variant conflict management: conceptualizing and

177
Variant Conflict Management: Conceptualizing and Investigating Team Conflict Management as a Configural Construct Yunzi Tan Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy under the Executive Committee of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY 2013

Upload: others

Post on 26-Mar-2022

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

 

 

Variant Conflict Management: Conceptualizing and Investigating Team Conflict Management as a Configural Construct

Yunzi Tan

Submitted in partial fulfillment of the

requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

under the Executive Committee of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences

COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY

2013

 

 

© 2013

Yunzi Tan All Rights Reserved

 

 

ABSTRACT

Variant Conflict Management: Conceptualizing and Investigating Team Conflict Management as a Configural Construct

Yunzi Tan

The key purpose of this dissertation was to empirically test a new conceptual model of

team conflict management (Tan, 2011). Central to this model is a configural team-level construct

called variant conflict management (VCM), which refers to the relative levels of cooperative and

competitive conflict management among members in a team. This model also identifies three key

antecedent categories likely to predict VCM in teams: salient conflict-relevant member

characteristics, team contextual determinants, and divergent team dynamics. Three archetypal

profiles of VCM, i.e., distinct distributional patterns in members’ conflict management

approaches, are also proposed: minimum, moderate and maximum VCM profiles. These three

profiles are further organized into five sub-types: minimum cooperative, minimum competitive,

moderate cooperative, moderate competitive, and maximum VCM profiles.

Specifically, this study sought to assess whether the proposed VCM profiles are present

in 79 student project teams. It also compared the effects of VCM (based on teams’ standard

deviation scores) and of mean team conflict management (based on teams’ means scores) on

three team outcomes: team conflict efficacy, members’ satisfaction with their teams’ conflict

management process, and team effectiveness. This study also investigated the relative effects of

the proposed VCM profiles on each of the three team outcomes. Three indicators representing

each of the antecedent categories, i.e., gender role diversity, team goal interdependence, and

subgroup formation, were also examined as potential predictors of VCM.

Using qualitative content coding analyses, it was revealed that all five VCM profiles

proposed in the model, i.e., minimum cooperative, minimum competitive, moderate cooperative,

 

 

moderate competitive, and maximum VCM profiles, were indeed evident in the teams sampled.

Three additional profiles described as ‘distributed,’ ‘multiple clusters’ and ‘midpoint cluster’

were also uncovered in the content coding analyses. In the supplementary latent class analyses,

four latent classes were identified. Two of these classes corresponded with two of the five

proposed VCM profiles: the moderate cooperative and moderate competitive VCM profiles. The

third latent class was aligned with the new ‘distributed’ profile identified in the content coding

analyses. As for the fourth latent class, it consisted of the other three proposed VCM profiles,

i.e., minimum cooperative, minimum competitive and maximum VCM profiles, as well as the

two additional profiles uncovered in the content coding analyses, i.e., ‘multiple clusters’ and

‘midpoint cluster’ profiles.

Comparisons among the five proposed VCM profiles of their effects on the three team

outcomes showed that teams with minimum cooperative VCM profiles reported higher levels of

team conflict efficacy than teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles, and they were also

more effective than teams with minimum competitive VCM profiles. Teams with minimum

competitive VCM profiles, on the other hand, reported the lowest levels of member satisfaction

compared to teams with the four other proposed VCM profiles; teams with minimum

competitive VCM profiles were also less effective than teams with minimum cooperative and

moderate cooperative VCM profiles. Teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles, relative to

those with moderate competitive VCM profiles reported greater team conflict efficacy and team

effectiveness.

The study results also found no significant effects of VCM (based on teams’ standard

deviation scores) and of mean team conflict management (based on teams’ means scores on

cooperative and competitive conflict management respectively) on team conflict efficacy,

 

 

members’ satisfaction with their teams’ conflict management process, and team effectiveness.

Additionally, no significant associations were found between the three proposed predictor

variables, i.e., gender role diversity, team goal interdependence and subgroup formation, and

VCM.

Implications of these findings for theory, research and practice, along with limitations

and future research directions, were also discussed.

  i  

TABLE OF CONTENTS

PAGE

LIST OF TABLES vii

LIST OF FIGURES ix

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS x

CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION 1

Overview 1

Study Purpose 3

Study Contributions 4

Key Perspectives Underlying the Variant Conflict Management Model

5

The ‘Interactionist’ Perspective: Person versus Situation 5

The ‘Process’ Perspective: Team Dynamics Matter 6

The ‘Levels’ Perspective: “The Whole does not Equal the Sum of its Parts”

8

Dissertation Structure 9

CHAPTER II: LITERATURE REVIEW 10

Overview 10

Team Conflict Management: A Critical Review 10

Common Theories and Typologies in Team Conflict Management Research

11

Antecedents of Team Conflict Management 15

Effects of Team Conflict Management 17

Conceptualizing Variant Conflict Management 19

 

  ii  

TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued)

PAGE

Variant Conflict Management Profiles 22

Minimum VCM 22

Moderate VCM 23

Maximum VCM 23

Effects of Variant Conflict Management 24

Relative Effects of VCM and Mean Cooperative Team Conflict Management

24

Relative Effects of VCM and Mean Competitive Team Conflict Management

27

Relative Effects of Various VCM Profiles 29

Antecedents of Variant Conflict Management 35

Salient Conflict-Relevant Member Characteristics 35

Gender Role Diversity 37

Team Contextual Determinants 39

Goal Interdependence 40

Divergent Team Dynamics 43

Subgroup Formation 44

CHAPTER III: METHODOLOGY 46

Overview 46

Participants 46

Sample 46

   

  iii  

TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued)

PAGE

Design and Procedures 48

Measures 49

Conflict Management Measures 49

VCM Profiles 49

Variant Conflict Management 50

Cooperative and Competitive Team Conflict Management

51

Predictor Measures 51

Gender Role Diversity

51

Team Goal Interdependence 52

Mixed Goal Interdependence 53

Subgroup Formation 53

Outcome Measures 54

Team Conflict Efficacy 54

Member Satisfaction with the Team Conflict Management Process

54

Team Effectiveness 54

Controls and Other Measures 54

Intra-Team Conflict Types 54

Perceived Conflict Importance and Intensity 55

Course Affiliation and Team Size 56

  iv  

TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued)

PAGE

Participant Demographics 56

CHAPTER IV: RESULTS 57

Overview 57

Preliminary Analyses 57

Descriptive Statistics 57

Team-Level Reliability and Data Aggregation Indices 57

Common Method Variance 59

Intercorrelational Analyses 59

Main Analyses

61

Assessing Presence of VCM Profiles in Teams 61

Hypothesis 1: Presence of Proposed VCM Profiles

61

Assessing Relative Effects of VCM and Mean Team Conflict Management on Team Outcomes

63

Hypotheses 2a to 2c: Cooperative Team Conflict Management versus VCM

63

Hypotheses 3a to 3c: Competitive Team Conflict Management versus VCM

67

Assessing Relative Effects of VCM Profiles on Team Outcomes

70

Hypotheses 4a to 4c: Minimum Cooperative VCM versus All Other VCM Profiles

70

Hypotheses 5a to 5c: Minimum Competitive VCM versus All Other VCM Profiles

72

Hypotheses 6a to 6c: Moderate Cooperative VCM versus Moderate Competitive VCM Profiles

73

  v  

TABLE OF CONTENTS

(Continued)

PAGE

Hypotheses 7a to 7c: Maximum VCM versus Moderate Competitive VCM Profiles

74

Assessing Potential Antecedents of VCM 76

Hypothesis 8: Gender Role Diversity and VCM 76

Hypothesis 9: Team Goal Interdependence and VCM

76

Hypothesis 10: Subgroup Formation as A Mediator

77

Supplementary Analyses 78

Assessing VCM Profiles using Latent Class Analyses 78

Exploring Group Differences among VCM Categories on Team Outcomes

81

VCM Categories on Team Conflict Efficacy

82

VCM Categories on Member Satisfaction with Team Conflict Management Process

83

VCM Categories on Team Effectiveness

83

CHAPTER V: DISCUSSION 84

Theoretical Implications 92

Research Implications 94

Practical Implications 95

Study Limitations 96

Future Research Directions 97

Conclusion

98

  vi  

TABLE OF CONTENTS (Continued)

PAGE

REFERENCES 100

APPENDICES 145

Appendix A: Email Requests for Study Recruitment and Participation

145

Appendix B: Study Questionnaire 147

Appendix C: Study Measures 159

  vii  

LIST OF TABLES

TABLE DESCRIPTION PAGE

1 Descriptive Statistics for all Key Team Variables 120

2 Reliability and Aggregation Indices for all Team Measures

121

3 Intercorrelations for all Key Team Variables 122

4 Coding Definitions of VCM Profiles used in the Content Coding Analyses

124

5 Classification of Teams based on the Content Coding Analyses

125

6 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Team Conflict Efficacy from Cooperative Team Conflict Management and VCM

126

7 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Member Satisfaction with Team Conflict Management Process from Cooperative Team Conflict Management and VCM

127

8 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Team Effectiveness from Cooperative Team Conflict Management and VCM

128

9 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Team Conflict Efficacy from Competitive Team Conflict Management and VCM

129

10 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Member Satisfaction with Team Conflict Management Process from Competitive Team Conflict Management and VCM

130

11 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Team Effectiveness from Competitive Team Conflict Management and VCM

131

12 Medians and Number of Cases for the Effects of VCM Profiles on Team Conflict Efficacy

132

13 Means and Standard Deviations for the Effects of VCM Profiles on Member Satisfaction with the Team’s Conflict Management Process and Team Effectiveness

132

14 Simple Regression Analysis Predicting VCM with Gender Role Diversity

133

  viii  

LIST OF TABLES (Continued)

TABLE DESCRIPTION PAGE

15 Polynomial Regression Analysis Predicting VCM with Team Goal Interdependence Variables

134

16 Simple Regression Analysis Predicting Subgroup Formation with Gender Role Diversity

135

17 Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Subgroup Formation as a Mediator between Gender Role Diversity and VCM

135

18 Model Fit Statistics for the Latent Class Analyses 136

19 Classification of VCM Profiles based on Latent Class Memberships

137

20 Medians and Number of Cases for the Effects of VCM Categories (Identified in the Latent Class Analyses) on Team Conflict Efficacy

138

21 Means and Standard Deviations for the Effects of VCM Categories (Identified in the Latent Class Analyses) on Member Satisfaction with the Team’s Conflict Management Process and Team Effectiveness

138

22 Summary Findings for the Main and Supplementary Analyses

139

  ix  

LIST OF FIGURES

FIGURE DESCRIPTION PAGE

1 Conceptual Model of Variant Conflict Management 113

2 Minimum VCM Profiles 114

3 Moderate VCM Profiles 115

4 Maximum VCM Profiles 116

5 VCM Profiles Identified from Content Coding 117

6 Graph showing Estimated Conditional Probabilities for the Four-Class Model Solution

118

7 VCM Profiles Identified from Latent Class Analyses 119

  x  

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

First and foremost, I would like to express my deepest gratitude to my two primary

program advisors and mentors, Dr. Loriann Roberson and Dr. Peter T. Coleman, for their

ongoing advisement and guidance, throughout my years as a Ph.D. student in the Social-

Organizational Psychology Program at Teachers College. Thank you both, for your positive

encouragement and support over the years, and for making my doctoral student experience a

most rewarding and fulfilling one. I would also like to thank the rest of my dissertation

committee, Dr. Madhabi Chatterji, Dr. James Westaby, Dr. Joel Brockner, and Dr. Lyle Yorks,

for their thoughtful questions and insightful comments, and for making my defense a truly

memorable and intellectually stimulating event.

Special thanks go to the sponsor of the Lim Kim San Fellowship for Ph.D. Students, i.e.,

the Lee Kong Chian School of Business at the Singapore Management University, for awarding

me this competitive fellowship, and in turn, for making the data collection and funding of my

dissertation possible. I would also like to express my thanks to my fellowship faculty host, Dr.

Hwee Hoon Tan, the rest of the Organizational Behavior and Human Resources Division faculty,

and the student participants, for making my fellowship visit a productive and engaging

experience.

I am also greatly indebted to my Ph.D. program community, particularly Dr. Marina

Field, Dr. Frank Golom, Mekayla Castro, Nishita Rai, and Apiwat Hanvongse. Thank you all for

your friendship, love and support throughout my Ph.D. journey, and I could never have made it

this far without having all of you along the way. To my closest friends and allies in life, Dr. Cui

Su, Dr. Thiam Hui Lee, Arwa Jumkawala, Lisa Choo, Lynn Jiang, Winston Len, Karen Frank,

Laurie Barrueta, and Dr. June Lee, this dissertation would never have been possible without your

  xi  

tireless encouragement and deep support for me as well. I would also like to thank Hui Hui

Chong and Lily Ng for volunteering to code data for my study.

To my immediate and extended family in Singapore: thank you for your continuing

support in my pursuit of the doctorate over the years, despite being thousands of miles away. I

finally did it – becoming the first Dr. in our family!

To my husband, Sebastian Christoph Wagner, and my son, Leo Rui Hao Wagner: Thank

you both for being the two greatest pillars of strength, joy, inspiration and love in my life!

Completing this Ph.D. journey would also never have been possible without you two by my side.

Ich liebe dich beide!

Last but not least, to my parents, Tan Beng Soon and Ling Lee Keow: I dedicate this

dissertation to both of you. Thank you for all that you have done for me, and for giving me your

best in support of my doctoral goals! (爸,妈:我把这本论文典献给你们。感谢你们多年以来

所为我做的一切,也感谢你们支持我当博士的目标!)

 

1  

CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION

Overview

For decades, organizational scholars and researchers have expended much effort studying

the antecedents, dynamics and consequences of conflict and its management (Lewicki, Weiss, &

Lewin, 1992). Today, we have amassed a considerable body of knowledge about how, when and

why conflict occurs, and at various levels of analysis; a significant portion of this knowledge has

also been dedicated to understanding how conflicts are managed or resolved (De Dreu &

Gelfand, 2008; Rahim, 2001). Despite this substantial attention to conflict management, most of

the research in this area has been directed at the individual and interpersonal levels (e.g.,

Deutsch, 1973; Rahim, 1983; Thomas, 1992), and to some extent, at the organizational level

(e.g., Pondy, 1967). By contrast, relatively little research has been conducted on conflict

management at the team level. This is particularly noteworthy given the increasing use of work

teams in organizations today (Ilgen, 1999). In light of this, researchers have begun to focus on

team-level conflict management in recent years (e.g., Alper, Tjosvold, & Law, 2000; Behfar,

Peterson, Mannix, & Trochim, 2008; De Dreu & Van Vianen, 2001; DeChurch, Hamilton, &

Haas, 2007; Kuhn & Poole, 2000).

However, in spite of this promising trend, current research in team conflict management

remains subject to several limitations. Arguably, one of the most significant limitations concerns

the heavy reliance on existing conflict management theories and typologies (e.g., Blake &

Mouton, 1964; Deutsch, 1949, 1973; Rahim, 1983; Thomas & Kilmann, 1974; Van de Vliert &

Euwema, 1994) that are typically conceptualized at the individual level. More specifically,

existing studies in team conflict management have tended to assume similar or shared conflict

management approaches among individual members in the team (e.g., Alper, et al., 2000;

 

2  

DeChurch & Marks, 2001; Jordan & Troth, 2004; Somech, Desivilya, & Lidogoster, 2008). For

example, a team may be characterized as having a cooperative, competitive, or avoidant conflict

management approach (Chen & Tjosvold, 2002a; De Dreu & Van Vianen, 2001; Tjosvold, Yu &

Wu, 2009). While a few studies in the literature have also looked at teams with two or more

conflict management approaches, such approaches were still assessed at the team rather than the

individual level; in other words, these multiple approaches within the team were still assumed to

be displayed by all individual members (e.g., Behfar, et al., 2008; Kuhn & Poole, 2000; Poole &

Dobosh, 2010).

Overall, this over-reliance on existing individual-level conflict management theories and

typologies inadvertently constrains our current understanding of team conflict management. So

far, we know very little about what causes individual team members to vary, rather than

converge, in their conflict management approaches within a given team. What are the critical

factors shaping variability in team conflict management, and what consequences might such

variability lead to? These are questions yet to be addressed in current team conflict management

research.

Further, some team conflict management scholars have highlighted the need for future

research to improve or refine our theorizing about conflict management at the team level (e.g.,

Behfar, et al., 2008; DeChurch & Marks, 2001). Research in other team-related domains, such as

group dynamics and social influence processes (e.g., Abrams, Wetherell, Cochrane, Hogg, &

Turner, 1990; Asch, 1952, 1956; Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004; Isenberg, 1986; Mackie, 1987;

Nemeth, 1986), and group composition (e.g., Earley & Mosakowski, 2000; Kanter, 1977; Lau &

Murnighan, 1998, 2005; Li & Hambrick, 2005) have also demonstrated evidence of instances in

which individuals may differ from others in their perceptions, attitudes and behaviors within a

 

3  

group context. Accordingly, such findings reinforce the importance of exploring when and how

variability in conflict management may occur in teams, and how such variability in turn affects

critical outcomes for individuals, teams or organizations.

Study purpose

Considering the above discussion, the purpose of this dissertation study was to begin

addressing the aforementioned limitations in existing team conflict management research.

Specifically, this study tested a new conceptual model of team-level conflict management

proposed by Tan (2011). Central to this model is a team-level construct, known as variant

conflict management (VCM), which refers to the distribution of cooperative and competitive

conflict approaches (Deutsch, 1949, 1973) among individual members of a team (Tan, 2011). In

this model, it is argued that conflict management behaviors, approaches or strategies can vary

among individual members when they handle conflicts within a team, and such variability can be

attributed to three key antecedent categories: salient conflict-relevant member characteristics,

team contextual determinants, and divergent team dynamics (Tan, 2011). Once VCM develops in

a team, it is in turn expected to lead to differential effects on important outcomes, such as team

performance and member satisfaction (Tan, 2011). It is also posited that such differential effects

will be a function of how cooperative and competitive conflict approaches are distributed among

team members, assuming that team members exert similar efforts in conflict handling with their

teams. Tan’s (2011) model suggests that these distributional patterns in conflict management

among individual members may be further organized into distinct profiles, that is, minimum,

moderate and maximum VCM profiles (see Figure 1 for this conceptual model).

To empirically test this model, content coding and latent class analyses were conducted to

assess the construct validity of the proposed VCM profiles (i.e., the patterns of variability in

 

4  

individual conflict management) in actual teams. Once the VCM profiles were established,

regression analyses and planned comparisons were carried out to examine the relationships

proposed in the model. In particular, I investigated the relative effects of VCM and mean

cooperative and competitive team conflict management in teams on three specific team outcome

variables: team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management

process, and team effectiveness. This study also examined the relationships between different

VCM profiles and these three team outcomes. Additionally, specific variables representing each

of the three antecedent categories predicting the development of VCM were identified, and the

relationships between these variables and VCM were assessed. Namely, these variables were

gender role diversity (as an indicator of ‘salient member characteristics’), perceived goal

interdependence among members (as a type of ‘team contextual determinant’), and subgroup

formation (as a form of ‘divergent team dynamics’).

Study contributions

This study contributed to the research literature in a number of ways. First, it added to

existing knowledge about team conflict management by assessing the presence of the VCM

profiles as conceptualized in Tan’s (2011) model. By assessing the relative effects of VCM and

mean team conflict management on team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s

conflict management process, and team effectiveness, this study also investigated whether and

the extent to which VCM may predict outcomes above and beyond traditional notions of team

conflict management in the literature. Further, this study helped broaden our understanding about

possible impacts of team conflict management by examining how different VCM profiles may

exert differential effects on critical outcomes for teams. Last but not least, this study identified

and examined specific factors that represent the three antecedent categories proposed in the

 

5  

model, and that are likely to cause variation in individual conflict management approaches

within teams.

In the following paragraphs, three key perspectives underlying the VCM model are

discussed. I refer to these as the ‘interactionist,’ ‘process,’ and ‘levels’ perspectives. It is

important to discuss these three perspectives, as they have been pivotal to the development of the

VCM model. An understanding of how these perspectives relate to the model should also allow

the reader to gain insights into the overall structure of the model, as well as how proposed

relationships among concepts in the model are organized.

Key perspectives underlying the variant conflict management model

The ‘interactionist’ perspective: Person versus Situation

The first perspective, that is, the ‘interactionist’ perspective, concerns the interaction

between person and situational factors in influencing human cognition, attitudes and behavior

(Lewin, 1946; Ross & Nisbett, 1991). In the conflict management literature, this ‘interactionist’

perspective has been dominant in shaping how conflict management scholars and researchers

theorize and conduct empirical inquiry on the topic. Today, most academics and practitioners

would agree that how people manage conflict is dependent on both person or individual factors

as well as situational characteristics. Examples of person-related factors predicting conflict

management include personality traits, such as self-monitoring (Baron, 1989), agreeableness

(Graziano, Jensen-Campbell, & Hair, 1996), extraversion, openness and conscientiousness (e.g.,

Antonioni, 1998; Moberg, 1998). Still, others involve cultural orientations (Ting-Toomey, et al.,

1991) and demographic attributes, such as race (Smith, Harrington, & Neck, 2000) and gender

(e.g., Gross & Guerrero, 2000; Rosenthal & Hautaluoma, 1988). As for situational predictors of

conflict management, these include power or status differences (e.g., Brewer, Mitchell, &

 

6  

Weber, 2002; Holt & DeVore, 2005; Renwick, 1975; Utley, Richardson, & Pilkington, 1989),

goal interdependence (Somech, 2008; Tjosvold, 1998), the opponent’s conflict approaches (e.g.,

Gross & Guerrero, 2000; Park & Antonioni, 2007), and trust (Davidson, McElwee, & Hannan,

2004).

Consistent with prior research on conflict management, the VCM model also draws from

the ‘interactionist’ perspective by suggesting that variability in team conflict management is also

likely to be a function of both person and situational factors. In this model, ‘salient conflict-

relevant member characteristics’ denote the category that captures possible person-related factors

while ‘team contextual determinants’ characterize situational factors within the team context.

Salient conflict-relevant member characteristics refer to the nature of and the extent to which

individual characteristics, such as demographic attributes, personality traits and cultural values,

are obvious or meaningful to members in conflict within the team, whether physically,

psychologically or both. Team contextual determinants, on the other hand, relate to the type and

strength of objective or subjective features in the team context that can affect how team members

think, feel or behave. For the empirical purposes of this study, I identified gender role diversity

as an internal team factor indicating salient conflict-relevant member characteristics, and

perceived goal interdependence as another variable denoting a type of team contextual

determinant. More detailed discussions on these variables, along with their proposed effects on

VCM, are provided in the next chapter.

The ‘process’ perspective: Team dynamics matter

The second key perspective, which I call the ‘process’ perspective, takes into account

aspects related to team dynamics. While it is important to know what person and/or situational

factors are likely to affect conflict management, it may be equally important to understand the

 

7  

underlying mechanisms through which these factors affect conflict management. The conflict

management literature has provided some evidence noting the importance of temporal and

process aspects in influencing conflict management behaviors in individuals and in groups (e.g.,

Baxter, 1982; Behfar, et al., 2008; Farmer & Roth, 1998; Jarboe & Witteman, 1996; Kuhn &

Poole, 2000; Poole & Dobosh, 2010; Rogers, 1987; Witteman, 1991). Other existing research,

especially in the group dynamics and group composition literatures, has also identified specific

processes or mechanisms through which individuals in groups may differ from one another in

terms of perceptions, attitudes and behaviors. In the area of group dynamics, such mechanisms

include majority and minority influence (e.g., Asch, 1952, 1956; Deutsch & Gerard, 1955;

Moscovici, 1976; Nemeth, 1986), group polarization (Isenberg, 1986), groupthink (Janis, 1972),

and group norms (e.g., Bettenhausen & Murnighan, 1985, 1991; Feldman, 1984). As for the

group composition literature, subgroup size and status (e.g., Blau, 1977; Kanter, 1977) and

coalition formation (e.g., Earley & Mosakowski, 2000; Gebhardt & Meyers, 1995; Lau &

Murnighan, 1998, 2005; Li & Hambrick, 2005) exemplify other aspects of the team interaction

process that have demonstrated effects on cognitive, affective or behavioral outcomes.

Following this ‘process’ perspective then, the VCM model considers the role of team

dynamics in predicting VCM by suggesting a third antecedent category, i.e., ‘divergent team

dynamics,’ in addition to salient conflict-relevant member characteristics and team contextual

determinants. Divergent team dynamics refer to aspects of the team interactions or mechanisms

through which individual members are influenced to deviate from others in the team. For this

study, I identified subgroup formation as an indicator of divergent team dynamics. Similar to

gender role diversity and goal interdependence, relevant literature and the hypothesized

 

8  

relationships between subgroup formation and VCM are discussed in greater detail in the next

chapter.

The ‘levels’ perspective: “The whole does not equal the sum of its parts”

The third perspective that underlies the VCM model, i.e., the ‘levels’ perspective,

involves determining the levels of analysis at which model constructs are specified. Given the

complexities often associated with the study of individual and group behavior in organizations,

levels theorists and scholars have reasoned and provided empirical support for the importance of

specifying the levels at which organizational phenomena are conceptualized, analyzed and

measured (e.g., Harrison & Klein, 2007; Klein, Dansereau, & Hall, 1994; Kozlowski & Klein,

2000). In the case of studying conflict management in teams, the levels at which this construct is

construed and measured are sometimes unclear or even unspecified (e.g., Desivilya & Yagil,

2005; Jarboe & Witteman, 1996; Jones & White, 1985). Even in instances where team conflict

management is explicitly construed and measured at the team level, researchers have generally

assumed that the construct of team conflict management possesses shared properties (Kozlowski

& Klein, 2000), and consequently, the aggregation of individual-level data to the team level is

often deemed as appropriate for measuring such constructs (e.g., Alper, et al., 2000; DeChurch &

Marks, 2001; Somech, 2008; for an exception, see Park & Park, 2008).

Contrary to such conventional assumptions and measurement practices, the VCM model

proposes an alternative way of conceptualizing and measuring team conflict management (Tan,

2011). Specifically, it suggests that team conflict management may be construed as a team-level

construct with configural properties (Kozlowski & Klein, 2000). This means that individual

members’ approaches in conflict management are not assumed to converge within the team;

rather, differences in these individual approaches may produce variability in conflict

 

9  

management at the team level, i.e., VCM, which will in turn lead to different effects on outcomes

(Tan, 2011). I expound on the conceptualization of VCM further in the following chapter.

Dissertation structure

The remainder of this dissertation is organized as follows. The next chapter critically

reviews existing research on team conflict management, followed by a discussion of the

conceptualization of VCM. In this chapter, I also present the theoretical and research-based

arguments supporting the research question and hypotheses examined in this study.

Following the literature review chapter, the methodology for the study is discussed. Here,

I provide detailed descriptions of the sampling strategy, research design, study procedures and

measures that were used. Next, I discuss preliminary, main, and supplementary analyses that

were conducted in this study. Finally, I conclude with a list of cited references and a set of

appendices containing materials that were used in the study.

 

10  

CHAPTER II: LITERATURE REVIEW

Overview

This chapter begins with a critical review of existing literature concerning team conflict

management. Next, the configural concept of team conflict management, i.e., variant conflict

management (VCM), is presented and discussed. Then, relative effects of VCM and mean team

conflict management on team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict

management process, and team effectiveness are considered and discussed. Following that, the

predicted effects of VCM profiles on the same three outcomes are also presented. Finally,

relevant literature on three antecedent variables of VCM, i.e., gender role diversity, perceived

goal interdependence, and subgroup formation, are reviewed. The hypothesized relationships

among these antecedent variables and VCM are also discussed at the end of the literature review

associated with each variable.

Team conflict management: A critical review

Team conflict management refers to the strategies, approaches or behaviors used by

individual members of a team to resolve or handle disagreements, tensions or incompatible

activities among one another (DeChurch & Marks, 2001). Despite an extensive and longstanding

research interest in conflict and its management (Lewicki, et al., 1992; Wall Jr. & Callister,

1995), empirical work on team conflict management has only garnered attention over the last

thirty years or so. Nevertheless, in this relatively short span of time, we have gathered a

considerable amount of knowledge about the antecedents, dynamics and effects of conflict

management in teams. In the subsequent paragraphs, existing theories and typologies of conflict

management that were frequently used in present team conflict management research will first be

 

11  

discussed, followed by summary reviews of the antecedents and consequences of team conflict

management that have been studied in the literature.

Common theories and typologies in team conflict management research

The conflict management literature abounds with conceptual frameworks characterizing

how conflict is managed (Rahim, 2001). In the narrower domain of team conflict management,

however, the most commonly used theories of conflict management in empirical research are

Deutsch’s (1949, 1973) theory of cooperation and competition, and Rahim and Bonoma’s (1979)

two-dimensional model of interpersonal conflict handling styles (for the former theory, e.g.,

Alper, et al., 2000; Chen, Liu & Tjosvold, 2005; Hempel, Zhang, & Tjosvold, 2008; Tjosvold,

Law, & Sun, 2006; for the latter theory, e.g., Desivilya & Yagil, 2005; Jordan & Troth, 2004;

Somech, et al., 2008).

Deutsch’s (1949, 1973) theory of cooperation and competition exemplified one of the

early one-dimensional models in conflict management. Specifically, he dichotomizes conflict

management into two mutually exclusive modes: cooperation and competition. When individuals

use a cooperative process during conflict management, they may view the conflict situation as a

mutual problem to be resolved, perceive similar interests and concerns with the other, adopt a

more trusting and friendly attitude toward the other, as well as communicate more and seek to

understand the other’s perspectives and needs (Deutsch, 2006). On the other hand, when

individuals use a competitive approach toward the conflict, they tend to view the conflict as a

zero-sum or win-lose situation, perceive less similarities in interests and concerns with the other,

adopt a less trusting and friendly attitude, as well as communicate less with the other party

(Deutsch, 2006).

 

12  

By contrast, Rahim and Bonoma’s (1979) model of conflict handling styles built upon

earlier conflict management models developed by Blake and Mouton (1964) and Thomas and

Kilmann (1974). This model differentiates conflict management styles in terms of two

dimensions: concern for self (the extent to which one attempts to satisfy one’s concerns) and

concern for others (the extent to which one attempts to satisfy others’ concerns). The modes of

interpersonal conflict handling styles proposed by Rahim and Bonoma (1979) include integrating

(high self- and other-concerns), obliging (low self- but high other-concerns), avoiding (low self-

and other-concerns), compromising (intermediate self- and other-concerns) and dominating (high

self- but low other-concerns). Furthermore, it has been noted that these five conflict styles could

also be organized along two other types of dimensions: integrative and distributive (Rahim,

2001). The integrative dimension (integrating – avoiding) refers to the degree to which both

one’s and others’ concerns are satisfied, while the distributive dimension (dominating – obliging)

refers to the ratio of satisfaction obtained for either one’s or others’ concerns (Rahim, 2001).

