uninterrupted infant sleep, development, and maternal mood · article uninterrupted infant sleep,...

10
ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD, a,b Christine Laganière, BSc, a,b,c Andrée-Anne Bouvette-Turcot, PhD, c Irina Pokhvisneva, MSc, c Meir Steiner, PhD, d Michael J. Meaney, PhD, c,e Hélène Gaudreau, PhD, c on behalf of the Mavan Research Team OBJECTIVES: Contrary to the importance of total sleep duration, the association between sleeping through the night and development in early infancy remains unclear. Our aims were to investigate the proportion of infants who sleep through the night (6- or 8-hour sleep blocks) at ages 6 and 12 months in a longitudinal cohort and to explore associations between sleeping through the night, mental and psychomotor development, maternal mood, and breastfeeding. METHODS: At 6 and 12 months of age, maternal reports were used to assess the longest period of uninterrupted infant sleep and feeding method ( n = 388). Two different criteria were used to determine if infants slept through the night: 6 and 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep. Mental and psychomotor developmental indices (Bayley Scales of Infant Development II) and maternal mood (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale) were measured at 6, 12, and 36 months of age. RESULTS: Using a definition of either 6 or 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep, we found that 27.9% to 57.0% of 6- and 12-month-old infants did not sleep through the night. Linear regressions revealed no significant associations between sleeping through the night and concurrent or later mental development, psychomotor development, or maternal mood (P > .05). However, sleeping through the night was associated with a much lower rate of breastfeeding (P < .0001). CONCLUSIONS: Considering that high proportions of infants did not sleep through the night and that no associations were found between uninterrupted sleep, mental or psychomotor development, and maternal mood, expectations for early sleep consolidation could be moderated. abstract a Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology and c Ludmer Center for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Canada; b Hôpital en Santé Mentale Rivière-des-Prairies, Centre intégré universitaire de santé et de services sociaux du Nord-de-Ille-de- Montréal, Montreal, Canada; d Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, Canada; and e Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore, Singapore Dr Pennestri conceptualized the study, conducted the statistical analyses, interpreted the data, drafted the initial manuscript, and reviewed the manuscript; Ms Laganière and Dr Bouvette-Turcot interpreted the data and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Ms Pokhvisneva supervised the statistical analyses, participated in the data interpretation, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Steiner participated in the study conceptualization and design, designed the data collection instruments, coordinated and supervised the data collection, interpreted the data, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Meaney acted as the principal investigator of this study, participated in the study conceptualization and design, interpreted the data, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Gaudreau coordinated and supervised the data collection, participated in the study conceptualization and design, PEDIATRICS Volume 142, number 6, December 2018:e20174330 WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Sleep plays a fundamental role in child development. Lack of sleep (total sleep duration) is associated with both physical and mental health problems in childhood. However, the specific association between consecutive sleep duration and development in early infancy remains unclear. WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: A high percentage of 6- and 12-month-old infants do not sleep through the night (uninterrupted sleep duration). When controlling for well-known confounding variables, no significant association was found between sleeping through the night, mental and psychomotor development, and maternal mood. To cite: Pennestri M-H, Laganière C, Bouvette-Turcot A-A, et al. Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood. Pediatrics. 2018;142(6):e20174330 by guest on March 24, 2020 www.aappublications.org/news Downloaded from

Upload: others

Post on 19-Mar-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

ARTICLE

Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal MoodMarie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD, a, b Christine Laganière, BSc, a, b, c Andrée-Anne Bouvette-Turcot, PhD, c Irina Pokhvisneva, MSc, c Meir Steiner, PhD, d Michael J. Meaney, PhD, c, e Hélène Gaudreau, PhD, c on behalf of the Mavan Research Team

OBJECTIVES: Contrary to the importance of total sleep duration, the association between sleeping through the night and development in early infancy remains unclear. Our aims were to investigate the proportion of infants who sleep through the night (6- or 8-hour sleep blocks) at ages 6 and 12 months in a longitudinal cohort and to explore associations between sleeping through the night, mental and psychomotor development, maternal mood, and breastfeeding.METHODS: At 6 and 12 months of age, maternal reports were used to assess the longest period of uninterrupted infant sleep and feeding method (n = 388). Two different criteria were used to determine if infants slept through the night: 6 and 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep. Mental and psychomotor developmental indices (Bayley Scales of Infant Development II) and maternal mood (Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale) were measured at 6, 12, and 36 months of age. RESULTS: Using a definition of either 6 or 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep, we found that 27.9% to 57.0% of 6- and 12-month-old infants did not sleep through the night. Linear regressions revealed no significant associations between sleeping through the night and concurrent or later mental development, psychomotor development, or maternal mood (P > .05). However, sleeping through the night was associated with a much lower rate of breastfeeding (P < .0001).CONCLUSIONS: Considering that high proportions of infants did not sleep through the night and that no associations were found between uninterrupted sleep, mental or psychomotor development, and maternal mood, expectations for early sleep consolidation could be moderated.

