advances in cognitive-social- personality theory: applications to … · advances in...

24
ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY KEY WORDS: Personality theory, Cognitive-Affective Processing System (CAPS), Coaching behaviors, Achievement goals, Motivational climate, Anxiety. ABSTRACT: Many theories and intervention techniques in sport psychology have a cognitive-behavioral emphasis, and sport psychologists have long been interested in individual differences. Recent developments in cognitive social personality theory offer new opportunities for understanding sport behavior. The finding of stable individual differences in situation- behavior relations has helped resolve the person-situation debate of past years, and idiographically-distinct behavioral signatures have now been demonstrated for coaching behaviors across differing game situations. Moreover, coaching behaviors are differentially related to athletes’ liking for the coach, depending on whether they occur during winning or losing game situations. Mischel and Shoda’s (1995) Cognitive-Affective Processing System offers a new template within which to study sport psychology constructs, such as achievement goal orientations and anxiety. Just as social cognitive theory can inform research, theory development, and interventions in sport psychology, research in sport settings can advance the future development of cognitive social personality theory. Correspondencia: Ronald E. Smith. Department of Psychology. University of Washington. Seattle, Washington, U.S.A. * This article is based on addresses at the Symposium Internacional de Actividad Física: Motivación y Dinámica de Equipos, Palma de Mallorca, May 2006 and to Col·legi Oficial de Psicolegs de les Illes Balears, Palma, Mallorca, May, 2008. Preparation of this article and the research reported herein were supported by Grant # 1523 from the William T. Grant Foundation — Fecha de recepción: 11 de Agosto de 2008. Fecha de aceptación: 27 de Octubre 2008. Universitat de les Illes Balears Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona Revista de Psicología del Deporte 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2 pp. 253-276 ISSN: 1132-239X

Upload: others

Post on 16-May-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY:

APPLICATIONS TO SPORTPSYCHOLOGY

Ronald E. Smith

ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY

KEY WORDS: Personality theory, Cognitive-Affective Processing System (CAPS), Coaching behaviors, Achievement goals,Motivational climate, Anxiety.ABSTRACT: Many theories and intervention techniques in sport psychology have a cognitive-behavioral emphasis, andsport psychologists have long been interested in individual differences. Recent developments in cognitive social personalitytheory offer new opportunities for understanding sport behavior. The finding of stable individual differences in situation-behavior relations has helped resolve the person-situation debate of past years, and idiographically-distinct behavioralsignatures have now been demonstrated for coaching behaviors across differing game situations. Moreover, coachingbehaviors are differentially related to athletes’ liking for the coach, depending on whether they occur during winning orlosing game situations. Mischel and Shoda’s (1995) Cognitive-Affective Processing System offers a new template withinwhich to study sport psychology constructs, such as achievement goal orientations and anxiety. Just as social cognitivetheory can inform research, theory development, and interventions in sport psychology, research in sport settings canadvance the future development of cognitive social personality theory.

Correspondencia: Ronald E. Smith. Department of Psychology. University of Washington. Seattle, Washington,U.S.A.

* This article is based on addresses at the Symposium Internacional de Actividad Física: Motivación y Dinámica de Equipos,Palma de Mallorca, May 2006 and to Col·legi Oficial de Psicolegs de les Illes Balears, Palma, Mallorca, May, 2008. Preparationof this article and the research reported herein were supported by Grant # 1523 from the William T. Grant Foundation

— Fecha de recepción: 11 de Agosto de 2008. Fecha de aceptación: 27 de Octubre 2008.

Universitat de les Illes BalearsUniversitat Autònoma de Barcelona

Revista de Psicología del Deporte2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2 pp. 253-276ISSN: 1132-239X

Page 2: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276 254

As a construct, personality arises from thefascinating spectrum of human individuality.We observe that people differ meaningfullyin the ways they customarily think, feel, andact. These distinctive behavior patterns helpdefine one’s identity as a person. The conceptof personality also rests on the observationthat a given person seems to behave some-what consistently over time and acrossdifferent situations. From this perceivedtemporal and situational consistency comesthe notion of personality traits thatcharacterize individuals’ customary ways ofresponding to their world. Although manydefinitions of personality have beenadvanced, virtually all share the coreassumption that personality exhibitscontinuity, stability, and coherence, i.e., thatit is organized in some fashion and serves as amajor internal determinant of behavior.

The Personality ParadoxOur intuition and personal observations

tell us that the coherence of personality isexpressed as some degree of consistency inbehavior across many different situations.However, when Walter Mischel (1968)reviewed the evidence, he came to a sur-prising conclusion: There was more evidencefor inconsistency than for consistency. Evenon a trait so central as honesty, people canshow considerable behavioral variabilityacross situations. In a classic study done inthe 1920s by Hartshorne and May (1928)thousands of children were given oppor-tunities to lie, steal, and cheat in a number ofdifferent settings: at home, in school, at aparty, and in an athletic contest. The rathersurprising finding was that “lying, cheatingand stealing as measured by the testsituations in this study are only very looselyrelated. . . . Most children will deceive incertain situations but not in others” (p. 411).

At about the same time, Newcomb (1929)found striking inconsistency of behavior in astudy of college students’ introversion-extraversion behaviors across an array ofsituations, and Mischel and Peake (1982)later reported similar findings for collegestudents on the trait of conscientiousness. Astudent might be highly conscientious in onesituation (e.g., coming to work on time)without being conscientious in another (e.g.,turning in class assignments on time). Manyother studies revealed similar behavioralinconsistency across different types ofsituations, and correlations between traitmeasures and behavior that rarely exceeded.30 (Pervin, 1994). Critics of personalityreferred to this modest .30 ceiling as the“personality coefficient.”

To some, Mischel’s (1968) conclusionthat the common assumption of consistencyin thought, affect, and behavior acrosssituations lacked empirical support called thevery concept of personality into question,and it evoked a bitter controversy that hasraged for nearly 40 years. One aspect was thecelebrated person versus situation debate,with some maintaining that the situation isthe prepotent influence on behavior and thatthe concept of personality is not neededbecause it accounts for such modest amountsof behavioral variance (Ross and Nisbett,1991). They reasoned that if personalitydifferences account for less than 10 percentof the variance in behavior (derived bysquaring the .30 personality coefficient), thensituational forces must account for the other91 percent. However, other evidence showedthat the situation did not account for morevariance than did individual differencevariables, even under controlled laboratoryconditions (Sarason, Smith and Diener,1975), and more recent evidence confirmsthe position that both personality variables

Page 3: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

255Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

and situations account for similar andnotable amounts of variance (Fleeson, 2004;Fournier, Moskowitz, and Zuroff, 2008).

Others argued that traits refer to averageamounts of behavior across differingsituations and that no trait theorist wouldhold that people should behave consistentlyin every situation. Their approach was toaggregate behavioral measures acrosssituations, deriving a mean behavior scoreand thereby achieving much highercorrelations with personality trait measures(e. g., Epstein, 1979). Though useful for pre-dictive purposes, aggregation basically ignoresthe issue of non-consistency, treatingsituational variability as error variance andfailing to account for the reasons for thevariability in behavior.

A third response to Mischel’s critique wasinteractionism, in which behavior was viewedas a function of an interacting person and

situation (Lewin, 1935; Magnusson andEndler, 1977). This approach had the meritof taking both the person and the situationinto account. In factorial designs involvingpersonality variables, situational factors, andtheir interaction, interactionists strengthenedtheir case by showing that the interactioneffect often accounted for more behavioralvariance than did either the person orsituation main effects. In sport research, anexample of person x situation interactionismcomes from a study by Smith and Smoll(1990). The situational variable in this studywas the behavior of coaches as codedobservationally during youth baseball gamesusing the Coaching Behavior AssessmentSystem (CBAS; Smith, Smoll and Hunt,1977). Factor analysis of the 12 behavioralcategories revealed a factor called Suppor-tiveness, on which positive reinforcement andencouragement following mistakes loaded

Figure 1. Athletes’ mean postseason evaluation of coaches who were either high (+1 SD) or low (-1 SD)in scores on the CBAS Supportiveness (Sup) factor. (Data from Smith and Smoll, 1990).

