child language teaching and therapy effectiveness of … language... · 2015-07-21 ·...

34
Child Language Teaching and Therapy 2014, Vol. 30(1) 7–40 © The Author(s) 2013 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0265659013512321 clt.sagepub.com Effectiveness of intervention for grammar in school-aged children with primary language impairments: A review of the evidence Susan Ebbels Moor House School and University College London, UK Abstract This article summarizes the evidence as regards the effectiveness of therapy for grammar for school-aged children with language impairments. I first review studies focusing on specific areas of grammar (both expressive and receptive targets) and then studies aiming to improve language more generally, several of which focus more on the effectiveness of different methods of delivery. I conclude that while there is a growing body of evidence in this area, there are still many gaps. The most concerning gap is the small amount of evidence of effectiveness of intervention for children with receptive as well as expressive language impairments. The evidence to date seems to indicate that these children need specialist, intensive help in order to make progress with their language. Further research is also needed to consider the relative impact of different types of interventions (or their combination) on children of different ages and with different language profiles, including establishing the most effective and/or cost-effective methods of delivery of these interventions. Keywords Evidence base, grammar, intervention, language impairment, review, school-aged children, therapy I Introduction Children with language impairments often have difficulties in many areas of language, but gram- mar is particularly affected. These children often produce short, simple sentences containing gram- matical errors and have difficulties understanding longer and more complex sentences. Language difficulties which are still present by school entry are likely to persist (Stothard et al., 1998), espe- cially if children have receptive as well as expressive language difficulties (Clark et al., 2007). Corresponding author: Susan Ebbels, Moor House School, Mill Lane, Hurst Green, Oxted, Surrey RH8 9AQ, UK. Email: [email protected] 512321CLT 30 1 10.1177/0265659013512321Child Language Teaching and TherapyEbbels research-article 2013 Article at University College London on June 25, 2015 clt.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Upload: others

Post on 29-Jul-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Child Language Teaching and Therapy2014, Vol. 30(1) 7 –40

© The Author(s) 2013Reprints and permissions:

sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.navDOI: 10.1177/0265659013512321

clt.sagepub.com

Effectiveness of intervention for grammar in school-aged children with primary language impairments: A review of the evidence

Susan EbbelsMoor House School and University College London, UK

AbstractThis article summarizes the evidence as regards the effectiveness of therapy for grammar for school-aged children with language impairments. I first review studies focusing on specific areas of grammar (both expressive and receptive targets) and then studies aiming to improve language more generally, several of which focus more on the effectiveness of different methods of delivery. I conclude that while there is a growing body of evidence in this area, there are still many gaps. The most concerning gap is the small amount of evidence of effectiveness of intervention for children with receptive as well as expressive language impairments. The evidence to date seems to indicate that these children need specialist, intensive help in order to make progress with their language. Further research is also needed to consider the relative impact of different types of interventions (or their combination) on children of different ages and with different language profiles, including establishing the most effective and/or cost-effective methods of delivery of these interventions.

KeywordsEvidence base, grammar, intervention, language impairment, review, school-aged children, therapy

I Introduction

Children with language impairments often have difficulties in many areas of language, but gram-mar is particularly affected. These children often produce short, simple sentences containing gram-matical errors and have difficulties understanding longer and more complex sentences. Language difficulties which are still present by school entry are likely to persist (Stothard et al., 1998), espe-cially if children have receptive as well as expressive language difficulties (Clark et al., 2007).

Corresponding author:Susan Ebbels, Moor House School, Mill Lane, Hurst Green, Oxted, Surrey RH8 9AQ, UK.Email: [email protected]

512321 CLT30110.1177/0265659013512321Child Language Teaching and TherapyEbbelsresearch-article2013

Article

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 2: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

8 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Such difficulties are likely to affect their ability to do well at school (Nippold, 2010) and hence their employment prospects (e.g. Law et al., 2009). Thus, there is a need to establish the best ways to help school-aged children with language impairments improve their language abilities and hence their educational and life prospects. Improving their use and understanding of grammar is likely to be one aspect of this.

This review concentrates on intervention studies that aim to improve understanding and use of grammar in school-aged children (over five years) with language impairments. Some studies investigate improvements in general language abilities (of which grammar is a major part) but do not specifically consider grammar. These may use a specific approach (e.g. Fast ForWord, Scientific Learning Corporation, 1998) or a therapy package containing many different ele-ments. The outcome measures of these studies are usually quite general, although some meas-ures more closely related to the intervention itself may also be included. I will consider these studies after examining those that use more specific intervention methods and outcome measures related to grammar.

The majority of published language intervention studies indicate that intervention is generally successful, regardless of the targets or methods used. However, a few important exceptions exist; these are often the studies with more rigorous designs. Many gaps in the evidence persist, where relatively little has been published; this is particularly the case for receptive language. Indeed, previous reviews of the effectiveness of therapy for children with language impairments have con-cluded that ‘the most substantial single gap in the literature … is the lack of good-quality literature about intervention for children with severe receptive language difficulties’ (Law et al., 2004) and that there is ‘an overall lack of evidence for approaches to effective treatment for children with RELI [receptive and expressive language impairment]’ (Boyle et al., 2010).

II Important variables in intervention studies

The ultimate goal of intervention research is to establish which method is the most effective, for which areas of language, for which children, using which method of delivery. The most important variables within the children are likely to be: age, severity and pervasiveness of language difficul-ties and any co-occurring difficulties. When considering different methods of delivery, the varia-bles include: who or what delivers the therapy – e.g. speech and language therapist (SLT), SLT assistant, teaching assistant (TA), teacher, parent, computer – and whether the therapy is delivered 1:1 (one-to-one) or with other children. If the therapy is delivered with other children: how many others (e.g. in a pair, small group, large group) and who are the other children (in terms of age, diagnosis, etc)? The duration and distribution of therapy are also important variables as, of course, is the precise nature of the therapy itself.

When appraising a particular study, it is important first to consider the research design. Some designs are much more robust than others, and this depends on the degree of experimental control provided by the study and hence how many other possible factors can be ruled out. For further discussion of these factors see Ebbels (2008: 150–52).

Practitioners are aided in their appraisal of the evidence by searchable websites, which rate articles or interventions according to the strength of their research design and hence the reliability of their findings. The SpeechBITE website http://www.speechbite.com (accessed September 2013) has the facility to search for published articles on all areas of speech and language therapy. The resulting studies are listed in order of the strength of their design, although single case experimen-tal designs have not yet been rated. The recently launched What Works website https://www.thecommunicationtrust.org.uk/whatworks (accessed September 2013), hosted by the Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 3: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 9

intervention methods or packages. The evidence for each is reviewed and rated as strong, moderate or indicative.

Another factor to consider when evaluating intervention studies is how specific or general the outcome measures are and how closely related they are to the intervention. The effectiveness of an intervention is also indicated by whether positive effects are maintained after intervention ceases and whether they generalize to similar linguistic targets and to spontaneous use and comprehension of language in a range of settings.

In this review, I start by considering studies specifically focused on grammar and review the evidence for different methods of intervention. Tables 1–3 show the key features of all the studies discussed; I therefore leave out many of these details from my discussion as the information can be found in the tables. The studies are grouped into tables by target area and sorted within each table according to the level of experimental control. This is so that practitioners wishing to focus on a particular area of language can quickly find the evidence relating to that area. More confidence can be placed in studies higher up the tables due to their stronger designs. Table 1 includes studies focusing on specific expressive language targets, and Table 2 includes studies focusing on specific receptive language targets. The studies in Table 3 also focus on expressive language, but not on specific targets. The tables do not include studies teaching artificial rules or novel linguistic forms. Following this, I review studies with more general language outcome measures that would be influenced by changes in grammatical ability, but include other areas of language.

III Intervention approaches aimed specifically at grammar

Two main approaches to improving grammar in school-aged children with language impairments have been studied: grammar facilitation and meta-linguistic methods. Grammar facilitation approaches are predominantly implicit and meta-linguistic approaches predominantly explicit (and usually involve visual support). In practice, a mixture of explicit and implicit approaches may be used, and the balance between the two may change as the child moves through therapy. Some stud-ies have examined the effectiveness of primarily implicit or explicit approaches, and some involve a combination. A few aim to compare the relative effectiveness of these two methods.

1 Implicit approaches

a Grammar facilitation methods. Grammar facilitation methods are the most widely investigated in intervention research studies. These aim to make target forms more frequent, which is hypoth-esized to help the child identify grammatical rules and give the child practice at producing forms they tend to omit. The studies are mostly with pre-school or early school-aged children, many of whom have expressive language difficulties only. Indeed, the focus of grammar facilitation meth-ods is on improving expressive language; receptive language is rarely mentioned. The most com-mon grammar facilitation approaches are: imitation, modelling or focused stimulation, and recasting.

(i) Imitation. Imitation approaches usually involve the adult providing a non-verbal stimulus (e.g. a picture) and a target form, which the child then imitates, receiving reinforcement for correct productions. The adult model and reinforcements are gradually reduced until the child produces the target in response to the non-verbal stimulus only. Two early randomized control trials (RCTs) showed that imitation approaches can be effective for improving production of syntax in general (Matheny and Panagos, 1978) and yes/no questions in particular (Mulac and Tomlinson, 1977). However, in the latter study, progress only generalized to other settings for those children who

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 4: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

10 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Tab

le 1

. St

udie

s ta

rget

ing

spec

ific

expr

essi

ve la

ngua

ge t

arge

ts.

Spec

ific

targ

ets

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

ar

inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

Expr

essi

ve

argu

men

t st

ruct

ure

Ebbe

ls e

t al

. (2

007)

18t

(9t:

Shap

e C

odin

g, 9

t: ve

rb

sem

antic

s), 9

c

11;0

–16;

1SL

I (re

cept

ive

and

expr

essi

ve)

met

alin

guis

tic:

Shap

e C

odin

g vs

. ver

b se

man

tics

dire

ct 1

:1ye

sR

CT

: 2

ther

apy

grou

ps p

lus

cont

rol g

roup

1 ×

30

min

utes

pe

r w

eek

(=

4.5

hour

s)

both

the

rapy

gro

ups

impr

oved

mor

e th

an

cont

rols

afte

r 3

mon

ths

to c

ontr

ol v

erbs

yes/

no q

uest

ion

form

atio

nM

ulac

and

T

omlin

son

(197

7)

6t (

3t: g

ram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n,

3t: g

ram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n +

tra

nsfe

r pr

ogra

mm

e),

3c (

artic

ulat

ion

trai

ning

)

4;4–

6;3

lang

uage

de

laye

d, p

lus

failu

re t

o us

e is

inte

rrog

ativ

e

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

imita

tion

(plu

s tr

ansf

er

prog

ram

me

for

3t)

dire

ct 1

:1,

plus

3t

pare

nts

give

n ta

sks

to d

o, b

ut

no m

entio

n of

tra

inin

g pa

rent

s

yes

RC

T: 2

gr

amm

ar

ther

apy

grou

ps p

lus

cont

rol g

roup

(a

rtic

ulat

ion

ther

apy)

2.8

hour

s (p

lus

1.92

ho

urs

tran

sfer

pr

ogra

mm

e fo

r 3t

)

both

gra

mm

ar t

hera

py

grou

ps im

prov

ed in

the

cl

inic

situ

atio

n

20–2

6 da

ys

afte

r en

d of

th

erap

y

to o

ther

se

ttin

gs o

nly

if ex

tend

ed

tran

sfer

tra

inin

g gi

ven

wh-

ques

tion

form

atio

nW

ilcox

and

Le

onar

d (1

978)

12t,

12c

3;8–

8;2

lang

uage

di

sord

ered

, al

l bel

ow 1

0th

perc

entil

e of

ex

pres

sive

m

easu

re,

com

preh

ensi

on

not

men

tione

d

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

mod

ellin

g

dire

ct 1

:1ye

sth

erap

y vs

. del

ayed

tr

eatm

ent

grou

p, R

CT

ex

cept

for

3 ch

ildre

n

not

stat

edth

erap

y gr

oup

impr

oved

m

ore

than

wai

ting

cont

rols

; per

form

ance

of

wai

ting

cont

rols

aft

er

ther

apy

not

disc

usse

d

not

mea

sure

dis

inve

rsio

n ge

nera

lized

to

wh-

ques

tions

finite

mor

phem

esT

yler

et

al.

(200

2)20

t (1

0t:

mor

phos

ynta

x th

en

phon

olog

y, 1

0t:

phon

olog

y th

en

mor

phos

ynta

x),

7c

3;0–

5;11

expr

essi

ve

lang

uage

and

ph

onol

ogic

al

impa

irm

ent

(7t

REL

I)

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

focu

sed

stim

ulat

ion

and

elic

ited

prod

uctio

n,

reca

sts

and

expa

nsio

ns

dire

ct 1

:1 p

lus

grou

p (1

:3)

with

gra

duat

e st

uden

t SL

T

inte

rns

yes

2 th

erap

y gr

oups

(r

ando

mly

as

sign

ed)

+

cont

rol g

roup

(n

ot r

ando

mly

as

sign

ed)

2 ×

30

min

utes

1:

1 pl

us 4

5 m

inut

es 1

:3

per

wee

k fo

r 12

wee

ks

initi

ally

, the

n an

othe

r 12

w

eeks

(=

20

hou

rs

mor

phos

ynta

x +

20

hour

s ph

onol

ogy

ther

apy)

afte

r 12

wee

ks,

mor

phos

ynta

x th

erap

y gr

oup

mad

e m

ore

prog

ress

tha

n co

ntro

ls

on m

orph

osyn

tax

(as

did

phon

olog

y gr

oup

on

phon

olog

y); p

hono

logy

th

erap

y di

d no

t im

prov

e m

orph

osyn

tax

alth

ough

re

vers

e w

as t

rue;

aft

er

24 w

eeks

, no

effe

ct o

f or

der

of t

hera

py

not

mea

sure

dm

orph

osyn

tax

inte

rven

tion

gene

raliz

ed t

o sp

onta

neou

s sp

eech

and

als

o to

pho

nolo

gica

l sk

ills

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 5: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 11

Spec

ific

targ

ets

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

ar

inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

finite

mor

phem

esT

yler

et

al.