abstract

aDepartment of Educational and Counselling Psychology and cLudmer Center for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Canada; bHôpital en Santé Mentale Rivière-des-Prairies, Centre intégré universitaire de santé et de services sociaux du Nord-de-I’lle-de-Montréal, Montreal, Canada; dDepartment of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, McMaster University, Hamilton, Canada; and eSingapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, Singapore, Singapore

Dr Pennestri conceptualized the study, conducted the statistical analyses, interpreted the data, drafted the initial manuscript, and reviewed the manuscript; Ms Laganière and Dr Bouvette-Turcot interpreted the data and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Ms Pokhvisneva supervised the statistical analyses, participated in the data interpretation, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Steiner participated in the study conceptualization and design, designed the data collection instruments, coordinated and supervised the data collection, interpreted the data, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Meaney acted as the principal investigator of this study, participated in the study conceptualization and design, interpreted the data, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Gaudreau coordinated and supervised the data collection, participated in the study conceptualization and design,

PEDIATRICS Volume 142, number 6, December 2018:e20174330

WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Sleep plays a fundamental role in child development. Lack of sleep (total sleep duration) is associated with both physical and mental health problems in childhood. However, the specific association between consecutive sleep duration and development in early infancy remains unclear.

WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: A high percentage of 6- and 12-month-old infants do not sleep through the night (uninterrupted sleep duration). When controlling for well-known confounding variables, no significant association was found between sleeping through the night, mental and psychomotor development, and maternal mood.

To cite: Pennestri M-H, Laganière C, Bouvette-Turcot A-A, et al. Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood. Pediatrics. 2018;142(6):e20174330

by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 2: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

Sleep plays a fundamental role in child development, and a growing body of evidence suggests that lack of sleep in children is associated with both physical and mental health problems.1 –5 Considering the high percentages of children with sleep problems (ranging from 20% to 35%), 6 – 8 interest has grown in sleep disorder prevention, resulting in behavioral interventions at increasingly younger ages.9, 10 Researchers in several recent studies have proposed such interventions in infants as young as 3 months of age.11, 12

For most parents, the time when infants should consolidate their sleep (a process often called “sleeping through the night”) is a major concern.13 Sleeping through the night is a different concept from total nocturnal sleep duration (which refers to the total sleep duration during the night), and it is also different from total sleep duration (which refers to the total sleep duration in a 24-hour period). Sleeping through the night is instead defined as the longest period of uninterrupted sleep without parental intervention (ie, sleep blocks of a specific duration, typically 6 or 8 hours).14 This is widely considered a developmental milestone that should be achieved at approximately age 5 or 6 months.14, 15 However, researchers in several studies have reported that not all infants conform to this classic developmental timeline.14, 16, 17 Although studies show that increasing percentages of infants sleep through the night as they grow up, they also show major interindividual differences.14, 18

Sleeping through the night at age 6 to 12 months is generally considered the gold standard in Western industrialized nations, and behavioral sleep training (such as controlled crying) is popular among parents and professionals.19, 20 However, the idea of night awakenings as being problematic in early development is

not unanimous, leading to conflicting advice from professionals.10, 21, 22 To get infants to sleep through the night, interventions such as delayed response and feeding during the night have been proposed.12, 23 However, many parents report that these methods are incongruent with their personal beliefs and find sleep interventions too difficult, particularly attempts to ignore infant crying.24

The beneficial effects of sufficient total sleep duration on emotional regulation, attention, memory, and executive functions are well documented in the literature.2, 25 – 31 Nevertheless, the specific contribution of sleeping through the night to infant development remains underinvestigated and unclear. Moreover, sleep interventions in early infancy are also meant to improve maternal well-being.32 Because poor maternal sleep quality is associated with increased maternal depressive symptoms, 33, 34 whether infants sleep through the night may influence maternal mood. Finally, breastfeeding and cosleeping are frequently mentioned factors associated with sleep fragmentation in infants.35, 36 Therefore, they are key factors to consider when investigating sleep-wake cycle consolidation.

Considering the importance of sleep in daily life and the conflicting messages parents receive about the importance of early sleep consolidation, it would be worthwhile to determine if sleeping through the night in early infancy is associated with early child development and maternal mood. Our aims in the current study were to (1) determine the percentage of infants who sleep through the night at ages 6 and 12 months (using 6- or 8-hour sleep blocks as criteria) in a longitudinal cohort; (2) determine feeding method as a function of sleeping through the night or not; and (3) explore associations

between sleeping through the night, mental development, psychomotor development, and maternal mood.