Page 4: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

256 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

very highly. Coaches were selected whosefactor scores on Supportiveness were onestandard deviation above and below themean. The personality variable was athletes’level of global self-esteem, divided into low,moderate, and high levels. The dependentvariable was postseason ratings of how muchthe athletes liked playing for the coach andwished to play for him in the future. As seenin Figure 1, significant main effects for likingwere found for both coach supportivenessand children’s self-esteem level. However, themost interesting result was the supportivenessx self-esteem interaction, which showed thatchildren low in self-esteem were especiallyresponsive to variations in coach suppor-tiveness, and they evaluated non-supportivecoaches very negatively. Self-esteem thusserved as a moderator variable that influencedthe relation between supportiveness andliking. This result was consistent with thecommon assumption that low self-esteemchildren are especially in need of self-enhancing sport experiences and are thereforemost strongly affected by their relationshipwith their coach, and especially their coach’ssupportive behaviors.

Still, even interactionism failed to providea totally satisfactory answer to what Bem andAllen (1974) dubbed the “personalityparadox”: How can we have a coherent andstable personality, yet show such incon-sistency in cognitive, affective, and overtbehavior across different situations?

Resolving the Personality ParadoxRecent advances in cognitive social

personality theory (formerly called cognitivesocial learning theory) have provided ananswer to the personality paradox (Mischeland Shoda, 1995, 1998, 1999; Shoda andMischel, 2000). A key finding is that whilepeople show considerable variability in the

same behavior across situations, they tend toshow high consistency in those behaviorswithin classes of situations that are similar toone another. In a study by Shoda, Mischeland Wright (1994), for example, childrenwere intensively observed within a residentialsummer camp over a 6-week period, and avariety of specific behaviors were coded,including verbal aggression. Idiographicanalyses of the children’s responses providedevidence for stable and consistent situation-behavior profiles across 5 different and well-defined classes of situations (teased by anotherchild, approached by another child, praised byan adult, warned by an adult, or punished byan adult). The children differed not only intheir total number of aggressive responses, asan aggression trait model would predict, butalso in the situations in which the behaviorsoccurred. However, this situational variationwas not random; it was well-structured formost children, and their situation-behaviorprofiles were often highly consistent overtime. Shoda et al. concluded that as peopleconfront certain classes of situations, theyexhibit distinctive behavioral signatures thatare the outward manifestation of personalityand that establish a person’s unique identity.Figure 2 shows a hypothetical behavior xsituation behavioral signature for two people.Although the mean level of the behavior isequal when aggregated across the threesituations, the situational patterning is verydifferent. However, this intraindividualpatterning, which provides key informationabout the person, is lost when behavior isdecontextualized through aggregation.

Do behavioral signatures occur in sportsas well? To find out, we analyzed datacollected from 13 youth baseball coaches over631 half-innings (at bat or in the field) of 53games (Smith, Shoda, Cumming, and Smoll,in press). Behaviors were coded using the

Page 5: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

257Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

Coaching Behavior Assessment System(Smith, Smoll and Hunt, 1977). Observersrecorded the time and score at the beginning

and end of each half-inning. Data wereexpressed as rates of behavior per minute ofobservation.

Figure 2. Hypothetical behavioral signatures of two persons whose behavioral means would be nearlyidentical if aggregated across three situations, but whose patterning of behavior differs markedly acrossthe situations.

Focusing on supportive and instructionalbehaviors, which constituted nearly 75% ofall coded behaviors, we plotted the rate ofthese two classes of behaviors over threepsychologically-salient game situations:leading in score by 2 or more runs at the endof the half inning, tied or within one run ofthe opponent, or losing by 2 or more runs toproduce behavioral signature profiles. Foreach of the two classes of behavior and threetypes of game situations (i. e., 6 situation-behavior combinations), we standardized the

rate scores separately in each type of situation(i. e., winning, losing, or close/tied). Thisprocedure removed the nomothetic in-fluences of the game situations on thecoaches’ behaviors, thereby revealing eachcoach’s idiographic pattern of situation-behavior relations. The z-score for each coachthus represents the rate at which that coachengaged in a specific type of behavior in aspecific situation relative to all of the coachesin the sample. We then randomly divided allof the observed half-innings into two sets so

Page 6: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

258 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

that we could not only determine if coachesdiffered in their behavior profiles within thethree game situations, but also could assesshow consistent or stable these profiles wereby correlating the two sets of situation-behavior data across Sets 1 and 2

The results provided strong support forthe existence of coaching behavioralsignatures. Coaches exhibited considerablevariability in their situation-behavior profiles,even when their overall rates of supportiveand instructional behaviors across the threegame situations were quite similar, and thebehavior x situation stability coefficients foreach coach revealed considerable evidence forstability. Nine of the 13 coaches werecharacterized by positive stability coefficientsfor both behaviors across the three gamesituations, and 20 of the 26 stabilitycoefficients equaled or exceeded +.30. Themean of these 20 coefficients was +.70.Behavioral signatures thus constitute a newway of conceptualizing and analyzingcoaching patterns, and it will be possible infuture research to relate them to othervariables, such as the athletes’ attitudestoward the coach, motivational andemotional outcomes, and dropout.

The study of behavioral signatures revealsthat there is indeed coherence andconsistency in behavior. This consistencyconsists, however, not across situations ingeneral, but across certain classes of situationsthat have similar psychological meaning, or“active ingredients” for the individual. Thebehavioral if….then… relations found inbehavioral signatures reflect the coherence ofthe underlying personality. It remains,however, to specify the underlying processesand dynamics that are involved in thiscoherence. Cognitive social theory attemptsto account for the internal level of coherencethrough a dynamic network of cognitive and

affective processes that process situationalcues and generate output behaviors,including behavioral signatures.

The Cognitive-Affective Processing System(CAPS)

Cognitive social theorists’attempts toresolve the personality paradox and accountfor behavioral signatures led to a search for anew conceptual model that could accountnot only for individual differences in themean or “average” levels of behavior acrosssituations that are the focus of trait con-ceptions, but also for the distinctive andunique ways that a person’s behavior canchange across situations. Such a model wouldnecessarily incorporate both situational anddispositional factors, but in a manner thatbuilt upon the traditional person-by-situationinteractional approach. Because of itscognitive emphasis, it would move beyondnominal situational factors (i. e., physical orsocial features) to their psychological ingre-dients as encoded or construed by the person.Likewise, dispositional variables would gobeyond static trait measures to specifycognitive-affective processes that becomeactivated by situational elements, interactwith and influence one another in a systemicand stable manner, and generate outputbehaviors.

The model began to take shape with atheoretical article by Mischel (1973) thatclosely followed his 1968 critique of theliterature. Mischel proposed as an alternativeto broad personality traits an approach thatfocused on psychological constructs that areknown to have causal influences on behavior,and suggested that what we call personalityreflects a coherent organization of thesemechanisms that differs from individual toindividual. Over the next 20 years, Mischel’soriginal model evolved into a Cognitive-

Page 7: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

259Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

Affective Processing System (CAPS) model(Mischel and Shoda, 1995; Shoda andMischel, 1998). This evolution was spurredby the development of informationprocessing, connectionist, and neuralnetwork models in areas such as perception,social cognition, and cognitive neuroscience(Read and Miller, 1998; Rumelhart andMcClelland, 1986). Connectionist modelsfocus on organized networks of cognitive-affective processing units (such as neurons)whose interconnections form a uniquenetwork. This network functions as anorganized whole and its units are activated bythe specific features of the stimuli that arebeing processed. Individuals differ from oneanother in the specifics of the units and inthe chronic accessibility of network elements,that is, the ease with which the particularcognitive-affective units become activated(Higgins, 1990). They also differ in the levelsof activation that occur in response to (a)

elements of the “psychological situation” thatis being processed and (b) the activity ofother associated units, which can stimulate,inhibit, or exert no influence on the unit.The dynamic interactions among the unitsthus mediate relations between situations andbehaviors in a manner that can be quite dis-tinctive for different individuals.

