explaining individual and cross-cultural differences in mirror...

127
Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror Self-recognition by Senay Cebioğlu M.A. (Psychology) Koç University, 2010 B.A. (Hons, Psychology) Ege University, 2006 Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Department of Psychology Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences © Senay Cebioğlu 2019 SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY Spring 2019 Copyright in this work rests with the author. Please ensure that any reproduction or re-use is done in accordance with the relevant national copyright legislation.

Upload: others

Post on 17-Oct-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences

in Mirror Self-recognition

by

Senay Cebioğlu

M.A. (Psychology) Koç University, 2010

B.A. (Hons, Psychology) Ege University, 2006

Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the

Requirements for the Degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

in the

Department of Psychology

Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences

© Senay Cebioğlu 2019

SIMON FRASER UNIVERSITY

Spring 2019

Copyright in this work rests with the author. Please ensure that any reproduction or re-use is done in accordance with the relevant national copyright legislation.

Page 2: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

ii

Approval

Name: Senay Cebioğlu

Degree: Doctor of Philosophy

Title: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror Self-recognition

Examining Committee: Chair: Tom Spalek Professor

Tanya Broesch Senior Supervisor Associate Professor

Jeremy Carpendale Supervisor Professor

Michelle Kline Supervisor Assistant Professor

Maureen Hoskyn Internal Examiner Associate Professor Faculty of Education

Mark Nielsen External Examiner Associate Professor School of Psychology The University of Queensland

Date Defended/Approved: April 3, 2019

Page 3: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

iii

Ethics Statement

Page 4: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

iv

Abstract

Mirror self-recognition (MSR) is considered the benchmark of the capacity to think about

oneself. Around 18 months of age, infants across cultures vary systematically in their

MSR abilities. Understanding the developmental processes that underlie these

differences is critical to understanding the ontogeny of human self-awareness. The

overarching goal of my dissertation was to identify the early experiences that are linked

to infants’ ability to self-recognize, with three independent but linked studies. In study 1, I

recruited 18- to 22 months-old infants and their mothers from two distinct sociocultural

environments: urban Canada and rural Vanuatu, a small-scale island society located in

the South Pacific. Consistent with previous cross-cultural research, ni-Vanuatu infants

passed the MSR test at significantly lower rates (7%) compared to Canadian infants

(68%). Among a suite of social-interactional variables, mothers’ imitation of their infants

during a short free play session with toys best predicted infants’ performance on the

MSR test. In addition, low passing rates among ni-Vanuatu infants could not be

attributed to reasons non-related to self-development (i.e., motivation to show mark-

directed behavior, understanding mirror-correspondence, representational thinking). In

study 2, I aimed to replicate my previous results on imitation, this time in a culturally

diverse urban Canadian sample of 15- to 21-month-old infants and their parents, in order

to bypass potential confounding factors related to the cross-cultural validity of the test. In

this study, mothers’ imitation of their infants observed during a short free play session

predicted infants’ MSR, while controlling for infants’ temperament. In study 3, I examined

whether the referential content of mothers’ child-directed speech systematically differed

between ni-Vanuatu and Canadian cultures. When interacting with their 18- to 28-month-

olds during a dyadic free play session, Canadian mothers referred significantly more to

their toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire), linked toddlers’ internal states

to their acts and perceptions, and produced more references to past events. These

results bring attention to the role that language interactions may be playing in the

emergence of a self-concept. Together, these 3 studies provide evidence that will help

us better understand the social-interactional processes underlying the development of

self-awareness in infancy.

Keywords: Mirror self-recognition; objective self-awareness; culture; imitation;

language interactions

Page 5: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

v

Dedication

I dedicate my dissertation to the children of Tanna. I caught sight of pure happiness as I

watched them play.

Page 6: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

vi

Acknowledgements

It is a pleasure to thank the people without whom I would not have been able to

accomplish this work. First and foremost, I would like to express my deepest gratitude to

my senior supervisor Dr. Tanya Broesch. She provided her generous help at every step

of this project and she changed my life by introducing me to fieldwork, and to my family

in Tanna Island, Vanuatu. I am also grateful to my committee members Dr. Jeremy

Carpendale and Dr. Michelle Kline for providing much appreciated feedback on this

work. Moreover, both of them have been role models; they have thought me how to be a

better academic, a better teacher, and a better person.

I would like to thank Dr. Kiley Hamlin and Van de Vondervoort, J. W., our

collaborators at the UBC Psychology Department, for their contribution to data collection,

and Dr. Kelly Marin for providing her expertise in language. I would also like to thank Dr.

Lara Aknin for her feedback at the earlier steps of the project, and Dr. Mike Maraun for

his vigilant statistical counselling. A special thanks goes to my former advisor Dr. Nazan

Aksan, for her continuing support. Last but not least, I gratefully acknowledge the

Graduate International Research Travel Award that was granted to me by the Graduate

and Postdoctoral Studies at SFU, which made my first trip to Vanuatu possible.

I would like to express my sincere appreciation to all the parents and children

who participated in this research. Our knowledge on infant development progresses

thanks to them. I am indebted to Rachel Iakin who is a ni-Vanuatu field worker affiliated

with the Cultural Center in Vanuatu. She worked as a research assistant in this project.

She welcomed me to the village of Lounikavik and helped me survive and thrive in a

culture that was initially foreign to me. I will be forever grateful for her help and

friendship. I am also grateful to Chief Tom Noam, my father in Tanna, for accepting me

into his family. Hilary Aime, a dear friend and a member of Culture and Development

Lab at SFU shared some of this journey with me. I am grateful for her valuable support

and companionship.

Several research assistants have contributed to this project including Sam Allen,

Sukhneet Bains, Diana Bajo, Sila Kiral, Jennifer Lawley, Kirsten Martin, Puneet Sekhon,

and Marieke Vandenhende. Most of them were undergraduate students who worked part

Page 7: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

vii

time jobs elsewhere to fund their education. The fact that they volunteered in this project

proves that they had high internal motivation to learn and to be part of the academic

community. Undergraduate research assistants fulfil an important role in the research

that is being conducted in our universities world-wide; they are the foot soldiers of

science.

My family has been incredibly supportive during all the years I have been away

from home to complete my PhD degree. They provided me with financial and moral

support. They endured years of yearning for their daughter so that she could achieve her

dreams. I am thankful for their support. I also would like to take the opportunity to thank

my friends in Vancouver, who have been a second family to me. Nural Sumbultepe, Ece

Sonmez, Ataman Avdan, and Burcak Dogan are a few names among many. One of the

advantages of being a psychologist is that you get to have friends who are mental health

professionals. I have a terrific support team constituting of 4 psychologist women whom I

met in Istanbul in 2008. They were a phone call or a text message away whenever I felt

overwhelmed with the adversities of life as a graduate student. Thank you, Ceren

Sonmez, Irem Guroglu, Beyza Ates, and Pinar Arslan. Two people helped me survive

the struggles of finishing up this work. I offer my gratitude and appreciation to my

colleague Andrea Smit who has been a huge source of support over the last few months

as both of our defence dates approached. And I thank Bruno Olaizola Mampaso for his

valuable emotional support during those daunting weeks prior to the defence. In

addition, I would like to thank my friend Beau Wallbridge for proofreading my dissertation

and my friend Canan Gunes for polishing my bibliography. Thank you Ishtar Beck,

Adrian de Bruyn, and Thais Amaral for helping me celebrate which was unsurprisingly

hard once it was all over. Finally, thanks to all friends and colleagues who have listened

to me, challenged me, and inspired me over the years either inside or outside the walls

of the university. It takes a village to write a dissertation, or to accomplish anything for

that matter.

Page 8: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

viii

Table of Contents

Approval .......................................................................................................................... ii

Ethics Statement ............................................................................................................ iii

Abstract .......................................................................................................................... iv

Dedication ....................................................................................................................... v

Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................ vi

Table of Contents .......................................................................................................... viii

List of Tables .................................................................................................................. xi

List of Figures................................................................................................................ xii

General Introduction ..................................................................................................... 1

Defining objective self-awareness ............................................................................... 1

Mirror Self-recognition ..................................................................................................... 2

Cross-cultural Variation in MSR Passing Rates ............................................................... 6

Methodology ................................................................................................................... 7

Research Methods and Theoretical Framework ........................................................... 7

Field Sites .................................................................................................................... 8

Three Studies on Self in Infancy ...................................................................................... 9

Chapter 1. Study 1. Understanding Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror Self-Recognition ........................................................................................................ 11

1.1. Abstract ............................................................................................................... 11

1.2. Introduction .......................................................................................................... 11

1.2.1. MSR Varies across Sociocultural Contexts .................................................. 13

1.2.2. Framing the Differences in Developmental Trajectories ............................... 14

1.2.3. Understanding the Developmental Mechanisms .......................................... 16

1.2.4. Alternative Explanations .............................................................................. 18

1.2.5. Summary and Hypotheses........................................................................... 21

1.3. Methods .............................................................................................................. 22

1.3.1. Sociocultural Contexts ................................................................................. 22

Tanna. .................................................................................................................... 23

Vancouver. ............................................................................................................. 23

1.3.2. Participants .................................................................................................. 24

1.3.3. Procedure .................................................................................................... 25

1.3.4. Measures ..................................................................................................... 27

Invisible displacement (ID) test............................................................................... 27

Mirror mediated behaviour (MMB) test ................................................................... 27

Mirror self-recognition (MSR) test ........................................................................... 28

Knee-marking test .................................................................................................. 29

Free Play sessions ................................................................................................. 29

1.3.5. Coding and Reliability .................................................................................. 30

Behavioral tasks ..................................................................................................... 30

Page 9: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

ix

Free play interactions ............................................................................................. 31

1.4. Results ................................................................................................................ 33

1.4.1. Differences in MSR Passing Rates .............................................................. 33

1.4.2. Examining Alternative Explanations for Cross-cultural Variability ................. 34

1.4.3. Predicting MSR ............................................................................................ 35

Correlations between social interactional variables and MSR ................................ 36

Comparing joint attention levels between Vanuatu and Canada ............................. 36

Comparing imitation levels between Vanuatu and Canada ..................................... 37

Summary of associations ....................................................................................... 38

Logistic regression Model Predicting MSR ............................................................. 38

1.5. Discussion ........................................................................................................... 39

Chapter 2. Study 2. Parental Imitation and Mirror Self-Recognition: Examining Within-Culture Variability .................................................................................. 44

2.1. Abstract ............................................................................................................... 44

2.2. Introduction .......................................................................................................... 44

2.3. Current Research ................................................................................................ 48

2.4. Methods .............................................................................................................. 48

2.4.1. Participants .................................................................................................. 48

2.4.2. Procedure .................................................................................................... 50

2.4.3. Measures ..................................................................................................... 50

2.4.4. Coding and Reliability .................................................................................. 52

2.5. Results ................................................................................................................ 54

2.5.1. Predicting Self-recognizers .......................................................................... 54

2.5.2. Describing the Effects of Culture, Gender and Free play Context on Imitation . .................................................................................................................... 55

2.5.3. Describing the Effects of Culture and Gender on Temperament .................. 57

2.6. Discussion ........................................................................................................... 57

Chapter 3. Study 3. Child-directed Speech as a Context for the Socialization of Self: A comparison of Canada and Vanuatu .................................................... 61

3.1. Abstract ............................................................................................................... 61

3.2. Introduction .......................................................................................................... 61

3.3. Methods .............................................................................................................. 66

3.3.1. Participants .................................................................................................. 66

3.3.2. Societies ...................................................................................................... 67

3.3.3. Procedure .................................................................................................... 67

3.3.4. Transcription and Translation of the Interactions ......................................... 69

3.3.5. Parsing the Transcriptions ........................................................................... 71

3.3.6. Coding ......................................................................................................... 71

General referential content ..................................................................................... 72

Referential content focused at the child .................................................................. 73

Referential content focused at the child’s internal states ........................................ 73

Past talk ................................................................................................................. 74

3.4. Results ................................................................................................................ 75

Page 10: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

x

3.4.1. General Referential Content ........................................................................ 75

3.4.2. Referential Content Focused on the Child ................................................... 76

3.4.3. Referential Content Focused on the Child’s Internal States ......................... 78

3.4.4. Past Talk ..................................................................................................... 80

3.5. Discussion ........................................................................................................... 80

General Discussion ..................................................................................................... 87

Maternal Imitation Predicts MSR ................................................................................... 87

Child-directed Speech Varies across Cultures............................................................... 89

MSR Is a Cross-culturally Valid Measure of Objective Self-awareness.......................... 90

The Path Forward ......................................................................................................... 91

References ................................................................................................................... 93

Appendix A. Supplementary Material on Study 1 .............................................. 107

Section A.1. Pictures of Testing Materials ................................................................... 107

Section A.2. Information on contingency tables used to compare imitation levels between cultures ....................................................................................................................... 111

Appendix B. Supplementary Material on Study 2 .............................................. 113

Section B.1. Comparing mother-infant and father-infant dyads .................................... 113

Section B.2. Procedural differences with regards to the number of visits ..................... 113

Section B.3. Procedural differences with regards to parents’ gender ........................... 114

Section B.4. Procedural differences with regards to the identity of the experimenter ... 114

Section B.5. Differences in children’s mean age due to procedural differences ........... 115

Page 11: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

xi

List of Tables

Table 1.1. Correlations between Social-interactional Variables and the MSR Test . 36

Table 1.2. Summarizing the Associations between Site, Social Interactions, and MSR ....................................................................................................... 38

Table 1.3. Regression Model Predicting MSR ......................................................... 39

Table 2.1. Logistic Regression with all Potential Predictors in the Model ................ 55

Table 3.1. Coding of the General Referential Content ............................................. 72

Table 3.2. Coding of the Referential Content Focused on the Child ........................ 73

Table 3.3. Coding of the Referential Content Focused on the Child’s Internal States ............................................................................................................... 74

Page 12: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

xii

List of Figures

Figure 1.1. Comparison of Ni-Vanuatu and Canadian infants’ passing rates for the MSR test and the control measures ....................................................... 35

Figure 3.1. Percentage distribution of referential content categories focused on the child in ni-Vanuatu and Canadian parents’ child-directed speech ........... 78

Figure 3.2. Percentage distribution of mind-minded and body-minded internal state references among ni-Vanuatu and Canadian parents’ total internal state references .............................................................................................. 80

Page 13: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

1

General Introduction

Infants begin to recognize themselves in a mirror around 18 months of age. This

seemingly simple ability marks the benchmark of objective self-awareness —ability to

think about oneself. Interestingly, cross-cultural research indicates that the age at which

infants start to recognize themselves in a mirror varies systematically between cultures

(Broesch, Callaghan, Henrich, Murphy, & Rochat, 2011; Kärtner, Keller, Chaudhary, &

Yovsi, 2012). Explaining how infants’ early experiences contribute to this variation is

critical to our understanding of the ontogeny of human self-awareness. The overarching

goal of my dissertation is to better understand the links between infants’ social-

interactive experience and self-recognition ability with three studies. In this general

introduction, I briefly introduce the underlying motivation for this research as well as my

theoretical framework and research methodology. Finally, I will present the specific

research questions I addressed in each study and summarize my findings.

Defining objective self-awareness

The concept of objective self-awareness refers to the ability to think about the

self. This ability marks the transition from a subjective level of self-experience to a more

complex form of self-experience where one becomes the object of one’s own thought

(Duval & Wicklund, 1972; James, 1890/1950; Mead, 1934/1967).

Subjective self-awareness refers to experiencing oneself as the source of

perception and action while the awareness remains at an organismic level (Duval &

Wicklund, 1972). In other words self-other distinction is functionally present and can be

observed in perception and action but not yet in thought. For example, newborns

differentiate their own touch from that of others, a phenomenon measured through the

rooting reflex (Rochat & Hespos, 1997). Starting from two months of age they become

engaged participants in face to face exchanges by taking turns (Bigelow & Rochat,

2006) and start to act agentically to reproduce the consequences of their actions

(Rochat & Striano, 1999). By nine months, they show signs that they have learned to

use others to regulate their behavior or affect as in the case of social referencing

(Feinman, 1982). However, they cannot yet dissociate from their subjective experience

and treat themselves as an object of thought. This difference becomes easier to observe

Page 14: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

2

when infants are presented with their reflected image in front of a mirror. Throughout the

first year and a half of their lives, infants express a growing interest in the mirror and

demonstrate an increasing appreciation of the synchronicity between their body

movements and the image in the mirror (Amsterdam, 1972; Dickie & Strader, 1974).

However, they cannot cognize that the infant in the mirror is them until they are about 18

months of age. The level of reflexive thinking that goes into recognizing the mirror image

as ‘me’ requires the infant to construe a mental representation of the self (Lewis &

Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Gallup, 1977; Rochat, 2009); in other words, an ‘idea of me’ (Lewis,

2003). This revolutionary idea is a human universal which can be considered as the first

step towards building a personhood that will integrate the individual into a complex

system of social, emotional, and moral network.

Mirror Self-recognition

For adults, the experience of objective self-awareness clearly manifests when we

look in the mirror and face the “self”. Whether infants also recognized themselves in the

mirror had long been a matter of curiosity but the conclusions had been discrepant

before the mirror self-recognition (MSR) test. Darwin thought that visual self-recognition

was present at 9 months (Darwin, 2010) and Gesell, after testing over 500 babies, had

doubted that recognition could occur in early childhood (Gesell, Thompson, & Amatruda,

1934). The challenge is to identify a reliable non-verbal indication of infants’ knowledge

of the identity of the mirror image since we cannot directly observe this construct. The

invention of the mirror self-recognition (MSR) test (Gallup, 1970; Amsterdam, 1972), also

known as the mark test, resolved this problem and became widely adopted by

developmental researchers thanks to its strong face validity (Amsterdam & Lewitt, 1980).

During the MSR test procedure, an experimenter surreptitiously places a mark

(typically a sticker or lipstick) on an infant’s forehead, cheek, or nose. The infant is then

presented with a mirror and the infant’s response to the mirror image is recorded. If the

infant shows mark-directed behavior (i.e., touches the mark on her face) upon seeing the

mirror, it is considered that they have recognized themselves and passed the test. Other

responses commonly observed among infants who fail the test include

sociable/playmate behaviors such as smiling at the mirror, mirror-directed behaviors

such as touching the mirror, moving one’s body and observing the mirror image, and

search behaviors such as checking behind the mirror (Amsterdam, 1972; Lewis &

Page 15: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

3

Brooks-Gunn, 1979). While they are all considered as a fail, it is difficult to interpret the

exact meaning of these behaviors. Sociable/playmate and mirror-directed behaviors

which are also common among animals such as birds, cats, dogs, and monkeys are

considered indication of wrongly identifying the mirror as someone else (Gallup, 1968).

Moving one’s body and observing the mirror image may be indicating an enjoyment of

the symmetry between kinesthetic and visual stimuli which would indicate that the infant

at least differentiates between these two perceptual stimuli. According to Rochat (2009),

this is an indication of a differentiated self, the very first level of subjective self-

awareness. On the other hand, a systematic exploration of the intermodal link between

the seen and felt movements may be an indication of a situated self (Rochat, 2009), a

relatively more advanced sense of subjective self where the infant is aware that the

image in the mirror is unique to the self and situated spatially in relation to the self.

Regardless, in both of these cases, infants are not yet able to identify who is in the

mirror. Finally, checking behind the mirror could either be interpreted as a sociable-

playmate behavior or a systematic exploration of the image out there which would be an

indication of a situated self. In either case, this behavior also does not imply self-

recognition.

Touching the mark on one’s face provides compelling evidence of self-

recognition because the infant is not only making a distinction between her body and the

image in the mirror but also directing her behavior purposefully towards their body

(Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979). Developmental observations show us that these

behaviors which we consider as failing the test are present among infants as early as 3

to 5 months and their display decreases as mark-directed behavior appears starting from

15- to 18-months of age (Amsterdam, 1972; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979). In addition,

mark-directed behavior is correlated with personal pronoun use (Courage, Edison, &

Howe, 2004; Lewis & Ramsay, 2004) and the display of “exposure embarrassment”, a

form of embarrassment that appears when we become aware of being seen by others

such as in the case of receiving a compliment (Lewis, Stanger, & Sullivan, 1989). These

evidence further point to the mark test’s ability to discriminate between different levels of

self-awareness.

Self-recognition is typically considered a demonstration of a self-concept in the

infant. The proponents of this rich interpretation suggest that in order to recognize the

mirror image as oneself, the infant must construe a mental representation of the self

Page 16: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

4

(Gallup, 1977; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Rochat, 2009), in other words, an idea of

me (Lewis, 2003). When a child is secretly marked on their face and shown a mirror,

they see a distorted (marked) image of themselves. In order to identify this as a distorted

image, the infant must compare the image in the mirror to a mental image of their

“typical” visual features. In other words, recognizing oneself in the mirror requires the

ability to hold both the existent and the non-existent models of their face in mind (Perner,

1991; Gergely, 1994; Parker & Mitchell, 1994). Recently, Nielsen and his colleagues

provided empirical evidence supporting the representational account of the MSR test

(Nielsen, Suddendorf, Slaughter, 2006). To do this, researchers invented a novel version

of the MSR test where toddlers are surreptitiously marked on their clothing (i.e., knees of

the pants) rather than their faces. Toddlers’ performance on the classical face-

recognition test and the knee-recognition test proved comparable. Then, in two other

studies, authors manipulated infants’ knowledge of how their clothing looked. They found

that those infants who had no correct knowledge of their clothing showed lower levels of

knee-recognition compared to those who had been exposed to their new clothing. These

results support the hypothesis that infants pass the MSR test by comparing two models

of self; one being how they look like in the mirror and one being how they think they

should look.

On the other hand, according to some researchers, the ability to recognize one’s

mirror image is a result of some lower level skills and does not imply a self-concept. For

example, Heyes (1994) argued that to pass the test, one only needs the ability to

distinguish between sensory input originating from the self and from outside the self.

Loveland suggests (1986) that passing the test requires discovering the affordances of a

reflecting surface. Finally, Mitchell’s (1993) inductive theory of visual self-recognition

suggests that passing the test requires both mature kinesthetic-visual matching and

understanding of mirror-correspondence. However, these lean interpretations cannot

explain findings such as infants being able to use mirrors’ reflective features to locate

objects before they can recognize their mirror image (Bertenthal & Fischer, 1978; Gouin

Décarie, Pouliot, & Poulin-Dubois, 1983). Moreover, infants can identify discrepancies

between kinesthetic and visual stimuli as early as 5 months of age (Bahrick & Watson,

1985) and being able to detect whether someone is imitating them does not differentiate

between toddlers who are recognizers and non-recognizers (Asendorpf, Warkentin, &

Baudonniere, 1996). Thus, based on the current evidence, it seems that lower levels

Page 17: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

5

skills such as kinesthetic-visual matching and understanding mirror correspondence are

not enough to pass the test.

Although it is the most widely adopted one, MSR is not the only test available to

measure objective self-awareness. Recently, self-recognition beyond the face (i.e., leg)

was shown to be a viable alternative to facial self-recognition (Nielsen, Suddendorf, &

Slaughter, 2006). Self-recognition in pictures and videos (Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979;

Povinelli, Landau, & Perilloux, 1996) has been examined as measures that are capturing

increasingly more complex aspects of visual self-recognition. Other indications of

objective self-awareness which do not involve visual recognition have also been

addressed. Some of these involve the use of the pronoun ‘me’ (Courage, Edison, &

Howe, 2004; Kärtner et al., 2012; LeVine, 1983, Lewis & Ramsay, 2004), expression of

self-conscious emotions such as embarrassment (Lewis, Sullivan, Stranger, & Weiss,

1989), and pretense involving the self at different levels of complexities (Pipp, Fischer, &

Jennings, 1987). Finally, researchers recently introduced different body awareness tasks

that measure infants’ ability to think about the relationship between their body and the

world of objects (e.g., Brownell, Zerwas, & Ramani, 2007; Moore, Mealiea, Garon, &

Povinelli, 2007). One of these is the body as obstacle (BAO) test where the infant is

asked to push a toy shopping cart forward. However, the cart is attached to the mat from

its back axle which prevents the child who is standing on the mat from pushing the cart

forward (Moore et al., 2007; Piaget, 1936/1977). To solve this task, the child must think

about the relationship between their body weight and the cart (i.e., ‘the chart is stuck

because of me, so I must move off the mat first’). These different tests clearly measure

independent but related domains or aspects of objective self-awareness. For example,

Moore and colleagues (2007) argue that the BAO test measures the understanding of

self as an object within the world of objects, as opposed to the MSR test that measures

the understanding of self as a person who is separate from other people in terms of

visual appearance.

In this dissertation, I am adopting the MSR for several reasons. First of all, the

test has strong face validity. Self-recognition is intuitively seen as central to one’s own

knowledge of themselves as a person. It is also the best studied test of in WEIRD

societies (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, Democratic; Henrich, Heine, &

Norenzayan, 2010). We know its age of onset, its correlates, and its relationship with

other milestones in the developmental trajectory. This offers us the capacity to judge the

Page 18: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

6

comparability of the test across cultures within a nomological framework. In addition, it is

easier to implement and to reliably code compared to some other indices of objective

self-awareness such as expression of self-conscious emotions. Moreover, particularly

because the face is not directly visible to the child like other body parts such as hands or

legs, developing a representation of one’s face should rely more heavily on social

experience rather than experience with the world of objects (Piaget, 1945/1962; Moore

et al., 2007). In this sense, I argue that the test exceptionally captures the social nature

of self-objectification that is stressed by the relational theories of objective self-

awareness (e.g., Mead, 1967; Cooley, 1964). Although I acknowledge that there are

arguments towards leaner interpretations of the MSR test (e.g., Heyes, 1994; Loveland,

1986), based on the arguments reviewed above, I adopt the traditional rich interpretation

which suggests that MSR pinpoints the emergence of a self-concept (Gallup, 1977;

Amsterdam, 1972; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979).

Cross-cultural Variation in MSR Passing Rates

Infants from Western, urban societies typically begin passing the MSR test at

approximately 18 months of age and most of them become self-recognizers by 24

months (Amsterdam, 1972; Courage, Edison & Howe, 2004; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn,

1979; Nielsen, Dissanayake, & Kashima, 2003). Unfortunately, as with other

developmental research, much of the research on this ability is based on urban, Western

sociocultural contexts (Henrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010; Kline, Shamsudheen, &

Broesch, 2018; Nielsen, Haun, Kärtner, & Legare, 2017). Recently, a handful of studies

have examined this ability in children across diverse sociocultural contexts and the

findings indicate significant cross-cultural variability (Broesch et al., 2011, Kärtner et al.,

2012; Keller et al., 2004; Ross et al., 2016). The cross-cultural differences observed in

MSR test performance are typically interpreted as a discrepancy in the age of onset and

taken as indication either that self-developmental trajectories are sensitive to variations

in infants’ early experiences (Keller et al., 2004; Keller, Kärtner, Borke, Yovsi, & Kleis,

2005). However, it was also argued that the test is not robust across cultures (Broesch

et al., 2011). Understanding the source of this variation is the main goal of this research

project.

Page 19: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

7

Methodology

Research Methods and Theoretical Framework

Cross-cultural research gives us access to a wider range of variability in early

experiences. We can use this variability to test the relationships between the variables of

interest and their theorized outcomes. In this dissertation, I used cross-cultural

comparison as a method to describe the relationships between social-interactive

experiences between toddlers and their caregivers and infants’ MSR abilities. However,

because cultures vary simultaneously along many dimensions of children’s early

experiences, it is crucial to target variables guided by theory (Campbell, 1961; Bornstein,

1980). In this dissertation, I took a constructivist, relational and developmental

perspective (Mead, 1934/1967; Piaget, 1950/1970; Vygotsky, 1934/1962). Based on this

perspective, all knowledge about self, others, and the world is constructed by the infant

through direct interaction with the world (Piaget, 1950/1970; Vygotsky, 1934/1962).

Moreover, knowledge pertaining to objective self-awareness is dependent on infants’

social experience since the objective self is an essentially social structure, an

organization derived from others’ attitudes towards oneself (Cooley, 1902/1964; Mead,

1934/1967). Finally, due to the relational nature of objective self-awareness (Cooley,

1902/1964; Mead, 1934/1967), the proximal developmental antecedents of objective

self-knowledge are found within secondary intersubjectivity rather than early infancy

(Carpendale & Lewis, 2011; Chapman, 1991; Mitchell, 1993; Moore, 2007; Rochat,

2001; Tomasello, 1995; Trevarthen, 1980). The decision to study the specific social-

interactional predictors I examined in this dissertation is a product of this theoretical

perspective and is elaborated in each of the three papers.

