergo plastic changes as well. It is possible

that the LC mediates plasticity on two fronts - (1) the LC itself undergoes plastic changes to

modulate noradrenaline release to the target site, and (2) the LC modulates the activity of the

post-synaptic neurons via adrenergic receptor binding.

6.3.3 Plasticity at the hypoglossal motor neuron

Although the LC itself can undergo plasticity, the LC can also mediate plasticity via α- or β-

adrenergic receptor binding on the post-synaptic cell. In my introduction, I discussed how

noradrenaline binding can induce plastic changes to the post-synaptic motor neuron. Briefly,

the binding of α1-adrenergic receptors triggers a signalling cascade that initiates potentiation of

motor neuron firing by activating diacyl-glycerol (DAG) to activate protein kinase C (PKC). PKC is

hypothesized to cause AMPA receptor phosphorylation and is the hypothesized mechanism

that potentiates glutamate-evoked cell firing (Feldman et al., 2005, Neverova et al., 2007). The

activation of DAG and subsequently PKC can also increase protein synthesis of BDNF (Juric et

al., 2008), which in turn can lead to the insertion of new ion channels at the synapse (Itami et

al., 2003). Studies have shown that α1-adrenergic receptor activation on hypoglossal motor

neurons alone can trigger hLTF (Neverova et al., 2007). This suggests that noradrenaline

released from the LC could be initiating synaptic changes onto hypoglossal motor neurons, and

these changes could be the mechanism underlying hLTF.

Several studies also showed that the hypoglossal motor neuron can indeed undergo plasticity

and exhibit long-term changes on motor neuron membrane properties. For example, chronic

intermittent hypoxia spanning three months can increase α1-adrenergic receptor expression

and decrease 5HT-2A receptor expression at the hypoglossal motor pool (Rukhadze et al.,

2010). This suggests the hypoglossal motor neuron can adapt by changing receptor expression

at the synapse.

Page 148: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

132

Desipramine, a noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor, was recently shown to abolish the reduction

in genioglossus activity associated with the transition from wakefulness to non-rapid eye

moment (NREM) sleep (Taranto-Montemurro et al., 2016), suggesting the increased action on

α1-receptors presumably on hypoglossal motor neurons can augment genioglossus muscle

tone. In addition, the activation of α1-adrenergic receptors could cause brain-derived

neurotrophic factor (BDNF) to bind to TrkB receptors at the hypoglossal motor neuron, thereby

inducing hLTF. Activation of TrkB receptors can initiate kinase cascades leading to enhanced

synaptic efficacy or insertion of new ion channels to the pre- or post- synaptic membrane (Itami

et al., 2003, Juric et al., 2008). This was supported in studies that investigated the mechanisms

underlying pLTF where it was found to require novel protein synthesis via BDNF for pLTF to

manifest (Baker-Herman et al., 2004, Baker-Herman and Mitchell, 2002). In addition, activation

of TrkB receptors on or near phrenic motor neurons can elicit pLTF (Golder et al., 2008). Similar

mechanisms are observed in hLTF. Specifically, TrkB receptor blockade at the hypoglossal motor

pool prevented apnea-induced hLTF while TrkB receptor activation alone at the hypoglossal

could elicit hLTF (Tadjalli, 2012). By altering the activity of TrkB receptors at the level of the

hypoglossal motor pool, it was possible to alter genioglossus motor output, suggesting hLTF to

be mediated by a TrkB-receptor mechanism within the hypoglossal motor pool.

6.4 Summary of proposed mechanism underlying hLTF

My working hypothesis stemmed from apnea-induced hLTF. The trigger (repeated apneas)

elicited hLTF via repeated modulation of vagus-mediated broncho-pulmonary feedback (Tadjalli

et al., 2010), indicating an obstruction of airflow. This signal (or lack thereof) is propagated via

vagal afferents to terminate at the NTS, the first critical structure in my hypothesized hLTF

neural circuit (Fig. 6.1). The NTS in turns send direct excitatory projections to the LC. I

hypothesize that this connection is glutamatergic based on 4 lines of evidence: (1) there is an

increase in LC activity following apnea-induced hLTF, suggesting an excitatory neurotransmitter

is involved, (2) recordings from LC neurons that receive NTS innervations showed an increase in

the firing rates of LC neurons during hypercapnia (Lopes et al., 2016), and (3) LC-projecting NTS

Page 149: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

133

neurons were shown using electron microscopy to possess structures that implicate they are

glutamatergic axon terminals (van Bockstaele et al., 1999), and (4) plasticity at the NTS has

been reported to involve glutamatergic signalling (Bonham et al., 2006, Kline, 2008). Together,

these data suggest that the NTS → LC connection is likely a glutamatergic connection. The

patterned activation of LC neurons by the NTS causes a recruitment of additional LC neurons to

release noradrenaline onto hypoglossal motor neurons (Christie et al., 1989, Ishimatsu and

Williams, 1996b). The LC then undergoes plastic changes by increasing its activity for persistent

noradrenaline release. Noradrenaline binds to α1-adrenergic receptors on hypoglossal motor

neurons to induce hLTF, potentially via increased excitability to the post-synaptic cell caused by

reduce potassium currents and AMPA channel insertion. The increased excitability of

hypoglossal motor neurons allows for increased genioglossus motor output (i.e., hLTF). I

thereby hypothesize that plasticity is occurring at two fronts: (1) increased noradrenaline

release from altered LC activity, and (2) increased excitability of hypoglossal motor neurons

through post-synaptic changes.

Page 150: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

134

Figure 6.1. Hypothesized neural circuit underlying hLTF. (A) Brain map showing the potential sources of noradrenaline mediating hLTF. The locus coeruleus (LC) was shown to be the critical structure mediating hLTF and the hypothesized circuit is outlined in purple (inset) and expanded in (B). (B) Repeated obstructive apneas modulate vagal afferent activity, which terminates in the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS). Cells in the NTS send glutamatergic (Glu) projections to activate locus coeruleus neurons to release noradrenaline (NA) directly onto the α1-adrenergic receptors on hypoglossal (XII) motor neurons to modulate hypoglossal (and therefore genioglossus) activity, effectively triggering hLTF. Intermittent stimulation of the locus coeruleus directly can also induce noradrenaline release onto hypoglossal motor neurons to trigger hLTF. I hypothesize that this is the neural circuit and mechanism underlying LTF of inspiratory genioglossus motor output.

6.5 Methodological considerations

Throughout this thesis, I chose 60 minutes to define hLTF in healthy animals. There are many

limitations with this approach as OSA patients experience apneas throughout the night (i.e.,

Page 151: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

135

longer than 60 minutes). In addition, my approach investigated a neural circuit prior to chronic

exposure to hypoxia and hypercapnia and thus may be less physiological relevant in OSA

patients. It may therefore be possible that these findings are less applicable in a clinical setting.

However, hLTF can be observed in OSA patients (Aboubakr et al., 2001, Younes et al., 2014),

suggesting that the capability for hLTF is present but may be compromised or insufficient at

mitigating the obstructions. In addition, studies using chronic intermittent hypoxia over a

course of 3 months showed changes in α1-adrenergic and 5-HT2A receptor expression on rat

hypoglossal motor neurons (Rukhadze et al., 2010), suggesting long-term plasticity may be

involved to help mitigate symptoms. My approach provides insight into an existing neural

circuit but cannot address the possible long-term changes that may have greater impact on

how hLTF manifests in a clinical setting.

Another consideration in these studies is the fact that all experiments conducted used

genioglossus EMG amplitude as my metric to define hLTF. Although the genioglossus muscle is

a major airway dilator muscle, other muscles contribute to airway patency (Fuller et al., 1999).

Without direct measurements on airway resistance or airflow, I can only postulate the potential

benefits of hLTF. However, hLTF is noradrenaline-dependent and ventilatory LTF was observed

mice absent serotonin in the central nervous system (Hickner et al., 2014). This suggests that

ventilatory LTF may be mediated by noradrenaline-dependent hLTF. In addition, measurements

of upper airway resistance and airflow are performed human studies using similar triggers (i.e.,

repeated bouts of hypoxia) where hLTF was observed (Aboubakr et al., 2001, Chowdhuri et al.,

2008, Griffin et al., 2012, Harris et al., 2006, Shkoukani et al., 2002), and likely involved an

increase in genioglossus muscle tone (Harris et al., 2006).

Other considerations include my approach to blood gas measurements. I did not measure blood

gases in my experiments. Instead, end-tidal CO2 measurements were recorded in animals as a

percent and no real units (e.g. mmHg) are available. In my experiments, ET-CO2 levels peaked at

6% at the end of the repeated apnea intervention which can be approximated to 46 mmHg

(Carroll, 1999). Ideally, direct measurements of arterial CO2 levels would have been more

Page 152: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

136

accurate as changes within a few mmHg can have a significant influence on the expression of

hLTF and diaphragm activity. Similarly, measurements of blood oxygen saturation were also

performed non-invasively using pulse oximetry to monitor arterial oxygenation levels. Although

a significant reduction in oxygen saturation was observed during apneas, actual arterial partial

pressures of oxygen were not measured. hLTF induced by intermittent hypoxia typically lowers

oxygen saturation to a partial pressure of 35-45 mmHg (Dale et al., 2017, Devinney et al., 2015,

Fuller et al., 2000). My approach using repeated apneas does not allow a direct comparison to

these studies.

One large limitation in this thesis involves the statistical analyses used to compare animals that

exhibited apnea-induced hLTF to animals that did not exhibit hLTF following LC inactivation.

Excluding animals that did not exhibit hLTF in one group may skew my statistical analyses

because it implies that 27-35% of animals would not have exhibited hLTF regardless of my

intervention. However, by simply comparing the number of animals that did and did not

express hLTF, it is very unlikely that the non-responders (i.e., 27-35% of animals) were

repeatedly observed in my LC inactivation or hypoglossal α1-receptor blockade studies. In

animals that were given repeated apneas, out of 14 animals, 9 animals expressed hLTF, 5 did

not (i.e., 65% of animals exhibited hLTF). In animals given intermittent light stimulation, 8

expressed hLTF, 3 did not (i.e., 73% of animals exhibited hLTF). In animals that got no

stimulation, 1 expressed hLTF, 4 did not (i.e., 25% of animals exhibit hLTF without intervention).