Building on processing dynamics modelsand on an earlier specification by Mischel(1973) of five “person factors” that might beof particular significance in understandingindividual differences, Mischel and Shoda(1995) advanced a new five-componentmodel that specified the major classes ofprocessing and behavior-generation units.This organized system of cognitive-affectiveunits, briefly described in Table 1, interactscontinuously with the social world in whichit functions, generating the person’sdistinctive patterns of behavior, or behavioralsignatures.

1. Encodings and personal constructs. Cognitive categories for the self, people, events, andsituations into which internal and external stimuli are sorted.

2. Beliefs and expectancies. Includes the person’s belief system as well as stimulus-outcome,response-outcome, self-efficacy, and locus of control expectancies.

3. Affects. Emotional responEncodings and personal constructs. Cognitive categories for theself, people, events, and situations into which internal and external stimuli are sorted.

4. Goals and values. Short- and long-term desired and undesired outcomes; valuesconcerning what is significant, moral, and good.

5. Skills and self-regulatory competencies. Include physical and mental competencies, self-standards and self-reinforcement processes, plans and strategies for attaining goals; abilitiesto exert internal control over cognitions, affect, and behavior.

From Smith, 2006, p. 6. Reprinted with permission

Table 1. Component Variables in the Cognitive-Affective Processing System (CAPS).

Page 8: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

A schematic representation of the CAPSmodel is shown in Figure 3. The cognitive-affective components are represented by theinterconnections shown within the circle.The encoding units respond to specificaspects of the situation (producing theinternally-construed psychological situation)and they both influence and are affected byother units (expectancies, goals, affects).Some links (shown by solid lines) activateother units, whereas other connections(shown by the dotted lines) are inhibitory in

260 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

nature, as when an athlete’s anxiety inhibitsconfident thoughts. The total pattern of acti-vations and inhibitions results in certainbehaviors, which may themselves alter the si-tuation (as represented by the arrow leadingback from behaviors to the situation). Thesebehaviors may also affect ongoing CAPSdynamics. For example, poor performanceduring an athletic event may trigger de-creased confidence, lowered efficacy beliefs,self-reproach, and negative affect that furtherundermines performance.

Figure 3. Schematic representation of the cognitive-affective processing system advanced by Mischel andShoda (1995). Within the circle are the CAPS mediating units, connected in a stable network ofrelations that characterize the individual. Solid lines represent positive activation, dotted linesinhibitory relations. Mediating units become activated initially by encodings of situational features, andoutput behaviours can reciprocally influence both the situation and the CAPS elements underlying thebehaviotal responses. Adapted with permission from Shoda and Smith (2004).

Enc: Encoding; Exp: Expectancy; V: Values; G: Goals; Aff: Affects B: Behavioral scripts /competencies

Page 9: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

Central to the CAPS formulation is thefact that the CAPS variables are not isolated,but rather are interconnected (Smith andShoda, in press). In the CAPS model, thefocus is not just on “how much” of aparticular unit (e.g., self-efficacy belief,performance anxiety, mastery goalorientation) a person has, but in how thesecognitive-affective units are organized withone another within the athlete, forming anetwork of interconnections that can operate,in a parallel rather than serial manner, atmultiple levels of accessibility, awareness, andautomaticity. Individual differences inpersonality reflect the fact that people differstably and uniquely in this network ofinterconnections. For a given individual thelikelihood that a particular feature of asituation triggers encoding (interpretation)A, which leads to thought B, emotion C,motive D, and behavior E may be relativelystable and predictable, reflecting a network ofchronically accessible associations amongcognitions and affects available to thatindividual. Thus, the CAPS model posits aninternal set of if,... then... relations as well theexternal situation-behavior if.... then....relations discussed earlier.

The CAPS, however, is not simplyreactive to external situations. The systemthat underlies an individual’s cognitive-affective and behavioral dynamics typicallycontains extensive internal feedback loopsthat can generate a flow of thoughts, feelings,and even behaviors without necessarilyrequiring an outside stimulus. Thus, when anathlete is in a depressed affective state, shemay be more likely to selectively encodenegative aspects of situations, attributeinadequacies to herself, and generatebehavioral withdrawal tendencies. Moreover,the elements in an individual’s CAPSnetwork are likely to form a system of

261Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

mutually supporting components. Forexample, the many beliefs we maintain arenot independent of each other, but supportone another in a way that helps us “makesense” of the world. Further, components ofa belief system are related to affectivereactions, goals and values, and behaviors, ina way that forms a coherent organic whole.Thus, in current cognitive social theory, if…then relations within the underlyingcognitive-affective processing system cons-titute the stable and coherent underlyingstructure that constitutes personality.

The CAPS model reflects the differencebetween dynamic and dispositional models(Mischel and Shoda, 1998). Dynamic modelsreflect a “bottom-up” approach that focuseson causal determinants and contrasts withwhat Salmon (1989) referred to as “top-down” approaches that appeal to broadfactors, such as traits, to account for regu-larities in behavior. In dispositionalapproaches behaviors are commonly ex-plained as a product of some underlying traitthat takes on the status of a causal factor. Forexample, an athlete engages in cooperativeand considerate behaviors because he or she ishigh on the trait of “agreeableness.” Incurrent personality psychology, this top-down approach is best represented by theFive Factor model, which regards regularitiesin behavior as stemming from individualdifferences on five factor analytically-derivedtraits: extraversion, agreeableness, cons-cientiousness, neuroticism, and openness toexperience (McCrae and Costa, 1997). Thesetraits presumably produce differences in“average” levels of trait-relevant behaviors,and behavioral inconsistencies across timeand/or situations are basically disregarded. Itis important to note that top-downexplanatory systems do not require anyknowledge about underlying causal

Page 10: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

262 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

mechanisms, and a construct can be positedwith no attempt being made to identify theprocesses responsible for its descriptive orpredictive value. Unfortunately, however,using the construct defined by the observedbehaviors as a casual explanation for thosebehaviors (i. e., the trait of “agreeableness” asan explanation for agreeable behavior) in theabsence of underlying causal mechanismsamounts to the logical error of circular rea-soning. To avoid circularity, the underlyingcauses must be conceptually independent ofthe behaviors they are designed to explain(Salmon, 1989). Dynamic or bottom-upmodels are much more difficult to formulateand test, but in the end they help us achieve amuch higher level of understanding ofpsychological processes (Bandura, 1986).

Applying the CAPS Model to SportsPhenomena

Cognitive social theory is arguably themost dynamic current personality model interms of research stimulation and application(Cervone and Shoda, 1999). It is beingapplied in the areas of social cognition (e. g.,Higgins, 1999), interpersonal relations(Baldwin, 1999), motivation (Grant andDweck, 1999), analysis and treatment ofclinical problems (Shoda and Smith, 2004),self-regulation processes (Cervone, Shadel,Smith and Fiori, 2006), and sportphenomena (Smith, 2006).

CAPS Representation of Sport PsychologyConstructs

Although every person’s CAPS, as aproduct of genetic endowment and life ex-periences, is unique, there also existsimilarities between people that cause themto exhibit certain dispositions. We shouldexpect people who exhibit specific dispo-sitions to have some commonalities in their

CAPS components and dynamics, and theCAPS may prove to be a useful way tounderstand and research the trait construct.For example, Mischel, Shoda, and Smith(2005) have analyzed a personality constructknown as rejection sensitivity (Downey &Feldman, 1997) from this perspective. Thesituational feature that activates thisdisposition is the selective encoding of aromantic partner’s behavior as uncaring or insome way rejecting. This encoding stimulatesexpectations and concerns about aban-donment, as well as feelings of anxiety, angerand resentment at the prospect of beingrejected. These expectations, beliefs, motives,and affects then activate behavioral scripts forcoercive and hostile behavior toward thepartner. These behaviors serve to alienate thepartner and may ultimately result in the veryrejection that was feared, thereby affirmingand strengthening future vigilance torejection as well as the other elements of thesystem. Interestingly, rejection-sensitivepeople are likely to be less hostile thanaverage and very caring of partners in si-tuations that are not encoded as threatening,illustrating the if…. then behavioral signatureof this personality disposition.