Cross-cultural validity of our measurements becomes an important concern when

conducting cross-cultural research. Currently, there is not enough empirical evidence to

reject the possibility that the observed differences in MSR passing rates are not, at least

partially, confounded by factors such as compliance norms (Broesch et al., 2011) and

mirror experience. To overcome these challenges, I took a multi-method approach. In

study 1, where I implemented the MSR test in Canada and Vanuatu, I used a battery of

control measures and directly tested three alternative explanations for cross-cultural

variation in MSR. In study 2, I aimed to replicate the results of study 1 by recruiting

exclusively from an urban middle-class sociocultural context in Vancouver and

Page 20: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

8

examining within-culture variability. I aimed to maximize within-culture variability in the

constructs of interest by recruiting from different self-identified cultural backgrounds (i.e.,

English-speaking monolingual families and Cantonese-speaking bilingual families).

Field Sites

I recruited my participants from two different field sites: urban Canada

(Vancouver) and rural Vanuatu (Tanna). These two societies were selected due their

distinct values sociocultural orientations that are theorized to be linked to differences in

the trajectory of self-development. The sociocultural perspective to self-development

suggests that children’s self-developmental trajectories are shaped to fit the sociocultural

pressures (Greenfield, Keller, Fuligni, & Maynard, 2003; Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Keller et al.,

2004; Keller et al., 2006). Based on this perspective, parents in cultures of

independence (typically Western and urban societies) put a higher emphasis on

supporting psychological autonomy in their children compared to parents in cultures of

interdependence (typically rural subsistence societies) and these differences are

manifested in the relatively early attainment of self-developmental milestones among

infants from cultures of independence (Keller et al., 2006). Consistently, researchers

observed higher MSR passing rates among toddlers from Western urban environments

compared to their counterparts from non-Western/rural and small-scale societies

(Broesch et al., 2011, Kärtner et al., 2012; Keller et al., 2004; Ross et al., 2016). As a

cosmopolitan city with a highly specialized, competitive job market, Vancouver shows

the typical characteristics of a culture of independence. As a small-scale society that has

a communal lifestyle, Tanna Island in Vanuatu shows the typical characteristics of a

culture of interdependence. Based on the predictions of the sociocultural perspective I

described (Greenfield et al., 2003; Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Keller et al., 2004, 2006), I

expected that infants’ social-interactive experiences and as well as their MSR abilities

would vary significantly in Vancouver and Tanna –making these two societies suitable

comparison groups for this study.

I was a resident of the city of Vancouver in Canada between 2011 and 2019. I

lived in this city for the duration of my graduate studies as an international student. My

experience in Tanna Island in Vanuatu consists of 3- to 8-week visits that span over 4

years. I spent a total of 6 months living in the village of Leneukivik in Western interior

Tanna. During my visits, I was hosted by a member of the local community, Rachel

Page 21: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

9

Iakin, who also worked as a field worker at the Cultural Center in Tanna –an

organization that governs researchers’ activities in the island. My stay in the village and

my research program was fully supported by the residents of the village as well the local

chief, Tom Noam, who has graciously accepted me into the community and bestowed a

traditional name on me. The recruitment process was lead with the permission and

support of local chiefs in West Tanna and through the personal connections of Rachel

Iakin. To reduce the effects of my presence, especially in infant participants who are not

used to interact with outsiders, I did not directly engage with participants during the study

procedure which was run by Rachel Iakin in participants’ local language.

Three Studies on Self in Infancy

In the first study, I examined cross-cultural differences in infants’ MSR abilities in

urban Canada and rural Vanuatu. As expected, I found that significantly fewer ni-

Vanuatu infants passed the MSR test between 18 and 22 months of age, compared to

urban Canadian infants. This study had two main goals. The first goal was to examine

whether parent-infant interactions observed during a semi-structured free play session

could explain differences in MSR performance. With this aim, I analysed the links

between culture, MSR, and three social-interactional variables (i.e., joint attention to

object, joint attention to self, and imitation). My findings pointed to one variable as the

predictor of self-recognition ability: maternal imitation. Another goal of this study was to

test whether cross-cultural differences in MSR test performance could be attributed to

reasons other than differences in the developmental trajectory of self-awareness. To

examine this question, I tested three alternative explanations using control measures.

The evidence I collected suggested that the differences observed in the MSR test

performance couldn’t be explained by these alternative hypotheses, therefore providing

support for the idea that the differences reflect true differences in the trajectory of self-

development.

In the second study, I aimed to replicate my previous results on imitation, this

time in a culturally diverse urban Canadian sample. Examining within-culture differences

has several advantages such as bypassing potential confounding factors related to the

cross-cultural validity of the test. This study also examined whether infant temperament

could explain some of the variation in MSR. Results revealed that only maternal imitation

predicted MSR. This study further underscores the link between imitation and MSR, and

Page 22: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

10

suggests that it could be a mechanism through which infants learn to think objectively

about their appearance. It also raises more questions with regards to the mechanisms

underlying the links between imitation and self-recognition.

In the third study, I turned my attention to language, which has long been seen to

be at the heart of self-awareness by theoreticians such as Vygotsky (1934/1962). There

is substantial empirical evidence suggesting that language interactions are very

important for the development of a self-concept in preschool children. However, we

know very little about the relations between language and thought during the pre-verbal

period. As a first step, this paper examines whether language interactions before the

second year systematically differ between ni-Vanuatu and Canadian societies in a way

that could give rise to differences in the emergence of self-concept. Specifically, I

investigated the content of mothers’ child-directed speech with their 21-month-old

toddlers in Vanuatu and Canada.

Together, these studies make a significant contribution to the MSR literature by

addressing some of the methodological challenges of cross-cultural research in MSR,

offering a theoretically informed roadmap for the examination of developmental

antecedents of objective self-awareness, and providing new empirical evidence that

advances our understanding of how developmental trajectories of self-awareness may

be shaped.

Page 23: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

11

Chapter 1. Study 1. Understanding Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror Self-Recognition

1.1. Abstract

Mirror self-recognition is considered to be the benchmark of objective self-

awareness —the ability to think about oneself. Recent cross-cultural research has

shown that there are systematic differences in infants’ MSR abilities between 18 and 24

months. Understanding whether these differences result from systematic variation in

early social experiences will help us understand the mechanisms through which

objective self-awareness develops. In this study, I examined MSR performance of 18- to

22-month-old infants and their mothers from two diverse sociocultural environments:

urban Canada and rural Vanuatu, a small-scale island society located in the South

Pacific. I had two main goals: 1) to identify the social-interactional correlates of MSR

ability in this cross-cultural sample, 2) to examine whether passing rates in Vanuatu

could be attributed to confounding factors unrelated to self-development. Consistent with

previous cross-cultural research, ni-Vanuatu infants passed the MSR test at significantly

lower rates (7%) compared to Canadian toddlers (68%). Among a suite of social

interactional variables, only mothers’ imitation of their infants that was observed during a

short free play session with toys predicted who passed the MSR test. In addition, low

passing rates among ni-Vanuatu infants could not be attributed to reasons unrelated to

self-development (i.e., motivation to show mark-directed behavior, understanding mirror-

correspondence, representational thinking). This suggests that imitation may have an

important role in shaping the construction of objective self-knowledge.

1.2. Introduction

The mirror self-recognition (MSR) test measures whether infants recognize

themselves in the mirror (Amsterdam, 1972; Gallup, 1970). This seemingly simple ability

marks an important milestone in the trajectory of self-development: the ability to

contemplate the “self”. During the MSR test procedure, an experimenter surreptitiously

Page 24: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

12

places a mark (typically a sticker or lipstick mark) on a child’s forehead, cheek, or nose.

The child is then presented with a mirror and the child’s response to the mirror image is

recorded. Children’s exhibition of self-directed behaviors such as manually investigating

their face while looking in the mirror or removing the mark from their face suggests that

they recognized the face in the mirror as their own. Therefore, it is concluded that they

have already developed a self-concept (Gallup, 1977), or as Lewis (2003) refers to it, an

idea of me (for leaner interpretations of the MSR test, see Heyes, 1994; Loveland, 1986;

Mitchell, 1993; Rochat & Zahavi, 2011). Having developed this idea of “me” marks the

initiation of objective self-awareness which refers to humans’ ability to treat the self as

an object of thought (Duval & Wicklund, 1972; James, 1890/1950; Mead, 1934/1967).

Identifying the developmental mechanisms involved in the formation of the idea of “me”

is critical for our quest to understand the ontogeny of humans’ self-awareness.

There are systematic differences across cultures in the age at which infants start

recognizing themselves in a mirror (Kärtner et al., 2012; Keller et al., 2004). These

findings are typically taken as an indication that the trajectory of self-development is

sensitive to variations in infants’ early experiences (Keller et al., 2004). The specific

behavioral mechanisms involved in shaping the developmental trajectories, however, are

not well understood. As a preliminary step towards understanding these mechanisms,

my main goal in this study was to identify the social-interactional correlates of infants’

performance on the mirror self-recognition (MSR) test (Amsterdam, 1972; Asendorpf et

al., 1996; Gallup, 1970) in a cross-cultural sample of infants recruited from urban

Canada and rural Vanuatu. These regions were selected, in part, due to their distinct

sociocultural orientations which are thought to be related to differences in children’s self-

developmental trajectories (Keller et al., 2004). Despite the fact that cross-cultural

research is an excellent tool to study the variation in developmental processes, it also

brings serious methodological challenges. Currently, we do not have enough empirical

evidence to reject the possibility that the observed differences in MSR passing rates are

not, at least partially, confounded by factors such as compliance norms, mirror

experience, or systematic differences in symbolic development. Thus, another goal of

this study was examine whether cross-cultural differences in MSR rates could be

explained by factors unrelated to self-development.

Page 25: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

13

1.2.1. MSR Varies across Sociocultural Contexts

Among infants recruited from Western, urban, middle-class societies, which are

typically classified as cultures of independence (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996), the onset of self-

recognition occurs between 18 and 24 months of age (Amsterdam, 1972; Courage et al.,

2004; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Lewis & Ramsay, 2004; Nielsen et al., 2003). In

Amsterdam’s pioneering study (1972) that was conducted with infants from urban

middle-class American families, none of the infants in the 15- to 17-month-old group

recognized themselves. However, the passing rates increased to 42% in the 18- to 20-

month-old group and to 65% in the 21- to 24-month-old group. In a recent longitudinal

study conducted with infants from middle-class Australian families, Nielsen and his

colleagues (Nielsen et al., 2003) reported that at 18 months, 50% of the infants passed

the MSR test and 88% of the sample became self-recognizers by 24 months. Similarly,

in another longitudinal study conducted with infants urban, middle-class American

families, Lewis and Ramsay (2004) reported that 47% of the infants passed the MSR

test by 18 months and all infants became recognizers by 24 months. These data clearly

indicate that at some point between 18 and 24 months, there is a developmental shift

that enables infants to refer to the reflected image as oneself.

However, participants recruited from these societies are increasingly viewed as

outliers in psychological functioning (Henrich et al. 2004; Kline et al., 2018; Nielsen et

al., 2017). Similarly, it is possible that the emergence of MSR as early as 18 months is

limited to WEIRD (Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic; Henrich et

al., 2010) societies. Cross-cultural research on MSR suggest that infants from the

Majority World1 may be following a different trajectory (Broesch et al., 2011; Kärtner et

al., 2012; Keller et al., 2004; Ross et al., 2016). The first observation of cross-cultural

differences in MSR was made by Keller and colleagues (Keller et al., 2004). Authors

tested 18- to 20-month-old infants from three distinct sociocultural contexts:

Cameroonian farmer families who live a subsistence lifestyle, middle-class Costa Rican

families, and urban Greek families. In this study, only 3% of Cameroonian infants passed

the MSR test in comparison to 68% of Greek infants. Infants from middle-class Costa

Rican families fell in the middle of these two groups with a passing rate of 50%. In

1 A term introduced by Çiğdem Kağıtçıbaşı (1996). It refers to the so‐called developing countries,

or the non‐Western world, where the majority of the world’s population resides.

Page 26: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

14

another study, authors examined 18- to 20-month-old infants from Germany and rural

Cameroon (Keller et al., 2005). Echoing their previous findings, passing rates of infants

from rural Cameroonian farmer families were only 15% compared to 73% of the infants

from urban German families. The authors interpreted the differences in passing rates as

a discrepancy in the trajectory of self-development across cultures.

Other studies replicated this pattern. Kärtner and colleagues (2012) conducted a

comprehensive study where they examined MSR in infants from urban Germany, urban

India, rural India, and rural Cameroon by testing a total of 276 infants from six age

cohorts (16-, 17-, 18-, 19-, 20-, and 21-month cohorts) for 6 consecutive weeks. They

found that even though passing rates increased with age in all four societies, infants

from urban settings (both Germany and India) performed significantly better regardless

of age. For example, when the 18-month cohorts were first assessed, passing rates

were 9% for the rural Cameroonian sample and 11% for the rural Indian sample,

compared to 42% for the German sample and 50% for the urban Indian sample. Most

recently, Ross and colleagues examined MSR in 15 to 18-month-old infants living in

rural Zambia, urban Scotland and urban Turkey. Consistent with previous research,

infants from Scotland and Turkey performed similarly on the MSR test (passing rates

were 47% and 41%, respectively), whereas same-aged Zambian infants passed at a

lower rate (15%). Taken together, this body of evidence suggests that there are

systematic differences in the onset of MSR across cultures (see Priel & Schonen, 1986

for a different result).

1.2.2. Framing the Differences in Developmental Trajectories

Keller and colleagues provide a theoretical framework linking self, society, and

the elements of the socialization process (Keller et al., 2004; Keller et al., 2005; Keller et

al. 2006). Following the steps of the pioneers of cross-cultural psychology (e.g., Whiting,

1963; LeVine, 1977), Keller and colleagues suggest that children’s self-developmental

trajectories are shaped to fit the sociocultural pressures and are mediated through

parenting. Based on this, different environmental challenges (e.g. subsistence activities)

create distinct understandings of psychological competence (Greenfield, Keller, Fuligni,

& Maynard, 2003). In contexts where cooperation is crucial for the group’s survival, such

as the rural subsistence societies (typically categorized as cultures of interdependence),

sustaining harmonious relationships is important for well-being. On the other hand, in

Page 27: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

15

urban/industrialized and typically Western societies (typically categorized as cultures of

independence) where individual achievement and competition are important for success,

individuation and self-maximization underlie an individual’s well-being. Therefore,

individuals need to hold different self-construals in order to adapt to these distinct

sociocultural environments (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). These ideal self-construals

manifest in parents’ socialization goals, beliefs about good parenting behaviors, and

parenting/caregiving practices (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Keller et al., 2006) which lead to the

socialization of a selfhood that is functional in the society. Within this framework, Keller

and colleagues suggest that early onset of self-recognition is a consequence of

autonomy-supporting socialization practices which are adopted by the parents in

cultures of independence (Keller et al., 2004; Keller et al., 2005). Consistent with Keller’s

ideas, Kärtner and colleagues’ study conducted with participants from two urban

(Germany and India), and two rural (India and Cameroon) societies showed that parents’

socialization goals that favour psychological autonomy in their children predicted

whether infants passed the MSR test while controlling for several confounding factors

including infants’ age and gender (Kärtner et al., 2012).

It is important to note that the framework reviewed above offers a functional

explanation and does not inform us of the proximal mechanisms that shape infants’ self-

developmental trajectories. Keller offers early caregiving practices (i.e., distal and

proximal parenting) as a bridge to this gap. The distal parenting style, which is typically

adopted by parents in cultures of independence, is distinguished by high levels of object

stimulation and face-to-face exchange. The proximal parenting style, which is typically

adopted by parents in cultures of interdependence, is distinguished by high levels of

body contact and body stimulation (Keller et al., 2009). Keller suggests that by adopting

a distal proximal style, WEIRD parents support the development of psychological

autonomy in their infants and prepare them for early onset of objective self-awareness

(Keller et al., 2004). In support of this view, in 2 cross-cultural studies, Keller and

colleagues found that mothers’ use of distal strategies as they interacted with their 3

month-old infants longitudinally predicted infants’ MSR abilities at 18-20 months (Keller

et al., 2004; Keller et al., 2005). These are pioneering studies as they demonstrate a link

between infants’ self-developmental outcomes and their early social experiences.

However, it is not clear precisely how infants’ experiences in dyadic exchanges as early

as 3 months is causally related to the emergence of objective self-awareness. The next

Page 28: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

16

step in self-development research should be to better understand the mechanisms of

development. In the section that follows, I review the developmental theories of self-

awareness and discuss why the experiences involved in the formation of objective self-

knowledge are to be found within the kinds of interactions that characterize the second

year of life, rather than early infancy.

1.2.3. Understanding the Developmental Mechanisms

If we want to understand the development of human self-awareness, it would be

useful to differentiate between developmentally more and less complex forms of self-

understanding and seek to identify the changing relationships between infants’

experience in the world and their self-knowledge. There is a rich body of literature

focused on conceptualizing different forms of self-knowledge (e.g., Amsterdam & Levitt,

1980; Duval & Wicklund, 1972; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Neisser, 1988; Rochat,

2003; Rochat, 2015). One distinction that appears among these discussions has

relevance from a developmental perspective: the distinction between subjective and

objective levels of self-awareness (Duval & Wicklund, 1972) or its conceptual

equivalents. Subjective self-awareness, which appears earlier in development, refers to

experiencing oneself as the source of perception and action (Duval & Wicklund, 1972).

Here, self-other distinction is functionally present and can be observed in perception and

action but not yet in thought. The objective level of self-awareness, which appears later

in development, refers to the ability to experience the self as an object of thought (Duval

& Wicklund, 1972; Gallup, 1977; Lewis, 2003; Rochat, 2009, Mead, 1934/1967). The

literature suggests that although there is developmental continuity between the

subjective and objective forms of self-awareness, they develop through different

mechanisms (Amsterdam & Levitt, 1980; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Neisser, 1991;

Rochat, 1997, Rochat, 2015). Infants’ earliest attempts to control their body and their

environment, their mastery of circular reactions, as well as their experience of

contingency during face-to-face exchanges may be serving as mechanisms that support

infants’ sense of subjective self-awareness (Fonagy, Gergely, & Target, 2007; Lewis &

Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Rochat, 1997; Rochat, 2015). The emergence of the objective self,

however, is reliant upon infants’ introduction to more complex exchanges, such as triadic

interactions.

Page 29: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

17

When I argue that the proximal developmental antecedents of objective self-

awareness are of triadic nature, I rely on theories that postulate the objective self as a

“looking glass self”, where individuals take a third person perspective towards

themselves (Cooley, 1902/1964; Mead, 1934/1967). Modern psychologists who take this

approach identify a special kind of interactive experience as the antecedent of objective

self-awareness: interactions that involve coordinating perspectives (e.g., Carpendale &

Lewis, 2011; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Martin, Sokol, Elfers, 2008; Mitchell, 1993;

Moore, 2007; Rochat, 2009; Tomasello, 1995). The ability to engage in such interactions

is dependent upon infants’ capacity to form joint attention which appears around 9

months and develops throughout the second year (Trevarthen, 1980; Zahavi & Rochat,

2015). Specifically, two types of interactions are emphasized: joint attention and

imitation. Consider the following triadic exchange. A toddler and a mother are playing a

game of throwing balls to each other. In this game, the toddler functions as both the

actor and the recipient and therefore needs to consider both perspectives in order to

sustain reciprocity. When it is the mother’s turn to throw the ball, the toddler expects the

mother to throw the ball to him or to her. Here, the form of self to be experienced is one

of self as object. Deservingly, triadic interactions have been referred to as the “epistemic

triangle” (Chapman, 1991) and the “fateful triangle of self” (Rochat, 2001). Tomasello

(1999) considers parental imitation a very special case of triadic interactions. Through

imitation, parents reorient their infants’ attention towards themselves, rather than a toy,

so that infants become the object of the dyad’s joint attention. Examples of imitation

during the second year may involve instances of social mirroring games where infants

and partners engage in reciprocally imitative coordinated actions (Mitchell, 1993), and

instances of synchronous imitation where partners simultaneously play in similar ways,

check up on the partner, coordinate their actions with that of the partner, and alternate

roles of imitator and model (Asendorpf et al., 1996). In all these imitation games, the

element of coordinating perspectives appears as infants deliberately match their partner

and watch how they look to others (Müller & Runions, 2003; Barresi & Moore, 1996).

Finally, Piaget (1945/1962, p.85) pointed out that, particularly because their face is not

directly visible to humans, imitating others’ facial movements is essential for infants to

construct a visual representation of their own face. To summarize, episodes of joint

attention and imitation may be functioning as the proximal developmental antecedents of

objective self-awareness.

Page 30: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

18

Empirical data to support these hypothesized mechanisms, however, is scarce.

No previous study has examined joint attention in relation to MSR. Yet, we have some

evidence that links imitation to self-developmental milestones in WEIRD samples. Hart

and Fegley (1994) observed that toddlers’ imitation of their mother during a free play

session with toys was associated with earlier MSR onset longitudinally, while their ability

to imitate complex actions with toys was not. Kristen-Antonow and colleagues (Kristen-

Antonow, Sodian, Perst, & Licata, 2015) found that responsiveness to being imitated by

an adult at 12 months was the strongest predictor of infants’ performance on a delayed

visual self-recognition task when they are 4 years old, together with MSR and verbal

intelligence. In addition, Asendorpf and colleagues (1996) observed a significant

relationship between infants’ MSR and their ability to engage in synchronous imitation,

concurrently. Based on the literature I reviewed above, I propose that cross-cultural

differences observed in MSR may be a result of systematic variation in infants’

participation in episodes of imitation and joint attention over the second year of their

lives. In this study, I only examine the concurrent relations between imitation, joint

attention, and MSR as a first step to shedding light on the developmental processes.

1.2.4. Alternative Explanations

Currently, there is not enough empirical evidence to reject the possibility that the

cross-cultural differences in MSR passing rates are not, at least partially, confounded by

factors unrelated to self-development. In this section, I outline three alternative

explanations and suggest that they must be carefully examined before we presume that

cross-cultural differences in MSR passing rates reflect systematic differences in infants’

self-developmental trajectories.

The first alternative explanation involves the procedure’s failure to elicit mark-

directed behavior in infants from the Majority World. In the classic MSR procedure

(Amsterdam, 1972), and in its cross-cultural implementations (e.g., Broesch et al., 2011;

Keller et al., 2004), infants are not prompted to remove the mark or trained in removing

the mark, even though mark-directed behavior is the sole criterion for passing the test.

This seems to work for infants from WEIRD societies, given the average response times

as short as 4.36 seconds (Courage et al., 2004). However, an internal motivation to

remove a mark cannot be assumed to be universal across human populations. For

example, Broesch and colleagues (2011) report that children up to 6-years-old (in rural

Page 31: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

19

Kenya) and children up to 4.5-years-old (in rural Fiji) did not self-refer during an MSR

procedure which lasted 30 seconds. Because these were typically-developing children, it

is implausible to conclude they had not developed objective self-awareness by 6 years

of age. Broesch and colleagues suggest that Kenyan and Fijian infants must have

demonstrated a form of compliance by not touching the mark placed on them by an adult

in an unfamiliar context. In support of this claim, they demonstrated that infants’

motivation to remove the mark from their face can be reduced among urban middle-

class American children by manipulating social norms (Rochat, Broesch, & Jayne 2012).

Consistent with this, Kärtner and colleagues (2012) found that, within the rural

Cameroonian sample, infants’ tendency to remove a visible mark from their own body

was related to their MSR performance. Overall, there are grounds to suspect that cross-

cultural differences in the MSR test performance reflect differences in infants’ motivation

to remove the mark.

The second alternative explanation involves potential differences in infants’

understanding of mirror correspondence. Understanding of mirror correspondence is the

ability to use the match between the mirror image and the real world, and is measured

though mirror-mediated behavior (MMB) tests where participants are expected to locate

a desired object outside of their visual field (e.g., toy or food) through the use of a mirror.

Infants from WEIRD samples pass the MMB test before they pass the MSR test

(Bertenthal & Fischer, 1978; Gouin Décarie, Pouliot, & Poulin-Dubois, 1983), and non-

human species who pass the MSR test also pass MMB test but not vice versa

(Anderson, 1986; Medina, Taylor, Hunt, & Gray, 2011; Povinelli, 1989). Based on this

evidence, Gergely (1994) and Mitchell (1993) argued that understanding mirror

correspondence is a necessary condition for self-recognition in a mirror. If that is the

case, infants who have less experience with mirrors may be disadvantaged in the MSR

test. Current evidence is inconclusive. Studies with WEIRD samples show that variability

in mirror experience is not correlated with MSR performance (Courage et al., 2004;

Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Vyt, 2001). However, it is possible that all infants from

WEIRD societies have been exposed to reflective surfaces in minimally required levels.

In contrast to infants growing up in WEIRD societies, infants (and adults) from small-

scale societies may have no exposure to reflective surfaces. Yet, we have very little

evidence from such environments. One cross-cultural study showed that infants without

prior mirror exposure indeed show lower passing rates in the MMB test (Priel & de

Page 32: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

20

Schonen, 1986). However, in the same study, MMB performance was not related to

MSR performance. In another study, Kärtner and colleagues (2012) found that mirror

experience did not affect MSR performance in their culturally diverse sample based on

the fact that infants from rural samples did not show a particularly steep learning curve

over six consecutive MSR assessments. Overall, more evidence is needed to reject the

possibility that a lack of previous exposure to mirrors might be contributing to

systematically lower passing rates in rural and small-scale societies.

Finally, a third alternative explanation suggests that cross-cultural differences in

MSR can be best explained by global differences in symbolic development, rather than

differences specific to self-development. According to Perner (1991), around 18 months

of age, infants start to be able process representations at the secondary level, which

means they can hold representations of multiple models at a time. Following Perner,

several MSR researchers considered secondary representations as a prerequisite for

the self-recognition (Gergely, 1994; Parker & Mitchell, 1994). When a child is secretly

marked on their face and shown a mirror, they see a distorted (marked) image of

themselves. In order to identify this as a distorted image, the infant must compare the

image in the mirror to a mental image of their “typical” visual features. In other words,

recognizing oneself in the mirror requires the ability to hold both the existent and the

non-existent models of their face in mind (Perner, 1991). Recently, Nielsen and his

colleagues provided empirical evidence supporting the representational account of the

MSR test (Nielsen, Suddendorf, Slaughter, 2006). To do this, researchers invented a

novel version of the MSR test where toddlers are surreptitiously marked on their clothing

(i.e., knees of the pants) rather than their faces. Toddlers’ performance on the classical

face-recognition test and the knee-recognition test proved comparable. Then, in two

other studies, authors manipulated infants’ knowledge of how their clothing looked. They

found that those infants who had no correct knowledge of their clothing showed lower

levels of knee-recognition compared to those who had been exposed to their new

clothing. These results support the hypothesis that infants pass the MSR test by

comparing two models of self; one being how they look like in the mirror and one being

how they think they should look. Based on this evidence, one possible explanation for

cross-cultural differences in MSR rates may be differences in infants’ ability to use

secondary representations.

Page 33: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

21

1.2.5. Summary and Hypotheses

In this study, I examined MSR abilities in 18- to 22-month-old infants recruited

from rural Vanuatu, a society of cultural interdependence, and urban Canada, a society

of cultural independence. Based on Keller’s cultural model of self-development and

based on previous cross-cultural literature, I expected significant differences in the age

of onset between the two societies. Specifically, I expected fewer ni-Vanuatu infants to

pass the MSR test compared to Canadian infants.

