improvements to situational awareness during approach and landing through enhanced and synthetic...

69
IMPROVEMENTS TO SITUATIONAL AWARENESS DURING APPROACH AND LANDING THROUGH ENHANCED AND SYNTHETIC VISION SYSTEMS by Justin Stephen Brown A Graduate Capstone Project Submitted to the Worldwide Campus in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements of the Degree of Master of Aeronautical Science Embry-Riddle Aeronautical University Worldwide Campus Pensacola, FL May 2011

Upload: justin-stephen-brown

Post on 16-Jul-2015

70 views

Category:

Engineering


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

IMPROVEMENTS TO SITUATIONAL AWARENESS DURING APPROACH AND LANDING THROUGH ENHANCED AND SYNTHETIC VISION SYSTEMS

by

Justin Stephen Brown

A Graduate Capstone Project Submitted to the Worldwide Campus

in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements of the Degree of Master of Aeronautical Science

Embry-Riddle Aeronautical University

Worldwide Campus Pensacola, FL

May 2011

Page 2: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

ii

IMPROVEMENTS TO SITUATIONAL AWARENESS DURING APPROACH AND LANDING THROUGH ENHANCED AND SYNTHETIC VISION SYSTEMS

by

Justin Stephen Brown

This Graduate Capstone Project

was prepared under the direction of the candidate’s Research Committee Member, Mr. William L. Little, Adjunct Associate Professor, Worldwide Campus

and the candidate’s Research Committee Chair, Dr. Peter B. Walker, Adjunct Assistant Professor, Worldwide Campus and has been

approved by the Project Review Committee. It was submitted to the Worldwide Campus in partial fulfillment of

the requirements for the degree of Master of Aeronautical Science

PROJECT REVIEW COMMITTEE:

___________________________________ William L. Little, MAS

Committee Member

___________________________________ Peter B. Walker, Ph.D.

Committee Chair

Page 3: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

iii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to thank my wife, Tamara Brown, MPT, for providing the support

and stamina required to care for our daughters while I worked on the Capstone project.

When we started down this journey we were still somewhat newlyweds. We managed

through two PCS moves, the birth of two daughters, two months of daily visits to the

NICU and countless evenings spent in front of the computer. I cannot thank Tamara

enough for her love and patience throughout the five years I've spent working on my

graduate education. Without her, this never would have happened.

I would also like to recognize my parents for teaching me the meaning of

dedication and hard work. Thank you for creating a strong work ethic in me.

I am also very thankful to my committee members who helped me finish this

project remotely with consistent and reliable communication. You all deserve a raise.

Finally, I am sincerely thankful to God for blessing me with family, friends, co-

workers and the means necessary that allowed me to achieve my goals.

Page 4: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

iv

ABSTRACT

Researcher: Justin Stephen Brown

Title: Enhanced and Synthetic Vision System’s Improvement to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing

Institution: Embry-Riddle Aeronautical University

Degree: Master of Aeronautical Science

Year: 2011

In June 1999, American Airlines flight 1420 overran runway 04R in Little Rock,

Arkansas and fell into a ravine killing 10 people including the captain. This accident

occurred during a thunderstorm and contains many of the characteristics involved in

approach and landing accidents. The Flight Safety Foundation’s (FSF) Approach and

Landing Accident Reduction (ALAR) study noted that 76 percent of all jet and turboprop

aircraft accidents involved landing overruns, loss of control, runway incursion, non-

stabilized approaches, or controlled flight into terrain (Killers in aviation, 1998).

Enhanced and synthetic vision systems are technological advances that intend to reduce

pilot workload, especially during the final phase of flight. The primary aim of this

research was to use simulation studies, proto-type research and statistical data to compare

mishap rates and pilot workload during visual flight rules (VFR) and instrument flight

rules (IFR) approaches to landing using traditional navigational aids and enhanced and

synthetic vision technology.

Page 5: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

v

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

PROJECT REVIEW COMMITTEE ii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS iii

ABSTRACT iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS v

LIST OF TABLES viii

LIST OF FIGURES ix

Chapter

I INTRODUCTION 1

Background of the Problem 1

Researcher's Work Role and Setting 5

Statement of the Problem 6

Limitations 7

Acronyms 8

II REVIEW OF RELEVANT LITERATURE AND RESEARCH 10

History of Navigational and Landing Aids 10

Developments in SVS 15

Developments in EVS 18

Situational Awareness Measurement 19

Page 6: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

vi

Prior Research of SVS and EVS 21

SVS for CFIT Prevention 22

SVS Symbology 23

Blending EVS and SVS 25

Statement of the Hypothesis 26

III RESEARCH METHODS 27

Research Model 27

Survey Population 27

Sources of Data 28

The Data Collection Device 28

Instrument Pretest 29

Instrument Reliability 30

Procedures 30

Treatment of Data 30

IV RESULTS 31

V DISCUSSION 40

VI CONCLUSION 44

VII RECOMMENDATIONS 46

REFERENCES 48

APPENDICES 52

Page 7: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

vii

A BIBLIOGRAPHY 52

B BASELINE HUD FOR IMC-DAY CONDITION 53

C SVS HUD FOR IMC-DAY CONDITION 54

D EVS HUD FOR IMC-DAY CONDITION 55

E COMBO HUD FOR IMC-DAY CONDITION 56

F BASELINE HUD FOR IMC-NIGHT CONDITION 57

G SVS HUD FOR IMC-NIGHT CONDITION 58

H EVS HUD FOR IMC-NIGHT CONDITION 59

I COMBO HUD FOR IMC-NIGHT CONDITION 60

Page 8: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

viii

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1 Pilot Flight Time in Hours 29

2 ANOVA Results for RMSE Data 31

3 Summary of F-Test Results on SAGAT Scores 36

4 Results of Post-Hoc Analysis for Display Effect on Pilot SA Types 38

Page 9: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

ix

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page

1 Four Course Radio Range 11

2 Very High Frequency Omnidirectional Radio Range 11

3 Early Forms of Runway Landing Aid Lighting 13

4 Elements of the Instrument Landing System 14

5 Universal Avionics SVS Display 17

6 EVS Using FLIR on Approach 18

7 RMSEs for Each Display Configuration 32

8 RMSEs for IMC Conditions 33

9 RMSEs for Display Configurations by IMC Condition 34

10 RMSEs for Leg by Display Configuration 35

11 Overall SA Scores for Each Display Configuration 37

12 SA Scores for Three Different Types of SA Being Measured 38

Page 10: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

1

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

Background of the Problem

The Flight Safety Foundation (FSF) conducted a study in 1998 with intentions of

reducing approach and landing accidents ("Killers in Aviation", 1998). The FSF study

found that 76 percent of all jet and turboprop aircraft accidents involved landing

overruns, loss of control, runway excursions, non-stabilized approaches, or controlled

flight into terrain (CFIT) ("Killers in Aviation", 1998). More specifically the FSF study

found that nearly 60% of ALAs involved CFIT ("Killers in Aviation", 1998). Similarly,

a study conducted by the National Aeronautics and Space Administration (NASA) in

2005 found that 66 percent of all jet accidents occur during the landing or final phase of

flight (NASA, 2006). The study also noted that 81 percent of these ALAs occurred

during instrument meteorological conditions (IMC) (NASA, 2006).

Take for example, the mishap of American Airlines flight 1420. In June 1999,

American Airlines flight 1420 overran runway 04R in Little Rock, Arkansas and fell into

a ravine killing 10 people including the captain. This accident occurred during a

thunderstorm and contains many of the characteristics involved in approach and landing

accidents (ALAs.) The ability of the pilot to determine critical information through

perceived visual cues of the outside environment may be limited by time of day and

various weather phenomena such as fog, rain, snow, and clouds. The Flight Safety

Foundation (FSF) conducted a study in 1998 with intentions of reducing approach and

landing accidents ("Killers in Aviation", 1998).

Various systems have been developed to reduce this accident rate and enhance

Page 11: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

2

safety and overcome the issues with limited outside visibility for the pilot, such as radio

navigation, instrument landing systems (ILS), ground proximity warning systems

(GPWS). More recent developments include moving map displays, Global Positioning

System (GPS) capability for improved navigational accuracy, terrain awareness warning

systems (TAWS) and enhanced GPWS.

Regardless of the technological advancement, the aircraft information display

concepts require pilots to perform various mental transformations of display data to a

mental picture of what the outside environment may look like (Prinzel et al., 2002). For

example, the TAWS technology may help to mitigate some factors causing CFIT, its use

generally follows the information processing model of warn then act and, therefore

requires the pilot to be reactive rather than proactive in dealing with terrain hazards

(Prinzel, Comstock, Glaab, Kramer, and Arthur, 2002).

Snow and Reising (1999) stated that what is needed in terms of aircraft

information systems in intuitive technologies that improve pilot SA with respect to spatial

orientation (relative to terrain and flight path) without requiring the pilot to divert visual

attention and cognitive resources away from possible external events and primary flight

references. A proactive system that can help prevent (versus just warn a pilot of) a

potential collision with terrain is needed (Prinzel et al., 2004a).