Achievement goal orientations. Smith(2006) has construed several popular sportpsychology constructs in CAPS terms. Forexample, achievement goal theory (Duda,2001; Roberts, Treasure and Kavassanu,1997) has been one of sport psychology’smost active areas of research and theorydevelopment. Derived, like the CAPS model,from a cognitive social conceptual frame-work, achievement goal theory focuses onunderstanding the function and the meaningof goal directed actions, based on howparticipants define success and how theyjudge whether or not they have demonstrated

Page 11: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

competence (Ames, 1992; Dweck, 1999;Nicholls, 1989). The central individualdifference construct in the theory is goalorientations that guide achievement per-ceptions and behavior.

Achievement goal theory posits twodifferent ways of defining success andconstruing one’s level of competence, labelingthem mastery (or task) orientation and egoorientation. Mastery-oriented people are self-referenced; they feel successful and competentwhen they have learned something new,experienced skill improvement, mastered thetask at hand, and/or given their best effort.For ego-oriented people, definitions ofpersonal success and demonstrated com-petence are other-referenced and based largelyon social comparison. The goal is to showthat one is superior to relevant others or toavoid appearing inferior to others. Ego-oriented people can feel successful if theyoutperform their peers or if they do as well asothers without concerted effort. Experiencingpersonal improvement or knowing that onedid his or her best would not in itselfoccasion subjective success and a sense ofdemonstrated competence for an ego-orientedperson. Indeed, knowing that one tried hardand failed to outperform others would causesuch a person to feel especially incompetent.

In placing achievement goal orientationswithin a CAPS template, Smith (2006)suggested that at the level of encodings,mastery-oriented athletes seek and encodeself-referenced information relevant topersonal improvement. This includesencoding cues from unsuccessful perfor-mances as information for improvement. Incontrast, the ego-oriented athlete seeks andencodes comparisons with others, and cuesfrom unsuccessful performance are encodedas evidence for insufficient ability.

At the level of beliefs and expectancies,

263Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

mastery-oriented athletes believe that abilityis changeable and expect that hard work andeffort are instrumental to self-improvement.When setbacks occur, they expect thatincreased effort and persistence will bringimprovement. They believe that sport’spurpose is to promote good citizenship andthe merits of hard work and cooperation.Ego-oriented athletes believe that ability islargely innate and that the need for higheffort is a sign of poor ability. They expectability to play the major role in success, andnegative outcomes yield attributions ofinsufficient ability and evoke discoura-gement. They also believe that sport’spurpose is to promote the self and earn theesteem of others.

As noted earlier, the goals of mastery andego-oriented athletes differ. The mastery-oriented athlete wants to master skills andenjoy activity for its own sake (intrinsicmotivation). Success is defined in a self-referenced manner. The goal for the ego-oriented athlete is to demonstrate superiorityover others and/or to avoid appearing inferiorto them. Successful goal attainment isdefined through social comparison.

At the level of affects, the mastery-oriented athlete can experience positiveemotional responses from high effort andimproved performance even in the absence ofwinning. Such an athlete is less likely toexperience fear of failure and negative self-evaluations if not victorious. In contrast, forthe ego-oriented athlete, positive affect iscontingent on outperforming others orwinning. Losing evokes feelings of discoura-gement and, if frequent, may evokedisengagement.

In terms of self-regulation, standards forself-reinforcement involve giving maximumeffort and achieving one’s potential in themastery-oriented athlete. Their focus on

Page 12: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

getting better encourages the development ofgoal-attainment strategies. In ego-orientedathletes, internal standards for self-reinforcement require positive comparisonswith others, or good performance with littleeffort. Such athletes are less likely to developself-improvement strategies based on effortbecause of their ability attributions forsuccess.

Sport performance anxiety. High perfor-mance anxiety can also be conceptualizedwithin the CAPS framework (Smith, 2006).The intensity and duration of anxiety areinfluenced by the nature of the competitivesituation in which the athlete is involved.Such situations differ in the demands theyplace upon the athlete, as well as the degreeof threat that they pose to important goalsand successful performance. Such factors asstrength of opponent, importance of thecontest, presence of significant others, anddegree of social support received fromcoaches and teammates can affect the amountof threat that the situation is likely to posefor the athlete (Martens, Burton, Vealey, &Smith, 1990; Smith, Smoll, & Passer, 2002).

Elements of the competitive situation areselectively encoded by the athlete. Goalsinfluence which elements of the situation aredeemed most significant. Where anxiety isconcerned, the balances between perceiveddemands, threats, and personal and si-tuational resources are the encoded elementsthat most heavily define the psychologicalsituation constructed by the athlete (Smith,1996). An athlete who defines the presentsituational demands as overwhelming, whoappraises his resources and skills asinadequate to deal with the demands, whoanticipates failure and/or disapproval as aresult of the demands/resources imbalance,and who defines his self-worth in terms of

264 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

success and/or the approval of others willperceive the situation as threatening ordangerous. Such encodings are likely toactivate expectancies of poor performance,rejection, and other negative outcomes,evoking the worry component of perfor-mance. They can also trigger low self-efficacybeliefs and an external locus of control which,in turn, influence subsequent encodings, orreappraisals, of the competitive situation. Themeanings attached to the expectedconsequences derive from the person’s beliefsystem, and they often involve theindividual’s self-reinforcement standards andcriteria for self-worth. Such personalstandards are an important aspect of the self-regulation element in the CAPS framework(Mischel & Shoda, 1995).

At the level of affect, negative encodingsof the situation can generate high levels ofphysiological arousal, and this arousal, inturn, feeds back into the ongoing process ofappraisal and reappraisal in a reciprocalfashion. High arousal may convince anathlete that he or she is “falling apart” andhelp generate even more negative encodings.Normally positive self-efficacy expectanciesmay be inhibited from activation, producinga sense of helplessness (Bandura, 1986).

The individual’s goals and motives are acentral component of the CAPS conceptionof anxiety. The athletic situation has strongachievement and social approval implications,and such motives can be either gratified orthreatened. Athletes who are prone to anxietyfear failure. Such fears can involve a variety ofconsequences, including shame andembarrassment, devalued self regard, uncer-tainties about one’s athletic future, loss ofinterest by significant others, and concernsabout upsetting significant others, such ascoaches and parents (Conroy, Willon, andMelzer, 2002). Goals influence the personal

Page 13: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

significance of situational stimulus elements,as well as the encodings, expectancies, andaffects they trigger. In turn, these cognitive-affective elements may influence currentmotives and goals by either increasing orreducing motivation.

Finally, self-regulatory skills andcompetencies play a central interactive role inperformance anxiety. Level of perceivedcompetence influences encodings andexpectancies as well as performance.Standards for self-reinforcement are linked togoals, encodings, and expectancies (Bandura,1986). Emotional self-regulation skills play acentral role in performance anxiety. If anathlete lacks good coping and anxiety-controlskills, a wide range of sport situations arelikely to be encoded as threatening (Martenset al., 1990). Inadequately-developed com-petencies may also make feared consequencesappear more likely and engender low self-efficacy for dealing with situational demands.Finally, emotional arousal evoked by thesecognitive processes may actually interferewith the application of existing skills and self-regulation competencies, as when an athlete’smotor and attentional abilities are degradedby anxiety-produced task-irrelevant responses(Nideffer and Sagal, 2001; Smith, 1996).

