two pathways to performance: affective- and motivationally

323
University of Central Florida University of Central Florida STARS STARS Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 2012 Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally- Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally- driven Development In Virtual Multiteam Systems driven Development In Virtual Multiteam Systems Miliani Jimenez-Rodriguez University of Central Florida Part of the Industrial and Organizational Psychology Commons Find similar works at: https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd University of Central Florida Libraries http://library.ucf.edu This Doctoral Dissertation (Open Access) is brought to you for free and open access by STARS. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 by an authorized administrator of STARS. For more information, please contact [email protected]. STARS Citation STARS Citation Jimenez-Rodriguez, Miliani, "Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally-driven Development In Virtual Multiteam Systems" (2012). Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019. 2209. https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd/2209

Upload: others

Post on 15-Jan-2022

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

University of Central Florida University of Central Florida

STARS STARS

Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019

2012

Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally-Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally-

driven Development In Virtual Multiteam Systems driven Development In Virtual Multiteam Systems

Miliani Jimenez-Rodriguez University of Central Florida

Part of the Industrial and Organizational Psychology Commons

Find similar works at: https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd

University of Central Florida Libraries http://library.ucf.edu

This Doctoral Dissertation (Open Access) is brought to you for free and open access by STARS. It has been accepted

for inclusion in Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019 by an authorized administrator of STARS. For more

information, please contact [email protected].

STARS Citation STARS Citation Jimenez-Rodriguez, Miliani, "Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally-driven Development In Virtual Multiteam Systems" (2012). Electronic Theses and Dissertations, 2004-2019. 2209. https://stars.library.ucf.edu/etd/2209

Page 2: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

TWO PATHWAYS TO PERFORMANCE:

AFFECTIVE- AND MOTIVATIONALLY-DRIVEN DEVELOPMENT IN VIRTUAL

MULTITEAM SYSTEMS

by

MILIANI JIMENEZ-RODRIGUEZ

M.S. University of Central Florida, 2010

B.A. The Pennsylvania State University, 2005

A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements

for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

in the Department of Psychology

in the College of Sciences

at the University of Central Florida

Orlando, Florida

Summer Term

2012

Major Professor: Leslie A. DeChurch

Page 3: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

ii

© 2012 Miliani Jiménez-Rodríguez

Page 4: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

iii

ABSTRACT

Multiteam systems are an integral part of our daily lives. We witness these entities in

natural disaster responses teams, such as the PB Oil Spill and Hurricane Katrina, governmental

agencies, such as the CIA and FBI, working behind the scenes to preemptively disarm terrorist

attacks, within branches of the Armed Forces, within our organizations, and in science teams

aiming to find a cure for cancer (Goodwin, Essens, & Smith, 2012; Marks & Luvison, 2012).

Two key features of the collaborative efforts of multiteam systems are the exchange of

information both within and across component team boundaries as well as the virtual tools

employed to transfer information between teams (Keyton, Ford, & Smith, 2012; Zaccaro, Marks,

& DeChurch, 2012).

The goal of this dissertation was to shed light on enabling the effectiveness of multiteam

systems. One means of targeting this concern was to provide insight on the underpinnings of

MTS mechanism and how they evolve. The past 20 years of research on teams supports the

central role of motivational and affective states (Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006; and Mathieu,

Maynard, Rapp, & Gibson, 2008) as critical drivers of performance. Therefore it was my interest

to understand how these critical team mechanisms unravel at the multiteam system level and

understanding how they influence the development of other important multiteam system

processes and emergent states. Specifically, this dissertation focused on the influence

motivational and affective emergent states (such as multiteam efficacy and multiteam trust) have

on shaping behavioral processes (such as information sharing-unique and open) and cognitive

emergent states (such as Transactive memory systems and shared mental models). Findings from

Page 5: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

iv

this dissertation suggest that multiteam efficacy is a driver of open information sharing in

multiteam systems and both types of cognitive emergent states (transactive memory systems and

shared mental models). Multiteam trust was also found to be a critical driver of open

information sharing and the cognitive emergent state transactive memory systems.

Understanding that these mechanisms do not evolve in isolation, it was my interest to

study them under a growing contextual state that is continuously infiltrating our work lives

today, under virtual collaboration. This dissertation sought to uncover how the use of distinct

forms of virtual tools, media rich tools and media retrievability tools, enable multiteam systems

to develop needed behavioral processes and cognitive emergent states. Findings suggest that the

use of media retrievability tools interacted with the task mental models in promoting the

exchange of unique information both between and within component teams of a multiteam

system.

The implications of these findings are twofold. First, since both motivational and

affective emergent states of members within multiteam systems are critical drivers of behavioral

processes, cognitive emergent states, and in turnmultiteam system performance; future research

should explore how we can diagnose as well as target the development of multiteam system level

efficacy and trust. Second, the virtual communication tools that providemultiteam systems

members the ability to review discussed materials at a later point in time are critical for sharing

information both within and across component teams depending on the level of shared cognition

that multiteam system members possess of the task.Therefore the ability to encourage the use

and provide such tools for collaborative purposes is beneficial for the successful collaboration of

multiteam systems.

Page 6: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

v

Para Mami, Papi, Junito,

Abuelo Luis, Abuela Milagros,

y Titi Tere

Page 7: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

vi

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

I want to thank my mentor Leslie and all my committee members, Dr. Salas, Dr. Zaccaro,

and Dr. Bowers for all your support throughout this dissertation process. Many have stated that

the dissertation process is an uphill battle, but I thank each and every one of you for making a

worthwhile and enjoyable experience. It was a pleasure and honor to have so many pioneers of

the team and MTS literature in one room guiding me through my dissertation work. Thank you

Leslie, Dr. Salas, Dr. Zaccaro, and Dr. Bowers.

Grazie Leslie! You have been a true mentor, friend, and role model over the last 10 years

(note to all, I have not been in graduate school that long). Since we first met while I was an

undergraduate you have championed me throughout my entire academic career and you have

been a big part of my success. A thousand thanks for your unconditional support throughout

these years.

Arigatou Toshio! My graduate school colleague and friend that has seen me through all

the tough times graduate school throws our way. You have been instrumental in my success

through countless classes, test preps, analyses, conference preparations, grant deadlines,

publication deadlines, dissertation work, and the list goes on and on. Thank you Toshio for

always being such a great friend and colleague. I look forward to our collaboration over the

years and our continued friendship.

Gratias tibi ago Dan! You have been the reason this entire dissertation was a success.

Thank you to your computer programming skills, for developing the simulation, for pulling all

objective indices, for talking about data with me and for accepting all my nagging emails. I truly

Page 8: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

vii

appreciate all your support throughout this process.

Thank you Marissa Shuffler! You have been a true friend throughout graduate school and

this fun dissertation process. I truly enjoyed our lunch and coffee breaks where we were

extremely productive developing new ideas for the future of MTS and virtual teams. I would also

like to thank Jessie Wildman for her support throughout this entire dissertation process, her help

with bootstrapping, and for being such a great friend and colleague.Thank you Jessie!

DELTA lab members: Celise, Eric, Serena, Scott, Jeremy, Adriane, Alissa, Allison,

Amanda, Ashley, Bianka, Christina, Christine, Chris, John, Kyle, Michelle, Mikael, Sklyer,

Corey, Budd, Anthony, Jesse, Melissa, and Kevin (last, but not least), thank you to all for your

hard work, long nights, and relentless commitment to the lab. This dissertation was made

possible thanks to each and everyone of you!! Thank you JA for P&P.

Gracias a mi familia!!! Mami, Papi, Junito, Abuela Milagros, Titi Anny, David Luis,

Martamaria, Tio David, Abuela Clara, Abuelo Oreste, Titi Blanqui, Titi Amalia, Maritza, he

Inma. A todos los que están conmigo y a los que me velan desde el cielo, Abuelo Luis y Titi

Tere. Este logro se lo dedico a todos ustedes que atreves de los años me han brindado el mas

grande apoyo que uno puede pedir, sus oraciones, sus palabras positivas y mas que nada su amor.

Gracias a todos por siempre creer en mi.

Mami, Papi, y Junito – ¡Mil gracias! ¡Lo logramos!

Page 9: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

viii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

LIST OF FIGURES ...................................................................................................................... xii

LIST OF TABLES ....................................................................................................................... xiv

CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................. 1

Statement of the Problem ............................................................................................................................... 1

Literature Review ............................................................................................................................................... 5

Multiteam Systems ....................................................................................................................................... 5

Models of Team and Multiteam Performance ................................................................................... 6

Emergent States ............................................................................................................................................. 9

Team and MTS Efficacy ............................................................................................................................................. 9

Team and MTS Trust ................................................................................................................................................ 12

Team and MTS Cognition........................................................................................................................................ 16

Transactive Memory Systems ........................................................................................................................ 17

Shared Mental Models ........................................................................................................................................ 18

Information Sharing ................................................................................................................................... 20

Virtuality ......................................................................................................................................................... 21

Theory Development and Hypotheses .................................................................................................... 24

Motivationally-Driven Development .................................................................................................. 26

Affect-Driven Development .................................................................................................................... 28

Media Retrievability Moderator ............................................................................................................ 31

Media Richness Moderator ..................................................................................................................... 33

CHAPTER TWO: METHOD ....................................................................................................... 35

Page 10: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

ix

Participants ......................................................................................................................................................... 35

Power Analysis .................................................................................................................................................. 35

Overview of the Experimental Protocol .................................................................................................. 37

MTS Task Environment ........................................................................................................................................... 37

Performance Episodes ............................................................................................................................................. 38

Playing Environment ................................................................................................................................................ 39

Training .......................................................................................................................................................................... 40

Networking the Task ................................................................................................................................................ 40

MTS Structure .................................................................................................................................................... 41

MTS Goal Hierarchy .................................................................................................................................................. 41

Individual Level .......................................................................................................................................................... 42

Component Team Level........................................................................................................................................... 42

Integration of Teams ................................................................................................................................................ 43

Procedure ............................................................................................................................................................ 44

Measures .............................................................................................................................................................. 45

MTS Efficacy ................................................................................................................................................................. 46

MTS Trust ...................................................................................................................................................................... 47

MTS Information Sharing ....................................................................................................................................... 47

MTS Transactive Memory Systems .................................................................................................................... 48

MTS Shared Mental Models ................................................................................................................................... 49

MTS Performance ...................................................................................................................................................... 49

Communication Medium ........................................................................................................................................ 51

Control Variables ....................................................................................................................................................... 52

CHAPTER THREE: RESULTS ................................................................................................... 54

Page 11: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

x

Overview of the Analyses Presented in the Results ........................................................................... 54

Theoretical Model Analyses ......................................................................................................................... 55

Motivationally-Driven Development .................................................................................................. 55

Affect-Driven Development .................................................................................................................... 59

Media Retrievability Moderator ............................................................................................................ 62

Media Richness Moderator ..................................................................................................................... 63

Follow-Up Exploratory Analyses Model 1 .............................................................................................. 64

Motivationally-Driven Development .................................................................................................. 64

Affectively-Driven Development .......................................................................................................... 66

Media Retrievability Moderator ............................................................................................................ 70

Media Richness Moderator ..................................................................................................................... 74

Follow-Up Exploratory Analyses Model 2 .............................................................................................. 76

Motivationally-Driven Development .................................................................................................. 77

Affect-Driven Development .................................................................................................................... 79

Media Retrievability Moderator ............................................................................................................ 82

Media Richness Moderator ..................................................................................................................... 84

Follow-Up Exploratory Analyses Model 3 .............................................................................................. 84

Motivationally-Driven Development .................................................................................................. 84

Affectively-Driven Development .......................................................................................................... 85

Media Retrievability Moderator ............................................................................................................ 90

Media Richness Moderator ..................................................................................................................... 94

CHAPTER FOUR: DISCUSSION ............................................................................................... 97

Motivational and Affective Emergent States Shaping the Dynamics in Multiteam Systems

Page 12: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

xi

.................................................................................................................................................................................................. 97

Shared Cognition in Multiteam Systems ................................................................................................. 99

Virtuality in Multiteam Systems .............................................................................................................. 101

Limitations and Potential Solutions ....................................................................................................... 103

Differences Between Teams and MTSs ........................................................................................... 103

Measurement Issues ............................................................................................................................... 107

Design Issues .............................................................................................................................................. 109

Implications for Future Research ........................................................................................................... 114

Theoretical Contributions .......................................................................................................................... 122

Practical Implications .................................................................................................................................. 123

Conclusion ........................................................................................................................................................ 125

APPENDIX A FIGURES .......................................................................................................... 127

APPENDIX B TABLES ............................................................................................................ 147

APPENDIX C SESSION TIMELINE ....................................................................................... 278

APPENDIX D PERFORMANCE MISSION INTELLIGENCE DISTRIBUTION .................. 280

APPENDIX E INTERNAL REVIEW BOARD HUMAN SUBJECTS PERMISSION LETTER

..................................................................................................................................................... 286

REFERENCES ........................................................................................................................... 289

Page 13: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

xii

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1: Theoretical model. ....................................................................................................... 128

Figure 2: Taskforce DELTA goal hierarchy. .............................................................................. 129

Figure 3: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 1..................................................................... 130

Figure 4: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 2..................................................................... 131

Figure 5: Follow-up exploratory analyses Model 3. ................................................................... 132

Figure 6: Information sharing uniqueness within team and multiteam TMS relationship

moderated by media retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 31. ........ 133

Figure 7: Information sharing uniqueness within team and multiteam TMS relationship

moderated by media retrievability social and task information exchanged – depiction of Model

33................................................................................................................................................. 134

Figure 8: Information sharing uniqueness between teams and multiteam task SMM relationship

moderated by media retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 66. ........ 135

Figure 9: Information sharing uniqueness within team and multiteam task SMM relationship

moderated by media retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 67. ........ 136

Figure 10: Information sharing uniqueness between teams and multiteam task SMM relationship

moderated by media retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 68. ........ 137

Figure 11: Information sharing uniqueness within team and multiteam task SMM relationship

moderated by media retrievability task information exchanged– depiction of Model 69. ......... 138

Figure 12: Multiteam TMS and information sharing uniqueness between teams relationship

Page 14: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

xiii

moderated by media retrievability social information exchanged – depiction of Model 123. ... 139

Figure 13: Multiteam task SMM and information sharing uniqueness between teams relationship

moderated by media retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 169. ...... 140

Figure 14: Multiteam task SMM and information sharing uniqueness within team relationship

moderated by media retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 178. ...... 141

Figure 15: Multiteam task SMM and information sharing uniqueness within team relationship

moderated by media retrievability social and task information exchanged – depiction of Model

181............................................................................................................................................... 142

Figure 16: Theoretical model findings. ....................................................................................... 143

Figure 17: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 1 findings. ................................................... 144

Figure 18: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 2 findings. ................................................... 145

Figure 19: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 3 findings. ................................................... 146

Page 15: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

xiv

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1: Dissertation Hypotheses Summarized .......................................................................... 148

Table 2: Zero Order Correlations of Study Variables ................................................................. 157

Table 3: Cronbach Alpha and Rwg Statistics for Study Variables ............................................. 160

Table 4: Multiteam Efficacy ....................................................................................................... 161

Table 5: Trust .............................................................................................................................. 164

Table 6: Information Sharing Psychometric Scale Items ........................................................... 165

Table 7: Transactive Memory Systems -Psychometric Scale ..................................................... 166

Table 8: Shared Mental Models .................................................................................................. 167

Table 9: Performance Indices ..................................................................................................... 168

Table 10: Controls....................................................................................................................... 169

Table 11: Zero Order Correlations between Control Variables and Study Variables ................ 170

Table 12: Analyses Regressing MTS IS Uniqueness on MTS Efficacy ..................................... 171

Table 13: Analyses Regressing MTS Performance on MTS IS Uniqueness .............................. 175

Table 14: Mediator Analyses: The MTS IS and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by

MTS TMS ................................................................................................................................... 177

Table 15: Analyses Regressing MTS IS Openness on MTS Trust ............................................. 179

Table 16: Analyses Regressing MTS Performance on MTS IS Openness ................................. 180

Table 17: Mediator Analyses: The MTS IS Openness and MTS Performance Relationship

Mediated by MTS SMM ............................................................................................................. 181

Page 16: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

xv

Table 18: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS Cognition Relationship

Moderated by Media Retrievability ............................................................................................ 184

Table 19: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Openness and MTS SMM Relationship Moderated

by Media Richness ...................................................................................................................... 188

Table 20: Analyses Regressing MTS IS Openness on MTS Efficacy ........................................ 190

Table 21: Mediator Analysis: The MTS IS Openness and MTS Performance Relationship

Mediated by MTS TMS .............................................................................................................. 192

Table 22: Analyses Regressing MTS IS Uniqueness on MTS Trust .......................................... 193

Table 23: Mediator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS Performance Relationship

Mediated by MTS SMM ............................................................................................................. 194

Table 24: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS SMM Relationship

Moderated by Media Retrievability ............................................................................................ 201

Table 25: Moderator Analyses: The IS Openness and MTS TMS Relationship Moderated by

Media Retrievability ................................................................................................................... 211

Table 26: Moderator Analysis: The MTS IS Openness and MTS SMM Relationship Moderated

by Media Retrievability .............................................................................................................. 214

Table 27: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Openness and MTS TMS Relationship Moderated

by Media Richness ...................................................................................................................... 219

Table 28: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS SMM Relationship

Moderated by Media Richness.................................................................................................... 220

Table 29: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS TMS Relationship Moderated

by Media Richness ...................................................................................................................... 223

Page 17: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

xvi

Table 30: Lagged Correlation Analysis of MTS Behavior and MTS Cognition Relationship ... 225

Table 31: Analyses Regressing MTS TMS on MTS Efficacy .................................................... 226

Table 32:Analyses Regressing MTS Performance on MTS TMS .............................................. 228

Table 33: Mediator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by

MTS IS Uniqueness .................................................................................................................... 229

Table 34: Analyses Regressing MTS SMM on MTS Trust ........................................................ 231

Table 35: Analyses Regressing MTS Performance on MTS SMM ............................................ 233

Table 36: Mediator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by

MTS IS Openness ....................................................................................................................... 235

Table 37: Moderator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS IS Uniqueness Relationship Moderated

by Media Retrievability .............................................................................................................. 238

Table 38: Moderator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS IS Openness Relationship Moderated

by Media Richness ...................................................................................................................... 241

Table 39: Analyses Regressing MTS SMM on MTS Efficacy ................................................... 243

Table 40: Analyses Regressing MTS TMS on MTS Trust ......................................................... 248

Table 41: Mediator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by

MTS IS Openness ....................................................................................................................... 249

Table 42: Mediator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by

MTS IS Uniqueness .................................................................................................................... 251

Table 43: Moderator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS IS Openness Relationship Moderated

by Media Retrievability .............................................................................................................. 257

Table 44: Moderator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS IS Uniqueness Relationship

Page 18: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

xvii

Moderated by Media Retrievability ............................................................................................ 259

Table 45: Moderator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS IS Openness Relationship Moderated

by Media Retrievability .............................................................................................................. 268

Table 46: Moderator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS IS Uniqueness Relationship

Moderated by Media Richness.................................................................................................... 273

Table 47: Moderator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS IS Openness Relationship Moderated

by Media Richness ...................................................................................................................... 276

Table 48: Moderator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS IS Uniqueness Relationship Moderated

by Media Richness ...................................................................................................................... 277

Page 19: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

1

CHAPTER ONE: INTRODUCTION

Statement of the Problem

The Mars Climate Orbiter was built in December 1998 in response to NASA’s efforts to

find more cost-effective solutions to interplanetary missions. The main purpose of the Mars

Climate Orbiter was to study the climate and weather of Mars and the history of water on Mars.