NASA and its industry partners have designed and prototyped crew-vehicle

interface technologies that strive to proactively overcome aircraft safety issues due to

low-visibility conditions by providing the operational benefits of clear day flight through

cockpit displays, regardless of the actual outside visibility conditions (Bailey, Parrish,

Kramer, Harrah, and Arthur, 2002).

Page 12: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

3

Enhanced and synthetic vision systems are technological advances that intend to

reduce pilot workload, especially during the final phase of flight. The titles are

occasionally used interchangeably; individually they are quite different and sometimes

competing technologies.

Synthetic Vision/System (SV/SVS) is a computer generated display image of the

out-of-the-cockpit scene topography based on aircraft attitude, high-precision navigation

instrumentation, and data on the surrounding terrain, obstacles, and cultural features.

SVS databases have been built to support this display technology with real-time integrity

in order to ensure pilot detection of real obstacles and to plan and verify accurate flight

navigation.

Prinzel et al. (2002) suggested that this display concept presents information to

the pilot with a level of accuracy and realism to flying under visual meteorological

conditions (VMC), regardless of the actual weather conditions. Laboratory and field

research experiments have successfully demonstrated both the safety and capability

benefits of SVS technologies in flight (Snow, Reising, Kiggett, and Barry, 1999), landing

(Prinzel et al., 2004a; Schnell, Kwon, Merchant, and Etherington, 2004) and taxi

operations (Wilson, Hoovey, Foyle, Williams, 2002). Prinzel et al. (2004a) suggests that

SVS display concepts are expected to reduce the occurrence of accident precursors

including:

• Pilot loss of attitude awareness

• Pilot loss of altitude awareness

• Pilot loss of vertical and lateral spatial awareness

• Pilot loss of terrain and traffic awareness on approach

Page 13: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

4

• Confusion regarding escape or go-around path after recognition of

flight problem

• Pilot loss of SA in relating to the runway environment and

incursion

• Unclear flight path guidance on initial approach

An Enhanced Vision/System (EV/EVS) uses electronic sensors such as forward

looking infrared radar (FLIR) or millimeter-wave radar (MMWR) to augment or enhance

the natural vision while flying an aircraft. Such instruments are used to penetrate weather

phenomena such as rain, snow, fog, and haze.

In 2007 business jets began incorporating EVS displays as a night-vision

technology to complement other traditional navigational aids. Based on the development

of EVS technology the U.S. Federal Aviation Regulations (FAR), Section 91.175 was

amended such that pilots conducting straight-in approaches may now operate aircraft

below publicized Decision Heights (DH) or Minimum Descent Altitudes (MDA), when

using an approved EVS presented on a HUD (Bailey, Kramer and Prinzell, 2006).

EVS instruments are designed to help the pilot see permanently located

obstructions such as buildings, trees, towers, power lines, and terrain in general and are

normally displayed on a heads-up display (HUD). The intended use of EVS parallels

SVS in that both strive to improve safety and SA in low visibility conditions that may

otherwise cause major flight accidents and to provide the operational benefits of VMC

(Bailey et al., 2006).

EVS and SVS technologies are designed to improve situational awareness (SA)

Page 14: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

5

for pilots flying the aircraft (PF) and non-flying pilots (NPF) alike. Investigative

interviews and the National Transportation and Safety Board (NTSB) research has shown

that accidents similar to the American Airlines crash, involve pilots that 1) are

overwhelmed with tasks during an IMC and/or night-time approach or; 2) have lost SA

during an IMC and/or night-time approach (Prinzel, Hughes, Arthur and Kramer, 2003).

It is hypothesized that EVS and SVS technology improvements will reduce the

rate of ALA accidents when compared to other navigational aids and traditional

instrument approach procedures by increasing SA and reducing pilot workload during

high-risk phases of flight such as the final approach to landing in low visibility situations.

Despite the technology’s infancy, NASA, the Federal Aviation Administration,

and many other private entities have tested this technology rigorously. The quantifiable

methods typically center on pilot workload and traditional physiological measurement

methods of stress and anxiety (Hughes, 2005c). Practical applications of this research

have promoted further mainstream research to determine safety improvements, allowing

for more efficient air carrier movement especially in congested areas susceptible to poor

weather. Financial effects can only be estimated until implementation is more complete.

Researcher’s Work Role and Setting

The researcher holds numerous Federal Aviation Administration (FAA) licenses

including Airline Transport Pilot (ATP) and a Boeing 737-300 type rating. Other

licenses and certificates include Airplane Single Engine Land (ASEL), Airplane Mutli-

Engine Land (AMEL), Military Instructor Pilot, Military Test Pilot, and Military Crew

Resource Management (CRM) Facilitator. He has served for 10 years as a Naval Aviator

with numerous sorties in wartime theaters.

Page 15: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

6

The researcher became interested in improvements of SA during low visibility

approaches to landing while deployed to Misawa, Japan. The deployment spanned the

winter months, and Misawa, Japan is located in the northern part of the main Japanese

island of Honshu. During this deployment, the researcher encountered numerous whiteout

conditions while attempting to land in Misawa. The researcher was flying as copilot

during a hazardous emergency landing during a snowstorm onto a snow-covered runway.

During this landing, the emergency arresting cable was covered by snow and

unknowingly left rigged and unreported, resulting in significant damage to the aircraft

upon landing. From this incident spawned seven years of research and education on how

to improve safety of flight during the segment of the flight that requires the most

situational awareness and removal of cluttering information.

Statement of the Problem

The FAA has authorized synthetic vision systems and enhanced vision systems

for use in flight with certain limitations. The popularity and integration has risen sharply

in its relatively short lifespan. Many pilots have not yet been exposed to the training

required to become proficient in use of EVS or SVS yet exposure is destined to come.

Without question this technology will offer more information to the pilot when visual

cues are no longer available.

The purpose of this project was to determine if EVS and SVS systems improve

SA during the approach and landing phase of flight, or if the addition of the new

technology increased avionic clutter causing a decrease in SA. How is SA or pilot

workload measured? Do experimental results show a decrease in pilot workload? Do

the results show improved performance? The importance of this study showed whether

Page 16: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

7

the implementation of additional technology in cockpit could reduce clutter and improve

the pilot’s ability to safely land an aircraft in less than VFR conditions.

Limitations

In general, conducting an experiment with real aircraft in real scenarios to

examine the effects of certain cockpit displays on human performance is difficult and

expensive. Even simulator experiments can prove to be costly and time consuming to

gather a large enough sample size of pilots, high tech instruments, etc.

Additionally, EVS and SVS are young technological advancements. Therefore, a

relatively small number of experiments or studies have been conducted on the effects on

human performance. The researcher detailed a meta-analytic approach to the relevant

experiments and studies in Chapter II regarding the early researchers involved in the

advent of synthetic and enhanced vision systems and their impact on pilot performance

and accident reduction.

Additionally, the researcher noted that the primary scientific study in his research

was Kim's simulator study (2009). Kim set the p-value at .05 to determine outliers that

occur by chance. However, Kim's sample size (see Table 3), involved 49 samples, which

would result in approximately 2 outliers with a p-value of .05. Perhaps Kim's results

would be more accurate with a p-value of .01.

Page 17: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

8

Acronyms

ALAR – Approach and Landing Accident Reduction AGL – Above Ground Level ALA – Approach and Landing Accidents ALA – Approach and Landing Accidents ANOVA – Analysis of Variance CAA – Civil Aeronautics Administration CFIT – Controlled Flight Into Terrain CRM – Crew Resource Management DH – Decision Height DIME – Database Integrity Monitoring Equipment DV – Dependent Variables EV – Enhanced Vision EVS – Enhanced Vision System FAA – Federal Aviation Administration FAR – Federal Aviation Regulation FLIR – Forward Looking Infrared Radar FMS – Flight Management System FOV – Field of View FSF – Flight Safety Foundation FTE – Flight Technical Errors GPWS – Ground Proximity Warning System HDD – Heads-Down Display HITS – Highway-In-The-Sky HUD – Heads-Up Display ICAO – International Civil Aviation Organization IFD – Integration Flight Deck IFR – Instrument Flight Rules ILS – Instrument Landing System IMC – Instrument Meteorological Conditions MDA – Minimum Descent Altitude MMWR – Millimeter Wave Radar NASA – National Aeronautics and Space Administration ND – Navigation Displays NPF – Pilot Not Flying NTSB – National Transportation and Safety Board PF – Pilot Flying PFD – Primary Flight Display RMSE – Root Mean Square Error SA – Situational Awareness SAGAT – Situation Awareness Global Assessment Technique SART – Situation Awareness Rating Technique SAS – Statistical Analysis Software STARs – Standard Terminal Arrival Routes

Page 18: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

9

SV – Synthetic Vision SVS – Synthetic Vision System SWORD – Subjective Workload Dominance TLX – Task Load Index USAF – United States Air Force VHF – Very High Frequency VFR – Visual Flight Rules VOR – Very High Frequency Omnidirectional Radio Range WAAS – Wide Area Augmentation System

Page 19: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

10

CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF RELEVANT LITERATURE AND RESEARCH

History of Navigational and Landing Aids

In the early days of flight, there were no navigation aids to help pilots find their

way. Pilots flew by looking out of their cockpit window for landmarks or by using road

maps. These visual landmarks and maps were fine for daylight operations, but airmail

operated around the clock and the need for mail to be delivered by air expanded.

