influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/fulltext01.pdf · environmental...

36
Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology Master Thesis Influences of environment and personality on cognitive judgment bias in chickens Emelie Jansson LiTH-IFM- Ex--15/3002--SE Supervisors: Hanne Løvlie and Josefina Zidar, Linköping University Examiner: Jordi Altimiras, Linköping University Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology Linköpings universitet SE-581 83 Linköping, Sweden

Upload: others

Post on 25-Sep-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology

Master Thesis

Influences of environment and personality on

cognitive judgment bias in chickens

Emelie Jansson

LiTH-IFM- Ex--15/3002--SE

Supervisors: Hanne Løvlie and Josefina Zidar,

Linköping University

Examiner: Jordi Altimiras, Linköping University

Department of Physics, Chemistry and Biology

Linköpings universitet

SE-581 83 Linköping, Sweden

Page 2: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

Rapporttyp Report category

Examensarbete

D-uppsats

Språk/Language

Engelska/English

Titel/Title:

Influences of environment and personality on cognitive judgment bias in chickens

Författare/Author:

Emelie Jansson

Sammanfattning/Abstract:

Cognitive processes include biases, such as cognitive judgment bias. Cognitive judgment bias influences

how the surrounding is interpreted, and this can differ between individuals. However, thus far no formal

framework exists to understand how cognitive judgment bias works. Here I investigated how

environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks. First I

investigated how two environmental factors affected the cognitive judgment bias of laying hen chicks.

Chicks were exposed to stress and/or environmental enrichment, and tested in a cognitive judgment bias

test before and after collective unpredictable stressors were presented. The results showed that chicks

living in enriched environments were faster to reach all cues after the collective stressors than chicks living

in non-enriched environments. Individual differences are often observed in animals, even when raised

under identical conditions; therefore I also investigated if variation in personality influence cognitive

judgment bias. Red junglefowl chicks were thereby raised in equal environments and exposed to

personality assays in addition to a cognitive judgment bias test. I demonstrated that less nervous chicks

were more optimistic towards ambiguous and negative cues than more nervous chicks. Also previous

studies have found indications of connections between cognitive bias and environment or personality. I

conclude that environmental enrichment can buffer the influence of stress on cognitive judgment bias and

that personality has a small influence on interpretations of stimuli. In the future, experiments in this field

should focus on exploring more aspects on how cognitive biases occurs to improve our understanding of

cognitive processes.

ISBN

LITH-IFM-A-EX—15/3002—SE

__________________________________________________

ISRN __________________________________________________

Serietitel och serienummer ISSN

Title of series, numbering

Handledare/Supervisor Hanne Løvlie and Josefina Zidar

Ort/Location: Linköping

Nyckelord/Keyword:

Cognitive judgment bias, personality, welfare, environmental enrichment

Datum/Date

2015-06-15

URL för elektronisk version

Institutionen för fysik, kemi och biologi

Department of Physics, Chemistry and

Biology

Avdelningen för biologi

Instutitionen för fysik och mätteknik

Page 3: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

Content

1 Abstract .............................................................................................. 2

2 Introduction ....................................................................................... 2

3 Methods .............................................................................................. 6

3.1 Influence of environmental factors on cognitive judgment bias.. 6

3.1.1 Animals and housing ................................................................ 6

3.1.2 Test room conditions ................................................................ 8

3.1.3 Associative learning ................................................................. 9

3.1.4 Cognitive judgment bias test .................................................. 11

3.2 Linking variation in personality to cognitive judgment bias ..... 12

3.2.1 Animals and housing .............................................................. 12

3.2.2 Associative learning ............................................................... 12

3.2.3 Cognitive judgment bias test .................................................. 13

3.2.4 Personality assays ................................................................... 13

3.3 Statistical analysis ...................................................................... 17

3.3.1 Influence of environmental factors on cognitive judgment bias

.................................................................................................17

3.3.2 Linking variation in personality to cognitive judgment bias.. 18

4 Results .............................................................................................. 18

4.1 Influence of environmental factors on cognitive judgment

bias.............. .......................................................................................... 18

4.2 Linking variation in personality to cognitive judgment bias ..... 21

5 Discussion ......................................................................................... 24

5.1 Influence of environmental factors on cognitive judgment

bias....... ................................................................................................. 25

5.2 Linking variation in personality to cognitive judgment bias ..... 26

5.3 Societal and ethical considerations ............................................ 27

6 Conclusions ...................................................................................... 28

7 Acknowledgements ......................................................................... 28

8 References ........................................................................................ 29

Page 4: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

2

1 Abstract

Cognitive processes include biases, such as cognitive judgment bias.

Cognitive judgment bias influences how the surrounding is interpreted,

and this can differ between individuals. However, thus far no formal

framework exists to understand how cognitive judgment bias works. Here

I investigated how environmental factors and personality influence

cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks. First I investigated how

two environmental factors affected the cognitive judgment bias of laying

hen chicks. Chicks were exposed to stress and/or environmental

enrichment, and tested in a cognitive judgment bias test before and after

collective unpredictable stressors were presented. The results showed that

chicks living in enriched environments were faster to reach all cues after

the collective stressors than chicks living in non-enriched environments.

Individual differences are often observed in animals, even when raised

under identical conditions; therefore I also investigated if variation in

personality influence cognitive judgment bias. Red junglefowl chicks

were thereby raised in equal environments and exposed to personality

assays in addition to a cognitive judgment bias test. I demonstrated that

less nervous chicks were more optimistic towards ambiguous and

negative cues than more nervous chicks. Also previous studies have

found indications of connections between cognitive bias and environment

or personality. I conclude that environmental enrichment can buffer the

influence of stress on cognitive judgment bias and that personality has a

small influence on interpretations of stimuli. In the future, experiments in

this field should focus on exploring more aspects on how cognitive biases

occurs to improve our understanding of cognitive processes.

2 Introduction

Cognition is the process of gathering, processing, storing and acting upon

information from the surrounding environment (Shettleworth 2010).

Animals rely on cognitive processes to be able to find food, shelter and

mates as well as to avoid predators. Individual variation in cognitive

abilities can therefore have great impact on animal lives (Shettleworth

2010). Some known individual difference in the cognitive process is

cognitive biases. One of these biases is cognitive judgment bias, defined

as a bias in the cognitive process where the interpretation of stimuli

differs dependent on the mood (i.e. a prolonged affective state, affective

state being subjective experiences of different valence that do not occur

as a response to a stimuli, Russell 2003, Mendl et al. 2010) of the

individual (Mathews & MacLeod 1985, Eysenck et al. 1991).

Page 5: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

3

Work on cognitive judgment bias in humans show that individuals

experiencing a negative mood (e.g. depression or anxiety) interpret

stimuli more negatively than individuals experiencing a positive mood

(e.g. happy or excited, e.g. Mathews et al. 1989, Eysenck et al. 1991). A

well-known example of cognitive judgment bias is when humans are

asked if a glass half-filled with liquid, is half full or half empty. Within

animal science, cognitive judgment bias was first investigated by Harding

and colleagues (2004). They observed that different environmental

conditions changed the response of male rats to sound cues indicating

reward or punishment (Harding et al. 2004). Rats that were housed in an

unpredictable environment showed more pessimistic responses to cues

predicting reward and ambiguous cues resembling the rewarded cue, than

animals living in predictable housing systems (Harding et al. 2004). Thus,

by observing animals’ reactions to ambiguous stimuli that are

intermediate to cues associated with punishment and reward, we can

interpret the individuals’ affective states.

