breakfast consumption and exercise interact to affect cognitive performance and mood later in the...

27
Accepted Manuscript Research report Breakfast consumption and exercise interact to affect cognitive performance and mood later in the day: a randomized controlled trial R.C. Veasey, J.T. Gonzalez, D.O. Kennedy, C.F. Haskell, E.J. Stevenson PII: S0195-6663(13)00148-7 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.011 Reference: APPET 1817 To appear in: Appetite Received Date: 29 October 2012 Revised Date: 12 April 2013 Accepted Date: 14 April 2013 Please cite this article as: Veasey, R.C., Gonzalez, J.T., Kennedy, D.O., Haskell, C.F., Stevenson, E.J., Breakfast consumption and exercise interact to affect cognitive performance and mood later in the day: a randomized controlled trial, Appetite (2013), doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.011 This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Upload: independent

Post on 13-May-2023

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Accepted Manuscript

Research report

Breakfast consumption and exercise interact to affect cognitive performance and

mood later in the day: a randomized controlled trial

R.C. Veasey, J.T. Gonzalez, D.O. Kennedy, C.F. Haskell, E.J. Stevenson

PII: S0195-6663(13)00148-7

DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.011

Reference: APPET 1817

To appear in: Appetite

Received Date: 29 October 2012

Revised Date: 12 April 2013

Accepted Date: 14 April 2013

Please cite this article as: Veasey, R.C., Gonzalez, J.T., Kennedy, D.O., Haskell, C.F., Stevenson, E.J., Breakfast

consumption and exercise interact to affect cognitive performance and mood later in the day: a randomized controlled

trial, Appetite (2013), doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.04.011

This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers

we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and

review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process

errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Breakfast consumption and exercise interact to affect cognitive performance and mood 1

later in the day: a randomized controlled trial 2

R.C.Veaseya,*, J. T. Gonzalez

a, D.O. Kennedy

a, C.F. Haskell

a, E.J. Stevenson

a 3

aBrain, Performance and Nutrition Research Centre, Faculty of Health and Life Sciences, 4

Northumbria University, Newcastle, Tyne and Wear, NE18ST, UK. 5 6 7 *Corresponding author: [email protected] (Rachel Veasey) 8

9 Not for publication: +44 (0)191 243 7253 10 11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30

31

32

Abstract 33

The current study assessed the interactive effect of breakfast and exercise on cognition and 34

mood. Twelve active males completed four trials; no breakfast-rest, breakfast-rest, no 35

breakfast-exercise or breakfast-exercise in a randomized, cross-over design. The trials 36

consisted of; breakfast or fast, a 2h rest, exercise (treadmill run) or equivalent rest, a 37

chocolate milk drink, a 90min rest and an ad libitum lunch. Cognitive performance and 38

mood were recorded frequently throughout each trial. Data was analysed as pre-exercise/rest, 39

during and immediately post exercise/rest and post-drink. No effects were found prior to 40

consumption of the drink. Post-drink, fasting before exercise increased mental fatigue 41

compared to consuming breakfast before exercise and fasting before rest. Tension increased 42

when breakfast was consumed at rest and when exercise was undertaken fasted compared to 43

omitting breakfast before rest. Breakfast before rest decreased Rapid Visual Information 44

Processing task speed and impaired Stroop performance. Breakfast omission improved Four 45

Choice Reaction Time performance. To conclude, breakfast before exercise appeared 46

beneficial for post-exercise mood even when a post-exercise snack was consumed. Exercise 47

reversed post-breakfast cognitive impairment in active males. 48

49

Trial registration: clinicaltrials.gov NCT01229345 50

51

Keywords 52

Breakfast; Exercise; Cognition; Mood; Active males 53

54

55

56

Introduction 57

Past research has explored the effects of pre-exercise nutrition on physical performance, 58

forming a set of guidelines for athletes and the active population to follow. For example, it is 59

recommended that carbohydrate is consumed prior to exercise to improve endurance capacity 60

(American Dietetic Association, 2009) . However, achieving optimal physical performance is 61

often not the sole motivation for exercise. Performance is a multi-dimensional paradigm, 62

which also encompasses aspects of cognition and mood. An acute bout of sub-maximal 63

exercise has been shown to improve some facets of cognitive performance, such as reaction 64

time (Etnier, et al., 1997) and decision making (Adam, Teeken, Ypelaar, Verstappen, & Paas, 65

1997; McMorris & Graydon, 1996), as well as mood (Scully, Kremer, Meade, Graham, & 66

Dudgeon, 1998), an effect often thought to be caused by increased arousal during the 67

recovery period (Lambourne & Tomporowski, 2010). Many of the general population 68

exercise regularly for these additional health benefits instead of, or in addition to, physical 69

performance enhancement. It has been found that consuming CHO during exercise can 70

enhance these cognitive (Ali & Williams, 2009; Collardeau, Brisswalter, Vercruyssen, 71

Audiffren, & Goubault, 2001) and mood (Lieberman, Falco, & Slade, 2002) effects 72

demonstrating an interaction between nutritional state and exercise on post-exercise cognitive 73

performance. 74

The timing of exercise likely influences pre-exercise nutritional intake. When exercise is 75

undertaken in the morning, it is perhaps more common to remain fasted due to lack of time. 76