In 1919, U.S. Army Air Service Lieutenant Donald L. Bruner began using bonfires to

help with night navigation (Preston, 1998). In February 1921, an airmail pilot named

Jack Knight flew the first all-night flight from North Platte, Nebraska to Chicago using

bonfires lit by Post Office staff, farmers and the public (Preston, 1998).

In 1926, the Aeronautics Branch of the Department of Commerce took over

responsibility of building lighted airways and by 1933, 18,000 miles of airway had lights

and 1,500 beacons were in place (Preston, 1988). Regardless of the complexity of

lighting systems, pilots were still required to maintain visual contact with the ground.

Radio communications developed rapidly during this same period allowing for

weather reports to be passed via two-way radio signals and teletypewriter. This let pilots

plan their flight path and diverts around foul weather to maintain VMC. In 1929, Army

Lt. James H. Doolittle became the first pilot to use aircraft instrument guidance solely for

take off, fly a set course, and land. Lt. Doolittle used a four-course radio (Figure 1)

range and radio marker beacons to indicate his distance from the runway (Preston, 1988).

An altimeter displayed his altitude, and a directional gyroscope with artificial horizon

helped Lt. Doolittle control his aircraft’s attitude, without seeing the ground. These

Page 20: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

11

technologies became the basis for many future developments in aviation navigation.

Figure 1. Four Course Radio Range

In May 1941, the Civil Aeronautics Administration (CAA) opened its first ultra-

high frequency radio range system for scheduled airline navigation, later expanding to

use such equipment to 35,000 miles of federal airways. In 1944, the CAA began testing a

static-free, very high frequency (VHF) omnidirectional radio range (VOR) (Figure 2)

that allowed pilots to navigate by watching a dial and needle on the instrument panel

rather than by listening to the radio signal audibly (Preston, 1988).

Figure 2. Very High Frequency Omnidirectional Radio Range (VOR)

By the middle of 1952, 45,000 miles of VHF and VOR airways, referred to as

Victor airways, supplemented the 70,000 miles of federally maintained low frequency

Page 21: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

12

airways. The CAA began to shut down the low and medium frequency four course radio

ranges (Preston, 1998). In 1961, the FAA began using distance-measuring equipment

(DME) on its entire system. DME allowed aircraft to determine their distance from

known checkpoints in order to confirm their position. DME with VOR greatly improved

accuracy in instrument approaches during less than visual conditions and during

nighttime operations.

By 1973 the last airway light beacon from the system in the 1920s was shut down

and by 1982, the first of 950 new navigation aids equipped with solid-state construction

and advanced features was installed. With the advancement of radar implementation into

the air traffic control system, air traffic made large improvements in managing the

increasing flow of traffic while also increasing safety.

Developments in landing aids progressed parallel to developments in navigational

aids. Airports began using rotating lights at the landing field so they could be found after

dark. In the early 1930s, airports installed the earliest forms of approach lighting. These

indicated the correct angle of descent and the correct alignment with the runway. The

approach path was called glidepath or glideslope. The colors of the lights, their rates of

flash became standard worldwide based on International Civil Aviation Organization

(ICAO) standards.

Radio navigation aids assisted in landing, such as the four-course radio range, in

which the pilot was guided by the strength of Morse code signals. The introduction of

the slope-line approach system was a first in landing aids. Developed in the 1940s, the

aid consisted of lights in a row (Figure 3) that allows the pilot a simple funnel of two

rows that led to the end of the runway (Komons, 1989). The system was inexpensive to

Page 22: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

13

build and operate and variations of this same lighting system are still used in airports

worldwide.

Figure 3. Early Forms of Runway Landing Aid Lighting; the top image shows a daytime view; the lower image illustrates a nighttime view

The instrument landing system (ILS) incorporated the best features of both

approach lighting and radio beacons with higher frequency transmissions. The ILS

painted an electronic picture of the glideslope onto a pilot’s cockpit instrument panel.

The first landing of a scheduled U.S. passenger airline using ILS was on January 26,

1938 as a Pennsylvania-Central Airlines Boeing 247 arrived in Pittsburgh from

Washington, D.C. and landed in a snowstorm using only the ILS system (Komons, 1989).

More than one type of ILS system (Figure 4) was tried and eventually the

Page 23: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

14

adaption consisted of a course indicator (localizer) that showed drift either left or right

from runway centerline, glidepath to signify drift above or below glideslope, and two

marker beacons to show progress of the approach to the landing field or runway

(Komons, 1989). Approach lighting and other visibility equipment are part of the ILS

and also aid the pilot in landing. The ILS remains very similar to the initial system and

continues to be relied upon as an accurate instrument approach landing aid as of the

writing of this paper.

Figure 4. Elements of the Instrument Landing System

The advent of the GPS provided an alternative source of precision information for

instrument approaches. In the U.S., the Wide Area Augmentation System (WAAS) has

been available to provide precision guidance to Category I standards since 2007. Satellite

based avionics acted as the foundation for the majority of improvements to precision

instrument navigational supplements and landing aids.

Similar to the rapid changes in technological advances for navigational aids in the

1920s and 1930s, the 2000s witnessed rapid upgrades in possibilities to improve aircrew

and passenger safety. However, the addition of more and more information proved to be

more of a distraction in certain situations, such as cell phone use while driving, than of an

Page 24: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

15

improvement. Therefore, technological progress was tempered with caution to eliminate

unnecessary information in circumstances that demand high levels of situational

awareness. This created a need for repeated and even continual experiments to measure

pilot performance while exposed to additional information with the objective to remove

unnecessary and distracting data.

In this section, various recent and historical research and studies pertaining to

EVS and/or SVS are reviewed from a human factors perspective and the influence on

situational awareness and pilot workload. It was the author’s intent to discuss the studies

and their assessment of specific features or configurations of EVS and/or SVS

technologies on pilot performance, in order to understand their effect on reducing pilot

workload and improving situational awareness during the approach and landing phases of

flight. Due to the difficulty in measuring situational awareness, the methods used during

these studies will also be discussed.

Developments in SVS

Throughout aviation history, developers have made attempts to improve safety and

increase the ability to successfully aviate and navigate through poor weather and poor

visibility conditions. For example, Jeppesen researched a detailed terrain database and

precision navigation sensors that would be a part of future-generation, three-dimensional

(3-D) flight management system (FMS.) The FMS would supply an on-board SVS with

position, altitude and heading data, along with terrain features, elevations and contours.

The SVS then projects a synthetic image of the outside world onto a HUD or HDD,

similar to the computer generated visuals in a flight simulator (see Figure 5) (Ramsey,

2004).

Page 25: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

16

Early developments of SVS had to overcome an Achilles’ heel: access to an

accurate terrain database that could be approved for IFR flight. The U.S. military

controlled access to one of the most precise terrain databases ever developed: the Defense

Mapping Agency’s 90 meter terrain survey that was developed for cruise missile

guidance and strike aircraft attack missions. A major step forward in terrain mapping

came with the February 2000 launch of the space shuttle Endeavor and its 11-day shuttle

radar topography mission (Ramsey, 2004). This accelerated the development of a system

flying in general aviation (GA) aircraft. Coupled with Jeppesen navigation charts, the

system displays a highway-in-the-sky that guides pilots through a series of waypoints,

standard terminal arrival routes (STARS), and ILS approaches to landing. Terrain,

obstructions, and traffic can also be depicted on the display (Ramsey, 2004).

Page 26: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

17

Figure 5. Universal Avionics SVS display

Developments in EVS

Enhanced vision systems are designed to use electronic sensors to augment or

enhance the natural vision while flying an aircraft (see Figure 6) (Hughes, 2005b). These

instruments are designed to help the pilot see permanently located obstructions, such as

buildings, trees, towers, power lines, and are normally displayed on a HUD.

Page 27: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

18

Figure 6. EVS using FLIR on approach FLIR provides excellent EVS image resolution and is relatively affordable

technology. Also, FLIR systems are compact and installation requires only minor

modifications to the airframe. FLIR could provide pilots with a high-resolution display

of the terrain and runway environment when attempting to make an approach to an

isolated airport on a clear night. However, fog and clouds severely degrade FLIR

performance, rendering it almost useless especially if the clouds are dense.

Clouds and fog, on the other hand, don’t affect MMWR, although heavy

precipitation may reduce its effective range. Yet MMWR doesn’t have as high of a

Page 28: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

19

resolution as FLIR. Compared to FLIR systems, MMWR systems are relatively

expensive and hard to adapt to civil aircraft because of their bulky antennas. A

MMWR’s ultra-short wavelength may require changes to an aircraft’s radome to ensure

that it is transparent to three and nine millimeter radio waves (George, 1995).