My first goal in this study was to identify social-interactional correlates of infants’

performance on the mirror self-recognition (MSR) test. Importantly, associations I

examined were motivated by developmental theories of self-awareness. I observed

social interactions during a 10-minute free play session between mothers and infants (5

minutes with toys and 5 minutes without toys). I operationalized two types of joint

attention: “joint attention to object” where mothers and infants engaged in triadic

interactions with toys and “joint attention to self” where infants themselves (rather than

an object) appeared as the object of the dyad’s joint attention (e.g., where is your

nose?). I operationalized 3 different types of imitation including child’s imitation of the

mother, mother’s imitation of the child and simultaneous imitation. Although I had the

general expectation that joint attention and imitation would be positively associated with

MSR, I did not set specific hypotheses with regards to each type of joint attention and

imitation because there was not enough theoretical or empirical basis to form such

detailed hypotheses. For example, I did not know whether child’s imitation of the mother

or mother’s imitation of the child would be a better predictor of MSR. In order to identify

the candidate predictors of MSR, I examined the associations between MSR, social-

interactional variables, and research site. Based on the postulation that higher MSR

passing rates among infants from WEIRD societies result from social experiences that

scaffold early understanding of self (Keller et al., 2004; Keller et al., 2005), I expected

that the candidate predictors of MSR would be a) positively correlated with MSR within

the entire sample and, b) observed at higher levels among the Canadian dyads.

Informed by the patterns that appeared in the data, I tested a regression model to predict

individual differences in MSR.

Another goal of this study was to test whether cross-cultural differences in MSR

rates could be explained by factors unrelated to self-development. I identified three

Page 34: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

22

alternative explanations. First, low MSR rates could be the result of low motivation to

remove the mark during the test. In order to test this possibility, I measured infants’

motivation to show mark-directed behavior through a knee-marking (KM) test (Nielsen &

Dissanayke, 2004; Kärtner et al., 2012) where infants were prompted to remove a mark

from their knees, a part of their body that they could see without the need of a mirror. I

hypothesized that if ni-Vanuatu infants in my sample failed the MSR test because they

were not motivated to remove the mark, then their KM passing rates would also be low,

and that their MSR performance would be associated with their KM performance.

Second, low MSR rates could be the result of ni-Vanuatu infants’ inability to use mirrors’

reflective functions. In order to test this possibility, I measured infants’ understanding of

mirror correspondence using a mirror-mediated behavior (MMB) test (Bertenthal &

Fischer, 1978; Courage et al., 2004) where infants were expected to locate a toy

presented from behind using the reflective features of the mirror. I hypothesized that if

ni-Vanuatu infants in my sample failed the MSR test because they were not able to use

mirror’s reflective features, their MMB passing rates would also be low and that ni-

Vanuatu infants’ MSR performance would be associated with their MMB performance.

Third, low MSR rates could be the result of differences in ni-Vanuatu infants’ trajectory of

symbolic development, specifically a relative delay in using secondary representations. I

measured infants’ ability to use secondary representations using an invisible

displacement (ID) test. The ID test (Piaget, 1937/1954) is an advanced measure of

object permanence which requires the use of secondary representations. It involves

finding an object following a series of invisible displacements and is typically correlated

with MSR within WEIRD samples. I hypothesized that if this hypothesis was true, ID

passing rates would also be low among ni-Vanuatu infants and that ni-Vanuatu infants’

MSR performance would be associated with their ID performance. This study improves

on previous cross-cultural research by testing all three of these alternative explanations

using control measures and adds to the cross-cultural validity of the MSR test.

1.3. Methods

1.3.1. Sociocultural Contexts

Tanna Island in Vanuatu is a small-scale island society that has a subsistence-

based economy and a communal lifestyle and has features thought to be prototypical of

Page 35: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

23

a culture of interdependence (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Keller et al., 2004). Vancouver in

Canada, a cosmopolitan society that has a free market economy and a highly

specialized, competitive job market, was selected as a prototypical culture of

independence (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Keller et al., 2004). Based on the framework I

described above, I expected low passing rates in ni-Vanuatu infants and high passing

rates in Canadian infants.

Tanna.

Tanna Island has a population of approximately 29,000 based on the 2009

census (Vanuatu National Statistics Office, 2009). The island consists of four

geographical regions. Each region is distinct in some ways but also similar in general

lifestyle and livelihood. Our research was conducted in Western Tanna (geographically

central on the island) in five communities consisting of both Kastom and Traditional

neighbouring communities. The communities are each led by a chief who governs

several hamlets consisting of several households. Each community contains a nakamal

which is the community meeting place for spiritual worship, political, social, and cultural

gatherings. Families in this central region do not have access to public amenities (no

provided water or electricity) and travel to the mainland is limited by the rough terrain. It

is about a half of a day’s walk to get to the nearest town (Lenekal town). At the time of

this research, there were many changes happening on the island – particularly after the

devastating cyclone Pam in 2015, with much aid and new foreign interests beginning to

invest in infrastructure. At the time of this study, however, most families in this region

would have had little exposure to Western parenting ideals. The culture of Tanna and

Vanuatu is one of cooperation and kinship. Based on these demographic characteristics,

Tanna island shows the typical characteristics of a society with interdependent or

collectivistic sociocultural orientation (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Triandis, 2001). This social

ecology presents similarities to other rural populations where researchers reported lower

levels of MSR compared to Western urban samples (Broesch et al., 2011; Keller et al.,

2004).

Vancouver.

Metro Vancouver is Canada’s third-largest metropolitan area with a population of

approximately 2.5 million. Its population is highly affluent with a median family income of

72,662 CAD (Statistics Canada, 2016) and highly educated with 91.8% of residents

Page 36: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

24

having high school level education or higher. The family structure is nuclear with an

average of only 2.5 persons per household (Statistics Canada, 2016). It is also an

ethnically and linguistically diverse city as 46.9% of its residents’ first language is an

immigrant language (Statistics Canada, 2016). Vancouver attracts highly skilled

international workers and the employment market is highly competitive. Based on these

demographics, Vancouver shows the typical characteristics of a society with an

independent or individualistic sociocultural orientation (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Triandis,

2001). This social ecology presents similarities to other Western urban populations

where researchers reported higher levels of MSR compared to non-Western rural

samples (Keller et al., 2004; Broesch et al., 2011).

1.3.2. Participants

I recruited 57 mother-toddler dyads —31 in Tanna, Vanuatu, and 26 in

Vancouver Canada. I recruited Canadian dyads from the Metro Vancouver Regional

District in BC, between 2013 and 2016, a) through a database of parents who previously

indicated they were interested in participating in studies at the Culture and Development

Lab at Simon Fraser University (SFU), b) a daycare centre located on SFU campus, or

c) posting flyers at community centres and public libraries.

All parents were mothers except one ni-Vanuatu father who participated with his

infant. I recruited ni-Vanuatu dyads during a one-month research visit to Tanna Island in

2015. After obtaining permission from the Vanuatu Cultural Centre and the local leaders,

I recruited participants following a list of infant birth records from the local hospital and

by word of mouth. I excluded six additional mother-toddler dyads from the database due

to fussiness (n = 4), experimental error (n = 1), and unknown age (n = 1).

The toddlers were, on average, 19.50 months old (range = 17.58 – 22.24, SD =

1.21). The mean age was 19.69 months (SD = 1.31) for Canadian toddlers, 19.34

months (SD = 1.13) for ni-Vanuatu toddlers. There were no significant differences

between the ages, t(1, 55) = 1.07, p = .289. There were 31 girls (n = 13 for Canada, n =

18 for Vanuatu), and 26 boys (n = 13 for Canada, n = 13 for Vanuatu). Gender

distribution was comparable across cultural groups, χ² (1, 57) = .37, p = .543. Mothers

were, on average, 34.64 years old (SD = 4.21). There were no differences between the

two cultural groups in mothers’ self-reported age, t(1, 50) = 1.80, p = .078. In Canada,

Page 37: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

25

mothers were highly educated, having spent an average of 18.84 years (SD = 3.36) in

formal education. In Tanna, however, only 69% of the mothers had received formal

education. Among those, the average years of schooling was 6.85 (SD = 1.98).

In Canada, 14 dyads were from English-speaking monolingual families, nine

dyads were from English-speaking bilingual families, and three dyads were from

monolingual families speaking a language other than English (one French-speaking, one

Portuguese-speaking, and one Chinese speaking). The three babies whose primary

languages aren’t English were all used to hearing English outside of their immediate

home environments, such as the daycare setting, and felt comfortable interacting with

the experimenter in English. All three families were currently residing in Canada. The

linguistic variability in the Vancouver sample reflects Vancouver’s cosmopolitan social

structure as described in the sociocultural contexts section above. In Vanuatu, twenty-

seven dyads were from monolingual families who spoke the local dialects of West

Tanna. Three infants were from multilingual families due to mothers from a different

island in Vanuatu being married to partners from Tanna. These infants were

predominantly spoken to in Bislama (the official language of Vanuatu).

1.3.3. Procedure

The experimenter and the dyad met at the testing location where the

experimenter informed the mother about the details of the study and took verbal

consent. The procedure started with a short warm-up session (approximately 5 minutes

or until the child appeared comfortable) during which the experimenter, the infants and

the mothers played with simple wooden blocks and a 12" baby-doll (a black doll in

Vanuatu, a white doll in Canada) and engaged in friendly dialogue. This session also

served as a familiarization for the stimuli that would be used later for testing. After the

warm-up session, the Invisible Displacement (ID) task was administered (see section

1.3.4 for full description of measures). After the ID task, the experimenter performed the

practice session for the MSR task where infants learned to remove a sticker from the

baby doll’s face and their own hands. This was followed by the familiarization phase for

the mirror. The experimenter brought a 40 x 50 cm sturdy wooden framed mirror with a

support stand and allowed the child to explore the mirror for approximately 2 minutes or

until the child lost interest in the mirror. After that, the MMB task, the MSR task, and the

knee marking task were administered in the listed order. During this time, the mother

Page 38: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

26

was allowed to stay next to the infant but was asked to stay silent and not to intervene.

Once these tasks were over, the experimental props were removed and the dyad was

left alone for a 10-minute free play session. The free play session began with five

minutes of free play without any toys. Following that, the experimenter entered the room,

delivered the toys (blocks and the baby doll) and left the dyad alone for another five

minutes. The entire procedure took 30 to 40 minutes and was video recorded by two

cameras (front view and left back view) for later coding. After the free play session, the

cameras were stopped and mothers were asked a series of demographic questions

involving mothers’ age, education, and infants’ health history and experience with

mirrors. Figure A.1. and Figure A.2. in Appendix A., Section A.1. illustrate the typical

setting for testing in Vanuatu and in Canada.

In Vancouver, I implemented the procedure in English. Data collection took place

either in the lab located at the SFU campus (n = 8), in participants’ homes (n = 10) or in

a daycare (n = 5). In Tanna, a local female research assistant implemented the

procedure in the local language. The local research assistant was a fluent speaker of

English, and I trained her to conduct the testing independently. The testing was

performed in the toddlers’ mother tongue which could be either one of the local dialects

spoken in West Tanna (n = 28) or Bislama (n = 3). Data collection took place either in

participants’ homes, in an available communal building such as a tent used by the village

as a classroom, or a suitable outdoor space in the village.

The translation of the study procedure was completed in two steps. First, the

procedure was translated from English to Bislama and back translated from Bislama to

English, by two separate local research assistants who were fluent in both languages.

Any inconsistencies were resolved through discussion. Once the procedure in Bislama

reflected the intended study procedure, the procedure was translated from Bislama to

Natuar (one of the local dialects spoken in West Tanna and the first language of the

experimenter) and back by two separate local research assistants. Again,

inconsistencies were resolved through discussion. The translations between Bislama

and Natuar were conducted orally since Natuar is an oral language and the process was

videotaped for documentation purposes.

Page 39: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

27

1.3.4. Measures

Invisible displacement (ID) test

The ID test is an advanced measure of object permanence and involves finding

an object following a series of invisible displacements (Piaget, 1937/1954). The ID test

used in this study was the 14th task in Uzgiris and Hunt’s (1975) battery developed to

measure the development of the visual pursuit of slowly moving objects. The materials

used for this task were three fabric covers (25 cm x 25 cm) and a small orange-color ball

that easily fit into the experimenter’s palm (see Appendix A, Section A.1., Figure A.2.).

During the test, the experimenter and the infant sit facing each other and the mother was

asked to place herself behind the infant (the infant could sit on mother’s lap) while

remaining silent. The experimenter placed the three covers in front of the infant, hid the

ball inside her palm, moved her fist sequentially under three covers, withdrew her hand

empty and said: ‘Where did the ball go? Can you find the ball?’ The experimenter waited

15 seconds for infants’ response, and if the child had not located the ball, repeated the

prompt up to two more times. The trial ended when the infant located the ball or when 45

seconds passed. The sequence was repeated three times.

Mirror mediated behaviour (MMB) test

Infants’ understanding of mirror correspondence was measured with an MMB

test which was adapted from previous procedures used by Bertenthal and Fisher (1978)

and Priel and Schonen (1986). The test aimed to measure infants’ capacity to locate

objects seen through the mirror. The materials used in this test were the mirror

previously used in the mirror familiarization phase and a finger puppet in the form of a

bee (see Appendix A, Section A.1, Figure A.4. for a picture of the finger puppet). Infants

sat in front of the mirror and the experimenter placed herself behind the infant. Mothers

were asked to sit slightly away (approximately 1 m.) on the left side of the infant and the

experimenter. The experimenter directed the infants’ attention to the mirror by asking the

child to look at the mirror and by pointing to the mirror. While the infant’s attention was

on the mirror, the experimenter showed the bee from behind the infant, above their right

shoulder. If the infant did not turn his back to locate the toy within 15 seconds, the

experimenter pointed to the mirror and asked the infant to get the puppet by saying:

‘Look, there is a bee. Can you get the bee?’. The experimenter repeated the prompt up

Page 40: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

28

to three times waiting 15 seconds after each prompt. The task ended either when infants

located the bee or by the end of a total of 60 seconds.

Mirror self-recognition (MSR) test

I assessed children’s MSR abilities with a procedure adapted from the original

MSR test (Amsterdam, 1972). In Amsterdam’s original procedure, toddlers are secretly

marked on their face, shown the mirror, and expected to remove the mark without

receiving any verbal prompts. However, over the 40 years of research using this

procedure, several modifications have been suggested in order to reduce the potential

for false negatives and ambiguous cases. These suggestions included training infants to

show mark-directed behavior, using verbal prompts for encouraging self-directed

behavior, and using control tests to measure infants’ proneness to show mark-directed

behavior in a clearly visible body part (Asendorpf et al., 1996; Broesch et al., 2011;

Kärtner et al., 2012). Based on these recommendations, I introduced a training phase

where infants learned to remove a sticker from a baby doll and from their hands and I

gave an open prompt to remove the sticker during the testing phase. A control measure

was also implemented and is described in the below section on the knee-marking test.

The materials used for this task were a baby doll (see Figure A.3. in Appendix A,

Section A.1. for a picture) and the standing mirror (see Figure A.1. in Appendix A,

Section A.1., for a picture). I used yellow post-it stickers to mark the infant. Stickers have

been previously used in cross-cultural research to increase the saliency of the mark

(e.g., Broesch et al., 2011). As an exception, five infants who were tested in Vancouver

were marked using a rouge lipstick rather than a sticker. During the training phase (see

details below), these infants learned to clean the mark using a wet wipe instead of

removing the sticker from the doll’s face.

During the training phase, which occurred after the ID task, infants learned to

remove a post-it sticker or rouge lipstick off a doll’s face and from their own hands

through six consecutive trials. The experimenter showed the infant a baby doll that was

marked with a sticker on its forehead and said: “Oh! Look, there is something on baby’s

face. We must take it off. Can you take it off?”. If the infant responded by removing the

sticker within 15 seconds, the experimenter thanked the child and said: “Well done, we

cleaned the baby’s face. It’s all clean now, thank you!”. If the infant did not respond, the

experimenter modeled the behavior herself. This was repeated three times. Next, the

Page 41: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

29

experimenter repeated the same procedure by marking the child on their hand three

times.

The testing phase occurred following the MMB task. To mark the child, the

experimenter covered the mirror and distracted the infant by making them giggle with the

bee puppet while she surreptitiously put a sticker on their forehead, above the hairline.

After the child was marked, the experimenter spent at least 15 seconds playing with the

child and the bee to make sure that the child hadn’t felt the marking, which would be

indicated by infants’ mark-directed behavior. The experimenter continued the MSR test

only if the child did not show any mark-directed behavior during this time.

For the testing phase, the experimenter uncovered the mirror and asked the

infant to look in the mirror without giving any further prompts. If the infant did not show

self-directed behavior within 15 seconds, the experimenter pointed to the mirror and

gave the first prompt: “Oh, look, there is something on the face”. This prompt was

repeated once more if the child didn’t show any self-directed behaviors. The next verbal

prompt openly asked the infant to remove the mark: “Look, there is something on the

face. We must take it off. Please take it off”. This last prompt was repeated twice more if

the child didn’t show any self-directed behaviors. By the end of a total of 90 seconds and

five verbal prompts, whether the infant responded or not, the task was deemed over.

Knee-marking test

The knee-marking task was implemented as a control task (only in Vanuatu) for

measuring infants’ motivation to remove a sticker from their own body. After the MSR

test was over, the experimenter visibly put a sticker on infants’ knee and said: “Oh, look,

there is something on the knee”. The experimenter gave the same prompt up to three

times waiting 15 seconds in between the prompts. However, most infants pass the test

by immediately removing the sticker from their knee.

Free Play sessions

Next, the experimenter removed the props and left the room for an initial 5

minutes of free play between the caregiver and her child, without any toys. After 5

minutes, the experimenter returned with the following toys: simple wooden blocks and a

baby doll. The experimenter left the room and the dyad was again left alone for a 5-

minute free play session with toys. After 5 minutes, the experimenter returned and

Page 42: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

30

ended the free play session. A trained research assistant watched all the videos,

synchronized the two video angles by merging them into one video and excluded

sections of the video when: a) the infant was out of the camera view, b) no interaction

was possible as a result of the child crying, or c) another external intrusion was

preventing the interaction. Finally, for the no-toy session, we removed the sections

where the mother used an object as a toy (e.g., mother takes out her wallet and offers it

to the child to play with).

I coded three separate social-interactional variables during the free play session:

imitation, joint attention to object, and joint attention to self. Imitation was coded during

both the free play without toys session, and the free play with toys session. Joint

attention to object was coded only during the free play with toys session, while joint

attention to self was coded only during the free play without toys session.

1.3.5. Coding and Reliability

Behavioral tasks

For the ID task, a pass was defined as finding the object by searching along the

path that the hand was observed to take, eventually searching in the last location of the

hand, or searching for the ball directly under the last cover. Finding the ball in one of the

three trials was enough for passing. To establish reliability, 10 cases (22% of the

available data) were coded by an independent coder. Percent agreement between

coders was 100% on the binomial variable of pass/fail.

For the MMB test, infants received a pass code if they turned their back either

immediately after seeing the bee or after receiving a prompt. Infants could have turned

from either side (i.e., clock-wise or counter clock-wise). To establish reliability, 10 cases

(21% of the available data) were coded by an independent coder. Percent agreement

between coders was 100% on the binomial variable of pass/fail.

For the MSR test, the training and the testing phases were coded separately. For

the training phase, the passing criterion was removing the sticker from the doll and from

their hand at least once. For the self-recognition phase, a pass was defined as removing

the sticker, touching the sticker without removing it, or bringing the hand to the forehead

repeatedly while looking in the mirror without showing other indications that would

Page 43: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

31

contradict with a pass (e.g., touching the mirror, looking behind the mirror). To establish

reliability, 10 cases (20% of the available cases) were coded by an independent coder.

Percent agreement between coders was 100% on the binomial variable of pass/fail, for

both the training and the test phases.

For the KM test, infants received a pass code if they removed the sticker from

their knees. To establish reliability, 8 cases (24% of the available cases) were coded by

an independent coder. Percent agreement between coders was 100% on the binomial

variable of pass/fail.

Free play interactions

Joint attention

Based on Adamson and colleagues’ operational definition, coordinated joint

attention to an object was defined as a state of engagement during which both the

mother and the infant are actively involved in the same object or event and the infant is

actively and repeatedly acknowledging the mother’s participation (Adamson, Bakeman,

& Deckner, 2004). For example, a joint attention episode could have started with the

infant holding a wooden block out to the mother and gesturing their request for the

blocks to be placed on top of another. This joint attention episode continues as long as

the infant alternates their attention between the mother and the blocks through looking,

turn taking, or gesturing. The joint attention state ends when no clear cues of joint

attention are present for a minimum of 10 seconds and/or when the child ceases the

interaction by cues such as turning their back to the mother/activity at hand or shifting

their attention to a new object/event.

During the free play without toys session, I coded coordinated joint attention to

the self/child. This variable aimed to capture dyadic mother-infant interactions where the

focus of the joint attention was the infant. I operationally defined joint attention to the self

as a state of engagement during which both the mother and the infant are actively

involved in attention directed toward the child’s body or action and the infant is actively

and repeatedly acknowledging the mother’s participation. This is a typical example of

coordinated joint attention on the self: the mother asks her infant to show his/her nose

and the infant responds by pointing to his/her nose. The following are other examples of

this state: the infant shows her mother his dance moves as requested, checks mother’s

Page 44: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

32

reaction by keeping eye gaze, and responds coyly; an episode where the child watches

her mother’s fingers point to her legs as per the game’s rules and tucks her leg in when

the song says its time; or an episode where the infant performs a communicative gesture

for asking to be picked up and the mother responds. In all of these cases, the infant (i.e.,

infant’s nose, dance moves, leg, or whole body) became the object of attention for the

dyad and the child was aware of the mother’s attention on herself.

To code the two joint attention variables, I recorded the beginnings and ends of

joint attention states (Bakeman & Adamson, 1984). Trained coders watched the

interaction videos in real time by pausing and re-winding as necessary and identified the

starting and end points of joint attention states. The coding was completed by two

coders. For establishing reliability on joint attention to external object, 11 cases (20% of

the available data) that were randomly selected were coded by both coders

independently. Cohen’s kappa was calculated by using each second as a unit of

analysis; agreement was tallied if the same state was coded at a given second

(Bakeman & Adamson, 1984). The kappa was .84, indicating high reliability. For joint

attention to self, 12 cases (26% of the available data) that were randomly selected were

coded by both coders independently. Again, the unit of analysis was one second.

Cohen’s kappa was .89, indicating high reliability between coders.

Gestural imitation

Gestural imitation was coded with a coding system adapted from that of Hart and

Fegley (1994) who coded for infants’ and children’s imitation during a mother-infant play

session with a set of toys. Gestural imitation was operationally defined as the

reproduction of the partner’s bodily action. During the free play session with toys,

following Hart and Fegley, I coded for two types of imitation: parent imitation and child

imitation. During the free play session without toys, however, I observed that infants and

mothers often engaged in ritualized imitation games where the infant and the mother

were performing a series of gestures synchronously. Therefore, I also coded for

simultaneous imitation during the free play without toys session.

I used an interval coding strategy to code imitation (Bakeman & Gottman, 1997).

The coder assigned one of the following mutually exclusive codes to each 3-second

segment: parent imitation, child imitation, simultaneous imitation (if applicable), and no

imitation. Child imitation code was assigned whenever the child reproduced a novel

Page 45: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

33

action performed by the mother. For example, the mother takes a wooden block, places

it on top of another block, looks at her child and pauses. If the child also takes a block

and places it on top of another block, the child’s action is coded as imitation. The novelty

principle posits that each imitation must follow a modeled action and that the imitator’s

further repetitions of the same action independent of the model are not coded. For

instance, if a child continues to play with the blocks by placing them on top of each other

without the mother modeling it, they do not receive further codes of imitation. Parent

imitation was assigned whenever the mother reproduced a novel action performed by

the child (e.g., the child claps, mother joins). Simultaneous imitation code is assigned

whenever the mother and the infant perform actions that mirror each other. For example,

the mother and the infant are rolling their forearms over one another as they sing the

wheels on the bus face to face. If no imitation was observed during the segment, a no-

imitation code was assigned.

The coding of the imitation was completed by two coders. For establishing

reliability, 10 cases for the imitation during the with-toys session (18% of the available

data) and 10 cases for the imitation during the without-toys session (22% of the available

data) that I selected randomly were coded independently by both coders. I calculated

Cohen’s kappa by taking 3-second segments as the unit of analysis. An agreement was

tallied for each 3-second segment that received the same code from both coders. The

kappa was .82, indicating high reliability.

1.4. Results

1.4.1. Differences in MSR Passing Rates

Only 2 out of 29 ni-Vanuatu infants (7%) passed the MSR test compared to 15

out of 22 Canadian infants (68%). A Fisher’s exact test shows that the proportion of

passing infants is significantly different between the two cultural groups (p = .000). The

relative risk value was calculated as 2.92, which suggests that ni-Vanuatu infants were

almost 3 times more likely to fail compared to Canadian infants.

Page 46: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

34

1.4.2. Examining Alternative Explanations for Cross-cultural Variability

To examine whether low MSR rates in Vanuatu could be attributed to factors un-

related to self-development, I examined ni-Vanuatu infants’ performance on the three

control measures. The knee-marking (KM) test was used to measure infants’ motivation

to show mark-directed behavior. All ni-Vanuatu infants who received the test (n = 24)

passed by successfully removing the sticker. Two of these infants also passed the MSR

test whereas 22 failed. This suggests that ni-Vanuatu infants’ low passing rates cannot

be attributed to a lack of motivation to remove the mark. The ID test was used in order to

measure infants’ capacity to use secondary representations when searching for hidden

objects. Out of 23 ni-Vanuatu infants who received the test, 19 (83%) passed. This rate

was significantly higher than their performance in the MSR test (p = .000, McNemar

exact test) and comparable to the performance of Canadian infants (p = .189, Fisher’s

exact test for homogeneity) where 14 out of 23 infants passed (61%). Moreover, I did not

find any relationship between infants’ performance on the MSR and ID tests either within

the Vanuatu sample or within the Canadian sample (p = 1.00 for both, Fisher’s exact test

of independence). This suggests that ni-Vanuatu infants’ low passing rates in MSR

cannot be attributed to differences in using secondary representations. The mirror-

mediated behavior (MMB) test was used in order to measure infants’ ability to

understand mirror correspondence. Out of 27 ni-Vanuatu infants who received the test,

13 (48%) passed. This rate was significantly higher compared to their performance on

the MSR test (p = .007, McNemar exact test). In addition, I found no significant

relationship between MMB and MSR performance within the Vanuatu sample (p = .481,

Fisher’s exact test for independence). In other words, those infants who passed the

MMB test were not more likely to pass the MSR test. In Vanuatu, the two infants who

passed the MSR test failed the MMB test and none of the infants who passed MMB test

passed the MSR test. On the other hand, the MMB rates in Vanuatu were still

significantly lower than for the Canadian infants’ (p = .000, Fisher’s exact test for

homogeneity), where all infants who received the MMB test (n = 21) passed. Moreover,

in Canada, all but one infant displayed the passing criteria (i.e., turned their back) before

they received a prompt, with an average reaction time of 3.94 seconds (SD = 2.39).

However in Vanuatu, no infant turned before they received a prompt, which was given

after 10.68 seconds (SD = 9.05), on average. The results on the MMB test suggest that

Page 47: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

35

although ni-Vanuatu infants had less expertise in using mirrors’ reflective features, this

does not appear to explain their performance on the MSR test. Figure 1.1 shows ni-

Vanuatu and Canadian infants’ passing rates for the MSR test and the control measures

(data is not available for the KM test in Canada).

Figure 1.1. Comparison of Ni-Vanuatu and Canadian infants’ passing rates for the MSR test and the control measures

1.4.3. Predicting MSR

In this study, I observed a suite of social-interactional behaviors during a 10-

minute free play session between infants and their mothers. In this section, my goal is to

identify which specific social-interactional variables are better predictors of infants’

performance on the MSR test. I expected that the candidate predictors would a) be

positively correlated with MSR within the entire sample, and b) be observed at higher

levels among the Canadian dyads. First, I present the correlations between the social-

interactional variables and MSR. Then, I present analyses comparing the levels at which

these variables are observed among ni-Vanuatu and Canadian dyads. Finally, I present

a logistic regression model predicting MSR performance.