Some individuals may choose to exercise in a fasted state due to the associated increase in fat 77

oxidation (Backhouse, Williams, Stevenson, & Nute, 2007; Gonzalez, Veasey, Rumbold, & 78

Stevenson, 2013). Although this may be beneficial if weight loss is a main goal of exercise, 79

if breakfast is ommitted to achieve this, dietary guidelines which state that breakfast 80

consumption is important for health and wellbeing are contradicted (NIfHaC, 2006). The 81

positive health benefits of consuming breakfast include mood enhancement (Kral, Heo, 82

Whiteford, & Faith, 2012) and improvement in some aspects of cognitive performance, in 83

particular, memory [for review see (Marangoni, et al., 2009)]. These benefits have been 84

attributed to its timing, consumed first thing in the morning, breaking the overnight fast and it 85

is thought that breakfast may be vital for optimum cognitive function at this point (Benton & 86

Parker, 1998; Pollitt & Mathews, 1998). Although the acute mood-enhancing effects of 87

breakfast do tend to diminish after a few hours (Smith, Clark, & Gallagher, 1999), consuming 88

a mid-morning snack has been shown to improve declining mood following a large breakfast 89

(Benton, Slater, & Donohoe, 2001). Even if guidelines for exercise recovery are followed and 90

energy is replaced immediately following exercise [for example, by consuming chocolate 91

milk (Karp, et al., 2006; Thomas K, 2009)], omitting breakfast and inducing a further energy 92

deficit through exercise following an overnight fast may have detrimental cognitive and 93

mood effects later in the day. 94

Trials which have investigated the effect of consuming or omitting breakfast prior to exercise 95

on cognitive performance and mood are uncommon in the literature and have produced 96

diverse results (Hill, Whitehead, & Goodwin, 2011; Paul, Rokusek, Dykstra, Boileau, & 97

Layman, 1996; Vermorel, Bitar, Vernet, Verdier, & Coudert, 2003). In a cross-sectional 98

study by Vermoral et al. (2003), it was suggested that when adolescents have a morning 99

physical education lesson they require a substantial breakfast for cognitive benefits later in 100

the day (Vermorel, et al., 2003). Recently, there was no difference identified in the mood 101

state of swimmers who either consumed or omitted CHO before a morning training session 102

(Hill, et al., 2011) although Paul et al. (1996) reported an increase in mental fatigue during 103

the recovery period following a 90min cycle if breakfast was omitted rather than consumed 104

beforehand, but found no effect on cognition. However, no study to date has looked at the 105

interactive effect of breakfast consumption or omission prior to exercise or rest on cognitive 106

performance and mood in the same sample, which may be an important factor (Hubert, King, 107

& Blundell, 1998). Therefore the focus of the present paper was to assess the effect of 108

breakfast consumption and breakfast omission prior to rest or exercise, followed by a post-109

exercise recovery drink to assess cognitive performance and mood response. It was 110

hypothesized that consuming breakfast prior to exercise would elicit the most beneficial 111

cognitive performance and mood effects following a morning run. 112

Methods 113

Initial screening/Preliminary measurements 114

Ethical approval for the study protocol was granted by the Ethics Committee from the School 115

of Life Sciences at Northumbria University. Prior to participation volunteers gave written 116

informed consent. All participants confirmed that they were non-smokers, in good health and 117

free from social drugs, medication and herbal and dietary supplements at an initial screening 118

visit. Participants had no history of, or current, head trauma, learning difficulties, attention 119

deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), dyslexia, migraines or gastric problems, a good 120

standard of English, equivalent to that of a native English speaker and were not high caffeine 121

consumers (<600mg per day). A further criterion for inclusion in the study was that 122

participants were at least moderately physically active, exercising for at least 30 minutes 5 123

times per week (confirmed using the International Physical Activity Questionnaire 124

[IPAQ;(Craig, et al., 2003)] and were able to run for 1h continuously. 125

Screening also comprised the collection of demographic information and training and 126

familiarisation with the cognitive tasks and experimental procedures. The battery of 127

cognitive tasks was repeated 3 times during this session to decrease the chance of learning 128

effects during main trials. Participants undertook two preliminary tests to establish 1) the 129

relationship between V˙ O2 and running speed on a flat treadmill using a 16-min test, and 2) 130

their V˙ O2max using an incremental treadmill test whereby the gradient was increased by 131

1%⋅min-1

to exhaustion as previously described in full detail (Williams, Nute, Broadbank, & 132

Vinall, 1990). The running speed equivalent to 60% of each participant‟s V˙ O2max was then 133

determined and the duration of exercise needed at this intensity to expend 2.9MJ was 134

calculated for each individual. The within subjects design with matched energy expenditure 135

during exercise intended to eradicate any significant effects that varying exercise durations 136

may have had on the results. On the day preceding the first trial, participants were required 137

to keep a record of their food intake and physical activity which was replicated the day 138

preceding subsequent trials. All 12 participants who attended the screening visit also 139

successfully completed the study. 140

Participants 141

Twelve, healthy, active men participated in the study. Their mean ± SD age, height, body 142

mass (BM), Body Mass Index (BMI) and maximal oxygen uptake (V˙ O2max) were 23.2 ± 143