Situational Awareness Measurement

Due to the difficulty and ambiguity surrounding situational awareness, there are

also difficulties in measuring situational awareness during studies such as those discussed

in this paper. The research, experiments and studies reviewed in the following chapters

use different SA measurement methods.

Situational awareness subjective workload dominance (SA-SWORD) is used to

assess and compare the pilot SA when using two or more different cockpit displays or

interfaces. The SWORD technique is a subjective workload assessment tool that has

been used both retrospectively and predictively. SWORD uses subjective paired

comparisons of tasks in order to provide a rating of workload for each individual task.

When using SWORD, participants rate one task’s dominance over another in terms of

workload imposed.

Situational awareness global assessment technique (SAGAT) is an objective,

diagnostic and sensitive metric that is highly validated for use in a variety of applications.

SAGAT has been successfully used to directly and objectively measure operator SA in

evaluating avionics concepts, display designs and interface technologies. With SAGAT,

mission simulations are frozen randomly, the system displays are turned off and the

simulation is suspended while operators quickly answer questions about their current

perceptions of the situation. Operator perceptions are then compared to the real situation

Page 29: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

20

to provide an objective measurement of SA.

The situational awareness rating technique (SART) is a direct self-rating measure

of SA that is more complex than a simple Likert scale. In 1989, R.M. Taylor developed

the SART by eliciting knowledge from pilots and aircrew. Through statistical techniques

he created the SART which consists of ten dimensions of questions to measure SA.

Cooper-Harper rating scale is a set of criteria used by pilots and flight test engineers

to evaluate the handling qualities of aircraft during flight tests. The scale ranges from 1

to 10, with 10 indicating the worst handling characteristics and 1 indicating the best. The

criteria are evaluative and therefore the scale is considered subjective.

The U.S. Air Force’s Revised Workload Estimation Scale (a 7-point Likert scale) is

a subjective technique used to measure mental workload in comparison with two or more

dependent variables.

NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX) is a subjective workload assessment tool.

NASA-TLX allows users to perform subjective workload assessments on operators

working with various human-machine systems. NASA-TLX is a dimensional rating

procedure that derives an overall workload score based on a weighted average of ratings

on six subscales. These subscales include mental demands, physical demands, temporal

demands, own performance, effort and frustration.

Root mean square error (RMSE) is a frequently used measure of the differences

between values predicted by a model or an estimator and the values actually observed

from the subject being modeled or estimated. In Kim’s study, RMSE was used to

measure the errors in flight path control by calculating the distance away from centerline

an aircraft or simulator drifted during the study.

Page 30: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

21

Prior Research of EVS and SVS

The use of EVS and SVS technology is expected to reduce aircraft accidents, in

particular CFIT, due to the display information enhancing pilot SA under low visibility

conditions, similar to a night-time or inclement approach to landing. Several studies have

been conducted to assess this expectation and to investigate the effect of specific

EVS/SVS features on pilot performance. In general, studies have focused on the effects

of display sizes and corresponding field of view (FOV), guidance images, and tunnel

images on flight path tracking performance, SA, workload, or subjective display ratings

(Kim, 2009).

Researchers such as Prinzel, Schnell, Arthur, Bailey, Hughes, McKinley, Kramer

and others used these measurement techniques to assess the pilot’s ability to safely

maintain an aircraft in stable flight through experiments with symbology, ground

proximity, instrument approach precision, and other variables discussed below. The

previous research has mainly focused on portions or certain aspects of the

implementation of new displays with more information into the cockpit. Sang-Hwan

Kim’s (2009) experiment combined the research into a measurement of all aspects into a

quantifiable study on how pilots respond to an efficient display of additional information.

The results of their work created the basis for Kim’s simulator experiment in 2009, which

combined numerous variables into one study.

Early SVS and EVS studies and articles were centered on the rapidly changing

technology and its integration into mainstream aviation. Prinzel et al. (2002) conducted

flight tests to evaluate the effects of three display concepts. These design concepts were

tested with the primary objective to determine which, if any, of the concepts improved

Page 31: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

22

SA and reduced pilot workload in IMC conditions on final approach. In general, Prinzel

et al. confirmed the hypotheses that SVS would provide safety and performance benefits

over traditional navigational instruments (2002).

With a similar objective, Prinzel et al. in another study in 2004 conducted two

experiments to examine the efficacy of SVS displays and to develop field of view (FOV)

and terrain texture recommendations for cockpit display design. In one of their

experiments, they investigated the effects of different types of displays for presenting

SVS information, two types of textures (photorealistic and generic) and two runway

conditions on performance, subjective preference ratings, workload and SA (using SA-

SWORD). Results demonstrated that the different display sizes did not affect flight

performance and that the use of the HUD for presenting SVS information reduced lateral

path error, as compared to the HDD. A reduction in lateral path error led to confirming

their hypothesis that a HUD would reduce pilot workload and improve situational

awareness over a HDD.

SVS for CFIT Prevention

The studies reviewed above have focused on nominal flight operations; however,

other research has been conducted to examine the efficacy of SVS technology for CFIT

prevention in off-nominal situations (Prinzel et al, 2003). In an experiment by Prinzel et

al. (2003), 10 display concepts, including two baseline conditions (a round-dials display

and a primary flight display), and various SVS textures were used to assess operator

CFIT detection. Results revealed that the use of SVS, in general, improved CFIT

detection.

In a second experiment, Prinzel et al (2003) evaluated four display concepts by

Page 32: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

23

measuring flight performance, SA and workload during a go-around situation.

Situational awareness was measured using the SART and SA-SWORD methods.

Workload was measured using modified Cooper - Harper ratings. Results confirmed that

the use of the SVS allowed pilots to detect CFIT more efficiently than baseline concepts.

SVS Symbology

These experiments demonstrated the general efficacy of the SVS concept.

Consequently, the effects of guidance and tunnel images, combined with SVS

technology, were investigated. Prinzel et al. (2004c) conducted two experiments to

compare different tunnel and guidance symbology concepts for synthetic vision display

systems presented on HDDs and HUDs. They evaluated the efficacy of these concepts

during complex, curved approaches under instrument meteorological conditions (IMC).

In the first experiment, they focused on a SVS primary flight display (PFD) and

examined four tunnel concepts compared to a baseline (no tunnel) configuration. They

also assessed three guidance symbologies by measuring mental workload using the

United States Air Force’s (USAF) Revised Workload Estimation Scale, SA using SART

and SA-SWORD, a subjective questionnaire, and RMSE flight path control measurement

(Kim, 2009). The results of the first experiment revealed the baseline condition to be

worse than other conditions including tunnel concepts, in terms of path control, workload

and SA. The second experiment evaluated two pathway tunnel concepts and two forms

of guidance for a HUD. Overall, the results demonstrated that presenting any kind of

tunnel feature could produce better performance in terms of RMSE, workload and SA.

Schnell et al. (2004) also evaluated a SVS HDD against traditional navigational

aids through conventional glass cockpit displays to assess whether SVS technology could

Page 33: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

24

improve pilot performance, SA and workload. Schnell et al. included navigation displays

(ND) in their simulation setup for providing pilots with more realistic flight situation

information. SA was measured using SAGAT, mental workload using the NASA-TLX

workload assessment tool, flight technical errors (FTE) and eye movements of pilots

when using three different configurations of flight decks.

The configurations included a conventional PFD with displays, a SVS PFD with

navigational displays, and a conventional PFD with an external display. This is an

exocentric display that depicts the planned flight path in the context of the surrounding

terrain. The depiction is centered on the aircraft (Kim, 2009). Results demonstrated, in

general, the use of the SVS display format to improve pilot performance by generating

reduced flight technical errors (FTEs), lower workload scores and short overall visual

scan length. Interestingly, there was no significant difference in SA across display

conditions. That is, the SVS PFD with terrain representation did not seem to improve the

terrain awareness of the pilot.

Schnell et al. (2004) deduced that pilots relied on and trusted the pathway tunnel

to the extent that they did not feel they needed to devote much attention to the aircraft-

terrain situation. Schnell et al. (2004) generalized that pilot workload measures were

lower in the SVS condition than with the conventional PFD.

Problems with using SVS technology included standardizing the quality of

database-oriented system information and controlling the quality of matching real and

synthetic information closely enough to certify the database for practical applications.

Bailey et al. (2002) introduced two possibilities of reducing SVS information errors: 1)

development of a complimentary system called Database Integrity Monitoring Equipment

Page 34: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

25

(DIME); and 2) incorporating use of EVS, real-time and non-database elements, blended

into the SVS display.

Blending EVS and SVS

EVS and SVS have been perceived as separate technologies, both designed to

improve pilot situational awareness but neither technology completely provided the entire

picture outside the cockpit. NASA developed a Sensor Enhanced – SVS (SE – SVS)

concept, which utilizes the beneficial aspects of EVS and SVS while mitigating the

negative aspects of each concept (e.g., calibration in SVS images with terrain and poor

EVS display quality due to meteorological conditions) (Bailey et al, 2002).