(200

3)40

t (1

0t: b

lock

of

mor

phos

ynta

x th

en p

hono

logy

, 10

t: bl

ock

of

phon

olog

y th

en

mor

phos

ynta

x,

11t:

alte

rnat

ing

wee

kly,

9t:

sim

ulta

neou

s ph

onol

ogy

and

mor

phos

ynta

x),

7c

3;0–

5;11

lang

uage

and

ph

onol

ogic

al

impa

irm

ent

(all

expr

essi

ve

impa

irm

ent,

som

e al

so

rece

ptiv

e)

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

focu

sed

stim

ulat

ion

and

elic

ited

prod

uctio

n,

reca

sts

and

expa

nsio

ns

dire

ct 1

:1 p

lus

grou

p (1

:3)

with

gra

duat

e st

uden

t SL

T

inte

rns

yes

4 th

erap

y gr

oups

(r

ando

mly

as

sign

ed)

+

cont

rol g

roup

(n

ot r

ando

mly

as

sign

ed)

2 ×

30

min

utes

1:

1 pl

us 4

5 m

inut

es 1

:3

per

wee

k fo

r 24

wee

ks

(= 2

0 ho

urs

mor

phos

ynta

x +

20

hour

s ph

onol

ogy

ther

apy)

afte

r 12

wee

ks,

mor

phos

ynta

x an

d al

tern

atin

g th

erap

y gr

oups

mad

e m

ore

prog

ress

tha

n co

ntro

ls

on m

orph

osyn

tax;

aft

er

24 w

eeks

: gre

ates

t ga

ins

in m

orph

osyn

tax

in a

ltern

atin

g th

erap

y gr

oup

with

larg

e ef

fect

(d

> 1

) (c

hang

es

in p

hono

logy

not

si

gnifi

cant

)

not

mea

sure

dm

orph

osyn

tax

inte

rven

tion

gene

raliz

ed t

o sp

onta

neou

s sp

eech

is, d

on’t

Leon

ard

(197

5)4t

, 4c

5–9

year

sde

ficie

ncie

s in

gr

amm

atic

al

expr

essi

on

with

no

use

of

is or

don

’t

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

mod

ellin

g

dire

ct 1

:1ye

sth

erap

y vs

. del

ayed

tr

eatm

ent

grou

p (n

ot

rand

omly

as

sign

ed)

1.25

hou

rsth

erap

y gr

oup

impr

oved

m

ore

than

wai

ting

cont

rols

; per

form

ance

of

wai

ting

cont

rols

aft

er

ther

apy

not

disc

usse

d

not

mea

sure

dno

t m

easu

red

wh-

ques

tion

and

pass

ive

form

atio

nEb

bels

and

va

n de

r Le

ly

(200

1)

4t11

–14

year

sSL

I (re

cept

ive

and

expr

essi

ve)

met

alin

guis

tic:

Shap

e C

odin

gdi

rect

1:1

yes

Mul

tiple

ba

selin

e2

× 3

0 m

inut

es

per

wee

k fo

r 10

wee

ks (

=

10 h

ours

) on

pa

ssiv

es, f

or

20 w

eeks

(=

20

hou

rs)

on

wh-

ques

tions

3/4

child

ren

show

ed

sign

ifica

nt p

rogr

ess

with

pa

ssiv

es, a

ll pr

ogre

ssed

w

ith w

h-qu

estio

ns

at 3

0 w

eeks

: pa

ssiv

es fo

r 2

child

ren,

w

h-qu

estio

ns

for

1 ch

ild

not

mea

sure

d

(Con

tinue

d)

Tab

le 1

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 6: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

12 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Spec

ific

targ

ets

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

ar

inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

subj

ect

pron

ouns

he

and

she

, po

sses

sive

-s,

past

te

nse

-ed

Smith

-Loc

k et

al

. (20

13a)

19t,

15c

5 ye

ars

SLI (

on

aver

age

both

ex

pres

sive

an

d re

cept

ive

lang

uage

af

fect

ed)

dire

ct t

each

ing

plus

gra

mm

ar

faci

litat

ion:

fo

cuse

d st

imul

atio

n,

reca

stin

g an

d im

itatio

n

smal

l gro

up

(3–5

chi

ldre

n)

wor

k w

ith

SLT

, tea

cher

an

d as

sist

ant

(who

had

re

ceiv

ed

trai

ning

and

m

anua

l)

yes

mat

ched

co

ntro

l gro

up

(not

ran

dom

ly

assi

gned

) an

d m

ultip

le

base

line

1 ×

1 h

our

wee

kly

for

8 w

eeks

(=

8

hour

s)

expe

rim

enta

l gro

up

impr

oved

mor

e th

an

cont

rols

, but

onl

y w

hen

rece

ived

the

rapy

, no

t du

ring

bas

elin

e;

effe

ct s

peci

fic t

o ta

rget

ed s

truc

ture

s; a

t si

ngle

-cas

e le

vel,

10/1

9 sh

owed

a s

igni

fican

t tr

eatm

ent

effe

ct, 3

/19

a no

n-si

gnifi

cant

effe

ct

in fa

vour

of t

reat

men

t; 6

mad

e no

pro

gres

s,

but

5 of

the

se h

ad

artic

ulat

ion

diffi

culti

es

whi

ch w

ould

inte

rfer

e w

ith p

rodu

ctio

n of

the

ta

rget

ed g

ram

mat

ical

co

nstr

uctio

n

not

mea

sure

dno

t m

easu

red

to o

ther

si

tuat

ions

. Did

no

t ge

nera

lize

to c

ontr

ol it

ems

regu

lar

past

ten

seK

ulka

rni e

t al

. (in

pre

ss)

2t8;

11 a

nd

8;10

part

icip

ant

A: l

angu

age

diso

rder

, pa

rtic

ipan

t B:

A

SD (

both

ha

d ex

pres

sive

an

d re

cept

ive

impa

irm

ents

)

Shap

e C

odin

g pl

us g

ram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n (r

ecas

ting

and

elic

ited

imita

tion)

phas

e 1:

1:1

w

ith S

LT;

phas

e 2:

ge

nera

lizat

ion

ther

apy:

ac

tiviti

es a

nd

advi

ce g

iven

to

tea

cher

s an

d pa

rent

s fo

r ca

rryo

ver

yes

mul

tiple

ba

selin

e de

sign

phas

e 1:

1 ×

30

min

utes

per

w

eek

with

SLT

fo

r 10

wee

ks

(= 5

hou

rs),

plus

3.5

hou

rs

with

TA

for

part

icip

ant

A (

tota

l =

8.5

hour

s;

0.5

hour

s fo

r pa

rtic

ipan

t B

(tot

al =

5.5

ho

urs)

; pha

se

2: S

LT c

arri

ed

out

4 se

ssio

ns

in c

lass

, par

ent

mee

ting

and

sess

ion

at

part

icip

ants

’ ho

mes

.

part

icip

ant

A: s

tabl

e ba

selin

e, t

hen

sign

ifica

nt

prog

ress

on

sent

ence

co

mpl

etio

n fo

r tr

eate

d an

d un

trea

ted

verb

s af

ter

phas

e 1,

pro

gres

s on

con

vers

atio

n ta

sk

only

aft

er p

hase

2;

no c

hang

e in

con

trol

st

ruct

ure;

par

ticip

ant

B: s

tabl

e ba

selin

e, t

hen

sign

ifica

nt p

rogr

ess

with

con

vers

atio

n af

ter

phas

e 1,

pro

gres

s in

se

nten

ce c

ompl

etio

n ta

sk o

nly

sign

ifica

nt

afte

r ph

ase

2; n

o ch

ange

in

con

trol

str

uctu

re

part

icip

ant

A: y

es fo

r 6

wee

ks; n

ot

mea

sure

d in

pa

rtic

ipan

t B

yes;

for

part

icip

ant

B,

gene

raliz

atio

n oc

curr

ed t

o co

nver

satio

n du

ring

pha

se

1; p

artic

ipan

t A

nee

ded

gene

raliz

atio

n th

erap

y (p

hase

2)

for

prog

ress

to

gen

eral

ize

to

conv

ersa

tion

Tab

le 1

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 7: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 13

Spec

ific

targ

ets

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

ar

inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

subj

ect

and

poss

essi

ve

pron

ouns

, pr

esen

t an

d pa

st

tens

e

Smith

-Loc

k et

al

. (20

13b)

31t:

(13t

: di

stri

bute

d th

erap

y, 1

8t:

conc

entr

ated

th

erap

y)

5 ye

ars

SLI (

17/3

1t

had

REL

I, re

mai

nder

ex

pres

sive

on

ly)

dire

ct t

each

ing

plus

gra

mm

ar

faci

litat

ion:

fo

cuse

d st

imul

atio

n,

reca

stin

g an

d im

itatio

n

As

Smith

-Loc

k et

al.

(201

3a),

but

com

pare

d w

eekl

y vs

. da

ily t

hera

py

yes

mat

ched

co

ntro

l gro

up

(not

ran

dom

ly

assi

gned

) an

d si

ngle

bas

elin

e

1 ×

60

min

utes

ei

ther

wee

kly

or d

aily

(=

8

hour

s)

dist

ribu

ted

grou

p m

ade

sign

ifica

ntly

m

ore

prog

ress

with

th

erap

y th

an d

urin

g ba

selin

e; c

once

ntra

ted

grou

p m

ade

sim

ilar

prog

ress

in b

asel

ine

and

with

the

rapy

, re

gard

less

of w

heth

er

mea

sure

d im

med

iate

ly

afte

r th

erap

y or

aft

er

8 w

eeks

(to

mat

ch

post

-the

rapy

tes

ting

peri

od o

f dis

trib

uted

gr

oup)

; ind

ivid

ual

anal

yses

sho

wed

6/1

3 of

dis

trib

uted

gro

up

show

ed s

igni

fican

t tr

eatm

ent

effe

ct w

hile

3/

18 o

f con

cent

rate

d gr

oup

did

so

not

mea

sure

dno

t m

easu

red

gram

mat

ical

cas

e in

Ger

man

Mot

sch

and

Rie

hem

ann

(200

8)

63t,

63c

(mod

ellin

g ap

proa

ches

, st

anda

rd

ther

apy)

8;6–

10;1

SLI (

84/1

26

had

rece

ptiv

e di

fficu

lties

)

‘Con

text

-op

timiz

atio

n’

(inco

rpor

ates

gr

amm

ar

faci

litat

ion

and

met

alin

guis

tic

appr

oach

es a

nd

wri

ting)

with

in r

egul

ar

less

ons

by

dual

tra

ined

te

ache

r-SL

T

yes

ther

apy

vs.

‘con

trol

’ gr

oup

(ass

igne

d ac

cord

ing

to w

heth

er

teac

her

sign

ed u

p fo

r ad

ditio

nal

trai

ning

)

expe

rim

enta

l gr

oup:

12

hou

rs

inco

rpor

ated

in

reg

ular

le

sson

s (o

n av

erag

e 17

m

inut

es, 4

×

per

wee

k);

cont

rol g

roup

: tim

e no

t st

ated

expe

rim

enta

l gro

up

impr

oved

mor

e on

use

of

dat

ive

case

; bot

h gr

oups

impr

oved

on

use

of a

ccus

ativ

e ca

se

yes,

for

3 m

onth

sno

t m

easu

red

(Con

tinue

d)

Tab

le 1

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 8: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

14 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Spec

ific

targ

ets

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

ar

inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

verb

arg

umen

t st

ruct

ure

Bold

erso

n et

al

. (20

11)

6t5;

3–6;

6ex

pres

sive

di

fficu

lties

th

at in

clud

ed

wor

d or

der

and

omis

sion

pr

oble

ms,

and

po

or v

erb

know

ledg

e an

d us

e

met

alin

guis

tic:

Col

ourf

ul

Sem

antic

s

dire

ct 1

:1ye

sgr

oup

stud

y,

sing

le b

asel

ine

30–4

5 m

inut

es,

2 ×

per

wee

k fo

r 8

wee

ks (

=

4–6

hour

s)

spec

ific

verb

tes

t an

d T

RO

G: i

mpr

oved

du

ring

bas

elin

e an

d th

erap

y (n

ot s

igni

fican

tly

diffe

rent

); R

APT

(b

oth

gram

mar

and

in

form

atio

n) a

nd b

us

stor

y (in

form

atio

n an

d m

ean

sent

ence

leng

th):

no p

rogr

ess

duri

ng

base

line,

but

sig

nific

ant

prog

ress

aft

er t

hera

py

not

mea

sure

dto

sta

ndar

dize

d te

sts

expr

essi

on a

nd

com

preh

ensi

on o

f pa

ssiv

es

Ric

hes

(201

3)2t

8;1

and

8;2

SLI (

rece

ptiv

e an

d ex

pres

sive

)us

age-

base

d pr

inci

ples

, usi

ng

‘con

stru

ctio

n gr

ound

ing’

and

‘c

onsp

irac

y’

dire

ct 1

:1ye

s2

case

stu

dies

, si

ngle

bas

elin

e;

cont

rol

mea

sure

(r

elat

ive

clau

ses)

for

one

child

20–3

0 m

inut

es

per

wee

k fo

r 6

wee

ks (

=

2 ho

urs,

30

min

utes

)

both

chi

ldre

n si

gnifi

cant

ly im

prov

ed

both

com

preh

ensi

on

and

prod

uctio

n of

pa

ssiv

es; c

hild

with

co

ntro

l mea

sure

did

not

im

prov

e on

thi

s

not

mea

sure

dno

t to

con

trol

m

easu

re

they

Cou

rtw

righ

t an

d C

ourt

wri

ght

(197

6)

8t (

4t: i

mita

tion,

4t

: mod

ellin

g)5–

10

year

sdi

sord

ered

in

use

of ‘t

hey’

(u

sed

‘them

’ in

stea

d)

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

mod

ellin

g vs

. im

itatio

n

dire

ct 1

:1no

2 th

erap

y gr

oups

(no

t ra

ndom

ly

assi

gned

)

3 ×

20

min

utes

(=

1 h

our)

Mod

ellin

g gr

oup

impr

oved

mor

e th

an

imita

tion

grou

p

not

mea

sure

dto

spo

ntan

eous

sp

eech

regu

lar

past

ten

se

and

plur

als

Seef

f-Gab

riel

et

al.

