group dynamics: theory, research, and practice · pdf file12.05.2014 · 2014 volume...

89
2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal Information Current issue feed Set PsycALERT: New issue update by email Teamwork Competency Test (TWCT): A step forward on measuring teamwork competencies. Pages 101121 Aguado, David; Rico, Ramón; SánchezManzanares, Miriam; Salas, Eduardo The effect of different phases of synchrony on pain threshold. Pages 122128 Sullivan, Philip J.; Rickers, Kate; Gammage, Kimberley L. Reactions to social inclusion and ostracism as a function of perceived ingroup similarity. Pages 129137 Sacco, Donald F.; Bernstein, Michael J.; Young, Steven G.; Hugenberg, Kurt Singlecase designs in group work: Past applications, future directions. Pages 138158 Macgowan, Mark J.; Wong, Stephen E. Team task conflict resolution: An examination of its linkages to team personality composition and team effectiveness outcomes. Pages 159173 O’Neill, Thomas A.; Allen, Natalie J. The impact of group norms and behavioral congruence on the internalization of an illegal downloading behavior. Pages 174188 Sansfaçon, Sophie; Amiot, Catherine E. Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions | Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions | Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions | Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions | Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions | Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions |

Upload: phunghanh

Post on 06-Mar-2018

222 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun)

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research,and PracticeEditor: David Marcus, PhD

Journal InformationCurrent issue feedSet PsycALERT: New issue update by email

Teamwork Competency Test (TWCT): A step forward on measuring teamwork competencies.Pages 101121Aguado, David; Rico, Ramón; SánchezManzanares, Miriam; Salas, Eduardo

The effect of different phases of synchrony on pain threshold.Pages 122128Sullivan, Philip J.; Rickers, Kate; Gammage, Kimberley L.

Reactions to social inclusion and ostracism as a function of perceived ingroup similarity.Pages 129137Sacco, Donald F.; Bernstein, Michael J.; Young, Steven G.; Hugenberg, Kurt

Singlecase designs in group work: Past applications, future directions.Pages 138158Macgowan, Mark J.; Wong, Stephen E.

Team task conflict resolution: An examination of its linkages to team personality composition and team effectivenessoutcomes.Pages 159173O’Neill, Thomas A.; Allen, Natalie J.

The impact of group norms and behavioral congruence on the internalization of an illegal downloading behavior.Pages 174188Sansfaçon, Sophie; Amiot, Catherine E.

Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions |

Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions |

Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions |

Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions |

Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions |

Abstract | Full Text PDF | Full Text HTML | Permissions |

Page 2: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Teamwork Competency Test (TWCT): A Step Forward onMeasuring Teamwork Competencies

David AguadoAutonomous University of Madrid and Knowledge

Engineering Institute

Ramón RicoAutonomous University of Madrid

Miriam Sánchez-ManzanaresCarlos III University of Madrid

Eduardo SalasUniversity of Central Florida

The Teamwork Knowledge, Skill, Ability Test (TWKSAT), designed to assess team-work competencies, has been widely used in both applied and academic contexts.However, studies have brought to light a number of reliability problems in the test. Inthis article, we describe 3 studies that (a) examined the functioning of the TWKSAT(N 135); (b) investigated a new measure, the Teamwork Competency Test, includingits metric properties (N 120); and (c) analyzed the convergent and predictive validityof the Teamwork Competency Test compared with the TWKSAT (N 91). Based onour results, we conclude that the TWKSAT does not adequately reflect the initialsubstantive model and has limitations with regard to reliability. The Teamwork Com-petency Test improves the TWKSAT by enhancing reliability, content validity, andsubstantiating the dimensional structure of the test.

Keywords: teamwork test, teamwork competencies, team performance

Work teams have responded effectively to theglobal challenges of our times, providing orga-nizations with the benefits of adaptability, pro-ductivity, and creativity above and beyond thecontributions individuals can make on their own(Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006; Salas, Sims, &Burke, 2005). However, teamwork requires sev-eral competencies to allow its members to ef-fectively integrate their contributions, functionas a unified whole, and make an “expert team”

out of a mere “group of experts” (Lawler &Worley, 2006).

Work teams bring to their mission muchmore than just the behaviors directly related tothe task at hand. Their members need to interactand cooperate if they are to synchronize effec-tively (Salas et al., 2005; Salas, Sims, & Klein,2004), which requires a specific set of behaviorsthat further the attainment of team goals. Em-pirical evidence shows that measures of knowl-edge and skills related to both a specific taskand teamwork predict individual performancein work contexts (e.g., McClough & Rogelberg,2003; Schmidt & Hunter, 1983). At the sametime, teams formed by people with strong team-work competencies display a specific range ofbehaviors, including the use of integrative(win–win) as opposed to distributive (win–lose)negotiating strategies. These findings are rele-vant as teams are often affected by interpersonalconflicts and faulty cooperation, which hinderoptimum performance (Hackman, 2002). Theavailability of an appropriate measure of team-work competencies may help minimize theseproblems (Stevens & Campion, 1994, 1999).

This article was published Online First May 12, 2014.David Aguado, Autonomous University of Madrid, Ma-

drid and Knowledge Engineering Institute; Ramón Rico,Autonomous University of Madrid; Miriam Sánchez-Manzanares, Carlos III University of Madrid; Eduardo Sa-las, University of Central Florida.

This research was partially supported by the Office ofNaval Research Collaboration and Knowledge Interoper-ability Program and Office of Naval Research Multidis-ciplinary University Research Initiative GrantN000140610446.

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-dressed to David Aguado, Autonomous University of Ma-drid, Ciudad Universitaria de Cantoblanco, Madrid 28049Spain. E-mail: [email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice © 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 18, No. 2, 101–121 1089-2699/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0036098

101

Page 3: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Over recent decades, scholars increasinglyhave underlined the importance of teamworkcompetencies for the effectiveness of groupsand work teams (e.g., Cannon-Bowers & Bow-ers, 2011; Cannon-Bowers, Tannenbaum, Salas,& Volpe, 1995; Chen, Donahue, & Klimoski,2004; Leach, Wall, Rogelberg, & Jackson,2005; Peeters, van Tuijl, Rutte, & Reymen,2006). In particular, Stevens and Campion(1994, 1999) proposed the Teamwork Knowl-edge, Skill, Ability Test (TWKSAT), whichidentifies and measures five transportable team-work competencies that are common to differ-ent types of teams (see Figure 1) and have beenrelated to different performance criteria (Mc-

Clough & Rogelberg, 2003; Stevens & Cam-pion, 1999).

However, studies employing the TWKSAThave reported low levels of reliability (Athana-saw, 2003; Chen et al., 2004; McClough &Rogelberg, 2003). Despite the TWKSAT’s ca-pacity to predict individual performance in teamtasks, there are no studies addressing the causesof the test’s low reliability or explaining itsdimensional structure. To resolve the problemsaffecting the assessment of teamwork compe-tencies, we (a) examined the metric propertiesof the TWKSAT items, as well as their contentvalidity and dimensional structure, and (b) pro-pose a new measure based on the Stevens and

I. Interpersonal KSAs

1. Conflict Resoluon

Recognize team conflict.

Recognize type and source of conflict and implement conflict resoluon strategies.

Employ integrave (win-win) negoaon strategies.

2. Collaborave Problem Solving

Idenfy situaons requiring parcipave group problem solving.

Recognize the obstacles to collaborave group problem solving and implement correcve acons.

3. Communicaon

Understand communicaon networks and ulize decentralized networks to enhance communicaon.

Communicate open and supporvely.

Listen in a non-evaluave way and use acve listening techniques.

Maximize consonance between non-verbal and verbal messages, and recognize and interpret the non-verbal messages of

others.

Engage in ritual greengs and small talk.

II. Self-management KSAs

4. Goal Seng and Performance Management

Establish specific, challenging and accepted team goals.

Monitor, evaluate, and provide feedback on both overall team performance and individual performance.

5. Planning and Task Coordinaon

Coordinate and synchronize acvies, informaon, and task interdependences.

Establish task and role expectaons of individual team members, and ensure proper balancing of workload in the team.

Figure 1. The Stevens and Campion model.

102 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 4: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Campion (1994, 1999) model, analyzing its re-liability and validity as a measure of teamworkcompetencies. Although recently some testshave been proposed to capture knowledge onteamwork (e.g., boundary-spanning activities,Marrone, Tesluk, & Carson, 2007; team roles,Mumford, Van Iddekinge, Morgeson, & Cam-pion, 2008), it is still necessary to develop testscapable of assessing teamwork competencies topredict employees’ performance in teams andthus design and develop high-performanceteams. We aimed to improve the assessment ofteamwork-related competencies in work set-tings, beyond the TWKSAT, one of the mostpopular measures in the field.

The TWKSAT as a Measure of TeamworkCompetency

Competencies are defined as the underlyingcharacteristics integrated with an individual’sknowledge, skills, and abilities that are causallyrelated to a referential criterion of effectiveand/or superior action in a specific job or situ-ation (Spencer & Spencer, 1993). In particular,researchers have stressed the key role of team-work competencies for the effectiveness ofwork teams (e.g., Cannon-Bowers et al., 1995;Chen et al., 2004).

Cannon-Bowers et al. (1995) identified eightcentral teamwork competencies, namely adapt-ability, shared understanding of the situation,performance monitoring and feedback, leader-ship, interpersonal relations, coordination, com-munication, and decision making. These schol-ars also made a key contribution by definingcompetencies on the basis of two dimensions:task competencies versus team competenciesand specific competencies versus general com-petencies. Given their relationship with perfor-mance in different teamwork situations, generalteam competencies (i.e., transportable compe-tencies) are of particular interest to organiza-tions. With this in mind, Stevens and Campion(1994, 1999) identified five transportable team-work competencies that are common to differ-ent types of teams: (1) conflict resolution, (2)collaborative problem solving, (3) communica-tion, (4) goal setting and performance manage-ment, and (5) planning and task coordination.These competencies are grouped in two moregeneral dimensions termed interpersonal com-petencies (defined by the first three items in the

list) and self-management competencies (repre-sented by the last two items). This distinction isbased on the idea that team effectiveness de-pends on its members’ ability to both managetheir relations appropriately and direct their ac-tions to carry out the tasks assigned by theorganization.

Perhaps the most important contributionmade by Stevens and Campion (1999) is theconstruction of the TWKSAT, which providesboth practitioners and academics with an instru-ment to measure key teamwork competencies.The available evidence suggests that theTWKSAT has considerable predictive validity.Thus, the TWKSAT measure of employees inreal work teams correlated with their perfor-mance in the team as evaluated by both super-visors (correlations of between .23 and .52) andcolleagues (correlations of between .21 and .34;McClough & Rogelberg, 2003; Stevens & Cam-pion, 1999). Furthermore, Chen et al. (2004)found that the TWKSAT was sensitive tochanges in the individual competency of uni-versity students after participation in a trainingprogram designed to develop their teamworkcompetencies.

However, other studies in which theTWKSAT was used have also consistently re-ported low reliability of the measure. Stevensand Campion (1999) originally found reliabilityof .80 (internal consistency), but this appears tobe an overestimate (McClough & Rogelberg,2003). In fact, the alpha coefficients in studiesemploying the TWKSAT were consistentlylower. For example, McClough and Rogelberg(2003) found an alpha coefficient of .59, andChen et al. (2004) found a coefficient of .64before training in teamwork skills and .82 aftertraining. Athanasaw (2003) obtained coeffi-cients of .66 for the complete scale and between.25 and .48 for each of the five competencies,and Leach et al. (2005) found a coefficient of.70. As McClough and Rogelberg point out,however, the TWKSAT was designed from amultidimensional standpoint and other situa-tional judgment tests share the low alpha coef-ficients found to date. In this type of test, inter-nal consistency-based reliability measuresshould be complemented with test–retest esti-mations (Clevenger, Pereira, Wiechmann,Schmitt, & Harvey, 2001; McDaniel, Morge-son, Finnegan, Campion, & Braverman, 2001).However, Chen et al. reported a test–retest cor-

103TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 5: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

relation of .60 in the control group for theirstudy. Overall, the available evidence suggeststhat the reliability of the TWKSAT could beimproved, despite the test’s predictive capacity.

No studies have been carried out to date toidentify the reasons for the TWKSAT’s lowreliability, either by examining the metric char-acteristics of the items or the degree to whichthe different contents proposed by Stevens andCampion (1994) are actually present in theTWKSAT. Nor has any attempt been made toexplain the dimensional structure of theTWKSAT, although this is crucial to identify-ing the scoring obtained from the test.

Overall, our main objective in this study wasto present a new measure of the teamwork-related competencies based on Stevens andCampion’s (1994) original model. To this end,we conducted three empirical studies. The firstwas devoted to analyzing the metric propertiesof the items and the content validity and dimen-sionality of the TWKSAT. In the second study,we proposed a new measure, testing the im-provements made on reliability and dimension-ality in a sample of university students. Finally,in the third study, we tested the validity of thenew measure in a sample of professional em-ployees.

Study 1

The aim of this first study was to analyze theitems composing the TWKSAT, examiningtheir content and dimensionality. To begin, wepresent the results of the descriptive analysisbased on the impact of each item on reliability.Following the strategy employed by Rovinelliand Hambleton (1977), we then go on to ana-lyze the test items in terms of content validity.Next, the dimensionality of the test is examinedusing factor analysis.

Method

TWKSAT translation. The TWKSATcomprises 35 multiple-response items, whichdescribe different situations that may arisewithin a work team.1 Respondents answer theitems by indicating how they would act in eachsituation. The questionnaire evaluates the fiveteamwork competencies identified by Stevensand Campion (1994). An example item is thefollowing (Stevens & Campion, 1999):

Your team wants to improve the quality and flow of theconversations among its members. Your team should:(A) use comments that build upon and connect to whatothers have already said; (B) set up a specific order foreveryone to speak and then follow it; (C) let teammembers with more to say determine the direction andtopic of conversation; (D) do all of the above.

The original version of the TWKSAT is writ-ten in English. We therefore applied the back-translation procedure (Brislin, 1970) to use thescale in Spanish and ensure the equivalence ofitems (Gibson, 1999). Three certified translatorsand a group of specialists from bilingual teamsin the United States and Spain were involved inthis process. The materials forming the testwere first translated into Spanish by the threetranslators. The Spanish version of the test wasthen translated back into English by the bilin-gual specialists. Finally, the back-translationwas reviewed by the research team to ensurethat the meaning of the items was consistent inthe different translations. As a final result, weobtained a scale in Spanish equivalent to theEnglish-language original.

Participants and procedure. The partici-pants in the study were 135 students from dif-ferent disciplines (78% information technologyengineering and 22% psychology) at a publicuniversity in Madrid. Men made up 71% of thetotal sample, and the average age was 23 years(SD 0.93). All of the participants were fromSpain and had Spanish as their mother tongue.

Participation in the study was mandatory forthe students, being part of a program of prac-tices in different subjects. Participants signed aconsent form regarding their participation in thestudy. They could voluntarily decline to be en-rolled in the research by not providing theirrecords without any adverse effect on their classgrades. At the beginning of the academic se-mester, the researchers administered theTWKSAT to the students as a part of their classrequirements. The objectives and reasoning be-hind the study were explained by the professorsat the end of the course. Each participant wasalso given a brief report with the scores ob-

1 The TWKSAT is a commercial test and we therefore donot provide the content of the items included in the test so asto protect the intellectual property rights of its authors. The testcan be acquired at http://www.vangent-hcm.com/Solutions/SelectionAssessments/SkillsAbilitiesAssessments/. We use theoriginal numbering of the items in our presentation of results tofacilitate understanding.

104 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 6: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

tained on the test and the meaning of each of theteamwork competencies.

The validity of the TWKSAT content wasanalyzed by three experts in the field of team-work. All raters had more than 10 years’ expe-rience in conducting team-based research andconsulting. The experts analyzed the TWKSATitems as described in the Results section.

Results

Description of items. Table 1 shows themean (difficulty index), standard deviation,

asymmetry, kurtosis, and discrimination indexfor each item on the scale, as well as the Cron-bach’s alpha estimated for all 35 items together.As shown, the range of difficulty varies between.08 (Item 26) and .93 (Item 6). With regard tothe discrimination index, four items (7, 12, 27,and 35) are negatively correlated with the scaleand another series of items (2, 4, 10, 11, 15, 17,24, and 25) display a positive but very lowadjusted correlation (.15). The total alpha forthe scale is .60. However, if the negativelycorrelated items and those with a correlation of

Table 1Descriptive Statistics for Study 1 Teamwork Knowledge, Skills, Attitudes TestOriginal Items

Item Mean SD

Discrimination index(corrected item-total

correlation) Skewness Kurtosis

01 0.59 0.49 .22 0.80 1.3402 0.39 0.49 .03 0.68 1.5303 0.79 0.41 .18 1.62 0.6304 0.68 0.47 .01 1.44 0.0805 0.53 0.50 .17 0.23 1.9406 0.93 0.25 .35 5.39 27.0207 0.49 0.50 .07 0.02 1.9908 0.91 0.29 .37 4.08 14.6009 0.34 0.47 .19 0.25 1.9310 0.72 0.45 .13 1.26 0.4011 0.50 0.50 .14 0.46 1.7812 0.29 0.45 .01 0.70 1.4913 0.71 0.46 .15 1.01 0.9614 0.29 0.45 .22 0.69 1.5115 0.70 0.46 .10 0.88 1.2216 0.11 0.32 .30 0.60 1.6317 0.69 0.46 .02 0.79 1.3718 0.16 0.37 .31 0.15 1.9719 0.70 0.46 .30 0.86 1.2520 0.76 0.43 .35 1.62 0.6321 0.56 0.50 .21 1.06 0.8522 0.61 0.49 .29 0.60 1.6323 0.50 0.50 .27 0.33 1.8824 0.40 0.49 .01 0.02 1.9925 0.32 0.47 .02 0.43 1.8026 0.08 0.28 .24 0.92 1.1427 0.38 0.49 .06 0.01 1.9928 0.68 0.47 .36 1.18 0.5829 0.22 0.41 .15 1.15 0.6730 0.15 0.36 .41 0.13 1.9831 0.64 0.48 .15 1.00 0.9932 0.09 0.29 .34 0.31 1.8933 0.61 0.49 .12 0.40 1.8334 0.23 0.42 .10 0.76 1.4135 0.50 0.50 .05 0.02 1.99

Note. N 135. .60.

105TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 7: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

less than .15 are discarded, the alpha scale risesto .71.

Content analysis. To examine the validityof the test content, we employed the strategyproposed by Rovinelli and Hambleton (1977) inwhich a group of experts expresses the degree towhich each item measures each of the previ-ously defined evaluation objectives. Accord-ingly, three experts separately analyzed the 35items composing the test, indicating the extentto which each item was representative of eachof the teamwork subcompetencies defined byStevens and Campion (1994) and relevant to themeasurement of the target. A score of 1 wasassigned if the item was considered appropriate,1 if not, and 0 if the expert in question wasunsure. The resulting index will be 1 when all ofthe experts concur that an item is appropriate inview of the target measured. The experts werealso asked to make any general comments onthe measurement items included.

Table 2 summarizes the results. A number ofitems display a high level of item–target con-gruence (.80), other items show a congruenceindex of less than .50 for all of the measurementtargets (27, 29, and 33), and Item 31 falls shortof the threshold value of .30 for inclusion in thetable (Thorn & Deitz, 1989). Table 2 also pro-vides valuable information related to the degreeto which the test items meet the proposed mea-surement objectives. There are no items associ-ated with the subcompetencies A2 (“Recognizethe type and source of conflict”), C1 (“Under-stand and use communication networks”), andD2 (“Monitor, assess and provide feedback onindividual and group performance”). Moreover,the number of items that are congruent with thedifferent measurement targets varies between asingle highly congruent item (23) for A3 (“Usewin–win strategies”) and four items (1, 4, 11,and 19) for D1 (“Help set specific, challengingand accepted objectives”).

Finally, all of the experts remarked in theircomments that the test items allow assessmentof only the respondents’ knowledge, althoughthe original Stevens and Campion (1994) modelrefers to “teamwork knowledge, skills and abil-ities.”

Dimensionality analysis. A confirmatoryfactor analysis (N 135) was carried out on thecorrelation matrix to examine the dimensional-ity of the test. As these are dichotomous (right/wrong) items, the tetrachoric correlation matrix

was analyzed using Mplus (Muthén & Muthén,2006). Following the substantive model pro-posed by Stevens and Campion (1994, 1999),we tested five models: (a) five-factor orthogonalmodel, (b) two-factor orthogonal model, (c)five-factor correlated model, (d) two-factor cor-related model, and (e) one-factor model.

Table 3 presents the fit indices established forthe different estimated models. The orthogonalsolutions of the five-factor and two-factor mod-els do not fit the data. Values in each index aresubstantially below the usual standards. How-ever, when correlation between factors is al-lowed, the model fits the data better. Even so,the only adjustment index for which a good fit isobtained is the root mean square error of ap-proximation (.05). A similar effect is ob-served with a single-factor model.

In addition, to deeply explore the dimension-ality of the items proposed, we conducted anexhaustive exploratory factor analysis of thedifferent factorial solutions from one to sevenfactors, including a review of both orthogonaland oblique rotations. The results do not allowus to clearly label the factors according to theStevens and Campion model (1994).

Criterion validity analysis. To providefurther evidence of the criterion-related validityof the TWKSAT, we correlated the scores in theTWKSAT against the criterion for individualperformance in a team task. A subsample of thestudy (n 30; 56% women; average age 23years) formed by the participants in an under-graduate teamwork course was observed duringthe resolution of a group decision-making task.The participants were randomly assigned tofive-member teams. The task required the teamsto generate effective measures to resolve trafficproblems caused by improper parking on a uni-versity campus.2 The teams were allowed 30min to complete the task and were video-recorded for subsequent analysis. Both teamtasks and video recording were standard fea-tures of the course, and the participants weretherefore familiar with these procedures. An adhoc code (see Appendix A) was designed forbehavioral observation. The code categories de-scribe specific behaviors associated with thefive teamwork competencies identified by Ste-vens and Campion (1994, 1999; e.g., “Request

2 The task materials are available on request.

106 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 8: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

additional information from team members”).Two groups of five judges independently ana-lyzed the recordings, noting the frequency ofteamwork behaviors associated with a particular

competency displayed by each of the teammembers. All of the judges were postgraduatestudents who were blind to the objectives of thestudy and independent of the research group.

Table 2Teamwork Knowledge, Skills, Attitudes Test Congruence Indexes

Item A1 A2 A3 B1 B2 C1 C2 C3 C4 C5 D1 D2 E1 E2

06 0.88 0.3507 0.65 0.4723 0.9210 0.55 0.3712 0.6426 0.6522 0.9727 0.3128 0.9229 0.3115 0.9632 0.7933 0.4534 0.6409 0.5816 0.94 0.7630 0.6014 0.58 0.9424 0.40 0.9435 0.59 0.9502 0.7917 0.9901 0.9504 0.9211 0.9719 1.0018 0.6208 0.88 0.7113 0.6220 0.7821 0.95 0.4125 0.55 0.3705 0.5903 0.71 0.8831

Note. A1 The knowledge, skills, and attitudes (KSA) to recognize and encourage desirable, but discourage undesirable, teamconflict; A2 The KSA to recognize the type and source of conflict confronting the team and to implement an appropriateconflict resolution strategy; A3 The KSA to employ an integrative negotiation strategy rather than the traditional distributivestrategy; B1 The KSA to identify situations requiring participative group problem solving and to utilize the proper degree andtype of participation; B2 The KSA to recognize the obstacles to collaborative group problem solving and implement appropriatecorrective actions; C1 The KSA to understand communication networks and to utilize decentralized networks to enhance thecommunication where possible; C2 The KSA to communicate openly and supportively; C3 The KSA to listen nonevalu-atively and appropriately use active listening techniques; C4 The KSA to maximize consonance between nonverbal and verbalmessages, and to recognize and interpret the nonverbal messages of others; C5 The KSA to engage in ritual greetings and smalltalk, and a recognition of their importance; D1 The KSA to help establish specific, challenging, and accepted team goals; D2 The KSA to monitor, evaluate, and provide feedback on both overall team performance and individual team member perfor-mance; E1 The KSA to coordinate and synchronize activities, information, and task interdependencies between members; E2 The KSA to help establish task and role expectations of individual team members and to ensure proper balancing of workloadin the team. To facilitate reading the indexes, indexes .30 have been eliminated; indexes .80 appear in bold italic type; andthe greater index obtained for each item appears in bold type.

107TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 9: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

They were trained in the use of the code andunderwent a trial period to align criteria anddiscuss inconsistencies. The mean interjudgereliability for the different competencies wasacceptable ( .87); therefore, we computedthe mean of their assessments to obtain a singlescore for each competency. Finally, we com-puted the mean of the scores for each individualand the five competencies, as in the case of thequestionnaire, to obtain a total teamwork com-petency score. As expected, the TWKSAT scorewas positively correlated with the observedmeasure of teamwork skills (r .43, p .05).

Discussion

The first study revealed certain weaknesses inthe TWKSAT (Stevens & Campion, 1999) thataffect its use in academic and professional con-texts. Specifically, the following deficiencieswere observed in three different facets: (a)Some items show very low reliability indicesaffecting the general reliability of the scale, (b)the contents of the original model proposed byStevens and Campion (1994) are not fully rep-resented in the test, and (c) the dimensionalstructure obtained from the factor analysis is notwell aligned with the substantive model.

With regard to the first weakness, the resultsof the study show that certain items should beeliminated from the scale, as their adjusted cor-relations with the total for the scale are less thanthe generally accepted standards.

Turning to the second weakness, the contentanalysis indicates that certain teamwork sub-competencies identified in the general modelare not picked up by the test items. Thus, noitems exist associated with the subcompetenciesA2 (“Recognize the type and source of con-flict”), C1 (“Understand and use communica-

tion networks”), and D2 (“Monitor, assess andprovide feedback on individual and group per-formance”). The content analysis also revealedthat certain items are not associated with any ofthe proposed measurement targets (Items 27,29, 31, and 33). In addition, the format of theitems is designed to measure “knowledge,” butnot skills or aptitudes.

Finally, Stevens and Campion (1994) estab-lished 14 teamwork subcompetencies in theirmodel, grouped into five competencies, whichare, in turn, integrated in two dimensions. How-ever, our results indicate a structure that tendstoward unidimensionality.

To sum up, the results of this study providean explanation of the low reliability indicesreported in previous research and suggest how amore reliable measurement could be obtainedfrom the TWKSAT without affecting its predic-tive capacity. In line with prior studies using theoriginal English version of the TWKSAT (Chenet al., 2004; McClough & Rogelberg, 2003;Stevens & Campion, 1999), the Spanish adap-tation of the test used in our study reveals sim-ilar correlation indices (r .43, p .05).

It is necessary to recognize that our resultsmay be influenced by cultural differences be-tween U.S.-based samples used by Stevens andCampion (1999) to develop and validate theTWKSAT and our Spain-based sample. Cross-cultural studies (e.g., Earley & Erez, 1997; Hof-stede, 1980, 1983a, 1983b) show that culturaldifferences may affect the processes and out-comes of individuals and groups in organiza-tions (Cox, Lobel, & McLeod, 1991; Gibson,1999). Thus, in contrast to North Americans,Spaniards tend to be (a) higher in collectivism,so they tend to put group interests first (Trian-dis, 1995) and look more actively for socialacceptance, strong group identity, and the de-velopment of personal relationships (Grimm,Church, Katigbak, & Reyes, 1999); (b) higherin power distance, leading them to behave moresubmissively with managers and avoid dis-agreements and feeling more comfortable work-ing in teams with strong directivity (Earley &Erez, 1997); (c) lower in action orientation(Maznevski, DiStefano, Gomez, Noorderhaven,& Wu, 1997), which could make them work lesshard (Hampden-Turner & Trompenaars, 1993)and be more resistant to work by objectives(Kluckhohn & Strodtbeck, 1961); and (d) morelikely to believe that external forces determine

Table 3Model Fit Indexes

Model RMSEA CFI TLI

Five factor (orthogonal) .082 .067 .008Two factor (orthogonal) .07 .324 .282Five factor (correlated) .04 .776 .762Two factor (correlated) .038 .807 .792One factor .041 .774 .76

N 135; RMSEA root mean square error of approxi-mation; CFI confirmatory fit index; TLI Tucker–Lewisfit index.

108 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 10: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

their successes and failures (Trompenaars,1993). According to these cultural differencesbetween Spanish and North American popula-tions, the way employees approach their profes-sional performance in teams and, therefore, theway employees respond to the TWKSAT maybe different. Thus, generalization of our find-ings to the context of teamwork in North Amer-ican cultures should be done with caution.

To conclude, our findings suggest the need toimprove the measurement of teamwork compe-tencies. The TWKSAT allows measurement ofonly the General Teamwork competency, whichconstricts examination of the conceptual rich-ness expressed in Stevens and Campion’s(1994) original model. Consequently, it is de-sirable to develop new items capable of captur-ing the different subcompetencies, as well asobtaining more reliable scores in the five com-petencies.

Study 2

The second study analyzed a new measure ofthe teamwork knowledge, skills, and abilitiesbased on Stevens and Campion’s (1994) model,which is called the Teamwork Competency Test(TWCT). For this purpose, we developed andadapted new items in light of the findings fromStudy 1. The new test comprises 36 items,which were administered to a sample of 120university students to analyze the functioning ofthe items and to test the TWCT’s reliability,content validity, and dimensionality.

Method

Item development for TWCT. Based onresults from Study 1, we developed new itemsto represent the 14 subcompetencies establishedby Stevens and Campion (1994). The itemswere worded in the observable behavior format.An initial set of 83 items was constructed,which was progressively refined to the final 36items. Based on interviews with three experts inthe fields of teamwork and organizational be-havior, several items were rephrased (sevenitems) or left out (31 items). The remaining 52items were applied to different samples and,taking into account the item statistics (correcteditem-total correlation and factor loadings), 16items were deleted. Using the selected 36 items,we conducted a pilot study on 26 members of

four software development programmers’ teamsfrom a small information technology company.All team members responded to the question-naire and provided feedback on the items. Mostitems were understood and perceived as unam-biguous. Some minor changes were made basedon their comments, but all the items were re-tained. As a result, the new measure contains 36items drafted in Spanish (see the complete scalein Appendix B) and uses a 4-point responsescale of frequency (1 never/almost never and4 always/almost always).

Participants and procedure. The samplecomprised 120 final-year psychology studentsat a large Spanish public university, 68.7% ofwhom were women. The average age was 23years (SD 0.96). The procedure was identicalto that in Study 1. The assistance of the sameexperts was used to analyze the validity of thetest content as in Study 1.

Results

Description of items and scales. Table 4summarizes the main descriptive statistics forthe questionnaire items. The mean scores for allitems were above 2.5 (the theoretical midpointon the response scale), except those related tothe Collaborative Problem Solving dimension,in which the averages were somewhat lower(between 1.94 and 2.28). The reliability indicesfor both the scales and the full questionnaire(.89) were satisfactory.

Content analysis. Table 4 presents thecongruence indices (Rovinelli & Hambleton,1977) for each item based on the subcompeten-cies proposed by Stevens and Campion (1994).The congruence indices for all of the items weresatisfactory (minimum 0.56, maximum 1.00). Finally, the whole content domain estab-lished in the model was represented by the itemsdeveloped.

Dimensionality analysis. We carried outvarious factor analyses to explore the dimen-sionality of the questionnaire above and beyondthe adequacy and relevance of the items. Giventhe high means for the items and the deviationof distributions away from normal, we used theminimum unweighted least squares method forfactor extraction, employing the FACTOR pro-gram (Lorenzo-Seva & Ferrando, 2006). Bar-tlett’s sphericity test (2 5557.6, df 703,p .001) and the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin index

109TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 11: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Table 4Teamwork Competency Test Item Statistics, Congruence Indexes, and Scale Reliabilities

Content

Item M SD A1 A2 A3 B1 B2 C1 C2 C3 C4 C5 D1 D2 E1 E2

1 3.34 0.67 1.0016 3.00 0.75 1.0019 2.91 0.74 0.56 0.8321 3.12 0.75 0.77 0.63 0.57 3.41 0.60 0.69 0.83

10 2.47 0.89 0.643 2.88 0.79 0.94

15 3.27 0.68 0.81 0.8111 3.00 0.86 0.8125 3.04 0.84 1.0014 2.71 0.74 0.7024 2.51 0.81 0.8326 2.61 0.76 1.005 2.83 0.70 0.95

17 3.27 0.70 0.9528 3.28 0.69 1.0029 3.31 0.68 0.692 3.43 0.61 0.78

27 3.37 0.63 1.008 3.57 0.61 0.899 3.13 0.75 0.83

12 3.25 0.79 1.0030 3.23 0.78 1.0035 2.70 0.75 1.006 2.99 0.76 0.87

20 3.03 0.81 0.9522 2.91 0.83 0.9531 3.00 0.77 1.0032 3.06 0.78 1.0036 2.86 0.80 0.954 3.06 0.72 0.87

23 3.33 0.75 1.0033 3.11 0.68 0.8734 3.07 0.68 0.8713 3.65 0.60 0.8918 3.05 0.73 1.00

Total 112.9 12.00

Note. A1 The knowledge, skills, and attitudes (KSA) to recognize and encourage desirable, but discourage undesirable,team conflict; A2 The KSA to recognize the type and source of conflict confronting the team and to implement anappropriate conflict resolution strategy; A3 The KSA to employ an integrative negotiation strategy rather than thetraditional distributive strategy; B1 The KSA to identify situations requiring participative group problem solving and toutilize the proper degree and type of participation; B2 The KSA to recognize the obstacles to collaborative group problemsolving and implement appropriate corrective actions; C1 The KSA to understand communication networks and to utilizedecentralized networks to enhance the communication where possible; C2 The KSA to communicate openly andsupportively; C3 The KSA to listen nonevaluatively and appropriately use active listening techniques; C4 The KSAto maximize consonance between nonverbal and verbal messages, and to recognize and interpret the nonverbal messagesof others; C5 The KSA to engage in ritual greetings and small talk, and a recognition of their importance; D1 The KSAto help establish specific, challenging, and accepted team goals; D2 The KSA to monitor, evaluate, and provide feedbackon both overall team performance and individual team member performance; E1 The KSA to coordinate and synchronizeactivities, information, and task interdependencies between members; E2 The KSA to help establish task and roleexpectations of individual team members and to ensure proper balancing of workload in the team.

110 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 12: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

(.66) confirmed that the item correlation matrixcould be factorized. The factorial solution wasobliquely rotated. Eleven factors were found tohave an eigenvalue of more than 1.00 afterextraction. The parallel analysis retained onlythe first eight factors extracted. In line with thisanalysis, the eight-factor model was the one thatbest reflected the initial substantive model, ex-plaining 56% of the total variance, with only18% residual errors of more than .05. The eight-factor solution was compared with a five-factorsolution, as proposed in the substantive model.The percentage variance explained was found tobe lower (43%), as was the percentage of resid-ual errors greater than .05 (33%). In addition,the residual mean squares error average showeda marginally acceptable value (.085) in theeight-factor model, but a clearly unacceptablevalue in the five-factor model (.11). Therefore,it appears that the eight-factor model better re-produces the analyzed data matrix comparedwith the five-factor model.

Table 5 presents the results obtained in theconfiguration matrix after oblique rotation. Al-though this matrix does not exactly reproducethe expected structure of 14 subcompetencies, adetailed analysis indicates that it reflects thesubstantive model—with some variations—onwhich the development of the items was based.The first factor extracted represents the ConflictResolution competency. The items developed inrelation to this competency (especially thosereferring to the recognition of conflict: Items 1,10, 15, 16, 19, and 21) are associated with thisfactor, as are the Communication competencyitems related to open communication and sup-port. They are joined by Item 18 (designed forthe Planning and Coordination competency).The second factor extracted reflects the Plan-ning and Coordination competency (Items 4,13, 23, 33, and 34). This factor also includesItem 8 (designed for the Communication com-petency). The items for the Goal Setting andPerformance Management competency appearin Factors 3 and 6. Items related to “monitoring,assessing and providing feedback on individualand group performance” (Items 6, 22, 31, and35) are associated with Factor 3, and thosereferring to “offering teammates feedback ontheir results” (Items 20, 32, and 36) are associ-ated with Factor 6. Factors 4 and 5 reflect theCollaborative Problem Solving competency.Factor 4 is associated with items referring to

“recognizing obstacles to participative problemsolving” (Items 14, 24, and 26), and Factor 5 isassociated with items referring to “identifyingsituations that require participation in decisionmaking” (Items 11 and 25). Finally, Factors 7and 8 reflect the Communication competency.The items related to “active listening, nonverbalcommunication and recognition of communica-tion networks” (Items 2, 3, 5, 7, 9, and 27) areassociated with Factor 7, and those related to“informal communication acts with other teammembers” (Items 12 and 30) are associated withFactor 8.

Finally, we performed a confirmatory factoranalysis (N 120) on the scores obtained forthe eight factors to verify whether the eightfactors extracted are associated with two gen-eral dimensions, as proposed in the substantivemodel, or with a single dimension as Study 1suggests. The following factors were assignedto the Interpersonal competencies dimension inthe two-dimensional model (see Figure 2): 1(Conflict Resolution), 4 (Collaborative ProblemSolving: Group), 5 (Collaborative ProblemSolving: Individual), 7 (Communication: ActiveListening), and 8 (Communication: Informal).The following factors were assigned to the Self-Management dimension: 2 (Planning), 3 (Per-formance Objective Management: Monitoring),and 7 (Performance Objective Management:Feedback).

In view of the weightings assigned to thedifferent variables, all of the parameters esti-mated in the two-factor model are significant(p .001), except for the relationship betweenFactor 4 and Factor 5 and the InterpersonalRelations dimension (p .001 and p .040,respectively). All of the parameters estimatedare again significant (p .001) for a one-factormodel, except for the relationship between Fac-tor 4 and Factor 5 and the General Teamworkfactor (p .002 and p .042, respectively).None of the standardized residuals attains avalue of |res_z| 2 for the one-factor model.However, a high value, |res_z| 2, appears inthe two-factor model for the relationship be-tween Factor 1 and Factor 7, although this is notan especially large deviation.

The values obtained for the goodness-of-fitestimators for both models indicate a good fit inboth models. For the one-factor model and two-factor model, the values were, respectively,2 0.181 and 0.242, standardized root mean

111TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 13: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

residual .048 and .047, root mean squareerror of approximation .037 and .031, com-parative fit index .976 and .983, and Tucker–Lewis index .966 and .975 (Figure 2).

Discussion

Overall, the results of the second study showthat the TWCT reasonably covers the wholecontent domain proposed by Stevens and Cam-pion (1994), is reliable, and its dimensionalstructure adequately reflects the original sub-

stantive model (although it does not do so ex-actly). With regard to the first point, the inter-judge analysis indicates that the TWCT itemsare representative of the 14 subcompetenciesdefined by the authors. In terms of reliability,the five scales present adequate Cronbach’s al-phas of more than .80, except the Conflict Res-olution scale, which has an alpha of .71. Thealpha for the total scale is also adequate (.89).Finally, the dimensionality analysis reveals alatent structure for the TWCT that is substan-

Table 5Exploratory Factor Analysis for Teamwork Competency Test Items Configuration Matrix

Factor

Item 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

28 0.93 0.09 0.05 0.02 0.10 0.02 0.03 0.0517 0.92 0.12 0.02 0.05 0.07 0.02 0.04 0.0829 0.90 0.11 0.03 0.05 0.10 0.06 0.06 0.1215 0.47 0.08 0.05 0.04 0.16 0.07 0.02 0.0419 0.35 0.03 0.05 0.06 0.09 0.16 0.19 0.0318 0.29 0.00 0.25 0.09 0.03 0.06 0.17 0.0301 0.24 0.07 0.12 0.10 0.24 0.14 0.16 0.0210 0.23 0.02 0.15 0.08 0.09 0.06 0.14 0.0521 0.21 0.05 0.06 0.13 0.04 0.29 0.03 0.0816 0.18 0.13 0.20 0.01 0.13 0.15 0.09 0.0534 0.05 0.94 0.02 0.01 0.03 0.10 0.06 0.0333 0.02 0.91 0.04 0.03 0.04 0.06 0.03 0.0104 0.05 0.84 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.09 0.07 0.0523 0.01 0.55 0.13 0.05 0.05 0.06 0.12 0.0208 0.04 0.22 0.01 0.01 0.05 0.07 0.11 0.0813 0.00 0.19 0.13 0.02 0.05 0.02 0.18 0.0631 0.06 0.05 0.93 0.03 0.02 0.01 0.06 0.0306 0.06 0.01 0.91 0.00 0.02 0.02 0.05 0.0122 0.00 0.05 0.87 0.02 0.01 0.01 0.03 0.0735 0.12 0.01 0.11 0.10 0.03 0.05 0.01 0.0826 0.02 0.01 0.01 0.95 0.06 0.03 0.07 0.0014 0.02 0.01 0.09 0.89 0.07 0.02 0.06 0.0124 0.12 0.03 0.08 0.80 0.05 0.02 0.03 0.0111 0.08 0.02 0.03 0.02 0.98 0.03 0.08 0.0225 0.07 0.02 0.03 0.05 0.93 0.03 0.10 0.0320 0.06 0.10 0.02 0.01 0.04 0.89 0.03 0.0232 0.09 0.14 0.02 0.05 0.01 0.85 0.03 0.0836 0.11 0.06 0.13 0.04 0.00 0.53 0.08 0.0927 0.08 0.06 0.01 0.04 0.07 0.09 0.90 0.0802 0.10 0.05 0.02 0.06 0.10 0.05 0.79 0.0303 0.16 0.04 0.08 0.00 0.03 0.13 0.39 0.0307 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.09 0.10 0.02 0.36 0.1505 0.09 0.03 0.14 0.01 0.07 0.08 0.25 0.0909 0.08 0.16 0.03 0.17 0.11 0.11 0.25 0.0030 0.02 0.01 0.05 0.01 0.03 0.03 0.03 1.0012 0.02 0.04 0.01 0.01 0.00 0.07 0.06 0.90

Note. N 120; Extraction unweighted least squares; Rotation Oblimin–Kaiser; Convergence 12 iterations. Itemsrationally linked with the factor are in bold type. Items not rationally linked with the factor and up to .10 value are in bolditalic type.

