central coherence

Upload: anacrisnu

Post on 14-Apr-2018

235 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/27/2019 Central Coherence

    1/22

    The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused Cognitive Stylein Autism Spectrum Disorders

    Francesca Happe1,3 and Uta Frith2

    Weak central coherence refers to the detail-focused processing style proposed to

    characterise autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The original suggestion of a core deficit in

    central processing resulting in failure to extract global form/meaning, has been challenged inthree ways. First, it may represent an outcome of superiority in local processing. Second, it

    may be a processing bias, rather than deficit. Third, weak coherence may occur alongside,

    rather than explain, deficits in social cognition. A review of over 50 empirical studies of

    coherence suggests robust findings of local bias in ASD, with mixed findings regarding weak

    global processing. Local bias appears not to be a mere side-effect of executive dysfunction, and

    may be independent of theory of mind deficits. Possible computational and neural models are

    discussed.

    KEY WORDS: Autism spectrum disorders; central coherence; cognitive style; individual differences;

    localglobal processing.

    Some individuals with autism spectrum disorders

    (ASD) can name the pitch of the pop as a cork

    comes out of a bottle, or identify dozens of brands of

    vacuum cleaner from their sound alone. Others can

    spot a misaligned book in a bookcase in seconds, or

    mimic foreign speech distinctions not usually notice-

    able to non-native speakers. These exceptional per-

    ceptual abilities may be maladaptive in so far as they

    may lead to distress at small changes in the environ-

    ment. Kanners original description of autism high-

    lighted this attention to detail and inability to

    experience wholes without full attention to theconstituent parts as one factor in the characteristic

    insistence on sameness: A situation, a performance, a

    sentence is not regarded as complete if it is not made

    up of exactly the same elements that were present at

    the time the child was first confronted with it. If the

    slightest ingredient is altered or removed, the total

    situation is no longer the same and therefore is not

    accepted as such... (Kanner, 1943, p. 246). Indeed, a

    persistent preoccupation with parts of objects is one

    of the diagnostic criteria for autistic disorder in

    current practice (DSM-IV, APA, 1994).

    Understanding perceptual processes in ASD

    may involve explaining both disordered and superiorprocessing. One cognitive theory that has specifically

    sought to address both deficits and assets in ASD is

    the weak coherence account. Frith (1989) drew

    attention to the tendency for typically developing

    children and adults to process incoming information

    for meaning and gestalt (global) form, often at the

    expense of attention to or memory for details and

    surface structure. This tendency, referred to by

    Bartlett (1932) as drive for meaning, was termed

    1 Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute

    of Psychiatry, Kings College London, London, UK.2 Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College, London,

    UK.3 Correspondence should be addressed to: Social, Genetic and

    Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Kings

    College London, De Crespigny Park, Denmark Hill, P.O. Box

    P080, SE5 8AF, London, UK. e-mail: [email protected]

    50162-3257/06/0100-0005/0 2006 Springer ScienceBusiness Media, Inc.

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, Vol. 36, No. 1, January 2006 ( 2006)

    DOI 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0

    Published Online: February 1, 2006

  • 7/27/2019 Central Coherence

    2/22

    central coherence by Frith. Individuals with ASD

    were hypothesised to show weak central coherence;

    a processing bias for featural and local information,

    and relative failure to extract gist or see the big

    picture in everyday life. This processing bias was

    evident in early work on verbal memory, showingrelatively little benefit from meaning (Hermelin &

    OConnor, 1967), and exact rather than corrected

    repetition (Aurnhammer-Frith, 1969). In early work

    using visuo-spatial tasks, ASD groups showed supe-

    riority at disembedding (Shah & Frith, 1983), greater

    reliance on adjacent elements in pattern extraction

    (Frith, 1970), and a reduced inversion effect in face

    processing (Langdell, 1978).

    Interest in the coherence account of ASD has

    grown rapidly since the early work by Frith (1989)

    and Happe (1999; Frith & Happe , 1994), with more

    than 60 publications relating to this topic publishedsince 1999, a fourfold increase on the previous 10-

    year period. In that time, and in response to empirical

    findings, the coherence account has been modified

    from Friths original conception in three important

    ways. First, the original suggestion of a core deficit in

    central processing, manifest in failure to extract

    global form and meaning, has changed from a

    primary problem to a more secondary out-

    comewith greater emphasis on possible superiority

    in local or detail-focused processing. Second, the idea

    of a core deficit has given way to the suggestion of a

    processing bias or cognitive style, which can be

    overcome in tasks with explicit demands for global

    processing. Last, the explanatory remit of the account

    has changed, with a recognition that weak coherence

    may be one aspect of cognition in ASD alongside,

    rather than causing/explaining, deficits in social

    cognition (e.g. theory of mind; Frith, 1989, revised

    2003).