Other important sport psychologyconstructs can also be conceptualized withinthe CAPS model, and the framework canprovide a template for the collection ofqualitative data on relevant factors. Here aresome possible questions for qualitativelyassessing CAPS variables:

1. How do you perceive the (relevant)situation, and yourself in relation to thesituation? (Encodings)

2. What do you expect will happen?How capable are you of doing what isrequired to achieve your goals? How muchpersonal control do you have in this

265Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

situation? What other personal beliefs areengaged by this situation? (Expectancies andbeliefs)

3. What outcomes do you want? Whichoutcomes do you wish to avoid? Which needscould be satisfied or frustrated in this sportenvironment? Which personal values areengaged in sport situations? (Goals andvalues)

4. Which emotions are aroused? Howintense and long-lasting are they? Whichsport situations arouse them? (Affects)

5. What are your personal standards foryourself? Which skills do you possess, andwhich ones do you lack? What strategies doyou use to attain your goals? Can youpostpone or delay gratification of short-termgoals in favor of longer-term ones? How wellare you able to control your thoughts,feelings, and behaviors? (Competencies andself-regulation skills) (Smith, 2006, p. 12)

Interventions to Influence the CAPSAs indicated by its earlier cognitive social

learning label, today’s cognitive social theoryplaces a strong emphasis on the role of expe-rience in shaping behavior and producingbehavior change. Although the CAPSnetwork has a dynamic structure with stableproperties, that system can be altered byeither internal or external influences. Notsurprisingly, therefore, cognitive social theoryhas inspired numerous interventions designedto effect personality change (Bandura, 2006;Cervone et al., 2006; Meichenbaum, 1985;Shoda and Smith, 2004; Vaughn, Rogers,Singhal, and Swalehe, 2000). These inter-ventions have been directed at a wide varietyof target behaviors, from anxiety disorders toself regulation, from athletic performance toAIDS prevention. One of the reasons thatinterventions based on this theoretical modelhave been so successful is that the theory

Page 14: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

specifies the mechanisms that control thetarget behaviors and personality charac-teristics. Interventions can then be tailored toinfluence these causal factors.

Earlier, I presented CAPS-based analysesof two constructs that are at the forefront ofcurrent sport psychology research, namely,achievement goal orientations and sportperformance anxiety. Within the CAPSsystem, achievement goal orientations wouldbe represented primarily as a goals com-ponent and anxiety as an affect component,although they clearly involve other CAPScomponents as well. Both of these constructshave been found to be related to coachingbehaviors in correlational studies of youthsport participants. Coaching behaviorsfurnish important situational cues that areencoded by athletes and that subsequentlyactivate other aspects of the system. One classof coaching behaviors that is particularlyinfluential on a wide range of personalcharacteristics and athlete behaviors fallunder the rubric of motivational climate.Motivational climate is the environmentalfactor most addressed and researched withinachievement goal theory (Duda, 2001;McArdle and Duda, 2002).

Motivational climate involves, in part,behaviors by coaches that promote andsupport mastery or ego achievement goalorientations in athletes through the pattern ofnormative influences, evaluative standards,rewards and sanctions, interpersonal inter-actions, and values they communicate withinthe achievement environment. Althoughmotivational climate is a multifacetedconstruct, a mastery (or task-involving)climate is characterized most centrally by acoach’s emphasis on self-referenced impro-vement, effort, attention to all athletes, and acooperative learning environment. An ego-initiating climate is marked by an emphasis

266 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

on outperforming others, a focus onoutcome, preferential attention to topperformers, and punishment for mistakes(Ames; 1992; Dweck, 1999). Research inboth educational and sport settings indicatesthat motivational climate is related to avariety of meaningful outcome variables,including achievement goal orientations,intrinsic motivation, enjoyment, beliefs aboutthe meaning of success, persistence in the faceof adversity, perceived ability, and emotionalresponses such as anxiety. In general, mastery-initiating climates are more frequentlyassociated with salutary outcomes, whereasego-initiating climates frequently are linkedto negative outcomes, including fear of failure(see Duda and Balaguer, 2007, for a review).

In an attempt to influence the motiva-tional climate created by coaches, we createdan intervention called the Mastery Approachto Coaching. This intervention, evolvedfrom Coach Effectiveness Training (Smith,Smoll, and Curtis, 1979), is presented as alive 75-minute workshop. The workshopprovides coaches with behavioral guidelinesderived from previous research on coachingbehaviors and their effects on athletes andfrom achievement goal research. Itsbehavioral guidelines focus on two majorthemes. First, we place strong emphasis onthe distinction between positive versusaversive control of behavior. In a series ofcoaching do’s and don’ts derived from thefoundational research on coaching behaviorsas measured by the CBAS coding system andtheir effects (Smith, Smoll, and Curtis,1978), coaches are encouraged to increasefour specific behaviors–positive reinfor-cement, mistake-contingent encouragement,corrective instruction given in a positive andencouraging fashion, and sound technicalinstruction. Coaches are urged to avoidnonreinforcement of positive behaviors,

Page 15: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

punishment for mistakes, and punitivetechnical instruction following mistakes.They are also shown how to establish teamrules early and reinforce compliance withthem to avoid discipline problems, and toreinforce socially supportive behaviors amongteam members. These guidelines are designedto increase positive coach-athlete interactions,enhance team solidarity, reduce fear offailure, and promote a positive atmospherefor skill development (Smoll and Smith, inpress). The behavioral guidelines are thusconsistent with the procedures designed byAmes (1992) and Epstein (1988) to create amastery learning climate in the classroom.

The second important theme in themastery-oriented guidelines, also derivedfrom CET and from achievement goal theoryand research, is a conception of success asgiving maximum effort and becoming thebest one can be, rather than an emphasis onwinning or outperforming others. Coachesare encouraged to emphasize and reinforceeffort as well as outcome; to help theirathletes become the best they can be bygiving individualized attention to all athletesand by setting personalized goals forimprovement; to define success as maxi-mizing one’s athletic potential; and toemphasize the importance of having fun andgetting better as opposed to winning at allcosts. Like the guidelines that foster positivecoach-athlete relations and team solidarity,these guidelines are designed to reduce fear offailure, to foster self-esteem enhancement byallowing athletes to take personal pride ineffort and improvement, and to create a moreenjoyable learning environment that increasesintrinsic motivation for the activity.

During the Mastery Approach workshop,the differences between a mastery- and ego-oriented motivational climate were explicitlydescribed; the creation of a mastery climate

267Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

was strongly recommended; and a list ofpositive effects of such a climate waspresented. The verbal presentation wassupplemented by modeling both desirableand undesirable methods of responding tospecific situations (e. g., performance andeffort, athlete mistakes). Accompanying theworkshop was a manual that summarized theprinciples presented in the workshop andgave coaches behavioral guidelines forcreating a mastery motivational climate (seeSmoll, Smith, Cruz, and Garcia-Mas [inpress] for a Spanish-language version).Coaches were also given self-monitoringforms containing nine items related to thebehavioral guidelines. On the form, theywere asked how often they engaged in therecommended behaviors in relevantsituations. For example, coaches were asked,“When athletes gave good effort (regardlessof the outcome), what percentage of thetimes did you respond with reinforcement?”They were asked to complete the formsimmediately after the next 10 practices orgames. This self-monitoring component ofthe intervention was intended to increasecoaches’ awareness of their behavior and toencourage their compliance with theguidelines.

