working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm

13
~ Pergamon Pll: S0028 3932(97)00082 I Neuropsycholo#ia, Vol. 35, No, 10. pp. 1341 1353, 1997 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved Printed in Great Britain 0028 3932/97 $17.00+0.00 Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm SHARON McDOWELL,*t JOHN WHYTE*t and MARK D'ESPOSITO+§ *Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Philadelphia, U.S.A.: tDepartment of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Temple University School of Medicine, Philadelphia, U.S.A.; ~.Department of Neurology, University of Pennylvania Medical Center, Philadelphia, U.S.A. (Received 28 Norember 1996: accepted 22 May 1997) Abstract--Although many individuals with traumatic brain injury (TBI) perform well on standard neuropsychological tests, they often exhibit marked functional difficulties. The functions which are impaired seem to be analogous to the role of the central executive system (CES) in Baddeley's [Workin 9 Memory, 1986, Oxford University Press, New York] widely accepted model of working memory. The purpose of this study was to investigate CES function in individuals with TBI with a dual-task paradigm. We studied 25 non-demented persons who were at various stages in their recovery from severe TBI and compared their performance on a dual- task paradigm to a group of age-matched controls. Our dual-task paradigm measured performance on a simple visual reaction time task both alone (baseline) and during concurrent tasks of articulation or digit span. Subjects were also assessed with other neuropsychological tests of executive function. TBI patients had slower reaction times on the primary task when performed alone (P< 0.05) and greater decrements in performance during dual-task conditions (P < 0.01). They also exhibited significantly greater deficits than control subjects on other measures of executive function. Although correlations between dual-task performance and other executive measures were quite low, principle components analysis suggested that a common factor does exist between these measures. These findings support the conclusion that TBI patients have a working memory impairment that is due to dysfunction of the CES and which may be related to executive function deficits as measured by standard neuropsychological testing. ~) 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. Key Words: executive function; frontal lobes; traumatic brain injury: working memory. Introduction Individuals with frontal lobe pathology, such as anterior cerebral artery infarction or traumatic brain injury (TBI), often exhibit marked functional difficulties in per- formance of complex tasks, even when functioning well in basic cognitive domains such as arousal, language, memory and perception [2~4]. The practical difficulties these people experience in everyday life seem to arise from gross oversights of important information, poor planning and time management, and a lack of judgment and spontaneous organizational skills which have been described as ~executive functions' [5]. These cognitive impairments and their functional sequelae have a dev- astating impact on individuals with frontal lobe lesions and their families, due to the difficulty these persons have § Address for correspondence: Mark D'Esposito, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, Department of Neurology, 3400 Spruce St, Philadelphia, PA 19104-4283, U.S.A.; e-mail: despo(a mail.med, upenn.edu. recovering normal function in activities of daily living and their requirement for supervision and cueing. Most often, these cognitive impairments have been defined empirically using performance on neuropsychological tasks, but a theoretical model could suggest which cog- nitive processes are involved and define ways to measure the deficits, thus offering significant advantages. Baddeley and Hitch's working memory model [6-8], may help to explain the functional difficulties experienced by individuals with frontal lobe injury. They define work- ing memory as a process for holding information on-line during the performance of other cognitive functions such as language comprehension or problem-solving. This model has three major components: two slave systems and a central executive system (CES). Separate slave sys- tems are responsible for temporarily storing verbal and non-verbal information, and the CES regulates the dis- tribution of limited attentional resources. The CES, anal- ogous to the Supervisory Attentional System (SAS) [7, 9] also coordinates information processing and controls cognitive functioning when novel tasks are performed or 1341

Upload: sharon-mcdowell

Post on 15-Sep-2016

213 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

~ Pergamon Pll: S0028 3932(97)00082 I

Neuropsycholo#ia, Vol. 35, No, 10. pp. 1341 1353, 1997 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved

Printed in Great Britain 0028 3932/97 $17.00+0.00

Working memory impairments in traumatic brain injury: evidence from a dual-task paradigm

S H A R O N M c D O W E L L , * t J O H N W H Y T E * t a n d M A R K D ' E S P O S I T O + §

*Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, Philadelphia, U.S.A.: tDepartment of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Temple University School of Medicine, Philadelphia, U.S.A.; ~.Department of Neurology, University of Pennylvania Medical Center,

Philadelphia, U.S.A.

(Received 28 Norember 1996: accepted 22 May 1997)

Abstract--Although many individuals with traumatic brain injury (TBI) perform well on standard neuropsychological tests, they often exhibit marked functional difficulties. The functions which are impaired seem to be analogous to the role of the central executive system (CES) in Baddeley's [Workin 9 Memory, 1986, Oxford University Press, New York] widely accepted model of working memory. The purpose of this study was to investigate CES function in individuals with TBI with a dual-task paradigm. We studied 25 non-demented persons who were at various stages in their recovery from severe TBI and compared their performance on a dual- task paradigm to a group of age-matched controls. Our dual-task paradigm measured performance on a simple visual reaction time task both alone (baseline) and during concurrent tasks of articulation or digit span. Subjects were also assessed with other neuropsychological tests of executive function. TBI patients had slower reaction times on the primary task when performed alone (P< 0.05) and greater decrements in performance during dual-task conditions (P < 0.01). They also exhibited significantly greater deficits than control subjects on other measures of executive function. Although correlations between dual-task performance and other executive measures were quite low, principle components analysis suggested that a common factor does exist between these measures. These findings support the conclusion that TBI patients have a working memory impairment that is due to dysfunction of the CES and which may be related to executive function deficits as measured by standard neuropsychological testing. ~) 1997 Elsevier Science Ltd.

Key Words: executive function; frontal lobes; traumatic brain injury: working memory.

Introduction

Individuals with frontal lobe pathology, such as anterior cerebral artery infarction or traumatic brain injury (TBI), often exhibit marked functional difficulties in per- formance of complex tasks, even when functioning well in basic cognitive domains such as arousal, language, memory and perception [2~4]. The practical difficulties these people experience in everyday life seem to arise from gross oversights of important information, poor planning and time management, and a lack of judgment and spontaneous organizational skills which have been described as ~executive functions' [5]. These cognitive impairments and their functional sequelae have a dev- astating impact on individuals with frontal lobe lesions and their families, due to the difficulty these persons have

§ Address for correspondence: Mark D'Esposito, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, Department of Neurology, 3400 Spruce St, Philadelphia, PA 19104-4283, U.S.A.; e-mail: despo(a mail.med, upenn.edu.

recovering normal function in activities of daily living and their requirement for supervision and cueing. Most often, these cognitive impairments have been defined empirically using performance on neuropsychological tasks, but a theoretical model could suggest which cog- nitive processes are involved and define ways to measure the deficits, thus offering significant advantages.