Page 48: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

36

Correlations between social interactional variables and MSR

I used point-biserial correlations to calculate the associations between MSR and

the social-interactional variables. The point-biserial correlation coefficient is a special

case of the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient which is employed when one

of the variables is dichotomous and the other is expressed in interval/ratio scale

(Sheskin, 2011). The variables used in these analyses were transformed to frequency

per minute scores representing the amount of joint attention and imitation observed per

minute in order to account for slight differences in the duration of each observation. As

shown in Table 1.1, only maternal imitation observed during the free play session with

toys was significantly correlated with MSR. Infants of mothers who imitated their infants

more frequently were more likely to pass the MSR test.

Table 1.1. Correlations between Social-interactional Variables and the MSR Test

Social-interactional variables rpb P value N

Joint attention to object -.00 .995 41

Joint attention to self .07 .642 49

No-toys maternal imitation .13 .434 41

No-toys child imitation -.05 .754 41

No-toys simultaneous imitation -.01 .959 41

Toys maternal imitation .47 .001 49

Toys child imitation -.03 .845 49

Note. P values < .05 are in boldface.

Comparing joint attention levels between Vanuatu and Canada

I used t-tests to compare the average duration of joint attention observed among

ni-Vanuatu and Canadian dyads. The joint attention variable used in this analysis

represented the duration (in seconds) of joint attention the dyad shared per minute.

Mean duration of joint attention to self was not significantly different between Canada

and Vanuatu t(1, 44) = -.312, p = .756. On average, Canadian dyads spent 6.47 seconds

(SD = 4.74) of every minute directing their joint attention on the infant whereas for ni-

Vanuatu dyads this duration was 6.03 seconds (SD = 4.75). There was also no

Page 49: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

37

difference in the duration of joint attention to object during free play with toys session t(1,

53) = -1.09, p = .281. On average, Canadian dyads spent 12.60 seconds (SD = 9.91) of

every minute directing their joint attention to an object or event while for ni-Vanuatu

dyads this duration was 9.88 seconds (SD = 8.67).

Comparing imitation levels between Vanuatu and Canada

I used Fisher’s exact tests to compare the levels of imitation observed among ni-

Vanuatu and Canadian dyads. For each imitation type, I created 2x2 contingency tables.

In these tables, columns represented the total frequency of 3-second segments where

imitation was and was not observed, whereas rows represented research sites (i.e.,

Vanuatu and Canada). This allowed me to compare the proportion of positive

observations (3-second-segments where imitation was observed) at the group level

while accounting for differences in the durations of observations at the individual level.

The contingency tables, as well as detailed information on how the cell sums were

calculated, are provided in the Appendix A, Section A.2. Bonferonni corrections were

applied to account for multiple comparisons within each family of comparisons (i.e.,

comparisons within the no-toys session and the toys session).

During the free play session with toys, Canadian mothers imitated their infants

significantly more than ni-Vanuatu mothers (p = .000). The odds of a Canadian mother

imitating her infant were 3.47 times higher than a ni-Vanuatu mother imitating her infant

at a given observation interval. There were no differences in the level at which Canadian

and ni-Vanuatu infants imitated their mothers (p = .224). During the free play session

without toys, no differences were observed in the level of imitation shown by Canadian

and ni-Vanuatu mothers (p = .049). However, ni-Vanuatu infants imitated their mothers

more than Canadian infants imitated their mothers (p = .000). The odds of a ni-Vanuatu

infant imitating their mother were 2.98 times higher compared to the odds of a Canadian

infant imitating their mother. Finally, during the free play session without toys, Ni-

Vanuatu dyads also showed higher levels of simultaneous imitation (p = .000). The odds

of a ni-Vanuatu dyad engaging simultaneous imitation were 1.76 times higher compared

to a Canadian dyad.

Page 50: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

38

Summary of associations

Only maternal imitation during the free play session with toys was positively

correlated with MSR. In addition, only maternal imitation during free play session with

toys was observed at significantly higher levels among Canadian dyads. These findings

pinpoint maternal imitation during the toys session as a potential predictor of MSR.

Results are summarized in Table 1.2. Here, green cells represent significant results. The

first two columns summarize the results of between-culture comparisons of imitation and

joint attention levels. When a variable was observed at higher levels in one of the sites,

the representing cell was depicted in green. The third column summarises the results of

correlation (rpb) analyses. When a social interactional variable was positively correlated

with MSR, the representing cell was shown in green.

Table 1.2. Summarizing the Associations between Site, Social Interactions, and MSR

Social-interactional variables Vanuatu Canada MSR

Joint attention to object

Joint attention to self

No-toys maternal imitation

No-toys child imitation

No-toys simult. imitation

Toys maternal imitation

Toys child imitation

Logistic regression Model Predicting MSR

Guided by the results presented in the previous section, I examined whether

maternal imitation scores observed during the toys session predicted individual

differences in MSR in the entire sample. I used a hierarchical logistic regression model

where toddler’s age and sex were entered in the first step as potential confounding

variables. The maternal imitation variable which was entered in the second step was first

subjected to square root transformations in order reduce skewness and then z-scored.

Maternal imitation significantly predicted MSR, χ²(1, 49) = 9.14, p = .002, with an odds

ratio of 3.00. A re-run of the analysis using a stepwise selection method in which I began

with all seven of the social interactional variables confirmed that maternal imitation

Page 51: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

39

observed during the free play session with toys is the only predictor of MSR in this

sample. Table 1.3 presents the regression coefficients, standard errors for regression

coefficients, and odds ratios for the predictors of the final model.

Table 1.3. Regression Model Predicting MSR

Step 1 Step 2

Model significance x2(2, 49) = .211, p = .900 x2(3, 49) = 11.36, p = .010

Social interactional variables

B SE (B) odds B SE (B) odds

Infant age -0.1 0.25 0.91 -0.09 0.29 0.92

Infant sex 0.13 0.6 1.14 -0.26 0.7 0.77

Maternal Imitation 1.1** 0.36 3

** < .01

1.5. Discussion

Results revealed significant differences in the MSR abilities of Canadian and ni-

Vanuatu infants. Only 7% of ni-Vanuatu infants passed the MSR test compared to 68%

of Canadian infants. These findings replicate previous cross-cultural literature where

infants from non-Western rural and small-scale societies pass at significantly lower rates

compared to urban Western samples (Broesch et al., 2011; Keller et al., 2004; Ross et

al. 2016).

The main objective of this study was to identify the social-interactional variables

that were related to passing the MSR test in this sample consisting of ni-Vanuatu and

Canadian infants. Based on relational approaches to objective self-awareness (e.g.,

Mead, 1934/1967; Moore, 2007; Rochat, 2009; Tomasello, 1995), I examined the

concurrent relations between MSR and an array of social-interactional variables: two

types of joint attention (i.e., joint attention to object and joint attention to self) and three

types of imitation (i.e., child’s imitation of the mother, mother’s imitation of the child and

simultaneous imitation). I hypothesized that if any of these variables had a role in

shaping the differences observed in ni-Vanautu and Canadian infants’ MSR abilities,

then that variable a) would be observed at higher levels among Canadian dyads, b)

Page 52: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

40

would be correlated with MSR in the entire sample, and c) significantly predict those

infants who pass the MSR test.

The results indicate that mother’s imitation of their infants during the free play

session with toys was the only predictor of MSR. During the free play session with toys,

Canadian mothers imitated their infants significantly more than did ni-Vanuatu mothers

and mothers’ imitation of their infants was positively correlated with infants’ MSR abilities

in the entire sample. In addition, in a logistic regression model, maternal imitation during

the free play session with toys significantly predicted infants’ MSR while controlling for

infants’ age and sex. In other words, the extent to which mothers imitated their infants

best distinguished who passed the MSR test among ni-Vanuatu and Canadian infants.

This study adds the first cross-cultural evidence to the empirical literature that links

imitation to the development of MSR (Kristen-Antonow et al., 2015, Hart & Fegley, 1994)

and provides support for the theoretical positions that view coordination of perspectives

at the heart of objective self-development (Carpendale & Lewis, 2011; Martin et al.,

2008; Mitchell, 1993; Moore, 2007; Rochat, 2009; Tomasello, 1995). Moreover, these

results put a special type of imitation –parents’ imitation of the infant– under the spotlight

of future studies interested in understanding the developmental processes involved in

the construction of objective self-awareness.

These results evoke more questions than they answer. In this study, mothers’

imitation of their infants predicted MSR but infants’ imitation of their mothers did not. This

suggests that the distinction between imitating and being imitated may have theoretical

relevance. Also, in this study, only mother’s imitation during the free play session with

toys predicted MSR, but mother’s imitation during the free play session without toys did

not. This suggests that whether imitation appears in a dyadic or triadic context may have

theoretical relevance as well. Exploring these questions further will help us understand

the details of the mechanisms through which imitation is involved in infants’ acquisition

of objective self-knowledge. Another question that arises is why joint attention was not

linked with MSR. During the joint attention episodes that appeared in the toy-play

session, dyads from both cultures engaged in joint activities such as building a tower.

Their joint attention was considered sustained as long as they demonstrated an

awareness of the other’s perspective via alternating gaze, turn taking, and gesturing.

However, this form of coordination of perspectives was not related to MSR. During the

joint attention episodes that appeared in the dyadic play session, dyads from both

Page 53: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

41

cultures engaged in games during which infants’ attention was directed to their own body

(e.g., “Where is your nose?”) or in games during which the dyad’s attention continuously

switched between infant’s and the mother’s body (e.g., a game where the mother points

to either her own leg or the infant’s leg). However, this form of coordination of

perspectives was not related to MSR either. It is possible that such interactions are still

relevant to infants’ self-development. However; the form of self-knowledge that is

acquired in these interactions may not be measured with the MSR test (Moore, Mealiea,

Garon, & Povinelli, 2007). Clearly, more theoretical and empirical work is needed to

understand the causal mechanisms through which infants construct various aspects of

objective self-awareness.

Another goal of this study was to test whether low passing rates among ni-

Vanuatu infants could be attributed to reasons unrelated to self-development such as

low motivation to show mark-directed behavior, difficulty in using secondary

representations, and difficulty in understanding mirror correspondence. I hypothesized

that if ni-Vanuatu infants did not pass the MSR test because they were not motivated to

remove the mark from their face, then they would pass the knee-marking (KM) test at

equally low levels. In addition, among ni-Vanuatu infants, performance in the KM test

would be associated with performance in the MSR test. This was not the case. All infants

who failed the MSR test successfully removed the mark from their knees. Similarly, I

hypothesized that if Vanuatu infants failed the MSR test because they were not able to

use secondary representations, then they would pass the invisible displacement (ID) test

at equally low levels. In addition, among ni-Vanuatu infants, performance in the ID test

would be associated with performance in the MSR test. This was not the case either. Ni-

Vanuatu infants were successful in using secondary representations when it comes to

searching for hidden objects. These findings demonstrate that low passing rates

observed among ni-Vanuatu infants cannot be attributed to a lack of motivation to show

mark-directed behavior or to a difficulty in using secondary representations. As such, this

study provides further support for the cross-cultural validity of the MSR test.

My examination of ni-Vanuatu infants’ understanding of mirror correspondence,

however, revealed inconclusive results. I hypothesized that if low MSR rates could be

attributed to infants’ inability to use the mirror’s reflective function, then we would expect

equally low passing rates in the mirror mediated behavior (MMB) test among ni-Vanuatu

infants. In addition, we would expect a relationship between infants’ performance in the

Page 54: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

42

two tests. Contrary to these hypotheses, among ni-Vanuatu infants, MMB passing rates

were significantly higher than MSR passing rates. On the other hand, MMB passing

rates were significantly lower in Vanuatu compared to Canada. Whereas all Canadian

infants who received the MMB test passed, fewer than half (48%) of the ni-Vanuatu

infants did so. In fact, I suspect that the differences may be even more pronounced than

the passing rates reflect. During the MMB test, if infants did not turn on their own within

10 seconds, a prompt asking the infant to get the toy was given by the experimenter.

Whereas all Canadian infants but one turned on their own before receiving the prompt,

none of the ni-Vanuatu infants turned without receiving a prompt. These results suggest

that, similar to Bedouin infants who didn’t have previous exposure to mirrors (Priel &

Schonen, 1986), ni-Vanuatu infants experienced difficulties in using a mirror’s reflective

features. These results do not support, however, the hypothesis that this relative

challenge is responsible for the low passing rates observed among ni-Vanuatu infants

because no relationship was observed between infants’ performance on the MSR test

and the MMB test. More research is needed to provide a definitive answer to the

question of whether there is a causal relationship between an understanding of mirror

correspondence and mirror self-recognition and whether such a relationship could

partially be responsible for cross-cultural differences observed in MSR passing rates.

This study has several limitations. One of these is the lack of variability in MSR

among ni-Vanuatu infants. In order to argue that the predictive value of maternal

imitation that appeared in this sample was more than a description of correlational

patterns, it is imperative to test whether maternal imitation explains within culture

variability in MSR among ni-Vanuatu infants. Another limitation is the cross-sectional

nature of the dataset. Longitudinal replications of these results will be critical in

establishing imitation as a developmental antecedent of self-recognition. Finally, I

observed the social-interactional variables during a short semi-structured free play

session. Although free play situations between infants and caregivers occur in various

cultures (Keller, 2007), the frequency of such interactions may vary. In addition, the free

play session with the set of toys I provided may be particularly unnatural for the ni-

Vanuatu dyads given that the toys provided (i.e., simple wooden blocks and a baby doll)

are rarely found in this context. Future studies should rely on natural observations to

examine the amount of imitation infants typically experience in each culture.

Page 55: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

43

Finally, I would like to add that the construction of objective self-awareness in the

infant is a long and multi-faceted process and the predictors of MSR cannot be limited to

imitation. One strong candidate that I did not address in this study is language.

Language is seen as one of the most pervasive mediums for the socialization of self

(Schieffelin & Ochs, 1986; Vygotsky, 1934/1962). Cross-cultural research on language

socialization typically focuses on the content of parents’ conversations with their children

who are active participants in these conversations —namely, children of preschool age

and older (e.g., Wang, 2001). This literature demonstrates that language interactions

shape children’s self-concepts in ways that are compatible with cultural expectations

(Fivush & Nelson, 2004). I suggest that there are reasonable grounds to expect that

systematic differences in parent-child language interactions are present even earlier,

before infants become active participants in these conversations. Future research should

examine the role of language in children’s acquisition of a self-concept.

Page 56: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

44

Chapter 2. Study 2. Parental Imitation and Mirror Self-Recognition: Examining Within-Culture Variability

2.1. Abstract

There are significant differences across cultures in the development of mirror

self-recognition (MSR), a classic test of the emergence of a sense of self. While several

explanations exist, the cross-cultural evidence point to differences in early social

experiences as a causal mechanism for the onset of MSR. In this study, I aimed to

examine and disentangle potential mechanisms underlying individual and group

differences by examining MSR in two cultural communities in an urban region of Canada

(self-identified native English and Cantonese speakers). Forty nine parents and their

toddlers participated in a structured free play procedure and infants received a mirror

self-recognition test. Additionally, I examined children’s temperament using the Early

Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Results revealed that there were no differences in

MSR passing rates between cultural groups. In the entire sample, parents’ imitation of

child’s behavior predicted children’s performance on the MSR test. This research helps

to identify possible mechanisms involved in the emergence of MSR ability and may act

as a springboard for investigating the role of imitation in shaping early self-development.

2.2. Introduction

The mirror self-recognition (MSR) test measures children’s capacity to identify

their reflected image as themselves (Amsterdam, 1972; Gallup, 1970). Children from

Western and urban populations pass the MSR test (e.g., by utilizing their reflection to

remove a mark surreptitiously placed on their face) starting from 18 months of age, with

passing rates gradually increasing over the second year. By 24 months, most toddlers

pass the MSR test and are noted to be “self-recognizers” (Amsterdam, 1972; Courage et

al., 2003; Lewis & Brooks-Gunn, 1979; Nielsen et al., 2003). This ability is an important

social cognitive milestone because it represents the emergence of a self-concept, or an

idea of me (Gallup, 1977; Lewis, 2003). This level of self-understanding is termed

Page 57: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

45

objective self-awareness (Duval & Wicklund, 1972; Neisser, 1988) and is different from

earlier forms of self-understanding because, for the first time, young children are moving

from their subjective experience of the world to an ability to take a third-person

perspective towards themselves (Mead, 1934/1967; Rochat, 2009; Tomasello, 1995).

Until recently, mirror self-recognition was considered a robust cognitive milestone

(Amsterdam, 1972; Gallup, 1970; Priel & Schonen, 1986) and individual differences in its

developmental trajectory were largely overlooked. However, recent cross-cultural

research shows significant differences between societies in the age at which young

children demonstrate MSR (Broesch et al., 2011; Kärtner et al., 2012; Keller et al., 2004;

Ross et al., 2016). This cross-cultural research suggests that children living in societies

characterized as rural and non-Western show significantly lower rates of MSR compared

to their age-matched peers from Western and urban societies. For example, only 3% of

rural Cameroonian children between 18 and 20 months self-referred during a MSR test

compared to 68% of urban Greek children (Keller et al., 2004). Similarly, Broesch and

colleagues (2011) found that passing rates were significantly lower in children living in

Fiji, Saint Lucia, Grenada, and Peru (< 33%) compared to children living in North

American cities and towns (>77%). Recently, Ross and colleagues studied MSR in

children living in rural Zambia, urban Scotland, and urban Turkey. Children from

Scotland and Turkey performed similarly on the MSR test (with passing rates of 47%

and 41%, respectively), whereas same-aged Zambian children passed at a significantly

lower rate (15%).

There are several explanations for these cross-cultural findings. One explanation

is concerned with the validity of the MSR test across diverse cultures, suggesting that

the methodological design does not capture the competence of participants in some

societies. One characteristic of the classic MSR test is that it relies on children’s

motivation to remove a mark on their face. However, in some societies, for various

reasons (e.g., compliance to authority, cultural face painting), children may not be

motivated to remove the mark. Evidence for this interpretation comes from a study by

Rochat, Broesch, and Jayne (2012) that examined how children’s motivational states

influence performance in the MSR. In this research, the authors set up a non-verbal

scenario in which they successfully alter children’s motivation to remove the mark by

creating a norm of marked faces or a norm of unmarked faces. Children removed the

mark significantly more often when unmarked faces were the norm, suggesting that

Page 58: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

46

children’s failure to remove a mark might be explained by motivational states as

opposed to failures in self-recognition.

Another explanation underscores the fact the MSR task assumes some level of

familiarity with mirrors’ reflective capacity as well as one’s own facial features. Children

from non-Western societies may have less experience with mirrors and other reflective

surfaces, and so have fewer opportunities to see themselves as a reflected image

(Kärtner et al., 2012; Priel & Schonen, 1986). In these cases, failure to pass the MSR

test may not reflect true differences in the emergence of a stable self-concept, but

instead reflect differences in infants’ reactions to the test design. By recruiting from

within the Canadian culture, I aimed to reduce the impact potential differences in

children’s motivation to remove the mark and their familiarity with mirrors.

Finally, the sociocultural perspective posits that differences in MSR passing rates

reflect true differences in self-recognition and that they are a result of differential

socialization of self across cultures. Based on this perspective, self-developmental

trajectories are shaped through parenting practices, which serve to maximize children’s

psychological functioning in a given society (Keller, 2003; Keller, 2007; Kağıtçıbaşı,

1996). Keller and colleagues suggest that early onset of self-recognition is a

consequence of autonomy-supporting socialization goals and practices which are

adopted by the parents in cultures of independence (Keller et al., 2004; Keller et al.,

2005). Consistently, Kärtner and colleagues’ study conducted with participants from two

urban (Germany and India), and two rural (India and Cameroon) societies showed that

parents’ socialization goals that favour psychological autonomy in their children

predicted whether infants passed the MSR test while controlling for several confounding

factors including infants’ age and gender (Kärtner et al., 2012). Moreover, in two cross-

cultural studies, Keller and colleagues found that mothers’ use of autonomy supporting

caregiving strategies (i.e., face-to face exchange and object stimulation) as they

interacted with their 3-month-old infants longitudinally predicted infants’ MSR abilities at

18-20 months (Keller et al., 2004; Keller et al., 2005).

However, it is not clear precisely how infants’ experiences in dyadic exchanges

as early as 3 months is causally related to the emergence of self-recognition abilities.

Developmental theories of self-awareness suggest that the proximal mechanisms

involved in the construction of objective self-awareness are to be found in the second

Page 59: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

47

year of life, when children start to interact with others as active social agents (Tomasello,

1995; Rochat, 2009; Müller & Runions, 2003; Moore, 2007). Following 12 months of age,

infants begin to engage in secondary intersubjectivity (Trevarthen, 1980). At this stage,

children view others as intentional agents, monitor other’s attention towards outside

entities, and coordinate their attention with that of their social partner (Tomasello, 1995;

Rochat, 2009). In turn, a whole new set of possibilities for triadic interactions becomes

possible. Rochat (2001) refers to these triadic interactions as the “fateful triangle of self”

(Rochat, 2001) while Chapman (1991) refers to them as the “epistemic triangle”.

Imitation may be seen as a very special case of such triadic interactions where the focal

point of the interaction is the dyad (Tomasello, 1995). Through imitation, parents are

able to reorient their infants’ attention towards themselves, rather than an object.

Moreover in mutually imitative games, the element of coordinating perspectives appears

as infants deliberately match their partner and watch how they look to others (Müller &

Runions, 2003; Barresi & Moore, 1996). Finally, Piaget (1945/1962) pointed out that

particularly because their face is not directly visible to humans, imitating others’ facial

movements is essential for infants to construct a visual representation of their own face.

In support of these hypotheses, Hart and Fegley (1994) observed that children’s

imitation of their mother during a free play session with toys was associated with earlier

MSR onset longitudinally, while their ability to imitate complex actions with toys was not,

underscoring the importance of social experience in acquiring self-knowledge. Along the

same lines, Kristen-Antonow and colleagues (Kristen-Antonow et al., 2015) found that

children’s performance on a delayed visual self-recognition task at 4 years of age was

most strongly predicted by infants’ responsiveness to being imitated by an adult at 12

months, and by MSR and verbal intelligence at 2 years. Both of these studies have been

conducted in Western urban samples. In a recent cross-cultural study, I have found that

maternal imitation of the child that was observed during a short free play session with

toys was the only variable that significantly predicted who passed the MSR test among a

sample of infants recruited from rural Vanuatu and urban Canada. These findings are

promising and suggest that imitation may be playing an important role in the

development of objective self-knowledge. In this study, I examined the associations

between MSR and three distinct types of imitation that typically appear in parent-infant

interactions in a culturally diverse sample recruited from the same socioecological

context.

Page 60: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

48

2.3. Current Research

Developmental theories of self-awareness propose that infants develop objective

self-awareness by actively participating in social interactions that underscore the

relations between self and other (Tomasello, 1995; Rochat, 2009; Müller & Runions,

2003; Moore, 2007). Imitation may be key to this process. In this study, I examined the

links between imitation and MSR in an urban Canadian sample. I recruited parents and

their children from two self-identified distinct cultural backgrounds, all living within the

same geographical location. By recruiting participants from the same ecological context,

I aimed to eliminate potential confounding effects of early experiences which might affect

MSR test performance (e.g., experience with mirrors) while maximizing variability in the

characteristics of social interactions. Furthermore, I measured parents’ assessment of

their child’s temperament to explore potential links between temperamental differences

and self-development. Because traditional Chinese culture promotes relatedness over

individuality (Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Keller et al., 2007), I expected parents coming

from this cultural background to be less likely to use strategies that promote early

development of self. Parents from traditional Chinese backgrounds have been described

as varying on a number of parenting behaviors and socialization goals such as

authoritarianism (Chuang & Su, 2009), face-to-face interactions and object-stimulation

(Keller et al., 2007), encouragement of children’s self-initiated behaviors, and

cooperation (Liu et al., 2005). With this methodological approach, the results of this

study can shed light on the developmental processes of self-awareness and inform

future investigations of cross-cultural differences in self-recognition.

2.4. Methods

2.4.1. Participants

Forty nine parents with their 16- to 21-month-old children living in the Greater

Vancouver Area of British Columbia, Canada participated in the study. Parents were

recruited from a list of families who agreed to be contacted to participate in studies on

child development. Families came from two self-identified cultural backgrounds: primarily

English-speaking (n = 29) or primarily Cantonese-speaking (n = 20). The toddlers in the

English-speaking group were predominantly exposed to English (97.17% of the time on

average) as indicated by a questionnaire given to parents. Parents in this group reported

Page 61: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

49

their children’s ethnic origin as either European or European mixed with another

ethnicity. The toddlers in the Cantonese-speaking group were predominantly exposed to

Cantonese (75.70% of the time on average). Parents in this group reported their

children’s ethnicity as East or South East Asian. I refer to these groups as English-

speaking and Cantonese-speaking throughout.

Children. The average age of participating children was 17.74 months (range =

15.40 – 21.40, SD = 1.41)2. The average age was 17.85 months (SD = 1.18) for

Cantonese-speaking babies, 17.66 months (SD = 1.55) for English-speaking babies, and

no significant differences appeared between the mean ages, t(1, 48) = -.46, p = .649.

Overall, 22 toddlers were girls and 28 were boys, and chi-square analyses showed that

the gender distribution was comparable across cultural groups (English-speaking, 16

girls; Cantonese-speaking, 13 girls), χ² (1, 50) = .22, p = .642.

Caregivers. Forty of the dyads were mother-infant pairs and nine were father-

infant pairs3. Among the nine father-infant pairs, six were from the English-speaking

group and three were from the Cantonese-speaking group. A Fisher’s exact test

indicated that the number of father-infant dyads was comparable between the cultural

groups (p = .720). Because the data extracted from father-infant and mother-infant

dyads were similar, I combined them (see Appendix B, section B.1., for details). The

number of years of formal education achieved by parents in this study was comparable

between the two language groups, chi-square test, χ² (2, 44) = 3.71, p = .156. Overall,

29.5% of the parents had either a high school education or some post-secondary

education, 52.3% had a bachelor’s degree, and 18.2% had graduate level education.

2 A subsample of participants completed the procedure in two separate visits (see the procedure section). For calculating the infant mean age reported here, I used the date at which these participants paid their first visit. The average infant age at MSR test and the mean age comparisons between the cultural groups based on infant age at MSR are presented in Appendix B, Section B.5. 3 The numbers reported here are based on the parent who was paired with the toddler during the free play session. Typically, one parent accompanied the toddler during the entire procedure. However, for a small number of participants, either a different parent was present during the MSR test (n = 2) or a different parent filled out the ECBQ (n = 2). More information on accompanying parents’ gender for different tasks is presented in Appendix B, Section B.3.

Page 62: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

50

2.4.2. Procedure

The data was collected at the UBC Centre for Infant Cognition by our

collaborators. A female experimenter greeted the parents and their children in the

laboratory, went over the procedure, and collected written informed consent from

parents. Then, the experimenter directed the participants to the testing area which

consists of a room furnished with a mat on the ground and left them alone for the 10-

minute free play session. The free play session began with five minutes of free play

without any toys. Following that, the experimenter entered the room, delivered a set of

toys (simple wooden blocks and a 12" baby-doll, see Appendix A, Section A.3. for

image) and left the dyad alone for another five minutes. After the free play session, the

experimenter entered the room and administrated the MSR test to the infant while the

mother was present in the room. Finally, participants were invited back to the waiting

area where parents completed the temperament questionnaire (ECBQ, Early Childhood

Behavior Questionnaire). All dyads received a certificate and a small prize (t-shirt, bath

toy, or book). The MSR and free play sessions were video recorded with three cameras,

to capture distinct video angles for later coding.

The English-speaking sample was tested in English by a female research

assistant. The Cantonese-speaking sample was tested in Cantonese by a female

research assistant. The Cantonese-speaking participants received either the English or

the Cantonese version of the temperament questionnaire. A subsample of 13 parent-

infant dyads completed the procedure in two separate visits. The details of the

procedure they went through, and other exceptions to the standard procedure, are

described in Appendix B, Section B.2., Section B.3., and Section B.4. An additional 11

parent-toddler dyads (n = 7 for English-speaking; n = 3 for Cantonese-speaking) were

excluded from the dataset due to fussiness (n = 5), parental interference (n = 4),

experimenter error (n = 1) and inability to complete the pre-test for MSR (n = 1).