In animals that received LC inactivation, all 9 animals did not express hLTF. Similarly, in animals

with hypoglossal α1-receptors blocked, all 8 animals did not express hLTF. By simply comparing

n-values, I would expect to observe hLTF in 25% (natural variation) to 73% (stimulation) of

animals following an intervention. When the LC was inactivated or hypoglossal α1-receptors

blocked, I did not get hLTF in any of the 17 animals tested. Therefore, the probability that 17

animals were coincidentally all non-responders is unlikely, suggesting LC activation and

hypoglossal α1-receptor activation is likely necessary for hLTF expression. Nonetheless, I

continue to address this concern using different statistical approaches.

Page 153: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

137

Current LTF studies have not set a precedent on how to address non-responders, but instead

continue to follow statistical analysis traditionally used (i.e., a one-way ANOVA or a two-way

ANOVA). The statistical approach that I introduced here addresses this problem by including all

animals (i.e., animals that exhibit hLTF and non-responders) in my statistical analysis. The chi-

square test and the Firth logistic regression provide insight into whether my intervention

prevented apnea-induced hLTF (i.e., not due to random chance), and showed that the

expression of hLTF following my intervention was in fact not due to random chance. In addition,

the ordinary least squares linear regression (OLS) can determine the strength and direction of

my intervention on the outcome and found that my intervention significantly influenced the

outcome of hLTF expression. These statistical solutions may not be the best approach but

provides what I believe is the start to addressing this caveat.

Lastly, the interpretation of my histology results allowed me to provide insight into where I

hypothesize plasticity is occurring within the hLTF circuit (NTS → LC → XII). However, these

interpretations were based on c-Fos results which is (1) correlative, and (2) inherently biased

towards cells that express c-Fos (i.e., the approach ignores cells that do not express c-Fos). My

interpretations could therefore undervalue the contribution of other cell groups that may be

involved with hLTF and plasticity. Ideally, this would be addressed using cell recordings from the

hypothesized structures in the hLTF circuit.

6.6 Significance of findings

I have shown that repeated apneas are a trigger that can elicit a naturally occurring form of

plasticity that can augment respiratory motor output. hLTF is naturally protective when faced

with repeated respiratory challenges. My findings have added to the field of respiratory motor

plasticity research by identifying the neural circuit that mediates hLTF (i.e., NTS → LC → XII).

This circuit underlies a naturally protective form of respiratory motor plasticity, which can help

identify new therapeutic targets to mitigate some respiratory disorders such as obstructive

sleep apnea (OSA).

Page 154: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

138

OSA is defined by a reduction in upper airway muscle tone during sleep can lead to obstructions

in the upper airways causing apneas. Wakefulness caused by a reflexive increase in ventilatory

effort from apnea-induced hypoxia and hypercapnia ends the apnea, only to repeat when the

patient resumes sleep. LTF of genioglossus motor output, which humans exhibit (Aboubakr et

al., 2001, Chowdhuri et al., 2008, Griffin et al., 2012, Harris et al., 2006, Schwartz et al., 2012,

Shkoukani et al., 2002), could potentially increase upper airway muscle tone, mitigating the

reduction that occurs during sleep, which may aid OSA patients.

In this thesis, I identified a novel trigger to elicit this form of plasticity. The LC is critical to hLTF

and stimulation of this structure alone can elicit hLTF. To date, LTF has focused largely on

changes to phrenic motor neuron activity. The identification of the LC as a trigger not only

allows for targeted therapeutic treatments, but also provides insight to the possible interaction

with current pharmaceutical treatments that have shown to be promising for treating OSA. For

example, clinical studies have shown desipramine, a noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor, to

prevent the natural reduction in genioglossus muscle tone during transition from wakefulness

to NREM sleep (Taranto-Montemurro et al., 2016). It may therefore be possible for a

noradrenaline reuptake inhibitor to elevate noradrenaline levels to maintain upper airway

muscle tone during sleep.

My findings have also contributed to the identification of potential sites where plasticity could

be occurring. It may be possible to trigger hLTF by inducing plasticity within the hLTF circuit. For

example, by increasing α1-adrenergic receptor expression at the hypoglossal motor pool

through chronic exposure to hypoxia (Rukhadze et al., 2010), it may be possible to increase

genioglossus motor output thereby mitigating the apnea. Taken together, these findings

provide a foundation to guide future studies into therapeutic directions to treat OSA.

Page 155: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

139

6.7 Future directions

Currently, I have shown that the LC is a critical structure in mediating hLTF. However, the

activity of the LC prior to hLTF induction and its activity following hLTF induction have not been

recorded. In this thesis, I show an increase in c-Fos expression and presumably LC activity. The

next step would be to use cell recording to quantify LC activity before, during, and after hLTF

induction. Furthermore, cell recordings taken from tripartite circuit could help determine the

site(s) of plasticity. By identifying the role each site plays, it may be possible to target structures

upstream of the motor pool to induce hLTF.

In addition, other neurotransmitters and neuromodulators require further investigation to

elucidate their role in hLTF. Although I have shown that the LC mediates hLTF via noradrenaline

binding to α1-adrenergic receptors on hypoglossal motor neurons, these findings do not

address the role of co-released neurotransmitters and modulators that may facilitate this

process. For example, serotonin is not required for apnea-induced hLTF (Tadjalli et al., 2010)

but still possess a large influence on hypoglossal motor neuron activity (Sood et al., 2005).

These neurotransmitters and neuromodulators require further investigation to decipher a more

comprehensive view of the mechanisms underlying hLTF.

It is my hope that LTF research will continue to grow in the field of respiratory plasticity such

that better treatments can be provided for respiratory disorders, and greater knowledge can be

achieved for plasticity research.

Page 156: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

140

References

Abbott SB, Coates MB, Stornetta RL, Guyenet PG (2013) Optogenetic stimulation of c1 and retrotrapezoid nucleus neurons causes sleep state-dependent cardiorespiratory stimulation and arousal in rats. Hypertension 61:835-841.

Abbott SB, Pilowsky PM (2009) Galanin microinjection into rostral ventrolateral medulla of the rat is hypotensive and attenuates sympathetic chemoreflex. American journal of physiology Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology 296:R1019-R1026.

Abbott SB, Stornetta RL, Fortuna MG, Depuy SD, West GH, Harris TE, Guyenet PG (2009) Photostimulation of retrotrapezoid nucleus phox2b-expressing neurons in vivo produces long-lasting activation of breathing in rats. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 29:5806-5819.

Aboubakr SE, Taylor A, Ford R, Siddiqi S, Badr MS (2001) Long-term facilitation in obstructive sleep apnea patients during NREM sleep. J Appl Physiol (1985) 91:2751-2757.

Abraham WC, Huggett A (1997) Induction and reversal of long-term potentiation by repeated high-frequency stimulation in rat hippocampal slices. Hippocampus 7:137-145.

Abraham WC, Mason SE, Demmer J, Williams JM, Richardson CL, Tate WP, Lawlor PA, Dragunow M (1993) Correlations between immediate early gene induction and the persistence of long-term potentiation. Neuroscience 56:717-727.

Adams LM, Foote SL (1988) Effects of locally infused pharmacological agents on spontaneous and sensory-evoked activity of locus coeruleus neurons. Brain Research Bulletin 21:395-400.

Aicher SA, Sharma S, Mitchell JL (2003) Structural changes in AMPA-receptive neurons in the nucleus of the solitary tract of spontaneously hypertensive rats. Hypertension 41:1246-1252.

Aldes LD (1990) Topographically organized projections from the nucleus subceruleus to the hypoglossal nucleus in the rat: a light and electron microscopic study with complementary axonal transport techniques. The Journal of comparative neurology 302:643-656.

Aldes LD, Chapman ME, Chronister RB, Haycock JW (1992) Sources of noradrenergic afferents to the hypoglossal nucleus in the rat. Brain Res Bull 29:931-942.

Andero R, Choi DC, Ressler KJ (2014) BDNF-TrkB receptor regulation of distributed adult neural plasticity, memory formation, and psychiatric disorders. Progress in molecular biology and translational science 122:169-192.

Andresen MC (1994) Nucleus tractus solitarius - gateway to neural circulatory control. Annu Rev Physiol 56:93-116.

Anikeeva P, Andalman AS, Witten I, Warden M, Goshen I, Grosenick L, Gunaydin LA, Frank LM, Deisseroth K (2011) Optetrode: a multichannel readout for optogenetic control in freely moving mice. Nature neuroscience 15:163-170.

Aoki C, Go C, Venkatesan C, Kurose H (1994) Perikaryal and synaptic localization of a a2A-adrenergic receptor-like immunoreactivity. Brain research 650:181-204.

Aston-Jones G, Bloom FE (1981) Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuations in the sleep-waking cycle. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 1:876-886.

Aston-Jones G, Bloom, F.E. (1981) Activity of norepinephrine-containing locus coeruleus neurons in behaving rats anticipates fluctuationsin the sleep-wake cycle. Neuroscience 1:876-886.

Aston-Jones G, Ennis M, Pieribone VA, Nickell WT, Shipley MT (1970) The brain nucleus locus coeruleus: restricted afferent control of a broad efferent network. Science 234:734-737.

Bacak BJ, Kim T, Smith JC, Rubin JE, Rybak IA (2016) Mixed-mode oscillations and population bursting in the pre-Bӧtzinger complex. pp 1-58 eLife.

Page 157: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

141

Bach KB, Mitchell GS (1996) Hypoxia-induced long-term facilitation of respiratory activity is serotonin dependent. Respiration physiology 104:251-260.

Bach KB, Mitchell GS (1998) Hypercapnia-induced long-term depression of respiratory activity requires alpha(2)-adrenergic receptors. Journal of Applied Physiology 84:2099-2105.

Badie-Mahdavi H, Lu X, Behrens MM, Bartfai T (2005) Role of galanin receptor 1 and galanin receptor 2 activation in synaptic plasticity associated with 3',5'-cyclic AMP response element-binding protein phosphorylation in the dentate gyrus: studies with a galanin receptor 2 agonist and galanin receptor 1 knockout mice. Neuroscience 133:591-604.

Baertsch NA, Baker-Herman TL (2013) Inactivity-induced phrenic and hypoglossal motor facilitation are differentially expressed following intermittent vs. sustained neural apnea. J Appl Physiol (1985) 114:1388-1395.