Another frequently applied model of conflict management in many team conflict

management studies is Van de Vliert and Euwema’s (1994) meta-taxonomy of conflict

management (e.g., Chou & Yeh, 2007; DeChurch, et al., 2007; DeChurch & Marks, 2001). Van

de Vliert and Euwema (1994) developed this meta-taxonomy to integrate the multiple conflict

management taxonomies and to offer a more parsimonious framework for assessing conflict

management behaviors. To that end, they identified two dimensions—agreeableness and

activeness—as common factors that interrelate the different typologies of conflict handling (Van

de Vliert & Euwema, 1994). In other words, the integration of the conflict taxonomies meant that

previously described conflict styles (e.g., cooperative, competitive, integrative, avoiding,

dominating, obliging and compromising) are interrelated in terms of their positive or negative

 

13  

relations with agreeableness and activeness. Van de Vliert & Euwema (1994) defined

agreeableness as “the extent to which conflict [management] behaviors make a pleasant and

relaxed rather than unpleasant and strainful impression” and activeness as “the extent to which

conflict [management] behaviors make a responsive and direct rather than inert and indirect

impression” (p. 676).

While the aforementioned theories and typologies (viz., Deutsch, 1949, 1973; Rahim &

Bonoma, 1979; Van de Vliert & Euwema, 1994) have been applied in most of team conflict

management research, still, other studies have chosen a different approach to conceptualize and

measure the construct. Instead of relying on a specific theory or framework, these studies used a

combination of conflict management theories and typologies in their conception and

measurement of team conflict management (e.g., Boros, et al., 2010; Farmer & Roth, 1998;

Jarboe & Witteman, 1996; Jones & White, 1985; Kuhn & Poole, 2000). Boros and associates

(2010), for instance, conceptualized team conflict management based on a combination of Rahim

and Bonoma’s (1979) model and a fourth dimension—third party intervention—identified by De

Dreu and Van Vianen (2001), but measured the same construct using a mix of scale items

developed by Rahim (1983), Janssen, Van de Vliert, and Veenstra (1999), and Putnam and

Wilson (1982). Jarboe and Witteman (1996), in a different study, construed team conflict

management as involving three modes: integrative, distributive, avoidance communication; these

modes were based on a number of conflict management typologies developed by Kilmann and

Thomas (1977), Putnam and Wilson (1982), and Rahim (1983).

Undoubtedly, the various existing conflict management theories in the literature have

aided greatly to our understanding of conflict management in teams. Yet, almost all current team

conflict management studies that rely on one or more existing conflict management theories do

 

14  

not take such within-team differences in conflict management approaches into account. Since

existing conflict management theories are largely conceptualized at the individual level, most

team conflict management researchers have, in turn, assumed that individual conflict

management styles will likely converge at the team level. As a result, there is little knowledge

about when and how differences in individual conflict management contribute, if at all, to

outcomes such as team effectiveness and satisfaction.

Fortunately, team conflict management researchers have begun to recognize such

limitations associated with the use of individual-level conflict management theory in team-level

research (e.g., Behfar, et al., 2008; DeChurch & Marks, 2001; Tekleab, et al., 2009). In recent

years, a number of them have also begun to use exploratory methods and emergent data, rather

than rely on existing models or theories, to extend our understanding of the antecedents,

dynamics and effects of team conflict management (e.g., Behfar, et al., 2008; Poole & Dobosh,

2010). For instance, Behfar and colleagues (2008) applied a number of exploratory methods,

such as concept mapping, multidimensional scaling and cluster analysis, and uncovered different

conflict management strategies used by 65 MBA student teams. They also found that these

strategies were associated with differing patterns of team performance and member satisfaction

over time. In another example, Poole and Dobosh (2010) studied the emergence of conflict

management patterns in jury deliberations and the effects of such patterns on jury decisions,

using interactional analysis on archival data. In their analyses, they found that different conflict

management approaches and norms developed in each deliberation, which in turn led to different

decision outcomes. Specifically, in one deliberation, conflicts were often held in check through

suppression of explicit disagreements and emphasis on the task at hand, which subsequently

resulted in quick decisions being made. In the other deliberation, disagreements were handled

 

15  

openly and for longer durations; however, most of these disagreements were left unresolved and

eventually led to an impasse in the final decision. Though these studies remain few and far in

between, they nevertheless indicate a need for new conceptions of team conflict management to

be developed in the literature.

Antecedents of team conflict management

Several antecedent factors that are likely to influence how individuals in teams manage

conflict have been identified in the literature. These factors may be further organized into two

general categories: ‘individual differences’ and ‘team contextual characteristics.’ The former

category concerns variables that stem from individual team members’ natural or innate

predispositions, preferences or orientations. Such variables include personality traits (e.g., Jones

& White, 1985; Kleinman, Palmon, & Lee, 2003), emotional intelligence (Jordan & Troth,

2004), self-efficacy (e.g., Desivilya & Eizen, 2005; Leon-Perez, Medina, & Munduate, 2011),

and cultural orientations (e.g., Boros, et al., 2010; Paul, Samarah, Seetharaman, & Mykytyn Jr.,

2004). As for context, variables that belong in this category describe specific characteristics or

aspects associated with the team context. These characteristics include team task type (e.g., Kuhn

& Poole, 2000; Oetzel, 1999; Poole & Dobosh, 2010), team size (Farmer & Roth, 1998), team

conflict types (e.g., DeChurch & Marks, 2001; Desivilya & Yagil, 2005; Tekleab, et al., 2009),

hierarchical positions among team members (Kleinman, et al., 2003), team identification (e.g.,

Desivilya & Eizen, 2005; Somech, et al., 2008), and perceived interdependence among members

(e.g., Desivilya & Eizen, 2005; Somech, 2008; Somech, et al., 2008).

Additionally, and to a limited extent, the literature also reveals another group of

antecedents that may be best described as factors relating to the dynamical or temporal aspects of

a team’s interaction or lifespan. Examples of these factors include team development stages

 

16  

(Baxter, 1982), team norms (Behfar, et al., 2008), team interaction periods (Witteman, 1991),

and time (Farmer & Roth, 1998; Poole & Dobosh, 2010). In spite of the few number of studies

available, this subset of research suggests that aside from individual differences and team

contextual factors, it is also important to examine the interactional patterns and dynamics that

characterize team conflict management over time. This is consistent with the views of group

development theorists (e.g., Bales, 1950; Gersick, 1988; Tuckman, 1965) and some group

conflict researchers (e.g., Behfar, et al., 2008; Greer, Jehn, & Mannix, 2008; Jehn & Mannix,

2001) as well.

Considering the antecedents discussed above, it is clear that a substantial amount of

research has been conducted to help us understand the predictors of team conflict management.

However, despite such research progress, it remains difficult to draw more systematic

conclusions about how these predictors influence team conflict management in general. One

reason for this difficulty may be attributed to the myriad ways in which the construct of team

conflict management has been conceptualized and measured. For example, Desivilya & Eizen

(2005) examined the effects of self-efficacy and team identification on team conflict

management. In their study, team conflict management is construed using Rahim’s (1983) five-

modal typology of conflict styles: integrating, obliging, avoiding, compromising and dominating

conflict styles. By contrast, Boros and colleagues (2010), in their study assessing the impact of

cultural member diversity on team conflict management, they conceptualized team conflict

management based on De Dreu and Van Vianen’s (2001) approach, which suggests only three

types of conflict styles: avoiding, contending and cooperating. Still, other researchers have used

different existing conflict management theories and frameworks to conceptualize team conflict

 

17  

management (e.g., Farmer & Roth, 1998; Hempel, Zhang, & Tjosvold, 2009; Jarboe &

Witteman, 1996; Jones & White, 1985; Kleinman, et al., 2003).

Accordingly, such research points to the need for new directions in the conception of

team conflict management so that more consistent conceptualizations of the construct can be

adopted by researchers, and in turn, more systematic conclusions can be made about the effects

different predictors may exert on conflict management at the team level.

Effects of team conflict management

When it comes to studying the impact of team conflict management on outcomes, a large

number of studies have focused on two types of outcomes: team effectiveness or performance

(e.g., Alper, et al., 2000; Behfar, et al., 2008; Chen & Tjosvold, 2002a; De Dreu & Van Vianen,

2001; DeChurch & Marks, 2001; Jones & White, 1985; Quigley, Tekleab, & Tesluk, 2007;

Tjosvold, Law, & Sun, 2006) and overall member satisfaction (e.g., Behfar, et al., 2008; De Dreu

& Van Vianen, 2001; DeChurch & Marks, 2001; Kleinman, et al., 2003; Quigley, et al., 2007).

Other outcomes of team conflict management include team functioning (i.e., voice, compliance

and helping behavior, De Dreu & Van Vianen, 2001), perceived warmth and productivity in

team atmosphere (Kleinman, et al., 2003), team innovativeness (Tjosvold, Yu, & Wu, 2009),

team cohesion (Tekleab, Quigley, & Tesluk, 2009), team justice (Chen & Tjosvold, 2002a),

organizational innovation (Chen, Liu, & Tjosvold, 2005), and organizational performance (Liu,

Fu, & Liu, 2009).

As discussed earlier, differences in how team conflict management has been construed in

the literature also pose challenges in comparing its effects across studies and gathering more

systematic observations about its benefits and drawbacks on outcomes (e.g., Behfar, et al., 2008;

De Dreu & Van Vianen, 2001; DeChurch & Marks, 2001; Kuhn & Poole, 2000; Quigley, et al.,

 

18  

2007). DeChurch & Marks (2001), for instance, examined the effects of team conflict

management and task conflict on group performance and member satisfaction. In their study,

team conflict management was conceptualized using Van de Vliert and Euwema’s (1994) meta-

taxonomy of conflict management; here, conflict management behaviors were described as

active or agreeable among team members. Their findings noted that active team conflict

management had no effect on team performance, while agreeable team conflict management

exerted a direct positive effect on member satisfaction (above and beyond the effects of task

conflict). Another study by Quigley, Tekleab, & Tesluk (2007), which also looked at the impact

of team conflict management and group conflict on team performance and satisfaction, used a

different approach to conceptualize the construct. In this case, team conflict management was

construed as the degree to which team members engaged in conflict management processes,

based on Cosier and Dalton’s (1990) approach. In their findings, they noted that team conflict

management was positively related to team satisfaction.

At first glance, the findings on the team conflict management-satisfaction relationship in

both studies may appear consistent or similar. However, a closer examination would reveal that

the results from DeChurch and Mark’s (2001) study indicated that it was the quality of team

conflict management (i.e., agreeable or active conflict management behaviors) that accounted for

its effects on member satisfaction, and not just the level of team conflict management present

(i.e., the extent of conflict management processes used), as suggested in Quigley, Tekleab &

Tesluk’s (2007) findings. As such, and in line with the earlier discussion on the antecedents of

team conflict management, this review of existing research on the effects of team conflict

management also highlights the need for further developments in our conception of team conflict

management, so we can understand its effects on critical outcomes more clearly and consistently.

 

19  

Following the above discussion on the need for new conceptions of team conflict

management to be developed in the literature, I discuss one such concept of team conflict

management, i.e., variant conflict management, in the next section.

Conceptualizing variant conflict management

Variant conflict management (VCM) is defined as the degree to and manner in which

conflict management approaches differ among individual members of a team (Tan, 2011).

Inherent in this definition is the emphasis on within-team variability, rather than similarity, in

how individual members manage team conflict. In the subsequent sections, I provide detailed

descriptions of the conceptualization of VCM, followed by a discussion of how VCM may in

turn affect important team outcomes.

In conceptualizing VCM, Tan (2011) draws from relevant theory and research in the

levels literature (Klein, et al., 1994; Kozlowski & Klein, 2000), including recent theory on group

diversity conceptualizations posited by Harrison and Klein (2007). Kozlowski and Klein (2000),

in their book on multilevel theory and research in organizations, argued that group phenomena

could be construed as unit-level constructs possessing global, shared or configural unit properties

(p. 29). Of these three construct types, configural constructs are most applicable to our

discussion of VCM here. Specifically, they explained that configural constructs “capture patterns

of individual perceptions or behavior within a unit” (p. 31). In other words, these constructs

describe the differential contributions of individual cognitions, emotions or behaviors and how

they combine to yield a team-level phenomenon. Team creativity, for example, may be construed

as a configural construct, in terms of the varying number of novel ideas or information individual

members contribute toward the collective creative output. Here, the individual contributions of

creative output are distinct among team members (e.g., some team members tend to produce

 

20  

more creative ideas than others) and depending on the compilation of these individual

contributions, the level of creativity for the team as a whole will in turn differ.

Based on Kozlowski and Klein’s (2000) notion of configural constructs, VCM is thus

conceptualized as a team-level construct with configural unit properties, as it too, is concerned

with varying patterns of individual conflict management approaches in a given team (Tan, 2011).

For instance, a team that is experiencing internal conflict may be characterized by most, if not

all, individual members being highly cooperative or competitive in their conflict management

approaches. Alternatively, it may comprise of two evenly sized subgroups of members that

employ markedly different conflict management approaches: one subgroup being highly

cooperative, and the other being highly competitive. Such differences in individual members’

display of conflict management reflect different patterns or distributions at the team level, and

hence, the emergence of VCM.

To further specify the construct of VCM, Tan (2011) applied a recent theoretical

framework on group diversity typologies, proposed by Harrison and Klein (2007). Harrison and

Klein (2007) defined group diversity as “the distribution of differences among the members of a

unit with respect to a common attribute” (p. 1200). They argued that group diversity may be

conceptualized and specified in terms of three types: separation, variety and disparity. Of these

three, separation-based diversity constructs are most relevant in the theorizing of VCM.

According to Harrison and Klein (2007), separation-based diversity constructs indicate the

distribution of specific attitudes, values or behaviors, in terms of similarity or dissimilarity, along

a single continuum. Akin to the concept of separation-based diversity constructs then, VCM

denotes a separation-based construct of diversity in conflict management (Tan, 2011). It

describes how individual conflict management approaches are distributed to a lesser (more

 

21  

similar) or greater (more dissimilar) extent along a single continuum. Here, Deutsch’s (1949,

1973) theory of cooperation and competition is applied in construing VCM as the distribution of

individual conflict management in terms of cooperative and competitive approaches along a one-

dimensional bi-polar continuum. In other words, members in a team may differ from one another

in their positions along the cooperation-competition continuum. For example, members may be

co-located closely with one another on the far ends of cooperation or competition, or they may

be evenly distributed along the continuum in terms of their cooperative and competitive

approaches to conflict.

There are several reasons supporting the application of Deutsch’s (1949, 1973) theory in

conceptualizing VCM. First, unlike other existing two-dimensional conflict management theories

and multi-modal typologies, Deutsch’s (1949, 1973) theory describes a one-dimensional

framework that is compatible with the notion of separation-based constructs in Harrison and

Klein’s (2007) framework. His theory also allows for greater parsimony in the conceptual

development of VCM given its focus on two fundamental modes of conflict management:

cooperation and competition. These two fundamental modes have also been found in other

existing conflict management typologies, e.g., the integrative and distributive dimensions in

Rahim and Bonoma’s (1979) model of conflict handling styles.

Second, as one of the most commonly used theoretical models in existing team conflict

management research, the application of Deutsch’s (1949, 1973) theory in the conceptualization

of VCM also contributes to our current knowledge of team conflict management by enhancing

consistency and interpretability of possible antecedents of VCM and the potential effects of

VCM on important outcomes.

 

22  

Third, from an empirical standpoint, Deutsch’s (1949, 1973) propositions have also been

tested extensively and received substantial support in the literature (Johnson & Johnson, 2005).

In a recent monograph, Johnson and Johnson (2005) noted that over 750 research studies have

been conducted to examine the relative effects of cooperation and competition on a wide range

of dependent variables (e.g., task achievement, interpersonal relationships, and self-esteem), and

the conditions under which they operate (p.325). They also found that the mean effect sizes for

the impact of cooperation and competition on various dependent variables were relatively strong

(0.57 to 0.93; see p.304). The majority of these studies also involved experimental research with

highly controlled conditions and random assignment procedures, thus suggesting high internal

validity of the findings observed (Johnson & Johnson, 2005).

Taken altogether, the aforementioned reasons suggest that the application of Deutsch’s

(1949, 1973) theory allows for the conceptualization of VCM to be based not just on sound

theory but also on significant empirical research.

Variant conflict management profiles

Variant conflict management (VCM) may be characterized in terms of three profiles:

minimum, maximum and moderate VCM (Tan, 2011). It is important to note that the first two

profiles—minimum and maximum VCM—represent ‘pure’ forms of how differential

distributions of individual conflict management approaches may be compiled in a team. In

reality, however, most work teams are likely to be characterized by the third profile, i.e.,

moderate VCM. More detailed discussions on each of these three profiles are offered in the

following paragraphs.

Minimum VCM. The minimum VCM profile describes a team in which its members are

co-located altogether on a given position along the conflict management continuum (see Figure

 

23  

2, Tan, 2011). This profile can be further organized into two sub-types: minimum cooperative

VCM and minimum competitive VCM. Minimum cooperative VCM exists when all members of

a team are cooperative in handling conflict, whereas minimum competitive VCM occurs when

all members are competitive in their individual conflict management approaches. Of the three

profiles, the minimum VCM profile is analogous to shared or homogeneous team conflict

management (Tan, 2011).

Moderate VCM. A moderate VCM profile describes a team where its members are

distributed unevenly along the conflict management continuum (see Figure 3, Tan, 2011). There

are also two possible sub-types depicting moderate VCM: the first sub-type describes a team

whereby the majority of its members are cooperative. The second sub-type depicts the opposite:

a team that consists of a competitive majority (Tan, 2011).

Maximum VCM. A team that possesses the maximum VCM profile is comprised of

members who are strongly and evenly split toward either end of the conflict management

continuum (see Figure 4, Tan, 2011). In other words, such a team consists of two evenly sized

subgroups: one that is highly cooperative, and the other highly competitive. The maximum VCM

profile also describes what Lau and Murnighan (1998) have proposed as a group ‘faultline’, i.e.,

a “hypothetical line that may split[s] a group into subgroups based on one or more attributes ” (p.

328). A team with the maximum VCM profile is analogous to a team with a strong faultline that

separates its members into two subgroups, based on how they manage conflict (Tan, 2011).

Considering the above discussion of VCM and the typology of its profiles, the first

objective of this dissertation study, then, was to investigate the presence and prevalence of the

proposed VCM profiles in actual teams. To that end, this study examined the following

hypothesis:

 

24  

H1: The five proposed VCM profiles, i.e., minimum cooperative, minimum

competitive, moderate cooperative, moderate competitive, and maximum VCM

profiles, will be present in teams.

Effects of variant conflict management

Earlier in this chapter, I provided arguments to support the case for assessing whether

differences among individual members’ conflict management approaches can be observed within

teams. More importantly, it is also reasoned that such differences in members’ conflict

management strategies within teams may affect team processes and outcomes in ways that may

not be explained by traditional concepts of team conflict management, i.e., similar or shared

conflict management approaches among members within teams. Specifically, it is expected that

the effects of VCM (based on teams’ standard deviation scores), relative to those of mean

cooperative team conflict management (based on teams’ mean cooperative conflict management

scores), will likely lead to lower levels of team outcomes, specifically team conflict efficacy,

members’ satisfaction with their teams’ conflict management process, and overall team

effectiveness (Hypotheses 2a to 2c). It is also posited that the effects of VCM, when compared to

those of mean competitive team conflict management (based on teams’ mean competitive

conflict management scores), will be associated with higher levels of team conflict efficacy,

members’ satisfaction with the team conflict management process, and team effectiveness

(Hypotheses 3a to 3c). I expound on my reasoning for the above hypotheses in the following

paragraphs.

Relative effects of VCM and mean cooperative team conflict management. The first team

outcome of interest, i.e., team conflict efficacy, may be defined as the extent to which members

believe they may successfully handle different conflict situations within the team (Alper, et al.,

 

25  

2000). Alper and colleagues (2000), in a field study with 61 self-managed production teams,

found that teams that were characterized as cooperative in their conflict management approaches,

based on their mean scores in cooperative conflict management, were more likely to report

higher levels of conflict efficacy (Alper, et al., 2000). Such findings suggest that similarly

cooperative members in a given team are able to handle disagreements constructively, address

one another’s concerns and interests, and ultimately create a shared sense of efficacy in dealing

with conflicts among themselves.

By contrast, a team with both cooperative and competitive members, i.e., a team with

VCM, is likely to experience greater difficulties in reaching shared consensus or bridging

opposing viewpoints to resolve the conflict at hand. Prior research examining the cooperative

and competitive dynamics in social interactions has found competition to be a more potent force

than cooperation (e.g., Kelley & Stahelski, 1970). Kelley and Stahelski (1970), for instance,

conducted a series of experiments about interpersonal interactions using the Prisoner’s Dilemma

game. Their findings concluded that cooperative parties are likely to be influenced by their

competitive opponents’ behaviors, and as a result, adopt more competitive strategies themselves.

Consequently, these interpersonal interactions become more competitive in nature, and in turn,

lead to more negative eventual outcomes for both parties.

Considering these findings, it is thus plausible that teams with VCM are also likely to

experience more competitive dynamics, as competitive members in such teams are more inclined

toward exerting their ideas and opinions over others, while cooperative members within the

teams are likely to struggle to find common ground with their competitive peers in order to

develop integrative solutions for the team as a whole. Over time, the interactions between the

cooperative and competitive members in the team will likely become more competitive in nature

 

26  

(Kelley & Stahelski, 1970). Therefore, this should also result in teams with VCM feeling less

confident in their ability to resolve conflicts effectively, i.e., experiencing lower levels of conflict

efficacy, compared to teams whose members share similar cooperative conflict management

approaches (Hypothesis 2a).

The second team outcome, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management

process, is concerned with the degree to which team members feel satisfied with how their teams

handle internal conflicts. Prior research has shown that group members’ preferences for

cooperative conflict management approaches were positively related to higher levels of

satisfaction with their group processes, including conflict management (Park & Park, 2008).

Moreover, this relationship between members’ conflict management approaches and satisfaction

with group processes was stronger when members were more similar in their cooperative

approaches within the group (Park & Park, 2008). Such findings suggest that, relative to

members who share similar cooperative conflict management approaches within the team, teams

whose members differ in their conflict management approaches, i.e., teams with VCM, are

therefore more likely to experience lower levels of satisfaction with their team processes,

including their team’s conflict management process (Hypothesis 2b).

The third team outcome, team effectiveness, is defined in this study as the extent to

which team members perceive themselves as being satisfied with their work, committed to the

goals of their work, and successful in managing their work (Alper, et al., 2000). Evidence from

the team conflict management literature has consistently revealed that cooperative teams were

associated with higher levels of team effectiveness, when compared to competitive teams (e.g.,

Alper et al., 2000; Somech, et al., 2008; Tjosvold, Law & Sun, 2006). In cooperative teams,

members were more likely to perceive less detrimental conflict within the team (Tjosvold, et al.,

 

27  

2006) and to hold higher levels of affective trust with one another (Hempel et al., 2008), thus

leading to greater team effectiveness. When it comes to teams whose members differ in their

conflict handling approaches (i.e., teams with VCM), the differences in which cooperative and

competitive members approach the same conflict within the same teams are likely to be

accompanied by differing perceptions and feelings toward the conflict issues involved.

Cooperative members may be motivated to seek out solutions that satisfy everyone, while

competitive members may be more concerned with addressing their own interests or motives.

Over time, the interplay between cooperative and competitive members will likely become more

competitive in nature (Kelley & Stahelski, 1970), and thus impair the team’s ability to perform

or function effectively. As such, a team with VCM will likely be less effective than a team

whose members share similar approaches in conflict management (Hypothesis 2c).

H2a: VCM (based on teams’ standard deviation scores) will be negatively associated

with team conflict efficacy, with mean cooperative team conflict management

scores being held constant.

H2b: VCM (based on teams’ standard deviation scores) will be negatively associated

with member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, with mean

cooperative team conflict management scores being held constant.

H2c: VCM (based on teams’ standard deviation scores) will be negatively associated

with team effectiveness, with mean cooperative team conflict management scores

being held constant.

Relative effects of VCM and mean competitive team conflict management. When it comes

to competitive teams, researchers have found that such teams, compared with cooperative ones,

were more likely to report lower levels of conflict efficacy (Alper et al., 2000). Competitive

 

28  

teams have also been found to be associated with lower levels of team effectiveness (e.g., Alper

et al., 2000; Somech, et al., 2008; Tjosvold, Law & Sun, 2006). Teams with all or mostly

competitive members approach internal conflicts from a ‘win-lose’ mindset, adopt more

combative stances, and seek to attain their own interests and goals at the expense of others’

(Deutsch, 1949, 1973). Members of competitive teams were also more likely to perceive more

detrimental conflict (Tjosvold et al., 2006) and to hold lower levels of cognitive trust with one

another (Hempel et al., 2008), thus resulting in lower levels of team effectiveness. In competitive

teams, it is possible that at least some members in these teams will feel less confident and able to

deal with the team’s conflicts as they may feel that they have ‘lost’ in the arguments and debates,

with their concerns remaining unmet or voices going unheard. Feelings about their team’s

conflict management process are also more likely to be negative. In these teams then, the overall

sense of conflict efficacy, member satisfaction about the team’s conflict management process,

and team effectiveness are thus likely to be adversely affected.

Teams with a mix of cooperative and competitive members, i.e., teams with VCM, on the

contrary, may benefit from having some cooperative members within them. Cooperative

members in these teams, assuming they are consistent and confident in their approaches, may be

able to elicit some degree of cooperation from their peers within the teams, in the form of

minority influence (Moscovici & Nemeth, 1974). While such minority influence of cooperative

members may be unlikely to overcome more potent and negative impact of their competitive

peers within the same team, it may nonetheless still be able to buffer some of the negative effects

associated with competitive approaches in the team. As such, it is possible that relative to

competitive teams, teams with VCM will experience lower levels of conflict efficacy, member

satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness respectively.

 

29  

H3a: VCM (based on teams’ standard deviation scores) will be negatively associated

with team conflict efficacy, with mean competitive team conflict management

scores being held constant.

H3b: VCM (based on teams’ standard deviation scores) will be negatively associated

with member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, with mean

competitive team conflict management scores being held constant.

H3c: VCM (based on teams’ standard deviation scores) will be negatively associated

with team effectiveness, with mean competitive team conflict management scores

being held constant.

Relative effects of various VCM profiles. Much research has been conducted to

understand the effects of team conflict management on various outcomes and at different levels

of analysis. Findings in the literature indicate that cooperative team conflict management is

generally associated with positive team outcomes, such as increased team efficacy (Alper, et al.,

2000), higher levels of affect-based trust among team members (Hempel, et al., 2008), and

enhanced team performance (e.g., Behfar, et al., 2008; Kuhn & Poole, 2000; Somech, et al.,

2008). Competitive team conflict management, on the other hand, has been linked to more

detrimental effects on outcomes such as lower team efficacy (Alper, et al., 2000), lower levels of

cognition-based trust among members (Hempel, et al., 2008), and reduced team performance

(e.g., Behfar, et al., 2008; Hempel, et al., 2008; Kuhn & Poole, 2000; Somech, et al., 2008).

Given the conceptual similarities between shared cooperative and competitive conflict

management and minimum cooperative and competitive VCM respectively, it is expected that

the minimum cooperative VCM profile is also likely to be associated with positive team

outcomes, whereas the minimum competitive VCM profile will likely be related to negative team

 

30  

outcomes. More specifically, I postulate that relative to all other VCM profiles, a minimum

cooperative VCM profile will likely lead to the highest levels of team conflict efficacy, member

satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness (Hypotheses

4a-4c); by contrast, when compared with all other VCM profiles, a minimum competitive VCM

profile will likely lead to the lowest levels of team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the

team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness (Hypotheses 5a-5c).

Unlike all other profiles with some or mostly competitive members, a minimum

cooperative VCM suggests that the cooperative approaches shared by all members in a team

should reinforce mutual active listening among members, the seeking of one another’s ideas and

concerns, and a shared motivation to identify and uncover important concerns that underlie the

team’s conflict (Deutsch, 1949, 1973). As a result, such open exchange of ideas and perspectives

that is engaged in a supportive climate should lend itself to more integrative solutions to the

conflict at hand (Deutsch, et al., 2006), and in turn, produce more positive team outcomes, e.g.,

higher team conflict efficacy (Alper, et al., 2000), greater member satisfaction with the team’s

conflict management process, and increased team effectiveness (Behfar, et al., 2008):

H4a: Minimum cooperative VCM profiles will be most positively associated with team

conflict efficacy.

H4b: Minimum cooperative VCM profiles will be most positively associated with

member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process.

H4c: Minimum cooperative VCM profiles will be most positively associated with team

effectiveness.

As for minimum competitive VCM, when compared with all other profiles with some or

mostly cooperative members, the competitive approaches shared by all members in the team

 

31  

serve to reinforce a ‘fixed-pie’ or ‘win-lose’ mentality among members, and create a highly

hostile and suspicious climate whereby members may view one another as barriers toward their

individual goals and motives (Deutsch, 1949, 1973). Such competitive dynamics should in turn

lead to reduced information sharing and decreased mutual understanding that could potentially

result in conflict resolution (Deutsch, et al., 2006); furthermore, levels of hostility may be

maintained or even escalated in ways that will hinder the team’s ability to resolve conflict

effectively or produce positive output (Behfar, et al., 2008; Pruitt & Kim, 2004). Accordingly,

this suggests more negative outcomes, such as lower levels of member satisfaction with the

team’s conflict management process, reduced team conflict efficacy (Alper, et al., 2000), and

decreased team effectiveness for a team with a minimum competitive VCM profile:

H5a: Minimum competitive VCM profiles will be most negatively associated with team

conflict efficacy.

H5b: Minimum competitive VCM profiles will be most negatively associated with

member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process.

H5c: Minimum competitive VCM profiles will be most negatively associated with team

effectiveness.

When it comes to moderate VCM profiles, their effects on team performance and

member satisfaction will depend on the specific moderate VCM profile scenario involved. Recall

that moderate VCM profiles may be categorized into two sub-types: one that is characterized by

a cooperative majority, and the other by a competitive majority. These two sub-types

representing moderate VCM are expected to produce different impacts on team performance and

satisfaction. In this study, it is predicted that relative to a moderate VCM with a competitive

majority, a moderate VCM with a cooperative majority will contribute to higher levels of team

 

32  

conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and team

effectiveness (Hypotheses 6a-6c).

In a team where most of its members are cooperative (cooperative majority) and the

remaining few are competitive (competitive minority), it is likely that the cooperative majority

will be able to exert its majority influence given its numerical advantage within the team,

especially early on in the team’s lifecycle (Moscovici, 1976, Nemeth, 1986). Accordingly, this

provides more structural stability for cooperative norms and standards to emerge and guide

member perceptions and behavior within the team (Axelrod, 1984; Bettenhausen & Murnighan,

1991; Deutsch & Gerard, 1955). At the same time, the competitive minority within the team is

likely to assert its position and viewpoints during the team discussion and interactions as well.