Baddeley and Hitch's working memory model [6-8], may help to explain the functional difficulties experienced by individuals with frontal lobe injury. They define work- ing memory as a process for holding information on-line during the performance of other cognitive functions such as language comprehension or problem-solving. This model has three major components: two slave systems and a central executive system (CES). Separate slave sys- tems are responsible for temporarily storing verbal and non-verbal information, and the CES regulates the dis- tribution of limited attentional resources. The CES, anal- ogous to the Supervisory Attentional System (SAS) [7, 9] also coordinates information processing and controls cognitive functioning when novel tasks are performed or

1341

1342 S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI

when existing habits have to be overridden. Complex tasks tax the CES, which must allocate its attentional capacity such that working memory can hold onto mul- tiple pieces of information at once. Since the CES is thought to have limited capacity, simultaneous per- formance of two tasks which use the same processing systems places increased demands on this system.

There is extensive evidence from both primate and human studies that working memory is subserved by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (PFC). Single-unit rec- ording and lesion studies performed on primates [10 12] have identified the PFC as critical for maintaining representations over time. Many investigations of the anatomy, metabolism and electrophysiology support the role of the PFC in spatial working memory [13 16]. Working memory in neurologically intact human subjects has been investigated using functional neuroimaging studies. A large number of studies show that memory slave systems for verbal and visuospatial information activate the PFC [17 19]. Furthermore, active manipu- lation of verbal and non-verbal information held in work- ing memory during a dual-task paradigm, a function of the CES, has also been shown to activate the PFC [20].

Evidence that the CES specifically is impaired after frontal damage is indirect since there has been only a single study of dual-task paradigms in such patients with focal frontal lesions. However, studies using other para- digms are suggestive of such an impairment. In one study, Shallice used the Tower of London task thought to tap SAS function [21]. Other executive problems found after frontal damage may be explained by a CES deficit. For example, CES impairment would not be evident during performance of well-learned or automatic tasks, but would make switching to a different set of responses difficult which could result in 'stuck-in-set' type of per- severations [22]. Several studies show that patients with frontal lesions do exhibit this behavior [2, 23, 24]. With complex or novel tasks, performance is more dependent on the overextended CES since associations between stimulus and response are weak and uncertain. This absence of strong associations and memory for ultimate task goals increases susceptibility to capture errors and inappropriate control of behavior by irrelevant aspects of the environment or tasks which elicit stronger, more automatic responses. Similarly, impaired concentration on tasks [25-28] and utilization behavior [29, 30] found in these patients may reflect this CES impairment. In this study, we use a dual-task paradigm to directly test the hypothesis that the CES is impaired after frontal damage.

As we have reviewed, performance on many apparently different tasks is reported to be impaired in individuals with frontal pathology (see Table 1). There are two approaches to accounting for the varied manifestations of frontal dysfunction: differentiated versus unified accounts. The differentiated accounts theorize the exis- tence of many different subfunctions of the PFC which affect performance on many different tasks. This approach, therefore, defines the cognitive impairments

Table 1. Tasks impaired after fromtal lobe injury

Task Reference

Context memory Conditional associative learning Go-no-go learning Action sequencing

Planning Recency judgment Cognitive estimation

Stroop interference test Verbal fluency Card-sorting

Schacter, 1987[64] Petrides, 1985[65]

Drewe, 1975166] Kolb & Milner, 1981167] Luria, 1965[68] Shallice, 1982121] Milner et al. 1991169] Shallice & Evans, 1978170] Smith & Milner, 1984171] Perret, 1974138] Milner, 1982172] Drewe, 1974157] Milner, 1963154], Milner, 196412]

with reference to performance on specific neuro- psychological tests and has used extensive testing of patients with frontal lobe damage to identify numerous behavioral functions of the frontal cortex. Use of this approach has also been successful in identifying and localizing many functional domains of the frontal lobe. There is now some evidence of anatomic specificity within the prefrontal cortex and this predicts dissociability of different executive deficits in patients [31].

Unified theories of executive function, on the other hand, propose that the heterogeneous tasks all require use of a similar process subserved by the PFC. Thus, a single impairment can cause performance problems on disparate tasks, not by affecting the individual cognitive operations, but by controlling their integration. For example, Kimberg and Farah [32] offered some con- firmation that a unified theory may be applicable to pat- ients with prefrontal damage; a single disruption in their computational model led to impairments on a variety of apparently different tasks analogous to the impairments seen in patients with frontal lobe injury. Baddeley, as well as other researchers [33, 9] have also developed a unified account for prefrontal function by proposing an execu- tive or controller which spans various cognitive domains and asserts a controlling influence over many cognitive networks. Impairment of this executive could, therefore, explain the failure in completion of complex tasks with success at the individual task components that is typically seen in patients with frontal damage. This account requires that either no dissociability of the disparate per- formance deficits be found or that dissociations be attri- buted to varying damage to the different modules being supervised.

One population of patients with frontal lobe damage is those individuals with TBI. TBI frequently causes local contusions and other lesions of prefrontal cortical areas [34, 35]. The diffuse axonal injury seen in this patient population disrupts dopaminergic inputs to the PFC [36]. It also disrupts neural connections between PFC and

S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI 1343

other areas o f the brain such as the posterior parietal cortex and subcortical regions which are activated during working memory tasks. Important ly , these individuals frequently also have executive function deficits. We chose, therefore, to use subjects with TBI to study the effects o f frontal lobe injury on working memory and its relationship to performance on other measures o f execu- tive function.

In the present study, we used Baddeley's model of working memory as a f ramework to explore the nature of the executive function deficits in individuals with TBI. This study examined the cognitive performance of indi- viduals who have had TBI on tests of the two slave systems, a direct measure of CES, and other commonly used clinical measures o f executive function. Our hypoth- esis was two-fold. First, we expected that TBI would result in working memory deficits secondary to an under- lying CES impairment. Secondly, we hypothesized that the CES impairment might account for their executive function deficits. Performance on tests o f CES capacity would therefore be related to performance on more stan- dard clinical measures of executive function.

Methods

Subjects

Experimental design and tasks

Each subject was given seven neuropsychological tests in each of two sessions. The first session was used only to familiarize the subject with the tasks and the data were discarded. This was done to make sure that the subject understood the tasks and to reduce the effects of practice during the actual testing session. Experimental sessions were brief ( < 60 min) in order to reduce mental and physical fatigue. Below are the actual test measures that were used. Subjects completed the tasks in the order described.

Measures

Neuropsychological testing, except for the Trail-mak- ing test and FAS test, was performed on an Apple Mac- intosh Powerbook, using an active matrix color moni tor set to 256 colors. The software for these tasks was custom written for this experiment using Think C 5.0.4. Measures were chosen to directly test verbal (digit span) and vis- uospatial (spatial delayed response task) slave systems of working memory and the CES (dual-task paradigm). In addition, four more commonly used measures of execu- tive function were chosen (Stroop Interference Test, Trail-making Test, Wisconsin Card Sort Test, and Con- trolled Oral Word Association Test). Each of these is described in detail below.