Cognitive judgment bias tests are generally performed by training the

animal to associate one cue (e.g. tone, colour or placement) with a reward

(mostly a treat) and its opposite (e.g. white vs. black or right vs. left),

with a lack of reward or a punishment (e.g. electric shock or white noise).

How the animal behaves towards intermediate, ambiguous, cues is then

investigated as indicative of the affective state of the animal (i.e. the

response to the ambiguous cues are interpreted with regards to the

previously rewarded and unrewarded or punished cues). Reactions to

ambiguous stimuli that resemble the response to unrewarded or punished

cues are interpreted as reactions of a more pessimistic individual

(Harding et al. 2004, Matheson et al. 2008). On the other hand, reactions

to ambiguous stimuli resembling the response to rewarded cues are

interpreted as the animal being more optimistic (Harding et al. 2004,

Matheson et al. 2008).

The knowledge that human cognitive judgment bias is influenced by their

mood has been used to further the research of animal welfare. Animal

welfare being defined as an individual's attempts and level of success in

coping with its environment (Broom 1986, 1991). Studies of animal

welfare highlight the importance of evaluating quality of life of animals

that we keep in captivity (Fraser et al. 1997). An important component of

animal welfare is the importance of subjective states of animals (Fraser et

al. 1997, Hewson 2003, Mendl et al. 2009). According to Duncan (1996)

it is even the most important part of assessing animal welfare. Previously,

focus has been on preventing poor welfare such as feather pecking in

chickens (e.g. Dixona 2008), tail biting in pigs (e.g. Sutherland et al.

Page 6: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

4

2009), or other stereotypic behaviours (repetitive behaviours seemingly

without a purpose, e.g. Mason & Latham 2004). However, more recently,

focus has been directed towards measures of positive welfare. One way

of estimating both positive and negative animal welfare is by observing

the subjective states of animals. Animal welfare can consequently be

measured by investigating the mood of animals (Russell 2003, Mendl et

al. 2010). To investigate the mood of animals cognitive judgment bias

tests have been used. Cognitive judgement bias tests are ideal to use as

they do not only measure the arousal of an individual’s mood (which

gives no distinction between positive and negative affective states), but

also the valence of it (which can be positive or negative, Mendl et al.

2009).

Variants of the method to measure cognitive judgment bias in animals

have been used to assess welfare in starlings (Sturnus vulgaris, Bateson

and Matheson 2007, Brilot et al. 2010, Matheson et al. 2008), chickens

(Gallus gallus, Salmeto et al. 2011, Seehuus et al. 2013), rats (Rattus

norvegicus, Enkel et al. 2010) and calves (Bos taurus, Neave et al. 2013).

These studies have found that enriched and/or predictable housing

conditions result in more optimistic animals, interpreted as animals with

better welfare. However, Wichman and colleagues (2012) did not find a

relationship between welfare and enrichment in a study on welfare in

laying hens. Moreover, the studies mentioned above have typically

focused on one single factor (e.g. stress or environmental enrichment) at

the time that could influence the emotional state of the animals and are

therefore potentially missing important factors that might influence the

individuals’ biases. To understand what influences variation in cognitive

judgment bias, it is vital to investigate how several different factors, such

as enrichment and stress (the non-specific response of the body to any

demand made upon it, Selye 1973) affect the response of an individual.

However, it is also important to investigate how interactions of factors

affects individual interpretation of stimuli as different stimuli might

influence each other.

While trying to understand animal emotions and welfare through

cognitive judgment bias tests, it has become apparent that there are large

individual differences in how individuals’ responses are affected. For

example individual differences have been observed when observing the

effects of stress on animal welfare (Wichman et al. 2012). Intraspecific

individual variation in behavioural responses over time and / or across

context (Gosling 2001, Dall et al. 2004, Réale et al. 2007) is used to

describe animal personality. Thereby personality could be the reason

behind the individual differences observed in cognitive judgment bias

Page 7: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

5

tests. Animal personality has been demonstrated in a broad range of

species (Gosling 2001), including the fowl (Favati et al. 2014a,b).

Personality gradients that animals are described along are typically

shyness-boldness, exploration-avoidance, activity, sociability,

aggressiveness (Réale et al. 2007), and nervousness (Gosling & John,

1999). Personality is known to interlink with variation in physiology,

commonly investigated in rodents and under the term ‘coping styles’

(Koolhaas et al. 1999, 2010). Further, connections between cognitive

processes and personality have been seen as success in associative

learning (a process in which a behavioural response becomes altered or

acquired as a consequence of sensory information from the environment,

Lachman 1997) have been connected to boldness (Dugatkin & Alfieri

2003). With this cognitive process shown to be related to variation in

personality it is possible that also biases in cognitive processes are related

to personality. In fact, attention bias has been shown to correlate with

personality in parrots (Amazona amazonica, Cussen & Mench, 2014).

However, no formal framework has, been developed for how personality

and cognitive biases may be related. Although one may predict that

individuals that are more nervous or stress-sensitive would be prone to

have more negative biases.

In order to thoroughly understand how cognitive judgment bias occurs

and what influences it, I here investigate why differences in cognitive

judgment bias occur among individuals by (1) investigating how

environmental enrichments and stress influence cognitive judgment bias

in chicks, and (2) investigating how variation in personality relates to

variation in cognitive judgment bias among chicks.

When exploring the influence of environmental complexity, stress and

personality on variation in cognitive judgment bias, I used two different

breeds of fowl (Gallus gallus). Fowl can easily be breed and are easy to

handle. Further, fowl are precocial, meaning that they can take care of

themselves already after they have hatched. As a consequence, parental

influences and other accumulated experiences can be minimized if using

young chicks. Chickens in the egg- and meat industries are exposed to a

lot of stressors, therefore I chose to use a common breed of laying hen,

the Bovans Robust for the first experiment (influence of stress and

environment on cognitive judgment bias). When testing the influences of

personality on cognitive judgment bias I found it more suitable to use a

breed that had not undergone strong artificial selection in the same way

as domesticated breeds have done. Strong selection typically reduce

individual differences and genetic variation. For this reason the wild

ancestor of our domesticated chickens, the Red Junglefowl, was used for

Page 8: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

6

the second experiment (influence of personality on cognitive judgment

bias).

3 Methods

3.1 Influence of environmental factors on cognitive judgment bias

3.1.1 Animals and housing

During the summer of 2014, female Bovans Robust chicks from Swefarm

AB were used to investigate potential differences in cognitive judgment

bias in chicks raised under different environmental conditions. At the day

of hatching, chicks (n = 96) were collected and transported to Funbo

research facility (SLU) at Lövsta, Uppsala, where they were housed in

eight equally sized pens with twelve chicks per pen (1.2 x 1.2 m, figure

2). The walls of the pens prevented visual, but not vocal, contact with

neighbouring chicks. Chicks were exposed to cognitive judgment bias

tests on two separate occasions, first 14 - 16 days post-hatching (n = 96),

and again 35 - 36 days post-hatching (n = 65, figure 1). The second

cognitive judgment bias test was performed after a unpredictable,

collective stressors had been introduced (figure 1). During the first test

the temperature was kept to around 28 °C with light maintained at a 16:8

hour light:dark ratio. During the second cognitive judgment bias test the

temperature was kept around 26 °C and lights maintained a 9:15 hour

light:dark ratio. The conditions of light and heat was used to simulate the

conditions in laying hen farms in Sweden at the different ages.

Page 9: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

7

Figure 1. Timeline overview of the different experimental phases that female Bovans Robust chicks were exposed to. See main text for details about each phase and test.