Considering the cooperative nature of the majority, it is likely that majority subgroup members

will be inclined to integrate the competitive minority’s concerns and perspectives, as part of their

attempt to achieve integrative and mutually shared outcomes for all. Furthermore, assuming that

the competitive minority is consistent and confident in conveying its position or differing

perspectives, some degree of minority influence is likely to occur (Moscovici & Nemeth, 1974),

and as a result, the integration of differing ideas into the overall team process will likely lead to

higher quality outcomes. With enhanced team output, members are likely to feel more confident

in their ability to manage internal conflicts, and to feel more satisfied with their conflict

management process as well.

On the other hand, a team with a competitive majority (and a cooperative minority) is

likely to experience relatively negative cognitive, affective and performance outcomes. Here, the

competitive majority is also likely to exert significant majority influence, given its larger

numerical size and representation within the team (Moscovici, 1976, Nemeth, 1986). This

 

33  

influence exerted by the competitive majority is likely to create more competitive dynamics

within the team as a whole over time, as cooperative minorities are likely to adapt their

cooperative approaches toward more competitive ones (Kelley & Stahelski, 1970). As

competitive dynamics dominate the nature of member interactions within the team, the overall

levels of team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management

process, and team effectiveness are in turn likely to suffer as a result. Taken altogether, it is thus

reasonable to expect that a moderate VCM with a cooperative majority, when compared with a

moderate VCM with a competitive majority, will likely hold stronger conflict efficacy beliefs,

generate greater levels of satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and work

more effectively:

H6a: Moderate VCM with a cooperative majority, relative to a moderate VCM with a

competitive majority, will be more positively associated with team conflict

efficacy.

H6b: Moderate VCM with a cooperative majority, relative to a moderate VCM with a

competitive majority, will be more positively associated with member satisfaction

with the team’s conflict management process.

H6c: Moderate VCM with a cooperative majority, relative to a moderate VCM with a

competitive majority, will be more positively associated with team effectiveness.

Finally, as for maximum VCM, it is posited that when compared to a moderate VCM

profile consisting of a competitive majority, a maximum VCM profile will contribute to more

positive, rather than negative, effects on team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the

team conflict management process, and overall team effectiveness (Hypotheses 7a-7c). Based on

Lau and Murnighan’s (1998) group faultline theory, a team with a maximum VCM profile may

 

34  

be described as having a strong faultline that separates its members into two even subgroups

based on their conflict management approaches. A team with a moderate competitive VCM

profile, on the other hand, would be described as possessing a relatively weaker faultline given

the uneven subgroup sizes of a competitive majority and a cooperative minority. In an empirical

test of their group faultline model, Lau and Murnighan (2005) found that contrary to their model

predictions and to prior research (e.g., Molleman, 2005; Thatcher, Jehn, & Zanutto, 2003),

groups with strong faultlines experienced less relationship conflict, more psychological safety

and increased satisfaction with their groups, relative to groups with weak faultlines (pp. 653-

654). They also found that cross-subgroup communications improved outcomes for groups with

weak faultlines, but not those with strong faultlines (Lau & Murnighan, 2005).

In a related study, O’Leary and Mortensen (2010), who sampled 62 geographically

dispersed teams and looked at how their different geographical configurations affected team

dynamics, noted that teams with subgroups reported less identification with the whole team,

lower transactive memory, increased conflict, and greater coordination problems, when

compared to teams without subgroups. Additionally, they also observed that these negative

effects were exacerbated for teams with minority subgroups (i.e., subgroups of uneven sizes) as

compared to those with non-minority subgroups (i.e., subgroups of comparable sizes).

Taken altogether, these research findings suggest that compared to teams with moderate

competitive VCM profiles, members in teams with maximum VCM profiles are likely to

experience more positive interactions with others in their own subgroups, and to connect less

with members of the other subgroup within the broader team as well. When dealing with conflict

situations, members in these teams may choose to minimize or avoid overt cross-subgroup

disagreements or contentions, and instead, prefer to handle the conflicts constructively within the

 

35  

psychologically safe boundaries of their own subgroups. Teams with moderate competitive

profiles, by contrast, are likely to experience more detrimental interactions between the

competitive majority and cooperative minority (Kelley & Stahelski, 1970; O’Leary &

Mortensen, 2010), which are in turn likely to contribute to lower levels of team conflict efficacy,

less member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and decreased team

effectiveness.

H7a: Maximum VCM, relative to a moderate VCM with a competitive majority, will be

more positively associated with team conflict efficacy.

H7b: Maximum VCM, relative to a moderate VCM with a competitive majority, will be

more positively associated with member satisfaction with the team’s conflict

management process.

H7c: Maximum VCM, relative to a moderate VCM with a competitive majority, will be

more positively associated with team effectiveness.

Antecedents of variant conflict management

Three antecedent categories are likely to contribute to the development of VCM in teams.

These categories are salient conflict-relevant member characteristics, team contextual

determinants, and divergent team dynamics. Further, in this study, I have identified specific

variables representing each of these three categories: gender role diversity, goal interdependence,

and subgroup formation. In the subsequent sections, I describe how the three antecedent

categories relate to VCM, followed by discussions of the hypothesized relationships between the

specific variables representing these categories and VCM, which were empirically examined in

the study.

Salient conflict-relevant member characteristics

 

36  

As noted earlier, salient conflict-relevant member characteristics refer to the degree in

which individual attributes, e.g., demographic dimensions, personality traits and cultural

orientations, are obvious or meaningful to members within a team during conflict situations.

Theories about similarity-attraction (Byrne, 1971; Newcomb, 1961), social identity (Hogg &

Terry, 2000; Tajfel & Turner, 1986) and social categorization processes (Tajfel, 1978; Turner,

1985) have generally argued that people tend to be attracted to or are more inclined to connect

with others deemed similar to them, and are less likely to relate to or interact with those

perceived as different from them.

Additionally, Swann’s self-verification theory (1983) has also reasoned that individuals

are motivated to seek verification about their thoughts and feelings (i.e., self-views) in a group

setting. As such, aligning oneself with similar others who presumably also hold similar values,

beliefs and worldviews (and at the same time, distancing oneself from perceived different others)

should increase the likelihood of validating one’s own self-views and enhance the sense that

one’s perspectives are heard and understood by others in the group.

Empirically, research in the team diversity and intergroup relations literatures have

provided substantial support for these theories (e.g., Brewer, 1995; Van Knippenberg &

Schippers, 2007; Williams & O'Reilly, 1998). In a team where individual characteristics or social

identity memberships (e.g., ethnicity, age and gender) are salient among individual team

members, members may be more drawn toward others possessing similar attributes or belonging

to the same social identity group. These members begin to interact more with one another,

establish stronger relational bonds, and eventually, develop or share similar perspectives,

attitudes or behaviors as a subgroup.

 

37  

Considering the above reasoning then, in a team conflict situation, members who align

themselves into subgroups based on salient and conflict-relevant individual characteristics, due

to social categorization, attraction-similarity and/or self-verification processes, may also be

likely to adopt relatively similar approaches in handling the team conflict within their subgroups

(Deutsch, 1985).

Gender role diversity. To test the proposed relation between salient conflict-relevant

member characteristics and VCM discussed above, I examine a specific indicator of salient and

conflict-relevant member characteristics, that is, gender role diversity, and its hypothesized

relationship with VCM in the proposed study. Gender role diversity may be described as the

extent to which individual members’ gender role orientations differ from one another within the

team. By gender role orientations, these refer to individuals’ psychological tendencies toward

masculine or instrumental traits (e.g., aggressiveness, analytical, and assertiveness), feminine or

expressive traits (e.g., affectionate, gentle, and understanding), or both traits, i.e., being

androgynous (Bem, 1974). Specifically, Bem (1974) reasoned that contrary to conventional

notions of masculinity and femininity being opposite ends of a one-dimensional continuum, both

concepts may be considered as independent dimensions. Not only could individuals be

characterized as being masculine or feminine, but they may also be characterized as being

androgynous, i.e., having both masculine and feminine personality characteristics (Bem, 1974).

Based on Bem’s (1974) early work on gender role orientations, many researchers have

examined the concept of gender role orientations and its relation with a variety of constructs,

including that of individual conflict management styles (e.g., Brewer, Mitchell & Weber, 2002;

Jurma & Powell, 1994; Portello & Long, 1994; Yelsma & Brown, 1985). Portello and Long

(1994), for example, found that masculine managers tend to use more dominating (competitive)

 

38  

conflict styles, while androgynous managers were more likely to use integrating (cooperative)

conflict styles. Jurma and Powell (1994), in another study, noted that organizational managers

who were perceived by their subordinates as displaying more androgynous behaviors were rated

as being most effective in managing conflict situations. Managers perceived as more masculine

by their subordinates, on the other hand, were rated as least effective in conflict management.

More recently, a survey study conducted by Brewer and colleagues (2002) reported that

individuals with masculine gender role orientations were more likely to use dominating styles,

those with feminine orientations were more likely to use avoiding styles, and those with

androgynous orientations were more likely to use integrating styles. Brewer et al.’s (2002)

findings were particularly noteworthy as they also demonstrated that these effects of gender role

orientations on conflict management styles were above and beyond those of physical gender or

biological sex differences. As such, this suggests that gender role orientation may be a better

predictor of conflict management than physical gender or biological sex (Brewer et al., 2002).

Given the above evidence on gender role orientation and conflict management, it is

therefore expected that differences in gender role orientations among members of a team, i.e.,

gender role diversity, will also likely affect the ways members handle conflict situations within

the team. Further, it is reasoned that gender role diversity in teams may have a positive

relationship with VCM. A low level of gender role diversity within a team indicates that all

members of a team are highly similar in their gender role orientations respectively. Such high

levels of similarity along gender role orientations further suggest that there is minimal basis for

members to categorize one another along this specific diversity attribute (Brewer, 1993; Tajfel &

Turner, 1986). Instead, highly similar members are likely to be drawn toward one another, in part

due to similarity/attraction processes (Byrne, 1971; Newcomb, 1961). If most, if not all,

 

39  

members are similar in a given type of gender role orientation, e.g., masculine gender roles, then

it is plausible that they are also likely to use similar conflict approaches, e.g., competitive

conflict handling strategies, in dealing with conflicts with one another within the team. In other

words, low levels of gender role diversity among team members should reduce the likelihood for

divergent individual behaviors, including variation in individuals’ conflict management

approaches during team conflict (Hempel, et al., 2008).

By contrast, high levels of gender role diversity will be more likely to contribute to the

formation of VCM instead. When team members perceive some differences in gender role

orientations as salient and meaningful among themselves, VCM should be more likely to develop

in the team when internal conflicts occur. Here, categorization processes will likely be activated,

as members become inclined to align with others who share their gender role orientations within

the team (Brewer, 1993; Tajfel & Turner, 1986). As members align with some but not others,

based on salient differences in gender role orientations activated during team conflict, this in turn

suggests weaker member identification at the team level, and thus a stronger likelihood for

members to rely on their own different conflict management approaches associated with their

specific gender role orientations, thus increasing the likelihood of VCM occurring within the

team.

H8: Gender role diversity will be positively associated with VCM.

Team contextual determinants

Aside from salient conflict-relevant member characteristics, team contextual

determinants are expected to be another key antecedent category likely to contribute to the

development of VCM. Further, theorists and researchers have shown that when the strength of

contextual influence on individuals is weak, individuals may be more inclined to perceive or

 

40  

behave in ways that reflect their personal predispositions, motives or attitudes (Meyer, Dalal, &

Hermida, 2010; Mischel, 1977).

In a team with contextual factors that exert relatively weak influence on its members

(e.g., one that lacks a strong raison d’être or a compelling team purpose), individual interests or

motivations may end up playing a larger role in influencing members’ perspectives, attitudes and

even behaviors. When such a team encounters conflict, members in the team may therefore rely

more on their own proclivities in conflict management. Some members may try to handle the

conflict more cooperatively, while others may do so in a more competitive fashion. Collectively

then, these differences should reflect variable conflict management approaches among individual

members, and thus, predict the occurrence of VCM.

Goal interdependence. The prominent social psychologist, Kurt Lewin (1939), once

noted that “[it] is not the similarity or dissimilarity of individuals that constitute a group, but

rather interdependence of fate” (pp. 165-166). Since Lewin’s observation, researchers have

found a considerable amount of support for the concept of interdependence, and its effects on a

variety of outcomes (see Johnson, 2003; Johnson & Johnson, 1989; Johnson, Maruyama,

Johnson, Nelson, & Skon, 1981; Tjosvold, 1986, 1998). While interdependence may be

construed in various ways, e.g., task, role, and outcome interdependence (Wageman, 1995), I

focus on perceived goal interdependence here, which can be defined as the extent and manner to

which members of a team perceive themselves as dependent on one another to achieve their

individual and/or team goals (Deutsch, 1949, 1973; Tjosvold, 1986, 1998). In other words, goal

interdependence is concerned with how goals are interlinked between two or more members in a

given team context.

 

41  

This definition of goal interdependence is also derived from Deutsch’s (1949, 1973)

theory of cooperation and competition, which includes a central proposition about the ways

individual goals are structured (i.e., goal interdependence), and how these ways in turn guide the

dynamics and outcomes of interpersonal interactions. Specifically, Deutsch (1949, 1973)

reasoned that there are three ways in which goals may be structured between individuals:

positive, negative and independent. Positive interdependence refers to the situation in which

individuals perceive their goals as positively correlated, i.e., individuals would achieve their

goals only if others achieve theirs as well. By contrast, negative interdependence refers to the

situation in which individual goals are negatively correlated: individuals would achieve their

goals only if others fail to achieve theirs. As for the third type of goal structure, independent

goals, this refers to the situation in which individuals perceive their goals to be independent of

one another. In this case, how and whether individuals achieve their goals is of little or no

concern to others (Deutsch, 1949, 1973; Tjosvold, 1986).

Although much of the empirical research supporting Deutsch’s propositions on goal

interdependence has been conducted at the individual level and in educational settings (Johnson,

Johnson, & Maruyama, 1983; Johnson, et al., 1981), Tjosvold and his colleagues have studied

this phenomenon in teams and in organizations (e.g., Alper, Tjosvold, & Law, 1998; Lu,

Tjosvold, & Shi, 2010; Tjosvold, 1986). For individuals and teams in organizations, they found

that positive interdependence or cooperative goals tend to predict higher levels of constructive

controversy (i.e., the open-minded discussion of intellectual conflicts, Tjosvold, 1998), which in

turn lead to enhanced outcomes such as greater team effectiveness (Alper, et al., 1998), improved

relationships (Tjosvold, 2002), and increased team innovation (Chen & Tjosvold, 2002b).

Negative interdependence or competitive goals, on the other hand, has been linked to lower

 

42  

levels of constructive controversy (e.g., Alper, et al., 1998), and consequently, more detrimental

team outcomes such as lower team effectiveness (Tjosvold, Wong, Nibler, & Pounder, 2002),

greater negative affect and reduced innovation (Chen, Tjosvold, & Su, 2005). As for independent

goals, research has also uncovered generally negative findings: independent goals have been

found to predict lower levels of constructive controversy (Chen & Tjosvold, 2002b), less open-

mindedness among team members (Tjosvold, 2002), less positive affect and reduced confidence

in future collaborations (Tjosvold, 1990). Overall, these findings suggest a critical role that goal

interdependence plays as a team contextual factor when it comes to understanding important

individual and team processes, including conflict management.

So far, prior research on goal interdependence has focused mainly on the ‘pure’ forms of

cooperative, competitive and/or independent goals (e.g., Alper, et al., 1998; Tjosvold, Law, &

Sun, 2006; Tjosvold, et al., 2002). Research and real-life observations, however, suggest that

individuals often also perceive the co-occurrence of positive and negative goals with one

another, i.e., mixed goal interdependence (Johnson & Johnson, 2005; Tjosvold, 1998). When

both cooperative and competitive goals exist in a team, individual differences in conflict

management approaches becomes likely to emerge among members as the contextual influence

of ‘pure’ cooperative or competitive goal interdependence on them is likely to be weakened.

Consequently, teams with mixed goals are also more likely to experience VCM compared to

cooperatively or competitively interdependent teams, since teams with cooperatively

interdependent members are more likely to manage conflicts similarly in a cooperative manner,

while teams with competitively interdependent members are also more likely to manage conflicts

similarly but competitively; such convergence in conflict management approaches suggest the

 

43  

reduced likelihood of VCM occurring in either cooperatively or competitively interdependent

teams.

H9: Cooperative and competitive goal interdependence among members will be

negatively associated with VCM, while mixed goal interdependence among

members will be positively associated with VCM.

Divergent team dynamics

The third antecedent category of VCM, divergent team dynamics, concerns the nature of

interaction processes and behavioral patterns in a team. Team dynamics that steer members’

cognition, attitudes or behaviors toward different directions, such as group polarization

(Isenberg, 1986; Myers & Lamm, 1976), subgroup formation and dynamics (Lau & Murnighan,

1998, 2005), and majority/minority influence (e.g., Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004; Deutsch &

Gerard, 1955; Moscovici, 1976; Nemeth, 1986), are likely to contribute to the development of

VCM in a team.

An example of divergent team dynamics includes the presence and strength of norms that

form within a team. Norms refer to informal rules that guide and regulate behavioral patterns

among members of a group (Feldman, 1984). In a team where its members have aligned

themselves into subgroups, the norms supporting these subgroups are likely to exert a powerful

effect on reinforcing certain attitudes and behaviors among subgroup members (Hackman,

1976). Strong norms in a subgroup can hold significant sway on how subgroup members’ behave

as they provide a clear sense of identity and belonging for subgroup members, guide shared

codes of conduct, and allow members to predict and interpret behaviors within the subgroup

(e.g., Bettenhausen & Murnighan, 1985, 1991; Chatman, 2010; Feldman, 1984). Suppose a team

that consists of a subgroup with relatively strong norms supporting collaborative behaviors and

 

44  

another subgroup with equally strong norms supporting competitive behaviors. In a conflict

situation, team members who identify with either subgroup are likely to display conflict

management behaviors that align with their subgroup norms. Members of the subgroup with

collaborative norms will display cooperative conflict management while those of the subgroup

with competitive norms will display competitive conflict management. For the team as a whole,

this should therefore lead to VCM development.

Subgroup formation. For this study, I focus on subgroup formation as an indicator of

divergent team dynamics in predicting the emergence of VCM. In the earlier sections, I have

alluded to how subgroup formation is likely to occur when individuals align themselves into

smaller entities or ‘cliques’ within a team, based on social categorization (Tajfel, 1978; Turner,

1985), similarity-attraction (Byrne, 1971; Newcomb, 1961), and/or faultline activation (Lau &

Murnighan, 1998, 2005). This suggests that the formation of subgroups in teams indicates the

presence of divergent team dynamics that are likely to influence individual and team behaviors.

As such, it may also be inferred that subgroup formation, only when it is based on salient

conflict-relevant member characteristics, would represent an important form of divergent team

dynamics in shaping the development of VCM in teams. In particular, I postulate that subgroup

formation is likely to mediate the predicted effects of gender role diversity on VCM (Hypothesis

10).

When member differences along gender role orientations are sufficiently salient and

meaningful during team conflict situations, it is likely that this specific diversity dimension will

in turn trigger social categorization processes (Tajfel, 1978; Turner, 1985), leading to in-

group/out-group distinction among members within the team, or in other words, subgroup

formation (Lau & Murnighan, 1998, 2005). Consequently, this process of in-group alignment

 

45  

and out-group differentiation may in turn encourage differences in conflict management styles to

emerge between subgroups. A subgroup that consists of more masculine members, for instance,

may use more competitive strategies to handle the team conflict situation, compared to strategies

used by another subgroup with more feminine members. For the team as a whole, it is therefore

likely that VCM will emerge.

H10: Subgroup formation will mediate the relationship between gender role diversity

and VCM.

 

46  

CHAPTER III: METHODOLOGY

Overview

This chapter describes the research design, sampling strategy, and study procedures that

were employed to empirically test the research questions and hypotheses discussed in the earlier

chapter. Measures that were used to collect data on the variables to be examined in this study are

also discussed here.

Participants

Sample. 884 undergraduate students majoring in business administration were recruited

for voluntary participation from several sections of two core courses titled, “Leadership and

Team Building (LTB)” and “Managing People at Work (MPW),” that were offered by the

Organizational Behavior and Human Resource Management (OBHR) division at the Singapore

Management University. 470 (53.2%) students enrolled in the LTB course were invited to

participate in this study via their course instructors, while 414 (46.8%) students enrolled in the

MPW course were invited to participate via the university’s subject pool system. A total of 417

students participated in this study, constituting a 47% response rate. 88 (18.7%) participants were

enrolled in the LTB course, while 329 (79.5%) participants were enrolled in the MPW course.

Since this study dealt with multilevel data (i.e., individuals nested within teams), the

traditional method of conducting a power analysis to estimate required sample sizes in single-

level designs cannot be directly translated to multilevel research (Scherbaum & Ferreter, 2009).

Instead, I follow recommendations from the multilevel literature which have suggested that

Level 2 sample sizes approaching 100 are appropriate if the variance components for the Level 2

variables are of interest in the study; such conclusions are based on simulation studies that have

 

47  

examined statistical power for variance components in a variety of sample sizes with different

effect sizes and intraclass correlations, and at various levels (Maas & Hox, 2005).

To be included in the final sample, participating teams also needed to meet two criteria:

first, the teams should already be established in the courses, and team members should have had

some level of interaction at the time of recruitment for this study. To assess the level of

interaction among team members, participants were asked to indicate the frequency of their

interactions with their team members throughout the semester (e.g., five or less times per week,

more than ten times per week) in the study. This is to ensure that team members have had

sufficient opportunities to interact with one another, and that they would have experienced

certain team processes, such as conflicts, within their teams. Second, members in the teams were

expected to work on a team project together for the academic semester. This is to ensure that

participants are members of “real” teams (Alderfer, 1977), rather than loosely defined work

groups.

As this study was concerned with within-team variance, it was also important to have

teams that were numerically large enough for member variability to emerge. Therefore, at least

three members from a given team were deemed to constitute one team unit. Researchers who

study groups have found that dyads possess different dynamics when compared to teams that

comprise of three or more members (Levine & Moreland, 1990).

Participating students from the LTB and MPW courses were required to work in the same

teams and each team had to work on a final team project throughout the academic semester.

Also, the team projects assigned to the student teams were identical across all the sections in

each course. Each individual participant was matched with their respective teams within the

course section that they were enrolled in, based on the team information he or she provided in the

 

48  

study. A total of 104 teams were identified from the participating students. 21 (20.2%) teams

were from the LTB course and 83 (79.8%) teams were from the MPW course.

After screening all 104 teams based on the team selection criteria described earlier, a total

of 79 teams were identified and included in the final analyses. These teams ranged from three to

eight members, with an average of four members per team (SD = 1.09, S2 = 1.19). Eight of these

teams were from the LTB course while the remaining 71 teams were from the MPW course.

Since the participating teams were recruited from two different courses, a series of t-tests (and

chi-square tests for categorical variables) was conducted between the eight LTB teams and a

random sample of eight MPW teams to determine whether there were any significant group

differences for various measures between teams based on course affiliation. No significant group

differences were found between both groups on measures assessing key variables for this study.

Design and procedures

This study was conducted using an online survey design. To recruit participants from the

MPW course, the study description was posted in the university’s subject pool system so that

students enrolled in the MPW course could sign up for voluntary participation in the second half

of the academic semester. This posting included general survey instructions, deadline for

participation, and a direct link to the online survey for this study. Participants from the MPW

course were also informed that they would receive their course credit upon completion of the

survey via the subject pool system.

At the same time, to recruit participants from the LTB course, I contacted all LTB course

instructors, via email, for their permission to recruit student teams from their courses for

voluntary participation in this study. Both the course instructors and participants were informed

of the study objectives at a general level, i.e., to understand how team characteristics affect team

 

49  

interactions, and how these interactions in turn affect team outcomes. Once the instructors’

permission had been obtained, the course instructors were asked to forward an email invitation to

their students, informing them of this research participation opportunity. Included in the email

was a link that would direct each participant to a set of online survey questionnaires via a secure

electronic survey hosting software program. Participants were then asked to complete the

questionnaires and to return their responses online. Participants were also assured that their

information would be kept strictly confidential, and used for the sole research purposes of this

study. At the end of the online survey, LTB participants were also informed of a specific location

where they could collect a small monetary incentive for their participation from the researcher.

Appendix A contains the emails for participant recruitment and study participation that

were distributed to the LTB course instructors and participants respectively, as described above.

See Appendix B for the full survey questionnaire.

Measures

In the following sections, I describe the measures that were used to assess the variables

involved in this study: conflict management approaches, gender role diversity, team goal

interdependence, mixed goal interdependence, subgroup formation, team conflict efficacy,

member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness. I

discuss the measures for assessing conflict management first, followed by the predictor

measures, and then the outcome measures. Other measures assessing control variables and

participant demographics are also discussed. All study measures are included in Appendix C.

Conflict management measures

VCM profiles. To assess the VCM profiles, I first gathered individual-level data on how

members manage conflict within their teams. To do so, I measured individual members’ conflict

 

50  

management approaches within their teams, based on adapted items from Chen, Liu and

Tjosvold’s (2005) measure for cooperative and competitive conflict management approaches in

teams. This measure consisted of two sub-scales: cooperative conflict management and

competitive conflict management. Both sub-scales were scored on a scale of 1 to 5 (1 = never; 3

= sometimes; 5 = always). The cooperative conflict management sub-scale comprised of five

items (e.g., “I work with my teammates to find a solution that will be good for all of us.”), while

the competitive conflict management sub-scale comprised of four reverse-coded items (e.g., “I

tend to overstate my position to get my way.”). One item from the cooperative sub-scale and two

items from the competitive sub-scale were omitted from the final analyses, due to low item-total

correlations. These omitted items were “I treat conflict as a mutual problem to solve with my

teammates,” “I try to get my teammates to agree with my position (reverse coded),” and “I tend

to view conflict as a win-lose contest (reverse coded).” The Cronbach’s alphas based on

individual ratings for the adapted cooperative sub-scale, competitive sub-scale, and overall scale

were .67, .65 and .60 respectively. Since VCM profiles were construed at the team level, it was

important to assess the group-level reliability of the construct as well (Snijders & Bosker, 1999,

p. 26). While the Cronbach’s alphas based on individual ratings for both subscales and the

overall scale were all below .70, the Cronbach’s alphas based on the teams’ mean scores for the

adapted cooperative sub-scale, competitive sub-scale, and overall scale were .70 and higher (see

Table 2). These suggest that reasonable levels of internal consistency were met at the team level.

As such, the overall conflict management scale was used in the final analyses.

Variant conflict management. Since the VCM construct was conceptualized as a

separation-based construct, I followed Harrison and Klein’s (2007) recommendation for

operationalizing separation-based constructs by calculating the standard deviation (SD) score for

 

51  

each team. In this case, the SD score on the overall conflict management scale was calculated for

each team to assess VCM. The within-team SD score was computed using the following

formula: √[Σ(Si-Smean)2/n], where S refers to the individual score on the conflict management

continuum, i refers to the ith member, and n refers to the total number of members within the

team (Harrison & Klein, 2007). The within-team SD scores ranged from a minimum value of 0

to a maximum value of 5.18, based on the formula: [(u-l)/2], where u and l represent the lower

and upper bounds of the conflict management continuum.

Cooperative and competitive team conflict management. Cooperative and competitive

team conflict management, on the other hand, were operationalized using teams’ means scores

on the cooperative and competitive conflict management sub-scales respectively. This is

consistent with the common approach of operationalizing team conflict management in the

literature (e.g., Alper, et al., 2000; DeChurch & Marks, 2001; Somech, 2008).

In order to justify data aggregation using teams’ means scores for cooperative and

competitive team conflict management, it was also necessary to establish reasonable levels of

group-level reliability, within-team agreement and between-team variability for the construct.

Further discussions of the group-level reliability and data aggregation indices for cooperative

team conflict management and all other team-level measures (viz., team goal interdependence,

subgroup formation, team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict

management process, team effectiveness, intra-team conflict types, team conflict intensity, and

team conflict importance) involved in this study are provided in the following chapter.

Predictor measures

Gender role diversity. To measure gender role diversity in teams, participants’

perceptions of their individual gender role orientations had to be assessed first. To assess gender

 

52  

role orientations, I used items based on the short-form version of the Bem Sex Role Inventory

(BSRI, Bem, 1981). Since the focus is on individuals’ gender role orientations within teams,

participants were asked to rate how well each of the 30 short-form BSRI items describes them

within the team on a seven-point scale (1 = never or almost never true; 7 = always or almost

always true). Once all the individual scores were gathered, I then used the median split method

to determine individuals’ gender role orientations. Individuals were identified as masculine if

their masculine and feminine scores were above and below the median respectively. The

opposite pattern, i.e., masculine and feminine scores below and above the median respectively,

was used to identify feminine individuals. Individuals with both masculine and feminine scores

above the median were identified as androgynous and those with both scores below the median

were classified as undifferentiated.

When all participants’ gender role orientations have been identified, I then calculated the

gender role diversity for each team, using Blau’s (1977) heterogeneity index. This index has

been commonly used in past team diversity research as an objective measure of diversity in

teams (e.g., Bantel & Jackson, 1989; Harrison, Price & Bell, 1998; Mohammed & Angell, 2004).

The following formula is used in calculating the Blau’s index: 1-Σpk2, where p refers to the

proportion of team members in the kth category. The Blau’s index ranges from 0 to 1, where a

value of 0 indicates complete team homogeneity and a value of 1 indicates the opposite, i.e.,

complete team heterogeneity.

Team goal interdependence. To measure members’ perceptions of goal interdependence

within teams, I used Lu, Tjosvold and Shi’s (2010) measure for assessing cooperative and

competitive goals in the teams. Items were scored on a five-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to

5 = strongly agree). This measure also consisted of two sub-scales: positive goal

 

53  

interdependence and negative goal interdependence. The positive goal interdependence sub-scale

comprised of five items (e.g., “In this team, members want each other to succeed.”) and the

negative goal interdependence sub-scale also comprised of five reverse-coded items (e.g., “In

this team, members like to show that they are superior to each other.”). The Cronbach’s alphas

based on individual ratings for the positive goal interdependence sub-scale, the negative goal

interdependence sub-scale and the overall scale were .67, .70 and .76 respectively. As for the

Cronbach’s alphas based on the team mean scores for both sub-scales and the overall scale, these

were all above .70 (see Table 2). Given that the overall scale had the highest alpha values based

on individual ratings and on teams’ mean scores, it was therefore used in the final analyses.

Mixed goal interdependence. The variable, mixed goal interdependence, was computed

using a subset of the team mean scores for team goal interdependence. Specifically, the score

distribution for the goal interdependence variable was divided into three parts, with the middle

part of the score distribution being selected as scores for mixed goal interdependence. In other

words, teams whose mean scores for the team goal interdependence measure lie between the

values of 34.90 (one standard deviation below the group mean in the distribution) and 40.22 (one

standard deviation above the group mean) were identified as having mixed goal interdependence.

Subgroup formation. To assess for the presence of subgroups within the team, I adapted

items from Cronin, Bezrukova, Weingart, et al.’s (2011) measure for subgroup formation in

teams (Cronbach’s alpha based on individual ratings = .80). This measure included the following

four items: “To what extent has your team split into subgroups?” “To what extent has your team

split into multiple or smaller cliques?” “To what extent has your team split into unevenly

balanced subgroups?” and “To what extent has your team split into two balanced subgroups?”