Two groups of subjects were tested in this study: patients with TB1 and controls. TBI was defined as concussion with loss of consciousness (Glasgow Coma Scale< 8) for more than 6 hours. All patients went through a medical hospitalization and inpatient rehabilitation. Testing was performed at least 6 weeks post-injury, and as long as 10 years. Only right-handed subjects with at least a high school education and English as a primary language were included. Subjects were excluded for penetrating head injury, dementia (Mini-Mental Status Exam score < 26), oculomotor, uncorrected acuity or visual field deficits as noted on medical records or by brief bedside exam, or a major mental illness. Subjects were also excluded for uncontrolled high blood pressure, pregnancy, and current use of centrally acting medi- cations since these patients were chosen for later involvement in a drug study. Control subjects were age-matched to the brain injury patients with the same exclusion criteria as were used for patients with the additional requirement that they had never suffered loss of consciousness from a traumatic brain injury.

Subjects with TBI were referred primarily from inpatient and outpatient rehabilitation programs at a rehabilitation hospital with a specialized TBI program. Since the patients in this study were drawn from a referral hospital, we did not have access to the acute CT or MRI scans. However, we did have radiology reports for 22 of our 25 TBI patients for scans performed from 1 day to 8 months post-injury. In these reports, frontal injury was noted in 18/22 subjects (6 left hemisphere injury, 5 right, 6 bilateral, 1 unknown). Two patients for which we did not have reports suffered facial fractures, one including a orbital fracture and the other requiring jaw reconstruction, suggesting frontal lobe injury may have occurred. Subjects were paid $10 per hour for their participation in the study. Not all subjects completed all tests, due to time constraints and the desire of one patient to discontinue the testing for social reasons. In addition, one of the TBI subjects was unable to perform the dual task with digit span at all.

Dual-task Paradigm

We chose simple reaction time to a visual target as our pr imary task. While simple reaction time is a relatively undemanding task, there is evidence that it reflects the attentional capacity available to a subject [37], hence allowing the concurrent measurement o f performance on more than one task. Subjects were told that a dot would appear on the computer screen at r andom intervals, at r andom positions on the screen. They were instructed to simply press the spacebar with a finger o f their dominan t hand as soon as possible after each dot appeared. Each of sixty-four trials represented the visual target appearance following one o f four possible interval delays after subject response, each used 25% of the time: 0.5sec, 1.0sec, 1.5 sec, and 2.0 sec. The target was a sharply demarcated black dot which appeared in one o f sixteen dot positions in r andom order, with location counterbalanced. The dot remained on the screen until the subject responded. Performance was measured as the mean reaction time across trials.

The reaction time task was performed three times. The first task performance was alone and was considered a baseline condition. For the second administration, the task was performed simultaneously with the subject coun- ting aloud from one to ten repeatedly, at a rate o f the subjects' own choice. This was selected to make minimal extra demands on the CES. However, al though the infor-

1344 S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI

mation load is minimal, combining it with reaction time requires concurrent performance of two activities.

During the third administration, subjects sim- ultaneously performed the reaction time task and an oral digit span task. The difficulty of this task was calibrated across subjects by performing the test using the subject's own digit span, determined to be the largest digit span which the subject was able to perform correctly three times consecutively, without failing three times con- secutively. This task was expected to place much greater demands on the CES. For each administration, it was stressed to the subjects that they should try to perform the primary reaction time task as quickly as possible.

[40]which is the most commonly used method today. In this method the examiner points out errors as they occur so that the patient can always complete the test. In this method, scoring is based on time alone. This method penalizes for errors indirectly through increased time requirement, and any bias based on speed of the examiner in pointing out errors or speed of the patient in com- prehending and correcting these errors is eliminated by having all subjects tested by the same examiner and using the difference score for this study.

Controlled Oral Word Association

Digit span task

In this task, subjects were asked to immediately repeat back random sequences of numbers of different spans. The test began with two-digit numbers and progressed one digit at a time. Subjects were advanced by one digit after three consecutively correct responses. The test was stopped after three consecutive incorrect responses, and the subject's digit span was then specified as the previous level.

Stroop Color Interference Test

Two conditions of the Stroop task [38] were used: the color-naming control condition, in which subjects had to name the colors of rectangles, and the conflict condition, in which subjects had to name the 'ink colors' in which different color names were printed. The color names and the ink colors were always in conflict. The Stroop stimuli were presented in five columns of sixteen against a black background, using the default Macintosh red, blue, green, and yellow colors. Subjects were asked to go as quickly as possible without making errors. The examiner timed the subjects, using a programmed timer in the computer which was activated with display of the task and stopped by pressing a computer key. Performance was defined as the time required for each subject to com- plete each form of the Stroop test. The numbers of errors was also recorded for each subject.

In this test of verbal associative fluency, [41, 42], the examiner asks subjects to say as many words as they can think of, excluding proper nouns, that begin with a given letter of the alphabet. The score is the sum of all accept- able words produced in three one-minute trials, using the letters F, A, and S.

Spatial Delayed-response Task ( Visual Slave System Test)

Subjects were administered a dot location task similar to that developed by Funahashi, Bruce, and Goldman- Rakic [10], in which the subject must recall the location of a black dot stimulus after a brief delay. The subject was seated in front of a computer monitor and asked to observe a central fixation point. The visual stimulus appeared for 0.2 sec in the periphery at any location on the screen within 10 deg of the fixation point (to avoid the subject's blind spot) excluding locations of 0, 90, 180, and 270deg (to avoid referencing to the exact vertical and horizontal). After the stimulus, the screen was blank for 8 sec and then an auditory tone signaled the subject to touch a point on the screen where the stimulus appeared and this location was marked by movement of the cursor. This was repeated 40 times. Performance was defined as the mean distance between the stimulus and the response over all the trials.

Card Sorting Test

Trail-making test

The Trail-making Test [39] consists of two parts. In the first part, (Trails A), a subject is timed while drawing a line joining consecutively numbered circles randomly arranged on a page. In the second section of the test, (Trails B), the subject is timed again while connecting letters and numbers in an alternating sequential manner (1-a-2-b...13). The rapid alternation between numbers and letters creates some interference which tests executive function. This was scored using Reitan's method

This was an adapted version of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [2, 23] presented on the computer. Subjects were asked to place cards from a stack onto one of four reference cards. The computer screen showed them their card below the reference card, with the word 'right' or 'wrong' as feedback. Subjects were told the computer would be checking to see if their card belonged with the reference card they chose. Subjects continued until they had either achieved six categories or placed 48 cards. The sequence of categories was always the same: color, then shape, then number. The category was shifted after six correct consecutive trials. The cards used were only those

S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI 1345

24 (repeated continuously) that shared no more than one attribute with any given card, following Nelson's variation, so that each card placement could be inter- preted unambiguously. Performance measures were the number of categories achieved, the number of cards placed correctly, the number of cards placed incorrectly, and the number of perseverations (cards placed in the same wrong category as an immediately preceding incor- rect response).