    Along with increased research interest, the

    notion of weak coherence or detail-focused process-

    ing style, has been received with enthusiasm and

    immediate recognition by the ASD community

    (affected individuals and their families). Autobio-

    graphical accounts of autism often describe frag-mented perception (Gerland, 1997). Weak coherence

    is seen as addressing aspects of ASD that some other

    accounts have neglected, such as areas of talent,

    super-acute perception, and lack of generalisation.

    For example, perceptual abnormalities such as hyper-

    sensitivity, clinically/anecdotally reported but little

    studied in research to date, may relate to context-free

    processing as expectations and context-based inter-

    pretation are known to modulate experience of

    sensory stimuli in neurotypicals (people without

    ASD). Just as there is a higher than usual occurrence

    of perfect pitch in ASD (Miller, 1999), so absolute,

    rather than relative, coding of other sensory stimuli

    may underlie some aspects of perceptual discomfort

    or fascination. Problems with generalisation of skillswould follow from weak coherence, if experiences are

    coded in terms of details. If people with ASD

    remember each exemplar rather than extracting

    prototypes (Klinger & Dawson, 2001), this would

    render recognition of situations that are alike

    problematic: only if a situation shares the key

    detail(s) with a previous experience, will generalisa-

    tion of skills occur (Plaisted, 2001; Rincover &

    Koegel, 1975).

    OVERVIEW OF RESEARCH

    Table I summarises published experimental

    group studies in which weak coherence in ASD is

    addressed, and those directly relevant studies

    described below. A number of studies, both within

    and beyond those explicitly addressing coherence, are

    directly relevant to perceptual processing in ASD.

    Relevant to perception in the auditory modality, are

    demonstrations of stable memory for exact pitches

    (Bonnel et al., 2003; Heaton, Hermelin, & Pring,

    1998), enhanced local processing (with intact global

    processing) of musical stimuli (Heaton, 2003;

    Mottron, Peretz, & Menard, 2000), reduced interfer-

    ence from melodic structure (combining pitch and

    timing effects) in music processing (Foxton et al.,

    2003), and a reduced McGurk effect (i.e. less

    influence from visual to auditory speech perception;

    DeGelder, Vroomen, & Van der Heide, 1991).

    Relevant to perception in the visual modality,

    individuals with ASD show raised thresholds for

    perceiving coherent motion (Bertone, Mottron,

    Jelenic, & Faubert, 2003; Milne et al., 2002; Spencer

    et al., 2000), and reduced susceptibility to visually

    induced motion (Gepner, Mestre, Masson, & Scho-

    nen, 1995; Gepner & Mestre, 2002). Superior visualsearch (Plaisted, ORiordan, & Baron-Cohen, 1998a;

    ORiordan, Plaisted, Driver, & Baron-Cohen, 2001),

    and superior discrimination learning of highly

    confusable patterns (Plaisted, ORiordan, & Baron-

    Cohen, 1998b), have also been reported. Active

    processes of visual grouping may be affected, shown

    in reduced gestalt grouping (Brosnan, Scott, Fox, &

    Pye, 2004), reduced susceptibility to visual illusions

    (Happe , 1996; but see, Ropar & Mitchell, 1999,

    6 Happe and Frith

  • 7/27/2019 Central Coherence

    3/22

    TableI.PublishedGroupStudies

    AssessingCentralCoherenceinAutismSp

    ectrumDisorders

    Reference

    Participants(groupmeans)

    Tasks

    Mainfindings

    Visuo-spatial

    EFT

    ShahandFrith(1983)

    20A

    SDCA13,MANV9.6

    CE

    FT

    ASDgroupaccuracy>

    MH,TD

    20TDMA-matched

    Qualitativeevidenceofmoreimmediatesuccess

    20M

    HCAandMAmatch

    BrianandBryson(1996)

    16A

    SDCA20,Ravensraw39,

    Disembedding:adapted(meaningful)CEFT

    item

    splusabstractandfragmenteditems

    Nogroupdifferencesofgroup

    conditioninteractions

    34%le,PPVTstscore77,raw120

    Recognition:asaboveplusdistractoritems

    RTtodisembeddedMeaningful>Abstract>Fragmented

    inallgroups

    15TDCA12,Ravensscore39,55%ile

    RecognitionaccuracyMeaningful>Abstract=

    Fragmented(atchance)inallgroups

    16TDCA12,PPVTstscore103,

    rawscore119

    JolliffeandBaron-Cohen

    (1997)

    17H

    FACA31,FIQ105

    EF

    T

    HFAandAspergergroupsfasterthanTDonEFT.