Figure 2. Layout of pen setup in the chicken house showing placement of pens in four different treatments that female Bovans Robust chicks were kept in. Enriched pens contain slated boxes, non-enriched pens do not contain slated boxes, pens housing chicks subjected to cold stress is marked with an ‘S’ and pens housing chicks not subjected to stress is marked with ‘US’.

Chicks were randomly assigned to four different treatment groups (n = 24

/ treatment): stressed non-enriched, unstressed non-enriched, stressed

enriched and unstressed enriched (figure 1, 2). The non-enriched pens

had wood shavings on the floor, a round feeder with a commercial chick

feed and a water bell. The enriched pens had access to cover where the

chicks could hide, four perches at different heights, a small roof over a

part of the top perch and wooden blocks on the floor to provide different

types of structures, as well as wood shavings on the floor, a round feeder

Page 10: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

8

with commercial chick feed and a water bell (figure 3). To simulate a

stressful event that could occur during transport of commercial chicks,

the chicks in the stressed treatment groups were exposed to a cold stress

at two days of age (hereafter called ‘cold stress’). This was done by

keeping pairs of chicks in boxes (20W x 15L x 15H cm) for six hours

while the room temperature was lowered to 18 - 20 °C. Pilot studies were

performed by Irene Campderrich Lecumberri prior to the test on another

set of chicks of the same breed showing that a temperature of 18 - 20 °C

lowered the chicks’ body temperature, whereby it was regarded to be

stressful for the chicks. Conversely, the chicks not subjected to the cold

stress had a temperature of 33 °C during the same time period. At 22 - 27

days of age, between the first and second cognitive bias test all chicks

from all four treatment groups were exposed to a battery of stressors

using unpredictable lighting and sound at 90 dB (animal and mechanical

noises as well as music) in their home pens (hereafter called ‘collective

stressors’, figure 1).

Figure 3. Drawing (not to scale) of an enriched pen for the female Bovans Robust chicks exposed to an enriched treatments. The pen is seen from a) above and b) the left side, where the slatted box is a cover, grey lines are perches, the black filled box is the roof over the top perch, the black boxes with grey filling are wooden blocks and the circles indicate water bell (black) and feeder (grey).

3.1.2 Test room conditions

During training and testing, four to twelve chicks from the same home

pen were collected using a small, dark box and transported to the testing

room, located in an adjacent building. Birds were left in the dark box a

maximum of 30 minutes while waiting to be tested. The test room had

covered windows and was lit by standard fluorescent lamps to prevent

UV-light from altering the colours of the cues used. The temperature in

the test room was 24 - 25 ˚C during the first test and reached 27 - 28 °C

Page 11: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

9

in the second test. The mean temperature was higher during the second

test because of the extreme temperature in Sweden during this period of

testing. This was not expected to affect the behaviour of the birds in any

biased manner, and all birds were exposed to the same temperature within

each test round.

3.1.3 Associative learning

In order to investigate variation in cognitive judgment bias among

treatment groups, we trained chicks to associate white (n = 47) or black

(n = 49) visual cues with a reward (a small piece of mealworm) after

which we investigated their reaction to three intermediate (grey) visual

cues. Arenas were constructed out of plastic boxes covered with single

faced corrugated cardboard. Prior to testing the chicks were trained to be

alone in the arena by placing groups into the arenas and successively

decreasing the group size until each chick was calm even when alone in

the arena. A small partition was put at the far short end of the test arena to

separate the cues and thereby make sure the chick had to make an active

choice (figure 4a). On either side of the partition, signs (9 x 9 cm) of

black and white colour were placed. The signs were squares of laminated

photo paper, 9 x 9 cm in size, placed on the ground, leaning against the

wall. In front of the signs were bowls (5Ø x 3H cm) made of transparent

plastic, inside of which laminated paper of black or white was lined to

show both from the inside and the outside to maximize the cue stimuli for

the chicks. During training the rewarded cue was always placed on the

left side of the partition for the first choice, after which right and left

placement of the cues were alternated randomly according to a pre-

determined schedule to prevent chicks from associating placement with a

reward.

Page 12: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

10

Figure 4. Drawing of the arena used for training (a) and tesintg (b) Bovans Robusta and Red Junglefowl chicks in a cognitive judgment bias test. Chicks were first trained to associate a colour cue with a reward before their reactions to also intermediate cues were scored. Cues are at the top of the figure and the chick at the bottom (closest to the handler). The size of the arena differed between the breeds; for Bovans Robust chicks the an arena had the size of 46W x 68L x 42H cm and for the Red Junglefowl chicks it was 28W x 37L x 18H cm.

At the start of a training session the chick was placed in the opposite

short side of the arena from the cues (figure 4a), closest to the handler,

with a hand initially held in front of the chick to prevent jumps or rushes

to the cues. In the beginning of a training session the chicks received help

finding the mealworm in the bowl by the handler tapping in the correct

direction with the fingers or lifting the chick to the correct cue. Once the

chick started walking towards the cues on its own upon being placed in

the arena, no further help was offered.

A training session lasted for 15 minutes, and chicks were allowed to

make an unlimited amount of trials during each session. Each choice the

chick made was noted as a correct choice (walking to the rewarded

colour), an incorrect choice (walking to the unrewarded colour) or as

help, if the chick had received help as explained above. If the chick made

an incorrect choice it was immediately picked up, meaning that it did not

receive a reward, and the cues were switched accorded to a pre-

determined pseudo-random order. The association between cue and

reward was considered to have been learned when the chick had made six

correct choices in a row. To learn the association chicks were allowed

three training sessions, after which 87 chicks had learned the association.

Upon reaching the criteria of six correct choices in a row the chick was

placed back into the cage and given at least one hour of rest before it was

exposed to a cognitive judgment bias test.

Page 13: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

11

3.1.4 Cognitive judgment bias test

We tested cognitive judgment bias by presenting different cues one by

one to the chicks. The cues consisted of the previously rewarded and

unrewarded cues together with three intermediate cues (25 %, 50 % and

75 % in between black and white), creating a ‘near rewarded’ cue, a

‘middle’ cue and a ‘near unrewarded’ cue, regardless of which cue chicks

had initially been trained to associate with a reward. The responses of

chicks towards each cue were scored separately.

Before the cognitive judgment bias test, we made sure that each chick had

learned the association between cue and reward by exposing them to a

‘refresh’ trial, where chicks again had to make six correct choices in a

row in the same fashion as described above. Once this association was

made, chicks were exposed to one cue at a time in the same arena as

described above. However, this time the partition was taken away and the

cue placed in the middle of the short end side (figure 4b), thereby

eliminating possible problems of chicks favouring one side.

The chick was placed in the arena with its back towards the cue (figure

4b) to make the chick see the cue first once it turned around, thereby

preventing the chick from making a decision before starting to move. A

stopwatch was started as soon as the chick turned its head towards the

cue, and latency until the chick had its head over the bowl was measured.

A maximum latency of 30 seconds to reach the cue was set to prevent

chicks from getting tired or weary of the test. If the chick jumped out of

the arena, it was given a score of the maximum latency (30 seconds). The

first and the second cue presented to chicks during this test were always

the rewarded cue followed by the unrewarded cue. These first two cues

were not included in the analysis since they were considered to be

acclimatisation trials to the new test set-up.