2.4.3. Measures

Mirror self-recognition test. I assessed children’s MSR abilities with a

procedure adapted from the original MSR test (Amsterdam, 1972). In Amsterdam’s

procedure, toddlers are secretly marked on their face, shown the mirror, and expected to

remove the mark without receiving any verbal prompt. However, over the past 40 years,

Page 63: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

51

several modifications have been suggested to reduce the potential for false negatives

and ambiguous cases. Some of these suggestions involved training children in showing

mark-directed behavior prior to the testing phase (Asendorpf et al., 1996) and using

verbal prompts (e.g., directly asking the child to clean the mark) during the testing phase

(Broesch et al., 2011). Another suggestion involved marking the child on a body part that

is visible without a mirror (e.g., leg or hand) to collect a control measure of children’s

motivation to show self-directed behavior (Kärtner et al., 2012). Based on these

recommendations, I modified Amsterdam’s procedure in three ways. First, I introduced a

training phase where toddlers learned to remove the mark from a stuffed toy dog’s face.

Second, during the testing phase, I gave verbal prompts encouraging toddlers to remove

the mark. Third, following the testing phase, I secretly marked toddlers on their hands to

determine motivation to remove marks from the body. The details of these three phases

of the MSR test are described below. During the test, toddlers sat on their parents’ lap

while parents were asked to stay silent, not to interact with their toddlers, and to close

their eyes immediately before toddlers were shown the mirror reflection, so as not to

influence their child’s behavior in any way.

Training phase. The training was designed to ensure that toddlers were able to

follow the prompt to remove the sticker. The experimenter presented the child with a

stuffed toy dog with three stickers on the dog’s face and asked, “Can I have a sticker?” If

the child did not respond by removing the sticker, the experimenter performed a

demonstration by removing one sticker from the dog’s face and repeated the prompt.

This phase continued until the all three stickers were removed from the dog’s face and

the child removed at least one of the stickers.

Mirror self-recognition phase. During the mirror self-recognition phase, the

experimenter surreptitiously placed a sticker on child’s forehead and presented them

with a mirror. Toddlers were encouraged to look at the mirror for 30 seconds and were

then prompted by asking, “Do you see a sticker?”. If the child did not respond (e.g., not

searching, freezing) within 15 seconds, the experimenter asked, “Can I have a sticker?”.

This phase ended either when the toddler passed the test (i.e., showing mark-directed

behavior) or when 30 seconds passed after the second prompt.

Hand-marking phase. After the training and the MSR test phases were complete,

I removed the mirror and the experimenter placed a sticker on the top of the child’s hand

Page 64: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

52

without their knowledge. The goal was to mark the child in a location on their body that

they could easily view without a mirror. This phase ended when the child removed or

gestured toward the sticker or when 30 seconds passed.

Toddler temperament. I measured children’s temperament using the Very Short

Form of the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire (ECBQ; Putnam, Gartstein, &

Rothbart, 2006) completed by the parents. For the English-speaking group, I used the

English-language version (Putnam, Gartstein, & Rothbart, 2006). For the Cantonese-

speaking group, I used two translated versions: Chinese and Chinese (Taiwan Usage).

The ECBQ is a widely-used measure of early childhood temperament. While the

standard form contains 201 items and takes about an hour to complete, the Very Short

Form consists of 36 items and takes about 10 minutes to complete. The ECBQ

measures 18 discrete traits embedded within three higher-order factors: Negative Affect

(Discomfort, Fear, Frustration, Motor Activation, Sadness, Perceptual Sensitivity,

Shyness, Soothability), Surgency (Impulsivity, Activity Level, High Intensity Pleasure,

Sociability, Positive Anticipation), and Effortful Control (Inhibitory Control, Attention

Shifting, Low Intensity Pleasure, Cuddliness, and Attentional Focusing) (Putnam,

Gartstein, & Rothbart, 2006). I calculated and assigned each child a score for each of

the three subscales: negative affect, surgency, and effortful control. Each subscale has

12 items that are rated on a 7-point-scale. The Very Short Form was reported to have a

factor structure that matches the standard form, to have adequate internal consistency,

and criterion validity (Putnam et al., 2010). The Cronbach’s alpha measures of internal

consistency were .70 for negative affect, .52 for surgency, and .76 for effortful control.

2.4.4. Coding and Reliability

Mirror self-recognition. One trained coder watched the video recordings and

coded children’s behavior as a categorical “pass” or “fail” during three phases of the

MSR test: training, mirror self-recognition, and hand-marking. For the training phase, a

pass was assigned when the child removed at least one sticker from the stuffed toy

dog’s face. All children retained in the sample passed this phase successfully, showing

that they were all able to remove a sticker when requested. The mirror self-recognition

test phase was coded from the time the mirror was lifted and the child was viewing their

self-reflected image until they either passed the test or 30 seconds had passed from the

Page 65: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

53

second prompt to remove the sticker. Children were assigned a pass if they repeatedly

searched with the hands around their forehead while looking in the mirror, or directly

touched or removed the sticker from their forehead. For the hand-marking phase, a pass

was assigned when children removed the sticker from the top of their hands or directly

pointed at the sticker. Again, all children passed this phase, suggesting that they were

motivated to show self-directed behavior. To establish reliability, sixteen videos (31% of

the total) were coded by a second research assistant. For the training and hand-marking

phases, the agreement between the coders was 100% on the binomial variable of

pass/fail. For the testing phase, coders were in agreement 94% of the time on the

binomial variable of pass/fail.

Imitation. A trained research assistant watched the video recorded parent-infant

free play interactions in real time by pausing and re-winding as necessary to identify

imitation behavior. I coded five minutes of free play session without toys and five

minutes of free play session with toys. Using a coding scheme adapted from Hart and

Fegley (1994), three kinds of imitation codes were defined: parent’s imitation of the

child’s behavior, child’s imitation of the parent’s behavior, and simultaneous imitation.

Imitation was defined as the reproduction of the partner’s novel bodily action within a

period of five seconds. To exemplify, the mother may take a wooden block, place it on

top of another block, look at her child and pause. If the child also takes a block and

places it on top of a block (within five seconds of the mother’s action), the child’s action

is coded as imitation. The novelty principle posits that each imitation must follow a

modeled action and that the imitator’s further repetitions of the same action independent

of the model are not coded. For instance, a child who continues to play with the blocks

by placing them on top of each other without the mother modeling it does not receive

further codes of imitation. Note that coordinated imitation was coded only during the free

play without toys session. It occurred whenever the parent and the child were engaged

in coordinated imitative actions (e.g., partners face each other and simultaneously lift

their arms up).

I used an interval coding strategy to code imitation (Bakeman & Gottman, 1997).

The coder assigned one of the following mutually exclusive codes to each 3-second

segment: parent imitation, child imitation, simultaneous imitation (if applicable), and no

imitation. Because the free play sessions varied slightly in the duration, I converted the

frequency scores to rate scores by multiplying the number of segments where imitation

Page 66: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

54

was observed with 60 and then dividing it by the total number of segments available. For

establishing inter-rater reliability, I coded a random selection of 10 free play sessions

with toys (20% of the sample) and 10 free play sessions without toys (20% of the

sample). Cohen’s kappa was .89 for the free play with toys context, and .85 for the free

play without toys context, indicating high inter-rater agreement between the two coders.

The final parent imitation, child imitation, and simultaneous imitation scores that

were used in the regression analyses were computed as follows. For parent and child

imitation, I combined the scores across the two free play contexts. Due to elevated

skewness and kurtosis, square root transformations were performed on parent imitation

scores for both contexts and on child imitation for the no-toys session (Tabachnick &

Fidell, 2007). Then, I calculated z scores and summed them across the two free play

contexts. The simultaneous imitation variable was recoded into a binary variable

(present, non-present) since these behaviors were rarely observed.

2.5. Results

2.5.1. Predicting Self-recognizers

Overall, 43% of all children self-referred in the mirror. The passing rate was 41%

among English-speaking children compared to 45% among Cantonese-speaking

children. No association appeared between MSR performance and cultural group, χ² (1,

49) = .06, p = .801. I examined the predictors of self-recognition using a combination of

hierarchical and stepwise logistic regression techniques. In the first step, I entered

potential confounding variables: culture, toddler’s age at the time of MSR test, and

toddler’s sex. In the initial run of the second step, I entered all of the candidate predictor

variables: negative affectivity, effortful control, surgency, parental imitation, child

imitation, and simultaneous imitation. Simultaneous imitation was used as dichotomous

variable since this was observed in very few dyads.

A test of the full model with all predictors against a constant-only model where no

variables are included was not significant, χ² (9, 49) = 14.00, p = .122, indicating that the

predictors, as a set, did not distinguish between children who passed and failed MSR.

Table 2.1 shows regression coefficients, Wald statistics, significance levels for the Wald

statistics, and odds ratios for each of the predictors. An investigation of the individual

Page 67: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

55

parameters indicated that only infant age and parental imitation significantly contributed

to the model, χ² (1, 49) = 4.17, p = .041 and χ² (1, 49) = 7.09, p = .008, respectively.

Based on a comparison of the log-likelihood ratios, this complex model with 9

independent variables proved no better fit for the data compared to a simpler model that

included three control variables in the first step and only parental imitation as the sole

predictor in the second step, χ² (5, 49) = 2.57, p > .05. This suggested that the additional

predictor variables in the complex model did not account for the observed variation and

were therefore removed from the model. The simple model consisting of three control

variables and parental imitation as predictor was significant χ² (4, 49) = 12.57, p = .014.

Imitation positively predicted MSR (χ² (1, 49) = 7.64, p = .006) with an odds ratio of 2.37.

Those children whose parents exhibited higher levels of imitation were more likely to be

self-recognizers. The effect of age was also significant (χ² (1, 49) = 3.90, p = .048, β =

2.41); imitation increased with age.

Table 2.1. Logistic Regression with all Potential Predictors in the Model

Potential predictors B Wald p Exp (B)

MSR age 1.08 4.17 .04 2.94

Gender 1.19 1.86 .17 3.28

Culture .96 .95 .33 2.62

Negative Affectivity -.73 1.25 .26 .48

Surgency -.10 .26 .87 .91

Effortful Control -.43 .79 .37 .65

Parental Imitation .92 7.09 .01 2.52

Child Imitation -.01 .01 .97 .99

Sim. imitation .21 .06 .81 .81

(Constant) -18.72 3.44 .06 .00

Note. P values < .05 are in boldface.

2.5.2. Describing the Effects of Culture, Gender and Free play Context on Imitation

I compared imitation levels based on cultural group, free play context, and

infants’ gender using Fisher’s exact tests. For each type of imitation, I created 2x2

contingency tables. These tables contained the total frequency of 3-second segments

Page 68: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

56

where imitation was observed and not observed. This approach allowed me to reliably

compare the proportion of 3-second segments where imitation was observed between

cultural groups despite the fact that the total frequency of 3-second segment

observations varied across individual dyads. Bonferonni corrections were applied for

each family to account for multiple comparisons.

Over the 5 minutes of free play session without toys, English-speaking and

Cantonese-speaking dyads exhibited similar levels of parent imitation (p = .26) and child

imitation (p = .41). However, more simultaneous imitation was observed in English-

speaking dyads’ interactions (p = .011). Over the 5 minutes of free play session with toys

as well, English-speaking and Cantonese-speaking dyads exhibited similar levels of

parent imitation (p = .26) and child imitation (p = .41).

The next family of comparisons involved comparisons of imitation levels between

play contexts. These analyses were conducted using the entire sample. Results showed

that more imitation was observed during the toys session for both mothers (p = .009) and

toddlers (p = .000). Neither parent imitation (r = .00, p = .99) nor toddler imitation (r =

.09, p = .53) was correlated across the two contexts. This result may partly be explained

by the coders’ observation that the lack of structured activity posed a challenge for

children to stay focused on the interaction during the no-toys session. Consistent with

our observations, parental imitation during the no-toys session was correlated with

children’s effortful control (r = .36, p = .013), while no such association was observed

during the session with toys (r = .07, p = .629). Overall, these results suggest that the

free play session with toys and the free play session without toys measured distinct

aspects of the dyad’s interactive experience, and that the free play session with toys

elicited higher levels of child imitation in this sample.

Finally, I examined whether parents’ imitation levels varied based on toddlers’

gender. During the no-toys session, we observed parents imitate their girls more than

their boys (p = .013). No differences were observed during the toys session (p = .14).

Child’s imitation did not vary based on gender. During both the free play session with

toys and without toys, boys and girls imitated their parents to the same level (p = .38, p =

1.00).

Page 69: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

57

2.5.3. Describing the Effects of Culture and Gender on Temperament

Independent samples t-tests showed that Cantonese-speaking and English-

speaking parents judged their children’ surgency levels and effortful control levels

comparably, t(1, 47) = .27, p = .789 and t(1, 47) = -.93, p = .359, respectively. However, I

found a significant difference in negative affectivity, t(1, 47) = -3.56, p = .001.

Cantonese-speaking parents reported higher levels of negative affectivity (M = 3.48, SD

= .64) for their children compared to English-speaking parents (M = 2.79, SD = .66),

consistent with previous work (Chen et al., 1998; Gartstein et al., 2006; Ahadi, Rothbart,

&Ye, 1993).

2.6. Discussion

In this study, I examined imitation and temperament as predictors of self-

recognition ability among Vancouverite toddlers. Participants were recruited from two

different self-identified cultural backgrounds in order to access variability in the

constructs of interest while restricting differences in the ecological context that may

affect the validity of the measurements. However, no differences in MSR performance or

interaction styles appeared between the Cantonese-speaking and English-speaking

participants. The MSR passing rates I observed in this sample (41% among English-

speaking toddlers and 45% among Cantonese-speaking toddlers) are similar to those

reported in other Western urban samples (Amsterdam, 1972; Lewis & Ramsay, 2004;

Nielsen et al., 2003). This suggests that all participants came from a population where

parents encouraged early development of self-awareness in their children, consistent

with the needs of the socioecological context in which they lived (Keller, 2003; Keller et

al., 2004).

Results showed that parents’ imitation of their toddlers during a 10-minute free

play session significantly predicted toddlers’ performance in the MSR task together with

age. These results are consistent with the results of study 1 where maternal imitation

observed during a short free play session with toys explained the remarkable cross-

cultural differences in infants’ MSR abilities among toddlers recruited from Vanuatu and

Canada. These results are also in line with previous studies that showed that toddler’s

imitation of their mothers during a free play session (Hart & Fegley, 1994) and toddler’s

responsiveness to being imitated longitudinally predicted self-recognition ability (Kristen-

Page 70: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

58

Antonow et al., 2015). Together, these findings indicate that imitation may be an

important mechanism for the development of objective self-awareness. My findings add

to the existing literature by bring another component into play —the distinction between

the imitator and the imitated. In this study, parents’ imitation of their toddlers during a

free play session predicted MSR ability whereas toddlers’ imitation of their parents or

simultaneous imitation by both partners did not. These results suggest that parental

imitation of infants’ actions may have a particular role in supporting the development of

infants’ visual self-representations. However, more work is needed to understand

precisely how parental imitation facilitates the development of self-representations.

I classify the explanations regarding this process into two distinct theoretical

groups. One group of arguments emphasize children’s visual limitations regarding

seeing their own body, especially their face, and point to the importance of the social

other as a means of perceptual outreach (Guillaume, 1926/1971, Piaget, 1945/1962). I

will refer to this camp as the Piagetian framework. Piaget states that during the fifth

stage of sensorimotor development, which precedes the symbolic representation

capacity, children start to systematically and purposefully explore their organs that are

not directly visible to them (e.g., face) through imitation. They find correspondences

between their organs and that of their social partners using successive assimilation and

accommodation strategies, and this process leads to the construction of facial

representations (Piaget, 1945/1962, p. 85). Here, Piaget underscores the role of the

parent as an imitator because infants may need to be scaffolded into these imitative

exchanges. To summarize, from a Piagetian perspective, parents who imitate their

children might be supporting their children’ ability to construct self-representations by

providing perceptual cues on body parts that they cannot see, and by allowing them

opportunities to explore the correspondences between their own organs and those of

others.

The other group of explanations emphasize the tricky ability to step outside of the

first-person experience, following Mead’s proposition that objective self-awareness

involves taking the attitudes of other individuals toward oneself (Mead, 1934/1967).

Researchers who accept the Meadian premise suggest that the ability to think about

oneself objectively first appears within the context of triadic interactions (Moore, 2007;

Müller & Runions, 2003; Rochat, 2009; Tomasello, 1995). During triadic interactions,

caregivers and children alternate their attention between an object and each other,

Page 71: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

59

allowing children to be situated at a referential position (Rochat, 2009). In these

episodes, children actively seek to become an object of attention and the social “me”

appears for the first time in the eye of the infant. Note that this perspective does not

require children having access to the mental states of others before their own, but rather

posits that the process leading to the formation of objective knowledge of self is

immersed in our activities with the other. Following Tomasello (1995), I interpret imitation

as a very specific form of joint attention. In imitation, as it is in triadic joint attention

episodes, the object of the dyad’s joint attention stands as “me”. In addition, the

correspondence between the first and the third person perspectives is emphasized by a

match of the visual stimuli with the infant’s kinesthetic feelings. I suggest that this

kinesthetic-visual matching provides ostensibility to the categorical self. In other words,

imitating infants helps them recognize the “me” in the imitator’s actions and to view

themselves from the perspective of the other. Overall, from a Meadian perspective,

parents who imitate their children may be supporting their children’s ability to objectify

themselves by scaffolding children’ grasp of the correspondence between the first and

the third perspective. Currently, we do not have enough data to support either the

Piagetian or the Meadian framework or to argue whether they are two separate

mechanisms that work together. I encourage future researchers to design studies that

would help us identify the mechanisms through which imitation supports the

development of self-recognition.

With respect to the null result on child imitation, this finding is more difficult to

interpret and is inconsistent with Hart and Fegley’s (1994) previous study where child

imitation predicted MSR. This finding also seems to be incompatible with the two

process explanations I reviewed above, namely the Piagetian and the Meadian

perspectives. Piaget does acknowledge the role that imitating parents play as facilitators.

Yet, he also posits that infants’ own imitation of their social partners allows them to

actively explore their own faces and to learn from their mistakes (Piaget, 1945/1962, pp.

44), and thus is critical to constructing self-representations. Similarly, from the Meadian

perspective, one could argue that alternating between the imitator and the imitated roles

would help the infant coordinate their perspective with their social partner, and to

internalize the third person perspective, as Müller and Runions suggest (2003).

It is possible that my coding of infant imitation was not suitable for measuring

these theories’ predictions. Piaget’s explanation underscores imitation as a means of

Page 72: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

60

perceptual outreach. It is possible that children’s representations of their face develop

through imitating others’ faces, but not necessarily by imitating other body parts or

actions. This is a distinction that was not captured in my coding scheme. Alternatively,

from a Meadian perspective, one could argue that I failed to measure the interpersonal

aspect of imitation. The coding system I used only coded for reproduction of the

partner’s action, regardless of whether the social partner attended to or acknowledged

being imitated. The same problem was also present for my coding of parental imitation.

Yet, infants may have been more attentive to their parents’ imitation of their actions at

this developmental stage, assuring joint attention every time their parents were imitating.

The mutual attending to the imitative actions was necessarily present for the

“simultaneous imitation” behaviors. However, this type of imitation rarely occurred

among the dyads in this sample. It is possible that the procedure I used in this study was

not suitable (or long enough) to observe these behaviors. I hope that this discussion will

lead future researchers to think more about the mechanisms through which imitation

promotes self-knowledge and encourage them to revise their coding systems

accordingly.

Finally, I could not find any significant relationship between infants’ temperament

and MSR ability. To my knowledge, this is the first study that directly explored this link

although there have been some speculations with regards to the role of temperamental

differences in the onset of MSR. Some suggested that high sociability could be related to

earlier self-recognition through providing infants more opportunities for social interaction

(Asendorph et al., 1996). Whereas others suggested that inhibition may be related to

earlier MSR onset (Lewis, Brooks-Gunn, & Jaskir, 1985). The lack of a significant finding

in the present study may be related to a lack of sensitivity in the measurement. A link

between temperament and MSR could explain some of the variability in MSR across

cultures, and warrants further investigation.

Page 73: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

61

Chapter 3. Study 3. Child-directed Speech as a Context for the Socialization of Self: A comparison of Canada and Vanuatu

3.1. Abstract

Language interactions are an important context for the socialization and

development of self. In this paper, I examined the referential content of mothers’ child-

directed speech with their 21-month-old toddlers in two distinct sociocultural

environments: urban Canada (a sociocultural context well-suited for independent models

of selfhood) and rural Vanuatu (a sociocultural context well-suited for interdependent

models of selfhood). My results showed that in both Vanuatu and Canada, over half of

all child-directed speech made reference to the child. Interestingly, Canadian mothers

referred significantly more to children’s internal states, whereas ni-Vanuatu mothers

referred significantly more to children’s actions. When referring to internal states,

Canadian mothers referred predominantly to mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire)

whereas ni-Vanuatu mothers predominantly referred to body-minded internal states

(e.g., perception). Moreover, Canadian mothers used more elaborate forms of language

through linking children’s desires and abilities to their acts and perceptions, whereas ni-

Vanuatu mothers did not. Finally, Canadian mothers produced more references to past

events compared to ni-Vanuatu parents, who almost exclusively referred to the present.

This study involves the first cross-cultural examination of the content of child-directed

speech with toddlers. Overall, my results point to striking differences in how Canadian

and ni-Vanuatu mothers represent their toddlers’ selves as they speak to their toddlers. I

discuss the results and their potential implications for children’s early self-developmental

outcomes in the context of cultural ideologies on selfhood.

3.2. Introduction

One of the most pervasive mediums of socialization is language (Schieffelin &

Ochs, 1986; Vygotsky, 1934/1962). Through language interactions, parents help their

Page 74: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

62

young children develop the kind of psychological competence that is compatible with

cultural models of self (Fivush & Nelson, 2004). Cross-cultural research on language

socialization typically focuses on the content of parents’ conversations with their children

who are active participants in these conversations —namely, children of preschool age

and older (e.g., Wang, 2001). Yet, there are reasonable grounds to expect that

systematic differences in parent-child language interactions are present even earlier,

before children’s first self-developmental outcomes emerge towards the end of the

second year (Amsterdam, 1972). In this paper, I examined the referential content of

child-directed speech in mothers of toddlers from two distinct sociocultural environments:

urban Canada and rural Vanuatu –a small-scale island society located in the South

Pacific. My main goal was to describe the variation in the ways parents focus on the self

when they speak to their toddlers.

Cultural models of self and others are implicit ideas held by the members of a

cultural group about the psychological traits of a successful member of the society

(Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Western, urban, post-industrial, and largely middle-class

societies endorse an independent model of self where there are clear boundaries

between self and others. In these societies, well-being is achieved through agentic and

emotional independence (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996). Under this independent model, in order to

live a meaningful life, a person needs to discover, maximize, and openly express her

uniqueness (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). On the other hand, rural, traditional,

subsistence based, or low affluence societies endorse an interdependent model of self

where the boundaries between self and others are more vaguely defined. Well-being is

achieved through agentic and emotional interdependence (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996). In order

to live a meaningful life, a person needs to fit in with her social network, maximize the

group’s success, and live harmoniously (Markus & Kitayama, 1991).

These independent and interdependent models of self are reflected in adults’ life

narratives and autobiographical memories (Chandler, Lalonde, & Teucher, 2004;

Leichtman, Wang, & Pillemer, 2003; McAdams, 2001), beliefs about what good

parenting should entail (Harkness et al., 2010; Kärtner et al., 2007), long-term

socialization goals (Hardwood, Schoelmerich, Ventura-Cook, Schulze & Wilson, 1996),

parenting behaviors such as child-directed speech (Hughes, Devine, & Wang, 2018;

Keller et al., 2007) and reminiscing styles (Hayne & MacDonald, 2003; Wang & Fivush,

2005). These models of self eventually shape new generations’ self-representations as

Page 75: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

63

observed in children’s autobiographical memories and self-descriptions (Wang, 2006;

Wang, Doan, & Song, 2010).

Children’s first self-representations emerge towards the end of their second year

of life and become increasingly abstract, detailed, integrated across time and context,

evaluative, and reflective of societal values governing self-construals (Damon & Hart,

1982; Harter; 1998; Rochat, 2003). Parent-child reminiscing has been identified as an

important context within which children develop their self-representations. Through

encouraging their children to talk about past experiences rather than the present,

parents help their children develop a sense of self that is permanent across time and

context and highlight the kinds of experiences that are significant to the self (Bruner

1987; Fivush & Haden, 1997; Nelson & Fivush, 2004; Welch-Ross, 2001). Moreover,

parents have the advantage to guide both the content and the style of these self-related

conversations and do so based on their cultural ideologies (for a review, see Fivush &

Haden, 2003). For example, Euro-American parents have been found to take a child-

centred, elaborative, and mind-minded approach to reminiscing. They engage in

elaborate conversations with their young children about past experiences as soon as

they are able to participate in such conversations. They scaffold children’s production of

more detailed personal narratives by asking questions about the experience; directing

the conversation’s focus on children’s internal states such as emotions, thoughts, and

preferences; elaborating on the causes and consequences of their internal states; and

provide evaluations of children’s experiences (Hayne & MacDonald, 2003; Leichtman et

al., 2003; Wang & Fivush, 2005; Wang, 2001, 2006). As such, they provide children with

opportunities to construct a sense of self where the individual’s own perspective is

central to the interpretation of the world – an independent model of self. On the other

hand, Asian parents have been found to be more likely to take a mother-centered (in

contrast to child-centered), pragmatic, and moral approach to reminiscing (for a review,

see Fivush & Haden, 2003, Wang, 2013). They focus on extracting information, such as

asking questions, rather than creating detailed narratives; they see parent-child

conversations as teaching opportunities and often make reference to behavioral

expectations. When they talk about past experiences beyond these functions, they are

less likely to focus on children’s internal states and more likely to focus on interpersonal

relations (Wang & Fivush, 2005; Leichtman et al., 2003; Wang, Leichtman, & Davies,

2000). By doing so, parents provide their children with opportunities to construct an

Page 76: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

64

interdependent sense of self which children will use to construct a socially-oriented

identity (Wang, 2006; Wang, 2013; Wang & Conway, 2004).

These outcomes have indeed been observed in children’s self-descriptions and

the content they provide in autobiographical memories. For example, Wang (2006) found

that, when asked to describe themselves, Euro-American 3-year-olds made more

references to private aspects of their selves (e.g., personal attributes) as opposed to

collective aspects (e.g., group membership) and produced more information on

autobiographical memory reports compared to their Chinese and Chinese immigrant

counterparts. These differences were mediated by parents’ reminiscing style where

maternal elaborations and evaluations contributed to better memory reports regardless

of culture. Similarly, when Wang and colleagues (2010) asked 3-year old children to

describe themselves, they found that Euro-American 3-year-olds provided more trait

self-representations and evaluative self-representations compared to their Chinese

immigrant counterparts. In this study, mothers’ use of internal state language predicted

self-outcomes independent of culture and maternal reminiscing style mediated the

effects of culture (Wang et al., 2010).

The evidence points towards the importance of language interactions for the

development of a self-concept, yet cross-cultural differences in self-developmental

trajectories arise even earlier —as early as 18 months— before infants are active

participants in reminiscing conversations. Cross-cultural research on the development of

mirror self-recognition (MSR) indicates significant variability in the timing of this

developmental milestone (Keller et al., 2005; Keller et al., 2004). We know very little

about whether language interactions before the second year systematically differ across

cultures in a way that could give rise to differences in the emergence of self-recognition

and, potentially, self-concept. Research on the content of child-directed speech in

toddlerhood is scarce and the existing data comes from Euro-American populations.