112 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 14: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

1,00

Interpersonal

1,00

Self-management

Communicaon

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

3,65

,52 ,25

,53

1,31

1,52

1,49

1,40

-2,17

2,05

1,60

2,13

1,40

2,61

1,96

1,91

,88

Conflict

Decision

Communicaon

Informal

Planning and

Monitoring and

Offering

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

1,00

e

2,43

2,05

1,60

2,10

1,41

2,63

2,02

1,98

1,00

Teamwork

3,48 ,50

,25

1,44

,52

1,28

1,44

1,40

Resoluon

ProblemSolving

Making

Communicaon

Coordinaon

Assessing

Feedback

ConflictResoluon

ProblemSolving

DecisionMaking

InformalCommunicaon

Planning andCoordinaon

Monitoring and

Assessing

OfferingFeedback

Figure 2. Confirmatory factor analysis solutions for the one-factor and two-factor models.

113TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 15: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

tially associated with Stevens and Campion’s(1994) model. However, the structure observeddoes not exactly reproduce either the structureof the 14 subcompetencies or the structure ofthe five competencies.

Our results suggest the need to reconfigurethe originally designed scales. This is particu-larly important regarding the Conflict Resolu-tion competency. The analyses of both dimen-sionality and congruence reveal a strongassociation between the Conflict Resolutionitems and the Communication items (related toopen communication and support). It seems rea-sonable to integrate the items in the Communi-cation subcompetency into the Conflict Resolu-tion competency, as the factor analysisindicates. This is also true with regard to Item18, which both the judges and the factor anal-ysis associate with that competency. The Con-flict Resolution scale with an alpha of .84 wouldthus be established with the same items as orig-inally designed. The Collaborative ProblemSolving scale is also maintained intact, integrat-ing the Factor 4 and Factor 5 items with analpha of .83. The Communication scale containsthe Factor 7 and Factor 8 items associated withcommunication items, except for open commu-nication and support, obtaining an alpha of .72.The Objective Management and Performancecompetency would be formed by all of the itemsfrom Factors 3 and 6 with an alpha of .82.Finally, the Planning and Coordination compe-tency integrates all of the items associated withFactor 2 ( .88), except Item 18, which ismoved to Conflict Resolution.

Study 3

The third study had a dual objective: (1) toobtain initial evidence for the convergent valid-ity of the new measure of teamwork compe-tency developed in Study 2, and (2) to comparethe new measure with the original test proposedby Stevens and Campion (1999). A sample ofemployees was used to analyze the relationshipbetween the TWCT and (a) the originalTWKSAT, (b) supervisor assessments of em-ployees’ teamwork competencies, and (c) self-assessments of competency by employeesthemselves.

Method

Sample. A total of 91 employees workingin a major Spanish power utility took part in thestudy. Men made up 81% of the total sample,and the average age was 29.6 years (SD 3.38). The measurements were made as part ofthe activities undertaken by the employeeswithin the framework of a mandatory skillsdevelopment program set up by the company.Participants signed a consent form regardingtheir participation in the study. They could de-cline to be enrolled in the research by not pro-viding their records without any adverse effecton their participation in the skills developmentprogram. We also obtained company consent touse the data in the research.

Measures.TWKSAT. We administered the original

version of the Stevens and Campion (1999) testused in Study 1.

TWCT. The version of the test designed inStudy 2 was administered. Six different mea-sures were established for each participant, con-sisting of a total score and a score for each ofthe five competencies.

Team performance: Supervisor assessment.The assessments of each employee’s immedi-ate superior were obtained specifically for thepurposes of the study and did not form part ofthe company’s usual performance manage-ment process. The information gathered onthe employees was strictly training-related.The supervisor questionnaire contained eightitems describing different teamwork behav-iors, to which they were asked to respond ona 4-point scale (1 completely disagree, 4 completely agree). An example item is “He orshe (the employee) participates actively inwork meetings (giving his or her opinion,asking questions, etc.).” The items weredrawn from a competency model used in thecompany.

Team performance: Self-assessment. Thesame eight-item questionnaire expressed inthe first person was also answered by eachemployee to assess his/her usual behavior inwork team situations.

Procedure. The supervisors’ assessments(Measure 3) and the self-assessments of theemployees (Measure 4) were obtained con-currently at the beginning of the course. Thesupervisors gave their responses online via a

114 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 16: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

user/key connection to a web page containingthe questionnaire and instructions for comple-tion. The TWKSAT and TWCT measures (re-spectively, Measures 1 and 2) were collectedat a first training session 1 week later. Bothquestionnaires were administered in a paper-and-pencil format, counterbalanced to avoideffects associated with the order of presenta-tion.

Results

Table 6 summarizes the descriptive statisticsand intercorrelations for all of the variables inthe study. The reliability of the two criteriameasures used was .70 for team performancesupervisor evaluation and .54 for team perfor-mance self-evaluation. The two versions of theTWKSAT were positively correlated (r .47,p .01), as were the assessments made by thesupervisors and the employees (r .25, p .05). As in Study 2, the competencies evaluatedin the TWCT were positively correlated (exceptCompetency B: Collaborative Problem Solv-ing). Also, the competencies as captured by theTWCT were positively correlated with the orig-inal TWKSAT, except Competency B.

The supervisor assessment correlation withthe TWCT was .34 (p .01) compared with acorrelation of .26 (p .05) with the TWKSAT.These correlations are not statistically different(T 0.76, p .05). A similar effect wasobserved in the employee self-assessments,which showed a correlation of .39 (p .01)with the TWCT and .21 (p .05) with theTWKSAT. These correlations are statistically

different (T 1.78, p .05). Analysis of theTWCT competencies shows that CompetenciesA (Conflict Resolution), D (Goal Setting andPerformance Management), and E (Planningand Coordination) are positively correlated bothwith the supervisor’s assessment (r .24, p .05; r .36, p .01; r .27, p .01,respectively) and with employees’ self-assess-ments (r .30, p .01; r .38, p .01; r .37, p .01, respectively).

Finally, we performed a hierarchical regres-sion analysis examining the proportion of thevariance in the scores obtained from the super-visors’ assessments due to the TWKSAT andTWCT, respectively. In the first step, theTWKSAT score was introduced, and the TWCTscore was introduced in a second step. Table 7indicates that the percentage of explained vari-ance increases significantly at 6.3%, R2 .063, p .05; F(2.88) 6.60; .285, p .05, when the TWCT score is introduced.

Discussion

The results of the third study provide favor-able empirical evidence for the convergent va-lidity of the TWCT. The total score in this newversion of the test is positively correlated withthe other measures in the study, namely (a) thescore in the original Stevens and Campion test(1999), (b) the supervisors’ assessments of theteamwork behavior of employees in their rou-tine work, and (c) the self-assessments made bythe employees themselves. Furthermore, the re-sults indicate that the Conflict Resolution, GoalSetting and Performance Management, and

Table 6Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations for Study 3 Measures

Measure M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

1. TWKSAT 21.9 2.3 .372. TWCT Total 109.0 10.9 .84 .47

3. TWCT KSA_A 30.5 4.6 .84 .36 .81

4. TWCT KSA_B 13.6 2.6 .26 .10 .42 .075. TWCT KSA_C 25.0 2.8 .35 .37 .59 .39 .006. TWCT KSA_D 19.9 3.3 .66 .41 .81 .59 .24 .33

7. TWCT KSA_E 19.9 2.4 .55 .33 .72 .43 .36 .29 .51

8. Team performance supervisor evaluation 16.2 3.3 .70 .26 .34 .24 .13 .16 .36 .27

9. Team performance self-evaluation 23.4 2.8 .54 .21 .39 .30 .08 .19 .38 .37 .25

Note. TWKSAT Teamwork Knowledge, Skills, Attitudes Test; TWCT Teamwork Competency Test; N 91;KSA_A Conflict Resolution; KSA_B Collaborative Problem Solving; KSA_C Communication; KSA_D GoalSetting and Performance Management; KSA_E Planning and Task Coordination. p .05. p .01.

115TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 17: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Planning and Coordination competencies arethe only competencies of the five contained inthe TWCT that correlate independently with thecriterion variables.

Contrary to our expectations, however, thecorrelations between the different subscales inthe test are not all either significant or positive.Specifically, Collaborative Problem Solving isweakly related to the other dimensions exceptGoal Setting and Performance Management andPlanning and Coordination.

Another interesting feature is the significant,positive correlation found between the originalTWKSAT and the supervisors’ assessments.This is in line with the original study carried outby Stevens and Campion (1999). Finally, com-parison of the predictive capacity of the twoversions of the TWKSAT suggests that theTWCT is a better predictor of the self-assessment of teamwork—but not of the super-visors’ assessments—than the original test.

General Discussion

Overall, the three studies highlight certainsignificant limitations in the original TWKSATdesigned by Stevens and Campion (1999), im-proving the metric characteristics of the test anddeveloping a new version. Available researchshows that the TWKSAT offers good predictivevalidity (McClough & Rogelberg, 2003), but itmay be improved in terms of reliability (e.g.,Athanasaw, 2003; Chen et al., 2004). Despitethe TWKSAT’s relevance as an appropriatemeasure of essential teamwork competencies,the absence of studies examining the causes ofthese reliability problems is surprising.

Based on the results of Study 1, (a) the reli-ability problems in the TWKSAT are due to the

poor functioning of certain items, (b) the do-main contents proposed by the authors are notsatisfactorily represented by the test items, and(c) the dimensionality of the test does not reflectthe original substantive model.

Study 2 presents the TWCT, which is a newdevelopment focused on the Stevens and Cam-pion (1999) model. The TWCT includes 36items in a 4-point frequency scale format anddrafted in “observable behaviors” statements.The dimensionality of the TWCT reproducesthe original substantive model better than itspredecessor, reflecting the assessment contentsdomain included in the model and offering re-liable scores in both the test total and each of themodel’s five dimensions.

Finally, Study 3 provides initial evidence forthe criterion validity of the TWCT comparedwith the original test in a sample of profession-als. The total TWCT score is positively corre-lated with the teamwork assessments made byboth supervisors and the employees themselves.Moreover, the TWCT predicts the supervisorassessment better than the TWKSAT.

Overall, our research provides empirical ev-idence concerning the method for valid, reliableteamwork competency assessments. The TWCToffers a more accurate measure of teamworkcompetency in terms of reliability than the orig-inal measure proposed by Stevens and Campion(1994), as well as greater conceptual richness interms of independent scores for the five compe-tencies identified by the authors. This impor-tantly allows differential analysis of the effectsof specific competencies on the performance ofdifferent types of teams. In addition, it makespossible the examination of whether improvingthe skills associated with a specific teamworkcompetency benefits the others. Our findingsalso help both academics and practitioners tobetter understand the low reliability indicatorsfor the TWKSAT and provide a new test(TWCT) that improves reliability and offers anindependent measure for each of the dimensionsproposed by Stevens and Campion (1999).

Considering the competencies measured bythe TWCT from the standpoint of the five majorteamwork dimensions proposed by Salas et al.(2005), the competencies measured at the indi-vidual level are found to be associated with twoof the core dimensions these authors describe,namely follow-up and monitoring of team per-formance and team orientation. However, other

Table 7Hierarchical Regression Analysis Over the TeamPerformance Supervisor Evaluation

Step df F R2

1 1, 89 6.42 .067

TWKSAT .259

2 2, 88 6.60 .063

TWKSAT .125TWCT .285

Note. TWKSAT Teamwork Knowledge, Skills, Atti-tudes Test; TWCT Teamwork Competency Test. p .05.

116 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 18: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

core dimensions of the model, such as teamleadership, replacement behaviors, and adapt-ability, do not appear in the TWCT. In addition,recent studies have shown the capacity ofknowledge tests on team roles related to thetask, team, and boundary-spanning activities toaccurately predict performance in teams (Mar-rone et al., 2007; Mumford et al., 2008). Futureresearch should consider this issue as theTWCT would gain in both quality and utility ifit included the mentioned dimensions.

Despite the above-mentioned contributions,our research is not without limitations. First, thesamples analyzed in the first two studies are ofa reasonable size but were provided by univer-sity students. If our results are to be generalized,the studies would need to be replicated in largersamples of employees. In particular, the factoranalysis conducted on the TWKSAT should bereplicated both in larger samples, given that oursample was smaller than the criterion of 10participants per item, and in American samplesto solve the potential effects of cultural differ-ences. As proposed by Ryan, Chan, Ployhart,and Slade (1999), beyond the translation of theTWKSAT, it is necessary to adapt the test con-sidering the equivalence of the measurementsobtained in culturally diverse populations. Sec-ond, the factor analyses carried out on theTWCT reveal a dimensional structure that issimilar to the substantive model employed butdoes not reflect it perfectly. Further studiesshould look into the reasons for this, particu-larly with regard to the functioning of the Col-laborative Problem Solving scale. In line withthe underlying theoretical model, it was ex-pected that this competency would correlatewith Conflict Management and Communica-tion, but only a weak association was found.Moreover, Collaborative Problem Solving cor-relates more closely with other competenciesrelated to team self-management. Analysis ofthe content of items (e.g., Item 11, “To addressthe trivial task-related issues, I do not need totalk first with all team members in order toreach a decision”) indicates that behaviors ofthis kind are related to effective coordination.

The number and type of competencies se-lected are critical issues. The competenciesoriginally proposed by Stevens and Campion(1994) do not exhaust the set of skills that couldbe considered to explain what people actuallydo in effective teamwork. For example, the

skills to develop shared mental models (Can-non-Bowers et al., 1995; Zaccaro, Rittman, &Marks, 2001), mutual trust (Webber, 2002), andteam leadership (Cannon-Bowers et al., 1995;Marks, Mathieu, & Zaccaro, 2001) may also becritical in explaining team performance. Asmentioned above, the development of measuresthat integrate these aspects would improve ourability to diagnose and predict teams’ perfor-mance based on the personal abilities of theindividuals who form them. Moreover, the useof advanced psychometric models like thosebased on item response theory (Hambleton &Swaminathan, 1985) would allow the design ofcomputerized adaptive tests able to optimize theadministration of a wide-ranging measure(Lord, 1970; Owen, 1975).

Finally, let us consider the practical implica-tions of this research. The information providedby the new TWCT is of great interest for peoplemanagement in organizations, as it shows man-agers, team leaders, and human resources pro-fessionals which employees’ competencies arecritical to select, train, and develop to improveteam performance.

By throwing light on candidates’ skills, thetest would facilitate selection and team compo-sition decisions. The access to information onfive different competencies means that the con-figuration of teams can be supported by a richerinformation base than would be possible usingonly a single general estimate. In addition, thedifferential diagnosis provided by the TWCT onthe stronger and weaker competencies of eachemployee will allow teams to customize train-ing and development interventions. This is par-ticularly valuable in view of the increasing de-mand for training programs to improveteamwork competencies (Chen et al., 2004).Finally, the behaviors made explicit by theTWCT could be used as a guide to determinethe behavioral anchors and sample behaviorsused in the design of performance assessmentand management tools.

References

Athanasaw, Y. A. (2003). Team characteristics andteam member knowledge, skills, and ability rela-tionships to the effectiveness of cross-functionalteams in the public sector. International Journalof Public Administration, 26, 1165–1203. doi:10.1081/PAD-120019926

117TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 19: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-translation for cross-cultural research. Journal of Cross-CulturalPsychology, 1, 185–216. doi:10.1177/135910457000100301

Cannon-Bowers, J. A., & Bowers, C. A. (2011).Team development and functioning. In S. Zedeck(Ed.), APA handbook of industrial and organiza-tional psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 597–650). Wash-ington, DC: American Psychological Association.doi:10.1037/12169-019

Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Tannenbaum, S. I., Salas, E.,& Volpe, C. E. (1995). Defining competencies andestablishing team training requirements. In R. A.Guzzo & E. Salas (Eds.), Team effectiveness anddecision making in organizations (pp. 333–380).San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Chen, G., Donahue, L. M., & Klimoski, R. J. (2004).Training undergraduates to work in organizationalteams. Academy of Management Learning and Ed-ucation, 3, 27– 40. doi:10.5465/AMLE.2004.12436817

Clevenger, J., Pereira, G. M., Wiechmann, D.,Schmitt, N., & Harvey, V. S. (2001). Incrementalvalidity of situational judgment tests. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 86, 410–417. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.3.410

Cox, T., Lobel, S., & McLeod, P. (1991). Effects ofethnic group cultural differences on cooperativeand competitive behavior on a group task. Acad-emy of Management Journal, 34, 827–847. doi:10.2307/256391

Earley, P. C., & Erez, M. (1997). The transplantedexecutive. New York, NY: Oxford UniversityPress.

Gibson, C. B. (1999). Do they do what they believethey can? Group efficacy and group effectivenessacross tasks and cultures. Academy of Manage-ment Journal, 42, 138–152. doi:10.2307/257089

Grimm, S. D., Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., & Reyes,J. A. S. (1999). Self-described traits, values, andmoods associated with individualism and collectiv-ism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 30, 466–500. doi:10.1177/0022022199030004005

Hackman, J. R. (2002). Leading teams: Setting thestage for great performances. Boston, MA: Har-vard Business School Press.

Hambleton, R. K., & Swaminathan, J. (1985). Itemresponse theory: Principles and applications. Bos-ton, MA: Kluwer.

Hampden-Turner, C., & Trompenaars, F. (1993). Theseven cultures of capitalism: Value systems forcreating wealth in the United States, Japan, Ger-many, France, Sweden, and the Netherlands. Gar-den City, NY: Doubleday.

Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: Inter-national differences in work-related values. Bev-erly Hills, CA: Sage.

Hofstede, G. (1983a). The cultural relativity of orga-nizational practices and theories. Journal of Inter-national Business Studies, 14, 75–89. doi:10.1057/palgrave.jibs.8490867

Hofstede, G. (1983b). Dimensions of national cul-tures in fifty countries and three regions. In J. B.Deregowksi, S. Dziurawiec, & R. C. Annis (Eds.),Expiscations in cross-cultural psychology (pp.335–355). Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger.

Kluckhohn, F., & Strodtbeck, F. L. (1961). Varia-tions in value orientations. Westport, CT: Green-wood Press.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Ilgen, D. R. (2006). Enhanc-ing the effectiveness of work groups and teams.Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7,77–124.

Lawler, E. E. III, & Worley, C. G. (2006). Built tochange. How to achieve sustained organizationaleffectiveness. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Leach, D. J., Wall, T. D., Rogelberg, S. G., & Jack-son, P. R. (2005). Team autonomy, performance,and member job strain: Uncovering the TeamworkKSA link. Applied Psychology: An InternationalReview, 54, 1–24. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.2005.00193.x

Lord, F. M. (1970). Some test theory for tailoredtesting. In W. H. Holtzman (Ed.), Computer as-sisted instruction, testing and guidance (pp. 139–183). New York: Harper & Row.

Lorenzo-Seva, U., & Ferrando, P. J. (2006).FACTOR: A computer program to fit the explor-atory factor analysis model. Behavioral ResearchMethods, 38, 88–91. doi:10.3758/BF03192753

Marks, M. A., Mathieu, J. E., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2001).A temporally based framework and taxonomy ofteam processes. Academy of Management Review,26, 356–376.

Marrone, J. A., Tesluk, P. E., & Carson, J. B. (2007).A multi-level investigation of antecedents and con-sequences of team member boundary spanning be-havior. Academy of Management Journal, 50,1423–1439. doi:10.5465/AMJ.2007.28225967

Maznevski, M. L., DiStefano, J. J., Gomez, C. B.,Noorderhaven, N. G., & Wu, P. (1997, October8–12). The cultural orientations framework andinternational management research. Paper pre-sented at the Academy of International Businessannual meeting, Guadalajara, Mexico.

McClough, A. C., & Rogelberg, S. G. (2003). Selec-tion in teams: An exploration of the TeamworkKnowledge, Skills and Ability Test. InternationalJournal of Selection and Assessment, 11, 56–66.doi:10.1111/1468-2389.00226

McDaniel, M. A., Morgeson, F. P., Finnegan, E. B.,Campion, M. A., & Braverman, E. P. (2001). Useof situational judgement tests to predict job per-formance: A clarification of the literature. Journal

118 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 20: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

of Applied Psychology, 86, 730–740. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.86.4.730

Mumford, T. V., Van Iddekinge, C. H., Morgeson,F. P., & Campion, M. A. (2008). The Team RoleTest: Development and validation of a team roleknowledge situational judgment test. Journal ofApplied Psychology, 93, 250–267. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.2.250

Muthén, B., & Muthén, L. K. (2006). Mplus [Com-puter software]. Los Angeles, CA: Author.

Owen, R. J. (1975). A Bayesian sequential procedurefor quantal response in the context of adaptivemental testing. Journal of the American StatisticalAssociation, 70, 351–356. doi:10.1080/01621459.1975.10479871

Peeters, M. A. G., van Tuijl, H. F. J. M., Rutte, C. G.,& Reymen, I. M. M. J. (2006). Personality andteam performance: A meta-analysis. EuropeanJournal of Personality, 20, 377–396. doi:10.1002/per.588

Rovinelli, R. J., & Hambleton, R. K. (1977). On theuse of content specialists in the assessment ofcriterion-referenced test item validity. Dutch Jour-nal of Educational Research, 2, 49–60.

Ryan, A. M., Chan, D., Ployhart, R. E., & Slade,L. A. (1999). Employee attitude surveys in a mul-tinational organization: Considering language andculture in assessing measurement equivalence.Personnel Psychology, 52, 37–58. doi:10.1111/j.1744-6570.1999.tb01812.x

Salas, E., Sims, D. E., & Burke, C. S. (2005). Is therea “big five” in teamwork? Small Group Research,36, 555–599. doi:10.1177/1046496405277134

Salas, E., Sims, D. E., & Klein, C. (2004). Cooper-ation at work. In C. D. Spielberger (Ed.), Encyclo-pedia of applied psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 497–505).

Boston, MA: Elsevier Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B0-12-657410-3/00533-X

Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1983). Individualdifferences in productivity: An empirical test ofestimates derived from studies of selection proce-dure utility. Journal of Applied Psychology, 68,407–414. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.68.3.407

Spencer, L. M., & Spencer, S. M. (1993). Compe-tence at work: Models for superior performance.New York, NY: Wiley.

Stevens, M. A., & Campion, M. J. (1994). Theknowledge, skill, and ability requirements forteamwork: Implications for human resource man-agement. Journal of Management, 20, 503–530.

Stevens, M. A., & Campion, M. J. (1999). Staffingwork teams: Development and validation of a se-lection test for teamwork settings. Journal of Man-agement, 25, 207–228. doi:10.1016/S0149-2063(99)80010-5

Thorn, D. W., & Deitz, J. C. (1989). Examiningcontent validity through the use of content experts.Occupational Therapy Journal of Research, 9,334–346.

Triandis, H. C. (1995). Individualism and collectiv-ism. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.

Trompenaars, F. (1993). Riding the waves of culture:Understanding cultural diversity in business. BurrRidge, IL: Irwin.

Webber, S. S. (2002). Leadership and trust facilitat-ing cross-functional team success. Journal of Man-agement Development, 21, 201–214. doi:10.1108/02621710210420273

Zaccaro, S. J., Rittman, A. L., & Marks, M. A.(2001). Team leadership. The Leadership Quar-terly, 12, 451–483. doi:10.1016/S1048-9843(01)00093-5

(Appendices follow)

119TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 21: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Appendix A

Observation Code Categories

Conflict Resolution• She/he negotiates considering reasons in-

stead of positions.• She/he collects information about the dif-

ferent sources of conflict.• She/he uses an integrative (win–win) nego-

tiation strategy when possible.

Collaborative Problem Solving• She/he describes problems in terms of be-

haviors, consequences, and feelings.• She/he avoids setting conclusions and as-

cribing motives to others quickly.• She/he focused on communalities to gener-

ate solutions (e.g., use brainstorming to gener-ate more options).

• She/he requests additional information.

Communication• She/he produces complete and specific

messages.• She/he asks for feedback about message

understanding.

• She/he makes summaries along a commu-nication interaction to demonstrate understand-ing.

• She/he communicates openly and support-ively so that he/she recognizes the others.

Goal Setting and Performance Management• She/he tries to define clearly the wished

results (SMART objectives).• She/he performs actions that promote oth-

ers’ success instead of hindering their progress.• She/he shows awareness about others’ per-

formance (e.g., asking for participation of themost active members).

Planning and Task Coordination• She/he makes questions for planning (e.g.,

How are we going to start the project? Whatresources are necessary? How much time wehave to invest?).

• She/he clarifies the procedures of execution(what we make the first, the second, etc.).

• She/he helps to associate tasks withteammates.

Appendix B

TWCT Items

1. When my work team is in conflict, I try tomake it explicit to find solution pathways.

2. When I interact with my team mates, I askquestions to better understand what they say.

3. When I disagree with others, I make an effortto focus on what we have in common insteadof centering on what separates us.

4. I plan my tasks effectively.5. I try to use the most appropriate team network

to communicate the different types of infor-mation, avoiding the same formal procedureall the time.

6. I often get involved in monitoring the taskperformance of other team members.

(Appendices continue)

120 AGUADO, RICO, SÁNCHEZ-MANZANARES, AND SALAS

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 22: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

7. When we face an internal conflict because ofa communication problem or misunderstand-ing, I try to solve it by asking questions andlistening to the people involved.

8. I look at people when they talk to me and Imodify my body language to show real inter-est in what they tell me.

9. I can easily recognize people’s emotionalstates by observing their nonverbal messages.

10. If someone in my team acts inappropriately, Italk privately with her/him, encouraging therest of the team to do the same.

11. To address the trivial task-related issues, I donot need to talk first with all team members sowe reach a decision.

12. I make an effort to talk about less importantthings with my peers for the sake of teamspirit and better internal communication.

13. Having knowledge about people’s skills andsituation requirements is critical to assigntasks properly.

14. Discussions without directions or guides canlead group members to make decisions thatthey would not make on their own.

15. When my personal interests are in conflictwith others’ interests, I tend to be honest inthe negotiation so that others understand myneeds.

16. I care and act to make team conflicts explicitin a way that they can be solved.

17. I play an active role in team meetings byoffering my opinions, asking questions, andexpressing my thoughts and ideas in a sincereand open way.

18. I often help others in my team to make clearthe roles and tasks they have to perform.

19. When I am upset about something, I expressmy discomfort to the group in a constructiveway, asking for solution alternatives.

20. I like to provide my peers with feedbackabout what they do and to assess and valuetheir work.

21. If something upsets me in my team, I do notlike to act as if nothing has happened.

22. I try to establish milestones in my work teamso that we can monitor our assigned tasks.

23. When I am involved in a team project, I careabout having clear plans concerning the tasksand the timing to accomplish them.

24. During group meetings, regulation is neces-sary to ensure that all members provide theiropinions and to avoid that only a few partic-ipate actively.

25. When performing tasks in which one is anexpert, the contributions made by other mem-bers are not that important.

26. In group decision meetings, it is more usual topromote cohesion and reach a majority agree-ment than to pay attention to divergent opin-ions.

27. I try listening to my peers’ opinions withoutevaluating their positions as good or bad.

28. When working in a group, I say what I thinkin an open and sincere way.

29. I expect my peers trust enough to tell me theaspects of my work that they most dislike.

30. I sometimes talk with my peers without anobjective, just for sharing a while together.

31. It is important for me to monitor the tasksassigned to each team member.

32. I provide my peers with relevant informationon how well I think the team tasks are pro-gressing.

33. When doing my job, I prioritize the tasksmost necessary for my teammates to com-plete their work.

34. I try to ensure that my outputs match theinputs needed by my peers to perform theirtasks.

35. For the sake of team work, I set objectiveswith moderate difficulty so that effort isneeded to accomplish them.

36. I often provide my peers with feedback ontheir task performance.

Received October 15, 2012Revision received November 7, 2013

Accepted December 14, 2013

121TEAMWORK COMPETENCY TEST

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 23: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

The Effect of Different Phases of Synchrony on Pain Threshold

Philip J. Sullivan, Kate Rickers, and Kimberley L. GammageBrock University

Synchronization of behavior between individuals has been found to result in a varietyof prosocial outcomes. The role of endorphins in vigorous synchronous activities(Cohen, Ejsmond-Frey, Knight, & Dunbar, 2010) may underlie these effects as endor-phins have been implicated in social bonding (Dunbar & Shultz, 2010). Althoughresearch on synchronous behavior has noted that there are 2 dominant phases ofsynchrony—in-phase and antiphase synchrony (Marsh, Richardson, Baron, & Schmidt,2006), research on the effect of synchrony on endorphins has only incorporatedin-phase synchrony. The current study examined whether both phases of synchronywould generate the synchrony effect. Twenty-two participants rowed under 3 counter-balanced conditions—alone, in-phase synchrony and antiphase synchrony. Endorphinrelease, as measured by pain threshold, was assessed before and after each session.Change in pain threshold during the in-phase synchrony session was significantlyhigher than either of the other 2 conditions. These results suggest that the synchronyeffect may be specific to just in-phase synchrony, and that social presence is not aviable explanation for the effect of synchrony on pain threshold.

Keywords: behavioral synchrony, endorphins, pain threshold, in-phase synchrony

People tend to be inclined to move syn-chronously, whether to an external stimuli orto partners. For example, children as young astwo have been found to impulsively synchro-nize themselves with a drum beat (Kirschner& Tomasello, 2009), and even minimalgroups, such as two individuals in rockingchairs, will spontaneously synchronize theirmotions (Richardson, Marsh, Isenhower,Goodman, & Schmidt, 2007). The relevanceof synchronized behavior may be seen in avariety of interpersonal outcomes that haveresulted from joint action or mimicry.Whether people spontaneously act in coordi-nation with each other, or one person mimicsthe actions of someone else, there appears tobe a robust effect of such actions on interper-sonal attraction and prosocial behavior.

Acting in synchrony with others has repeat-edly been found to increase perceptions of in-terpersonal attraction between participants. Peo-

ple have been shown to infer greatercohesiveness on individuals who move in syn-chrony compared with those who move inde-pendently (Lakens & Stel, 2011). Synchroniza-tion of individuals’ movements with others hasbeen shown to result in higher ratings of inter-personal attraction and affiliation (Bernieri,1988; LaFrance & Broadbent, 1976; Miles,Nind, & Macrae, 2009).

Hove and Risen (2009) conducted a series ofexperiments on interpersonal attraction betweenindividuals engaged in synchronized behavior.In one study, participants engaged in synchro-nized finger tapping with an experimenter for a2.5 min trial. Degree of synchrony, as measuredby percentage of finger taps occurring within100 ms, was significantly correlated with higherratings of liking for the experimenter. Theirsecond experiment manipulated interpersonalsynchrony and included a baseline measure ofliking. Results showed that individuals ran-domly assigned to a condition in which theytapped in synchrony with the experimenter re-ported significantly greater liking for the exper-imenter than those in either the asynchrony oralone conditions. There were no significantbaseline differences in liking for the experi-menter.

Philip J. Sullivan, Kate Rickers, and Kimberley L. Gam-mage, Department of Kinesiology, Brock University.

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-dressed to Philip J. Sullivan, Department of Kinesiology,Brock University, St. Catharines, ON, L2S 3A1. E-mail:[email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice © 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 18, No. 2, 122–128 1089-2699/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/gdn0000001

122

Page 24: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Synchrony has also been repeatedly linked toa variety of prosocial behaviors. Individualswho act in synchrony tend to display signifi-cantly greater cooperation (Valdesolo, Ouyang& DeSteno, 2010; Wiltermuth & Heath, 2009),norm compliance (Wiltermuth, 2012), compas-sion (Valdesolo & DeSteno, 2011), and gener-osity (Stel, van Baaren, & Vonk, 2007) thanindividuals who do not act in synchrony.

Wiltermuth and Heath (2009) investigatedthe effect of synchronized behavior and coop-eration in small groups. Participants walkedaround campus in groups of three in one of twoconditions. In the synchronized condition, theywalked in step; in the nonsynchronized condi-tion, they walked normally. In a postwalk co-operative game, individuals in the synchronizedcondition behaved significantly more coopera-tively than those in the nonsynchronized condi-tion. A second experiment in the same studyreplicated this effect, even after controlling forcommon identity and common fate within thegroups.

In a study conducted by Valdesolo, Ouyange,and DeSteno (2010), individuals were randomlyassigned to one of two conditions. They rockedin rocking chairs in pairs in either a synchro-nized or unsynchronized fashion for two 90 strials. After this manipulation, the pairs had toparticipate in a cooperative joint action task.Subjects also completed ratings of similarity,connectedness, and liking for their partners.Pairs in the synchronized condition completedthe cooperative task in significantly less timethan those in the unsynchronized condition.Synchrony also resulted in higher perceptionsof connectedness and liking.

Recent research may have uncovered a po-tential mechanism for these social effects ofsynchrony. It appears that synchronous behav-iors, particularly vigorous synchronous behav-iors, elevate participants’ endorphin levels,which have been linked to social bonding inhumans and primates (Dunbar, 2010; Dunbar &Shultz, 2010). It has been suggested that endog-enous endorphins may mediate between affiliativestimuli (e.g., shared synchronous movements) andaffiliation (DePue & Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005.Dunbar and Shultz (2010) stated that socialgrooming in primates builds intense social bondsthrough the release of endorphins, “which pro-vides a psycho-pharmacological mechanismthat enables two individuals to build a bonded

relationship with some kind of deeply emo-tional basis” (p. 782).

Because brain endorphins do not cross theblood–brain barrier, and can only be measuredthrough an invasive lumbar puncture (Boeker etal., 2008; Dearman & Francis, 1983), painthreshold has been commonly used as an indi-cator of endorphin activity. (Dunbar et al.,2012; Jamner & Leigh, 1999; Zillman, Rock-well, Schweitzer, & Sundar, 1993). Pain thresh-old is understood to be a valid indicator ofendorphin activity because endogenous opioidsare released during pain sensation (DePue &Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005; Mueller, 1981).Specifically, the use of a blood pressure cuff toinduce specific ischemic pain has been found tobe a valid and reliable, as well as noninvasive,protocol to assess endorphin levels (Estebe, LeNaoures, Chemaly, & Ecoffey, 2000; Ryan &Kovacic, 1966).

Cohen, Ejsmond-Frey, Knight, and Dunbar(2010) investigated synchronized behaviors in ateam of male collegiate rowers, using the painthreshold test as a noninvasive measure of cen-tral endorphin activity. They found a differencewhen teammates rowed in synchronicity in avirtual boat compared with when they per-formed the same workout alone. Specifically,the synchronized condition produced a signifi-cantly greater increase in pain threshold frompre- to postexercise than the solitary session.This was despite the fact that workload wasstandardized between the two conditions.Cohen et al. (2010) concluded that vigoroussynchronized activity results in a “synchronyeffect” whereby endorphin activity is increased.

This synchrony effect has been replicated bySullivan and Rickers (2013) who used the sameprotocol as Cohen et al. (2010), with partici-pants who were strangers as well as rowingteammates. Because of the role of endorphins insocial bonding, the study investigated whetherthe use of teammates in Cohen’s study mayhave influenced the results. Sullivan and Rick-ers found that when individuals rowed for 45min with either teammates or strangers, theystill displayed significantly higher changes inpain threshold than when they rowed alone.

One aspect of synchrony that may be relevantto the synchrony effect is the phase of syn-chrony. There are two dominant phases of syn-chrony—in-phase and antiphase (Marsh, Rich-ardson, Baron, & Schmidt, 2006). In-phase

123SYNCHRONY EFFECT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 25: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

synchrony is characterized by movements beingin perfect unison whereas antiphase symmetryis characterized by alternating patterns of move-ment between two individuals. For example, iftwo children were side by side on swings, in-phase movement would be if they both swingforward and then backward together. Antiphasesynchrony would be seen when one childswings forward as the other swings back (Miles,Nind, Henderson, & Macrae, 2010). Humansappear to prefer in-phase synchrony (Marsh etal., 2006; Richardson et al., 2007). Furthermore,in-phase synchrony appears to have a morepowerful effect on some of the outcomes notedabove (Macrae, Duffy, Miles, & Lawrence,2008).

Although these two different phases mayboth affect outcomes of synchronized behav-ior, to date the synchrony effect has only beeninvestigated with in-phase synchrony. Thecurrent study investigated the role of differentphases of behavioral synchrony on the syn-chrony effect. Specifically, rowing under sol-itary, in-phase and antiphase synchrony con-ditions were examined to see if all wouldproduce the same effect on pain threshold thatwas seen by Cohen et al. (2010) and Sullivanand Rickers (2013). Because previous re-search has noted significantly greater in-creases in endorphin levels after synchronouscompared with solitary activity, it was pre-dicted that there would be a significant dif-ference between solitary and in-phase syn-chrony. Because antiphase synchrony has notyet been studied with respect to the synchronyeffect, no specific hypotheses were put for-ward regarding this condition.

Method

Participants

Twenty-two (9 male, 13 female) individualsparticipated in the study. Participants ranged inage from 19 to 55 years (M 31.14, SD 13.13). Men’s average height was 73.11 in.(SD 1.54) and weight was 200.56 pounds(SD 23.91). Women’s average height was67.62 in. (SD 1.94) and weight was 145.00(SD 12.75). All participants had rowed for atleast 1 year.

Procedures

Before data collection, this study was ap-proved by the IRB of the primary author. Beforeparticipation, all participants completed in-formed-consent forms and the Physical ActivityReadiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q; CanadianSociety for Exercise Physiology, 2002). ThePAR-Q is a 7-item measure to assess whetherparticipants can safely partake in physical activity.Every participant was required to answer to ques-tions using either yes or no responses. If theyreplied with “yes” to any of the questions, theywere to obtain permission from their doctors to bephysically active. If they answered “no” to allquestions, it was safe for them to participate inphysical activity. All participants were able topartake in the procedure.

The experiment used a randomized counter-balanced repeated measures design. Participantsrowed in all of three conditions—alone, and intwo synchronized condition with a partner. Inthe in-phase synchrony condition, both rowerskept in the same movement pattern (i.e., bothrowers would be at the fully extended and fullcontracted positions of the stroke at the sametime). In the antiphase synchrony condition,rowers kept at opposite points of the movementpattern (i.e., when one rower was fully ex-tended, the other rower would be fully con-tracted, and vice versa). These conditions werecounterbalanced so that one third of the partic-ipants rowed alone first, one third rowed in thein-phase synchrony condition first, and the re-maining third rowed in the antiphase synchronycondition first.

Intensity of the workout was operationalizedby average 500 m split time (i.e., the time takento row 500 m on the ergometer machine) overthe 30 min session. This value was recordedduring the first session and participants matchedthe intensity on subsequent trials. By slight ma-nipulations of the drag factor, the experimenterswere able to establish synchrony between par-ticipants while allowing individuals to maintainthe same intensity (i.e., stroke rate) in all threeconditions.