METHODS

Participants

Participants were part of the longitudinal birth cohort study Maternal Adversity, Vulnerability, and Neurodevelopment (MAVAN). Sleep measures were available for 388 mother-infant dyads at 6 months of age (182 girls and 206 boys; Table 1) and for 369 mother-infant dyads at 12 months of age (176 girls and 193 boys; 308 participants completed sleep measures at both time points). Pregnant mothers were recruited between 13 and 20 weeks’ gestation from obstetric clinics in Montreal, Québec, and Hamilton, Ontario (Canada). Inclusion criteria were aged ≥18 years and fluency in English or French. Mothers who reported serious obstetric complications, chronic illness, congenital disease, or other serious medical conditions were excluded. Infants with serious complications during delivery, serious medical conditions, or who were born at <37 weeks’ gestation were also excluded. Mothers signed a consent form approved by the ethics committee of the Douglas Mental Health University Institute (Montreal) and St Joseph’s Healthcare (McMaster University, Hamilton).

Measures and Procedures

Sleeping Through the Night

When infants in the study were 6 and 12 months old, mothers responded to a questionnaire about their infant’s sleeping habits during the last 2 weeks (adapted from the Self-Administered Questionnaire for the Mother).37 To determine if infants slept through the night, mothers answered the following question: “During the night, how many consecutive hours does your child sleep without waking up?” In their

PENNESTRI et al2 by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 3: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

review, Henderson et al14 identified 6 or 8 hours of consecutive sleep as the 2 most often used criteria in the literature to define sleeping through the night. Accordingly, we used both criteria (6 or 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep) as definitions of sleeping through the night or not.

Child Development

Mental and psychomotor developmental indices were measured at the infant’s home at ages 6, 12, and 36 months (Bayley Scales of Infant Development II)38 by trained research assistants. This instrument includes both mental (cognitive and language) and motor (gross and fine motor skills) developmental scales and is used to assess development in early childhood.

Maternal Mood

Maternal mood was assessed during pregnancy (third trimester) and at 6, 12, and 36 months postnatally with the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.39 Items on the scale reflect symptom frequency during the previous week. This instrument has been validated in postpartum samples.40

Feeding Method

Feeding method was assessed at 6 and 12 months of age with the retrospective breastfeeding questionnaire.41 At each time point, infants were classified as breastfed or not.

Covariates

The Self-Administered Questionnaire for the Mother also contains questions on cosleeping status and total sleep duration in a 24-hour period. Given the well-known associations between education, income, sleep, and child development, 42, 43 socioeconomic status (SES) was assessed (as reported by the mother during pregnancy). Statistics Canada’s low-income cutoff (before tax) was used to assess income level (high versus low). Maternal education was dichotomized between low and high education (high education: at least some college education). SES was broken down into the following 2 categories: high SES (high income and high mother education) and middle to low SES (low on at least 1 category).

Statistical Analyses

The percentage of infants who slept through the night in our cohort was calculated at ages 6 and 12 months by using a 6-hour criterion and an 8-hour criterion. χ2 tests were used to assess the relationships between sleeping through the night and infant sex, SES, and breastfeeding. The significance level for the descriptive statistics was set at P < .05. Linear regressions were used to assess the associations between sleeping through the night or not and mental development, psychomotor development, and maternal depression (concurrently and for the next available time point). For all regressions, infant sex, SES, breastfeeding status,

cosleeping status, and total sleep duration in a 24-hour period were used as covariates. In addition, prenatal maternal mood was used as a covariate in regressions that predicted maternal mood to control for initial levels before birth. Statistical analyses were conducted by using IBM SPSS Statistics 24 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY). The Holm-Bonferroni method was used to adjust for multiple comparisons (regressions).

RESULTS

Sleeping Through the Night or Not at 6 Months of Age Using the 6-Hour Criterion

In Fig 1A, we show the percentage of infants who slept through the night or not at 6 months of age using the 6-hour criterion (as reported by mothers). Whereas 62.4% of mothers reported that their infant got ≥6 hours of consecutive sleep (6-hour sleep blocks), 37.6% reported <6 hours. A higher percentage of girls than boys slept through the night (69.8% vs 55.8%; χ2 = 8.02, P = .005). SES was similar between infants who slept through the night and those who did not (χ2 = 0.33, P = .57). In the group that slept through the night, 55.0% of the infants were breastfed at age 6 months; however, this proportion was much higher in the group that did not sleep through the night (80.8%; χ2 = 26.67, P < .0001; Fig 2A).

Sleeping through the night or not at 6 months of age was not associated with concurrent mental development (β = −1.78, P = .10), psychomotor development (β = −1.33, P = .39), or maternal mood (β = −.96, P = .34). Similar results were observed for the same outcomes at 12 months of age (mental development: β = −1.89, P = .17; psychomotor development: β = −.75, P = .67; maternal mood β = 1.17, P = .27).