Based on previous research results andtheoretical expectations, we expected thatathletes’ encodings of coaches’ masteryclimate behaviors would influence bothachievement goal orientations and anxiety.Specifically, a mastery climate should increasemastery goal orientations and reduce ego goalorientation in athletes, and it should alsoreduce anxiety. A mastery climate would alsobe expected to reduce the anxiety-arousingpotential of the sport environment for severalreasons. First, a conception of success asoutperforming and comparing oneself withothers (which is characteristic of ego-

Page 16: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

involving climates) heightens evaluationapprehension and fosters worry and anxiety(Walling, Duda and Chi, 1993). In contrast,a mastery climate serves to minimize socialcomparison and to focus athletes’ attentionon personal development and task mastery.In such an environment, athletes should beless likely to experience threat concerningtheir ability to outperform others andtherefore experience less anxiety (McArdleand Duda, 2002). In line with thesetheoretical predictions, a mastery-involvingclimate is associated with lower anxiety thanis an ego-involving climate (Papaiannou andKouli, 1999; Walling et al., 1993; Yoo,2003). Moreover, a mastery climate alsoincreases enjoyment of sport activities, whichis negatively associated with anxiety(Boixadós, Cruz, Torregrosa and Valiente,2004; Newton and Duda, 1999).

To test these hypotheses in a youth sportsample of 10-14 year old basketball players,we developed new measures of all threevariables because existing measures weredesigned for older athlete populations andhad reading levels too high for this age group.All items on these scales have assessed readinglevels of 9 years or below. The MotivationalClimate Scale for Youth Sports (MCSYS;Smith, Cumming and Smoll, 2008) measuresathletes’ reports of their coaches’ mastery andego climate behaviors. Achievement goalorientations were measured by theAchievement Goal Scale for Youth Sports(AGSYS; Cumming, Smith, Smoll, Standageand Grossbard, 2008), and anxiety wasassessed using the Sport Anxiety Scale-2 (SAS-2; Smith, Smoll, Cumming and Grossbard,2007).

Effects of the motivational climateintervention on goal orientations and anxietywere tested by comparing a sample of 20basketball coaches who were trained in theMastery Approach with a control condition

268 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

consisting of 17 untrained coaches (Smith,Smoll and Cumming, 2007; Smoll, Smithand Cumming, 2007). Athletes on the 37teams were administered the achievementgoal and anxiety measures at the beginningand end of the season, and they completedthe MCSYS at the end of the season to reportthe extent to which their coaches engaged inmastery- and ego-initiating behaviors overthe course of the season.

Analysis of the motivational climate datarevealed that the trained coaches hadsignificantly higher mastery scores than didthe control coaches, indicating that theMastery Approach to Coaching interventionhad its desired effects on coaching behaviors.We assumed that these behavioral differenceswould be encoded by athletes in a mannerthat influenced the goal and affectcomponents of the CAPS. In support of thisprediction, we found significant Time xGroup interactions for both variables.Athletes who played for the trained coachesshowed significant increases on the Masteryorientation scale of the AGSYS, andsignificant decreases on the Ego orientationscale. In contrast, athletes who played for theuntrained coaches showed no significantchanges in goal orientation scores during theseason (Smoll et al., 2007).

Sport performance anxiety was alsoinfluenced by the intervention. Figure 4shows the results for total scores on the SAS-2 over the course of the season. Again, asignificant Groups x Time interaction wasfound, and follow up tests revealed a signi-ficant anxiety decrease in the experimentalcondition and a significant increase in anxietyin the control condition as competitivepressures increased over the season. Thus, thecoach intervention not only influencedcoaching behaviors, but consequentlychanged two key components of the CAPS,namely goals and affect.

Page 17: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

The exact paths by which changeoccurred are not clear, for the relationsbetween situational features and CAPS com-ponents create a number of possibilities, asdo connections between internal cognitive-affective units. It is possible, for example,that the intervention created change inachievement goals and anxiety independentlyof one another. It may be that differentcomponents of the motivational climateselectively influence achievement goals andanxiety. Thus, the coach’s focus on strivingfor personal improvement may be an activeingredient of the situation that influencesmovement in the athletes toward a masterygoal orientation. Another element, namely

269Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

the coach’s avoidance of criticism for mis-takes, may be an active ingredient inreducing anxiety. Another possibility,however, is that anxiety change was mediatedby a shift toward a mastery goal orientation,which served to reduce the threat value of theathletic environment by removing pressuresto outperform others and win. To examinethese possibilities, I correlated masteryclimate scores with achievement goal andanxiety change scores, and the latter changescores with one another. Mastery climatescores on the MCSYS were significantlycorrelated with increases in mastery goalorientation and decreases in ego goalorientation, and they were also significantly

Figure 4. Changes in total scores on the Sport Anxiety Scale-2 in athletes who were exposed to coachestrained to create a mastery motivational climated compared with athletes who played for untrainedcoaches. (Data from Smith et al., 2007)

Page 18: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

correlated with decreases in anxiety.However, the goal orientation change scoreswere not significantly correlated with anxietychange scores. This pattern of results suggeststhat anxiety reduction was not mediated bychanges in goal orientations, and that thesevariables may be influenced by different“active ingredients” of motivational climate.Future research on relations among situa-tional factors and CAPS elements is one ofthe greatest needs in using this dynamicmodel of psychological processes in sportpsychology research.

Conclusions

Cognitive social theory is a strong andvibrant force within contemporary theorydevelopment and research in personality.Further, its influence is being realized inextensions to other disciplines, includingsport psychology (e. g., Smith, 2006). Inaddition to stimulating basic research oncognitive-affective processes and dynamics, itis also stimulating applications in many areasof psychology.

The CAPS formulation has played animportant role in helping to resolve the long-standing person-situation controversy thatwas inititiated by Mischel’s (1968) con-clusion that there is little evidence for cross-situational consistency in behavior. The so-called personality paradox is resolved in thefindings that stability does indeed exist, butat the level of if….then behavioral signatures.In sports, behavioral signatures have beenclearly demonstrated among coaches whosesituation-behavior profiles differ in a stablefashion. Additionally, it has been found thatcoaching behaviors have different impact onathletes, depending on the game situations inwhich they occur. For example, the rate atwhich coaches engage in supportive behaviors

270 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

in game situations when the team is aheadpredict athletes’ liking for the coach (r = .50),whereas rate of supportive behaviors duringlosing game situations are unrelated to liking.Conversely, rate of punitive behaviors givenduring losing game situations are negativelyrelated (r = -.37) to liking for the coach,whereas punishment during winningsituations is not related to liking (Smith etal., in press). Interestingly, rates of supportiveand punitive behaviors are only weaklyrelated to liking for the coach when thebehaviors are aggregated across winning andlosing game situations. This illustrates thecost of decontextualizing behaviors andthereby ignoring situational variations thatmight influence relations with othervariables.

I believe that cognitive social personalitytheory should be particularly useful for sportpsychologists. Models like the CAPS (whichcomprises only one part of the broadertheoretical orientation) can be usefultemplates for assessing athlete and situationalcharacteristics, stimulating research, andguiding the development of interventions.Many of the interventions routinely used bysport psychologists in their performanceenhancement work, such as goal setting,stress management, imagery (mentalsimulation) and attention control are deriveddirectly from the cognitive-behavioraltradition (Sousa, Cruz, Torregrosa, Vilchesand Viladrich, 2006; Sousa, Smith and Cruz,2008; Smith, 2006).

Cognitive social theory not only focuseson both the situation and the person inunderstanding behavior, but new advancestakes this a step further by showing thatvariations in behavior across situations showlawful regularity and coherence in the formof behavioral signatures. In this theoreticalmodel, the person, the environment, and

Page 19: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

behavior all influence one another inreciprocal causal relations, and the person hasan agentic role in behavior in that it is theindividual who not only selects and mentallyconstructs the situations that are encoun-tered, but who also can change their meaningand thus their impact (Bandura, 1986).Current research stimulated by self-determination theory (Deci and Ryan, 2000;Vallerand, 2001 ) is clearly consistent withthis principle.

Just as advances in social cognitive per-sonality theory can help advance theorydevelopment, research, and interventions in

271Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

sport psychology, the sport psychologist’swork can help advance the theory. Sport is awonderful environment to study virtually anypsychological process, and its public natureand easily accessible performance measuresmake it especially attractive in the study ofsituational, individual difference, andbehavioral variables. The researchsummarized above on behavioral signaturesin coaches and on motivational climateeffects illustrate only a beginning in howsport psychologists can use and contribute tothe further development of personalitytheory.