2.4 y, 177.6 ± 7.0 cm, 77.2 ± 5.3 kg, 24.5 ± 2.0 kg/m2 and 50.7 ± 1.2 mL · kg

_1 · min

_1, 144

respectively. Participants abstained from alcohol and vigorous exercise for 24 h and fasted 145

for 12 h, prior to the start of each trial. 146

Cognitive and Mood Measures 147

The cognitive tasks and mood measures selected had shown previous sensitivity to nutritional 148

and/or exercise interventions. All tasks and visual analogue scales were administered using 149

the Computerised Mental Performance Assessment System (COMPASS, Northumbria 150

University), a programme used to present computerized standard psychometric tests. 151

COMPASS has been used in several previous nutritional intervention studies and has been 152

shown to be sensitive to acute cognitive enhancement and impairment with a variety of 153

substances [e.g. (Haskell, Kennedy, Wesnes, & Scholey, 2005; Jackson, Deary, Reay, 154

Scholey, & Kennedy, 2012; D. Kennedy, et al., 2010; D. O. Kennedy, et al., 2011; Scholey, 155

et al., 2010; Wightman, Haskell, Forster, Veasey, & Kennedy, 2012)]. Presentation of tasks 156

and mood scales was via a laptop computer. All responses were recorded via a button 157

response box (comprising buttons for Yes/No, Left/Right, Blue/ Green/Yellow /Red and a 158

central reaction time button). Each set of tasks took approximately 13 min to complete. 159

Participants were tested individually and were required to wear ear defenders throughout the 160

cognitive tasks to minimise noise distraction. 161

Simple Reaction Time (SRT; ~90 sec) 162

An arrow pointing upwards appeared in the centre of the computer screen at irregular 163

intervals (between 1 and 3.5 sec). The participant was instructed to press the centre button on 164

the response pad when the arrow appeared, responding as quickly and as accurately as 165

possible. The task was scored for overall reaction time (RT) to 30 stimuli. 166

167

Stroop task (~2 min) 168

Words describing one of four colours („RED‟, „YELLOW‟, „GREEN‟, „BLUE‟) were 169

presented in different coloured fonts in the centre of the computer screen. The participant 170

pressed one of four coloured response buttons in order to identify the font colour (e.g., if the 171

word „GREEN‟ was presented in a blue font, the correct response would be to respond with 172

the blue button). The presented words were either „congruent‟ (word and font are the same 173

colour) or „incongruent‟ (word and font are different colours) and were presented in a random 174

order. In total, 120 words were presented. The task was scored for RT and accuracy of 175

responses to „congruent‟ and „incongruent‟ words. 176

Four Choice Reaction Time (FCRT; ~90 sec) 177

Four direction arrow keys were displayed on the computer screen. The arrows „lit up‟ at 178

irregular intervals (between 1 and 3.5 sec), one at a time. Participants were instructed to use 179

the index finger of their dominant hand to press the corresponding button on the response pad 180

(left/right/up/down) when an arrow became lit, responding as quickly and as accurately as 181

possible. In total, 32 stimuli were presented. The task was scored for RT and accuracy of 182

responses. 183

N-Back (~2 min) 184

A series of single letters appeared on the screen, presented one at a time. If the letter that 185

appeared was also presented 3 letters previously in the series, participants were asked to press 186

the „YES‟ button on the response pad and if the letter that appeared was not presented 3 187

letters previously in the series, to press the „NO‟ button on the response pad, responding as 188

quickly and accurately as possible to every letter presented. In total, 36 stimuli were 189

presented, encompassing 12 target pairs. The task was scored for RT and accuracy. 190

Rapid visual information processing task (RVIP; 5 min) 191

A series of single digit numbers between 1 and 9 appeared on the screen continuously at a 192

rate of 100 per min-1

for 5 minutes. Participants were required to use the index finger of their 193

dominant hand to press the centre button on the response box, reacting as quickly and 194

accurately as possible, when they identified three odd or even digits presented in succession. 195

Eight correct target strings were presented each minute. The task was scored for percentage 196

of target strings correctly detected, average RT for correct detections and number of false 197

alarms. 198

Mental Fatigue and Task Difficulty Visual Analogue Scales 199

Two single visual analogue scales measuring mental fatigue and task difficulty were 200

completed at the end of each set of tasks. Each scale was labelled “not at all” (left end of 201

scale) and “extremely” (right end of scale). 202

Mood and Physical State Visual Analogue Scales 203

A set of mood and physical state visual analogue scales [adapted from (Rogers, Richardson, 204

& Elliman, 1995)] were completed before each set of cognitive tasks measuring subjective 205

ratings of mood and physical state (“relaxed,” “alert,” “jittery,” “tired,” “tense,” “headache, ” 206

“overall mood”). Participants were asked to click on a 100mm line on the computer screen to 207

grade their current subjective status for each state. Each scale was labelled “not at all” (left 208

end of scale) and “extremely” (right end of scale), except for “overall mood” which was 209

labelled “very bad” and “very good”. During exercise trials, participants also completed a 210

single VAS immediately following the exercise session rating to what extent they enjoyed the 211

run. 212

All VAS scales were scored as mm along the line towards „extremely‟. 213

Treatments/Test meals 214

The nutritional content of the breakfast, snack and lunch provided in the study are detailed in 215