Bailey, Kramer and Prinzel (2006) compared the general effects of blending

EVS/SVS concepts with and without pathway tunnel images. The comparisons focused

on the fusion of the two displays during low-visibility and landing operations. In the

experiments, four HUD display concepts were tested. This included simulated approach

to landing with the use of SVS displays on approach then transitioned to an EVS image at

500’ above ground level (AGL). This transition was designed primarily because an EVS

camera can only give a usable image when the aircraft is low and close to the ground

(Bailey et al., 2006). Bailey et al. (2006) measured flight path errors and pilot control

inputs during each experimental trial. They also collected subjective questionnaires,

workload ratings, and SA ratings using SA – SWORD and SART. The results of the

study showed that significant improvements in pilot SA without increases in workload

could be provided by the fusion display and the pathway tunnel image (Bailey et al.,

2006).

These results confirmed previous studies (Alexander, Wickens and Hardy, 2003;

Page 35: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

26

McKinley, Heidhausen, Cramer and Krone, 2005; Wickens, Horrey, Nune and Hardy,

2004), which showed a synthetic tunnel image to improve flight performance. Through

these studies, it can be stated that the critical objective of improving flight performance

and reducing pilot workload might be largely facilitated by the presentation of tunnel

images in a HUD (Kim, 2009).

Statement of Hypothesis

To summarize, the studies mentioned in this literature review support evidence

that advanced synthetic images in cockpit displays, including blended or fused images

with EVS, synthetic tunnels and terrain features for approach to landing, improve flight

performance and/or pilot SA, and reduce workload (Alexander et al., 2003; McKinley et

al., 2005; Prinzell et al., 2002, 2003; Wickens et al., 2004). Highway-in-the-sky (HITS)

or tunnel images have demonstrated to be a significant factor in improving pilot

performance, particularly flight path accuracy (Alexander et al., 2003).

The findings of combined terrain feature utilities SVS – HUD and EVS – HUD in

Kim's study were hypothesized to improve pilot SA and generated lower mental

workload for pilots than traditional navigational aids (Arthur et al., 2005). Additionally,

the null hypothesis was determined that Kim's experiment would show no change in the

results following the baseline simulator flights as compared to the other test flights.

Page 36: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

27

CHAPTER III

RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Research Model

The Graduate Capstone Project included a meta-analytic approach of multiple studies

and experiments in collaboration to answer the hypothesis. The project employed a

descriptive quantitative research model involving a laboratory experiment conduct by

S.H. Kim in 2009 as the primary focus. Kim's results were collected via SAGAT

measurement devices and three dependent variables (DV) were analyzed with a one-way

analysis of variance (ANOVA) and f-tests.

A meta-analytic approach is a statistical examination of multiple scientific studies and

not an actual scientific study itself. This approach can be used in situations where large

sample sizes are not available, such as in this case with few scientific studies based on the

dependent variable of situational awareness. The strength of a meta-analytic approach

can more powerfully estimate the true effect size as opposed to a smaller effect size

derived in a single study under a given set of assumptions and conditions. The weakness

of this approach was that the sources of bias were not controlled by the method.

The experiment was first developed in consideration of the data collection method

that would be most valid. As previously mentioned, measuring SA is difficult and the

methods are limited. The SAGAT method was employed to quantify the results of Kim’s

experiment (2009).

Survey Population

The sample size in Kim's study was eight pilots recruited to participate in the lab

experiment. The requirements for selection included previous flying experience in

Page 37: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

28

commercial aircraft with glass cockpit displays. Due to the relatively recent

developments in EVS and SVS, experience with the use of this technology was not

expected. Data from the demographic survey for the eight pilots include: all pilots were

male; the average age was 58.6 with a standard deviation of 14.4 years; all pilots had

glass cockpit experience; the average flight hour experience was 11,043.8 hours with a

standard deviation of 7,893.1; three pilots had experience with a HUD display in either

actual flight or in a simulator; two pilots had experience with SVS systems (mean of 4

hours) and one pilot had EVS experience (6 hours) in a simulator (Kim, 2009).

Sources of Data

Primary sources of data include task load measurement study conducted by Kim in

2009 at North Carolina State University. Other studies, as cited previously, have been

conducted on specific methods or specific modes of EVS/SVS. However, Kim’s study

(2009) was most appropriate for the specific task of testing the hypothesis that pilot

workload during approach and landing is decreased with the use of EVS and SVS.

The Data Collection Device

Each pilot in Kim’s study completed nine trials. Eight of these trials followed a

within-subjects variable design and one additional trial for collected subject verbal

protocols to measure cognitive task analysis. The data from the first eight trials were

collected using SAGAT measurements of situational awareness. Responses to SAGAT

queries represent a binomial variable – either correct or incorrect. The nature of this

measurement violates quantified statistical test assumptions. With the use of a valid

arcsine function, the data was transformed and traditional parametric data analysis can be

satisfied (Kim, 2009). One-way ANOVAs were then conducted on the transformed

Page 38: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

29

SAGAT scores for overall SA, SA by levels and SA by types.

The ninth and last trial data was collected through verbal questioning and

measurement based on the NASA Task Load Index (NASA-TLX) model. Another one-

way ANOVA was used to normalize the rating scores for individual workload scaling.

In addition, all nine trials included a physiological workload measurement – incremental

heart rates (∆HR) – compared to the HR during rest (baseline HR) (Kim, 2009). A single

variable ANOVA was conducted to analyze the effect of display conditions, visibility,

nighttime effects on HR.

Instrument Pretest

All data collection devices were pre-tested to ensure their accuracy and

comprehensiveness. Kim’s experiment (2009) was conducted on a low-fidelity simulator

developed by Integration Flight Deck (IFD) inputs from NASA’s Langley Research

Center, which provided pilots with a full-mission simulator. An ex-Air Force C-130

check pilot with experience with advanced HUDs and experience with NASAs IFD

simulator tested the different HUD configurations in each trial and validated the

simulator.

Table 1. Pilot Flight Time Data in Hours

Pilot Hours Mean Standard Deviation

Total Instrument Time 3781.3 4064.7

Total Night Time 3660.0 3471.8

Total Flight Time 11043.8 7893.1

Total Time Last 12 Months 230.6 204.8

Page 39: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

30

Instrument Reliability

SAGAT is the most widely validated of all SA measurement techniques (Endsley,

2000). According to Endsley (2000) numerous studies have been performed to assess the

validity of the SAGAT method and the evidence suggests that it is a valid metric of SA.

The study by Endsley (2000) included two sets of simulation trials on four fighter pilots

with mean scores of .98, .99, .99 and .92.

The SAGAT method, when used to measure SA through the low-fidelity simulator,

maintains its reliability through simplified binomial variable testing. Either correct or

incorrect responses were measured through pilot inputs on the simulator and then

collected and stored on the simulator’s PC-based computer.

Procedures

Kim’s simulator experiment was conducted on four different segments of a typical

commercial flight. The pilots flew these segments first without EVS/SVS to establish a

baseline. Then the pilots flew using SVS and EVS separately followed by a combination

of EVS and SVS. The dependent variables (DV) measured during these trials were

spatial awareness, system awareness and task awareness, which are all functions of SA.

Treatment of Data

All statistical analyses were performed using Statistical Analysis Software (SAS) (Kim,

2009). Residual plots were used to examine and verify linearity, constant variance of

error terms, and independence of error terms and normality of error term distribution

(Kim, 2009). An alpha level of 0.05 was used to identify any significant effects and

interactions.

Page 40: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

31

CHAPTER IV

RESULTS

Flight Path Control Performance

The results of the experiment are listed in the tables below including the ANOVA

results on RMSE data for each of the display configurations (baseline, SVS, EVS,

combination), ANOVA results on pilot SA (overall and by SA type – spatial awareness,

system awareness, and task awareness), and ANOVA results for pilot workload (Kim,

2009).

Flight path control performance is illustrated in Table 2 and in Figure 7.

Table 2 ANOVA Results for RMSE Data

Source df Type III SS Mean Square F P Display 3 0.324819 0.108273 14.37 <.0001 IMC 1 0.458698 0.458698 60.89 <.0001 Leg 3 0.021173 0.007058 0.94 0.4236 IMC Leg 3 0.0582812 0.017604 2.34 0.0746 Display IMC 3 0.211148 0.070383 9.34 <.0001 Display Leg 9 0.176666 0.01963 2.61 .0007 Display IMC Leg 9 0.07069 0.007854 1.04 .407

Page 41: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

32

Figure 7. RMSEs for each display configuration

ANOVA results (see Figure 8) also revealed that there was a significant effect of

the IMC condition on tracking performance (F(1,223)=60.9, p<.0001). The IMC-day

condition (M=14.3) was associated with greater errors in the tracking task than the IMC-

night condition (M=12.0) (Kim, 2009).

`

12.9  

15.7  

11.3  

14.3  

0  

2  

4  

6  

8  

10  

12  

14  

16  

18  RM

SE  

Display  Con0iguration  

RMSEs  for  Display  Con0iguration  

Baseline  

SVS  

EVS  

Combo  

Page 42: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

33

Figure 8. RMSEs for IMC Conditions.