PEDIATRICS Volume 142, number 6, December 2018 3

TABLE 1 Characteristics of the MAVAN Cohort

Total Sample at 6 Months of Age, n = 388

SES, low, high, % 21.8, 78.2Sex, girl, boy, % 46.9, 53.1Maternal age at birth, y, mean ± SD 30.53 ± 4.94Maternal mood at 6 mo, using the CES-D, mean ± SD 11.36 ± 9.90Infant mental development at 6 mo, using the Bayley

Scales of Infant Development II, mean ± SD97.32 ± 8.33

Infant motor development at 6 mo, using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development II, mean ± SD

98.36 ± 11.84

CES-D, Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale.

by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 4: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

Sleeping Through the Night or Not at 6 Months of Age Using the 8-Hour Criterion

Using the 8-hour criterion, we found that 43.0% of mothers reported that their infant got at least 8 hours of consecutive sleep, whereas more than half of mothers (57.0%) reported <8 hours (Fig 1B). A slightly higher percentage of girls than boys slept through the night (47.8% vs 38.8%; χ2 = 3.17, P = .08), but this difference was not statistically significant. SES was similar for the 2 groups (χ2 = 0.10, P = .76). Again, the group that did not sleep through the night included a higher percentage of breastfed infants (76.5% vs 49.1%; χ2 = 31.19; P < .0001; Fig 2B).

Using the 8-hour criterion, we found that sleeping through the night or not at 6 months of age was not associated

with concurrent mental development (β = −1.53, P = .15), psychomotor development (β = −2.14, P = .15), or maternal mood (β = .12, P = .90). Comparable results were observed for all variables at 12 months of age (mental development: β = −1.78, P = .19; psychomotor development: β = −.77, P = .66; maternal mood: β = 1.51, P = .14).

Sleeping Through the Night or Not at 12 Months of Age Using the 6-Hour Criterion

At 12 months, a higher percentage of mothers reported that their infant slept at least 6 consecutive hours (72.1%), but 27.9% still reported <6 hours (Fig 1C). Again, the percentage of girls compared with boys who slept through the night was slightly higher but not statistically significant (76.7% vs 67.9%; χ2 = 3.57, P = .059),

and no association with SES was observed (χ2 = 1.01, P = .32). Nonetheless, breastfeeding status and consecutive sleep duration remained strongly associated. In the group that slept through the night, only 23.8% of infants were breastfed; however, in the group that did not sleep through the night, half of the infants were still breastfed (56.4%; χ2 = 34.96, P < .0001; Fig 2C).

Using the 6-hour criterion, we found that sleeping or not through the night at 12 months of age was not associated with concurrent mental development (β = −1.19, P = .45), psychomotor development (β = 1.43, P = .49), or maternal mood (β = −1.52, P = .19). Similar results were observed when assessing the same outcomes at 36 months of age (mental development: β = −1.51, P = .41; psychomotor development: β = −1.59, P = .48; and maternal mood: β = −.08, P = .96).

Sleeping Through the Night or Not at 12 Months of Age Using the 8-Hour Criterion

In Fig 1D, we show the percentage of infants who slept through the night at 12 months of age using the 8-hour criterion. 56.6% of mothers reported that their infant slept ≥8 consecutive hours, whereas 43.4% of mothers reported <8 hours. No significant associations were found between sleeping through the night and the infant’s sex (χ2 = 2.38, P = .12) or SES (χ2 = 1.83, P = .18). However, breastfeeding status and sleeping through the night remained strongly associated; 22.1% of infants who slept through the night were breastfed at 12 months of age as opposed to 47.1% in the group that did not sleep through the night (χ2 = 25.24; P < .0001; Fig 2D).

Using the 8-hour criterion, we found that sleeping through the night or not at 12 months of age was not associated with concurrent mental development (β = −.92, P = .50), psychomotor development

PENNESTRI et al4

FIGURE 1Percentages of infants sleeping or not through the night in the MAVAN cohort. A, Infants at 6 months of age who attained 6 hours of uninterrupted sleep. At 6 months of age, 62.4% of infants were sleeping through the night as measured by the 6-hour criterion; 37.6% of infants were not. B, Infants at 6 months of age who attained 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep. At 6 months of age, 43.0% of infants were sleeping through the night as measured by the 8-hour criterion; 57.0% of infants were not. C, Infants at 12 months of age who attained 6 hours of uninterrupted sleep. At 12 months of age, 72.1% of infants were sleeping through the night as measured by the 6-hour criterion; 27.9% of infants were not. D, Infants at 12 months of age who attained 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep. At 12 months of age, 56.6% of infants were sleeping through the night as measured by the 8-hour criterion; 43.4% of infants were not.

by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 5: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

(β = −.29, P = .87), or maternal mood (β = .10, P = .92); there was also no association with the same outcomes at 36 months of age with mental development (β = −2.90, P = .06), psychomotor development (β = −1.85, P = .33), or maternal mood (β = 1.53, P = .22).