After the first two cues, the order of cues was presented according to a

pre-determined, pseudo-randomized schedule. Each intermediate cue was

presented to the chicks two times and the rewarded and unrewarded cues

were presented eight and seven times respectively, for a total of 21 cue

presentations. The rewarded cue was rewarded throughout the cognitive

judgment bias test to keep the chicks motivated to investigate the cues,

but this was the only cue where chicks received a reward as this was the

only cue they were pre-trained to interpret as positive.

For the second cognitive bias test the arenas were larger to adjust to the

larger size of the older chicks. But from his, everything else was

performed as described as above, with the chicks being through a

Page 14: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

12

‘refresh’ trial before proceeding to the cognitive bias test. At the time of

the second cognitive judgment bias test 22 chicks had passed away,

whereby the remaining 65 chicks were tested.

3.2 Linking variation in personality to cognitive judgment bias

3.2.1 Animals and housing

Red Junglefowl chicks (males and females) from 29 different families (n

= 1 - 4 from each family) were used to investigate the relationship

between cognitive judgment bias and personality. The study was carried

out in the spring of 2014 at Linköping University. Sixty eight chicks were

hatched, out of which 50 were randomly selected to take part in the

cognitive judgment bias test. The remaining 18 chicks lived with the test

chicks, however they were not used in this study. Chicks were housed in

pens (70W x 72L x 60H cm) in the laboratory where the experiments

were to take place. Initially, chicks were all housed together, but as they

grew larger they were randomly parted into mixed-sex groups in five

pens.

In their home pens, chicks had access to a perch and a heating plate for

warmth, as well as commercial chick feed in a feed bell and water in a

water bell. Room temperature in the laboratory was kept around 25 ˚C

throughout the five weeks of testing.

3.2.2 Associative learning

From the day of hatching, the chicks were accustomed to getting handled

and being alone in the test arena. The test arena was a mesh arena

covered with single faced corrugated cardboard. Thereafter, at three days

of age, learning began by teaching the chicks to associate a randomly

assigned colour cue, white (n = 25) or black (n = 25), with a reward (a

piece of mealworm). As in the test above, a partition was put up in one

short end of the arena to clearly separate the cues placed on either side

(figure 4a). The same cues as above, consisting of a sign with a matching

bowl in front of it, were used. As the test room had no windows UV-light

was not an issue, and the room was also here lit up by standard

fluorescent lighting.

As in the previous test the assigned rewarded colour was placed on the

left side for the first choice in a trial however it was then altered every

time the chick had chosen a cue. Each choice was marked in a protocol in

the same way as described above and this continued for 15 minutes or

until the chick had made a correct choice five times in a row, whereby the

association was considered learned. In total chicks were allowed seven

Page 15: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

13

trials of fifteen minutes each to learn the association between cue and

reward, after which all chicks had learned the association. During each

session the chick could choose between the colours for as many times as

it had time for, as mentioned above. When the association was learned

the session was ended and the chick put back in the home pen and given

at least an hour of rest before it was tested in the cognitive judgment bias

test. For more details about the associative learning, see above.

3.2.3 Cognitive judgment bias test

A cognitive judgment bias test was performed to investigate differences

in how chicks with known personality interpret their surroundings. At this

time the same five colour cues as in the test described above (the

previously rewarded and unrewarded cues as well as three intermediate

cues) were presented to the chicks one at a time. Chick responses were

recorded by measuring latency to reach each cue in the same fashion as

described above.

Directly following a successful ‘refresh’ trial of associative learning,

where they had to choose the correct bowl six times in a row. Afterwards

the chicks (at the time six to nine days old) were exposed to a cognitive

judgment bias test, as described above.

Each intermediate cue was presented three times and the rewarded and

unrewarded cues were presented eleven to thirteen times each in a

pseudo-randomized pre-determined order, for a total of 33 cue

presentations. In this test the previously rewarded cue kept being

rewarded to keep the chicks motivation up throughout the test. The

rewarded cue was always presented first followed by the unrewarded cue.

However, these two initial cues were not included in the analysis, as

mentioned above. The maximum latency to reach the cue was set to 60

seconds. A stopwatch was started as soon as the chick turned its head

towards the cue, from the position of having the back towards the cue,

and latency until the chick had its head over the bowl was measured.

Jumping out of the arena was recorded as the maximum latency of 60

seconds.

3.2.4 Personality assays

Upon finishing the cognitive judgment bias test the chicks were exposed

to a range of personality assays (n = 49, one chick died previous to

personality assaying).

Page 16: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

14

3.2.4.1 Novel arena and novel object

Novel arena (NA) and novel objet (NO) tests are commonly used and

validated tests for investigating personality in fowl (Forkman et al. 2007;

Favati et al. 2014a,b). Chicks were tested in NA and NO tests when 27-

29 days old, with a NA test directly followed by a NO test. Two arenas,

made out of solid wood walls with a mesh roof, were set up next to each

other on the floor in the lab. In these arenas the floors were covered with

rubber mats, upon which some wood shavings were sprinkled. Two

empty food bells and one empty large water bell were set up at the same

place in each arena so that the chick could not see the entire arena from

the start position, thereby encouraging exploration. Cameras from two

different angles were used to record chick behaviour during the tests.

Two chicks were tested simultaneously in two identical arenas. The

handlers placed the birds in the upper left corner of each arena (figure 5)

and then hid behind a screen during the test.

Figure 5. Drawing of arenas used to test responses of Red Junglefowl chicks to novel arenas and novel objects. Large, unfilled, circles indicate placement of large water bell, small, grey filled, circles indicate placements of food bells, black triangles indicates placements of cameras, the dotted lines divides the imaginary squares used to record activity and the chick placement at the beginning of the novel arena test is in the top left corner. Each arena measures (114L x 76W x 40H cm).

After the chicks had been exposed to the NA for ten minutes lights were

turned off and a NO, being a yellow, round stuffed animal with big eyes

and a tail (figure 6), were placed into each arena as far away from the

chick as possible. Lights were then turned back on after the handlers had

hidden behind the screen. This procedure was used in order to influence

the chicks as little as possible as the NO was put into the arena. Chicks

were exposed to the NO test for ten minutes, after which they were put

Page 17: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

15

back into the home cage. In between each trial the arenas were cleaned to

remove any faeces left from the previously tested bird.

Figure 6. Stuffed animal used as novel object to investigate personality in chicks (ca. 10 cm in diameter and 20 cm tail).

I later decoded the videos, recording latency to move, latency to explore

all six imaginary areas the arenas were divided into, number of square

changes, number of escape attempts, and the chick's behaviour every ten

seconds using instantaneous sampling (table 1). The behaviours observed

were chosen in order to measure fear response and activity in the chicks.

Page 18: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

16

Table 1. Observed behaviours of Red Junglefowl chicks during instantaneous sampling of behavioural responses in novel arena and novel object test

Behaviour Explanation

Vigilance The chick has its neck stretched out, while either

walking around or standing still with legs

straight.

Eat The chick holds its head in a position lower than

in a relaxed, neutral position, or it pecks on the

ground or at an object (not the novel object).

Activity The chick is moving around in the arena, body

position or pace not important.

Stand The chick is in a still position, legs extended so

that the stomach does not touch the ground.

Other Behaviours not listed above (e.g. lying down).