In September 1999 the Mars Climate Orbiter lost contact with Earth’s team. Rather than enter the

Mars orbit, it crashed into the planet’s surface. The Orbiter cost 193 million dollars to build, and

the cost of the total mission amounted to 327.6 million dollars (figure includes launching,

mission operations, and space craft development). The Orbiter was built by teams distributed

between Colorado and California, and reports suggest the failure can be largely traced to human

collaborative errors (Thompson, 2010; Hinds & Weisband, 2003; Webley, 2010). Teams at the

two sites were exchanging information electronically and did not detect that they were designing

key parts using different units of measurement: metric and English. In what way did the

virtuality of teams play a role in the Orbiter failure?

On September 11, 2001, 19 Al-Qaeda terrorists took control of four commercial flights

and used them to attack the World Trade Center and the Pentagon, ultimately killing 2,996

people (including the 19 terrorists). These attacks represent an enormous failure of the US

intelligence agencies. Multiple agencies were responsible for analyzing intelligence, including

the Central Intelligence Agency (CIA) and the Federal Bureau of Investigation (FBI). The 9-11

Page 20: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

2

Commission's Final Report identified the failure of the FBI and CIA to share information with

each other as a critical breakdown. Republican commissioner and former secretary of the Navy,

John Lehman, noted, “We need to ensure the fusion and sharing of all intelligence that could

have helped us to avoid 9/11” (2004, Associated Press). In what way did the organizational

boundaries play a role in the 9-11 terrorist attacks?

The Orbiter and 9-11 terrorist attacks illustrate the potential consequences that can arise

when organizational collectives fail to accomplish their goals. These examples illustrate two

important structural/contextual factors that complicate teamwork: virtuality and group

boundaries, and two processes critical to these collectives: cognition and information sharing.

This dissertation seeks to advance our understanding of the cognitive and informational

processes so important to the success of virtual, multi-team collectives.

Structuring work around teams has become a steady trend in organizations. The goals

teams strive for are often not the ultimate criteria of interest in organizations – rather, team goals

are intermediate building blocks of larger goals. Teams specialize and need to coordinate with

other teams. The examples above both illustrate the consequences of breakdowns in the handoffs

between teams. The unit of analysis capturing the dynamics of multiple teams has been termed

the multiteam system (Mathieu, Marks, & Zaccaro, 2001). As organizations adopt these new and

more complex infrastructures, researchers need to expand knowledge on the inner workings of

multiteam systems. To do this one pertinent area we must focus on is the modes of

communication utilized by these systems and how such media impacts information sharing.

Communication is the means by which teams create an understanding of the task and arrive at

key decisions.

Page 21: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

3

With the increasing transparency of communication technologies, the use of virtual teams

and the scope of the projects they undertake are both increasing. Past research has focused on

understanding the effects of virtuality on team effectiveness by comparing virtual teams to face-

to-face teams (Baltes, Dickson, Sherman, Bauer, & LaGanke, 2002). Yet, the reality is that very

little work today is conducted without some form of virtual communication. Workers rely on

instant messaging, online chatting, online message boards, email, teleconferencing, video

conferencing, and many more electronic tools to facilitate collaboration. Each of these tools

mentioned has its pros and cons and can be characterized along three dimensions of virtuality

proposed by Kirkman and Mathieu (2005): use of virtual tools, level of synchronicity, and

informational value. We need to understand how information exchanged through distinct media

can be more useful to teams in developing distinct cognitive structures, such as mental models

and transactive memory systems, both of which have been shown to predict team performance

(Lewis, 2003; Mohammed, Ferdanzi, & Hamilton, 2010; DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2010).

Accordingly, this dissertation makes five contributions to the team effectiveness

literature. First, this dissertation will add new knowledge to an increasingly important and little

understood area of teamsresearch: multiteam systems (MTS; Mathieu, Marks, Zaccaro, 2001;

DeChurch & Mathieu, 2009; Zaccaro, Marks, & DeChurch, 2011). As organizations continue to

structure work around teams as the basic unit, teams need to collaborate across boundaries as

components of larger systems of teams. This dissertation will add foundational knowledge to our

understanding of the inner workings of multiteam systems.

Second, this dissertation will shed light on the mechanisms through which two emergent

states known to be important to teams – motivation and affect- shape the dynamics of multiteam

Page 22: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

4

systems. It is important to examine the relative impact of motivational and affective states on

behavioral processes and cognitive emergent states and how they influence multiteam system

functioning. By uncovering the mechanisms through which motivational and affective emergent

states impact multiteam system performance we can better inform the development of

scientifically-based interventions.

Third, this dissertation will contribute to our knowledge on the type of information

sharing that takes place in virtual teams and the processes that lead to such behaviors. Meta-

analytic work by Mesmer-Magnus, DeChurch, Jiménez, Wildman, and Shuffler (2011) and

Mesmer-Magnus and DeChurch (2009) notes that information sharing is positively related to

team performance. The authors of these two meta-analyses have discovered that the type of

information shared among team members is distinctly predictive of team performance dependent

on team context. Mesmer-Magnus and DeChurch concluded that in face-to-face teams unique

information sharing is more predictive of performance than open information sharing although

teams tend to engage more in this form of information sharing. Mesmer-Magnus et al. deduced

that yes, unique information sharing is more predictive of performance but only for face-to-face

teams; for virtual teams open information sharing is more predictive of team performance than

unique information sharing, even though virtual team members tend to engage in more unique

information sharing. Therefore, the next steps should focus on understanding what team

processes encourage the exchange of both unique and open information sharing. This dissertation

will look at how collective efficacy and trust shape the exchange of information that takes place

between teams.

Fourth, this dissertation will explore the development of cognition in these complex

Page 23: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

5

systems. Research has shown that three distinct mediators, taking the form of processes and

emergent states, have been the essence of effective teams. These mediating mechanisms have

been classified into affective or motivational states, behavioral processes, and cognitive

emergent states (Kozlowski & Bell, 2002; Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006; and Mathieu et al., 2008)

sometimes referred to as the ABCs of teamwork (Salas, Rosen, Burke, & Goodwin, 2009). Meta-

analytic work by DeChurch and Mesmer-Magnus (2010) has shown that cognition is one of the

most influential mechanisms that contributes to team performance. This meta-analytic evidence

demonstrates the importance of team cognition as a driver of team performance, therefore

understanding how cognition is developed and how cognition influences other virtual multiteam

mechanisms is critical for the advancement of the effective functioning of multiteam systems.

Fifth, as most multiteam systems operate with some degree of physical distance, it is

critical to understand the impact of virtual communication on their functioning. This dissertation

will explore the impact of virtuality on multiteam dynamics. Research on the impact of virtuality

has often represented virtuality at two points, comparing and contrasting virtual teams to their

face-to-face counterparts. This study takes a more advanced approach and tests the impact of two

virtuality tools 1) media rich and 2) media retrievability tool (provide high informational value)

on the development of behavioral and cognitive processes.

Literature Review

Multiteam Systems

Keeping on the cutting edge of business today has led many organizations to venture out

Page 24: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

6

beyond teams. As the nature of organizational work changes due to trends toward globalization,

flattening of organizational structure, and outsourcing of work, much work ends up being

accomplished by system of teams working together rather than just one team. In many instances,

many organizations have to collaborate with teams in other departments within the same

organization, with teams in other organizations, or be part of global teams. Recent work by

Mathieu, Marks, and Zaccaro (2001) defined “two or more teams that interface directly and

interdependently in response to environmental contingencies toward the accomplishment of

collective goals” as multiteam systems (from this point forward MTS or system will be used

interchangeably to refer to multiteam systems; p. 290).

As noted by Zaccaro, Marks, and DeChurch (2011) one of the key defining features of

MTSs is the interdependence that takes place both within and across teams. In the earliest work

on MTS, Mathieu, Marks, and Zaccaro (2001) define the interdependence that takes place in

MTSs as “a state by which entities have mutual reliance, determination, influence, and shared

vested interest in processes they use to accomplish work activities (p. 293).”It is through these

distinct forms of interdependence, resource interdependence, process interdependence, and

outcome interdependence (Mathieu, Marks, Zaccaro, 2001; DeChurch & Mathieu, 2009;

Zaccaro, Marks, DeChurch, 2011; Mathieu, 2011) that MTSs successfully attain both proximal

team goals and distal system goals.

Models of Team and Multiteam Performance

As noted by McGrath (2000), teams are dynamic, complex, adaptive systems. As such,

team researchers have sought a way to understand the intervening mechanismsthrough which

Page 25: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

7

teams convert inputs into outputs. The first framework for understanding team performance is

the input-process-output (IPO) model (McGrath, 1964; Hackman, 1987); the second is the

expanded input-mediator-output-input (Ilgen, Hollenbeck, Johnson, & Jundt, 2005) model. The

premise behind both models is that inputs (e.g., team composition, training programs, leadership)

shape group states and interaction processes (e.g., cohesion, trust, collective efficacy,

coordination, backup behavior, development of transactive memory systems, and shared mental

models). Group states and processes, in turn, impact team effectiveness (e.g., speed to market,

team effectiveness, team efficiency, and reduced errors).

Over the years, teams researchers have posited that there are three critical types of

processes that are vital to team effectiveness. These mechanisms are organized into three

overarching categories: 1) affective/motivational, 2) behavioral, and 3) cognitive (DeChurch &

Mesmer-Magnus, 2010; Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006; Salas, Rosen, Burke, & Goodwin, 2009).

Research on these mechanisms has made salient the importance of distinguishing between team

processes (mechanisms that change inputs to outputs) and emergent states (constructs that

emerge over time as team members interact and the team develops”; p. 79; Kozlowski & Ilgen,

2006). Work by Marks, Mathieu, and Zaccaro (2001) provided a taxonomy to classify team

processes and team emergent states. They defined team processes as “interdependent acts that

convert inputs to outcomes through cognitive, verbal, and behavioral activities directed toward

organizing task work to achieve collective goals” (p. 357; Marks, Mathieu, & Zaccaro, 2001). In

other words, team processes are the mechanisms by which team members take inputs, such as

team expertise and convert them into team outputs. Emergent states, on the other hand, have

been defined as “constructs that characterize properties of the team that are typically dynamic in

Page 26: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

8

nature and vary as a function of team context, inputs, processes, and outcomes” (p.357; Marks,

Mathieu, & Zaccaro, 2001).

Multiteam system states and processes, like team states and processes, can be categorized

into three overarching constructs: affective, behavioral, and cognitive. MTS emergent states are

dynamic properties of the system: they represent a capacity for action; they shape and constrain

interaction between teams; and they evolve as component teams in the system interact over time.

Similarly, multiteam processes are behavioral interactions between teams through which team

inputs are transformed into multiteam outcomes.

The primary distinction between team and MTS processes and states is the level of focus.

In team processes and states, members joined toward a team goal are interacting, whereas with

multiteam processes, the interactions are occurring among the members of distinct teams

(Zaccaro, Marks, & DeChurch, 2011; DeChurch & Mathieu, 2009). Team processes are termed

intrateam processes in the context of a multiteam system. Intrateam processes such as backup

behavior commence among the team members of one’s team. Interteam or multiteam processes

involve the interactions among members of distinct teams, such as information exchange or

coordination between teams. Research has suggested that when component teams work toward

their own team goals, intrateam processes are more predictive of performance (Marks et al.,

2005), whereas when teams work toward the system’s goal, interteam processes are the driving

mechanisms that enable success.

Work by DeChurch and Zaccaro (2010) has noted that multiteam system effectiveness

requires an understanding of both the dynamics occurring within teams and the dynamics

occurring between teams. For instance, DeChurch and Marks (2006) found leadership aimed at

Page 27: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

9

the between-team interface improves multiteam performance by way of inter-, and not int-team

process improvement. Similarly, work by Mathieu, Cobb, Marks, Zaccaro, and Marsh (2004)

found that when training was focused on within team processes, system performance was not as

effective as when training was directed at cross-team processes. This evidence, suggests that it is

important to understand the determinants of multiteam system processes and performance.

Emergent States

Within the team literature a number of affective and motivational attitude constructs have

been noted as imperative for the functioning of effective teams (Salas, Rosen, Burke, &

Goodwin, 2009; Mathieu et al., 2008): for instance, collective orientation (Driskell & Salas,

1992; Mohammed & Angell, 2004), collective efficacy (Bandura, 1986; Zaccaro, Blair, Peterson,

& Zazaries, 1995), team learning orientation (Bunderson & Sutcliffe, 2003), team cohesion

(Beal, Cohen, Burke, & McLendon, 2003; Zaccaro, Gualtieri, & Minionis, 1995), trust (Salas,

Sims, & Burke, 2005), team empowerment (Mathieu, Gilson, & Ruddy, 2006; Kirkman, Rosen,

Tesluck, & Gibson, 2004), and team goal commitment (Aubé & Rousseau, 2005). These

emergentstates are thought to be the mechanisms that drive team members to act in a certain way

due to the circumstances they foresee. As such these forms of emergent states and how they

impact multiteam system processes are important for the future effective functioning of

multiteam systems.

Team and MTS Efficacy

As noted in the paragraph above a number of motivational variables (e.g., collective

Page 28: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

10

orientation and collective efficacy) exist. One construct thatcan manifest itself distinctly at the

team and system level is efficacy. Collective or team efficacy is defined as a team’s shared belief

in their capability to successfully perform a given task (Bandura, 1997). If team members have

high confidence in their team’s ability to perform a given task, then teams are more likely to

successfully perform when faced with novel and unexpected situations. A team’s confidence in

the team’s likelihood to succeed is what motivates them to perform when faced with challenging

and novel tasks. Therefore, collective or team efficacy is an emergent state that acts as a driver

of behavior based on team members’ experience of working together. The time and experience

of working on a task together provides members a platform to base their potentialsuccess and in

turn future actionsMathieu et al., 2010).

MTS efficacy goes beyond team members focusing on their team’s potential and taking

stock in the possibilities of the system’s ability to successfully accomplish its goal. Due to the

inherent interdependence between teams of a multiteam system, the capability of teams in a

system effectively completing their team’s task while simultaneously completing the system’s

task becomes a critical driver of performance. Having faith in the ability of other teams’

capabilities, when limited exposure may exist between them, can provide a stronger indicator of

behavior that will take place between teams. Thus, the reliance on such teams for the success of

the mission at hand puts this process at the forefront of our understanding in how it affects

multiteam functioning.

Research focusing on the effects of efficacy on team processes has been scant. Work has

primarily reviewed the effects of collective efficacy on team performance (Gully, Incalcaterra,

Joshi, & Beaubien, 2002; Stajkovic, Lee, & Nyberg, 2009; Srivastava, Bartol, & Locke, 2006;

Page 29: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

11

Campion, Medsker, & Higgs, 1993; Campion, Papper & Medsker, 1996; Gibson, 1999; Shea &

Guzzo, 1987; Gully, Incalcaterra, Joshi, & Beaubien, 2002), job satisfaction, lower levels of

exhaustion and turnover intentions (Zellars, Hochwarter, Perrewe, Miles, Kiewitz, 2001), and the

mediating role that team efficacy has on the relationship between team cognition and

performance (Mathieu, Rapp, Maynard &Mangos, 2010; Liu & Zang, 2010). Work by Gully et

al. (2002) suggests that the relationship between team efficacy and team performance is

enhanced when task interdependence between teams is high. If we turn back to the definition of

an MTS, one defining feature is the interdependence between teams. Specifically, MTSs are

defined as highly interdependent coupling of teams that strive toward a shared system goal while

simultaneously attaining their individual team goal. Focusing on collective efficacy at the

individual team level is distinct from the system level. At the system level, a number of factors

come into play. For example, the competing nature between teams for resources, the ability to

simultaneously work towards team and system goals, and the time constraints placed on the

completion of simultaneous goals.

Building on team efficacy and the definition posited by Gibson (1999), multiteam system

efficacy can be viewed as a between-team emergent state where teams in the system believe

other teams in the system are capable of successfully performing a given task. More specifically,

each team would believe that other teams are capable of attaining their individual team goal as

well as successfully contribute to the system level goal. If a system experiences high multiteam

system efficacy then the system will have the confidence that it will see its way through

unforeseen events or challenges when faced with them. If a system possess low MTS efficacy,

they are less likely to believe that component teams within the system are capable of attaining

Page 30: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

12

their individual goals or their contributions to the system level goal. As a result, teams may be

less motivated to engage in behaviors that benefit the system and in turn the system may fail on

their given task.

Team and MTS Trust

As organizations reap the benefits of teams, more and more employees are working

together. Interdependent collaboration affords employees a stepping board to develop bonds and

relationships with fellow teammates. One such bond created is trust, which has been defined as

“the willingness of a party to be vulnerable to the actions of another party based on the

expectation that the other will perform a particular action important to the trustor, irrespective of

the ability to monitor or control that other party” (p.712; Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995).

Researchers have focused on the theoretical trust model proposed by Mayer, Davis, and

Schoorman (1995), comprised of 3 dimensions: ability, benevolence, and integrity. Ability one

of the principle components states that the trustor must believe or have faith that the trustee is

well versed in his or her area of expertise in order to trust him or her on future tasks (Mayer et

al., 1995). Benevolence has been defined as the belief that the trustee is doing things out of good

will and has no egotistic motives (Mayer et al. 1995). Lastly, integrity has been defined as a

trustor’s perceptions that the trustee follows a set of implicitly defined rules found to be

acceptable by each (Mayer et al., 1995).