Baker-Herman TL, Fuller DD, Bavis RW, Zabka AG, Golder FJ, Doperalski NJ, Johnson RA, Watters JJ, Mitchell GS (2004) BDNF is necessary and sufficient for spinal respiratory plasticity following intermittent hypoxia. Nature neuroscience 7:48-55.

Baker-Herman TL, Mitchell GS (2002) Phrenic long-term facilitation requires spinal serotonin receptor activation and protein synthesis. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 22:6239-6246.

Baker-Herman TL, Strey KA (2011) Similarities and differences in mechanisms of phrenic and hypoglossal motor facilitation. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 179:48-56.

Baker TL, Fuller DD, Zabka AG, Mitchell GS (2001) Respiratory plasticity: differential actions of continuous and episodic hypoxia and hypercapnia. Respiration physiology 129:25-35.

Baker TL, Mitchell GS (2000) Episodic but not continuous hypoxia elicits long-term facilitation of phrenic motor output in rats. The Journal of physiology 529.1:215-219.

Barraco RA, Ergene E, Dunbar JC, El-Ridi MR (1990) Cardiorespiratory Response Patterns Elicited by Microinjections of Neuropeptide Y in the Nucleus Tractus Solitarius. Brain Research Bulletin 24:465-485.

Basting TM, Burke PG, Kanbar R, Viar KE, Stornetta DS, Stornetta RL, Guyenet PG (2015) Hypoxia silences retrotrapezoid nucleus respiratory chemoreceptors via alkalosis. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 35:527-543.

Benowitz LI, Routtenberg A (1997) GAP-43: an intrinsic determinant of neuronal development and plasticity. TINS 20:84-91.

Berger AJ, Herbert DA, Mitchell RA (1978) Properties of apneusis produced by reversible cold block of the rostral pons. Respiration physiology 33:323-337.

Berlucchi G (2002) The origin of the term plasticity in the neurosciences: Ernesto Lugaro and chemical synaptic transmission. Journal of the history of the neurosciences 11:305-309.

Berquin P, Bodineau L, Gros F, Larnicol N (2000) Brainstem and hypothalamic areas involved in respiratory chemoreflexes: a Fos study in adult rats. Brain research 857:30-40.

Bhaskaran D, Freed CR (1988) Changes in Neurotransmitter Turnover in Locus Coeruleus Produced by Changes in Arterial Blood Pressure. Brain Research Bulletin 21:191-199.

Biancardi V, Bicego KC, Almeida MC, Gargaglioni LH (2008) Locus coeruleus noradrenergic neurons and CO2 drive to breathing. Pflugers Archiv : European journal of physiology 455:1119-1128.

Blanco CE, Dawes GS, Hanson MA, McCooke HB (1984) The response to hypoxia of arterial chemoreceptors in fetal sheep and new-born lambs. The Journal of physiology 351:25-37.

Bliss TVP, Lomo T (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the denrate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. The Journal of physiology 232:331-356.

Blitz DM, Ramirez JM (2002) Long-term modulation of respiratory network activity following anoxia in vitro. Journal of neurophysiology 87:2964-2971.

Page 158: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

142

Blum R, Konnerth A (2005) Neurotrophin-Mediated Rapid Signaling in the Central Nervous System: Mechanisms and Functions. Physiology 20:70-78.

Bocchiaro CM, Feldman JL (2004) Synaptic activity-independent persistent plasticity in endogenously active mammalian motoneurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 101:4292-4295.

Bodineau L, Larnicol N (2001) Brainstem and hypothalamic areas activated by tissue hypoxia: Fos-like immunoreactivity induced by carbon monoxide inhalation in the rat. Neuroscience 108:643-653.

Bonham AC, Chen CY, Sekizawa S, Joad JP (2006) Plasticity in the nucleus tractus solitarius and its influence on lung and airway reflexes. J Appl Physiol (1985) 101:322-327.

Bonham AC, McCrimmon DR (1990) Neurones in a discrete region of the nucleus tractus solitarius are required for the Breuer-Hering reflex in rat. The Journal of physiology 427:261-280.

Boone TB, Aldes LD (1984) The ultrastructure of two distinct neuron populations in the hypoglossal nucleus of the rat. Exp Brain Res 54:321-326.

Bullitt E (1990) Expression of c-Fos-like protein as a marker for neuronal activity following noxious stimulation in the rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 296:517-530.

Byrum CEG, P.G. (1987) Afferent and Efferent Connections of the A5 Noradrenergic Cell Group in the Rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 261:529-542.

Caille D, Vibert JF, Hugelin A (1981) Apneusis and apnea after parabrachial or Kolliker-Fuse n. lesion; influence of wakefulness. Respiration physiology 45:79-95.

Cajal SR (1911) Histologie du Systeme Nerveux de l’Homme et des Vertebres,. New York: Oxford University Press.

Cao Y, Ling L (2010) Urethane inhibits genioglossal long-term facilitation in un-paralyzed anesthetized rats. Neuroscience letters 477:124-128.

Cao Y, Zwillich CW, Berthon-Jones M, Sullivan CE (1992) Increased normoxic ventilation induced by repetitive hypoxia in conscious dogs. J Appl Physiol 73:2083-2088.

Carroll P (1999) Respiratory monitoring. RN 62:68-72. Carter ME, Yizhar O, Chikahisa S, Nguyen H, Adamantidis A, Nishino S, Deisseroth K, de Lecea L (2010)

Tuning arousal with optogenetic modulation of locus coeruleus neurons. Nature neuroscience 13:1526-1533.

Cedarbaum JM, Aghajanian GK (1978) Afferent projections to the rat locus coeruleus as determined by a retrograde tracing technique. The Journal of comparative neurology 178:1-16.

Chan E, Steenland HW, Liu H, Horner RL (2006) Endogenous excitatory drive modulating respiratory muscle activity across sleep-wake states. American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine 174:1264-1273.

Chan RKW, Peto CA, Sawchenko PE (2000) Fine Structure and Plasticity of Barosensitive Neurons in the Nucleus of Solitary Tract. The Journal of comparative neurology 422:338-351.

Chan RKW, Sawchenko PE (1994) Spatially and Temporally Differentiated Patterns of c-fos Expression in Brainstem Catecholaminergic Cell Groups Induced by Cardiovascular Challenges in the Rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 348:433-460.

Choi SJ, Panhelainen A, Schmitz Y, Larsen KE, Kanter E, Wu M, Sulzer D, Mosharov EV (2015) Changes in neuronal dopamine homeostasis following 1-methyl-4-phenylpyridinium (MPP+) exposure. The Journal of biological chemistry 290:6799-6809.

Chowdhuri S, Pierchala L, Aboubakr SE, Shkoukani M, Badr MS (2008) Long-term facilitation of genioglossus activity is present in normal humans during NREM sleep. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 160:65-75.

Chowdhuri S, Pranathiageswaran S, Franco-Elizondo R, Jayakar A, Hosni A, Nair A, Badr MS (2015) Effect of age on long-term facilitation and chemosensitivity during NREM sleep. J Appl Physiol (1985) 119:1088-1096.

Page 159: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

143

Christie MJ, Williams JT, North RA (1989) Electrical Coupling Synchronizes Subthreshold Coeruleus Neurons in vitro from Neonatal Rats. Neuroscience 9:3584-3589.

Cline HT (1993) Adivity-dependent plasticity in the visual systems of frogs and fish. TINS 14:104-111. Coates EL, Li A, Nattie EE (1993) Widespread sites of brain stem ventilatory chemoreceptors. J Appl

Physiol (1985) 75:5-14. Cohen MI (1971) Switching of the respiratory phases and evoked phrenic responses produced by rostral

pontine electrical stimulation. The Journal of physiology 217:133-158. Collingridge GL, Kehl SJ, McLennan H (1983) Excitatory amino acids in synaptic transmission in the

schaefer colateral-commissural pathway of the rat hippocampus. The Journal of physiology 334:33-46.

Consolo S, Baldi G, Russi G, Civenni G, Bartfai T, Vezzani A (1994) Impulse flow dependency of galanin release in vivo in the rat ventral hippocampus. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 91:8047-8051.

Conte WL, Kamishina H, Reep RL (2009) Multiple neuroanatomical tract-tracing using fluorescent Alexa Fluor conjugates of cholera toxin subunit B in rats. Nature protocols 4:1157-1166.

Costa-Silva JH, Zoccal DB, Machado BH (2011) Chronic intermittent hypoxia alters glutamatergic control of sympathetic and respiratory activities in the commissural NTS of rats. American journal of physiology Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology 302:R785-793.

Coumis U, Davies CH (2002) The effects of galanin on long-term synaptic plasticity in the CA1 area of rodent hippocampus. Neuroscience 112:173-182.

Dale EA, Fields DP, Devinney MJ, Mitchell GS (2017) Phrenic motor neuron TrkB expression is necessary for acute intermittent hypoxia-induced phrenic long-term facilitation. Experimental neurology 287:130-136.

Dampney RAL (1994) Functional Organization of Central Pathways Regulating the Cardiovascular System. Physiological reviews 74:323-364.

de Castro D, Lipski J, Kanjhan R (1994) Electrophysiological study of dorsal respiratory neurons in the medulla oblongata of the rat. Brain research 639:49-56.

Deacon NL, McEvoy DR, Stadler DL, Catcheside PG (2017) Intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia during sleep does not induce ventilatory long term facilitation in healthy males. J Appl Physiol 1-31.

DeBock F, Kurz J, Azad SC, Parsons CG, Hapfelmeier G, Zieglgänsberger W, Rammes G (2003) α2-Adrenoreceptor activation inhibits LTP and LTD in the basolateral amygdala: involvement of Gi/o-protein-mediated modulation of Ca2+-channels and inwardly rectifying K+-channels in LTD. European Journal of Neuroscience 17:1411-1424.

Del Negro CA, Morgado-Valle C, Feldman JL (2002) Respiratory rhythm: an emergent network property? Neuron 34:821-830.

Devinney MJ, Fields DP, Huxtable AG, Peterson TJ, Dale EA, Mitchell GS (2015) Phrenic long-term facilitation requires PKCtheta activity within phrenic motor neurons. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 35:8107-8117.

Devoto P, Flore G (2006) On the Origin of Cortical Dopamine: Is it a Co-Transmitter in Noradrenergic Neurons? Current neuropharmacology 4:115-125.

