understanding dyspnea as a complex individual experience

6
Maturitas 76 (2013) 45–50 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Maturitas journa l h om epa ge: www.elsevier.com/locate/maturitas Review Understanding dyspnea as a complex individual experience Anja Hayen , Mari Herigstad, Kyle T.S. Pattinson Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX3 9DU, United Kingdom a r t i c l e i n f o Article history: Received 29 May 2013 Accepted 5 June 2013 Keywords: Dyspnea Psychology Neuroimaging Gender Obesity Aging a b s t r a c t Dyspnea is the highly threatening experience of breathlessness experienced by patients with diverse pathologies, including respiratory, cardiovascular, and neuromuscular diseases, cancer and panic dis- order. This debilitating symptom is especially prominent in the elderly and the obese, two growing populations in the Western world. It has further been found that women suffer more strongly from dyspnea than men. Despite optimization of disease-specific treatments, dyspnea is often inadequately treated. The immense burden faced by patients, families and the healthcare system makes improving management of chronic dyspnea a priority. Dyspnea is a multidimensional sensation that encompasses an array of unpleasant respiratory sensa- tions that vary according to underlying cause and patient characteristics. Biopsychological factors beyond disease pathology exacerbate the perception of dyspnea, increase symptom severity and reduce quality of life. Psychological state (especially comorbid anxiety and depression), hormone status, gender, body weight (obesity) and general fitness level are particularly important. Neuroimaging has started to uncover the neural mechanisms involved in the processing of sensory and affective components of dyspnea. Awareness of biopsychological factors beyond pathology is essential for diagnosis and treatment of dyspnea. Increasing understanding the interactions between biopsychological factors and dyspnea per- ception will enhance the development of symptomatic treatments that specifically address each patient’s most pressing needs at a specific stage in life. Future neuroimaging research can provide objective mark- ers to fully understand the role of biopsychological factors in the perception of dyspnea in the hope of uncovering target areas for pharmacologic and non-pharmacologic therapy. © 2013 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Contents 1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45 2. Perception of dyspnea . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 3. The impact of emotions on dyspnea perception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 46 4. Dyspnea and cognitive processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 47 5. Challenges for dyspnea management . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48 6. Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Contributors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Competing interest . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Funding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 Provenance and peer review . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49 This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Com- mons Attribution-NonCommercial-No Derivative Works License, which permits non-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 1865 234544; fax: +44 1865 234072. E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Hayen). 1. Introduction Dyspnea is the highly distressing experience of breathless- ness that makes simple everyday activities like playing with the grandchildren, going to the shops or simply getting dressed in the morning a major challenge. This chronic debilitating burden affects an increasingly large group of patients with respiratory diseases 0378-5122/$ see front matter © 2013 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.maturitas.2013.06.005

Upload: reading

Post on 24-Apr-2023

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

R

U

AN

ARA

KDPNGOA

C

mno

0h

Maturitas 76 (2013) 45– 50

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Maturitas

journa l h om epa ge: www.elsev ier .com/ locate /matur i tas

eview

nderstanding dyspnea as a complex individual experience�

nja Hayen ∗, Mari Herigstad, Kyle T.S. Pattinsonuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, John Radcliffe Hospital, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX3 9DU, United Kingdom

a r t i c l e i n f o

rticle history:eceived 29 May 2013ccepted 5 June 2013

eywords:yspneasychologyeuroimagingenderbesityging

a b s t r a c t

Dyspnea is the highly threatening experience of breathlessness experienced by patients with diversepathologies, including respiratory, cardiovascular, and neuromuscular diseases, cancer and panic dis-order. This debilitating symptom is especially prominent in the elderly and the obese, two growingpopulations in the Western world. It has further been found that women suffer more strongly fromdyspnea than men. Despite optimization of disease-specific treatments, dyspnea is often inadequatelytreated. The immense burden faced by patients, families and the healthcare system makes improvingmanagement of chronic dyspnea a priority.

Dyspnea is a multidimensional sensation that encompasses an array of unpleasant respiratory sensa-tions that vary according to underlying cause and patient characteristics. Biopsychological factors beyonddisease pathology exacerbate the perception of dyspnea, increase symptom severity and reduce qualityof life. Psychological state (especially comorbid anxiety and depression), hormone status, gender, bodyweight (obesity) and general fitness level are particularly important. Neuroimaging has started to uncoverthe neural mechanisms involved in the processing of sensory and affective components of dyspnea.

Awareness of biopsychological factors beyond pathology is essential for diagnosis and treatment ofdyspnea. Increasing understanding the interactions between biopsychological factors and dyspnea per-ception will enhance the development of symptomatic treatments that specifically address each patient’smost pressing needs at a specific stage in life. Future neuroimaging research can provide objective mark-ers to fully understand the role of biopsychological factors in the perception of dyspnea in the hope ofuncovering target areas for pharmacologic and non-pharmacologic therapy.

© 2013 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

ontents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 452. Perception of dyspnea . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 463. The impact of emotions on dyspnea perception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 464. Dyspnea and cognitive processing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 475. Challenges for dyspnea management . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 486. Conclusion. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49

Contributors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49Competing interest . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49Funding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49Provenance and peer review . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 49

� This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Com-ons Attribution-NonCommercial-No Derivative Works License, which permits

on-commercial use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided theriginal author and source are credited.∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +44 1865 234544; fax: +44 1865 234072.