Table 1. Participants were given a maximum of 15 min to consume the breakfast. The post-216

exercise/rest snack was served at room temperature and participants were instructed to finish 217

the drink within 5 min. Participants confirmed verbally that the drink was well liked. 218

Each portion of the ad libitum lunch was prepared in advance and re-heated in the microwave 219

for 2.5 minutes as required. 220

Procedure/ Experimental protocol (Fig. 1) 221

Each participant completed four trials, in a randomised, cross-over design, consisting no 222

breakfast and rest (NB NE), breakfast and rest (B NE), no breakfast and exercise (NB E) or 223

breakfast and exercise (B E). The first trial was undertaken between 48 h and 14 days of the 224

initial screening visit. Trials were separated by ≥48 hours and all trials were performed under 225

similar laboratory conditions. Trials began at 0730 h (±15 min). After confirming 226

compliance to the study restrictions, a baseline completion of the cognitive tasks and mood 227

scales was then undertaken, before participants were administered the test breakfast or 228

remained fasted. During the 2 h rest period which followed, cognitive performance and 229

mood were measured at 60 and 120 min. In between these periods, participants were allowed 230

to read, write or watch a DVD. In the exercise trials (NB E and B E), participants then 231

completed a treadmill run at 60% of their V˙ O2, until 2.9MJ had been expended with heart 232

rate and rate of perceived exertion (RPE) measured at 10 min intervals throughout. Exercise 233

intensity was maintained throughout this period via an online gas analysis system (Metalyzer 234

3B, 164 Cortex, Germany). On rest days (NB NE and B NE), participants rested for the 235

equivalent amount of time. Cognitive performance and mood were reassessed before 236

participants were administered a test drink, followed by a 90 min rest period where cognitive 237

performance and mood was assessed at 30 and 75 min. This was followed by an ad libitum 238

lunch where participants were asked to consume enough food to feel satisfied to a normal 239

level. After lunch, they completed the cognitive tasks and mood scales for a final time and 240

were then free to leave the laboratory. Heart rate was monitored throughout each trial using 241

telemetry (Polar T31 transmitter, Polar Electro Oy, HQ, Professorintie 5, FIN-90440 242

Kempele, Finland). Water intake was recorded during each first rest trial and first exercise 243

trial and was approximately matched in the subsequent rest and exercise trials. 244

245

2.8 Statistical Analysis 246

Data was split into three distinct parts for analysis: Post-breakfast (assessing the effects of 247

breakfast consumption vs. breakfast omission only), during and/or immediately post-248

exercise/rest (to assess the immediate interaction between the breakfast and exercise 249

interventions) and from post drink (to assess the cognitive responses to the test drink). For 250

each analysis, change values from the same baseline were used. 251

Cognitive task repetition was used to calculate sample size. Based on the 4 repetitions 252

completed after both interventions had taken place, a power calculation revealed that a 253

sample size of 12 would provide statistical power to detect large effects above 80% with an 254

alpha level of .05 (Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, & Buchner, 2007). Before the main statistical 255

analysis, paired sample t tests were used to compare pre-dose baseline data to assess for 256

differences in performance across the study days. Scores for each individual task outcome 257

(SRT, Stroop, FCRT, N-back, RVIP, mental fatigue VAS, difficulty VAS and mood and 258

physical state VAS) were analysed as „change from baseline‟ using Minitab 16 (Minitab Ltd, 259

Coventry, UK). Separate analysis was conducted for data collected pre-exercise/rest (two-260

way repeated measures AVOVA, breakfast (NB = 0, B = 1) x repetition) and during and post 261

exercise/rest (two-way repeated measures ANOVA [repetition × breakfast (NB = 0, B = 262

1)×exercise(NE = 0, E = 1)]) and post-drink (two-way repeated measures ANOVA [repetition 263

× breakfast (NB = 0, B = 1)×exercise(NE = 0, E = 1)]). The latter analysis also allowed the 264

detection of any main effects of breakfast (irrespective of exercise) and of exercise 265

(irrespective of breakfast). Planned comparisons (using t tests calculated with the Mean 266

Squares Error) were then employed to show where the significance lay in significant 267

interactions shown by the ANOVA, which were corrected using Bonferroni where 268

appropriate. An alpha level of .05 was used for all statistical tests. Means ± Standard Error 269

Mean are reported for significant findings. 270

Results 271

There were no significant differences between mean baseline scores for any of the treatment 272

conditions or outcome measures. Significant main effects and interactions for COMPASS 273

tasks, mood and physical state VAS are reported below. Due to a data capture error only 11 274

participants were included in the N-back and Stroop analysis and 9 in the RVIP analysis. 275

Post-breakfast Analysis 276

No effects on cognitive performance or mood were observed during the 2h post-breakfast 277

period. 278

Post-exercise/Rest Analysis 279

No cognitive performance or mood variable effects were observed immediately following the 280

exercise or rest period. 281

Post-drink Analysis 282

Cognitive Performance 283

No effects were observed for the simple reaction time or N-back tasks or task difficulty. 284