Dobbins EG, Feldman JL (1994) Brainstem network controlling descending drive to phrenic motoneurons in rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 347:64-86.

Dobbins EG, Feldman JL (1995) Differential Innervation of Protruder and Retractor Muscles of the Tongue in Rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 376-394.

Dodig PI, Pecotic R, Valic M, Dogas Z (2012) Acute intermittent hypoxia induces phrenic long-term facilitation which is modulated by 5-HT1A receptor in the caudal raphe region of the rat. Journal of sleep research 21:195-203.

Page 160: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

144

Dragunow M, Faull RLM, Jansen KLR (1990) MK-801, an antagonist of NMDA receptors, inhibits injury-induced c-Fos protein accumulation in rat brain. Neuroscience letters 109.

Dremencov E, El Mansari M, Blier P (2007) Distinct electrophysiological effects of paliperidone and risperidone on the firing activity of rat serotonin and norepinephrine neurons. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 194:63-72.

Duffin J (1990) The chemoreflex control of breathing and its measurement. Canadian Journal of Anaesthesia 37:933-942.

Duffin J (2004) Functional organization of respiratory neurones: a brief review of current questions and speculations. Experimental physiology 89:517-529.

Duffin J (2005) Role of acid-base balance in the chemoreflex control of breathing. J Appl Physiol (1985) 99:2255-2265.

Dunwiddie T, Lynch G (1978) Long-term potentiation and depression of synaptic responses in the rat hippocampus: localization and frequency dependency. The Journal of physiology 276:353-367.

Edge D, O'Halloran KD (2015) Chronic Intermittent Hypoxia Blunts the Expression of Ventilatory Long Term Facilitation in Sleeping Rats. Adv Exp Med Biol 860:335-342.

Everitt BJ, Hokfelt T, Terenius L, Tatemoto K, Mutt V, Goldstein M (1984) Differential co-existence of neuropeptide y (NPY)-like immunoreactivity with catecholamines in the central nervous system of the rat. Neuroscience 11:443-462.

Ezure K, Tanaka I (2000) Identification of deflation-sensitive inspiratory neurons in the dorsal respiratory group of the rat. Brain research 883:22-30.

Fazlali Z, Ranjbar-Slamloo Y, Adibi M, Arabzadeh E (2016) Correlation between Cortical State and Locus Coeruleus Activity: Implications for Sensory Coding in Rat Barrel Cortex. Frontiers in neural circuits 10:14.

Feldman JL, Loewy AD, Speck DF (1985) Projections from the Ventral Respiratory Group to Phrenic and Intercostal Motoneurons in Cat: An Autoradiographic Study. Neuroscience 5:1993-2000.

Feldman JL, Mitchell GS, Nattie EE (2003) Breathing: rhythmicity, plasticity, chemosensitivity. Annual review of neuroscience 26:239-266.

Feldman JL, Neverova NV, Saywell SA (2005) Modulation of hypoglossal motoneuron excitability by intracellular signal transduction cascades. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 147:131-143.

Fenik V, Marchenko V, P. J, Davies RO, Kubin L (2002) A5 cells are silenced when REM sleep-like signs are elicited by pontine carbachol. J Appl Physiol 93:1448-1456.

Fenik VB, Fung SJ, Lim V, Chase MH (2013) Quantitative analysis of the excitability of hypoglossal motoneurons during natural sleep in the rat. Journal of neuroscience methods 212:56-63.

Fenik VB, Rukhadze I, Kubin L (2008) Inhibition of pontine noradrenergic A7 cells reduces hypoglossal nerve activity in rats. Neuroscience 157:473-482.

Fitzgerald R (1995) Lung Biology in Health and Disease: A History of Breathing Physiology. In: The Regulation of Breathing (Proctor, D. F., ed), pp 303-333 New York: Marcel Dekker Inc.

Florin-Lechner SM, Druhan JP, Aston-Jones G, Valentino RJ (1996) Enhanced norepinephrine release in prefrontal cortex with burst stimulation of the locus coeruleus. Brain research 742:89-97.

Fregosi RF, Fuller DD (1997) Respiratory-related control of extrinsic tongue muscle activity. Respiration physiology 110:295-306.

Fritschy J, Grzanna R (1990) Distribution of locus coeruleus axons within the rat brainstem demonstrated by phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin anterograde tracing in combination with dopamine-b-hydroxylase immunofluorescence. The Journal of comparative neurology 293:616-631.

Fuller DD (2005) Episodic hypoxia induces long-term facilitation of neural drive to tongue protrudor and retractor muscles. J Appl Physiol (1985) 98:1761-1767.

Fuller DD, Bach KB, Baker TL, Kinkead R, Mitchell GS (2000) Long term facilitation of phrenic motor output. Respiration physiology 121:135-146.

Page 161: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

145

Fuller DD, Williams JS, P.L. J, Fregosi RF (1999) Effect of co-activation of tongue protrudor and retractor muscles on tongue movements and pharyngeal airflow mechanics in the rat. The Journal of physiology 519.2:601-613.

Fuller DD, Zabka AG, Baker TL, Mitchell GS (2001) Phrenic long-term facilitation requires 5-HT receptor activation during but not following episodic hypoxia. J Appl Physiol (1985) 90:2001-2006; discussion 2000.

Fung SJ, Chase MH (2015) Postsynaptic inhibition of hypoglossal motoneurons produces atonia of the genioglossal muscle during rapid eye movement sleep. Sleep 38:139-146.

Fung SJ, Manzoni D, Chan JY, Pompeiano O, Barnes CD (1991) Locus coeruleus control of spinal motor output. Progress in brain research 88:395-409.

Fung SJ, Reddy VK, Liu RH, Wang Z, Barnes CD (1994) Existence of glutamate in noradrenergic locus coeruleus neurons of rodents. Brain Research Bulletin 35:505-512.

Furtinger S, Pirker S, Czech T, Baumgartner C, Ransmayr G, Sperk G (2001) Plasticity of Y1 and Y2 Receptors and Neuropeptide Y Fibers in Patients with Temporal Lobe Epilepsy. Neuroscience 21:5804-5812.

G.D. F, Smith JC, Feldman JL (1993) Generation and Transmission of Respiratory Oscillations in Medullary Slices: Role of Excitatory Amino Acids. Journal of neurophysiology 70:1497-1515.

Gandolfi D, Cerri S, Mapelli J, Polimeni M, Tritto S, Fuzzati-Armentero MT, Bigiani A, Blandini F, Mapelli L, D'Angelo E (2017) Activation of the CREB/c-Fos Pathway during Long-Term Synaptic Plasticity in the Cerebellum Granular Layer. Frontiers in cellular neuroscience 11:184.

Gao WJ, Ji RR (2009) c-Fos or pERK, which is a better marker for neuronal activation and central sensitization after noxious stimulation and tissue injury? The Open Pain Journal 2:11-17.

Gargaglioni LH, Hartzler LK, Putnam RW (2010) The locus coeruleus and central chemosensitivity. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 173:264-273.

Gerst DG, 3rd, Yokhana SS, Carney LM, Lee DS, Badr MS, Qureshi T, Anthouard MN, Mateika JH (2011) The hypoxic ventilatory response and ventilatory long-term facilitation are altered by time of day and repeated daily exposure to intermittent hypoxia. J Appl Physiol (1985) 110:15-28.

Getting PA (1989) Emerging principles governing the operation of neural networks. Ann Rev Neurosci 12:185-204.

Golder FJ, Ranganathan L, Satriotomo I, Hoffman M, Lovett-Barr MR, Watters JJ, Baker-Herman TL, Mitchell GS (2008) Spinal adenosine A2a receptor activation elicits long-lasting phrenic motor facilitation. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 28:2033-2042.

Goodman CS, Shatz CJ (1993) Developmental Mechanisms that Generate Precise Patterns of Neuronal Connectivity. Neuron 10:77-98.

Gottschalk WA, Jiang H, Tartaglia N, Feng L, Figurov A, Lu B (1999) Signalling mechanisms mediating BDNF modulation of synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus. Learning and Memory 6:243-256.

Gradinaru V, Zhang F, Ramakrishnan C, Mattis J, Prakash R, Diester I, Goshen I, Thompson KR, Deisseroth K (2010) Molecular and cellular approaches for diversifying and extending optogenetics. Cell 141:154-165.

Gray PA, Rekling JC, Bocchiaro CM, Feldman JL (1999) Modulation of Respiratory Frequency by Peptidergic Input to Rhythmogenic Neurons in the PreBo¬tzinger Complex. Science 286:1566-1568.

Griffin HS, Pugh K, Kumar P, Balanos GM (2012) Long-term facilitation of ventilation following acute continuous hypoxia in awake humans during sustained hypercapnia. The Journal of physiology 590:5151-5165.

Guyenet PG, Wang H (2001) Pre-Bo¨tzinger Neurons With Preinspiratory Discharges “In Vivo” Express NK1 Receptors in the Rat. American Physiological Society 86:438-446.

Page 162: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

146

Guzowski JF, McNaughton BL, Barnes CA, Worley PF (1999) Environment-specific expression of the immediate-early gene Arc in hippocampal neuronal ensembles. Nature neuroscience 2:1120-1124.

Gysbrechts B, Wang L, Trong NN, Cabral H, Navratilova Z, Battaglia F, Saeys W, Bartic C (2016) Light distribution and thermal effects in the rat brain under optogenetic stimulation. Journal of biophotonics 9:576-585.

Hakuno H, Oyamada Y, Murai M, Ito Y, Yamaguchi K (2004) Effects of inactivation and stimulation of locus coeruleus on respiratory activity of neonatal rat. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 140:9-18.

Han X (2012) Optogenetics in the nonhuman primate. Progress in brain research 196:215-233. Hannon J, Hoyer D (2008) Molecular biology of 5-HT receptors. Behavioural brain research 195:198-213. Harris DP, Balasubramaniam A, Badr MS, Mateika JH (2006) Long-term facilitation of ventilation and

genioglossus muscle activity is evident in the presence of elevated levels of carbon dioxide in awake humans. American journal of physiology Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology 291:R1111-1119.

Hawkins RD, Kandel ER, Siegalbaum SA (1993) Learning to modulate transmitter release: Themes and Variations in Synaptic Plasticity. Ann Rev Neurosci 16:625-665.