There was no significant effect of leg and no interaction of IMC condition and leg

on RMSE. However, ANOVA results revealed an interaction effect among the display

and IMC conditions. Figure 9 shows the interaction plot. This graph indicates the SVS-

HUD under IMC-day condition produced higher RMSE than the same HUD during IMC-

night conditions. In general, IMC-day conditions were associated with higher RMSE

then IMC-night conditions (Kim, 2009).

0.0  

2.0  

4.0  

6.0  

8.0  

10.0  

12.0  

14.0  

16.0  

Visibility  Con=iguration  

RMSE  

RMSEs  for  IMC  Condition  

IMC-­‐Day  

IMC-­‐Night  

Page 43: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

34

Figure 9. RMSEs for display configuration by IMC condition. ANOVA results also revealed a significant interaction effect between display and

flight leg on RMSE (F(9,223)=2.61, p=.007). Figure 10 shows the RMSEs for each

display configuration for the four simulator legs. The SVS-HUD yielded higher RMSEs

and the EVS-HUD produced lower RMSEs across legs. However, the Combo-HUD in

Leg 4 generated higher tracking error than the other displays in other legs. There was no

three-way interaction effect among the experimental manipulations (F(9,223)=1.04,

p=.4070).

12.8  

19.0  

12.5  14.2  

12.1   11.7  10.4  

12.0  

0.0  

2.0  

4.0  

6.0  

8.0  

10.0  

12.0  

14.0  

16.0  

18.0  

20.0  

Basleline   SVS   EVS   Combo  

RMSE  

Display  Con0iguration  by  IMC  Condition  

IMC  Day  

IMC  Night  

Page 44: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

35

Figure 10. RMSEs for leg by display configuration.

Pilot Situational Awareness

Results of an ANOVA applied to pilot SA revealed significant main and

interaction effects of display configuration, IMC condition, and leg of flight on SAGAT

scores including overall SA, and for various levels of SA. Table 3 presents a summary of

F-test results on the situational awareness SAGAT scores.

12.3  13.2  

11.9   12.5  

14.6   14.9   15.5  

13.5  

10.3  12.0   12.7  

10.7  

12.7   12.2   12.4  

15.3  

0.0  

2.0  

4.0  

6.0  

8.0  

10.0  

12.0  

14.0  

16.0  

18.0  

Leg  1   Leg  2   Leg  3   Leg  4  

RMSE  

Leg  (Phase  of  Flight)  

Leg  by  Display  Con0iguration  

Baseline  

SVS  

EVS    

Combo  

Page 45: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

36

Table 3

Summary of F-test Results on SAGAT Scores

Ind. Vrbls

Overall SA

Levels of SA Types of SA

Level 1 Level 2 Level 3 Spatial Awareness

System Awareness

Task Awareness

Display F(3,224)=3.04 F(3,224)=2.37 F(3,224)=1.42 F(3,224)=1.73 F(3,224)=1.1 F(3,224)=4.55 F(3.224)=.82

p=.0300 p=.0712 p=.2374 p=.1614 p=.3517 p=.0041 p=.4823

IMC F(1,224)=1.22 F(1,224)=.44 F(1,224)=5.99 F(1,224)=56 F(1,224)=2.3 F(1,224)=3.99 F(1,224)=3.39

p=.2701 p=.5096 p=.0151 p=4555 p=.1306 p=.0469 p=.0668

Leg F(3,224)=.76 F(3,224)=2.28 F(3,224)=1.15 F(3.224)=10.62 F(3,224)=9.37 F(3,224)=10.36 F(3,224)=11.12

p<.0001 p=.0799 p=.3292 p<.0001 p<.0001 p<.0001 p<.0001

Display IMC

F(3,224)=.76 F(3,224)=.88 F(3.224)=1.45 F(3.224)=.14 F(3.224)=.57 F(3,224)=.99 F(3, 224)=1.68

p=.5188 p=.4532 p=.2289 p=.9334 p=6334 p=.3978 p=.1711

Display Leg

F(9,224)=1.91 F(9,224)=1.32 F(9,224)=1.45 F(9,224)=1.66 F(9,224)=.55 F(9,224)=2.31 F(9,224)=.84

p=.0509 p=.2282 p=.1686 p=.1009 p=.8386 p=.0167 p=.5827

IMC Leg

F(3,224)=1.77 F(3.224)=.66 F(3,224)=.56 F(3,224)=4.28 F(3,224)=.97 F(3,224)=2,82 F(3,224)=.97

p=.1544 p=.5748 p=.6412 p=.0059 p=.4064 p=.0386 p=.4091

Display IMC Leg

F(9,224)=.62 F(9,224)=.63 F(9,224)=1.08 F(9,224)=.86 F(9,224)=1.05 F(9,224)=.42 F(9,224)=.22

p=.7786 p=.7722 p=.3754 p=.5604 p=.4030 p=.92237 p=.9911

ANOVA results revealed significant effects of HUD configuration

(F(3,224)=3.04, p=.03) and leg of flight (F(3,224)=9.75, p<.0001) on overall SA score.

A post-hoc analysis categorized the display into two groups: one group consisted of SVS,

Baseline, and the Combination of EVS and SVS, condition, which produced higher SA

scores; and another group consisted of Baseline, Combo, and EVS conditions, which

were associated with lower SA scores (see Figure 11) (Kim, 2009).

Page 46: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

37

Figure 11. Overall SA Scores for Each Display Configuration

Regarding the effect of display configuration on the three types of pilot SA,

ANOVA results revealed that a significant effect on pilot system awareness

(F(3,224)=4.55, p=.0041). Table 4 shows the post-hoc analyses for the effect of display

configuration on the three types of pilot SA and Figure 12 illustrates the same

information. It can be observed that system awareness was degraded by the EVS-HUD

while the other display effects were comparable.

ANOVA results also revealed the effect of IMC condition to be significant on

system awareness was associated with higher SA scores for system awareness than the

IMC-night condition (Kim, 2009).

44  

46  

48  

50  

52  

54  

56  

58  

60  

Display  Con=iguration  

Percent  Correct  for  SA  Queries  

Overall  Scores  for  Display  Con0iguration  

Baseline  

SVS  

EVS  

Combo  

Page 47: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

38

Table 4

Results of post-hoc analysis for display effect on pilot SA types

Spatial Awareness System Awareness Task Awareness Baseline (M=47.9%) SVS (M=47.2%) EVS (M=40.5%) Combo (M=51.8%)

Baseline (M=61.5%) SVS (M=61.2%) EVS (M=41.9%) Combo (M=51.4%)

Baseline (M=78.2%) SVS (M=85.3%) EVS (M=83.3%) Combo (M=78.2%)

Figure 12. SA Scores for three different types of SA being measured according to display

configuration

Cognitive Task Analysis

The results of the cognitive task analysis (CTA) included three types of outcomes:

lists of sequential tasks, lists of non-sequential tasks and critical outcomes from pilots.

Patterns of pilot shifting attention about the HUD were examined.

47.9  

61.5  

78.2  

47.2  

61.2  

85.3  

40.5   41.9  

83.3  

51.8   51.4  

79.4  

0  

10  

20  

30  

40  

50  

60  

70  

80  

90  

Spatial  Awareness   System  Awareness   Task  Awareness  

Percent  Correct  Answers  

Types  of  SA  

SA  Scores  for  Types  of  SA  by  Display  Con0iguration  

Baseline  

SVS  

EVS    

Combo  

Page 48: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

39

There were no specific differences in the sequential tasks among the experimental

manipulations, which suggested the display configurations and IMC conditions do not

affect sequential flight tasks (Kim, 2009). Trigger events were based on flight scenario

and confirmed by observations of common pilot behaviors in the simulator. Examples of

trigger events included ATC broadcasts, altimeter announcements, and critical changes in

flight status (e.g. runway appearing through clouds, intercepting final approach). Each

event or task consisted of an associated flow of elementary actions.

The analysis of non-sequential tasks revealed required pilot behaviors in

controlling the aircraft independent of the task timing. Non-sequential tasks included

tracking the flight path marker (FPM), airspeed control, altitude control, heading control,

vertical speed control, and radar altitude. Throughout the verbal protocol and analysis,

Kim revealed that some tasks have several alternative methods of performance based on

pilot preference, from training or experience as well as the status of the flight (2009).

Based on analysis of non-sequential tasks, inferences can be made on how

specific behaviors required information affect pilot workload and SA in task

performance. For example, Kim noticed two methods for acquiring vertical speed. The

first method observed included the use of the vertical speed indicator (VSI), while the

second method was pilot inference of the current speed based on the vertical motion of

the flight path marker between the horizon and pitch control (2009).

Similar to the sequential tasks analysis, the non-sequential behaviors among

display configurations and IMC conditions did not reveal differences in pilot

performance due to the display features, such as terrain imagery.