 

54  

These items were scored on a 1 to 5 scale (1 = not at all or to a very small extent; 5 = to a very

large extent).

Outcome measures

Team conflict efficacy. To measure members’ levels of conflict efficacy within teams, I

used an existing five-item measure developed by Alper et al. (2000). This measure was rated on

a five-point scale (1 = strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree). The Cronbach’s alpha based on

individual ratings for this measure was .79, thus indicating high levels of internal consistency

reliability.

Member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process. To assess members’

levels of satisfaction with how their teams manage internal conflicts, I adapted and used one of

the sub-scales in the Subjective Value Inventory developed by Curhan, Elfenbein, and Xu

(2006). This measure consisted of four items and was rated on a seven-point scale (1 = not at all;

4 = moderately; 7 = perfectly). The Cronbach’s alpha based on individual ratings for this

measure was .73.

Team effectiveness. Team members’ perceptions of their teams’ effectiveness were

measured using an existing five-item measure derived from prior studies (Alper et al., 1998;

Barker et al., 1988; Tjosvold, Law & Sun, 2006). This measure was rated on a five-point scale (1

= strongly disagree; 5 = strongly agree). The measure also exhibited very high levels of internal

consistency reliability based on individual ratings, with a Cronbach’s alpha of .90.

Controls and other measures

Intra-team conflict types. In this study, it was also important to control for the effects of

intra-team conflict types, as studies have shown that intra-team conflicts could significantly

affect the impact of conflict management on team functioning and outcomes (e.g., Behfar, et al.,

 

55  

2008; DeChurch & Marks, 2001; Desivilya & Yagil, 2005, Tjosvold, Law & Sun, 2006).

Tjosvold and colleagues (2006), for instance, reported that team perceptions of intra-team

conflicts, such as task and relational conflicts, could mediate the relationship between the teams’

conflict management approaches and their levels of team effectiveness. Specifically, they found

that teams with competitive conflict management approaches were likely to perceive more task

and relational conflicts, whereas teams with cooperative conflict management approaches were

likely to perceive less task and relational conflicts; Higher levels of task conflicts were also

related to more negative managerial ratings of team effectiveness (Tjosvold, Law & Sun, 2006).

So, to assess members’ perceptions of conflicts within the team, I applied Jehn and

Mannix’s (2001) scale to measure three types of within-team conflicts: task, relationship and

process conflicts. The overall measure comprised of nine items and the three sub-scales consisted

of three items each. All items for this measure are rated on a five-point scale. The Cronbach’s

alpha based on individual ratings for the overall measure was .88.

Perceived conflict importance and intensity. Past research has found that individual

perceptions of conflict importance and intensity could affect their conflict management

approaches as well (for the former, e.g., Jehn, 1997; Rosenthal & Hautaluoma, 1988; for the

latter, e.g., Andrews & Tjosvold, 1983, Barker, Tjosvold & Andrews, 1988). As such, it was also

important to measure and control for potential effects of members’ perceptions of the importance

and intensity of conflicts experienced within their teams. So, to assess individual perceptions of

conflict importance, participants were asked to rate the extent to which they view conflict within

the team as important, on a five-point scale (1 = not at all or to a very small extent; 5 = to a large

extent).

 

56  

As for the measure on conflict intensity in teams, this was assessed using participants’

scores on four items in the Jehn and Mannix’s (2001) intra-team conflict type measure. Together,

these four items assessed the extent to which overall conflict (i.e., task, relationship and process

conflicts combined) occurs within the team (see items 1, 4, 6 and 8 of the “Intra-team Conflict

Types” measure under Appendix C). The Cronbach’s alpha based on individual ratings for this

measure was .78.

Course affiliation and team size. Given that the participating teams were recruited from

two different courses, participants’ course affiliations would be controlled for in the analyses. In

this study, team size would also be controlled for, since team size can affect team functioning

and outcomes differently (Brewer & Kramer, 1986; Steiner, 1972).

Participant demographics. Members of the participating teams were also asked to

provide information on the following demographic and team-related characteristics: age, gender,

ethnicity, country of origin, years of living in Singapore, area of specialization for the degree,

years of prior and current work experience, team name, and course affiliation (see Section V of

Appendix B for the survey items that were used to gather this data). In the final sample, the

participants’ ages ranged from 17 to 27 (M = 21.7, SD = 1.65, S2 = 2.71). 53.9% of the

participants were female (n = 179), 45% were male (n = 151), and 0.6% identified with “other,

e.g., transgender” (n = 2). As for ethnicity, 86.4% of the participants identified themselves as

Chinese (n = 286), 5.4% as Indian (n = 18), 1.8% as biracial or multiracial (n = 6), 1.5% as

Malay (n = 5), and 4.8% as Other (n = 16). On average, participants had 25 years of living in

Singapore, and 1.5 years of prior and current work experience.

 

57  

CHAPTER IV: RESULTS

Overview

In this chapter, I discuss the preliminary, main and supplementary analyses that were

conducted to address the research questions and hypotheses stated in this study. First,

preliminary analyses, which included descriptive statistics and correlational analyses among all

key variables, are presented. Next, the main analyses used to assess the VCM construct and

proposed relationships among model variables are reported: qualitative content coding analyses

to assess the presence and prevalence of the proposed VCM profiles in teams; hierarchical

regressions to compare the relative effects of standard deviations-based VCM scores and means-

based cooperative and competitive team conflict management scores on team conflict efficacy,

member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness

respectively; planned comparisons to compare the effects of various VCM profiles on the three

team outcomes; simple, curvilinear and mediational regressions to examine gender role diversity,

team goal interdependence and subgroup formation as possible antecedents of VCM

respectively. Finally, supplementary analyses that assessed the VCM construct using latent class

analyses and group differences among identified latent classes on the three team outcomes are

also presented and discussed.

Preliminary analyses

Descriptive statistics

Table 1 shows the means, standard deviations and variances for all key team-level

variables. The means for these variables ranged from .62 to 37.57, the standard deviations ranged

from .15 to 3.57, and the variances ranged from .02 to 12.71.

Team-level reliability and data aggregation indices

 

58  

Since this study was concerned with team-level constructs, sufficient levels of team-level

reliability, within-team agreement, and between-team variability were needed in order to justify

data aggregation for all relevant team-level measures. To do so, I calculated the team-level

reliability scores based on the teams’ means (Snijders & Bosker, 1999, p. 26), within-group

interrater agreement indices or rwgs (James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1984, 1993), and intraclass

correlations (Bliese, 2000) using the aggregated team-level data for each measure. The rwg

measures the degree to which individual ratings within a group are interchangeable, and values

of rwgs that are greater or equal to .70 are considered good indicators of sufficient within-team

agreement (George & Bettenhausen, 1990). As for intraclass correlations, these are measured in

two forms: ICC (1), which assesses the extent to which variability in individual ratings is

explained by group membership, and ICC (2), which is concerned with estimating the reliability

of group means. ICC (l) values of .12 or higher and ICC (2) values of .70 or higher would

indicate sufficient support for within-team agreement and between-team variability respectively

(Bliese, 2000).

Table 2 shows the team-level reliability scores and data aggregation indices for all the

team-level measures. All measures exhibited reasonable to high levels of reliability according to

the group means-based Cronbach’s alphas (range of .70 to 94). As for the aggregation indices,

the median rwgs for all measures were .70 or greater, thus indicating acceptable levels of within-

team agreement. The ICC (1) values for all the measures also met the recommended value of .12

or higher, except for the masculine sub-scale and the overall gender role orientation measure.

The ICC (2) values, on the other hand, ranged from .45 to .93. This indicated that some of the

measures did not exhibit reasonable levels of between-team variability (see Table 2 for these

 

59  

measures). Nonetheless, since the team reliabilities and rwgs for all the team measures were

acceptable, it was determined that data aggregation for the team-level variables was justified.

Common method variance

Considering that all variables were assessed using self-report measures in the same

survey, it is important to determine whether common method variance, i.e., variance that stems

from the measurement method rather than from the intended constructs to be assessed

(Podsakoff, et al., 2003), may be present. One common statistical technique used in diagnosing

the presence of common method variance is the Harman’s single-factor test (Podsakoff & Organ,

1986; Podsakoff et al., 2003). In this test, an exploratory or confirmatory factor analysis is

conducted in which all variables in the study are loaded. The unrotated factor solution is then

examined to determine the number of factors accounting for the variance in the variables. The

Harman’s single-factor test assumes that if a significant amount of variance was present, a single

factor would therefore emerge from the factor analysis. Alternatively, one single factor would

account for most of the co-variance among the variables (Podsakoff & Organ, 1986; Podsakoff et

al., 2003).

Based on this approach, I therefore conducted the Harman’s single-factor test to assess

whether common method variance would pose an issue in the interpretation of the study results.

To do so, scale items for all the team-level predictor and outcome variables involved in this

study were loaded into a principal-components factor analysis. The results revealed 18 factors

with eigenvalues of one or greater that were extracted from the unrotated solution. Further, the

first and largest factor to emerge from the solution accounted for only 24.5% of the total

variance. As such, it was concluded that common method variance did not pose a problem in this

research.

 

60  

Intercorrelational analyses

Zero-order correlations among all key continuous and dichotomous team-level variables

using Pearson product-moment coefficients were also conducted (see Table 3). Cooperative team

conflict management, based on teams’ means scores for the cooperative conflict management

sub-scale, was significantly correlated with three predictor variables, team goal interdependence

(r = .48, p < .001), mixed goal interdependence (r = .56, p < .001) and subgroup formation (r = -

.31, p < .01). It was also correlated with all three team outcomes: team conflict efficacy (r = .36,

p < .01), members’ satisfaction with their teams’ conflict management process (r = .23, p < .05),

and team effectiveness (r = .46, p < .001). Higher levels of cooperative team mean conflict

management were also found to be significantly associated with lower levels of intra-team

conflict (r = -.39, p < .001) and lower levels of team conflict intensity (r = -.49, p < .001). By

contrast, VCM, based on teams’ standard deviation scores for the overall conflict management

scale, was uncorrelated with any of the team-level variables. Accordingly, these correlational

findings suggested that initial support for Hypotheses 2a to 2c, which predicted effects of VCM

on team conflict efficacy, members’ satisfaction with team conflict management process, and

team effectiveness, above and beyond those of cooperative team conflict management was not

found.

Competitive team conflict management, based on teams’ means scores for the

competitive conflict management sub-scale, was significantly correlated with two of the

predictor variables: team goal interdependence (r = -.39, p < .01) and subgroup formation (r =

.41, p < .01). It was, however, not related to any of the three outcome variables. As such, initial

support for Hypotheses 3a to 3c, which predicted the effects of VCM on team conflict team

conflict efficacy, members’ satisfaction with team conflict management process respectively, and

 

61  

team effectiveness above and beyond those of competitive team conflict management was also

not established.

Gender role diversity, one of the proposed predictors of VCM, was only significantly

related to course affiliation (r = .23, p < .05). This suggests that initial support for Hypothesis 8,

which predicted a positive relationship between gender role diversity and VCM, was also not

found.

The second proposed predictor of VCM, team goal interdependence, was negatively

correlated with subgroup formation (r = -.45, p < .001), and positively correlated with all three

team outcomes: team conflict efficacy (r = .44, p < .001), members’ satisfaction with team

conflict management process (r = .47, p < .001), and team effectiveness (r = .55, p < .001).

Cooperative team goal interdependence (i.e., higher team goal interdependence scores) was also

related to lower levels of intra-team conflict (r = -.55, p < .001) and team conflict intensity (r = -

.61, p < .001). Accordingly, this indicates that initial support for Hypothesis 9, which predicted a

curvilinear relationship between team goal interdependence and VCM, was also not established.

Finally, as for subgroup formation, the third antecedent that was predicted to mediate the

relationship between gender role diversity and VCM in Hypothesis 10, it was found to be

uncorrelated with gender role diversity (r = -.18, ns) and VCM (r = -.04, ns). Given these

findings, initial support for Hypothesis 10 was thus not provided.

Main analyses

Assessing presence of VCM profiles in teams

Presence of proposed VCM profiles. Hypothesis 1 states that the five proposed VCM

profiles, i.e., minimum cooperative, minimum competitive, moderate cooperative, moderate

competitive, and maximum VCM profiles, will be present in teams. To test this hypothesis, I

 

62  

conducted a content coding analysis. To do so, the individual-level responses to all items in the

overall conflict management scale were first aggregated for each member within a team. Then,

the aggregated conflict management score for each individual member was plotted along a one-

dimensional continuum for all members in each team. A Microsoft Excel worksheet was created

to visually plot the distribution of individual conflict management scores for each team along a

single row. Since 79 teams were included in the analyses, 79 rows were created, with each row

representing the spread of individual scores per team.

Using the VCM profile descriptions as a coding scheme, three trained coders who were

blind to the study questions and hypotheses reviewed these rows for all teams, and were

instructed to classify each team into one of five proposed VCM profiles: minimum cooperative

VCM, moderate cooperative VCM, maximum VCM, moderate competitive VCM, and minimum

competitive VCM. If the team’s row of scores did not characterize any one of the five VCM

profiles, coders were instructed to classify the team under a sixth category labeled as “other”.

Whenever a team was classified as “other”, the coder also provided detailed comments to

describe the specific distribution of individual scores for that team. The specific definitions of

the coding categories are provided in Table 4.

In cases where coding discrepancies among coders occurred, the majority rule was

applied. For example, if two of the three coders rated a “3” and the remaining coder rated a “1”,

the former was applied as the coding category for the given team. Once the coding from all three

coders has been completed, Cohen’s Kappas were calculated between pairs of coders to

determine the level of inter-rater agreement on the coding. The Cohen’s Kappas were .96, .96

and .96, thus indicating high levels of consistency across coders’ ratings.

 

63  

Table 5 shows the final counts of teams that were classified across the six coding

categories. Based on these counts, it was observed that in this sample, most teams exhibited

moderate cooperative VCM profiles, followed by moderate competitive VCM, minimum

cooperative VCM, maximum VCM, and minimum competitive VCM profiles. As for the teams

that were classified under the “other” category, a closer examination of the comments provided

by coders suggested that these teams may be further organized into three new profiles:

‘distributed,’ ‘multiple clusters,’ and ‘midpoint cluster.’ Teams that may be described as

‘distributed’ contained individual scores that were relatively spread out throughout the entire

conflict management continuum. Teams described as ‘multiple clusters’ contained score

distributions where there was an absence of a majority cluster; instead, multiple small clusters

were observed along the conflict management continuum. Finally, for teams that may be

described as ‘midpoint cluster’, these contained score distributions where all scores were co-

located around the midpoint of the continuum. See Figure 5 for all eight profiles identified.

In summary, to address Hypothesis 1 based on the findings from the content coding

analysis, it was therefore concluded that the various proposed VCM profiles are indeed present

in teams. Furthermore, other types of team profiles such as those characterized as ‘distributed,’

‘multiple clusters,’ and ‘midpoint cluster’ profiles that were not previously conceptualized in the

proposed VCM theory were also present in the teams sampled. In the analyses, teams with

moderate cooperative VCM profiles were most common, followed by those with moderate

competitive, minimum cooperative, maximum, and minimum competitive VCM profiles.

Assessing relative effects of VCM and mean team conflict management on team outcomes

Cooperative team conflict management versus VCM. The next set of hypotheses, i.e.,

Hypotheses 2a-2c, is concerned with comparing the relative effects of cooperative team conflict

 

64  

management scores (based on teams’ means scores) and VCM (based on teams’ standard

deviation scores) on team conflict efficacy, members’ satisfaction with their team’s conflict

management process, and team effectiveness respectively. Specifically, Hypothesis 2a stated that

VCM scores will be associated with lower levels of team conflict efficacy, with cooperative team

conflict management scores being held constant. Hypothesis 2b predicted that VCM scores will

be associated with lower levels of members’ satisfaction with their teams’ conflict management

process, with cooperative team conflict management scores being held constant. As for

Hypothesis 2c, VCM scores will also be associated with lower levels of team effectiveness, with

cooperative team conflict management scores being held constant. Hierarchical multiple

regression analyses were used to test these three hypotheses.

Prior to testing Hypothesis 2a, preliminary analyses were first performed to test

assumptions of homoscedasticity, independence, linearity, normality, and multicollinearity

among the variables involved in this analysis. The variables included cooperative team conflict

management, VCM, team conflict efficacy, intra-team conflict, team conflict intensity, team

conflict importance, team goal interdependence, mixed goal interdependence, and team subgroup

formation. The variables, intra-team conflict and team conflict intensity, were found to be highly

intercorrelated, indicating the presence of multicollinearity. To address this, team conflict

intensity, and not intra-team conflict, was used as a control variable in the hypothesis test, given

the former’s stronger correlation with the dependent variable, team conflict efficacy. Team goal

interdependence and mixed goal interdependence were also highly multicollinear, and both were

correlated with team conflict efficacy as well. In this case, only team goal interdependence was

included as a control variable since it was more strongly associated with team conflict efficacy

than mixed goal interdependence. Team subgroup formation was also included as a control

 

65  

variable in this analysis as it, too, was significantly correlated with the team conflict efficacy.

Course affiliation and team size were not included as control variables in this analysis as they

were found to be uncorrelated with team conflict efficacy.

In the hierarchical regression analysis, the control variables, i.e., team conflict intensity,

team conflict importance, team goal interdependence and team subgroup formation, were entered

at Step 1 in the regression model. Both cooperative team conflict management and VCM were

entered as predictor variables at Step 2 in the model.

Table 6 presents the findings for the hierarchical regression analysis. The regression

results showed that the four control variables, team conflict intensity, team conflict importance,

team goal interdependence and team subgroup formation, explained 33.3% of the variance in

team conflict efficacy, F (4, 74) = 9.25, p < .01. The entry of cooperative team conflict

management and VCM at Step 2 explained an additional 2.9% of the variance in team conflict

efficacy; however, this additional variance was not a significant contribution in the model, F (2,

72) = 6.83, ns. In the final model, only team goal interdependence (β = .28, p < .05) and team

conflict importance (β = .27, p < .05) were statistically significant predictors of team conflict

efficacy. Neither cooperative team conflict management (β = .14, ns) nor VCM (β = -.14, ns)

significantly predicted team conflict efficacy. Overall, these findings suggest that Hypothesis 2a

was not supported.

To test Hypothesis 2b, preliminary analyses were also first conducted to ensure that no

variables involved in this analysis violated the assumptions of homoscedasticity, independence,

linearity, normality, and multicollinearity. At Step 1 of the hierarchical regression, the variables,

team mean age, team conflict intensity, and team goal interdependence were entered as controls

in the regression model. Team mean age and team goal interdependence were controlled for in

 

66  

this analysis as both were significantly correlated with the dependent variable, member

satisfaction with team conflict management process. Course affiliation, team size, and team

conflict importance were omitted in this analysis since they were uncorrelated with member

satisfaction with team conflict management process. Despite significant associations with

member satisfaction, mixed goal interdependence and intra-team conflict were also excluded as

control variables in the model due to their high multicollinearity with team goal interdependence

and team conflict intensity respectively. Cooperative team conflict management and VCM were

entered as predictor variables at Step 2 in the model equation.

Table 7 shows the results of the hierarchical regression analysis. In Model 1, the control

variables explained 28.2% of the total variance in predicting satisfaction with team conflict

management process, F (3, 75) = 9.84, p < .01. The inclusion of cooperative team conflict

management and VCM as predictors in Model 2 accounted for an additional variance of 0.6% in

the final model; however, this additional variance was not a statistically significant contribution,

F (2, 73) = 5.90, ns. Only team mean age (β = .21, p < .05) and team goal interdependence (β =

.39, p < .01) significantly predicted satisfaction with team conflict management process in the

final model. Accordingly, this indicates that neither cooperative team conflict management nor

VCM were significant predictors of member satisfaction with team conflict management, after

controlling for the effects of team mean age, team conflict intensity, and team goal

interdependence. As such, Hypothesis 2b was not supported.

Finally, Hypothesis 2c was also tested using a hierarchical regression analysis.

Preliminary analyses were also conducted to ensure that no variables involved in this analysis

violated the assumptions of homoscedasticity, independence, linearity, normality, and

multicollinearity. Three control variables, i.e., team conflict intensity, team goal

 

67  

interdependence, and subgroup formation, were entered in the first block of the regression

model. Team goal interdependence and subgroup formation were added as controls due to their

significant correlations with team effectiveness. Despite their significant correlations with team

effectiveness, mixed goal interdependence and intra-team conflict were not included as control

variables due to their high multicollinearity with team goal interdependence and team conflict

intensity respectively. Since course affiliation, team size and team conflict importance were not

correlated with team effectiveness, these were also not controlled for in the analysis. Cooperative

team conflict management and VCM were entered as predictor variables in the second block.

The hierarchical regression results revealed that the three control variables accounted for

36.8% of the variance in team effectiveness, F (3, 75) = 14.54, p < .01, while the two predictor

variables contributed an additional 5.3% of the variance in team effectiveness, F (2, 73) = 5.90, p

< .01 (see Table 8). A closer look at the coefficients in the final model showed that only

cooperative team conflict management (β = .25, p < .05) significantly predicted team

effectiveness, even after team conflict intensity, team goal interdependence, and subgroup

formation were controlled for. VCM, on the other hand, did not have a significant effect on team

effectiveness (β = .05, ns). Accordingly, these findings suggest that Hypothesis 2c was not

supported.

Competitive team conflict management versus VCM. The third set of hypotheses, i.e.,

Hypotheses 3a-3c, is concerned with comparing the relative effects of competitive team conflict

management scores (based on teams’ means scores) and VCM (based on teams’ standard

deviation scores) on team conflict efficacy, members’ satisfaction with their team’s conflict

management process, and team effectiveness respectively. Specifically, Hypothesis 3a stated that

VCM scores will be associated with higher levels of team conflict efficacy, with competitive

 

68  

team conflict management scores being held constant. Hypothesis 3b predicted that VCM scores

will be associated with higher levels of members’ satisfaction with their teams’ conflict

management process, with competitive team conflict management scores being held constant. As

for Hypothesis 3c, VCM scores will also be associated with higher levels of team effectiveness,

with competitive team conflict management scores being held constant. Similar to Hypotheses 2a

to 2c, a series of hierarchical multiple regression analyses were also used to test Hypotheses 3a

to 3c.

Prior to testing Hypothesis 3a, preliminary analyses were also performed to test

assumptions of homoscedasticity, independence, linearity, normality, and multicollinearity

among the variables involved in this analysis. Four control variables, i.e., team conflict intensity,

team conflict importance, team goal interdependence and team subgroup formation, were entered

at Step 1 in the regression model. Both competitive team conflict management and VCM were

entered as predictor variables at Step 2 in the model.

Table 9 presents the findings for the hierarchical regression analysis. In the final model,

the entry of competitive team conflict management and VCM at Step 2 explained an additional

2.5% of the variance in team conflict efficacy; however, this additional variance was not a

significant contribution in the model, F (2, 72) = 6.69, ns. In the final model, only team goal

interdependence (β = .35, p < .05) and team conflict importance (β = .26, p < .05) were

statistically significant predictors of team conflict efficacy. Neither competitive team conflict

management (β = .11, ns) nor VCM (β = -.13, ns) significantly predicted team conflict efficacy.

Overall, these findings suggest that Hypothesis 3a was not supported.

To test Hypothesis 3b, preliminary analyses were also first conducted to ensure that no

variables involved in this analysis violated the assumptions of homoscedasticity, independence,

 

69  

linearity, normality, and multicollinearity. At Step 1 of the hierarchical regression, the variables,

team mean age, team conflict intensity, and team goal interdependence were entered as controls

in the regression model. Competitive team conflict management and VCM were entered as

predictor variables at Step 2 in the model equation.

Table 10 shows the results of the hierarchical regression analysis. The inclusion of

competitive team conflict management and VCM as predictors in Model 2 accounted for an

additional variance of 3.7% in the final model; however, this additional variance was not a

statistically significant contribution, F (2, 73) = 6.84, ns. Only team mean age (β = .21, p < .05)

and team goal interdependence (β = .44, p < .01) significantly predicted satisfaction with team

conflict management process in the final model. Accordingly, this indicates that neither

competitive team conflict management nor VCM were significant predictors of member

satisfaction with team conflict management, after controlling for the effects of team mean age,

team conflict intensity, and team goal interdependence. As such, Hypothesis 3b was not

supported.

Finally, Hypothesis 3c was also tested using a hierarchical regression analysis.

Preliminary analyses were also conducted to ensure that no variables involved in this analysis

violated the assumptions of homoscedasticity, independence, linearity, normality, and

multicollinearity. Three control variables, i.e., team conflict intensity, team goal

interdependence, and subgroup formation, were entered in the first block. Competitive team

conflict management and VCM were entered as predictor variables in the second block.

The hierarchical regression results revealed that the entry of competitive team conflict

management and VCM as predictors in Model 2 accounted for an additional variance of 2.1% in

the final model; however, this additional variance was also not a statistically significant

 

70  

contribution, F (2, 73) = 9.27, ns (see Table 11). Accordingly, these findings suggest that

Hypothesis 3c was also not supported.

Assessing relative effects of VCM profiles on team outcomes

Minimum cooperative VCM versus all other VCM profiles. The next set of hypotheses,

i.e., Hypotheses 4a-4c, was concerned with the relative effects of minimum cooperative VCM

profiles and all other VCM profiles on team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the

team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness respectively. Hypothesis 4a stated

that relative to teams with all other VCM profiles, teams with minimum cooperative VCM

profiles are likely to report the highest levels of team conflict efficacy. Similarly, Hypothesis 4b

predicted that relative to teams with all other VCM profiles, teams with minimum cooperative

VCM profiles are likely to report the highest levels of members’ satisfaction with the team’s

conflict management process. As for Hypothesis 4c, it stated that relative to teams with all other

VCM profiles, teams with minimum cooperative VCM profiles are also likely to report the

highest levels of team effectiveness.

Prior to testing Hypotheses 4a to 4c, preliminary analyses were performed to test the

assumptions of homoscedasticity, independence, linearity, normality, and multicollinearity. The

preliminary analyses revealed that the distribution of team conflict efficacy scores for teams with

moderate competitive VCM profiles was significantly non-normal, W (10) = .77, p < .01. As

such, Hypothesis 4a was tested using the Mann-Whitney test. Hypotheses 4b and 4c, on the other

hand, were each tested using planned comparisons. To test all three hypotheses, the VCM

profiles as identified in the earlier content coding analysis were entered as a grouping variable of

five levels, with each level representing one VCM profile.

 

71  

Prior to testing Hypothesis 4a, dummy variables were first created to compare the

minimum cooperative VCM profiles (“1”) against the four other VCM profiles (“0”) in four

Mann-Whitney U Tests. A Bonferroni correction was applied, so all effects were reported at a

.0125 level of significance. Based on the Mann-Whitney U test findings, it was observed that the

team conflict efficacy scores for teams with minimum cooperative VCM profiles (Mdn = 19.45,

n = 6) were not significantly higher than those for teams with moderate cooperative VCM

profiles (Mdn= 19.71, n = 38), U = 101.50, z = -.43, ns, teams with maximum VCM profiles

(Mdn = 19.25, n = 5), U = 12.00, z = -.55, ns, or teams with minimum competitive VCM profiles

(Mdn = 18.33, n = 3), U = 1.50, z = -1.95, ns. However, teams with minimum cooperative VCM

profiles did report significantly higher levels of team conflict efficacy compared to those with

moderate competitive VCM profiles (Mdn = 18.58, n = 10), U = 5.00, z = -2.71, p < .01, r = .68.

These findings thus suggest that Hypothesis 4a was partially supported (see Table 12).

Results of Hypothesis 4b indicated the planned comparison test contrasting minimum

cooperative VCM against the other four VCM profiles was also not significant: t (57) = 1.30, ns

(see Table 13 for the group means and standard deviations). Accordingly, this suggests that

Hypothesis 4b was not supported.

Finally, for the test of Hypothesis 4c, the planned comparison test contrasting minimum

cooperative VCM against the other four VCM profiles was significant: t (57) = 2.47, p < .05, r =

.31 (see Table 13). Further post-hoc comparisons, using a Bonferroni’s correction, indicated that

the mean team effectiveness scores for teams with minimum cooperative VCM profiles (M =

19.80, SD = 1.15) were only significantly higher than those for teams with minimum competitive

VCM profiles (M = 15.47, SD = 1.53). Overall, this suggests that Hypothesis 4c was only

partially supported.

 

72  

Minimum competitive VCM profile versus all other VCM profiles. Hypotheses 5a to 5c

were concerned with comparing the effects of minimum competitive VCM profiles against all

other VCM profiles on team conflict efficacy, members’ satisfaction with the team’s conflict

management process, and team effectiveness respectively. As noted earlier, given the non-

normal distribution of team conflict efficacy scores for teams with moderate competitive VCM

profiles, Hypothesis 5a was therefore tested using the the Mann-Whitney test. Hypotheses 5b and

5c were each tested using planned comparisons.

To test Hypothesis 5a, dummy variables were created to compare the minimum

competitive VCM profiles (“1”) against the four other VCM profiles (“0”) in four Mann-

Whitney U Tests. A Bonferroni correction was applied, so all effects were reported at a .0125

level of significance. Finding from the Mann-Whitney U tests indicated that none of the

comparisons were significant: team conflict efficacy scores for teams with minimum competitive

VCM profiles (Mdn = 18.33, n = 3) were not significantly lower than those for team with

minimum cooperative VCM profiles (Mdn = 19.45, n = 6), U = 1.50, z = -1.95, ns, teams with

moderate cooperative VCM profiles (Mdn= 19.71, n = 38), U = 101.50, z = -.43, ns, teams with

maximum VCM profiles (Mdn = 19.25, n = 5), U = 12.00, z = -.55, ns, or teams with moderate

competitive VCM profiles (Mdn = 18.58, n = 10), U = 12.50, z = -.42, ns (see Table 12). These

findings thus suggest that Hypothesis 5a was not supported.

Results of Hypothesis 5b indicated that the planned comparison test contrasting minimum

competitive VCM against the other four VCM profiles was significant: t (73) = -2.14, p < .05, r

= .27 (see Table 13). Teams with minimum competitive VCM profiles (M = 19.19, SD = 2.17)

reported lowest levels of member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process,

relative to those with minimum cooperative VCM (M = 22.55, SD = 1.75), moderate cooperative

 

73  

VCM (M = 21.88, SD = 2.30), maximum VCM (M = 22.60, SD = 3.10), and moderate

competitive VCM (M = 21.27, SD = 1.69). Accordingly, these results suggest that Hypothesis 5b

was supported.

Finally, for the test of Hypothesis 5c, the planned comparison test contrasting minimum

competitive VCM against the other four VCM profiles was also significant: t (57) = -3.44, p <

.01, r = .41 (see Table 13). Specifically, the mean team effectiveness scores for teams with

minimum competitive VCM profiles (M = 15.47, SD = 1.53) were only significantly lower than

those for teams with minimum cooperative VCM (M = 19.80, SD = 1.15), and than those for

teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles (M = 19.04, SD = 1.75). Overall, this suggests

that Hypothesis 5c was only partially supported.