E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Hayen).

378-5122/$ – see front matter © 2013 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All righttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.maturitas.2013.06.005

1. Introduction

Dyspnea is the highly distressing experience of breathless-ness that makes simple everyday activities like playing with the

grandchildren, going to the shops or simply getting dressed in themorning a major challenge. This chronic debilitating burden affectsan increasingly large group of patients with respiratory diseases

ts reserved.

4 turitas 76 (2013) 45– 50

lhdwflFbwo£

tefastaierta

mmbpmapatonapio

2

obpampdtpub

s([cr[hio

Fig. 1. Dyspnea is a multidimensional set of experiences that is closely linked to

6 A. Hayen et al. / Ma

ike chronic obstructive pulmonary disease (COPD) and asthma,eart failure and terminal cancer [1] and is a main symptom of panicisorder [2]. Dyspnea is a strong predictor of mortality in patientsith COPD and heart failure and amongst the most common causes

or emergency department visits [3,4]. Due to its chronicity, costlyong-term treatment of dyspnea is necessary in many patients.or example, the annual direct treatment costs of COPD, the fifthiggest killer disease in the UK with over 30,000 annual deaths,ere estimated at £820 per patient with an additional £820 of sec-

ndary economic costs, accumulating to a total societal cost of over2.1 billion [5]. Similar figures are reported from the USA [6].

Dyspnea itself often influences the course of disease and symp-oms often persist after medical treatment options have beenxhausted [7]. Dyspnea can lead to early cessation of exercise, andear of dyspnea might discourage patients from engaging in dailyctivities, resulting in reduced cardiovascular fitness and muscletrength, which again increase the feeling of dyspnea, thus ini-iating and perpetuating a spiral of decline [3]. Patients, familynd friends endure great emotional suffering when facing loss ofndependence and increasing physical distress [8]. Many patients,specially those who have smoked, are obese or do not engage inegular exercise, feel that they have brought their symptoms uponhemselves, which makes them feel guilty and less likely to seekppropriate medical help [9].

This review will highlight variations in perception and treat-ent success of dyspnea that present challenges for dyspneaanagement. Evidence from clinical and laboratory studies will

e reviewed to show how emotions affect the perception of dys-nea and how understanding the neural mechanisms behind theseodulations can aid dyspnea management. The challenges of man-

ging dyspnea in older people, obese people and women, threeopulations particularly vulnerable to dyspnea, will be highlightednd we will suggest how future research can help to understandhe neural mechanisms involved in the perception of dyspnea inrder to better target treatment. Increased understanding of theeural mechanisms behind dyspnea will allow us to individuallyddress the most important aspects of dyspnea suffering in eachatient. While research on the neural mechanisms of dyspnea is still

n its infancy, it can reveal important directions for improvementsf dyspnea therapy and future research.

. Perception of dyspnea

Dyspnea is not one experience, but encompasses a whole rangef sensations (e.g. air hunger, feeling of increased effort, rapidreathing) that are highly subjective. Individual variation betweenatients in dyspnea perception and resulting impairment is largend reaches beyond the underlying cause [10]. Treatment of the pri-ary cause of dyspnea is essential, but despite optimum treatment,

atients often continue to suffer from dyspnea and the associatedecrease in quality of life [7]. In conjunction with disease-specificreatments, symptomatic treatments can help to manage dys-nea. To improve and optimally target these treatments, a betternderstanding of the mechanisms underlying dyspnea is necessaryecause “one size does not fit all”.

One model of dyspnea perception describes a primary sen-ory component (intensity) and a primary affective componentunpleasantness) [11]. These components can vary independently12]. These primary components are followed by a secondary, moreognitive affective component that leads to long-term emotionalesponses (suffering) that affect future dyspnea-related behavior

11]. Dyspnea is a subjective experience and while animal modelsave given us insight into mechanisms behind specific pathologies,

t is difficult to derive information about the subjective experiencef dyspnea from animal models; these models are ill equipped

almost every aspect of a patient’s physiological and psychological state. The brain isthe main relay station integrating these complex internal and external experienceswith physiological respiratory signals.

to study the mechanisms underlying dyspnea perception. Muchresearch on dyspnea mechanisms has hence been performed inhealthy individuals exposed to laboratory dyspnea. Replicating theemotional component of dyspnea in a laboratory environment isdifficult as laboratory dyspnea does not cause the existential fearsdyspnea sufferers encounter in daily life, hence patient studies willbe necessary in order to fully comprehend all aspects of dyspnea.

Neuroimaging research has confirmed the important role ofemotional processing in the perception of dyspnea. There is evi-dence for a common emotion-related human brain network thatunderlies the perception of aversive bodily sensations such as dys-pnea and pain [13]. When respiratory stimuli are applied to healthyvolunteers in a non-threatening laboratory context, they primar-ily activate somatomotor areas [14]. When laboratory dyspnea isevoked, the limbic system, an array of brain structures involved inemotional processing, and paralimbic areas like the insula (impli-cated in interoception, emotion and cognition) are activated [15].This research hints at different neuronal networks being respon-sible for the different aspects of dyspnea perception, but futureresearch in clinical populations is necessary to disentangle the neu-ronal processes implicated in the sensory and affective processingof chronic dyspnea.