Moving beyond the traditional three-dimensional approach of trust, researchers have also

conceptualized trust as being a rational or social process (Jarvenpaa, Knoll, & Leidner, 1998;

Kramer & Tyler, 1996). The rationale posits that trust is a calculated factor based on a person’s

Page 31: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

13

self interest (Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995). Whereas the social perspective relies on

people’s moral obligations or how they may believe they should act towards others (Kramer &

Tyler, 1996; Mayer, Davis, & Schoorman, 1995). Whether one takes the traditional three

dimensional approach (ability, benevolence, and integrity) or the two dimensional focus

(rationale and social), it is safe to conclude that trust is an emergent process that is shaped

through continuous exposure and the experience of working with members of a team.

Meta-analytic work on trust at the individual level has shown that trust is a positive driver

of three job performance dimensions (Colquitt, Scott, & LePine, 2007). Specifically, the authors

found, through accumulation of the literature, that trust is positively related to task performance

and citizenship behavior and negatively related to counterproductive work behavior. In addition

the authors found that trust was positively linked to individual’s propensity to take risks (Colquitt

et al., 2007). Team trust has also been linked to outcome variables such as team member’s

attitudes towards the organization, task performance, and team satisfaction (Costa, 2003).

Work focusing on the effects of team trust on team processes suggests that team trust has

been positively linked to team members engaging in cooperative behavior and negatively related

to team members monitoring coworkers’ behavior (Costa, Roe, and Taillieu, 2001). In other

words the more trust team members possess for teammates, the more likely they are to help each

other out when they believe team members need assistance, but at the same time, they are less

likely to consistently be monitoring their teammates situation, because they have faith in their

teammates to perform their tasks. Contextual factors, such as work autonomy bring in new

dynamics. Work by Langferd, (2004) took into consideration the affect of trust on the negative

relationship between work autonomy and performance. Langferd found that teams that the

Page 32: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

14

negative relationship between autonomy and performance is intensified when team s experience

low levels of trust rather than when the level of trust is high. Thus, in self-managing teams the

presence of too much trust could be harmful if the task at hand permits for greater levels of work

autonomy. Additionally, Langfred concluded that in self-managing teams, the presence of some

level of monitoring must be in place in order to avoid processes loss and poor performance.

Thus far, this review of the team trust literature has been based on face-to-face teams. With the

prevalence on virtual teams and the reliance on a number of virtual communication tools that

limit face-to-face interaction, developing virtual trust in teams has become a challenge for both

practitioners and scientist.

Virtual teams are placed at a disadvantage that face-to-face teams do not encounter.

Although, video conferencing provides virtual teams with the ability to meet face-to-face with

team members, virtual teams tend to experience far fewer of these exchanges, even if they are

through some form of virtual medium. Virtual team research considering the interdependence

between team members has noted that trusts among members is positively related to

communication under high levels of interdependence (Rico, Alcover, Sanchez-Manzanares, &

Gil, 2009). Work focusing on the distribution of team members suggests, that dispersion can

have detrimental effects on trust. Work by Polzar, Crisp, Jarvenpaa, and Kim (2006) found that

teams with two colocated subgroups, and therefore more colocated peers, exhibit less trust than

fully dispersed teams. Additionally, the authors noted that the size of the dispersed groups,

particularly those that have fewer equally sized subgroups, experience less trust than teams

arranged in more similar configurations across locations (Polzar et al., 2006).

Additionally, the limited face-to-face interaction also places virtual teams at a

Page 33: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

15

disadvantage in establishing personal bonds and relationships with fellow teammates. Many of

us have heard, if not partaken in, of the infamous water cooler conversation with fellow

coworkers. It is during these times that teammates not only get to know each other on a more

personal level, but also that idea generation continues once formal meetings are over. Through

these extra interactions with teammates where members get to know each other’s work and get to

exchange ideas, team members begin to build trust and value for members of their team.

Qualitative research of trust in virtual teams has shown that teams differ in the strategies that

help them develop trust. Teams that showed high trust engaged in a number of strategies such as

being more proactive andkeeping an optimistic tone throughout their interaction (Jarvenpaa

Knoll, & Leidner, 1998). The authors also noted that in teams exhibiting high trust individuals

took initiative in taking on task responsibilities and time management was explicit and process-

based. Teams characterized as having high trust tended to providepredictable and substantive

feedback to members. Lastly, teams that were noted as having high trustwere more likely to

engage in intense bursts of interaction/communication between members (Jarvenpaa et al.,

1998). The noted limitations that virtual teams are exposed to can be translated to MTS. In

many instances the means of communication for component teams both within and across team

boundaries is through some form of virtual collaboration tool.

In MTS, teams simultaneously work independently toward their own team goals while

also working interdependently towards a system goal (DeChurch & Mathieu, 2009; Mathieu,

Marks, & Zaccaro, 2001). Team’s research has shown that team trust can be beneficial, by

increasing team information sharing (Mayer, 1995), providing backup behavior to team members

(Costa et al., 2001), and engaging in more citizenship behavior (Colquitt, 2007). Yet, it is still

Page 34: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

16

not clear how trust emanates itself at the system level. Expanding on Mayer et al. (1995) work on

trust, MTS trust can be defined as the willingness of a team to be vulnerable to the actions of

another team, based on the expectation that the other team will perform a particular action

important for the system’s performance. Limited research exists on our understanding of how

MTS trust is developed and once developed how it affects MTS processes. Work by Serva,

Fuller, and Mayer (2005) showed that trust across teams is developed through the actions taken

by a team and another team’s perception of those actions. In other words, reciprocal trust is

established throughout a team’s life cycle because teams are able to exhibit their vulnerability to

trust one another and continuous exposure of this behavior prompts similar behavior to occur.

Building onto these and previous findings on trust research in both teams and multiteam systems

is the next step in obtaining a clear understanding of how trust manifests itself at the system

level.

Team and MTS Cognition

Team cognition represents the “structure of collective perception, cognitive structure, or

knowledge organization, and knowledge or information acquisition” (pp.81, Kozlowski & Ilgen,

2006). Research has shown that distinct types of cognition have been theorized (Cannon-Bowers,

Salas, & Converse, 1993) and shown (Mohammed, Ferdanzi, & Hamilton, 2010; DeChurch &

Mesmer-Magnus, 2010) to be among the most critical drivers of team performance. Team

cognition has been discussed as more than an individual process, but rather it is a collective level

phenomenon (Klimoski & Mohammed, 1994) that emerges through interaction. Over the years

two predominate constructs comprise the team cognition literature, transactive memory systems

Page 35: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

17

(Wegner, 1986; 1995, Wegner, Giuliano, & Hertel, 1985; Lewis, 2003; Austin, 2003; Faraj &

Sproull, 2000) and mental models (Cannon-Bowers, Salas, & Converse, 1993; Klimoski &

Mohammed, 1994; Mohammed & Dumville, 2001).

Transactive Memory Systems

The first construct is transactive memory systems (TMS); Wagner (1987) defined TMS

as a cognitively interdependent system based on a set of individual memory systems, which

provides knowledge of which members of the team possess particular task relevant knowledge

(Wagner, 1987; Mohammed & Dumville, 2001). The study of TMS involves “the prediction of

group behavior through an understanding of the manner in which groups process and structure

information” (pp. 187, Wagner, 1987). TMS can be conceptualized as a knowledge network of

unique information possessed by team members. “When each team member learns in a general

sense what other team members know in detail, the team can draw on the detailed knowledge

distributed across members of the collective” (pp. 85. Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006). The

development of transactive memory has been attributed to three (Lewis, 2003) to four (Austin,

2003) dimensions that target the following areas: 1) awareness of where knowledge lies within

the team, 2) trust or credibility in others’ knowledge, and 3) the ability to effectively coordinate

the retrieval of information (Lewis, 2003; Wegner, 1987).

Research on TMS has posited that antecedents of TMS include task interdependence

(Zhang, Hempel, Han, & Tjosvold, 2007), cooperative goal interdependence (Zhang et al., 2007),

support for innovation (Zhang et al., 2007), team stability (Akgun, Byrne, Keskin, Lynn, &

Imamoglu, 2005), and team familiarity (Akgun, 2005; Smith-Jentsch, Kraiger, Canon-Bowers, &

Page 36: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

18

Salas, 2009). TMS has also been linked to distinct team processes and outcomes, such as backup

behavior (Smith-Jentsch et al., 2009), trust (Akgun et al., 2005), team efficacy (Smith-Jentsch et

al., 2009), external evaluations (Austin, 2003), and internal evaluations (Austin, 2003), team

learning (Akgun, Bryne, Keskin, Lynn, 2006), collective mind (Yoo & Kanawattanachai, 2001),

team performance (Austin, 2003; He, Butler, & King, 2007; Lewis, 2003; Zhang et al., 2007),

virtual team performance (Yoo & Kanawattanachai, 2001; Kanawattanachai & Yoo, 2007), goal

attainment (Austin, 2003), and speed to market (Zhang et al., 2007). As the evidence

suggestTMS has received substantial attention as a means via which teams can function more

effectively. In conjunction with TMS over the years teams researchers have also promoted the

investigation of shared mental models as a way to enhance team effectiveness.

Shared Mental Models

Shared Mental Models (SMM) have been defined as the team’s shared understanding of

their teammates, teamwork, taskwork, and equipment (Cannon-Bowers, Salas, & Converse,

1993). SMM are a way of understanding the implicit coordination that takes place between team

members based on their shared understanding of the situation or mission. Over the years, four

distinct SMM have been discussed in the literature. First,the teammate SMM consists of the

shared understanding between team members' expertise and their understanding of how each

member contributes to the task at hand. Second, the teamwork SMM focuses on the interactions

that must take place between members in order to successfully attain the team’s goal. Third, the

taskwork SMM is a shared understanding of the task at hand and how to best approach the

problem space. Lastly, the equipment SMM consists of a shared understanding of the tools and

Page 37: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

19

technology used during the task at hand. Mathieu et al. (2000, 2005) articulate that these four

reflect two overarching types: task and team.

A recent review of the SMM literature by Mohammed, Ferdanzi, and Hamilton (2010)

shows that SMM have been linked to a number of distinct processes (Mathieu, Heffner,

Goodwin, Salas, & Cannon-Bowers, 2000; Mathieu, Heffner, Goodwin, Cannon-Bowers, &

Salas, 2005) such as back-up-behavior (Marks, Sabella, Burke, & Zaccaro, 2002), coordination

(Marks, Sabella, Burke, & Zaccaro, 2002), and communication (Marks, Zaccaro, & Mathieu,

2000). Additionally, SMM have been related to a number of team effectiveness indicators, such

as team performance (Cooke, Kiekel, & Helm, 2001; Lim & Klein, 2006; Mathieu, Heffner,

Goodwin, Salas, & Cannon-Bowers, 2000; Mathieu, Heffner, Goodwin, Cannon-Bowers, &

Salas, 2005; Edwards, Day, Arthur, and Bell, 2006; DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2010), team

viability (Rentsch & Klimoski, 2001), and client satisfaction (Rentsch & Klimoski,

2001).Although these findings have provided us a stepping stone for building our knowledge of

how these SMM may function in MTS. As noted earlier, our understanding of how teams

function cannot simply be aggregated to explain MTS functioning (DeChurch & Zaccaro, 2010).

As a result, the question still remains, how is cognition represented at the MTS level?

And how does this pattern of system-level cognition impact performance? One team’s

understanding of another team’s expertise and shared understanding of the task at hand, would

be beneficial for MTSs. We can see that MTSs would benefit in ways, such as having the ability

to coordinate faster, via implicit coordination. Additionally, as component teams of MTSs

become more dependent on technology for communication and sharing of information, they are

likely to be more efficient by limiting the time they spent communicating with members that do

Page 38: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

20

not have the knowledge relevant for the task at hand and going directly to members that hold

vital task relevant information. Lastly, having a shared understanding of how the system plans

on approaching a given situation, may serve fruitful in the event that systems have to adapt to

situational factors, for instance, receiving information that is not pertinent to their own team’s,

but is to the other team’s task. A system’s ability to possess a shared schema of the situation

should enable more effective communication and in turn performance by the system as a whole.

Information Sharing

Information sharing (IS) is a behavioral process by which collectives exchange relevant

tasks and knowledge with one another (Staples & Webster, 2008; Stasser & Titus, 1985, 1987).

Work by Stasser and Titus (1985, 1987) has rooted the IS literature by noting that teams posses

both a) shared information (information that all members of a team hold) and b) unshared/unique

information (information that is uniquely held by members of a team). Our understanding of

collectives is that they can benefit from pooling distinctly held knowledge, and in doing so, reach

better decisions than if they would be based on common information. For some time now

researches have sought to understand why IS in collectives tends to be biased toward the

discussion of common information. Work by Stasser and Titus (1985, 1987) suggest that “groups

tend to be dominated by information that members hold in common before a group discussion

and information that supports members’ existing preferences” (pp. 1467).

Recent meta-analytic work by Mesmer-Magnus and DeChurch (2009) echoes these

findings by suggesting that teams are more inclined to share commonly held information than

unique information even though the latter has been found to be more predictive of team

Page 39: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

21

performance. Therefore, if teams tend to experience issues fully aggregating unique information

in teams, the problem likely compounds as an additional boundary is introduced.

Furthermore, how would information sharing affect interactions, such as the development

of cognitive processes and outcomes, such as team performance? IS at the system level is a

critical issue, when we take into consideration the success of multiple teams and the system as a

whole. If a team does not have all information at hand, prior to making a decision, the likelihood

of the team performing successfully is diminished. As noted by De Dreu, Nijstad, and van

Knippenberg (2008) through task communication, teams are able to develop a shared

understanding of the task and arrive at a better solution. A team’s performance will in turn have

compounding effect on system performance. More importantly, when we turn to MTS, IS

becomes more complex.

MTS IS can be defined as the exchange of information between component teams in a

system. MTS IS can be conceptualized in terms of its openness and uniqueness both within and

across teams of a system. The openness of MTS IS is the forthcoming and frequent exchange of

information within and across team boundaries. The uniqueness of MTS ISis the extent to which

information initially contained by a single team member is exchanged with members within and

across his other team.

Virtuality

Virtual communication has enabled work to be coordinated and accomplished by larger,

more diverse sets of individuals. Virtuality has also greatly increased the flexibility of work

enabling individuals to connect from different locations and at different times. With this

Page 40: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

22

improved capacity to connect through technology comes a range of positive and negative

implications. Virtuality hasinfiltrated our work world and has enabled much advancement, such

as international collaborations, global ventures, teleworking from distinct locations and the

ability to stay in touch any time. However, the Orbiter example clearly illustrates the

consequences that can result if communication media do not allow the same quality of

information exchange afforded by face-to-face contact. Organizations are now conducting work

virtually, heavily relying on email, conference calls, instant messaging services, video

conferencing, shared work space, and many more virtual tools.

Although virtual communication has been adopted due to its ability to provide individuals

the ability to coordinate business from distinct locations, unite experts of individuals from

different time zones, provides flexibility from where work is conducted, and provide faster

communication, much of the research thus far conducted on virtual teams limits our

understanding of teams use of virtual tools because the comparison has been between virtual

teams, i.e., those for whom all information exchange is through some type of technology, and

face-to-face teams, i.e., those for whom all information exchange occurs in person (Baltes,

Dickson, Sherman, Bauer, & LeGanke, 2002). Given that most teams use a variety of virtual

tools, and use them for different purposes, much more can be learned about virtual effectiveness

by considering how particular tools enable the exchange of particular types of information, and

differentially affect the core aspects of team functioning.

Past work on virtual teams which compares virtual and face-to-face teams generally

supports three conclusions. First, computer-mediated communication is related to a decrease in

team effectiveness when compared to face-to-face teams (Hedlund, Ilgen, & Hollenbeck, 1998;

Page 41: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

23

Baltes et al., 2001). Second, virtual teams are less efficient, due to the additional time it takes to

communicateas compared to face-to-face teams (Baltes et al., 2002). Third, members of virtual

teams tend to be less satisfied when compared to face-to-face teams (Baltes et al., 2002).

This sort of analysis is not particularly useful for two reasons. First, it assumes that

“virtuality” is a leverage point or choice for the organization when often it is not. Virtual

communication has become the norm. Even so called face-to-face teams often communicate

significant amounts of information using virtual tools. Second, this analysis doesn’t account for

differences in the type of virtual communication. Tools differ on multiple dimensions including

how synchronous they are, and the richness of cues they convey. These features likely have

important implications of their relative utility in sending/receiving different types of information.

Work by Kirkman and Mathieu (2005) provided a taxonomy of the distinct features that

describe virtual teams: use of virtual tools, synchronicity of interaction, and informational value.

The latter two dimensions provide a starting point to conceptualize the benefits and drawbacks of

various tools. Specifically, tools that are more synchronous are tools that provide for virtual

communication to occur in real time, such as video chats and conference calls. Tools that are

asynchronous imply that a lag occurs between the time a person sends a message and the

intended receiver receives it. Informational value or media richness implies that there is a “lower

level of virtuality” (pp. 703). More specifically, Skype or video conference calls would be

considered to have high media richness because they provide for both, verbal and non-verbal

(visual) communication. Electronic chat rooms or email would be considered to have low media

richness because they only provide a platform for verbal communication or written

communication (Kirkman & Mathieu, 2005).

Page 42: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

24

I propose an important additional dimension, retrievability, is extent to which the tool

affords a written record of the interaction that can easily be referred back to by team members.

The need for this explanatory dimension comes from a recent meta-analysis on virtuality and IS

in teams. Mesmer-Magnus, and her colleagues found that whereas unique IS is more predictive

of performance for face-to-face teams, in virtual teams, IS openness is more predictive of

performance than is unique IS. A way to interpret this finding is that the type of IS needs to

match the tool being utilized. Asynchronous and low richness tools are stripped of social cues

and therefore, teams need the “openness” dimension of IS – i.e., reiterating what another member

has said – in order to built needed emergent states and enhance their effectiveness. Conversely,

in face-to-face teams, or those using very rich tools like video chat, it is the unique IS that is

critical to performance. These teams can rely on the media to provide rich information about the

comprehension of information by their teammates, and teammates reactions to information.

Therefore, the tool provides access to these proxies for IS openness.

One concern that this brings to our attention is the impact the virtual communication can

pose for multiteam system collaboration. How do distinct virtual tools help or hinder the

information being shared among members? Furthermore, how does sharing information through

virtual tools impact the development of team processes, such as IS processes and the

development of cognitive processes? Are certain tools more conducive to teams sharing unique

or open information?

Theory Development and Hypotheses

Organizations are progressively becoming virtual. Virtual organizations are defined as “a

Page 43: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

25

collection of geographically distributed, functionally and/or culturally diverse entities that are

linked by electronic forms of communication and rely on lateral, dynamic relationships for

coordination” (p. 693; DeSanctis & Monge, 1999). Virtual teams that reside within these

organizations have been noted to be heavily electronically dependent due to their remote

collaboration (Kirkman & Mathieu, 2005; Gibson & Cohen, 2003; Gibson & Gibbs, 2006). The

collaboration that takes place in virtual organizations can be viewed through a multiteam system

lens.