(201

2)1t

5;1

Spee

ch a

nd

lang

uage

di

fficu

lties

past

ten

se:

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n (m

odel

ling

and

elic

itatio

n) +

m

etal

ingu

istic

(v

isua

l sym

bols

); pl

ural

s:

phon

olog

y th

erap

y

dire

ct 1

:1 +

ca

rryo

ver

from

mot

her

and

scho

ol

notr

eate

d vs

. un

trea

ted

verb

s

1 ×

30

min

utes

pe

r w

eek

for

10 w

eeks

(=

5

hour

s) +

ca

rryo

ver

from

m

othe

r an

d sc

hool

past

ten

se: s

igni

fican

t pr

ogre

ss; p

lura

ls: /

s/ a

nd

/z/ p

rodu

ced

corr

ectly

in

mon

omor

phem

ic

wor

ds a

fter

pho

nolo

gy

ther

apy;

/s/ g

ener

aliz

ed

to p

lura

ls; /

z/ p

lura

l pr

oduc

ed a

s [d

] an

d th

en a

fter

furt

her

phon

olog

ical

the

rapy

as

[dz

]

yes,

for

past

te

nse

for

8 w

eeks

to u

ntar

gete

d re

gula

r ve

rbs,

bu

t no

t to

ir

regu

lar

verb

s

Tab

le 1

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 9: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 15

Spec

ific

targ

ets

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

ar

inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

use

and

inve

rsio

n of

aux

is (

3 ch

ildre

n), u

se o

f he

(on

e ch

ild)

Ellis

- W

eism

er

and

Mur

ray-

Br

anch

(19

89)

4t5;

5–6;

11ex

pres

sive

la

ngua

ge d

elay

(o

ne a

lso

had

phon

olog

ical

an

d co

mpr

ehen

sion

di

fficu

lties

)

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

mod

ellin

g vs

. m

odel

ling

+ e

voke

d pr

oduc

tion

dire

ct 1

:1no

4 ca

se s

tudi

es:

alte

rnat

ing

trea

tmen

ts,

not

mul

tiple

ba

selin

e

4.67

–7.5

hou

rsbo

th a

ppro

ache

s ef

fect

ive

for

is (c

hild

ren

with

exp

ress

ive

lang

uage

del

ay),

no

prog

ress

with

‘he’

for

child

with

add

ition

al

diffi

culti

es

not

mea

sure

dno

t m

easu

red

expr

essi

ve

lang

uage

, es

peci

ally

ar

gum

ent

stru

ctur

e

Spoo

ner

(200

2)2t

6;3

and

9;9

expr

essi

ve

and

rece

ptiv

e la

ngua

ge

diso

rder

, plu

s w

ord

findi

ng

diffi

culti

es a

nd

dysp

raxi

a

met

alin

guis

tic:

Col

ourf

ul

Sem

antic

s

dire

ct 1

:1no

pre-

vs.

pos

t-te

st, i

nclu

ding

so

me

stan

dard

sc

ores

appr

ox 2

2 ho

urs

one

child

pro

gres

sed

in a

rgum

ent

stru

ctur

e;

both

chi

ldre

n im

prov

ed

othe

r ar

eas

of

expr

essi

ve la

ngua

ge

not

mea

sure

dto

gen

eral

la

ngua

ge t

ests

past

ten

se

mor

phol

ogy

Ebbe

ls (

2007

)9t

11–1

3 ye

ars

SLI (

REL

I)m

etal

ingu

istic

: Sh

ape

Cod

ing

dire

ct g

roup

+

(fo

r 2

child

ren)

1:2

nopr

e- v

s.

post

-tes

t, no

t st

anda

rd

scor

es

1 ho

ur p

er

wee

k fo

r 16

w

eeks

(=

16

hou

rs)

+ a

ppro

x 4

hour

s fo

r 2

child

ren

6 ch

ildre

n im

prov

ed

with

gro

up t

hera

py; 2

im

prov

ed o

nly

afte

r ad

ditio

nal p

aire

d th

erap

y

not

mea

sure

dto

spo

ntan

eous

w

ritin

g

expr

essi

ve

argu

men

t st

ruct

ure

Brya

n (1

997)

1t5;

10ex

pres

sive

la

ngua

ge

diso

rder

met

alin

guis

tic:

Col

ourf

ul

Sem

antic

s

dire

ct 1

:1no

pre-

vs.

po

st-t

est,

not

stan

dard

sc

ores

appr

ox 2

2 ho

urs

mos

t se

nten

ces

cont

aine

d co

rrec

t ar

gum

ent

stru

ctur

e af

ter

ther

apy

not

mea

sure

dto

a g

ener

al

lang

uage

te

st a

nd

spon

tane

ous

spee

ch in

cla

ss

Not

es. A

t nu

mbe

r of

par

ticip

ants

, t =

tre

ated

and

c =

con

trol

; 1:1

= o

ne-t

o-on

e; A

SD =

aut

istic

spe

ctru

m d

isor

der;

RA

PT =

Ren

frew

Act

ion

Pict

ure

Tes

t (R

enfr

ew, 2

003)

; RC

T =

ran

dom

-iz

ed c

ontr

olle

d tr

ial;

REL

I = R

ecep

tive

and

Expr

essi

ve L

angu

age

Impa

irm

ent;

SLI =

spe

cific

lang

uage

impa

irm

ent;

SLT

= s

peec

h an

d la

ngua

ge t

hera

pist

; TA

= t

each

ing

assi

stan

t.

Tab

le 1

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 10: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

16 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Tab

le 2

. St

udie

s ta

rget

ing

spec

ific

rece

ptiv

e la

ngua

ge t

arge

ts.

Spec

ific

targ

ets

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

ar

inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

com

preh

ensi

on

of c

oord

inat

ing

conj

unct

ions

Ebbe

ls e

t al

. (20

13)

14t

(7

wer

e w

aitin

g co

ntro

ls)

11;3

–16

;1pr

imar

y la

ngua

ge

impa

irm

ents

(R

ELI)

met

alin

guis

tic:

Shap

e C

odin

g1:

1 w

ith

SLT

yes

RC

T: t

hera

py

vs. w

aitin

g co

ntro

l gro

up

1 ×

30

min

utes

per

w

eek

for

8 w

eeks

(=

4

hour

s)

sign

ifica

nt

prog

ress

w

ith t

hera

py

on t

arge

ted

conj

unct

ions

yes

for

4 m

onth

sye

s to

T

RO

G-2

, n

ot t

o pa

ssiv

es

reve

rsib

le

sent

ence

s (p

assi

ves,

co

mpa

rativ

es

and

sent

ence

s in

clud

ing

prep

ositi

ons)

Bish

op e

t al

. (20

06)

24t

(12t

: m

odifi

ed

spee

ch,

12t:

unm

odifi

ed

spee

ch)

9c

8–13

ye

ars

rece

ptiv

e la

ngua

ge

impa

irm

ent

acou

stic

ally

m

odifi

ed v

s.

unm

odifi

ed

spee

ch

com

pute

r 1:

1ye

s2

ther

apy

grou

ps p

lus

cont

rol

grou

p:

min

imiz

atio

n m

etho

d

1.5–

7.25

hou

rsno

diff

eren

ces

betw

een

grou

ps

n/a

n/a

com

preh

ensi

on

of p

assi

ves

and

wh-

ques

tions

Ebbe

ls

and

van

der

Lely

(2

001)

4t11

;8–

12;9

SLI (

REL

I)m

etal

ingu

istic

: Sh

ape

Cod

ing

dire

ct 1

:1ye

s4

case

st

udie

s,

mul

tiple

ba

selin

e

2 ×

30

min

utes

per

w

eek

for

10

wee

ks (

= 1

0 ho

urs)

on

pass

ives

, for

20

wee

ks (

=

20 h

ours

) on

w

h-qu

estio

ns

3/4

child

ren

prog

ress

ed

with

pas

sive

s,

2/2

prog

ress

ed

with

wh-

ques

tions

at 3

0 w

eeks

not

mea

sure

d

com

preh

ensi

on

of d

ativ

e an

d w

h-co

mpa

rativ

e qu

estio

ns

Ebbe

ls

(200

7)3t

11;8

–12

;9SL

I (R

ELI)

met

alin

guis

tic:

Shap

e C

odin

gdi

rect

1:1

yes

3 ca

se

stud

ies,

m

ultip

le

base

line

2 ×

30

min

utes

per

w

eek

for

10

wee

ks (

= 1

0 ho

urs)

2/3

child

ren

prog

ress

ed

with

dat

ive,

2/

2 pr

ogre

ssed

w

ith w

h-co

mpa

rativ

e qu

estio

ns

not

mea

sure

dno

t m

easu

red

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 11: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 17

Spec

ific

targ

ets

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

ar

inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

expr

essi

on a

nd

com

preh

ensi

on

of p

assi

ves

Ric

hes

(201

3)2t

8;1

and

8;2

SLI (

REL

I)us

age-

base

d pr

inci

ples

, usi

ng

‘con

stru

ctio

n gr

ound

ing’

and

‘c

onsp

irac

y’

dire

ct 1

:1ye

s2

case

st

udie

s, s

ingl

e ba

selin

e;

cont

rol

mea

sure

for

one

child

20–3

0 m

inut

es

per

wee

k fo

r 6

wee

ks (

= 2

.5

hour

s)

both

chi

ldre

n si

gnifi

cant

ly

impr

oved

bot

h co

mpr

ehen

sion

an

d pr

oduc

tion

of p

assi

ves;

ch

ild w

ith

cont

rol

mea

sure

did

no

t im

prov

e on

thi

s

not

mea

sure

dno

t m

easu

red

com

preh

ensi

on

and

prod

uctio

n of

str

uctu

res

invo

lvin

g sy

ntac

tic

‘mov

emen

t’ (r

elat

ive

clau

ses

and

topi

caliz

atio

n)

in H

ebre

w

Levy

and

Fr

iedm

ann

(200

9)

1t12

;2sy

ntac

tic S

LI

(REL

I)m

etal

ingu

istic

(c

olou

r-co

ded

verb

s an

d th

eir

argu

men

ts a

nd

expl

icitl

y ta

ught

to

mov

e th

em

to d

iffer

ent

posi

tions

in t

he

sent

ence

)

1:1

with

re

sear

cher

nopr

e- v

s.

post

-tes

t (n

ot s

tand

ard

scor

es)

20–6

0 m

inut

es

× 1

6 se

ssio

ns,

over

6 m

onth

s (=

?11

hou

rs)

sign

ifica

nt

prog

ress

in

mos

t ar

eas

targ

eted

not

mea

sure

dye

s, t

o w

h-qu

estio

ns

(whi

ch w

ere

not

dire

ctly

ta

rget

ed)

Not

es. S

ee n

otes

at

Tab

le 1

.

Tab

le 2

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 12: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

18 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)T

able

3.

Stud

ies

targ

etin

g im

prov

emen

ts in

exp

ress

ive

lang

uage

(no

t ta

rget

spe

cific

).

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

arin

terv

entio

n m

etho

d

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

Fey

et a

l. (1

993)

(p

hase

1)

21t

(11t

: cl

inic

ian

grou

p, 1

0t:

pare

nt g

roup

), 9c

(w

aitin

g co

ntro

ls, w

ho

late

r go

t: 4

clin

icia

n, 4

pa

rent

)

3;8–

5;10

clin

ical

ly

sign

ifica

nt

impa

irm

ent

of e

xpre

ssiv

e gr

amm

ar; 1

3 ha

d R

ELI a

nd 5

ha

d no

n-ve

rbal

IQ

in 7

0s

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

mod

ellin

g/fo

cuse

d st

imul

atio

n pl

us r

ecas

ting

(plu

s im

itatio

n fo

r cl

inic

ian

grou

p)

clin

icia

n gr

oup:

di

rect

: 1:1

+

gro

up

(4–6

ch

ildre

n);

pare

nt

grou

p:

indi

rect

th

roug

h pa

rent

gr

oups

and

in

divi

dual

pa

rent

se

ssio

ns

yes

RC

T: 2

th

erap

y gr

oups

plu

s co

ntro

l gr

oup

clin

icia

n gr

oup:

60

hou

rs

(chi

ldre

n);

pare

nt g

roup

: 56

hou

rs

(par

ents

)

both

the

rapy

gr

oups

impr

oved

m

ore

than

con

trol

s (w

ho s

how

ed

no c

hang

e) o

n de

velo

pmen

tal

sent

ence

sco

res

(DSS

); w

aitin

g co

ntro

ls s

how

ed

sim

ilar

prog

ress

w

hen

they

re

ceiv

ed c

linic

ian

ther

apy,

but

not

pa

rent

the

rapy

; m

ore

relia

ble

prog

ress

in

clin

icia

n gr

oup

not

mea

sure

d (b

ut s

ee F

ey e

t al

., 19

97)

to

spon

tane

ous

spee

ch

Fey

et a

l. (1

997)

(p

hase

2)

18t

(9t:

clin

icia

n gr

oup,

9t

: par

ent

grou

p), 1

0c

(dis

mis

sed

grou

p w

ho

had

rece

ived

in

terv

entio

n in

ph

ase

1)

not

stat

ed:

?4;1

–6;3

?

mar

ked

dela

ys

in g

ram

mar

de

velo

pmen

t

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

mod

ellin

g/fo

cuse

d st

imul

atio

n pl

us r

ecas

ting

(plu

s im

itatio

n fo

r cl

inic

ian

grou

p)

as fo

r Fe

y et

al.

(199

3)ye

sR

CT

: 2

ther

apy

grou

ps p

lus

cont

rol

grou

p

clin

icia

n gr

oup:

60

hou

rs

(chi

ldre

n, in

ad

ditio

n to

60

in fi

rst

stud

y),

pare

nt g

roup

: 15

hou

rs

(par

ents

, in

addi

tion

to 5

6 in

firs

t st

udy)

both

the

rapy

gr

oups

impr

oved

m

ore

than

con

trol

s

dism

isse

d gr

oup

show

ed

no c

hang

e;

ther

efor

e pr

ogre

ss

from

pre

viou

s ph

ase

1 st

udy

mai

ntai

ned

for

5 m

onth

s

to

spon

tane

ous

spee

ch

Mat

heny

an

d Pa

nago

s (1

978)

16t

(8t:

synt

ax

ther

apy,

8t:

artic

ulat

ion

ther

apy)

, 8c

5;5–

6;10

‘func

tiona

l ar

ticul

ator

y an

d sy

ntac

tic

prob

lem

s’

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

imita

tion

dire

ct 1

:1ye

sR

CT

: 2

ther

apy

grou

ps p

lus

cont

rol

grou

p

unsp

ecifi

ed

over

5 m

onth

sbo

th g

roup

s m

ade

sign

ifica

nt p

rogr

ess

in b

oth

synt

ax

and

artic

ulat

ion;

co

ntro

l gro

up

mad

e no

pro

gres

s

not

mea

sure

dto

gen

eral

la

ngua

ge

test

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 13: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 19

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

arin

terv

entio

n m

etho

d

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

Gill

am e

t al

. (20

12)

16t

(8t:

CLI

,8t

: DLI

), 8c

6;0–

9;0

lang

uage

im

pair

edC

LI v

s. D

LI

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

drill

s, fo

cuse

d st

imul

atio

n an

d re

cast

s

smal

l gr

oups

(3/

4 ch

ildre

n)

with

SLT

yes

2 th

erap

y gr

oups

ra

ndom

ly

assi

gned

; no

n-ra

ndom

ly

assi

gned

co

ntro

l gr

oup

3 ×

50

min

pe

r w

eek

for

6 w

eeks

= 1

5 ho

urs

both

CLI

and

DLI

im

prov

ed m

ore

than

con

trol

s on

se

nten

ce-le

vel

mea

sure

s; C

LI

(but

not

DLI

) al

so im

prov

ed

gene

ral n

arra

tive

com

preh

ensi

on

and

expr

essi

on

(alth

ough

not

on

spe

cific

m

acro

stru

ctur

e m

easu

re);

CLI

ha

d la

rger

effe

ct

size

s th

an D

LI o

n bo

th s

ente

nce

and

narr

ativ

e m

easu

res

not

mea

sure

dno

t m

easu

red

Nel

son

et

al. (

1996

)7t

4;7–

6;7

SLI,

< 1

.25

SD o

n M

LU

and

sent

ence

im

itatio

n

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

imita

tion

vs.

reca

stin

g

dire

ct 1

:1ye

sIn

divi

dual

ta

rget

s as

sign

ed t

o 2

ther

apy

met

hods

vs.

no

the

rapy

mea

n of

18.