Four Choice Reaction Time 285

No interactions between breakfast and exercise were observed for this task. A main effect of 286

breakfast on FCRT accuracy was observed [F (1, 10) = 5.04, p = .046] with a lower 287

percentage of correct responses seen when breakfast was consumed (-0.4 ± 0.4%) compared 288

to omitted (1.1 ± 0.5%; Fig. 2). 289

Stroop 290

A significant exercise x breakfast interaction was observed for Stroop task accuracy [F (1, 291

7)=7.84, p = .019]. Comparisons revealed poorer accuracy in the B NE condition (-0.4 ± 292

0.6%) compared to the B E condition (0.6 ± 0.6 %; t(10) = 3.96, p = .001) the NB E condition 293

(-0.4 ± 0.5%; t(10) = 3.13, p = .013) and the NB NE condition (1.5 ± 0.5%, t(10) = 4.71, p < 294

.001; Fig. 3a). 295

296

Rapid Visual Information Processing task 297

A significant exercise x breakfast interaction was observed for the RVIP task [F (1, 5)=10.74, 298

p = .011]. Comparisons revealed a quicker RT in the NB NE condition (-14.6 ± 6.5ms) than 299

in the B NE condition (25.8 ± 9.1ms; t(8) = 3.23, p = .010; Fig. 3b). 300

301

Mental fatigue ratings 302

A significant breakfast x exercise interaction was seen [F (1, 8) = 11.89, p = .005] with 303

significantly higher mental fatigue ratings reported in the NB E condition (25.9 ± 2.7mm) 304

compared to the NB NE (10.9 ± 2.9; t(11) = 3.60, p = .003) and B E condition ( 11.1 ± 305

2.6mm; t(11) = 3.55, p = .003; Fig. 3c). 306

307

Mood and Physical State VAS 308

No significant effects of breakfast or exercise on ratings of relaxation, alertness, jitteriness, 309

tiredness or overall mood were observed. 310

Tension ratings 311

A breakfast x exercise interaction was found for ratings of tension [F (1, 8) = 9.75, p = .010]. 312

Tension was significantly higher in the B NE (3.8 ± 2.9mm) and NB E (5.8 ± 2.5mm; t(11) = 313

3.13, p = .013) conditions compared to the NB NE condition (-12.5 ± 2.1; t(11) = 2.79, p = 314

.036; Fig. 3d). 315

316

Heart rate, RPE and Exercise Enjoyment VAS 317

There were no significant differences observed for RPE or enjoyment VAS ratings during the 318

exercise trials. Due to a large amount of missing data points, data for heart rate was not 319

analysed. 320

Discussion 321

This study investigated the effects of breakfast consumption or omission prior to exercise or 322

rest on cognitive performance and mood in healthy, habitually active males. The results 323

suggest that breakfast consumption and exercise interact to influence some aspects of 324

cognitive performance and mood. 325

Significant effects were only observed following consumption of the drink. Breakfast had a 326

negative impact on Four Choice Reaction Time (FCRT) accuracy, irrespective of exercise 327

intervention. Consumption of breakfast, as opposed to fasting, prior to rest also reduced speed 328

of Rapid Visual Information Processing (RVIP) and Stroop task accuracy, with exercise or 329

omitting breakfast reversing the latter effect. Breakfast omission prior to exercise led to 330

significantly higher mental fatigue ratings than breakfast omission prior to rest or breakfast 331

consumption prior to exercise. An increase in tension was also seen when exercise was 332

undertaken in a fasted state and when breakfast was consumed followed by rest, as compared 333

to no breakfast followed by rest. 334

The findings of decrements to performance following breakfast may seem surprising in light 335

of research demonstrating a positive effect of breakfast [for review see (Hoyland, Lawton, & 336

Dye, 2008)] . However, the majority of positive effects of breakfast have been shown on tests 337

of delayed verbal memory. Reaction time is often reported to decrease with increased feelings 338

of hunger (Fischer, Colombani, & Wenk, 2004), and impairment in concentration following a 339

high caloric breakfast has also been reported (Michaud, Musse, Nicolas, & Mejean, 1991). It 340

is important to note that decrements following breakfast were only demonstrated when 341

measured at least 3 hours later and following consumption of a chocolate drink. It is possible 342

that these detrimental findings in the current study reflect a positive effect of the drink only 343

observed when breakfast has not been consumed, or a negative outcome of the interaction 344

between breakfast and the drink. 345

Paul et al., (1996) failed to find any effect of breakfast consumption prior to cycling on 346

cognitive performance, whereas in the current study negative effects of breakfast on RVIP 347

reaction time and Stroop accuracy were eradicated by combining with an exercise bout. 348

The literature does suggest that exercise is stimulating (Lambourne & Tomporowski, 2010) 349

and an increase in arousal post-exercise can decrease RT (Etnier, et al., 1997) and facilitate 350

the speed of decision making (Adam, et al., 1997; McMorris & Graydon, 1996). Similarly, 351

higher tension ratings reported in the current study when exercise was undertaken, or 352

breakfast was consumed, in isolation were eradicated by combining breakfast consumption 353

and exercise. In line with the current finding of mitigation of mental fatigue through the 354

consumption of breakfast prior to exercising, Paul et al., (1996) previously reported lower 355

ratings of post-exercise central fatigue following breakfast consumption, rather than 356

omission, prior to cycling (Paul, et al., 1996). The authors suggested that the protein in the 357

breakfast likely attenuated an exercise-induced rise in brain serotonin, a neurotransmitter 358

known to increase fatigue sensitivity (Newsholme E.A., Acworth, & Blomstrand, 1987), 359

although physical fatigue during exercise measured using RPE was not affected by prior 360

breakfast consumption, a finding replicated in the current study. In addition, brain 361

requirements for fuel (i.e. glucose) may be high during the post-exercise recovery period (Ide, 362