3. The impact of emotions on dyspnea perception

Based on the concept of total pain proposed by Dame CicerlySaunders in the 1960s, a model of total dyspnea has highlightedthe vicious circle in which dyspnea affects and is affected by physi-cal, psychological, spiritual and social aspects of a patient’s life [16].While an array of potential modulators for the perception of dys-pnea is presented in Fig. 1, this review will focus on the impact ofnegative emotions, and the challenges that age, obesity and genderpresent to dyspnea management and research.

An individual’s emotional disposition, current mood, level ofgeneral anxiety and anticipation of dyspnea, as well as the increasedattention paid to one’s respiration can all influence how dyspnea

is perceived. Depression and anxiety are two major comorbiditiesof respiratory disease (e.g. approx. 40% of patients with COPD suf-fer from depression [17] and approx. 30% have comorbid anxietydisorders [18]) and have been shown to almost double mortality

turita

r2b

p(pparwscibeu

aptpcshrsacetaslutpt

cpstsaeaiataphdihsehpgopdhd

A. Hayen et al. / Ma

ates, diminish social and physical functioning by approximately5%, increase frequency of hospitalization by 10% and length of stayy approximately 3 days [19].

Emotions are powerful modulators of the perception of dys-nea. This has been shown in asthmatics, where strong emotionsboth positive and negative) decrease pulmonary function, butositive emotions during a rollercoaster ride led to reduced dys-nea besides lower lung function [20]. High negative emotionality,s present in depression, was shown to increase the strength ofeported dyspnea in response to laboratory stimuli in individualsith normal lung function [21] and is associated with increased

ymptom reporting in patients with respiratory disease [22], espe-ially when sensations are ambiguous or low in intensity [3]. Theres evidence that dyspnea improvement during pulmonary reha-ilitation is correlated with a decrease in experience of negativemotions [23], but the driving force behind this interaction isnknown.

Neuroimaging can be used to identify specific neuronal mech-nisms of respiratory disease that can help us to understand therocessing of dyspnea in particular conditions. Evidence from func-ional magnetic resonance imaging (FMRI) suggests that emotionalrocessing might be altered in asthma. The anterior cingulateortex and the insula, both implicated in the regulation of theensitivity of the response to respiratory stimuli [24], might beyperresponsive in individuals with asthma and other stress-elated conditions [25], leading to a perception of increasedeverity of respiratory stimuli, increased use of medication [26]nd increased frequency of emergency hospital visits. A weak per-eption of dyspnea in asthmatics, as e.g. occurs when positivemotion is experienced, can lead to a dangerous lack of necessaryreatment [20]. Dyspnea perception might vary between patientsnd within the same patient at different times, which might haveevere consequences. Neuroimaging might be able to provide a bio-ogical marker for the subjective perception of dyspnea. A betternderstanding of the neuronal mechanisms involved in the mul-idimensional perception of respiratory symptoms can highlightarticularly vulnerable patients and dangerous situations in ordero minimize the risks associated with over- and under-treatment.

Besides the strong negative impact of emotions on dyspnea per-eption, few studies have looked at the specific contributions ofarticular emotions on the perception of dyspnea. Much researchtill needs to be done to fully understand the impact of emo-ions on the perception of dyspnea. The following section willummarize knowledge about the intricate relationship betweennxiety and dyspnea. In the context of dyspnea, we divide anxi-ty into general anxiety and anxiety relating directly to breathingnd breathlessness (dyspnea-specific anxiety). High general anx-ety increases the perception of dyspnea in patients [27] and isssociated with decreased quality of life [28]. High anxiety inurn induces maladaptive breathing that intensifies the unpleas-nt sensations elicited by respiratory stimuli [29], generating aositive-feedback loop. Dyspnea-related anxiety was shown toave a mediating effect on the relationship between anxiety andyspnea in patients [30] and increased perception of dyspnea

n response to respiratory stimuli in healthy volunteers, whileigh general anxiety did not increase dyspnea perception in theame experiment [29]. The importance of dyspnea-specific anxi-ty might explain why anxiolytics (e.g. benzodiazepines), besidesaving shown positive results for managing dyspnea in individualatients, have not been successful for improving dyspnea in clinicalroup trials in patients with increased general anxiety but with-ut measures of dyspnea-specific anxiety [31]. This indicates that

atients with dyspnea-specific anxiety might benefit more from ayspnea-focused treatment, like pulmonary rehabilitation, whichas been shown to decrease both exercise-induced dyspnea andyspnea-specific anxiety in COPD patients [30].