DISCUSSION

The results of this study indicate that by using a commonly accepted definition (6 hours of consecutive sleep), a high percentage of infants in our cohort did not sleep through the night at either age 6 months (37.6%) or 12 months (27.9%). Using an 8-hour criterion for consecutive sleep, we found that more than half of the 6-month-old infants did not sleep through the night (57.0%),

whereas 43.4% of infants at 12 months of age did not sleep through the night. These results fall within the range reported in a recent review.14 Moreover, a higher percentage of girls than boys slept through the night at some time points. This sex-based difference is consistent with results from another recent, large, longitudinal study that found more awakenings in boys compared with girls in infancy.44

In the present sample of typically developing infants, we were unable to find any significant associations between sleeping through the night or not at 6 and 12 months of age and variations in mental or psychomotor development. Whereas the beneficial effects of sufficient total sleep duration in childhood

and adolescence are well known, the associations between sleep-wake cycle patterns and development are much less straightforward during the first year of life.45 Although a few studies found associations between certain polysomnographic sleep measures and development in early infancy, 46, 47 most of these studies did not specifically measure consecutive sleep duration. To our knowledge, only 1 study conducted in 1985 revealed an association between the longest consecutive sleep duration and higher mental development scores at 24 months of age.48 However, this study was performed in a small sample of preterm infants. Considering that individual differences may also be linked to brain maturation, other authors have questioned the existence of a straightforward relationship between sleep and early development.45, 49

Moreover, we found no associations between sleeping through the night or not and postnatal maternal mood. This is noteworthy because maternal sleep deprivation is often invoked to support the introduction of early behavioral interventions.33, 34 Perhaps maternal total sleep duration (during the night and/or daytime naps) or fatigue could be better predictors of maternal well-being. Future studies could document mothers’ sleep quality and durations in association with their infants’ sleep patterns, considering not only total sleep duration but also sleeping through the night. Earlier time points should also be assessed to clarify if associations are present before 6 months of age.

Importantly, our results reveal a strong relationship between breastfeeding and not sleeping through the night. This association was present at 6 and 12 months of age as measured by both the 6- and 8-hour criteria. Studies have documented the many benefits of breastfeeding for both children and mothers, including lower

PEDIATRICS Volume 142, number 6, December 2018 5

FIGURE 2Percentages of breastfed infants as a function of sleeping or not through the night in the MAVAN cohort. A, Infants at 6 months of age who attained 6 hours of uninterrupted sleep. In the group that slept through the night (6-hour criterion), 55.0% of infants were breastfed at the age of 6 months as opposed to 80.8% in the group that did not sleep through the night (χ2 = 26.67; P < .0001). B, Infants at 6 months of age who attained 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep. In the group that slept through the night (8-hour criterion), 49.1% of infants were breastfed at the age of 6 months as opposed to 76.5% in the group that did not sleep through the night (χ2 = 31.19; P < .0001). C, Infants at 12 months of age who attained 6 hours of uninterrupted sleep. In the group that slept through the night (6-hour criterion), 23.8% of infants were breastfed at the age of 12 months as opposed to 56.4% in the group that did not sleep through the night (χ2 = 34.96; P < .0001). D, Infants 12 months of age who attained 8 hours of uninterrupted sleep. In the group that slept through the night (8-hour criterion), 22.1% of infants were breastfed at the age of 12 months as opposed to 47.1% in the group that did not sleep through the night (χ2 = 25.24; P < .0001).

by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 6: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

risks of respiratory infections, gastroenteritis, and asthma.50 Breastfeeding also has a positive impact on cognitive functions.51 The World Health Organization recommends exclusive breastfeeding for the first 6 months of age and partial breastfeeding for up to 2 years or for as long as the mother and child wish.52 However, over the same developmental period, parents are encouraged to teach their infant to sleep through the night, and breastfeeding is often invoked among the factors associated with sleep fragmentation.35 This conflicting advice may undoubtedly confuse parents, and contradictory information was specifically identified as a risk factor for maladaptation in new parents.53 Yet, the results of our study do not allow for the drawing of any causality between not sleeping through the night and breastfeeding; this should be evaluated in future studies.

Some authors have questioned the use of sleep behavioral techniques in young infants because sleep consolidation is a developmental process that is influenced by interindividual variations.22 Moreover, some mothers have reported feeling tense and depressed when they tried to get their infant to sleep through the night, explaining that for them rapid sleep consolidation was not a priority.54 In addition, authors have proposed that because parents are routinely asked about sleep consolidation at medical follow-up meetings, this may implicitly suggest that infants should sleep through the night.55, 56 In a recent review, authors concluded that behavioral interventions at <6 months of age neither reduced crying nor prevented sleep problems, and they noted side effects such as anxiety and a premature cessation of breastfeeding.21 However, the conclusions of this review have been questioned by others, 10 again reflecting the controversy

over the desirability of early sleep consolidation.

In the absence of a consensus on the importance of the early onset of sleeping through the night, breastfeeding, and sleep-related parental practices, it was shown that experts often refer to their own values or experience when advising parents.57 However, these values are not necessarily congruent with every parent’s needs. It is important to teach behavioral sleep interventions when there is a need and on parental request. However, parental expectations and sleep arrangements vary considerably according to culture and values.58 Therefore, health professionals should also take into consideration alternative parental values, especially in nonclinical samples.