1 se

ssio

ns

(leng

th n

ot

stat

ed)

trea

ted

targ

ets

bett

er t

han

untr

eate

d ar

eas;

ta

rget

s pr

oduc

ed

quic

ker

and

gene

raliz

ed m

ore

with

rec

astin

g

not

mea

sure

dto

sp

onta

neou

s sp

eech

at

hom

e

Cul

atta

an

d H

orn

(198

2)

4t4;

6–9;

2la

ngua

ge

diso

rder

ed,

prim

arily

ex

pres

sive

onl

y

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

mod

ellin

g/fo

cuse

d st

imul

atio

n pl

us r

ecas

ting

dire

ct 1

:1ye

s4

case

st

udie

s:

mul

tiple

ba

selin

e

14.2

5–20

.25

hour

s90

% a

ccur

acy

reac

hed

on

trai

ned

targ

ets,

lit

tle p

rogr

ess

on s

econ

d ta

rget

du

ring

bas

elin

e pe

riod

.

yes,

for

at le

ast

3.5

wee

ksto

sp

onta

neou

s sp

eech

(Con

tinue

d)

Tab

le 3

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 14: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

20 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Gra

mm

arin

terv

entio

n m

etho

d

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

Cam

arat

a et

al.

(199

4)

21t

4;0–

6;10

SLI,

prim

arily

ex

pres

sive

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

reca

stin

g vs

. im

itatio

n

dire

ct 1

:1no

2 ta

rget

s pe

r ch

ild

rand

omly

as

sign

ed t

o 2

ther

apy

met

hods

20 h

ours

succ

ess

of m

etho

ds

vari

ed b

etw

een

child

ren;

imita

tion

led

to q

uick

er

elic

ited

prod

uctio

n

not

mea

sure

dto

sp

onta

neou

s sp

eech

, qu

icke

r w

ith

reca

stin

g th

an

imita

tion

Cam

arat

a an

d N

elso

n (1

992)

4t4;

9–5;

11SL

I (re

cept

ive

lang

uage

un

affe

cted

)

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

reca

stin

g vs

. im

itatio

n

dire

ct 1

:1no

2 or

4

targ

ets

per

child

ra

ndom

ly

assi

gned

to

2 th

erap

y m

etho

ds

16–3

2 ho

urs

succ

ess

of m

etho

ds

vari

ed b

etw

een

targ

ets

not

mea

sure

dto

sp

onta

neou

s sp

eech

, qu

icke

r w

ith

reca

stin

g th

an

imita

tion

Tyl

er a

nd

Wat

ters

on

(199

1)

12t

(6t:

lang

uage

in

terv

entio

n,

6t:

phon

olog

ical

in

terv

entio

n)

3;7–

5;7

lang

uage

and

ph

onol

ogic

ally

im

pair

ed (

all

expr

essi

ve;

com

preh

ensi

on

vari

ed a

lthou

gh

gene

rally

bet

ter

than

exp

ress

ion)

gram

mar

fa

cilit

atio

n:

stor

ies

read

an

d re

-tol

d in

clud

ing

targ

ets,

rol

e-pl

ay in

clud

ing

targ

ets,

el

icita

tion

and

mod

ellin

g

dire

ct

1:2

with

st

uden

t SL

T

no2

ther

apy

grou

ps (

not

rand

omly

as

sign

ed),

no c

ontr

ol

grou

p.

2 ×

per

wee

k fo

r 9

wee

ks

(leng

th o

f se

ssio

n no

t st

ated

)

no s

igni

fican

t di

ffere

nces

in

MLU

bet

wee

n gr

oups

pre

- or

po

st-t

hera

py; n

o si

gnifi

cant

cha

nges

in

MLU

n/a

n/a

Gue

ndou

zi

(200

3)2t

7;0

and

6;10

SLI

met

alin

guis

tic:

sim

ilar

to

Col

ourf

ul

Sem

antic

s

not

stat

ed,

dire

ct 1

:1

impl

ied

no2

case

st

udie

s pr

e-

vs. p

ost-

test

, no

sta

ndar

d sc

ores

not

stat

edon

e ch

ild m

ade

prog

ress

on

MLU

; on

e di

d no

t

not

mea

sure

dto

sp

onta

neou

s sp

eech

Not

es. S

ee n

otes

at

Tab

le 1

; CLI

= c

onte

xtua

lized

, lite

ratu

re-b

ased

nar

rativ

e in

terv

entio

n; D

LI =

dec

onte

xtua

lized

lang

uage

inte

rven

tion;

MLU

= m

ean

leng

th o

f utt

eran

ce.

Tab

le 3

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 15: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 21

received additional sessions with the clinician and parent, where the target form was elicited in the context of conversation and stories.

(ii) Modelling / focused stimulation with or without evoked production. In modelling and focused stimulation approaches, the child is not required to respond, merely to listen to examples of the target structure. Modelling approaches direct the child’s attention to the stimuli but do not give explicit guidance on which particular features to attend to. Focused stimulation, in contrast, does not direct the child’s attention to the model in any way. Evoked production may follow the model-ling or focused stimulation period. In this case, the child produces a novel utterance that uses the same rule as the model and then receives feedback. The degree of modelling is gradually reduced as the child begins to use the new rule productively.

One study showed that modelling without evoked production was effective in teaching auxiliary is and auxiliary inversion to three children with expressive language delays, but the addition of evoked production led to a more stable learning pattern (Ellis-Weismer and Murray-Branch, 1989). However, neither method was successful in teaching he to a fourth child with both expressive and receptive language difficulties.

Two studies found modelling with evoked production improved the ability of an experimental group to produce is and don’t (Leonard, 1975) and wh-questions accurately (Wilcox and Leonard, 1978). The delayed therapy groups made no progress until they too received therapy. The latter study showed generalization of is inversion to other wh-constructions requiring inversion.

Courtwright and Courtwright (1976) compared the effectiveness of modelling vs. imitation methods for teaching correct use of they in subject position (as opposed to them). The children in both groups improved on their initial performance, but those in the modelling group showed greater progress. They found a similar advantage for modelling when teaching children to produce an artificial grammatical rule (Courtwright and Courtwright, 1979). However, two studies (Connell, 1987; Connell and Stone, 1992) showed that modelling alone seemed to be less effective for teach-ing novel derivational morphemes to children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) than just imitation or modelling plus imitation, particularly if they were required to produce the new form. The differing results of these studies may be due to the nature of the invented rules, which for the Connell studies involved derivational morphology, but for the Courtwright and Courtwright (1979) study involved a novel sentence structure.

(iii) Recasting. Recasting methods are designed to be non-intrusive conversational procedures. The adult does not initiate teaching directly, but manipulates play activities to increase the chances of the child using targeted grammatical forms. When the child fails to use the target form or makes an error, the adult immediately follows his or her utterance with a modified version that includes the target form (a ‘recast’). The theory behind this approach is that the child is more likely to be interested in what the adult is saying if it links semantically to the situation and the child’s own prior utterance. The immediate contrast between the two forms should also focus the child’s atten-tion on the features of the utterances that differ. In addition, the child does not need to parse the adult’s meaning and thus should have more processing resources available for analysing the target form in the recast.

Three studies compared the effectiveness of recasting vs. imitation for increasing production of a range of morphosyntactic structures in children with SLI (Camarata and Nelson, 1992; Camarata et al., 1994; Nelson et al., 1996). These found targets treated with either type of intervention improved more than untreated targets, but Camarata et al. (1994) found target forms occurred spontaneously after fewer presentations using recasting than imitation. In contrast, imitation led to faster elicited production of the target. However, there is evidence of an interaction of target type,

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 16: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

22 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

child and intervention method. Camarata and Nelson (1992) found that children acquired the pas-sive construction faster with recasting, whereas they acquired the gerund faster with imitation. Individual variation was revealed in Camarata et al. (1994) when three of the 21 participants only acquired targets with imitation and three only with recasting.

More recent studies have begun to unpack the necessary features for recasting approaches to be effective. This includes: the density of recasts, the similarity of the recast to the original sentence, whether they serve a corrective function, and whether the child’s initial levels of use of the target matter. I discuss these studies below as they have clinical implications for recasting therapy, but they do not appear in the tables because they are either with pre-schoolers, or use novel linguistic forms or are not intervention studies.

The original studies demonstrating the efficacy of recasting by Camarata, Nelson and colleagues (discussed above) used recast rates of between 0.7 and 1.8 recasts per minute. Subsequent studies have shown that lower levels (0.47; Proctor-Williams and Fey, 2007) and conversation-like densi-ties (Proctor-Williams et al., 2001) do not seem to benefit children with SLI (unlike typically developing children). Another study (Fey and Loeb, 2002) with recasting densities similar to the original studies (one per minute) found no effect of recasting on the ability of 3-year-old children with SLI to produce auxiliaries or modals. They suggested the children may not have been ready to benefit from recasts and that recasting may be best when the children are already using the target form to a certain extent rather than for encouraging use of a new form. Indeed, Saxton (2000) showed that typically developing children need to use a grammatical form above 50% of the times required before they can benefit from corrective recasts. It appears that recasts do not necessarily need to be corrective. Hassink and Leonard (2010) found that conversationally relevant recasts containing a new form were facilitative for pre-schoolers with SLI even when the recasts served no direct corrective function.

Thus, it seems that for recasting to be maximally effective, the recasts need to be of high density and the children need to already use the target to a certain extent. It does not seem to matter whether or not the recast corrects an error.

(iv) Combined grammar facilitation approaches. Some intervention studies have used a combi-nation of the methods discussed above. In particular, modelling with evoked production together with recasting has been shown to be effective for generalization of newly learned grammatical rules to spontaneous discourse (Culatta and Horn, 1982) and for increasing grammatical accuracy and range (Fey et al., 1993; 1997; Gillam et al., 2012; Tyler et al., 2002). The studies by Fey and colleagues also investigated the role of parents in the delivery of intervention. These revealed a significant effect of intervention, whether delivered by parent or clinician, although the children in the clinician groups made more reliable progress.

A series of studies by Tyler and colleagues investigated the effectiveness of grammar facilita-tion (and phonological therapy) approaches with children with both language and phonological impairments. An early study (Tyler and Watterson, 1991) found no significant effect of grammar facilitation therapy on the mean length of utterance (MLU) of these children. However, two later studies (Tyler et al., 2002, 2003) showed that children receiving a block of grammar facilitation therapy focused on morphosyntax, improved their production of finite morphemes (and indeed their phonology) more than a control group who received no therapy. Tyler et al. (2002) addition-ally showed that the amount of progress in finite morphemes was the same regardless of whether the children received morphosyntactic therapy before or after phonological therapy. However, Tyler et al. (2003) showed that the largest gains were found in children receiving therapy that alter-nated weekly between a focus on phonology and morphosyntax. The children were assigned ran-domly to groups, thus there was no consideration of the extent to which each child’s morphological

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 17: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 23

difficulties were due to phonological factors. This may have contributed to the highly variable responses to therapy in this study.

Indeed, a study including both implicit and explicit therapy (Smith-Lock et al., 2013a, discussed below) found that children whose articulation difficulties interfered with production of targeted grammatical constructions made no progress with grammatical therapy. A single case study (Seeff-Gabriel et al., 2012) considered the impact of phonology on the targeted morphosyntactic struc-tures and varied the therapy accordingly. Phonology was not considered to affect his production of past tense and, indeed, grammatical therapy (mostly using grammar facilitation methods) improved his production of the regular past tense. However, phonology was considered to affect his produc-tion of plurals, as he could not accurately produce /s/ or /z/. In this case, phonological therapy led to increased marking of plurals.

A recent study (Gillam et al., 2012) found combined grammar facilitation approaches were more effective when embedded in a story context than when presented in a decontextualized way. However, a large scale RCT (Gillam et al., 2008; see Table 5 for details) compared grammar facili-tation approaches with two other interventions: Fast ForWord (reviewed below) and computer-based language games and also with a control intervention: ‘academic enrichment’ (computer games focusing on Maths, Science and Geography). They found no significant advantage for any group. Indeed, the language intervention groups only showed greater progress than the ‘academic enrichment’ group on blending words. Thus, the grammar facilitation group fared no better on language measures than the other groups, including the ‘control’ academic enrichment group.

b Usage-based approach. Riches (2013) evaluated an alternative implicit approach (a usage-based approach; e.g. Tomasello, 2003), which takes into account the gradual development of grammati-cal structures in typically developing children. Riches evaluated this approach with reference to passives. The intervention gradually built up to a full event passive, starting with state passives (e.g. I like my sausages chopped) which could be interpreted as an adjective, via ambiguous pas-sives (e.g. I want my sausages chopped) to event passives (e.g. the sausages were chopped by the cat). He found that two children with receptive and expressive SLI significantly improved their comprehension and production of passives, but not the control structure (relative clauses).

c Summary of implicit approaches. The effectiveness of implicit methods has been investigated in a range of studies including some RCTs. These generally indicate that these methods are effective for improving expressive morphology and syntax in pre-school and early school-aged children with expressive language delays and disorders when delivered 1:1 by an SLT or parent. However, the study by Gillam et al. (2008) indicates that this may be no more effective than ‘academic enrichment’. For children with co-occurring phonological impairments, the impact of these on specific grammatical targets should be considered, as should delivering an alternating phonologi-cal and grammatical approach.

2 Explicit approaches

Meta-linguistic approaches provide predominantly explicit teaching of language, often in the con-text of specific visual cues. Once the child has learned a new rule, some grammar facilitation methods (especially recasting) may be used alongside visual templates and explicit references to the child’s errors, and more context may begin to be added. These approaches are based on the hypotheses that children with primary language impairments have difficulties learning grammar implicitly and benefit from explicit teaching of the rules (for further discussion, see Ebbels et al., 2013).

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 18: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

24 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

a Colourful semantics. Colour coding is frequently used in meta-linguistic approaches. Colourful Semantics (Bryan, 1997) colour codes thematic roles in sentences in order to help children identify thematic roles and create a variety of argument structures. Several uncontrolled case studies have been carried out using this or similar methods (Bryan, 1997; Guendouzi, 2003; Spooner, 2002). Also, a group study (Bolderson et al., 2011) found improved expressive language after therapy, compared with no progress during a baseline period. Progress on receptive language was seen both during baseline and the therapy period and thus the changes could be due to maturation or practice at the tests.

b Shape coding. A related meta-linguistic approach (Shape Coding, Ebbels, 2007) uses a combi-nation of shapes, colours and arrows to indicate phrases, parts of speech and morphology respec-tively. It was originally conceived as a combination of the ‘Colour Pattern Scheme’ (Lea, 1970) and ‘Colourful Semantics’ systems, but was further developed so that it can also show complex sentence structures and verb morphology. Each shape is linked to a question word and colour.