Moderate cooperative VCM profiles versus moderate competitive VCM profiles. The next

set of hypotheses, i.e., Hypotheses 6a to 6c, compared the effects of moderate cooperative VCM

profiles and moderate competitive VCM profiles on team conflict efficacy, members’

satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness respectively.

Given the non-normal distributions of team efficacy scores for teams with moderate competitive

VCM profiles, Hypothesis 6a was thus examined using the Mann-Whitney U tests. Hypotheses

6b and 6c were each tested using planned comparison tests instead.

Hypothesis 6a stated that compared to teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles,

teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles will be associated with higher levels of team

conflict efficacy. A dummy variable was created and used to compare teams with moderate

cooperative VCM profiles (“1”) and those with moderate competitive VCM profiles (“0”) in the

Mann-Whitney U test. Results of the Mann-Whitney U test was significant: U = 101.00, z = -

2.26, p < .05, r = .33 (see Table 12). This indicated that the team conflict efficacy scores for

 

74  

teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles (Mdn = 19.71, n = 38) were significantly higher

than those for teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles (Mdn = 18.58, n = 10). Therefore,

Hypothesis 6a was supported.

Hypothesis 6b predicted that relative to teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles,

teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles are likely to report higher levels of members’

satisfaction with their team’s conflict management process. Results of the planned comparison

test contrasting moderate cooperative VCM profiles against moderate competitive VCM profiles

on member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process scores was non-significant:

t (57) = .77, ns (see Table 13). In other words, teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles

(M = 21.88, SD = 2.30) were not significantly more satisfied with their conflict management

process than teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles (M = 21.27, SD = 1.69).

Hypothesis 6b was therefore not supported.

When it comes to Hypothesis 6c, this indicated that relative to teams with moderate

competitive VCM profiles, teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles are also likely to

report higher levels of team effectiveness. Results of the planned comparison test contrasting

moderate cooperative VCM profiles against moderate competitive VCM profiles on team

effectiveness was significant: t (57) = 2.02, p < .05, r = .26 (see Table 13). The mean team

effectiveness scores for teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles (M = 19.04, SD = 1.75)

were thus significantly higher than those for teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles (M

= 17.83, SD = 1.29). As such, Hypothesis 6c was supported.

Maximum VCM profiles versus moderate competitive VCM profiles. Hypotheses 7a to 7c

were concerned with comparing the effects of maximum VCM profiles and moderate

competitive VCM profiles on team conflict efficacy, members’ satisfaction with the team’s

 

75  

conflict management process and team effectiveness respectively. Hypothesis 7a was analyzed

using the Mann-Whitney U Test due to a violation of the non-normality assumption for teams

with moderate competitive VCM profiles, while Hypotheses 7b and 7c were each examined with

planned comparisons.

Hypothesis 7a stated that relative to teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles,

teams with maximum VCM profiles are likely to report higher levels of team conflict efficacy.

To test this hypothesis, a dummy variable was created to compare teams with moderate

competitive VCM profiles (“1”) and those with maximum VCM profiles (“0”) and applied in the

Mann-Whitney U test. Results of the Mann-Whitney U test revealed a marginally significant

difference in team conflict efficacy scores between teams with maximum VCM profiles (Mdn =

19.25, n = 5) and those with moderate competitive VCM profiles (Mdn = 18.58, n = 10), U =

9.50, z = -1.91, p < .10 (see Table 12). Therefore, this indicated that Hypothesis 7a was not

supported.

Hypothesis 7b predicted that relative to teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles,

teams with maximum VCM profiles are also likely to report higher levels of members’

satisfaction with team conflict management process. Results of the planned comparison test

contrasting teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles against teams with maximum VCM

profiles on member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process scores was non-

significant: t (57) = -1.09, ns (see Table 13). In other words, teams with maximum VCM profiles

(M = 22.60, SD = 3.10) were not significantly more satisfied with their conflict management

process than those with moderate competitive VCM profiles (M = 21.27, SD = 1.69).

Accordingly, Hypothesis 7b was not supported.

 

76  

Finally, for Hypothesis 7c, it was hypothesized that relative to teams with moderate

competitive VCM profiles, teams with maximum VCM profiles are also likely to report higher

levels of team effectiveness. Results of the planned comparison test contrasting teams with

maximum VCM profiles against teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles on team

effectiveness were also non-significant: t (57) = -1.54, ns (see Table 13). The mean team

effectiveness scores for teams with maximum VCM profiles (M = 19.25, SD = 2.38) were not

significantly higher than those for teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles (M = 17.83,

SD = 1.29). As such, Hypothesis 7c was also not supported.

Assessing potential antecedents of VCM

Gender role diversity. Hypothesis 8 predicted a positive relationship between gender role

diversity and VCM (i.e., The higher the levels of gender role diversity, the more likely VCM will

occur in teams). To test Hypothesis 8, a simple regression analysis was conducted. Table 14

shows the findings for the simple regression analysis. The findings revealed that gender role

diversity did not predict VCM significantly, F (1, 76) = .77, ns. As such, Hypothesis 8 was not

supported.

Team goal interdependence. Hypothesis 9 predicted an inverted U-shaped curvilinear

relationship between different types of goal interdependence and VCM (i.e., cooperative and

competitive goal interdependence will be negatively associated with VCM, while mixed goal

interdependence will be positively associated with VCM). A polynomial regression analysis was

used to test this hypothesis.

A higher order variable of team goal interdependence (squared team goal

interdependence) was created in order to test the nonlinear relationship proposed. Next, both

team goal interdependence and squared team goal interdependence variables were then centered

 

77  

and entered into the model to improve interpretability of parameter estimates and to reduce

multicollinearity between predictor variables.

Table 15 shows the findings for the polynomial regression analysis. The findings

revealed that neither team goal interdependence nor its higher order variable significantly

predicted VCM, F (2, 76) = 1.35, ns. Therefore, Hypothesis 9 was also not supported.

Subgroup formation as a mediator. Hypothesis 10 predicted the mediating role of

subgroup formation between gender role diversity and VCM. It was tested using mediational

regression analyses, based on Baron and Kenny’s (1986) approach. According to Baron and

Kenny (1986), three conditions need to be established in order to demonstrate mediation effects:

(1) the antecedent has to be related to the outcome; (2) the antecedent has to be related to the

mediator; (3) the relationship between the antecedent and the outcome will be partially or fully

removed when the mediator is controlled for (p.1177).

To test Hypothesis 10, the first mediational condition that gender role diversity is related

to VCM has to be established. Since Hypothesis 8 was not supported and earlier correlational

analyses also indicated that gender role diversity was not correlated with VCM, this first

condition was therefore not established.

The second mediational condition stated that gender role diversity has to be related to

subgroup formation. A simple regression analysis was conducted to test this relationship. Results

from the regression analysis indicated that gender role diversity was unrelated to subgroup

formation, F (1, 76) = 2.29, ns (see Table 16). Therefore, the second mediational condition was

also not established.

Finally, the third mediational condition required the full or partial elimination of the

relationship between gender role diversity and VCM is removed when subgroup formation is

 

78  

controlled for. A hierarchical multiple regression analysis was conducted to test this condition.

Subgroup formation was entered as the first predictor in Step 1 of the model, and gender role

diversity as the second predictor in Step 2. Results from the hierarchical regression analysis

noted that both subgroup formation and gender role diversity did not contribute significantly in

the final model, F (2, 75) = .51, ns (see Table 17). Considering these findings, Hypothesis 10

was therefore not supported.

Supplementary analyses

Given the preliminary, exploratory and main findings reported above, I also conducted

additional supplementary analyses to empirically assess the presence of the various VCM

profiles uncovered in the content coding process, using a series of latent class analyses. Based on

the categories identified in the latent class analyses, I further explored possible group differences

among these VCM categories on the three key outcome variables involved in this study, i.e.,

team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and

team effectiveness.

Assessing VCM profiles using latent class analyses

In addition to the qualitative content coding analyses described earlier, a series of latent

class analyses (LCA) was also conducted to examine whether the distinctly different

distributional patterns of conflict management in the teams, i.e., the proposed VCM profiles, may

be observed in the teams sampled. Lawrence and Zyphur (2011) recently argued and

demonstrated the utility of using LCA for identifying and measuring organizational faultlines in

work groups. More importantly, they argued that LCA may be particularly useful when “the

central concept involves a profile, such as a profile of individuals’ personality traits, a profile of

high- versus low-performing groups or a profile of the network attributes of industries” (p. 52).

 

79  

LCA can be used as a measurement model to uncover latent clusters or distinct subgroups that

may exist in teams (Lawrence & Zyphur, 2011). As such, it is expected that the LCA technique

should be a useful analytical tool for uncovering the presence of different VCM profiles in

teams.

To conduct the LCAs, I first created eight binary indicators or items (with values of “0”

and “1”), based on all the VCM profile categories identified in the content coding analysis. For

example, one of the eight items would represent the minimum cooperative VCM profile, with

teams characterizing this profile coded as “1” and teams that do not coded as “0”. When it comes

to the “other” VCM profile category, three items were created, with each representing one of the

three types identified in the content coding analysis: ‘distributed,’ ‘multiple clusters,’ and

‘midpoint cluster.’

A series of LCAs were then conducted to compare models with two- through eight-class

solutions using the software, Mplus Version 6.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998 – 2010). The two-

class solution represents the simplest possible solution that aligns with Deutsch’s (1949, 1973)

one-dimensional conceptual framework of cooperative and competitive conflict management,

while the eight-class solution represents the maximum possible number of latent classes

identified in the content coding analysis.

Overall, the LCA literature recommends using a combination of information criteria and

fit indexes (or likelihood-based tests) in determining the most appropriate number of latent

classes in LCA modeling (Finch & Bronk, 2011; Nylund, Asparouhov & Muthén, 2007).

Statistical Information Criteria (IC), such as the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC), the

Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) and the Sample-size Adjusted Bayesian Information

Criterion (aBIC), are typical indices used to guide the decision on the number of latent classes

 

80  

identified in LCA modeling (Nylund, et al., 2007). Generally, the lower the value of these

indices, the better the model fit. Of the three, aBIC has been found to be superior to the other two

ICs in LCA modeling simulation studies (Yang, 2006).

The Lo-Mendell-Rubin adjusted Likelihood Test (LMRT) and the Bootstrap Likelihood

Ratio Test (BLRT) are two recommended fit indexes used to perform significance testing in

evaluating model fit (Nylund et al., 2007). The LMRT compares two neighboring class models

and determines whether there is improvement in model fit with the addition of one more class.

When the test value for LMRT is significant at the p-value of .05, this indicates that the model

with k classes fits the data better than the model with k – 1 classes. As for the BLRT, this test

uses bootstrap samples to estimate log likelihood difference distribution between the k – 1 and k

class models (Nylund et al., 2007). Similar to the LMRT, when the test value for the BLRT is

significant at p-value of .05, this also suggests that the k class model is sufficient in fitting the

observed data compared to the k – 1 class model.

Table 18 shows the fit statistics for the various model comparisons in the LCAs. Based

on the fit statistics, the model with a four-class solution appears to fit the data best. The four-

class model solution has the second lowest aBIC value, coupled with both significant LMRT and

BLRT results. Both the significant LMRT and BLRT results suggest that four (k) classes fit the

observed data better than three (k - 1) classes.

Figure 6 shows a graph depicting the estimated conditional probabilities for the four-class

solution. The x-axis represents the eight binary items, while the y-axis represents the range of

conditional probabilities for each class. Each line on the graph represents a distinct latent class

identified in the analysis. The estimated conditional probabilities are specific to each class and

provide information on the probability of each team as belonging in that class. For example, the

 

81  

conditional probability for Class 1 is 26.6%, and this indicates that 26.6% of all the teams are

most likely to belong to Class 1 rather than to the other three classes.

Table 19 shows how the various proposed VCM profiles corresponded with the four

distinct latent classes identified in the analysis (for a graphical depiction, see Figure 7). The

“Count” and “Proportions” columns refer to the number and percentage of teams that are

classified into each latent class respectively. This four-class model solution identified four

distinct latent classes or categories, three of which aligned with the VCM profiles identified in

the content coding analysis. These three categories were moderate cooperative VCM, moderate

competitive VCM, and ‘distributed’ profiles. Consistent with earlier observations in the content

coding analysis, the highest number of teams also had moderate cooperative VCM profiles. The

next most prevalent profiles were moderate competitive VCM and ‘distributed’ profiles. The last

latent class identified in the analysis consisted of all the teams that were described as having

minimum cooperative VCM, maximum VCM, minimum competitive VCM, ‘multiple clusters,’

and ‘midpoint cluster’ profiles (for simplicity, I refer to this class as the ‘combination’ category).

There were 17 such teams in this category, and together, they made up 26.6% of all the teams

examined.

The “Threshold estimate” column in Table 7 refers to the threshold values of teams

belonging to the binary items for the four-class model. Larger positive threshold values indicate

a lower likelihood of teams belonging to a given item (i.e., having a value of “1” instead of “0”

for that item), while large negative values indicate the opposite (Finch & Bronk, 2011). Three of

the four latent classes each had threshold estimates of -15.000. These estimates indicated the

highest possible likelihood of teams belonging to each class or VCM profile.

Exploring group differences among VCM categories on team outcomes

 

82  

VCM categories on team conflict efficacy. Using the four VCM categories identified in

the LCAs, I then proceeded to explore possible group differences among these four categories in

terms of their effects on the three team outcomes: team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction

with the team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness. Preliminary analyses were

conducted to assess the assumptions of homoscedasticity, independence, linearity, normality, and

multicollinearity, and it was found that the distributions of team conflict efficacy scores for

teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles, W (10) = .26, p < .01, and teams with profiles

that fall under the ‘combination’ category were significantly non-normal, W (21) = .89, p < .05.

As a result, the Kruskall-Wallis Test and a series of Mann-Whitney U tests were used to assess

group differences in team conflict efficacy scores among the four VCM categories identified in

the latent class analyses. On the other hand, group differences in member satisfaction with the

team’s conflict management process and team effectiveness scores among the four VCM

categories were explored using one-way between-groups ANOVAs.

Findings from the Kruskall-Wallis test showed that there was no significant overall

difference in team conflict efficacy scores across all four VCM categories: H (3) = 5.74, ns (see

Table 20). To follow-up on this finding, dummy variables were created to compare each of the

four VCM categories with one another in six Mann-Whitney U tests. Two of the six Mann-

Whitney U test findings were significant: Teams with profiles under the ‘combination’ category

(i.e., minimum cooperative, maximum, minimum competitive, ‘multiple clusters,’ and ‘midpoint

cluster’; Mdn = 19.25, n = 21) reported significantly higher levels of team conflict efficacy than

teams with moderate competitive VCM profiles (Mdn = 18.58, n = 10), U = 54.00, z = -2.16, p <

.05, r = .39; Teams with the moderate cooperative VCM profiles (Mdn = 19.71 n = 37) also

 

83  

reported significantly higher levels of team conflict efficacy than teams with moderate

competitive VCM profiles (Mdn = 18.58, n = 10), U = 101.00, z = -2.26, p < .05, r = .33.

VCM categories on member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process.

Results from the one-way ANOVA revealed that there was no significant group difference in

member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process scores across all four VCM

categories, F (3, 75) = .29, ns (see Table 21).

VCM categories on team effectiveness. The ANOVA findings showed that there was no

significant overall group difference in team effectiveness scores across all four VCM categories

as well, F (3, 75) = 1.03, ns (see Table 21).

Table 22 provides a summary of all the findings reported in the main and supplementary

analyses in this chapter.

 

84  

CHAPTER V: DISCUSSION

The overall purpose of this dissertation study was to empirically test Tan’s (2011)

conceptual model, which proposes a new team-level construct of conflict management, known as

variant conflict management (VCM). In this model, VCM is defined as the degree and pattern of

differences in the ways individual members handle conflicts cooperatively and/or competitively

among themselves within teams. Further, VCM is posited to consist of five distributional

patterns or ‘profiles’ that characterize member differences in conflict handling: minimum

cooperative, minimum competitive, moderate cooperative, moderate competitive and maximum

VCM profiles. Tan’s (2011) model also suggests that factors within three antecedent categories,

namely salient conflict-relevant member characteristics, team contextual determinants and

divergent team dynamics, will predict the development of VCM. The model further notes that the

various VCM profiles are likely to exert differential effects on key team outcomes, such as

member satisfaction and team effectiveness; the effects of VCM (based on teams’ standard

deviation scores) are also likely to be above and beyond those of traditional concepts of team

conflict management, i.e., homogeneous or means-based team conflict management (Tan, 2011).

This study sought to accomplish four key research objectives. The first objective was to

assess whether the five proposed VCM profiles (minimum cooperative, minimum competitive,

moderate cooperative, moderate competitive and maximum VCM profiles) are evident in the

student project teams sampled. Based on findings from the qualitative content coding analyses, it

was observed that within-team differences in members’ conflict management approaches could

indeed be organized into distinct configurations aligned with the five VCM profile types

proposed in Tan’s (2011) model. Moreover, it was also noted that among the teams sampled, a

majority of them exhibited moderate cooperative VCM profiles, followed by moderate

 

85  

competitive, minimum cooperative, maximum, and minimum competitive VCM profiles. Such

frequency distributions of VCM profiles are understandable given the nature of the sampled

teams. Since this study involved teams comprised of students who needed to rely on one another

in order to complete their team projects, it is reasonable to expect that team members would be

relatively cooperative with one another when handling team conflict situations in the process of

working on their projects. Additionally, the interdependent nature of the team task would also

preclude most teams from developing minimum competitive VCM profiles, since it would be

challenging for highly competitive members to work together effectively toward their team

goals.

Aside from the five VCM profiles discussed above, the content coding analyses also

revealed other VCM profiles that were not included in Tan’s (2011) conceptual model. This set

of VCM profiles were tentatively described as ‘distributed,’ ‘multiple clusters,’ and ‘midpoint

cluster.’ Teams with the ‘distributed’ VCM profiles were characterized by members who were

relatively spread out along the conflict management continuum; in these teams, no discernible

clusters were observed along the continuum as well. As for teams with the ‘multiple clusters’

VCM profiles, these were characterized by the presence of smaller subgroups or clusters that

were distributed along the conflict management continuum; no single majority subgroup was

observed in such profiles. When it comes to teams described with the ‘midpoint cluster’ VCM

profiles, these were characterized by all members being co-located around the midpoint of the

conflict management continuum.

One possible explanation for the emergence of the ‘distributed’ and ‘multiple clusters’

VCM profiles could be that team members were not subject to relatively strong situational or

team-level pressures that would sway them to act more similarly with most of their teammates

 

86  

(Meyer, et al., 2010; Mischel 1977). As a result, members in such teams may be free to rely on

their own natural proclivities in conflict management when conflict situations arise in their

teams, thus leading to more diversity or spreading out of individual conflict approaches within

the teams. More research would certainly be needed to better understand the conditions under

and mechanisms through which ‘distributed’ and ‘multiple clusters’ VCM profiles are likely to

occur.

Further, the presence of ‘midpoint cluster’ VCM profiles among some of the teams

observed suggests that individual members may not always veer toward the cooperative or

competitive end of the one-dimensional conflict management continuum, as postulated in Tan’s

(2011) model. Rather, it is possible for teams to comprise of all members whose preferences

involve using conflict management strategies that lie somewhere in between cooperative and

competitive conflict management. Such conflict management strategies also parallel the concepts

of compromising, accommodating or obliging styles of conflict management, as discussed in

other existing conflict management theories or typologies (e.g., Rahim & Bonoma, 1979;

Thomas & Kilmann, 1974). As such, the presence of ‘midpoint cluster’ VCM profiles in teams

suggests that the proposed VCM theory would also require more development and refinement in

order to better explain and predict the possibilities of such distributional patterns in conflict

management approaches among team members.

From an empirical standpoint, the supplementary latent class analyses (LCAs) provided

further validation to some of the VCM profiles proposed in the model, as well as the need to

consider additional profiles in the model. Specifically, the LCAs identified two of the proposed

VCM profiles, i.e., moderate cooperative and moderate competitive VCM profiles, as distinct

latent classes in the data. The new ‘distributed’ profile was also identified as a distinct latent

 

87  

class in the LCAs, thus supporting the discovery of this profile in the content coding analyses.

The LCA results were also consistent with those from the content coding analyses in noting that

the moderate cooperative VCM profiles were most prevalent in the teams sampled.

The LCA findings supported the conclusion that other profiles that are not currently

included in Tan’s (2011) model may be present in teams as well. In particular, the analyses

revealed a “combination” category that encompasses three of the proposed VCM profiles (i.e.,

minimum cooperative, maximum and minimum competitive VCM profiles) and two of the new

profiles (i.e., ‘multiple clusters’ and ‘midpoint cluster’) identified in the content coding. While

these findings may, in part, be due to the small sample sizes associated with these profiles, they

nonetheless indicate the need for future research to assess the possibility of new profiles that may

emerge by using larger team samples in comparable settings.

The second research objective of this study was to examine whether the overall degree of

variability in team members’ conflict management approaches, i.e., VCM based on teams’

standard deviation scores, would predict team outcomes (team conflict efficacy, members’

satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and team effectiveness) above and

beyond conventional measures of team conflict management, i.e., cooperative and competitive

team conflict management based on group means’ scores respectively. Preliminary correlational

analyses showed that consistent with prior research (e.g., Alper et al., 2000), mean cooperative

team conflict management was positively associated with team outcomes, i.e., team conflict

efficacy, member satisfaction and team effectiveness. In the main hypothesis tests, however,

these effects of cooperative team conflict management on team conflict efficacy and member

satisfaction were removed when control variables, such as team goal interdependence and

conflict intensity, were included. Such findings also supported earlier research in noting that the

 

88  

effects of mean cooperative team conflict management on team outcomes are, in part, dependent

on other factors such as goal interdependence (e.g., Alper et al., 1998) and conflict intensity

(e.g., Barker et al., 1988).

When it comes to competitive team conflict management, however, findings from both

the correlations and main hypothesis tests contradicted those of prior research (e.g., Alper et al.,

2000; Tjosvold, Law & Sun, 2006): mean competitive team conflict management was found to

be uncorrelated with team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction, and team effectiveness. This

absence of effects may be explained by the relatively cooperative nature of the teams sampled.

Since the teams in this study were found to be generally cooperative, the individual and team

scores obtained on the competitive conflict management sub-scale were therefore likely to be

lower (i.e., less competitive) compared to other teams sampled in earlier studies. As such, the

relatively cooperative team sample in this study would also result in a reduced likelihood of

detecting significant effects of competitive team conflict management on team outcomes.

As for overall VCM, the correlational analyses indicated that it was uncorrelated with

team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management process, and

team effectiveness. Overall VCM was also uncorrelated with cooperative and competitive team

conflict management. Results from the main hypothesis tests also indicated that it did not predict

all three team outcome variables above and beyond mean cooperative or competitive team

conflict management. This lack of effects of overall VCM on team outcomes, above and beyond

those of mean team conflict management may be partly due to the fact that the same measure

was used to assess the team conflict management and VCM variables. Since the conflict

management measure was adapted to assess individual perceptions about individuals’ conflict

handling approaches within the team, rather than individual perceptions about the team’s conflict

 

89  

handling approaches as a whole, this adjustment may have affected the construct validity of team

conflict management as well (Chan, 1998). As such, future research should attempt to assess the

team conflict management and VCM constructs using separate and different measures or

instruments. Other reasons that may explain the absence of effects of overall VCM on team

outcomes include the generally cooperative nature of the teams sampled and the collectivistic

cultural context (Singapore) in which these teams were embedded. Such team and contextual

characteristics are thus likely to lower the degree of overall variability in members’ conflict

management approaches, and as such, also reduce the likelihood of finding significant effects of

overall VCM on team outcomes.

Additionally, the lack of associations between overall VCM and team outcomes also

suggests that VCM may be better construed as a categorical rather than a continuous variable.

This observation is further supported by the significant associations between specific VCM

profiles and the various team outcomes examined in the planned comparisons. Findings from the

planned comparisons and correlational analyses seem to suggest that how variability in

members’ conflict management strategies are organized within their teams, i.e., VCM profiles,

may be associated with distinctly different dynamics and interaction patterns (e.g., different

numbers of subgroups formed or the amount of intense conflicts experienced) that, in turn, can

produce differential effects on important team outcomes. When only the impact on the overall

degree of members’ conflict management differences within their teams, i.e., overall VCM, was

examined, then, it is possible that the distinctly different dynamics and processes associated with

different VCM profiles would have been masked or overlooked in the analyses, thus leading to

possible ‘canceling out’ of opposing effects in the findings.

 

90  

Accordingly, the definition of the VCM construct should be revised to indicate only the

patterns, and not the degree, of differences in which members handle conflicts within their

teams.

The third research objective of this study was to compare the effects of various VCM

profiles on team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team’s conflict management

process, and team effectiveness. Results from the study revealed that teams with minimum

cooperative or moderate cooperative VCM profiles reported higher levels of conflict efficacy

than those with moderate competitive VCM profiles. Teams with minimum competitive VCM

profiles reported the lowest levels of member satisfaction, when compared to teams with the

other four VCM profiles proposed in Tan’s (2011) model. Teams with minimum cooperative

VCM profiles were found to be more effective than those with minimum competitive VCM

profiles. Teams with moderate cooperative profiles were also more effective than those with

minimum competitive VCM profiles, as well as those with moderate competitive VCM profiles.

As for comparisons between teams with maximum and moderate competitive VCM profiles, no

significant effects were observed between these two types of teams on all three outcomes.

Taken altogether, it can be concluded that these findings on various VCM profiles’

impact on team outcomes also paralleled prior research on the effects of cooperative and

competitive conflict management in teams (e.g., Alper et al., 2000; Tjosvold, Law & Sun, 2006).

Overall, teams with VCM profiles that are more cooperative in nature (i.e., minimum

cooperative and moderate cooperative VCM profiles) are more likely to experience positive team

outcomes (i.e., higher levels of team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction with the team conflict

management process, and team effectiveness), when compared to those with VCM profiles that

 

91  

are more competitive in nature (i.e., minimum competitive and moderate competitive VCM

profiles).

As for the absence of effects comparing maximum and moderate competitive VCM

profiles, this may be due to the relatively small group sizes involving teams with these two

profile types. It was also observed in the data, based on the qualitative content coding analyses,

that some of the teams that were coded as having maximum VCM profiles may not have enough

“distance” between the opposing subgroups within them. In other words, the two subgroups in

these teams may not be far apart enough for opposing cooperative and competitive dynamics to

truly emerge and significantly influence team outcomes. Future investigations should attempt to

identify large enough samples of teams with “true” maximum VCM profiles and to replicate the

comparison study of how these teams may differ from those with moderate competitive VCM

profiles in affecting team outcomes.

Last but not least, the fourth research objective of this study was to investigate whether

gender role diversity, team goal interdependence and subgroup formation are likely to predict the

development of VCM in teams. Contrary to the hypotheses, all three predictor variables were

found to be unrelated to VCM in both the correlations and main hypothesis tests. For the lack of

association between gender role diversity and VCM, one possible explanation may be that at the

team level, differences in members’ gender role orientations among two or more members within

the teams were simply not salient enough to affect team functioning and outcomes. The

relatively low standard deviation and variance scores obtained for gender role diversity (SD =

.15, S2 = .02) in Table 1 also suggest that there was limited variability in this variable across the

teams sampled, thus perhaps making the construct less salient than expected. Prior studies have

suggested that saliency in member differences may play a role in determining whether diversity

 

92  

in member attributes affect important outcomes (e.g., Zellmer-Bruhn, Maloney, Bhappu, et al.,

2010). Further research should therefore examine whether team members perceive salient

differences in gender role orientations among one another in their teams, and in turn, whether

such salient differences influence team outcomes.

The absence of relationship between team goal interdependence and VCM was

surprising, given the consistently strong associations between goal interdependence and conflict

management that have been noted in past research (e.g., Tjosvold, 1998). This absence of

associations may be traced back to the earlier conclusion that the VCM construct would be better

conceived as a categorical rather than a continuous variable. In a similar vein, possible opposing

effects of cooperative and competitive goal interdependence on overall VCM may have been

masked by the continuous nature of the VCM construct conceptualized here. Future research

should therefore examine whether team goal interdependence would significantly predict the

various VCM profiles instead.

As for the lack of evidence supporting subgroup formation as a mediator in the gender

role diversity-VCM relationship, this may in part be due to the ‘generic’ nature of the subgroup

formation measure. Since the measure items only surveyed for the presence and nature of

subgroups in the teams, it was unclear whether the subgroups formed were, in fact, relevant or

specific to the team conflicts experienced within the teams. Future research should adapt the

subgroup formation measure such that it is conflict-specific. Additional research is also needed

to identify other potential mediating factors that may influence possible antecedent-VCM

linkages.

Theoretical implications

 

93  

From a theoretical perspective, this study contributed to existing theory on conflict

management by examining Tan’s (2011) VCM model, which is the first theoretical model that

was formulated to describe and explain conflict management at the team level. Study results also

provided initial support for the presence of VCM profiles as proposed in Tan’s (2011) model,

corroborated prior research on team conflict management and on existing conflict management

theories, as well as provided insights into possible extensions or refinements of the VCM model

with the discovery of ‘new’ VCM profiles.

Most notably, findings from this study offered preliminary support to the argument that

the present study of team conflict management based on teams’ means scores limits our

understanding of the phenomenon. While some of the findings did support prior research

regarding the impact of mean cooperative and competitive team conflict management on team

outcomes (e.g., Alper et al., 2000; Tjosvold, Law & Sun, 2006), it was also observed that

individual members do not always converge or share similar conflict management approaches

within their teams. This divergence or diversity of individual differences in conflict management

approaches was evidenced by the presence of distinct VCM profiles uncovered in both the

content coding and latent class analyses in this study. Findings from this study also indicated that

different VCM profiles could exert differential effects on important outcomes, such as team

conflict efficacy, member satisfaction and team effectiveness. The uncovering of ‘new’ VCM

profiles in these analyses also indicates the need to further refine or extend the proposed VCM

model.

From the study results, it was also inferred that VCM might be better conceptualized in

terms of the distributional patterns of individual conflict management approaches within teams

(i.e., VCM profiles), rather than the extent of such differences within teams (i.e., standard

 

94  

deviations-based VCM). This inference is supported by the fact that some of the VCM profiles

were found to influence critical team outcomes differently, whereas VCM, when based on team

standard-deviation scores, did not have a significant effect on the same team outcomes

examined. Accordingly, assessing the degree to which members differ in the ways they handle

team conflicts alone is insufficient in advancing our knowledge in team conflict management.

Researchers need to explore beyond shared team conflict management, and to further assess the

patterns in which differences in members’ conflict management approaches are or can be

manifested within teams.

Research implications

  From an empirical standpoint, this study was one of the first to examine the presence of

variability in individual conflict management at the team level, how such variability may impact

team outcomes differently from traditional concepts of team conflict management, as well as

how such variability may form distinct and meaningful patterns that have consequential effects

on important team outcomes. Furthermore, this study also investigated whether gender role

diversity, team goal interdependence, and subgroup formation predicted variability in individual

conflict management within teams.