Participants rowed with the same partner inboth synchrony conditions and pairs alwaysconsisted of rowers of the same gender. Allparticipants were prohibited from listening tomusic or consuming sport drinks during theirtrials. There was a week gap between sessions

124 SULLIVAN, RICKERS, AND GAMMAGE

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 26: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

for all participants. All sessions were held in theafternoon or early evening.

Measures of pain threshold were taken beforeand after all trials using the blood pressure cuffprotocol used by Cohen et al. (2010). All painmeasurements were taken by the experimenterand recorded 1 min before the start and con-clusion of each trial. In all conditions, the sameexperimenter assessed pain threshold for all par-ticipants. This was achieved by staggering theend of workouts of participants by 30 s each.The pain thresholds of all participants wereassessed away from all others being tested. Theblood pressure cuff was placed above the elbowon the nondominant arm of each participant.Ischemic pain was induced through manualpumping of the cuff to increase pressure. Par-ticipants expressed the point at which pressurebecame uncomfortable by saying “now,” atwhich point the pressure was recorded and thecuff was removed. This protocol has been com-monly used in studies on behavioral synchrony(Cohen et al., 2010; Dunbar et al., 2012; Sulli-van & Rickers, 2013), and previous research hassupported its validity as a measure of painthreshold. It has been shown to be highly cor-related with other measures of pain threshold,including visual analog scales (Estebe et al.,2000) and gross pressure ischemia (Ryan &Kovacic, 1966). Furthermore, it has been foundto have a high test–retest reliability within par-ticipants (r .87; Ryan & Kovacic, 1966).

Materials

Two Concept2 Model D indoor rowing ma-chines (i.e., “ergometers”) were set up in therowing training center of the university. Painthreshold was measured using an AMG MedicalInc. Professional Series sphygmomanometer(blood pressure cuff). Units of pressure wererecorded in millimeters of mercury (mmHg).

Results

Because pain threshold differs by individual(Dunbar et al., 2012), change in pain thresholdfrom pre- to postscore was used as the depen-dent variable in analyses. Although there aresome concerns with using raw change scores insuch analyses, research has shown that whenusing reliable scores, particularly physiologicalmeasures, it is advisable to use raw change

scores (Dimitrov & Rumrill, 2003). Pre- andpostpain threshold scores by condition areshown in Table 1. All three conditions dis-played mean increases in pain threshold frompre- to postactivity, which is consistent with theeffect of vigorous exercise on endorphin activ-ity (Boecker et al., 2008). Change in painthreshold was normally distributed in all threeconditions. Average prescores did not differ be-tween conditions (F (2, 63) 0.89, p .416),and there were significant pre- to postchanges inthe solitary (F (1, 21) 24.10, p 001),in-phase (F (1, 21) 69.69, p 001) andantiphase conditions (F (1, 21) 22.04, p 001). There were no statistically significant dif-ferences between genders in preactivity painthreshold scores or changes scores in any of thethree conditions.

Before testing the hypotheses, the data wasexamined to see if it the assumption of spheric-ity was upheld. Mauchly’s test was nonsignifi-cant (W .929, p .05), indicating that therewas homogeneous variance between the condi-tions, and that the data was appropriate for arepeated measures analysis. A repeated mea-sures analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed asignificant effect for condition (F (2, 42) 4.12, p .023, 2 .16). Observed powerwithin the design was .70. Bonferroni adjustedcontrast analyses revealed that the in-phase syn-chrony condition was significantly differentfrom both the solitary (F (1, 21) 5.27, p .03, 2 .20) and antiphase (F (1, 21) 8.99,p .007, 2 .30) conditions. There was nota significant difference between the solitary andantiphase conditions (F (1, 21) 0.41, p .05).

Discussion

The current study was designed to investigateif two different phases of synchrony would both

Table 1Means and SDs of Raw and Change PainThreshold Scores by Condition

ConditionPrescore(M, SD)

Postscore(M, SD)

Change score(M, SD)

Solitary 178.64 (38.95) 210.00 (51.73) 31.36 (29.96)In-phase 196.82 (55.41) 247.73 (50.89) 50.91 (28.60)Antiphase 194.55 (51.89) 224.09 (52.34) 29.55 (29.52)

Note. Values are in millimeters of mercury (mmHg).

125SYNCHRONY EFFECT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 27: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

show the “synchrony effect” of elevated painthreshold, which has previously only been doc-umented with in-phase synchrony (Cohen et al.,2010; Sullivan & Rickers, 2013). Within thecurrent study, it was found that when individu-als rowed in an in-phase synchronous condition,they reported a significantly greater increase(from pre- to postactivity) in pain threshold thaneither the solitary or antiphase synchrony con-ditions. There was no significant difference be-tween the antiphase and solitary condition.Therefore, the synchrony effect appears to beonly a result of in-phase synchrony.

At this point, it appears that the synchronyeffect is a fairly robust phenomenon. It hasrepeatedly been found that individuals have asignificantly higher change in pain thresholdafter vigorous activity in synchrony with othersthan after the same vigorous activity performedalone (Cohen et al., 2010; Sullivan & Rickers,2013). This has been found with different sam-ples, including teammates and strangers, andboth males and females. Whereas Cohen et al.’s(2010) original study only included male rowerswho were teammates, Sullivan and Rickers(2013) included strangers as well as teammatesand males as well as females, and the currentstudy also found the effect using both male andfemale participants.

As pain threshold has been interpreted as anindication of endorphin activity (Cohen et al.,2010; Dunbar et al., 2012), it appears that vig-orous synchronized activity may produce a sig-nificant “rush” in endorphins compared with thesame activity performed alone. Endorphinshave been implicated in social bonding, al-though the exact mechanism is not clearly un-derstood. As noted above, it is possible thatendorphins mediate affiliative stimuli, such assynchronous movements, and affiliation (DePue& Morrone-Strupinsky, 2005). If this is thecase, it may be that synchronized movements,particularly in-phase synchrony, which appearsto be preferred by people (Marsh et al., 2006;Richardson et al., 2007), causes endorphin re-lease. In the case of vigorous synchronized ac-tivities, such as rowing, this endorphin release isabove and beyond what is induced by the exer-cise itself (Cohen et al., 2010). Because one ofthe effects of endorphins, in addition to painthreshold, is feelings of well-being (Dunbar &Shultz, 2010), this shared synchrony would re-sult in stronger social bonds among participants.

Therefore, it is possible that through the syn-chrony effect, vigorous synchronized activityreleases endorphins, which in turn increase so-cial bonding. This process would be consistentwith speculation that collective activities in-crease cohesion among group members in ac-tivities such as marching in step and rhythmicdancing and singing during religious activities(McNeill, 1995). Through the inducement ofendorphins, the synchrony effect may serve as amechanism for this increased cohesion.

A secondary implication of the finding thatin-phase synchrony produces an effect on en-dorphins whereas antiphase synchrony does notis that this synchrony effect now appears to beindependent of any social facilitation effect.The designs of Cohen et al., (2010) and Sullivanand Rickers (2013) both included a confound-ing factor—the synchrony condition was alsothe only group condition. In tasks requiringphysical conditioning demands, such as rowing,the presence of others should result in perfor-mance increases (Strauss, 2002). Although per-formance was constant in both conditions inCohen et al.’s and Sullivan and Ricker’s re-search, it may be that other responses wereaffected. The presence of others has beenknown to have an effect on unlearned re-sponses, particularly cardiovascular activity.Blascovich, Mendes, Hunter, and Salomon(1999) found that the presence of others duringwell-learned tasks could produce a “challenge”response (i.e., an increase in cardiovascular ac-tivity). Because exercise stimulates endorphinrelease (Boeker et al., 2008), such a changecould be responsible for the change in endor-phins seen by Cohen and her colleagues. It mayhave been possible that the endorphin effect thatCohen attributed to synchrony was actually amere presence effect. However, given the cur-rent results, where a second group condition(i.e., the antiphase synchrony) did not producethis synchrony effect, it appears that the effecton endorphins is because of synchrony, in par-ticular in-phase synchrony, rather than any so-cial facilitation effect.

The present results extend our understandingof the synchrony effect. Consistent with previ-ous research, they have supported the findingthat vigorous synchronized activity causes anincrease in pain threshold, which appears to bean indication of elevated endorphin activity.This effect now appears to be the result of

126 SULLIVAN, RICKERS, AND GAMMAGE

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 28: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

synchrony, as opposed to social coaction, andspecifically in-phase synchrony. Future researchis still required to fully understand this syn-chrony effect. Specifically, the studies to datehave only used the synchronized activity ofrowing. Would the same synchrony effect beseen with other activities such as running instep, or fitness classes? It would also be inter-esting to see if the effect could be replicatedwith synchronized nonvigorous activities suchas tai chi or walking in step. Furthermore, wewould note that the links between synchronyand endorphins and cohesion are currently spec-ulative and research is required to fully opera-tionalize this potential mechanism. To fully un-derstand this phenomenon, we need to know ifsynchrony directly effects endorphins and if so,does this synchrony-induced endorphin activityaffect cohesion. As interesting as the effect ap-pears to be, the time is ripe for more research onthe basic mechanics and ramifications of thissynchrony effect.

However, there are several limitations to thecurrent design, which are probably not limitedto the present study. The current sample, likeCohen at al.’s (2010) and Sullivan and Rickers(2013) comprised experienced rowers. It is pos-sible that experienced rowers may associatesynchrony with good teamwork and the resul-tant pain threshold effect may be related to thissocial effectiveness as opposed to any syn-chrony effect. Furthermore, there are some po-tential demand characteristics is the study. Inthis protocol, the experimenter is not blind tothe condition of the participants, and it is pos-sible that this may have influenced the results.Future research on the effect of behavioral syn-chrony on pain threshold or endorphin activityshould use more strict methodologies, includingblinded conditions and exit questionnaires toprobe participants’ suspicions about researchquestions.

References

Bernieri, F. (1988). Coordinated movement and rap-port in teacher-student interactions. Journal ofNonverbal Behavior, 12, 120–138. doi:10.1007/BF00986930

Blascovich, J., Mendes, W. B., Hunter, S. B., &Salomon, K. (1999). Social “facilitation” as chal-lenge and threat. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 77, 68–77. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.1.68

Boeker, H., Sprenger, T., Spilker, M. E., Henriksen,G., Koppenhoefer, M., Wagner, K. L., . . . Tolle,T. R. (2008). The runner’s high: Opioidergicmechanisms in the human brain. Cerebral Cortex,18, 2523–2532. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn013

Canadian Society for Exercise Physiology. (2002).Physical Activity Readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q).Retrieved from http://www.csep.ca/communities/c574/files/hidden/pdfs/par-q.pdf

Cohen, E. E., Ejsmond-Frey, R., Knight, N., & Dun-bar, R. I. (2010). Rowers’ high: Behavioural syn-chrony is correlated with elevated pain thresholds.Biology Letters, 6, 106 –108. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0670

Dearman, J., & Francis, K. T. (1983). Plasma levelsof catecholamines, cortisol, and beta- endorphinsin male athletes after running 26.2, 6, and 2 miles.The Journal of Sports Medicine and Physical Fit-ness, 23, 30–38.

DePue, R. A., & Morrone-Strupinsky, J. V. (2005). Aneurobehavioral model of affiliative bonding: Im-plications for conceptualization a human trait ofaffiliation. Behavioral Brain Science, 28, 313–385.doi:10.1017/S0140525X05290064

Dimitrov, D. M., & Rumrill, Jr, P. D. (2003). Pretest-posttest designs and measurement of change.Work: A Journal of Prevention, Assessment andRehabilitation, 20, 159–165.

Dunbar, R. I. M. (2010). The social role of touch inhumans and primates: Behavioural function andneurobiological mechanisms. Neuroscience andBiological Reviews, 34, 260–268. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.07.001

Dunbar, R. I. M., Baron, R., Frangoou, A., Pearce, E.,van Leeuwen, J. C., Stow, J., . . . van Vugt, M.(2012). Social laughter is correlated with an ele-vated pain threshold. Proceedings of The RoyalSociety Biological Sciences, 279, 1161–1167. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.1373

Dunbar, R. I., & Shultz, S. (2010). Bondedness andsociality. Behavior, 147, 775–803. doi.10.1163/000579510X501151

Estebe, J. P., Le Naoures, A., Chemaly, L., & Ecof-fey, C. (2000). Tourniquet pain in a volunteerstudy: Effect of changes in cuff width and pres-sure. Anasthesia, 55, 21–26.

Hove, J. M., & Risen, L. J. (2009). It’s all in thetiming: Interpersonal synchrony increases affilia-tion. Social Cognition, 27, 949–960.

Jamner, L. D., & Leigh, H. (1999). Repressive/defensive coping, endogenous opioids and health:How a life so perfect can make you sick. Psychi-atry Research, 8, 17–31. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00134-6

Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Joint drum-ming: Social context facilitates synchronization inpreschool children. Journal of Experimental Child

127SYNCHRONY EFFECT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 29: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Psychology, 102, 299 –314. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.07.005

LaFrance, M., & Broadbent, M. (1976). Group rap-port: Posture sharing as a nonverbal indicator.Group and Organizational Management, 1, 328–333. doi:10.1177/105960117600100307

Lakens, D., & Stel, M. (2011). If they move in sync,they must feel in sync: Movement synchrony leadsto attributions of rapport and entitavitiy. SocialCognition, 29, 1–14. doi:10.1521/soco.2011.29.1.1

Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K., & Law-rence, J. (2008). A case of hand waving: Actionsynchrony and person perception. Cognition, 109,152–156. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.07.007

Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., Baron, R. M., &Schmidt, R. C. (2006). Contrasting approaches toperceiving and acting with others. Ecological Psy-chology, 18, 1–38. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco1801_1

McNeill, W. H. (1995). Keeping together in time:Dance and drill in human history. Cambridge,MA: Harvard University Press.

Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., Henderson, Z., & Macrae,C. N. (2010). Moving memories: Behavioral syn-chrony and memory for self and others. Journal ofExperimental Social Psychology, 46, 457–460.doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.12.006

Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N. (2009).The rhythm of rapport: Interpersonal synchronyand social perception. Journal of ExperimentalSocial Psychology, 45, 585–589. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.002

Mueller, G. P. (1981). Beta-endorphin immunoreac-tivity in rat plasma: Variations in response to dif-ferent physical stimuli. Life Sciences, 29, 1669–1674. doi.10.1016/0024-3205(81)90069-2

Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W.,Goodman, J. R., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rockingtogether: Dynamics of intentional and uninten-tional interpersonal coordination. Human Move-

ment Science, 26, 867–891. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.07.002

Ryan, E. D., & Kovacic, C. R. (1966). Pain toleranceand athletic participation. Perceptual and MotorSkills, 22, 383–390. doi:10.2466/pms.1966.22.2.383

Stel, M., van Baaren, R. B., & Vonk, R. (2007).Effects of mimicking: Acting prosocially and be-ing emotionally moved. European Journal of So-cial Psychology, 38, 965–976. doi:10.1002/ejsp.472

Strauss, B. (2002). Social facilitation in motor tasks:A review of theory and research. Psychology ofSport and Exercise, 3, 237–256. doi:10.1016/S1469-0292(01)00019-x

Sullivan, P., & Rickers, K. (2013). The effects ofbehavioral synchrony in groups of teammates andstrangers. International Journal of Sport and Ex-ercise Psychology, 1–6. doi:10.1080/1612197X.2013.750139

Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony andthe social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11,262–266. doi:10.1037/a0021302

Valdesolo, P., Ouyang, J., & DeSteno, D. (2010). Therhythm of joint action: Synchrony promotes coop-erative ability. Journal of Experimental SocialPsychology, 46, 693–695. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.004

Wiltermuth, S. (2012). Synchrony and destructiveobedience. Social Influence, 7, 78 – 89. doi:10.1080/15534510.2012.658653

Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchronyand cooperation. Psychological Science, 20, 1–5.doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.x

Zillman, D., Rockwell, S., Schweitzer, K., & Sundar,S. (1993). Does humor facilitate coping with phys-ical discomfort? Motivation Emotion, 17, 1–21.doi:10.1007/BF00995204

Received February 17, 2013Revision received July 18, 2013

Accepted July 22, 2013

128 SULLIVAN, RICKERS, AND GAMMAGE

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 30: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Reactions to Social Inclusion and Ostracism as a Function ofPerceived In-Group Similarity

Donald F. SaccoThe University of Southern Mississippi

Michael J. BernsteinPennsylvania State University–Abington

Steven G. YoungBaruch College, City University of New York

Kurt HugenbergMiami University

Although ostracism is a powerfully aversive experience, recent evidence identifiesfactors capable of moderating the impact of ostracism, such as in-group status and thegroup’s essential nature. In the current work, 67 Caucasian American participants (47women) were included or ostracized by either same-race (i.e., Caucasian American) orother-race (i.e., African American) targets on a between-subjects basis while playingthe game Cyberball. Participants then indicated the extent to which they felt similar tothe other Cyberball players as well as how satisfied their basic needs (e.g., belonging-ness, self-esteem) were during the game. Consistent with past research, we found thatin-group and out-group status moderated the magnitude of reactions to social inclusionand ostracism; that is, ostracism hurts more and social inclusion feels better when it isimplemented by fellow in-group as opposed to out-group members. Importantly, weextend these previous findings by demonstrating that differential reactions to socialinclusion and ostracism are mediated by changes in participants’ self-perceived simi-larity with in-group members. These results identify a potential mechanism responsiblefor the differential impact of in-group–out-group status on reactions to social inclusionand ostracism.

Keywords: ostracism, intergroup relations, in-group–out-group, threat

Humans are intensely social animals. Assuch, we place great value on social connectionsand group memberships, with our self-esteemand emotional states being closely linked to oursocial interactions and relationships (Baumeis-ter & Leary, 1995). As a result, being sociallyrejected or ostracized is an intensely aversivesocial experience, whereas being socially ac-cepted is decidedly positive (MacDonald &

Leary, 2005; Williams, 2007). However, therehas been some debate as to whether all experi-ences of ostracism and social inclusion areequally impactful. Although initial research in-dicated that aversive responses to ostracism arereflexive and unmoderated by situational factors(e.g., Gonsalkorale & Williams, 2007; Williams& Zadro, 2005), more recent research suggeststhat responses to social ostracism are sensitiveto several factors, including characteristics ofthe individuals engaging in the act of ostracism.For example, Bernstein and colleagues (2010)demonstrated that being included or ostracizedby in-group members exacerbates the intensityof the experience (i.e., being ostracized by anin-group hurts more than being ostracized by anout-group).

Whereas these past findings demonstrate thatin-group status is an important moderator ofreactions to social inclusion and ostracism, re-search has yet to identify the mechanism under-

Donald F. Sacco, Department of Psychology, The Uni-versity of Southern Mississippi; Michael J. Bernstein, De-partment of Psychological and Social Sciences, Pennsylva-nia State University–Abington; Steven G. Young,Department of Psychology, Baruch College, City Universityof New York; Kurt Hugenberg, Department of Psychology,Miami University.

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-dressed to Donald F. Sacco, Department of Psychology, TheUniversity of Southern Mississippi, Owings-McQuaggeHall, 118 College Drive 5025, Hattiesburg, MS 39406.E-mail: [email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice © 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 18, No. 2, 129–137 1089-2699/14/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/gdn0000002

129

Page 31: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

lying these differential reactions. To addressthis research lacuna, we tested the hypothesisthat inclusion within and ostracism by an in-group can have more powerful influences on theself (e.g., influencing belongingness and self-esteem) precisely because such experiences canchange perceptions of self-similarity to the in-group, which serves as a psychological media-tor of the increased intensity of in-group inclu-sion and ostracism. To this end, we first reviewthe literature on the need to belong, in-group–out-group status, and perceptions of similarity,and then provide direct experimental evidencefor our proposed mediational hypothesis.

The Need to Belong

Whereas stable social bonds have numerousphysical and psychological benefits, unfulfilledbelongingness needs have a decidedly negativeimpact on well-being (Baumeister & Leary,1995). Specifically, acute experiences of ostra-cism induce negative mood and thwart basicsocial needs satisfaction (e.g., self-esteem, be-longing; Williams & Zadro, 2005). Importantly,these strong reactions to the experience of os-tracism are largely unaffected by various indi-vidual difference measures that seem intuitivelyrelevant (e.g., trait self-esteem and social anxi-ety; Leary, Haupt, Strausser, & Chokel, 1998;Zadro, Boland, & Richardson, 2006).

Indeed, prior work has even found that ostra-cism lowers mood and basic need satisfactionequally, regardless of the characteristics of theostracizing party. For example, Williams andcolleagues (2000) found that responses to ostra-cism did not differ based on in-group–out-group distinctions (e.g., PC vs. Mac users; seealso Smith & Williams, 2004). Furthermore,individuals’ basic needs satisfaction is threat-ened regardless of whether participants believethat they were ostracized by fellow participants(i.e., peers), by confederates who were trainedto ostracize them, or even by a computer pro-grammed to arbitrarily ostracize them (Zadro,Williams, & Richardson, 2004). Finally, Gon-salkorale and Williams (2007) even found thatostracism perpetrated by a disliked out-group(i.e., KKK members) is experienced as pain-fully as that perpetrated by a valued in-group.

Nonetheless, not all sources of affiliationequally satisfy belonging needs. Specifically,valued social groups are a vital source of social

and material support, offering protection andaccess to mates (e.g., Correll & Park, 2005).Indeed, in-group bias, or the tendency to favorin-groups over out-groups (Tajfel, 1982), is re-flected in numerous behaviors, ranging frommore positive evaluations of in-groups, greaterlevels of prosocial behavior toward fellow in-group members, and allocation of more re-sources to in-groups (e.g., Ferguson & Kelley,1964; Hein, Silani, Preuschoff, Batson, &Singer, 2010; Tajfel, Billig, Bundy, & Flament,1971). Consistent with the importance of socialin-groups, Bernstein and colleagues (2010) re-cently reported two studies demonstrating thatostracism is more painful and social inclusionmore positive when carried out by fellow in-group members (cf. Goodwin, Williams, &Carter-Sowell, 2010). In their first study, partic-ipants were either ostracized or socially in-cluded by racial in-group or out-group mem-bers; participants reported more threatenedaffiliation needs following racial in-group ostra-cism (relatively to ostracism by racial out-groupmembers), but more satisfied basic affiliationneeds following social inclusion by racial in-group as opposed to out-group members. Theirsecond study replicated this basic finding withpolitical group membership; that is, participantsreported more social pain when ostracized byindividuals who shared their own party affilia-tion (as opposed to ostracism by confederates ofa different party affiliation), but greater satisfac-tion when socially included by individuals withthe same political party affiliation as their own.Importantly, these authors found that thisstrengthened experience of in-group inclusionand ostracism occurred most strongly for per-ceivers who were made to see the in-group ashighly essentialized (i.e., participants who wereled to believe that these groups possessed char-acteristics defined as relatively inborn, immuta-ble, or genetically based; see Prentice & Miller,2007).

Perceived Self-Similarity and Reactions toIn-Group–Out-Group Inclusion and

Ostracism

In the current work, we tested the hypothesisthat the experience of becoming psychologi-cally similar to the including in-group or dis-similar from the ostracizing in-group would me-diate the effect of in-group–out-group status on

130 SACCO, BERNSTEIN, YOUNG, AND HUGENBERG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 32: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

reactions to inclusion and ostracism. Across nu-merous kinds of relationships, individuals pre-fer to interact with others who are similar acrossa variety of domains, including intelligence andphysical attractiveness (e.g., Feingold, 1988;Jensen, 1978). Furthermore, similarity is posi-tively related to relationship satisfaction andlongevity in both platonic and romantic rela-tionships (e.g., Acitelli, Kenny, & Weiner,2001; Gonzaga, Campos, & Bradbury, 2007;Russell & Wells, 1991).

Interestingly, this relation between similarityand liking is bidirectional, such that positiveinteractions with others lead to increased per-ceptions of similarity. According to Morry’s(2005) attraction-similarity model, attractionleads to perceptions of similarity between theself and others and these perceptions of simi-larity provide numerous benefits, ranging fromfeeling more understood and validated to in-creased feelings of positive affect and decreasedloneliness. Put simply, we are likely to affiliatewith similar others (e.g., Acitelli et al., 2001)but also perceive those we affiliate with as moresimilar to the self, and these feelings of inter-personal similarity are important for promptingboth initial liking and securing the stable andlong-term relationships that best satisfy belong-ing needs (Baumeister & Leary, 1995).

Based on these previous findings, we suggestthat inclusion by in-group members creates per-ceptions of enhanced similarity with membersof that group, an outcome we propose may beresponsible for enhanced need satisfaction fol-lowing inclusion by in-group members. Con-versely, ostracism by in-group members may beinterpreted as evidence of dissimilarity betweenthe self and group, which may sharpen the neg-ative experience of social exclusion. Specifi-cally, to the extent that inclusion by in-groupmembers makes the in-group more attractive toan individual, that person should indicateheightened perceived similarity with that in-group, resulting in higher levels of interpersonalsatisfaction; conversely, ostracism by in-groupmembers should lead individuals to perceivethat group as less attractive, resulting in reducedperceptions of similarity with the in-group andpotential interpersonal dissatisfaction (i.e.,Morry, 2005). However, because out-groups arecategorized as essentially dissimilar from theself, simple acts of inclusion and exclusion maylead to little change in perceived similarity to

out-group members, and thus may be unrelatedto the experiences of ostracism and social in-clusion.

To test our hypothesis, we manipulated therace of supposed confederates in the context ofthe Cyberball paradigm (Williams & Jarvis,2006), thereby allowing us to independentlymanipulate whether participants experiencedsocial inclusion or ostracism and whether thatexperience was initiated by members of one’sown racial in-group or out-group. Followingeach of these experiences, we assessed partici-pants’ perceived similarity with the Cyberball“confederates” they played with as well as theirbasic needs satisfaction following the game.This design allowed us to replicate previousfindings that inclusion and ostracism perpe-trated by racial in-groups produce more intenseinterpersonal reactions (both positive and neg-ative) as well as to test the hypothesis that theeffects of in-group inclusion and ostracism onbasic affiliation needs (dis)satisfaction are me-diated by changes in perceived similarity within-group members.

Method

Participants

The sample was composed of 67 (47 women;Mage 18.97 years, SD 1.28) CaucasianAmerican undergraduate students at a largemidwestern university in the United States whoparticipated in exchange for partial course cred-it; non-Caucasian Americans did not participatein this study.1 A 2 (inclusionary status: ostra-cism, inclusion) 2 (agent race: same-race,other-race) between-subjects design was em-ployed.

Materials

Basic Needs Scale. To assess participants’reactions to social inclusion and ostracism, weadministered the Basic Needs Scale, used ex-tensively in prior research (Williams et al.,2000; Zadro, Williams, & Richardson, 2004).This questionnaire assesses four social needsknown to fluctuate with experiences of socialinclusion and ostracism: belonging, perceived

1 No effects of participant sex were observed; therefore,this variable is not discussed further.

131SOCIAL INCLUSION, OSTRACISM, AND SIMILARITY

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 33: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

control, self-esteem, and meaningful existence(for a review, see Williams, 2007). The ques-tionnaire included 16 items assessing perceivedthreats to these four needs (e.g., “I felt discon-nected,” “I felt I had control over the situation,”“I felt liked,” “I felt meaningless”); we askedparticipants to provide their responses based onhow they felt during the game (reflexive) ratherthan based on how they currently felt (reflec-tive). Participants responded to each of the 16questions using a 7-point Likert scale (1 notat all to 7 extremely), scored such that highernumbers indicated more fulfillment of the re-spective need (i.e., higher numbers indicated agreater experience of belonging, higher self-esteem, etc.).

Perceived similarity. Using the same7-point Likert scale used to assess basic needssatisfaction, we also asked participants the ex-tent to which they agreed with the statement“The other participants are similar to me.”

Procedure

The procedure was adapted closely from thatof Bernstein and colleagues (2010). Upon theirarrival in a university laboratory, participantsprovided informed consent and were escorted toindividual cubicles. Specifically, they were in-formed that they were participating in a studyinvolving online communication and mental vi-sualization, and that because the study was oc-curring in multiple departments on the univer-sity campus, they would be playing with otherstudents located elsewhere on campus. In actu-ality, the other participants were computer-controlled agents programmed to either sociallyinclude or ostracize the participants on a be-tween-subjects basis. To facilitate our experi-mental design, we told participants that theywould be able to see a photograph of the twoparticipants with whom they would be playingduring the game itself, and that the two partic-ipants would be able to see a picture of them(thus allowing for our manipulation of agentrace). Participants were then instructed to standagainst a white wall while the experimentertook a digital photograph of them, which theexperimenter then ostensibly uploaded to thecomputer network prior to the start of the game.

After having their picture taken and complet-ing a series of filler questions supposedly mea-suring “mental visualization” capabilities, par-

ticipants were directed to play Cyberball, anInternet ball-tossing game ostensibly designedto help people hone their mental visualizationskills. Importantly, our White participants saweither two White faces (i.e., in-group members)or two Black faces (i.e., out-group members) asfellow players (on a between-subjects basis, tomanipulate racial in-group and out-group mem-bership). In addition, participants were ran-domly assigned to either receive the ballroughly one third of the time (inclusion condi-tion) or to receive the ball only twice at thebeginning and then never again for the remain-ing throws (ostracism condition).

Participants then completed the similarityquestion and the Basic Needs Scale. Partici-pants were debriefed and thanked for their par-ticipation. We used a funneled debriefing ques-tionnaire to determine whether participantswere suspicious about any aspect of our exper-imental procedures, including the Cyberballmanipulation. Using an open-ended format, par-ticipants were asked, “Did you find anythingodd or suspicious during the study?” and “Didyou think anything in the study was fake?” Ifparticipants answered affirmatively to eitherquestion, they were asked to elaborate on theirinitial answer.

Results

Manipulation Check Effectiveness

To determine whether our Cyberball manip-ulation was believable to participants, we codedparticipants’ responses to our funneled debrief-ing questions for suspicion. Specifically, wecreated a “player suspicion” variable and notedany participants who indicated that they foundthe two Cyberball players generally or their raceto be fake, odd, or suspicious. Anyone mention-ing such a comment in either question wasassigned a 1, and all others who did not mentionanything were assigned a 0. This coding re-sulted in a total of six of 67 respondents havingmade comments regarding some aspect of theplayers being fake, odd, or suspicious. Impor-tantly, only one participant’s suspicion was ac-tually based on the race of the Cyberball play-ers.

We conducted analyses to determine whetherthese differences varied across experimentalconditions. We first conducted a chi-square

132 SACCO, BERNSTEIN, YOUNG, AND HUGENBERG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 34: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

analysis, using Cyberball target race (Caucasianvs. African American) as the independent vari-able. This analysis yielded a nonsignificant dif-ference, 2(1) 0.67, p .41, indicating thatsuspicion about the players did not vary basedon whether participants were playing withWhite or Black agents. We next conducted asimilar chi-square analysis using Cyberball con-dition (social inclusion vs. ostracism) as theindependent variable; again, this analysis re-vealed no difference between conditions,2(1) 0.35, p .56. To determine whetherthese factors (social experience and agent race)interacted, we conducted a binary logistic re-gression, dummy coding (1) inclusion Blackagents, ostracism White agents, and ostra-cism Black agents against the referent groupof inclusion White agents (0). The regressionwas nonsignificant, 2(3) 2.82, p .42, foreach of the three dummy-coded variables en-tered into the logistic regression.

In a final analysis, we dummy coded bothsocial experience (0 exclusion, 1 inclu-sion) and agent race (0 Black, 1 White)and entered them simultaneously as factors in abinary logistic regression, using our “playersuspicion” factor as the dependent variable.This analysis allowed us to examine the effectof each of these factors while also accountingfor the variability due to the other factor. Thisanalysis also yielded a nonsignificant regres-sion, 2(2) 0.98, p .61.

All evidence suggested that suspicion aboutthe Cyberball players, including their race, didnot differ between conditions of our experi-ment. Furthermore, this identical experimentalprocedure has been used in previous work andsimilarly low levels of suspicion were docu-mented (see Bernstein et al., 2010). As such, thecurrent procedure possessed an adequate levelof experimental realism.

Basic Needs

The individual needs were highly related( .94); as such, we formed a composite basicneeds score for each participant (higher valuesrepresent more satisfied basic needs; see Mc-Connell, Brown, Shoda, Stayton, & Martin,2011, for similar procedures). We then sub-jected these scores to a 2 (inclusionary status:inclusion vs. ostracism) 2 (agent race: same-race vs. other-race) between-subjects analysis

of variance (ANOVA).2 A main effect of inclu-sionary status emerged, F(1, 63) 105.73, p .001, p

2 .63, such that included participants(M 4.44, SE 0.14) reported substantiallymore fulfilled basic needs than did ostracizedparticipants (M 2.50, SE 0.13). Impor-tantly, this main effect was qualified by a sig-nificant Inclusionary Status Agent Race in-teraction, F(1, 63) 5.41, p .023, p

2 .079(see Figure 1). Simple effects analyses revealedthat participants ostracized by same-race agents(M 2.22, SD 0.58) had less fulfilled basicneeds than did those ostracized by other-raceagents (M 2.78, SD 0.88), t(35) 2.20,p .032, d 0.75. Although participants in-cluded by same-race agents were directionallymore satisfied (M 4.61, SD 0.83) than werethose included by other-race agents (M 4.28,SD .73), this effect did not reach statisticalsignificance, t(28) 1.15, p .20, d 0.42.This significant interaction between inclusion-ary status and target race, however, is consistentwith previous findings indicating that althoughostracism always feels worse than social inclu-sion, such effects can be qualified by the in-group– out-group relationship of the rejecterand rejected (Bernstein et al., 2010).

Mediation Analysis

We then investigated whether the strongerresponses to in-group inclusion and ostracismwere mediated by changes in the perceived sim-ilarity of the self to in-group members.3 Wepredicted that participants included by same-race participants should see the self as moresimilar to the agents, relative to participantsexperiencing ostracism. We further predictedthat increased perceived similarity should bepositively correlated with increased fulfillmentof basic affiliation needs. Finally, we predictedthat the relationship between inclusionary statusand basic needs would be mediated by the

2 Treating each of the four subscales as separate depen-dent variables in four separate ANOVAs had no effect onthe interaction pattern (ps .07).

3 Because perceived similarity and belongingness are po-tentially psychologically similar constructs, we conducted acorrelational analysis to determine how strongly they wererelated to one another, r(67) .49, p .01. Because thesetwo concepts were only moderately correlated, perceivedsimilarity was an adequate variable to include as a psycho-logical mediator.

133SOCIAL INCLUSION, OSTRACISM, AND SIMILARITY

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 35: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

aforementioned increase in perceived similarity.However, we believe this relationship shouldonly be true for participants playing with same-race agents. We hypothesized that de facto dis-similarity with racial out-groups would be un-affected by the relatively minor inclusionaryand exclusionary behaviors used in our task,and as such, we did not expect similarity tomediate the relationship between inclusionarystatus and basic needs satisfaction.

We therefore conducted a moderated media-tion analysis (i.e., a conditional indirect effect;see Preacher, Rucker, & Hayes, 2007) to exam-ine our specific hypothesis, and we used thePreacher et al. (2007) existing SPSS macro toconduct this analysis. Before testing the moder-ated mediation, we began by dummy coding theindependent variable of inclusionary status (1 inclusion, 0 ostracism) and entered it as apredictor of the composite basic needs score.We also included our similarity measure as ourmediator and target race as the moderating vari-able (0 White, 1 Black). This analysisproduced a significant overall model, F(3,63) 5.82, p .014, R2 .22, which includedour previously described direct effect on thebasic needs, such that inclusion increased basicneeds satisfaction relative to ostracism (B 1.94, p .001). We next examined whether thedirect effect was mediated by perceptions ofsimilarity. To test this hypothesis, we employedthe bootstrapping method developed byPreacher and Hayes (2004, 2008). This methodrequires that a relationship between an indepen-dent variable and a dependent variable exist tobe mediated, and that the “indirect effect be

statistically significant in the direction predictedby the mediation hypothesis” (Preacher &Hayes, 2004, p. 719). Having already estab-lished these requirements, we next performedthe bootstrapping procedures using the macrosprovided by Preacher and Hayes (2008) to de-termine whether the direct effect of inclusionarystatus on basic needs was mediated by percep-tions of similarity. This analysis yielded a 95%bias-corrected confidence interval (based on5,000 bootstrap samples) for the indirect effectthat did not include zero (95% CI [0.066, 0.70]).This analysis indicated that perceptions of per-ceived similarity mediated the direct effect.Greater social inclusion led to stronger percep-tions of similarity (B 0.92, p .002), and assimilarity increased, so too did participants’ ba-sic needs satisfaction (B 0.28, p .001). Thedirect effect, however, was mediated when in-cluding similarity (B 1.68, p .001).

However, we wished to examine whether thiswas true both when playing with same-race andother-race agents (i.e., did target race moderatethe mediated effect?). We conducted a moder-ated meditation analysis using Model 7 with theW moderator being target race (0 White, 1 Black). This yielded two bootstrapping models,each with 5,000 iterations. When participantsplayed with White agents, similarity did indeedmediate the effect of inclusion and ostracism onbasic needs satisfaction (95% bias-correctedconfidence interval did not include zero; 95%CI [0.079, 0.91]). However, similarity did notmediate the effect when participants playedwith Black agents (95% bias-corrected confi-dence interval did include zero; 95% CI [0.

Figure 1. The effect of inclusion and ostracism on basic needs satisfaction as a function ofthe in-group–out-group status of the agents.

134 SACCO, BERNSTEIN, YOUNG, AND HUGENBERG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 36: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

054, 0.39]). Thus, our moderated mediation hy-pothesis was supported.

Discussion

Consistent with past research, we found thatthe social pain stemming from ostracism ismore severe when administered by fellow in-group members (e.g., Bernstein et al., 2010).Conversely, social inclusion is experienced asrelatively more positive when administered byin-group relative to out-group members. Impor-tantly, we found that these enhanced effects ofsocial inclusion and ostracism observed in thein-group condition are mediated by perceivedsimilarity with those agents of inclusion or os-tracism. Not only do in-group members seemmore similar to the self when they include par-ticipants (and more dissimilar when they ostra-cize participants), but changes in the perceptionof similarity with agents mediate the relation-ship between inclusionary status and the fulfill-ment of basic affiliation needs. Critically, thisrelationship was only true when participantsplayed the game with racial in-group members;perceptions of similarity did not change, regard-less of the inclusionary experience, in the racialout-group condition (i.e., moderated media-tion).

Several theoretical positions are congruentwith our findings. First, out-group members arenaturally constrained with respect to how simi-lar they may be perceived to in-group members.Among racial out-groups, for whom the signalsof group differences (i.e., skin tone) are salient(e.g., Ito & Urland, 2003), it may be difficult toever completely overcome that difference solong as “race” is the primary category that de-fines the targets. This explanation is furthersupported by evidence showing that out-groupsare perceived as being more homogenous ascompared with in-groups (Park, Ryan, & Judd,1992). It may be that when individuals think ofin-group members, they are better able to imag-ine a greater degree of variation in how similarthey are to themselves; receiving positive feed-back (such as acceptance) may enhance theseperceptions of similarity, because positive inter-personal interactions can increase perceivedsimilarity (Morry, 2005). At the same time,receiving negative feedback may result in per-ceiving the agent as less similar to themselves(perhaps the group as well) as a means of dis-

tancing oneself from the aggressor and perhapsalso protecting generally positive perceptions ofthe in-group (e.g., Doosje, Ellemers, & Spears,1995). Alternatively, ostracism from the in-group may threaten one’s perceived standingwithin the in-group (Williams, 2007), causingthe observed dissimilarity and intensified nega-tivity.

Although past theory and research (e.g., Wil-liams, 2007) proposed that early stage reactionsto ostracism are essentially reflexive and insen-sitive to situational factors, the current resultsand other recent work (Bernstein et al., 2010)suggest that responses to ostracism are not sim-ply “all or nothing,” and instead can be morenuanced and contextualized. Furthermore, thecurrent results also mesh nicely with social paintheory (MacDonald & Leary, 2005), which pro-poses that social and psychical pain are phe-nomenologically and physiologically linked ex-periences (e.g., Eisenberger, Lieberman, &Williams, 2003). To the extent that reactions toostracism are similar to responses to physicalpain, it is sensible that the most serious socialinjuries (e.g., being ostracized by in-groups)should result in the most acute experiences ofsocial pain.