An RCT (Ebbels et al., 2007) investigated production of verb argument structure and compared therapy using Shape Coding with therapy focusing on verb semantic representations and a control therapy (focused on inferencing, which was not predicted to have any effect on verb argument structure). Both the Shape Coding and verb semantic methods were based on detailed hypotheses regarding the underlying reasons for the participants’ difficulties with verb argument structure and both groups made significant progress, particularly in linking arguments to the correct syntactic positions (i.e. reducing errors such as ‘she is filling the water into the glass’). Progress generalized to control verbs and was maintained three months after intervention ceased. The Shape Coding group also used more optional arguments after therapy. The control group showed no progress in verb argument structure.

A second RCT (Ebbels et al., 2013) focused on comprehension of coordinating conjunctions (but not, neither nor, not only but also). We found that those receiving Shape Coding therapy improved their comprehension of the targeted conjunctions significantly more than the waiting controls, who then also made progress when they too received therapy. Progress also led to increased scores on the Test of Reception of Grammar (TROG-2, Bishop, 2003). However, we found no generalization to comprehension of passives. Analyses of child-related factors showed no predictors of which children would improve the most with the therapy. The predictors considered including non-verbal and visual processing abilities, which while correlated with each other, were not correlated with any language measure or progress with therapy.

Ebbels and van der Lely (2001) investigated the efficacy of Shape Coding for improving expres-sion and comprehension of passives and wh-questions. Three of the four participants showed sig-nificant progress in both their comprehension and production of passives. Two had difficulties comprehending wh-questions pre-therapy and both showed significant progress in this area. All four participants showed short-term progress with the production of wh-questions, but only one participant maintained this at a significant level by follow-up. The three participants who responded best participated in a follow-up study (Ebbels, 2007) targeting comprehension of the dative con-struction (e.g. the boy is giving the girl the rabbit) and wh-comparative questions (e.g. what is bigger than a cat? vs. what is a cat bigger than?). All three received intervention on datives, but only two received intervention for wh-comparatives due to a change of SLT. Two of the three par-ticipants showed significant progress in their comprehension of dative constructions. The third was hypothesized to have additional short-term memory difficulties that made progress on this area more difficult, due to the need to remember the order of three key nouns. However, this participant made significant progress in comprehension of wh-comparative questions, as did the other partici-pant who was taught this structure.

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 19: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 25

The studies of Shape Coding discussed above all involved individual therapy sessions. However, an uncontrolled study on the use of the past tense in writing (Ebbels, 2007) involved group teach-ing. A class of nine pupils (aged 11–13 years) were taught using the Shape Coding system during English lessons. Six used the past tense more after the class sessions, but two more made progress only when they received additional intervention in a pair. Possible explanations are either that they merely needed more intervention time, or that they needed a more individualized approach which could be provided in a pair, but not in a group of nine.

Until recently, all the studies of Shape Coding have been with secondary-aged children. However, Kulkarni et al. (in press) investigated its effectiveness for improving oral use of the past tense by two younger children (8-year-olds) in both structured tasks and conversation. We also considered whether additional generalization therapy was required for participants to use target forms in their spontaneous speech. One participant improved markedly in sentence completion but required the generalization therapy before gaining in the conversational task. The other made more modest gains in both areas without recourse to the generalization therapy.

c Other explicit approaches. The effectiveness of an explicit meta-linguistic approach has also been investigated in Hebrew in a single uncontrolled case study (aged 12;2). Levy and Friedman (2009) investigated its effectiveness for improving the comprehension and production of structures involving syntactic ‘movement’ (relative clauses and topicalization) in Hebrew. Their method was similar to Colourful Semantics and Shape Coding in that they colour coded verbs and their argu-ments (as in Colourful Semantics) and they explicitly taught movement showing the link between the moved item and its trace (as in Shape Coding; see Ebbels and van der Lely, 2001). They found significant progress in most areas targeted and also generalization to areas not targeted, such as wh-questions.

d Summary of explicit approaches. Studies of metalinguistic approaches indicate that they can be effective for school-aged children with language impairments, including those of secondary age and those with receptive language difficulties. There is no evidence of whether these approaches are effective when delivered by anyone other than an SLT.

3 Comparison of explicit and implicit approaches

A few studies have compared the effectiveness of implicit vs. explicit therapy for improving expressive language. These have found conflicting results, which may be due to the age of the children, the target of therapy, or the design of the studies. One study (Swisher et al., 1995) found that younger children with SLI (age 4–6 years) learned to generalize novel bound morphemes trained in a story context to untrained vocabulary stems better with implicit than explicit training. In contrast, Finestack and Fey (2009) found that children with language impairment (aged 6–8 years) learned to use novel verb inflections better with explicit than implicit training. Motsch and Riehemann (2008) found German children with SLI (aged 8–10 years) learned the dative case bet-ter with explicit than implicit intervention, whereas both methods were equally effective for improving the accusative. However, there were several possible biases in this study; for example, the more willing and knowledgeable teachers carried out the explicit intervention. It is also unclear whether the two groups received equal amounts of intervention.

These three studies together appear to indicate that implicit approaches may be more effective for younger children (under 6 years) while explicit approaches may be more appropriate for older children. However, given the limited range of targets and the fact that all the targets involved expressive morphology, much more work needs to be done to draw any firm conclusions.

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 20: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

26 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

4 Combination of explicit and implicit approaches

In a study of the effectiveness of grammar therapy in a school setting, Smith-Lock et al. (2013a) showed a significant effect of direct explicit teaching combined with grammar facilitation tech-niques on the ability of 5-year-olds with SLI to produce subject pronouns, possessive s and past tense -ed. Their experimental group improved more than the control group, but only when they received intervention (in a group), not during baseline. The effects did not generalize to untreated grammatical targets. At a single-case level, most children showed a treatment effect. Six made no progress, but five of these had articulation difficulties. A follow-up study (Smith-Lock et al., 2013a) showed that this approach was only effective when provided in eight weekly sessions rather than eight daily sessions.

Some of the studies discussed above (e.g. Kulkarni et al., in press), while predominantly using explicit methods, also used some grammar facilitation methods. Indeed, in clinical practice, the two are often combined. Given that 50% usage of a targeted structure may be required before recasting can be effective, it may be that explicit methods could be used for initial teaching and then recasting could be used thereafter. However, further research is needed to establish whether a combination of approaches is more effective than purely implicit or explicit therapy and, if so, how and when the two methods should be combined.

Several studies have focused on both narrative and grammar abilities in parallel. These are shown in Table 4. These tend to have an explicit approach to narrative structure and an implicit approach to grammar. Specifically, the ‘narrative based language intervention’ (NBLI) used in Swanson et al. (2005) and Fey et al. (2010) explicitly taught narrative structure while using gram-mar facilitation approaches to teach grammar. Swanson et al. (2005) found their intervention improved the quality of the children’s narratives, but not their grammatical abilities. The authors suggest this could be due to limited processing resources, such that children with SLI only focus on explicit targets. Fey et al. (2010) also found NBLI did not yield significant improvements in the grammatical production of children with SLI, but did improve their narrative comprehension (as this study primarily focused on the effectiveness of Fast ForWord, it is shown in Table 5).

Some other studies (Davies et al., 2004; Petersen et al., 2008, 2010) used explicit therapy to focus mainly on narrative production, but also included some work on expressive grammar. These found progress on narrative ability and grammar. However, the scores for narrative and grammar were often conflated, so it is difficult to know whether the positive change was in both areas.

IV Language interventions not specific to grammar

1 Acoustically modified speech (including Fast ForWord, FFW; Scientific Learning Corporation, 1998)

Intervention studies using acoustically modified speech have focused mainly on receptive rather than expressive language and are shown in Table 5. They are based on the theory that children with SLI have difficulty processing rapid or brief stimuli (Tallal et al., 1985) and aim to improve this underlying deficit by training the auditory system using acoustically modified speech. The chil-dren’s general language abilities are hypothesized to improve as a direct consequence of their improved temporal processing abilities. An early study of FFW reported that children’s language comprehension improved significantly (Tallal et al., 1996). However, there were several problems with the design of this study.

Independent case study investigations of FFW (Friel-Patti et al., 2001; Gillam et al., 2001; Loeb et al., 2001) showed the majority of children made some progress with some areas of language,

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 21: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 27T

able

4.

Stud

ies

targ

etin

g na

rrat

ive

and

gram

mar

tog

ethe

r.

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Inte

rven

tion

met

hod

Met

hod

of

deliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

ho

urs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

Pete

rsen

et

al.

(201

0)

3t6;

3, 6

;5,

8;1

neur

omus

cula

r im

pair

men

t an

d co

-mor

bid

REL

I

‘lite

rate

nar

rativ

e in

terv

entio

n’,

emph

asiz

ed

mac

rost

ruct

ure

and

som

e as

pect

s of

nar

rativ

e m

icro

stru

ctur

e,

part

icul

arly

cau

sal

and

tem

pora

l su

bord

inat

ing

conj

unct

ions

1:1

with

SL

T (

75%

of

sess

ions

) an

d ps

ycho

logi

st

(25%

)

yes

mul

tiple

ba

selin

e10

× 6

0 m

inut

es, 4

da

ys p

er

wee

k (=

10

hour

s)

mac

rost

ruct

ure:

bet

ter

narr

ativ

es p

rodu

ced

duri

ng in

terv

entio

n th

an

duri

ng b

asel

ine;

tar

gete

d m

icro

stru

ctur

e: im

prov

ed

use

of m

arke

d an

d un

mar

ked

caus

al r

elat

ions

, but

no

chan

ge in

tem

pora

l adv

erbi

al

subo

rdin

ate

clau

ses;

soc

ial

valid

ity: 1

5 un

derg

rads

rat

ed

base

line

narr

ativ

es a

s po

ores

t at

leas

t 67

%, 9

3% a

nd 1

00%

of

time

for

the

thre

e pa

rtic

ipan

ts

mac

rost

ruct

ure:

m

aint

aine

d fo

r on

e ch

ild, p

ossi

bly

for

anot

her,

no

data

for

thir

d;

caus

al r

elat

ions

: fo

r on

e ch

ild

mac

rost

ruct

ure:

to

ver

bally

pr

ompt

ed

narr

ativ

es;

mic

rost

ruct

ure:

to

mos

t no

n-ta

rget

ed a

reas

fo

r 2

child

ren

and

to a

few

ar

eas

for

othe

r ch

ild

Pete

rsen

et

al.

(200

8)

12t

6;4–

9;1

lang

uage

impa

ired

expl

icitl

y ta

ught

st

ory

gram

mar

co

mpo

nent

s, p

lus

caus

ality

, tem

pora

l co

ncep

ts a

nd d

ialo

gue

grou

ps o

f 3–4

yes

ther

apy

vs.

base

line

peri

od

4 ×

90

min

utes

pe

r w

eek

for

4 w

eeks

(=

24

hou

rs)

sign

ifica

nt in

crea

se in

sto

ry

com

plex

ity w

ith in

terv

entio

n,

none

dur

ing

base

line

(sco

re

incl

uded

poi

nts

for

stor

y gr

amm

ar, c

ausa

lity,

tem

pora

l m

arke

rs a

nd d

ialo

gue)

not

test

edno

t te

sted

Dav

ies

et a

l. (2

004)

31t

mea

n ag

e: 5

;11

teac

hers

id

entif

ied

child

ren

with

diff

icul

ty

desc

ribi

ng,

expl

aini

ng a

nd

conv

eyin

g ev

ents

(o

nly

one

on S

LT

case

load

)

met

alin

guis

tic u

sing

co

lour

cod

ing

for

wh-

wor

ds, p

lus

prac

tice

at r

e-te

lling

and

ge

nera

ting

stor

ies

grou

p (1

×

per

wee

k w

ith S

LT, 2

×

per

wee

k w

ith a

lear

ning

su

ppor

t as

sist

ant)

nopr

e- v

s.

post

-tes

t, no

t st

anda

rd

scor

es; u

sed

age

equi

vale

nt

scor

es t

o co

ntro

l for

m

atur

atio

n

3 ×

40

min

utes

pe

r w

eek

for

8 w

eeks

(=

16

hou

rs)

part

icip

ants

impr

oved

the

ir

desc

ript

ions

of p

ictu

res

and

narr

ativ

es in

ter

ms

of in

form

atio

n, g

ram

mar

(in

clud

ing

conn

ectiv

es)

and

stor

y qu

ality

; mor

e pr

ogre

ss

in t

erm

s of

age

equ

ival

ent

than

m

onth

s w

hich

had

pas

sed

not

test

edte

ache

rs

repo

rted

in

crea

sed

conf

iden

ce in

cl

ass

and

bett

er

able

to

liste

n an

d co

ntri

bute

ap

prop

riat

ely

in c

lass

Swan

son

et a

l. (2

005)

10t

6;11

–8;9

SLI (

of w

hom

7

REL

I)N

BLI:

addr

esse

s bo

th

narr

ativ

e (e

xplic

it te

achi

ng p

lus

stor

y re

-te

lling

and

gen

erat

ion)

an

d gr

amm

ar

(impl

icitl

y us

ing

imita

tion,

mod

ellin

g an

d re

cast

ing)

; cyc

lical

go

al a

ttac

k st

rate

gy: 2

w

eeks

on

each

tar

get

dire

ct 1

:1 w

ith

SLT

nopr

e- v

s.

post

-tes

t, no

t st

anda

rd

scor

es

3 ×

50

min

utes

pe

r w

eek

for

6 w

eeks

(=

15

hou

rs)

8/10

mad

e cl

inic

ally

sig

nific

ant

prog

ress

in n

arra

tive

qual

ity;

1/0

mad

e cl

inic

ally

sig

nific

ant

prog

ress

in n

umbe

r of

di

ffere

nt w

ords

; no

sign

ifica

nt

gain

s fo

r an

y gr

amm

atic

al

outc

omes

not

mea

sure

dno

t m

easu

red

Not

es. S

ee n

otes

at

Tab

le 1

; NBL

I = n

arra

tive

base

d la

ngua

ge in

terv

entio

n.

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 22: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

28 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)T

able

5.

Stud

ies

targ

etin

g im

prov

emen

t in

gen

eral

rec

eptiv

e an

d/or

exp

ress

ive

lang

uage

(as

sess

ing

effe

ctiv

enes

s of

Fas

t Fo

rWor

d, F

FW).

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Inte

rven

tion

met

hod(

s)M

etho

d of

de

liver

yC

ontr

ols?

Tot

al h

ours

the

rapy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

mai

ntai

ned?

Prog

ress

gen

eral

ized

?