Consistent with the reminiscing literature I reviewed above, these studies suggest that

parents’ focus on toddlers’ internal states predicts children’s own internal state language

at two years (Dunn, Bretherton, & Munn, 1987). In fact, Euro-American parents refer to

their pre-verbal infants’ internal states during infant-directed speech as early as 3 to 6

months of age (Meins, Fernyhough, Fradley, & Tuckey, 2001; Penman, Cross, Milgrom-

Friedman, & Measres, 1983; Sherrod, Crawley, Peterson, & Bennett, 1978) and do so

more than mothers of Chinese 3-month-olds (Keller et al., 2007). Referring to internal

Page 77: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

65

states as early as 3 months reflects Euro-American mothers’ mind-mindedness, which is

defined as mothers’ proclivity to treat their infants’ as an individual with a mind, rather

than merely as a creature with needs that must be satisfied (Meins, 1997). This

construct is measured by examining parents’ descriptions of their infants and parents’

reports on whether they attribute meaning to their children’s early vocalizations. These

measures are consistent between 6 months and 3 years of age, correlate with parents’

mental state language during child-directed speech at 3 years, and eventually predict

children’s theory of mind abilities at 5 years (Meins et al., 2002). Recently, Hughes and

colleagues (Hughes, Devine, & Wang, 2018) examined mind-mindedness cross-

culturally and showed that parents of 3 to 4.5 year-old children from UK were

significantly higher in mind-mindedness compared to parents from Hong-Kong, and that

these differences explained the variation in children’s theory of mind skills. These results

are consistent with Harkness and colleagues’ research on parenting ethnotheories

(Harkness & Super, 1996; Harkness et al., 2010), suggesting that US parents are

particularly focused on their children’s needs for cognitive stimulation compared to non-

Western cultures, as well as some Mediterranean European cultures.

In light of the research which suggests that Euro-American parents’ particular

focus on their children’s ability to develop an individual mind is consistent across infancy

and early childhood (Hughes et al., 2018; Harkness et al., 2010; Meins et al., 2002), we

would expect to observe the patterns demonstrated by the cross-cultural reminiscing

research in child-directed speech with toddlers. To my knowledge, I present the first

cross-cultural examination of child-directed speech in toddlerhood, a developmental

stage that is critical for the emergence of self-concept. In this study, my main goal was to

document similarities and differences in the referential content of child-directed speech

of mothers and their 18- to 28-month-old toddlers from two sociocultural environments

that are expected to vary significantly in terms of cultural ideologies: urban Canada and

rural Vanuatu. With this aim, I examined the prevalence and the characteristics of

mothers’ references to their toddlers. I provided detailed analyses of internal state

references to capture different aspects of subjectivity. Finally, I examined the prevalence

to mothers’ reference to past events. Results are discussed within the framework of

socialization of self in relation to others.

Page 78: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

66

3.3. Methods

3.3.1. Participants

Thirty-five toddlers (n = 16 for Canada, n = 19 for Vanuatu) and their primary

caregivers participated in this study. There were 20 girls (n = 7 for Canada, n = 13 for

Vanuatu), and 15 boys (n = 9 for Canada, n = 6 for Vanuatu). Gender distribution was

comparable across cultural groups, χ² (1, 35) = 2.16, p = .142. The average age of

participating children was 21.31 months (range = 18.23 – 28.43, SD = 2.53). Canadian

infants were significantly younger than ni-Vanuatu children, t(1,33) = 2.81, p = .008. The

average age was 22.31 months (range = 19.10 – 28.43, SD = 1.18) for ni-Vanuatu

toddlers and 20.12 months (range = 18.23 – 22.97, SD = 1.39) for Canadian toddlers.

All Canadian caregivers were mothers. Mothers’ average age was 34.75 years

(SD = 5.15) and they achieved, on average, 19.06 years (SD = 3.51) of formal

education. All families were either English-speaking (n = 12) or bilingual families (n = 4)

and were comfortable interacting in English. Ni-Vanuatu caregivers consisted of 17

mothers, one father, and one adolescent older sibling. Caregivers’ average age was

28.78 years (SD = 7.30), based on self-report; however their self-reported age cannot be

expected to be exact as it is not customary to remember birth dates or ages. Only 26%

of the caregivers (n = 5) had attended formal schooling, and for fewer than 5 years, on

average. All ni-Vanuatu participants came from monolingual families who spoke the local

dialects of Eastern Tanna Island.

A total of nine dyads were excluded from the database. In Vanuatu, three dyads

were excluded due to fussiness and one dyad was excluded because the caregiver was

a preadolescent child. In Canada, two dyads were excluded due to fussiness, and three

dyads were excluded because a significant portion of these interactions was in a non-

English language (one Japanese, one French, and one German), as mothers preferred

to switch from English during the free play interactions. I decided to exclude these

interactions in order to avoid introducing unwarranted translation bias into the data.

These mothers were initially recruited because they reported that they were comfortable

interacting with their children in English.

Page 79: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

67

3.3.2. Societies

Metro Vancouver is a city with a population of approximately 2.5 million people,

making it the third-largest metropolitan area in Canada (Statistics Canada, 2016). It is an

affluent city with a median total family income of over 70,000 CAD (Statistics Canada,

2016). Its population is highly educated; 92% of its population has high school level

education or higher. The family structure is nuclear with an average of 2.5 persons per

household (Statistics Canada, 2016). It is also an ethnically and linguistically diverse city

as 47% of its residents’ first language is an immigrant language (Statistics Canada,

2016). Vancouver attracts highly skilled international workers and the employment

market is highly competitive. Based on these demographics, Vancouver shows the

typical characteristics of a society with an independent, individualistic sociocultural

orientation (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Triandis, 2001).

Tanna Island is in the Tafea province of the Republic of Vanuatu, located in the

South Pacific. It has a population of 29,000 based on the 2009 census (Vanuatu

National Statistics Office, 2009). Residents on this island live in small communities of

fewer than 100 inhabitants where neighbors consist of extended family members.

Families are reliant on subsistence agriculture and small cash crops for survival. Formal

education is relatively recent and literacy skills in the adult population are low. Birth rates

are high and children participate in the work force by helping their parents in child

rearing and plantation responsibilities. There is very little integration with a market

economy, very little media access (no electricity in the villages at the time of this study),

and as a result, very little influence from Western cultures. Based on these

demographics, Tanna Island shows the typical characteristics of a society with an

interdependent or collectivistic sociocultural orientation (Kağıtçıbaşı, 1996; Triandis,

2001).

3.3.3. Procedure

As part of a study on self-development, I video recorded mothers and their

infants during a ten-minute free play session after a series of developmental tasks that

took approximately 20 minutes. The free play session consisted of five minutes of free

play without any toys followed by five minutes of free play with a set of select toys. In this

study, I only examined the free play without toys session, which is representative of

Page 80: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

68

typical mother-child interactions, as toys are not traditionally used and are rarely found in

this community. During the free play without toys session, I asked mothers to sit on a

soft mat on the floor and play freely with their children as they would do on a regular day.

I recorded the sessions using two video cameras from two different angles (front view

and left back view). In both Canada and Vanuatu, I obtained mothers’ consent verbally.

The procedure was stopped if the child was distressed or fussy.

I collected the Canadian data between 2013 and 2016. I recruited mothers from

the Metro Vancouver region of British Columbia, Canada, through a list of mothers who

previously agreed to participate in studies on child development, as well as through

flyers posted in childcare services at Simon Fraser University (SFU), SFU campus,

public libraries, and community centres. I tested participants either in the laboratory (n =

5), in participants’ homes (n = 7) or in a daycare (n = 4).

I collected the ni-Vanuatu data between 2014 and 2015 during two separate field

trips to Tanna Island. Before starting data collection, I obtained research permits from

the Vanuatu Cultural Centre and then permission to do research in these villages by the

local leaders. I recruited in villages of East Tanna and conducted the study either in

participants’ homes or in a suitable open area in the village. The testing was done in the

local language by a local research assistant who was a native speaker of the local

language and also spoke English fluently. I translated the study procedure from English

to the local language in two steps. First, a local research assistant who is fluent in both

English and Bislama (the official language of Vanuatu) translated the procedure from

English into Bislama in writing. Another local research assistant who was fluent in both

languages back translated from Bislama to English, again in writing, and any

inconsistences were resolved through discussion (back translation). Once the procedure

in Bislama reflected the intended study procedure, another round of translation and back

translation was made from Bislama to local language by two separate local research

assistants who were fluent in both languages. Again, all inconsistencies were resolved

through discussion between translators. This last step of translation and back translation

was conducted orally because the local language is an oral language, and was

videotaped for documentation purposes.

Page 81: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

69

3.3.4. Transcription and Translation of the Interactions

The transcription of the free play interactions (though videos in Canada, online in

Tanna) began as soon as the researcher left the room, or if the mother-infant pairs

began to interact. The mean duration of transcribed interactions was 4 minutes 44

seconds, and was comparable between societies, t(1, 33) = .55, p = .583. In Canada, I

edited the videos of the free play interactions prior to transcription in order to obtain

uninterrupted interactions. A trained research assistant watched all the videos,

synchronized the two video angles by merging them into one video and excluded the

following parts using the cropping function of the i-Movie program: a) the infant is out of

the camera view, b) no interaction is possible as a result of child crying, or c) another

external intrusion is preventing the interaction. Next, a research assistant watched these

edited videos using Windows Media Player and transcribed mothers’ and infants’ speech

verbatim. A second researcher independently watched all of the videos in order to

review the transcript created by the first research assistant, and recorded all

disagreements. The disagreements were resolved by a third researcher.

The interactions from Tanna were translated into English prior to analysis. An

important disadvantage of working with translated linguistic data is the difficulty of

accurately transferring the meaning between languages (Esposito, 2001; Temple &

Young, 2004). In order to reduce threats to the validity of the data, I worked with only

one interpreter. The source language was the translator’s native language and she was

fluent in the target language (English). As a mother from the same community, she was

knowledgeable in the ways of communication of the group being studied, and she was

blind to the hypotheses of the study. In addition, I translated a subsample of the data

together with a second interpreter (again, a mother from the same community) and

compared the output of the content analysis from both translations as a validity check

(Twinn, 1997). The interactions were translated orally and transcribed directly in English

without being transcribed in the original language since the source language is an oral

language. During the translation process, I worked closely with the translator. The

process was as follows: I paused the video after each utterance (i.e., continuous piece of

speech beginning and ending with a clear pause) and re-played as many times as

necessary to achieve accurate comprehension and translation by the interpreter. The

interpreter listened to the utterance and orally translated it from the local language to

English. The English translation provided was handwritten by me on a notebook.

Page 82: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

70

Unedited footages were used during this process. Prior to video replay, I selected the

video angle that provided the clearest audio and made exclusion decisions as the

transcription process was taking place based on the identical rules that were used for

the editing of the Canadian videos (see the above paragraph). For example, the

translation was stopped when the child walked away from the testing area and resumed

when the child came back. The lost duration was noted in order to keep track of the total

duration of the interaction. In Vancouver, a trained research assistant typed my

handwritten transcriptions. I compared the typed transcriptions to the original

handwritten ones and confirmed that no mistakes were made.

Finally, to prepare the transcriptions for coding, I removed all nursery rhymes,

song lyrics, and children’s speech content from the transcriptions, leaving only

spontaneous child-directed speech from the mother as data for this project. I excluded

nursery rhymes and lyrics in Vanuatu because their content involved archaic words (i.e.,

words that are no longer in common use) whose translation required linguistic expertise

that was not available. In order to reach consistency, I also excluded nursery rhymes

and song lyrics from the Canadian transcriptions.

In order to establish reliability for the translation of ni-Vanuatu interactions, a

separate local ni-Vanuatu research assistant translated five randomly selected

interactions (26% of the Vanuatu sample) from the local language into English through

the same process described above. Next, I parsed and coded the transcriptions of these

five reliability cases, and compared them to the original dataset. I examined the

consistency of the data with a series of two-way mixed, absolute, single measures ICCs.

The ICCs calculated for the general referential content system varied between .90 and

.98, indicating high consistency (Cicchetti, 1994). The ICCs calculated for the referential

content focused on the child ranged between .84 and .97, indicating high consistency -

except for the elaborate internal state code. I was not able to calculate an ICC statistic

for the elaborate internal state code because this code was observed only once in one of

the translator’s transcriptions based on the five randomly selected interactions. I was

also not able to calculate ICC scores for the referential content system focused on

children’s internal states because the scores that resulted from the two sets of

translations were almost identical, indicating a lack of variability. Finally, I observed only

one instance where past talk appeared in one of the translator’s transcription. Overall,

my analysis of the two sets of translations suggests that the content of the interactions

Page 83: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

71

could be reliably translated and that a minimal amount of measurement error was

introduced through the translation process.

3.3.5. Parsing the Transcriptions

The unit of analysis for this dataset was chosen as a communication unit (c-unit)

(Loban, 1976). This allowed me to reliably parse the translated ni-Vanuatu transcriptions

given the absence of an expert speaker who could be trained to parse utterances at the

time of translation. Structurally, a c-unit is defined as an independent clause and its

modifiers (Loban, 1976). Semantically, a c-unit is defined as a group of words which

cannot further be divided without the loss of their essential meaning (Loban, 1976). In

this study, I defined c-units in both structural and semantic terms. This approach was

used because single-word utterances or utterances that lack clausal structure were

common in my data. Whenever an utterance qualified for a clause, the utterance was

segmented based on structural definition. Based on this, both of the following utterances

were parsed as one c-unit: “Sit down”; “If you cry, I will leave you”. Whenever an

utterance did not qualify for a clause, the segmentation was made based on the

semantic definition. Based on this, both the one-word phrase “what?” and three-word

phrase “the other game” were parsed as one c-unit. In order to establish reliability with

the researcher who parsed the transcriptions, a trained research assistant independently

parsed nine randomly selected transcriptions (26% of the sample). The percent

agreement between the parsers was 98%.

3.3.6. Coding

I coded mothers’ spontaneous child-directed speech for past talk, general

referential content, referential content focused on the child, and referential content

focused on the child’s internal states. For past talk and general referential content, I

coded all c-units.

Past talk coding identified the c-units where mothers made reference to the past.

For coding the general referential content, I categorized each c-unit under four mutually

exclusive categories: child, mother/dyad, environment, and other. Those c-units that

were categorized under the child category were then coded for referential content

focused on the child. In this coding system, I categorized the targeted c-units under four

Page 84: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

72

mutually exclusive content categories: action, simple internal state, elaborate internal

state, and objectification. Finally, I selected those c-units that were categorized under

the internal state category (either simple or elaborate) and coded them for referential

content focused on the child’s internal states. In this coding system, I categorized the

targeted c-units under six mutually exclusive categories: four mind-minded categories

(desire, thought/belief, ability/volition, and emotion) and two body-minded categories

(perception and physiological). The details of the coding systems are described below.

General referential content

In this coding system, I coded the c-units exhaustively based on their general

referential content. I categorized each c-unit into one of the four mutually exclusive

referent categories: 1) child, 2) mother/dyad, 3) environment, 4) other/non-determinable.

The referent categories were based on Morikawa, Shand, and Kosawa (1988). A “child”

code was assigned whenever the c-unit was concerned the child. A “mother/dyad” code

was assigned whenever the c-unit concerned the mother or the dyad. An “environment”

code was assigned whenever the c-unit was about an object or a third person. Finally,

the “other/non-determinable” code was assigned whenever the c-unit couldn’t be

categorized under the three other categories confidently or whenever the c-unit didn’t

have a clear referent. Examples for each referential category are presented in Table 3.1.

In order to establish reliability with the researcher who completed the coding, a trained

research assistant independently coded eight randomly selected transcriptions (23% of

the sample). The kappa score was .95.

Table 3.1. Coding of the General Referential Content

Referent category Examples

Child Come back here. Are you hungry? Where is your nose?

Mother or Dyad Mother will sing. I am going now. We will play naronaro.

Environment Daddy is at home. What color is this stripe? The caterpillar is over there.

Other/un-determinable Good job. Okay. This one.

Page 85: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

73

Referential content focused at the child

This coding system focused on those c-units where the child appeared as the

referential content. In order to identify the attributes that mothers chose to refer to in their

children, I coded the targeted c-units into four mutually exclusive categories: 1) action, 2)

simple internal state, 3) elaborate internal state, and 4) objectification. I assigned the

code “action” whenever the child was invited to perform an action, or the mother was

describing the child’s action. I assigned the code “simple internal state simple” whenever

the c-unit referred to the child’s internal state such as emotions or thoughts. I assigned

the code “internal state elaborate” whenever the c-unit was a complex sentence that

referred to either two separate internal states or one internal state and an action in the

same c-unit. Finally, I assigned the code “objectified” when the child herself, the child’s

body part, or the child’s possession appeared as an object in the sentence. One-word c-

units consisting of the child’s name or the pronoun you were also classified under this

category. Examples for each referential category are presented in Table 3.2. In order to

establish reliability, a second researcher independently coded eight randomly selected

transcriptions (23% of the sample). The kappa score was .92.

Table 3.2. Coding of the Referential Content Focused on the Child

Referent category Example

Action Say hi. Come over here. Sing a song to the baby.

Internal state [simple] Are you dizzy? You want the horse? You are so focused on this task.

Internal state [elaborate] Do you want to sing the alphabet? Can you hop like a bunny? Wanna see mama on the camera?

Objectified Where is Lucy (child’s name)? Is that your tummy? Mom will carry you.

Referential content focused at the child’s internal states

This coding system focused on those c-units where the referent was the child’s

internal states (either simple or elaborate). I identified six different categories of internal

states which I combined under two major categories: mind-minded internal states and

body-minded internal states. Mind-minded internal states involved those c-units where

mothers referred to their child’s mental or psychological processes. I coded four sub-

Page 86: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

74

categories: desire, thought, ability/volition, and emotion. Body-minded internal states

involved those c-units where mothers referred to their child’s bodily sensations. I coded

two subcategories: perception and physiological. This coding scheme was based on

Bretherton and Beegly (1982). I assigned the “desire” code when the c-unit referred to

the child’s wants and wishes; a “thought/belief” code when the c-unit referred to the

child’s mental activities; an “ability/volition” code when the c-unit referred to the child’s

capacity to perform a certain action; an “emotion” code when the child’s internal state

was an emotion, a feeling, or a mood; a “perceptual” code when a perceptual sense

such as seeing or hearing was referred to; and finally, a “physiological” code whenever

the child’s physiological state such as hunger or sleepiness was referred to. I called the

first four categories (i.e., desire, thought and belief, ability, and emotion) “mind-minded

internal states” in reference to Meins’s concept of mind-minded comments (Meins,

1997), whereas I called the last two categories (i.e., perception and physiology) “body-

minded internal states”. Examples of for each referential category are presented in Table

3.3. The coding was completed by the lead author. In order to establish reliability, a

research assistant trained by the lead author independently coded eight randomly

selected transcriptions (23% of the sample). The kappa score was .97.

Table 3.3. Coding of the Referential Content Focused on the Child’s Internal States

Referent category Referent sub-category Examples

Mind-minded internal states

Desire What do you want? What would you like to play?

Thought/belief Do you know the next verse? Remember that song?

Ability/volition Can you find me the blue card? You gotta sit down please.

Emotion Do you like blue? Don’t worry about it.

Body-minded internal states

Perceptual Do you see the dog coming? Do you hear the airplanes?

Physiological Are you thirsty? You are getting sleepy.

Past talk

In this coding system, we coded c-units exhaustively into two mutually exclusive

categories: 1) past talk, 2) other. We assigned a “past talk” code when the mother used

past tense in the c-unit (e.g., “Were you gentle with the baby?”). We put all other c-units

Page 87: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

75

into the “other” category. In order to establish reliability, a trained research assistant

independently coded 8 randomly selected transcriptions (23% of the sample). The kappa

score was 1.00, indicating perfect agreement.

3.4. Results

3.4.1. General Referential Content

I compared the general referential content of Ni-Vanuatu and Canadian mothers’

child-directed speech using a one-way between-subjects multivariate analysis of

variance (MANOVA). A MANOVA was performed on four independent variables (child,

mother/dyad, environment, and other/non-determinable) where culture served as the

group factor. There were no univariate or multivariate outliers. Assumptions of

homogeneity of variance-covariance matrices, linearity, and multicollinearity were met. I

observed that the scores for the “mother/dyad” variable were moderately positively

skewed. However, the MANOVA analysis only requires multivariate normality of

sampling distributions, and the univariate F test is considered robust against non-

normality as long as the error term is greater than 20 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007), a

criterion that was met in our case. Based on the Wilks’ criterion, culture had a significant

effect on the outcomes as a set, F(4, 30) = 3.42, p =.020. The multivariate eta squared

measure, η2 = .313, indicated that approximately 31% of multivariate variance of the

dependent variables is associated with the group factor. Follow-up univariate ANOVAs

indicated that only the “other/non-determinable” variable was significantly different

between Vanuatu and Canada transcriptions, F(1, 33) = 11.37, p = .002, η2 = .27.

Canadian mothers produced twice as many c-units of this category (M = 19.0%, SD =

6.57%) compared to ni-Vanuatu mothers (M = 9.53%, SD = 9.47%).

There were no significant differences in the proportions of mothers’ references to

their children, to themselves or to the dyad, and to the environment (p > .34 for all).

Approximately half of child-directed speech made reference to the child in both Vanuatu

(M = 58. 30%, SD = 25.42%) and Canada (M = 51.31%, SD = 14.63%). The next most

common referent was the environment; approximately 20% of child-directed speech fell

under this category in both Vanuatu (M = 22.60, SD = 18.61%) and Canada (M = 18.15,

SD = 11.76%). The least common referent was the mother or the dyad; this category

Page 88: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

76

constituted only 9.57% of child directed speech in Vanuatu (SD = 12.34%), and 11.54%

of child-directed speech in Canada (SD = 6.69%).

3.4.2. Referential Content Focused on the Child

I examined whether ni-Vanuatu and Canadian mothers varied in terms of the

attributes they chose to refer to in their children. For this, I compared the distribution of

referential content categories focused on the child (i.e., “action”, “simple internal state”,

“elaborate internal state”, and “objectified”) in ni-Vanuatu and Canadian parents’ child-

directed speech. A MANOVA analysis was performed on three dependent variables:

action, internal state, and objectification, where culture served as the group factor. There

were no univariate or multivariate outliers. Evaluations of assumptions of homogeneity of

variance-covariance matrices, linearity, and multicollinearity were satisfactory. The

scores for the “action” and the “objectification” variables were moderately positively

skewed. However, MANOVA analysis only requires normality of sampling distributions,

and the F test is considered robust against modest non-normality as long as the error

term is greater than 20 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007), a criterion that was met in our case.

Based on the Wilks’ criterion, culture had a significant effect on the set of outcomes, F(3,

33) = 5.44, p =.004. The observed power was .90 based on the alpha level of .05. The

multivariate eta squared measure, η2 = .345, indicated that approximately 35% of

multivariate variance in dependent variables is associated with the group factor. Follow-

up univariate ANOVAs indicated that the action variable, F(1. 33) = 9.39, p = .004, and

the total internal states variable, F(1, 33) = 5.99, p = .020, were significantly different

between Vanuatu and Canada samples. The difference observed in the objectification

variable did not reach significance F(1, 33) = 3.33, p = .077. In Vanuatu, mothers

referred significantly more to their infants’ actions; 39.47% of all the child-directed

speech referred to children’s actions (SD = 26.98%) compared to 17.5% in Canada (SD

= 10.34). On the other hand, Canadian mothers referred significantly more to their

children’s internal states. Among all child-directed speech, 18.91% referred to children’s

internal states (SD = 9.14) compared to 10.14% in Vanuatu (SD = 11.62%).

The differences in use of internal state references became even more articulated

when I examined them at two levels of complexity: simple and elaborate. As explained in

more detailed under the methods section, a c-unit was coded as simple internal state

when mothers singly referred to one internal state category. For example, the mother

Page 89: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

77

may have referred to the child’s desire (e.g., “What do you want?”). A c-unit was coded

as elaborate internal state when mothers referred to either two kinds of internal states at

once (e.g., “You want to see mama?”) or when they referred to the child’s internal state

combined with the child’s action (e.g., “Can you jump?”). The use of these categories

was compared using non-parametric tests because the distributions severely violated

the normality assumption. Fourteen out of 19 ni-Vanuatu mothers (74%) referred to their

children’s internal states in simple form, compared to 15 out of 16 Canadian mothers

(94%), which was equally common among ni-Vanuatu and Canadian mothers, χ²(34) =

2.46, p = .117. On average, 9.68% of all the c-units were coded as simple internal state

in ni-Vanuatu mothers’ transcriptions (SD = 11.64%), compared to 5.78% in Canadian

mothers’ transcriptions (SD = 3.56%). A Mann-Whitney U test suggested that the means

of the ranks of the two groups were equal, p = .868.

However, there were remarkable differences in the use of elaborate internal state

forms. Only two out of 19 ni-Vanuatu mothers referred to their children’s internal states

in elaborate form, compared to 15 out of 16 Canadian mothers and this was statistically

significant, χ²(34) = 24.08, p = .000. In addition, Canadian mothers relied more heavily in

this form. In Canada, on average, 13.13% (SD = 8.77%) of all child-directed speech was

in this form compared to only 0.46% in Vanuatu (SD = 1.43%). A Mann-Whitney U test

suggested that the means of the ranks of the two groups were significantly different, p =

.000. Interestingly, Canadian mothers used elaborate internal state forms even more

often than simple internal state forms, t(15) = -3.50, p = .003.

Page 90: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

78

Figure 3.1. Percentage distribution of referential content categories focused on the child in ni-Vanuatu and Canadian parents’ child-directed speech

3.4.3. Referential Content Focused on the Child’s Internal States

Next, I examined the internal state categories that mothers have used when they

referred to children’s internal states. In order to analyze to what extent mothers preferred

to refer to mind-minded internal states over body-minded internal states in their children,

I created a ‘mind over body’ variable through subtracting the frequency of c-units that

were coded under the body-minded internal state category from the frequency of c-units

that were coded under the mind-minded category and dividing the result with the total

frequency of c-units that were coded under the internal state category [(#mind-minded –

#body-minded) / (#mind-minded + #body-minded)]. The result is a value between -1 and

+1 where those mothers who have only referred to mind-minded internal states get a

score of +1, those mothers who have only referred to body-minded internal states get a

score of -1, those mothers who have equally referred to both get a score of 0, and others

get scores in between, proportionate to how many more c-units they produced referring

to their children’s mind-minded internal states. The mothers who didn’t refer to their

children’s internal states at all, all of whom were from Vanuatu (n = 5), were not included

in the analyses. A Mann-Whitney U test showed that the means of the ranks of the two

Page 91: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

79

groups were significantly different, p = .000. Figure 3.2 shows the distribution of the

percentage of c-units that were coded under mind-minded and body-minded categories

among internal state references separately for ni-Vanuatu and Canadian mothers. As

can be seen in the table, ni-Vanuatu mothers predominantly used body-minded internal

state references whereas Canadian mothers predominantly used mind-minded internal

state references.

In Tanna, the most common code was perception (e.g., “See the caterpillar”,

“Listen”). The majority of internal state references fell under this category (M = 76.89%,

SD = 37.29%), followed by the physiological category (e.g., “Is your stomach full?”, “If

you cry, I will leave you”) which constituted 11.61% of all c-units that referred to

children’s internal states (SD = 27.94%). Desire constituted another 8.04% (SD =

18.09%), thought/belief constituted another 1.79% (SD = 6.68%), and finally

ability/volition constituted 1.68% (SD = 6.29%) of all c-units that referred to children’s

internal states. I did not observe any reference to children’s emotions. In Canada, the

most common code was desire (e.g., “What would you like to play?”, “Do you want to

sing the ants?”). Over 40% of the c-units that referred to children’s internal states fell

under this category (M = 42.57, SD = 34.2%) followed by the ability/volition category

(e.g., “How fast can you go?”, “You can look, but don’t touch it”), which constituted 33.

42% of internal state references (SD = 30.91%). Finally, thought/belief constituted 8.03%

(SD = 10.95%), physiological constituted 3.23% (SD = 6.82%), and emotion constituted

2.26% (SD = 5.44%) of all c-units that referred to children’s internal states.

Page 92: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

80

:

Figure 3.2. Percentage distribution of mind-minded and body-minded internal state references among ni-Vanuatu and Canadian parents’ total internal state references

3.4.4. Past Talk

I examined the extent to which ni-Vanuatu and Canadian mothers referred to

past events in their child-directed speech. Not all mothers in the sample referred to past

events; however past talk appeared in significantly more mothers’ transcriptions in

Canada, χ²(1, 35) = 8.43, p = 004. Nine out of 16 Canadian mothers (56%) referred to at

least one past event compared to only two out of 19 ni-Vanuatu mothers (11%). In

Canada c-units that referred to a past event consisted of 2.49% of all child-directed

speech (SD = 3.65%) on average whereas this rate was only .10% among ni-Vanuatu

mothers (SD = .31). A Mann-Whitney U test showed that the means of the ranks of the

two groups were significantly different, p = .002.