By examining these relationships, this study contributed to our current knowledge of

team conflict management with preliminary evidence supporting the presence of member

differences in conflict management within teams, in terms of distinct configurations, that could,

in turn, exert varying effects on team conflict efficacy, member satisfaction and team

effectiveness. The study findings also indicated that there are nuanced differences in terms of

how cooperative or competitive conflict management in teams may influence certain team

outcomes. For instance, it was noted that teams with minimum cooperative VCM profiles were

 

95  

more effective than teams with minimum competitive VCM profiles, but not those with moderate

competitive VCM profiles. On the other hand, teams with moderate cooperative VCM profiles

were more effective than both teams with minimum and moderate competitive VCM profiles.

What, then, are some reasons that may account for such differences?

One possible direction to explore would be the interplays among within-team conflicts

(especially intense and important ones), team goal interdependence, and subgroup formation, in

the VCM profile-team outcome linkages. Based on the correlational findings, it was noted that

both cooperative and competitive conflict management were strongly correlated with team goal

interdependence, subgroup formation, within-team conflict and conflict intensity. Specifically,

these correlations suggest that cooperative teams are likely to have more positive interdependent

goals among members, fewer opposing subgroups, less within-team conflicts (especially intense

ones), and improved eventual team outcomes. By contrast, competitive teams are likely to have

more negative interdependent goals among members, more conflicting subgroups, and increased

within-team conflicts that are also experienced as intense and important. As such, it may be

inferred from these correlations that whether a team’s VCM profile is relatively cooperative or

competitive in nature and how the nature of the profile is patterned (i.e., whether there is a

cooperative or competitive majority subgroup, or no majority subgroups at all) can significantly

influence the team’s dynamics and processes, which can in turn lead to improvements or

detriments on team functioning and effectiveness. Accordingly, these observations and

conclusions help extend our current understanding about the mechanisms and factors that explain

how individual differences in conflict management within teams affect team process and outputs.

Practical implications

 

96  

As for practice-related implications, findings from this study may be able to help

managers, team leaders or self-managed teams better understand how differences in the ways

their team members handle conflict situations can significantly impact effective team processes

and outcomes. For example, a team may recognize that when its members are organized into a

cooperative majority subgroup and a competitive minority subgroup in their conflict

management approaches; this configuration may actually facilitate its performance compared to

when all its members are highly competitive in handling conflict situations.

Furthermore, by identifying specific VCM profiles that are likely to enhance rather than

undermine effectiveness or team functioning, managers, team leaders or self-managed teams

may also be better able to steer their teams away from more ‘detrimental’ VCM profiles or

promote the development of more ‘productive’ ones in their teams. Encouraging a team to shift

from a moderate competitive VCM profile to a minimum cooperative or moderate cooperative

VCM profile by providing opportunities for team members to learn and apply more cooperative

conflict management skills, for instance, may in turn help increase the team’s effectiveness.

Last but not least, results from this study may also help inform managers, team leaders or

self-managed teams about specific factors that are likely to influence the development of specific

VCM profiles in teams. For example, promoting positive interdependent goals among its

members and discouraging the formation of member ‘cliques’ or subgroups may help reduce the

likelihood of the team developing a minimum competitive or moderate competitive VCM

profile.

Study limitations

As with all research, this study was also subject to a number of limitations. As a cross-

sectional field study, it is important to note that the results obtained in this study only indicate

 

97  

correlational, and not causal, relationships among the variables examined. The teams sampled in

this study were also comprised of student project teams with relatively young members, and with

clear team goals that existed for a relatively short period of time (i.e., an academic semester).

Although such a sample may suggest limited generalizability of the results, prior research has

shown that studies using student teams often found comparable findings to those using

organizational teams (see Van Vianen & De Dreu, 2001).

Since the teams involved in this study were identified based on available member

responses, and that a considerable number of the teams included in the analyses were not

complete teams, the variance of scores within the teams may have been restricted, and thereby

reducing the likelihood of detecting more significant effects. The sizes of teams sampled were

also relatively small (i.e., 3 to 8). Future investigations should attempt to use complete intact

teams and with larger team samples.

The use of unequal and relatively small group sizes used in the group comparison

analyses among VCM profiles, and the application of less powerful non-parametric tests also

suggest a lower likelihood in detecting significant effects. As such, the results obtained from

these analyses should also be interpreted with caution.

Future research directions

Considering the findings obtained in this study, there are numerous questions that may be

answered and addressed in future research. For instance, researchers should attempt to further

refine Tan’s (2011) VCM theory, given the discovery of other possible VCM profiles in teams.

Specifically, one may develop additional theorizing to explain the occurrence and impact of the

‘distributed,’ ‘multiple clusters,’ and ‘midpoint cluster’ VCM profiles.

 

98  

Researchers should also consider developing new measures or methods to assess VCM

profiles instead of relying on existing survey instruments. For example, using objective

measures, such as behavioral observations of participants’ conflict management approaches in

teams over time, may help reduce potential self-report biases and better assess the presence and

nature of VCM profiles formed within teams. The use of other research designs, such as

experiments and longitudinal surveys, may also help better establish the validity and causality of

relationships among the VCM construct, various VCM profiles, with their potential predictors

and outcomes.

Other variables representing each of the three antecedent categories (viz., salient conflict-

relevant member characteristics, team contextual determinants, and divergent team dynamics) of

VCM and its profiles should also be identified and examined in future investigations. For

example, investigators may want to consider examining the impact of cultural diversity among

team members, as another possible indicator of salient member characteristics, on various VCM

profiles. Cultural differences among members within teams have been found to influence team

conflict management (Boros et al., 2010), and as such, these differences may also be likely to

influence patterns of variability in how members manage internal team conflicts.

Future studies should also look into using organizational team samples or other types of

teams in various organizational settings, such that the predictors and outcomes of VCM profiles

may be better understood across a broader range of teams and contexts.

Conclusion

As the formation and use of work teams continue to accelerate in contemporary

organizations, the need to understand the conditions and mechanisms that can either enhance or

undermine effective team conflict management will undoubtedly also increase in both

 

99  

importance and relevance to organizational scholars, managers and team leaders. To that end, I

urge both current and future researchers to advance the VCM theory, by taking on the challenges

of further examining the nature and impact of VCM profiles. It is with much hope that we may

begin to take further strides on our journey to deepen our knowledge of team conflict

management, particularly as it relates to enriching our understanding of how differences in

individual conflict handling approaches may develop and in turn affect team process and

important outcomes.

 

100  

REFERENCES

Abrams, D., Wetherell, M., Cochrane, S., Hogg, M. A., & Turner, J. C. (1990). Knowing what to think by knowing who you are: Self-categorization and the nature of norm formation, conformity and group polarization. British Journal of Social Psychology, 29(2), 97-119.

Alderfer, C. P. (1977). Group and intergroup relations. In J. R. Hackman & J. L. Suttle (Eds.), Improving the quality of work life (pp. 227-296). Santa Monica, CA: Goodyear.

Alper, S., Tjosvold, D., & Law, K. S. (1998). Interdependence and controversy in group decision making: Antecedents to effective self-managing teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 74(1), 33-52.

Alper, S., Tjosvold, D., & Law, K. S. (2000). Conflict management, efficacy and performance in organizational teams. Personnel Psychology, 53(3), 625-642.

Andrews, I. R., & Tjosvold, D. (1983). Conflict management under different levels of conflict intensity. Journal of Occupational Behavior, 4(3), 223-228.

Antonioni, D. (1998). Relationship between the Big Five personality factors and conflict management styles. International Journal of Conflict Management, 9(4), 336-355. doi: 10.1108/eb022814

Asch, S. E. (1952). Some forms of interpersonal influence. In G. E. Swanson, T. M. Newcomb & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in social psychology. New York, NY: Holt.

Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and submission to group pressure: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70(9), Whole No. 416.

Axelrod, R. (1984). The evolution of cooperation. New York, NY: Basic Books.

Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction process analysis: A method for the study of small groups. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley Press.

Bantel, K. A., & Jackson, S. E. (1989). Top management and innovations in banking: Does the composition of the top team make a difference? Strategic Management Journal, 10(S1), 107-124. doi: 10.1002/smj.4250100709

Barker, J., Tjosvold, D., & Andrews, I. R. (1988). Conflict approaches of effective and ineffective project managers: A field study in a matrix organization. Journal of Management Studies, 25(2), 167-178. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6486.1988.tb00030.x

Baron, R. S. (1989). Personality and organizational conflict: Effects of the type a behavior pattern and self-monitoring. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 44(2), 281-296.

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173-1182.

 

101  

Baxter, L. A. (1982). Conflict management: An episodic approach. Small Group Research, 13(1), 23-42. doi: 10.1177/104649648201300102

Behfar, K. J., Peterson, R. S., Mannix, E. A., & Trochim, W. M. K. (2008). The critical role of conflict resolution in teams: A close look at the links between conflict type, conflict management strategies, and team outcomes. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93(1), 170-188. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.93.1.170

Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42(2), 155-162. doi: 10.1037/h0036215

Bem, S. L. (1981). Bem Sex Role Inventory: A professional manual. Palo Alto, Ca: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Bettenhausen, K. L., & Murnighan, J. K. (1985). The emergence of norms in competitive decision-making groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 30, 350-372.

Bettenhausen, K. L., & Murnighan, J. K. (1991). The development of an intragroup norm and the effects of interpersonal and structural challenges. Administrative Science Quarterly, 36(1), 20-35.

Blake, R. R., & Mouton, J. S. (1964). The managerial grid. Houston, TX: Gulf.

Blau, P. M. (1977). Inequality and heterogeneity: A primitive theory of social structure. New York, NY: Free Press.

Bliese, P. D. (2000). Within-group agreement, non-independence, and reliability: Implications for data aggregation and analysis. In K. J. Klein & S. W. J. Kozlowski (Eds.), Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organizations: Foundations, extensions, and new directions. (pp. 349-381): San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Boros, S., Meslec, N., Curseu, P. L., & Emons, W. (2010). Struggles for cooperation: conflict resolution strategies in multicultural groups. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 25(5), 539.

Brewer, M. B. (1993). Social identity, distinctiveness, and in-group homogeneity. Social Cognition, 11(1), 150-164. doi: 10.1521/soco.1993.11.1.150

Brewer, M. B. (1995). Managing diversity: The role of social identities. In S. E. Jackson & M. N. Ruderman (Eds.), Diversity in work teams: Research paradigms for a changing workplace. (pp. 47-68). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Brewer, M. B., & Brown, R. J. (1998). Intergroup relations. In D. T. Gilbert & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 554–594). New York: McGraw-Hill.

Brewer, M. B., & Kramer, R. M. (1986). Choice behavior in social dilemmas: Effects of social identity, group size, and decision framing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(3), 543-549. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.3.543

 

102  

Brewer, N., Mitchell, P., & Weber, N. (2002). Gender role, organizational status, and conflict management styles. International Journal of Conflict Management, 13(1), 78.

Byrne, D. (1971). The attraction paradigm. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Chan, D. (1998). Functional relations among constructs in the same content domain at different levels of analysis: A typology of composition models. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(2), 234-246.

Chatman, J. A. (2010). Norms in mixed sex and mixed race work groups. Academy of Management Annals, 4, 447 - 484.

Chen, G., Liu, C., & Tjosvold, D. (2005). Conflict management for effective top management teams and innovation in China. Journal of Management Studies, 42(2), 277-300.

Chen, G., & Tjosvold, D. (2002a). Conflict management and team effectiveness in China: The mediating role of justice. Asia Pacific Journal of Management, 19(4), 557-572. doi: 10.1023/a:1020573710461

Chen, G., & Tjosvold, D. (2002b). Cooperative goals and constructive controversy for promoting innovation in student groups in China. Journal of Education for Business, 78(1), 46.

Chen, Y., Tjosvold, D., & Su, S. F. (2005). Goal interdependence for working across cultural boundaries: Chinese employees with foreign managers. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29(4), 429-447.

Chou, H.-W., & Yeh, Y.-J. (2007). Conflict, conflict management and performance in ERP teams. Social Behavior & Personality: An International Journal, 35(8), 1035-1047.

Cialdini, R. B., & Goldstein, N. J. (2004). Social Influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55(1), 591-621.

Cosier, R. A., & Dalton, D. (1990). Positive effects of conflict: A field assessment. International Journal of Conflict Management, 1, 81-92.

Cronin, M. A., Bezrukova, K., Weingart, L. R., & Tinsley, C. H. (2011). Subgroups within a team: The role of cognitive and affective integration. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 32(6), 831-849. doi: 10.1002/job.707

Curhan, J. R., Elfenbein, H. A., & Xu, H. (2006). What do people value when they negotiate? Mapping the domain of subjective value in negotiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 493-512. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.493

Davidson, J. A., McElwee, G., & Hannan, G. (2004). Trust and power as determinants of conflict resolution strategy and outcome satisfaction. Peace & Conflict, 10(3), 275-292.

De Dreu, C. K. W., & Gelfand, M. J. (Eds.). (2008). The psychology of conflict and conflict management in organizations. London, England and New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

 

103  

De Dreu, C. K. W., & Van Vianen, A. E. M. (2001). Managing relationship conflict and the effectiveness of organizational teams. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22(3), 309-328. doi: 10.1002/job.71

DeChurch, L. A., & Marks, M. A. (2001). Maximizing the benefits of task conflict: The role of conflict management. International Journal of Conflict Management, 12(1), 4-22. doi: 10.1108/eb022847

DeChurch, L. A., Hamilton, K. L., & Haas, C. (2007). Effects of conflict management strategies on perceptions of intragroup conflict. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 11(1), 66-78. doi: 10.1037/1089-2699.11.1.66

Desivilya, H. S., & Eizen, D. (2005). Conflict management in work teams: The role of social self-efficacy and group identification. International Journal of Conflict Management, 16(2), 183-208.

Desivilya, H. S., & Yagil, D. (2005). The role of emotions in conflict management: The case of work teams. International Journal of Conflict Management, 16(1), 55-69.

Deutsch, M. (1949). A theory of cooperation and competition. Human Relations, 2, 129-152.

Deutsch, M. (1973). The resolution of conflict: Constructive and destructive processes. New Haven, NY: Yale University Press.

Deutsch, M. (1985). Interdependence and psychological orientation. Distributive justice: A social-psychological perspective. New Haven, NY: Yale University Press.

Deutsch, M. (2006). Cooperation and competition. In M. Deutsch, P. T. Coleman & E. C. Marcus (Eds.), The handbook of conflict resolution: Theory and practice (pp. 1-42). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(3), 629-636.

Earley, P. C., & Mosakowski, E. (2000). Creating hybrid team cultures: An empirical test of transnational team functioning. Academy of Management Journal, 43(1), 26-50.

Farmer, S. M., & Roth, J. (1998). Conflict-handling behavior in work groups: Effects of group structure, decision processes, and time. Small Group Research, 29(6), 669-713. doi: 10.1177/1046496498296002

Feldman, D. C. (1984). The development and enforcement of group norms. Academy of Management Review, 9(1), 47-53.

Finch, W. H., & Bronk, K. C. (2011). Conducting confirmatory latent class analysis using Mplus. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 18(1), 132-151. doi: 10.1080/10705511.2011.532732

Gebhardt, L. J., & Meyers, R. A. (1995). Subgroup influence in decision-making groups. Small Group Research, 26(2), 147-168. doi: 10.1177/1046496495262001

 

104  

George, J. M., & Bettenhausen, K. (1990). Understanding prosocial behavior, sales performance, and turnover: A group-level analysis in a service context. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75(6), 698-709. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.75.6.698

Gersick, C. J. G. (1988). Time and transition in work teams: Toward a new model of group development. Academy of Management Journal, 31(1), 9-41.

Graziano, W. G., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Hair, E. (1996). Perceiving interpersonal conflict and reacting to it: The case for agreeableness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 820-835.

Greer, L. L., Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. (2008). Conflict transformation: A longitudinal investigation of the relationships between different types of intragroup conflict and the moderating role of conflict resolution. Small Group Research, 39(3), 278-302. doi: 10.1177/1046496408317793

Gross, M. A., & Guerrero, L. K. (2000). Managing conflict appropriately and effectively: An application of the competence model to Rahim's organizational conflict styles. International Journal of Conflict Management, 11(3), 200-226.

Hackman, J. R. (1976). Group influences on individuals in organizations. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology (pp. 1455-1525). Chicago, IL: Rand-McNall.

Harrison, D., & Klein, K. (2007). What's the difference? Diversity constructs as separation, variety, or disparity in organizations. The Academy of Management Review, 32(4), 1199-1228.

Harrison, D. A., Price, K. H., & Bell, M. P. (1998). Beyond relational demography: Time and the effects of surface- and deep-level diversity on work group cohesion. Academy of Management Journal, 41, 96-107.

Hempel, P. S., Zhang, Z. X., & Tjosvold, D. (2008). Conflict management between and within teams for trusting relationships and performance in China. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(1), 41-65.

Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. (2000). Social identity and self-categorization processes in organizational contexts. Academy of Management Review, 25(1), 121-140.

Holt, J. L., & DeVore, C. J. (2005). Culture, gender, organizational role, and styles of conflict resolution: A meta-analysis. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 29(2), 165-196.

Ilgen, D. R. (1999). Teams embedded in organizations: Some implications. American Psychologist, 54(2), 129-139. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.54.2.129

Isenberg, D. J. (1986). Group polarization: A critical review and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(6), 1141-1151. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.50.6.1141

 

105  

James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1984). Estimating within-group interrater reliability with and without response bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 69, 85-98.

James, L. R., Demaree, R. G., & Wolf, G. (1993). rwg: An assessment of within-group interrater agreement. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 306-309.

Janis, I. L. (1972). Victims of groupthink. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.

Janssen, O., Van de Vliert, E., & Veenstra, C. (1999). How task and person conflict shape the role of positive interdependence in management teams. Journal of Management, 25(2), 117-141.

Jarboe, S. C., & Witteman, H. R. (1996). Intragroup conflict management in task-oriented groups. Small Group Research, 27(2), 316-338. doi: 10.1177/1046496496272007

Jehn, K. A. (1997). A qualitative analysis of conflict types and dimensions in organizational groups. Administrative Science Quarterly, 42(3), 530-557.

Jehn, K. A., & Mannix, E. A. (2001). The dynamic nature of conflict: A longitudinal study of intragroup conflict and group performance. Academy of Management Journal, 44(2), 238-251.

Johnson, D. W. (2003). Social interdependence: Interrelationships among theory, research and practice. American Psychologist, 58(11), 934-945.

Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (1989). Cooperation and competition: Theory and research. Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company.

Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2005). New developments in social interdependence theory. Genetic, Social & General Psychology Monographs, 131(4), 285-358.

Johnson, D. W., Johnson, R. T., & Maruyama, G. (1983). Interdependence and interpersonal attraction among heterogeneous and homogeneous individuals: A theoretical formulation and a meta-analysis of the research. Review of Educational Research, 53(1), 5-54. doi: 10.3102/00346543053001005

Johnson, D. W., Maruyama, G., Johnson, R. T., Nelson, D., & Skon, S. (1981). Effects of cooperative, competitive, and individualistic goal structures on achievement: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 47-62.

Jones, R. E., & White, C. S. (1985). Relationships among personality, conflict resolution styles, and task effectiveness. Group & Organization Management, 10(2), 152-167. doi: 10.1177/105960118501000204

Jordan, P. J., & Troth, A. C. (2004). Managing emotions during team problem solving: Emotional intelligence and conflict resolution. Human Performance, 17(2), 195-218.

Jurma, W. E., & Powell, M. L. (1994). Perceived gender roles of managers and effective conflict management. Psychological Reports, 74(1), 104-106. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1994.74.1.104

Kanter, R. (1977). Men and women of the organization. New York, NY: Basic Books.

 

106  

Kelley, H. H., & Stahelski, A. J. (1970). Social interaction basis of cooperators' and competitors' beliefs about others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16(1), 66-91.

Kilmann, R. H., & Thomas, K. W. (1977). Developing a forced-choice measure of conflict-handling behavior: The "Mode" instrument. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 37(2), 309-325. doi: 10.1177/001316447703700204

Klein, K. J., Dansereau, F., & Hall, R. J. (1994). Levels issues in theory development, data collection, and analysis. Academy of Management Review, 19(2), 195-229.

Kleinman, G., Palmon, D., & Lee, P. (2003). The effects of personal and group level factors on the outcomes of simulate auditor and client teams. Group Decision and Negotiation, 12(1), 57-84.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Klein, K. J. (2000). A multilevel approach to theory and research in organizations: Contextual, temporal, and emergent processes Multilevel theory, research, and methods in organizations: Foundations, extensions, and new directions. (pp. 3-90): San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Kuhn, T., & Poole, S. (2000). Do conflict management styles affect group decision making? Evidence from a longitudinal field study. Human Communication Research, 26(4), 558-590.

Lau, D. C., & Murnighan, J. K. (1998). Demographic diversity and faultlines: The compositional dynamics of organizational groups. Academy of Management Review, 23(2), 325-340.

Lau, D. C., & Murnighan, J. K. (2005). Interactions within groups and subgroups: The effects of demographic faultlines. . Academy of Management Journal, 48(4), 645-659.

Lawrence, B. S., & Zyphur, M. J. (2011). Identifying organizational faultlines with latent class cluster analysis. Organizational Research Methods, 14(1), 32-57. doi: 10.1177/1094428110376838

Leon-Perez, J. M., Medina, F. J., & Munduate, L. (2011). Effects of self-efficacy on objective and subjective outcomes in transactions and disputes. International Journal of Conflict Management, 22(2), 170-189. doi: 10.1108/10444061111126693

Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. (1990). Progress in small group research. Annual Review of Psychology, 41(1), 585-634. doi: doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.41.020190.003101

Lewicki, R. J., Weiss, S. E., & Lewin, D. (1992). Models of conflict, negotiation and third party intervention: A review and synthesis. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13(3), 209-252.

Lewin, K. (1997). ‘When facing danger.’ Resolving social conflicts and field theory in social sciences (pp. 116-121). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. (Original work published 1939).

 

107  

Lewin, K. (1997). Behavior and development as a function of the total situation. Resolving social conflicts and field theory in social sciences (pp. 337-381). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. (Original work published 1946).

Li, J., & Hambrick, D. C. (2005). Factional groups: A new vantage on demographic faultlines, conflict, and disintegration in work teams. Academy of Management Journal, 48(5), 794-813.

Liu, J., Fu, P., & Liu, S. (2009). Conflicts in top management teams and team/firm outcomes: The moderating effects of conflict-handling approaches. International journal of Conflict Management, 20(3), 228-250. doi: 10.1108/10444060910974867

Lu, J. F., Tjosvold, D., & Shi, K. (2010). Team training in China: Testing and applying the theory of cooperation andc ompetition. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40(1), 101-134. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2009.00565.x

Maas, C. J. M., & Hox, J. J. (2005). Sufficient sample sizes for multilevel modeling. Methodology: European Journal of Research Methods for the Behavioral and Social Sciences, 1(3), 86-92. doi: 10.1027/1614-2241.1.3.86

Mackie, D. M. (1987). Systematic and nonsystematic processing of majority and minority persuasive communications. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 41-52. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.41

Meyer, R. D., Dalal, R. S., & Hermida, R. (2010). A review and synthesis of situational strength in the organizational sciences. Journal of Management, 36(1), 121-140. doi: 10.1177/0149206309349309

Mischel, W. (1977). The interaction of person and situation. In D. Magnusson & N. S. Endler (Eds.), Personality at the crossroads: Current issues in interactional psychology (pp. 333-352). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Moberg, P. J. (1998). Predicting conflict strategy with personality traits: Incremental validity and the Five Factor model. International Journal of Conflict Management, 9(3), 258-285.

Mohammed, S., & Angell, L. C. (2004). Surface- and deep-level diversity in workgroups: Examining the moderating effects of team orientation and team process on relationship conflict. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25(8), 1015-1039. doi: 10.1002/job.293

Molleman, E. (2005). Diversity in demographic characteristics, abilities and personality traits: Do faultlines affect team functioning? Group Decision and Negotiation, 14(3), 173.

Moscovici, S. (1976). Social influence and social change. London, England and New York, NY: Academic Press.

Moscovici, S., & Nemeth, C. (1974). Social influence: II. Minority influence. . In C. Nemeth (Ed.), Social psychology: Classic and contemporary integrations. Oxford: Rand McNally.

Muthén, L.K. and Muthén, B.O. (1998-2010). Mplus User’s Guide (6th ed.).

 

108  

Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

Myers, D. G. (1978). Polarizing effects of social comparison. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 14(6), 554-563.

Nemeth, C. (1986). Differential contributions of majority and minority influence. Psychological Review, 93(1), 23-32. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.93.1.23

Newcomb, T. M. (1961). The acquaintance process: New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

Nylund, K. L., Asparouhov, T., & Muthén, B. O. (2007). Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 14(4), 535-569. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396

O'Leary, M. B., & Mortensen, M. (2010). Go (con)figure: Subgroups, imbalance, and isolates in geographically dispersed teams. Organization Science, 21(1), 115-131. doi: 10.1287/orsc.1090.0434

Oetzel, J. G. (1999). The influence of situational features on perceived conflict styles and self-construals in work groups. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23(4), 679-695.

Paul, S., Samarah, I. M., Seetharaman, P., & Mykytyn Jr., P. P. (2004). An empirical investigation of collaborative conflict management style in group support system-based global virtual teams. Journal of Management Information Systems, 21(3), 185-222.

Park, H., & Antonioni, D. (2007). Personality, reciprocity, and strength of conflict resolution strategy. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 110-125.

Park, H. S., & Park, M. (2008). Multilevel effects of conflict management preferences on satisfaction with group processes. International Journal of Conflict Management, 19(1), 57-71. doi: 10.1108/10444060810849182

Podsakoff, P. M., & Organ, D. W. (1986). Self-reports in organizational research: Problems and prospects. Journal of Management, 12(4), 531-544. doi: 10.1177/014920638601200408

Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Lee, J. Y., & Podsakoff, N. P. (2003). Common method biases in behavioral research: A critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(5), 879-903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

Pondy, L. R. (1967). Organizational conflict: Concepts and models. Administrative Science Quarterly, 12(2), 296-320.

Poole, M. S., & Dobosh, M. (2010). Exploring conflict management processes in jury deliberations through interaction analysis. Small Group Research, 41(4), 408-426.

Portello, J. Y., & Long, B. C. (1994). Gender role orientation, ethical and interpersonal conflicts, and conflict handling styles of female managers. Sex Roles, 31(11/12), 683-701.

 

109  

Pruitt, D. G., & Kim, S. H. (2004). Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate and settlement (3rd ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Putnam, L. L., & Wilson, C. (1982). Communicative strategies in organizational conflict: Reliability and validity of a measurement scale. In M. Burgoon (Ed.), Communication Yearbook (Vol. 6, pp. 629-652). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Quigley, N. R., Tekleab, A. G., & Tesluk, P. E. (2007). Comparing consensus- and aggregation-based methods of measuring team-level variables. Organizational Research Methods, 10(4), 589-608.

Rahim, M. A. (1983). A measurement of styles of handling interpersonal conflict. Academy of Management Journal, 26, 368-376.

Rahim, M. A. (2001). Managing conflict in organizations (3rd ed.). Westport, CT: Quorum Books.

Rahim, M. A., & Bonoma, T. V. (1979). Managing organizational conflict - Model for diagnosis and intervention. Psychological Reports, 44(3), 1323-1344.

Renwick, P. A. (1975). Perception and management of superior-subordinate conflict. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 13(3), 444-456. doi: 10.1016/0030-5073(75)90062-8

Rogers, S. J. (1987). The dynamics of conflict behavior in a mediated dispute. In J. A. Lemmon (Ed.), Mediation Quarterly (Vol. 18, pp. 61-71). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Rosenthal, D. B., & Hautaluoma, J. (1988). Effects of importance of issues, gender, and power of contenders on conflict management style. Journal of Social Psychology, 128(5), 699 - 701.

Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. E. (1991). The person and the situation: Perspectives of social psychology: New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Scherbaum, C. A., & Ferreter, J. M. (2009). Estimating statistical power and required sample sizes for organizational research using multilevel modeling. Organizational Research Methods, 12(2), 347-367. doi: 10.1177/1094428107308906

Smith, W. J., Harrington, K. V., & Neck, C. P. (2000). Resolving conflict with humor in a diversity context. Journal of Managerial Psychology, 15(6), 606-625. doi: 10.1108/02683940010346743

Snijders, T., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to basic and advanced multilevel modeling. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications Inc.

Somech, A. (2008). Managing conflict in school teams: The impact of task and goal interdependence on conflict management and team effectiveness. Educational Administration Quarterly, 44(3), 359-390. doi: 10.1177/0013161x08318957

 

110  

Somech, A., Desivilya, H. S., & Lidogoster, H. (2008). Team conflict management and team effectiveness: the effects of task interdependence and team identification. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 30(3), 359-378.

Steiner, I. D. (1972). Group process and productivity. New York, NY: Academic Press.

Swann, W. B., Jr. (1983). Self-verification: Bringing social reality into harmony with the self. In J. Suls & A. G. Greenwald (Eds.), Social psychological perspectives on the self (Vol. 2, pp. 33-66). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Tajfel, H. (1978). Social categorization, social identity, and social comparison. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 61-76). London, England: Academic Press.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7-24). Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall.

Tan, R. Y. (2011). Variant conflict management: A conceptual model on varying conflict management approaches within work teams. Paper presented at the 24th Annual Conference of the International Association for Conflict Management, Istanbul, Turkey. http://ssrn.com/paper=1872913

Tekleab, A. G., Quigley, N. R., & Tesluk, P. E. (2009). A longitudinal study of team conflict, conflict management, cohesion, and team effectiveness. Group & Organization Management, 34(2), 170-205. doi: 10.1177/1059601108331218

Thatcher, S. M. B., Jehn, K. A., & Zanutto, E. (2003). Cracks in diversity research: The effects of diversity faultlines on conflict and performance. Group Decision and Negotiation, 12(3), 217-241.

Thomas, K. W., & Kilmann, R. H. (1974). The Thomas-Kilmann conflict mode instrument. Tuxedo: Xicom.

Thomas, K. W. (1992). Conflict and conflict management: Reflections and update. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 13(3), 265-274.

Ting-Toomey, S., Gao, G., Trubisky, P., Yang, Z., Kim, H. S., Lin, S., et al. (1991). Culture, face maintenance, and styles of handling interpersonal conflict: A study in five cultures. International Journal of Conflict Management, 2(4), 275-296. doi: 10.1108/eb022702

Tjosvold, D. (1986). The dynamics of interdependence in organizations. Human Relations, 39(6), 517-540.

Tjosvold, D. (1990). Flight crew collaboration to manage safety risks. Group & Organization Management, 15(2), 177-177.