This dissertation examines MTS interacting in a context characterized by virtual

communication between teams, and asks which factors determine their success? The model

presented in Figure 1 posits that two emergent states shape the types of process interactions that

commence among teams. The affective route, initiated by trust between teams, impact the IS

openness, which in turn affects the development of shared mental models, and ultimately MTS

performance. The second motivational route, initiated by efficacy, shapes the sharing of unique

information, transactive memory, and in turn, MTS performance. Lastly, the strength of the

relationships between information sharing processes and performance are posited to depend on

the richness and retrievability of the tool used to transmit information across teams. The current

study enables MTS to choose how and when to communicate using three available tools: instant

messaging, voice call, and video call. These tools can be ordered according to the extent to

which retrievability is maximized (text>voice, video) and information richness is maximized

(video>voice, text). This dissertation will test the extent to which not only the type of IS or type

of communication media impacts the development of MTS cognition, but additionally, that the

match between sending particular types of information using particular tools optimizes the

Page 44: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

26

development of two cognitive architectures in MTS. These two pathways to performance,

motivationally –driven and affectively-driven.

Motivationally-Driven Development

In order to understand the motivationally-driven development, resource allocation theory

is referenced. Resource allocation theory posits that an individual's “cognitive resources can be

conceptualized as an undifferentiated pool, representing the limited capacity of the human's

information-processing system” (p. 663; Kanfer & Ackerman, 1989). The authors note that

attentional demands are greater in the early stages of a task, specifically if the task is novel in

nature. Resource allocation can help us understand individual motivation. For instance,

individuals that are part of MTS have to determine whether to allocate resources towards their

team or towards the MTS. If individuals perceive high efficacy towards the MTS, then they

believe that the MTS is capable of attaining its goals. If individuals possess low efficacy towards

the MTS, then individuals believe that the MTS does not the ability to accomplish the tasks at

hand. In the case that individuals believe that the MTS possess the capability to successfully

attain its goals, this will lead individuals to allocate resources to system level goals. This

motivational state then creates the energy pool oriented toward the MTS task. The next question

is: how does this drive state translate into concrete interaction capable of influencing

performance?

Research on virtual teams, and on MTS, has posited that the probability of performing

well is enhanced via a number of distinct processes (Marks et al., 2005; DeChurch & Marks,

2006). Amongst the distinct processes, one critical behavioral processes for effective teams is

Page 45: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

27

information sharing (Bunderson & Sutcliffe, 2002; Staples & Webster, 2008; Stasser & Titus,

1985, 1987). Meta-analytic evidence has supported the idea that information sharing is essential

for team performance (Mesmer-Magnus & DeChurch, 2009).

The literature has suggested that teams tend to engage in two distinct types of IS: unique

IS and open IS. Unique IS refers to the distribution of information prior to a collaborative

discussion, specifically, “information that is unshared before discussion”; p. 1476 & 1477;

Stasser & Titus, 1985). This information is not shared by other members of a team and is critical

in order to incorporate when making a decision. Without unique IS the decision arrived at may

not be the best or most well informed decision.

IS is a critical behavioral process in teams. Due to the complexity of MTS (e.g., distinct

teams contributing towards a system goal, while simultaneously working towards their own

goals) ISuniqueness is the crux that allows systems to function effectively. One question that

arises is what promotes MTSIS uniqueness, when members could simply share information

within their own team? In other words, why would members of a team expend resources

towards the system’s performance? If all members of a system share in their belief that the

system is capable of attaining its goals, then they would be more inclined to share unique

information then when efficacy is low.

H1: MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS IS uniqueness.

H2: MTS IS uniqueness is positively related to MTS performance.

As teams engage in unique MTS IS uniqueness they are able to begin building a shared

understanding of the situation/task at hand (Hollingshead & Brandon, 2003). As teams begin to

build a shared understanding through communication, they begin to understand how each

Page 46: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

28

individual within the team can individually contribute his or her knowledge to the task

(Hollingshead & Brandon, 2003). Specifically, awareness is brought to the expertise that each

individual can supply. This awareness provides team members the ability to communicate

information in a timely manner (provides for implicit coordination) when members are aware

who the information should be communicated to. Thus, via unique cross team IS, teams are able

to build a system level TMS which provides them the ability to directly coordinate more

efficiently and effectively across teams (Austin, 2003; He, Butler, & King, 2007; Lewis, 2003;

Zhang et al., 2007).

H3: MTS IS uniqueness is positively related to MTS performance through (i.e., mediated

by) the development of MTS TMS. As teams exchange more MTS IS uniqueness, they will

develop more accurate MTS TMS than if they exchange less MTSIS uniqueness. The more

accurate a system's TMS, the better the system will perform. Thus, TMS acts as a

mediator in the MTS IS uniqueness- MTS performance relationship.

Affect-Driven Development

Whereas the first state influencing MTS interaction is motivational, members of distinct

teams need the drive to allocate resources to the larger system goal, a second emergent state is

also a necessary precursor to MTS effectiveness: trust. The physical separation of members and

component teams limits the developmental processes that traditional teams experience that are

critical in fostering trust. For instance, virtual team members are less likely to share information

about them via electronic communication, limiting their ability to establish personal relationships

with members of their own team and component teams. The literature on geographically

Page 47: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

29

dispersed teams emphasizes the lack of trust developed between virtual team members as an

issue that hinders successful performance (Jarvenpaa & Leidner, 1999).

As teams begin to trust each other they are more inclined to partake in problem solving,

become more forward with providing task relevant information, are more critical and question

each other, and provide assistance to other members when they believe it is required. Work by

Staples and Webster (2008) has shown that trust within teams leads to greater IS across local,

virtual, and hybrid teams (part of the team is local and part of the team is remote). In many

instances, the feeling of belonging develops into trust, providing members the ability to be

vulnerable and share information with other members. As noted earlier, IS has been described as

the sharing of unique or open information. IS openness has been defined as a “conscious and

deliberate attempts on the partof team members to exchange work-related information, keep one

another apprised of activities, and inform one another of key developments” (p. 881; Bunderson

& Sutcliffe, 2002). If individuals believe they can trust one another with task related

responsibilities, they will be more open to share information that is pertinent to the task at hand.

Work by Ridings, Gefen, and Arinze (2002) showed that within teams, individuals that trusted

one another’s ability and integrity both gave and got more information than their non trusting

counterparts.

H4: MTS team trust is positively related to MTS IS openness. Such that when teams feel

more MTS trust they are more inclined to engage in MTS IS openness.

Distinct processes have been noted to be critical for the success of teams. One such

behavioral process is communication (He, Butler, & King, 2007) which is the means by which IS

openness takes place. Meta-analysis compiling research over 20 years on the relationship

Page 48: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

30

between team IS (both unique and open) and performance has shown a consistently positive

relationship (Mesmer-Magnus & DeChurch, 2009; Mesmer-Magnus, DeChurch, Jiménez,

Wildman, & Shuffler, 2011; Yoo &Kanawattanachai, 2001). Therefore, as systems engage in

MTSIS openness, the flow of information being exchanged between teams is greater, providing

members more instances to understand the importance of task relevant information, which can

result in better performing systems.

H5: MTS IS openness is positively related to MTS performance.

As teams engage in more IS openness teams may encounter a number of advantages and

disadvantages. First, individuals may register the sharing of information as a welcoming culture.

Second, team members are likely to reiterate information already pointed out by a team member

when engaging in open information sharing. Third, teams may be more inclined to believe the

information that is being processed and shared by members of the team; this has been noted as a

common pitfall of hidden profiles (Stasser & Titus, 1985; 1987; Stasser, Taylor, & Hanna, 1989;

Mesmer-Magnus & DeChurch, 2009). Fourth, similar to my previous point, individuals are more

likely to trust members who vocalize information in a way that supports the information they

possess. Lastly, although commonly viewed as a downside of IS openness, teams are likely to

place more weight on information that is supported by other members and as a result, limit their

discussion of unique information among group members. As teams place more weight on

information that is openly shared, they are more apt to create a shared understanding among all

members of the situation, how to approach the situation, and the coordination that should take

place among members (He et al., 2007). Research has shown that communication has been

linked to the development of mental models; little has focused on the type of information shared

Page 49: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

31

that helps teams build much cognitive architectures. As such through the repetition of

information openly exchanged between teams, it is expected that a shared mental model will be

developed among system members.

H6: MTS IS openness is positively related to MTS performance through MTS mental

model similarity. As MTS engage in more open cross team information sharing they are

likely to develop a more similar MTS mental model. The more similar team’s mental

models’ are to other teams, the better their MTS performance.

Media Retrievability Moderator

As organizations are delving into virtual business, geographic dispersion of teams has

made teams more reliant on electronic communication tools (Cramton, 2001, Gibson & Gibbs,

2006). Although electronic communication affords teams a number of benefits, such as

immediate communication, collaboration across countries, and diversity of expertise and culture,

virtual communication can also have its drawbacks (Gibson & Gibbs, 2006). Virtual

communication can result in a misunderstanding of information due to the limited verbal and

visual cues involved, logistical and technological constraints that limits informal spontaneous

interaction, and hindering knowledge interpretation(DeSanctis and Monge, 1999). Over the

years a number of virtual tools have been developed to assist with or daily work interactions.

The evolution of these tools has provided distinct features that attempt to mitigate some of the

downfalls previously noted.

Virtual communication can take the form of, but is not limited to, video conferencing,

teleconferencing, email, groupware, and electronic text messaging (e.g., GChat, AIM, etc.). The

Page 50: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

32

primary purpose of virtual communication is to share valuable information among members in a

timely manner. A number of researchers have focused on the distinct types of information

relayed via communication mediums. Research suggests that distinct types of information are

exchanged between members of virtual teams (Weisband, 1992). For instance, work by Hiltz,

Johnson, Turoff, 1986 suggest that virtual teams tend to focus more on task-oriented

communication than face-to-face teams, whereas Boardia, DiFonzo, and Chang (1999) found no

difference between the two types of teams. Recent meta-analytic work on team virtuality and

information sharing found that in virtual teams, IS openness (or the breadth of information

shared, independent of the original distribution of that information) resulted in greater team

performance than IS uniqueness (information that is unshared or distributed among system

members; Mesmer-Magnus et al., 2011), whereas meta-analytic findings of face-to-face teams

has concluded the opposite; IS uniqueness resulted in greater team performance than IS openness

(Mesmer-Magnus & DeChurch, 2009). Based on these findings, the authors concluded next steps

for team virtuality research include having a better understanding of “the extent to which

different virtual tools effectively support different group processes” (Mesmer-Magnus et al.,

2011).

Towards this aim, this dissertation introduces the idea of information friendly retrievable

tools (i.e., media retrievability tools), the extent to which a technological tool enables the

information to be retrieved later. Typically, email, shared sites, and chat messaging are high

media retrievable tools, whereas voice and video are not low media retrievability tools.

Retrievability represents one large advantage of more virtual, distant, less rich tools. First,

information can be stored and is available when it is needed by a teammate. Second, it can be

Page 51: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

33

interpreted later with reflection, by ones’ teammate improving comprehensiveness. Third, by

virtue of its archival nature, it ought to facilitate the development of transactive memory,

enabling team members to note who knows what information and how it can be retrieved,

without having to fully digest the information as it is sent.

Additionally, media retrievability is likely more important to the utility of unique, as

compared to open, IS. Given the different functions served by unique and open IS posited by

Mesmer-Magnus and her colleagues (2011), the uniqueness of information ought to benefit from

being somewhat anonymized in its delivery, and available for retrieval throughout the course of

group interaction. Therefore, it is predicted that retrievability moderates the strength of the

relationship between MTS IS uniqueness and MTS TMS accuracy:

H7: Communication retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS IS

uniqueness and MTS TMS, such that when systems engage in more MTS IS unique via

media retrievable tools, the relationship between MTS IS unique and MTS TMS will be

stronger whereas the relationship will be weaker when systems engage in less exchanged

of information via media retrievability tools.

Media Richness Moderator

Teams research has shown that when arriving at group decisions, teams are more inclined

to spend a disproportionate amount of time engaging in IS openness (Stasser & Titus, 1985,

1987; Stasser, 2003; Mesmer-Magnus & DeChurch 2009). Part of the reason teams engage in IS

openness is because members tend to discuss information that is supportive of their decision.

Information that refutes an initial decision is discarded because individual dislike it.

Page 52: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

34

Additionally, more weight is placed on information that many people can corroborate. As such,

teams that engage in IS openness can benefit from the aforementioned repetition in information

being discussed. As all members openly share information, they are able to develop shared

mental schemas of the situation. The repetition of information provides teams the ability to

develop a shared schema of the situation.

One of the most effective ways to share information in a timely manner is through

communication media characterized as rich, carrying a wide range of visual and auditory

information. These forms of communication provide higher value of the information being

conveyed. Medium high in richness provide individuals communicating through them the ability

to engage in conversation as if they were in a face-to-face conversation these forms of media

provides individuals the ability to read body language, pick up on tone inflection, and interpret

silence better than less rich media, such as emails, message boards, and text messages. This

feature ought to be particularly important for the openness of IS. When communicating

information that builds relationships, demonstrates agreement with information, or allows

members to reframe and modify information. For this reason, it is expected that the strength of

the relationship between IS openness and shared mental models will be moderated by media

richness:

H8: Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS IS openness and MTSSMM,

such that when systems engage in MTS IS openness using more rich media (e.g., video

communication), SMMs will be more similar whereas the relationship will be weaker

when exchange occurs through less rich media (e.g., text messaging).

Page 53: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

35

CHAPTER TWO: METHOD

Participants

The sample consisted of 328undergraduate psychology students from a large southeastern

university participated in this study in exchange for research credit, extra credit, or $40. Of the

328 participants, 54% were female and 46% were male. The ethnicity of the sample included

60% Caucasian, 11% African American, 17% Hispanic, 5% Pacific Islander, 5% Asian, and 2%

Middle Eastern. Ages ranged from 17 to 38. Approximately 87% of participants were 18 or 19

years of age.

The 328 participants were assigned to 82, 4-person MTS (2 2-person teams). Team

assignment was kept intact throughout the duration of the study. At no point in time did the

entire MTS come together or interact face-to-face. As all hypotheses specified relationships at

the MTS level of analysis, the effective sample size was 82. This sample size was determined

based on a power analysis which was conducted a priori in order to determine the minimum

sample size needed to detect a given effect size.

Power Analysis

As defined by Cohen(1992), statistical power is influenced by three factors: sample size,

alpha level, and effect size. Of these three factors, researchers have control over sample size and

alpha levels (although over the years, researchers have come to the agreement that alpha levels

Page 54: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

36

should be within a specified range α = .01-.05). The following equation was used to calculate

the sample size for this study (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken, 2003).

n = (L/ƒ2) +k+ 1

First, following the steps established by Cohen et al. (2003) an alpha level was selected; α

= .05. Second, effect size = ƒ2

= R2/1-R

2 was calculated using an effect size obtained form

previous literature in the area of information sharing in virtual teams. For this study, an effect

size of .46 was used based on meta-analytic work by Mesmer-Magnus and colleagues (2011).

Therefore, ƒ2 = .21/.79 = .27. Next, L was determined by utilizing Table E.2 in Cohen et al.

(2003) which is a factor of the degrees of freedom (KB) of the hypothesized model, for this study

KB = 9, the pre-established power = .80, the pre-establishedalpha level (α = .05). Based on table

E.2 (Cohen et al., 2003) the L for corresponding to these statistics was 15.65. Lastly, k = the

number of variables in the study, for this study k = 9.

n = (15.65/.27) + 9 + 1

n = 57.96+10

n = 67.96 = 68

Therefore, in this study a total of68 data points were required to ensure a power of .80.

With a total of 82 data points, the statistical power for testing the current hypotheses was .89.

Using an electronic power calculator (Lenth, 2006) the following statistics were used to generate

the power of 89: sample of 82, alpha set at .05, 8 regressors, and a R2 of .21 (previously

established by the literature.). The only exception was in the analyses testing hypotheses 6, 8, 12,

13, 15, 17, 22-24, 26, 27, 30, and 32-34. These hypotheses involved shared mental models, and

due to missing data, the sample size for these analyses ranged between 49 and 70, and the

Page 55: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

37

statistical power for these analyses were.65 and .85. Using the same electronic power calculator

(Lenth, 2006) the following statistics were used to generate the power of 65: sample of 49, alpha

set at .05, 6 regressors, and a R2 of .20 (obtained from this study). Again, using the same

electronic power calculator (Lenth, 2006) the following statistics were used to generate the

power of 85: sample of 70, alpha set at .05, 6 regressors, and a R2 of .20 (obtained from this

study).

Overview of the Experimental Protocol

MTS Task Environment

The SURREALISM MTS task was utilized for this research study. This task mimics an

emergency management system with 3 teams, comprised of 2, 2 person live teams and 1

simulated team of 15 convoy trucks traveling through the region with medical supplies. This

platform was designed based on a commercially available real-time strategy PC video game:

World in Conflict (Sierra Entertainment, 2007). SURREALISM created a highly controlled 10

cell by 10 cell region in which all MTSs encountered the same enemies at the exact same

locations. In order to ensure that all MTSs experienced the exact same enemies each received the

same

Within the region there were three types of zones: safe zones, non-hotspot zones, and

hotspot zones. The safe zones indicated that there were no enemies that would inflict damage on

the convoy traveling through the region. The non-hotspot zones had enemy forces that would

Page 56: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

38

inflict minimal damage on the convoy. Additionally, the non-hotspot zones consisted of enemies

that could be neutralized by one specific team. In other words, both teams were not necessary at

that point in the map in order to neutralize the enemies found within that region. The hotspots

zones were considered the most dangerous spots within the region. The enemies within this area

would inflict the most damage on the convoy units traveling through the region. These hotspot

areas were designed in order to promote cross team coordination because in addition to

exchanging information about the enemies in the region, both teams were necessary in order to

neutralize the enemies within a hotspot zone prior to having the convoy travel through it safely.

Intelligence about the entire 100 cell region was dispersed among all four members of the

MTS. The MTS had all the information to determine which regions within the map were safe,

non-hotspot, or hotspot zones. To successfully identify these zones, MTS members had to share

information both within and across teams. Intelligence was distributed to MTSs prior to the

beginning of each mission.

Performance Episodes

Each MTS completed two missions during which performance was assessed. Mission 1

lasted 30 minutes and served as a practice task. Mission 2 lasted 60 minutes and was used for all

data collection. Each mission was designed to unfold as Marks et al. (2001) noted, where

episodes are comprised of both transition (or planning) phases and action (or playing) phases. At

the beginning of the practice and performance missions, the MTS had 10 minutes to plan

followed by time to execute their plan. During the practice mission the action phase consisted of

20 minutes, whereas during the actual performance mission, the action phasewas split into two

Page 57: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

39

sessions, one 35 minute session and one 25 minute session.