3.5. Discussion

Analysis of general referential content categories revealed that ni-Vanuatu and

Canadian mothers referred equally to their children, to themselves or the dyad, and to

the environment during child-directed speech. In both Vancouver and Tanna, the most

common referent category was the child; more than half of all child-directed speech

Page 93: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

81

made reference to the child. In other words, in both cultures, mothers organized their

language interactions around their toddlers. This suggests that perhaps the critical

differences lie not in the extent to which mothers’ speech to their children is about them,

but rather in what aspects of the children's self they highlight. I found a significant

difference in the proportion of the c-units that I coded as ‘other/non-determinable’.

Canadian mothers produced more c-units that were coded under this category. It is

unclear what this finding reflects. One possibility is that it reflects Canadian mothers’

verbosity. Canadian mothers produced more c-units per minute compared to ni-Vanuatu

mothers. As such, they may have used more ‘fillers’ –one-word utterances that do not

have a clear referent (e.g., “Okay”). Future research should look more closely at this kind

of sweeping category.

There were significant differences in referential content focused on the child. Ni-

Vanuatu mothers referred significantly more to their infants’ actions; nearly 40% of all the

child-directed speech referred to children’s actions compared to approximately 20% in

Canada. Although I didn’t code for function, I observed that in both cultures, the c-units

that were coded under this category mostly consisted of action requests. Some ni-

Vanuatu interactions almost entirely constituted of action requests. For example, one ni-

Vanuatu mother repeated her request ten times until she received a response, a typical

situation for ni-Vanuatu interactions.

On the other hand, Canadian mothers referred significantly more to their

children’s internal states, such as this continuous excerpt from one mother: “What are

we gonna do now? What do you want to play? Remember that song? Yeah. Do you like

the bee? Do you like baby? Yeah, what does baby do? Baby. Yeah, were you gentle

with baby?”. Overall, by referring to their children’s thoughts, emotions, and preferences,

Canadian mothers draw attention to their children as individuals who have a rich inner

psychological experience. Also, Canadian mothers made references to enduring

personality characteristics in their children (e.g., “You’re so persistent.”). These findings

are consistent with the literature suggesting that Euro-American mothers are high in

mind-mindedness (Meins et al., 2001; Hughes et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2010).

The differences in the use of internal state references became even more

pronounced when I examined them at two levels of complexity: simple and elaborate.

Recall that simple internal state references consisted of phrases that singly referred to

Page 94: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

82

one internal state (e.g., “Do you see it?”), whereas the elaborate internal state form

consisted of those phrases where the mothers referred to either two kinds of internal

states at once (e.g., ‘You want to see mama?’) or when they referred to the child’s

internal state combined with the child’s action (e.g., Can you jump?). There were no

differences in the level at which ni-Vanuatu and Canadian mothers used the simple form.

However, there were remarkable differences in the use of elaborate internal state forms;

whereas Canadian mothers preferred the elaborate over the simple form, ni-Vanuatu

mothers used it sporadically. To illustrate the differences, I present two excerpts. In both

excerpts, the mothers are encouraging their child to sing and dance, but they do so in

different ways in terms of complexity and elaboration.

Vanuatu mother: “Sing a song. Stand up. Stand up and dance. Stand up and

dance. Stand up and dance. Stand up and dance.”

Canadian mother: “We’re gonna do some songs. Can you clap your hands? Can

you stomp your feet? Good boy! What else do we know? How about the wheels on the

bus? What do the windows do? The windows go up and down.”

The use of high levels of elaborate forms is consistent with the literature

suggesting that Euro-American mothers are highly elaborate (e.g., Wang, 2006;

Leichtman et al., 2003). During reminiscing, highly elaborate mothers scaffold their

children into providing more detailed memory reports and elaborate on the relations

between events and children’s internal states. The common interpretation is that, by

doing so, mothers help their infants reflect on the event in ways that highlight what the

event means to the self (e.g., Fivush, Haden, & Reese, 2006). I think that my results

resonate with this interpretation. When Canadian mothers used this form, they typically

combined an internal state referring to the child’s desire or ability with a perception (e.g.,

“Wanna see mama on the camera?”) or an action (e.g., “Want to play Pat-a-Cake or

Ring-around-the-Rosie?”). Using such combinations, mothers readily present their

toddlers with the psychological link between their desire or ability and a subjective

experience or an action. As such, they highlight its personal meaning for the child. In

comparison, take this ni-Vanuatu mother who simply says “Ningi?” to her child. In this

phrase, the mother is offering to play a game called ningi. However, she is only referring

to the game itself and avoids presenting a psychological connection between the game

and her child.

Page 95: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

83

Canadian mothers also used more objectification; however, the differences were

only marginally significant (p = .077). This result may be reflecting the common use of

games where the child or the child’s body part appeared as the object of the dyad’s

attention (i.e., “Where is your nose?”). Some theorists suggested that these games are

important for the emergence of a self-concept (Mitchell, 1993; Tomasello, 1995).

Despite the fact that Canadian mothers referred more to internal states overall,

internal state references were present in both cultures. Differences emerged in the kinds

of internal states mothers focused on. Results suggested that Canadian mothers

consistently chose mind-minded states over body-minded internal states and focused

particularly on desire and ability/volition. This highlights Canadian mothers’ proclivity to

support an independent self in their children. Canadian mothers did not refer to any

internal state indiscriminately. They particularly referred to those internal states where

the child appears as a willful agent who has desires and capabilities/challenges when

acting in the world. On the other hand, ni-Vanuatu mothers predominantly chose body-

minded internal states over mind-minded ones and focused particularly on toddlers’

perceptions. The implications of this preference are less clear. It is possible that frequent

references to body-minded internal states will support bodily self-awareness in toddlers -

speculatively an important skill in Vanuatu.

Finally, I observed low levels of past talk in both cultural groups. My free play

procedure did not elicit much spontaneous past talk for this age group. But even so,

differences were observable; Canadian mothers produced more past talk than ni-

Vanuatu mothers who almost entirely stayed in the present. When Canadian mothers

referred to past events, they referred to recent past events rather than distant ones.

Overall, my results demonstrate that there are striking differences in Canadian

and ni-Vanuatu mothers’ child-directed speech. These differences are consistent with

independent and interdependent cultural ideologies. Mothers from Vancouver were

highly mind-minded and elaborate when they spoke to their toddlers. They often referred

to their toddlers’ internal states with a particular focus on mind-minded internal states

such as desires and abilities. They used elaborate internal state language to emphasize

the subjective value of the experiences or the actions that they referred to in their

toddlers. They also introduced some past talk referring to recent events (e.g., those that

happened earlier during their visit to the lab) and used these instances in order to build a

Page 96: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

84

coherent narrative of the child’s experience. In contrast, ni-Vanuatu mothers were low in

mind-mindedness and were often repetitive. They frequently referred to their children’s

activities through repeated action requests and did not build connections between these

activities and their child’s desires or abilities. When they referred to internal states, they

limited their references to body-minded internal states such as perceptions and

physiological experiences. Finally, they refrained almost entirely from past talk and

focused on children’s immediate activities and sensations. However, note that these

results and interpretations do not reflect the quality of the relationship between Ni-

Vanuatu mothers and their toddlers. Though I did not code for responsiveness or

attachment, I have indeed observed that some Ni-Vanuatu dyads were very harmonious

and affectionate. Their interactions were organized around synchronous singing and

dancing, or routine games involving bodily imitation and coordination rather than

conversational interactions. Future research may look into whether elaboration and use

of internal state language is correlated with positive socio-emotional outcomes in ni-

Vanuatu children, as is the case in Euro-American samples (e.g., Laible & Thompson,

2000; Meins et al., 2001).

This study has several limitations. First of all, the ni-Vanuatu interactions were

translated before analysis. In modern cross-linguistic research, it is increasingly

acknowledged that translation is not an objective transmission process; rather it’s is an

interpretative process affected by the interpreter’s subjective experience of the meaning

as well as their skills in recreating the original meaning in the target language (Temple &

Young, 2004; Esposito, 2001; Xian, 2008). It is possible that, at times, the English

translations reflected my interpreter’s interpretation of the socioemotional context.

Whereas I do not see this as a threat to the validity of the data for the purpose of this

study, her linguistic expertise may have posed a larger problem. As a member of the

community, my translator was an expert in the local culture and language. However, she

did not have the same level of expertise in English. Although she spoke English fluently,

English was her second language. Moreover, she was not familiar with the Canadian

culture as someone who has never left her local community. Therefore, at times, she

may have failed to select the best ways to express the original meaning in the target

language. This may have diminished the quality of the ni-Vanuatu transcriptions,

especially when it comes to translating internal state terms. This being said, in this study,

I focused on identifying the presence or absence of basic referent categories; a measure

Page 97: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

85

that is relatively immune to translations biases. The best way to conduct this study would

have been to train a native speaker of the language to analyze the data in the original

language. However this option was not available to me because as did not have

proficiency in the local language. To my defence, this compromise allowed me to study

an undocumented oral language that we wouldn’t be able to study otherwise.

Other limitations of the study are as follows. I observed the social-interactional

variables during a short semi-structured free play session. Although free play situations

between infants and caregivers occur in various cultures (Keller, 2007), the frequency

and the organization of such interactions may vary. A designated “play time” may have

been experienced as unnatural among the ni-Vanuatu dyads. Future studies should rely

on natural observations to examine the amount of imitation infants typically experience in

each culture. Another limitation of my study is its focus on the development of

individuality in the child. A task for future should be to take a more emic approach and

try to understand how ni-Vanuatu mothers are using language interactions to promote

the development of an interdependent self in their children. Another limitation was that I

had to work with a small sample size which may have limited my ability to detect

statistical differences. Finally, the cultural comparison maybe confounded by rural/urban

or high/low SES differences. Extending these investigations into urbanized ni-Vanuatu

samples or making comparisons between rural communities with and without a history of

formal schooling may be an important next step for future research.

The cross-cultural differences I observed in the child-directed speech are

consistent with the cross-cultural reminiscing literature which shows that Euro-American

mothers are more likely to use internal state language and elaboration whereas Asian

mothers are more likely to take a pragmatic approach during joint reminiscing episodes.

As such, this study extends the existing literature by demonstrating that the cultural

ideologies can also be traced in mothers’ child-directed speech with children as young

as toddlers. The developmental consequences of these differences can be significant for

infants in the age group I studied. Linguistic reference is defined as a communicative

exchange where one partner directs the other’s attention towards a particular object in

the world (Bruner, 1974; Tomasello, 1999). By frequently referring to their toddlers’

internal states, Canadian mothers may be directing their children’s attention towards

themselves and facilitating the emergence of their conceptual self as an object of

thought. Future research should examine whether mothers’ references to internal states

Page 98: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

86

in child-directed speech with toddlers explains the cross-cultural and within culture

differences in the emergence of self-representations. In addition, future research should

look more closely into the use of body-minded internal state references in non-Western

rural societies as these may be fulfilling a special function in the socialization of selfhood

in those cultures.

Page 99: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

87

General Discussion

The overarching goal of this dissertation was to identify the social-interactive

experiences that were linked to infants’ mirror self-recognition (MSR) ability. In study 1, I

examined how a suite of social-interactive behaviors observed during a 10-minute free

play session correlated with MSR in a sample constituting of urban Canadian and rural

ni-Vanuatu infants and their mothers. This study showed that the extent to which

mothers imitated their infants best distinguished who passed the MSR test among ni-

Vanuatu and Canadian infants. In study 2, I examined whether imitation explained within

culture variability in MSR in a culturally diverse urban Canadian sample. Consistent with

the results of study 1, this study showed that maternal imitation predicted self-

recognition among urban Canadian infants along with age. Together, these two studies

suggest that imitation may be a strategy through which Canadian mothers are

scaffolding objective self-awareness skills in their infants. In study 3, I compared how ni-

Vanuatu and Canadian mothers spoke to their infants during a dyadic free play session.

In this study, I used speech as an alternative measure to describe the similarities and

differences in ways the social interactions are organized in these two cultures. Results

showed that whereas ni-Vanuatu mothers referred significantly more to their toddlers’

actions, Canadian mothers referred significantly more to their toddlers’ internal states.

Moreover, Canadian mothers created a narrative where the infant appeared as an

individual who has desires, capabilities, and challenges when acting in the world. These

differences in speech content may be indicative of a pragmatic context that highlights

infants’ agency and may support early self-awareness alongside maternal imitation.

Together, these three studies help us better understand how early social interactions

may be shaped to support an early acquisition of objective self-awareness within the

Western urban sociocultural context. Below I discuss my results in more detail under

three separate titles. Finally, I make recommendations for future research and describe

how we can build a research program dedicated to understanding the ontogeny of

objective self-awareness.

Maternal Imitation Predicts MSR

In the first paper, I argued that the proximal developmental antecedents of objective self-

awareness should be sought within infants’ social interactive experiences over the

Page 100: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

88

second year of life and identified a number of potential predictors. I examined my

hypotheses among a cross-cultural sample consisting of urban Canadian and rural ni-

Vanuatu infants (Study 1) and a culturally diverse urban Canadian sample (Study 2). In

study 1, I observed that Canadian mothers imitated their infants significantly more

compared to ni-Vanuatu mothers during the free play session with toys. In addition,

mothers’ imitation of their infants during this free play session was positively correlated

with MSR in the entire sample. Finally, the regression models pinpointed this maternal

imitation variable as the only predictor of MSR. Similarly, in study 2, parental imitation

predicted urban Canadian infants’ MSR abilities together with infants’ age. Based on

convergent evidence from study 1 and study 2 and based on previous theoretical

(Mitchell, 1993; Moore, 2007; Tomasello, 1995; Tomasello, 1999) and empirical work

(Hart & Fegley, 1994; Kristen-Antonow et al., 2015), I propose that parental imitation

may be scaffolding the development of objective self-awareness during the second year

of life. In order to further examine this possibility, future research should examine the

predictive power of maternal imitation among ni-Vanuatu infants, as well as in various

sociocultural contexts. In addition, longitudinal evidence must be provided in order to

argue that parental imitation is a developmental antecedent of self-recognition.

This is the first study that differentiated between three distinct forms of imitation

(i.e., mother’s imitation of infants, infants’ imitation of mother, and simultaneous

imitation). In both study 1 and study 2, only mothers’ imitation of infants predicted

infants’ MSR ability. This suggests that the identity of the imitator may be important.

Examining this question further will help us understand how precisely imitation facilitates

the development of self-representations. In paper 2, I classified the theoretical

explanations of the mechanisms underlying the effects of imitation into two groups: the

Piagetian approach and the Meadian approach. The Piagetian approach underscores

the importance of perceptual outreach (Guillaume, 1926/1971; Piaget, 1945/1962). On

the other hand, the Meadian approach underscores the tricky ability to step outside of

the first-person experience following Mead’s proposition that objective self-awareness

involves taking the attitudes of other individuals toward oneself (Mead, 1934/1967).

Currently, we do not have enough data to support either framework. I encourage future

researchers to design studies which would help us identify the mechanisms through

which imitation supports the development of self-recognition.

Page 101: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

89

Child-directed Speech Varies across Cultures

The role of language has been prominent in theories of socialization of self

(Schieffelin & Ochs, 1986; Vygotsky, 1934/1962). However, its role in the emergence of

a self-concept has not been studied. Research on language socialization typically focus

on the content of parents’ conversations with their children who are active participants in

these conversations; namely, children of preschool age and older (e.g., Wang, 2001). In

paper 3, I presented striking differences in how Canadian and ni-Vanuatu parents

organized their language interactions with their pre-verbal toddlers. During child-directed

speech, Canadian mothers referred significantly more to toddlers’ internal states

whereas ni-Vanuatu mothers referred significantly more to toddlers’ actions. When

referring to internal states, Canadian mothers referred predominantly to mind-minded

internal states (e.g., desire) whereas ni-Vanuatu mothers predominantly referred to

body-minded internal states (e.g., perception). Moreover, Canadian mothers used more

elaborate forms of language though linking children’s desires and abilities to their acts

and perceptions, whereas ni-Vanuatu mothers did not. Finally, Canadian mothers

produced more references to past events compared to ni-Vanuatu parents who almost

exclusively referred to the present. The developmental consequences of these

differences may be significant for MSR development. By frequently referring to their

toddlers as though they were active agents who have desires and

capabilities/challenges, Canadian mothers may be supporting the emergence of a self-

concept in the toddlers. We know little about the processes that would underlie such an

effect. However, based on a perspective of language as activity (i.e., Carpendale &

Lewis, 2004; Chapman, 1987; Schieffelin & Ochs, 1986; Vygotsky, 1934/1962,

Wittgenstein, 1958), we can argue that for this age group, the processes underlying the

effects of child-directed speech would occur through the pragmatic aspects of the

language interactions. This interpretation calls for an examination of the relationship

between the referential content of child-directed speech, the social/communicative

behaviors (e.g., joint attention, imitation) occurring within the interactions, and the

development of a self-concept in the child. Overall, this study brings attention to

language interactions as an alternative way to measure how parents are organizing their

interactions with their toddlers in ways that will support their individuality.

Page 102: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

90

MSR Is a Cross-culturally Valid Measure of Objective Self-awareness

The striking cross-cultural differences in MSR passing rates are typically

interpreted as an indication of underlying differences in self-developmental trajectories.

However, there is evidence to suspect that the way infants from rural traditional societies

respond to the MSR test reflects differences unrelated to their objective self-awareness

ability (Broesch et al., 2011). In this dissertation, I aimed to gather more evidence

towards the cross-cultural validity of the procedure by directly testing alternative

hypotheses. My findings add to the existing evidence supporting the MSR test’s

adequacy to motivate self-directed behavior in non-WEIRD populations –provided that

the suggested modifications (i.e., training and prompting) are ensured. In addition, I

presented the first evidence suggesting that low passing rates in Vanuatu could not be

attributed to global differences in representational capacity. This result has implications

for not only the cross-cultural validity of the test but also the construct validity of the test.

The fact that ni-Vanuatu infants were successful in using secondary representations for

searching for an invisibly displaced object but not for recognizing their mirror image

suggests that the MSR test measures specifically self-related representations.

However, my findings on infants’ understanding of mirror correspondence were

inconclusive. I hypothesized that if low MSR rates could be attributed to infants’ inability

to use a mirror’s reflective function, then we would expect equally low passing rates in

the mirror mediated behavior (MMB) test among ni-Vanuatu infants. In addition, we

would expect a relationship between infants’ performance in the two tests. Results

showed that MMB passing rates were significantly lower in Vanuatu compared to

Canada. These results do not support, however, the hypothesis that this relative

challenge is responsible for the low passing rates observed among ni-Vanuatu infants.

Ni-Vanuatu infants still performed better on the MMB test compared to the MSR test and

no relationship was observed between infants’ performance on the MSR test and the

MMB test. More research is needed to provide a definitive answer to the question of

whether there is a causal relationship between an understanding of mirror

correspondence and mirror self-recognition, and whether such a relationship could

partially be responsible for cross-cultural differences observed in MSR passing rates.

Based on these results, I recommend caution for future researchers who will study MSR

in traditional societies. Measuring mirror correspondence with improved control tests

Page 103: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

91

and/or providing ample mirror experience will help in ruling out potential difficulties for

infants who do not have the same mirror experience as infants from WEIRD societies.

The Path Forward

Developmental science is largely based on the study of WEIRD populations. The

handful of studies examining non-WEIRD samples, however, indicate that social

cognitive development varies across cultures (Henrich et al., 2010; Kline et al., 2018;

Nielsen et al., 2017). Consistent with this picture, the limited cross-cultural literature I

reviewed suggests that the well-established 18-month-old shift in MSR ability may not

generalize beyond Western urban populations (Keller et al., 2004; Kärtner et al. 2012;

Broesch et al., 2011). Cross-cultural research can fulfil a significant role in understanding

the ontogeny of objective self-awareness by explaining the mechanisms that lead to the

systematic variation in self-developmental outcomes. I hope that this dissertation will

provide cross-cultural researchers with a theoretical framework and a methodological

toolkit to study the developmental antecedents of MSR across different sociocultural

environments.

I think that the first next step in cross-cultural MSR research is to describe the

developmental trajectory of MSR in diverse populations beyond comparing passing rates

at 18- to 24-months range which is the typical age of onset in WEIRD populations. The

second next step is, naturally, to go beyond describing age norms and trying to

understand how infants’ social experiences shape the development of objective self-

awareness within different sociocultural contexts. As a first step towards this goal, I have

designed a new project. Over the summer of 2018, I traveled to Vanuatu and collected

similar data, this time by recruiting 60 18- to 36-month-old ni-Vanuatu infants and their

mothers. This new dataset offers a wide range of variability in MSR abilities, as well as in

imitation levels, which will allow me to test whether imitation and child-directed speech

explain variability in MSR among ni-Vanuatu children.

Finally, I would like to point to the importance of not only moving beyond the

WEIRD populations but also beyond WEIRD tests. Recently, there have been valuable

attempts create measures that capture distinct domains of objective self-knowledge

(e.g., Moore et al., 2007; Ross et al., 2017). Expending our capacity to measure different

Page 104: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

92

aspects of self-awareness across domains and across development (Rochat, 2003) will

allow us to understand the full scope of self-development in infancy.

Page 105: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

93

References

Ahadi, S. A., Rothbart, M. K., & Ye, R. (1993). Children's temperament in the US and China: similarities and differences. European Journal of Personality, 7(5), 359-377. doi: 10.1002/per.2410070506

Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Developmental Psychobiology, 5(4), 297-305. doi:10.1002/dev.420050403

Amsterdam, B. K., & Levitt, M. (1980). Consciousness of self and painful self-consciousness. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 35, 67-83. doi: 10.1080/00797308.1980.11823105

Anderson, J. R. (1986). Mirror-mediated finding of hidden food by monkeys (Macaca Tonkeana and M. Fascicularis). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 100(3), 237-242. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.237

Asendorpf, J. B., Warkentin, V., & Baudonnière, P. (1996). Self-awareness and other-awareness. II: Mirror self-recognition, social contingency awareness, and synchronic imitation. Developmental Psychology, 32(2), 313-321. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.2.313

Adamson, L. B., Bakeman, R., & Deckner, D. F. (2004). The development of symbol‐infused joint engagement. Child Development, 75(4), 1171-1187. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00732.x

Bahrick, L. E., & Watson, J. S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive€ “ visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21(6), 963–973. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.21.6.963

Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1987). Applying observational methods: A systematic view. In J. D.Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 818–854). New York: John Wiley.

Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 1278 – 1289. doi: 10.2307/1129997

Barresi, J., & Moore, C. (1996). Intentional relations and social understanding. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19, 107-122. doi: 10.1017/s0140525x00041790

Bertenthal, B. I., & Fischer, K. W. (1978). Development of self-recognition in the infant. Developmental Psychology, 14(1), 44-50. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.14.1.44

Bornstein, M. (1980). Cross-cultural developmental psychology. In Bornstein, M. (Ed.), Comparative methods in psychology (pp. 231-281). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

Page 106: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

94

Brownell, C. A., Zerwas, S., & Ramani, G. B. (2007). “So Big”: The Development of Body Self-Awareness in Toddlers. Child Development, 78(5), 1426–1440. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01075.x

Broesch, T., Callaghan, T., Henrich, J., Murphy, C., & Rochat, P. (2011). Cultural variations in children’s mirror self-recognition. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42(6), 1018-1029. doi: 10.1177/0022022110381114

Beeghly, M., Bretherton, I., & Mervis, C. B. (1986). Mothers' internal state language to toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 247-260. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1986.tb01016.x

Bretherton, I., & Beeghly, M. (1982). Talking about internal states: the acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Developmental Psychology, 18, 906-921. doi: 10.1037%2F0012-1649.18.6.906

Bruner, J. S. (1974). From communication to language -a psychological perspective. Cognition, 3(13), 255-287. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(74)90012-2

Bruner, J. (1987). Life as narrative. Social Research, 54, 11–32. Retrieved from https://www-jstor-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/stable/40970444?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contents

Campbell, D. T. (1961). The mutual methodological relevance of anthropology and psychology. Psychological Anthropology, 333–352. Retrieved from https://search-ebscohost-com.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login.aspx?direct=true&db=ant&AN=239415&site=ehost-live

Carpendale J. I. M., & C. Lewis, C. (2011). Self constructed in culture. In K. J. de López, & T. G. B. Hansen (Eds.), Development of self in culture (pp. 25-40). Denmark: Aalborg University Press.

Carpendale, J. I. M., & Lewis, C. (2004). Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children’s social understanding within social interaction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(1), 79–151. doi: 10.1017\/s0140525x04000032

Carpenter, E. (1976). The tribal terror of self-awareness. In P. Hockings (Ed.), Principles of visual anthropology, (pp. 451-461). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter. doi:10.1515/9783110221138.479

Chapman, M. (1987). Inner Processes and Outward Criteria: Wittgenstein’s Importance for Psychology. Meaning and the Growth of Understanding, 103–127. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-83023-5_7

Chapman, M. (1991). The epistemic triangle: Operative and communicative components of cognitive competence. In M. Chandler & M. Chapman (Eds.), Criteria for competence: Controversies in the conceptualization and assessment of children¨s abilities (pp. 209-228). Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erbaum Associates, Inc.

Page 107: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

95

Chuang, S. S., & Su, Y. (2009). Do we see eye to eye? Chinese mothers’ and fathers’ parenting beliefs and values for toddlers in Canada and China. Journal of Family Psychology, 23(3), 331–341. doi: 10.1037/a0016015

Chen, X., Hastings, P. D., Rubin, K. H., Chen, H., Cen, G., & Stewart, S. L. (1998). Child-rearing attitudes and behavioral inhibition in Chinese and Canadian toddlers: A cross-cultural study. Developmental Psychology, 34(4), 677–686. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1037/0012-1649.34.4.677

Cooley, C. H. (1964). Human nature and the social order. New York: Schocken Books. (Original work published 1902)

Chandler, M. J., Lalonde, C. E., & Teucher, U. (2004). Culture, continuity, and the limits of narrativity: A comparison of the self-narratives of Native and Non-Native youth. In C. Daiute & C. Lightfoot (Eds.), Narrative analysis: studying the development of individuals in society (pp.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. doi:10.4135/9781412985246.n3.4

Cicchetti, D. V. (1994). Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychological Assessment, 6(4), 284-290. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.6.4.284

Courage, M. L., Edison, S. C., & Howe, M. L. (2004). Variability in the early development of visual self-recognition. Infant Behaviour and Development, 27(4). 509-532. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2004.06.001

Damon, W. & Hart, D. (1982). The development of self-understanding from infancy through adolescence. Child Development, 53(4), 841-864. doi:10.2307/1129122

Darwin, C. (1877). A Biographical Sketch of an Infant. Scientific American, 4(86supp), 1372–1373. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican08251877-1372bsupp

Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 132–139. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.132

Dickie, J. R., & Strader, W. H. (1974). Development of mirror image responses in infancy. Journal of Psychology, 88, 333–337. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1974.9915745

Duval, T. S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York: Academic.