s 76 (2013) 45– 50 47

Anticipatory anxiety is the anxiety felt when anticipatingspecific future events, so in the case of dyspnea, it is acute dyspnea-specific anxiety. Patients remember triggers of their dyspnea andanticipate dyspnea and alter their behavior when encounteringthese situations in the future [32], e.g. when dyspnea is experi-enced while walking up the stairs, patients will likely be wary ofusing the stairs and try to avoid walking upstairs in the future.An initially neutral stimulus (e.g. the stairs) begins to carry thenegative associations related to dyspnea, i.e. the patient becomesconditioned to this association. If negatively cued stimuli cannotbe avoided, learnt anticipation of dyspnea in response to a stim-ulus might increase the negative perception of respiratory stimuli[33], so patients are more likely to feel out-of breath because theyknow they are climbing the stairs and they expect to feel breath-less. The fear of future dyspnea and resulting avoidance of activitiesthat might induce dyspnea generate a positive feedback loop thatdrives the spiral of decline and leads to increased perception ofdisability at comparable pulmonary function [34]. On the neuronallevel, brain activation during anticipation of dyspnea was found tocorrelate with brain activation during the perception of dyspnea[35]. Anticipation of pain has further shown that brain activationduring anticipation can alter brain activity during pain perceptionand can amplify pain perception [36]. The power of anticipation toamplify the subsequent experience makes understanding the neg-ative impact of acute dyspnea-specific anxiety vital for introducingtargeted treatments to break the spiral of decline and to increasequality of life in affected patients. FMRI could be used to provideneurobiological markers for potential anxiolytic effects. Drug inter-vention studies suggest that the medial prefrontal cortex, an areacentral to conscious threat appraisal, might be a useful target areafor dyspnea and anxiety interactions [37,38].

4. Dyspnea and cognitive processing

Emotion modulates the neural processing of respiratory sensa-tions on a higher-order level [39] and might hence interact withnon-emotional mental processes, such as attention, expectationor catastrophizing, which have been found to modulate aversiveexperiences like pain or dyspnea [40]. Limited attentional resourcesare allocated according to emotional salience. Hence, if attention isused to focus on the threat of dyspnea, lack of cognitive resourcesmight impair performance on everyday tasks. There is evidence thathigh dyspnea-specific anxiety might reduce an individuals’ abilityto allocate attention to cognitive tasks and that this impairment isenhanced in a negative emotional context relative to a neutral emo-tional context [39]. Reduced cognitive performance might impair apatient’s quality of life. While exercise has been proven beneficialto cognitive function, it needs to be investigated whether this effectstretches to dyspnoeic patients.

The allocation of attention plays an important role in treat-ment. During exacerbations of their conditions, patients with lungdisorders are often told to focus on their breathing. Laboratory evi-dence has shown that interoceptive self-awareness does decreasethe strength of perceived dyspnea [41], but that this positive cog-nitive manipulation is susceptible to interruptions by negativeemotions, [41]. This makes these maneuvers more challengingfor dyspnea patients with comorbid depression and/or anxietydisorders. In apparent contradiction to the positive effects ofinteroceptive self-awareness, external distraction has been shownto alleviate the unpleasantness of dyspnea during exercise [42]and could therefore be used to improve exercise performance.

Distraction from pain was shown to decrease brain activity inareas commonly implicated in the processing of pain intensity,whilst increasing activity in the frontal cortex and the cingu-late cortex [43]. Similar mechanisms could be employed during

4 turita

dasr

5

tcdpmiddnacasppa

paWowbmrimah(dvcvmtad

tptcbrwmmmm[

dmpna

8 A. Hayen et al. / Ma

istraction from dyspnea. Understanding the important role ofttention in the modulation of dyspnea could lead to relativelytraight-forward and cost-effective interventions that may helpelieve dyspnea.

. Challenges for dyspnea management

Besides being affected by psychological state, dyspnea percep-ion can be shaped by an individual’s particular life situation (e.g.ontext). A growing population of patients suffering from chronicyspnea are elderly, with approximately a third of elderly peo-le (over 75) at home being affected by dyspnea [44]. Comorbidedical conditions, low fitness levels, and/or functional limitations

n older adults [45] make this population especially vulnerable toeconditioning and deterioration. Additionally, the onset of chronicyspnea is gradual and in older patients it is often disguised as aormal part of aging. Mistaking dyspnea due to respiratory diseases a normal sign of aging delays diagnosis of underlying medi-al conditions and start of treatment. Increasing public awarenessnd patient education are vital to encourage elderly patients toee dyspnea as potentially serious and to present early with dys-nea symptoms. Understanding the neural processes leading toarticular dyspnea sensations has the potential to improve its man-gement in elderly patients.

Improving the management of dyspnea in an aging Westernopulation with an unhealthy diet, increasingly sedentary lifestylend growing rates of obesity is of vital importance. In 2008, theorld Health Organization estimated that 1.4 billion adults were

verweight, including 200 million men and 300 million womenho were obese [46]. Dyspnea is strongly associated with increased

ody mass index and decreased physical activity [47]. Obesityechanically affects respiration by decreasing tidal volume and

esidual capacity [48]. It also leads to altered hormone levels (e.g.ncreased estrogen), but whether and how obesity-induced hor-

onal changes increase dyspnea is not yet understood [48]. There is strong association between asthma and obesity and losing weightas been shown to improve asthma [48]. Additional comorbiditiese.g. joint pain) makes obese dyspnea patients especially prone toeconditioning and there is evidence indicating that obese indi-iduals more frequently experience negative emotions [49], whichould further increase dyspnea perception and suffering. A holisticiew toward improving both dyspnea and obesity are vital for opti-um long-term patient care. Neuroimaging could be a useful tool

o understand the role the burdens of obesity (e.g. deconditioning,ltered hormone levels and negative emotions) play in modulatingyspnea with a view of better targeting treatments.