Data analysis

Demographic comparisons between patients and con- trols were made with the Chi Square statistic for gender and the Mann-Whitney U statistic for age and education. Overall comparisons of patients and controls were done with a repeated measure ANOVA for dual-task per- formance and a MANOVA lbr executive test perform- ance. Both statistical tests were performed with ranked data since raw data violated the assumptions of univ- ariate and multivariate homogeneity of variance [43, 44]. We used the Mann Whitney U statistic for post-hoc test- ing of individual performance differences between TBI patients and age-matched control subjects, because the data were not normally distributed. Spearman cor- relations were performed to assess the relationships between continuous variables.

Performance

Experimental data were obtained to test working mem- ory function using oral digit span and a delayed spatial response task to evaluate the working memory slave sys- tems and the dual-task paradigm to measure CES func- tion. More standard clinical measures of executive function used were the Stroop Color Interference Test, the Trail-making Test, the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test.

Several measures had more than one condition (Dual- task, Stroop, Trail-making). Performance on these tasks was therefore characterized with respect to two broad dimensions: baseline task performance and decrement on a second condition compared to baseline. Performance on each of these tasks was measured by response time; therefore performance decrement was calculated as the difference between speeds on the secondary and the cor- responding baseline tasks. Finally an exploratory prin- cipal components analysis was performed to assess possible relationships between CES capacity, as mea- sured by performance decrements as above on the dual- task paradigm, and the more standard tests of executive function.

Calculation of PerJormance decrement

Patients and controls had significantly different speeds for baseline tasks in the Trail-making Test, Stroop Test, and Dual-task Paradigm. Therefore, in order to compare performance decrements, it was important to know whether performance decrement was related to baseline speed in normal subjects. In order to evaluate this issue, we examined correlations between the baseline and the decrement among the control subjects in each of these tests. The decrement scores during secondary task per- formance were not significantly correlated with baseline speed (Spearman Correlation rho's -0 .022 to -0 .352, P's.921 to.086). The only relationship that approached significance was dual-task counting performance. In this task, however, the correlation was in the opposite direc- tion from that predicted if slower motor and cognitive processing in simpler tasks leads to greater slowing in more difficult tasks: here, slower reaction time was associ- ated with reduced decrements. Based on these results, a simple difference score was selected as an appropriate measure of performance decrement.

Results

Subject demoqraphics

Gender distribution was comparable in patient and control groups (see Table 2) [Pearson Chi Square (1, N = 50)= 1.049, P = 0.306]. The two groups did not differ significantly with respect to age [Mann Whitney U (25,25)=325.5, P=0.801]. However, since the groups were not perfectly balanced and the sample size was small, the effects of age and gender on performance were assessed. Neither gender nor age had any significant relationship to performance measures in controls. Age and gender, then, are unlikely sources of bias.

Unfortunately, our group of TBI patients and controls were not well matched for education level [medians of 14 vs. 16 years: Mann-Whitney U (25,25)= 465, P = 0.003]. However, individual Spearman Correlation tests revealed no significant correlation between baseline performance

Table 2. Subject demographics

TBI Control subjects subjects Probability

Gender 21 males, 18 males, 0.302 4 females 7 females (Chi Square,

I d.f. = 1.049)

Age median; 32 years 32 years 0.801 range (15-55) (15-57) (Mann-Whitney

U, 25,25 = 325.5)

Education 14 years 16 years 0.003 median: range (9-18) (8-22) (Mann-Whitney

U, 25,25 = 465)

1346 S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI

and education level among controls, and between only one executive function measure (verbal fluency Spearman rho=0.439, P=0.041). Nevertheless, performance differences found between patients and controls were verified on a subsample which was well-matched for edu- cation level (see 'Education as a confound' section)

Group analyses

Baseline speed. Patients and controls had significantly different speeds for baseline tasks in the Stroop Test, Trail-making Test, and Dual-task Paradigm [MANOVA F=24.3 (3,43) P<0.001]. Median reaction time for TBI patients was 390 msec compared to 303 msec for controls (P < 0.001). Similarly, performance on the Stroop Color Control Test and Trail-making A Test was significantly slower in patients than controls (median of 67sec vs. 45 sec, P<0.001 and 48 vs. 25, P<0.001) respectively.

Dual-task measure. Both patients and controls showed consistent performance decrements on visual reaction time tasks during dual-task performance (See Table 3) (Wilcoxon signed ranks tests on control subjects: Z=3.73, 4.29; P<0.001, <0.001 and Wilcoxon signed ranks tests on TBI patients: Z=4.29 , 4.20; P<0.001, <0.001) for dual task with counting and dual task with digit span respectively. Digit span performance was also consistently worse for both subject groups during dual- task performance than when performed alone (Wilcoxon signed rank tests on controls subjects: Z = - 2 . 6 6 8 , P=0.008 and TBI subjects: Z=3.945, P<0.001), dem- onstrating that subjects did attempt to perform both tasks simultaneously, rather than engaging in a speed-accuracy trade-off.

The impact of TBI on CES function was examined

statistically with a repeated-measures ANOVA with simple reaction time, reaction time while counting and reaction time during digit span performance as the within subject factor and TBI patients vs. control subjects as the between subject factor. The effects of subject group [F(1,45) = 30.7, P < 0.001] and test condition [F(2,90) = 260.1, P < 0.001] were significant for this com- parison with a strong interaction effect [F(2,90)=6.4, P=0.002], demonstrating a larger impairment of dual- task performance in patients (see Fig. 1 A).

Baseline as a conjounding variable

Although the performance baselines did not correlate with performance decrements in control subjects, the large baseline differences between groups were still a con- cern as a possible source of confounding for these vari- ables. In order to address this concern, a separate analysis was performed for dual-task performance, by pairing a subsample of TBI patients with control subjects matched to within 6ms for baseline reaction time performance [MWU (12,12)= 72, P = 1.00], rather than for age. Paired subject performance was then assessed using the Mann- Whitney-U Test. Evaluation of the dual-task paradigm in this way revealed that reaction time performance dec- rement while performing dual-tasks remained sig- nificantly higher for TBI patients than controls [MWU (12,12)= 33, 24; P=0.024, 0.01 for dual task with coun- ting and digit span, respectively], see Fig. 1 (B).

Education as a confounding variable

Demographic differences represent another source of potential bias. As discussed earlier, we were unsuccessful

Table 3. Effect of TBI on performance of CES tests

Set of subjects Test

TBI subjects Control subjects

n Mean S.D. n Mean S.D.

All TBI and control subjects

RT-matched subset of subjects

Reaction time alone 24 390 130 25 303 33 Reaction time with 24 532 242 25 341 38

counting Reaction time with digit 23 890 438 24 506 114

span Dual-task reaction time 24 142 133 25 39 41

with counting decr. Dual-task reaction time 23 500 370 24 200 104

with digit span decr. Reaction time alone 12 318 35 12 318 35 Reaction time with 12 437 147 12 352 38

counting Reaction time with digit 12 804 462 11 501 108

span Dual-task reaction time 12 120 119 12 34 48

with counting decr. Dual-task reaction time 12 489 437 11 175 80

with digit span decr.