Page 49: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

40

CHAPTER V

DISCUSSION

Flight Path Control

Errors in tracking were evaluated as a measure of flight path control performance

using RMSE calculations. It was assumed that higher RMSEs would be associated with

less attention to the FPM indicative of degraded path control performance. Flight path

control performance was affected by pilot display configuration and visibility condition

(Kim, 2009). These results were in-line with the expectation for the HUD content to

drive variation in flight performance under the various environment conditions.

EVS generated lower RMSE scores, the Baseline and Combo conditions were

comparable to each other and SVS induced the greatest flight path control. This was not

in-line with the expectation that the Baseline display would generate the lowest errors in

flight path control followed increasingly be SVS, EVS and Combo displays.

Primarily, the grid lines depicting terrain features in Kim's experiment, generated

by the SVS were often confused by pilots with the FPM and the tunnel features which

also consisted of lines. This confusion may have diverted pilot attention from the FPM in

order to discriminate other features from the SVS imagery and caused higher tracking

errors. Secondly, the thermal returns, or moisture images, from the EVS did not appear

to produce pilot confusion. EVS imagery appeared to compel pilots to focus more on the

FPM in the display and this produced lower tracking errors. Finally, due to the negating

effects of SVS increasing RMSE and EVS decreasing RMSE, the Combo display was

comparable to the Baseline display configurations.

Page 50: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

41

Pilot Situational Awareness

Pilot SA was measured using SAGAT techniques and was affected by the

experimental manipulations in Kim's experiment. There were several results that

contradicted the hypothesis.

Overall SA scores were higher with SVS displays and lower for EVS displays,

while the effect of each of these configurations was not different from the Baseline and

Combo displays. The use of EVS degraded system awareness, which involved pilot

understanding of iconic information in the display (see Figure 12.) Degradation in SA

while using EVS may be attributed to the thermal features frequently washing-out iconic

features that presented system information and pilot focus on the FPM to perform the

tracking task which caused negligence in attending to system information. Therefore,

pilots who used EVS features produced higher tracking performance (lower RMSEs) but

had lower system awareness. This is suggestive of a cognitive tunneling effect. This is

caused by the thermal features from the EVS in the HUD.

The visibility conditions had no effect on overall situational awareness. However,

there were significant effects on specific levels and types of SA. While IMC during

night-time operations were associated with higher levels of SA scores, it induced lower

system awareness scores. This is suggestive that night flying increased pilot

comprehension of overall flight information with decreased understanding of system

information in the HUD (Kim, 2009). Additionally, mean scores for other levels and

types of SA across display configuration and legs were higher in the IMC-night

condition.

No significant interaction effects were found on overall SA scores. However,

Page 51: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

42

there were significant effects on several levels and types of pilot SA. Display

configuration caused differences in system awareness among the different legs of flight.

The use of SVS produced higher system awareness in Legs 1 and 2, but the Baseline

configuration produced higher system awareness scores for Legs 3 and 4 (Kim, 2009).

This may be explained by the actual terrain and runway being visible through the out-of-

cockpit view.

Among the three types of pilot SA, spatial awareness and task awareness were not

affected by display configuration and visibility condition. The effects of most

experimental manipulations were significant on system awareness.

Pilot Workload

Both subjective and physiological pilot workload measures (NASA TLX and

heart rate measurements) were not affected by the experimental manipulations, including

display configurations or IMC conditions. Although a previous study (Kaber et al., 2008)

found subjective workload rating measured by NASA-TLX to be affected by levels of

display clutter, Kim's study could not reveal the impact of display configuration on the

experiment and nature of the task (2009). This result was unexpected and may be

attributed to the constraints on the experiment and the nature of the task. The primary

assignment for the experimental pilots was not actual flight control but tracking the flight

path.

Cognitive Task Analysis Results

Three types of results were derived from the pilot think aloud portion of the

experiment. The number of tasks and amount of information for each leg of flight

affected system and task awareness. The non-sequential task analysis demonstrated how

Page 52: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

43

pilots control the aircraft and which alternative behaviors can be employed to perform the

specific task along with the relevant information. Pilot comments during the think-aloud

session provided useful information for interpreting the effect of the display

configurations on performance. The use of SVS terrain features produced confusion with

the tunnel and FPM features. EVS caused a cognitive tunneling effect that compelled

pilots to concentrate on the FPM instead of iconic information. The use of terrain

features should not be considered after the runway is visible during VMC conditions

because of the potential to create display clutter effects.

Page 53: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

44

CHAPTER VI

CONCLUSIONS

The objective of this study was to determine if advancements in avionics

technology, such as EVS and SVS in HUD configurations, could improve flight path

control, pilot situational awareness and pilot workload during landing approach under

instrument meteorological conditions. In this study, a quasi meta-analytic approach was

used to compare results from numerous experiments. A heavy focus was placed on S. H.

Kim's experiment in 2009 that tested pilot performance under various situations involving

EVS and SVS variables.

Flight Path Control

In Kim's study, videos of HUD content for approach to runway 16R at KRNO

were prepared and then presented to pilots on a lab simulator (Kim, 2009). The pilots

were tasked with tracking a flight path in the stimuli while following a flight scenario and

understanding the flight situation. Several pilot studies mentioned earlier in this project

by Prinzell, Schnell, Kramer, Hughes, etc. revealed that the use of tunnel features

improved pilot performance. Kim's experiment revealed that HUD content with EVS

images of terrain features were most effective for facilitating SA on system status

information as compared to providing pilots with spatial information (2009).

Pilot Workload

Both subjective and physiological pilot workload measurements (NASA-TLX and

heart rate changes, respectively) were not affected by increased demands in the

experimental manipulations. Initially, this was thought to not fall in line with the

hypothesis that pilot workload would not be the same under the experimental conditions

Page 54: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

45

(Kim, 2009). However, it was expected that the subjective and physiological

measurements would show a parallel movement with the experimental variables. Since

pilot workload measurements remained unchanged under increased demands, it was

determined that, in effect, pilot workload would decrease under normal demands.

Therefore, it can be said that EVS and SVS improve pilot workload conditions during

landing approach under IMC conditions.

Cognitive Task Analysis

Analyses on the subjective-questioning, think-aloud sessions and verbal probes

revealed three types of results. The number of tasks and amount of information for each

leg of flight affected system awareness and task awareness. This may be explained by

the practice effect whereby the pilots became more familiar with the simulator. The non-

sequential task analysis demonstrated how pilots control the aircraft and which

alternative habits can be employed to perform the specific task along with relevant

information. The use of SVS terrain features produced confusion with the tunnel and

flight path marker features (Kim, 2009). EVS caused a cognitive tunneling effect that

compelled pilots to concentrate on the FPM instead of other important information. The

use of terrain features should not be considered after the runway is visible on the

instrument approach because of the potential to create display clutter effects.

Page 55: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

46

CHAPTER VII

RECOMMENDATIONS

Based on the results of Kim's experiment and the previous studies and research,

directions for future research include investigating advanced HUD design and the use of

HDD for SVS flight path control during landing approach prior to the runway

environment coming into view. Primarily, the effects of SVS during more realistic flight

conditions or perhaps actual flight conditions should be further evaluated on a larger,

more heavily funded scale. Various avionics manufacturers have employed SVS in more

of a mainstream modality and its benefits have proven effective in managing pilot

workload and improving situational awareness in low visibility conditions (Hughes,

2006). EVS features with HUD usage have proven more costly and less likely than SVS

to become mainstream. Again, further experiments with wider breadth to provide a more

realistic flight scenario can more accurately measure RMSE on EVS effects on spatial

awareness past the traditional decision height position.

Also, more studies should be conducted regarding the design of advanced HUDs

to determine the optimal features to include. Iconic features can prove confusing to pilots

when too many features are presented. Likewise, too few features foster a visual search

for more information, which then increases the pilot workload. Therefore, cognitive task

analyses involving the best use of symbols, numbers and out-of-cockpit images should be

studied to determine the optimum display configuration.

Although it may be possible for pilots to manipulate display configurations in

order to achieve most feature combination or display properties under particular flight

conditions, this may cause additional cognitive workload for pilots. It would be desirable

Page 56: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

47

to present pilots with optimal display feature combinations or display sets according to

dynamic changes in flight situations. The HUD interface in Kim's experiment presented

pilots with a runway outline and glideslope reference line instead of tunnel features when

the aircraft descended below 500' AGL, which was preferred by most pilots.

In short, numerous future studies and experiments are required before bold

statements regarding EVS in the landing transition can be made. Many of the Level C

and D full flight simulators in use by airlines and military training facilities would serve

as testing and training devices for future SVS and EVS research.

Page 57: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

48

REFERENCES

Alexander, M. J., Wickens, C. D., & Hardy, T. J. (2003). Examining the effects of

guidance symbology, display size, and field of view on flight performance and

situation awareness. Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society

47th Annual Meeting. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics

Society.

Arthur, J. J., Kramer, L. J., & Bailey, R . E. (2005). Flight test comparison between

Enhanced Vision (FLIR) and Synthetic Vision Systems. Proceedings of

Enhanced and Synthetic Vision (SPIE) 2005, Vol. 5802, 25-36.