Horn, & Secher, 1999); if fuel availability is poor, this could be detrimental for mood 363

(Benton & Parker, 1998; Pollitt & Mathews, 1998). Consuming a high CHO meal prior to 364

exercise may reduce this effect by increasing pre-exercise glycogen stores (Hargreaves, 365

Hawley, & Jeukendrup, 2004). These results suggest that consuming breakfast before 366

exercise is beneficial for post-exercise mental state, supporting the original hypothesis. 367

However, it has also been previously reported that breakfast consumption or omission prior to 368

swimming does not alter post-exercise mood (Hill, et al., 2011), suggesting that exercise 369

mode can influence how pre-exercise nutritional state alters post-exercise mood. 370

371

It is important to note that whilst the majority of participants in this study were habitual 372

breakfast consumers, four participants (25%) did not consume breakfast often enough to be 373

classed as habitual consumers. Although there is some evidence to suggest that long-term 374

breakfast habits may influence response to acute breakfast consumption (Halsey, et al., 2012; 375

Pereira, et al., 2011), given that consuming breakfast prior to exercise was not a completely 376

novel situation for any of the participants, it is unlikely this would have significantly 377

influenced the results. Another caveat in the current study was that tolerance to the 378

nutritional interventions was not measured. Although participants confirmed verbally that the 379

test drink and pasta lunch were well liked, some participants did not find the test breakfast 380

pleasant to eat. Findings showing that consuming breakfast which is different in composition 381

from that which is normally consumed can negatively affect mood (Lluch, Hubert, King, & 382

Blundell, 2000) may explain the detrimental impact of breakfast in the current study. 383

Information on the habitual morning exercise routine of this sample would have also been 384

beneficial when assessing the effect of the chosen interventions. 385

Currently, this data only extends to males. Conducting a similar study using a female 386

population, who are generally under-represented in the literature, would add to knowledge in 387

this research area. 388

This is believed to be the first study to establish the effect of consuming or omitting a 389

breakfast meal prior to exercise or rest on cognitive performance and mood. The results 390

suggest that in habitually active males, poorer pre-exercise nutritional state can negatively 391

influence mood state post-exercise after a post-exercise snack. Also, breakfast can adversely 392

affect cognitive performance, an effect reversed by undertaking exercise. 393

394

Acknowledgements 395

This study was completed as part of the PhD of R.C.V., funded by Northumbria University. 396

Data collection was conducted by R.C.V and J.G. All authors contributed to the study design 397

and to the writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript. The 398

authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest. 399

400

References 401

Adam, J. J., Teeken, J. C., Ypelaar, P. J. C., Verstappen, F. T. J., & Paas, F. G. W. (1997). Exercise-402 induced arousal and information processing. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28, 403 217-226. 404

Ali, A., & Williams, C. (2009). Carbohydrate ingestion and soccer skill performance during 405 prolonged intermittent exercise. Journal of Sports Sciences, 27, 1499 - 1508. 406

American Dietetic Association, D. o. C. a. t. A. C. o. S. M. (2009). Position of the American Dietetic 407 Association, Dietitians of Canada, and the American College of Sports Medicine: Nutrition 408 and Athletic Performance. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 109, 509-527. 409

Backhouse, S. H., Williams, C., Stevenson, E., & Nute, M. (2007). Effects of the glycemic index of 410 breakfast on metabolic responses to brisk walking in females. European Journal of Clinical 411 Nutrition, 61, 590-596. 412

Benton, D., & Parker, P. Y. (1998). Breakfast, blood glucose, and cognition. American Journal of 413 Clinical Nutrition, 67, 772S-778S. 414

Benton, D., Slater, O., & Donohoe, R. T. (2001). The influence of breakfast and a snack on 415 psychological functioning. Physiology and Behavior, 74, 559-571. 416

Chepenik, L. G., Cornew, L. A., & Farah, M. J. (2007). The Influence of Sad Mood on Cognition. 417 Emotion, 7, 802-811. 418

Collardeau, M., Brisswalter, J., Vercruyssen, F., Audiffren, M., & Goubault, C. (2001). Single and 419 choice reaction time during prolonged exercise in trained subjects: influence of carbohydrate 420 availability. European Journal of Applied Physiology, 86, 150-156. 421

Craig, C. L., ., Marshall, A. L., Sja-stra-m, M., Bauman, A. E., Booth, M. L., Ainsworth, B. E., Pratt, 422 M., Ekelund, U., Yngve., A., Sallis, J. F., & Oja, O. (2003). International Physical Activity 423 Questionnaire: 12-Country Reliability and Validity. Medicine and Science in Sports and 424 Exercise, 35, 1381-1395. 425

Etnier, J. L., Salazar, W., Launders, D. M., Petruzzello, S. J., Han, M., & Nowell, P. (1997). The 426 influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. 427 Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 19, 249-277. 428

Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power 429 analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research 430 Methods, 39, 175-191. 431

Fischer, K., Colombani, P. C., & Wenk, C. (2004). Metabolic and cognitive coefficients in the 432 development of hunger sensations after pure macronutrient ingestion in the morning. Appetite, 433 42, 49-61. 434

Gonzalez, J. T., Veasey, R. C., Rumbold, P. L. S., & Stevenson, E. J. (2013). Breakfast and exercise 435 contingently affect postprandial metabolism and energy balance in physically active males. 436 British Journal of Nutrition, FirstView, 1-12. 437

Halsey, L. G., Huber, J. W., Low, T., Ibeawuchi, C., Woodruff, P., & Reeves, S. (2012). Does 438 consuming breakfast influence activity levels? An experiment into the effect of breakfast 439 consumption on eating habits and energy expenditure. Public Health Nutrition, 15, 238-245. 440

Hargreaves, M., Hawley, J. A., & Jeukendrup, A. (2004). Pre-exercise carbohydrate and fat ingestion: 441 effects on metabolism and performance. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22, 31-38. 442

Haskell, C. F., Kennedy, D. O., Wesnes, K. A., & Scholey, A. B. (2005). Cognitive and mood 443 improvements of caffeine in habitual consumers and habitual non-consumers of caffeine. 444 Psychopharmacology, 179, 813-825. 445

Hill, K. M., Whitehead, J. R., & Goodwin, J. K. (2011). Pre-workout carbohydrate supplementation 446 does not affect measures of self-assesed vitality and affect in college swimmers J Sports Sci 447 Med, 10, 478-482. 448

Hoyland, A., Lawton, C. L., & Dye, L. (2008). Acute effects of macronutrient manipulations on 449 cognitive test performance in healthy young adults: A systematic research review. 450 Neuroscience &amp; Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 72-85. 451

Hubert, P., King, N. A., & Blundell, J. E. (1998). Uncoupling the Effects of Energy Expenditure and 452 Energy Intake: Appetite Response to Short-term Energy Deficit Induced by Meal Omission 453 and Physical Activity. Appetite, 31, 9-19. 454

Ide, K., Horn, A., & Secher, N. H. (1999). Cerebral metabolic response to submaximal exercise. 455 Journal of Applied Physiology, 87, 1604-1608. 456

Jackson, P. A., Deary, M. E., Reay, J. L., Scholey, A. B., & Kennedy, D. O. (2012). No effect of 12 457 weeks‟ supplementation with 1 g DHA-rich or EPA-rich fish oil on cognitive function or 458 mood in healthy young adults aged 18–35 years. British Journal of Nutrition, 107, 232–1243. 459

Karp, J. R., Johnston, J. D., Tecklenburg, S., Mickleborough, T. D., Fly, A. D., & Stager, J. M. 460 (2006). Chocolate Milk as a Post-Exercise Recovery Aid. International Journal of Sport 461 Nutrition and Exercise Metabolism, 16, 78-91. 462

Kennedy, D., Veasey, R., Watson, A., Dodd, F., Jones, E., Maggini, S., & Haskell, C. (2010). Effects 463 of high-dose B vitamin complex with vitamin C and minerals on subjective mood and 464 performance in healthy males. Psychopharmacology, 211, 55-68. 465

Kennedy, D. O., Dodd, F. L., Robertson, B. C., Okello, E. J., Reay, J. L., Scholey, A. B., & Haskell, 466 C. F. (2011). Monoterpenoid extract of sage (Salvia lavandulaefolia) with cholinesterase 467 inhibiting properties improves cognitive performance and mood in healthy adults. Journal of 468 Psychopharmacology, 25, 1088-1100. 469

Kral, T. V. E., Heo, M., Whiteford, L. M., & Faith, M. S. (2012). Effects on Cognitive Performance 470 of Eating Compared With Omitting Breakfast in Elementary Schoolchildren. Journal of 471 Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 33, 9-16 472

Lambourne, K., & Tomporowski, P. (2010). The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task 473 performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Research, 1341, 12-24. 474

Lieberman, H. R., Falco, C. M., & Slade, S. S. (2002). Carbohydrate administration during a day of 475 sustained aerobic activity improves vigilance, as assessed by a novel ambulatory monitoring 476 device, and mood. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 76, 120-127. 477

Lluch, A., Hubert, P., King, N. A., & Blundell, J. E. (2000). Selective effects of acute exercise and 478 breakfast interventions on mood and motivation to eat. Physiology and Behavior, 68, 515-479 520. 480

Marangoni, F., Poli, A., Agostoni, C., Di Pietro, P., Cricelli, C., Brignoli, O., Fatati, G., Giovannini, 481 M., Riva, E., Marelli, G., Porrini, M., Rotella, C. M., Mele, G., Iughetti, L., & Paoletti, R. 482 (2009). A consensus document on the role of breakfast in the attainment and maintenance of 483 health and wellness. Acta Biomed, 80, 166-171. 484

McMorris, T., & Graydon, J. (1996). The effect of exercise on the decision-making performance of 485 experienced and inexperienced soccer players. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 486 67, 109-114. 487