1000.00

900.00

800.00

700.00

600.00

500.00

400.00

300.00

200.00

1000.00

900.00 - o - TBI patients l

800.00

700.00

600.00

U 500.00

400.00

300.00

200.00

(a) Effect of TBI on dual-task reaction time performance

I I I

T

---O-- TBI patients f l con ol subje/[

R'T RT+Count RT+DigSpan

(b) Effect of TBI on dual-task reaction time performance for RT matched subset

! I I

S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI 1347

R'T RT+Count RT+DigSpan

Fig. 1. In this figure, the mean reaction times for both TBI and control subjects are plotted for three conditions: alone, with simultaneous performance of a digit span task. This dem- onstrates that reaction time decrements for dual task per-

formance are greater in TBl patients.

and education level as covariates, and our result did not change (subject group effect [F(2,42)= 4.567, P = 0.016]).

Working memory slave systems. Evaluation of the two working memory slave systems with individual Mann- Whitney U comparisons revealed a significant dim- inishment of immediate spatial memory after TBI (MWU=216, P=0.013) and a similar trend for verbal memory (MWU =320.5, P=0.058).

t::recutire.function measures. Executive function of the two groups was compared using a MANOVA with each of these measures as dependent variables and subject group as the independent variable. The effect of subject group was significant (P < 0.001) for this comparison with an effect size of 0.632. Post-hoc comparisons of individual tests showed that individuals with TBI had impaired executive function as measured by all tests but the Wis- consin Card Sort (see Table 4).

Two of the executive function tests (Stroop Inter- ference and Trail-making Tests) are similar to the dual- task paradigm, in that they consist of a simple task which is primarily dependent on speed and a more complex task which requires executive function. When these are evaluated and displayed in the manner of the dual-task paradigm, the performance profiles are strikingly similar to the dual-task results as shown in Fig. 2 (A) and (B).

Baseline Junction as a con[bunding variable

Similarly, when new paired data sets were constructed pairing patients and controls for the baselines (Stroop Color Control Times within 3sec (MWU=31.5 , P=0.958) and Trail-making A Times within 2sec (MWU=34 , P=0.833)) the differences remained sig- nificant ( M W U = 12 and 6, P=0.036, 0.006, respec- tively). Other individual tests revealed that verbal fluency was also significantly reduced after TBI.

Echwation as a con[bunding variable

in matching our subject groups for education, which could confound our results. Although education level had only a very low correlation with reaction time in our subject group (Spearman rho = - 0.172, P = 0.41 ), we therefore created a subset of 14 pairs of patients and controls from our original cohorts who had comparable levels of education [MWU (1,28) P=0.89], as well as gender [chi square (1, N = 50) P=0.403] and age [MWU (1,28) P = 0.747] and repeated our tests for groups differ- ences on this subset. Reaction time decrements during the dual-task performance remained significantly higher for TBI patients than for control subjects [MWU (N=29,28) =25, 16; P=0.001, <0.001 for dual task with counting and digit span respectively]. We also re-did the ANOVA above as an ANCOVA with both reaction time

Using the comparable education subset of TBI and control subjects as discussed above and significance between the two groups remained the same for all execu- tive tests in this smaller subset of patients. Verbal fluency, the only executive function test which correlated with education level in our larger subject group, remained significantly worse in patients with TBI than controls. Although education level did not correlate strongly with baseline performance on the Trail-making or Stroop Interference tests (Spearman r h o = - 0 . 1 4 1 and 0.109, P = 0.522 and 0.612, respectively), we also performed an ANCOVA on the executive measures using both edu- cation and the baseline measures as covariates to check for any possible confounding of the results. Once again, the effect of subject group remained highly significant [F(5, 33)=4.226, P=0.004].

1348 S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI

Table 4. Effect of TBI on performance of clinical executive function tests

Test

TB1 Control

n Mean n Mean MWU

Statistic P

Stroop color control time 25 67 Stroop colour interference time 25 106 Stroop color interference decr 25 39 Trailmaking A Time 25 48 Trailmaking B Time 25 113 Trailmaking test decrement 20 66 Wisconsin card sort test categories 20 4.6 Wisconsin card sort test perseverations 20 3.12 FAS test verbal fluency 24 29

24 45 61 .5 <0.001 24 70 83 <0.001 24 26 500 <0.001 23 25 67 <0.001 23 49 30 <0.001 23 23.5 500 <0.001 23 5 212.5 0.65 23 2.1 234 0.901 22 54 2 6 . 5 <0.001

*Items in bold represent executive effects of interest.

Relationship between CES function and other measures of executive function

Since the CES allocates attention between two tasks, performance decrement on the dual-task paradigm should be a measure of this aspect of working memory. A relationship between these decrements and other execu- tive measures would suggest the operation of the central executive in working memory and standard executive function tests. A principal components analysis was per- formed to investigate a possible overlap between these cognitive functions. The raw TBI data for the seven vari- ables (both dual-task reaction time decrements, the Stroop Interference Decrement, the Trail-making Dec- rement, the FAS Verbal Fluency Measure, and the two Wisconsin Card Sort Test Measures) were entered into a principal components analysis with Varimax rotation and the component structure for a two factor solution is shown in Table 5 with the individual component loading for each variable included. The tests loading heavily on component one were those which assessed executive func- tion while those loading on component two were those affecting CES capacity. This two component system accounts for 57% of the variance after rotation, and the eigenvalues for the two components were 2.02 and 1.98, respectively. Principal components analysis using other

rotations and performed with ranked data yielded almost identical results.

In fact, this relationship between performance on the Stroop Test, Trail-making Test and Dual-task Measures does not occur in our control subjects. In controls, unlike TBI patients, the Stroop Interference and Trail-making tests did not load on the factor with the CES tests (see Table 6), with 61% of the variance explained by these two rotated components. Again, the results did not change when the analysis was performed on ranked data or rotated differently.

Discussion

The results of this study show that the CES component of working memory as measured by dual-task perform- ance, is impaired after TBI. The working memory slave system performance was also worsened, suggesting a more widespread working memory impairment in these subjects. The results also lend support to the hypothesis that the other executive deficits in this patient population may be related to the dysfunction of their CES. Specifi- cally, the performance by individuals with TBI on tests sensitive to CES dysfunction was related to that on some commonly used clinical tests of executive function.