The findings of this exploratory study should be replicated in larger normative cohorts. It is possible that results from a larger sample would reveal associations between sleeping through the night, mental development, psychomotor development, and maternal mood. Nevertheless, this study included 388 participants, and although several time points, criteria, and outcome measures were tested, no significant associations were found. In addition, the strength of our results is supported by the fact that no previous study to our knowledge has demonstrated a clear association between these measures and consecutive sleep duration.

It is possible that some awakenings were not signaled to the mothers by infants or that mothers did not hear their infant. Although it would be informative to replicate these results by using an objective measure of sleep (actigraphy or videosomnography), the present results reflect the general parental perception of wondering when their infant will (or should) sleep through the night. Variables such as the frequency of nocturnal breastfeeding,

use of behavioral sleep interventions, introduction of solid food, and parental sleep exhaustion were not available in our study and should be measured in future studies.

CONCLUSIONS

New mothers appear to be greatly surprised about the degree of sleep disturbance and exhaustion that they experience.59 As a potential protective strategy, mothers could be more informed about the normal development of the sleep-wake cycle instead of only focusing on methods and interventions. The transition to parenthood is a vulnerable period of life, and it could be reassuring for parents to learn that in a typically developing cohort up to 37.6% of infants do not sleep 6 consecutive hours at age 6 months and up to 27.9% do not at age 12 months. Although there is a clear need to replicate these findings, we found no associations between infants sleeping through the night at 6 or 12 months of age and variations in their mental or motor development and maternal well-being. However, a significantly higher rate of breastfeeding was found in infants who did not sleep through the night. Keeping in mind the wide variability in the age when an infant starts to sleep through the night, expectations for early sleep consolidation could be moderated.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank the MAVAN research team. Thanks to Margaret McKyes for linguistic editing.

PENNESTRI et al6

ABBREVIATIONS

MAVAN:  Maternal Adversity, Vulnerability, and Neurodevelopment

SES:  socioeconomic status

by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 7: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

REFERENCES

1. Quist JS, Sjödin A, Chaput JP, Hjorth MF. Sleep and cardiometabolic risk in children and adolescents. Sleep Med Rev. 2016;29:76–100

2. Touchette E, Petit D, Tremblay RE, Montplaisir JY. Risk factors and consequences of early childhood dyssomnias: new perspectives. Sleep Med Rev. 2009;13(5):355–361

3. Paavonen EJ, Räikkönen K, Lahti J, et al. Short sleep duration and behavioral symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in healthy 7- to 8-year-old children. Pediatrics. 2009;123(5). Available at: www. pediatrics. org/ cgi/ content/ full/ 123/ 5/ e857

4. Chaput JP, Gray CE, Poitras VJ, et al. Systematic review of the relationships between sleep duration and health indicators in school-aged children and youth. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2016;41(6 suppl 3):S266–S282

5. Fatima Y, Doi SA, Mamun AA. Longitudinal impact of sleep on overweight and obesity in children and adolescents: a systematic review and bias-adjusted meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2015;16(2):137–149

6. Byars KC, Yolton K, Rausch J, Lanphear B, Beebe DW. Prevalence, patterns, and persistence of sleep problems in the first 3 years of life. Pediatrics. 2012;129(2). Available at: www. pediatrics. org/ cgi/ content/ full/ 129/ 2/ e276

7. Gaylor EE, Burnham MM, Goodlin-Jones BL, Anders TF. A longitudinal follow-up study of young children’s

sleep patterns using a developmental classification system. Behav Sleep Med. 2005;3(1):44–61

8. Owens J. Classification and epidemiology of childhood sleep disorders. Prim Care. 2008;35(3):533–546, vii

9. Gradisar M, Jackson K, Spurrier NJ, et al. Behavioral interventions for infant sleep problems: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2016;137(6):e20151486

10. Crichton GE, Symon B. Behavioral management of sleep problems in infants under 6 months–what works? J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2016;37(2):164–171

11. Santos IS, Bassani DG, Matijasevich A, et al. Infant sleep hygiene counseling (sleep trial): protocol of a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. 2016;16(1):307

12. St James-Roberts I, Sleep J, Morris S, Owen C, Gillham P. Use of a behavioural programme in the first 3 months to prevent infant crying and sleeping problems. J Paediatr Child Health. 2001;37(3):289–297

13. Thiedke CC. Sleep disorders and sleep problems in childhood. Am Fam Physician. 2001;63(2):277–284

14. Henderson JM, France KG, Blampied NM. The consolidation of infants’ nocturnal sleep across the first year of life. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15(4):211–220

15. Anders TF, Halpern LF, Hua J. Sleeping through the night: a developmental perspective. Pediatrics. 1992;90(4):554–560

16. Moore T, Ucko LE. Night waking in early infancy. I. Arch Dis Child. 1957;32(164):333–342

17. Michelsson K, Rinne A, Paajanen S. Crying, feeding and sleeping patterns in 1 to 12-month-old infants. Child Care Health Dev. 1990;16(2):99–111