3.2.4.2 Tonic immobility

A validated test of fear responses in chicks is the tonic immobility test

(TI, Forkman et al. 2007). In the TI test, latency until the chick starts to

move is used to measure fear. A long duration in TI is considered to show

higher levels of fear. The TI test was performed on all chicks in this study

at 30 days of age by the same observer, in order to make sure that it was

done in the same way for all chicks. During this test a chick was placed

on its back in a v-shaped construction (figure 7). After being placed on

the back, chicks were held down for fifteen seconds with one of the

handlers’ hands gently holding down its chest while the other hand

covered the head. As the fifteen seconds ended the pressure of the hand

on the chick was released and a stopwatch was started. During the time

when the chick was on its back the handler avoided eye contact to

minimize human influence of the duration of the TI. Latency to first head

movement and latency until the chick jumped up on its feet were

recorded in seconds. Each chick was given three trials to go into tonic

Page 19: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

17

immobility, which was regarded as achieved when the chick stayed on its

back for three seconds or longer. The number of trials it took to induce

tonic immobility was also noted and if the attempts to reach tonic

immobility failed the latency was noted as zero seconds and the chick

was released back into the home cage. A maximum latency of ten

minutes was set for the TI, after which the chick was helped up from the

position by a gentle nudge to its side and maximum latency (600 s) was

noted.

Figure 7. V-shaped construction used to induce Red Junglefowl chicks in tonic immobility to investigate fear response as a part of a personality assessment.

3.3 Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using R Studio Version 0.98.1062.

3.3.1 Influence of environmental factors on cognitive judgment bias

When investigating environmental impact on cognitive judgment bias,

linear mixed-effect models (LMM) were used for the first and second

cognitive judgment bias test. Mean latencies per chick and cue were

calculated per test (creating a total of ten mean latencies per chick) and

log transformed before used as the response variable. The variables ‘cold

stress’ (i.e. whether the chick was exposed to a cold stress or not),

‘environmental enrichment’ (i.e. whether the chick was raised in an

enriched environment or non-enriched environment) were included in the

LMMs as fixed effects. The interaction between cold stress and

environmental enrichment was initially included but was later dropped

from the final model because it was not significant. ‘Pen’ (i.e. pen 1 - 8)

and ‘ID’ (i.e. the individual id number of the chicks) were added as

random effects. An Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed on the

LMMs from the first and second cognitive judgment bias test separately

to search for significant results. Post-hoc unpaired t-tests were performed

when there were a significant effect in the LMMs to investigate if there

was a specific factor, such as one cue, that influenced the results, this was

Page 20: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

18

not the case. Furthermore a Mann- Whitney U test was performed to

investigate if the differences detected could be because of differences in

learning.

3.3.2 Linking variation in personality to cognitive judgment bias

To reduce the behaviour variables obtained from the personality assays a

principal component analyses (PCA) was performed. The behaviours

added to the PCA were latency to move and latency to visit all areas from

the novel arena test, activity, vigilance, number of escape attempts and

number of square changes from both the novel arena and the novel object

test, and latency to move head and latency to jump up from the tonic

immobility test (for more information about these behaviours and other

behavioural responses, see above under Personality assays). Four

principal components (PCs) had eigen values larger than 1 and were

consider from the PCA. These PCs contained data describing mainly

variation in activity (PC 1), fearfulness (PC 2), escape proneness (PC 3)

and nervousness (PC 4, table 2). Individual scores were obtained for each

PC and analysed using a LMM to investigate if personality could predict

individual scores in the cognitive judgment bias test. Mean latencies per

chick and cue from the cognitive judgment bias test (for a total of five

mean latencies per chick) were used as response variable and the four

PCs were added as fixed effects. As random effects ‘ID’ was used. Due to

lack of significant effects, the variables ‘Sex’ (i.e. male, female),

‘Family’ (i.e. family 1 - 29) and PC*Sex interactions, were dropped from

the model. An ANOVA was used to search for significant results from

the final model of LMM. The interactions between PC and Cue (the cues

used in the test, i.e. rewarded, near rewarded, middle, near unrewarded

and unrewarded cues) were further investigated in a linear model (LM)

carried out for each ambiguous cue (i.e. one for each of the three grey

cues). These LMs were searched for significant results using ANOVAs.

4 Results

4.1 Influence of environmental factors on cognitive judgment bias

Chicks required 9 - 89 choices to learn the association between cue and

reward, with no effect of cold stress (w = 995, p = 0.67) or enrichment (w

= 918, p = 0.82). Neither stress nor enrichment had an influence on how

the chicks performed in the first cognitive judgment bias test, prior to the

collective stressors (table 2, figure 8a). During the second cognitive

judgment bias test, stress once again had no effect on latency to reach the

cues (table 2). However, chicks that were raised in enriched environments

had significantly shorter latencies to reach the cues in comparison to

Page 21: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

19

chicks that had lived in non-enriched environments (table 2, figure 8b).

The difference was explained by chicks in non-enriched pens being

slower to reach the cues in the second cognitive judgment bias test (df =

1, χ² = 44.03, p = 0.00000000003), not by chicks from enriched pens

being faster (df = 1, χ² = 0.01, p = 0.91). This indicates that enrichment

buffered stress and that chicks in enriched environments were able to

have a more positive cognitive judgment bias in comparison to chicks

living in non-enriched environments, after the collective stressors.

Table 2. Results from ANOVA when performing an linear mixed effects model. It was investigated if stress and / or enrichment have an effect of response to the cognitive judgment bias tests in female Bovans Robust chicks.

Variable χ² Df p-value

First cognitive judgment bias test

Enrichment 0.10 1 0.75

Stress 0.007 1 0.93

Second cognitive judgment bias test

Enrichment 5.07 1 0.02*

Stress 1.13 1 0.29

* p < 0.05

Page 22: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

20

Figure 8. Responses of female Bovans Robust chicks to two cognitive judgment bias tests. Chicks were tested before (a) and after (b) being subjected to unpredictable stressors. Chicks displayed more optimistic responses (lower latency to reach the cues) in the second test when living in enriched environments in comparison to chicks living in non-enriched environments. Mean latencies ± standard error are presented. * p<0.05, ** p<0.01

Page 23: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

21

4.2 Linking variation in personality to cognitive judgment bias

Red Junglefowl chicks needed 13 - 54 choices to learn the association

between colour (black or white) and reward. Results from the cognitive

judgment bias test show that chicks displayed a shorter latency to reach

the rewarded cue in comparison to all other cues, and that latencies to

reach the cues increased with its resemblance to the unrewarded cue

(figure 9). The PCA clustered behaviours into 4 components describing

variation in activity, fearfulness, escape proneness and nervousness (table

3). Performing a LMM and ANOVA showed a significant interaction

between individual nervousness score and latency to reach the cues,

depending on cue (table 4). Analysing each cue on its own showed no

effect of the PCs on latency to reach the rewarded cue, the cue close to

rewarded or middle cue (table 5). However, the cues near unrewarded and

unrewarded were positively correlated with nervousness, meaning that

more nervous individuals were slower to reach the near unrewarded

(figure 10a) and unrewarded (figure 10b) cues (table 5).

Figure 9. Responses of red junglefowl chicks in a cognitive bias test. Chicks were faster (latencies in seconds) towards rewarded cues, and slowest towards unrewarded cues. Mean ± standard error are presented.

Page 24: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

22

Table 3. Personality scores describing variation among red junglefowl chicks. The personality gradients are obtained from a principal component analysis (PCA) based on data from Novel arena (NA)-, Novel object (NO)- and Tonic immobility (TI) tests. Bold numbers indicate factors loading over 0.3, which are considered having a strong loading in the PC.