Coh

en e

t al

. (20

05)

50t

(23t

: FFW

, 27

t: ot

her

com

pute

r pr

ogra

ms)

, 27c

6–10

ye

ars

SLI (

rece

ptiv

e an

d ex

pres

sive

)FF

W v

s. o

ther

co

mpu

ter

prog

ram

sco

mpu

ter

1:1

yes

RC

T: 2

the

rapy

gr

oups

plu

s co

ntro

l gro

up

6–60

hou

rs (

FFW

), 2–

82 (

othe

r pr

ogra

ms)

ther

apy

grou

ps im

prov

ed

no m

ore

than

con

trol

gr

oup

n/a

n/a

Gill

am e

t al

. (20

08)

162t

(54

t: FF

W,

54t:

CA

LI, 5

4t:

ILI),

54c:

AE

6–9

year

sno

rmal

non

verb

al

skill

s, la

ngua

ge

abili

ties

1.2

SDs

or

mor

e be

low

mea

n

FFW

vs.

oth

er

com

pute

r pr

ogra

ms

(lang

uage

and

non

-la

ngua

ge)

vs. I

LI:

lang

uage

faci

litat

ion

appr

oach

es, s

ome

in c

onte

xt o

f st

orie

s

all 1

:1. F

FW,

AE

and

CA

LI:

com

pute

r,

ILI:

SLT

yes

RC

T: 3

lang

uage

in

terv

entio

n gr

oups

, plu

s ‘c

ontr

ol g

roup

’ (w

ho h

ad A

E)

100

min

utes

× 5

da

ys p

er w

eek,

fo

r 6

wee

ks (

= 5

0 ho

urs)

all g

roup

s m

ade

sign

ifica

nt p

rogr

ess

on

CA

SL, t

oken

tes

t an

d ba

ckw

ard

mas

king

, no

effe

ct o

f gro

up; o

nly

effe

ct o

f gro

up o

n bl

endi

ng w

ords

sub

test

of

CT

OPP

whe

re

lang

uage

gro

ups

> A

E

yes,

furt

her

prog

ress

af

ter

6 m

onth

s fo

r al

l gro

ups

for

CA

SL

and

toke

n te

st;

blen

ding

pro

gres

s al

so s

how

ed fu

rthe

r pr

ogre

ss, b

ut t

his

was

sig

nific

antly

hi

gher

for

CA

LI a

nd

FFW

, tha

n IL

I and

AE

yes,

to

gene

ral

lang

uage

tes

ts.

Effe

ct s

izes

gre

ates

t on

CA

SL (

mai

nly

expr

essi

ve la

ngua

ge);

smal

ler

effe

cts

on

toke

n te

st s

ugge

stin

g ex

pres

sive

lang

uage

im

prov

ed m

ore

Fey

et a

l. (2

010)

30t

(23

com

plet

ed

stud

y an

d an

alys

ed: 7

t: FF

W/N

BLI,

7t:

NBL

I/FFW

, 9t:

Wai

t/N

BLI)

6–8

year

s12

/23

SLI,

11/2

3 N

LIFF

W v

s. N

BLI

incl

uded

sto

ry

rete

ll, s

ente

nce

imita

tion

and

stor

y ge

nera

tion

(rec

asts

fo

r st

orie

s)

FFW

: co

mpu

ter

1:1,

NBL

I: SL

T 1

:1

yes

RC

T: T

hree

th

erap

y gr

oups

(o

ne a

cted

as

wai

ting

cont

rol

in p

hase

1)

FFW

: 100

min

utes

pe

r da

y fo

r 24

se

ssio

ns (

= 4

0 ho

urs)

, NBL

I: 5

× 1

00 m

inut

es

per

fort

nigh

t fo

r 12

ses

sion

s (=

20

hour

s)

mos

t ch

ildre

n ha

d di

fficu

lties

pro

gres

sing

th

roug

h FF

W g

ames

; no

sig

nific

ant

diffe

renc

e be

twee

n gr

oups

on

gram

mar

mea

sure

s;

afte

r ph

ase

1: t

wo

ther

apy

grou

ps im

prov

ed

narr

ativ

e co

mpr

ehen

sion

bu

t on

ly s

igni

fican

tly

bett

er t

han

wai

ting

cont

rols

whe

n tw

o gr

oups

com

bine

d; o

ver

who

le s

tudy

: nar

rativ

e im

prov

ed m

ost

in N

BLI/

FFW

gro

up a

nd le

ast

in F

FW/N

BLI g

roup

, su

gges

ting

that

NBL

I im

prov

es n

arra

tive

com

preh

ensi

on, b

ut o

nly

if no

t pr

eced

ed b

y FF

W

not

mea

sure

dto

sta

ndar

dize

d na

rrat

ive

com

preh

ensi

on t

est

Gill

am e

t al

. (20

01)

4t (

2t: F

FW, 2

t: ot

her

com

pute

r pr

ogra

ms,

tw

o id

entic

al

6;11

–7;

6la

ngua

ge

diso

rder

ed (

two

with

bor

derl

ine

rece

ptiv

e

FFW

vs.

oth

er

com

pute

r pr

ogra

ms

com

pute

r 1:

1no

child

ren

rand

omly

as

sign

ed t

o ea

ch

100

min

utes

per

da

y, 5

day

s a

wee

k fo

r 4

wee

ks (

= 3

3 ho

urs)

All

child

ren

mad

e so

me

prog

ress

not

mea

sure

dto

spo

ntan

eous

sp

eech

and

gen

eral

la

ngua

ge t

ests

for

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 23: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 29

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Inte

rven

tion

met

hod(

s)M

etho

d of

de

liver

yC

ontr

ols?

Tot

al h

ours

the

rapy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

mai

ntai

ned?

Prog

ress

gen

eral

ized

?

twin

s as

sign

ed

to d

iffer

ent

prog

ram

s)

lang

uage

, tw

o w

ith

mod

erat

e-se

vere

re

cept

ive

lang

uage

di

fficu

lties

)

type

of t

hera

py,

pre-

vs.

pos

t-te

st in

clud

ing

stan

dard

sco

res

one

FFW

chi

ld a

nd 2

on

oth

er p

rogr

ams

Loeb

et

al.

(200

1)4t

5;6–

8;1

spee

ch-la

ngua

ge

impa

irm

ent

(2/4

ha

d re

cept

ive

diffi

culti

es)

FFW

com

pute

r 1:

1no

pre-

vs.

pos

t-te

st, i

nclu

ding

st

anda

rd s

core

s

30–5

0 se

ssio

ns

(hou

rs u

ncle

ar, b

ut

prob

ably

50–

84

hour

s)

3/4

(less

impa

ired

) ch

ildre

n co

mpl

eted

pr

ogra

mm

e; a

ll ch

ildre

n m

ade

gain

s on

som

e st

anda

rdiz

ed t

ests

, but

m

any

lang

uage

are

as

show

ed n

o ch

ange

afte

r 3

mon

ths,

61%

of

gai

ns m

aint

aine

dto

som

e ge

nera

l la

ngua

ge t

ests

. Li

ttle

cha

nge

in

spon

tane

ous

spee

ch,

few

diff

eren

ces

repo

rted

by

pare

nts

and

teac

hers

Frie

l-Pat

ti et

al.

(200

1)

5t5;

10–

9;2

lang

uage

lear

ning

di

sabl

ed (

2 al

so a

tten

tion

defic

it di

sord

er);

only

1/5

had

re

cept

ive

lang

uage

di

fficu

lties

FFW

com

pute

r 1:

1no

pre-

vs.

pos

t-te

st, i

nclu

ding

st

anda

rd s

core

s

100

min

utes

× 5

da

ys a

wee

k fo

r 31

–32

wee

ks fo

r 4/

5 (=

52–

53 h

ours

). O

ne (

less

impa

ired

) pa

rtic

ipan

t re

ache

d di

smis

sal c

rite

ria

afte

r 16

day

s (=

27

hour

s).

3/5

child

ren

show

ed

mod

est

impr

ovem

ents

on

a fe

w m

easu

res;

the

se

incl

uded

the

tw

o le

ss

impa

ired

chi

ldre

n w

ho

had

reac

hed

dism

issa

l cr

iteri

a fr

om F

FW

not

mea

sure

dto

som

e ge

nera

l la

ngua

ge t

ests

for

3–5

child

ren.

Not

to

spon

tane

ous

spee

ch.

Tal

lal e

t al

. (19

96)

(stu

dy 2

)

22t

(11t

: FFW

, 11

t: un

mod

ified

sp

eech

)

5;2–

10;0

lang

uage

lear

ning

im

pair

ed

(rec

eptiv

e an

d ex

pres

sive

)

FFW

vs.

unm

odifi

ed

spee

chco

mpu

ter

1:1

no2

ther

apy

grou

ps (

not

rand

omly

as

sign

ed),

no

cont

rol g

roup

, no

t st

anda

rd

scor

es

3 ho

urs

× 5

day

s pe

r w

eek

+ 1

–2

hour

s a

day,

7 d

ays

a w

eek

for

4 w

eeks

(=

88–

116

hour

s)

both

gro

ups

impr

oved

co

mpr

ehen

sion

; si

gnifi

cant

ly m

ore

with

m

odifi

ed t

han

unm

odifi

ed

spee

ch

not

mea

sure

dto

gen

eral

lang

uage

te

sts

Tal

lal e

t al

. (19

96)

(stu

dy 1

)

7t5;

9–9;

1la

ngua

ge le

arni

ng

impa

ired

(r

ecep

tive

and

expr

essi

ve)

FFW

com

pute

r 1:

1no

pre-

vs.

po

st-t

est,

not

stan

dard

sco

res

3 ho

urs

× 5

day

s pe

r w

eek

+ 1

–2

hour

s a

day,

7 d

ays

a w

eek

for

4 w

eeks

(=

88–

116

hour

s)

lang

uage

com

preh

ensi

on

impr

oved

sig

nific

antly

not

mea

sure

dto

gen

eral

lang

uage

te

sts

Not

es. S

ee n

otes

at

Tab

le 1

; AE

= a

cade

mic

enr

ichm

ent;

CA

LI =

com

pute

r as

sist

ed la

ngua

ge in

terv

entio

n; C

ASL

= C

ompr

ehen

sive

Ass

essm

ent

of S

poke

n La

ngua

ge; C

TO

PP =

Com

preh

ensi

ve T

est

of P

hono

logi

cal

Proc

essi

ng (

Wag

ner

et a

l., 1

999)

; FFW

= F

ast

ForW

ord;

ILI =

indi

vidu

aliz

ed la

ngua

ge in

terv

entio

n; N

BLI =

nar

rativ

e ba

sed

lang

uage

inte

rven

tion.

Tab

le 5

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 24: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

30 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

although the children with the most severe language impairments appeared to benefit the least. However, recent independent large-scale RCTs (Cohen et al., 2005; Fey et al., 2010; Gillam et al., 2008) found control groups showed equal progress to those receiving FFW (or similar acoustically modified speech; see Bishop et al., 2006; details shown in Table 2) and a recent meta-analysis, which is the strongest form of evidence (Strong et al., 2011) has concluded ‘there is no evidence … that FFW is effective as a treatment for children’s oral language or reading difficulties’ (p. 224).

2 Language intervention packages

Several recent studies have considered the effectiveness of intervention packages delivered in dif-ferent ways and are shown in Table 6. These studies include a range of intervention approaches and targets with the aim of improving language generally, and their main focus is on establishing whether non-SLTs can effectively provide intervention.

Studies by Boyle and McCartney and colleagues of commonly used interventions delivered in mainstream schools found that children with ELI made more progress than controls in expressive language on the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-3 (CELF-3, Semel et al., 2006) when they received intervention by an SLT or SLT assistant employed by the research project (Boyle et al., 2007, 2009). This contrasted with children with RELI who made no progress relative to controls with either receptive or expressive language. Indeed, on the Receptive Language Scale, children receiving therapy (whether or not they had receptive language difficulties) showed no greater progress than controls.

However, a follow-up study (McCartney et al., 2011), using the same intervention but delivered by school staff (teachers, deputy head teachers, language support teachers and classroom assis-tants), found no effect of intervention for either receptive or expressive language relative to the Boyle et al. (2009) historical controls. The most likely reason for this is probably the limited amount of intervention actually delivered. However, this model of working through education staff is one followed by many SLT services.

The major difference between the Boyle et al. (2007; 2009) and McCartney et al. (2011) studies was in the background and employment those delivering the intervention. In Boyle et al. (2007; 2009), they were employed by the researchers running the study and were psychology graduates. In the McCartney et al. (2011) study, they were school staff, with many other demands on their time. Indeed, 54% of the teachers who had implemented the intervention in this study agreed or strongly agreed that ‘this method of working expects too much of the teacher’ (McCartney et al., 2010: 362). Even after modification of the language therapy support model and revision of the manual (now called the Strathclyde Language Intervention Programme, SLIP) following feedback from some of the teachers, potential users (teachers who had not been involved in earlier studies) were unsure whether implementing the model and delivering the language-learning activities would be realistic (McCartney et al., 2010). These studies are extremely worrying as they indicate that a very common model of therapy in the UK may be unrealistic and ineffective.

Nevertheless, an ‘enhanced consultative model’, using SLT assistants employed by the SLT service (Mecrow et al., 2010), was effective. Progress on targets (both speech and language, recep-tive and expressive) was significantly greater than progress on control areas. They also found sig-nificant change on the CELF-Preschool receptive and expressive language scales. However, they did not split the analyses to see if the effectiveness of therapy varied between target areas or between different groups of children.

It is not the case that school staff cannot effectively deliver intervention if they are well enough trained, supported and monitored. A small-scale study (Hutchinson and Clegg, 2011) indicates that language groups delivered by well-trained and supported education professionals can improve

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 25: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 31T

able

6.

Stud

ies

asse

ssin

g di

ffere

nt m

odel

s of

ser

vice

del

iver

y (t

arge

ts a

nd in

terv

entio

n m

etho

d no

t th

e fo

cus)

.

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Lang

uage

In

terv

entio

n m

etho

d

Met

hod

of d

eliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

Boyl

e et

al.

(200

9)13

1t (

34t:

dire

ct 1

:1,

31t:

dire

ct

grou

p, 3

3t:

indi

rect

1:1

,32

t: in

dire

ct

grou

p), 3

1 c

6;0–

11;5

prim

ary

lang

uage

del

ay

IQ >

75

(86

REL

I, 75

ELI

)

man

ualiz

ed

inte

rven

tion

prog

ram

me

cove

ring

co

mpr

ehen

sion

m

onito

ring

, vo

cabu

lary

de

velo

pmen

t, gr

amm

ar a

nd

narr

ativ

es

dire

ct 1

:1 w

ith S

LT

vs. d

irec

t gr

oup

with

SL

T v

s. in

dire

ct 1

:1

SLT

A v

s. in

dire

ct

grou

p w

ith S

LTA

yes

RC

T: 4

the

rapy

gr

oups

plu

s co

ntro

l gro

up

(nor

mal

th

erap

y)

3 ×

30–

40

min

utes

per

w

eek,

ove

r 15

w

eek,

ave

rage

38

ses

sion

s (=

19

–25

hour

s)

no s

igni

fican

t di

ffere

nce

betw

een

type

s of

de

liver

y; p

roje

ct

ther

apy

> c

ontr

ol fo

r ex

pres

sive

lang

uage

for

ELI;

child

ren

with

REL

I no

mor

e pr

ogre

ss t

han

cont

rols

; no

grou

p m

ade

mor

e pr

ogre

ss

than

con

trol

s on

re

cept

ive

mea

sure

s;

non-

verb

al IQ

not

si

gnifi

cant

pre

dict

or o

f la

ngua

ge o

utco

mes

yes

12

mon

ths

late

r fo

r EL

I ch

ildre

n on

ex

pres

sive

m

easu

res

prog

ress

on

stan

dard

ized

tes

ts

McC

artn

ey

et a

l. (2

011)

38t

6–11

yrs

(mea

n:

8;9)

lang

uage

im

pair

men

t, IQ

>

75

(incl

udin

g so

me

with

R

ELI)

sam

e in

terv

entio

n pr

ogra

mm

e as

Boy

le e

t al

. (2

009)

by s

choo

l sta

ff, v

ia

‘con

sulta

tive

mod

el’,

activ

ities

from

m

anua

l; pl

anne

d 3

× p

er w

eek

for

15

wee

ks

yes

prog

ress

co

mpa

red

retr

ospe

ctiv

ely

to c

ontr

ols

in B

oyle

et

al.