Bailey, R. E., Parrish, R. V., Kramer, L. J., Harrah, S., & Arthur, J. J. (2002). Technical

challenges in the development of a NASA Vision System Concept. Proceedings

of the North Atlantic Treaty Organization (NATO) Enhanced and Synthetic

Vision Systems.

Bailey, R. E., Kramer, L. J., & Prinzell, L. J. (2006). Crew and display concepts

evaluation for synthetic/enhanced vision systems. Proceedings of the SPIE,

Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Conference 2006.

Endsley, M. R. & Garland, D. J. (2000). Situation awareness analysis and measurement.

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Flight Safety Foundation. (2007). Approach and landing safety demands improvement. In

Approach and landing accident reduction (Data Systems and Analysis).

George, F. (1995). EVS: Is it feasible? Business & Commercial Aviation, 6, 74-77.

Hughes, D. (2004). Clearing the fog. Aviation Week & Space Technology, 161, 48.

Hughes, D. (2005). MMW ready to roll. Aviation Week & Space Technology, 163, 62.

Page 58: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

49

Hughes, D. (2005). Picture this. Aviation Week & Space Technology, 163, 65.

Hughes, D. (2005). Seeing and believing. Aviation Week & Space Technology, 163, 59.

Hughes, D. (2006). Virtual vfr for bizjets. Aviation Week & Space Technology, 165, 66.

Kaber, D.B., Kim, S-H, Kaufmann, K. Veil, T., Alexander, A.L., Stelzer, E.M., Prinzel,

L. & Allamandola, A. (2008). Modeling the effect of HUD visual properties,

pilot experience and flight scenario. Proceedings of the NASA Aviation Safety

Technical Conference. Denver, CO. October 21-23.

Killers in aviation: FSF task force presents facts about approach-and-landing and

controlled-flight-into-terrain accidents. (1998). Flight Safety Digest, November -

December 1998.

Kim, S.H. (2009). Examining and explaining the effects of non-iconic conformal features

in advanced heads-up displays on pilot performance. Retrieved from ProQuest.

(UMI: 3357736)

Komons, N. (1989). Bonfires to beacons. Smithsonian Institution Press. Washington,

D.C.

McKinley, J. B., Heidhausen, E., Cramer, J. A., & Krone, N. J. (2005). Flight testing of

an airborne SVS with Highway-In-The-Sky on a head-up display. Proceedings of

the Digital Avionics System Conference, DASC 2005, 2, 13.C.1-1-13.C.1.1-9.

NASA. (2006). Synthetic vision would give pilots clear skies all the time. In (NASA,

Ed.) Find It @ NASA. Retrieved 25 July 2006, from

http://www.nasa.gov/centers/langley/news/factsheets/SynthVision.html.

Page 59: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

50

NTSB American Airlines 1420 report (McDonnell Douglas MD-82, Registration:

N215AA). (Docket Management System (DMS) No. DCA99MA060). (2002).

Washington, D.C. (10).

Preston, Edmund. (1998) FAA historical chronology, civil aviation and the federal

government 1926-1996. Department of Transportation, Federal Aviation

Administration Office of Public Affairs, Washington.

Prinzell, L. J., Kramer, L. J., Comstock, J. R., Bailey, R. E., Hughes, M. F., & Parish, R.

V. (2002). NASA synthetic vision EGE flight test. Proceedings of the Human

Factors and Ergonomics Society 46th Annual Meeting. Santa Monica, CA:

Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.

Prinzell, L. J., Hughes, M. F., Arthur, J. J., & Kramer, L. J. (2003) Synthetic vision CFIT

experiments for GA and commercial aircraft: “A picture is worth a thousand

lives”. Proceeding of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 47th Annual

Meeting. Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.

Prinzell, L. J., Comstock, J. R., Glaab, L. J., Kramer, L. J., & Arthur, J. J. (2004). The

efficiency of head-down and head-up synthetic vision display concepts for retro

and forward-fit of commercial aircraft. International Journal of Aviation

Psychology, 14(1), 53-57.

Prinzell, L. J., Kramer, L. J., Arthur, J. J., Bailey, R. E., & Comstock, R. J. (2004).

Comparison of head-up and head-down “Highway-In-The-Sky” tunnel and

guidance concepts for synthetic vision displays. Proceeding of the Human

Factors and Ergnonomics Soceity 48th Annual Meeting. Santa Monica, CA.

Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.

Page 60: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

51

Prinzell, L. J., Arthur, J. J., Kramer, L. J., Bailey, R. E. (2004). Pathway concepts for

head-down synthetic vision displays. Proceedings of the SPIE, Enhanced and

Synthetic Vision 2004. 5423-02.

Ramsey, J. (2004). Synthetic vision: no longer futuristic. Aviation Today. 14, 48.

Schnell, T. Kwon, Y., Merchant, S. & Etherington, T. (2004). Improved flight technical

performance in flight decks equipped with synthetic vision information system

display. International Journal of Aviation Psychology, 14, 79-102.

Snow, M.P., Reising, J.M., Kiggett, K.K. U & Barry, T.P. (1999). Flying complex

approaches using a head-up display: effects on visibility and display type. In

Proceedings of the 10th International Aviation Psychology Symposium

Tailwind and snow forced Southwest 737-700 off runway. (2005, January 20). Flight

International, 168(5016), pp. 7.

Wickens, C. D., Alexander, A. L., Horrey, W. J., Nune, A. & Hardy, T. J. (2004). Traffic

and flight guidance depiction on a synthetic vision system display: The effects of

clutter on performance and visual attention allocation. Proceedings of the Human

Factors and Ergonomics Society 48th Annual Meeting. Santa Monica, CA:

Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.

Wilson, J.R., Hoovey, B.L., Foyle, D.C., & Williams, J.L. (2002) Comparing pilot’s taxi

performance, situation awareness and workload using command-guidance,

situation-guidance and hybrid head-up display symbologies. In Proceedings of

the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 46th Annual Meeting. Santa

Monica, CA: Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.

Page 61: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

52

APPENDIX A

BIBLIOGRAPHY

American Psychological Association (2010). Publication manual of the American

Psychological Association (6th ed.) Washington, D.C.

Bender, A. Clark, R., Hanrahan, P., Harsha, W., McMasters, B., Murphy, E. et al.

(Eds.) (2005). Graduate/technical management capstone project

guidelines (6th ed.). Daytona Beach, FL: Embry-Riddle Aeronautical

University, Extended Campus

StatPlus (Version 5.7.8) (2009) [Computer Software]. Vancouver, B.C.

AnalystSoft Inc.

Page 62: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

53

APPENDIX B

BASELINE HUD FOR IMC-DAY CONDITION

!"#

!

!! !!

!! ! ! !$%&'()#"*+,*#-./)0%1)#234#56(#789:;.<#=61;%>%61*#

!"#$%$ !"#$&$

!"#$'$ !"#$($

Page 63: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

54

APPENDIX C

SVS HUD FOR IMC-DAY CONDITION

!"#

!

!! !!

!!$%&'()#*+,-+#./.#012#34(#56789:;#<4=9%>%4=+

!"#$%$ !"#$&$

!"#$'$ !"#$($

Page 64: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

55

APPENDIX D

EVS HUD FOR IMC-DAY CONDITION

!"#

## #

!

!!$%&'()#*+,-+#./0#123#45(#6789:;<#=5>:%?%5>+

!"#$%$ !"#$&$

!"#$'$ !"#$($

Page 65: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

56

APPENDIX E

COMBO HUD FOR IMC-DAY CONDITION

!"#

## #

!

!!$%&'()#*+,!+#-./0.#123#4.(#56-789:#;.<8%=%.<+

!"#$%$ !"#$&$

!"#$'$ !"#$($

Page 66: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

57

APPENDIX F

BASELINE HUD FOR IMC-NIGHT CONDITION

!"#

## #

!

!!$%&'()#*+,-+#./0)1%2)#345#67(#89:;2%&<=#>72?%=%72+

!"#$%$ !"#$&$

!"#$'$ !"#$($

Page 67: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

58

APPENDIX G

SVS HUD FOR IMC-NIGHT CONDITION

!!"

"" " "

!

!!#$%&'(")*+,*"-.-"/01"23'"45678$%9:";38<$:$38*

!"#$%$ !"#$&$

!"#$' !"#$($

Page 68: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

59

APPENDIX H

EVS HUD FOR IMC-NIGHT CONDITION

!"#

## #

!

!!$%&'()#*+,-+#./0#123#45(#6789:%&;<#=5:>%<%5:+

!"#$%$ !"#$&$

!"#$'$ !"#$($

Page 69: Improvements to Situational Awareness During Approach and Landing Through Enhanced and Synthetic Vision Systems

60

APPENDIX I

COMBO HUD FOR IMC-NIGHT CONDITION

!"#

## #

!

!!$%&'()#*+,,+#-./0.#123#4.(#56-78%&9:#;.8<%:%.8+!

!"#$%$ !"#$&$

!"#$'$ !"#$($