AVANCES DE LA TEORÍA COGNITIVO-SOCIAL DE LA PERSONALIDAD: APLICACIONES A LA PSICOLOGÍADEL DEPORTE

PALABRAS CLAVE: Teoría de la personalidad, Sistema de Procesamiento Cognitivo-Afectivo (CAPS), Conductas delentrenador, Objetivos de logro, Clima motivacional, Ansiedad.RESUMEN: Muchas teorías y técnicas de intervención en psicología del deporte tienen un énfasis cognitivo-conductual ylos psicólogos del deporte han estado interesados desde hace tiempo en las diferencias individuales. Los desarrolloscrecientes en la teoría cognitivo social de la personalidad ofrecen nuevas oportunidades para entender el comportamientodeportivo. El descubrimiento de diferencias individuales estables en las relaciones situación-comportamiento ha ayudado aresolver el debate persona-situación en los últimos años, y las rúbricas conductuales ideográficamente distintivas se handemostrado ahora en los comportamientos de los entrenadores en diferentes situaciones de juego. Además, loscomportamientos de los entrenadores se relacionan de manera diferenciada con las preferencias de los deportistas por elentrenador, en función de que ocurran en situaciones en que se está ganando o perdiendo. El Sistema de ProcesamientoCognitivo-Afectivo de Mischel y Shoda (1995) ofrece una nueva herramienta para estudiar constructos de psicología deldeporte como los objetivos de logro y la ansiedad. Así como la teoría cognitivo-social puede informar a la investigación, eldesarrollo de teoría, y las intervenciones en psicología del deporte, la investigación en los ámbitos deportivos puede avanzarel futuro desarrollo de la teoría cognitivo-social de la personalidad.

PROGRESSOS NA TEORIA SOCIO-COGNITIVA DA PERSONALIDADE: APLICAÇÕES NA PSICOLOGIA DODESPORTO

PALAVRAS-CHAVE: Teoria da personalidade, Sistema de Processamento Cognitivo-Afectivo (SPCA), Comportamentos dotreinador, Objectivos de realização, Clima motivacional, Ansiedade.RESUMO: Várias teorias e técnicas de intervenção na psicologia do desporto possuem uma ênfase cognitivo-comportamental, e os psicólogos do desporto estão desde há muito interessados nas diferenças individuais. Recentesdesenvolvimentos na teoria socio-cognitiva da personalidade oferecem novas oportunidades para compreender ocomportamento desportivo. A descoberta de diferenças individuais estáveis nas relações situação-comportamento, têmajudado a resolver o debate dos últimos anos sobre a interacção pessoa-situação, e têm sido agora demonstrados distintospadrões ideográficos para os comportamentos dos treinadores em diferentes situações de jogo. Além disso, oscomportamentos dos treinadores estão relacionados diferencialmente com o apreço dos atletas pelo treinador, dependendode ocorrerem em situações competitivas de vitória ou derrota. O Sistema de Processamento Cognitivo-Afectivo de Mischel& Shoda (1995), oferece um novo moldelo para o estudo de alguns constructos da psicologia do desporto, tais como aorientação para os objectivos de realização e a ansiedade. Enquanto a teoria socio-cognitiva pode auxiliar a investigação, odesenvolvimento da teoria, e as intervenções na psicologia do desporto, a investigação no contexto desportivo podecontribuir para o desenvolvimento futuro da teoria socio-cognitiva da personalidade.

Page 20: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

References

Ames, C. (1992). Achievement goals and adaptive motivational patterns: The role of theenvironment. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 161-176).Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Baldwin, M. W. (1999). Relational schemas: Research into social-cognitive aspects ofinterpersonal experience. In D. Cervone and Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality:Social-cognitive bases of personality consistency, variability, and organization (pp. 127-154).NY: Guilford.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory. EnglewoodCliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Bandura, A. (2006). Going global with social cognitive theory: From prospect to paydirt. In S. I.Donaldson, D. E. Berger and K. Pezdek (Eds.). Applied psychology: New frontiers andrewarding careers (pp. 69-102). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Boixadós, M., Cruz, J., Torregrosa, M. and Valiente, L. (2004). Relationship among motivationalclimate, satisfaction, perceived ability and fair play attitudes in young soccer players.Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 301-317.

Burton, D. (1998). Measuring competitive state anxiety. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sportand exercise psychology measurement (pp. 129-148). Morgantown, WV: Fitness InformationTechnology.

Cervone, D. and Shoda, Y. (Eds.) (1999). The coherence of personality: Social-cognitive bases ofpersonality consistency, variability, and organization. NY: Guilford.

Cervone, D., Shadel, W. G., Smith, R. E. and Fiori, M. (2006). Self-regulation: Reminders andsuggestions from personality science. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 55, 333-385.

Conroy, D. E., Willow, J. P. and Metzler, J. N. (2002). Multidimensional fear of failuremeasurement: The Performance Failure Appraisal Inventory. Journal of Applied SportPsychology, 14, 76-90.

Cumming, S. P., Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., Standage, M. and Grossbard, J. R. (2008).Development and validation of the Achievement Goal Scale for Youth Sports. Psychology ofSport and Exercise.

Deci, E. L. and Ryan, R. M. (2000). The support of autonomy and the control of behavior. In E.T. Higgins and A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Motivational science: Social and personalityperspectives (pp. 128-145). New York: Psychology Press.

Downey, G. and Feldman, S. I. (1996). Inplications of rejection sensitivity for intimaterelationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1327-1343.

Duda, J. L. (2001). Achievement goal theory in sport: Pushing the boundaries and clarifying somemisunderstandings. In G. C. Roberts (ED.), Advances in motivation in sport and exercise(pp. 129-182). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Duda, J. L. and Balaguer, I. (2007). Coach-created motivational climate. In S. Jowett and D.Lavallee (Eds.), Social psychology in sport (pp. 117-130). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Duda, J. L. and Hall, H. K. (2001). Achievement goal theory in sport: Recent extensions andfuture directions. In R. Singer, H. Hausenblas and C. Janelle (Eds.), Handbook of sportpsychology (2nd ed., pp. 417-443). New York: Wiley.

272 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

Page 21: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories and goals: Their role in motivation, personality, and development.Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.

Epstein, J. (1988). Effective schools or effective students? Dealing with diversity. In R. Haskinsand B. MacRae (Eds.), Policies for America’s public schools (pp. 89-126). Norwood, NJ:Ablex.

Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior. I. On predicting most of the people much of thetime. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1097-1126.

Fleeson, W. (2004). Moving personality beyond the person-situation debate: The challenges andthe opportunities of within-person variation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13,83-87.

Fournier, M. A., Moskowitz, D. S. and Zuroff, D. C. (2008). Integrating dispositions, signatures,and the interpersonal domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 531-545.

Grant, H. and Dweck, C. S. (1999). A goal analysis of personality and personality coherence. InD. Cervone and Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality: Social-cognitive bases ofpersonality consistency, variability, and organization (pp. 345-371). NY: Guilford.

Hartshorne, H. and May, M. A. (1928). Studies in the nature of character: Vol. 1 Studies in deceit.New York: Macmillan.

Higgins, E. T. (1990). Personality, social psychology, and person-situation relations: Standardsand knowledge activation as a common language. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook ofpersonality: Theory and research (pp. 301-338). New York: Guilford Press.

Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.Magnusson, D. and Endler, N. S. (Eds.) (1977. Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in

interactional psychology. Hilldsale, NJ: Erlbaum.Martens, R., Burton, D., Vealey, R. S. and Smith, D. (1990). Competitive anxiety in sport.

Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.McCrae, R. R. and Costa, P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American

Psychologist, 52, 509-516.McArdle, S. and Duda, J. L. (2002). Implications of the motivational climate in youth sports. In

F. L. Smoll and R. E. Smith (Eds.), Children and youth in sport: A biopsychosocial perspective(2nd ed., 409-434). Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt.