Personality gradients

Active Fearful Escape

prone Nervous

NA activity 0.37 -0.02 0.08 -0.28

NA vigilance 0.15 0.42 -0.41 0.33

NA latency to move -0.21 -0.18 -0.27 0.29

NA latency to visit all areas -0.29 0.22 0.10 0.21

NA number of escape attempts 0.22 0.09 0.52 0.49

NA number of square changes 0.38 0.08 0.12 -0.34

NO activity 0.39 -0.01 -0.22 -0.08

NO vigilance 0.26 0.25 -0.41 0.32

NO number of escape attempts 0.29 -0.09 0.41 0.41

NO number of square changes 0.39 0.06 -0.09 -0.05

TI latency to move head -0.21 0.45 0.21 -0.13

TI latency to jump up -0.07 0.68 0.14 -0.20

Eigen value 2.17 1.27 1.15 1.06

Proportion of variance 39.1 % 13.5 % 11.0 % 9.4 %

Accumulative proportion of

variance 39.1 % 52.6 % 63.6 % 73.0 %

Page 25: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

23

Table 4. ANOVA output from comparison between personality principle component analysis (PCA) and data from a cognitive judgment bias test in Red Junglefowl chicks using a linear mixed effects model (LMM). The components compared are the four PCs (active, fearful, escape prone and nervous, see table 3)

Variable χ² Df p-value

Sex 0.49 1 0.48

Active 0.0007 1 0.98

Fearful 1.10 1 0.30

Escape prone 0.10 1 0.75

Nervous 2.01 1 0.16

Active*Cue 2.04 4 0.73

Fearful*Cue 1.13 4 0.89

Escape prone*Cue 7.58 4 0.11

Nervous*Cue 14.50 4 0.006**

Active*Sex 0.62 1 0.43

Fearful*sex 0.69 1 0.41

Escape prone*sex 1.10 1 0.29

Nervous*sex 0.79 1 0.37

** p < 0.01

Page 26: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

24

Table 5. Results from linear models (LM) per each cue used in a cognitive judgment bias test compared to ‘Nervousness’ (see table 3) observed in Red Junglefowl chicks.

Cue F-value Df p-value

Rewarded 0.01 1 0.90

Near rewarded 0.71 1 0.41

Middle 0.73 1 0.40

Near unrewarded 4.95 1 0.03*

Unrewarded 7.95 1 0.007**

* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01

Figure 10. Relations between ‘nervousness’ (see table 3) and latency to reach two different cues in a cognitive judgment bias test in Red Junglefowl chicks. ‘Nervousness’ correlated with latency to reach a) the near unrewarded cue (r = 0.34, p = 0.03) and b) the unrewarded cue (r = 0.21, p = 0.007).

5 Discussion

I have here demonstrated that chicks housed in enriched environments

were faster to reach ambiguous cues in a cognitive judgment bias test

performed after a battery of collective stressors, in comparison to chicks

Page 27: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

25

housed in non-enriched environments. This suggests that living in

enriched environments buffers the influences of unpredictable stressors

and keeps chicks more optimistic. When exploring effects of personality

on cognitive judgment bias, I demonstrated a negative correlation

between nervousness and optimism. This indicates that the more nervous

a chick is, the more negatively it interprets its surroundings. However, the

influence of personality on optimism was limited.

5.1 Influence of environmental factors on cognitive judgment bias

Results from the cognitive judgment bias tests showed that chicks housed

in enriched environments are more optimistic in comparison with chicks

living in non-enriched environments after being subjected to

unpredictable stressors. Thus it is possible that enrichment buffers stress,

indicating that providing enrichment to chicks supports better welfare and

helps them to cope with unpredictable stressors, should they happen to

occur.

The finding that unpredictable environments, such as the one created with

the collective stressors, induce a more pessimistic judgment bias,

supports previous studies which have found a similar pattern (Harding et

al. 2004, Reefmann et al. 2012). However, here I also showed that

environmental enrichment can buffer the negative influence on cognitive

judgment bias since chicks living in enriched environments showed no

altered response from the first cognitive judgment bias tests to the

second. When physiological and behavioural responses to stress

previously have been investigated, environmental enrichment has been

found to reverse the effects of stress (e.g. Francis et al. 2002, Laviola et

al. 2004, Del Arco et al. 2007). Nevertheless, it has previously not been

investigated how environmental enrichment affects stress responses by

investigating the animals’ subjective experience, as delineated in this

thesis.

The collective stressors, which consisted of unpredictable sound and light

schedules, led to more pessimistic responses in the cognitive judgment

bias test from the chicks that lived in non-enriched environments.

Latencies to reach the cues differed between chicks living in enriched and

non-enriched environments in a way that resembled previous findings,

which linked pessimistic responses towards both unrewarded and

rewarded cues to depression in rats (Enkel et al. 2010, Richter et al.

2012). What these studies found were that rats bred to model human

depression exhibited reduced anticipation of positive events in addition to

an increased anticipation of negative events (Enkel et al. 2010, Richter et

al. 2012). As chicks living in non-enriched environments increased their

Page 28: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

26

latencies to reach both the rewarded, unrewarded and ambiguous cues,

they could have been induced into a depressed state. This would be bad

for their welfare, as the cognitive judgment bias test indicates.

No effects of initial stress or environmental enrichment were seen in the

first cognitive judgment bias test. It is possible that the initial stressor was

less stressful in comparison to the collective, unpredictable stressors.

Further it is possible that the initial stressor did not alter the behaviours of

the chicks, whereas the other stressors did because of their

unpredictability, which have been seen to have an effect as stated above.

However, this does not explain the lack of effect of environmental

enrichment in the cognitive judgment bias test before the collective

stressors. It is possible that the environment does not have a large effect

on cognitive judgment bias until other factors, such as the unpredictable

stress in this case, are applied. This is a possibility that should be

investigated in future studies.

My results demonstrated that providing enrichment to chicks made them

more optimistic when unpredictable stressors occurred. This in turn

provides support that environment influences cognitive judgment bias,

and thus optimism, for chicks.

5.2 Linking variation in personality to cognitive judgment bias

When investigating the link between variation in personality and

optimism, I showed that personality has a significant effect on

interpretation of stimuli in a cognitive judgment bias test. However, this

effect was rather small. Other studies have also found personality and

cognition to be related, for example in the form of learning (e.g. Dugatkin

& Alfieri 2003). In psittacines, more neurotic animals had a more

negative attention bias (Cussen & Mench, 2014), showing similarities

with my findings of nervousness being positively correlated with

pessimism.

There is a possibility that the tests used to investigate personalities in the

chicks did not represent the full range of their personalities, and that some

other aspect of personality would better explain variation in optimism.

However, the novel arena-, novel object-, and tonic immobility tests

performed here were chosen in order to cover a variety of situations in

which personality differences have previously been found. Novel arena-

and novel object tests are methods of assessing behavioural differences in

a variety of species (e.g. mice (Hood & Quigley 2008), pigs (Janczak et

al. 2003), rabbits (e.g. Andersson et al. 2014) and cattle (MacKay et al.

2014). Also the tonic immobility test is a test that is used in different

types of animals, including chickens (Jones 1984), muroid rodents

Page 29: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

27

(Webster et al. 1981) and pigs (e.g. Janczak et al. 2003). The three

personality assays used have also been reviewed and deemed fit for

testing fear response (Forkman et al. 2007).

However, because personality only had a limited influence of

performance in a cognitive judgment bias test after environmental

influences have been minimised, it is still unclear what causes the

observed individual differences in cognitive judgment bias tests. It is

possible that observed individual differences arise from variations in

other cognitive biases such as attention, and memory bias. Further, the

observed variation can be dependent on variation in genetics, physical

status, social status or other individual differences. Future studies should

investigate the contributions to variation in responses to cognitive

judgement bias tests further.