Haxhiu MA, Tolentino-Silva F, Pete G, Kc P, Mack SO (2001) Monoaminergic neurons, chemosensation and arousal. Respiration physiology 129:191-209.

Haxhiu MA, Yung K, Erokwu B, Cherniack NS (1996) CO2-induced c-fos expression in the CNS catecholaminergic neurons. Respiration physiology 105:35-45.

Hebb DO (1949) Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological Theory: Psychology Press. Hedner J, Hedner T, Jonason J, Lundberg D (1982) Evidence for a dopamine interaction with the central

respiratory control system in the rat. Eur J Pharmacol 81:603-615. Hickner S, Hussain N, Angoa-Perez M, Francescutti DM, Kuhn DM, Mateika JH (2014) Ventilatory long-

term facilitation is evident after initial and repeated exposure to intermittent hypoxia in mice genetically depleted of brain serotonin. J Appl Physiol 116:240-250.

Hilaire G (2006) Endogenous noradrenaline affects the maturation and function of the respiratory network: possible implication for SIDS. Autonomic neuroscience : basic & clinical 126-127:320-331.

Hilaire G, Viemari JC, Coulon P, Simonneau M, Bevengut M (2004) Modulation of the respiratory rhythm generator by the pontine noradrenergic A5 and A6 groups in rodents. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 143:187-197.

Hirakawa H, Nakamura T, Hayashida Y (1997) Effect of carbon dioxide on autonomic cardiovascular responses to systemic hypoxia in conscious rats. The American journal of physiology 273:R747-754.

Hoffman MS, Nichols NL, Macfarlane PM, Mitchell GS (2012) Phrenic long-term facilitation after acute intermittent hypoxia requires spinal ERK activation but not TrkB synthesis. J Appl Physiol (1985) 113:1184-1193.

Holets VR, Hokfelt T, Rokaeus A, Terenius L, Goldstein M (1988) Locus coeruleus neurons in the rat containing neuropeptide y, tyrosine hydroxylase or galanin and their efferent projections to the spinal cord, cerebral cortex and hypothalamus. Neuroscience 24:893-906.

Holloway BB, Viar KE, Stornetta RL, Guyenet PG (2015) The retrotrapezoid nucleus stimulates breathing by releasing glutamate in adult conscious mice. The European journal of neuroscience 42:2271-2282.

Horner RL (1996) Motor Control of the Pharyngeal Musculature and Implications for the Pathogenesis of Obstructive Sleep Apnea. Sleep 19:827-853.

Horner RL (2008) Pathophysiology of obstructive sleep apnea. J Cardiopulm Rehabil Prev 28:289-298.

Page 163: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

147

Horner RL, Liu X, Gill H, Nolan P, Liu H, Sood S (2002) Selected Contribution: Effects of sleep-wake state on genioglossus vs. diaphragm muscle responses to CO2 in rats. J Appl Physiol 92:878-887.

Hsiao C, Lahiri S, Mokashi A (1989) Peripheral and central dopamine receptors in respiratory control. Respiration physiology 76:327-336.

Huang YY, Kandel ER (1997) Recruitment of Long-lasting and Protein Kinase A-dependent Long-term Potentiation in the CA I Region of Hippocampus Requires Repeated Tetanization. Learning and Memory 1:74-82.

Hunanyan AS, Petrosyan HA, Alessi V, Arvanian VL (2013) Combination of chondroitinase ABC and AAV-NT3 promotes neural plasticity at descending spinal pathways after thoracic contusion in rats. Journal of neurophysiology 110:1782-1792.

Hung J, Whitford EG, Parsons RW, Hillman DR (1990) Association of sleep apnoea with myocardial infarction in men. The Lancet 336:261-264.

Huxtable AG, MacFarlane PM, Vinit S, Nichols NL, Dale EA, Mitchell GS (2014) Adrenergic a1-receptor activation is sufficient, but not necessary for phrenic long-term facilitation. J Appl Physiol 116:1345-1352.

Ip MSM, Lam B, Lauder IJ, Tsang KWT, Chung KF, Mok YW, Lam WK (2001) A Community Study of Sleep-Disordered Breathing in Middle-aged Chinese Men in Hong Kong. CHEST Journal 119:62-69.

Ip MSM, Lam B, Tang LCH, Lauder IJ, Ip TY, Lam WK (2004) A Community Study of Sleep- Disordered Breathing in Middle-Aged Chinese Women in Hong Kong. CHEST Journal 125:127-134.

Isackson PJ, Huntsman MM, Murray KD, Gall CM (1991) BDNF mRNA Expression Is Increased in Adult Rat Forebrain after Limbic Seizures: Temporal Patterns of Induction Distinct from NGF. Neuron 6:937-948.

Ishimatsu M, Williams JT (1996a) Synchronous Activity in Locus Coeruleus Results from Dendritic Interactions in Pericoerulear Regions. Neuroscience 16:5196-5204.

Ishimatsu M, Williams JT (1996b) Synchronous activity in locus coeruleus results from dendritic interactions in pericoerulear regions. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 16:5196-5204.

Itami C, Kimura F, Kohno T, Matsuoka M, Ichikawa M, Tsumoto T, Nakamura S (2003) Brain-derived neurotrophic factor-dependent unmasking of "silent" synapses in the developing mouse barrel cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100:13069-13074.

Janczewski WA, Feldman JL (2006) Distinct rhythm generators for inspiration and expiration in the juvenile rat. The Journal of physiology 570:407-420.

Janssen PL, Fregosi RF (2000) No evidence for long-term facilitation after episodic hypoxia in spontaneously breathing, anesthetized rats. J Appl Physiol (1985) 89:1345-1351.

Jedrzejewska-Szmek J, Luczak V, Abel T, Blackwell KT (2017) beta-adrenergic signaling broadly contributes to LTP induction. PLoS computational biology 13:e1005657.

Jiang C, Lipski J (1990) Extensive monosynaptic inhibition of ventral respiratory group neurons by augmenting neurons in the B6tzinger complex in the cat. Exp Brain Res 81:639-648.

Jiao XX, Gonzalez-Cabrera PJ, Xiao L, Bradley ME, Abel PW, Jeffries WB (2002) Tonic Inhibitory Role for cAMP in a1a-Adrenergic Receptor Coupling to Extracellular Signal-Regulated Kinases 1/2. The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics 303:247-256.

Johnson SM, Koshiya N, Smith JC (2001) Isolation of the Kernel for Respiratory Rhythm Generation in a Novel Preparation: The Pre-Bo¨tzinger Complex “Island”. Rapid Communication 1772-1776.

Jones LS, Gauger LL, Davis JN (1985) Anatomy of Brain Alpha1-Adrenergic Receptors: In Vitro Autoradiography With [125I]-Heat. The Journal of comparative neurology 231:190-208.

Joubert F, Loiseau C, Perrin-Terrin AS, Cayetanot F, Frugiere A, Voituron N, Bodineau L (2016) Key Brainstem Structures Activated during Hypoxic Exposure in One-day-old Mice Highlight

Page 164: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

148

Characteristics for Modeling Breathing Network in Premature Infants. Frontiers in physiology 7:609.

Juric DM, Loncar D, Carman-Krzan M (2008) Noradrenergic stimulation of BDNF synthesis in astrocytes: mediation via alpha1- and beta1/beta2-adrenergic receptors. Neurochemistry international 52:297-306.

Kalia M, Sullivan JM (1982) Brainstem Projections of Sensory and Motor Components of the Vagus Nerve in the Rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 211:248-264.

Kaliszewska A, Bijata M, Kaczmarek L, Kossut M (2012) Experience-dependent plasticity of the barrel cortex in mice observed with 2-DG brain mapping and c-Fos: effects of MMP-9 KO. Cereb Cortex 22:2160-2170.

Karunakaran S, Chowdhury A, Donato F, Quairiaux C, Michel CM, Caroni P (2016) PV plasticity sustained through D1/5 dopamine signaling required for long-term memory consolidation. Nature neuroscience 19:454-464.

Katakura N, Chandler SH (1990) An iontophoretic analysis of the pharmacologic mechanisms responsible for trigeminal motorneuronal discharge during masticatory-like activity in the guinea pig. Journal of neurophysiology 63:356-369.

Kemp N, McQueen J, Faulkes S, Bashir ZI (2000) Different forms of LTD in the CA1 region of the hippocampus: role of age and stimulus protocol. The European journal of neuroscience 12:360-366.

Kempadoo KA, Mosharov EV, Choi SJ, Sulzer D, Kandel ER (2016) Dopamine release from the locus coeruleus to the dorsal hippocampus promotes spatial learning and memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113:14835-14840.

Kiehn O, Sillar KT, Kjaerulff O, McDearmid JR (1999) Effects of noradrenaline on locomotor rhythm-generating networks in the isolated neonatal rat spinal cord. The Journal of physiology 82:741-746.

Kinkead R, Bach KB, Johnson SM, Hodgeman BA, Mitchell GS (2001) Plasticity in respiratory motor control: intermittent hypoxia and hypercapnia activate opposing serotonergic and noradrenergic modulatory systems. Comparative biochemistry and physiology Part A, Molecular & integrative physiology 130:207-218.

Kline DD (2008) Plasticity in glutamatergic NTS neurotransmission. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 164:105-111.

Kline DD, Mendelowitz D (2012) Intermittent Hypoxia Alters the Function of Cardiovascular Neurons and Reflex Pathways in the Brainstem. pp 71-83.

Kogan JH, Nestler EJ, Aghajanian GK (1992) Elevated basal firing rates and enhanced responses to 8-Br-cAMP in locus coeruleus neurons in brain slices from opiate-dependent rats. Eur J Pharmacol 211:47-53.

Koshiya N, Smith JC (1999) Neuronal pacemaker for breathing visualized in vitro. Nature 400:360-363. Kott JN, Westrum LE, Bhatia AM (1991) Is phaseotus vulgaris leucoagglutinin (PHA-L) a useful marker for

labeling neural grafts? Journal of Neural Transplatation and Plasticity 2:249-253. Lalley PM (2008) Opioidergic and dopaminergic modulation of respiration. Respiratory physiology &

neurobiology 164:160-167. Lam B, Ip MS, Tench E, Ryan CF (2005) Craniofacial profile in Asian and white subjects with obstructive

sleep apnoea. Thorax 60:504-510. Lee HK, Kameyama K, Huganir RL, Bear MF (1998) NMDA Induces Long-Term Synaptic Depression and

Dephosphorylation of the GluR1 Subunit of AMPA Receptors in Hippocampus. Neuron 21:1151-1162.