Playing Environment

For both the performance episodes each participant sat an individual workstation outfitted

with a networked pc, microphone-equipped headset, and 2 displays. One display provided the

game interface and the other served as the communication console where Skype was displayed.

In the interface console, students were presented with a 10 cell by 10 cell map of the Kazbar

region. Within the console, the intended functions are to a) move around the map in an effort to

identify enemy threats and hotspots based on intelligence reports received by command, b) zone

areas as hotspots, based on a participants rules of engagement, c) neutralize enemy threats

located within identified hotspots, and d) order the convoy of troops carrying humanitarian aid to

war-torn troops to move through the region once it has been cleared of all potential threats. In

order to complete these tasks, participants coordinated their action via the communication

console.

The communication console consists of the Skype version 5.3 (Skype Premium, 2012),

with group video. Within this console each participant had the ability to see all members of the

taskforce in his or her contact list along the left hand side of the interface. Through the Skype

interface, participants had the option of engaging in chat conversations, audio conversations, and

video conversations with every member of the taskforce during both the transition (i.e., planning)

and action (i.e., playing) phases of the task. This version of Skype was selected because it

provided the taskforce members the option to partake in a group video conversation, allowing

multiple members to be involved in a video conference call. In conjunction with the Skype

Page 58: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

40

software, the Pamela software (Pamela, 2012) was used to record all Skype interactions for later

coding. In order to ensure that all participants had the knowledge to successfully perform the

task, each was trained on their individual roles, the game, and the communication console.

Training

The training portion of the study was conducted in two adjacent training rooms. Each

training room had a high performing PC and a 28-inch monitor at the end of a conference table.

MTSs in the current study included two 2-person component teams, the Phantom team and the

Stinger team. Each component team was situated in one of these two training rooms and did not

interact face-to-face during the training.The training setupallowed for both team members to be

trained simultaneously by a research assistant. At the end of each training session, the

participants were given a training check questionnaire to ensure that they understood their role

and the task. Answers to the training check questionnaire were reviewed as a group and any

questions answered incorrectly were explained to participants. Upon completion of the training

participants were taken to their work stations and asked to engage in a practice session.

Participants were notified that they were engaging in a practice session and they were

encouraged to ask the experimenter questions if there was any confusion about their role, the

task, or the communication console.

Networking the Task

In order for all four MTS members to be linked, this task required the use of a control

room that included two high performance PCs to allow the experiment to network team members

Page 59: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

41

on the same domain, collect real-time data for each participant, team and MTS. Participant

stations were equipped with a high performance PCs, two 21-inch widescreen monitors, noise

reducing headsets, and webcams. A total of 7 networked high performing PC computers and 11

28-inch monitors, 4 noise reducing headsets, and 4 webcams were required for this

experimentation.

MTS Structure

MTS Goal Hierarchy

SURREALISM was selected for this study because it provided designed to create a task

that incorporates the key aspects of MTS theory, where team members are interdependent with

respect to a team level goal, and where two teams are interdependent with respect to a system-

level goal. The goals of the mission are broken down by level in Figure 2. First at the lowest

level, along the bottom of Figure 2, the team level, team members on the Phantom team were

required to plot intelligence and neutralize counter insurgency enemies, such as insurgent threats.

The members on the Stinger team were required to plot intelligence and neutralize ordinance

disposal enemies, such as improvised explosive devices (IED). At the middle level, or the team

level, each team is tasked with neutralizing identified hotspot cells/areas. These cells were

identified to cause the greatest damage to the convoy traveling through the region. Lastly, at the

highest level, the top level of Figure 2, the system level, the taskforce’s goal is to safely move

the convoy through the region while limiting the damage it received while traveling through the

region.

Page 60: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

42

Individual Level

The Phantom team was comprised of two members. At the individual level, the Recon

Officer was tasked with a) plotting intelligence obtained from command, b) identifying hotspots

based on his or her rules of engagement, and c) rezoning hotspot cell locations as red, whereas

the Field Specialist was tasked with a) neutralizing insurgent hotspots identified by his or her

recon officer following his or her rules of engagement and b) rezoning once designated hotspot

areas as green, after they were cleared of all enemies. By working together the Phantom team

would neutralize the appropriate enemies and clear the region in order for the convoy to travel

through it while limiting the amount of counter insurgents that would inflict damage on the

convoy.

Similarly, at the individual level, the Stinger team, like the Phantom team, was comprised

of two members. The Recon Officer was tasked with a) plotting intelligence obtained from

command, b) identifying hotspots based on his or her rules of engagement, and c) zoning hotspot

cell locations as blue. The Field Specialist was tasked with a) neutralizing improvised explosive

devices (IED) hotspots identified by his or her recon officer following his or her rules of

engagement and b) rezoning once designated hotspot areas as green, once they were cleared of

all enemies. By working together the Stinger team would neutralize the appropriate enemies and

clear the region in order for the convoy to travel through it while limiting the amount of IEDs

that would inflict damage on the convoy.

Component Team Level

Page 61: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

43

At the team level, the goal was to clear the region of any enemy hotspots, specifically of

IED hotspots for the Stinger team and insurgent hotspots for the Phantom team, and it was not

possible for either the Stinger or Phantom team to achieve this goal without collaboration

between the recon officer and the field specialist. For example the Phantom Recon Officer’s task

was to plot intelligence and identify areas that are potential hotspots. The Phantom Recon

Officer did not have the capabilities of neutralizing any enemies in the region and had to rely on

his or her Phantom Field Specialist in order to successfully attain the team’s goal, of clearing the

area of insurgent threats. Similarly, the Phantom Field Specialist did not have the zoning

capabilities as the Phantom Recon Officer and was not able to identify cells as potential hotspot

regions that must be neutralized. Therefore, the Phantom Field Specialist had to await his or her

Phantom Recon Officer’s zoning of regions as hotspots before engaging the enemy threats,

otherwise attempting to neutralize all threats would be an inefficient use of his or her time and

doing so would not allow the Phantom Field Specialist enough time to clear the region of all

insurgent hotspots.

Integration of Teams

At the highest level, the multiteam system level, the goal was to move the convoy

through the war-torn regions as quickly as possible while limiting the amount of damage

inflicted on the convoy. In order to accomplish this goal each team had to communicate with

each other acknowledging that the region was cleared of threats. Coordination between the teams

on when it was safe to move the convoy was essential for the convoy to receive the last amount

of damage.

Page 62: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

44

Procedure

Participants reported to the laboratory and were directed into one of two team rooms.

This was done to insure that the Phantom team members did not interact with the Stinger team

members face-to-face prior to the study. Next participants completed demographic

questionnaires. Following demographic measures, participants were given a 45 minute training

session as a team, where mission objectives, as well as individual role responsibilities were

reviewed. This training was delivered in person by a trained experimenter. After the training

session, participants were escorted to their stations and engaged in two missions.

Mission 1 (i.e., practice mission), lasted a total of 30 minutes. Itfollowed immediately

after participants were trained. Next, participants engaged in a 10 minute transition (i.e.,

planning) phase. Communication with all taskforce memberswas done via virtual communication

(i.e., communication interface). Following the transition phase participants completed measures

as indicated in Appendix A. Next participants engaged in a 20 minute action (i.e., playing)

phase. At the end of the action phase, participants completed a number of measures as depicted

in Appendix A.

Mission 2 (i.e., performance mission)followed immediately after Mission 1. This

mission lasted a total of 60 minutes. Similar to mission 1, participants were given 10 minutes to

engage in strategizing and planning for the mission. During the transition (i.e., planning) phase,

participants were able to communicate with their MTS members via their choice of virtual

communication (e.g., video conference call, audio conference call, or chat). Post the transition

Page 63: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

45

phase, participants were asked to complete a set of measures. Following measure completion,

participants engaged in a 55 minute action (i.e., playing) phase. This action phase was split into

two parts with measure completion in the middle. The first half of the action (i.e., playing) phase

lasted 35 minutes and the second half lasted 20 minutes. At the end of the study as depicted in

Appendix A, performance indices were obtained. In the following session

Measures

Appendix A provides a timeline of the study. It details the time when each of the

following measures was collected throughout each session. Table 2 provides zero-order

correlations for all study variables. Table 3 provides all Cronbach alphas and aggregation

indices for study variables. All Cronbach alphas were analyzed and considered acceptable, with

the exception of the alpha associated with the trust measure. Analyses were conducted and an

alpha was calculated considering the removal of each item. Results of the analysis suggested

that the highest alpha was achieved by including all scale items when aggregating to the scale

level. Removal of any one item resulted in significant drop of the Cronbach alpha associated

with the scale score. Therefore, all items were considered when obtaining a trust score for each

participant. Next analyses were conducted to support the aggregation of individual scores to the

MTS level. Rwg (j)indices of within-group agreement (James, Demaree, & Wolf, 1984) were

calculated for each measure. This index provides a within-group estimation of agreement for

each item ranging from 0-1, where higher scores indicate greater agreement between group

members on a particular construct. Table 3 provides all rwgstatistics for the aggregation of all

Page 64: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

46

study variables from the individual level to the MTS level.

MTS Efficacy

Collective efficacy was measured using 5 distinct scales, all presented in Table 4. It was

of interest to determine the best way to target multiteam level constructs. Therefore multiple

ways of targeting this construct were included solely for this construct in order to avoid

participant fatigue. The first form of multiteam efficacy consisted of a task focus efficacy. Task

efficacy was measured using a 4-item scale. A sample item of the scale consists of “How far do

you think your team and the other team can move the convoy in the upcoming trial?” Responses

were scored on a 1-5, where 1 = 0%, 2 = 25%, 3 = 50%, 4 = 75%, and 5= 100%. An average

was calculated, where higher scores indicated greater levels of multiteam task efficacy.

The second and third multiteam efficacy measure consisted of an adapted 11-item

measure based on Chen, Gully, and Eden’s (2001) general self-efficacy scale. These items were

adapted to refer to each member’s perception of the taskforce in two distinct ways. The first

consisted of refereeing to the taskforce as two distinct teams and the second consisted of

referring to the taskforce as a whole (e.g., the taskforce). A sample item of the team reference

multiteam efficacy scale consisted of “My team and the other team will be able to achieve most

of the goals that are set for the taskforce”whereas a sample item of the taskforce reference

consists of “Our taskforce will be able to achieve most of the goals that are set for the

taskforce”. Response were rated on a Likert-type scale where 1 = Strongly Disagree, 2 =

Disagree, 3 = Neutral, 4 = Agree, and 5 = Strongly Agree. Scores for this scale were averaged,

where higher scores indicated greater levels of multiteam efficacy.

Page 65: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

47

Lastly, the fourth and fifth way of targeting multiteam efficacy, was through another task

focused measure based on the Bandura (1986) task efficacy measure. This measure consisted of

8 items. A sample item of the first set of 8 questions consisted of “I believe that our taskforce

team can finish the mission in the upcoming mission”. Responses were scored as 0 = no and 1 =

yes. A total was calculated for the 8 questions asked; higher scores indicated greater degrees of

multiteam efficacy. A sample of the second set of questions consists of “Based on your above

answer, how certain are you?” Participants were asked to respond by providing a percentage

from 0%-100%; greater number indicated higher levels of multiteam efficacy. Each of the initial

8 Bandura questions was followed immediately by the Bandura percentage question.

MTS Trust

Trust was assessed using a 7-item scale. A sample item consists of “The other team will

keep our team in mind when performing the mission”. All items are provided in Table 5.

Responses were scored on a 5 point Likert-type scale ranging from 1= strongly disagree, 2 =

disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, and 5 = strongly agree. Please see Table 2 for all scale items.

Scores were averaged where higher scores indicated greater level of multiteam trust.

MTS Information Sharing

Information sharing opennesswas measured using a 3-item measure adapted from the

Bunderson and Sutcliffe (2002) measure. A sample item is “The members of the other team

worked hard to keep our team up to date on their activities.” All items are presented in Table 6.

Responseswere scored on a Likert-type scale format where 1 = very strongly disagree, 2 =

Page 66: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

48

disagree, 3 = neither disagree or agree, 4 = agree, and5 = very strongly agree. Responses

across all three items were averaged and higher scores indicated greater levels of multiteam

information sharing openness.

Information sharing uniqueness wascalculated using objective data pulled from the task.

Appendix B provides an overview of how the task relevant information was distributed across all

taskforce members. Scores were obtained for the unique information shared both between

teams and within team. The information sharing unique between teams score consisted of the

amount of information that was exchanged between players of distinct teams that was initially

distributed to only one member of the taskforce. The information sharing unique within team

score consisted of the amount of information that was exchanged between players of the same

team that was initially distributed to only one of the two members engaging in the exchange of

information.

MTS Transactive Memory Systems

The TMS measure consisted of a 10-item scale adapted from the Lewis (2003) TMS scale

commonly used in the literature to assess TMS. The original scale consists of three dimensions:

specialization, credibility, and coordination. For the sake of this study, only the specialization

and credibility dimensions of the scale were included. Each subscale was comprised of 5 items.

A sample item from the specialization scale consisted of “I have knowledge about an aspect of

the task that no other member of the taskforce has.” A sample item from the credibility scale

consisted of “I trust that task knowledge of the other members is credible”. All items are

presented in Table 7. The response scale for these items followed a Likert-type scale format

Page 67: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

49

where 1 = completely disagree, 2 = disagree, 3 = neutral, 4 = agree, and5 = completely agree.

MTS Shared Mental Models

Three shared mental models were measured using the pairwise comparison approach:

multiteam goal shared mental model, multiteam task shared mental model, and multiteam team

shared mental model. Each shared mental model consisted of 6, 5, and 7 nodes or statements

that were compared and rated. Participants were asked to review the nodes presented along the

top of the measure and along the left hand side of the measure. Taking one pairing at a time,

participants were asked to provide a rating on a Likert-type scale where 1 = minimal or no

relationship, 2 = weakly related, 3 = moderately related, 4 = somewhat strongly related, and5 =

very strongly related of how related the two tasks are to one another. All statements presented to

participants to rate are presented in Table 8.

A similarity index was used to assess mental models. The similarity index was computed

using the Quadratic Assignment Procedure (QAP) in UCINET (Borgatti, Everett, & Freeman,

1992). QAP analysis can be utilized to assess the similarity between two matrices. QAP

analyses provide a correlation equivalent to a Pearson correlation which indicates the sharedness

or similarity in the pattern of relationships between team members’ shared mental models.

MTS Performance

A recent review of the literature (Murase et al., 2010) has shown that an overwhelming

amount of team literature has placed an emphasis on team effectiveness as criteria by which to

Page 68: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

50

measure team performance. Table 9 provides an outline of how performance indices were

conceptualized. Multiteam Effectiveness was measured using the taskforce’s ability to move the

convoy safely through the Kazbar region, through a predefined path, while having the least

amount of damage inflicted on the convoy. It was explained to participants that the convoy

would not deviate from the path previously defined and that it would only move along when

ordered by one of the members of the taskforce. This predefined path was provided to

participants in order to provide more control of the game environment. This provided for all

taskforces to experience the same environment, the same amount of attacks on the convoy and

required them to travel through the same amount of cells to get to the final destination.

Multiteam effectiveness was assessed by taking into account whether taskforce members

followed the rules of engagement while neutralizing the war-torn region and riding it of counter

insurgents and IEDs. The rules of engagement consisted of 1) all threats in a cell were reported,

2) threat within a cell is correctly neutralized, and 3)convoy travels safely through the cell. If

these rules of engagement were not followed in order then the convoy would receive damage.

The greater damage received by the convoy, the more points were deducted from the taskforce’s

overall score at the end of the task. Although effectiveness is an important performance criterion,

it is not the only means by which performance was assessed. A second criteria to measure system

performance was included, multiteam efficiency.

Multiteam efficiency was an objective task derived index operationalized as the

taskforce’s ability to move the convoy along a predefined path in the shortest amount of time as

possible. The task gave us the ability to track the time when each taskforce reached the final

destination cell. All taskforces were given 55 minutes to accomplish their goal of reaching the

Page 69: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

51

final destination cell. At the 55 minute mark all taskforces were asked to stop. The amount of

time was then recorded. A high score indicated a less efficient taskforce, whereas low score

indicated a highly efficient taskforce.

Communication Medium

In contrast to much prior work on virtuality where teams are restricted to certain media

and then groups of participants using one media or another are compared, this study allows

participants to choose the media that they use to communicate and to change this fluidly as the

task u folds. Thus, communication media includes a set of measured process variables reflecting

how much time the team spent using a particular type of media.

In order to assess media retrievability, three unobtrusive measures were derived using the

chat logs as recorded using the Pamela software. The first consisted of social information

exchanged via media retrievability tools (i.e., chat). The second entailed task focused messages

sent via media retrievability tools, and the third was an aggregate of the two that was assigned a

time value. The time value consisted of the summation of the following: the amount of social

messages multiplied by 4 seconds and the amount of task specific messages multiplied by 10

seconds. The selection of 4 and 10 seconds were due to the information that was being relayed

across participants. Four seconds were assigned to social related messages because they mainly

consisted of messages that were very short and did not require many cognitive resources, such

as: want to grab food after, whereas 10 seconds were assigned to each task related message

because it consisted of having participants sit read their intelligence and copy it into a chat

window. A sample task related message was I8 – Humanitarian Tent (63, 361). These

Page 70: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

52

messages, although short, were less intuitive and required participates to continuously refer to

their intelligence sheets in order to input it correctly into their chats. For each of the three

measures of media retrievability, the higher the number the more messages or time spent using

the media retrievability tools.

Media richness consisted of the total amount of time that media rich tools were

employed. Each Pamela video and audio conference call recording was reviewed. The following

was extracted from each recording: 1) amount of time per video where taskforce members were

present and visual (i.e., a live feed of their interaction that showed their face) cues could be

interpreted, 2) amount of time per video where taskforce members were present and only their

voices could be heard (i.e., no live feed of their interaction could be seen), and 3) the total

amount of individuals present for each exchange described in points 1 and 2. Next the product of

steps 1 and 3 was multiplied by 1.5 and the product of steps 2 and 3 were multiplied by1,

essentially staying the same. Since video conversations are viewed as more rich, the exchange

that consisted of video was awarded a greater number, thus multiplied by1.5. Lastly, the scores

obtained from the video and the audio calculations were summed and this summation comprised

the media richness index.