Nonetheless, a number of open questions stillremain. First, given that the current work usedrace as the operational definition of in-groupand out-group, it may appear unclear whetherthe current effects are due to an in-group effector due to status or power differences among thein-group and out-group. Although a sensiblealternative explanation, this hypothesis is noteasily congruent with previous findings. Indeed,Bernstein and colleagues (2010) found that bothBlack and White participants showed equiva-lent effects when socially included or ostracizedby racial in-group versus out-group members.Clearly, this pattern of data does not align easilywith a status or power interpretation of race.Second, in the current work, we did not testwhether similar results would be observed forother in-group– out-group distinctions. Here,too, we can point to past research to address thisissue. Given that the differential responses toostracism and social inclusion by in-group andout-group members only appear to occur forhighly essentialized groups (e.g., race; seeBernstein et al., 2010), but not for more arbi-trary groups or groups with weak affiliations(e.g., PC vs. Mac users; Smith & Williams,

135SOCIAL INCLUSION, OSTRACISM, AND SIMILARITY

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 37: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

2004), this indicates that the current mediationalpattern for in-groups would likely hold only forsubjectively relevant and highly essentializedin-groups.

In conclusion, consistent with past research,we found that reactions to social inclusion andostracism are exacerbated when perpetrated byfellow racial in-group members (compared withracial out-group members). Importantly, theseexacerbated reactions stemming from in-groupinclusion and ostracism are explained by fluc-tuations in perceived self-similarity with in-group members. Future research would benefitby understanding additional mechanisms poten-tially responsible for differentiated reactions toinclusion and ostracism by in-group members.

References

Acitelli, L. K., Kenny, D. A., & Weiner, D. (2001).The importance of similarity and understanding ofpartners’ marital ideals to relationship satisfaction.Personal Relationships, 8, 167–185. doi:10.1111/j.1475-6811.2001.tb00034.x

Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The needto belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments asa fundamental human motivation. PsychologicalBulletin, 117, 497–529. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497

Bernstein, M. J., Sacco, D. F., Young, S. G., Hugen-berg, K., & Cook, E. (2010). Being “in” with thein-crowd: The effects of social exclusion and in-clusion are enhanced by the perceived essentialismof the ingroup and outgroup. Personality and So-cial Psychology Bulletin, 36, 999 –1009. doi:10.1177/0146167210376059

Correll, J., & Park, B. (2005). A model of the ingroupas a social resource. Personality and Social Psy-chology Review, 9, 341–359. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0904_4

Doosje, B., Ellemers, N., & Spears, R. (1995). Per-ceived intragroup variability as a function of groupstatus and identification. Journal of ExperimentalSocial Psychology, 31, 410–436. doi:10.1006/jesp.1995.1018

Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams,K. D. (2003, October 10). Does rejection hurt? AnfMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302,290–292. doi:10.1126/science.1089134

Feingold, A. (1988). Matching for attractiveness inromantic partners and same-sex friends: A meta-analysis and theoretical critique. PsychologicalBulletin, 104, 226–235. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.104.2.226

Ferguson, C. K., & Kelley, H. H. (1964). Significantfactors in overevaluation of own-group’s product.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 69, 223–228.doi:10.1037/h0046572

Gonsalkorale, K., & Williams, D. (2007). The KKKwon’t let me play: Ostracism even by a despisedoutgroup hurts. European Journal of Social Psy-chology, 37, 1176–1186. doi:10.1002/ejsp.392

Gonzaga, G. C., Campos, B., & Bradbury, T. (2007).Similarity, convergence, and relationship satisfac-tion in dating and married couples. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 93, 34–48.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.93.1.34

Goodwin, S. A., Williams, K. D., & Carter-Sowell,A. R. (2010). The psychological sting of stigma:The costs of attributing ostracism to racism. Jour-nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 612–618. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.02.002

Hein, G., Silani, G., Preuschoff, K., Batson, C. D., &Singer, T. (2010). Neural responses to ingroup andoutgroup members’ suffering predict individualdifferences in costly helping. Neuron, 68, 149–160. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003

Ito, T. A., & Urland, R. (2003). Race and gender onthe brain: Electrocortical measures of attention tothe race and gender of multiply categorizable in-dividuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 85, 616–626. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.4.616

Jensen, A. R. (1978). Genetic and behavioral effectsof nonrandom mating. In R. T. Osborne, C. E.Noble, & N. J. Weyl (Eds.), Human variation:Biopsychology of age, race, and sex (pp. 51–105).New York, NY: Academic Press.

Leary, M. R., Haupt, A. L., Strausser, K. S., &Chokel, J. T. (1998). Calibrating the sociometer:The relationship between interpersonal appraisalsand state self-esteem. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 74, 1290–1299. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1290

MacDonald, G., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why doessocial exclusion hurt? The relationship betweensocial and physical pain. Psychological Bulletin,131, 202–223. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.2.202

McConnell, A. R., Brown, C. M., Shoda, T. M.,Stayton, L. E., & Martin, C. E. (2011). Friendswith benefits: On the positive consequences of petownership. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 101, 1239–1252. doi:10.1037/a0024506

Morry, M. M. (2005). Relationship satisfaction as apredictor of similarity ratings: A test of the attrac-tion-similarity hypothesis. Journal of Social andPersonal Relationships, 22, 561–584. doi:10.1177/0265407505054524

Park, B., Ryan, C. S., & Judd, C. M. (1992). Role ofmeaningful subgroups in explaining differences inperceived variability for in-groups and out-groups.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,553–567. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.553

136 SACCO, BERNSTEIN, YOUNG, AND HUGENBERG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 38: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS andSAS procedures for estimating indirect effects insimple mediation models. Behavior ResearchMethods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717–731.doi:10.3758/BF03206553

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptoticand resampling strategies for assessing and com-paring indirect effects in multiple mediator mod-els. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891.doi:10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., & Hayes, A. F.(2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypoth-eses: Theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multi-variate Behavioral Research, 42, 185–227. doi:10.1080/00273170701341316

Prentice, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2007). Psychologi-cal essentialism of human categories. Current Di-rections in Psychological Science, 16, 202–206.doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00504.x

Russell, R. J., & Wells, P. A. (1991). Personalitysimilarity and quality of marriage. Personality andIndividual Differences, 12, 407–412. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(91)90057-I

Smith, A., & Williams, K. D. (2004). R U there?Effects of ostracism by cell phone messages.Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice,8, 291–301. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.8.4.291

Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergrouprelations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 1–39.doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.33.020182.000245

Tajfel, H., Billig, M. G., Bundy, R. P., & Flament, C.(1971). Social categorization and intergroup be-haviour. European Journal of Social Psychology,1, 149–178. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420010202

Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review ofPsychology, 58, 425– 452. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641

Williams, K. D., Cheung, C. K. T., & Choi, W.(2000). Cyberostracism: Effects of being ignoredover the Internet. Journal of Personality and So-cial Psychology, 79, 748–762. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748

Williams, K. D., & Jarvis, B. (2006). Cyberball: Aprogram for use in research on ostracism and in-terpersonal acceptance. Behavior Research Meth-ods, 38, 174–180. doi:10.3758/BF03192765

Williams, K. D., & Zadro, L. (2005). Ostracism: Anindiscriminate early detection system. In K. D.Williams, J. P. Forgas, & W. von Hippel (Eds.),The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion,rejection, and bullying (pp. 19–34). New York,NY: Psychology Press.

Zadro, L., Boland, C., & Richardson, R. (2006). Howlong does it last? The persistence of the effects ofostracism in the socially anxious. Journal of Ex-perimental Social Psychology, 42, 692–697. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.10.007

Zadro, L., Williams, K. D., & Richardson, R. (2004).How low can you go? Ostracism by a computer issufficient to lower self-reported levels of belong-ing, control, self-esteem, and meaningful exis-tence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,40, 560–567. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2003.11.006

Received June 22, 2013Revision received August 27, 2013

Accepted August 27, 2013

E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!

Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will beavailable online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ andyou will be notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!

137SOCIAL INCLUSION, OSTRACISM, AND SIMILARITY

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 39: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Single-Case Designs in Group Work:Past Applications, Future Directions

Mark J. Macgowan and Stephen E. WongRobert Stempel College of Public Health & Social Work, Florida International University

This paper examined how group work researchers and practitioners have used single-case designs (SCDs) to evaluate interventions for improving group processes andoutcomes. Fifty-one group work studies published from 1972 to 2012 using SCDs andaddressing a wide range of problems were identified. The 8 types of SCDs applied inthese studies are discussed in relation to their methodology, relative strengths andlimitations, and applications in group work research and practice evaluation. Selectedstudies, which varied in quality and rigor, are described to illustrate design features andutility for group work. The review reveals the challenges and opportunities in usingSCDs, and it provides recommendations for designs best suited to answer particularquestions.

Keywords: group work, single-case designs, single-subject designs, single-system designs,practice evaluation

Single-case designs (SCDs), also known assingle-subject, single-participant, N-of-1, andsingle-system evaluation designs, can evaluateeffects of an intervention with only one subjector group. In most SCDs, repeated measures aretaken on a single subject or group under no-treatment (baseline) and treatment conditions,and the subject or group serves as its own con-trol. Controlled SCDs demonstrate experimentalcontrol by replicating treatment effects withineach single-case study through procedures such asrepeatedly introducing and removing the interven-tion; successively introducing the interventionacross different targets, settings, or subjects;randomly alternating between various condi-tions; or incrementally changing the level of theintervention. Results of SCDs are typicallygraphed for visual analysis, but there are also

numerous tests for determining statistically sig-nificant change.

SCDs have been structured to examine spe-cial features of the group modality. The designmay focus on functioning of individuals withinthe group, a subgroup of individuals, or thewhole group. SCDs can also include variablesthat reflect the group experience, such as mem-ber satisfaction, nature of interactions, and lead-ership. There are many potential focal pointssuch as intervention theory, individual groupmembers, group structures, group processes andleadership, all of which relate to group workoutcomes (Burlingame, Strauss, & Joyce,2013).

Benefits of Single-Case Designs forGroup Work

SCDs are considered a useful research designfor clinical practice across professions includ-ing psychology, social work, special education,and counselor education (Bloom, Fischer, &Orme, 2009; Heppner, Kivlighan, & Wampold,2008; Horner et al., 2005; Kazdin, 2003; Lun-dervold & Belwood, 2000; Vonk, Tripodi, &Epstein, 2007). They can be a valid source ofdata in determining empirically supported inter-ventions (Chambless et al., 1998). In groupwork it has been recommended as a valuable

Mark J. Macgowan and Stephen E. Wong, Robert Stem-pel College of Public Health & Social Work, Florida Inter-national University.

Both authors contributed equally to the writing of thispaper and are listed in alphabetical order.

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-dressed to Mark J. Macgowan or Stephen E. Wong, Schoolof Social Work, Robert Stempel College of Public Healthand Social Work, Florida International University, 11200SW 8th Street, Miami, FL 33199. E-mail: [email protected]; [email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice © 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 18, No. 2, 138–158 1089-2699/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/gdn0000003

138

Page 40: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

evaluation method (Garrett, 2005; Toseland &Rivas, 2012; Zastrow, 2006).

There are a number of important advantagesof SCDs for research and practice. SCDs offer ameans of obtaining valuable pilot data beforelarger between-groups designs are utilized(Petry, Martin, & Finocche, 2001; Petry, Wein-stock, & Alessi, 2011). Between-groups designsrequire a control group and a sufficient numberof subjects to satisfy the power requirements ofthe statistical test to be used for data analysis.However, SCDs can be conducted with a singleperson or one group containing as many mem-bers who attend. SCDs are also the most prac-tical technology available for evaluating one’sown practice, which is required by the Ameri-can Group Psychotherapy Association (AGPA,2007), Association for Specialists in GroupWork (ASGW, 2000, 2007), and the Associa-tion for the Advancement of Social Work withGroups (AASWG, 2006). Another benefit ofSCDs is their flexibility, as they can accommo-date a trial-and-error process allowing clini-cians to try out different group interventions inthe search for an effective one (Newman &Wong, 2004). This also makes SCDs particularlywell suited for practitioners using evidence-basedgroup treatments with diverse populations. Groupworkers using SCDs can determine whether anevidence-supported intervention or technique,validated with other populations, is effectivewith the particular cultural groups they are serv-ing (Macgowan, 2008; Macgowan & Hanbidge,2014).

Previous Reviews

Compared with between-groups designs,SCDs seldom appear in published group workresearch. Reviews of the history of group workresearch since the 1950s made no mention ofSCDs (Barlow, Burlingame, & Fuhriman, 2000;Barlow, Fuhriman, & Burlingame, 2004; Feld-man, 1987; Frank, 1975; Horne & Rosenthal,1997; McGrath, 1997). In a review of 54 studiesin a decade of group work research up to 1994,only two SCDs were identified (Tolman & Mo-lidor, 1994). As the authors noted, the lack ofstudies might have been a reflection of “publi-cation biases against such studies, rather thanresearcher rejection of such designs” (Tolman& Molidor, 1994, p. 152).

Regardless of the reasons, the focus in groupresearch has been on between-groups designs,with little attention on SCD technology. Withrespect to clinical practice, relatively few prac-titioners use SCDs, but instead rely on informalmethods to determine progress (Foster, Watson,& Young, 2002; Galassi & Gersh, 1993; Ven-timiglia, Marschke, Carmichael, & Loew,2000). In group work, the findings are similar.A survey of the use of SCDs by 54 schoolpractitioners found that none used SCDs in theirgroups but rather relied on subjective methods(Garrett, 2005, p. 251).

There have been a number of reviews ofSCDs in group work (Fike, 1980; Johnson,Beckerman, & Auerbach, 2001; Patterson &Basham, 2002; Robison, Morran, & Hulse-Killacky, 1989; Rose, 1977), a few of which canbe summarized. In his book on behavioral groupwork, Rose (1977, pp. 66–72) described sevenapplications of SCDs, illustrating five simplebaseline-treatment designs (AB), one reversaldesign (ABAB), and one multiple-baseline de-sign (MBD). These examples illustrated how touse SCDs to assess change in variables such asmember-to-member interactions, memberpraise statements, leader-to-member interac-tions, leader suggestion giving, positive affec-tive responses, suggestion-opinion giving re-sponses, information-giving responses, andsubjective appraisal of intervention proceduresby group members.

Fike (1980) noted how SCDs could be usedflexibly to capture the nature of change ingroups. He described the possible relationshipsbetween multiple formative/process variablesand outcome variables in groups. He argued thatdesigns simply recording individual outcomes,“do not give attention to all of the relevantgroup information” (Fike, 1980, p. 42). Accord-ing to Fike, SCDs should also include othergroup variables such as member satisfactionwith the group, quality of interactions, andgroup leadership.

Robison and colleagues (Robison et al.,1989) described two SCDs (ABAB and MBD)and statistical procedures (time-series analysesand nonparametric tests) that group workers coulduse to evaluate the effectiveness of their interven-tions. They cautioned that ABAB designs mightnot be able to determine causality with somedependent variables, such as cohesion, as thedevelopmental processes may occur irrespec-

139SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 41: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

tive of an intervention and might not be revers-ible through withdrawal of treatment. In suchcases, a MBD across clients or groups could beused to demonstrate causality. To improve theinternal validity of findings from a multiple-baseline across groups design, the authors rec-ommended random assignment of clients intothe different groups.

Purpose and Scope of Review

This paper advances previous reviews by in-cluding more recent studies; examining abroader range of SCD designs, their individualmethodologies, and relative strengths and weak-nesses; and making recommendations for prac-tice and research with groups. This review isrestricted to SCDs involving facilitated smallgroups whose purpose is to improve the psy-chosocial or socioemotional functioning of itsmembers. The term small group “implies theability of members to identify themselves asmembers, to engage in interaction, and to ex-change thoughts and feelings among themselvesthough verbal, nonverbal, and written commu-nication processes. Members can meet face-to-face, by telephone or video, or through com-puter networks” (Toseland & Rivas, 2012,p. 11).

We used a number of methods to locate andidentify SCDs in the literature. We restricted thesearch to scholarly journals of all years, up to2012. We searched a number of databases in-cluding social service abstracts, PsycINFO, So-ciofile, Medline, and Social Work Abstracts.The reference lists of other reviews (Garrett,2005; Johnson et al., 2001; Maughan, Christian-sen, Jenson, Olympia, & Clark, 2005; Smith,2012) were also examined, in addition to thosein recent SCD studies. The following termsrelated to group work and SCDs were used tolocate the literature, searching in the title, key-words, and abstract: single subject, single sys-tem, single case, multiple baseline, withdrawal,reversal, alternating treatments design, AB,ABA, BAB, ABAB, appearing with any of thefollowing terms: group work, group counseling,group treatment, group intervention, group ther-apy, group prevention, group approach, grouppsychotherapy, group training, and group in-struction. After an iterative process to selectstudies meeting the above criteria, 51 studieswere found.

In Table 1 and the following section we re-view SCDs used in extant group work research,beginning with the simplest and progressing tothe more complex designs. We briefly describethe logic behind each design, what the designcan accomplish, and how it was properly orless-than optimally applied in previous studies.We also note when these designs were used toinvestigate particular questions about groupprocesses or outcomes. Discussion and recom-mendations for research and practice follow.

Review of Single-Case Designs in GroupWork Studies

The first SCD in group work appeared in1972 with an additional five published in thatdecade (see Table 1). There were 15 publishedin the 1980s, 11 in the 1990s, and 19 since theyear 2000, representing a considerable increasefrom the previous decade. The discipline withthe most publications is Psychology followedby Social Work. Most of the studies have orig-inated in the United States but studies were alsoconducted in Argentina, New Zealand, and theUnited Kingdom.

B Design (Treatment Only)

Four of the publications in Table 1 used a Bdesign, which first appeared in the 1980s. It isthe simplest SCD and it is uncontrolled, con-sisting of merely taking repeated measurementsduring treatment. When outcome measures arecollected in this manner, various results arepossible: (a) measures might change in the de-sired direction, indicating a clinical improve-ment; (b) measures might not change signifi-cantly, indicating a lack of improvement despitetreatment; (c) measures might change in theundesired direction, indicating clinical deterio-ration; or (d) some intermediate result that fallsbetween a and b or between b and c. Theseresults are important for deciding whether tocontinue treatment and for making future treat-ment recommendations, making the B designvaluable for clinicians. However, although theB design is easy to do and it is superior tounsystematic or no collection of outcome data,it has serious limitations. If the client does im-prove during treatment, numerous alternativeexplanations such as history, maturation, testing(Campbell & Stanley, 1966; Cook & Campbell,

140 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 42: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Tab

le1

Sing

le-C

ase

Des

igns

Pre

viou

sly

App

lied

inG

roup

Wor

kR

esea

rch

Des

ign

Bri

efD

escr

iptio

nA

dvan

tage

sD

isad

vant

ages

Con

side

ratio

nsfo

rG

roup

Wor

kSt

udie

sU

sing

this

Des

ign

Cas

eSt

udy

&U

ncon

trol

led

Des

igns

BM

onito

rou

tcom

em

easu

res

duri

ngtr

eatm

ent.

Gra

phic

disp

lay

can

show

impr

ovem

ent,

noch

ange

,or

wor

seni

ngas

soci

ated

with

inte

rven

tion.

Sim

ples

t&

easi

est

SCD

toco

nduc

t.D

oes

not

requ

ire

ano

-tre

atm

ent

base

line

phas

e.

Una

ble

todr

awca

usal

infe

renc

es.

Susc

eptib

leto

conf

ound

sfr

omhi

stor

y,m

atur

atio

n,te

stin

g,na

tura

lhe

alin

g,&

extr

aneo

usev

ents

.

Prev

ious

rese

arch

ers

have

used

this

desi

gnto

mon

itor

iffo

rmat

ive

grou

ppr

oces

ses

are

occu

rrin

gas

expe

cted

.

(Cet

ingo

k&

Hir

ayam

a,19

83;

Font

ao&

Mer

gent

hale

r,20

08;

O’B

rien

etal

.,19

99;

Patte

rson

&B

asha

m,

2002

)

AB

&re

late

dde

sign

sA

fter

colle

ctin

gst

able

base

line

data

orda

tam

ovin

gin

aco

unte

r-th

erap

eutic

dire

ctio

n,ap

ply

the

inte

rven

tion

for

ape

riod

atle

ast

equa

lto

the

leng

thof

base

line.

Rel

ativ

ely

easy

&pr

actic

alto

cond

uct.

Som

etim

esno

tpo

ssib

leto

dela

ytr

eatm

ent

toco

llect

base

line

data

.Su

bsta

ntia

lch

ange

sev

iden

tin

asi

ngle

AB

desi

gnca

non

lysu

gges

ta

trea

tmen

tef

fect

.Sh

ould

not

mak

eca

usal

infe

renc

esfr

omth

isde

sign

beca

use

itis

susc

eptib

leto

conf

ound

sfr

omex

tran

eous

even

ts.

Mus

tco

nsid

erth

epo

ssib

ility

that

emer

gent

chan

ges

coin

cidi

ngw

ithtr

eatm

ent

are

due

tode

velo

pmen

tal

orin

tern

algr

oup

proc

esse

s.

(Hal

l,20

06;

John

son,

etal

.,20

01;

Led

gerw

ood,

etal

.,20

08[c

ount

erba

lanc

edA

B]

;N

elso

n&

Kel

ley,

2001

;R

ubin

,19

91;

Stev

ens,

2006

;T

osel

and,

Kre

bs,

&V

ahse

n,19

78;

Wad

e,O

rtiz

,&

Gor

man

,20

07[i

nter

rupt

edtim

e-se

ries

desi

gn])

AB

C,

AB

CD

AB

1B

2,

AB

1B

2B

3,

&re

late

dde

sign

s

Whe

nin

itial

trea

tmen

t(B

orB

1)

prov

esun

satis

fact

ory,

appl

ya

seco

ndtr

eatm

ent

(C)

orth

esa

me

trea

tmen

tat

ahi

gher

inte

nsity

(B2).

Ifpe

rfor

man

ceun

der

Cor

B2

prov

esun

satis

fact

ory,

appl

ya

thir

dtr

eatm

ent

(D)

orth

esa

me

trea

tmen

tat

anev

enhi

gher

inte

nsity

(B3).

Acc

omm

odat

esa

flexi

ble

tria

l&

erro

rpr

oces

s.A

seri

esof

inte

rven

tions

can

beap

plie

dun

tila

satis

fact

ory

outc

ome

isat

tain

ed.

Lik

ean

AB

desi

gn,

issu

scep

tible

toco

nfou

nds

from

extr

aneo

usev

ents

that

mig

htco

inci

dew

ithth

ese

emin

gly

effe

ctiv

etr

eatm

ent.

Add

ition

alth

reat

sto

this

desi

gnin

clud

eca

rryo

ver

effe

cts

from

prio

rtr

eatm

ents

.

Dev

elop

men

tal

&in

tern

algr

oup

proc

esse

sm

ayal

soco

nfou

ndth

ein

tern

alva

lidity

ofth

ese

desi

gns.

(Fry

,et

al.,

2009

[AB

C]

;Ph

aneu

f&

McI

ntyr

e,20

11[A

BC

D]

)

(tab

leco

ntin

ues)

141SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 43: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Tab

le1

(con

tinu

ed)

Des

ign

Bri

efD

escr

iptio

nA

dvan

tage

sD

isad

vant

ages

Con

side

ratio

nsfo

rG

roup

Wor

kSt

udie

sU

sing

this

Des

ign

Exp

erim

enta

l&

Con

trol

led

Des

igns

AB

A,

BA

B,

AB

AB

,A

BC

BC

,A

B1B

2B

1B

2,

&ot

her

reve

rsal

desi

gns

Rem

ove

anap

pare

ntly

effe

ctiv

etr

eatm

ent

orre

turn

toth

eco

nditi

onth

atpr

eced

edth

eap

pare

ntly

effe

ctiv

etr

eatm

ent

tore

vers

eob

serv

edim

prov

emen

ts,

&th

enre

intr

oduc

eth

etr

eatm

ent

tore

prod

uce

and

repl

icat

eits

effe

cts.

Rep

licat

ion

oftr

eatm

ent

effe

cts

prov

ides

ast

rong

dem

onst

ratio

nof

expe

rim

enta

lco

ntro

lov

erth

ede

pend

ent

vari

able

(goo

din

tern

alva

lidity

).D

esig

nca

nbe

cond

ucte

dw

ithon

lyon

epa

rtic

ipan

tor

one

grou

p.

Eth

ical

conc

erns

abou

tin

tent

iona

llyre

vers

ing

gain

sas

soci

ated

with

atr

eatm

ent.

Not

appl

icab

lew

ithso

me

inte

rven

tions

(e.g

.,ps

ycho

educ

atio

nal)

beca

use

thei

ref

fect

sar

eno

tre

vers

ible

.

Cer

tain

grou

ppr

oces

ses

(e.g

.,co

hesi

on,

role

diff

eren

tiatio

n)ar

eun

likel

yto

bere

vers

ible

mak

ing

the

desi

gnun

wor

kabl

e.

(Hau

serm

an,

etal

.,19

72[A

BA

B];

Hel

mus

,et

al.,

2003

[AB

A];

Kir

by,

etal

.,20

08;

Lin

sk,

etal

.,19

75[A

BA

B]

;M

arr

&Fa

irch

ild,

1993

[AB

AA

];M

artin

ez&

Won

g,20

09[A

BA

B]

;M

outo

n&

Stan

ley,

1996

[AB

A];

Petr

y,et

al.,

2001

[AB

1B

2C

A];

Vac

caro

,19

90[A

BA

B];

Web

er,

1980

[AB

AB

])

Con

curr

ent

mul

tiple

-bas

elin

ede

sign

Sim

ulta

neou

sly

colle

ctda

taon

two

orm

ore

equi

vale

ntba

selin

es.

Aft

erob

tain

ing

stab

lepr

e-in

terv

entio

nda

ta,

appl

yth

ein

terv

entio

nto

the

first

base

line.

Aft

erob

serv

ing

clea

rga

ins

onth

efir

stba

selin

e,ap

ply

the

inte

rven

tion

toth

ese

cond

base

line.

Rep

eat

this

proc

ess

with

thir

d&

subs

eque

ntba

selin

es.

Des

ign

does

not

requ

ire

are

vers

al&

ther

efor

eis

not

com

plic

ated

bypo

ssib

leet

hica

lco

ncer

ns&

irre

vers

ibili

ty.

As

the

trea

tmen

tis

repl

icat

edac

ross

diff

eren

tbe

havi

ors,

mem

bers

,or

grou

ps,

evid

ence

for

the

gene

raliz

abili

ty(e

xter

nal

valid

ity)

ofth

ein

terv

entio

nis

obta

ined

.

Des

ign

requ

ires

two

orm

ore

inde

pend

ent

base

lines

(i.e

.,si

mila

rbe

havi

ors,

setti

ngs,

part

icip

ants

,or

grou

ps).

Inte

rven

tion

isde

laye

dfo

rse

cond

&su

bseq

uent

base

lines

.

MB

Dac

ross

part

icip

ants

with

inth

esa

me

grou

pis

vuln

erab

leto

carr

y-ov

erac

ross

mem

bers

due

tom

odel

ing

effe

cts

&ch

ange

sin

grou

pin

tera

ctio

npa

ttern

s.G

ener

aliz

atio

nac

ross

part

icip

ants

orgr

oups

cann

otbe

dist

ingu

ishe

dfr

omlo

ssof

expe

rim

enta

lco

ntro

l(i

.e.,

extr

aneo

usef

fect

s).

(Bat

es,

1980

;C

lark

,C

unni

ngha

m,

&C

unni

ngha

m,

1989

;Fo

xx,

McM

orro

w,

Bitt

le,

&N

ess,

1986

;Fo

xx,

etal

.,19

83;

Gro

nna,

Sern

a,K

enne

dy,

&Pr

ater

,19

99;

Hal

l,et

al.,

2000

;

Hal

l,Sc

hles

inge

r,&

Din

een,

1997

;H

anse

n,et

al.,

1985

;

Han

sen,

St.

Law

renc

e,&

Chr

isto

ff,

1989

;

142 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 44: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Tab

le1

(con

tinu

ed)

Des

ign

Bri

efD

escr

iptio

nA

dvan

tage

sD

isad

vant

ages

Con

side

ratio

nsfo

rG

roup

Wor

kSt

udie

sU

sing

this

Des

ign

Kel

ly,

Lau

ghlin

,C

laib

orne

,&

Patte

rson

,19

79;

Kel

ly,

Wild

man

,U

rey,

&T

hurm

an,

1980

;L

umpk

in,

Silv

erm

an,

Wee

ms,

Mar

kham

,&

Kur

tines

,20

02[p

artia

lno

ncon

curr

ent

MB

D];

Phan

euf

&M

cInt

yre,

2007

;R

oess

ler

&L

ewis

,19

84;

Sern

a,Sc

hum

aker

,Sh

erm

an,

&Sh

eldo

n,19

91;

Tra

cey,

etal

.,19

74;

Tym

chuk

,A

ndro

n,&

Rah

bar,

1988

)N

oncu

rren

tm

ultip

le-b

asel

ine

desi

gn

The

NC

MB

Dre

sem

bles

asu

cces

sion

ofA

Bde

sign

s.In

itsst

rict

est

form

the

leng

thof

the

diff

eren

tba

selin

esva

ry&

are

rand

omly

assi

gned

toa

seri

esof

clie

nts

orgr

oups

.

Des

ign

does

not

requ

ire

runn

ing

mul

tiple

base

lines

sim

ulta

neou

sly.

The

NC

MB

Dre

plic

ates

trea

tmen

tef

fect

sac

ross

part

icip

ants

orgr

oups

&th

eref

ore

dem

onst

rate

sge

nera

lity

with

each

succ

essi

veba

selin

e.

Req

uire

sm

ultip

lepa

rtic

ipan

tsor

grou

ps.

Bec

ause

they

are

star

ted

atdi

ffer

ent

poin

tsin

time,

ther

eis

less

likel

ihoo

dof

carr

yove

ref

fect

sac

ross

part

icip

ants

&gr

oups

.

(Con

nors

,et

al.,

1984

;

Edl

eson

,et

al.,

1985

;

Ket

tlew

ell,

etal

.,19

92;

Lee

s&

Ron

an,

2008

)

(tab

leco

ntin

ues)

143SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 45: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Tab

le1

(con

tinu

ed)

Des

ign

Bri

efD

escr

iptio

nA

dvan

tage

sD

isad

vant

ages

Con

side

ratio

nsfo

rG

roup

Wor

kSt

udie

sU

sing

this

Des

ign

Alte

rnat

ing

trea

tmen

tsde

sign

Bas

elin

ese

ssio

ns&

trea

tmen

tse

ssio

nsar

era

pidl

yal

tern

ated

acco

rdin

gto

ara

ndom

sche

dule

.

Effi

cien

tde

sign

that

allo

ws

the

com

pari

son

ofm

ultip

letr

eatm

ents

with

ina

shor

tpe

riod

oftim

e.C

ansh

owtr

eatm

ent

effe

cts

even

whe

nba

selin

eda

tais

tren

ding

inth

ede

sire

ddi

rect

ion.

Bri

efpr

esen

tatio

nof

trea

tmen

tsm

aym

inim

ize

irre

vers

ibili

ty&

mul

tiple

trea

tmen

tin

terf

eren

ce.

Dif

ficul

tto

use

with

com

plex

ther

apy

prot

ocol

sth

atin

volv

ea

cum

ulat

ive

build

ing

proc

ess.

Giv

enra

pidl

yal

tern

atin

gtr

eatm

ents

,th

ere

isth

eri

skof

inte

ract

ion

effe

cts

with

typi

cal

grou

pde

velo

pmen

tev

ents

.

Can

beus

edto

test

effe

cts

ofin

terv

entio

nson

outc

omes

&fo

rco

mpa

ring

the

effe

cts

ofin

terv

entio

nson

with

in-g

roup

cond

ition

s(e

.g.,

enga

gem

ent)

.

(Bla

ke,

etal

.,19

90;

Gaj

ar,

etal

.,19

84;

Kas

tner

&M

ay,

2009

;

Won

g,et

al.,

1988

)

Mix

edor

com

bine

dde

sign

sC

ombi

ning

SCD

sor

embe

ddin

gon

eSC

Dw

ithin

anot

her

can

incr

ease

desi

gnm

anip

ulat

ions

topr

ovid

em

ore

dem

onst

ratio

nsof

expe

rim

enta

lco

ntro

l&

answ

erco

mpl

exqu

estio

ns.

Can

perm

itm

ore

soph

istic

ated

inqu

iry

&re

finem

ent

ofin

terv

entio

ns(e

.g.,

afte

rde

mon

stra

ting

anef

fect

ive

ther

apy

asse

ssin

gst

rate

gies

topr

omot

ege

nera

lizat

ion)

.

Mix

ed&

com

bine

dde

sign

sre

quir

ehi

gher

profi

cien

cyin

the

use

ofSC

Dm

etho

dolo

gy.

Com

plex

desi

gns

coul

dbe

stru

ctur

edto

prod

uce

sequ

entia

lch

ange

sin

grou

ppr

oces

ses

&gr

oup

outc

omes

tote

stth

ere

latio

nshi

psbe

twee

nth

ose

vari

able

s.

(Koh

ler

&Fo

wle

r,19

85;

Won

g,et

al.,

1996

)

A

rtic

leis

furt

her

revi

ewed

inth

ete

xtof

this

pape

r.

144 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 46: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

1979), and natural healing (Wong, 2010) pre-vent one from concluding that treatment causedthose improvements.

Group process measures can also be plottedduring a B design, revealing fluctuations in vari-ables such as verbal interaction patterns andgroup cohesion, which might mediate outcomesfor group members. A few group researchershave monitored group process during treatmentin this manner (Fontao & Mergenthaler, 2008;O’Brien, Korchynsky, Fabrizio, McGrath, &Swank, 1999; Patterson & Basham, 2002). Forexample, O’Brien and colleagues (1999) mea-sured positive process, satisfaction, and nega-tive process during stress management groups,and reported the findings to the group. In addi-tion, the intervention included increasing posi-tive interactions (e.g., active helping) during thelatter sessions of the group program. The re-searchers found that ratings of positive processand satisfaction increased significantly acrosssessions. These investigators further reportedthat satisfaction ratings were inversely corre-lated to cardiovascular reactivity to stress, whilenegative process ratings were positively corre-lated to this physiological measure. The authorsspeculated that the planned emphasis on posi-tive interaction may have improved ratings overtime. More recently, Fontao and Mergenthaler(2008) examined the verbal content of femalepatients at four measurement points in a psy-chodynamic group for eating disorders. As pre-dicted by the researchers, group members’ lan-guage patterns (“emotion-abstraction patterns”)significantly related to particular therapeuticfactors, which also suggested the presence of apositive therapeutic cycle. The researchersnoted that such language patterns may be usedto identify the occurrence of therapeutically im-portant points in the process of group work.

Although process measures can serve asproximal indicators of change and are importantfor assessing whether desired therapeutic pro-cesses unfold as planned, the ultimate test ofgroup therapy is showing improvement in clientoutcomes. Cetingok and Hirayama (1983) useda B design consisting of four assessments con-ducted at the first, third, fifth, and seventhweeks of their program to assess whether par-ticipation in groups was associated with gains inhealth care knowledge and mental health statusfor elderly persons with diabetes, heart disease,and hypertension. The investigators found no

significant gains in any of their dependent mea-sures. Cetingok and Hirayama explained theirunexpected findings by noting that their elderlysubjects probably entered the program well in-formed about their health care needs and appar-ently became suspicious that the mental healthassessment data might be used to expel themfrom their rent-subsidized, high-rise apart-ments.

AB Design (Baseline; Treatment)

The AB design is a substantial improvementover the B design and unlike the latter could beconsidered an actual “design” in that it providesa minimal degree of experimental control. Con-trol comes from the baseline data (during whichno treatment is administered) to which treat-ment data is compared. If the baseline data arestable or moving in a countertherapeutic direc-tion and this pattern changes shortly after theintroduction of the intervention, these resultscan give evidence that the intervention might beeffective. Unlike the B, the AB design elimi-nates maturation (i.e., processes associated withthe mere passage of time) and testing (i.e., mea-surement reactivity) as alternative explanationsof observed change (Campbell & Stanley,1966). Unfortunately, the AB design is stillsusceptible to “history” or extraneous eventsthat happen to coincide with treatment (Camp-bell & Stanley, 1966; Hersen & Barlow, 1976),such as the arrival or departure of groupmembers. Eight studies used this design sincefirst appearing in the literature in the 1990s,with most published since the year 2000 (seeTable 1).

Rubin (1991) aggregated a set of AB designsto evaluate outreach counseling and supportgroups for battered women. A formidable chal-lenge to his evaluation effort was that interven-tions used at the agency were “somewhat unsys-tematic,” theoretically eclectic, “idiosyncratic,”and “. . . varied from session to session and acrosspractitioners (group leaders)” (p. 340). With twosets of measures collected on each woman, onescoring desired thoughts, feelings, and behav-iors and the other scoring abusive behavior bythe spouse, neither measure showed positivechange that could be attributed to agency inter-ventions.

Johnson et al. (2001) obtained more favor-able results when examining whether leader-

145SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 47: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

initiated changes influenced group processes inthree AB design studies implemented by under-graduate students and practitioners. The threestudies included (a) student leaders of an ado-lescent girls’ group encouraging the develop-ment of trusting relationships between membersand the effect on subsequent group attendance,(b) interventions to increase participation by the“silent majority” in a group of veterans andthe number of members later speaking up in thegroup, and (c) encouragement of self-expres-sion through puppetry in children who had ex-perienced loss and emotional trauma and thenumber of children then using this mode ofexpression. Changes in dependent measuresfrom the A to B phases were statistically sig-nificant in the first two studies. In the thirdstudy, statistical tests were not used, but therewas 200% improvement from baseline to inter-vention mean scores.

Ledgerwood, Alessi, Hanson, Godley, andPetry (2008) used counterbalanced AB designs(A1A2B; A1BA2) across four clinics to gaugethe effects of contingency management onattendance at substance abuse therapy groups.Three randomly selected clinics were exposedto an A1A2B design composed of the followingconditions: An 8-week “baseline” of standardtreatment (A1), 16 additional weeks of standardtreatment (A2), and 16 weeks of standard treat-ment plus contingency management (materialincentives for attendance) (B). A fourth ran-domly selected clinic was exposed to an A1BA2

design, which resembled the previous designexcept that it presented the last two phases inreverse order. Clients who were enrolled in theclinics when contingency management was im-plemented attended a significantly greater per-centage of therapy sessions than clients whowere enrolled in treatment without contingencymanagement.

ABC, ABCD, AB1B2, AB1B2B3, and RelatedDesigns

If the outcome in the B phase of an ABdesign is unsatisfactory, a second intervention(C) can be tested in an ABC design, or the sameintervention at a higher magnitude (B2) can betested in an AB1B2 design. If the outcome of Cor B2 proves unsatisfactory, a third treatment(D) can be tested in an ABCD design, or thesame treatment can be tested at an even higher

magnitude (B3) in an AB1B2B3 design. Thishighlights one of the advantages of SCDs;namely, their flexibility and ability to incorpo-rate an evidence-based, trial-and-error processwithin clinical practice. This flexibility is cru-cial for accommodating the variable responsesof individuals and groups to particular interven-tions.

Although the ABC, ABCD, AB1B2,AB1B2B3, and related designs permit the mod-ification of interventions until the client orgroup has improved to a satisfactory level, thesedesigns are unable to determine which of theinterventions, if any, actually produced the im-provement. This is because these designs aremerely complicated versions of AB designs andare also susceptible to extraneous events thatmight have coincided with any of the seeminglyeffective treatment phases. In addition, se-quence and carryover effects are possible inABC and ABCD designs in that positivechanges in C or D phases actually might be dueto delayed effects of previous treatment phases.Interpretation of group work data is even trick-ier, because the conduct and affect of groupmembers and the nature of the group experienceis expected to change as a group moves throughdevelopmental stages.

Two studies have recently utilized these de-signs. Fry, Botterill, and Pring (2009) evaluatedtwo formats of group-based speech-languagetherapy on the stuttering of an 18-year-old maleclient (in a group of up to 10 members) using anABC design with a maintenance phase. After a5-week baseline (A), 2 weeks of intensive groupinstruction was provided during one treatmentphase (B), followed by 5 weeks of consolida-tion/self-management training in a second treat-ment phase (C). Finally, follow-up measureswere taken during a 10-month maintenancephase. Compared to the A phase, there was asignificant reduction in the percent of stutteredsyllables per session and in the duration ofstuttering in the B and C phases, and this im-provement continued or increased in the main-tenance phase. Although introduction of theinterventions was associated with decreasedstuttering, as intended, it was not known if theinterventions were responsible for this improve-ment.