5.3 Societal and ethical considerations

Investigations of welfare in the common laying hen, which was done in

the first experiment in this thesis, is an important part of egg production

since it is important to be able to house the laying hens in a suitable way.

Although it is mostly important from an ethical point of view to have

animals with good welfare producing the food we eat it has also been

seen that animals with better welfare produce higher quality and quantity

of animal products (e.g. Castellini & A Dal Bosco 2002, Pascual-Alonso

et al. 2015). Consumers have, in addition, become more and more

concerned about the welfare of the animals that produce their food

(Blokhuis et al. 2003) in particular in pork and poultry production

(Verbeke & Viaene 2000). This means that products produced using

practices with better welfare aspects will probably sell better than

products where the welfare aspect has not been taken into consideration.

Research regarding personality influence on cognitive judgment bias is

important in trying to understand how the human mind works. Although

bird brains are different to mammalian brains, homologues of several

areas exist, such as the mammalian neocortex being homologous to the

dorsal ventricular ridge in birds (Dugas-Ford et al. 2013). Furthermore, it

is of interest to explore species resembling those of humans to further our

understanding of how our minds work more in depth without having to

perform tests in humans. This is especially prevalent since more invasive

studies might be of interest to perform and since animal environments are

more possible to control and affect. Invasive studies and studies done

when the study subjects are housed in a controlled environment are not

possible o perform in humans, whereby an animal model is of great use in

Page 30: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

28

future studies. By using animals to understand our mind we can get

deeper into functions that are thus far unexplored.

6 Conclusions

In this study, I first showed that environmental enrichments could buffer

the negative influences unpredictable stressors can have on the cognitive

judgment bias in female Bovans Robust chicks. In turn, this supports that

environmental enrichments can buffer negative moods and thereby poor

welfare in these animals. No effects of initial environmental enrichment

nor early cold stress were detected in the animals. Perhaps the lack of

effect indicates that environmental enrichments are most important when

other factors, such as stressors, are prevalent. Furthermore the lack of

effect from the initial stressor might indicate that cold stress was easier to

recover from than the large stressor of unpredictable stimuli.

In the second test, I showed that personality has a limited influence on

individual cognitive judgment bias in Red Junglefowl chicks. This

influence consists of individuals scoring higher in nervousness also

portraying more pessimistic interpretations of unrewarded stimuli. With

this influence being small and individual differences being observed in

cognitive judgment bias in previous studies, there question remains on

where these differences originate from. This question is interesting to

investigate in future studies, along with questions of the underlying

physiological mechanism explaining variation in cognitive judgment

biases.

7 Acknowledgements

I would like to thank my supervisors Hanne Løvlie and Josefina Zidar for

all feedback and support. I would also like to thank Alexandra Balogh,

Anna Favati and Johan Almberg for help collecting data in Linköping. A

special thank you goes to Linda Keeling and Irene Campderrich

Lecumberri for inviting 'the Linköping chick crew' to take part in their

study. Furthermore I would like to thank Anette Wichman, Helena Jones,

Rebecca Katajamaa and Sonja Broberg for their help with data collection

in Uppsala. Lastly I thank the people helping me finishing the report,

consisting of Charlotte Rosher, Mikaela Hanson and Johan Jönsson, as

well as my examiner Jordi Altimiras.

Page 31: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

29

8 References

Andersson A, Laikre L & Bergvall UA. 2014. Two shades of boldness:

novel object and anti-predator behavior reflect different personality

dimensions in domestic rabbits. Journal of Ethology. 32, 123-136.

Bateson M, Desire S, Gartside AE & Wright GA. 2011. Agitated

honeybees exhibit pessimistic cognitive biases. Current Biology. 12,

1070-1073.

Bateson M & Matheson SM. 2007. Performance on a categorisation task

suggests that removal of environmental enrichment induces

‘pessimism’ in captive European starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Animal

Welfare. 16, 33-36.

Bateson M & Kacelnik A. 1995. Preferences for fixed and variables food

sources–variability in amount and delay. Journal of the Experimental

Analysis of Behavior. 63, 313-329

Blokhuis HJ, Jones RB, Geers R, Miele M & Veissier I. 2003. Measuring

and monitoring animal welfare: Transperacy in the food product

quality chain. Animal Welfare. 12, 445-455.

Boleij H, van't Klooster J, Lavrijsen M, Kirchhoff S, Arndt SS & Ohl F.

2012. A test to identify judgment bias in mice. Behavioural Brain

Research. 233, 45-54.

Brilot BO, Asher L & Bateson M. 2010. Stereotyping starlings are more

‘pessimistic’. Animal Cognition. 13, 721-731.

Broom DM. 1986. Indicators of poor welfare. British Veterinary Journal.

142, 524-526.

Broom DM. 1991. Animal welfare: concepts and measurements. Journal

of Animal Science. 69, 4167-4175.

Castellini C, Mugnai C & Dal Bosco A. 2002. Effect of organic

production system on broiler carcass and meat quality. Meat Science.

60, 219-225.

Cussen VA & Mench JA. 2014. Personality predicts cognitive bias in

captive psittacines, Amazona amazonica. Animal Behaviour. 89, 123-

130.

Page 32: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

30

Dall SRX, Houston AI & McNmara JM. 2004. The behavioural ecology

of personality: consistent individual differences from an adaptive

perspective. Ecology Letters. 7, 734-739.

Del Arco A, Segovia G, Garrido P, de Blas M & Mora Francisco. 2007.

Stress, prefrontal cortex and environmental enrichment: Studies on

dopamine and acetylcholine release and working memory

performance in rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 176, 267-273.

Digman JM. 1990. Personality structure: emergence of the five-factor

model. Annual Review of Psychology. 41, 417-440.

Dixona LM. 2008. Feather pecking behaviour and associated welfare

issues in laying hens. Avian Biology Research. 1, 73-87.

Dougas-Ford J, Rowell JJ & Ragsdale CW. 2012. Cell-type homologies

and the origins of the neocortex. Proceedings of the National

Academy of Sciences. 109, 16974-16979.

Dugatkin LA & Alfieri MS. 2003. Boldness, behavioural inhibition and

learning. Ethology Ecology and Evolution. 15, 43-49.

Duncan IJH. 1996. Animal welfare defined in terms of feelings. Acta

Agriculturae Scandinavica Section A, Animal Science. Supplement

27: 29-35

Dweck CS & Leggett EL. 1988. A social-cognitive approach to

motivation and personality. Psychological Review. 95, 256-273.

Enkel T, Gholizadeh D, von Bohlen und Halbach O, Sanchis-Segura C,

Hurlemann R, Spanagel R, Gass P & Vollmayr B. 2010. Ambiguous-

cue interpretation is biased under stress- and depression-like states in

rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 35, 1008-1015.

Eysenck MW, Mogg K, May J, Richards A & Mathews A. 1991. Bias in

interpretation of ambiguous sentences related to threat in anxiety.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 100, 144-150.

Favati A, Leimar O & Løvlie H. 2014a. Personality predicts social

dominance in male domestic fowl. PLoS ONE, 9, e103535.

Favati A, Leimar O, Radesäter T & Løvlie H. 2014b. Social status and

personality: stability in social state can promote consistency of

behavioural responses. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B.

281, 20132531.

Page 33: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

31

Forkman B, BoissyA, Meunier-Salaün M-C, Canali E & Jones RB. 2007.