Control Variables

The following variables were measured and their relations to key study variables

examined to determine their utility as potential control variables. Two question pertaining to PC

Video game and Video game experience as well as two question pertaining to virtual

communication were asked. All questions can be found in Table 10. Table 11 portrays the

Page 71: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

53

correlations between each of the four potential controls and all study variables. Based on these

correlations the following control variables were considered, IMing, PC video play experience

and Video play experience. Further analysis were conducted to determine if PC game

experience and Video game experience should be aggregated into one construct. The correlation

between the two of r = -.35, p<.01 led to the aggregation of the variables into one control

variable. The new aggregated variable in conjunction with the IMing variable were used in all

analyses as control variables.

Page 72: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

54

CHAPTER THREE: RESULTS

Overview of the Analyses Presented in the Results

The results are divided into four sections. The first section discusses the results of the

analyses depicted in the Theoretical Model that was initially proposed. Figure 1 depicts the

relationships tested. Tables 12-19, Models 1-36, summarize the relationships studied to test

Hypotheses 1-8. The second section of the discussion includes the relationships depicted in the

Follow-up Exploratory Analyses Model 1 (please see Figure 3). These relationships still shadow

the pattern of relationships that were predicted in the Theoretical Model, for instance,

motivational/affect emergent states predict behavioral processes, behavioral processes predict

cognitive emergent states, and finally cognitive emergent states predict performance. The

difference between the two models is that rather than efficacy predicting IS uniqueness, efficacy

predicts IS openness. Rather than IS uniqueness predicting TMS, IS uniqueness predicts SMM.

Tables 20-29, Models 37-96, summarize the relationships studied.

Based on results of the Theoretical Model and the Follow-Up Exploratory Model 1

additional exploratory analyses were conducted to look at the relationship between behavioral

processes and cognitive emergent states. Table 30 summarized results of lagged correlations that

were conducted to determine if behavior processes predicted cognitive emergent states or vice

versa. Based on the results two subsequent sections were included in the results.

The third section of this discussion includes the relationships depicted in the Follow-up

Page 73: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

55

Exploratory Model 2. Figure 4 provides a depiction of all the relationships studied. Rather than

considering motivational/affective emergent states predicting behavioral processes and in turn

behavioral processes predicting cognitive emergent states, and cognitive emergent states

predicting performance; the relationships studied in this model focuses on motivational/affective

emergent states predicting cognitive emergent states, cognitive emergent states predicting

behavioral processes, and behavioral processes predicting performance. Tables 31-38, Models

97-131, summarizethe relationships studied.Lastly, the fourth section of this discussion, takes

into consideration all the relationships depicted in the Follow-up Exploratory Model 3. In this

model motivational/affective emergent states predict cognitive emergent states, cognitive

emergent states predict behavioral processes, and behavioral processes predict performance.

Figure 5 provides a depiction of all the relationship studied. Tables 39-48, Models 132-200

summarize the relationships studied.

Theoretical Model Analyses

Motivationally-Driven Development

Hypothesis 1 stated that MTS efficacy would be positively related to MTSIS uniqueness.

Table 12, Models 1-10, summarize all analyses used to test this hypothesis. To test Hypothesis 1,

multiple regression was employed. To test the effects of MTS efficacy on MTS IS uniqueness,

MTS IS uniqueness was regressed onto the control variables, (aggregate of PC and Video game

experience and IMing, explanation indicating why these variables were selected is discussed in

the Method section of this document) in Step 1 and in Step 2 onto MTS efficacy. Hypothesis 1

Page 74: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

56

was not supported for the dependent variable MTS IS between teams; the standardized beta

(presented in Step 2 of Models 1-5) associated with MTS efficacy was not statistically significant

(ßMTS Task Efficacy= .00, ns; ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (team) = .02, ns;ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (MTS) = .09, ns;ßMTS

Bandura Percentage= .04, ns; and ßMTS Bandura Yes/No = .12, ns). Hypothesis 1 was also not supported for

the dependent variable MTS IS within team; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of

Models 6-10) associated with multiteam efficacy was not statistically significant (ßMTS Task

Efficacy= .15, ns; ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (team) = .15, ns;ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (MTS) = .10, ns;ßMTS Bandura

Percentage= .11, ns; and ßMTS Bandura Yes/No = -.02, ns).

Hypothesis 2 stated that MTSIS uniqueness is positively related to MTS performance,

(efficiency and effectiveness). To testHypothesis 2,multiple regression analyses were employed.

Table 13, Models 11-14, summarize all analyses used to test this hypothesis. In Step 1 of

Models11-14, MTSperformance was regressed onto the control variables and in Step 2 onto

MTS IS uniqueness. Hypothesis 2 was not supported for the dependent variable MTS efficacy;

the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Models 11 and 12) associated with MTS IS

uniqueness was not statistically significant (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams = .20, p <.10; ßMTS IS

Uniqueness Within Team = .17, ns). Hypothesis 2 was supported for the dependent variable MTS

effectiveness; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Models 13 and 14) associated with IS

uniqueness was statistically significant (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams = .23, p <.05; ßMTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team = .22, p <.05).

To test for mediation the bootstrapping technique has been viewed as a superior approach

over the traditional Baron and Kenny (1986) method. Bootstrapping affords us a number of

advantages to testing mediation. For instance, bootstrapping does not restrict us to the

Page 75: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

57

assumption of normality, which is often a problem in small sample sizes. Additionally,

bootstrapping provides us the ability to test the mediation effects in small samples providing us

statistics regarding the indirect effect that allow us to determine the magnitude of indirect effects.

Based on these advantages to the bootstrapping technique for testing indirect effects in

conjunction with the Preacher and Hayes (2004) method of testing for mediation was employed.

The Preacher and Hayes methodology was selected because it has been viewed as a superior

choice when testing for mediation hypotheses (MacKinnon, Fairchild, & Fritz, 2007;

MacKinnon, Krull, & Lockwood, 2000).The Preacher and Hayes method to test for mediation

allows one to test and estimate the mediated effect providing a better understanding of the

magnitude of the mediator in a mediated relationship. Additionally, work by Kenny, Kashy, and

Bolger (1998) suggest that for mediation to be present, the first step established by Baron and

Kenny (1986) is not necessary. The Preacher and Hayes method provides us some flexibility

around the strict steps presented by Baron and Kenny’s.

In order to test for mediation the Preacher and Hayes (2004) SPSS Macro titled Process

(Preacher & Hayes; http://www.afhayes.com/spss-sas-and-mplus-macros-and-code.html) was

adapted. All mediation analysis and results are based on 1,000 bootstrap samples. This macro

provides statistics for five paths (MacKinnon et al., 2007; Preacher & Hayes), four of which have

been outlined by Baron and Kenny (1986). First, Path a = the relationship between the

independent variable and the mediator. Second, Path b = the relationship between the mediator

and the dependent variable. Third, Path c = the relationship between the independent variable

and the dependent variable (also referred to as the total effect; Preacher & Hayes). Fourth, c´=

can be viewed as the left over variance not explained by the mediator (also referred to as the

Page 76: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

58

direct path; Preacher &Hayes). Lastly, Path ab = the relationship between the independent

variable and the dependent variable adjusting for the mediator (referred to as the indirect path;

Preacher & Hayes, 2004). In order to show that mediation is present, Preacher and Hayes suggest

that a significant indirect path must be present. For the purpose of presenting the mediation

results, statistical significant for each of the following paths will be discussed: total, direct, and

indirect path. In the event that the indirect path is significant and the BCa 95% confidence

interval does not include zero, we have sufficient evidence for mediation (Preacher & Hayes).

Hypothesis 3 stated that MTS IS uniqueness is positively related to MTS performance

(efficiency and effectiveness) through (i.e., mediated by) MTS TMS. This hypothesis was tested

employing bootstrapping software following the Preacher and Hayes (2004) methodology to test

for indirect effects. Table 14, Models 15-18, summarize all analyses used to test this hypothesis.

Hypothesis 3 was not supported for the dependent variable MTS efficacy.

As depicted in Model 15 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness on MTS efficiency was

significant, 2.50,p< .10 indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent

variable. The direct effect was, 2.55,p< .05, and the indirect effect through the proposed

mediator was -.06,ns, with a 95% BCa CI (bias-corrected confidence interval) of -.79 to .48

(1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include zero, it can be

concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated

by MTS TMS.

Model 16 depicts that the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness on MTS efficiency was

2.09,ns, indicating that the independent variable was notrelated to the dependent variable. The

direct effect was 2.16, p< .10 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was -.07, ns,

Page 77: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

59

with a 95% BCa CI of -.94 to .51 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval

did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and MTS

performance was not mediated by MTS TMS.

Model 17 depicts that the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness on MTS efficiency was.68,

p< .05, indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was .70, p< .05and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was -.02,ns, with a

95% BCa CI of -.23 to .17 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did

include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and MTS

performance was not mediated by MTS TMS.

Model 18 depicts that the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness on MTS efficiency was.63,

p< .05, indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was not significant .66, ns while the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was -

.02, ns, with a 95% BCa CI of -.27 to .16 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence

interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and

MTS performance was not mediated by MTS TMS.

Affect-Driven Development

Hypothesis 4 stated that MTS trust is positively related to MTS IS openness. To test

Hypothesis 4, multiple regression was employed. Table 15, Model 19, summarizes the analysis

used to test this hypothesis. To test the effects of MTS trust on MTS IS openness, MTS IS

openness was regressed onto the control variables, in Step 1 andin Step 2 onto MTS trust.

Hypothesis 4 was supported; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Model 19) associated

Page 78: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

60

with MTS trust was statistically significant (ß MTS Trust= .38, p< .01).

Hypothesis 5 stated that MTSIS openness is positively related to MTS performance

(efficiency and effectiveness). Multiple regression analyses were conducted to test this

hypothesis. Table 16, Models 20 and 21, summarize all analyses used to test this hypothesis.

InStep 1 of Models 20 and 21, MTSperformance was regressed onto the control variables and in

Step 2 onto MTS IS openness. Hypothesis 2 was supported for the dependent variable MTS

efficiency; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Model 20) associated with MTS IS

openness was statistically significant (ßMTS IS Openness = .31, p < .01). Hypothesis 2 was also

supported for the dependent variable MTS effectiveness; the standardized beta (presented in Step

2 of Model 21) associated with MTS IS openness was not statistically significant (ßMTS IS Openness

= .35, p < .01).

Hypothesis 6 stated that MTS IS openness is positively related to MTS performance

(efficiency and effectiveness) through (i.e., mediated by) MTS SMM. This hypothesis was

tested employing bootstrapping software following the Preacher and Hayes (2004) methodology

to test for indirect effects. Table 17, Models 22-27, summarize all analyses used to test this

hypothesis. Hypothesis 6 was not supported for the dependent variable MTS efficiency.

The total effect of MTS IS openness on MTS efficiency for Model 22 was 3.71, p< .05,

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was 3.57, p< .05and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS Goal SMM)

was .14, ns, with a 95% BCa CI of -.47 to 1.26 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the

confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

Page 79: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

61

For Model 23 the total effect of MTS IS openness on MTS efficiency was 3.88, p< .01,

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was 3.80, p< .01 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS Task SMM)

was.08, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -1.04 to .98 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the

confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 24 the total effect of MTS IS openness on MTS efficiency was 5.07, p< .01,

indicating that the independent variable is related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was 4.91, p< .01 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS Team SMM)

was.17, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.22 to 2.18 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the

confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

Hypothesis 6 was not supported for the dependent variable MTS effectiveness. The total

effect of MTS IS openness on MTS effectiveness for Model 25 was significant 1.14, p< .01,

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was not significant when the mediator (i.e., MTS Goal SMM) was included 1.18, ns, while the

indirect effect through the proposed mediator was -.05, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.29 to .10

(1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include zero, it can be

concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated

by MTS SMM.

For Model 26 the total effect of MTS IS openness on MTS effectiveness was 1.16, p<

.01, indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct

Page 80: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

62

effect was 1.03, p< .01 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS Task

SMM) was .13, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.11 to .39 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the

confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 27 the total effect of MTS IS openness on MTS effectiveness was .97, p< .05,

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was.92, ns, and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS Team SMM) was

.05, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.05 to .42 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence

interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and

MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

Media Retrievability Moderator

Hypothesis 7 stated that media retrievability would moderate the relationship between

MTS IS uniqueness and MTS TMS.Table 18, Models 28-33, summarize all analyses used to test

this hypothesis. To test Hypothesis 7 multiple regression was employed.In Step 1 of Models 28-

33, MTS TMS was regressed onto the control variables, the centered MTS IS uniqueness

variable and the centered media retrievability variable, and in Step 2 onto the multiplicative

interaction term (centered MTS IS uniqueness within team by the centered media retrievability

task). Hypothesis 7 was supported; the standardized beta (presented in Models 31 and 33)

associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were statistically significant Model 28 (ßMTS IS

Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Retrievability Social = -.08, ns), Model 29 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media

Retrievability Social = -.36, ns), Model 30 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Retrievability Task = -.08, ns),

Page 81: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

63

Model 31 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Task = -.24, p < .05),Model 32 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness

Between Team X Media Retrievability Aggregate = -.07, ns), and Model 33 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media

Retrievability Aggregate) = -.24, p < .05).

In order to determine the effect of the interaction, means were plotted. Although the

interaction term in step 2 of Model 31 was found to be significant, as shown in Figure 6, the plot

of the means did not depict any clear pattern between the effects of MTS IS uniqueness within

team and media retrievability task specific on MTS TMS. Figure 6 suggest that regardless of

unique information exchanged within teams and the amount of task specific exchanged via

media retrievability tools MTS develop a moderate level of transactive memory systems. The

same process was conducted to determine the effect of the interaction for Model 33. Once again,

although the interaction term in step 2 of Model 33 was found to be significant, as shown in

Figure 7, the plot of the means did not depict any clear pattern between the effects of MTS IS

uniqueness within team and media retrievability aggregate on MTS TMS. Figure 7 suggests that

regardless of the unique information exchanged within teams and the amount of both social and

task relevant information exchange via media retrievability tools MTS develop a moderate level

of transactive memory systems.

Media Richness Moderator

Hypothesis 8 stated media richness moderated the relationship between MTSIS openness

and MTSSMM.Table 19, Models 34-36, summarize all analyses used to test this hypothesis. To

test Hypothesis 8 multiple regression was employed.In Step 1 of Models 34-36, MTSSMM was

regressed onto the control variables, the centered MTS ISopenness variable and the centered

Page 82: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

64

mediarichness variable, and in Step 2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTS

ISopennesswithin team by the centered mediarichness). Hypothesis 8 was not supported; the

standardized beta (presented in Models 34-36) associated with the interaction term Step 2 were

not statistically significant Model 34 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Richness = -.26, p < .10), Model 35 (ßMTS

IS Openness X Media Richness) = -.15, ns), and Model 36 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Richness) = .06, ns).

Follow-Up Exploratory Analyses Model 1

Additional analyses were conducted following the same sequence of relationships as

those described in the Theoretical Model, emergent states predicting behavioral processes, which

were expected to promote the development of cognitive emergent states and lastly impact MTS

performance. To differentiate between the Theoretical Model and these analyses, the model has

been labeled the Follow-up Exploratory Analyses Model 1 and is depicted in Figure 3. The

difference between the Follow-up Exploratory Analyses Model 1 and the Theoretical Model is

the expected relationship associated with the emergent state predictor and behavioral process as

well as the relationships associated with the behavioral process and cognitive emergent states.

Summary of the relationships analyzed can be found in Table 1.

Motivationally-Driven Development

MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS IS openness was tested employing regression.

Table 20, Models 37-41, summarize all analyses used to test this relationship. In Step 1 MTS IS

openness was regressed onto the control variables and in Step 2 onto MTS efficacy. Results

support this relationship; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Models 37-41) associated

Page 83: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

65

with MTS efficacy was statistically significant (ßMTSTask Efficacy= .50, p < .01; ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy

(team) = .47, p < .01; ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (MTS) = .42, p < .01; ßMTS Bandura Percentage= .26, p < .05; and

ßMTS Bandura Yes/No = .12, ns).

MTS IS openness is positively related to MTS performance (efficiency and effectiveness)

through (i.e., mediated by) MTS TMS was tested employing bootstrapping software following

the Preacher and Hayes (2004) methodology to test for indirect effects. Table 21, Models 42 and

43, summarize all analyses used to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 42 and 43, MTS

performance was regressed onto the control variables and IS openness and in Step 2 onto MTS

TMS.

The MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS IS openness relationship was not

supported for the dependent variable MTS efficiency. For Model 42 the total effect of MTS IS

openness on MTS efficiency was 3.90, p< .01 indicating that the independent variable was

related to the dependent variable. The direct effect was 3.65, p< .01 and the indirect effect

through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS TMS) was .24, ns, with a 95% BCa CI of -1.53 to 2.08

(1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include zero, it can be

concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated

by MTS SMM.

The MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS IS openness relationship was not

supported for the dependent variable MTS effectiveness. For Model 43 the total effect of MTS

IS openness on MTS effectiveness was 1.01, p< .01 indicating that the independent variable was

related to the dependent variable. The direct effect was .79, p< .05 and the indirect effect through

the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS TMS) was .22, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.12 to .63 (1,000

Page 84: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

66

bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that

the relationship between IS uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS TMS.

Affectively-Driven Development

MTS trust is positively related to MTS ISuniqueness was tested employing

regression.Table 22, Models 44 and 45, summarize the analysis used to test this relationship. In

Step 1 MTS IS uniqueness was regressed onto the control variables and in Step 2 onto MTS

trust. This relationship was not supported; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Modes

44 and 45) associated with MTS IS uniqueness was not statistically significant Model 44 (ßMTSIS

Uniqueness Between Teams= .08, ns) and Model 45 (ßMTSIS Uniqueness Within Teams= .06, ns).

MTS IS uniqueness is positively related to MTS performance (efficiency and

effectiveness) through (i.e., mediated by) MTS SMM was tested employing bootstrapping

software following the Preacher and Hayes (2004) methodology to test for indirect effects.

Table 23, Models 46-57, summarize the analysis used to test this relationship. The MTS IS

uniqueness is positively related to MTS performance through MTS SMM was not supported for

the dependent variable MTS efficiency.

For Model 46 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness between teams on MTS efficiency

was 2.89, p< .05 indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable.

The direct effect was 2.82, p< .05 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .07,

ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.20 to 1.13 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence

interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and

MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

Page 85: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

67

For Model 47 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness within team on MTS efficiency was

1.85, nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The

direct effect was 1.91, nsand the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS Goal

SMM) was -.06, nswith a 95% BCa CI of -.94 to .30 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the

confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 48 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness between teams on MTS efficiency

was 3.20, p< .05indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable.