Legallois M (1813) Experiments on the principle of life. Philadelphia: Thomas.

Page 165: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

149

Lendvai B, Stern EA, Chen B, Svoboda K (2000) Experience-dependent plasticity of dendritic spines in the developing rat barrel cortex in vivo. Nature 404:876-881.

Levitt P, Moore RY (1979) Origin and organization of brainstem catecholamine innervation in the rat. The Journal of comparative neurology 186:505-528.

Li A, Nattie E (2006) Catecholamine neurones in rats modulate sleep, breathing, central chemoreception and breathing variability. The Journal of physiology 570:385-396.

Li YM, Shen L, Peever JH, Duffin J (2003) Connections between respiratory neurones in the neonatal rat transverse medullary slice studied with cross-correlation. The Journal of physiology 549:327-332.

Lim DC, Veasey SC (2010) Neural injury in sleep apnea. Current neurology and neuroscience reports 10:47-52.

Ling L, Fuller DD, Bach KB, Kinkead R, Olson Jr. BE, Mitchell GS (2001) Chronic Intermittent Hypoxia Elicits Serotonin-Dependent Plasticity in the Central Neural Control of Breathing. Neuroscience 21:5381-5388.

Lomo T (1966) Frequency potentiation of excitatory synaptic activity in the dentate area of the hippocampal formation. Acta Physiol Scand 68:128.

Lopes LT, Patrone LG, Li KY, Imber AN, Graham CD, Gargaglioni LH, Putnam RW (2016) Anatomical and functional connections between the locus coeruleus and the nucleus tractus solitarius in neonatal rats. Neuroscience 324:446-468.

Lu J, Sherman D, Devor M, Saper CB (2006) A putative flip-flop switch for control of REM sleep. Nature 441:589-594.

Lui S, Torontali Z, Tadjalli A, Peever J (2018) Brainstem Nuclei Associated with Mediating Apnea-Induced Respiratory Motor Plasticity. Scientific reports 8.

Lujan HL, Palani G, DiCarlo SE (2010) Structural neuroplasticity following T5 spinal cord transection: increased cardiac sympathetic innervation density and SPN arborization. American journal of physiology Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology 299:R985-R995.

Mahamed S, Strey KA, Mitchell GS, Baker-Herman TL (2011) Reduced respiratory neural activity elicits phrenic motor facilitation. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 175:303-309.

Mahn M, Prigge M, Ron S, Levy R, Yizhar O (2016) Biophysical constraints of optogenetic inhibition at presynaptic terminals. Nature neuroscience 19:554-556.

Mair RD, Zhang Y, Bailey KR, Toupin MM, Mair RG (2005) Effects of clonidine in the locus coeruleus on prefrontal- and hippocampal-dependent measures of attention and memory in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 181:280-288.

McGuire M, Zhang Y, White DP, Ling L (2002) Effect of hypoxic episode number and severity on ventilatory long-term facilitation in awake rats. J Appl Physiol (1985) 93:2155-2161.

McGuire M, Zhang Y, White DP, Ling L (2004) Serotonin receptor subtypes required for ventilatory long-term facilitation and its enhancement after chronic intermittent hypoxia in awake rats. American journal of physiology Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology 286:R334-341.

McGuire M, Zhang Y, White DP, Ling L (2005) Phrenic long-term facilitation requires NMDA receptors in the phrenic motonucleus in rats. The Journal of physiology 567:599-611.

Mellen N, Janczewski WA, Bocchiaro CM, Feldman JL (2003) Opioid-Induced Quantal Slowing Reveals Dual Networks for Respiratory Rhythm Generation. Neuron 37:821-826.

Mendez P, Bacci A (2011) Assortment of GABAergic plasticity in the cortical interneuron melting pot. Neural plasticity 2011:976856.

Merrill EG, Fedorko L (1984) Monosynaptic inhibition of phrenic motoneurons: a long descending projection from Botzinger neurons. Neuroscience 4:2350-2353.

Page 166: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

150

Michaloudi HC, el Majdoubi M, Poulain DA, Papadopoulos GC, Theodosis DT (1997) The noradrenergic innervation of identified hypothalamic magnocellular somata and its contribution to lactation-induced synaptic plasticity. Journal of neuroendocrinology 9:17-23.

Mifflin SW (1992) Arterial chemoreceptor input to nucleus tractus solitarius. American journal of physiology Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology 263:R368-375.

Millhorn DE (1986) Stimulation of raphe (obscurus) nucleus causes long-term potentation of phrenic nerve activity in cat. The Journal of physiology 381:169-179.

Millhorn DE, Eldridge FL, Waldrop TG (1980a) Prolonged stimulation of respiration by a new central neural mechanism. Respiration physiology 41:81-103.

Millhorn DE, Eldridge FL, Waldrop TG (1980b) Prolonged stimulation of respiration by endogenous central serotonin. Respiration physiology 42:171-188.

Minichiello L, Calella AM, Medina DL, Bonhoeffer T, Klein R, Korte M (2002) Mechanism of TrkB-mediated hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neuron 36:121-137.

Mohan R, Duffin J (1997) The effect of hypoxia on the ventilatory response to carbon dioxide in man. Respiration physiology 108:101-115.

Molkov YI, Rubin JE, Rybak IA, Smith JC (2017) Computational models of the neural control of breathing. Wiley interdisciplinary reviews Systems biology and medicine 9.

Moore RY (1979) Central catecholamine neuron systems: anatomy and physiology of the norepinephrine and epinephrine systems. Ann Rev Neurosci 2:113-168.

Moreira TS, Takakura AC, Damasceno RS, Falquetto B, Totola LT, Sobrinho CR, Ragioto DT, Zolezi FP (2011) Central chemoreceptors and neural mechanisms of cardiorespiratory control. Brazilian journal of medical and biological research = Revista brasileira de pesquisas medicas e biologicas / Sociedade Brasileira de Biofisica [et al] 44:883-889.

Morgan JI, Cohen DR, Hempstead JL, Curran T (1987) Mapping patterns of c-fos expression in the central nervous system after seizure. Science 237:192-198.

Morris BJ, Johnston HM (1995) A role for hippocampal opioids in long-term functional plasticity. TINS 18:350-355.

Morris KF, Shannon R, Lindsey BG (2001) Changes in cat medullary neurone firing rates and synchrony following induction of respiratory long-term facilitation. The Journal of physiology 532:483-497.

Mouradian RD, Sessler FM, Waterhouse BD (1991) Noradrenergic potentiation of excitatory transmitter action in cerebrocortical slices: evidence for mediation by an alpha 1 receptor-linked second messenger pathway. Brain research 546:83-95.

Muzerelle A, Scotto-Lomassese S, Bernard JF, Soiza-Reilly M, Gaspar P (2016) Conditional anterograde tracing reveals distinct targeting of individual serotonin cell groups (B5-B9) to the forebrain and brainstem. Brain structure & function 221:535-561.

Nakamura S, Sakaguchi T, Kimura F, Aoki F (1988) The role of alpha1-adrenoceptor-mediated collateral excitation in the regulation of the electrical activity of locus coeruleus neurons. Neuroscience 27:921-929.

Nakayama H, Smith CA, Rodman JR, Skatrud JB, Dempsey JA (2003) Carotid body denervation eliminates apnea in response to transient hypocapnia. J Appl Physiol (1985) 94:155-164.

Natarajan K, Berk BC (2006) Crosstalk Coregulation Mechanisms of G Protein-Coupled Receptors and Receptor Tyrosine Kinases. Methods in Molecular Biology 332:51-77.

Neverova NV, Saywell SA, Nashold LJ, Mitchell GS, Feldman JL (2007) Episodic stimulation of alpha1-adrenoreceptors induces protein kinase C-dependent persistent changes in motoneuronal excitability. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 27:4435-4442.

Page 167: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

151

Niebert M, Vogelgesang S, Koch UR, Bischoff AM, Kron M, Bock N, Manzke T (2011) Expression and function of serotonin 2A and 2B receptors in the mammalian respiratory network. PloS one 6:e21395.

Nieto FJ, Young TB, Lind BK, Shahar E, Samet JM, Redline S, D'Agostino RB, Lebowitz MD, Pickering TG (2000) Association of sleep-disordered breathing, sleep apnea, and hypertension in a large community-based study. JAMA : the journal of the American Medical Association 284:1829-1836.

Nitz D, Siegel J (1997) GABA release in the locus coeruleus as a function of sleep/wake state. Neuroscience 78:795-801.

Okazaki M, Takeda R, Yamazaki H, Haji A (2002) Synaptic mechanisms of inspiratory off-switching evoked by pontine pneumotaxic stimulation in cats. Neuroscience Research 44:101-110.

Olson EB, Jr., Bohne CJ, Dwinell MR, Podolsky A, Vidruk EH, Fuller DD, Powell FL, Mitchel GS (2001) Ventilatory long-term facilitation in unanesthetized rats. J Appl Physiol (1985) 91:709-716.

Onimaru H, Homma I (2003) A Novel Functional Neuron Group for Respiratory Rhythm Generation in the Ventral Medulla. Neuroscience 23:1478-1486.

Oyamada Y, Andrzejewski M, Muckenhoff K, Scheid P, Ballantyne D (1999) Locus coeruleus neurones in vitro: pH-sensitive oscillations of membrane potential in an electrically coupled network. Respiration physiology 118:131-147.

Panuchuk-Voloshina N, Haugland RP, Bishop-Stewart J, Bhalgat MK, Millard PJ, Mao F, Leung WY, Haugland RP (1999) Alexa Dyes, a Series of New Fluorescent Dyes that Yield Exceptionally Bright, Photostable Conjugates. Journal of Histochemistry and Cytochemistry 47:1179-1188.

Parkis MA, Bayliss DA, Berger AJ (1995) Actions of norepinephrine on rat hypoglossal motoneurons. Journal of neurophysiology 74:1911-1919.

Paxinos G, Watson C (1998) The rat brain: in stereotaxic coordinates. San Diego, California: Academic Press, Inc.

Peever JH, Shen L, Duffin J (2002) Respiratory pre-motor control of hypoglossal motorneurons in the rat. Neuroscience 110:711-722.