Meichenbaum, D. (1985). Stress inoculation training. New York: Pergamon.Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Wiley. Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconceptualization of personality.

Psychological Review, 80, 252-283.Mischel, W. (2004). Toward an integrative science of the person. Annual Review of Psychology, 55,

74-97.Mischel W. and Peake, P. K. (1982). Beyond déjà vu in the search for cross-situational

consistency. Psychological Review, 89, 730-755.Mischel, W. and Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality:

Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personalitystructure. Psychological Review, 102, 246-268.

Mischel, W. and Shoda, Y. (1998). Reconciling processing dynamics and personality dispositions.Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 229-258.

273Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

Page 22: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

Mischel, W., Shoda, Y. and Smith, R. E. (2004). Introduction to personality: An integrativeapproach (7th ed.). New York: Wiley.

Newcomb, T. M. (1929). Consistency of certain extrovert-introvert behaviors in 51 problem boys.New York: Columbia University, Teachers College, Bureau of Publications.

Newton, M. L. and Duda, J. L. (1999) The interaction of motivational climate, dispositional goalorientation and perceived ability in predicting indices of motivation. International Journalof Sport Psychology, 30, 63-82.

Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic education. Cambidge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.

Nideffer, R. M. and Sagal, M.-S. (2001). Concentration and attention control training. In J. M.Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth to peak performance (4th ed., pp.312-332). Mountain View, CA: Mayfield.

Papaioannou, A. and Kouli, O. (1999). The effect of task structure, perceived motivationalclimate and goal orientations on students’ task involvement and anxiety. Journal of AppliedSport Psychology, 11, 51-71.

Pervin, L. A. (1994). A critical analysis of trait theory. Psychological Inquiry, 5, 103-113.Read, S. J. and Miller, L. C. (Eds.) (1998). Connectionist and PDP models of social reasoning and

social behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Roberts, G. C., Treasure, D. C. and Kavussanu, M. (1997). Motivation in physical activity

contexts: An achievement goal perspective. In M. L. Maehr and P. R. Pintrich (Eds.),Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 413-447). Greenwich, CT: JAI.

Ross, L. and Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology.New York: McGraw-Hill.

Rumelhart, D. E. and McClelland, J. L. (1986) Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in themicrostructure of cognition, Vols. 1 and 2. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Salmon, W. C. (1989). Four decades of scientific explanation. In P. Kitcher and W. C. Salmon(Eds.), Minnesota studies in the philosophy of science: Vol 3. Scientific explanation (pp. 3-2219). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

Sarason, I. G., Smith, R. E. and Diener, E. (1975). Personality research: Components of varianceattributable to the person and the situation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32,199-204.

Shoda, Y. and Mischel, W. (1998). Personality as a stable cognitive-affective activation network:Characteristic patterns of behavior variation emerge from a stable personality structure. InS. J. Read and L. C. Miller (Eds.), Connectionist and PDP models of social reasoning andsocial behavior. (pp. 175-208). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Shoda, Y., Mischel, W. and Wright, J. C. (1994). Intraindividual stability in the organization andpatterning of behavior: Incorporating psychological situations into the idiographic analysisof personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 674-687.

Shoda, Y. and Smith, R. E. (2004). Conceptualizing personality as a cognitive-affective processingsystem: A framework for models of maladaptive behavior patterns and change. BehaviorTherapy, 35, 147-165.

Shoda, Y., Mischel, W. and Wright, J. C. (1994). Intra-individual stability in the organizationand patterning of behavior: Incorporating psychological situations into the idiographicanalysis of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 674-687.

274 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

Page 23: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

Smith, R. E. (1996). Performance anxiety, cognitive interference, and concentration enhancementstrategies in sport. In I. G. Sarason, B. R. Sarason and G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Cognitiveinterference: Theories, methods, and findings (pp. 75-109). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Smith, R. E. (2006). Understanding sport behavior: A cognitive-affective processing systemsapproach. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 18, 1-27.

Smith, R. E., Cumming, S. P. and Smoll, F. L. (2008). Development and validation of theMotivational Climate Scale for Youth Sports. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 20, 116-136.

Smith, R. E. and Shoda, Y. (in press). Personality as a cognitive-affective processing system. In P.Corr and G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality. Cambridge,England: Cambridge University Press.

Smith, R. E., Shoda, Y., Cumming, S. P. and Smoll, F. L. (in press). Behavioral signatures at theballpark: Intraindividual consistency of adults’ situation-behavior patterns and theirinterpersonal consequences. Journal of Research in Personality.

Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L. and Cumming, S. P. (2007). Effects of a motivational climateintervention for coaches on children’s sport performance anxiety. Journal of Sport andExercise Psychology, 29, 39-59.

Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L. and Curtis, B. (1978). Coaching behaviors in Little League Baseball. InF. L. Smoll and R. E. Smith (Eds.) Psychological perspectives in youth sports (pp. 173-201).Washington, D.C.: Hemisphere Publishing Corp.

Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L. and Curtis, B. (1979). Coach effectiveness training: A cognitive-behavioral approach to enhancing relationship skills in youth sport coaches. Journal ofSport Psychology, 1, 59-75.

Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L. and Hunt, E. B. (1977). A system for the behavioral assessment ofathletic coaches. Research Quarterly, 48, 401-407.

Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L. and Passer, M. W. (2002). Sport performance anxiety in children andyouth. In F. L. Smoll and R. E. Smith (Eds.), Children and youth in sports: Abiopsychosocial perspective (2nd ed.). Dubuque, IA: Kendall/Hunt Publishers.

Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., Cumming, S. P. and Grossbard, J. R. (2006). Measurement ofmultidimensional sport performance anxiety in children and adults: The Sport AnxietyScale-2. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 28, 479-501.

Smoll, F. L. and Smith, R. E. (in press). Conducting psychologically oriented coach trainingprograms: A social-cognitive approach. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology:Personal growth to peak performance (6th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

Smoll, F. L., Smith, R. E., Cruz, J. and Garcia-Mas, A. (in press). Claves para ser un entrenadorexcelente. Barcelona: INDE.

Smoll, F. L., Smith, R. E. and Cumming, S. P. (2007). Effects of a psychoeducationalintervention for coaches on changes in child athletes’ achievement goal orientations.Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology ,1, 23-46.

Sousa, C., Cruz, J., Torregrosa, M., Vilches, D. and Viladrich, C. (2006). Evaluación conductualy programa de asesoramiento personalizado a entrenadores (PAPE) de deportistas jóvenes.Revista de Psicología del Deporte, 15, 263-278.

Sousa, C., Smith, R. E. and Cruz, J. (2008). An individualized behavioral goal-setting program forcoaches. Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology, 2, 258-277.

275Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...

Page 24: ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL- PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO … · ADVANCES IN COGNITIVE-SOCIAL-PERSONALITY THEORY: APPLICATIONS TO SPORT PSYCHOLOGY Ronald E. Smith ADVANCES

Theeboom, M., De Knop, P. and Weiss, M. (1995). Motivational climate, psychologicalresponses and motor skill development in children’s sport: A field-based interventionstudy. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 3, 294-311.

Vallerand, R. J. (2001). A hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in sport andexercise. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in sport and exercise motivation (pp. 263-320).Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

Vaughn, P. W., Rogers, E. M., Singhal, A. and Swalehe, R. M. (2000). Environmental educationand HIV/AIDS prevention: A field experiment in Tanzania. Journal of HealthCommunication, 5, 81-100.

Walling, M. D., Duda, J. L. and Chi, L. (1993). The Perceived Motivational Climate in SportQuestionnaire: Construct and predictive validity. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,15, 172-183.

Yoo, J. (2003). Motivational climate and perceived competence in anxiety and tennisperformance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 96, 403-413.

276 Revista de Psicología del Deporte. 2008. Vol. 17, núm. 2, pp. 253-276

Smith, Ronald E. Advances in Cognitive-Social-Personality...