18. Goodlin-Jones BL, Burnham MM, Gaylor EE, Anders TF. Night waking, sleep-wake organization, and self-soothing in the first year of life. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2001;22(4):226–233

19. Maute M, Perren S. Ignoring children’s bedtime crying: the power of Western-oriented beliefs. Infant Ment Health J. 2018;39(2):220–230

20. Loutzenhiser L, Hoffman J, Beatch J. Parental perceptions of the effectiveness of graduated extinction in reducing infant night-wakings. J Reprod Infant Psychol. 2014;32(3):282–291

21. Douglas PS, Hill PS. Behavioral sleep interventions in the first six months of life do not improve outcomes for mothers or infants: a systematic review. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2013;34(7):497–507

22. Blunden SL, Thompson KR, Dawson D. Behavioural sleep treatments and night time crying in infants: challenging the status quo. Sleep Med Rev. 2011;15(5):327–334

23. Mindell JA, Kuhn B, Lewin DS, Meltzer LJ, Sadeh A; American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Behavioral treatment of bedtime problems and night

PEDIATRICS Volume 142, number 6, December 2018 7

participated in the data interpretation, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

DOI: https:// doi. org/ 10. 1542/ peds. 2017- 4330

Accepted for publication Sep 10, 2018

Address correspondence to Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD, Department of Educational and Counselling Psychology, McGill University, Education Building, Room 614, 3700 McTavish St, Montreal, QC H3A 1Y2, Canada. E-mail: [email protected]

PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).

Copyright © 2018 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

FUNDING: Funded by the Ludmer Center for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Canadian Institutes of Health Research, the Norlien Foundation (Calgary, Canada), the WOCO Foundation (London, Canada), McGill University, Fondation des Petits Trésors, and Fonds de recherche du Québec-Santé.

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found online at http:// www. pediatrics. org/ cgi/ doi/ 10. 1542/ 2018- 2589.

by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 8: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

wakings in infants and young children. Sleep. 2006;29(10):1263–1276

24. Etherton H, Blunden S, Hauck Y. Discussion of extinction-based behavioral sleep interventions for young children and reasons why parents may find them difficult. J Clin Sleep Med. 2016;12(11):1535–1543

25. Dahl RE. The impact of inadequate sleep on children’s daytime cognitive function. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 1996;3(1):44–50

26. Fulda S, Schulz H. Cognitive dysfunction in sleep disorders. Sleep Med Rev. 2001;5(6):423–445

27. Durmer JS, Dinges DF. Neurocognitive consequences of sleep deprivation. Semin Neurol. 2005;25(1):117–129

28. Jones K, Harrison Y. Frontal lobe function, sleep loss and fragmented sleep. Sleep Med Rev. 2001;5(6):463–475

29. Walker MP. Sleep-dependent memory processing. Harv Rev Psychiatry. 2008;16(5):287–298

30. Weaver TE. Outcome measurement in sleep medicine practice and research. Part 2: assessment of neurobehavioral performance and mood. Sleep Med Rev. 2001;5(3):223–236

31. Sadeh A. Consequences of sleep loss or sleep disruption in children. Sleep Med Clin. 2007;2(3):513–520

32. Symon B, Crichton GE. The joy of parenting: infant sleep intervention to improve maternal emotional well-being and infant sleep. Singapore Med J. 2017;58(1):50–54

33. McDaniel BT, Teti DM. Coparenting quality during the first three months after birth: the role of infant sleep quality. J Fam Psychol. 2012;26(6):886–895

34. Meltzer LJ, Mindell JA. Relationship between child sleep disturbances and maternal sleep, mood, and parenting stress: a pilot study. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21(1):67–73

35. Touchette E, Petit D, Paquet J, et al. Factors associated with fragmented sleep at night across early childhood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(3):242–249

36. Ramamurthy MB, Sekartini R, Ruangdaraganon N, Huynh DHT, Sadeh A, Mindell JA. Effect of

current breastfeeding on sleep patterns in infants from Asia-Pacific region. J Paediatr Child Health. 2012;48(8):669–674

37. Petit D, Touchette E, Paquet J, Montplaisir J. Sleep: development and associated factors. In: The Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (QLSCD 1998-2002) From Birth to 29 Months. Vol 2. Quebec, Canada: Institut de la Statistique du Québec; 2002:1–67

38. Bayley N. Manual for the Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Cooperation; 1993

39. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385–401

40. Moszkowski RJ, Stack DM, Girouard N, Field TM, Hernandez-Reif M, Diego M. Touching behaviors of infants of depressed mothers during normal and perturbed interactions. Infant Behav Dev. 2009;32(2):183–194

41. Jonas W, Atkinson L, Steiner M, Meaney MJ, Wazana A, Fleming AS; MAVAN Research Team. Breastfeeding and maternal sensitivity predict early infant temperament. Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(7):678–686