Respiratory disease affects both men and women [50–53], buthere is evidence for emotions playing a bigger role in dyspneaerception in women. Female COPD patients are twice as likelyo suffer from comorbid anxiety and depression [54]. The per-eption of dyspnea in women with respiratory disease seems toe more strongly influenced by socio-emotional factors and lesselated to physiological measures compared to men [55]. Womenith dyspnea also perceive poorer quality of life compared toen with the same level of dyspnea [22,53]. Women respondore emotionally (e.g. anger toward self) to their dyspnea thanen, which is associated with lower general wellbeing [56] andight amplify the negative consequences of dyspnea in women

57].It is currently not clear whether gender differences in

yspnea perception arise from differences in physiology, hor-

onal make-up, social and cultural experiences and/or emotional

rocessing strategies. Neuroimaging studies of dyspnea haveot (yet) investigated gender differences in the neural mech-nisms of its perception, but there is evidence that men and

s 76 (2013) 45– 50

women use different neural pathways when processing emo-tions [58]. Inconclusive results of behavioral and neuroimagingstudies investigating responses to complex multifactorial stim-uli (e.g. pain) might indicate that so-called ‘gender effects’ onthe perception of these stimuli might be due to variationsin other biopsychological factors that often vary with gender[59].

The effects of sex hormones on respiration, emotion and painperception make it likely that differences in dyspnea percep-tion between men and women might be related to differencesin sex hormone levels. The three major sex hormones (estro-gen, progesterone, and testosterone) have been shown to act atsites throughout the body, including the central nervous sys-tem [60] and the cardiovascular [61] system. Testosterone, whilepresent at low levels in women, is predominant in men. The effectsof testosterone on respiration are complex (increased ventila-tion response to hypoxia [62] and worsening of obstructive sleepapnoea [63]). Testosterone elevates mood [64], has been found todecrease during stress, including anticipatory stress and to increasepain perception [65], but its effects on dyspnea have not beeninvestigated.

Progesterone and estrogen are predominant in women, withlow concentrations in men. During the menstrual cycle, proges-terone levels are low during the first half of the cycle, and thenincrease, peaking in the middle of the luteal phase [66]. Proges-terone has been shown to stimulate respiration [62] and elevatemood [64]. Estrogen peaks during ovulation and is increased duringthe luteal phase of the cycle [66]. Estrogen increases progesterone-induced respiratory stimulation [67] and elevates mood [64]. Thesestrong variations in estrogen and progesterone levels throughout awoman’s cycle will likely affect her respiration, mood and percep-tion of dyspnea. The neural effects of these hormones on dyspneaperception have not been shown, but evidence from pain studiessuggests that increases in estrogen and progesterone seem to beassociated with enhancement of the affective component of thepain experience, but the neural mechanism are not understood[66].

Pregnancy and menopause are two important life events inwomen that are often associated with dyspnea. Over 60% ofhealthy pregnant women are affected by dyspnea [68]. During preg-nancy, the body changes: the diaphragm is elevated, metabolismincreases, levels of estrogen and progesterone increase and ven-tilation and respiratory drive increase. Mood changes, fatigue andstress associated with the pregnancy make pregnancy an emotion-ally challenging time for many women. When a pregnant womancomplains of dyspnea, the clinician should determine whether hersymptoms are due to an underlying disease or present a nor-mal physiologic response to pregnancy. If no pathology is found,cognitive reappraisal of dyspnea from a life-threatening warningsign to a normal part of the physical and emotional burden ofpregnancy decreases suffering. Understanding the neuronal mech-anisms behind this reappraisal might inform the study of thebrain mechanisms behind the emotional threat in more chronicdyspnea.

Dyspnea was found to have a prevalence of 20% in menopausalwomen in the UK [69]. During menopause, estrogen and proges-terone decrease, which might negatively affect mood [64] leadto increased anxiety and could in turn increase dyspnea percep-tion. Additionally, pharmacological treatment of hot flushes andemotional symptoms occurring during menopause might in itselfinduce dyspnea. Being aware of hormonal changes, medicationsand their interactions and potential side-effects is vital for man-

aging dyspnea in menopausal women. Addressing how hormonalchanges influence brain chemistry and emotional processing inthe limbic system might help to better manage dyspnea duringmenopause in future.

turita

6

sucttkpttfditmumo

C

tasfi

C

F

PcRO

P

R

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

A. Hayen et al. / Ma

. Conclusion

In this review, we have shown that dyspnea is a complex,ubjective sensation, and that its successful treatment dependspon viewing dyspnea as a composite, assessing psychologicalomorbidities, hormonal changes and mood changes as well ashe underlying respiratory cause. Future research should aimo address the role of modulating factors and to improve ournowledge of the mechanisms involved in dyspnea processing,articularly the effects of attention, self-awareness and distrac-ion. The important role that aging, gender and obesity play inhe modulation of dyspnea is under-researched and warrantsurther attention. Integrating behavioral measures and carefullyesigned neuroimaging protocols will allow us to clearly define

ntricacies of the neural pathways and spatial organization ofhe respiratory network involved in dyspnea perception and its

odulation. Accurate diagnosis and a greater awareness andnderstanding of modulating factors can facilitate targeted treat-ents of dyspnea and subsequently dramatically improve clinical

utcomes.