(200

9) s

tudy

mea

n nu

mbe

r co

ntac

ts 2

6 (r

ange

: 8–7

0),

leng

th o

f co

ntac

t no

t re

cord

ed.

no c

hang

e on

any

m

easu

res;

no

diffe

renc

e be

twee

n th

erap

y gr

oup

and

Boyl

e et

al.

(200

9)

cont

rols

n/a

n/a

Mec

row

et

al. (

2010

)35

t4;

2–6;

1014

REL

I, 7

spee

ch a

nd

REL

I, 5

spee

ch

and

ELI,

3 sp

eech

onl

y,

1 sp

eech

and

R

ELI

vari

ed

acco

rdin

g to

sp

ecifi

c ta

rget

s

‘enh

ance

d co

nsul

tativ

e ap

proa

ch’

STA

(lik

e SL

TA

, wor

k un

der

guid

ance

of

spec

ialis

t te

ache

r or

te

ache

r/SL

T m

ade

avai

labl

e to

sch

ools

) pr

ovid

ed 1

:1 t

hera

py;

STA

als

o tr

aine

d sc

hool

sta

ff

yes

com

pare

d ch

ange

bet

wee

n tr

eate

d an

d un

trea

ted

beha

viou

rs

(with

in

part

icip

ants

).

aim

: 4 ×

1

hour

ses

sion

s pe

r w

eek

for

10 w

eeks

. A

ctua

l: m

ean

of 3

9 se

ssio

ns,

45–6

0 m

inut

es

each

(=

29–

39

hour

s)

targ

et b

ehav

iour

s im

prov

ed s

igni

fican

tly

mor

e th

an c

ontr

ol

beha

viou

rs; r

eplic

ated

w

ith s

econ

d se

t of

ta

rget

s

for

3–12

m

onth

ssi

gnifi

cant

gai

ns

on C

ELF-

pre-

scho

ol r

ecep

tive

and

expr

essi

ve

stan

dard

sco

res,

bu

t no

t on

DEA

P;

gene

raliz

atio

n to

ot

her

sett

ings

and

co

nver

satio

n no

t m

easu

red

Hut

chin

son

Cle

gg

(201

1)

12t,

6cm

ean:

6;

9la

ngua

ge

diffi

culti

es

iden

tifie

d by

sc

hool

sta

ff,

not

on S

LT

case

load

activ

ities

ad

dres

sing

lis

teni

ng,

mem

ory,

co

mpr

ehen

sion

an

d ex

pres

sion

trai

ned

teac

her

or T

A

in g

roup

s of

6ye

sth

erap

y vs

. co

ntro

l gro

up

(not

ran

dom

ly

assi

gned

, fro

m

diffe

rent

sch

ool)

1 ×

30

min

utes

pe

r w

eek

for

8 w

eeks

(4

hour

s)

No

prog

ress

on

BPV

S. C

hild

ren

in

inte

rven

tion

scho

ol

(but

not

con

trol

s)

impr

oved

on

all a

spec

ts

of B

us s

tory

not

mea

sure

dpr

ogre

ss o

n st

anda

rdiz

ed t

est

(Bus

sto

ry)

(Con

tinue

d)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 26: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

32 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Stud

yN

umbe

r of

pa

rtic

ipan

tsA

geD

iagn

osis

Lang

uage

In

terv

entio

n m

etho

d

Met

hod

of d

eliv

ery

Con

trol

s?T

otal

hou

rs

ther

apy

Res

ults

Prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d?Pr

ogre

ss

gene

raliz

ed?

Bow

yer-

Cra

ne e

t al

. (2

008)

151t

(7

6t: o

ral

lang

uage

, 75t

: ph

onol

ogy

and

read

ing)

mea

n 4;

9ch

ildre

n w

ith lo

wes

t vo

cabu

lary

an

d w

ord

reas

onin

g in

eac

h sc

hool

(no

t al

l lan

guag

e im

pair

ed)

oral

lang

uage

: w

ork

on

voca

bula

ry,

narr

ativ

e,

ques

tion

answ

erin

g an

d ge

nera

tion;

gr

amm

atic

al

erro

rs r

e-ca

st

alte

rnat

ing

1:1

and

grou

p w

ith t

rain

ed

and

supp

orte

d T

As

follo

win

g a

man

ual

noR

CT

: 2 t

hera

py

grou

ps, n

o co

ntro

l gro

up

daily

al

tern

atin

g 1:

1 (2

0 m

inut

es)

and

grou

p (3

0 m

inut

es)

for

20 w

eeks

(=

45

hou

rs)

oral

lang

uage

gro

up

impr

oved

sig

nific

antly

m

ore

than

pho

nolo

gy

and

read

ing

grou

p on

ta

ught

voc

abul

ary

and

RA

PT g

ram

mar

; tre

nd

tow

ards

mor

e pr

ogre

ss

on B

us s

tory

; no

diffe

renc

e on

list

enin

g co

mpr

ehen

sion

prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d fo

r 6

mon

ths

prog

ress

on

stan

dard

ized

tes

ts

Bow

yer-

Cra

ne e

t al

. (2

011)

68t

(31t

: ora

l la

ngua

ge, 3

7t:

phon

olog

y an

d re

adin

g)

mea

n 4;

8la

ngua

ge

impa

ired

(29

SL

I and

39

gene

ral d

elay

)

oral

lang

uage

: w

ork

on

voca

bula

ry,

narr

ativ

e,

ques

tion

answ

erin

g an

d ge

nera

tion;

gr

amm

atic

al

erro

rs r

e-ca

st

daily

, alte

rnat

ing

1:1

and

grou

p w

ith

trai

ned

and

supp

orte

d T

As

follo

win

g a

man

ual

noR

CT

: 2 t

hera

py

grou

ps, n

o co

ntro

l gro

up

alte

rnat

ing

1:1

(20

min

utes

) an

d gr

oup

(30

min

utes

) fo

r 20

wee

ks =

45

hour

s

oral

lang

uage

gro

up

impr

oved

sig

nific

antly

m

ore

than

pho

nolo

gy

and

read

ing

grou

p on

ta

ught

voc

abul

ary

and

RA

PT g

ram

mar

(bo

th

SLI a

nd g

ener

al-d

elay

ch

ildre

n); n

o di

ffere

nce

on c

ompr

ehen

sion

or

narr

ativ

e

prog

ress

m

aint

aine

d fo

r 6

mon

ths

on t

augh

t vo

cabu

lary

, bu

t no

t on

gr

amm

ar

prog

ress

on

stan

dard

ized

tes

ts

Not

es. S

ee n

otes

at

Tab

le 1

; CEL

F =

Clin

ical

Eva

luat

ion

of L

angu

age

Fund

amen

tals

; DEA

P =

Dia

gnos

tic E

valu

atio

n of

Art

icul

atio

n an

d Ph

onol

ogy

(Dod

d et

al.,

200

2); S

LTA

= s

peec

h an

d la

ngua

ge

ther

apy

assi

stan

t.

Tab

le 6

. (C

ontin

ued)

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 27: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 33

expressive language. In this study, a teacher from a collaborative team of specialist teachers and SLTs who initially delivered a whole-school training package was in the school for two days a week during the project. Thus, the education professionals delivering the intervention were well-supported and their provision was closely monitored.

Several studies from the education literature (e.g. Bowyer-Crane et al., 2008) have also shown that, if well-supported and closely monitored, TAs can successfully deliver intervention that improves expressive language, taught vocabulary and literacy in children with literacy difficulties. Follow-up analyses (Bowyer-Crane et al., 2011) of only the children from the Bowyer-Crane et al. (2008) study who had language impairments showed that intensive intervention from a highly trained and well supported TA can improve understanding of taught vocabulary and expressive grammar (although not comprehension) in children with language impairments, regardless of their non-verbal IQ level.

V Variables in intervention

1 Targets of intervention

Implicit, grammar facilitation methods have focused on production of a wide range of morphologi-cal and syntactic targets; however, language comprehension has been largely ignored as a focus of intervention (with the exception of the usage-based approach by Riches, 2013). Studies using acoustically modified speech have focused on general language abilities, not specific morphologi-cal or syntactic targets (with the exception of Bishop et al., 2006). Studies of explicit methods have mainly focused on specific areas of grammar (both comprehension and production). The few stud-ies that have considered maintenance of progress generally show that progress is maintained but does not usually continue after intervention has ceased.

2 Child factors

The majority of studies of implicit grammar facilitation methods reported here either do not mention the receptive language status of their participants (seven studies), or state that the majority of their participants have age appropriate comprehension (four studies). Eight of the studies reviewed included children with RELI, but of those, three showed no greater progress than controls. The other five did not investigate whether those with vs. without receptive language difficulties differed in the amount of progress they made, but analysed them as a whole group which may mask any differences between them. However, a case series (Ellis-Weismer and Murray-Branch, 1989) found that the three children with expressive difficulties only made progress, but the one with RELI did not.

The participants in studies of explicit meta-linguistic methods have usually had both receptive and expressive language difficulties (regardless of whether the targets of intervention were recep-tive or expressive). However, these different participant profiles could be a function of age, as those children whose language difficulties persist are often those who have more pervasive diffi-culties (Bishop and Edmundson, 1987). The majority of grammar facilitation studies have been carried out with children under the age of seven, often with pre-schoolers, although a few studies using these methods include children up to 10 years of age. In contrast, studies of explicit approaches have involved a wider age range, but have tended to focus on older children.

Direct comparisons of explicit and implicit methods indicate that explicit methods may be bet-ter for older children and implicit for younger, but differing responses by age have not been meas-ured in a single study. Also, the relationship between age, severity and response to different intervention approaches remains to be considered. Ebbels et al. (2013), did look for correlations

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 28: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

34 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

between progress and age (and indeed non-verbal abilities) and found no correlations, but all par-ticipants were over 11 years. Direct comparisons of the two main approaches (explicit vs. implicit) with different age groups and different levels of severity (especially as regards expressive lan-guage) are therefore now required.

3 Methods of delivery

The overwhelming majority of studies aiming to directly improve children’s grammatical abilities involve 1:1 delivery of intervention by an SLT or (for the modified speech studies) by a computer. However, several recent studies have focused on the effectiveness of education staff delivering intervention. These have found that well-trained and supported assistants employed directly by the SLT service, or research teams, or very closely monitored, can improve the expressive language of children with expressive language difficulties only, whether the intervention is delivered 1:1 or in groups. However, a ‘consultative model’, where a programme is left for school staff to carry out with little support and monitoring, does not seem to be effective.

Disappointingly, standard therapy packages (whether delivered by an SLT or assistant, 1:1 or in groups) do not seem to improve the receptive or expressive language abilities of children with RELI. However, several studies indicate that explicit therapy methods, either alone, or in combina-tion with more implicit methods, delivered by an SLT can be effective for improving both receptive and expressive language in this group. Implicit therapy methods alone may be effective for this group, but we cannot evaluate this until analyses of progress of children with RELI have been car-ried out separately from those with purely expressive difficulties.

VI Implications

1 Future research

Many areas of grammar have been targeted in intervention studies but many gaps remain. In par-ticular, grammar facilitation methods have focused only on expressive language, primarily with younger children, many of whom do not have receptive language difficulties. In contrast, metalin-guistic methods have focused on both comprehension and production skills, but mostly with older children. Thus, both these methods should be investigated further with different age groups and receptive language status, for both comprehension and production of language.

The relative benefits of the two main approaches, and indeed their combination, also needs to be investigated with different age groups and with those with and without receptive language dif-ficulties. Such studies will require large numbers of participants. Even if age and receptive lan-guage skills are held constant, the effect size of a difference between two interventions is likely to be much smaller than between an intervention and control (where significant effects can be found in quite small studies). Varying age and receptive language status will require even larger numbers of participants in order to find significant effects within different groups.

Children with receptive language difficulties appear to be the least likely to progress with ther-apy, but also are those the most in need. Therefore studies are urgently needed with this group to establish which aspects of intervention are crucial in enabling these children to make progress.

2 Clinical implications

The intervention research base needs further development before clinicians can make reliable judgements regarding the appropriateness of different intervention approaches and methods of

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 29: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 35

delivery for individual children for particular grammatical targets. However, it is important for clinicians to make informed decisions using the evidence available. I would suggest that clinicians who wish to target the grammatical difficulties of a school-aged child, should first establish which areas of grammar are causing difficulties. Then, they should decide which areas they wish to treat and in which order. These decisions should be based on factors such as functional importance (e.g. the impact on access to the curriculum and friendships), typical developmental order of acquisition and a plan of how to proceed from one target to another, as one target may require prior learning of another.

Having decided on the targets, they should then consider whether any particular method of intervention has been shown to be effective (preferably in a study including experimental control) for that target, for children of a similar age, diagnosis and level of severity to the child they wish to treat. The tables in this paper are grouped by language target and sorted by level of experimental control to aid clinicians in this process.

The final step is to choose the method of delivery. The research evidence is primarily based on 1:1 delivery of therapy by an SLT. For a variety of reasons, clinicians may not be able or wish to offer this method of delivery, but they should be aware that a change in the method of delivery may affect the effectiveness of the intervention. It seems that delivering therapy via assistants and/or in groups can be effective for improving expressive language in children with ELI, but only if the assistants are well trained, supported and closely monitored to ensure that they do actually carry out the intervention. Indeed, McCartney et al. (2011) recommend that ‘SLT and school services adopting a consultancy model require a careful activity audit to be undertaken’ (p. 80).

However, for children with RELI, the limited evidence to date of effective intervention indi-cates that progress may only be made when intervention is delivered by an SLT, as in the studies of explicit therapy methods which mostly involve such children (even when focusing on expressive targets). However, if a clinician decides to use other methods of delivery for children with RELI, they should evaluate closely what they have done and share their findings with the rest of the SLT community.

VII Conclusions

In recent years, the quality and quantity of studies investigating the effectiveness of intervention for grammar in school-aged children has greatly improved. We can have reasonable confidence in the effectiveness of some interventions for particular types of children, but we have yet to compare directly the effectiveness of these different approaches with different types of children in order to establish which method is the most effective for which children using which method of delivery.