Peng YJ, Nanduri J, Raghuraman G, Souvannakitti D, Gadalla MM, Kumar GK, Snyder SH, Prabhakar NR (2010) H2S mediates O2 sensing in the carotid body. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 107:10719-10724.

Pham J, Young A, Kuo YT, O’Driscoll DM (2018) Polysomnographic comparison between Chinese and Caucasian patients with obstructive sleep apnea. Sleep and Biological Rhythms 16:223-230.

Qian Z, Gu Y (2005) In vivo measurement of absorption coefficient (ua) in rat brain and statistic analysis. Chinese Optic Letters 3.

Rajkowski J, Majczynski H, Clayton E, AstonJones G (2004) Activation of monkey locus coeruleus neurons varies with difficulty and performance in a target detection task. Journal of neurophysiology 92:361-371.

Reig R, Mattia M, Compte A, Belmonte C, Sanchez-Vives MV (2010) Temperature modulation of slow and fast cortical rhythms. Journal of neurophysiology 103:1253-1261.

Ritucci NA, Dean JB, Putnam RW (1997) Intracellular pH response to hypercapnia in neurons from chemosensitive areas of the medulla. Rapid Communication R433-R441.

Roda F, Pio J, Bianchi AL, Gestreau C (2004) Effects of anesthetics on hypoglossal nerve discharge and c-Fos expression in brainstem hypoglossal premotor neurons. The Journal of comparative neurology 468:571-586.

Rukhadze I, Fenik VB, Benincasa KE, Price A, Kubin L (2010) Chronic intermittent hypoxia alters density of aminergic terminals and receptors in the hypoglossal motor nucleus. American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine 182:1321-1329.

Page 168: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

152

Rukhadze I, Kubin L (2007) Differential pontomedullary catecholaminergic projections to hypoglossal motor nucleus and viscerosensory nucleus of the solitary tract. Journal of chemical neuroanatomy 33:23-33.

Rungta RL, Osmanski BF, Boido D, Tanter M, Charpak S (2017) Light controls cerebral blood flow in naive animals. Nature communications 8:14191.

Rutherford LC, Nelson SB, Turrigiano GG (1998) BDNF Has Opposite Effects on the Quantal Amplitude of Pyramidal Neuron and Interneuron Excitatory Synapses. Neuron 21:521-530.

Ryan S, Nolan P (2009) Episodic hypoxia induces long-term facilitation of upper airway muscle activity in spontaneously breathing anaesthetized rats. The Journal of physiology 587:3329-3342.

Saha S, Spary EJ, Maqbool A, Asipu A, Corbett EK, Batten TF (2004) Increased expression of AMPA receptor subunits in the nucleus of the solitary tract in the spontaneously hypertensive rat. Brain research Molecular brain research 121:37-49.

Sakai K, Touret M, Salvert D, Leger L, Jouvet M (1977) Afferent Projections to the Cat Locus Coeruleus as Visualized by the Horseradish Peroxidase Technique. Brain research 119:21-41.

Sakamoto H, Fukuda S, Minakawa Y, Sawamura S (2013) Clonidine induces sedation through acting on the perifornical area and the locus coeruleus in rats. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol 25:399-407.

Sanders M, Moore S (1983) Inspiratory and expiratory partitioning of airway resistance during sleep in patients with sleep apnea. Am Rev Respir Dis 127:554-558.

Sanders M, Rogers R, Pennock B (1985) Prolonged expiratory phase in sleep apnea. A unifying hypothesis. Am Rev Respir Dis 131:401-408.

Satriotomo I, Dale EA, Dahlberg JM, Mitchell GS (2012) Repetitive acute intermittent hypoxia increases expression of proteins associated with plasticity in the phrenic motor nucleus. Experimental neurology 237:103-115.

Sauerland EK, Mitchell SP (1970) Electromyographic activity of the human genioglossus muscle in response to respiration and to positional changes of the head. Bulletin of the Los Angeles neurological societies 2:69-73.

Sawczuk A, Mosier KM (2001) Neural control of tongue movement with respect to respiration and swallowing. Crit Rev Oral Biol Med 12:18-37.

Scharf MT, Woo NH, Lattal M, Young JZ, Nguyen PV, Abel T (2002) Protein synthesis is required for the enhancement of long-term potentation and long-term memory by spaced training. Journal of neurophysiology 87:2770-2777.

Schaser AJ, Stang K, Connor NP, Behan M (2012) The effect of age and tongue exercise on BDNF and TrkB in the hypoglossal nucleus of rats. Behavioural brain research 226:235-241.

Schjetnan AG, Escobar ML (2012) In vivo BDNF modulation of hippocampal mossy fiber plasticity induced by high frequency stimulation. Hippocampus 22:1-8.

Schwartz AR, Barnes M, Hillman D, Malhotra A, Kezirian E, Smith PL, Hoegh T, Parrish D, Eastwood PR (2012) Acute upper airway responses to hypoglossal nerve stimulation during sleep in obstructive sleep apnea. American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine 185:420-426.

Schwarz LA, Miyamichi K, Gao XJ, Beier KT, Weissbourd B, DeLoach KE, Ren J, Ibanes S, Malenka RC, Kremer EJ, Luo L (2015) Viral-genetic tracing of the input-output organization of a central noradrenaline circuit. Nature 524:88-92.

Schwarz PB, Mir S, Peever JH (2014) Noradrenergic modulation of masseter muscle activity during natural rapid eye movement sleep requires glutamatergic signalling at the trigeminal motor nucleus. The Journal of physiology 592:3597-3609.

Seager MA, Huff KD, Barth VN, Phebus LA, Rasmussen K (2004) Fluoxetine administration potentiates the effect of olanzapine on locus coeruleus neuronal activity. Biological psychiatry 55:1103-1109.

Segal M (2005) Dendritic spines and long-term plasticity. Nature reviews Neuroscience 6:277-284.

Page 169: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

153

Sessle BJ, Henry JL (1985) Effects of Enkephalin and 5-Hydroxytryptamine on Solitary Tract Neurones Involved in Respiration and Respiratory Reflexes. Brain research 327:221-230.

Shahar E, Whitney CW, Redline S, Lee ET, Newman AB, Nieto FJ, O'connor GT, Boland LL, Schwarz JE, Samet JM (2001) Sleep-disordered breathing and cardiovascular disease. American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine 163:19-25.

Sharifullina E, Ostroumov K, Nistri A (2004) Activation of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors enhances efficacy of glutamatergic inputs to neonatal rat hypoglossal motoneurons in vitro. The European journal of neuroscience 20:1245-1254.

Shatz CJ (1992) The Developin Brain. Scientific American 267:60-67. Shen L, Duffin J (2002) Caudal expiratory neurones in the rat. Pflugers Archiv : European journal of

physiology 444:405-410. Shirey MJ, Smith JB, Kudlik DE, Huo BX, Greene SE, Drew PJ (2015) Brief anesthesia, but not voluntary

locomotion, significantly alters cortical temperature. Journal of neurophysiology 114:309-322. Shkoukani M, Babcock MA, Badr MS (2002) Effect of episodic hypoxia on upper airway mechanics in

humans during NREM sleep. J Appl Physiol (1985) 92:2565-2570. Siwek DF, Knapp CM, Kaur G, Datta S (2014) Dorsal subcoeruleus nucleus (SubCD) involvement in

context-associated fear memory consolidation. Exp Brain Res 232:1535-1545. Smith CC, Greene RW (2012) CNS dopamine transmission mediated by noradrenergic innervation. The

Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 32:6072-6080. Smith JC, Abdala AP, Koizumi H, Rybak IA, Paton JF (2007) Spatial and functional architecture of the

mammalian brain stem respiratory network: a hierarchy of three oscillatory mechanisms. Journal of neurophysiology 98:3370-3387.

Song G, Poon CS (2017) α2-adrenergic blockade rescues hypoglossal motor defense against obstructive sleep apnea. JCI insight 2:1-16.

Sood S, Morrison JL, Liu H, Horner RL (2005) Role of endogenous serotonin in modulating genioglossus muscle activity in awake and sleeping rats. American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine 172:1338-1347.

Stanton PK, Sarvey JM (1985) Depletion of Norepinephrine, But Not Serotonin, Reduces Long-term Potentiation in the Dentate Gyrus of Rat Hippocampal Slices. Neuroscience 5:2169-2176.

Steenland HW, Liu H, Sood S, Liu X, Horner RL (2006) Respiratory activation of the genioglossus muscle involves both non-NMDA and NMDA glutamate receptors at the hypoglossal motor nucleus in vivo. Neuroscience 138:1407-1424.

Stettner GM, Fenik VB, Kubin L (2012) Effect of chronic intermittent hypoxia on noradrenergic activation of hypoglossal motoneurons. J Appl Physiol (1985) 112:305-312.

Strittmatter SM, Vartanian T, Fishman MC (1992) GAP-43 as a Plasticity Protein in Neuronal Form and Repair. Journal of neurobiology 23:507-520.

Stujenske JM, Spellman T, Gordon JA (2015) Modeling the Spatiotemporal Dynamics of Light and Heat Propagation for In Vivo Optogenetics. Cell reports 12:525-534.

Surprenant A, North RA (1988) Mechanism of synaptic inhibition by noradrenaline acting at a2-adrenoceptors. Proc R Soc Lond B 234.

Sved AF, Felsten G (1987) Stimulation of the locus coeruleus decreases arterial pressure. Brain research 414:119-132.

Syed Z, Lin HS, Mateika JH (2013) The impact of arousal state, sex, and sleep apnea on the magnitude of progressive augmentation and ventilatory long-term facilitation. J Appl Physiol (1985) 114:52-65.

Szabo ST, de Montigny C, Blier P (2000) Progressive attenuation of the firing activity of locus coeruleus noradrenergic neurons by sustained administration of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 3:1-11.

Page 170: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

154

Szymusiak R, McGinty D (2008) Hypothalamic regulation of sleep and arousal. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1129:275-286.

Tadjalli A (2012) Plasticity of Vagal Origin: Identification and characterization of a novel form of neural plasticity in respiratory motor control. In: Cell and Systems Biology, vol. PhD, pp 1-242: University of Toronto.