Table 5. Principle components analysis for TBI patients

TEST

Raw data Ranked data

Comp 1 Comp 2 Compl Comp 2

Wisc. card sort categories Wisc. card sort perseverations FAS verbal fluency test

Reaction time + counting deer Trailmaking decrement Reaction time + digit span decr Stroop color interference deer

0.902 0.090 -0.830 -0.023 -0.896 -0.108 0.704 0.164 0.518 -0.379 0.610 -0.269

0.061 0.736 0.408 0.707 -0.128 0.667 -0.199 0.657 0.335 0.656 0.367 0.751

-0.057 0.636 -0.256 0.633

Eigenvalues are 2.13,1.88, 1.29, 0.76, 0.61, 0.19, and 0.14 respectively

S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI 1349

(a)

120.00

110.00

100.00

90.00

80.00

70.00

60.00

50.00

40.00

Effect of TBI on Stroop Interference Test decrements

i i

TBI patients

co

Stroop Coior Control Stroop Color Interference

(b) Effect of TBI onTrail-making Test decrement

140.00 ' '

- 0 - - TBI pat ients

120.00 - 0 - c

100.00 ._~ b-

80.00

60.00

40.00

20.00 • . Trail-making A Trail-making B

Fig. 2. In this figure, the mean time required for the performance of Stroop Colour Interference Test for both TB1 and control subjects is plotted for both the Color Control and Color Inter- ference conditions. This demonstrates that reaction time dec- rements for dual task performance are greater in TBI patients. (B) In this figure, the mean time required for the performance of the Trail-making A and Trail-making B conditions. This demonstrates that performance time decrements for Trail-mak-

ing performance is greater in TBI patients.

In this study, both individuals with TBI and demo- graphically comparable control subjects exhibited slowed reaction times during dual-task performance which mea- sures CES capacity. The greater performance decrement in TBI patients during dual tasks, however, suggests a CES function impairment related to TBI. This result remained true even when baseline performances were matched, thus eliminating lower arousal level and slower response speed as explanations for this worsened per- formance profile. Although relatively little work has been done evaluating working memory, and specifically CES capacity, in individuals with TBI, the presence of CES capacity deficits is consistent with a similar study by Hartman, Pickering, and Wilson [45]. Since, in addition, the subjective complaint of inability to do two things at once correlates highly with the inability to return to work [4@ dual-task performance may be a very sensitive mea- sure of 'real-world' function for this patient population. In facl, since most activities of daily living are performed with other tasks or planning of tasks concurrently, this appears to be a useful direction for further study.

Perlbrmance of TBI patients on the more standard clinical executive function tests was also examined in this study. Difficulty with the Trail-making and FAS test after TBI confirmed previous clinical studies [45, 47-51]. A performance decrement on the Stroop Color Interference Test for TBI patients was also found in this study. The evidence in the literature for a stroop effect in TBI pat- ients is mixed. Careful review of these studies reveals many potential causes for these conflicting results includ- ing sampling differences: in level of injury severity [52], length of recovery time [52], etiology of injury, location of injury [53], and method of stroop effect measurement. Finally, the lack of impairment on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance appears surprising, since numerous studies have verified poorer performance in patients with focal frontal lobe lesions [2, 54 56, 47, 57]. However, other studies have found that some patients with clear frontal pathology have no difficulty with the test [4, 58]. Still other studies which specifically tested TBI patients have not shown this impairment [59, 60, 61] and even among TBI patients who had anosmia, and

Table 6. Principle components for control subjects

TEST

Raw DATA

Comp 1 Comp 2

Ranked DATA

Compl Comp2

Stroop interference test decr Trailmaking test decrement Wisc. card sort test categories Wisc. Card sort test perseverations FAS Verbal Fluency Test

Reaction time + Digit span decr Reaction time+counting decr

0.844 -0.094 0.785 -0.066 0.779 I). 137 0.841 0.145

-0.745 -0.144 -0.656 -0.107 0.671 -0.054 0.478 0.074

-0.424 I).438 -0.590 0.275

0.276 0.886 -0.149 0.919 -0.036 0.827 0.260 0.891

Eigenvalues are 2.6, 1.69, 1.01, 0.72, 0.48, 0.34 and 0.14, respectively.

1350 S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI

were thus likely to have focal frontal lobe damage, only 8 of 20 individuals had abnormal performance [62]. A further study of TBI patients showed no significant per- formance difference between individuals with or without frontal lobe brain damage [63].

The principal components analysis revealed some link- age between performance on the dual-task paradigm and two measures of executive function. This result provides some insight for distinguishing between differentiated and unified accounts of prefrontal function. Close exam- ination of the Trail-making Test and the Stroop Inter- ference Test, the tests which shared a common factor with the CES tests reveals some striking similarities among the tests. Both of these tests consist of a baseline level task which measures arousal level, task attention, and pro- cessing speed and a more difficult task level which mea- sures functions analogous to those defined as requiring CES function. The Trail-making B Test requires main- tenance and manipulation of two sets of data, numbers and letters; effectively functioning as a dual task. The Stroop Color Interference Test, by requiring the subject to suppress a practiced, automatic response pattern and apply a new response pattern, conforms again to Bad- deley's definition of the CES as "controlling cognitive function when novel tasks are performed or when existing habits have to be overridden" [1]. TBI subject per- formance on these tests was strikingly similar to per- formance on the dual-task paradigm. Interestingly, the relationship between performance on the dual-task para- digm and these two tests occurred only in subjects with TBI.

The previously mentioned study of dual-task per- formance after TBI also suggested a relationship with executive task function, but for both patients and con- trols [45]. This study used correlation coefficients, however, and was performed with raw Stroop Conflict and Trail-making B results, rather than the performance decrements for each of these tests. These correlations, therefore, could be explained simply by injury severity producing slower performance. In fact, we did not find significant correlations between performance decrements and dual-task performance (Rho = 0.01~).39, P = 0.823- 0.066), although correlations did increase when run on raw data from the Stroop and Trail-making tests anal- ogous to the method used by Hartman et al. (Rho = 0.562, 0.510, respectively).

The relationship between CES capacity and only two of the executive function measures in our study suggests that the prefrontal cortex supports different types of executive processes, and that CES impairment may only be related to one type. Good performance on the Stroop and Trail-making Tests may require good CES capacity to be able to hold information on-line in order to manipu- late or act on it. Since the verbal fluency appears to be a very different type of ability, one of generating rather than holding and shifting information, it is not surprising that it is not related to dual-task performance. The lack of relationship with Wisconsin Card Sorting Test is more

surprising since this test does, in fact, require the ability to hold information on-line while shifting set. However, our TBI patients did not have any significant impairment in performance of this test. This hypothesis, that some but not all executive functions are related to CES capacity, lends itself to sub specialization of PFC func- tion, a concept that is gaining support from anatomical data [31]. A difficulty with this explanation that tasks with related performance share an executive process is that we would expect to find this relationship in control subjects as well as TBI patients. We did not find the relationship, though, possibly because the performance variance in CES functions among controls was small in contrast to variance in content-specific performance which obscured the relationship.

An alternative explanation of our results is that these tasks that were related indeed tap cognitively distinct operations, but they are all damaged together because they are supported by anatomic structures which lie in close proximity. This explanation would account for finding a relationship in TBI patients but not in control subjects.

In addition, there are two other possible explanations for these findings which are not theoretically driven. One possibility is that the relationship between measures seen is simply related to injury severity in this diffusely injured population, so that individuals with more significant executive function deficits also exhibit more significant CES impairment. However, severity is not sufficient to explain the relationship between dual-task performance and the Stroop and Trails tests, since performance on the FAS test of verbal fluency was more strongly affected by traumatic brain injury [Mann-Whitney U (24,22)= 501.5] than any other test, but was unrelated to per- formance on dual tasks. These relationships do not necessarily support the idea that different brain regions support different executive functions, since one region with different disruption thresholds for these tasks (verbal fluency being the lowest) could also account for this data.