    17A

    SCA28,FIQ107

    Mo

    difiedReydrawingtask

    Nogroupdifferencesondrawingtask

    17TDCAandFIQmatched

    RoparandMitchell(2001)19autismCA14,VMA7

    EF

    T,Blockdesign

    Autism>

    MH,TDonEFTandBlockDesign(AS=TD

    11-year-oldgroup)

    11A

    SCA12,VMA10

    20M

    HCA13,VMA7

    37TDCA8&11

    Jarroldetal.(2000)

    17A

    SDCA10,VMA8

    CE

    FT,DASBD(+Belieftasks)

    InASDandTDchildren,EFTandBDnegativelycorre-

    latedwithToMscoresonceVMAandC

    Apartialledout

    24TDCA5

    Pre

    schoolEFT,DASBD(+Belieftasks)

    EFTnegativelycorrelatedwithEyesTestscore

    60T

    AdultsCA1825

    EF

    TandEyesTask

    Morganetal.(2003)

    21A

    SDCA4,VMA33months,

    Leite

    r(NVIQ)95

    Pre

    schoolEFT

    ASDfasterthancontrolsonEFT(accuracyns)

    21TD/MHCAandnVIQmatched

    PatternConstruction(DAS)(+joint

    attention,pretendplayratings)

    ASD>ControlsonPatternConstruction

    Correlationscoherence

    jointattention,pretendplayns

    BlockDesign

    ShahandFrith(1993)

    10H

    ighIQASDPIQ97

    We

    chslerBlockDesign

    7/10HiIQ(scaledscores1319)and6/10loIQASD(ss

    915)BDpersonalpeaksubtest

    10LowIQ(meanPerformancesubte

    stscorefor85%,

    regardlessofToM

    performance

    Weak Coherence 7

  • 7/27/2019 Central Coherence

    4/22

    TableI.Continued

    Reference

    Participants(groupmeans)

    Tasks

    Mainfindings

    Pring,Hermelin,&Heavey

    (1995)

    18A

    SDCA26,RavensMA12

    Blo

    ckdesigntask;meaningfulscenes,and

    We

    chsler-likedesigns

    ArtisticallytalentedTDfasterthanASD

    onmeaningful

    scenes.

    18T

    DMAmatched

    ArtisticallytalentedTDandallASD(regardlessofartistic

    talent)fasterthannonartisticTD

    Both

    groupedbyartisticability

    Hie

    rarchicalfigures

    Ozonoff,Strayer,

    McMahon,&Filloux

    (1994)

    14A

    SD,CA12,FIQ100

    Na

    vonHierarchicalFigurestask;largeletter

    com

    posedofsmallersameordifferent

    lett

    er(selectiveattention)

    Nogroupdifferences;allgroupsshowedglobaladvantage

    andinterference

    14T

    DCAandIQmatched

    14T

    ourettesyndromeCAand

    IQm

    atched

    Mottronetal.(1999)

    11H

    FACA15,RavensIQ110

    Na

    vonHierarchicalFigures(divided

    attention)

    HFA,butnotTD,groupshowaglobaladvantage

    11T

    DCAandIQmatched

    Palmermentalsynthesistask

    Nogroupdifferencesineffectofgoodnes

    sofPalmer

    figures

    Plaistedetal.(1999)

    17A

    SDCA10,ravensscore33

    Na

    vonHierarchicalFiguresinDividedand

    Selectiveattentionconditions

    Nogroupdifferencesinselectiveattentioncondition

    17TD

    CAandRavensrawscore

    matc

    hed

    ASDshownoglobaladvantageindivide

    dattentioncon-

    dition(TDdomakefewererrorsfortargetsatglobalthan

    locallevel)

    Rinehartetal.(2000)

    12H

    FACA10,FIQ94

    Na

    vonHierarchical(numbers)Figures(selec-

    tiveattention)

    Allgroupsshowedexpectedglobaladvan

    tageandinter-

    ference

    12A

    SCA12,FIQ104

    HFA,butnotASgroup,showedmorelocalinterference

    thanTD

    12+

    12TDCAandIQmatched

    Rinehartetal.(2001)

    12H

    FACA10,FIQ94

    Na

    vonHierarchical(numbers)Figures

    (dividedattention)

    HFARTtogloballeveltargetsslowedw

    henprevious

    targetatlocallevel,comparedwithTDg

    roup(deficit

    movingfromlocaltoglobalprocessing)

    12A

    SCA12,FIQ104

    NosuchgroupdifferenceforASgroup

    12+

    12TDCAandIQmatched

    Mottronetal.(2003)

    12H

    FACA16,IQ105110

    Hierarchicalfigures

    Nogpdifference(butalsonoglobaladvantageinTD

    group)