Michaud, C., Musse, N., Nicolas, J. P., & Mejean, L. (1991). Effects of breakfast-size on short-term 488 memory, concentration, mood and blood glucose. Journal of Adolescent Health, 12, 53-57. 489

Newsholme E.A., Acworth, I. N., & Blomstrand, E. (1987). Amino acids, brain neurotransmitters and 490 a functional link between muscle and brain that is important in sustained exercise. Benzi G, 491 ed. Advances in myochemistry. , London: John Libby Eurotext Ltd, 127-138. 492

NIfHaC, E. (2006). Obesity guidance on the prevention, assessment and management of overweight 493 and obesity in adults and children. Agency LHD. 494

Paul, G. L., Rokusek, J. T., Dykstra, G. L., Boileau, R. A., & Layman, D. K. (1996). Preexercise meal 495 composition alters plasma large neutral amino acid responses during exercise and recovery. 496 American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 64, 778-786. 497

Pereira, M., Erickson, E., McKee, P., Schrankler, K., Raatz, S., Lytle, L., & Pellegrini, A. (2011). 498 Breakfast Frequency and Quality May Affect Glycemia and Appetite in Adults and Children 499 Journal of Nutrition, 141, 163-168. 500

Pollitt, E., & Mathews, R. (1998). Breakfast and cognition: an integrative summary. The American 501 Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 67, 804S-813S. 502

Rogers, P. J., Richardson, N. J., & Elliman, N. A. (1995). Overnight caffeine abstinence and negative 503 reinforcement of preference for caffeine-containing drinks. Psychopharmacology, 120, 457-504 462. 505

Scholey, A., Ossoukhova, A., Owen, L., Ibarra, A., Pipingas, A., He, K., Roller, M., & Stough, C. 506 (2010). Effects of American ginseng (Panax quinquefolius) on neurocognitive function: an 507 acute, randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover study. Psychopharmacology, 508 212, 345-356. 509

Scully, D., Kremer, J., Meade, M. M., Graham, R., & Dudgeon, K. (1998). Physical exercise and 510 psychological well being: a critical review. British Journal of Sports Medicine, 32, 111-120. 511

Smith, A. P., Clark, R., & Gallagher, J. (1999). Breakfast Cereal and Caffeinated Coffee: Effects on 512 Working Memory, Attention, Mood, and Cardiovascular Function. Physiology and Behavior, 513 67, 9-17. 514

Thomas K, M. P., Stevenson E. . (2009). Improved endurance capacity following chocolate milk 515 consumption compared with 2 commercially available sport drinks. . Applied Physiology, 516 Nutrition, and Metabolism, 34, 78-82. 517

Vermorel, M., Bitar, A., Vernet, J., Verdier, E., & Coudert, J. (2003). The extent to which breakfast 518 covers the morning energy expenditure of adolescents with varying levels of physical activity. 519 European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 57, 310-315. 520

Wightman, E. L., Haskell, C. F., Forster, J. S., Veasey, R. C., & Kennedy, D. O. (2012). 521 Epigallocatechin gallate, cerebral blood flow parameters, cognitive performance and mood in 522 healthy humans: a double-blind, placebo-controlled, crossover investigation. Human 523 Psychopharmacol, Clinical and Experimental, 27, 177-186. 524

Williams, C., Nute, M. G., Broadbank, L., & Vinall, S. (1990). Influence of fluid intake on endurance 525 running performance. A comparison between water, glucose and fructose solutions. European 526 Journal of Applied Physiology and Occupational Physiology, 60, 112-119. 527

Figure Legends 528

Fig. 1. Study Schematic 529

Fig. 2. The effect of breakfast consumption or omission (B or NB) on Four Choice Reaction 530 Time accuracy following a mixed-macronutrient drink in active males. Values are change 531

from baseline ± SEM, (n = 12, *p<.05) 532

Fig. 3. The effects of prior breakfast consumption or omission (B or NB) and exercise or rest 533

(E or NE) on (a) Stroop accuracy (n = 11), (b) Rapid Visual Information Processing task 534 reaction time (n = 9), (c) mental fatigue (n = 12) and (d) tension (n = 12) following a mixed-535 macronutrient drink in active males. Values are change from baseline ± SEM, (*p<.05; 536

**p<.01; *** p<.001) 537

538

Table 1. Nutritional content of study foods

kJ/Kcal

Carbohydrate

(g)

Protein

(g)

Fat

(g)

Fibre

(g)

Breakfast

72g Syrup flavour porridge

oats 1129/271 48 6 5 6

360ml Semi-skimmed milk 752/180 17 13 7 0

Total 2560/451 65 19 18 6

Snack

250ml Chocolate milk 713/175 25 8 4 2

Lunch

125g Penne Pasta 1863/444 90 15 2 1

250g Bolognase pasta sauce 440/110 15 2 4 6

15g Oilve Oil 555/135 0 0 15 0

40g Cheddar Cheese 686/249 Trace 10 14 0

Total 3544/938 105 27 35 7

539

Fig. 1 540

541

542

Fig. 2 543

544

545

Fig. 3 546

547

548

549

This study examined the interaction between breakfast and exercise in active males

When breakfast was ommitted, post-exercise mental fatigue increased

Where breakfast inhibited cognitive function, exercise reversed this effect

Breakfast consumption prior to exercise may benefit mood