A group intervention has also been added toother therapeutic modalities in a stepwise ABCtreatment design. Phaneuf and McIntyre (2011)

146 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 48: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

used such a design in a three-tier parent trainingprogram involving parents of preschool chil-dren with developmental disabilities (n 8parent–child dyads). Each tier included an in-tervention component followed by an evalua-tion to determine who either responded favor-ably to the intervention, or who would needfurther service in the next intervention tier. Thefirst phase included 3 weeks of individual par-ticipants listening to or reading materials drawnfrom Webster-Stratton’s Incredible Years pro-gram, followed by an evaluation to determineeither who responded and would be scheduledfor a 3-month follow up assessment, or whowould move on to the second intervention tier.The second tier involved 11 weeks of Webster-Stratton’s group-based Incredible Years ParentTraining Program, followed by another evalua-tion. Those who responded were scheduledfor the 3-month follow up, and those whoneeded the final tier received a single session ofindividualized video feedback training and werescheduled for the last 3-month follow up. Allbut one child displayed reductions in observedinappropriate behavior or parental ratings ofchild behavior problems.

ABA, BAB, ABAB, ABCBCB, AB1B2B1B2,and Other Reversal Designs

Inherent weaknesses of AB, ABC, andABCD designs can be rectified by removing theseemingly effective treatment and returning tothe previous experimental condition to reverseobserved improvements, and then reintroducingthe treatment to replicate its positive effects.Replication of treatment effects by reapplyingan intervention and reproducing its results is apowerful demonstration of causality (Sidman,1960). The drawbacks of reversal designs in-clude ethical concerns about removing an ap-parently effective treatment merely for the pur-pose of demonstrating its efficacy, irreversibleeffects of certain interventions (such as psy-choeducational therapies conveying knowledgethat cannot be removed), and changes in certaingroup-based dependent variables (e.g., cohe-sion) attributable to group maturation that alsomight be irreversible. Ten reversal designs havebeen used in group work since the early 1970s,representing the second most utilized SCD (seeTable 1).

In a landmark study, both for its use of pre-cise observational measures of group interac-tion and SCD evaluation methodology, Linsk,Howe, and Pinkston (1975) included severalABAB designs to gauge the effect of question-asking on appropriate verbalizations by elderlyresidents in activity groups in a nursing home.Following a baseline phase in which group ac-tivities were conducted in the agency’s usualmanner, a second phase gauged the effects ofthe group leader asking residents as many ques-tions as possible during the group activity. In athird phase, the group leader returned to thecustomary manner of directing the group. In thefourth and final treatment phase, the groupleader resumed the procedure of increased ques-tion-asking. Linsk and colleagues showed thatmore frequent questions doubled the number ofappropriate verbalizations relative to baselinesessions using the agency’s usual procedure.

Weber (1980) investigated the impact of vid-eotape feedback on adolescents in inpatientgroup therapy, hypothesizing that it would in-crease verbal expressions of warmth and de-crease expressions of hostility and flight. Dur-ing experimental sessions, the first 45 minutesof each group session was videotaped and thenimmediately played back to the members. Theeffects of videotaping and playback were eval-uated within an ABAB design with each phaselasting 2 weeks. There were inconsistent resultsacross the three measures of verbal affect. Therewas some support for an increase in warmthexpressed during videotaping. However, trendswithin phases were unstable with warmth in-creasing in the initial baseline phase withoutvideotape feedback and decreasing rapidly atthe end of the final videotape feedback phase.For flight there was some reduction in the num-ber of responses during videotape feedback;but, here too, flight responses were decliningsteadily at the end of both baseline phases with-out videotape feedback. Finally, hostilityshowed no consistent reduction during the pres-ence of videotape feedback. Different statisticaltests for change in the SCD data yielded con-flicting results, showing how these analyses canbe contradictory. Contributing to the inconsis-tent findings was high variability within thephases and a predetermined length for all phases(2 weeks). Sometimes experimental conditionswere altered before data within each phase hadstabilized. Had the investigator waited until data

147SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 49: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

within phases had stabilized before changingconditions, he might have been able to moreclearly identify the levels and trends withinphases and then been able to detect changes insubsequent phases.

Martinez and Wong (2009) utilized anABAB design to evaluate the effects of writtenand telephone prompts on attendance at groupsupport meetings for women survivors of do-mestic abuse. Participants of this study wereresidents of a transitional housing program whoupon admission to the program were notified ofthe group support meetings along with otherservices offered by the agency. After 2 weeks ofbaseline in which no additional steps were takento increase attendance at the support meetings, acolorful written invitation to attend the meetingwas delivered and a single phone call was madeto each resident the morning of the meeting for2 weeks. Next, the baseline condition of noprompts was reinstituted for 2 weeks, whichwas followed by a final 3-week phase in whichwritten and telephone prompts were reintro-duced. During baseline phases attendance waslimited to the same three residents. During thefirst prompting phase the number of residents inattendance doubled. However, when promptswere discontinued attendance fell back to thelevel observed in baseline. In the final prompt-ing phase the number of residents again in-creased, this time to more than triple what wasrecorded in baseline.

Multiple-Baseline Designs (MBDs)

MBDs have been the most frequently usedSCD in group work (k 21), probably becausethey lack troublesome reversal phases and be-cause group work involves multiple clientswhose progress sometimes can be monitoredwithin separate baselines. Also contributing tothe common use of this design is the prevalenceof social skills training in group work whereMBDs have been utilized to assess the effects ofsequential training of specific social skills. Thissection will review several illustrative groupwork studies using concurrent and nonconcur-rent MBDs.

Concurrent MBD. As noted above, ethicalconcerns and irreversibility of certain types ofchange are limitations of ABAB and other re-versal designs. Concurrent MBDs do not con-tain reversal phases and circumvent these prob-

lems by simultaneously collecting data on twoor more independent baselines—consisting ei-ther of different behaviors/targets, the same be-haviors/targets in different settings, or the samebehaviors/targets in different group members(or groups)—and then sequentially introducingthe intervention across these baselines. If im-provements occur when, and only when, anintervention is successively applied to two ormore of these baselines, then the intervention’seffectiveness is demonstrated. Because concur-rent MBDs replicate treatment effects acrossbehaviors or across group members, they candemonstrate some treatment impact acrossthose two dimensions. Drawbacks to using con-current MBDs can be the lack of two or moreindependent but equivalent baselines (whichmust respond similarly to the same interven-tion) and the requirement to delay the interven-tion for the second and subsequent baselines.The concurrent MBD was used in 17 groupwork studies.

Hansen, St. Lawrence, and Christoff (1985)delivered a problem-solving skills interventionfor challenging situations in community livingto seven patients in a partial hospitalizationprogram. Hansen and colleagues utilized in-structions, modeling, behavior rehearsal, andverbal reinforcement during group sessions tosuccessively teach skill components of problemidentification, goal definition, solution evalua-tion, evaluation of alternatives, and selection ofthe best alternative. Group data plotted within aconcurrent MBD across skill componentsshowed that the five skills were rarely exhibitedduring baseline phases, but these same skillsshowed immediate and significant increases fol-lowing training.

Hall, Dineen, Schlesinger, and Stanton(2000) used a MBD to evaluate the effective-ness of group training in verbal interactive skillsfor developmentally disabled adults in a voca-tional training program. Six complex verbalskills (e.g., “social conversation”) were succes-sively taught with instructions, modeling, re-hearsal, and feedback over a 7-week period, andthe effects of training were evaluated with aconcurrent MBD across responses. There weremixed or null effects of the training procedureon clients’ rated skills. Hall and colleaguesnoted that this poor outcome might have beendue to their training schedule that required lead-ers to begin training on additional skills before

148 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 50: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

previously trained skills had been mastered, andthe low proficiency of group trainers who weremostly first-year graduate students.

Nonconcurrent MBD (NCMBD).NCMBDs are a variant of the MBD in whichthe baselines do not all start at the same time orrun simultaneously. If implemented strictly ac-cording to the first published description of thisdesign (Watson & Workman, 1981), the pre-treatment baseline phases are of different lengthsand the assignment of these different baselinelengths to individual clients is randomized. In thepublished literature, however, NCMBDs contain-ing different baseline lengths and random as-signment of these baselines has been inconsis-tent. Because the separate baselines of a NCMBDare initiated at different times, and can even bestarted after the completion of other baselines, aNCMBD is essentially a series of AB designsand is relatively easy to carry out. Furthermore,because replication of intervention effects in theseparate AB designs of the NCMBD are con-ducted with different participants, this designdemonstrates some generality of interventioneffects across subjects. There were four exam-ples of this design in the literature (see Table 1).

Connors, Johnson, and Stuckey (1984) im-plemented an NCMBD in a study of brief psy-choeducational group therapy for the treatmentof bulimia. Treatment consisted of education,self-monitoring, goal setting, assertion training,relaxation, and cognitive restructuring, whichwas applied with two groups of 10 normal-weight bulimic women, one group 3 weeks be-fore the other. The investigators reported a 70%reduction in binge/purge episodes and signifi-cant improvements in self-esteem, depression,assertiveness, and attitudes about eating thatcoincided with the introduction of treatment foreach group.

Edleson, Miller, Stone, and Chapman (1985)used a “modified” MBD that resembled a NCMBDto evaluate a 12-week, small-group treatmentfor men who battered their partners. Groupswere guided through topics of analyzing chainsof events leading to violent situations, progres-sive relaxation, cognitive restructuring, problemsolving, taking the partner’s view of the prob-lem, and negotiation skills. Three groups begantreatment at three different points in time. Edle-son and colleagues reported substantial declinesin the mean incidence of self-reported spouseabuse in two of the three groups as they began

participating in the program, which they pointedto as evidence of the program’s effectiveness. Acouple of features of this study, however, limitits ability to demonstrate treatment efficacy: (a)the baseline data for all three groups were re-constructed retrospectively, which greatly in-creased the possibility of error; and (b) grouptreatment was preceded by 2 weeks of intakeinterviews, during which all groups showed im-mediate “decreases” in spouse abuse from base-line levels. The decline during the interviewperiod, before any treatment was administered,suggests that decreases in abuse may have beendue to the intake interviews or participants’admission to the program rather than the treat-ment itself.

Kettlewell, Mizes, and Wasylyshyn (1992)met all of the strict requirements for a NCMBDin a study evaluating group cognitive–behav-ioral treatment for bulimia. Kettlewell and hisassociates used a comprehensive treatment thatincluded information on medical and psycho-logical consequences of bulimia, goal setting,functional analysis of the binge-purge cycle,selecting an appropriate weight for oneself, cop-ing strategies, combating irrational cognitionsabout thinness, and ways to maintain progress.This treatment was applied after 2, 6, or 10weeks of stable baselines with three groups ofclients, the length of baseline for each groupbeing randomly determined. The weekly rate ofbinging and purging markedly declined for allthree groups after the onset of treatment and,with the exception of one client, lower rates ofeating disorders were mostly maintained duringfollow-up assessments.

Alternating Treatments Design (ATD)

Group workers may want to know whichintervention is most efficacious for their mem-bers, and the ATD (Barlow & Hayes, 1979) is adesign specifically for the purpose of comparingtwo or more interventions. The ATD makescomparisons between interventions by rapidlyalternating (e.g., from one group session to thenext) between two or more treatment conditionsand baseline conditions on a random schedule.Sometimes a sign or cue is used to identify thesession (e.g., “Positive Interactions Session”) sothat participants can distinguish which condi-tion is currently in effect. The resulting data aregraphed and data points from the same condi-

149SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 51: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

tion are connected by lines to form curves rep-resenting group members’ performance duringthe different conditions. The distance separatingthe curves represents the differential effectsof the various conditions. The ATD is an effi-cient design that can reveal differences betweenconditions after a small number of sessions.Potential problems of the ATD are contrast ef-fects and multiple-treatment interference, whichare experimental artifacts brought on by closejuxtaposition of intervention conditions orcross-contamination and interaction of the dif-ferent conditions, respectively. Given thatgroups develop and mature over time, there is agood likelihood of interdependency betweenphases, producing such interaction effects.These threats can be reduced by ending theATD with a final phase in which the interven-tion with the best outcome is presented aloneconsecutively for several sessions. If perfor-mance within this final phase is similar to thatrecorded in the earlier alternating treatmentsphase of the study, then the results were not dueto contrast effects or multiple-treatment inter-ference.

As seen in Table 1, the first publications thatincluded the ATD in group work appeared inthe 1980s (k 2), with only two more over thenext two decades. Wong and colleagues (1988)used an ATD to assess the effects of two typesof structured group activities (e.g., team sports,art projects) on bizarre psychotic and appropri-ate behaviors in 10 male chronic psychiatricpatients on a hospital ward. On a random sched-ule, the group of patients participated in one ofthree different types of sessions: (a) structuredgroup activities with positive reinforcement (to-kens earned through active participation), (b)structured group activities with noncontingentreinforcement (tokens given regardless of thelevel of participation), or (c) baseline sessions(control sessions in which no structured activitywas offered). Psychotic behavior was signifi-cantly lower (70%) and appropriate behaviorwas substantially higher (approximately 500%)in the recreational sessions as opposed to thebaseline sessions. Data for the two recreationalconditions were roughly equivalent indicatingthat the beneficial effect was due to the recre-ational activities and not contingent reinforce-ment.

Blake, Owens, and Keane (1990) used anATD to evaluate the efficacy of a single pack-

age intervention, verbal and written announce-ments of daily activities plus individual feed-back and praise, on patients’ attendance atgroup activities on a postacute psychiatric tran-sition unit. On a weekly basis for the 20 weeksof the study, these procedures to encourage at-tendance alternated randomly with the unit’scustomary procedure of having nursing staffverbally remind patients that they were ex-pected to attend all groups as part of their treat-ment. Attendance steadily increased in bothconditions during the length of the study, butattendance was significantly higher in theprompting and feedback condition.

Kastner and May (2009) utilized an ATD togauge the effects of action-oriented techniques(AO) on group climate (using the Group Cli-mate Questionnaire–Short Version, MacKenzie,1983) and disruptive out-of-group behavior on agroup of seven middle-school students referredfor behavioral or emotional difficulties. Groupsessions consisted of discussion on topics se-lected by group members and alternated ran-domly between sessions with AO (i.e., thedouble, the empty chair, role reversal, and psy-chodramatic and behavioral role play) and ses-sions without AO. There were no significantdifferences between the two conditions in thefrequency of disruptive behaviors, ratings ofgroup engagement, or ratings of avoidance.However, ratings of engagement increased andratings of avoidance decreased significantlyover time, whereas ratings of conflict were un-changed.

Mixed and Combined Designs

Although articles and textbooks on SCDspresent these formats as relatively fixed struc-tures, the essence of all of these designs is thesystematic manipulation of the independentvariable to produce a corresponding pattern ofchanges in the dependent variable that is un-likely to have been due to random fluctuation orextraneous events. Researchers who appreciatethe underlying logic of SCDs can fully exploittheir flexibility by mixing or combining themanipulations characteristic of the different de-signs (e.g., reversals, successive application oftreatment across behaviors) to fit the needs ofthe particular study or to investigate additionalquestions of interest (Kazdin, 2011; Kennedy,2005; Smith, 2012; Wong & Liberman, 1981).

150 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 52: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

The major drawback to mixed and combineddesigns is that they are more intricate and re-quire more sophisticated understanding of SCDmethodology.

Kohler and Fowler (1985) used a combina-tion of concurrent MBDs and ABA designs in astudy teaching three young children to invitepeers to play and to use social amenities (e.g.,“thank you,” “please”). The study evaluatedeffects of group training and the termination oftraining (during the reversal phase) on the chil-dren’s behavior and their peers’ responses out-side of group in a classroom. For two of thethree children, the MBD showed that trainingincreased invitations and amenities performedby the children and those responses were recip-rocated by their peers. Termination of trainingresulted in a reduction of invitations and ame-nities, but not to the near zero levels observed inthe initial baseline. For the third child (who wasonly taught invitations), group training in-creased the rate of invitations in the target childbut these responses were not reciprocated by herpeers, and termination of training resulted in areturn to the low levels of invitations observedin baseline. For this child, additional training ofthe target child and her peers along with grouprewards was implemented to raise the numberof invitations. This condition increased both thenumber of invitations given by the child andmade by her peers, and when these procedureswere terminated, the level of invitations re-mained above that recorded in previous baselinephases. The complicated questions about lastingeffects of training and the testing of proceduresto increase invitations for the third child couldnot have been answered by using a MBD or anABA design alone. Additional experimentalmanipulations were needed for these purposes.

Wong, Morgan, Crowley and Baker (1996)used a mixed ABC design and a concurrentMBD across subjects to evaluate minimal in-situ training to promote generalization of socialskills taught to three adolescent psychiatric in-patients. After collecting baseline data (A), the“Stacking the Deck” table game (Foxx, McMor-row, & Schloss, 1983) (B) was used to teachand reinforce appropriate interactive skills tothe three adolescents. The game involved re-sponding to social scenarios described on gameplaying cards, modeling of correct responses,giving praise and the opportunity to move one’sgame piece after correct responses to the situa-

tion, and positive (monetary) reinforcement.Covert generalization assessments conductedon the hospital ward revealed that despite sub-stantial increases in trained responses in thegame setting, these skills were rarely exhibitedon the ward where patients resided. To promotegeneralization of the skills beyond the gamesituation, minimal in-situ training involving in-termittent verbal prompts and tangible rein-forcement (C) were administered when theskills were used during social interactions onthe ward. Minimal in-situ training was success-fully applied with each of the three adolescentsimmediately producing substantial improve-ment in skill usage on the ward in each patient,thereby replicating treatment effects in the pat-tern of a concurrent MBD across subjects. Thus,this study utilized an ABC design to show thatgame training (B) failed to produce changes thatgeneralized to the more naturalistic ward set-ting, but that minimal in-situ training (C) didengender those changes, which was proven byreplicating those improvements across subjectsin the concurrent MBD.

Discussion and Recommendations

Although group work researchers have notused SCDs frequently, they have used a rangeof designs to evaluate diverse interventionsaimed at changing a broad spectrum of depen-dent variables. The number of published reportsusing SCDs in group work found in this review(k 51) is substantially larger than the tworeported in Tolman and Molidor’s paper (1994).After a decrease in publications in the 1990s,there has been growth in the past decade. Per-haps this is an encouraging sign.

SCDs have been used to investigate bothprocesses and outcomes of group therapy usingboth individual and group levels of measure-ment. Most studies focused on group outcomes,across a broad range of target areas, includingsocial skills, eating disorders, anxiety, self-expression, warmth and hostility, and self-efficacy. The variables focusing on the groupitself (e.g., processes) included attendance(Blake et al., 1990; Helmus, Saules, Schoener,& Roll, 2003; Johnson et al., 2001; Kirby, Ker-win, Carpenedo, Rosenwasser, & Gardner,2008; Martinez & Wong, 2009; Petry et al.,2001), verbal participation (Gajar, Schloss,Schloss, & Thompson, 1984; Hauserman, Zwe-

151SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 53: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

back, & Plotkin, 1972; Johnson et al., 2001;Kohler & Fowler, 1985; Linsk et al., 1975;Tracey, Briddell, & Wilson, 1974), group inter-action (Toseland, et al., 1978), language pat-terns (Fontao & Mergenthaler, 2008), groupclimate (Kastner & May, 2009), and satisfactionwith group (O’Brien, et al., 1999; Patterson &Basham, 2002).

Most studies used the individual as the levelof measurement and then derived a group meanscore for analytical purposes. This method ofanalysis, though convenient and appropriate inmany cases, obscures individual and subgroupdifferences. Patterson and Basham (2002) ex-amined satisfaction with group at both the indi-vidual and group levels using an innovativemapping procedure, showing the importance ofexamining both simultaneously. Rather than us-ing individual scores or the mean score for thegroup as the unit of analysis, this approachshowed variability in both individual and groupoutcomes. Researchers should be aware of thebenefits and limitations of individual and grouplevels of measurement and analysis, and selectthe most appropriate method.

The quality and rigor of group work studiesusing SCD methodology varied to a large de-gree. SCDs are merely evaluation tools and theycannot rise above other key components of theclinical study. For example, in the case of Rubin(1991), SCDs were unable to produce consistentor interpretable results for interventions that theauthor himself admitted were unsystematic andvacillated from session to session. It is unclearhow such a study could yield reliable results, orif such results emerged how they would beunderstood. This also raises the question aboutwhat treatments lend themselves to SCDs. Asindicated in Table 1, most interventions incor-porated forms of behavior therapy, but othersutilized solution focused (Nelson & Kelley,2001), experiential (Patterson & Basham,2002), couples group therapy (Nelson & Kelley,2001), and other well-defined techniques (John-son et al., 2001). This shows that SCDs canaccommodate many different types of therapy.However, whatever theoretical treatment ap-proach is being evaluated, it must be appliedsystematically and consistently to produce in-terpretable results. This is true of SCDs as wellas between-groups designs.

The study by Edleson and colleagues (1985)presented a different set of concerns by using a

reconstructed baseline and interpreting the result-ing data in a questionable manner. The doubtfulaccuracy of reconstructed baseline data was aweakness in itself, but that combined with desiredchanges occurring prior to the introduction of ther-apy should have discouraged the authors fromdrawing conclusions about treatment efficacy.Most of the other studies cited here, however,applied SCDs in a technically sound manner andtheir results were more definitive.

Recommendations for Research

Use of SCDs in group work appears to be atan intermediate level of development where anumber of designs have been used and somefairly often, but there are opportunities for morevaried and refined applications. Group workresearchers have not always distinguished be-tween uncontrolled case study SCDs (e.g., B,AB, ABC) and controlled experimental SCDs(e.g., ABAB, MBDs), only the latter of whichreplicate treatment effects and thereby permitlogical inference of treatment efficacy. Thereare efforts underway to raise the standards forSCD research. Guidelines have been developedfor rating SCDs based on their level of evidencefor demonstrating a causal relationship (Smith,2012). According to such guidelines (RomeiserLogan, Hickman, Harris, & Heriza, 2008; WhatWorks Clearinghouse, 2013), a MBD with threebaselines could provide a convincing demon-stration of treatment efficacy, but a MBD withless than three baselines could not. This is note-worthy, as the proposed standard is more rigor-ous and stringent than that previously presentedin published research and textbooks coveringSCDs (Bailey & Burch, 2002; Cooper, Heron,& Heward, 2007).

Future research could apply SCDs not used inprevious group work studies. One example isthe Changing Criterion Design, which is appli-cable in situations where a dependent variablecan be tightly controlled (Hall & Fox, 1977;Hartmann & Hall, 1976). This design requiresone group member or group and one indepen-dent variable that produces stepwise changes inthe dependent variable. For example, it may beunreasonable to expect that a group memberwith limited social competence be fully en-gaged in the group within a few sessions as aresult of an engagement-building intervention.Instead, incremental engagement objectives

152 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 54: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

may be set over a number of group sessions(e.g., 25% more engaged per set of sessions),leading to full engagement as the ultimate goal.Other novel designs and experimental manipu-lations that could be used to explore group workquestions or to strengthen the internal validityof a SCD study involve randomization. Pointsin time that interventions are started or the orderin which group members or groups are exposedto treatment are randomly determined, and suchrandomization can be incorporated into almostany SCD (Edgington, 1975; Kratochwill &Levin, 2010). Randomization is likely to requireSCDs with longer baseline phases, but this maybe acceptable in some group work research (see,for example, Toseland, et al., 1978).

An important question that group workerscould explore using SCDs is whether groupprocesses directly impact group outcomes. Thisresearch could be conducted with two-stageSCDs where both processes and outcomes aresimultaneously monitored, and data are ana-lyzed to see if changes in relevant processesreliably precede changes in desired outcomes.This type of investigation would be difficult toconduct using conventional between-groups re-search designs, but would be manageable usingwithin-groups SCDs. There are many measuresof group processes, such as group climate(MacKenzie, 1983, as used in Kastner & May,2009) and group engagement (Macgowan &Newman, 2005), but the most frequently stud-ied variable has been cohesion, which has beenreliably associated with outcomes in manygroup studies (Burlingame, McClendon, &Alonso, 2011; Burlingame et al., 2006; Strauss,Burlingame, & Bormann, 2008). As Fike (1980)argued, it is important to capture not only indi-vidual outcomes, but also factors that reflect thegroup experience on individuals and on thegroup, such as satisfaction and the nature ofinteractions.

Recommendations for Practice

The goals, resources, and constraints forpractice differ substantially from that for re-search, and recommendations for SCDs forpractice should reflect those differences (Wong,2010). Recall that our review of group researchfrom 1972 to 2012 found an average of onlyslightly more than one SCD study published peryear. Guidelines for SCD research calling for

more rigorous designs with stronger evidencefor causality could have a suppressive effect onthe usage of these designs in evaluating prac-tice. Standards that devalue weaker SCDs mightresult in even fewer SCDs being applied andless attempts to empirically evaluate groupwork. Individual evaluation, even if imperfect,is preferable to automatically assuming that atreatment validated in a prior between-groupsstudy will be equally effective with the group ofclients one is currently working. It is alsoclearly superior to practice relying on tradition,popularity of techniques, personal belief, orother approaches not based on client data.

There has been much discussion about theimportance of developing and/or adapting exist-ing treatments to maximize efficacy with par-ticular client populations (Benish, Quintana, &Wampold, 2011; Castro, Barrera, & Steiker,2010; Chen, Kakkad, & Balzano, 2008). LaRoche and Christopher (2008) noted that noempirically supported treatment has met all ofthe criteria set forth by the APA Task Force fortreatment efficacy (American Psychological As-sociation, 2006) for any minority group. Inadapting existing group treatments, componentsare added that attend to culture, while retainingthe “essential” ingredients of the establishedgroup treatment (Macgowan & Hanbidge,2014). A MBD across different groups or set-tings could be used to test the relative effective-ness of a group intervention that is progres-sively adapted to include important culturalcomponents. If the SCD is done rigorously(Chambless et al., 1998; Kratochwill & Levin,2010), these data can contribute to the develop-ment of an empirically supported group inter-vention appropriate for that population.

Uncontrolled case study designs such as theB, AB, and ABC designs should be used inclinical practice, but with full awareness of theirlimited inferential power. Such designs do notprovide the controls or replications necessary toallow clinicians to logically conclude that theirinterventions caused whatever changes mighthave occurred. Nevertheless, these designs arevaluable because “even if the therapist and cli-ents are not certain that the intervention was thesole cause of the changes, it still makes it pos-sible to ascertain the degree to which goals arebeing achieved” (Rose, 1977, p. 67).

Group work practitioners need not be con-strained by earlier group work evaluative meth-

153SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 55: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

ods. They can apply more refined variants of theB design. Examples include the Periodic Treat-ments Design (Hayes, 1981; Hayes, Barlow, &Nelson-Gray, 1999) and the Repeated Pretest-Posttest Design (Thyer & Curtis, 1983), whichare controlled designs that can evaluate an in-tervention during a single intervention phasethat is neither preceded nor followed by a base-line phase. These designs can demonstrate ex-perimental control by replicating improvementsoccurring soon after periodic exposure to inter-vention (e.g., a technique to improve group co-hesion) while showing the absence of such im-provements at other times during the phase. Inaddition, the internal validity of other case studydesigns, such as the AB or ABC, can bestrengthened with the earlier mentioned proce-dure of randomly selecting the starting point forinterventions (Edgington, 1975; Kratochwill &Levin, 2010; Toseland, et al., 1978).

Conclusions

This paper has elucidated how SCDs have beenused in group work by reviewing the designs inpublished studies, discussing their respectivestrengths and limitations, and examining proce-dural variations producing data that were rela-tively clear or confounded. Most of the groupwork studies reviewed here successfully usedSCDs to accomplish their purpose—to systemat-ically evaluate the effects of their interventions onprocesses and outcomes. Group researchers canlearn from the mistakes and build upon thestrengths of these earlier studies to become morefacile with this research methodology. AlthoughSCD technology has been used in group worksince the early 1970s, there are many importantquestions that still need to be investigated andgroup researchers have only begun to tap the po-tential of these evaluative formats.

References

AASWG. (2006). Standards for social work practicewith groups (2nd ed.). Retrieved from http://www.aaswg.org/files/AASWG_Standards_for_Social_Work_Practice_with_Groups.pdf

AGPA. (2007). Practice guidelines for group psy-chotherapy. Retrieved September 5, 2013, fromhttp://www.agpa.org/guidelines/index.html

American Psychological Association. (2006). Evi-dence-based practice in psychology: APA Presi-

dential Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice.American Psychologist, 61, 271–285. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.61.4.271

ASGW. (2000). Professional standards for the train-ing of group workers. Retrieved September 6,2013, from http://www.asgw.org/training_standards.htm

ASGW. (2007). Association for Specialists in GroupWork: Best practice guidelines 2007 revisions. Re-trieved August 6, 2013, from http://www.asgw.org/PDF/Best_Practices.pdf

Bailey, J. S., & Burch, M. R. (2002). Research meth-ods in applied behavior analysis. Thousand Oaks,CA: Sage.

Barlow, A., Burlingame, G. M., & Fuhriman, A.(2000). Therapeutic application of groups fromPratt’s “thought control classes” to modern grouppsychotherapy. Group Dynamics: Theory, Re-search, and Practice, 4, 115–134. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.4.1.115

Barlow, D. H., & Hayes, S. C. (1979). Alternating treat-ments design: One strategy for comparing the effectsof two treatments in a single subject. Journal ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 12, 199–210. doi:10.1901/jaba.1979.12-199

Barlow, S. H., Fuhriman, A., & Burlingame, G. M.(2004). The history of group counseling and psy-chotherapy. In J. L. DeLucia-Waack, D. A. Ger-rity, C. R. Kalodner, & M. Riva (Eds.), Handbookof group counseling and psychotherapy (pp. 3–22).Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. doi:10.4135/9781452229683.n1

Bates, P. (1980). The effectiveness of interpersonalskills training on the social skill acquisition ofmoderately and mildly retarded adults. Journal ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 13, 237–248. doi:10.1901/jaba.1980.13-237

Benish, S. G., Quintana, S., & Wampold, B. E.(2011). Culturally adapted psychotherapy and thelegitimacy of myth: A direct-comparison meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 58,279–289. doi:10.1037/a0023626

Blake, D. D., Owens, M. D., & Keane, T. M. (1990).Increasing group attendance on a psychiatric unit:An alternating treatments design comparison.Journal of Behavior Therapy and ExperimentalPsychiatry, 21, 15–20. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(90)90044-L

Bloom, M., Fischer, J., & Orme, J. G. (2009). Eval-uating practice: Guidelines for the accountableprofessional (6th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson Allyn& Bacon.

Burlingame, G. M., Strauss, B., & Joyce, A. (2013).Change mechanisms and effectiveness of smallgroup treatments. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Berginand Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy andbehavior change (6th ed., pp. 640–689). NewYork: Wiley.

154 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 56: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Burlingame, G. M., McClendon, D. T., & Alonso, J.(2011). Cohesion in group therapy. Psychother-apy, 48, 34–42. doi:10.1037/a0022063

Burlingame, G. M., Strauss, B., Joyce, A., MacNair-Semands, R., MacKenzie, K. R., Ogrodniczuk, J.,& Taylor, S. (2006). CORE Battery–Revised: Anassessment toolkit for promoting optimal groupselection, process, and outcome. New York, NY:American Group Psychotherapy Association.

Campbell, D. T., & Stanley, J. C. (1966). Experimen-tal and quasi-experimental designs for research.Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.

Castro, F. G., Barrera, M., & Steiker, L. K. H. (2010).Issues and challenges in the design of culturallyadapted evidence-based interventions. Annual Re-view of Clinical Psychology, 6, 213–239. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-033109-132032

Cetingok, M., & Hirayama, H. (1983). Evaluatingthe effects of group work with the elderly: Anexperiment using a single-subject design. SmallGroup Behavior, 14, 327–335. doi:10.1177/104649648301400304

Chambless, D. L., Baker, M. J., Baucom, D. H.,Beutler, L. E., Calhoun, K. S., Crits-Christoph, P.,. . . Woody, S. R. (1998). Update on empiricallyvalidated therapies, II. The Clinical Psychologist,51, 3–16.

Chen, E. C., Kakkad, D., & Balzano, J. (2008).Multicultural competence and evidence-basedpractice in group therapy. Journal of Clinical Psy-chology, 64, 1261–1278. doi:10.1002/jclp.20533

Clark, M. L., Cunningham, L. J., & Cunningham,C. E. (1989). Improving the social behavior ofsiblings of autistic children using a group problemsolving approach. Child & Family Behavior Ther-apy, 11, 19–33. doi:10.1300/J019v11n01_02

Connors, M. E., Johnson, C. L., & Stuckey, M. K.(1984). Treatment of bulimia with brief psychoe-ducational group therapy. The American Journalof Psychiatry, 141, 1512–1516.

Cook, T. D., & Campbell, D. T. (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design & analysis issues for fieldsettings. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally College Pub.Co.

Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper SaddleRiver, N. J.: Pearson/Merrill-Prentice Hall.

Edgington, E. S. (1975). Randomization tests forone-subject operant experiments. The Journal ofPsychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 90,57–68. doi:10.1080/00223980.1975.9923926

Edleson, J. L., Miller, D. M., Stone, G. W., & Chap-man, D. G. (1985). Group treatment for men whobatter. Social Work Research & Abstracts, 21,18–21. doi:10.1093/swra/21.3.18

Feldman, R. A. (1987). Group work knowledge andresearch: A two-decade comparison. Social Workwith Groups, 9, 7–14. doi:10.1300/J009v09n03_03

Fike, D. F. (1980). Evaluating group intervention.Social Work with Groups, 3, 41–51. doi:10.1300/J009v03n02_04

Fontao, M. I., & Mergenthaler, E. (2008). Therapeuticfactors and language patterns in group therapy appli-cation of computer-assisted text analysis to the ex-amination of microprocesses in group therapy: Pre-liminary findings. Psychotherapy Research, 18, 345–354. doi:10.1080/10503300701576352

Foster, L. H., Watson, T. S., & Young, J. S. (2002).Single-subject research design for school counsel-ors: Becoming an applied researcher. ProfessionalSchool Counseling, 6, 146–154.

Foxx, R. M., McMorrow, M. J., Bittle, R. G., & Ness,J. (1986). An analysis of social skills generaliza-tion in two natural settings. Journal of AppliedBehavior Analysis, 19, 299–305. doi:10.1901/jaba.1986.19-299

Foxx, R. M., McMorrow, M. J., & Schloss, C. N.(1983). Stacking the deck: Teaching social skills toretarded adults with a modified table game. Jour-nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 16, 157–170.doi:10.1901/jaba.1983.16-157

Frank, J. D. (1975). Group psychotherapy research25 years later. International Journal of Group Psy-chotherapy, 25, 159–162.

Fry, J. P., Botterill, W. M., & Pring, T. R. (2009).The effect of an intensive group therapy pro-gram for young adults who stutter: A singlesubject study. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 12–19. doi:10.1080/17549500802600990

Gajar, A., Schloss, P. J., Schloss, C. N., & Thomp-son, C. K. (1984). Effects of feedback and self-monitoring on head trauma youths’ conversationskills. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17,353–358. doi:10.1901/jaba.1984.17-353

Galassi, J. P., & Gersh, T. L. (1993). Myths, miscon-ceptions, and missed opportunity: Single-case de-signs and counseling psychology. Journal ofCounseling Psychology, 40, 525–531. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.40.4.525

Garrett, K. J. (2005). School social workers’ evalu-ation of group work practices. Children & Schools,27, 247–252. doi:10.1093/cs/27.4.247

Gronna, S. S., Serna, L. A., Kennedy, C. H., & Prater,M. A. (1999). Promoting generalized social inter-actions using puppets and script training in anintegrated preschool. A single-case study usingmultiple baseline design. Behavior Modification,23, 419–440. doi:10.1177/0145445599233005

Hall, J. A., Dineen, J. P., Schlesinger, D. J., & Stan-ton, R. (2000). Advanced group treatment fordevelopmentally disabled adults with social skilldeficits. Research on Social Work Practice, 10,301–326.

Hall, J. A., Schlesinger, D. J., & Dineen, J. P. (1997).Social skills training in groups with developmen-

155SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 57: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

tally disabled adults. Research on Social Work Prac-tice, 7, 187–201.

Hall, K. R. (2006). Using problem-based learningwith victims of bullying behavior. ProfessionalSchool Counseling, 9, 231–237.

Hall, R. V., & Fox, R. G. (1977). Changing-criteriondesigns: An alternate applied behavior analysisprocedure. In B. Etzel, J. Leblanc, & D. M. Baer(Eds.), New developments in behavioral research:Theory, method, and application (pp. 151–166).New York, NY: Wiley.

Hansen, D. J., St. Lawrence, J. S., & Christoff, K. A.(1985). Effects of interpersonal problem-solvingtraining with chronic aftercare patients on prob-lem-solving component skills and effectiveness ofsolutions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-chology, 53, 167–174. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.53.2.167

Hansen, D. J., St. Lawrence, J. S., & Christoff, K. A.(1989). Group conversational-skills training with in-patient children and adolescents: Social validation,generalization, and maintenance. Behavior Modifica-tion, 13, 4–31. doi:10.1177/01454455890131001

Hartmann, D. P., & Hall, R. V. (1976). The changingcriterion design. Journal of Applied BehaviorAnalysis, 9, 527–532. doi:10.1901/jaba.1976.9-527

Hauserman, N., Zweback, S., & Plotkin, A. (1972).Use of concrete reinforcement to facilitate verbalinitiations in adolescent group therapy. Journal ofConsulting and Clinical Psychology, 38, 90–96.doi:10.1037/h0032410

Hayes, S. C. (1981). Single case experimental designand empirical clinical practice. Journal of Consult-ing and Clinical Psychology, 49, 193–211. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.49.2.193

Hayes, S. C., Barlow, D. H., & Nelson-Gray, R. O.(1999). The scientist practitioner: Research andaccountability in the age of managed care (2nded.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Helmus, T. C., Saules, K. K., Schoener, E. P., & Roll,J. M. (2003). Reinforcement of counseling atten-dance and alcohol abstinence in a community-based dual-diagnosis treatment program: A feasi-bility study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors,17, 249–251. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.17.3.249

Heppner, P. P., Kivlighan, D. M., & Wampold, B. E.(2008). Research design in counseling (3rd ed.).Belmont, CA: Thomson Brooks/Cole.

Hersen, M., & Barlow, D. H. (1976). Single caseexperimental designs: Strategies for studying be-havior change. New York, NY: Pergamon Press.

Horne, A. M., & Rosenthal, R. N. (1997). Researchin group work: How did we get where we are?Journal for Specialists in Group Work, 22, 228–240. doi:10.1080/01933929708415527

Horner, R. H., Carr, E. G., Halle, J., McGee, G.,Odom, S., & Wolery, M. (2005). The use of single-

subject research to identify evidence-based prac-tice in special education. Exceptional Children, 71,165–179.

Johnson, P., Beckerman, A., & Auerbach, C. (2001).Researching our own practice: Single system de-sign for groupwork. Groupwork, 13, 57–72.

Kastner, J. W., & May, W. (2009). Action-orientedtechniques in adolescent group therapy. Group, 33,315–327.

Kazdin, A. E. (2003). Research design in clinicalpsychology (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

Kazdin, A. E. (2011). Single-case research designs:Methods for clinical and applied settings (2nd ed.).New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Kelly, J. A., Laughlin, C., Claiborne, M., & Patter-son, J. (1979). A group procedure for teaching jobinterviewing skills to formerly hospitalized psychi-atric patients. Behavior Therapy, 10, 299–310.doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(79)80020-9

Kelly, J. A., Wildman, B. G., Urey, J. R., & Thur-man, C. (1980). Group skills training to increasethe conversational repertoire of retarded adoles-cents. Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 1, 323–336. doi:10.1300/J473v01n04_02

Kennedy, C. H. (2005). Single-case designs for edu-cational research. Boston, MA: Pearson/A & B.