ontributors

Anja Hayen: conceptualized and wrote the manuscript, concep-ualized and generated the figure. Mari Herigstad: conceptualizednd wrote the manuscript, conceptualized the figure. Kyle Pattin-on: conceptualized and wrote the manuscript, conceptualized thegure.

ompeting interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

unding

Funding was received by Anja Hayen, Mari Herigstad and Kyleattinson from the Medical Research Council (MRC) for this arti-le. AH and KP were supported by the National Institute for Healthesearch (NIHR), Oxford Biomedical Research Centre based atxford University Hospitals, NHS Trust and University of Oxford.

rovenance and peer review

Commissioned and externally peer reviewed.

eferences

[1] Solano JP, Gomes B, Higginson IJ. A comparison of symptom prevalence in faradvanced cancer, AIDS, heart disease, chronic obstructive pulmonary diseaseand renal disease. J Pain Symptom Manage 2006;31(1):58–69.

[2] Smoller JW, Pollack MH, Otto MW, Rosenbaum JF, Kradin RL. Panic anxiety,dyspnea, and respiratory disease. Theoretical and clinical considerations. Am JRespir Crit Care Med 1996;154(1):6–17 [Epub 1996/07/01].

[3] American Thoracic Society. Dyspnea. Mechanisms, assessment, and manage-ment: a consensus statement. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1999;159(1):321–40.

[4] Abidov A, Rozanski A, Hachamovitch R, et al. Prognostic significance ofdyspnea in patients referred for cardiac stress testing. N Engl J Med2005;353(18):1889–98 [Epub 2005/11/04].

[5] Britton M. The burden of COPD in the U.K.: results from the confronting COPDsurvey. Respir Med 2003;97(Suppl C):S71–9 [Epub 2003/03/22].

[6] Hoyert DL, Jiaquan X. Deaths preliminary data for 2011. Division of Vital Statis-tics; 2012. Contract no. 6.

[7] Mahler DA, Selecky PA, Harrod CG, et al. American College of Chest Physiciansconsensus statement on the management of dyspnea in patients with advancedlung or heart disease. Chest 2010;137(3):674–91.

[8] Gysels M, Bausewein C, Higginson IJ. Experiences of breathlessness:

a systematic review of the qualitative literature. Palliat Support Care2007;5(3):281–302.

[9] Plaufcan MR, Wamboldt FS, Holm KE. Behavioral and characterologicalself-blame in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. J Psychosom Res2012;72(1):78–83 [Epub 2011/12/28].

[

s 76 (2013) 45– 50 49

10] Mahler DA, Harver A, Lentine T, Scott JA, Beck K, Schwartzstein RM. Descrip-tors of breathlessness in cardiorespiratory diseases. Am J Respir Crit Care Med1996;154(5):1357–63 [Epub 1996/11/01].

11] Lansing RW, Gracely RH, Banzett RB. The multiple dimensions of dyspnea:review and hypotheses. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 2009;167(1):53–60.

12] Banzett RB, Pedersen SH, Schwartzstein RM, Lansing RW. The affective dimen-sion of laboratory dyspnea: air hunger is more unpleasant than work/effort.Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2008;177(12):1384–90.

13] von Leupoldt A, Sommer T, Kegat S, et al. Dyspnea and pain share emotion-related brain network. NeuroImage 2009;48(1):200–6.

14] Hayen A, Herigstad M, Kelly M, et al. The effects of altered intrathoracic pres-sure on resting cerebral blood flow and its response to visual stimulation.NeuroImage 2012;66C:479–88 [Epub 2012/10/31].

15] Herigstad M, Hayen A, Wiech K, Pattinson KT. Dyspnoea and the brain. RespirMed 2011;105(6):809–17.

16] Kamal AH, Maguire JM, Wheeler JL, Currow DC, Abernethy AP. Dyspnea reviewfor the palliative care professional: assessment, burdens, and etiologies. J PalliatMed 2011;14(10):1167–72.

17] Norwood RJ. A review of etiologies of depression in COPD. Int J Chronic Obstruc-tive Pulm Dis 2007;2(4):485–91.

18] Porzelius J, Vest M, Nochomovitz M. Respiratory function, cognitions,and panic in chronic obstructive pulmonary patients. Behav Res Therapy1992;30(1):75–7.

19] Ng TP, Niti M, Tan WC, Cao Z, Ong KC, Eng P. Depressive symptoms andchronic obstructive pulmonary disease: effect on mortality, hospital readmis-sion, symptom burden, functional status, and quality of life. Arch Intern Med2007;167(1):60–7 [Epub 2007/01/11].

20] Rietveld S, van Beest I. Rollercoaster asthma: when positive emotionalstress interferes with dyspnea perception. Behav Res Ther 2007;45(5):977–87.