Tjosvold, D. (1998). Cooperative and competitive goal approach to conflict: Accomplishments and challenges. Applied Psychology, 47(3), 285-313. doi: 10.1111/j.1464-0597.1998.tb00025.x

 

111  

Tjosvold, D. (2002). Managing anger for teamwork in Hong Kong: Goal interdependence and open–mindedness. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 5(2), 107-123. doi: 10.1111/1467-839x.00098

Tjosvold, D., Law, K. S., & Sun, H. (2006). Effectiveness of Chinese teams: The role of conflict types and conflict management approaches. Management and Organization Review, 2(2), 231-252. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8784.2006.00040.x

Tjosvold, D., Wong, A., Nibler, R., & Pounder, J. S. (2002). Teamwork and controversy in undergraduate management courses in Hong Kong: Can the method reinforce the message? Swiss Journal of Psychology, 61(3), 131-138. doi: 10.1024//1421-0185.61.3.131

Tjosvold, D., Yu, Z., & Wu, P. (2009). Empowering individuals for team innovation in China: Conflict management and problem solving. Negotiation & Conflict Management Research, 2(2), 185-205. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-4716.2009.00036.x

Tuckman, B. W. (1965). Developmental sequence in small groups. Psychological Bulletin, 63(6), 384-399. doi: 10.1037/h0022100

Turner, J. C. (1985). Social categorization and the self-concept: A social cognitive theory of group behavior. In E. J. Lawler (Ed.), Advances in Group Processes: Theory and Research (Vol. 2, pp. 77-122). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Utley, M. E., Richardson, D. R., & Pilkington, C. J. (1989). Personality and interpersonal conflict management. Personality and Individual Differences, 10(3), 287-293.

Van de Vliert, E., & Euwema, M. C. (1994). Agreeableness and activeness as components of conflict behaviors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 674-687. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.674

Van Knippenberg, D., & Schippers, M. C. (2007). Work group diversity. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 515-541. doi: doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085546

Wageman, R. (1995). Interdependence and group effectiveness. Administrative Science Quarterly, 40(1), 145-180.

Wall Jr., J. A., & Callister, R. R. (1995). Conflict and its management. Academy of Management Journal, 21(3), 515-558.

Williams, K. Y., & O'Reilly, C. A. (1998). Demography and diversity in organizations: A review of 40 years of research. Research in Organizational Behavior, 20, 77-140.

Witteman, H. (1991). Group member satisfaction: A conflict-related account. Small Group Research, 22(1), 24-58. doi: 10.1177/1046496491221003

Yang, C. C. (2006). Evaluating latent class analysis models in qualitative phenotype identification. Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 50(4), 1090-1104.

 

112  

Yelsma, P., & Brown, C. T. (1985). Gender roles, biological sex, and predisposition to conflict management. Sex Roles, 12(7/8), 731-747.

Zellmer-Bruhn, M. E., Maloney, M. M., Bhappu, A. D., & Salvador, R. (2008). When and how do differences matter? An exploration of perceived similarity in teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 107(1), 41-59.

 

113  

Figu

re 1

. Con

cept

ual m

odel

of v

aria

nt c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t

Varia

nt C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t (V

CM

)

Ove

rall

VC

M

VC

M

prof

iles

Salie

nt c

onfli

ct-r

elev

ant

mem

ber c

hara

cter

istic

s

Team

con

text

ual

dete

rmin

ants

Div

erge

nt te

am

dyna

mic

s

Team

out

com

es

Min

. C

oop.

M

in.

Com

p.

Mod

. C

omp.

M

ax.

Mod

. C

oop.

 

114  

Coo

pera

tion

Com

petit

ion

Figu

re 2

. Min

imum

VC

M p

rofil

es

Min

imum

co

oper

ativ

e V

CM

: A

ll m

embe

rs

are

coop

erat

ive.

Min

imum

co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

: A

ll m

embe

rs

are

com

petit

ive.

 

115  

Coo

pera

tion

Com

petit

ion

Figu

re 3

. Mod

erat

e V

CM

pro

files

Mod

erat

e co

oper

ativ

e V

CM

: A

coo

pera

tive

maj

ority

exi

sts

in

the

team

.

Mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

: A

com

petit

ive

maj

ority

exi

sts

in

the

team

.

 

116  

Coo

pera

tion

Com

petit

ion

Figu

re 4

. Max

imum

VC

M p

rofil

e

Mem

bers

are

eq

ually

pol

ariz

ed

tow

ard

eith

er e

nd

of th

e co

ntin

uum

.

 

117  

Figu

re 5

. VC

M p

rofil

es id

entif

ied

from

con

tent

cod

ing

VC

M

prof

iles

Min

. C

oop.

M

in.

Com

p.

Mod

. C

omp.

M

ax.

Mod

. C

oop.

M

ulti.

cl

uste

rs

Mid

pt.

clus

ter

Dis

trib.

 

118  

0 0.

1 0.

2 0.

3 0.

4 0.

5 0.

6 0.

7 0.

8 0.

9 1

1 2

3 4

5 6

7 8

Cla

ss 1

, 26.

6%

Cla

ss 2

, 12.

7%

Cla

ss 3

, 48.

1%

Cla

ss 4

, 12.

7%

Figu

re 6

. Gra

ph s

how

ing

estim

ated

con

ditio

nal p

roba

bilit

ies

for t

he fo

ur-c

lass

mod

el s

olut

ion

Estimated conditional probabilities

Num

ber o

f ind

icat

ors

 

119  

VC

M

prof

iles

Min

. C

oop.

M

in.

Com

p.

Mod

. C

omp.

Max

.

Mod

. C

oop.

D

istri

.

Figu

re 7

. VC

M p

rofil

es id

entif

ied

from

late

nt c

lass

ana

lyse

s Mul

. C

lust

r M

idpt

. C

lust

r.

 

120  

Table 1

Descriptive Statistics for All Key Team Variables (N = 79)

Variable M SD S2 Min Max Predictors

1. Cooperative team conflict management

15.72 1.24 1.54 11.33 19.00

2. Competitive team conflict management

4.19 0.95 0.91 2.33 6.90

3. Variant conflict management

2.25 0.92 0.84 0 5.18

4. Gender role diversity 0.62 0.15 0.02 0 0.86 5. Team goal

interdependence 37.56 2.66 7.09 30.67 44.00

6. Mixed goal interdependencea

37.57 1.45 2.11 35.00 40.00

7. Subgroup formation 7.67 1.92 3.67 4.00 12.00 Outcomes

8. Team conflict efficacy 19.38 1.49 2.23 16.00 23.67 9. Satisfaction with team

conflict management process

21.85 2.24 5.01 17.00 26.40

10. Team effectiveness 18.74 1.96 3.86 13.33 22.75 Controls

11. Intra-group conflict 17.54 3.57 12.71

12.33 28.75

12. Team conflict intensity 7.95 1.74 3.01 5.25 14.25 13. Team conflict importance 2.33 0.59 0.35 1.00 3.75 14. Team sizeb 4.22 1.00 0.99 3.00 8.00

Other 15. Team agec 21.72 0.95 0.90 19.33 25.75 16. Team interactiond 1.51 0.42 0.17 1.00 3.00

Note. a n = 56. b Team size is based on the number of members who participated from each team. c Team age is calculated using the average age of members in each team. d Team interaction is calculated using the mean scores for reported level of interaction among members in each team.

 

121  

Table 2

Reliability and Aggregation Indices for All Team Measures (N = 79)

Variable α Median rWG (J)

ICC (1) ICC (2)

Predictors 1. Conflict management .70 .92 .18 .57

Cooperation .73 .91 .21 .51 Competition .70 .74 .32 .48

2. Team goal interdependence .81 .96 .17 .68 Positive .78 .95 .40 .77 Negative .73 .89 .16 .49

3. Gender role orientationa .78 .97 .06 .56 Masculine .84 .95 .11 .54 Feminine .87 .94 .27 .77

4. Subgroup formation .82 .80 .45 .76 Outcomes 5. Team conflict efficacy .78 .95 .38 .75 6. Satisfaction with team conflict

management process .74 .92 .37 .79

7. Team effectiveness .94 .95 .74 .93 Controls 8. Intra-team conflict .88 .96 .31 .80 9. Team conflict intensitya .79 .91 .17 .45

Note. a n = 78, due to missing data. α = Cronbach’s alpha based on the group means of individual ratings for each measure. ICC (1) = intra-class correlation for individual ratings based on a one-way random effects ANOVA. ICC (2) = intra-class correlation for team mean ratings based on a one-way random effects ANOVA.

 

122  (R

ae) Y

unzi

Tan

“V

aria

nt C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t: C

once

ptua

lizin

g an

d In

vest

igat

ing

Team

Con

flict

Man

agem

ent a

s a C

onfig

ural

Con

stru

ct”

Rev

ised

Dis

serta

tion

Ana

lyse

s and

Fin

ding

s – O

ctob

er 2

012

!

13!

Tabl

e 3

Inte

rcor

rela

tions

for a

ll K

ey T

eam

Var

iabl

es

Var

iabl

e 1

2 3

4 5

6 7

8 9

Pred

icto

rs

1.

C

oope

rativ

e te

am c

onfli

ct

man

agem

ent

!

2.

Com

petit

ive

team

con

flict

m

anag

emen

t -.4

3***

!

3.

Var

iant

con

flict

man

agem

ent

.04

-.02

!

4.

Gen

der r

ole

dive

rsity

.1

5 -.1

1 -.1

2 !

5.

Team

goa

l int

erde

pend

ence

.4

8***

-.3

9***

.0

2 .0

9 !

6.

M

ixed

goa

l int

erde

pend

ence

a .5

6***

-.2

5 .0

9 -.0

5 1.

00**

* !

7.

Subg

roup

form

atio

n -.3

1**

.41*

**

-.07

-.18

-.45*

**

-.22

!

Out

com

es

8.

Te

am c

onfli

ct e

ffic

acy

.36*

* -.1

1 -.2

1 .1

8 .4

4***

.3

6**

-.24*

!

9.

Satis

fact

ion

with

team

co

nflic

t man

agem

ent p

roce

ss

.23*

-.0

6 .0

9 -.0

3 .4

7***

.4

0**

-.09

.52*

** !

10. T

eam

eff

ectiv

enes

s .4

6***

-.1

8 .0

1 .2

0 .5

5***

.4

3***

-.3

1**

.62*

**

.65*

**

Con

trol

s

11. C

ours

e af

filia

tionb

.15

-.06

.01

.23*

.1

6 .2

3 -.1

5 .2

0 .0

3 12

. Tea

m si

ze

.05

-.04

.11

-.07

.06

.05

.18

.15

.19

13. I

ntra

-team

con

flict

-.3

9***

.3

8***

.1

5 -.0

9 -.5

5***

-.3

9**

.53*

**

-.27*

-.3

0**

14. T

eam

con

flict

inte

nsity

-.4

9***

.4

2***

.1

3 -.1

2 -.6

1***

-.4

6***

.5

2***

-.3

3**

-.34*

* 15

. Tea

m c

onfli

ct im

porta

nce

-.11

.27*

.0

1 .0

5 -.0

4 -.0

5 .4

1***

.2

4*

.15

Oth

er

16

. Tea

m a

ge

.23*

-.1

9 .0

7 -.0

8 .0

4 -.0

1 -.0

3 .0

4 .2

7*

17. T

eam

ass

ignm

entc

.06

-.07

.05

.14

.18

-.14

.07

-.02

.05

18. T

eam

inte

ract

ion

.15

.02

-.03

.16

.06

.23

.12

.09

-.01

Not

e. T

he N

var

ies f

rom

72

– 79

due

to o

mis

sion

of o

utlie

rs. a

n =

53 –

56

due

to o

mis

sion

of o

utlie

rs. b

Cou

rse

affil

iatio

n =

1, “

Man

agin

g Pe

ople

at W

ork”

cou

rse;

2, “

Lead

ersh

ip a

nd T

eam

-bui

ldin

g” c

ours

e. c

Team

ass

ignm

ent =

1, “

yes”

; 0, “

no”.

* p

< 0

5. *

* p

< .0

1. *

** p

< .0

01.

 

123  (R

ae) Y

unzi

Tan

“V

aria

nt C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t: C

once

ptua

lizin

g an

d In

vest

igat

ing

Team

Con

flict

Man

agem

ent a

s a C

onfig

ural

Con

stru

ct”

Rev

ised

Dis

serta

tion

Ana

lyse

s and

Fin

ding

s – O

ctob

er 2

012

!

14!

Tabl

e 3

(Con

tinue

d)

Inte

rcor

rela

tions

for a

ll K

ey T

eam

Var

iabl

es

Var

iabl

e 10

11

12

13

14

15

16

17

18

10

. Tea

m e

ffec

tiven

ess

!

Con

trol

s

11. C

ours

e af

filia

tionb

.16

!

12

. Tea

m si

ze

.18

-.06

!

13. I

ntra

-team

con

flict

-.4

2***

.0

3 .1

8 !

14. T

eam

con

flict

inte

nsity

-.4

8***

-.0

1 .1

8 .9

5***

!

15

. Tea

m c

onfli

ct im

porta

nce

.22

.12

.25*

.4

2***

.3

7**

!

O

ther

16. T

eam

age

.1

9 -.4

1***

.1

1 -.1

2 -.1

2 -.0

1 !

17

. Tea

m a

ssig

nmen

tc .1

0 -.2

1 .1

5 -.0

6 -.0

7 .1

2 .1

5 !

18. T

eam

inte

ract

ion

.10

.17

-.03

.04

.03

.18

-.17

.25*

!

N

ote.

The

N v

arie

s fro

m 7

2 –

79 d

ue to

om

issi

on o

f out

liers

. a n =

53

– 56

due

to o

mis

sion

of o

utlie

rs. b

Cou

rse

affil

iatio

n =

1, “

Man

agin

g Pe

ople

at W

ork”

cou

rse;

2, “

Lead

ersh

ip a

nd T

eam

-bui

ldin

g” c

ours

e. b

Team

ass

ignm

ent =

1, “

yes”

; 0, “

no”.

* p

< 0

5. *

* p

< .0

1. *

** p

< .0

01.

 

124  

Table 4

Coding Definitions of Variant Conflict Management Profiles for the Content Coding Analysis

Coding category Variant conflict management profile

Definition

1 Minimum cooperative When all scores are clustered between the midpoint and the high end of the cooperative conflict management continuum.

2 Moderate cooperative When a majority cluster of scores is observed between the midpoint and the high end of the cooperative conflict management continuum.

3 Maximum When two distinct and evenly sized clusters are observed along the continuum.

4 Moderate competitive When a majority cluster of scores is observed between the midpoint and the low end of the cooperative conflict management continuum.

5 Minimum competitive When all scores are clustered between the midpoint and the low end of the cooperative conflict management continuum.

6 Other When the distribution of individual scores for the team does not fall into any of the above five categories. (If a team falls under this category, each coder also provides detailed comments describing the specific nature of the score distribution for the given team.)

 

125  

Table 5

Classification of Teams Based on the Content Coding Analysis

Coding category Variant conflict management profile N 1 Minimum cooperative 6 2 Moderate cooperative 38 3 Maximum 5 4 Moderate competitive 10 5 Minimum competitive 3 6 Other

‘Distributed’ ‘Multiple clusters’ ‘Midpoint cluster’

17 10 4 3

Note. The Cohen’s Kappas for the coders’ ratings were .96, .96, and .96.

 

126  

Table 6

Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Team Conflict Efficacy from Cooperative Team Conflict Management and Variant Conflict Management (N = 79)

Team conflict efficacy Model 1 Model 2

Predictor B SE B β B SE B β Step 1:

Team goal interdependence .17 .07 .30* .16 .08 .28* Subgroup formation -.12 .09 -.16 -.14 .10 -.18 Team conflict intensity -.25 .13 -.29 -.17 .14 -.20 Team conflict importance .70 .30 .28* .69 .30 .27*

Step 2: Cooperative team conflict management .17 .13 .14

Variant conflict management -.23 .17 -.14

R .58** .60 R2 .33** .36 Adj. R2 .30** .31 ΔR2 .33** .03

* p < .05. ** p < .01.

 

127  

Table 7

Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Member Satisfaction with Team Conflict Management Process from Cooperative Team Conflict Management and Variant Conflict Management (N = 79)

Member satisfaction with team conflict management process

Model 1 Model 2 Predictor B SE B β B SE B β

Step 1: Team goal interdependence .36 .11 .43** .33 .12 .39** Team conflict intensity -.08 .17 -.07 -.13 .18 -.10 Team age .53 .23 .23* .49 .24 .21*

Step 2: Cooperative team conflict management .06 .20 .03

Variant conflict management .18 .26 .07

R .53** .54 R2 .28** .29 Adj. R2 .25** .24 ΔR2 .28** .01

* p < .05. ** p < .01.

 

128  

Table 8

Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Team Effectiveness from Cooperative Team Conflict Management and Variant Conflict Management (N = 79)

Team effectiveness Model 1 Model 2

Predictor B SE B β B SE B β Step 1:

Team goal interdependence .32 .09 .44** .24 .09 .33* Subgroup formation .00 .11 .00 .05 .11 .05 Team conflict intensity -.26 .15 -.23 -.29 .16 -.26

Step 2: Cooperative team conflict management .39 .16 .25*

Variant conflict management .11 .21 .05

R .61** .65* R2 .37** .42* Adj. R2 .34** .38* ΔR2 .37** .05*

* p < .05. ** p < .01.

 

129  

Table 9

Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Team Conflict Efficacy from Competitive Team Conflict Management and Variant Conflict Management (N = 79)

Team conflict efficacy Model 1 Model 2

Predictor B SE B β B SE B β Step 1:

Team goal interdependence .17 .07 .30* .19 .08 .35* Subgroup formation -.12 .09 -.16 -.16 .10 -.21 Team conflict intensity -.25 .13 -.29 -.19 .14 -.23 Team conflict importance .70 .30 .28* .67 .30 .26*

Step 2: Competitive team conflict management .18 .17 .11

Variant conflict management -.20 .17 -.13

R .58** .60 R2 .33** .36 Adj. R2 .30** .30 ΔR2 .33** .03

* p < .05. ** p < .01.

 

130  

Table 10

Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Member Satisfaction with Team Conflict Management Process from Competitive Team Conflict Management and Variant Conflict Management (N = 79)

Member satisfaction with team conflict management process

Model 1 Model 2 Predictor B SE B β B SE B β

Step 1: Team goal interdependence .36 .11 .43** .37 .11 .44** Team conflict intensity -.08 .17 -.07 -.21 .18 -.16 Team age .53 .23 .23* .51 .24 .21*

Step 2: Competitive team conflict management .46 .25 .20

Variant conflict management .23 .26 .09

R .53** .57 R2 .28** .32 Adj. R2 .25** .27 ΔR2 .28** .04

* p < .05. ** p < .01.

 

131  

Table 11

Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Team Effectiveness from Competitive Team Conflict Management and Variant Conflict Management (N = 79)

Team effectiveness Model 1 Model 2

Predictor B SE B β B SE B β Step 1:

Team goal interdependence .32 .09 .44** .32 .09 .44** Subgroup formation .00 .11 .00 -.01 .12 -.01 Team conflict intensity -.26 .15 -.23 -.34 .16 -.30*

Step 2: Competitive team conflict management .29 .21 .14

Variant conflict management .18 .21 .08

R .61** .62 R2 .37** .39 Adj. R2 .34** .35 ΔR2 .37** .02

* p < .05. ** p < .01.

 

132  

(Rae

) Yun

zi T

an

“Var

iant

Con

flict

Man

agem

ent:

Con

cept

ualiz

ing

and

Inve

stig

atin

g Te

am C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t as a

Con

figur

al C

onst

ruct

” R

evis

ed D

isse

rtatio

n A

naly

ses a

nd F

indi

ngs –

Oct

ober

201

2 !

26!

Tabl

e 12

Med

ians

and

Num

ber

of C

ases

for

the

Effe

cts

of V

aria

nt C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t Pro

files

on

Team

Con

flict

Effi

cacy

V

aria

nt c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t pro

files

M

inim

um

coop

erat

ive

M

oder

ate

coop

erat

ive

M

axim

um

M

oder

ate

com

petit

ive

M

inim

um

com

petit

ive

Var

iabl

e M

dn

n

Mdn

n

M

dn

n

Mdn

n

M

dn

n

Team

con

flict

eff

icac

y 19

.45

6

19.7

1 38

19.2

5 5

18

.58

10

18

.33

3

! Tabl

e 13

Mea

ns a

nd S

tand

ard

Dev

iatio

ns fo

r th

e Ef

fect

s of

Var

iant

Con

flict

Man

agem

ent P

rofil

es o

n M

embe

r Sa

tisfa

ctio

n w

ith th

e Te

am’s

C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t Pro

cess

and

Tea

m E

ffect

iven

ess

V

aria

nt c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t pro

files

M

inim

um

coop

erat

ive

M

oder

ate

coop

erat

ive

M

axim

um

M

oder

ate

com

petit

ive

M

inim

um

com

petit

ive

Var

iabl

e M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

M

embe

r sat

isfa

ctio

n w

ith

the

team

’s c

onfli

ct

man

agem

ent p

roce

ss

22.5

5 1.

75

21

.88

2.30

22.6

0 3.

10

21

.27

1.69

19.1

9 2.

17

Team

eff

ectiv

enes

s 19

.80

1.15

19.0

4 1.

75

19

.25

2.38

17.8

3 1.

29

15

.47

1.53

!

 

133  

Table 14

Simple Regression Analysis Predicting Variant Conflict Management with Gender Role Diversity

Variable B SE B β t p Gender role diversity -.70 .80 -.10 -.88 .38

Note. R2 = .01 (N = 78, ns).

 

134  

Table 15

Polynomial Regression Analysis Predicting Variant Conflict Management with Team Goal Interdependence Variables

Variable B SE B β t p Team goal interdependence -.01 .04 -.02 -.15 .88 Team goal interdependence2 .02 .01 .18 1.63 .11

Note. Centered variables were used in this analysis. R2 = .03 (N = 79, ns).  

 

135  

Table 16

Simple Regression Analysis Predicting Subgroup Formation with Gender Role Diversity

Variable B SE B β t p Gender role diversity -2.51 1.66 -.17 -1.51 .13

Note. R2 = .03 (N = 78, ns).

Table 17

Hierarchical Regression Analysis Predicting Subgroup Formation as a Mediator between Gender Role Diversity and Variant Conflict Management (N = 78)

Variant conflict management Model 1 Model 2

Predictor B SE B β B SE B β Step 1:

Subgroup formation -.02 .06 -.04 -.03 .06 -.06 Step 2:

Gender role diversity -.78 .82 -.11

R .04 .12 R2 .00 .01 Adj. R2 -.01 -.01 ΔR2 .00 .01

* p < .05. ** p < .01.

 

136  

Table 18

Model Fit Statistics for the Latent Class Analyses

Model AIC BIC aBIC LMRT BLRT 8-class 401.944 570.175 346.308 p < .05 p < .05 7-class 392.262 539.168 343.678 p < .05 p > .05 6-class 382.061 507.642 340.530 p < .05 p > .05 5-class 373.588 477.843 339.109 p < .05 p > .05 4-class 367.023 449.954 339.597 p < .05 p < .05 3-class 367.063 428.669 346.689 p < .05 p < .05 2-class 368.198 408.479 354.877 p < .05 p < .05

Note. The LCAs were conducted with 100 random starts and 100 optimizations.

 

137  

Table 19

Classification of Variant Conflict Management Profiles Based on Latent Class Memberships (N = 79)

Latent class Variant conflict management profilea Count Proportion Threshold

estimate 1 Minimum cooperative

Maximum Minimum competitive ‘Multiple clusters’ ‘Midpoint cluster’

6 5 3 4 3

7.6% 6.3% 3.8% 5.1% 3.8%

0.916 1.163 1.792 1.447 1.792

2 ‘Distributed’ 10 12.7% -15.000

3 Moderate cooperative 38 48.1% -15.000

4 Moderate competitive 10 12.7% -15.000

Note. aThese profiles were identified in the content coding analyses.

 

138  (R

ae) Y

unzi

Tan

“V

aria

nt C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t: C

once

ptua

lizin

g an

d In

vest

igat

ing

Team

Con

flict

Man

agem

ent a

s a C

onfig

ural

Con

stru

ct”

Rev

ised

Dis

serta

tion

Ana

lyse

s and

Fin

ding

s – O

ctob

er 2

012

!

30!

Tabl

e 20

Med

ians

and

Num

ber

of C

ases

for

the

Effe

cts

of V

aria

nt C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t Cat

egor

ies

(Ide

ntifi

ed in

the

Late

nt C

lass

Ana

lyse

s)

on T

eam

Con

flict

Effi

cacy

V

aria

nt c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t cat

egor

ies

‘C

ombi

natio

n’

M

oder

ate

coop

erat

ive

‘D

istri

bute

d’

M

oder

ate

com

petit

ive

Var

iabl

e M

dn

n

Mdn

n

M

dn

n

Mdn

n

Team

con

flict

eff

icac

y 19

.25

21

19

.71

38

19

.25

10

18

.58

10

! Tabl

e 21

Mea

ns a

nd S

tand

ard

Dev

iatio

ns fo

r th

e Ef

fect

s of

Var

iant

Con

flict

Man

agem

ent C

ateg

orie

s (I

dent

ified

in th

e La

tent

Cla

ss

Anal

yses

) on

Mem

ber

Satis

fact

ion

with

the

Team

’s C

onfli

ct M

anag

emen

t Pro

cess

and

Tea

m E

ffect

iven

ess

V

aria

nt c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t cat

egor

ies

‘C

ombi

natio

n’

M

oder

ate

coop

erat

ive

‘D

istri

bute

d’

M

oder

ate

com

petit

ive

Var

iabl

e M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

Mem

ber s

atis

fact

ion

with

th

e te

am’s

con

flict

m

anag

emen

t pro

cess

21

.96

2.30

21.8

8 2.

30

22

.13

2.56

21.2

7 1.

69

Team

eff

ectiv

enes

s 18

.69

2.04

19.0

4 1.

75

18

.63

2.92

17.8

3 1.

29

! !

 

139   !

1!

Tabl

e 22

Sum

mar

y Fi

ndin

gs fo

r th

e M

ain

and

Supp

lem

enta

ry A

naly

ses

Res

earc

h H

ypot

hese

s St

atis

tical

Ana

lyse

s Fi

ndin

gs

H1:

Th

e fiv

e pr

opos

ed V

CM

pro

files

, i.e

., m

inim

um

coop

erat

ive,

min

imum

com

petit

ive,

mod

erat

e co

oper

ativ

e, m

oder

ate

com

petit

ive,

and

max

imum

V

CM

pro

files

, will

be

pres

ent i

n te

ams.

Qua

litat

ive

cont

ent

codi

ng

Yes

. All

five

prop

osed

VC

M p

rofil

es

wer

e id

entif

ied

in th

e co

nten

t cod

ing

anal

ysis

. Add

ition

al p

rofil

es w

ere

also

un

cove

red;

thes

e w

ere

desc

ribed

as

‘dis

tribu

ted,

’ ‘m

ultip

le c

lust

ers,’

and

‘m

idpo

int c

lust

er’ V

CM

pro

files

. See

Ta

bles

4 &

5.

H

2a:

VC

M w

ill b

e ne

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am c

onfli

ct

effic

acy,

with

mea

n co

oper

ativ

e te

am c

onfli

ct

man

agem

ent s

core

s bei

ng h

eld

cons

tant

.

Hie

rarc

hica

l mul

tiple

re

gres

sion!

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

6.

H2b

: V

CM

will

be

nega

tivel

y as

soci

ated

with

mem

ber

satis

fact

ion

with

the

team

’s c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t pr

oces

s, w

ith m

ean

coop

erat

ive

team

con

flict

m

anag

emen

t sco

res b

eing

hel

d co

nsta

nt.

Hie

rarc

hica

l mul

tiple

re

gres

sion!

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

7.

H2c

: V

CM

will

be

nega

tivel

y as

soci

ated

with

team

ef

fect

iven

ess,

with

mea

n co

oper

ativ

e te

am c

onfli

ct

man

agem

ent s

core

s bei

ng h

eld

cons

tant

.

Hie

rarc

hica

l mul

tiple

re

gres

sion!

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

8.

! !

 

140  

!

2!

Tabl

e 22

(Con

tinue

d)

Sum

mar

y Fi

ndin

gs fo

r th

e M

ain

and

Supp

lem

enta

ry A

naly

ses

Res

earc

h H

ypot

hese

s St

atis

tical

Ana

lyse

s Fi

ndin

gs

H3a

: VC

M w

ill b

e po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am c

onfli

ct

effic

acy,

with

mea

n co

mpe

titiv

e te

am c

onfli

ct

man

agem

ent s

core

s bei

ng h

eld

cons

tant

.

Hie

rarc

hica

l mul

tiple

re

gres

sion!

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

9.

H3b

: VC

M w

ill b

e po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith m

embe

r sa

tisfa

ctio

n w

ith th

e te

am’s

con

flict

man

agem

ent

proc

ess,

with

mea

n co

mpe

titiv

e te

am c

onfli

ct

man

agem

ent s

core

s bei

ng h

eld

cons

tant

.

Hie

rarc

hica

l mul

tiple

re

gres

sion!

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

10.

H3c

: VC

M w

ill b

e po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am

effe

ctiv

enes

s, w

ith m

ean

com

petit

ive

team

con

flict

m

anag

emen

t sco

res b

eing

hel

d co

nsta

nt.

Hie

rarc

hica

l mul

tiple

re

gres

sion!

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

11.

H4a

: M

inim

um c

oope

rativ

e V

CM

pro

files

will

be

mos

t po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am c

onfli

ct e

ffic

acy.

Man

n-W

hitn

ey U

Te

sts

Parti

ally

supp

orte

d. T

eam

s with

m

inim

um c

oope

rativ

e V

CM

pro

files

re

porte

d hi

gher

leve

ls o

f tea

m c

onfli

ct

effic

acy

than

team

s with

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

. See

Tab

le

12.

H4b

: M

inim

um c

oope

rativ

e V

CM

pro

files

will

be

mos

t po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith m

embe

r sat

isfa

ctio

n w

ith

the

team

’s c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t pro

cess

.

Plan

ned

com

paris

ons

Not

supp

orte

d. T

able

13.

H4c

: M

inim

um c

oope

rativ

e V

CM

pro

files

will

be

mos

t po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am e

ffec

tiven

ess.

Plan

ned

com

paris

ons

Parti

ally

supp

orte

d. T

eam

s with

m

inim

um c

oope

rativ

e V

CM

pro

files

w

ere

mor

e ef

fect

ive

than

team

s with

m

inim

um c

ompe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

. Se

e Ta

ble

13.

 

141  

!

3!

Tabl

e 22

(Con

tinue

d)

Sum

mar

y Fi

ndin

gs fo

r th

e M

ain

and

Supp

lem

enta

ry A

naly

ses

Res

earc

h H

ypot

hese

s St

atis

tical

Ana

lyse

s Fi

ndin

gs

H5a

: M

inim

um c

ompe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

will

be

mos

t ne

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am c

onfli

ct e

ffic

acy.

Man

n-W

hitn

ey U

Te

sts

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

12.

H5b

: M

inim

um c

ompe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

will

be

mos

t ne

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith m

embe

r sat

isfa

ctio

n w

ith

the

team

’s c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t pro

cess

.

Plan

ned

com

paris

ons

Supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

13.

H5c

: M

inim

um c

ompe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

will

be

mos

t ne

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am e

ffec

tiven

ess.