The direct effect was 2.98, p< .05and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e.,

MTS Task SMM) was .22, nswith a 95% BCa CI of -.40 to 1.42 (1,000 bootstrap resamples).

Because the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship

between IS uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 49 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness within team on MTS efficiency was

2.99, p< .05 indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The

direct effect was 2.79, p< .10 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS

Task SMM) was .25, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.21 to 1.43 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because

the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 50 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness between teams on MTS efficiency

was 3.26, p< .05indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable.

The direct effect was 3.33, nsand the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS

Team SMM) was -.09, nswith a 95% BCa CI of -1.26 to .33 (1,000 bootstrap resamples).

Page 86: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

68

Because the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship

between IS uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 51 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness within team on MTS efficiency was

2.56, nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The

direct effect was 2.42, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS Team

SMM) was .11, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.19 to 1.72 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the

confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 52 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness between teams on MTS effectiveness

was .77, p< .05 indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable.

The direct effect was .76, p< .05 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .00,

ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.09 to .19 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence

interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and

MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 53 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness within team on MTS effectiveness

was .56, ns indicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable.

The direct effect was .00, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS

Goal SMM) was .00, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.06 to .12 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because

the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 54 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness between teams on MTS effectiveness

was .93, nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable.

Page 87: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

69

The direct effect was .79, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS

Task SMM) was .14, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.01 to .49 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because

the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 55 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness within team on MTS effectiveness

was .95, p< .01 indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable.

The direct effect was .84, p< .05 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS

Task SMM) was .13, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.01 to .43 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because

the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

For Model 56 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness between teams on MTS effectiveness

was .71, p< .05 indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable.

The direct effect was insignificant when the mediator was .73, p< .05 and the indirect effect

through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS Team SMM) was -.02, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.25

to .09 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include zero, it can be

concluded that the relationship between IS uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated

by MTS SMM.

For Model 57 the total effect of MTS IS uniqueness within team on MTS effectiveness

was .55, nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable.

The direct effect was .51, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS

Team SMM) was .03, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.05 to .34 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because

the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between IS

Page 88: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

70

uniqueness and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS SMM.

Media Retrievability Moderator

Media retrievability moderates the relationship betweenMTSIS uniqueness between

teams and MTSSMM was tested employing hierarchical regression. To test this relationship,

multiple regression was employed.Table 24, Models 58-75, summarize the analysis used to test

this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 58-75, MTSSMM was regressed onto the control variables,

the centered MTS IS uniqueness variable and the centered media retrievability variable, and in

Step 2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered multiteam IS uniqueness within team by

the centered media retrievability task). The media retrievability moderates the relationship

between MTS IS uniqueness and MTS SMMrelationship was not supported for the dependent

variable MTS Goal SMM; the standardized beta (presented in Models 58-63) associated with the

interaction term in Step 2 were not statistically significant:for Model 58 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between

Teams X Media Retrievability Social = .15, ns), for Model 59 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Social =

.11, ns), for Model 60 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Retrievability Task = .03, ns), for Model 61

(ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Task = -.13,ns), for Model 62 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Team X

Media Retrievability Aggregate = .01, ns), and for Model 63 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability

Aggregate= -.18, ns). The media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS IS

uniqueness and MTS SMMrelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS Task

SMM; the standardized beta (presented in Models 64-69) associated with the interaction term in

Step 2 were not statistically significant: Model 64 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Retrievability

Page 89: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

71

Social = -.12, ns), for Model 65 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Social = -.26, ns), for Model

66 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Retrievability Task = -.31, p< .01), for Model 67 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team X Media Retrievability Task = -.38, p < .01), for Model 68 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Team X Media

Retrievability Aggregate = -.35, p< .01), and for Model 69 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability

Aggregate) = -.37, p < .01). The media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS IS

uniqueness and MTS SMMrelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS Team

SMM; the standardized beta (presented in Models 70-75) associated with the interaction term in

Step 2 were not statistically significant: for Model 70 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Retrievability

Social = .12, ns), for Model 71 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Social = -.14, ns), for Model

72 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Retrievability Task = .01, ns), for Model 73 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team X Media Retrievability Task = .03, ns), for Model 74 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Team X Media Retrievability

Aggregate = -.01, ns), and for Model 75 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Aggregate) = .01, ns).

In order to determine the effect of the interaction in Model 66, means were plotted.

Although the interaction term in step 2 of Model 66 was found to be significant, as shown in

Figure 8, the plot of the means did not depict any clear pattern between the effects of multiteam

information sharing uniqueness between teams and media retrievability task specific on

multiteam task SMM. The plot depicted the lines to be parallel. Figure 8 suggests that

regardless of the unique information exchanged between teams and the amount of task relevant

information exchanged via media retrievability tools MTS do not develop similar MTS task

SMM.

In order to determine the effect of the interaction in Model 67, means were plotted.

Although the interaction term in step 2 of Model 67 was found to be significant, as shown in

Page 90: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

72

Figure 9, the plot of the means did not depict any clear pattern between the effects of multiteam

information sharing uniqueness within team and media retrievability task on multiteam task

SMM. The plot depicted the lines to be parallel. Figure 9 suggests that regardless of the unique

information exchanged within team and the amount of task relevant information exchange via

media retrievability tools MTS develop do not develop similar SMM.

In order to determine the effect of the interaction in Model 68, means were plotted.

Although the interaction term in step 2 of Model 68 was found to be significant, as shown in

Figure 10, the plot of the means did not depict any clear pattern between the effects of multiteam

information sharing uniqueness between teams within team and media retrievability social and

task aggregate on multiteam task SMM. The plot depicted the lines to be parallel. Figure 10

suggests that regardless of the unique information exchanged between teams and the amount of

task relevant information exchange via media retrievability tools MTS do not develop a MTS

TMS.

In order to determine the effect of the interaction in Model 69, means were plotted.

Although the interaction term in step 2 of Model 69 was found to be significant, as shown in

Figure 11, the plot of the means did not depict any clear pattern between the effects of multiteam

information sharing uniqueness within team and media retrievability social and task aggregate on

multiteam task SMM. The plot depicted the lines to be parallel. Figure 11 suggests that

regardless of the unique information exchanged within team and the amount of both social and

task relevant information exchange via media retrievability tools MTS do not develop similar

MTS task SMM.

Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS IS openness and MTS

Page 91: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

73

TMSwas tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 25, Models 76-78, summarize the

analysis used to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 76-78, MTS TMS was regressed onto

the control variables, the centered MTS IS openness variable and the centered media

retrievability variable and in Step 2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTS IS

openness by centered media retrievability). The media retrievability moderates the relationship

between MTS IS openness and MTS TMSrelationship was not supported; the standardized beta

(presented in Models 76-78) associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were not statistically

significant; for Model 76 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability Social = -.16, ns), for Model 77 (ßMTS IS

Openness X Media Retrievability Task = .06, ns), and for Model 78 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability Aggregate =

.08, ns).

Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS IS opennessand

MTSSMM. Table 26, Models 79-87, summarize the analysis used to test this relationship. In

Step 1 of Models 79-87, MTSSMM similarity was regressed onto the control variables, the

centered MTS IS openness and the centered media retrievability variable and in Step 2 onto the

multiplicative interaction term (centered MT S IS openness by centered media retrievability).

The media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS IS openness and MTS

SMMrelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS Goal SMM; the

standardized beta (presented in Models 79-87) associated with the interaction term in Step 2

were statistically significant: Model 79 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability Social = .26, ns), for Model

80 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability Task = .19, ns), and for Model 81 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability

Aggregate) = .18, ns). Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS IS openness

and MTS SMMrelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS Task SMM; the

Page 92: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

74

standardized beta (presented in Models 82-84) associated with the interaction term in Step 2

were statistically significant: Model 82 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability Social = .10, ns), for Model

83 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability Task = -.08, ns), and for Model 84 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability

Aggregate) = -.07, ns). Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS IS openness

and MTS SMMrelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS Team SMM; the

standardized beta (presented in Models 85-87) associated with the interaction term in Step 2

were statistically significant:for Model 85 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability Social = -.14, ns), for

Model 86 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Retrievability Task = -.09, ns), and for Model 87 (ßMTS IS Openness X Media

Retrievability Aggregate) = -.13, ns).

Media Richness Moderator

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTSIS openness and MTS TMSwas

tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 27, Model 88,summarizes the analysis used to

test this relationship. In Step 1 of Model 88, MTS TMS was regressed onto the controls, the

centeredMTS IS openness and centered media richness and in Step 2 onto the multiplicative

interaction term (centered MTS IS openness by centered media richness).Media richness

moderates the relationship between MTS IS openness and MTS TMS relationship was not

supported; the standardized beta (presented in Models 88) associated with the interaction term in

Step 2 was not statistically significant (ßMTS IS Openness X Media Richness = .03, ns).

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS IS uniqueness and

MTSSMMwas tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 28, Models 89-94, summarize

Page 93: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

75

the analysis used to test this relationship.In Step 1, MTSSMM was regressed onto the control

variables, the centered MTS IS uniqueness and centered media richness and in Step 2 onto the

multiplicative interaction term (centered MTS IS uniqueness by centered media richness).

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS IS uniqueness and MTS

SMMrelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS Goal SMM; the

standardized beta (presented in Models 89 and 90) associated with the interaction term in Step 2

were not statistically significant: for Model 89 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Richness = -.10, ns)

and for Model 90 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Richness= -.04, ns). Media richness moderates

the relationship between MTS IS uniqueness and MTS SMMrelationship was not supported for

the dependent variable MTS Task SMM; the standardized beta (presented in Models 91 and 92)

associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were not statistically significant: for Model 91

(ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Richness = .11, ns) and for Model 92 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X

Media Richness = .06, ns). Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS IS uniqueness

and MTS SMM relationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS Team SMM; the

standardized beta (presented in Models 93 and 94) associated with the interaction term in Step 2

were not statistically significant: for Model 93 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Richness = -.12, ns)

and for Model 94 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Richness = -.05, ns).

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS IS uniqueness and MTS

TMSwas tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 29, Models 95 and 96summarize all

analyses used to test this relationship.In Step 1 of Models 95 and 96MTS TMS was regressed

onto the control variables, the centered MTS IS uniqueness and centered media richness and in

Step 2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTS IS uniqueness by centered media

Page 94: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

76

richness). Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS IS uniqueness and MTS

TMSrelationship was not supported; the standardized beta (presented in Models 95 and 96)

associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were not statistically significant Model 95 (ßMTS IS

Uniqueness Between Teams X Media Richness = -.10, ns) and for Model 96 (ßMTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media

Richness = -.14, ns).

Follow-Up Exploratory Analyses Model 2

In the previous two sections of the results a number of meditational analyses were

conducted testing the effects of MTS behavior (i.e., MTS IS) on MTS performance through MTS

cognition (MTS TMS and MTS SMM). These analyses yielded results that indicating that MTS

cognition was not a mediator of the MTS behavior and MTS performance relationships. Results

led to the conclusion that both MTS behavior and MTS cognition are predictors of MTS

performance. Based on these findings subsequent analyses were conducted to explore the MTS

behavior and MTS cognition relationship. Lagged correlations analyses were conducted and

reported in Table 30. The correlations take into account MTS behavior at Time 1 and look at its

relationship on MTS cognition at Time 2, similarly, lagged correlations were calculated for MTS

cognition at Time 2 and look at its relationship on MTS behavior at Time 3.

Analyses suggest that distinct relationships exist between the MTS IS and MTS cognition

variables. The strength of the 8 relationships were assessed. Strength was interpreted as

inferring causality between study constructsthat were collected at distinct points in time during

the tenure of each MTS. Correlational analysis focusing on MTS IS at Time 1 predicting MTS

cognition at Time 2 suggest that out of the 8 correlations 1correlation was stronger, whereas

Page 95: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

77

7correlations were stronger when focusing on the MTS cognition at Time 2 predicting MTS IS at

Time 3 relationship.These results were encouraging in taking an alternative view of the dynamics

that play out between MTS cognitive emergent states and behavioral processes. For this reason

the relationships previously established in both the Theoretical Model and the Exploratory Model

were adapted to reflect the effects of MTS cognition on MTS behavior. Figures 4 and 5 portray

these relationships, where MTS motivational/affective emergent states predicted MTS cognitive

emergent states and MTS cognitive emergent states predict MTS behavioral processes.

Motivationally-Driven Development

MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS TMSwas tested employing regression. Table

31, Models 91-101, depict all analyses analyzed to test this relationship. In Step 1, MTS TMS

was regressed onto the control variables, and in Step 2 ontoMTS efficacy. MTS efficacy is

positively related to MTS TMS relationship was supported; the standardized beta (presented in

Step 2 of Models 97-101) associated with MTS efficacy were statistically significant (ßMTS Task

Efficacy = .51, p < .01; ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (team) = .46, p < .01;ß MTS Taskforce Efficacy (MTS) = .43, p <

.01;ßMTS Bandura Percentage = .38, p < .01; and ßMTS Bandura Yes/No = .01, ns).

MTS TMS is positively related to MTS performance (efficiency and effectiveness) was

tested employing regression. Table 32, Models 102 and 103 summarize all analyses to test this

relationship. In Step 1 of Models 102 and 103, MTSperformance was regressed onto the control

variables and in Step 2 onto MTSTMS. The MTS TMS is positively related to MTS

performancerelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS efficiency; the

standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Model 102) associated with MTS TMS was not

Page 96: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

78

statistically significant Model 102 (ßMTS TMS= .20, p < .10). The MTS TMS is positively related

to TMS performancerelationship was supported for the dependent variable MTS effectiveness;

the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Model 103) associated with MTS TMS was

statistically significant Model 103 (ßMTS TMS = .29, p< .01).

MTS TMS is positively related to MTS performance (efficiency and effectiveness)

through (i.e., mediated by) MTS IS uniquenesswas tested employing bootstrapping software

following the Preacher and Hayes (2004) methodology totest for indirect effects. Table 33,

Models 104-107, summarize all relationships analyzed to test this hypothesis. To test for MTS

performance, in Step 1 of Models 104-107, MTS efficiency was regressed onto the control

variables and MTS TMS and in Step 2 onto MTS IS uniqueness.

The MTS TMS is positively related to MTS performance through MTS IS

uniquenessrelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS efficiency. As

depicted in Model 104 the total effect of MTS TMS on MTS efficiency was 2.45, p< .10

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was 1.87, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .95, ns with a 95% BCa

CI of -.04 to 2.03 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include zero,

it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS performance was not

mediated by MTS IS uniqueness between teams.

Model 105 depicts the total effect of MTS TMS on MTS efficiency was 2.45, p< .10

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was 2.33, p< .10 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .15, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.19 to 1.11 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

Page 97: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

79

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS performance was not

mediated by MTS IS Uniqueness within team.

MTS TMS is positively related to MTS performance through MT IS uniqueness was not

supported for the dependent variable MTS effectiveness. As depicted in Model 106 the total

effect of MTS TMS on MTS effectiveness was .83, p< .01 indicating that the independent

variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect was .72, p< .05 and the indirect

effect through the proposed mediator was .18, ns with a 95% BCa CI of .03 to .48 (1,000

bootstrap resamples). Although the confidence interval did not include zero, the coefficient was

not statistically significant and it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS TMS and

MTS performance was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness between teams.

Model 107 depicts the total effect of MTS TMS on MTS effectiveness was .83, p< .01

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was .80, p< .01 and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .04, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.03 to .27 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS performance was not

mediated by MTS IS Uniqueness within team.

Affect-Driven Development

MTS trust is positively related to MTSSMM was tested employing hierarchical

regression. Table 34, Models 108-110, summarize all relationships analyzed to test this

hypothesis. In Step 1 of Models 108-110, MTSSMM was regressed onto the control variables

and in Step 2 onto MTS trust. MTS trust is positively related to MTS SMM was not supported

Page 98: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

80

for the dependent variable MTS Goal SMM; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Model

108) associated with MTS trust was not statistically significant (ßMTS Trust = .16, ns). MTS trust is

positively related to MTS SMM was not supported for the dependent variable MTS Task SMM;

the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Model 109) associated with MTS trust was not

statistically significant (ßMTS Trust = .10, ns). MTS trust is positively related to MTS SMM was

not supported for the dependent variable MTS Team SMM; the standardized beta (presented in

Step 2 of Model 110) associated with MTS trust was not statistically significant (ßMTS Trust = -.10,

ns).

MTSSMM is positively related to MTS performance (efficiency and effectiveness).Table

35, Models 111-116, summarize all analyses used to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Models

111-113, MTSperformance was regressed onto the control variables and in Step 2 onto

MTSSMM. The MTS SMM is positively related to MTS performance relationship was not

supported for the dependent variable MTS efficiency; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2

of Models 111-113) associated with MTS efficacy was not statistically significant for Model 111

(ßMTSGoal SMM= .11, ns), for Model 112 (ßMTS Task SMM = .13, ns), and for Model 113 (ßMTS Team

SMM= -.14, ns).The MTS SMM is positively related to MTS performancerelationship was

supported for the dependent variable MTS effectiveness; the standardized beta (presented in Step

2 of Models 114-116) associated with MTS SMM was statistically significant for one model:

Model 114 (ßMTS Goal SMM= .03, ns), for Model 115 (ßMTS Task SMM = .25, p< .05), and for Model

116 (ßMTS Team SMM= -.16, ns).

MTS SMM is positively related to MTS performance (efficiency and effectiveness)

through (i.e., mediated by) MTS IS openness was tested employing bootstrapping software

Page 99: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

81

following the Preacher and Hayes (2004) methodology to test for indirect effects. Table 36,

Models 117-122, summarizes all analyses used to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 117-

119, MTS performance was regressed onto the control variables and MTS SMM and in Step 2

onto MTS IS openness. This relationship was supported as depicted in Model 121.

The total effect of MTS Goal SMM on MTS efficiency for Model 117 was 1.39, ns

indicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was .57, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS IS openness)

was .95, nswith a 95% BCa CI of .12 to 2.48 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Although the

confidence interval did not include zero, the coefficient was not statistically significant and it can

be concluded that the relationship between MTS Goal SMM and MTS performance was not

mediated by MTS IS openness.

The total effect of MTS Task SMM on MTS efficiency for Model 118 was 1.63, ns

indicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was .22, nsand the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS IS openness)

was 1.34, p< .10 with a 95% BCa CI of .23 to 3.15 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Although the

confidence interval did not include zero, the coefficient was not statistically significant and it can

be concluded that the relationship between MTS Task SMM and MTS performance was not

mediated by MTS IS openness.

The total effect of MTS Team SMM on MTS efficiency for Model 119 was -1.72, ns

indicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -1.23, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS IS

openness) was -.34, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -2.00 to .81 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because

Page 100: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

82

the confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS

Team SMM and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS IS openness.