21] Bogaerts K, Notebaert K, Van Diest I, Devriese S, De Peuter S, Van den Bergh O.Accuracy of respiratory symptom perception in different affective contexts. JPsychosom Res 2005;58(6):537–43.

22] Di Marco F, Verga M, Reggente M, et al. Anxiety and depression inCOPD patients: the roles of gender and disease severity. Respir Med2006;100(10):1767–74.

23] von Leupoldt A, Taube K, Lehmann K, Fritzsche A, Magnussen H. The impact ofanxiety and depression on outcomes of pulmonary rehabilitation in patientswith COPD. Chest 2011;140(3):730–6 [Epub 2011/04/02].

24] Rosenkranz MA, Davidson RJ. Affective neural circuitry and mind-body influ-ences in asthma. NeuroImage 2009;47(3):972–80.

25] Rosenkranz MA, Busse WW, Johnstone T, et al. Neural circuitry underlying theinteraction between emotion and asthma symptom exacerbation. Proc NatlAcad Sci USA 2005;102(37):13319–24.

26] Main J, Moss-Morris R, Booth R, Kaptein AA, Kolbe J. The use of reliever med-ication in asthma: the role of negative mood and symptom reports. J Asthma2003;40(4):357–65.

27] Dales RE, Spitzer WO, Schechter MT, Suissa S. The influence of psy-chological status on respiratory symptom reporting. Am Rev Respir Dis1989;139(6):1459–63.

28] Huang TY, Moser DK, Hsieh YS, Gau BS, Chiang FT, Hwang SL. Moderating effectof psychosocial factors for dyspnea in Taiwanese and American heart failurepatients. J Nurs Res 2013;21(1):49–58.

29] Alius MG, Pane-Farre CA, Von Leupoldt A, Hamm AO. Induction of dyspneaevokes increased anxiety and maladaptive breathing in individuals with highanxiety sensitivity and suffocation fear. Psychophysiology 2013.

30] Janssens T, De Peuter S, Stans L, et al. Dyspnea perception in COPD: asso-ciation between anxiety, dyspnea-related fear, and dyspnea in a pulmonaryrehabilitation program. Chest 2011;140(3):618–25 [Epub 2011/04/16].

31] Simon ST, Higginson IJ, Booth S, Harding R, Bausewein C. Benzodiazepines forthe relief of breathlessness in advanced malignant and non-malignant dis-eases in adults. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2010;1:CD007354 [Epub 2010/01/22].

32] Ley R. The modification of breathing behavior. Pavlovian and operant controlin emotion and cognition. Behav Modif 1999;23(3):441–79.

33] De Peuter S, Van Diest I, Lemaigre V, et al. Can subjective asthma symptoms belearned? Psychosom Med 2005;67(3):454–61.

34] Yellowlees PM, Ruffin RE. Psychological defenses and coping styles inpatients following a life-threatening attack of asthma. Chest 1989;95(6):1298–303.

35] Paulus MP, Flagan T, Simmons AN, et al. Subjecting elite athletes to inspiratorybreathing load reveals behavioral and neural signatures of optimal performersin extreme environments. PLoS ONE 2012;7(1):e29394.

36] Fairhurst M, Wiech K, Dunckley P, Tracey I. Anticipatory brainstem activity pre-dicts neural processing of pain in humans. Pain 2007;128(1–2):101–10 [Epub2006/10/31].

37] Sripada RK, Marx CE, King AP, Rampton JC, Ho SS, Liberzon I. Allopreg-nanolone elevations following pregnenolone administration are associatedwith enhanced activation of emotion regulation neurocircuits. Biol Psychiatry2013.

38] Evans KC, Simon NM, Dougherty DD, et al. A PET study of tiagabine treat-

ment implicates ventral medial prefrontal cortex in generalized social anxietydisorder. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009;34(2):390–8 [Epub 2008/06/10].

39] von Leupoldt A, Chan PY, Esser RW, Davenport PW. Emotions and neu-ral processing of respiratory sensations investigated with respiratory-relatedevoked potentials. Psychosom Med 2013;75(3):244–52 [Epub 2013/03/06].

5 turita

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

[

0 A. Hayen et al. / Ma

40] Wiech K, Tracey I. The influence of negative emotions on pain: behav-ioral effects and neural mechanisms. NeuroImage 2009;47(3):987–94 [Epub2009/06/02].

41] Petersen S, Ritz T. The role of fearful beliefs in the relationship between situ-ational self-awareness and report of breathing-related sensations. Br J HealthPsychol 2011;16(Pt 2):359–72.

42] von Leupoldt A, Taube K, Schubert-Heukeshoven S, Magnussen H, Dahme B. Dis-tractive auditory stimuli reduce the unpleasantness of dyspnea during exercisein patients with COPD. Chest 2007;132(5):1506–12.

43] Bantick SJ, Wise RG, Ploghaus A, Clare S, Smith SM, Tracey I. Imaging howattention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain 2002;125(Pt2):310–9 [Epub 2002/02/15].