Kettlewell, P. W., Mizes, J. S., & Wasylyshyn, N. A.(1992). A cognitive-behavioral group treatment ofbulimia. Behavior Therapy, 23, 657–670. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80227-8

Kirby, K. C., Kerwin, M. E., Carpenedo, C. M.,Rosenwasser, B. J., & Gardner, R. S. (2008). In-terdependent group contingency management forcocaine-dependent methadone maintenance pa-tients. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 41,579–595. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-579

Kohler, F. W., & Fowler, S. A. (1985). Trainingprosocial behaviors to young children: An analysisof reciprocity with untrained peers. Journal ofApplied Behavior Analysis, 18, 187–200. doi:10.1901/jaba.1985.18-187

Kratochwill, T. R., & Levin, J. R. (2010). Enhancingthe scientific credibility of single-case interventionresearch: Randomization to the rescue. Psycholog-ical Methods, 15, 124–144. doi:10.1037/a0017736

La Roche, M., & Christopher, M. S. (2008). Cultureand empirically supported treatments: On the roadto a collision? Culture & Psychology, 14, 333–356. doi:10.1177/1354067X08092637

Ledgerwood, D. M., Alessi, S. M., Hanson, T., God-ley, M. D., & Petry, N. M. (2008). Contingencymanagement for attendance to group substanceabuse treatment administered by clinicians in com-munity clinics. Journal of Applied Behavior Anal-ysis, 41, 517–526. doi:10.1901/jaba.2008.41-517

Lees, D. G., & Ronan, K. R. (2008). Engagement andeffectiveness of parent management training (in-credible years) for solo high-risk mothers: A mul-

156 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 58: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

tiple baseline evaluation. Behaviour Change, 25,109–128. doi:10.1375/bech.25.2.109

Linsk, N., Howe, M. W., & Pinkston, E. M. (1975).Behavioral group work in a home for the aged.Social Work, 20, 454–463.

Lumpkin, P. W., Silverman, W. K., Weems, C. F.,Markham, M. R., & Kurtines, W. M. (2002).Treating a heterogeneous set of anxiety disordersin youths with group cognitive behavioral therapy:A partially nonconcurrent multiple-baseline eval-uation. Behavior Therapy, 33, 163–177. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(02)80011-9

Lundervold, D. A., & Belwood, M. F. (2000). Thebest kept secret in counseling: Single-case (N1)experimental designs. Journal of Counseling &Development, 78, 92–102. doi:10.1002/j.1556-6676.2000.tb02565.x

Macgowan, M. J. (2008). A guide to evidence-basedgroup work. New York, NY: Oxford UniversityPress. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195183450.001.0001

Macgowan, M. J., & Hanbidge, A. S. (2014). Ad-vancing evidence-based group work in communitysettings: Methods, opportunities, and challenges.In J. L. DeLucia-Waack, C. R. Kalodner, & M.Riva (Eds.), The handbook of group counselingand psychotherapy (2nd ed., pp. 303–317). Thou-sand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Macgowan, M. J., & Newman, F. L. (2005). Thefactor structure of the Group Engagement Mea-sure. Social Work Research, 29, 107–118. doi:10.1093/swr/29.2.107

MacKenzie, K. R. (1983). The clinical application ofa group climate measure. In R. R. Dies & K. R.MacKenzie (Eds.), Advances in group psychother-apy: Integrating research and practice (pp. 159–170). New York, NY: International UniversitiesPress.

Marr, D. D., & Fairchild, T. N. (1993). A problem-solving strategy and self-esteem in recoveringchemically dependent women. Alcoholism Treat-ment Quarterly, 10, 171–186. doi:10.1300/J020V10N01_11

Martinez, K. K., & Wong, S. E. (2009). Usingprompts to increase attendance at groups for sur-vivors of domestic violence. Research on SocialWork Practice, 19, 460 – 463. doi:10.1177/1049731508329384

Maughan, D. R., Christiansen, E., Jenson, W. R.,Olympia, D., & Clark, E. (2005). Behavioral par-ent training as a treatment for externalizing behav-iors and disruptive behavior disorders: A meta-analysis. School Psychology Review, 34, 267–286.

McGrath, J. E. (1997). Small group research: Thatonce and future field: An interpretation of the pastwith an eye to the future. Group Dynamics: The-ory, Research, and Practice, 1, 7–27. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.1.1.7

Mouton, S. G., & Stanley, M. A. (1996). Habit re-versal training for trichotillomania: A group ap-proach. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 3,159–182. doi:10.1016/S1077-7229(96)80036-8

Nelson, T. S., & Kelley, L. (2001). Solution-focusedcouples group. Journal of Systemic Therapies, 20,47–66. doi:10.1521/jsyt.20.4.47.23085

Newman, F. L., & Wong, S. E. (2004). Progress andoutcome assessment of individual patient data:Selecting single subject design and statistical pro-cedures. In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), Use of psycho-logical testing for treatment planning & outcomeassessment (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 273–289). Mah-wah, NJ: Erlbaum.

O’Brien, W. H., Korchynsky, R., Fabrizio, J.,McGrath, J., & Swank, A. (1999). Evaluatinggroup process in a stress management intervention:Relationships between perceived process and car-diovascular reactivity to stress. Research on SocialWork Practice, 9, 608 – 630. doi:10.1177/104973159900900506

Patterson, D. A., & Basham, R. E. (2002). A datavisualization procedure for the evaluation of grouptreatment outcomes across units of analysis. SmallGroup Research, 33, 209 –232. doi:10.1177/104649640203300203

Petry, N. M., Martin, B., & Finocche, C. (2001).Contingency management in group treatment: Ademonstration project in an HIV drop-in center.Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 21, 89–96.doi:10.1016/S0740-5472(01)00184-2

Petry, N. M., Weinstock, J., & Alessi, S. M. (2011).A randomized trial of contingency managementdelivered in the context of group counseling. Jour-nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79,686–696. doi:10.1037/a0024813

Phaneuf, L., & McIntyre, L. L. (2007). Effects ofindividualized video feedback combined withgroup parent training on maternal inappropriatebehavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,40, 737–741.

Phaneuf, L., & McIntyre, L. L. (2011). The appli-cation of a three-tier model of intervention toparent training. Journal of Positive BehaviorInterventions, 13, 198 –207. doi:10.1177/1098300711405337

Robison, F. F., Morran, D. K., & Hulse-Killacky, D.(1989). Single-subject research designs for groupcounselors studying their own groups. Journal forSpecialists in Group Work, 14, 93–97. doi:10.1080/01933928908411892

Roessler, R. T., & Lewis, F. D. (1984). Conversationskill training with mentally retarded and learningdisabled sheltered workshop clients. Rehabilita-tion Counseling Bulletin, 27, 161–171.

Romeiser Logan, L., Hickman, R. R., Harris, S. R., &Heriza, C. B. (2008). Single-subject research de-sign: Recommendations for levels of evidence and

157SINGLE-CASE DESIGNS GROUP WORK

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 59: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

quality rating. Developmental Medicine & ChildNeurology, 50, 99–103. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.02005.x

Rose, S. D. (1977). Group therapy: A behavioralapproach. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Rubin, A. (1991). The effectiveness of outreachcounseling and support groups for battered wom-en: A preliminary evaluation. Research on SocialWork Practice, 1, 332–357. doi:10.1177/104973159100100401

Serna, L. A., Schumaker, J. B., Sherman, J. A., & Sheldon,J. B. (1991). In-home generalization of social interac-tions in families of adolescents with behavior prob-lems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 733–746. doi:10.1901/jaba.1991.24-733

Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research:Evaluating experimental data in psychology. NewYork, NY: Basic Books.

Smith, J. D. (2012). Single-case experimental de-signs: A systematic review of published researchand current standards. Psychological Methods, 17,510–550. doi:10.1037/a0029312

Stevens, R. E. S. (2006). Research ‘Alive’ in thesocial work classroom: Teaching graduate socialwork students to evaluate practice effectiveness.Arete, 29, 62–68.

Strauss, B., Burlingame, G. M., & Bormann, B.(2008). Using the CORE-R battery in group psy-chotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64,1225–1237. doi:10.1002/jclp.20535

Thyer, B. A., & Curtis, G. C. (1983). The repeatedpretest-posttest single-subject experiment: A newdesign for empirical clinical practice. Journal ofBehavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,14, 311–315. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(83)90073-3

Tolman, R. M., & Molidor, C. E. (1994). A decade ofsocial group work research: Trends in methodol-ogy, theory, and program development. Researchon Social Work Practice, 4, 142–159. doi:10.1177/104973159400400202

Toseland, R., Krebs, A., & Vahsen, J. (1978). Chang-ing group interaction patterns. Journal of SocialService Research, 2, 219–232.

Toseland, R. W., & Rivas, R. F. (2012). An introduc-tion to group work practice (7th ed.). Boston, MA:Allyn & Bacon.

Tracey, D. A., Briddell, D. W., & Wilson, G. T.(1974). Generalization of verbal conditioning toverbal and nonverbal behavior: Group therapywith chronic psychiatric patients. Journal of Ap-plied Behavior Analysis, 7, 391–402. doi:10.1901/jaba.1974.7-391

Tymchuk, A. J., Andron, L., & Rahbar, B. (1988). Effec-tive decision-making/problem-solving training withmothers who have mental retardation. AmericanJournal on Mental Retardation, 92, 510–516.

Vaccaro, F. J. (1990). Application of social skillstraining in a group of institutionalized aggressiveelderly subjects. Psychology and Aging, 5, 369–378. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.5.3.369

Ventimiglia, J. A., Marschke, J., Carmichael, P., &Loew, R. (2000). How do clinicians evaluate theirpractice effectiveness? A survey of clinical socialworkers. Smith College Studies in Social Work, 70,287–306. doi:10.1080/00377310009517593

Vonk, M. E., Tripodi, T., & Epstein, I. (2007). Re-search techniques for clinical social workers (2nded.). New York, NY: Columbia University Press.

Wade, C. M., Ortiz, C., & Gorman, B. S. (2007).Two-session group parent training for bedtimenoncompliance in Head Start preschoolers. Child& Family Behavior Therapy, 29, 23–55. doi:10.1300/J019v29n03_03

Watson, P. J., & Workman, E. A. (1981). The non-concurrent multiple baseline across-individualsdesign: An extension of the traditional multiplebaseline design. Journal of Behavior Therapy andExperimental Psychiatry, 12, 257–259. doi:10.1016/0005-7916(81)90055-0

Weber, L. A. (1980). The effect of videotape andplayback on an in-patient adolescent group. Inter-national Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 30,213–227.

What Works Clearinghouse. (2013). Procedures andstandards handbook (Version 3.0). Retrieved fromhttp://ies.Ed.gov/ncee/wwc/pdf/reference_resources/wwc_procedures_v3_0_draft_standards_handbook.pdf

Wong, S. E. (2010). Single-case evaluation designs forpractitioners. Journal of Social Service Research, 36,248–259. doi:10.1080/01488371003707654

Wong, S. E., & Liberman, R. P. (1981). Mixed single-subject designs in clinical research: Variations of themultiple-baseline. Behavioral Assessment, 3, 297–306.

Wong, S. E., Morgan, C., Crowley, R., & Baker, J. N.(1996). Using a table game to teach social skills toadolescent psychiatric inpatients: Do the skillsgeneralize? Child & Family Behavior Therapy, 18,1–17. doi:10.1300/J019v18n04_01

Wong, S. E., Wright, J., Terranova, M. D., Bowen,L., Zarate, R., & Zarate, R. (1988). Effects ofstructured ward activities on appropriate and psy-chotic behavior of chronic psychiatric patients.Behavioral Residential Treatment, 3, 41–50. doi:10.1002/bin.2360030104

Zastrow, C. (2006). Social work with groups: A com-prehensive workbook (6th ed.). Belmont, CA:Thomson.

Received February 15, 2013Revision received November 4, 2013

Accepted November 5, 2013

158 MACGOWAN AND WONG

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 60: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Team Task Conflict Resolution: An Examination of Its Linkages toTeam Personality Composition and Team Effectiveness Outcomes

Thomas A. O’NeillUniversity of Calgary

Natalie J. AllenWestern University

By drawing attention to task conflict resolution and reporting on its empirical relationswith key team variables, the present study offers a new perspective on team effective-ness. Specifically, we examined how three “dark” personality traits (Manipulativeness,Narcissism, and Secondary Psychopathy) relate to team conflict resolution, teaminnovation, and team task performance in student engineering design project teams.Interestingly, results indicate that mean team levels of Secondary Psychopathy is themost important predictor, and task conflict resolution is a mediator linking meanSecondary Psychopathy to team task performance. Furthermore, the prediction of teamperformance by Secondary Psychopathy is as strong as is any of the “Big 5” personalityvariables investigated in meta-analyses.

Keywords: team conflict, task conflict, task conflict resolution, team effectiveness, Dark Triad,personality, psychopathy

Work teams form the essential buildingblocks of modern organizations (Hackman,2002). As noted by Kozlowski and Bell (2003),the adoption of teams emerged, in large part,from global competition and an emphasis oninnovation, which created a need for organiza-tions to be increasingly flexible and adaptable.Identifying the determinants of work team pro-cesses, innovation, and performance, therefore,is fundamental to our understanding of organi-zational effectiveness (see Sundstrom, McIn-tyre, Halfhill, & Richards, 2000).

Perhaps not surprisingly, team conflict is oneof the most researched team processes (seeKorsgaard, Jeong, Mahony, & Pitariu, 2008).Consider the recent meta-analysis by de Wit,Greer, and Jehn (2012); in that research, theauthors report on data from 116 studies involv-ing almost 9,000 teams. Many of the studies

place emphasis on task conflict, which is theextent to which team members have differentopinions, perspectives, and views of the task(Jehn, 1995). Task conflict is of particular in-terest because, unlike most types of conflict, ithas been argued to be conducive to team per-formance (Amason, 1996). Meta-analyses,however, indicate that task conflict is weaklyrelated to team performance, on average (e.g.,O’Neill, Allen, & Hastings, 2013).

A potential limitation of task conflict re-search is that it ignores whether resolutions ofthose conflicts were actually reached. This is animportant issue, because a team with unresolvedconflicts may experience impasses, deadlocks,and barriers to forward movement, whereas ateam with resolved conflicts likely integrates,combines, and synthesizes divergent views andachieves consensus on how best to proceed (seeRobey, Farrow, & Franz, 1989). Our review ofthe literature presented below, however, sug-gests that empirical research examining bothantecedents and consequences of task conflictresolution is lacking. In light of the potentialimportance of task conflict resolution, we con-sider both innovation and task performance aspossible team outcomes in a sample of engi-neering design project teams. We also investi-gate potential antecedents (i.e., input variables)

Thomas A. O’Neill, Department of Psychology, Univer-sity of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada; Natalie J. Allen,Department of Psychology, Western University, London,Ontario, Canada.

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-dressed to Thomas A. O’Neill, Department of Psychology,University of Calgary, 2500 University Drive, N.W., Cal-gary, AB Canada T2N 1N4, P: 403-220-5207. E-mail:[email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice © 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 18, No. 2, 159–173 1089-2699/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/gdn0000004

159

Page 61: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

that could be useful for pinpointing the condi-tions in which task conflicts are most likely tobe resolved. Specifically, in this research, wefocus on team-level Manipulativeness, Narcis-sism, and Secondary Psychopathy. These traitsare similar, although not identical, to the “DarkTriad” (Paulhus & Williams, 2002), which in-cludes a common core of selfishness, remorse-lessness, disagreeableness, and aggressiveness(Jonason, Webster, Schmitt, Li, & Crysel,2012). Although Judge and LePine (2007)called for team research on dark traits, currentliterature has primarily focused on the Five-Factor Model (Bell, 2007; Driskell, Goodwin,Salas, & O’Shea, 2006).

Given our examination of team inputs (per-sonality composition), processes (task conflictresolution), and outcomes (innovation and taskperformance), the current research design pres-ents the opportunity to investigate the role oftask conflict resolution as a process variable thatmediates the effects of team personality com-position on team outcomes. As LePine, Buck-man, Crawford, and Methot (2011) noted intheir recent review, the intervening processesand mechanisms linking team personality inputsto team outcomes are understudied and largelyunknown. This study aims to address that gap.

Theoretical Development

Task Conflict Resolution

In the current research we draw attention tothe issue of task conflict resolution and investi-gate its associations with team inputs and out-comes. As described by Robey et al. (1989),“conflict resolution is apparent when membershave fewer objections and indicate more agree-ment with each other . . . it indicates a solutionthat is acceptable to the entire group” (p. 1174).Wall and Callister (1995) noted that conflictresolutions “may be in the form of explicit ortacit agreement” (p. 525), and Kuhn and Poole(2000) viewed conflict resolution as “coming toa mutually agreeable solution” (p. 570). Wedefine task conflict resolution as the extent towhich different opinions, viewpoints, and per-spectives were resolved.

As mentioned, recent meta-analyses summa-rizing the substantial body of research involvingtask conflict and team performance report smalleffect sizes (e.g., de Wit et al., 2012; O’Neill et

al., 2013). Resolving task conflicts, on the otherhand, could be critical to team outcomes be-cause team members have encountered and ad-equately addressed divergent viewpoints re-garding the direction in which the team ought toproceed. Compared with research on conflicttypes, there is scant consideration of the extent towhich a team actually resolves conflicts, the typeof conflicts resolved, and whether resolutionspredict team outcomes (e.g., De Dreu, 2011).We also note that there is research on the strat-egies, processes, and styles of negotiating con-flicts, resolving disputes, and allocating scarceresources (e.g., Behfar, Peterson, Mannix, &Trochim, 2008; Katz & Kahn, 1978; Rubin,Pruitt, & Kim, 1994), but there is little empiricalresearch informing our understanding of theantecedents and consequences of task conflictresolution.

Theoretical work on conflict resolution byRobey and colleagues (i.e., Robey & Farrow,1982; Robey et al., 1989) deals with factorsleading to satisfactory conflict resolution (seealso Barki & Hartwick, 1994). Notable findingsfrom that research suggest participation leads topower and influence, which in turn is positivelyassociated with satisfactory resolution of con-flict. However, those studies were conducted atthe individual level and, therefore, they ignorewhether the team, as a collective, was satisfiedwith resolutions. Furthermore, the studies con-found the different types of conflict (e.g., task,relationship), and conflict resolution is treatedas the final outcome rather than as a processaffecting team innovation and task perfor-mance. In a notable exception, Jehn, Greer,Levine, and Szulanksi (2008) offer a three-itemmeasure of task conflict resolution efficacy,which was related positively to team viabilityand emergent states. However, the constructpertains to “the belief that conflict can be easilyresolved” (p. 465) rather than actual conflictresolutions. Greer, Jehn, and Mannix (2008)used single-item scales to measure each of task,relationship (i.e., interpersonal incompatibili-ties), and process (e.g., work-structuring incom-patibilities) conflict resolutions. Arguably, mul-tiitem scales might be more reliable and couldprovide better coverage of the content domains,and consideration of team outcomes (e.g., teamtask performance) might shed new light on theimportance of task conflict resolution.

160 O’NEILL AND ALLEN

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 62: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Task Conflict Resolution andTeam Outcomes

Team innovation involves developing a cre-ative or novel solution that is also practical andfeasible in its development and application(e.g., Amabile, 1983; Drach-Zahavy & Somech,2001a, 2001b), whereas team task performancerefers to the overall quality of the team’s task-related output (e.g., Hackman, 1987). This dis-tinction suited the context of the current study.Specifically, the objective of the engineeringproject teams involved was to develop a noveland effective approach to demonstrating how anenvironmentally friendly technology creates re-newable energy (e.g., team innovation), and todeliver an extensive report documenting theteam’s design and the prototype’s capabilities(team task performance).

Teams capable of resolving task conflicts arelikely to perform well and reach high levels ofinnovation. Drawing from social interdepen-dence theory, compatible team-member goalsappear to facilitate effective conflict handling,which in turn results in identifying novel andhigh-quality solutions (see Deutsch, 2006). Ad-ditionally, based on information processing the-ory, we suggest unresolved conflicts may arousethe autonomic nervous system, thereby inhibit-ing critical analysis of the task and creativethinking (e.g., Carnevale & Probst, 1998). In-deed, teams with positive interpersonal relation-ships exhibit greater creativity than do teams inconflict (e.g., Craig & Kelly, 1999).

In project teams of the nature studied here,we anticipated this would be particularly impor-tant because meeting milestones and keepingforward momentum was essential to team inno-vation and team task performance (see Chioc-chio, 2007). Behfar et al.’s (2008) qualitativestudy of project teams found that high-performing teams used pluralistic strategies in-volving either a preemptive focus on addressingpotential conflicts, or quickly and efficientlycontaining conflicts by invoking rules to resolveconflicts (e.g., majority rules). Teams failing toaddress and to resolve different viewpoints arelikely to experience stalls or deadlocks that in-hibit smooth progress. In sum, we predict thefollowing:

H1a: Task conflict resolution will be posi-tively correlated with team innovation.

H1b: Task conflict resolution will be posi-tively correlated with team task performance.

Input-Process-Output (IPO) Model andTeam Personality Composition

In the current research we applied the well-known IPO model of team effectiveness (seeMcGrath, 1984). The IPO model is best consid-ered a heuristic that organizes theorizing abouthow team inputs (e.g., personality) affect teamprocesses (e.g., task conflict resolution) and, inturn, team outcomes (e.g., innovation, task per-formance). In the context of the present re-search, task conflict resolution is theorized to bea mechanism by which personality compositionimpacts team outcomes.

Variables organized under the IPO frame-work are conceptualized at the team level and,therefore, personality variables must be aggre-gated prior to hypothesis testing. Operationaliz-ing traits using the within-team mean, as we dohere, invokes an additive theory of aggregation(see Chan, 1998). Based on the additive theory,the arithmetic mean of team member scores ismost relevant because it is the overall “abun-dance” of the trait, within the team, that isexpected to be most consequential to the team’swork (e.g., LePine, Hollenbeck, Ilgen, & Hed-lund, 1997; LePine, 2003; O’Neill & Allen,2011).

Dark Traits as Antecedents of TaskConflict Resolution

One potential approach is the “Dark Triad,”which encompasses Narcissism, Machiavellian-ism, and Psychopathy (Paulhus & Williams,2002). Narcissism describes people who holdfeelings of superiority, grandiosity, and entitle-ment (e.g., Ames, Rose, & Anderson, 2006;Raskin & Terry, 1988). Rather than focusing onMachiavellianism, we chose a central dimen-sion of Machiavellianism referred to as Manip-ulativeness (see Paunonen & Jackson, 2000).Manipulativeness involves the willingness touse of flattery, ingratiation, and deception toadvance personal agendas (see Paunonen, Had-dock, Forsterling, & Keinonen, 2003). We viewthe content of the Manipulativeness dimensionof Machiavellianism as highly relevant to theteams’ capacities to resolve conflicts. Although

161TEAM TASK CONFLICT RESOLUTION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 63: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

individuals high on Manipulativeness use im-pression management techniques to advancetheir own agenda, they do not necessarily pos-sess the charm, appeal, and magnetism of Ma-chiavellians (cf. Paunonen & Jackson, 2000).Finally, we considered Secondary Psychopathy,which involves impulsivity, lack of goal com-mitment, delinquency, irresponsibility, and dif-ficulties establishing relationships (e.g., Leven-son, Kiehl, & Fitzpatrick, 1995; McHoskey,Worzel, & Szyarto, 1998). Although the currenttraits do not overlap perfectly with the DarkTriad, the Dark Triad literature is relevant toformulating our hypotheses.

Affective events theory (AET; Weiss & Cro-panzano, 1996) is a framework for understand-ing how Manipulativenss, Narcissism, and Sec-ondary Psychopathy may interfere with theresolution of task conflict. One proposition thatis consistent with AET is that personality affectsthe occurrence of events at work and how thoseevents translate into affective experiences,which in turn have implications for behavior(see Wegge, van Dick, Fisher, West, & Daw-son, 2006). Common tendencies of individualshigh on dark traits similar to those considered inthis study include the use of adversarial anddisruptive behaviors, and engagement in malev-olent interpersonal dealings (e.g., Jonason &Webster, 2012). As such, these individualswould likely exacerbate the challenge of effec-tively resolving task conflicts by arousing neg-ative emotions. This could occur through theiruse of “hard tactics” such as threats, punish-ments, and manipulations (e.g., Jonason, Slom-ski, & Partyka, 2012), lack of self-control andunderstanding of behavioral consequences (e.g.,Jonason & Tost, 2010), emotional intelligencedeficits (e.g., Jordan & Troth, 2004; Petrides,Vernon, Schermer, & Veselka, 2011), and em-pathy limitations (e.g., Jonason & Krause,2013; Wai & Tiliopoulos, 2012). Negative emo-tions and arousal in teams high on dark traitswould interfere with effective information pro-cessing (e.g., Carnevale & Probst, 1998), whichis likely to detract from problem-focused dis-cussions aimed at resolving task conflicts. Ac-cordingly, we offer the following predictions:

H2: Mean Manipulativeness, Narcissism, andSecondary Psychopathy will be negatively cor-related with task conflict resolution.

In the current study we measured darktraits, conflict, and outcomes at three distinct

and sequential time periods. Although we donot use an experimental design and, as a con-sequence, causality cannot be unequivocallydetermined, the ordering of measurement co-incides with the likely temporal ordering ofthese variables. Specifically, we theorize thatpersonality would most likely be an anteced-ent (not a mediator or outcome), and that taskconflict resolution would most likely precedeteam outcomes.

H3: Task conflict resolution will mediate therelationships between team personality vari-ables (Manipulativeness, Narcissism, and Sec-ondary Psychopathy) and team outcome vari-ables (innovation and performance).

Dark Traits and Team Outcomes

Although we argued for an indirect linkageinvolving the dark traits and team outcomes,theory also supports direct effects. Manipulativeindividuals engage in impression managementstrategies involving ingratiation and self-promotion (see Paunonen et al., 2003), which,when viewed as insincere, tend to be associatedwith distrust and skepticism (e.g., Baron, 1986).As several team members attempt to manipulateeach another, the team atmosphere would likelybe rife with suspicion, which would not likelypromote team collaboration, innovation, or per-formance. Team members high on Narcissismmight have excessively optimistic views aboutthe quality of their work or their own contribu-tions even in light of evidence to the contrary.Furthermore, narcissistic individuals are seenby their group members as domineering, ingen-uous, and lacking in interpersonal intelligence(e.g., Rauthmann, 2012), and a team of suchindividuals would not likely be effective. Fi-nally, teams with members high on SecondaryPsychopathy would likely derail the team’sprogress because of their members’ impulsivity,aggressiveness, and emotional instability (cf.McHoskey et al., 1998; Paulhus & Williams,2002). In sum, we propose the following:

H4a: Mean Manipulativeness, Narcissism,and Secondary Psychopathy will be negativelycorrelated with team innovation.

H4b: Mean Manipulativeness, Narcissism,and Secondary Psychopathy will be negativelycorrelated with team task performance.

162 O’NEILL AND ALLEN

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 64: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Method

Participants and Teamwork Context

Data were collected from 344 team membersworking in 81 engineering project design teamsfor 6.5 months. The participants were studentsin an engineering design course at a large Ca-nadian university. Teams had four (n 61) orfive (n 20) members, and 85% of participantswere male (M age 18.6 years, SD 2.4).Teams were formed pseudorandomly. Specifi-cally, individuals were asked to rank order theirproficiency on “skill sets” (i.e., communication,computer, planning, hands on), and then teamswere built such that they had one individualwith a ranking of “1” on each dimension. Thecourse instructors preferred this method to fullrandom assignment.

Our observations indicate teams took greatpride in designing their devices and presentingthem at a high-profile design showcase in whichthe city mayor, university president, engineer-ing dean and faculty, and the public were inattendance. This “major design project” was thefocus of team innovation and team task perfor-mance in the current research. The projectswere complicated and inherently high onknowledge interdependence because teammembers tended to become experts in particulardesign areas critical to the prototype’s innova-tion, functionality, and documentation. Thus,the teams in this sample fit well within theaccepted definition of work teams: highly inter-dependent members working on consequentialtasks and having a collective purpose (e.g., Al-len & West, 2005; Hackman, 1987).

Procedure and Timeline

Participants were invited to complete person-ality and conflict surveys during weekly designclasses. Personality data were collected in thefirst design class before participants were as-signed to their teams; conflict processes wereassessed 6 months later, 2 weeks before com-pleting the major design project. Team innova-tion ratings were based on prototype photos andwritten descriptions collected a week beforefinal project reports were due. Team task per-formance was assessed after the major designproject was submitted at the 6.5-month mark.

Measures

Time 1: Personality. Manipulativenesswas measured using Paunonen’s (2002) scalefrom the Supernumerary Personality Inventory(SPI). The scale contains 15 items with 5-pointLikert-type response options ( .70). Exam-ple items are as follows: By flattering someoneI can make him or her more apt to agree withme and I will pretend to be extremely interestedin what a person is saying in order to getsomething. Narcissism was measured using the16-item forced-choice form of the NarcissisticPersonality Inventory ( .67; Ames et al.,2006). Specifically, respondents were asked tochoose which of two statements are most likethem, such as I really like to be the center ofattention and It makes me uncomfortable to bethe center of attention. Another item set wasPeople sometimes believe what I tell them and Ican make anybody believe anything I want themto. Secondary Psychopathy was measured using10 items from the Levenson Self-Report Psy-chopathy Scale ( .63; Levenson et al., 1995)with 5-point Likert-type response options. Ex-ample items are I find myself in the same kindsof trouble, time after time and I have been in alot of shouting matches with other people.

Time 2: Task conflict resolution and taskconflict. To measure task conflict resolution,we observed teams in their engineering designlaboratories over a 6 month period. Extensivediary notes were taken during the approximate100 hours we spent, overall, observing variousteams receiving instructions, training, and feed-back, and during brainstorming, decision-making exercises, and design work. Specifi-cally, the first author attended 4 of the 8 weeklydesign labs on a rotating basis during whichteams were engaged with various group activi-ties including the focal project. Through thisinformal team task analysis, we identified fourkey project milestones: a) problem definition, b)deciding on the team’s design concept, c) de-veloping the team’s prototype, and d) preparingthe team presentation. Next, we adopted thefollowing general form across four items: theextent to which different opinions, viewpoints,and perspectives were resolved before settlingon the team’s ______________. For each item,the blank contained problem definition, designconcept, prototype, and presentation ( .89),respectively. Jehn’s (1995) four-item measure

163TEAM TASK CONFLICT RESOLUTION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 65: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

of task conflict was also administered ( .89).Scale anchors for both measures ranged from 1(A very small amount) to 5 (A lot).

Time 3: Team innovation. Team innova-tion was assessed using Amabile’s (1983) con-sensual assessment technique, which involvescollecting innovation judgments from domain-relevant experts. Five expert judges who wereengineering graduate students rated the innova-tion of each design prototype. The targets ofexperts’ ratings were short, written descriptionsand 4 6 in. color photographs of the teamprototypes. The relative percentile method(RPM) formed the basis of the rating scaleemployed to assess team innovation (see Goffin,Gellatly, Paunonen, Jackson, & Meyer, 1996).The current application of the RPM requiredjudges to rate each team’s level of innovation ona scale ranging from zero to 100. Team inno-vation was scored as the average of each judge’srating of each team prototype. An intraclasscorrelation (ICC) was computed for assessingthe reliability of multiple raters rating team in-novation on a continuous scale [ICC(2) .71;Shrout & Fleiss, 1979]. We note that the expertswere course teaching assistants leading differ-ent design studios, and they were each familiarwith a unique set of approximately 10 of the 81projects. However, all judges rated all projectsin a fully crossed design. Therefore, any possi-ble bias introduced by familiarity with the proj-ects would likely be limited after averagingacross all five judge ratings, and the stronginterrater reliability supports this view.

Time 4: Team task performance. Teamswere required to submit reports of approxi-mately 100 pages containing background re-search, documentation of the theoretical andobserved capabilities of the team’s design con-

cept, CAD sketches, mathematical modeling,and implications. The assessment of team taskperformance consisted of ratings provided byexperienced course instructors. Because ratersdid not rate the same teams, we controlled forthe possibility of raters using different perfor-mance distributions by z-standardizing withinraters (which is common practice in similarsituations; O’Neill & Allen, 2012; Wageman &Gordon, 2005).

Results

Aggregation

Task conflict resolution and task conflict are“shared-unit” constructs; therefore, within-teamagreement and between-team heterogeneity isrequired to support construct validity prior toaggregation (Kozlowski & Klein, 2000). Ac-cording to Bliese (2000), ICCs indicate the pro-portion of variance explained by group mem-bership [that is, ICC(1)] and the reliability ofthe group mean [that is, ICC(2)]. Table 1 con-tains ICCs for the shared-unit constructs of taskconflict and task conflict resolution, which wereall significantly greater than zero. Furthermore,Jehn et al. (2008) interpreted ICCs of this mag-nitude (and lower) as sufficient for justifyingaggregation. The personality variables were ag-gregated using the mean but were not expectedto demonstrate within-group agreement becauseteams were formed pseudorandomly, traits areconfigural unit properties (i.e., not shared; Koz-lowski & Klein, 2000), and their function istheorized to be additive across team members(LePine, 2003; O’Neill & Allen, 2011).

Table 1Means, Standard Deviations, ICCs, and Correlations Involving Team-Level Study Variables

Variable ICC(1) ICC(2) M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Manipulativeness 2.91 0.212. Narcissism 4.95 1.38 .40

3. Secondary psychopathy 2.34 0.23 .17 .29

4. Task conflict .24 .54 2.13 0.51 .17 .21 .26

5. Task conflict resolution .14 .40 3.91 0.55 .02 .12 .30 .39

6. Team innovation 53.96 17.38 .05 .06 .21 .11 .147. Team task performance 0.00 0.96 .06 .08 .30 .18 .30 .37

Note. n 81. p .05.

164 O’NEILL AND ALLEN

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 66: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Tests of Hypotheses

We predicted task conflict resolution wouldbe positively related to team innovation (H1a).Although in the predicted direction, this relationdid not reach significance (see Table 1). Taskconflict resolution was expected to positivelycorrelate with team task performance (H1b). Asthis was the case, H1b was supported.

We anticipated that Manipulativeness, Narcis-sism, and Secondary Psychopathy would be neg-atively related to task conflict resolution (H2).Although mean Manipulativeness and Narcissismwere not related to task conflict resolution, therewas a significant negative relation involving Sec-ondary Psychopathy (see Table 1). We also ex-pected that the effect of the dark personality traitson team outcomes would be mediated by taskconflict resolution (H3). Preacher and Hayes(2004) recommended the bootstrapping approachfor testing the significance of the indirect effect ofthe predictor on the outcome through the interven-ing variable. Specifically, bias-corrected upperand lower 95% confidence intervals were calcu-lated using 1000 bootstrapped samples. Analysessupported the significance of the indirect effect forone model (see Figure 1): Secondary Psychopathy¡ task conflict resolution ¡ team task perfor-mance (unstandardized indirect effect .28,CI95% .68 to .06). This indicates a one-unitincrease in mean Secondary Psychopathy was re-lated to a .28 decrease in team task performancethrough task conflict resolution. Furthermore, thedirect effect of Secondary Psychopathy on teamtask performance remained significant after takinginto account the indirect effects, suggesting Sec-ondary Psychopathy has both direct and indirectpaths to team task performance.1

Regarding team outcomes, Manipulativeness,Narcissism, and Secondary Psychopathy shouldbe related negatively to team innovation (H4a).However, none of the relations reached conven-tional levels of significance (see Table 1). Turn-ing to team task performance (H4b), of the threetraits examined, a significant negative associa-tion was found only for Secondary Psychopa-thy.

Supplemental Analyses

Given the pattern of results involving person-ality, Secondary Psychopathy appeared to bethe most important predictor of the teamwork

variables (see Table 1). One procedure for com-paring the importance of predictors is relativeimportance analysis (e.g., Johnson, 2000),which overcomes the limitations of both zero-order correlations as well as betas from standardmultiple regression (e.g., Johnson & LeBreton,2004). Table 2 contains the results of the rela-tive importance analysis as well as overallmodel R2 values. Model R2 values indicate thatthe personality traits, as a set, accounted for12% of the variance in team task performanceand 10% of the variance in task conflict resolution.Overall prediction of task conflict and team inno-vation was nonsignificant. In terms of the relativeimportance of the three personality traits, theresults clearly indicate that Secondary Psychop-athy was dominant. Of the proportion of vari-ance explained by the personality variables as aset, Secondary Psychopathy accounted for 57%(task conflict), 78% (team task performance),82% (team innovation), and 91% (task conflictresolution). Thus, Secondary Psychopathy wasthe most important predictor of the team processand outcome variables studied.

Discussion

The overarching purpose of this study was toinvestigate the role of task conflict resolution asa team process linking dark personality traits toteam outcomes. This is notable because the ac-tual resolution of task conflict has receivedmuch less consideration than has the occurrenceof task conflict, yet resolution could be at leastas important. Taken together, the set of hypoth-eses we forwarded involving these relations re-ceived partial support. In our view, the contri-bution of the current study is threefold. First,compared with task conflict, findings for theresolution of task conflict appear more intrigu-ing and theoretically informative. Task conflictresolution was significantly related to team taskperformance, and it was a mediator betweenSecondary Psychopathy and team task perfor-mance. This is novel given the dearth of past

1 For completeness, we considered task conflict as a me-diator and as a moderator in moderated mediation analyses.The moderated mediation analyses examined the role oftask conflict moderating either the effect of task conflictresolution on team outcomes or personality traits on taskconflict resolution (Hayes, 2013). We did not find anysignificant effects for the function of task conflict in any ofthese roles.

165TEAM TASK CONFLICT RESOLUTION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 67: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

research on task conflict resolution and the ex-isting focus on task conflict levels. Indeed, theauthors of recent meta-analyses found 83(O’Neill et al., 2013) and 116 (de Wit et al.,2012) articles investigating conflict types andteam performance. Consistent with informationprocessing theory, results from those meta-analyses suggested, with few exceptions, thattask conflict is either unrelated to or harmful forteam performance (as it was in the present re-search). Accordingly, a continued focus on taskconflict itself may be limiting (see alsoDeChurch, Mesmer-Magnus, & Doty, 2013).Our findings are important for theory and prac-tice because they imply that the resolution oftask conflict may be a critical factor. This fitswell within social interdependence theory, asinterdependence often leads to conflict and,therefore, the manner in which the conflict ismanaged may be most important (see Johnson,

2003; Tjosvold, 1998). It is also consistent withthe project management literature (e.g., Chioc-chio & Lafrenière, 2009) given that resolvedtask conflict is needed to progress smoothly andefficiently through project stages. Thus, it maybe advantageous for researchers and practitio-ners to emphasize strategies that help teamswork through task conflicts rather than to en-courage teams to engage in task conflict withoutaddressing its resolution.

A second novel contribution of the currentresearch involves the examination of team per-sonality composition from the perspective ofdark traits (cf. Judge & LePine, 2007). Relativeto Manipulativeness and Narcissism, SecondaryPsychopathy dominated the prediction of taskconflict resolution, team innovation, and teamtask performance. Interestingly, Secondary Psy-chopathy exhibited stronger relations with teamtask performance than did any of the Big Five,

Table 2Relative Importance Analysis of Team-Level Personality Variables

Criterion and statistic Manipulativeness Narcissism Secondary psychopathy R2

Task conflict .08 .12 .21† .09Raw relative weight .02 .03 .05Regression weight as a percentage of R2 16 28 57

Task conflict resolution .05 .05 .30 .10

Raw relative weight .00 .01 .09Regression weight as a percentage of R2 1 8 91

Team innovation .11 .04 .22† .06Raw relative weight .01 .00 .05Regression weight as a percentage of R2 11 7 82

Team task performance .09 .21 .32 .12

Raw relative weight .01 .02 .10Regression weight as a percentage of R2 5 17 78

Note. n 81. p .05.

.39* -.72*

Secondary Psychopathy

Task Conflict Resolution

Team Task Performance

-.94*

Figure 1. Mediation model with unstandardized path coefficients. All paths in the model aresignificant including the direct path from Psychopathy to team task performance aftercontrolling for the indirect effect through task conflict resolution. The indirect effect was.28. p .05.

166 O’NEILL AND ALLEN

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 68: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

in terms of uncorrected correlations, reported inpast meta-analyses (e.g., Bell, 2007). Character-istic patterns of behavior of individuals high onSecondary Psychopathy include difficulties re-lating to others, emotional outbursts, impulsiv-ity, and lack of goal commitment (Levenson etal., 1995; McHoskey et al., 1998). Consistentwith theories such as affective events theory,teams of individuals with high Secondary Psy-chopathy likely experienced frequent and re-peated cycles of negative affective interactions,potentially resulting in reinforcing downwardspirals (cf. Peterson & Behfar, 2003). For ex-ample, individuals high on Secondary Psychop-athy often use social influence tactics involvingcoercion, debasement, and threats (Jonason etal., 2012); tend to be dishonest and lack humil-ity (Lee & Ashton, 2005); exhibit self-controldifficulties (Jones & Paulhus, 2011; Jonason &Tost, 2010); appear to engage in various formsof aggression, cheating, and bullying (Boddy,2010; Coyne & Thomas, 2008); and tend tohold social dominance beliefs (Hodson, Hogg,& MacInnis, 2009).