Plan

ned

com

paris

ons

Parti

ally

supp

orte

d. T

eam

s with

m

inim

um c

ompe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

w

ere

less

eff

ectiv

e th

an te

ams w

ith

min

imum

coo

pera

tive

and

mod

erat

e co

oper

ativ

e V

CM

pro

files

. See

Tab

le

13.

H6a

: M

oder

ate

coop

erat

ive

VC

M p

rofil

es, r

elat

ive

to

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

, will

be

mor

e po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am c

onfli

ct e

ffic

acy.

Man

n-W

hitn

ey U

Te

sts

Supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

12.

H6b

: M

oder

ate

coop

erat

ive

VC

M p

rofil

es, r

elat

ive

to

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

, will

be

mor

e po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith m

embe

r sat

isfa

ctio

n w

ith

the

team

’s c

onfli

ct m

anag

emen

t pro

cess

.

Plan

ned

com

paris

ons

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

13.

H6c

: M

oder

ate

coop

erat

ive

VC

M p

rofil

es, r

elat

ive

to

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

, will

be

mor

e po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am e

ffec

tiven

ess.

Plan

ned

com

paris

ons

Supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

13.

!

 

142  

!

4!

Tabl

e 22

(Con

tinue

d)

Sum

mar

y Fi

ndin

gs fo

r th

e M

ain

and

Supp

lem

enta

ry A

naly

ses

Res

earc

h H

ypot

hese

s St

atis

tical

Ana

lyse

s Fi

ndin

gs

H7a

: M

axim

um V

CM

pro

files

, rel

ativ

e to

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

, will

be

mor

e ne

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am c

onfli

ct e

ffic

acy.

Man

n-W

hitn

ey U

Te

sts

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

12.

H7b

: M

axim

um V

CM

pro

files

, rel

ativ

e to

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

, will

be

mor

e ne

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith m

embe

r sat

isfa

ctio

n w

ith th

e te

am’s

co

nflic

t man

agem

ent p

roce

ss.

Plan

ned

com

paris

ons

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

13.!

H7c

: M

axim

um V

CM

pro

files

, rel

ativ

e to

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

, will

be

mor

e ne

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith te

am e

ffec

tiven

ess.

Plan

ned

com

paris

ons

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

13.!

H8:

G

ende

r rol

e di

vers

ity w

ill b

e po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed

with

VC

M.

Sim

ple

regr

essi

on

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

14.

H9:

C

oope

rativ

e an

d co

mpe

titiv

e go

al in

terd

epen

denc

e w

ill b

e ne

gativ

ely

asso

ciat

ed w

ith V

CM

, whi

le m

ixed

go

al in

terd

epen

denc

e w

ill b

e po

sitiv

ely

asso

ciat

ed

with

VC

M.

Poly

nom

ial m

ultip

le

regr

essi

on

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

15.

H10

: Su

bgro

up fo

rmat

ion

will

med

iate

the

rela

tions

hip

betw

een

gend

er ro

le d

iver

sity

and

VC

M.

Med

iatio

nal

regr

essi

ons b

ased

on

Bar

on &

Ken

ny’s

(1

986)

app

roac

h

Not

supp

orte

d. S

ee T

able

s 16

and

17.

!

 

143  

!

5!

Tabl

e 22

(Con

tinue

d)

Sum

mar

y Fi

ndin

gs fo

r th

e M

ain

and

Supp

lem

enta

ry A

naly

ses

Res

earc

h Q

uest

ions

St

atis

tical

ana

lyse

s Fi

ndin

gs

Supp

lem

enta

ry a

naly

ses:

A

sses

sing

the

pres

ence

of t

he

prop

osed

VC

M p

rofil

es in

the

mod

el

empi

rical

ly

Late

nt c

lass

ana

lyse

s Th

e be

st fi

tting

mod

el in

the

anal

yses

iden

tifie

d fo

ur la

tent

cl

ass c

ateg

orie

s. Tw

o of

thes

e ca

tego

ries c

orre

spon

ded

with

the

prop

osed

VC

M p

rofil

es: m

oder

ate

coop

erat

ive

and

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

. The

oth

er tw

o id

entif

ied

cate

gorie

s wer

e ‘d

istri

bute

d’ a

nd a

com

bina

tion

of ‘m

ultip

le c

lust

ers,’

‘mid

poin

t clu

ster

,’ m

inim

um

coop

erat

ive,

max

imum

, and

min

imum

com

petit

ive

VC

M

prof

iles.

See

Figu

re 5

, Tab

les 1

8 an

d 19

.

Expl

orin

g gr

oup

diff

eren

ces i

n te

am

conf

lict e

ffic

acy

scor

es a

mon

g th

e fo

ur V

CM

cat

egor

ies i

dent

ified

in

the

late

nt c

lass

ana

lyse

s

Kru

skal

l-Wal

lis T

est;

M

ann-

Whi

tney

U T

ests

for

follo

w-u

p co

mpa

rison

s

No

sign

ifica

nt o

vera

ll gr

oup

diff

eren

ce in

team

con

flict

ef

ficac

y sc

ores

acr

oss t

he fo

ur V

CM

cat

egor

ies;

How

ever

, tw

o of

the

six

Man

n-W

hitn

ey U

test

com

paris

ons w

ere

sign

ifica

nt:

1.

Team

s with

pro

files

und

er th

e ‘c

ombi

natio

n’ c

ateg

ory

(i.e.

, min

imum

coo

pera

tive,

max

imum

, min

imum

co

mpe

titiv

e, ‘m

ultip

le c

lust

ers,’

and

‘mid

poin

t clu

ster

’; M

dn =

19.

25, n

= 2

1) re

porte

d hi

gher

leve

ls o

f tea

m

conf

lict e

ffic

acy

than

team

s with

mod

erat

e co

mpe

titiv

e V

CM

pro

files

(Mdn

= 1

8.58

, n =

10)

, U =

54.

00, z

= -

2.16

, p <

.05,

r =

.39.

See

Tab

le 2

0.

2.

Team

s with

the

mod

erat

e co

oper

ativ

e V

CM

pro

files

(M

dn =

19.

71 n

= 3

7) a

lso

repo

rted

high

er le

vels

of

team

con

flict

eff

icac

y th

an te

ams w

ith m

oder

ate

com

petit

ive

VC

M p

rofil

es (M

dn =

18.

58, n

= 1

0), U

=

101.

00, z

= -2

.26,

p <

.05,

r =

.33.

See

Tab

le 2

0.

 

144  

!

6!

Tabl

e 22

(Con

tinue

d)

Sum

mar

y Fi

ndin

gs fo

r th

e M

ain

and

Supp

lem

enta

ry A

naly

ses

Res

earc

h Q

uest

ions

St

atis

tical

ana

lyse

s Fi

ndin

gs

Expl

orin

g gr

oup

diff

eren

ces i

n m

embe

r sat

isfa

ctio

n w

ith te

am

conf

lict m

anag

emen

t pro

cess

scor

es

amon

g th

e fo

ur V

CM

cat

egor

ies

iden

tifie

d in

the

late

nt c

lass

ana

lyse

s

One

-way

AN

OV

A

N

o si

gnifi

cant

ove

rall

grou

p di

ffer

ence

in m

embe

r sa

tisfa

ctio

n w

ith th

e te

am’s

con

flict

man

agem

ent

proc

ess s

core

s acr

oss a

ll fo

ur V

CM

cat

egor

ies,

F (3

, 75

) = .2

9, n

s. Se

e Ta

ble

21.

Expl

orin

g gr

oup

diff

eren

ces i

n te

am

effe

ctiv

enes

s sco

res a

mon

g th

e fo

ur

VC

M c

ateg

orie

s ide

ntifi

ed in

the

late

nt c

lass

ana

lyse

s

One

-way

AN

OV

A

No

sign

ifica

nt o

vera

ll gr

oup

diff

eren

ce in

team

ef

fect

iven

ess s

core

s acr

oss a

ll fo

ur V

CM

cat

egor

ies,

F (3

, 75)

= 1

.03,

ns.

See

Tab

le 2

1.

!

 

145  

Appendix A

Email Request for Recruiting Study Participants

Subject Line: Request for Recruiting Participants for a Dissertation Study on Teamwork Experiences

Dear Professor [NAME],

I am writing to request for your assistance in recruiting participants for my dissertation study on teamwork experiences. This dissertation research is being conducted as part of the Lim Kim San Research Fellowship at the Singapore Management University, under the supervision of Dr. Tan Hwee Hoon, Associate Professor of Organizational Behavior and Human Resources, and at Teachers College, Columbia University, under the supervision of Dr. Peter T. Coleman, Associate Professor of Psychology and Education.

The main purpose of this study is to investigate the effects of team characteristics on teamwork processes, and how these processes may in turn affect important outcomes. For this study, I am interested in recruiting intact student teams whose members are expected to work together on a final team project throughout the academic semester.

Based on the description of the course that you are currently teaching, it appears that the students in your course would meet the sample criterion for my study. As such, I would greatly appreciate it if you would allow me to invite your students to participate in this study. This study will be administered in the form of a short online survey, and all information obtained will be kept strictly confidential and will only be used solely for the purposes of this research.

Your assistance in my efforts to recruit participants would be greatly appreciated, and it would help me tremendously in completing my dissertation.

THANK YOU for considering my request above, and I look forward to hearing from you soon.

Sincerely,

(Rae) Yunzi Tan Visiting Lim Kim San Research Fellow Organizational Behavior and Human Resources Lee Kong Chian School of Business Singapore Management University Doctoral Candidate Program in Social-Organizational Psychology Department of Organization and Leadership Teachers College, Columbia University

 

146  

Appendix A (Continued)

Email Request for Study Participation

Dear Professor [NAME],

Thank you very much for agreeing to assist me in recruiting participants for my dissertation study.

Please feel free to forward the following message with your students, so they can be made aware of this voluntary opportunity for research participation:

************************************************************************

Subject Line: Request for Participation in a Study on Teamwork Experiences

Dear [PARTICIPANT NAME],

This is an email to inform you of a voluntary opportunity for research participation. The main purpose of this research is to investigate the effects of team characteristics on teamwork processes, and how these processes may in turn affect important outcomes. Your participation is important because the results will help us better understand individuals’ teamwork experiences and team effectiveness.

This study is being administered in the form of a short online survey. To begin this survey, please click on the following link: [INSERT LINK HERE].

This survey will take approximately 20 minutes to complete. All information gathered in this study will be kept strictly confidential and used solely for the purposes of this research. You will receive more information on how you may collect the monetary incentive offered for your participation on the Informed Consent Form prior to beginning the survey, and again at the end of the survey.

Please complete this survey no later than 11.59pm, March 31, 2012. If you have any questions or would like to receive a copy of the study results, please do not hesitate to email me. Thank you again for your participation!

Sincerely,

(Rae) Yunzi Tan Visiting Lim Kim San Research Fellow Organizational Behavior and Human Resources Lee Kong Chian School of Business Singapore Management University Doctoral Candidate Program in Social-Organizational Psychology Department of Organization and Leadership Teachers College, Columbia University

 

147  

Appendix B

Study Questionnaire

Teamwork Experiences Survey

Thank you for participating in this survey study on teamwork experiences. In this survey, you will be asked to respond to questions concerning your perceptions of and experiences in working with your peers on a specific team project this semester.

As you complete this survey, please provide your responses pertaining to the team you have been working with in the course: OBHR 001 – Leadership and Team Building / OBHR 101 – Management of People at Work.

Please complete this survey by no later than 11:59pm on March 31, 2012.

The survey consists of five sections, and would take approximately 30 minutes to complete. Your participation is entirely voluntary. Any information gathered will be kept strictly confidential and used solely for the purposes of this research.

On the next page, you will see the Informed Consent Form related to this research. Please read this information carefully and indicate your consent to participate in this study where noted.

Thank you once again for your participation!

 

148  

Appendix B (Continued)

Informed Consent Form

DESCRIPTION OF THE RESEARCH: You are invited to participate in a research study on teamwork experiences. Specifically, this study is intended to investigate the effects of team characteristics on teamwork processes, and how these processes may in turn affect important outcomes. You will be asked to complete a short online survey questionnaire that assesses your perceptions of, and experiences in working with a project team throughout the academic semester. All information gathered in this study will be kept strictly confidential and will be used solely for the purposes of this research.

The research is conducted by (Rae) Yunzi Tan, a doctoral candidate in the Social-Organizational Psychology program at Teachers College, Columbia University, under the supervision of Dr. Peter T. Coleman, Associate Professor of Psychology and Education. This research is also conducted at the Lee Kong Chian School of Business at the Singapore Management University, as part of the Lim Kim San Research Fellowship program offered by the Organizational Behavior and Human Resources Division, and under the supervision of Dr. Tan Hwee Hoon, Associate Professor of Organizational Behavior and Human Resources.

TIME INVOLVEMENT: Your participation will involve completing an online questionnaire that will take approximately 20 minutes.

RISKS AND BENEFITS: The risks and possible benefits associated with this study are minimal, as the probability of encountering harm or discomfort in this study is no greater than what is typically encountered in a classroom activity or discussion on an important team-related issue. Some of the questions are of a sensitive nature and may cause some discomfort as you reflect on your team experiences.

However, there are also potential benefits that you may gain from this research. By responding to questions about their perceptions of and experiences with working in teams, you may gain novel and valuable insights about your team experiences. You will also be offered access to the results upon completion of the study.

Participation in this study is entirely voluntary. You may refuse to participate in the study, or withdraw at any time by closing your browser. Refusal or withdrawal from participation at any time will involve no penalty or no loss of accrued benefits to which you are otherwise entitled.

PAYMENTS: (For LTB participants) You will receive a small monetary incentive of $5.00 as payment for your participation. You may collect this payment at Room 5031 between 1:00 to 5:00pm from March 15 to 30, 2012 (Mon-Fri only). (For MPW participants) You will receive a research credit value of 0.5 for your participation.

 

149  

Appendix B (Continued)

DATA STORAGE TO PROTECT CONFIDENTIALITY: All responses to this survey will be kept completely confidential. Your responses will be entered into a database that can only be accessed by the researcher. Any personal or identifying information will be recoded using pseudonyms or non-identifiers as soon as the data is collected, so that responses cannot be linked directly back to individual participants in the database.

HOW WILL RESULTS BE USED: The results of this study will be used to fulfill the partial requirements for a Doctor of Philosophy degree from Teachers College, Columbia University. The reporting of the results will be done in a manner to ensure that all participants’ identities are completely anonymous.

FOR QUESTIONS OR WHOM TO CONTACT: If, at any time, you have questions regarding the research or your participation, you may contact the investigator, Ms. (Rae) Yunzi Tan, at 6828-0597. The project has been reviewed and approved by the Teachers College, Columbia University Human Subjects Committee. If, at any time, you have comments or concerns regarding the conduct of the research or questions about your rights as a research subject, you may contact the Teachers College, Columbia University Institutional Review Board /IRB. The phone number for the IRB is (212) 678-4105. Or, you can write to the IRB at Teachers College, Columbia University, 525 West 120th Street, New York, NY, 10027, Box 151. (SMU IRB contact info: Institutional Review Board Secretariat, Ms. Stephanie Tan, at 6828-1925, or email [email protected])

By clicking on the link below, and by submitting your responses, it is understood that you have consented to participate in the research.

[INSERT LINK HERE]

Principal Investigator’s Signature: Date: February 15, 2012

(Rae) Yunzi Tan Visiting Lim Kim San Research Fellow Organizational Behavior and Human Resources Lee Kong Chian School of Business Singapore Management University Doctoral Candidate Program in Social-Organizational Psychology Department of Organization and Leadership Teachers College, Columbia University

 

150  

Appendix B (Continued)

Participant’s Rights Form

Principal Investigator: (Rae) Yunzi Tan Research Title: Teamwork Experiences

• I have read the Research Description described in the preceding page. I have had the opportunity to ask questions about the purposes and procedures, via email, regarding this study. I have received a copy of the Research Description/Informed Consent Form and this Participant's Rights document.

• I understand that my participation is voluntary. Refusal to participate will involve no penalty. I understand that I may discontinue participation at any time without penalty or loss of accrued benefits (Benefits are accrued in proportion to the amount of study complete or otherwise stated by the researcher) to which I am otherwise entitled. I declare that I am at least 18 years of age.

• The researcher may withdraw me from the research at his/her professional discretion. • If, during the course of the study, significant new information that has been developed

becomes available which may relate to my willingness to continue to participate, the investigator will provide this information to me.

• Any information derived from the research project that personally identifies me will not be voluntarily released or disclosed without my separate consent, except as specifically required by law.

• If at any time I have any questions regarding the research or my participation, I can contact the investigator, who will answer my questions. The investigator's phone number is 6828-0597.

• If at any time, I have comments or concerns regarding the conduct of the research or questions about my rights as a research subject, I should contact the Teachers College, Columbia University Institutional Review Board /IRB. The phone number for the IRB is (212) 678-4105. Or, I can write to the IRB at Teachers College, Columbia University, 525 W. 120th Street, New York, NY, 10027, Box 151. (SMU IRB contact info: Institutional Review Board Secretariat, Ms. Stephanie Tan, at 6828-1925, or email [email protected])

• The written materials will be viewed only by the principal investigator and members of the research team. Written materials ( ) may be viewed in an educational setting outside the research OR ( ) may NOT be viewed in an educational setting outside the research.

• My signature below means that I agree to participate in this study.

Participant's signature (please type your name): _______________Date: ____/____/____

For the Principal Investigator: “I have explained and defined in detail the research procedures in which the participant has consented to participate.”

Principal Investigator’s signature: Date: February 15, 2012

To begin the survey, please click on the link below: [INSERT LINK HERE]

 

151  

Appendix B (Continued)

Section I: Team Characteristics

This section contains several characteristics pertaining to individual attributes and traits within your project team for the course: OBHR 001 - Leadership and Team Building / OBHR 101 - Management of People at Work.

Please rate how well each of the following describes you in this team, on a scale of 1 (never or almost never true) to 7 (always or almost always true):

Characteristics

1 = Never to Almost Never True to 7 = Always to Almost Always True

1. Tender 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 2. Defends own beliefs 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 3. Aggressive 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 4. Willing to take risks 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 5. Compassionate 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 6. Understanding 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 7. Gentle 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8. Warm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 9. Assertive 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 10. Sympathetic 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 11. Sensitive to the needs of others 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 12. Independent 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 13. Strong personality 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 14. Affectionate 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 15. Dominant 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 16. Forceful 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 17. Eager to soothe hurt feelings 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 18. Leadership ability 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 19. Willing to take a stand 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

 

152  

Appendix B (Continued)

Section II: Team Functioning

This section contains several statements assessing your perceptions of how your project team has functioned throughout the semester for the course: OBHR 001 - Leadership and Team Building / OBHR 101- Management of People at Work.

Please rate how strongly you agree or disagree with each of the statements below.

1 2 3 4 5 Strongly

Agree Agree Neither Agree

nor Disagree Disagree Strongly

Disagree

1. In this team, members ‘swim or sink’ together. 1 2 3 4 5

2. In this team, members give high priority to the things they want to accomplish and low priority to the things other team members want to accomplish.

1 2 3 4 5

3. In this team, members seek compatible goals. 1 2 3 4 5

4. In this team, members like to show that who is superior to one other.

1 2 3 4 5

5. In this team, members want one other to succeed.

1 2 3 4 5

6. When members work together in this team, they usually have common goals.

1 2 3 4 5

7. In this team, members structure things in ways that favor each member’s own goals rather than the goals of other team members.

1 2 3 4 5

8. In this team, members have a ‘win-lose’ relationship.

1 2 3 4 5

9. In this team, members’ goals go together. 1 2 3 4 5

10. In this team, members’ goals are incompatible with one other.

1 2 3 4 5

 

153  

Appendix B (Continued)

Section II: Team Functioning (Continued)

This section contains several statements assessing your perceptions of how your project team has functioned throughout the semester for the course: OBHR 001 - Leadership and Team Building / OBHR 101 - Management of People at Work.

Please rate the frequency or intensity to which each of the statement describes your team.

Never Occasionally Sometimes Often Always 1 2 3 4 5

Not At All or To a Very Small

Extent

To a Small Extent

To a Moderate Extent

To a Large Extent

To a Very Large Extent

11. How much conflict of ideas is there in this team? 1 2 3 4 5

12. To what extent has your team split into subgroups? 1 2 3 4 5

13. How much relationship tension is there in this team? 1 2 3 4 5

14. How frequently do members have disagreements within this team about the task of the project?

1 2 3 4 5

15. How much conflict is there in this team about task responsibilities?

1 2 3 4 5

16. How much emotional conflict is there in this team? 1 2 3 4 5

17. To what extent has your team split into uneven subgroups?

1 2 3 4 5

18. How often do your teammates have conflicting opinions about the project you are working on?

1 2 3 4 5

19. To what extent has your team split into multiple or smaller cliques?

1 2 3 4 5

20. How often are there disagreements about who should do what in this team?

1 2 3 4 5

21. How often do members get angry while working in this team?

1 2 3 4 5

22. To what extent has your team split into two balanced subgroups?

1 2 3 4 5

23. How often do members disagree about resource allocation in this team?

1 2 3 4 5

24. To what extent do members view conflict in this team as important?

1 2 3 4 5

 

154  

Appendix B (Continued)

Section III: Team Interactions

This section contains several statements pertaining to your perceptions of how you have interacted with your teammates throughout the semester for the course: OBHR 001 - Leadership and Team Building / OBHR 101 - Management of People at Work.

Please rate the extent to which each of the following statement describes you in this team.

1 2 3 4 5 Never Occasionally Sometimes Often Always

1. I encourage a “we are in it together” attitude

with my teammates. 1 2 3 4 5

2. I tend to view conflict as a win-lose contest. 1 2 3 4 5

3. I try to get my teammates to agree with my position.

1 2 3 4 5

4. I work with my teammates to find a solution that will be good for all of us.

1 2 3 4 5

5. I try to persuade my teammates to make concessions without necessarily making concessions myself.

1 2 3 4 5

6. I tend to overstate my position to get my way. 1 2 3 4 5

7. I treat conflict as a mutual problem to solve with my teammates.

1 2 3 4 5

8. I work in a way that will enable everyone to get what he or she really wants.

1 2 3 4 5

9. I work with my teammates to combine the best of positions to make an effective decision.

1 2 3 4 5

 

155  

Appendix B (Continued)

Section IV: Team Outcomes

This section assesses your beliefs and feelings toward your teamwork experiences for the course: OBHR 001 - Leadership and Team Building / OBHR 101 - Management of People at Work.

For each question below, please select a number from 1-7 that most accurately reflects your opinion. If you encounter a particular question that is not applicable to your teamwork experience, simply select “N/A.”

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 N/A Not At All Moderately Perfectly Not

Applicable

Please rate how strongly you agree or disagree with each of the following statements by selecting the appropriate number.

1 2 3 4 5 Strongly

Agree Agree Neither Agree

nor Disagree Disagree Strongly

Disagree

1. Do you feel that your teammates listened to your concerns?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 N/A

2. How satisfied are you with the ease (or difficulty) of reaching an agreement within the team?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 N/A

3. Would you characterize the team conflict management process as fair?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 N/A

4. Did your teammates consider your wishes, opinions, or needs?

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 N/A

5. Generally speaking, team members are very satisfied with their work.

1 2 3 4 5

6. The way this team manages its work inspires its team members to enhance team performance.

1 2 3 4 5

7. Team members feel a strong commitment to their work. 1 2 3 4 5

8. All things considered, this team is highly pleased with the way it manages its work.

1 2 3 4 5

9. Team members feel highly committed to the goals of their work.

1 2 3 4 5

 

156  

Appendix B (Continued)

Section IV: Team Outcomes (Continued)

This section assesses your beliefs and feelings toward your teamwork experiences for the course: OBHR 001 - Leadership and Team Building / OBHR 101 - Management of People at Work.

Please rate how strongly you agree or disagree with each of the following statements by selecting the appropriate number.

1 2 3 4 5 Strongly

Agree Agree Neither Agree

nor Disagree Disagree Strongly

Disagree

10. I believe that this team will manage the following conflicts in an effective manner: a. Among team members concerning personality differences 1 2 3 4 5 b. Among team members concerning work habits 1 2 3 4 5 c. Among team members concerning work roles 1 2 3 4 5 d. Among team members concerning scheduling 1 2 3 4 5 e. Among team members concerning the best way to get the

project done 1 2 3 4 5

 

157  

Appendix B (Continued)

Section V: Participant Demographics

Please answer the following questions about yourself and your team. All information collected will be kept strictly confidential and will be used solely for the purposes of this research.

Your name: ________________________________________________________________

Course title and section number: ________________________________________________

Name of your team (if any): ____________________________________________________

Number of members in your team (including yourself): ______________________________

Names of members in your team (including yourself): _______________________________

Were you assigned to your team by the course instructor (select one)? Yes / No

In general, how often do you interact with your teammates throughout the semester in person, phone and/or online (select one below)?

• Rarely (Less than 5 times a week) • Moderately (5-10 times a week) • Frequently (More than 10 times a week)

Age: _____________ Years of prior and current work experience: _____________________

Gender (select one): Male Female Other (e.g., transgender)

Ethnicity (select one): Chinese Malay Indian Biracial/Multiracial Other (please indicate): _______________________________________

Country of origin (where you were born): _________________________________________

Years of living in Singapore: ___________________________________________________

Area of specialization for your degree: ___________________________________________

 

158  

Appendix B (Continued)

Debriefing Message

Debrief

You have reached the end of the survey. Thank you again for your participation in this study. The specific objective of this research is to examine how team member characteristics and team contextual factors affect the development of variability in team conflict management, and how such variability in turn affect team outcomes.

Once again, please note that all your responses in this survey will be kept strictly confidential and will only be used for the purposes of this research.

(For LTB participants) You will receive $5.00 as payment for your participation. To collect your payment, please go to Room 5031 between 1:00 to 5:00 PM, from March 15 to 30, 2012 (Mon-Fri only). A member of the research team for this study will be available to assist with your collection.

(For MPW participants) You will receive a research credit value of 0.5 for your participation.

If you have any further questions or concerns about this study, please do not hesitate to contact me at [email protected] or call 6828-0597.

Sincerely,

(Rae) Yunzi Tan Visiting Lim Kim San Research Fellow Organizational Behavior and Human Resources Lee Kong Chian School of Business Singapore Management University Doctoral Candidate Program in Social-Organizational Psychology Department of Organization and Leadership Teachers College, Columbia University

 

159  

Appendix C

Study Measures

Conflict Management Approaches (Chen, Liu, & Tjosvold, 2005)

All items are rated on a five-point scale (1 = “never”; 3 = “sometimes”; 5 = “always”).

Cooperation

1. I encourage a “we are in it together” attitude with my teammates. 2. I work with my teammates to find a solution that will be good for all of us. 3. I treat conflict as a mutual problem to solve with my teammates 4. I work in a way that will enable everyone to get what they really want. 5. I work with my teammates to combine the best of positions to make an effective decision.

Competition

6. I try to get my teammates to agree with my position. 7. I try to persuade my teammates to make concessions without necessarily making

concessions myself. 8. I tend to view conflict as a win-lose contest. 9. I tend to overstate my position to get my way.

Gender Role Orientations (Bem, 1981)

How well does each of the following describe you in this team? (Items are rated on a scale of 1 = “never or almost never true” to 7 = “always or almost always true”)

Goal Interdependence (Lu, Tjosvold, & Shi, 2010)

All items are rated on a five-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree”; 5 = “strongly agree”).

Positive Interdependence

1. In this team, members ‘swim or sink’ together. 2. In this team, members want each other to succeed.

Masculinity Femininity 1. Assertive 1. Understanding 2. Leadership ability 2. Sympathetic 3. Dominant 3. Eager to soothe hurt feelings 4. Strong personality 4. Sensitive to needs of others 5. Forceful 5. Compassionate 6. Aggressive 6. Affectionate 7. Willing to take a stand 7. Gentle 8. Independent 8. Warm 9. Defends own beliefs 9. Tender 10. Willing to take risks

 

160  

Appendix C (Continued)

3. In this team, members seek compatible goals. 4. In this team, the goals of team members go together. 5. When we work together in this team, we usually have common goals. Negative Interdependence

6. In this team, members structure things in ways that favor their own goals rather than the goals of other team members.

7. In this team, members have a ‘win-lose’ relationship. 8. In this team, members like to show that they are superior to each other. 9. In this team, members’ goals are incompatible with each other. 10. In this team, members give high priority to the things they want to accomplish and low

priority to the things other team members want to accomplish.

Subgroup Formation (Cronin, Bezrukova, Weingart, & Tinsley, 2011)

All items are rated on a five-point scale (1 = “not at all or a very small extent”; 5 = “to a very large extent”).

1. To what extent has your team split into subgroups? 2. To what extent has your team split into multiple or smaller cliques? 3. To what extent has your team split into uneven subgroups? 4. To what extent has your team split into two balanced subgroups?

Team Conflict Efficacy (Alper, Tjosvold & Law, 2000)

All items are rated on a five-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree”; 5 = “strongly agree”).

I believe that our team will manage the following conflicts in an effective manner:

1. Among team members concerning personality differences 2. Among team members concerning work habits 3. Among team members concerning work roles 4. Among team members concerning scheduling 5. Among team members concerning the best way to get the project done

Satisfaction with Team Conflict Management Process (Curhan, Elfenbein, & Xu, 2006)

All items are rated on a seven-point scale (1 = “not at all”; 4 = “moderately”; 7 = frequently)

1. Do you feel your teammate(s) listened to your concerns? 2. Would you characterize the team conflict management process as fair? 3. How satisfied are you with the ease (or difficulty) of reaching an agreement within the

team? 4. Did your teammate(s) consider your wishes, opinions, or needs?

 

161  

Appendix C (Continued)

Perceived Team Effectiveness (Alper, Tjosvold, & Law, 1998; Barker, Tjosvold, & Andrews, 1988; Tjosvold, Law, & Sun, 2006)

All items are rated on a five-point scale (1 = “strongly disagree”; 5 = “strongly agree”).

1. Generally speaking, team members are very satisfied with their work. 2. Team members feel a strong commitment to their work. 3. Team members feel highly committed to the goals of their work. 4. The way this team manages its work inspires its teammates to enhance team

performance. 5. All things considered, this team is highly pleased with the way it manages its work.

Intra-team Conflict Types (Jehn & Mannix, 2001)

All items are rated on a five-point scale (1 = “never”; 3 = “sometimes”; 5 = “always”), except items 1, 4, 6 and 8, which are rated on a scale of 1 = “not at all or to a very small extent” to 5 = “to a very large extent”.

Task Conflict

1. How much conflict of ideas is there in this team? 2. How frequently do members have disagreements within this team about the task of the

project? 3. How often do your teammates have conflicting opinions about the project you are

working on?

Relationship Conflict

4. How much relationship tension is there in this team? 5. How often do members get angry while working in this team? 6. How much emotional conflict is there in this team?

Process Conflict

7. How often are there disagreements about who should do what in this team? 8. How much conflict is there in this team about task responsibilities? 9. How often do members disagree about resource allocation in this team?

Intra-team Conflict Importance

To what extent do members view conflict in this team as important? (Item is rated on a scale of 1 = “not at all or a very small extent” to 5 = “to a very large extent”)