44] Ho SF, O’Mahony MS, Steward JA, Breay P, Buchalter M, Burr ML. Dyspnoea andquality of life in older people at home. Age Ageing 2001;30(2):155–9 [Epub2001/06/08].

45] Nelson ME, Rejeski WJ, Blair SN, et al. Physical activity and public health inolder adults: recommendation from the American College of Sports Medicineand the American Heart Association. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2007;39(8):1435–45[Epub 2007/09/01].

46] World Health Organization. Obesity and overweight. Fact sheet no.311; 2013. http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs311/en/index.html[accessed 15.05.13].

47] Gerlach Y, Williams MT, Coates AM. Weighing up the evidence-a systematicreview of measures used for the sensation of breathlessness in obesity. Int JObes (Lond) 2013;37(3):341–9 [Epub 2012/04/26].

48] Weiss ST. Obesity: insight into the origins of asthma. Nat Immunol2005;6(6):537–9 [Epub 2005/05/24].

49] Pasco JA, Williams LJ, Jacka FN, Brennan SL, Berk M. Obesity and the relationshipwith positive and negative affect. Aust N Z J Psychiatry 2013 [Epub 2013/03/26].

50] de Marco R, Locatelli F, Sunyer J, Burney P. Differences in incidence of reportedasthma related to age in men and women. A retrospective analysis of thedata of the European Respiratory Health Survey. Am J Respir Crit Care Med2000;162(1):68–74.

51] Mannino DM. COPD: epidemiology, prevalence, morbidity and mortality, anddisease heterogeneity. Chest 2002;121(5 Suppl), 121S–6S.

52] Melgert BN, Ray A, Hylkema MN, Timens W, Postma DS. Are there reasons

why adult asthma is more common in females? Curr Allergy Asthma Rep2007;7(2):143–50.

53] Naberan K, Azpeitia A, Cantoni J, Miravitlles M. Impairment of quality oflife in women with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Respir Med2012;106(3):367–73.

[[

s 76 (2013) 45– 50

54] Laurin C, Lavoie KL, Bacon SL, et al. Sex differences in the prevalence of psy-chiatric disorders and psychological distress in patients with COPD. Chest2007;132(1):148–55 [Epub 2007/05/17].

55] de Torres JP, Casanova C, de Garcini AM, Jaime AA, Celli BR. COPD hetero-geneity: gender differences in the multidimensional BODE index. Int J ChronicObstructive Pulm Dis 2007;2(2):151–5.

56] Ketelaars CA, Schlosser MA, Mostert R, Huyer Abu-Saad H, Halfens RJ, WoutersEF. Determinants of health-related quality of life in patients with chronicobstructive pulmonary disease. Thorax 1996;51(1):39–43.

57] Ninot G, Fortes M, Poulain M, et al. Gender difference in coping strategies amongpatients enrolled in an inpatient rehabilitation program. Heart Lung J Crit Care2006;35(2):130–6.

58] Weisenbach SL, Rapport LJ, Briceno EM, et al. Reduced emotion processing effi-ciency in healthy males relative to females. Social Cogn Affect Neurosci 2012.

59] Racine M, Tousignant-Laflamme Y, Kloda LA, Dion D, Dupuis G, Choiniere M.A systematic literature review of 10 years of research on sex/gender and painperception – part 2: do biopsychosocial factors alter pain sensitivity differentlyin women and men? Pain 2012;153(3):619–35.

60] Aloisi AM, Bonifazi M. Sex hormones, central nervous system and pain. HormBehav 2006;50(1):1–7 [Epub 2006/01/21].

61] Traupe T, Stettler CD, Li H, et al. Distinct roles of estrogen receptors alpha andbeta mediating acute vasodilation of epicardial coronary arteries. Hypertension2007;49(6):1364–70 [Epub 2007/05/02].

62] Saaresranta T, Polo O. Hormones and breathing. Chest 2002;122(6):2165–82[Epub 2002/12/12].

63] Schneider BK, Pickett CK, Zwillich CW, et al. Influence of testosterone on breath-ing during sleep. J Appl Physiol 1986;61(2):618–23 [Epub 1986/08/01].

64] Douma SL, Husband C, O’Donnell ME, Barwin BN, Woodend AK. Estrogen-related mood disorders: reproductive life cycle factors. ANS Adv Nurs Sci2005;28(4):364–75 [Epub 2005/11/18].

65] Choi JC, Chung MI, Lee YD. Modulation of pain sensation by stress-related testosterone and cortisol. Anaesthesia 2012;67(10):1146–51 [Epub2012/07/19].

66] Vincent K, Tracey I. Sex hormones and pain: the evidence from functionalimaging. Curr Pain Headache Rep 2010;14(5):396–403 [Epub 2010/08/11].

67] Behan M, Zabka AG, Thomas CF, Mitchell GS. Sex steroid hormones and the

neural control of breathing. Respir Physiol Neurobiol 2003;136(2–3):249–63[Epub 2003/07/11].

68] Tenholder MF, South-Paul JE. Dyspnea in pregnancy. Chest 1989;96(2):381–8.69] Ward T, Scheid V, Tuffrey V. Women’s mid-life health experiences in urban UK:

an international comparison. Clim J Int Menopause Soc 2010;13(3):278–88.