Tadjalli A, Duffin J, Peever J (2010) Identification of a novel form of noradrenergic-dependent respiratory motor plasticity triggered by vagal feedback. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 30:16886-16895.

Takakura AC, Moreira TS, Colombari E, West GH, Stornetta RL, Guyenet PG (2006) Peripheral chemoreceptor inputs to retrotrapezoid nucleus (RTN) CO2-sensitive neurons in rats. The Journal of physiology 572:503-523.

Talley EM, Lei Q, Sirois JE, Bayliss DA (2000) TASK-1, a two-pore domain K+ channel, is modulated by multiple neurotransmitters in motoneurons. Neuron 25:399-410.

Tan W, Janczewski WA, Yang P, Shao XM, Callaway EM, Feldman JL (2008) Silencing preBotzinger complex somatostatin-expressing neurons induces persistent apnea in awake rat. Nature neuroscience 11:538-540.

Tang F, Lane S, Korsak A, Paton JF, Gourine AV, Kasparov S, Teschemacher AG (2014) Lactate-mediated glia-neuronal signalling in the mammalian brain. Nature communications 5:3284.

Tangel DJ, Mezzanotte WS, White DP (1995) Respiratory-related control of palatoglossus and levator palatini muscle activity. J Appl Physiol (1985) 78:680-688.

Taranto-Montemurro L, Edwards BA, Sands SA, Marques M, Eckert DJ, White DP, Wellman A (2016) Desipramine Increases Genioglossus Activity and Reduces Upper Airway Collapsibility during Non-REM Sleep in Healthy Subjects. American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine 194:878-885.

Teppema LJ, Veening JG, Kranenburg A, Dahan A, Berkenbosch A, Olievier C (1997) Expression of c-fos in the rat brainstem after exposure to hypoxia and to normoxic and hyperoxic hypercapnia. The Journal of comparative neurology 388:169-190.

Terada J, Nakamura A, Zhang W, Yanagisawa M, Kuriyama T, Fukuda Y, Kuwaki T (2008) Ventilatory long-term facilitation in mice can be observed during both sleep and wake periods and depends on orexin. J Appl Physiol (1985) 104:499-507.

Torontali Z (2012) The Nucleus of the Solitary Tract is Necessary for Apnea-Induced Respiratory Plasticity. In: Cell and Systems Biology, vol. M.Sc, pp 1-89: University of Toronto.

Travagli RA, Dunwiddie T, Williams JT (1995) Opioid inhibition in locus coeruleus. Journal of neurophysiology 74:518-528.

Trudeau LE (2004) Glutamate co-transmission as an emerging concept in monoamine neuron function. Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience : JPN 29:296-310.

Tsuda K, Yokoo H, Goldstein M (1989) Neuropeptide Y and Galanin in Norepinephrine Release in Hypothalamic Slices. Hypertension 14:81-86.

Turner DL, Mitchell GS (1997) Long-term facilitation of ventilation following repeated hypoxic episodes in awake goats. The Journal of physiology 499.2:543-550.

Tye KM, Deisseroth K (2012) Optogenetic investigation of neural circuits underlying brain disease in animal models. Nature reviews Neuroscience 13:251-266.

Valente P, Casagrande S, Nieus T, Verstegen AM, Valtorta F, Benfenati F, Baldelli P (2012) Site-specific synapsin I phosphorylation participates in the expression of post-tetanic potentiation and its enhancement by BDNF. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 32:5868-5879.

Valic M, Pecotic R, Pavlinac I, Valic Z, Peros K, Dogas Z (2010) Microinjection of methysergide into the raphe nucleus attenuated phrenic long-term facilitation in rats. Exp Brain Res 202:583-589.

Page 171: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

155

van Bockstaele EJ, Peoples J, Telegan P (1999) Efferent Projections of the Nucleus of the Solitary Tract to Peri-Locus Coeruleus Dendrites in Rat Brain: Evidence for a Monosynaptic Pathway. The Journal of comparative neurology 412:410-428.

Van Bockstaele EJ, Saunders A, Commons KG, Liu XB, Peoples J (2000) Evidence for coexistence of enkephalin and glutamate in axon terminals and cellular sites for functional interactions of their receptors in the rat locus coeruleus. The Journal of comparative neurology 417:103-114.

Vankov A, Herve-Minivielle A, Sara SJ (1995) Response to novelty and its rapid habituation in locus coeruleus neurons of the freely exploring rat. The European journal of neuroscience 7:1180-1187.

Vazey EM, Aston-Jones G (2014) Designer receptor manipulations reveal a role of the locus coeruleus noradrenergic system in isoflurane general anesthesia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 111:3859-3864.

Viemari JC, Bevengut M, Burnet H, Coulon P, Pequignot JM, Tiveron MC, Hilaire G (2004a) Phox2a gene, A6 neurons, and noradrenaline are essential for development of normal respiratory rhythm in mice. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 24:928-937.

Viemari JC, Bevengut M, Coulon P, Hilaire G (2004b) Nasal Trigeminal Inputs Release the A5 Inhibition Received by the Respiratory Rhythm Generator of the Mouse Neonate. Journal of neurophysiology 91:746-758.

Volgin DV, Fay R, Kubin L (2003) Postnatal development of serotonin 1B, 2A and 2C receptors in brainstem motoneurons. The European journal of neuroscience 17:1179-1188.

Volgin DV, Mackiewicz M, Kubin L (2001) a1B receptors are the main postsynaptic mediators of adrenergic excitation in brainstem motoneurons, a single-cell RT-PCR study. Journal of chemical neuroanatomy 22:157-166.

Wagner PG, Eldridge FL (1990) Development of short-term potentiation of respiration. Respiration physiology 82:129-140.

Wang JQ, Arora A, Yang L, Parelkar NK, Zhang G, Liu X, Choe ES, Mao L (2005) Phosphorylation of AMPA Receptors. Molecular Neurobiology 32:237-249.

Wang X, Pinol RA, Byrne P, Mendelowitz D (2014) Optogenetic stimulation of locus ceruleus neurons augments inhibitory transmission to parasympathetic cardiac vagal neurons via activation of brainstem alpha1 and beta1 receptors. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 34:6182-6189.

Watanabe S, Hoffman DA, Migliore M, Johnston D (2002) Dendritic K+ channels contribute to spike-timing dependent long-term potentiation in hippocampal pyramidal neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 99:8366-8371.

Weinshenker D, Holmes PV (2016) Regulation of neurological and neuropsychiatric phenotypes by locus coeruleus-derived galanin. Brain research 1641:320-337.

Wetmore C, Olson L, Bean AJ (1994) Regulation of Brain-derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF) Expression and Release from Hippocampal Neurons Is Mediated by Non-NMDA Type Glutamate Receptors. Neuroscience 14:1688-1700.

Widdicombe J (2001) Airway receptors. Respiration physiology 125:3-15. Wilkerson JE, Mitchell GS (2009) Daily intermittent hypoxia augments spinal BDNF levels, ERK

phosphorylation and respiratory long-term facilitation. Experimental neurology 217:116-123. Wirth KJ, Steinmeyer K, Ruetten H (2013) Sensitization of upper airway mechanoreceptors as a new

pharmacologic principle to treat obstructive sleep apnea: investigations with AVE0118 in anesthetized pigs. Sleep 36:699-708.

Xu ZQ, Zheng K, Hokfelt T (2005) Electrophysiological studies on galanin effects in brain--progress during the last six years. Neuropeptides 39:269-275.

Page 172: Neural Circuitry of Upper Airway Respiratory Plasticity

156

Yamamoto K, Lalley P, Mifflin S (2015) Acute intermittent optogenetic stimulation of nucleus tractus solitarius neurons induces sympathetic long-term facilitation. American journal of physiology Regulatory, integrative and comparative physiology 308:R266-275.

Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Davidson TJ, Mogri M, Deisseroth K (2011) Optogenetics in neural systems. Neuron 71:9-34.

Yokhana SS, Gerst DG, 3rd, Lee DS, Badr MS, Qureshi T, Mateika JH (2012) Impact of repeated daily exposure to intermittent hypoxia and mild sustained hypercapnia on apnea severity. J Appl Physiol (1985) 112:367-377.

Yoshii A, Constantine-Paton M (2010) Postsynaptic BDNF-TrkB signaling in synapse maturation, plasticity, and disease. Developmental neurobiology 70:304-322.

Younes M, Loewen A, Ostrowski M, Hanly P (2014) Short-term potentiation in the control of pharyngeal muscles in obstructive apnea patients. Sleep 37:1833-1849.

Young T, Palta M, Dempsey JA, Skatrud JB, Weber S, Badr MS (1993) The occurence of sleep-disordered breathing among middle-aged adults. The New England Journal of Medicine 328:1230-1235.

Young T, Peppard PE, Gottlieb DJ (2002) Epidemiology of Obstructive Sleep Apnea. American journal of respiratory and critical care medicine 165:1217-1239.

Zafra F, Castren E, Thoenen H, Lindholm D (1991) Interplay between glutamate and y-aminobutyric acid transmitter systems in the physiological regulation of brain-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth factor synthesis in hippocampal neurons. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 88:10037-10041.

Zanella S, Roux JC, Viemari JC, Hilaire G (2006) Possible modulation of the mouse respiratory rhythm generator by A1/C1 neurones. Respiratory physiology & neurobiology 153:126-138.

Zhang W, Mifflin SW (2007) Modulation of synaptic transmission to second-order peripheral chemoreceptor neurons in caudal nucleus tractus solitarius by alpha1-adrenoreceptors. The Journal of pharmacology and experimental therapeutics 320:670-677.

Zhang Y, McGuire M, White DP, Ling L (2003) Episodic phrenic-inhibitory vagus nerve stimulation paradoxically induces phrenic long-term facilitation in rats. The Journal of physiology 551:981-991.

Zhao Y, Fang Q, Straub SG, Sharp GW (2008) Both G i and G o heterotrimeric G proteins are required to exert the full effect of norepinephrine on the beta-cell K ATP channel. The Journal of biological chemistry 283:5306-5316.

Zhong H, Minneman KP (1999) a1-Adrenoceptor subtypes. European Journal of Pharmacology 375:261-276.

Zhu Y, Fenik P, Zhan G, Mazza E, Kelz M, Aston-Jones G, Veasey SC (2007) Selective loss of catecholaminergic wake active neurons in a murine sleep apnea model. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience 27:10060-10071.