A parallel challenge is using this evidence wisely in clinical practice. Clinicians and services are under pressure to deliver effective interventions at the lowest possible cost, and at times effective-ness and cost may indicate different intervention or methods of delivery. A balance has to be struck. However, clinicians should ensure they do not waste everyone’s time and money providing inter-vention which has been shown to be ineffective, even if it is the cheapest option. Providing inef-fective intervention benefits nobody.

We also need to be very clear about the difference between:

•• evidence that an intervention is ineffective; and•• no evidence that an intervention is effective.

In the former case, we should not provide the intervention, even if we / the children / their parents / schools / commissioners like it. In the latter case, the intervention may be effective or ineffective:

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 30: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

36 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

we just do not know. If there is evidence that another intervention is effective, then that should be used. But if there is no evidence, we should use a best-fit approach combined with clinical experi-ence and then evaluate its effectiveness for the particular combination of target and child factors with which we are faced.

All clinical work has the potential to be a research project with the addition of experimental control. This can be achieved, for example, by use of waiting lists as waiting controls, using school holidays as baseline periods, or having control areas for each targeted area (for discussion of the value of case studies, see Vance and Clegg, 2013). Small group studies and even small-scale RCTs can also be carried out within clinical services, involving children with a profile relevant to that particular service and targeting priority areas. If the effect sizes are large enough (and hence clini-cally important), these can be significant even with relatively small numbers of participants. Indeed, the small-scale clinically-based RCTs that I have led have had only 14 (Ebbels et al., 2013), 15 (Ebbels et al., 2012) and 27 participants (Ebbels et al., 2007). All showed significant differences between intervention and controls (although not between interventions), because the effect sizes were large. Thus, RCTs need not require huge amounts of money to run (unless small effects are expected, such as comparisons of interventions) and should be possible for SLT services to carry out with appropriate support. If research becomes more embedded in our clinical practice, we have the potential to improve our evidence base dramatically, which will benefit both the SLT profession and the children we serve.

Declaration of conflicting interest

The author declares that there is no conflict of interest.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

References

Bishop DVM (2003) The Test for Reception of Grammar: TROG 2. London: Psychological Corporation.Bishop DVM and Edmundson A (1987) Language-impaired 4-year-olds: Distinguishing transient from per-

sistent impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 52: 156–73.Bishop DVM, Adams CV, and Rosen S (2006) Resistance of grammatical impairment to computerized com-

prehension training in children specific and non-specific language impairments. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 41: 19–40.

Bolderson S, Dosanjh C, Milligan C, Pring T, and Chiat S (2011) Colourful semantics: A clinical investiga-tion. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 27: 344–53.

Bowyer-Crane CA, Snowling MJ, Duff F, and Hulme C (2011) Response to early intervention of children with specific and general language impairment. Learning Disabilities: A Contemporary Journal 9: 107–21.

Bowyer-Crane C, Snowling MJ, Duff FJ, Fieldsend E, Carroll JM, Miles J, et al. (2008) Improving early language and literacy skills: Differential effects of an oral language versus a phonology with reading intervention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 49: 422–32.

Boyle J, McCartney E, Forbes J, and O’Hare A (2007) A randomised controlled trial and economic evaluation of direct versus indirect and individual versus group modes of speech and language therapy for children with primary language impairment. Health Technology Assessment 11: 1–160.

Boyle J, McCartney E, O’Hare A, and Forbes J (2009) Direct versus indirect and individual versus group modes of language therapy for children with primary language impairment: Principal outcomes from a randomized controlled trial and economic evaluation. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 44: 826–46.

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 31: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 37

Boyle J, McCartney E, O’Hare A, and Law J (2010) Intervention for mixed receptive-expressive language impairment: A review. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 52: 994–99.

Bryan A (1997) Colourful semantics. In: Chiat S, Law J, and Marshall J (eds) Language disorders in children and adults: Psycholinguistic approaches to therapy. London: Whurr, 143–61.

Camarata SM and Nelson KE (1992) Treatment efficiency as a function of target selection in the remediation of child language disorders. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 6: 167–78.

Camarata SM, Nelson KE, and Camarata MN (1994) Comparison of conversational-recasting and imitative procedures for training grammatical structures in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 37: 1414–23.

Clark A, O’Hare A, Watson J, Cohen W, Cowie H, Elton R, et al. (2007) Severe receptive language disor-der in childhood-familial aspects and long-term outcomes: Results from a Scottish study. Archives of Disease in Childhood 92: 614–19.

Cohen W, Hodson A, O’Hare A, Boyle J, Durrani T, McCartney E, et al. (2005) Effects of computer-based intervention using acoustically modified speech (Fast ForWord-language) in severe mixed receptive-expressive language impairment: Outcomes from a randomized control trial. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 48: 715–29.

Connell PJ (1987) An effect of modeling and imitation teaching procedures on children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 30: 105–13.

Connell PJ and Stone CA (1992) Morpheme learning of children with specific language impairment under controlled instructional conditions. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 35: 844–52.

Courtwright JA and Courtwright IC (1976) Imitative modelling as a theoretical base for instructing language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 19: 655–63.

Courtwright JA and Courtwright IC (1979) Imitative modeling as a language intervention strategy: The effects of two mediating variables. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 22: 366–88.

Culatta B and Horn D (1982) A program for achieving generalization of grammatical rules to spontaneous discourse. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 47: 174–80.

Davies P, Shanks B, and Davies K (2004) Improving narrative skills in young children with delayed language development. Educational Review 56: 271–86.

Dodd B, Zhu H, Crosbie S, Holm A, Ozanne A. Diagnostic evaluation of articulation and phonology. London: Harcourt.

Ebbels S (2008) Improving grammatical skill in children with specific language impairment. In: Norbury CF, Tomblin B, and Bishop DVM (eds) Understanding developmental language disorders. Hove: Psychology Press, 149–74.

Ebbels S and van der Lely H (2001) Meta-syntactic therapy using visual coding for children with severe per-sistent SLI. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 36: 345–50.

Ebbels SH (2007) Teaching grammar to school-aged children with specific language impairment using Shape Coding. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 23: 67–93.

Ebbels SH, Maric N, Murphy A, and Turner G (2013) Improving comprehension in adolescents with severe receptive language impairments: A randomised control trial of intervention for coordinating conjunc-tions. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders. Article first published online: DOI: 10.1111/1460-6984.12047.

Ebbels SH, van der Lely HKJ, and Dockrell JE (2007) Intervention for verb argument structure in children with persistent SLI: A randomized control trial. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 50: 1330–49.

Ellis-Weismer S and Murray-Branch J (1989) Modeling versus modeling plus evoked production train-ing: A comparison of 2 language intervention methods. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 54: 269–81.

Fey ME and Loeb DF (2002) An evaluation of the facilitative effects of inverted yes–no questions on the acquisition of auxiliary verbs. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 45: 160–74.

Fey ME, Cleave PL, and Long SH (1997) Two models of grammar facilitation in children with language impairments: Phase 2. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 40: 5–19.

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 32: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

38 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Fey ME, Cleave P, Long SH, and Hughes DL (1993) Two approaches to the facilitation of grammar in chil-dren with language impairment: An experimental evaluation. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 36: 141–57.

Fey ME, Finestack LH, Gajewski BJ, Popescu M, and Lewine JD (2010) A preliminary evaluation of Fast ForWord-language as an adjuvant treatment in language intervention. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 53: 430–49.

Finestack LH and Fey ME (2009) Evaluation of a deductive procedure to teach grammatical inflections to children with language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 18: 289–302.

Friel-Patti S, DesBarres K, and Thibodeau L (2001) Case studies of children using Fast ForWord. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 10: 203–15.

Gillam SL, Gillam RB, and Reece K (2012) Language outcomes of contextualized and decontextualised language intervention: Results of an early efficacy study. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 43: 276–91.

Gillam RB, Crofford JA, Gale MA, and Hoffman LM (2001) Language change following computer-assisted language instruction with Fast ForWord or Laureate Learning Systems software. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 10: 231–47.

Gillam RB, Loeb DF, Hoffman LM, Bohman T, Champlin CA, Thibodeau L, et al. (2008) The efficacy of Fast ForWord language intervention in school-age children with language impairment: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 51: 97–119.

Guendouzi J (2003) ‘SLI’, a generic category of language impairment that emerges from specific differences: A case study of two individual linguistic profiles. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics 17: 135–52.

Hassink JM and Leonard LB (2010) Within-treatment factors as predictors of outcomes following conversa-tional recasting. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 19: 213–24.

Hutchinson J and Clegg J (2011) Education practitioner-led intervention to facilitate language learning in young children: An effectiveness study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 27: 151–64.

Kulkarni A, Pring T, and Ebbels S (in press) Evaluating the effectiveness of therapy based around Shape Coding to develop the use of regular past tense morphemes in two children with language impairments. Child Language Teaching and Therapy.

Law J, Garrett Z, and Nye C (2004) The efficacy of treatment for children with developmental speech and lan-guage delay/disorder: A meta-analysis. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 47: 924–43.

Law J, Rush R, Schoon I, and Parsons S (2009) Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: Literacy, mental health, and employment outcomes. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 52: 1401–16.

Lea J (1970) The colour pattern scheme: A method of remedial language teaching. Hurst Green: Moor House School.

Leonard LB (1975) Developmental considerations in the management of language disabled children. Journal of Learning Disabilities 8: 44–49.

Levy H and Friedmann N (2009) Treatment of syntactic movement in syntactic SLI: A case study. First Language 29: 15–50.

Loeb DF, Stoke C, and Fey ME (2001) Language changes associated with Fast ForWord-language: Evidence from case studies. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 10: 216–30.

Matheny N and Panagos JM (1978) Comparing the effects of articulation and syntax programs on syntax and articulation improvement. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 9: 50–56.

McCartney E, Ellis S, Boyle J, Turnbull M, and Kerr J (2010) Developing a language support model for main-stream primary school teachers. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 26: 359–74.

McCartney E, Boyle J, Ellis S, Bannatyne S, and Turnbull M (2011) Indirect language therapy for children with persistent language impairment in mainstream primary schools: Outcomes from a cohort interven-tion. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 46: 74–82.

Mecrow C, Beckwith J, and Klee T (2010) An exploratory trial of the effectiveness of an enhanced con-sultative approach to delivering speech and language intervention in schools. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 45: 354–67.

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 33: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

Ebbels 39

Motsch HJ and Riehemann S (2008) Effects of ‘context-optimization’ on the acquisition of grammatical case in children with specific language impairment: An experimental evaluation in the classroom. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 43: 683–98.

Mulac A and Tomlinson CN (1977) Generalization of an operant remediation program for syntax with lan-guage delayed children. Journal of Communication Disorders 10: 231–43.

Nelson KE, Camarata SM, Welsh J, Butkovsky L, and Camarata M (1996) Effects of imitative and conversa-tional recasting treatment on the acquisition of grammar in children with specific language impairment and younger language-normal children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 39: 850–59.

Nippold MA (2010) Back to school: Why the speech-language pathologist belongs in the classroom. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 41: 377–78.

Petersen DR, Gillam SL, and Gillam RR (2008) Emerging procedures in narrative assessment: The index of narrative complexity. Topics in Language Disorders 28: 115–30.

Petersen DB, Gillam SL, Spencer T, and Gillam RB (2010) The effects of literate narrative intervention on children with neurologically based language impairments: An early stage study. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 53: 961–81.

Proctor-Williams K and Fey ME (2007) Recast density and acquisition of novel irregular past tense verbs. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 50: 1029–47.

Proctor-Williams K, Fey ME, and Loeb DF (2001) Parental recasts and production of copulas and articles by children with specific language impairment and typical language. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 10: 155–68.

Renfrew C (2003) The Action Picture Test. 4th edition. Oxford: Speechmark Publishing.Riches N (2013) Treating the passive in children with specific language impairment: A usage-based approach.

Child Language Teaching and Therapy 29: 155–69.Saxton M (2000) Negative evidence and negative feedback: Immediate effects on the grammaticality of child

speech. First Language 20: 221–52.Scientific Learning Corporation (1998) Fast ForWord-language [computer software]. Berkley, CA: Scientific

Learning Corporation.Seeff-Gabriel B, Chiat S, and Pring T (2012) Intervention for co-occurring speech and language difficulties.

Child Language Teaching and Therapy 28: 123–35.Semel E, Wiig EH, and Secord WA (2006) Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals: 3rd edition: CELF

3. London: Harcourt Assessment.Smith-Lock KM, Leitao S, Lambert L, and Nickels L (2013a) Effective intervention for expressive grammar

in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 48: 265–82.

Smith-Lock K, Leitao S, Lambert L, Prior P, Dunn A, Cronje J, et al. (2013b) Daily or weekly? The role of treatment frequency in the effectiveness of grammar treatment for children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 15: 255–67.

Spooner L (2002) Addressing expressive language disorder in children who also have severe receptive lan-guage disorder: A psycholinguistic approach. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 18: 289–313.

Stothard SE, Snowling M, Bishop DVM, Chipchase BB, and Kaplan CA (1998) Language-impaired pre-schoolers: A follow-up into adolescence. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 41: 407–18.

Strong GK, Torgerson CJ, Torgerson D, and Hulme C (2011) A systematic meta-analytic review of evidence for the effectiveness of the ‘Fast ForWord’ language intervention program. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 52: 224–35.

Swanson LA, Fey ME, Mills CE, and Hood LS (2005) Use of narrative-based language intervention with children who have specific language impairment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 14: 131–43.

Swisher L, Restrepo MA, Plante E, and Lowell S (1995) Effect of implicit and explicit rule presentation on bound- morpheme generalization in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 38: 168–73.

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Page 34: Child Language Teaching and Therapy Effectiveness of … Language... · 2015-07-21 · Communication Trust complements the SpeechBITE website as it allows searches for particular

40 Child Language Teaching and Therapy 30(1)

Tallal P, Stark RE, and Mellits D (1985) The relationship between auditory temporal analysis and receptive language-development: Evidence from studies of developmental language disorder. Neuropsychologia 23: 527–34.

Tallal P, Miller SL, Bedi G, Byma G, Wang XQ, Nagarajan SS, et al. (1996) Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science 271: 81–84.

Tomasello M (2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Tyler A, Lewis KE, Haskill A, and Tolbert LC (2003) Outcomes of different speech and language goal attack strategies. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 46: 1077–94.

Tyler AA and Watterson KH (1991) Effects of phonological versus language intervention in preschoolers with both phonological and language impairment. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 7: 141–60.

Tyler AA, Lewis KE, Haskill A, and Tolbert LC (2002) Efficacy and cross-domain effects of a morphosyntax and a phonology intervention. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools 33: 52–66.

Vance M and Clegg J (2013) Use of single case study research in child speech, language and communication interventions. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 28: 255–58.

Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, and Rashotte CA (1999) Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

Wilcox JM and Leonard LB (1978) Experimental acquisition of wh-questions in language-disordered chil-dren. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 21: 220–39.

at University College London on June 25, 2015clt.sagepub.comDownloaded from