Esposito, N. (2001). From Meaning to Meaning: The Influence of Translation Techniques on Non-English Focus Group Research. Qualitative Health Research, 11(4), 568–579. doi:10.1177/104973201129119217

Page 108: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

96

Feinman, S. (1982). Social referencing in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 28, 445–470. Retrieved from https://search-ebscohost-com.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login.aspx?direct=true&db=eue&AN=507482576&site=ehost-live

Fivush, R., & Nelson, K. (2004). Culture and language in the emergence of autobiographical memory. Psychological Science, 15(9), 573–577. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00722.x

Fivush, R., & Haden, C.A. (Eds.). (2003). Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. doi: 10.4324/9781410607478

Fivush, R., & Haden, C. A. (1997). Narrating and representing experience: Preschoolers’ developing autobiographical accounts. In P. W. van den Broek, P. J. Bauer, & T. Bourg (Eds.), Developmental spans in event comprehension and representation: Bridging fictional and actual events. (pp. 169–198). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=1997-97467-006&site=ehost-live

Fivush, R., Haden, C. A., & Reese, E. (2006). Elaborating on elaboration: Maternal reminiscing style and children’s socioemotional outcome. Child Development, 77(6), 1568–1588. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00960.x

Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., & Target, M. (2007). The parent-infant dyad and the construction of the subjective self. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 48(3-4), 288-328. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01727.x

Gallup, G. G. (1968). Mirror-image stimulation. Psychological Bulletin, 70(6, Pt.1), 782–793. doi:10.1037/h0026777

Gallup, G. (1970). Chimpanzees: Self-recognition. Science, 167, 86-87. doi:10.1126/science.167.3914.86

Gallup, G. G. (1977). Self-recognition in primates: A comparative approach to the bidirectional properties of consciousness. American Psychologist, 32, 329–338. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.32.5.329

Gergely, G. (1994). From self-recognition to theory of mind. In S. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & M. L. Boccia (Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: Developmental perspectives (pp. 51-60). New York, NY US: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511565526

Gesell, A., Thompson, H., & Amatruda, C. S. (1934). Infant behavior: Its genesis and growth. doi:10.1037/11333-000

Page 109: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

97

Gouin-Décarie, T., Pouliot, T., & Poulin-Dubois, D. (1983). Image spéculaire et genèse de la reconnaissance de soi: Une analyse hiérarchique [Mirror image and development of self-recognition: A hierarchical analysis]. Enfance, 35, 99–115. doi: 10.3406/enfan.1983.2805

Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 461. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145221

Gartstein, M. A., Gonzalez, C., Carranza, J. A., Ahadi, S. A., Ye, R., Rothbart, M. K., & Suh Wen, Y. (2006). Studying cross-cultural differences in the development of infant temperament: people’s republic of China, the United States of America, and Spain. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 37(2), 145-161. doi:10.1007/s10578-006-0025-6

Guillaume, P. (1971). Imitation in children (E.P. Halperin). Oxford: U. Chicago Press. (Original work published 1926)

Harkness, S., Super, C. M., Bermúdez, M. R., Moscardino, U., Rha, J.-H., Mavridis, C. J., … Zylicz, P. O. (2010). Parental ethnotheories of children’s learning. In D. F. Lancy, J. Bock, & S. Gaskins (Eds.), The anthropology of learning in childhood. (pp. 65–81). Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2010-03678-004&site=ehost-live

Heyes, C. M. (1994). Social learning in animals: categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 69(2):207-31. doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.1994.tb01506.x

Hart, D., & Fegley, S. (1994). Social imitation and the emergence of a mental model of self. In S. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, M. L. Boccia (Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: Developmental perspectives (pp. 51-60). New York, NY: Cambridge University. doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511565526.011

Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X

Harter, S. (1998). The development of self-representations. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development., Vol. 3, 5th ed. (pp. 553–617). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons Inc. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2005-03132-009&site=ehost-live

Harwood, R. L., Schoelmerich, A., Ventura-Cook, E., Schulze, P. A., & Wilson, S. P. (1996). Culture and class influences on Anglo and Puerto Rican mothers' beliefs regarding long-term socialization goals and child behavior. Child Development, 67(5), 2446-2461. doi:10.1111/j.1467-624.1996.tb01867.x

Page 110: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

98

Hayne, H., & MacDonald, S. (2003). The socialization of autobiographical memory in children and adults: The roles of culture and gender. In R. Fivush & C. A. Haden (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives (pp. 99 – 120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. doi:10.1002/icd.374

Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1996). Introduction. In S. Harkness & C. M. Super (Eds.), Parents’ cultural belief systems: Their origins, expressions, and consequences (pp. 1–23). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Heyes, C. M. (1994). Reflections on self-recognition in primates. Animal Behaviour, 47(4), 909–919. doi:10.1006/anbe.1994.1123

Hughes, C., Devine, R. T., & Wang, Z. (2018). Does parental mind-mindedness account for cross-cultural differences in preschoolers’ theory of mind?. Child Development, 89(4), 1296-1310. doi:10.1111/cdev.12746

James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology (Vol 1). New York, NY: Dover. (Original work published 1890)

Kağıtçıbaşı, C. (1996). Family and human development across cultures: A view from the other side. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Kline, M. A., Shamsudheen, R., & Broesch, T. (2018). Variation is the universal: making cultural evolution work in developmental psychology. Philosophical Transactions Of The Royal Society Of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 373(1743). https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1098/rstb.2017.0059

Keller, H., Borke, J., Staufenbiel, T., Yovsi, R. D., Abels, M., Papaligoura, Z., & ... Su, Y. (2009). Distal and proximal parenting as alternative parenting strategies during infants’ early months of life: A cross-cultural study. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 33(5), 412-420. doi:10.1177/0165025409338441

Keller, H. (2003). Socialization for competence: Cultural models of infancy. Human Development, 46(5), 288–311. doi: 10.1159/000071937

Kärtner, J., Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Yovsi, R. D., & Nandita, C. (2007). Manifestations of autonomy and relatedness in mothers' accounts of their ethnotheories regarding child care across five cultural communities. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(5), 613-628. doi:10.1177/0022022107305242

Kärtner, J., Keller, H., Chaudhary, N., & Yovsi, R. D. (2012). The development of mirror self-recognition in different sociocultural contexts. Monographs of The Society for Research In Child Development, 77(4), 1-101. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-834.2012.00691.x

Keller, H. (2007). Cultures of infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Page 111: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

99

Keller, H., Abels, M., Borke, J., Lamm, B., Su, Y., Wang, Y., & Lo, W. (2007). Socialization environments of chinese and euro-american middle-class babies: parenting behaviors, verbal discourses and ethnotheories. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 31(3), 210-217. doi: 10.1177/0165025407074633

Keller, H., Yovsi, R., Borke, J., Kärtner, J., Jensen, H., & Papaligoura, Z. (2004). Developmental consequences of early parenting experiences: self-recognition and self-regulation in three cultural communities. Child Development, 75(6), 1745-1760. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00814.x

Keller, H., Lamm, B., Abels, M., Yovsi, R., Borke, J., Jensen, H., & ... Chaudhary, N. (2006). Cultural models, socialization goals, and parenting ethnotheories: a multicultural analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37(2), 155-172. doi:10.1177/0022022105284494

Keller, H., Kärtner, J., Borke, J., Yovsi, R., & Kleis, A. (2005). Parenting styles and the development of the categorical self: A longitudinal study on mirror self-recognition in Cameroonian Nso and German families. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 29(6), 496-504. doi: 10.1177/01650250500147485

Kristen-Antonow, S., Sodian, B., Perst, H., & Licata, M. (2015). A longitudinal study of the emerging self from 9 months to the age of 4 years. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 789. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00789

Laible, D. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2000). Mother-child discourse, attachment security, shared positive affect and early conscience development. Child Development, 71(5), 1424-40. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00237

Leichtman, M. D., Wang, Q., & Pillemer, D. B. (2003). Cultural variations in interdependence and autobiographical memory: Lessons from Korea, China, India, and the United States. In R. Fivush & C. A. Haden (Eds.), Autobiographical memory and the construction of a narrative self: Developmental and cultural perspectives. (pp. 73–97). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2003-02703-004&site=ehost-liv

LeVine, R. A. (1977). Child rearing as cultural adaptation. Culture and Infancy, 15–27. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=ant&AN=288999&site=ehost-live

Levine, L. E. (1983). Mine: Self-definition in 2-year-old boys. Developmental Psychology, 19(4), 544–549. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.19.4.544

Lewis, M. (2003). The emergence of consciousness and its role in human development. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1001, 104-133. doi: 10.1196/annals.1279.007

Page 112: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

100

Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self. New York, NY: Plenum Press.

Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2004). Development of self-recognition, personal pronoun use, and pretend play during the 2nd Year. Child Development, 75(6), 1821-1831. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00819.x

Lewis, M., Stanger, C., & Sullivan, M. W. (1989). Deception in 3-year olds. Developmental Psychology, 25, 439–443. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.439

Loban, W. (1976). Language development: Kindergarten through grade twelve. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English.

Liu, Mowei, Chen, Xinyin, Rubin, Kenneth H, Zheng, Shujie, Cui, Liying, Li, Dan, & . . . Wang, Li. (2005). Autonomy vs. connectedness-oriented parenting behaviours in Chinese and Canadian mothers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(6), 489-495. doi: 10.1080/01650250500147063

Loveland, K. A. (1986). Discovering the affordances of a reflecting surface. Developmental Review, 6, 1–24. doi:10.1016/0273-2297(86)90001-8

Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224-253. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224

Martin, J. (2013). Life positioning analysis: An analytic framework for the study of lives and life narratives. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 33(1), 1-17. doi:10.1037/a0025916

Martin, J., Sokol, B. W., & Elfers, T. (2008). Taking and coordinating perspectives: From prereflective interactivity, through reflective intersubjectivity, to meta-reflective sociality. Human Development, 51(5-6), 294-317. doi: 10.1159/000170892

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5, 100–122. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.100

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Wainwright, R., Das Gupta, M., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2002). Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Development, 73(6), 1715–26. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00501

McAdams, D. P. (2001). The psychology of life stories. Review of General Psychology, 5, 100–122. doi:10.1037/1089-2680.5.2.100

Morikawa, H., Shand, N., & Kosawa, Y. (1988). Maternal speech to prelingual infants in Japan and the United States: relationships among functions, forms and referents. Journal of Child Language, 15(2), 237-256. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900012356

Page 113: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

101

Mead, G. H. (1967). Mind, self, and society from the standpoint of a social behaviourist. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1934)

Meins, E., Fernyhough, C., Fradley, E., & Tuckey, M. (2001). Rethinking maternal sensitivity: mothers' comments on infants' mental processes predict security of attachment at 12 months. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(5), 637-648. doi: 10.1017/s0021963001007302

Meins, E. (1997). Essays in developmental psychology. Security of attachment and the social development of cognition. Hove, England: Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis.

Medina, F. S., Taylor, A. H., Hunt, G. R., & Gray, R. D. (2011). New Caledonian crows’ responses to mirrors. Animal Behaviour, 82(5), 981-993. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.033

Mitchell, R. W. (1993). Mental models of mirror-self-recognition: Two theories. New Ideas in Psychology, 11(3), 295-325. doi:10.1016/0732-118x(93)90002-u

Moore, C. (2007). Understanding self and others in the second year. In C. A. Brownell & C. B. Kopp (Eds.), Socioemotional development in the toddler years: Transitions and transformations. (pp. 43–65). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2007-15136-002&site=ehost-live

Moore, C., Mealiea, J., Garon, N., & Povinelli, D. J. (2007). The development of body self-awareness. Infancy, 11(2), 157-174. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2007.tb00220

Morikawa, H., Shand, N., & Kosawa, Y. (1988). Maternal speech to prelingual infants in Japan and the United States: Relationships among functions, forms and referents. Journal of Child Language, 15(2), 237-256. doi: 10.1017/s0305000900012356

Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1(1), 35-59. doi:10.1080/09515088808572924

Neisser, U. (1991). Two perceptually given aspects of the self and their development. Developmental Review, 11(3), 197–209. doi: 10.1016/0273-2297(91)90009-d

Nielsen, M., & Dissanayake, C. (2004). Pretend play, mirror self-recognition and imitation: A longitudinal investigation through the second year. Infant Behavior and Development, 27(3), 342-365. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2003.12.006

Nielsen, M., Dissanayake, C., & Kashima, Y. (2003) A longitudinal investigation of self–other discrimination and the emergence of mirror self-recognition. Infant Behaviour and Development, 26(2), 213-226. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(03)00018-3

Page 114: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

102

Nielsen, M., Haun, D., Kärtner, J., & Legare, C.H. (2017). The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 31-38. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.04.017

Nielsen, M., Suddendorf, T., & Slaughter, V. (2006). Mirror self-recognition beyond the face. Child Development, 77(1), 176-185. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00863.x

Parker S. T. & Mitchell, R. W. (1994). Evolving self-awareness. In S. Parker, R. W. Mitchell, & M. L. Boccia (Eds.), Self-awareness in animals and humans: Developmental perspectives (pp. 51-60). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511565526.029

Penman, R., Cross, T., Milgrom-Friedman, J., & Meares, R. (1983). Mothers' speech to prelingual infants: A pragmatic analysis. Journal of Child Language, 10(1), 17-34. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900005109

Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. London: MIT Press.

Piaget, J. (1954). The development of causality. In The construction of reality in the child (M. Cook, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. (Original work published 1937)

Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood (C. Cattegno & F. M. Hodgson, Trans.). New York, NY: Norton & Company Inc. (Original work published 1945)

Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic epistemology (E. Duckworth, Trans.). New York, NY: Columbia University Press. (Original work published 1950)

Piaget, J. (1977). The origin of intelligence in the child. Harmondsworth, England: Penguin. (Original work published 1953)

Pipp, S., Fischer, K. W., & Jennings, S. (1987). Acquisition of self and mother knowledge in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 86–96. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.86

Povinelli, D. J. (1989). Failure to find self-recognition in Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) in contrast to their use of mirror cues to discover hidden food. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103, 122-131. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.103.2.122

Povinelli, D. J., Landau, K. R., & Perilloux, H. K. (1996). Self-recognition in young children using delayed versus live feedback: Evidence of a developmental asynchrony. Child Development, 67, 1540-1554.

Priel, B., & de Schonen, S. (1986). Self-recognition: A study of a population without mirrors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41(2), 237-250. doi: 10.1016/0022-0965(86)90038-X

Page 115: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

103

Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M.A., & Rothbart, M. K, (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 29 (3), 386-401.

Putnam, S. P., Jacobs, J., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2010, March). Development and assessment of short and very short forms of the Early Childhood Behavior Questionnaire. Poster session presented at International Conference on Infant Studies, Baltimore, MD.

Qi, W., & Conway, M. A. (2004). The stories we keep: autobiographical memory in American and Chinese middle-aged adults. Journal of Personality, 72(5), 911-938. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00285.x

Rochat, P. (2001). Origins of self-concept. In G. Bremner, & A. Fogel, G. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development (pp. 191-212). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

Rochat, P. (2003). Five levels of self-awareness as they unfold early in life. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 12(4), 717-731. doi:10.1016/S1053-8100(03)00081-3.

Rochat, P. (2009). Others in mind: Social origins of self-consciousness. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Rochat, P. (2015). Layers of awareness in development. Developmental Review, 38, 122-145. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2015.07.009

Rochat, P., Broesch, T. & Jayne, K. (2012). Social awareness and early self recognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1491-1497. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.04.007

Rochat, P. & Zahavi, D. (2011). The uncanny mirror: A re-framing of mirror self-experience. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(2), 204-213. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.007

Rochat, P. (2001). Origins of self-concept. In G. Bremner, & A. Fogel (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of infant development (pp. 191-212). Malden: Blackwell Publishing. doi: 10.1002/9780470996348.ch7

Rochat, P. (1997). Early development of the ecological self. In C. E. Dent-Read & P. E. Zukow-Goldring (Eds.), Evolving explanations of development: Ecological approaches to organism–environment systems (pp. 91–121). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi: 10.1037/10265-003

Rochat, P., & Hespos, S. J. (1997). Differential rooting response by neonates: Evidence for an early sense of self. Early Development & Parenting, 6(3–4), 105–112. https://doi-org.proxy.lib.sfu.ca/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199709/12)6:3/4<105::AID-EDP150>3.0.CO;2-U

Page 116: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

104

Rochat, P., Querido, J. G., & Striano, T. (1999). Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of protoconversation in early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 35(4), 950–957. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.950

Ross, J., Yilmaz, M., Dale, R., Cassidy, R., Yildirim, I. & Suzanne Zeedyk, M. (2017). Cultural differences in self-recognition: The early development of autonomous and related selves? Developmental Science, 20, 1-13. doi:10.1111/desc.12387

Sherrod, K. B., Crawley, S., Petersen, G., & Bennett, P. (1978). Maternal Language to Prelinguistic Infants: Semantic Aspects. Infant Behavior and Development, 1, 335-45.

Statistics Canada (2016). Focus on Geography Series, 2016 Census (Catalogue number 98-404-X2016001). Retrieved from https://www12.statcan.gc.ca/census-recensement/2016/as-sa/fogs-spg/Facts-CSD-Eng.cfm?TOPIC=5&LANG=Eng&GK=CSD&GC=5915022

Sheskin, D. J. (2004). Handbook of parametric and nonparametric statistical procedures (3rd ed.). Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC.

Schieffelin, B. B., & Ochs, E. (1986). Language socialization. Annual Review of Anthropology, 15(1), 163-191. doi:10.1146/annurev.an.15.100186.001115

Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Pearson/Allyn & Bacon.

Temple, B., & Young, A. (2004). Qualitative Research and Translation Dilemmas. Qualitative Research, 4(2), 161–178. doi:10.1177/1468794104044430

Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. (pp. 103–130). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=1995-97586-005&site=ehost-live

Trevarthen, C. (1980). The foundations of intersubjectivity: Developments of interpersonal and cooperative understanding in infants. In D. R. Olson (Ed.), The social foundations of language and thought: Essays in honor of Jerome S. Bruner (pp. 316-342). New York, NY: Norton. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=mzh&AN=1980303234&site=ehost-live

Triandis, H. C.(2001). Individualism-collectivism and personality. Journal of Personality, 69(6), 907– 924. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.696169

Twinn, S. (1997). An exploratory study examining the influence of translation on the validity and reliability of qualitative data in nursing research. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 26(2), 418–423. doi:10.1046/j.1365-2648.1997.1997026418.x

Page 117: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

105

Uzgiris, I. C., & Hunt, J. V. (1975). Assessment in infancy: Ordinal scales of psychological development. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois Press.

Vanuatu National Statistics Office. (2009). 2009 National Population and Housing Census Analytical Report (Vol. 2). Retrieved from the Vanuatu National Statistics Office website: https://vnso.gov.vu/index.php/component/advlisting/?view=download&fileId=1995

Vygotsky, L. (1962) Thought and language (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Trans.). Cambridge, Massacusetts: The M.I.T. Press (Original work published 1934)

Vyt, A. (2001). Processes of visual self-recognition in infants: Experimental induction of “mirror” experiences via video self-image presentation. Infant and Child Development, 10(4), 173–187. doi: 10.1002/icd.277

Wang, Q., Doan, S. N., & Song, Q. (2010). Talking about internal states in mother-child reminiscing influences children's self-representations: A cross-cultural study. Cognitive Development, 25(4), 380-393. doi: 10.1016\/j.cogdev.2010.08.007

Wang, Q. (2001). 'Did you have fun?' American and Chinese mother-child conversations about shared emotional experiences. Cognitive Development, 16, 693–715. doi: 10.1016/s0885-2014(01)00055-7

Wang, Q., Leichtman, M. D., & Davies, K. (2000). Sharing memories and telling stories: American and Chinese mothers and their 3-year-olds. Memory, 8(3), 159 – 177. doi:10.1080/096582100387588

Wang, Q., & Fivush, R. (2005). Mother–child conversations of emotionally salient events: Exploring the functions of emotional reminiscing in European American and Chinese Families. Social Development, 14(3), 473–495. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005.00312.x

Wang, Q. (2006). Relations of maternal style and child self-concept to autobiographical memories in Chinese, Chinese immigrant, and European American 3-year-olds. Child Development, 77(6), 1794–1809. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00974.x

Wang, Q. (2013). The cultured self and remembering. In P. J. Bauer & R. Fivush (Eds.), The Wiley handbook on the development of children’s memory (Vols. 1-3, pp. 605–625). Chichester, West Sussex: Wiley-Blackwell.

Wang, Q., & Conway, M. A. (2004). The stories we keep: Autobiographical memory in American and Chinese middle-aged adults. Journal of Personality, 72(5), 911-938. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00285.x

Page 118: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

106

Welch-Ross, M. (2001). Personalizing the temporally extended self: Evaluative self-awareness and the development of autobiographical memory. In C. Moore & K. Lemmon (Eds.), The self in time: Developmental perspectives. (pp. 97–120). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers. Retrieved from http://proxy.lib.sfu.ca/login?url=https://search.ebscohost.com/login.aspx?direct=true&db=psyh&AN=2001-06248-005&site=ehost-live

Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophical investigations / tr. by G. E. M. Anscombe. (2d ed.).

Whiting, B. B. (1963). Six cultures: Studies of child rearing. Oxford, England: Wiley

Xian, H. (2008). Lost in translation? Language, culture and the roles of translator in

cross‐cultural management research. Qualitative Research in Organizations and Management: An International Journal, 3(3), 231–245. doi:10.1108/17465640810920304

Zahavi, D., & Rochat, P. (2015). Empathy≠sharing: Perspectives from phenomenology and developmental psychology. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 36, 543-553. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.05.008

Page 119: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

107

Appendix A. Supplementary Material on Study 1

Section A.1. Pictures of Testing Materials

Figure A.1. Picture illustrating the typical setting of testing in Vanuatu. This picture was taken during the “familiarization with the mirror” phase. Note: Participants’ faces have been blurred. Participants gave consent to be videotaped/photographed and granted permission to the researcher for the use of images in educational/academic settings.

Page 120: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

108

Figure A.2. Picture illustrating the typical setting of testing in the laboratory in Canada. This picture was taken during the Invisible Displacements test. Note: Participants’ faces have been blurred. Participants gave consent to be videotaped/photographed and granted permission to the researcher for the use of images in educational/academic settings.

Page 121: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

109

Figure A.3. The toys used during the free play session with toys for Study 1 and Study 2

Page 122: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

110

Figure A.4. The finger puppet used during the MMB test for Study 1

Page 123: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

111

Section A.2. Information on contingency tables used to compare imitation levels between cultures

I used contingency tables to compare imitation levels between cultures. When

comparing joint attention levels, I used a rate score representing the duration (in

seconds) of joint attention dyads shared per minute, in order to deal with slight

differences in total duration of observation that was available for each dyad. However,

for imitation, contingency tables were deemed as a more suitable strategy due to the

nature of the data. The coding system for imitation produced count data. The values

obtained from each dyad represented the total number of 3-second segments where a

certain type of imitation behavior was observed. Each free play session (typically 5

minutes) was divided into 3-second segments (typically 100 segments). Each 3-second

segment received either an absent code, or one of the imitation codes (i.e. child

imitation, mother imitation, simultaneous imitation). For example, if a dyad’s free play

session lasted for four minutes and 57 seconds, they had 99 3-second segments. If,

during this duration, the mother imitated their infant twice, and the child imitated their

mother once, the counts were distributed as follows: 97 (no imitation), 2 (maternal

imitation), 1(child imitation). In the 2x2 contingency tables, columns represented the total

frequency of 3-second segments where a certain imitation was observed (and was not

observed) whereas rows represented research sites (i.e., Vanuatu and Canada). In other

words, each cell represented the total number 3-second segments where the target code

was observed among all the dyads from either Canada or Vanuatu. Using Fisher’s exact

tests, I compared the proportion of each type of imitation was observed in Canada and

Vanuatu. Table A.1. depicts the distribution of maternal imitation codes observed during

the free play session with toys. Table A.2. depicts the distribution of child imitation codes

observed during the free play session with toys. Table A.3. depicts the distribution of

maternal imitation codes observed during the free play session without toys. Table A.4.

depicts the distribution of child imitation codes observed during the free play session

without toys. Finally, Table A.5. depicts the distribution of simultaneous imitation codes

observed during the free play session without toys.

Page 124: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

112

Table A.1. Comparing maternal imitation during the free play session with toys

Maternal imitation (# of 3-sec segments)

Other (# of 3-sec segments)

Canada 37 2399 Vanuatu 11 2473

Table A.2. Comparing child imitation during the free play session with toys

Child imitation (# of 3-sec segments)

Other (# of 3-sec segments)

Canada 60 2376 Vanuatu 76 2408

Table A.3. Comparing maternal imitation during the free play session without toys

Mother imitation (# of 3-sec segments)

Other (# of 3-sec segments)

Canada 25 1675 Vanuatu 13 1771

Table A.4. Comparing child imitation during the free play session without toys

Child imitation (# of 3-sec segments)

Other (# of 3-sec segments)

Canada 12 1688 Vanuatu 37 1747

Table A.5. Comparing simultaneous imitation during the free play session without toys

Simultaneous imitation (# of 3-sec segments)

Other (# of 3-sec segments)

Canada 134 1566 Vanuatu 234 1550

Page 125: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

113

Appendix B. Supplementary Material on Study 2

Section B.1. Comparing mother-infant and father-infant dyads

I compared weather mothers and fathers imitated their toddlers to the same

extent, using Fisher’s exact tests. No significant differences were found (p = .48). I

examined whether children’ ECBQ scores varied between mother and father-reports

using independent samples t-tests. Results showed that fathers and mothers evaluated

their children’ temperamental characteristics similarly for negative affectivity, surgency,

and effortful control, t(1, 47) = .12, p = .903, t(1, 47) = .88, p = .385, and t(1, 47) = -.14, p

= .889, respectively.

Section B.2. Procedural differences with regards to the number of visits

Typically, children were tested in one single visit (n = 37). The details of the

standard procedure are described in the methods section. However, a subsample of the

English-speaking toddlers completed the testing in two visits (n = 13). These participants

were already scheduled to take the MSR test as part of a study related to prosocial

development. So they were called in the lab to participate only in the free play session

and complete the ECBQ questionnaire before their originally scheduled visit. In their next

visit to the lab, toddlers played a game related to sharing food treats with a stuffed

animal before participating in the MSR task. The free play session and later MSR task

were administered by the same female experimenter, and the MSR procedure was the

same for both groups of children (single visit, two visits). Average time between the two

visits was 2.28 months (SD = .66). A Fisher’s exact test showed that there was no

significant association between receiving the MSR test on a separate visit and MSR

performance (p = .211).

Page 126: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

114

Section B.3. Procedural differences with regards to parents’ gender

Typically, either the mother or the father was paired with the infant during testing.

However, for a small number of dyads (n = 4), participating parent varied between tasks.

In 2 cases, mothers were present during the first visit (they participated in free play and

filled out the ECBQ) whereas fathers were present during the second visit when children

received the MSR test. In one case where the procedure was completed in a single visit,

the mother was present during both the MSR test and the free play session but the

ECBQ was completed by the father. Finally, in one other case, the father was present

during both the MSR test and the free play session but the ECBQ was completed by the

mother. Overall, nine children were paired with their fathers during the free play session

as opposed to 42 children who were paired with their mothers. Eleven children received

the MSR test in the presence of their fathers as opposed to 40 children who received the

test in the presence of their mothers. Finally, nine children’s ECBQ questionnaires were

filled out by their fathers as opposed to 41 children whose questionnaire was filled out by

their mothers.

Section B.4. Procedural differences with regards to the identity of the experimenter

Typically, Cantonese-speaking participants were tested by one Cantonese-

speaking research assistant and English-speaking participants were tested by one

English-speaking research assistant. However, as exception, two dyads among the

Cantonese-speaking group were tested in English by the English-speaking

experimenter. These two dyads were initially classified in the English-speaking group

based on prior information provided by the parents and therefore scheduled to be tested

by an English-speaking experimenter. However, they were later moved to the

Cantonese-speaking group due to changes in daily language exposure within the family.

In addition, a third female research assistant who was fluent in both languages tested

two of the Cantonese-speaking dyads in Cantonese and two of the English-speaking

dyads in English. A Fisher’s exact test confirmed that there was no significant

relationship between the identity of the experimenter and the passing rates, suggesting

that no experimenter effect is present (p = .240).

Page 127: Explaining Individual and Cross-cultural Differences in Mirror ...summit.sfu.ca/system/files/iritems1/19203/etd20169.pdftheir toddlers’ mind-minded internal states (e.g., desire),

115

Section B.5. Differences in children’s mean age due to procedural differences

Because 13 children completed the procedure in two separate visits, their ages

varied between the visits. In the methods section, I reported the average toddler age

based on the age at which toddlers gave their first visit to the lab for participating in the

free play session. No differences were present in this age between cultural groups, t(1,

49) = -.66, p = .39. However, when the comparison was made based on the age at

which children received the MSR test, I found a significant difference between the

cultural groups. A t-test comparing children’s mean ages at the time of MSR indicated

that English-speaking toddlers were significantly older than the Cantonese-speaking

toddlers, t(1, 49) = 2.42, p = .02). While the Cantonese-speaking toddlers were 17.93

months (range = 16.53 – 21.4, SD = 1.21), English-speaking toddlers were 18.65

months (range = 17.23 – 19.93, SD = .77); 2 weeks older than the Cantonese-speaking

children.