A critical review of fear tests used on cattle, pigs, sheep, poultry and

horses. Physiology & Behavior. 92, 340-374.

Francis DD, Diorio J, Plotsky PM & Meaney MJ. 2002. Environmental

enrichment reverses the effects of maternal separation on stress

reactivity. The Journal of Neuroscience. 22, 7840-7843.

Fraser D, Weary DM, Pajor EA & Milligan BN. 1997. A scientific

conception of animal welfare that reflects ethical concerns. Animal

Welfare. 6, 187-205.

Gosling SD. 2001. From mice to men: what can we learn about

personality from animal research? Psychological Bulletin. 1, 45-86.

Gosling SD & John OP. 1999. Personality dimensions in nonhuman

animals: A cross species review. Current Directions in Psychological

Science. 8, 69-75.

Harding EJ, Paul ES & Mendl, M. 2004. Cognitive bias and affective

state. Nature. 427, 312.

Herrmann E, Call J, Hernández-Lloreda MV, Hare B & Tomasello M.

2007. Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition:

The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science. 317, 1360-1366.

Hewson CJ. 2003. What is animal welfare? Common definitions and their

practical consequences. The Canadian Veterinary Journal. 44, 496-

499.

Hood KE & Quigley KS. 2008. Exploratory behavior in mice selectively

bred for developmental differences in aggressive behavior.

Developmental Psychobiology. 50, 32-47. DOI: 10.1002/dev.20264.

Janczak AM, Pedersen LJ & Bakken M. 2003. Aggression, fearfulness

and coping styles in female pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science.

81, 13-28.

Jones RB. 1984. Experimental novelty and tonic immobility in chickens

(Gallus domesticus). Behavioural Processes. 9, 255-260.

von Keyserlingk MAG, Rushen J, de Passilé AM & Weary DM. 2009.

The welfare of dairy cattle- Key concepts and the role of science.

Journal of Dairy Science. 92, 4101-4111.

Page 34: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

32

Koolhaas JM, Korte SM, De Boer SF, van Der Vegt BJ, van Reenen H,

Hopster CG, De Jong IC, Ruis MAW & Blokhuis HJ. 1999. Coping

styles in animals: Current status in behavior and stress-physiology.

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 23, 925-935.

Koolhaas JM, de Boer SF, Coppens CM & Buwalda B. 2010.

Neuroendocrinology of coping styles: Towards understanding the

biology of individual variation. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology. 31,

307-321.

Lachman SJ. 1997. Learning is a process: Toward an improved definition

of learning. The Journal of Psychology. 131, 477-480.

Lassen J, Sandøe P & Forkman B. 2006. Happy pigs are dirty! -

Conflicting perspectives on animal welfare. Livestock Science. 103,

221-230.

Laviola G, Rea M, Morley-Fletcher S, Di Carlo S, Bacosi A, De Simone

R, Bertini M & Pacifici R. 2004. Beneficial effects of enriched

environment on adolescent rats from stressed pregnancies. European

Journal of Neuroscience. 20, 1655-1664.

MacKay JRD, Haskell MJ, Deag JM & van Reenen K. 2014. Fear

responses to novelty in testing environments are related to day-to-day

activity in the home environment in dairy cattle. Applied Animal

Behaviour Science. 152, 7-16.

Mason GJ & Latham NR. 2004. Can't stop, won't stop: Is stereotypy a

reliable animal welfare indicator? Animal Welfare. 13, 57-69.

Matheson S, Asher L & Bateson M. 2008. Larger, enriched cages are

associated with "optimistic" response biases in captive European

starlings (Sturnus vulgaris). Applied animal behaviour science. 109,

374-383.

Mathews A & MacLeod C. 1985. Selective processing of threat cues in

anxiety states. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 23, 563-369.

Mathews A, Richards A & Eysenck M. 1989. Interpretation of

homophones related to threat in anxiety states. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology. 98, 31-34.

Mendl M, Burman OHP & Paul ES. 2010. An integrative functional

framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings of

the Royal Society of London B. 277, 2895-2904.

Page 35: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

33

Mendl M, Burman OHP, Parker RMA & Paul ES. 2009. Cognitive bias

as an indicator of animal emotion and welfare: Emerging evidence

and underlying mechanisms. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 118,

161-181.

Neave HW, Daros RR, Costa JHC, von Keyserlingk MAG & Weary DM.

2013. Pain and pessimism: Dairy calves exhibit negative judgment

bias following hot-iron disbudding. PLoS ONE. 8, e80556.

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0080556

Pascual-Alonso M, Miranda-de la Lama GC, Aguayo-Ulloa L, Ezquerro

L, Villarroel M, Marín RH & Maria GA. 2015. Effect of postweaning

handling strategies on welfare and productive traits in lambs. Journal

of Applied Animal Welfare Science. 18, 42-56.

Pearce JM. 2008. Animal learning and cognition [3rd edition].

Psychology Press. New York.

Réale D, Reader SM, Sol D, McDougall PT & Dingemanse NJ. 2007.

Integrating animal temperament within ecology and evolution.

Biological Reviews. 82, 291-318.

Reefmann N, Muehlemann T, Wechsler B & Gygax L. 2012. Housing

induced mood modulates reactions to emotional stimuli in sheep.

Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 136, 146-155.

Richter SH, Schick A, Hoyer C, Lankisch K, Gass P & Vollmayr B.

2012. A glass full of optimism: Enrichment effects on sognitive bias

in a rat model of depression. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioural

Neuroscience. 12, 527-542.

Russell JA. 2003. Core affect and the psychological construction of

emotion. Psychological Review. 110, 145-172.

Salmeto AL, Hymel KA, Carpenter EC, Brilot BO, Bateson M & Sufka

KJ. 2011. Cognitive bias in the chick anxiety-depression model.

Brain research. 1373, 124-130.

Schmitt V, Pankau B & Fischer J. 2012. Old world monkeys compare to

apes in the primate cognition test battery. PLoS ONE. 7, e32024.

Seehuus B, Mendl M, Keeling L & Blokhuis H. 2013. Disrupting

motivational sequences in chicks: are there affective consequences?.

Applied animal behaviour science. 148, 85-92.

Page 36: Influences of environment and personality on cognitive ...882864/FULLTEXT01.pdf · environmental factors and personality influence cognitive judgment bias in Gallus gallus chicks

34

Selye H. 1973. The Evolution of the Stress Concept: The originator of the

concept traces its development from the discovery in 1936 of the

alarm reaction to modern therapeutic applications of syntoxic and

catatoxic hormones. American Scientist. 61, 692-699.

Shettleworth SJ. 2010. Cognition, evolution, and behavior [2nd edition].

Oxford University Press. New York.

Shettleworth SJ. 2013. Fundamentals of comparative cognition. Oxford

University Press. New York.

Sutherland MA, Bryer PJ, Krebs N & McGlone JJ. 2009. The effect of

method of tail docking on tail-biting behaviour and welfare of pigs.

Animal Welfare. 18, 561-570.

Verbeke WAJ & Viaene J. 2000. Ethical challenges for livestock

production: Meeting consumer concerns about meat safety and animal

welfare. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics. 12, 141-

151.

Webster DG, Lanthorn TH, Dewsbury DA & Meyer ME. 1981. Tonic

immobility and the dorsal immobility response in twelve species of

muroid rodents. Behavioral and Neural Biology. 31, 32-41.

Wichman A, Keeling LJ & Forkman B. 2012. Cognitive bias and

anticipatory behaviour of laying hens housed in basic and enriched

pens. Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 140, 62-69.