Given the above, placing a number of indi-viduals high on Secondary Psychopathy in ateam will likely lead to numerous negative af-fective interactions, disputes, and arguments. Itmay be advisable, therefore, to build teams suchthat overall levels of Secondary Psychopathyare as low as possible. One way to accomplishthis involves the “seeding” method outlined byHumphrey, Hollenbeck, Meyer, and Ilgen(2007). When a trait is known to combine ad-ditively (such that increasing or decreasing theteam mean on that trait is helpful for perfor-mance), they would suggest assessing the traitand distributing the highest scorers across teamsrather than concentrating them in any one team.With criterion validity evidence and an analysisof the personality-based work requirementssuggesting the trait is job relevant (see Arthur,Edwards, Bell, Villado, & Bennett, 2005), thecase for the legal defensibility of using the testto make personnel decisions with seeding meth-ods could be strengthened (cf. Society for In-dustrial-Organizational Psychology Principles,2003, and local employment law).

A third contribution is that we identify taskconflict resolution as a mechanism linking teampersonality composition, specifically mean lev-els of Secondary Psychopathy, to team taskperformance. This suggests a team of individu-

als high on Secondary Psychopathy will tend toexperience failures in resolving task conflicts,which, in turn, results in difficulties moving theproject forward and achieving high task perfor-mance. Thus, although meta-analyses highlightthe association between personality and perfor-mance in teams (e.g., Bell, 2007), in the currentstudy we report on one of the few empiricalfindings supporting a linking mechanismthrough which team personality composition in-fluences team performance. As mentionedabove, teams high on mean Secondary Psychop-athy contain individuals who are likely hostile,aggressive, impulsive, and unable to remaincommitted to goals. It appears teams of suchindividuals experience barriers to resolving taskconflicts and to finding agreement regardingavenues toward high-quality task work. In turn,this was associated with reductions in team ef-fectiveness. Importantly, both indirect and di-rect connections to team task performance weresupported in the model, suggesting mean Sec-ondary Psychopathy has at least two distinctpaths through which it impacts team task per-formance. Investigating additional team pro-cesses, such as psychological safety (Edmond-son, 1999), communication quality and quantity(Barrick, Stewart, Neubert, & Mount, 1998),and other potentially relevant processes arepromising avenues for future research.

On the Role of Mixed Motives

It is worth considering whether a lack ofmixed motives and hierarchical goal structuresmight explain why Manipulativeness and Nar-cissism were unrelated to team conflict pro-cesses and team outcomes. The underlying ra-tionale for the relevance of Manipulativenessand Narcissism is that they should be related toindividuals advancing their own objectives,agendas, and goals at the expense of pursuingthe team’s collective purpose. Findings fromstudies examining social interdependence the-ory indicate that this situation leads to intrateamcompetition, thereby contributing to selectiveinformation sharing, an argumentative approachto differences of opinion, distrust, and a closed-minded interpretation of new information (seereviews by Johnson, 2003; Johnson & Johnson,1989). But whether team members had proselfmotives is somewhat of an open question.

167TEAM TASK CONFLICT RESOLUTION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 69: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Our observations and familiarity with theteamwork context indicate that members quitelikely experienced varying degrees of diver-gence in individual- and team-level motives.First, participants had not yet specialized in aparticular subfield of engineering, and thereforethe team was faced with identifying one or twosubfields on which their prototype would bebased (e.g., mechanical, electrical, software, ur-ban, robotics). Given that team members oftenhad different interests, this decision likely cre-ated some variance between individual andteam preferences. In turn, we expect that thiscreates the conditions for manipulativeness,conceit, and selfishness to inhibit conflict reso-lution and joint effort toward a collective pur-pose. Second, team members likely had diver-gence on the level of quality and detail they feltwas needed, how many hours and meetings toinvest, the nature of member roles and time-lines, and so forth. Here again, manipulativeand egotistical behaviors would hinder integra-tion, compromise, and consensus, thereby con-tributing to conflict and poor team outcomes. Tothe extent that team members had different pref-erences and goals such as those described here,it is almost certain that some individuals’ ob-jectives would be at odds with team objectives.As a result, we speculated that teams high onManipulativeness or Narcissism would suffer.The extent to which individual and group mo-tives and goals were distinct, however, is diffi-cult to pinpoint, and future studies investigatingManipulativeness, Narcissism, and related traitsin teams may benefit from considering goalhierarchies and goal incongruences (e.g., De-Shon, Kozlowski, Schmidt, Milner, & Wiech-mann, 2004).

Future Research

In light of the current study, we view teamtask conflict resolution as a promising avenuefor advancing team conflict research. In addi-tion to the positive association with team taskperformance, team task conflict resolution ex-hibited a negative relation with task conflict.This suggests a number of teams experienceddifficulties with persistent and unresolved taskconflicts, which may well have reduced theirability to function effectively. Correlations be-tween task conflict resolution and the dark per-sonality traits were in the negative direction,

which supports the construct validity of the taskconflict resolution measure. Thus, future re-search on task conflict resolution would seemfruitful.

A second direction for future research in-volves studying how conflict is resolved. Thereis a voluminous literature on conflict manage-ment approaches (e.g., Deutsch, 1973) andstyles (e.g., Rahim, 1983), providing evidencethat cooperative goals and integrative styles areassociated with resolved conflicts (see Lewicki,Weiss, & Lewin, 1992). But there is a need forresearch on how to promote effective ap-proaches to conflict management and resolu-tion, and what specific strategies and behaviorslead to effective resolutions. For example,would it matter whether disagreements are re-solved through voting, autocracy, or avoidance?Recently, Behfar et al., (2008) identified spe-cific strategies used by teams to resolve con-flicts. Characteristic of high-performing andhighly satisfied teams was equity-focused reso-lution (e.g., work assignments based on skill).Characteristic of low-performing and unsatis-fied teams was adhocracy-focused resolution(e.g., avoidance of conflict and group membersin general). Tactics such as voting and majorityrules were associated with high performancebut, interestingly, low satisfaction. On the otherhand, equality rules that promoted cohesivenesswere associated with low performance and highsatisfaction. What might drive this research for-ward is scale development and empirical exam-ination of how these different resolution strate-gies impact resolution itself, satisfaction withthe resolution, and team outcomes. This coulddeepen our understanding of the specific behav-iors that are needed for effective conflict reso-lution.

In the way of a third avenue for future re-search, it may be valuable to consider relation-ship conflict resolution and process conflict res-olution. The current research focused on taskconflict resolution as a first step, as this type ofconflict has been studied extensively and hasbeen described as the “productive” form of con-flict (e.g., De Dreu, 1997). It seemed to us thattask conflict resolution may be promising, andour research provides cause for optimism in thisregard. Greer et al. (2008) found process con-flict resolution moderated the relation betweenearly process conflict and later process, task,and relationship conflict such that resolutions

168 O’NEILL AND ALLEN

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 70: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

buffered continuing conflict occurrences. Thesefindings are encouraging despite being based onsingle-item conflict resolution scales; therefore,development of multiitem scales would likelybe advantageous.

Limitations and Conclusion

First, although the dark traits investigated inthe current research are similar to the DarkTriad, and we draw from the Dark Triad litera-ture, there are some important differences. Wefocused on Manipulativeness, which is a featureof Machiavellianism but is not identical. Machi-avellianism is a broader trait including charm,charismatic tendencies (see Christie, Geis, &Berge, 1970), and self-control (see Jones &Paulhus, 2011), and the fact that Manipulative-ness was not predictive of teamwork variablesdoes not rule out the potential predictiveness ofMachiavellianism. We also considered Second-ary Psychopathy because we expected that itwould have little overlap with Manipulativenessrelative to Primary Psychopathy. However, theDark Triad contains coverage of Primary Psy-chopathy, such as cynicism and callousness (Jo-nason & Webster, 2010), which is strongly cor-related with Machiavellianism (e.g., McHoskeyet al., 1998). Therefore, we had a theoreticalinterest in considering traits that are slightlydifferent from the Dark Triad, and our findingsshould not be assumed to fully capture the ef-fects of the Dark Triad on team conflict pro-cesses or team outcomes. Second, despite theuse of a cross-sectional design with three mea-surement periods that corresponded to our the-oretical causal ordering of the relevant vari-ables, the mediation analyses do not allowdefinitive “cause-and-effect” conclusions (Judd& Kenny, 2010). Thus, experiments with fullrandomization to conditions should be con-ducted. Third, replication of the current researchis necessary to shed light on the robustness andgeneralizability of the findings. Fourth, al-though we used a unidimensional measure ofNarcissism, this trait consists of subdimensionsconcerning entitlement, leadership, self-absorp-tion, and superiority (Emmons, 1987). Thesesubdimensions may have differential relationswith teamwork variables, and therefore the cur-rent study may not have detected nontrivialfacet-level findings (cf. O’Neill & Allen, 2011).On the other hand, Manipulativeness is a unidi-

mensional construct (Paunonen & Jackson,2000) and Secondary Psychopathy is a narrowermeasure of a broad Psychopathy construct(Ross, Bye, Wrobel, & Horton, 2008), suggest-ing the issue may not be as relevant for theselatter traits.

The current research emphasizes the role oftask conflict resolution and provides evidenceof a positive relation with team task perfor-mance. Furthermore, task conflict resolutionwas found to operate as an indirect linkingmechanism between mean Secondary Psychop-athy and team task performance. Thus, it seemsnew research focusing on the resolution of con-flict, rather than its levels, could be useful forenriching our understanding of the role of con-flict processes in organizational work teams.Finally, in addition to influencing both task con-flict resolution and overall task conflict levels,mean Secondary Psychopathy exhibited nega-tive relations with team task performance, andat a marginally significant level, team innova-tion. Thus, further investigations of SecondaryPsychopathy may be of considerable value foradvancing theory on team personality composi-tion.

References

Allen, N. J., & West, M. A. (2005). Selecting forteamwork. In A. Evers, N. Anderson, & O.Voskuijl (Eds.), The Blackwell handbook of per-sonnel selection (pp. 476–494). Oxford, UK:Blackwell Press.

Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology ofcreativity. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. doi:10.1007/978-1-4612-5533-8

Amason, A. C. (1996). Distinguishing the effects offunctional and dysfunctional conflict on strategicdecision making: Resolving a paradox for topmanagement teams. Academy of ManagementJournal, 39, 123–148. doi:10.2307/256633

Ames, D. R., Rose, P., & Anderson, C. P. (2006). TheNPI-16 as a short measure of narcissism. Journalof Research in Personality, 40, 440–450. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2005.03.002

Arthur, W., Edwards, B. D., Bell, S. T., Villado,A. J., & Bennett, W. (2005). Team task analysis:Identifying tasks and jobs that are team based.Human Factors, 47, 654 – 669. doi:10.1518/001872005774860087

Barki, H., & Hartwick, J. (1994). User participation,conflict, and conflict resolution: The mediatingroles of influence. Information Systems Research,5, 422–438. doi:10.1287/isre.5.4.422

169TEAM TASK CONFLICT RESOLUTION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 71: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Baron, R. A. (1986). Self Presentation in Job Inter-views: When there can be “too much of a goodthing”. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 16,16–28. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1986.tb02275.x

Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., Neubert, M. J., &Mount, M. K. (1998). Relating member ability andpersonality to work-team processes and team ef-fectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83,377–391. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.3.377

Behfar, K. J., Perterson, R. S., Mannix, E. A., &Trochim, M. K. (2008). The critical role of conflictresolution in teams: A close look at the linksbetween conflict type, conflict management strat-egies, and team outcomes. Journal of Applied Psy-chology, 93, 170–188. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.170

Bell, S. T. (2007). Deep-level composition variablesas predictors of team performance: A metaanaly-sis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 595–615.doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.595

Bliese, P. D. (2000). Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: Implications for dataaggregation and analysis. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J.Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, andmethods in organizations: Foundations, exten-sions, and new directions (pp. 349–381). SanFrancisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Boddy, C. R. (2010). Corporate psychopaths, bully-ing and unfair supervision in the workplace. Jour-nal of Business Ethics, 100, 367–379. doi:10.1007/s10551-010-0689-5

Carnevale, P. J., & Probst, T. M. (1998). Socialvalues and social conflict in creative problem solv-ing and categorization. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 74, 1300–1309. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1300

Chan, D. (1998). Functional relations among con-structs in the same content domain at differentlevels of analysis: A typology of compositionmodels. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 234–246. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.83.2.234

Chiocchio, F. (2007). Project team performance: Astudy of electronic task and coordination commu-nication. Project Management Journal, 38, 97–109.

Chiocchio, F., & Lafrenière, A. (2009). A projectmanagement perspective on student’s declarativecommitments to goals established within asyn-chronous communication. Journal of ComputerAssisted Learning, 25, 294–305. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2729.2009.00310.x

Christie, R., Geis, F. L., & Berger, D. (1970). Studiesin Machiavellianism (p. ). New York, NY: Aca-demic Press.

Coyne, S. M., & Thomas, T. J. (2008). Psychopathy,aggression, and cheating behavior: A test of thecheater-hawk hypothesis. Personality and Individ-

ual Differences, 44, 1105–1115. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.11.002

Craig, T. Y., & Kelly, J. R. (1999). Group cohesive-ness and creative performance. Group Dynamics:Theory, Research, and Practice, 3, 243–256. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.3.4.243

DeChurch, L. A., Mesmer-Magnus, J. R., & Doty, D.(2013). Moving beyond relationship and task con-flict: Toward a process-state perspective. Journalof Applied Psychology, 98, 559–578. doi:10.1037/a0032896

De Dreu, C. K. W. (1997). Productive conflict: Theimportance of conflict management and conflictissue. In C. K. W. De Dreu & E. Van De Vliert(Eds.), Using conflict in organizations (pp. 10–22). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. doi:10.4135/9781446217016.n2

De Dreu, C. K. W. (2011). Conflict at work: Basicprinciples and applied issues. In S. Zedeck (Ed.),APA Handbook of industrial and organizationalpsychology (Vol. 3, pp. 461–493). Washington,DC: APA. doi:10.1037/12171-013

DeShon, R. P., Kozlowski, S. W. J., Schmidt, A. M.,Milner, K. R., & Wiechmann, D. (2004). A mul-tiple-goal, multilevel model of feedback effects onthe regulation of individual and team performance.Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 1035–1056.doi:10.1037/0021-9010.89.6.1035

Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict: Con-structive and destructive processes. New Haven,CT: Yale University Press. doi:10.1177/000276427301700206

Deutsch, M. (2006). Cooperation and competition. InM. Deutsch, P. T. Coleman, & E. C. Marcus(Eds.), The handbook of conflict resolution: The-ory and practice (2nd ed., pp. 23–42). San Fran-cisco, CA: Wiley

de Wit, F. R. C., Greer, L. L., & Jehn, K. A. (2012).The paradox of intragroup conflict: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 360–390. doi:10.1037/a0024844

Drach-Zahavy, A., & Somech, A. (2001a). Under-standing team innovation: The role of team pro-cesses and structures. Group Dynamics: Theory,Research and Practice, 5, 111–123. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.5.2.111

Drach-Zahavy, A., & Somech, A. (2001b). Can wewin them all? Benefits and costs of structure andflexible innovation implementations. Group Dy-namics: Theory, Research and Practice, 25, 217–234. doi:10.1002/job.239

Driskell, J. E., Goodwin, G. F., Salas, E., & O’Shea,P. G. (2006). What makes a good team player?Personality and team effectiveness. Group Dynam-ics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 10, 249–271.doi:10.1037/1089-2699.10.4.249

Edmondson, A. C. (1999). Psychological safety andlearning behavior in work teams. Administrative

170 O’NEILL AND ALLEN

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 72: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Science Quarterly, 44, 350–383. doi:10.2307/2666999

Emmons, R. A. (1987). Narcissism: Theory and mea-surement. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 52, 11–17. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.1.11

Goffin, R. D., Gellatly, I. R., Paunonen, S. V., Jack-son, D. N., & Meyer, J. P. (1996). Criterion vali-dation of two approaches to performance ap-praisal: The behavioral observation scale and therelative percentile method. Journal of Businessand Psychology, 11, 23–33. doi:10.1007/BF02278252

Greer, L. L., Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. (2008).Conflict transformation: A longitudinal investiga-tion of the relationships between different types ofintragroup conflict and the moderating role of con-flict resolution. Small Group Research, 39, 278–302. doi:10.1177/1046496408317793

Hackman, J. R. (1987). The design of work teams. InJ. W. Lorsch (Ed.), Handbook of organizationalbehavior (pp. 315–342). Englewood Cliffs, NJ:Prentice Hall.

Hackman, J. R. (2002). Leading teams: Setting thestage for great performances. Boston, MA: Har-vard Business School Press.

Hayes, A. R. (2013). Introduction to mediation, mod-eration, and conditional process analysis: A re-gression-based approach. New York, NY: Guil-ford Press.

Hodson, G., Hogg, S. M., & MacInnis, C. C. (2009).The role of “dark personalities” (Narcissism, Ma-chiavellianism, Psychopathy), big five personalityfactors and ideology in explaining prejudice. Jour-nal of Research in Personality, 43, 686–690. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.005

Humphrey, S. E., Hollenbeck, J. R., Meyer, C. J., &Ilgen, D. R. (2007). Trait configurations in self-managed teams: A conceptual examination of theuse of seeding for maximizing and minimizingtrait variance in teams. Journal of Applied Psy-chology, 92, 885–892. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.92.3.885

Jehn, K. A. (1995). A multimethod examination ofthe benefits and detriments of intragroup conflict.Administrative Science Quarterly, 40, 256–282.doi:10.2307/2393638

Jehn, K. A., Greer, L., Levine, S., & Szulanski, G.(2008). The effects of conflict types, dimensions, andemergent states on group outcomes. Group Decisionand Negotiation, 17, 465–495. doi:10.1007/s10726-008-9107-0

Johnson, D. W. (2003). Social interdependence: Inter-relationships among theory, research and practice.American Psychologist, 58, 934–945. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.11.934

Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1989). Coopera-tion and competition: Theory and research. Edina,MN: Interaction Book.

Johnson, J. W. (2000). A heuristic method for estimat-ing the relative weight of predictor variables in mul-tiple regression. Multivariate Behavioral Research,35, 1–19. doi:10.1207/S15327906MBR3501_1

Johnson, J., & LeBreton, J. M. (2004). History anduse of relative importance indices in organizationalresearch. Organizational Research Methods, 7,238–257. doi:10.1177/1094428104266510

Jonason, P. K., & Krause, L. (2013). The emotionaldeficits associated with the Dark Triad traits: Cog-nitive empathy, affective empathy, and alexithy-mia. Personality and Individual Differences, 55,532–537. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2013.04.027

Jonason, P. K., Slomski, S., & Partyka, J. (2012). Thedark triad at work: How toxic employees get theirway. Personality and Individual Differences, 52,449–453. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.008

Jonason, P. K., & Tost, J. (2010). I just cannot controlmyself: The dark triad and self-control. Personal-ity and Individual Differences, 49, 611–615. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.031

Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2010). The dirtydozen: A concise measure of the dark triad. Psy-chological Assessment, 22, 420–432. doi:10.1037/a0019265

Jonason, P. K., & Webster, G. D. (2012). A proteanapproach to social influence: Dark Triad personal-ities and social influence tactics. Personality andIndividual Differences, 52, 521–526. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.11.023

Jonason, P. K., Webster, G. D., Schmitt, D. P., Li,N. P., & Crysel, L. (2012). The antihero in popularculture: Life history theory and the dark personal-ity traits. Review of General Psychology, 16, 192–199. doi:10.1037/a0027914

Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2011). The role ofimpulsivity in the dark triad of personality. Per-sonality and Individual Differences, 51, 679–682.doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.011

Jordan, P. J., & Troth, A. C. (2004). Managing emo-tions during team problem solving: Emotional intel-ligence and conflict resolution. Human Performance,17, 195–218. doi:10.1207/s15327043hup1702_4

Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (2010). Data analysis insocial psychology: Recent and recurring issues. InS. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & L. Gardner (Eds.),Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., Vol. 1,pp. 115–139). Mahwah, NJ: Wiley.

Judge, T. A., & LePine, J. A. (2007). The bright anddark sides of personality: Implications for person-nel selection in individual and team contexts. In J.Langen-Fox, C. L. Cooper, & R. J. Klimoski(Eds.), Research companion to the dysfunctionalworkplace: Management challenges and symptoms

171TEAM TASK CONFLICT RESOLUTION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 73: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

(pp. 332–355). London, UK: Edward Elgar Pub-lishing Ltd. doi:10.4337/9781847207081.00028

Katz, D., & Kahn, R. L. (1978). The social psychol-ogy of organizations (2nd ed.). New York, NY:Wiley and Sons.

Korsgaard, M. A., Jeong, S. S., Mahony, D. M., &Pitariu, A. H. (2008). A multilevel view of intra-group conflict. Journal of Management, 34, 1222–1252. doi:10.1177/0149206308325124

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Bell, B. S. (2003). Workgroups and teams in organizations. In W. C. Bor-man, D. R. Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.), Hand-book of psychology (Vol. 12): Industrial and orga-nizational psychology (pp. 333–375). New York,NY: Wiley. doi:10.1002/9781118133880.hop212017

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Klein, K. J. (2000). A mul-tilevel approach to theory and research in organi-zations: Contextual, temporal, and emergent pro-cesses. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.),Multilevel theory, research, and methods in orga-nizations: Foundations, extensions, and new direc-tions (pp. 3–90). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Kuhn, T., & Poole, M. S. (2000). Do conflict manage-ment styles affect group decision making? Evidencefrom a longitudinal field study. Human Communica-tion Research, 26, 558–590. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2000.tb00769.x

Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2005). Psychopathy, Ma-chiavellianism, and Narcissism in the five-factormodel and the HEXACO model of personalitystructure. Personality and Individual Differences,38, 1571–1582. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.016

LePine, J. A. (2003). Team adaptation andpostchange performance: Effects of team compo-sition in terms of members’ cognitive ability andpersonality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88,27–39. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.1.27

LePine, J. A., Buckman, B. R., Crawford, E. R., &Methot, J. R. (2011). A review of research onpersonality in teams: Accounting for pathwaysspanning levels of theory and analysis. HumanResource Management Review, 21, 311–330. doi:10.1016/j.hrmr.2010.10.004

LePine, J. A., Hollenbeck, J. R., Ilgen, D. R., &Hedlund, J. (1997). Effects of individual differ-ences on the performance of hierarchical decision-making teams: Much more than g. Journal of Ap-plied Psychology, 82, 803–811. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.82.5.803

Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K., & Fitzpatrick, C. M.(1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes in a non-institutionalised population. Journal of Personalityand Social Psychology, 68, 151–158. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.68.1.151

Lewicki, R. J., Weiss, S. E., & Lewin, D. (1992).Models of conflict, negotiation, and third partyintervention: A review and synthesis. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 13, 209 –252. doi:10.1002/job.4030130303

McGrath, J. E. (1984). Groups: Interaction and per-formance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

McHoskey, J. W., Worzel, W., & Szyarto, C. (1998).Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 74, 192–210.doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.192

O’Neill, T. A., & Allen, N. A. (2011). Personalityand the prediction of team performance. EuropeanJournal of Personality, 25, 31–42. doi:10.1002/per.769

O’Neill, T. A., & Allen, N. A. (2012). Team meetingattitudes: Conceptualization and investigation of anew construct. Small Group Research, 43, 186–210. doi:10.1177/1046496411426485

O’Neill, T. A., Allen, N. A., & Hastings, S. E.(2013). Examining the “pros” and “cons” of teamconflict: A team-level meta-analysis of task, rela-tionship, and process conflict. Human Perfor-mance, 26, 236–260. doi:10.1080/08959285.2013.795573

Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The darktriad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellian-ism, and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Per-sonality, 36, 556 –563. doi:10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6

Paunonen, S. V. (2002). Design and construction ofthe Supernumerary Personality Inventory. Re-search Bulletin (Vol. 763). London, Ontario: Uni-versity of Western Ontario.

Paunonen, S. V., Haddock, G., Forsterling, F., &Keinonen, M. (2003). Broad versus narrow per-sonality measures and the prediction of behavioracross cultures. European Journal of Personality,17, 413–433. doi:10.1002/per.496

Paunonen, S. V., & Jackson, D. N. (2000). What isbeyond the Big Five? Plenty! Journal of Researchin Personality, 68, 821–835. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00117

Peterson, R. S., & Behfar, K. J. (2003). The dynamicrelationship between performance feedback, trust,and conflict in groups: A longitudinal study. Or-ganizational Behaviour and Human Decision Pro-cesses, 92, 102–112. doi:10.1016/S0749-5978(03)00090-6

Petrides, K. V., Vernon, P. A., Schermer, J. A., &Veselka, L. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence andthe dark triad traits of personality. Twin Research andHuman Genetics, 14, 35–41. doi:10.1375/twin.14.1.35

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS andSAS procedures for estimating indirect effects insimple mediation models. Behavior ResearchMethods, Instruments & Computers, 36, 717–731.doi:10.3758/BF03206553

172 O’NEILL AND ALLEN

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 74: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Rahim, M. A. (1983). A measure of styles of han-dling interpersonal conflict. Academy of Manage-ment Journal, 26, 368–376. doi:10.2307/255985

Raskin, R., & Terry, H. (1988). A principle compo-nents analysis of the narcissistic personality inven-tory and further evidence of its construct validity.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,890–902. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.54.5.890

Rauthmann, J. F. (2012). The Dark Triad and inter-personal perception: Similarities and differences inthe social consequences of Narcissism, Machiavel-lianism, and Psychopathy. Social Psychologicaland Personality Science, 3, 487–496. doi:10.1177/1948550611427608

Robey, D., & Farrow, D. L. (1982). User involve-ment in information system development: A con-flict model and empirical test. Management Sci-ence, 28, 73–85. doi:10.1287/mnsc.28.1.73

Robey, D., Farrow, D. L., & Franz, C. R. (1989). Groupprocess and conflict in system development. Man-agement Science, 35, 1172–1191. doi:10.1287/mnsc.35.10.1172

Ross, S. R., Bye, K., Wrobel, T. A., & Horton, R. S.(2008). Primary and secondary psychopathic char-acteristics and the schedule for non-adaptive andadaptive personality (SNAP). Personality and In-dividual Differences, 45, 249–254. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.007

Rubin, J. Z., Pruitt, D. G., & Kim, S. H. (1994).Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate, and settle-ment (2nd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Shrout, P. E., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass cor-relations: Uses in assessing rater reliability. Psy-chological Bulletin, 86, 420–428. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420

Society for Industrial-Organizational Psychology.(2003). Principles for the validation and use of

personnel selection procedures (4th ed.). BowlingGreen, OH: SIOP.

Sundstrom, E., McIntyre, M., Halfhill, T., & Rich-ards, H. (2000). Work groups: From the Haw-thorne studies to work teams of the 1990s andbeyond. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, andPractice, 4, 44–67. doi:10.1037//1089-2699.4.1.44

Tjosvold, D. (1998). Cooperative and competitivegoal approach to conflict: Accomplishments andchallenges. Applied Psychology, 47, 285–342. doi:10.1111/j.1464-0597.1998.tb00025.x

Wageman, R., & Gordon, F. M. (2005). As the twigis bent: How group values shape emergent taskinterdependence in groups. Organization Science,16, 687–700. doi:10.1287/orsc.1050.0146

Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective andcognitive empathic nature of the dark triad ofpersonality. Personality and Individual Differ-ences, 52, 794–799. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.008

Wall, J. A., & Callister, R. R. (1995). Conflict and itsmanagement. Journal of Management, 21, 515–558. doi:10.1177/014920639502100306

Wegge, J., van Dick, R., Fisher, G. K., West, M. A.,& Dawson, J. F. (2006). A test of basic assump-tions of affective events theory (AET) in call cen-tre work. British Journal of Management, 17, 237–254. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8551.2006.00489.x

Weiss, H. M., & Cropanzano, R. (1996). Affectiveevents theory: A theoretical discussion of thestructure, causes and consequences of affectiveexperiences at work. Research in OrganizationalBehavior, 18, 1–74.

Received May 7, 2013Revision received November 14, 2013

Accepted November 20, 2013

173TEAM TASK CONFLICT RESOLUTION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 75: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

The Impact of Group Norms and Behavioral Congruence on theInternalization of an Illegal Downloading Behavior

Sophie Sansfaçon and Catherine E. AmiotUniversité du Québec a Montréal

This experimental study (n 115) investigates how group norms and individuals’congruence with these norms predicted internalization (i.e., self-determination) of anillegal behavior. We manipulated ingroup norms in favor of versus against illegaldownloading of software, and assessed participants’ behavioral intentions, their moti-vations for emitting this specific behavior, and how they subjectively experienced theexperimental situation (compartmentalizing their illegal behavior; feeling conflicted).Participants were more likely to endorse the behavior that was normative in theiringroup. Also, participants were more self-determined to engage in the behavior whenthey intended to behave congruently with a norm against illegal downloading, andwhen they intended to behave incongruently with a norm in favor of illegal download-ing. Participants who did not intend to engage in illegal downloading felt less com-partmentalized and conflicted than participants who intended to engage in illegaldownloading. Results are discussed in light of self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan,2000) and social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1986).

Keywords: dissent, group norms, illegal behavior, norm and behavior congruence, self-determination

What drives people to behave illegally, suchas by stealing or cheating? One could say thatthe reasons why people act illegally are instru-mental, and serve to access valued resources(Lee & Allen, 2002) or gain social approval(Baumeister & Vohs, 2004). For example, stu-dents may have an external motivation to ille-gally download a computer software from theInternet because they need that program to suc-ceed in their studies. Other students may engagein the illegal behavior in a more internalizedmanner, because they feel that it is right forstudents not to pay for computer software be-cause they don’t have a salary yet, and that the

software companies are already making a lot ofmoney. Still, we can wonder about the variablesthat facilitate the internalization of such illegalbehavior within the self. Previous studies havedemonstrated that group norms influence notonly the emission of negative behaviors (Frone& Brown, 2010; Kura, Shamsudin, & Chauhan,2013; Robinson & O’leary-Kelly, 1998), butalso the internalization of those behaviors(Amiot, Sansfaçon, & Louis, 2013; Amiot,Sansfaçon, Louis & Yelle, 2012). When per-ceiving one’s group norm as encouraging a be-havior, it is possible that those behaviors be-come internalized by individual groupmembers, such that they are part of their self(Haslam & Reicher, 2007) and are engaged inout of pleasure or because they align with one’svalues (Amiot, Sansfaçon, & Louis, 2013; inpress). In the current research, we aim to test, inan experimental context, whether a situationallysalient norm influences the immediate internal-ization of the normative behaviors.

In fact, past studies have showed that whenan ingroup is important to the individual, thenorms of this group can become internalizedand impact significantly on the behaviors ofindividual group members (e.g., Fielding, Mc-

Sophie Sansfaçon and Catherine E. Amiot, Départementde Psychologie, Université du Québec a Montréal, Mon-tréal, Québec, Canada.

This research was facilitated thanks to grants from theSocial Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada(SSHRC) and from the Fond pour la recherche en santé–Québec (FRSQ) to Catherine E. Amiot.

Correspondence concerning this article should be ad-dressed to Sophie Sansfaçon, Département de psychologie,Université du Québec a Montréal, C.P. 8888, Succ. Centre-Ville, Montréal, PQ, H3C 3P8. E-mail: cpf.sansfaç[email protected]

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice © 2014 American Psychological Association2014, Vol. 18, No. 2, 174–188 1089-2699/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0035797

174

Page 76: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Donald, & Louis, 2008; Terry & Hogg, 1996).Relying on self-determination theory (Deci &Ryan, 2000) and intergroup theories based onthe social identity approach (Tajfel & Turner,1986), the first goal of the current study is toinvestigate the internalization of a particularillegal behavior, namely, the illegal download-ing of computer software. This behavior is pri-vate, anonymous, and relatively common (Bel-gian Anti-Piracy Federation, 2009); it wasconsidered relevant to study in relation to socialidentification and internalization processesgiven that social influence and norms operatestrongly under conditions of isolation and ano-nymity (Spears, Postmes, Lea, & Wolbert,2002). We expected this illegal behavior to beinternalized when social norms and cues aresalient.

Our second goal is to investigate how partic-ipants subjectively feel when they behave inline with group norms or not. To this aim, weassessed how engaging in an illegal behavior iscompartmentalized and to what extent it cancause conflict within the self. We also explorehow participants subjectively perceive how dif-ferent sources of influence guided their behaviorin the experimental setting. Doing so opens thedoor to a new subjective perspective on groupnorms that departs from traditional norms re-search, which has typically looked at objective(e.g., manipulated) sources of normative influ-ence that operate outside of individuals’ aware-ness (Vorauer & Miller, 1997).

Social Norms Promote Internalization

Social norms are useful guides for what isconsidered appropriate behavior in group set-tings. Norms represent what is done and pre-scribed by the other group members (Jacobson,Mortensen, & Cialdini, 2011). Despite beingpotentially coercive (Turner, 1991), norms canalso be internalized by group members andcome to be followed freely and out of choice(Amiot, Sansfaçon, & Louis, 2013). Internaliza-tion refers to the process by which an individualis taking in the values and behaviors of a groupand integrating them within their sense of self,by making them their own (Kelman, 1961).This process operates beyond acting for per-sonal gain, or to avoid the negative conse-quences that derive from violating the norm(Kerr, Garst, Kiehle, & Harris, 1997). From a

social identity perspective, when an individualidentifies with a group, the group identity be-comes a part of the self, and so do the norms ofthis group (Turner, 1991). Whether norms arefollowed or not also depends on the social con-text: when a group membership is salient in asocial context and this membership is importantto the person’s self-definition, group norms willbecome activated and will guide one’s behavior(Smith & Louis, 2008; Terry & Hogg, 1996).This internalization is also facilitated by intra-group processes, including communication thattakes place within the ingroup (Kerr & Kauf-man-Gilliland, 1994) and consensualizationprocesses (Turner, 1991).

For example, at a hockey game, if a hockeyfan identifies strongly as a fan of the team andperceives that his or her team group is encour-aging him/her in engaging in a harmful behav-iors against fans of other team, s/he will tend toengage in the normative behavior and internal-ize this behavior to a greater extent (Amiot,Sansfaçon, & Louis, in press). Importantly, theintergroup perspectives propose that the inter-nalization process should take place regardlessof whether the normative group behaviors areprosocial or potentially harmful (Turner, 1991)– such as whether behaviors are illegal or not.

Self-Determination Theory

Previous studies from the organizational lit-erature have confirmed that motivations to en-gage in illegal behaviors in the workplace canbe diverse, from thrill seeking to role modeling,and to feelings that such behaviors allow torestore justice (Robinson & Greenberg, 1998).This suggests that negative behavior can beemitted out of different motives. Herein wefocus on motives that range from being drivenby external sources, to behaviors that are fol-lowed for more internal reasons (Amiot, Sans-façon, & Louis, 2013). To concretely capturethe possible motivations for people’s behaviorsand the extent to which these behaviors—including normative behaviors—are internal-ized, the present research relies on self-determination theory (SDT; Deci & Ryan,2000). SDT is a motivational theory that hasbeen applied mostly to understanding the moti-vations that underlie constructive social behav-iors (e.g., volunteering; Millette & Gagné,2008). SDT proposes that when psychological

175NORM INTERNALISATION AND COMPARTMENTALISATION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 77: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

needs (autonomy, relatedness, competence) aresatisfied by the social environment, behaviorstend to be emitted out of self-determined mo-tives (internalized), and individuals reporthigher well-being (Lynch, La Guardia, & Ryan,2009). For SDT theorists, motivations varyalong a continuum ranging from reasons that arenot internalized and are not considered as partof the self (non–self-determined), to reasonsthat are considered internalized and part of theself (self-determined). More precisely, the non–self-determined motivations include amotiva-tion, external regulation, and introjected regula-tion. Amotivation represents a total absence ofmotivation. External regulation is present whenan individual acts out of external contingencies,such as to obtain rewards or to flee punishment.Introjected regulation occurs when the individ-ual is acting out of internal pressure, so as not tofeel bad or guilty.

The more internalized and self-determinedmotivations include identified regulation, inte-grated regulation, and intrinsic motivation.Identified regulation is present when individualsfeel that engaging in the behavior is importantfor them and allows them to reach valued goals.Integrated regulation is present when individu-als act because the behavior is coherent withtheir personal objectives and values. Finally,intrinsic motivation occurs when individualsbehave out of pleasure and for the satisfactionof engaging in the activity itself.

These motivations have been explored in var-ious contexts (Vallerand, 1997), and are begin-ning to be applied to group settings (e.g., Amiot& Sansfaçon, 2011). In the current research, weemploy the SDT framework not only to captureinternalization, but also because this frameworkproposes that socially harmful behaviors will bemore difficult to internalize in the self (Deci &Ryan, 2000). Whereas the intergroup ap-proaches do not make distinctions between so-cially harmful and beneficial group norms withrespect to their capacity to become internalizedin the self (Turner, 1991), SDT clearly putslimits to this capacity for internalization by sug-gesting that potentially harmful social behaviorsare fuelled by a lack of autonomy and self-determination, and that such behaviors occurwhen one’s basic psychological needs havebeen thwarted (Deci & Ryan, 2000). In fact,SDT is a humanistic theory that assumes thathumans strive to act effectively and in a proso-

cial manner (Deci & Ryan, 2000). Empirically,studies have found that abusive supervision inthe workplace context is linked to organiza-tional deviance, and that this link is mediated byneed satisfaction (Lian, Ferris, & Brown, 2012).In the current research, we expected, based onSDT, that harmful behaviors—such as illegalbehaviors—should hence trigger compartmen-talization and intraindividual conflict and un-ease given that such behaviors clash with theperson’s true goals and aims.

Prior studies confirmed that prosocial (ratherthan harmful) norms are easier to internalizeand that group norms and individuals’ behav-ioral congruence with the norms do have animpact on the internalization of a specific nor-mative behavior (Amiot et al., 2012). Thesestudies tested competing hypotheses based onSIT versus SDT about the possibility of groupnorms to be internalized and endorsed out ofself-determination. The findings tended to sup-port SDT’s perspective: When an important in-group was encouraging a pro-social behavior,such as distributing resources equally betweengroups (prosocial parity norm), and individualswere behaving congruently with this norm, thisbehavior was more strongly internalized andself-determined (Amiot et al., 2012). The re-verse pattern was observed for harmful norms infavor of discrimination: When one’s ingroupencouraged discrimination, it is individuals whobehaved incongruently with this norm (by en-gaging instead in parity) who were more self-determined. These results suggest that ingroupnorms do have an impact on the internalizationof the behavior, and that engaging in a poten-tially harmful social behavior tends to be moredifficult to internalize in the self, even if thisbehavior is proscribed by ingroup norms. Aswell, and in line with Packer (2008), partici-pants who went against a socially harmful normin favor of discrimination by engaging in apro-social behavior instead may have drawntheir motivation from particularly internalizedvalues. Risking to go against the ingroup normscan be costly for the individual with regard ofhis or her membership within a group, whichsuggests that deviant behaviors are engaged inout of deeply held values and beliefs.

The current study will test whether thesame results are observable when we manip-ulate norms that concern an illegal behavior,namely the illegal downloading and use of

176 SANSFAÇON AND AMIOT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 78: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

computer software. This type of behavior isconsidered to be socially harmful from a legaland a moral point of view. The law considerspirated software downloading as theft (Copy-right Board of Canada, 2009). Downloadingsoftware illegally does not recognize the workconducted by software designers and com-puter companies. Moreover, not paying for aproduct does not encourage software devel-opment and increases the costs of software forbuyers (BAF, 2009). We expected that thistype of behavior will be possible to changevia the manipulation of situational normsgiven that it is an anonymous behavior(Spears et al., 2002) that is considered quitebenign in terms of its consequences (Aguiar& Martens, 2013). Furthermore, differentstudies conducted among university studiesreport diverging statistics regarding the fre-quency of illegal downloading behaviors: Forexample, whereas a Belgian survey estab-lished that 90% of students engage in theillegal downloading of software (BAF, 2009),a French survey revealed that about 40% ofstudents engage in such behavior (JDN,2004). This divergence suggests that normsregarding the prevalence of illegal download-ing are somewhat vague, and that the normsmanipulated in this context may hence berelied on more strongly as a means to resolvethis ambiguity and vagueness (Smith, Hogg,Martin, & Terry, 2007). This allows us toexpect that the norm manipulation used in thecurrent study will have a potent impact onparticipants’ downloading behaviors and ontheir internalization of these behaviors.