42. Seo WH, Kwon JH, Eun SH, Kim G, Han K, Choi BM. Effect of socio-economic status on sleep. J Paediatr Child Health. 2017;53(6):592–597

43. Milne S, McDonald J, Kayrouz N. Determinants of developmental progress in pre-schoolers referred for neuro-developmental diagnosis. J Paediatr Child Health. 2016;52(11):1004–1011

44. Netsi E, Santos IS, Stein A, Barros FC, Barros AJD, Matijasevich A. A different rhythm of life: sleep patterns in the first 4 years of life and associated sociodemographic characteristics in a large Brazilian birth cohort. Sleep Med. 2017;37:77–87

45. Ednick M, Cohen AP, McPhail GL, Beebe D, Simakajornboon N, Amin RS. A review of the effects of sleep during the first year of life on cognitive, psychomotor, and temperament development. Sleep. 2009;32(11):1449–1458

46. Scher A. Infant sleep at 10 months of age as a window to cognitive development. Early Hum Dev. 2005;81(3):289–292

47. Becker PT, Thoman EB. Rapid eye movement storms in infants: rate of occurrence at 6 months predicts mental development at 1 year. Science. 1981;212(4501):1415–1416

48. Anders TF, Keener MA, Kraemer H. Sleep-wake state organization, neonatal assessment and development in premature infants during the first year of life. II. Sleep. 1985;8(3):193–206

49. Freudigman KA, Thoman EB. Infant sleep during the first postnatal day: an opportunity for assessment of vulnerability. Pediatrics. 1993;92(3):373–379

50. Eidelman AI. Breastfeeding and the use of human milk: an analysis of the American Academy of Pediatrics 2012 breastfeeding policy statement. Breastfeed Med. 2012;7(5):323–324

51. Smithers LG, Kramer MS, Lynch JW. Effects of breastfeeding on obesity and intelligence: causal insights from different study designs. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169(8):707–708

52. World Health Organization; UNICEF. Global Strategy for Infant and Young Child Feeding. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 2003

53. Barimani M, Vikström A, Rosander M, Forslund Frykedal K, Berlin A. Facilitating and inhibiting factors in transition to parenthood - ways in which health professionals can support parents. Scand J Caring Sci. 2017;31(3):537–546

54. Germo GR, Goldberg WA, Keller MA. Learning to sleep through the night: solution or strain for mothers and young children? Infant Ment Health J. 2009;30(3):223–244

55. Ball HL. Breastfeeding, bed-sharing, and infant sleep. Birth. 2003;30(3):181–188

56. Marchand L, Morrow MH. Infant feeding practices: understanding the decision-making process. Fam Med. 1994;26(5):319–324

57. Jenni OG, O’Connor BB. Children’s sleep: an interplay between culture and biology. Pediatrics. 2005;115(suppl 1):204–216

58. Mindell JA, Sadeh A, Kohyama J, How TH. Parental behaviors and sleep outcomes in infants and toddlers: a cross-cultural comparison. Sleep Med. 2010;11(4):393–399

59. Kennedy HP, Gardiner A, Gay C, Lee KA. Negotiating sleep: a qualitative study of new mothers. J Perinat Neonatal Nurs. 2007;21(2):114–122

PENNESTRI et al8 by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 9: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2017-4330 originally published online November 12, 2018; 2018;142;Pediatrics 

Mavan Research TeamPokhvisneva, Meir Steiner, Michael J. Meaney, Hélène Gaudreau and on behalf of the

Marie-Hélène Pennestri, Christine Laganière, Andrée-Anne Bouvette-Turcot, IrinaUninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood

ServicesUpdated Information &

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/142/6/e20174330including high resolution figures, can be found at:

Referenceshttp://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/142/6/e20174330#BIBLThis article cites 57 articles, 7 of which you can access for free at:

Subspecialty Collections

http://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/sleep_medicine_subSleep Medicineal_issues_subhttp://www.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/development:behaviorDevelopmental/Behavioral Pediatricsfollowing collection(s): This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the

Permissions & Licensing

http://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtmlin its entirety can be found online at: Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or

Reprintshttp://www.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtmlInformation about ordering reprints can be found online:

by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from

Page 10: Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood · ARTICLE Uninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood Marie-Hélène Pennestri, PhD,a, b Christine Lagani

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2017-4330 originally published online November 12, 2018; 2018;142;Pediatrics 

Mavan Research TeamPokhvisneva, Meir Steiner, Michael J. Meaney, Hélène Gaudreau and on behalf of the

Marie-Hélène Pennestri, Christine Laganière, Andrée-Anne Bouvette-Turcot, IrinaUninterrupted Infant Sleep, Development, and Maternal Mood

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/142/6/e20174330located on the World Wide Web at:

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is

1073-0397. ISSN:60007. Copyright © 2018 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print

the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois,has been published continuously since 1948. Pediatrics is owned, published, and trademarked by Pediatrics is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it

by guest on March 24, 2020www.aappublications.org/newsDownloaded from