The other possible confounding factor which may explain our findings is that all three related tasks are time- pressured tasks; since TBI patients are well known to have slower processing speeds, these tests might be related simply because of their slowed processing. Verbal fluency did not appear to be a time-pressured task in our subjects. The clinical intuition from testing our TBI subjects sug- gested that they generally did not produce words slowly and steadily, but rather that they produced a few words, and then stopped or perseverated on words already pro- duced. Statistical tests confirm this notion: (Spearman correlations between fluency and reaction time rho's of 0.07 and - 0.286, P = 0.756 and 0.186 for control subjects and TBI patients, respectively). Thus, any intrinsic relationship between working memory and executive functions might be overshadowed by a global slowing in processing. This last explanation could be tested by evaluating the relationship between executive function

S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI 1351

tests and a dual-task paradigm which measures per- formance quality rather than speed.

In conclusion, impairment on the dual-task paradigm after traumatic brain injury reflects a specific cognitive deficit within working memory, an impaired central executive system. This impaired performance was related to difficulties with two of the more standard measures of executive function, Stroop and Trails, after TBI. Although the cause of this relationship is ambiguous from our data, CES dysfunction may account for some of the impaired performance on executive tasks that is commonly found in these patients, through shared cog- nitive operations or anatomic localization. With either explanation, dual-task performance can be used as a sen- sitive marker for the higher level cognitive deficits these patients experience. In addition, although these impair- ments are generally described empirically, working mem- ory theory may provide a framework which would enable us to better define and measure these executive deficits, and may aid in the ability to find and test treatments for the associated disabilities experienced after frontal lobe injury.

Acknowledgements--The first author wishes to thank Dr. Susan Kohn for helping to focus her research goals and establish a collaborative relationship with Dr Mark D'Esposito.This pro- ject was supported in part by grants # 5T32HD07425 (NCMRR), NS01762 (NINDS), McDonnell-Pew Program in Cognitive Neuroscience, and by funds from the Moss Rehabili- tation Research Institute.

References

1. Baddeley, A., Working Memory. Oxford University Press, New York, 1986.

2. Milner, B., Some effects of frontal lobectomy in man. In The Frontal Granular Cortex and Behavior, ed. J. Warren and K. Akert. McGraw-Hill, New York, 1964, pp. 313-334.

3. Stuss, D. T. and Benson, D. F., The Frontal Lobes. Raven Press, New York, 1986.

4. Eslinger, P. J. and Damasio, A. R., Severe dis- turbance of higher cognition following bilateral fron- tal lobe ablation, patient EVR. Neurology, 1985, 35, 1731-1741.

5. Burgess, P. W. and Shallice, T., Response suppression, initiation and strategy use following frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia, 1996, 34(4), 263-273.

6. Baddeley, A. D. and Hitch, G. J., Working Memory. In Recent Advances in Learning and Motivation, ed. G. Bower, Academic Press, New York, 1978, pp. 47- 90.

7. Baddeley, A. D. and Hitch, G. J., Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 1994, 8(4), 485-493.

8. Baddeley, A., The development of the concept of

working memory, implications and contributions of neuropsychology. In Neuropsychological Impair- ments of Short-term Memory, ed. G. Vallar and T. Shallice. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1990.

9. Shallice, T. and Burgess, T., Higher-order impair- ments and frontal lobe lesions in man. In Frontal Lobe Function and Dysfunction, ed. H. S. Levin, H. M. Eisenberg and A. L. Benton. Oxford University Press, New York, 1991, pp. 125-138.

10. Funahashi, S., Bruce, C. and Goldman-Rakic, P., Mnemonic coding of visual space in the monkey's dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuro- physiology, 1989, 61, 331-349.

11. Goldman-Rakic, P., Chafee, M. and Friedman, H., Allocation of function in distributed circuits. In Brain Mechanisms of Perception and Memory, From Neuron to Behavior, ed. T. Ono et al. Oxford Uni- versity Press, New York, 1993, pp. 445-456.

12. Fuster, J., The Prefrontal Cortex, Anatomy, Physi- ology, and Neuropsychology of the Frontal Lobes, 2nd edn. Raven Press, New York, 1989.

13. Goldman-Rakic, P. and Friedman, H., The circuitry of working memory revealed by anatomy and meta- bolic imaging. In Frontal Lobe Function and Dys- function, ed. H. Levin, H. Eisenberg, and A. Benton. Oxford University Press, New York, 1991, pp. 72- 91.

14. Goldman-Rakic, P., Circuitry of the prefrontal cor- tex and the regulation of behavior by rep- resentational memory, in Handbook of physiology, the nervous system, higher functions of the brain. American Physiological Society, Bethesda, 1987, 00, 373-417.

15. Goldman-Rakic, P. S., Circuitry of primate pre- frontal cortex and regulation of behavior by rep- resentational memory. In Handbook of Physiology, The Nervous System, ed. F. Plum, American Physio- logical Society, Bethesda, MD, 1987, pp. 373-417.

16. Freedman, M. and Oscar-Berman, M., Bilateral frontal lobe disease and selective delayed response deficits in humans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 1986, 100, 337-342.

17. McCarthy, G., Functional neuroimaging of memory. The Neuroscientist, 1995, 1, 155-163.

18. Petrides, M. et al., Functional activation of the human frontal cortex during the performance of ver- bal working memory tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 1993, 90, 878-882.

19. Petrides, M. et al., Dissociation of human mid-dor- solateral from posterior dorsolateral frontal cortex in memory processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1993, 90, 873-877.

20. D'Esposito, M. et al., The neural basis of the central executive system of working memory. Nature, 1995, 378, 279-281.

21. Shallice, T., Specific impairments of planning. Philo- sophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 1982, 298, 199-209.

22. Sandson, J. and Albert, M. L., Varieties of per- severation. Neuropsychologia, 1984, 22, 715-732.

23. Nelson, H. E., A modified card sorting test sensitive to l¥ontal lobe defects. Cortex, 1976, 12, 313-324.

1352 S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI

24. Cicerone, K. D., Lazar, R. M. and Shapiro, W. R., Effects of frontal lobe lesions on hypothesis sampling during concept formation. Neuropsychologia, 1983, 21,513-524.

25. Knight, R. T., Decreased response to novel stimuli after prefrontal lesions in man. EEG Clinical Neuro- phys., 1984, 52, 571-582.

26. Knight, R. T. et al., The effects of frontal cortical lesions on event-related potentials during auditory selective attention. EEG Clinical Neurophys., 1981, 59, 571-582.

27. Salmaso, D. and Denes, G., Role of the frontal lobes on an attention task. A signal detection analysis. Perception and Motor Skills, 1982, 54, 1147-1150.