Subjective Processes

The experiment will also explore three newvariables that capture individual group mem-bers’ subjective experiences of the norm inter-nalization process. More specifically, we inves-tigate how individual group memberscompartmentalize their illegal behaviors withinthemselves, the level of internal pressure orconflict they subjectively feel when they intendto engage or not in a normative illegal behavior,and which specific sources of normative influ-ence they believe are influencing them whenintending to engage (or not) in illegal behaviors.

Compartmentalization

Compartmentalization is a term used whenreferring to distinct parts of the self that are keptseparated from each other (Ryan & Deci, 2003).Compartmentalization of behaviors occurswhen individuals behave in a way that fits withthe social identity and the social norms that aresalient in one specific social context, but with-out linking back this behavior to other life con-texts or to their own personal values. For exam-ple, a prisoner can act in an illegal manner whens/he is in the prison context and his or herprisoner identity is salient, but can follow lawsand regulations when s/he is in another group,her/his family for instance (Tripp, 2009). In thiscase, the illegal behavior does not represent theindividual as a whole, but represents the persononly in one particular context. Compartmental-ization should be experienced to a greater extentwhen individuals intend to engage in illegalbehaviors, because these kinds of behaviors aregenerally disapproved of by society, and need tobe kept in a distinct and hermetic part of the selfso as not to contaminate their entire sense of self(Ryan & Deci, 2003). We expect that individu-als who intend to act illegally (who intend tobehave congruently with a downloading normor incongruently with a no-downloading norm)will report higher compartmentalization thanthose who intend to act legally.

Intraindividual Conflict

Similarly, conflict arises when an individualfeels that there is a discrepancy between differ-ent parts of him or herself, such as betweendiverging norms or values (Benet-Martínez &Haritatos, 2005). Conflict may arise when theindividual acts in a way that does not representhis or her own personal values and attitudes, orwhen following group norms that are pro-scribed by another of his or her social groups.Because acting illegally is considered to be asocially harmful behavior and also based onSDT postulates, we hypothesize that intraindi-vidual conflict will be positively related to com-partmentalization. Moreover, we expect that in-dividuals who intend to act illegally (whointend to behave congruently with a download-ing norm or incongruently with a no-download-ing norm) will feel more conflict than those whoact legally.

177NORM INTERNALISATION AND COMPARTMENTALISATION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 79: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Perceived Influence

According to some researchers, the socialinfluence process is an implicit, unconsciousmechanism (Vorauer & Miller, 1997), whichalso implies that the subjective perception ofnorms can be biased (Baer, Stacy & Larimer,1991). Still, we can wonder how individualsperceive what has influenced them in a specificsituation, such as group norms or personal val-ues (Van Bavel, Packer, & Cunningham, 2008).This study will hence explore how participantssubjectively perceive different possible sourcesof influence—namely the situation, their ownpersonal values and sense of morality, and in-group norms—and how they consider that thesesources guided their behavior in the experimen-tal context. Exploring these subjective percep-tions is important given that multiple sources ofinfluence have the potential to guide people’sbehaviors in a specific context (i.e., attitudesand social norm; Ajzen, 2001) and that we wishto capture what influences people’s illegal ac-tions, from their own point of view.

The perception of what influenced one’s ownbehavior is probably a process that relates to thecompartmentalization process: A person whocompartmentalizes his or her behavior associ-ates and restricts this behavior to a particularlife context. In this sense, the more a personcompartmentalizes his or her behavior, the morelikely we expect that he or she will perceive thatit is a specific group or the situation itself (ratherthan his or her own morality or personal values)that influenced the behavior. We hence expectthat the different sources of influence will relatedifferently with compartmentalization.

The Present Research

Following past studies (Amiot et al., 2012),the first objective of this research is to testwhether a “prodownloading norm” (i.e., a normin favor of illegal downloading) versus a “no-downloading norm” (i.e., a norm that is notagainst illegal downloading) influences partici-pants’ behavioral intentions to engage in illegaldownloading behaviors. The second objective isto investigate how individuals’ congruence/incongruence with these norms influences theirself-determination (as a proxy for internaliza-tion) for engaging in this behavior. A thirdobjective is to capture participants’ subjective

experiences in the norm internalization process,and more specifically their feelings of compart-mentalization and intraindividual conflict, alongwith the sources of influence that they perceivedas guiding their behavior in the experiment. Bydoing so, the present study goes a step further ina line of research bridging SDT and intergrouptheories. By testing these hypotheses with apotentially malleable illegal behavior, and in-vestigating these additional subjective pro-cesses, this study will allow a broader look atthe consequences of engaging in and followingsocially “harmful” norms. Moreover, whereasother studies have explored the internalizationof negative social norms within correlationaldesign in real life contexts (e.g., discriminationamong hockey fans: Amiot, Sansfaçon, &Louis, in press), the present study will testwhether a situational group norm will influencethe immediate internalization of such behav-iors, in an experimental context.

To reach these goals, we present an experi-mental study in which we manipulated groupnorms and assessed participants’ congruencewith these norms. In line with past studies onnorm internalization (Amiot et al., 2012), wedecided to manipulate the descriptive norm here(individual’s perception of what other groupmembers do), instead of injunctive norms (per-ception of what group members encouraged in-dividual to do; Reno, Cialdini, & Källgren,1993: Smith & Louis, 2008; Smith et al., 2008).

Based on prior research, the following hy-potheses were made: (H1) Ingroup norms willinfluence participants’ behavioral intentions:Participants exposed to a prodownloading in-group norm will themselves report stronger in-tentions to engage in illegal prodownloading incomparison with participants exposed to a no-downloading ingroup norm. In line with priorstudies (Amiot et al., 2012), H2a predicts thatparticipants who act congruently with the no-downloading norm (and hence who do not in-tend to behave illegally themselves) will bemore self-determined than those who behaveincongruently with this norm. Among partici-pants in the prodownloading condition, thosewho intend to behave incongruently with thenorm should report higher self-determinationthan those who intend to behave in line with thisnorm (H2b). H3 and H4 predict that participantswill report higher compartmentalization andmore conflict when they intend to act illegally,

178 SANSFAÇON AND AMIOT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 80: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

that is, both when they plan to act congruentlywith a prodownloading norm or incongruentlywith a no-downloading norm. Based on a sub-jective approach to group norms, H5 predictsthat compartmentalization will be positively re-lated to intraindividual conflict. We also antic-ipate that the more a person compartmentalizeshis or her behavior, the more likely we expectthat he or she will perceive that it is a specificgroup or the situation itself (rather than his orher own morality or personal values) that influ-enced his or her behavior (H6). Finally, giventhe lack of prior studies on how individualssubjectively perceive different sources of influ-ence, we will explore how these sources ofinfluence are perceived across experimentalconditions.

Method

Participants and Design

Participants were 123 students enrolled at aFrench-speaking university in Montréal. Thestudy employed a between-participants designwhere we manipulated social identity salience(personal vs. student identity; Haslam, Oakes,Reynolds, & Turner, 1999)1 and ingroup norm-s(prodownloading vs. no-downloading of com-puter software). No participant had suspicionsabout what the goal of the study was, but 8participants did not understand the ingroupnorm manipulation correctly and were excludedfrom the sample. The final sample (N 115)included 90 females and 24 males (1 participantdid not report their gender), who were on aver-age 22.50 years old (SD 4.74).

Procedure

Participants were presented with informationabout the general context of illegal softwaredownloading among university students. Theywere told that studying at University requiresbuying and downloading costly computer pro-grams, and that some of the students may ille-gally download those programs. In line withpast research (Smith & Louis, 2008), partici-pants were then informed that a survey had beenconducted among students of their University(UQAM) to gather their opinions and behaviorsregarding illegal downloading. Bar graphs werepresented and illustrated the results observed

when asking students the following question:Do you personally illegally download and usecomputer software? For participants in thedownloading norm condition, the graphsshowed that 76% of students said that they wereengaging in illegal downloading while 24%were not. For participants in the no-download-ing norm condition, these percentages were re-versed.

To strengthen the norm manipulation further,participants read a series of five opinion state-ments about the target issue of computer soft-ware downloading, ostensibly from five stu-dents from their program who had participatedin the survey before them. The goal with thismanipulation was to provide participants withfurther ideological justifications for endorsingthe norm (Amiot & Bourhis, 2003). These state-ments indicated that four of five students fromtheir program either were engaging in the be-havior themselves (or not, depending on theexperimental condition), and that one of fivewas neutral. As a manipulation check, partici-pants were asked whether the graphs and com-ments showed that the majority of the studentsat their university were engaging in the behaviorthat involves illegally downloading computersoftware or not.

1 Identity Salience Manipulation. In line with past re-search (Haslam et al., 1999) and to test whether identitysalience accentuates the internalization of social norms(Smith & Louis, 2008), a social identity manipulation wasfirst presented. This identity salience manipulation wasbased on Haslam et al. (1999): half of the participants wereprimed with their personal identity, and the other half withtheir social identity as a student of their university. Partic-ipants in the social identity condition were asked to answerfour questions pertaining to their student identity. The samequestions were asked to participants in the personal identitycondition, but concerning themselves as individuals. Fol-lowing the manipulation, two manipulation check questionswere asked: To what extend it is easy for you to describeyourself as a UQAM student? (high identification condition:M 4.25; ST 1.50; low identification condition: M 4.55; SD 1.35); To what extent your UQAM studentidentity is important for you? (high identification condition:M 5.09; ST 1.35; low identification: M 5.20; SD 1.26), and answered on a 1 to 7 Likert scale, ranging fromNot at all to Extremely. To assess the impact of the identitysalience manipulation, ANOVAs were conducted on thetwo manipulation check questions. No significant differenceemerged between the two conditions on the two questions.The identity salience manipulation did not show any mainor interactive effects with the norms manipulation on self-determination index, t(112) 1.127, p .262; t(112 .497, p .633).

179NORM INTERNALISATION AND COMPARTMENTALISATION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 81: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Measures

Behavioral intentions. To assess partici-pants’ behavioral intentions, participantswere asked whether they would illegallydownload and use software if their program ofstudy requires it (yes/no). Answers to thisquestion were coded to indicate that partici-pants’ behavior was either congruent or in-congruent with their ingroup norm.

Motivation for emitting the behaviordisplayed. Participants were then asked tothink of the reasons why they intend to en-gage in this behavior. They were then pro-vided with 24 items based on previously val-idated SDT scales (Guay, Mageau, &Vallerand, 2003). These items measured amo-tivation (I am not really sure that this behav-ior is really worth it; .74), externalregulation (because its allows me to do whatis expected from me; .71), introjectedregulation (because I would have feltashamed if I had behave in another way; .90), identified regulation (because I believethat it is important to behave in that way; .65), integrated regulation (because this man-ner of behaving is in line with my values; .92), and intrinsic motivation (because I de-rived satisfaction from behaving this way; .93). Each of the six motivation types in-cluded four items. An index of self-determinedmotivation was created by using the followingformula: (3 intrinsic motivation 2 inte-grated regulation 1 identified regulation) (1 introjected regulation 2 external regu-lation 3 amotivation; see Vallerand &Bissonnette, 1992). It is common for SDTresearchers to compute an overall self-determination index (Ryan & Connell, 1989;Vallerand, 1997; Vallerand & Bissonnette,1992). Greater scores on this index representhigher self-determination motives relative tonon–self-determination motives. For thismeasure and the following ones, 1 (not at all)to 7 (extremely) Likert scales were used (theindex ranges from 36 up to 36).

Compartmentalization. Based on Ryanand Deci’s (2003) conceptualization of com-partmentalization, a 5-item scale was createdto measure the extent to which participants’illegal downloading behavior in the context ofthis study was compartmentalized in theirsense of self (My downloading behaviors do

not represent who I am as a person; Mydownloading behaviors are only emitted inthe current context; My downloading behav-iors are specific to a particular context of mylife; My downloading behaviors representonly a small part of who I am as a person; Mydownloading behaviors are associated with aparticular situation in my life; .84).

Intraindividual conflict. To assess theconflict between the behavior participants in-tend to emit and their personal values, fiveitems were adapted from the Benet-Martínezand Haritatos’ (2005) cultural conflict scale(e.g., My behaviors pertaining to computerdownloading are in conflict with my personalvalues; I feel torn between my downloadingbehaviors and my personal values; .86).

Perceived influence. To assess the per-ceived influence of different possible sources,we asked participants to what extent fivesources influenced their downloading behav-ior in the experiment: The situation, theirpersonal values, their morality, the softwarecompanies, and other UQAM students.

Results

Manipulations Checks: Norm andBehaviors Displayed

To assess the impact of the ingroup normmanipulation on participants’ behavioral inten-tions, chi-square analyses were conducted. Amain effect of the ingroup norm emerged,2(df 1, n 114) 10.10, p .001: Par-ticipants exposed to the prodownloading normdisplayed more downloading intentions in com-parison with participants exposed to the no-downloading norm. More specifically, amongparticipants whose ingroup endorses the normin favor of illegal downloading, 79% (n 44)displayed downloading intentions and 21%(n 12) did not display downloading inten-tions. Among participants whose ingroup en-dorsed a no-downloading norm, 50% (n 29)displayed downloading intentions and 50%(n 29) did not display downloading inten-tions. These results confirm H1: Ingroup normssignificantly influenced participants’ behavioralintentions.

180 SANSFAÇON AND AMIOT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 82: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Descriptive Statistics and CorrelationsAmong All Variables

Table 1 displays the correlations betweenthe variables for participants in all conditions.As could be expected because self-determina-tion reflects greater internalization and engag-ing in behaviors because they reflect one’schoices and values, the self-determination in-dex correlates positively with perceiving thatwhat influenced one in the experiment aretheir personal values (r .44, p .01) andmorality (r .41, p .01), but negativelywith perception of situational influence (r .20, p .05). Moreover, supporting H5,compartmentalization correlates with intrain-dividual conflict (r .40, p .01) and withsituational influence (r .41, p .01), whichis also coherent with the definition of com-partmentalization as taking place when dis-tinct behaviors are kept separated from eachother in the self and associated only withspecific situations (Ryan & Deci, 2003).Compartmentalization also correlates nega-tively with the perception that morality influ-enced one’s behavior (r .22, p .05),which is also coherent with the concept andwith H6. Finally, and in line with social iden-tity theory, perceiving the students as influ-ential is related to identifying with the studentingroup (r .21, p .05).

Motivation for Emitting the Behavior

To assess the impact of the ingroup normand the congruence of participants’ intentionsrelative to this norm on self-determination, a

2 (ingroup norm: prodownloading/no-down-loading norm) 2 (congruence: participant’sbehavioral intentions are congruent/incongru-ent with the ingroup norm) ANOVA was con-ducted on the self-determination index. Themain effect of the ingroup norm was signifi-cant, F(1, 112) 10.67, p .001, 2 .09:Participants in the no-downloading norm con-dition were more self-determined (M 12.04; SD 10.90) than participants in theprodownloading norm condition (M 3.80;SD 9.16). The main effect of congruencewas not significant, F(1, 112) 0.01, p .923, 2 .000. A significant interaction wasobserved between ingroup norm and congru-ence (Figure 1): F(1, 112) 17.295, p .001, 2 .14; participants whose behavioralintentions were congruent with the no-downloading norm condition (and who didnot engage in illegal downloading) reportedgreater self-determination (M 8.54; SD 7.81) than participants whose behavioral in-tentions were incongruent with this norm (andwho engaged in illegal downloading instead;M 1.69; SD 9.32; F(1, 57) 8.86, p .004, 2 .14). In contrast, participantswhose behavioral intentions were incongruentwith a prodownloading norm (such that theydid not engage in illegal downloading) weremore self-determined (M 3.13; SD 9.80)than participants whose behavioral intentionswere congruent with this norm (and whohence engaged in illegal downloading; M 3.40; SD 4.88, F(1, 55) 10.02, p .003, 2 .16). These findings support H2aand H2b.

Table 1Descriptive Statistics and Correlations Among All Variables

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

1. Index 7.71 8.322. Compartmentalization 3.49 1.76 .40

3. Intraindividual conflict 2.69 1.36 .50 .40

4. Influence: Situation 5.15 1.57 .20 .41 .175. Influence: Personal values 4.85 1.78 .44 .15 .17 .086. Influence: Morality 4.56 2.03 .41 .22 .08 .00 .87

7. Influence: Software companies 3.23 2.01 .11 .05 .12 .23 .12 .128. Influence: Students 2.52 1.67 .04 .07 .11 .16 .14 .42

9. Student identification 4.78 1.17 .06 .15 .16 .02 .01 .09 .21

Note. M mean; SD standard deviation. p .05. p .01. p .001.

181NORM INTERNALISATION AND COMPARTMENTALISATION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 83: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Compartmentalization

To assess the impact of the ingroup norm andthe congruence of participants’ intentions rela-tive to this norm on their perceptions of com-partmentalization, a 2 (ingroup norm: prodown-loading/no-downloading) 2 (congruence:congruent/incongruent) ANOVA was con-ducted on the measure of compartmentalization.The main effects of the ingroup norm, F(1,108) 0.92 p .338, 2 .008, and congru-ence, F(1, 108) 0.01, p .948, 2 .000,

were not significant. However, a significant in-teraction was observed between ingroup normand congruence, F(1, 108) 16,03, p .001,2 .129. Interpretation of this interaction (seeFigure 2) revealed that when the ingroup normwas a no-downloading one, participants whosebehavioral intentions were congruent with thisnorm (those who did not engage in the illegalbehavior) reported lower compartmentalization(M 2.44; SD 1.25) than participants whosebehavioral intentions were incongruent withthis norm (and who did engage in illegal down-

Figure 1. Self-determination as a function of ingroup norm and congruence.

Figure 2. Compartmentalization as a function of ingroup norm and congruence.

182 SANSFAÇON AND AMIOT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 84: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

loading; M 3.81; SD 1.92; F(1, 55) 10,13, p .002, 2 .156). In contrast, whenthe ingroup norm was a prodownloading one,participants whose behavioral intentions werecongruent with this norm (and who engaged inthe illegal behavior) reported greater compart-mentalization (M 4.16; SD 1.62) thanparticipants whose behavioral intentions wereincongruent with this norm (and who did notengage in illegal downloading; M 2.75; SD 1.59, F(1, 55) 6,72, p .012, 2 .112).These findings support H3.

Intraindividual Conflict

To assess the impact of the ingroup norm andthe congruence of participants’ intentions rela-tive to this norm on their feelings of intraindi-vidual conflict, a 2 (ingroup norm: prodown-loading/no-downloading) 2 (congruence:congruent/incongruent) ANOVA was con-ducted on the conflict variable. The main effectsof the ingroup norm, F(1, 107) .01 p .942,2 .000, and congruence, F(1, 107) .31,p .577, 2 .003, were not significant.However, a significant interaction was observedbetween ingroup norm and congruence, F(1,107) 15.07, p .001, 2 .12. Interpreta-tion of this interaction (see Figure 3) revealedthat participants whose behavioral intentionswere congruent with the no-downloading norm

(those who did not engage in the illegal behav-ior) reported lower conflict (M 1.95; SD 1.04) than participants whose behavioral inten-tions were incongruent with this norm (and whodid engage in the illegal behavior; M 3.18;SD 1.68; F(1, 54) 10.66, p .002, 2 .165). In contrast, when the ingroup norm was aprodownloading one, participants whose behav-ioral intentions were congruent with this norm(and who did engage in the illegal behavior)reported greater conflict (M 3.01; SD 1.22)than participants whose behavioral intentionswere incongruent with this norm (and who didnot engage in illegal behavior; M 2.09; SD 0.67, F(1, 53) 5.64, p .021, 2 .096),supporting H4.

Perceived Influence of Different Sources

To assess the impact of group norms and thecongruence of participants’ intentions relativeto the ingroup norm on their perceptions of theinfluence of different sources, we conducted a 2(ingroup norm: prodownloading/no-download-ing) 2 (congruence: congruent/incongruent)ANOVA with the five sources of normativeinfluence as a repeated-measures factor. A sig-nificant main effect of the ingroup normemerged, F(1, 104) 5.02, p .027, 2 .046, showing that participants generally per-ceived these sources of influence to be stronger

Figure 3. Intraindividual conflict as a function of ingroup norm and congruence.

183NORM INTERNALISATION AND COMPARTMENTALISATION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 85: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

when the ingroup norm was not supporting il-legal downloading (M 4.26; SD 0.17) thanwhen this norm was supporting illegal down-loading (M 3.77; SD 0.17). A significantmain effect for the source of influence emerged,F(5, 100) 48.38, p .001, 2 .708.Planned contrasts conducted to locate the dif-ferences among the sources showed that partic-ipants perceived the sources of influence per-taining to the situation (M 5.04; SD 0.16)and to their personal values (M 4.97; SD 0.17) to be stronger than their morality (M 4.77; SD 0.19; F(1, 10) 3.49, p .029,2 .045). Moreover, morality was seen as amore influential source than software compa-nies (M 3.19; SD 0.22; F(1, 10) 22.16,p .043, 2 .039). Finally, students fromparticipants’ university were perceived as hav-ing the least influence on their own behavior(M 2.41; SD 0.18; F(1, 104) 52.34, p .006, 2 .070).

A three-way interaction emerged between in-group norm, congruence, and source of norma-tive influence, F(5, 100) 4.36, p .001, 2 .179. Simple effect tests revealed that for thesources of normative influence pertaining tosoftware companies and to students, the norm

by congruence interactions were not significant,Fs .75 to 1.34, ps .25 to .39, 2 .007 to.01. For the 3 other sources (i.e., personal val-ues, morality, situation), significant norm bycongruence interactions emerged (Figure 4;Fs 10.117 to 13.28, ps .000 to .026, 2 .04 to .11). Participants in the prodownloadingnorm condition who intended to behave congru-ently with this norm perceived that it was thesituation (M 5.46; SD 1.50) that guidedthem more than their personal values (M 3.93; SD 1.79; F(1, 40) 22.98, p .001,2 .37), followed by their morality (M 3.66; SD 1.93; F(1, 40) 4.91, p .032,2 .10). In contrast, participants who in-tended to behave congruently with a no-downloading norm perceived that their personalvalues (M 5.89; SD 1.45) and their moral-ity (M 5.89; SD 1.44; F(1, 27) 00 p 1.00, 2 .00) guided them more in theirbehavior than the situation (M 4.79; SD 1.55; F(1, 27) 10.45, p .003, 2 .28).There is no significant difference for partici-pants who intended to behave either incongru-ently with a no-downloading or a prodownload-ing norm (Fs 2.33 to 2.40, ps .10 to .17,2 .08 to .13).

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

Perc

eive

d In

fluen

ce o

f So

urce

No-downloadingNorm-

Congruence

Pro-downloadingNorm-

Congruence

No-downloadingNorm-

Incongruence

Pro-downloadingNorm-

Incongruence

Influence of Personal Values Influence of One's MoralityInfluence of Situation

Figure 4. Perceived influence as a function of ingroup norm, congruence, and three sourcesof influence (personal values, morality, situation).

184 SANSFAÇON AND AMIOT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 86: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Discussion

The first objective of this experiment was toinvestigate the motives that underlie an illegalnormative behavior (i.e., illegally downloadingcomputer software or not) and to measure theextent to which this behavior can be situation-ally internalized in the self and endorsed forself-determined motivations. As a second objec-tive, this experiment tested the impact of amanipulated norm and of participants’ inten-tions to behave in congruence with this norm onvariables that captured participants’ subjectiveperceptions in the internalization process,namely compartmentalization, intraindividualconflict, and perceived sources of influence.This study used an established methodologyfrom the social norms literature (Smith & Louis,2008). Our findings confirmed the role playedby behavioral congruence in the internalizationof a potentially socially harmful behavior, whileproviding new information on participants’ sub-jective perceptions in this process. Investigatingthese ideas allows a better comprehension of themotivations that drive some illegal behaviors. Inaddition, identifying the subjective processesthat are activated when people plan to act ille-gally allows to capture what the perpetrators ofthese actions experience when engaging in suchbehaviors, and to develop more targeted inter-vention programs aimed at reducing illegal be-havior such as theft.

The ingroup norms we manipulated did in-fluence participants’ intentions to engage inprodownloading versus no-downloading be-haviors, confirming H1. Also, participantswho intended to behave congruently withtheir ingroup’s no-downloading norm weremore self-determined compared to partici-pants whose behavioral intentions were notcongruent with this norm (i.e., and whoplanned to behave illegally instead), whichconfirms H2a. In addition, participants whosebehavioral intentions were incongruent withthe prodownloading norm reported higherself-determination compared to participantswhose behavioral intentions were congruentwith this prodownloading ingroup norm, sup-porting H2b. Hence, above and beyond the nor-mative pressure, individuals internalize moreeasily a prolegal than an illegal behavior. Theseresults correspond with SDT and replicateAmiot and colleagues’ findings (2012), by

showing that prosocial behaviors tend to bemore self-determined, even if the individual isacting against his or her group norm. Moreover,these prior studies directly measured the behav-ior, whereas the current study assessed behav-ioral intentions. It is reassuring to observe sim-ilar results across the studies.

The current results are also consistent withliterature that states that an individual may de-viate from his or her group norms if s/he con-siders that it is better for the group to do so;such as because doing so may improve thegroup situation (Packer, 2008), or if the ingroupnorms go against one’s own moral values(Hornsey, Smith, & Begg, 2007). Still, it wouldbe interesting to further investigate whether thenorm that encourage illegally downloadingcomputer software is perceived as being accept-able for the individual and as being hurtfulversus good for the group. Even if it is illegal,accessing the latest software can be perceived asgood when it help the group to maintain highstandards which may then allow its members togain social recognition and status. The percep-tion of how the norm helps (vs. hurts) the groupmay influence the internalization process byfacilitating the acceptation or rejection of thenorm within his or her self. Furthermore, devi-ating from ingroup norms when these norms areperceived as harmful may represent a strategythat group members use to realign their grouptoward more prosocial actions and norms(Packer, 2008).

Moreover, participants who intended to actillegally—both by behaving incongruently witha no-downloading norm and by failing to followa no-downloading norm—felt more compart-mentalization and intraindividual conflict thanparticipants who did not intend to engage in theillegal downloading behavior (supporting H3and H4). These results confirm that compart-mentalization and intraindividual conflict aremost strongly activated when participants in-tend to behave illegally, regardless of ingroupnorms: Behaving incongruently with the normof an important ingroup per se did not inducesuch high levels of compartmentalization orconflict. These variables may help to protect theself from the negative impact of behaving ille-gally. While the current data do not provideexact information about which specific mecha-nisms may be blocking the internalization ofillegal norms and also making these norms

185NORM INTERNALISATION AND COMPARTMENTALISATION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 87: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

harder to feel comfortable about (e.g., intuitiveknowledge about what’s right vs. wrong), this isan exciting direction for future research whichalign with SDT postulates.

Participants’ perceptions of the five sourcesof influence also differed as a function of normsand congruence. When participants were in-tending to behave legally, they were more likelyto perceive that it was their personal values/morality that guided their behavior. When theyintended to behave illegally, they stated that thesituational sources influenced them the most.These findings could be attributable to a classicself-serving bias (Miller & Ross, 1975),whereby people attribute their positive behav-iors to internal causes (their values) and attri-bute their negative behaviors to external causes(the situation). These findings could also relateto the notion of compartmentalization; whenparticipants behave illegally, they may becomemore prone to perceive the situation as influ-encing them and to dissociate themselves per-sonally from their action by putting a distancebetween their self (i.e., their own values andmorality) and their behavior.

Interestingly, and despite the fact that ouringroup norm manipulation had a strong effecton participants’ intentions in the experiment,participants were less likely to perceive thatstudents from their university had influencedtheir behavioral intentions. This pattern of find-ings is also congruent with literature suggestingthat explicit and implicit processes are not al-ways congruent (Briñol, Petty, & Wheeler,2006) and that what is really influencing a per-son’s behavior is often different from what theperson’s subjectively reported as influencinghim/her (Nisbett & Wilson, 1977). Still, thecorrelation between identifying with the studentingroup and perceiving the group as a source ofinfluence suggest that the more participantsidentify with this ingroup, the more they con-sider the student group as influencing them,which is coherent with social identity theoryand its approach to group norms (e.g., Terry &Hogg, 1996; Turner, 1991). Future studiesshould further investigate these subjective per-ceptions, how they shape and predict differenttypes of normative behaviors, and what are thepotential effects of this mismatch between per-ceptions and behaviors.

It will also be interesting to further testwhether the results obtained herein can be ob-

served in different life contexts and over thelonger-term. It is important to note that thecurrent research investigated the immediate im-pact of a situational norm in an experimentalcontext. Still, we can wonder about the processof internalization over time in real life contextsand of how repeating behaviors over time maylead to the internalization of these behaviors.Further studies should investigate these longer-term processes by using longitudinal design.For example, we could follow over a 1-yearperiod a cohort of new university students asthey are in the process of endorsing new socialnorms, including potentially harmful norms(e.g., cheating in exams). By assessing repeat-edly over this period of time variables such asperceived norms, the frequency of the norma-tive behavior and self-determination to engagein this behavior, and degree of social identifi-cation with the group, such research will allowto paint a more complete picture of the inter-nalization process over time. Moreover, the rateof internalization of group norms is possiblylinked to changes in identification with thegroup, such that a normative behavior becomesmore congruent and internalized in the person’sself when this person develops a stronger senseof identification with the group (Terry & Hogg,1996). Understanding the temporal and longer-term processes involved in norm internalizationcould be useful to eventually create programs orcampaigns that aim to reduce illegal behaviorsin the workplace or school context. Consideringthe immediate versus the long-term impact of agroup norm on the behaviors and their internal-ization will also provide a more complete ac-count for why individuals engage in those neg-ative behaviors and how such behaviorsdevelop.

References

Aguiar, L., & Martens, B. (2013). Digital musicconsumption on the Internet: Evidence from Click-stream Data. European Commission: Institute forprospective technological studies. doi:10.2791/83798

Ajzen, I. (2001). Nature and operation of attitudes.Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 27–58.

Amiot, C. E., & Bourhis, R. Y. (2003). Discrimina-tion and the positive-negative asymmetry effect:Ideological and normative processes. Personalityand Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 597–608. doi:10.1177/0146167203029005005

186 SANSFAÇON AND AMIOT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 88: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

Amiot, C. E., & Sansfaçon, S. (2011). Motivations toidentify with social groups: Positive and negativeconsequences. Group Dynamics: Theory, Re-search, and Practice, 15, 105–127. doi:10.1037/a0023158

Amiot, C. E., Sansfaçon, S., & Louis, W. R. (2013).Investigating the motivations underlying harmfulsocial behaviors and the motivational nature ofsocial norms. Journal of Applied Social Psychol-ogy, 43, 2146–2157. doi:10.1111/jasp.12167

Amiot, C. E., Sansfaçon, S., & Louis, W. R. (inpress). How normative and social identificationprocesses predict self-determination to engage inderogatory behaviours against outgroup hockeyfans. European Journal of Social Psychology.

Amiot, C. E., Sansfaçon, S., Louis, W. R., & Yelle,M. (2012). Testing the role of group norms andbehavioral congruence in the internalization of dis-criminatory and parity behaviors. Personality andSocial Psychology Bulletin, 38, 63–76. doi:10.1177/0146167211429804

Baer, J. S., Stacy, A., & Larimer, M. (1991). Biasesin the perception of drinking norms among collegestudents. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 52, 580–586.

Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2004). Four rootsof evil. In M. G. Arthur (Ed.), The social psychol-ogy of good and evil, (pp. 87–101). New York,NY: Guilford Press.

Belgian Anti-Piracy Federation. (2009). Étude sur letéléchargement auprès des jeunes en Belgique(Study on illegal downloading in Belgium Youth).Press Release.

Benet-Martínez, V., & Haritatos’, J. (2005). Bicul-tural Identity Integration (BII): Components andsocio-personality antecedents. Journal of Person-ality, 73, 1015–1050. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00337.x

Briñol, P., Petty, R. E., & Wheeler, S. C. (2006).Discrepancies between explicit and implicit selfconcepts: Consequences for information process-ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,91, 154–170. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.154

Copyright Board of Canada. (2009). Copying forPrivate Use. Copyright Art of Canada, subsection,83(8).

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and“why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry,11, 227–268. doi:10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01

Fielding, K. S., McDonald, R., & Louis, W. R.(2008). Theory of planned behavior, identity, andintentions to engage in environmental activism.Journal of Environmental Psychology, 28, 318–326. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.03.003

Frone, M. R., & Brown, A. L. (2010). Workplacesubstance-use norms as predictors of employeesubstance use and impairment: A survey of US

workers. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs,71, 526–534.

Guay, F., Mageau, G. A., & Vallerand, R. J. (2003).On the hierarchical structure of self determinedmotivation: A test of top-down, bottom-up, recip-rocal, and horizontal effects. Personality and So-cial Psychology Bulletin, 29, 992–1004. doi:10.1177/0146167203253297

Haslam, S. A., Oakes, P. J., Reynolds, K. J., &Turner, J. C. (1999). Social identity salience andthe emergence of stereotype consensus. Personal-ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 809–818.doi:10.1177/0146167299025007004

Haslam, S. A., & Reicher, S. (2007). Beyond thebanality of evil: Three dynamics of an integration-ist social psychology of tyranny. Personality andSocial Psychology Bulletin, 33, 615– 622. doi:10.1177/0146167206298570

Hornsey, M. J., Smith, J. R., & Begg, D. (2007).Effects of norms among those with moral convic-tion: Counter-conformity emerges on intentionsbut not behaviors. Social Influence, 2, 244–268.doi:10.1080/15534510701476500

Jacobson, R. J., Mortensen, C. R., & Cialdini, R.(2011). Bodies obliged and unbound: Differenti-ated response tendencies for injunctive and de-scriptive social norms. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 100, 433–448.

JDN. (2004). Téléchargement illégal: On continuemalgré les risques. (Illegal downloading: We con-tinue regardless of the risks.) Retrieved on Sep-tember 20, 2013, from http://www.journaldunet.com/0405/040514sondage.shtml

Kelman, H. C. (1961). Processes of opinion change.Public Opinion Quarterly, 25, 57–78. doi:10.1086/266996

Kerr, N. L., Garst, J., Kiehle, D., & Harris, S. (1997).That still, small voice: Commitment to cooperateas an internalized vs. a social norm. Personalityand Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 1300–1311.doi:10.1177/01461672972312007

Kerr, N. L., & Kaufman-Gilliland, C. M. (1994).Communication, commitment, and cooperation insocial dilemmas. Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 66, 513–529. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.66.3.513

Kura, K. M., Shamsudin, F. M., & Chauhan, A.(2013). Modeling the influence of group normsand self-regulatory efficacy on workplace deviantbehaviour. Asian Social Science, 9, 113–122.

Lee, K., & Allen, N. J. (2002). Organizational citi-zenship behavior and workplace deviance: Therole of affect and cognition. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 87, 131–142. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.87.1.131

Lian, H., Ferris, L. D., & Brown, D. J. (2012). Doestaking the good with the bad make things worse?How abusive supervision and leader–member ex-

187NORM INTERNALISATION AND COMPARTMENTALISATION

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.

Page 89: Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice · PDF file12.05.2014 · 2014 Volume 18, Issue 2 (Jun) Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice Editor: David Marcus, PhD Journal

change interact to impact need satisfaction andorganizational deviance. Organizational Behaviorand Human Decision Processes, 117, 41–52. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.10.003

Lynch, M. F., La Guardia, J. G., & Ryan, R. M.(2009). On being yourself in different cultures:Ideal and actual self-concept, autonomy support,and well-being in China, Russia, and the UnitedStates. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4,290–304. doi:10.1080/17439760902933765

Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Sell-serving biasesin the attribution of causality: Fact or fiction?Psychological Bulletin, 82, 213–225. doi:10.1037/h0076486

Millette, V., & Gagné, M. (2008). Designing volun-teers’ tasks to maximize motivation, satisfactionand performance: The impact of job characteristicson volunteer engagement. Motivation and Emo-tion, 32, 11–22. doi:10.1007/s11031-007-9079-4

Nisbett, R. E., & Wilson, T. D. (1977). Telling morethan we can know: Verbal reports on mental pro-cesses. Psychological Review, 84, 1–259.

Packer, D. J. (2008). On being both with us andagainst us: A normative conflict model of dissentin social groups. Personality and Social Psy-chology Review, 12, 50 –72. doi:10.1177/1088868307309606

Reno, R. R., Cialdini, R. B., & Kallgren, C. A.(1993). The transsituational influence of socialnorms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 64, 104 –112. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.64.1.104

Robinson, S. L., & Greenberg, J. (1998). Employeesbehaving badly: Dimensions, determinants and di-lemmas in the study of workplace deviance. InC. L. Cooper & D. M. Rousseau (Ed.), Trends inorganizational behavior (Vol. 5, pp. 1–30). NewYork, NY: Wiley.

Robinson, S. L., & O’Leary-Kelly, A. M. (1998).Monkey see, monkey do: The influence of workgroups on the antisocial behavior of employees.Academy of Management Journal, 41, 658–672.doi:10.2307/256963

Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locusof causality and internalization: Examining rea-sons for acting in two domains. Journal of Person-ality and Social Psychology, 57, 749–761. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.57.5.749

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2003). On assimilatingidentities to the self: A self-determination theoryperspective on internalization and integrity withincultures. In M. R. Ryan & E. L. Deci (Eds.),Handbook on self & identity (pp. 253–272). NewYork, NY: The Guilford Press.

Smith, J. R., Hogg, M. A., Martin, R., & Terry, D. J.(2007). Uncertainty and the influence o groupnorms in the attitude-behaviour relationship. Brit-

ish Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 769–792.doi:10.1348/014466606X164439

Smith, J. R., & Louis, W. R. (2008). Do as we sayand as we do: The interplay of descriptive andinjunctive group norms in the attitude-behaviorrelationship. British Journal of Social Psychology,47, 647–666. doi:10.1348/014466607X269748

Smith, J. R., Terry, D. J., Manstead, A. S. R. Louis,W. R., Kotterman, D., & Wolfs, J. (2008). Theattitude-behavior relationship in consumer con-duct: The role of norms, past behavior, and self-identity. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148,311–333. doi:10.3200/SOCP.148.3.311-334

Spears, R., Postmes, T., Lea, M., & Wolbert, A.(2002). When are net effects of gross products?The power of influence and the influence of powerin computer-mediated communication. Journal ofSocial Issues, 58(1), 91–107. doi:10.1111/1540-4560.00250

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identitytheory of inter-group behavior. In S. Worchel &L. W. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergrouprelations. Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.

Terry, D. J., & Hogg, M. A. (1996). Group normsand the attitude-behavior relationship: A role forgroup identification. Personality and Social Psy-chology Bulletin, 22, 776 –793. doi:10.1177/0146167296228002

Tripp, B. (2009). Fathers in jail: Managing dual iden-tities. Applied Psychology in Criminal Justice, 5,26–56.

Turner, J. C. (1991). Social influence. Belmont, CA:Thomson Brooks/Cole Publishing Co.

Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchicalmodel of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. InM. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimentalsocial psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 271–360). SanDiego, CA: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60019-2

Vallerand, R. J., & Bissonnette, R. (1992). Intrinsic,extrinsic, and amotivational styles as predictors ofbehavior: A prospective study. Journal of Person-ality, 60, 599–620. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1992.tb00922.x

Van Bavel, J. J., Packer, D. J., & Cunningham, W. A.(2008). The neural substrates of in-group bias afunctional magnetic resonance imaging investiga-tion. Psychological Science, 19, 1131–1139. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02214.x

Vorauer, J. D., & Miller, D. T. (1997). Failure torecognize the effect of implicit social influence onthe presentation of self. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 73, 281–295. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.2.281

Received September 24, 2012Revision received December 14, 2013

Accepted December 17, 2013

188 SANSFAÇON AND AMIOT

Thi

sdo

cum

ent

isco

pyri

ghte

dby

the

Am

eric

anPs

ycho

logi

cal

Ass

ocia

tion

oron

eof

itsal

lied

publ

ishe

rs.

Thi

sar

ticle

isin

tend

edso

lely

for

the

pers

onal

use

ofth

ein

divi

dual

user

and

isno

tto

bedi

ssem

inat

edbr

oadl

y.