    101

    2TDmatchedonCA&IQ

    Fra

    gmentedletterrecognition

    Nogpdifference(butalsonomaineffect

    ofcondition)

    Silhouetteidentification

    Nogpdifference(butalsonomaineffect

    ofcondition)

    Long-short-rangegrouping

    Nogpdifference

    Disembedding

    ASD>

    TD,embeddingeffectsTDgroup

    only

    VisualIllusion

    Happe(1996)

    25A

    SDCA13,VMA7

    Jud

    geillusionsin2Dand3Dforms(verbal

    responsetoillusionsoncards)

    ASDsuccumbtofewerillusionsthanMH

    andTD

    21T

    DCA7

    ASDshowlessbenefitfrom3DdisembeddingthandoMH

    andTD

    26M

    HCAandVMAmatched

    ASD=MH,TDnumbercorrectfor3Dillusions

    8 Happe and Frith

  • 7/27/2019 Central Coherence

    5/22

    RoparandMitchell(1999)23autismCA13,VMA7

    Adjustpartsofcomputerisedillusions

    Participantsinallgroupsstronglysuscep

    tibletoillusions

    inbothformats,nosignificantgroupdiff

    erences

    13A

    SCA14,VMA14

    Verbalresponsetoillusionsoncards

    17M

    HCA10,VMA6

    41TDCA8&11,15adults

    RoparandMitchell(2001)19autismCA14,VMA7

    Adjustpartsofcomputerisedillusions

    Nogroupdifferencesinsusceptibilitytoillusions

    11A

    SCA12,VMA10

    ReyFiguredrawing,BD,EFT,EFT

    Reyfiguregavenoevidenceofgroupdifferencesinglobal

    approach.Performanceonillusionsnotr

    elatedtoperfor-

    manceonvisuo-spatialtasks

    20M

    HCA13,VMA7

    37TDCA8&11

    Dra

    wing

    Mottronetal.(1999)

    10H

    FACA19,PIQ112

    Copyingdrawings(realobjects,non-objects,

    possibleandimpossiblefigures)

    HFAgroupdrawmorelocalfeaturesatstartofcopyand

    arelessslowedbyimpossibilityoffigure,

    comparedwith

    controls

    11TDCAandPIQmatched

    Boothetal.(2003)

    30A

    SDCA11,FIQ100

    Drawingfromanexample

    ASDgroupshowedmoredetail-focuseddrawingstyle

    (25%startwithdetail,drawfragments,33%violatecon-

    figuration)vs.TDandADHD

    31TDCAandFIQmatched

    30A

    DHDCAandFIQmatched

    Mo

    tioncoherence

    Spenceretal.(2000)

    23A

    SD(noCAorIQdetails)

    Mo

    tioncoherencethresholdtask

    ASDraisedmotion,butnotform,coherencethresholds

    relativestoTD

    50V

    MAmatched(711)

    Formcoherencethresholdtask

    19T

    adults

    Milneetal.(2002)

    25A

    SDCA12,Ravensrawscore41

    Mo

    tioncoherencethresholdtask

    ASDraisedmotioncoherencethresholdvs.TD

    22TDCAandRavensmatched

    Bertoneetal.(2003)

    12H

    FACA12,IQ101

    Sen

    sitivityforfirst-andsecond-ordermotion

    HFA=TDonfirst-order(luminance-defined)motion

    12TDCA13

    HFAworsethanTDatdetectingsecond-order(texture-

    defined)motion

    Pellicanoetal.(2005)

    20A

    SDCA10,Ravensrawscore40

    Flickercontrastsensitivity

    Nogroupdifference

    20TDCAandRavensmatched

    Globaldotmotion(GDM)task

    ASDhigherthresholdsthanTD

    CE

    FT

    ASDfasterthanTD.InverserelationCE

    FTRTxGDM

    thresholdsinASDonly

    Fac

    es(selectedstudies)

    Hobson,Ouston,andLee

    (1988)

    17A

    SDCA19,BPVSrawscore65,Ravens

    rawscore36

    Up

    rightfaceidentity/emotionmatching,blank

    -

    mo

    uth/forehead

    ASDemotionmatchingdeclinedwithfew

    ercues,MHless

    soASD>MHmatchinginvertedfaces

    17M

    HCA19,BPVS66,Ravens22

    Inv

    ertedfaceidentity/emotionmatching

    TeunisseanddeGelder

    (2003)

    17H

    FACA19,VIQ90

    Inv

    ersioneffecttask

    Allgroupsworseatrecognisinginvertedthanuprightfaces

    24TDCA910

    Compositeeffecttask

    Non-alignedcompositesrecognisedfasterthanwholes

    (compositeeffect)inTadults,TD(p