28. Stuss, D. T. et al., Long-term effects of prefrontal leucotomy. An overview of neuropsychologic residuals. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1981, 3, 13 32.

29. Lhermitte, F., Utilization behavior and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 1983, 106, 237- 255.

30. Shallice, T. et al., The origins of utilization behavior. Brain, 1989, 112, 1587 1598.

31. Owen, A., Evans, A. and Petrides, M., Evidence for a two-stage model of spatial working memory pro- cessing within the lateral frontal cortex. A positron emission tomography study. Cerebral Cortex, 1996, 6, 31-38.

32. Kimberg, D. and Farah, M., A unified account of cognitive impairments following frontal damage, the role of working memory in complex, organized behavior. Journal of Experimental Psychology,Learn- ing, Memory and Cognition, 1993, 122, 411-428.

33. Cohen, D. and Servan-Schreiber, D., Context, cortex, and dopamine, a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review, 1992, 99, 45-77.

34. Holburn, A. H. S., Mechanisms of head injuries. Lancet, 1943, 2, 411-438.

35. Adams, J. H. et al., The contusion index, a quan- titative approach to cerebral contusions in head injury. Neuropathology and Applied Neurobiology, 1980, 6, 319-324.

36. Adams, J. H. et al., Diffuse axonal injury due to non- missile injury in humans. An analysis of 45 cases. Annals of Neurology, 1982, 12, 557-563.

37. Posner, M. I., Chronometric Explorations of Mind. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., 1978.

38. Perret, E., The left frontal lobe of man and sup- pression of habitual responses in verbal categorical behaviour. Neuropsychologia, 1974, 12, 323-330.

39. Army Individual Test Battery, Manual of Directions and Scoring. 1944, Washington, DC, War Depart- ment. Adjutant General's Office.

40. Reitan, R. M., Instructions and procedures for administering the psychological test battery used at the Neuropsychology Laboratoryno date, unpub- lished manuscript, Indianapolis, IN.

41. Spreen, O. and Strauss, E., A Compendium of Neuro- psychological Tests, Administration, Norms, and Commentary. Oxford University Press, New York, 1991.

42. Benton, A. L. and Hamsher, K. D. S., Muttilingual

Aphasia Examination 1976. University of Iowa, Iowa City, 1976.

43. Akritas, M. G., Limitations of the rank transform procedure, a study of repeated measures designs. Journal of the American Statistical Association 86(414), 1991, 457m60.

44. Akritas, M. G. and Arnold, S. F., Fully non- parametric hypotheses for factorial designs I. Mul- tivariate repeated measures design. Journal of the American Statistical Association 89(425), 1994, 336- 343,

45. Hartman, A., Pickering, R. and Wilson, B., Is there a central executive deficit after severe head injury? Clinical Rehabilitation, 1992, 6, 133-140.

46. Van Zomeren, A. H. and Van Den Burg, W., Residual complaints of patients two years after sev- ere head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 1985, 48, 21-28.

47. Stuss, D. et al., Subtle neuropsychological deficits in patients with good recovery after closed head injury. Neurosurgery 1(1), 1985, 41-47.

48. Levin, H. S., Grossman, R. G. and Kelly, P. J., Aphasic disorder in patients with closed head injury. Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 1976, 39, 1062 1070.

49. Sarno, M. T., The nature of verbal impairment after closed head injury. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 1980, 168, 685-692.

50. Spreen, O. and Benton, A.L., Neurosensory Center Comprehensive Examination for Aphasia 1969, Department of Psychology, Univerisity of Victoria, Neuropsychology Laboratory, Department of Psy- chology.

51. Janowsky, J. S. et al., Cognitive impairment fol- lowing frontal lobe damage and its relevance to human amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 1989, 103, 548 560.

52. McLean, A. et al., The behavioral sequelae of head injury. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 1983, 5, 361-376.

53. Vendrell, P. et al., The role of prefrontal regions in the Stroop task. Neuropsychologia 33(3), 1995, 341- 352.

54. Milner, B., Effects of different brain regions on card sorting. Archives of Neurology, 1963, 9, 90-100.

55. Stuss, D. et al., Evidence for the involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex in memory functions, and inter- ference effect. Journal of Comp. Physiology and Psy- chology, 1982, 96, 913--925.

56. Stuss, D. et al., The involvement of the orbitofrontal cerebrum in cognitive tasks. Neuropsychologia, 1983, 21,235-248.

57. Drewe, E. A., The effect of type and area of brain lesion on Wisconsin card sorting test. Cortex 10(2), 1974, 159-170.

58. Heck, E. and Bryer, J., Superior sorting and cat- egorizing ability in a case of bilateral frontal atrophy, an exception to the rule. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 1986, 8, 313-316.

59. Goldstein, F. et al., Neurobehavioral consequences of closed head injury in older adults. Journal of Neur- ology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 1994, 57, 961- 966.

S. McDowell et al./Working memory impairments in TBI 1353

60. Newcombe, F., Rabbitt, A. and Briggs, M., Minor head injury, pathophysiological or iatrogenic seque- lae? Journal of Neuroogy, Neurosurgeo,, and Psychiatry, 1994, 57, 709--716.

61. Gentilini, M. et al., Neuropsychological evaluation of mild head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neuro- surgery, and Po, chiatry, 1985, 48, 137-140.

62. Martzke, J., Swan, C. and Yarney, N., Postraumatic amnesia and orbital frontal damage, neuro- pschological and neuropsychiatric correlates. Neuro- psychology, 1991, 5, 213 225.

63. Anderson, C., Bigler, E. and Blatter, D., Frontal lobe lesions, diffuse damage, and neuropsychological functioning in traumatic brain-injured patients. Jour- nal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 1995, 17(6), 900--908.

64. Schacter, D. L., Memory, amnesia, and frontal lobe function. Psychobiology, 1987, 15, 21 36.

65. Petrides, M., Deficts on conditional associative learn- ing after frontal- and temporal-lobe lesions in man. Neuropsychologia, 1985, 23, 601-614.

66. Drewe, E. E., Go-no go learning after frontal lobe lesions in humans. Cortex, 1975, 11, 8-16.

67. Kolb, B. and Milner, B., Performance of complex arm and facial movements after focal brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 1981, 19, 491-503.

68. Luria, A. R., Two kinds of motor perseveration in massive injury of the frontal lobes. Brain, 1965, 88, 1-10.

69. Milner, B., Corsi, A. and Leonard, G., Frontal lobe contribution to recency judgements. Neuro- psychologia, 1991, 29, 601-618.

70. Shallice, T. and Evans, M. E., The involvement of the frontal lobes in cognitive estimation. Cortex, 1978 14, 294-303.

71. Smith, M. L. and Milner, B., Differential effects of frontal-lobe lesions on cognitive estimation and spatial memory. Neuropsychologia, 1984, 22, 697 705.

72. Milner, B., Petrides, M. and Smith, M. L., Frontal lobes and the temporal organization of memory. Human Neurobiology, 1985, 4, 137 142.