The total effect of MTS Goal SMM on MTS effectiveness for Model 120 was .08, ns

indicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -.19, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS IS openness)

was .32, p< .10 with a 95% BCa CI of .10 to .67 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Although the

confidence interval did not include zero, the coefficient was not statistically significant and it can

be concluded that the relationship between MTS Goal SMM and MTS performance was not

mediated by MTS IS openness.

The total effect of MTS Task SMM on MTS effectiveness for Model 121 was.75, p < .05

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was .36, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS IS openness) was

.36, p< .05 with a 95% BCa CI of .13 to .73 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence

interval did not include zero, can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Task SMM

and MTS performance was mediated by MTS IS openness.

The total effect of MTS Team SMM on MTS effectiveness for Model 122 was -.47,

nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -.37, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator (i.e., MTS IS openness)

was -.06, ns with a 95% BCa CI of -.35 to .16 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the

confidence interval did include zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS

Team SMM and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS IS openness.

Media Retrievability Moderator

Page 101: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

83

Media retrievability moderatesthe relationship between MTS TMS and MTS IS

uniqueness was tested using hierarchical regression. Table 37, Models 123-128, summarize all

analyses used to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Model 123-128, MTS IS uniqueness was

regressed onto the control variables, the centered MTS TMS and centered media retrievability

and in Step 2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTS TMS by centered media

retrievability). Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS IS

uniquenessrelationship was supported for MTS IS uniqueness between teams; the standardized

beta (presented in Models 123-125) associated with the interaction term in Step 2 yielded a

significant beta weights for Model 123 (ß MTS TMS X Media Retrievability Social = -.26, p < .05), for Model

124 (ß MTS TMS X Media Retrievability Task = -.14, ns), and for Model 125 (ß MTS TMS X Media Retrievability

Aggregate = -.15, ns). Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS

IS uniqueness relationship was not supported for MTS IS uniqueness within team; the

standardized beta (presented in Models 126-128) associated with the interaction term in Step 2

yielded no significant beta weights for Model 126 (ß MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Social

= -.18, ns), for Model 127 (ß MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Task = -.17, ns), and Model

128 (ß MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team X Media Retrievability Aggregate) = -.17, ns).

Figure 12 depicts the results of Model 123 and the MTS TMS and MTS performance

relationship moderated by communication retrievability social specific. Although the interaction

term in Model 123 suggest an interaction between MTS TMS and communication retrievability,

the plot of the interaction does not portray any clear interaction between the two variables in

predicting MTS IS unique between teams.

Page 102: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

84

Media Richness Moderator

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTSSMM and MTS IS openness was

tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 38, Models 129-131, depict all analyses to test

this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 129-131, MTS IS openness was regressed onto the control

variables, the centered MTSSMM and the centered media richness and in Step 2 onto the

multiplicative interaction term (centered multiteam SMM by centered media richness). Media

richness moderates the relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS openness relationship was

not supported; the standardized beta (presented in Models 129-131) associated with the

interaction term in Step 2 for all analyses was not significant: for Model 129 (ß MTS Goal SMM X Media

Richness = -.26, p < .10), for Model 130 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media Richness = -.10, ns), and for Model 131

(ß MTS Team SMM X Media Richness = .06, ns).

Follow-Up Exploratory Analyses Model 3

As noted earlier, both the Theoretical and Exploratory Models were expanded once

conducting lagged correlations to look at the effects of cognition on behavior. As noted in

Figure 5, take into consideration the effect that motivational/affective states have on predicting

cognitive emergent states, the effect of cognitive emergent states predicting behavioral

processes, and the effect of behavioral processes predicting performance are tested.

Motivationally-Driven Development

Page 103: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

85

MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS SMM was tested employing hierarchical

regression. Table 39, Models 132-146, summarize all analysesused to test this relationship. In

Step 1 MTS SMM was regressed onto the control variables and in Step 2 onto MTS efficacy.

MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS SMM relationship was supported for the dependent

variable for MTS Goal SMM; a number of standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Models 132-

136) associated with MTS efficacy were significant: for Model 132 (ßMTS Task Efficacy= .20, p <

.10), for Model 133 (ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (team) = .32, p < .05), for Model 134 (ß MTS Taskforce Efficacy

(MTS) = .24, p < .05) for Model 135 (ßMTS Bandura Percentage= .16, ns), and for Model 136 (ßMTS Bandura

Yes/No = .17, ns). MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS SMM relationship was supported for

the dependent variable for MTS Task SMM; a number of standardized beta (presented in Step 2

of Models 132-136) associated with MTS efficacy for Model 137 (ßMTS Task Efficacy= .11, ns), for

Model 138 (ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (team) = -.05, ns), for Model 139 (ß MTS Taskforce Efficacy (MTS) = -.06, ns)

for Model 140 (ßMTS Bandura Percentage= -.02, ns), and for Model 141 (ßMTS Bandura Yes/No = .03, ns).

Lastly, MTS efficacy is positively related to MTS SMM relationship was not supported for the

dependent variable for MTS Team SMM; a number of standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of

Models 132-136) associated with MTS efficacy for Model 142 (ßMTS Task Efficacy= -.09, ns), for

Model 143 (ßMTS Taskforce Efficacy (team) = -.07, ns), for Model 144 (ß MTS Taskforce Efficacy (MTS) = -.06, ns)

for Model 145 (ßMTS Bandura Percentage= .04, ns), and for Model 146 (ßMTS Bandura Yes/No = .21, ns).

Affectively-Driven Development

MTS trust is positively related to MTS TMSwas tested employing hierarchical

regression. Table 40, Model 147, summarizes the analyses used to test this relationship. To test

Page 104: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

86

the effects of MTSTMS, in Step 1 of Model 147, MTSTMS was regressed onto the control

variables and in Step 2 onto MTStrust. The MTS trust is positively related to MTS TMS

relationship was supported; the standardized beta (presented in Step 2 of Model 147) associated

with MTS trust was statistically significant (ßMTS Trust = .37, p< .01).

MTS TMS is positively related to MTS performance (efficiency and performance)

through MTS IS openness was tested employing bootstrapping software following the Preacher

and Hayes (2004) methodology to test for indirect effects. Table 41, Models 148 and 149,

summarize all analyses to test this relationship. This relationship was supported as depicted in

Model 148 and 149.

MTS TMS is positively related to MTS performance through MTS IS openness was

supported for the dependent variable MTS efficiency. As depicted in Model 148 the total effect

of MTS TMS on MTS efficiency was 2.45, ns indicating that the independent variable was not

related to the dependent variable. The direct effect was .45, ns and the indirect effect through the

proposed mediator was 2.11, p< .05 with a 95% BCa CI of .36 to 3.87 (1,000 bootstrap

resamples). Because the confidence interval did not include zero, it can be concluded that the

relationship between MTS TMS and MTS performance was mediated by MTS IS openness.

MTS TMS is positively related to MTS performance through MTS IS openness was

supported for the dependent variable MTS effectiveness. As depicted in Model 149 the total

effect of MTS TMS on MTS effectiveness was .83, p< .01 indicating that the independent

variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect was .40, ns and the indirect

effect through the proposed mediator was .46, p< .05 with a 95% BCa CI of .12 to .85 (1,000

bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did not include zero, it can be concluded

Page 105: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

87

that the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS performance was mediated by MTS IS

openness.

MTS SMM is positively related to MTS performance (efficiency and effectiveness)

through MTS IS uniqueness was tested employing bootstrapping software following the Preacher

and Hayes (2004) methodology to test for indirect effects. Table 42, Models 150-161, summarize

all analyses to test this relationship. This relationship was supported as depicted in Model 151.

For Model 150 the total effect of MTS Goal SMM on MTS efficiency was 1.39,

nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was .92, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .81, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.20 to 2.43 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Goal SMM and MTS performance

was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness between teams.

For Model 151 the total effect of MTS Goal SMM on MTS efficiency was 1.63, ns

indicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -.01, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was 1.65, p< .05 with a

95% BCa CI of .48 to 3.29 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Although the confidence interval did not

include zero, the fact that both Paths A and B were not statistically significant put into question

whether it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Goal SMM and MTS

performance was mediated by MTS IS uniqueness within team.

For Model 152 the total effect of MTS Task SMM on MTS efficiency was -1.72,

nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -1.87, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .33, ns with a 95%

Page 106: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

88

BCa CI of -1.20 to 2.16 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Task SMM and MTS performance

was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness between teams.

For Model 153 the total effect of MTS Task SMM on MTS efficiency was 1.39, ns

indicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was 1.31, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .11, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.18 to 1.16 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Task SMM and MTS performance

was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness within team.

For Model 154 the total effect of MTS Team SMM on MTS efficiency was 1.63,

nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was 1.10, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .52, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.40 to 1.72 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Team SMM and MTS performance

was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness between teams.

For Model 155 the total effect of MTS Team SMM on MTS efficiency was -1.72,

nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -1.89, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .16, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.57 to 1.26 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Team SMM and MTS performance

was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness within team.

For Model 156 the total effect of MTS Goal SMM on MTS effectiveness was .08,

Page 107: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

89

nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -.03, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .18, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.06 to .58 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Goal SMM and MTS performance

was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness between teams.

For Model 157 the total effect of MTS Goal SMM on MTS effectiveness was .75, p< .05

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was .39, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .36, nswith a 95% BCa CI

of .10 to .84 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Although the confidence interval did not include zero,

the coefficient was not statistically significant and it can be concluded that the relationship

between MTS Goal SMM and MTS performance was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness

within team.

For Model 158 the total effect of MTS Task SMM on MTS effectiveness was -.47,

nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -.49, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .06, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.24 to .42 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Task SMM and MTS performance

was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness between teams.

For Model 159 the total effect of MTS Task SMM on MTS effectiveness was .08, ns

indicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was .04, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .05, ns a 95% BCa

CI of -.05 to .25 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include zero, it

Page 108: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

90

can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Task SMM and MTS performance was not

mediated by MTS IS uniqueness within team.

For Model 160 the total effect of MTS Team SMM on MTS effectiveness was .75, p< .05

indicating that the independent variable was related to the dependent variable. The direct effect

was .60, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .15, ns with a 95% BCa CI

of -.03 to .55 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include zero, it

can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Team SMM and MTS performance was not

mediated by MTS IS uniqueness between teams.

For Model 161 the total effect of MTS Team SMM on MTS effectiveness was -.47,

nsindicating that the independent variable was not related to the dependent variable. The direct

effect was -.51, ns and the indirect effect through the proposed mediator was .04, ns with a 95%

BCa CI of -.13 to .34 (1,000 bootstrap resamples). Because the confidence interval did include

zero, it can be concluded that the relationship between MTS Team SMM and MTS performance

was not mediated by MTS IS uniqueness within team.

Media Retrievability Moderator

Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS IS

opennesswas tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 43, Models 162-164, summarize

all relationships analyzed to test this hypothesis. In Step 1 of Models 162-164, MTS IS openness

was regressed onto the control variables, the centered MTSTMS, and centered media

retrievability and in Step 2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTS TMS by the

centered media retrievability).Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS

Page 109: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

91

TMS and MTS IS openness was not supported; the standardized beta (presented in Models 162-

164) associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were not statistically significant for Model

162 (ß MTS TMS X Media Retrievability Social = .06, ns), for Model 163 (ß MTS TMS X Media Retrievability Task = .01,

ns), and for Model 164 (ß MTS TMS X Media Retrievability Aggregate = .02, ns).

Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS

uniqueness was tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 44, Models 165-182, summarize

all analyses used to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 165-182, MTS IS uniqueness was

regressed onto the control variables, the centered MTStask SMM, and centered media

retrievability social and in Step 2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTS

ISuniqueness by the centered media retrievability social).Media retrievability moderates the

relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS uniquenessrelationship was supported for the

dependent variable MTS IS between teams; onestandardized beta (presented in Models 165-173)

associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were statistically significant for Model 165 (ß MTS

Goal SMM X Media Retrievability Social = -.11, ns), for Model 166 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media Retrievability Social = -.20,

ns), for Model 167 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Social = -.01, ns), for Model 168 (ß MTS Goal SMM X

Media Retrievability Task = -.03, ns), for Model 169 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media Retrievability Task = -.23, p< .05), for

Model 170 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Task = .02, ns),for Model 171 (ß MTS Goal SMM X Media

Retrievability Aggregate = -.09, ns), for Model 172 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media Retrievability Aggregate = -.23, p< .10),

and for Model 173 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Aggregate = -.01, ns). Media retrievability

moderates the relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS uniquenessrelationship was

supported for the dependent variable MTS IS within team; a number of standardized beta

(presented in Models 174-182) associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were statistically

Page 110: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

92

significant for Model 174 (ß MTS Goal SMM X Media Retrievability Social = -.01, ns), for Model 175 (ß MTS Task

SMM X Media Retrievability Social = -.20, ns), for Model 176 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Social = .32, p <

.10), for Model 177 (ß MTS Goal SMM X Media Retrievability Task = -.14 ns), for Model 178 (ß MTS Task SMM X

Media Retrievability Task = -.30, p< .05), for Model 179 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Task = .07, ns),for

Model 180 (ß MTS Goal SMM X Media Retrievability Aggregate = -.21, ns), for Model 181 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media

Retrievability Aggregate = -.30, p< .05), and for Model 182 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Aggregate = .06,

ns).

Figures 13-15 depict the moderated relationships for Models 169, 178, and 181. Figure

13 depicts the results of Model 169 which suggests that if members of a MTS possess a highly

similar task shared mental model, they are more likely to engage in greater unique information

exchanged between teams if they limit the time they use media retrievability tools to share task

relevant information. In other words it appears that members use these tools more effectively

because they not only limit the amount of time using them, but also share unique information via

this media. In the case of MTSs where members do not share similar task mental models,

component teams of a MTS are more likely to benefit from using media rich tools to share task

relevant information because they will engage in more between team unique information

exchange.

Figure 14 depicts the results of Model 178 and the relationship between MTS task SMM

and MTS IS uniqueness within team moderated by the use of media retrievability tools to

exchange task specific information. Results of the graph suggest that regardless of level of MTS

task SMM; component teams share a moderate level of unique information with members of

their own team when highly relying on media retrievability tools to exchange task relevant

Page 111: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

93

information. Results also suggest that if MTS component team members limit their use of media

retrievability tools to exchange task relevant information and they do not have similar mental

models they share a moderate amount of unique information with members of their own teams;

whereas if members share a similar mental model then they are more likely to share more unique

information with members of their own teams. In sum these findings note the importance of

mental models and how they can impact the reliance of media retrievability tools to share

information with members of their own teams.

Figure 15 depicts the results of model 181 and the relationship between MTS task SMM

and MTS IS uniqueness within team moderated by the time MTS employee the use of media

retrievability tools. Results of the graph depict similar patterns as those described above for

Figure 14. If members of a MTS do share similar mental models, then the limited time they

spend communicating with MTS members via media retrievability tools results in greater unique

IS with members of their own team. In other words, one can potentially conclude that members

are efficiently communicating unique information to members of their own component team

when they posses similar mental models. When MTS members do not share a similar task SMM,

the limited use of media retrievability tools reduces the amount of unique information exchanged

with members of their own team, whereas the greater use of media retrievability tools

encourages unique IS with members of their own team.

Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS

opennesswas tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 45, Models 183-191, summarize all

analyses to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 183-191, MTS IS openness was regressed

onto the control variables, the centered MTS SMM, and centered media retrievability and in Step

Page 112: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

94

2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTS SMM by centered media retrievability).

Media retrievability moderates the relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS

opennessrelationship was not supported; the standardized beta (presented in Models 183-191)

associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were not statistically significant: for Model 183 (ß

MTS Goal SMM X Media Retrievability Social = .07, ns), for Model 184 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media Retrievability Social = -

.12, ns), for Model 185 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Social = -.08, ns), for Model 186 (ß MTS Goal

SMM X Media Retrievability Task = .22, p< .10), for Model 187 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media Retrievability Task = -.06,

ns), for Model 188 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Task = -.07, ns),for Model 189 (ß MTS Goal SMM X

Media Retrievability Aggregate = .23, ns), for Model 190 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media Retrievability Aggregate = -.04, ns),

and for Model 191 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Retrievability Aggregate = -.08, ns).

Media Richness Moderator

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS

uniquenesswas tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 46, Models 192-197, summarize

all analyses used to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 192-197, MTS IS uniqueness was

regressed onto the control variables, the centered MTSSMM, and centered mediarichness and in

Step 2 the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTSSMM by centered

mediarichness).Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS

uniquenessrelationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS IS between teams;

standardized beta (presented in Models 192-194) associated with the interaction term in Step 2

were not statistically significant for Model 192 (ß MTS Goal SMM X Media Richness = -.10, ns), for Model

193 (ß MTS Task SMM X Media Richness = -.09, ns), and for Model 194 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Richness = .12,

Page 113: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

95

ns). Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS SMM and MTS IS uniqueness

relationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS IS within team; standardized beta

(presented in Models 195-197) associated with the interaction term in Step 2 were not

statistically significant for Model 195 (ß MTS Goal SMM X Media Richness = .13, ns), for Model 196 (ß MTS

Task SMM X Media Richness = .03, ns), and for Model 197 (ß MTS Team SMM X Media Richness = -.07, ns).

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS IS openness

analyses to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Model 198, MTS IS openness was regressed onto

the control variables, the centered MTSTMS, and centered mediarichness and in Step 2 onto the

multiplicative interaction term (centered MTSTMS by centered mediarichness). Media richness

moderates the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS IS openness relationship was not

supported; standardized beta (presented in Model 198) associated with the interaction term in

Step 2 was not statistically significant for Model 198 (ß MTS TMS X Media Richness = -.07, ns).

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS IS uniqueness

was tested employing hierarchical regression. Table 48, Models 199 and 200, summarize the

analyses used to test this relationship. In Step 1 of Models 299 and 300, MTS IS uniqueness was

regressed onto the control variables, the centered MTSTMS, and centered mediarichness and in

Step 2 onto the multiplicative interaction term (centered MTSTMS by centered mediarichness).

Media richness moderates the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS IS uniqueness

relationship was not supported for the dependent variable MTS IS uniqueness between teams;

standardized beta (presented in Model 199) associated with the interaction term in Step 2 was not

statistically significant for Model 199 (ß MTS TMS X Media Richness = -.11, ns). Media richness

moderates the relationship between MTS TMS and MTS IS uniqueness relationship was not

Page 114: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

96

supported for the dependent variable MTS IS uniqueness within team; standardized beta

(presented in Model 199) associated with the interaction term in Step 2 was not statistically

significant for Model 200 (ß MTS TMS X Media Richness = -.09, ns).

Table 1 provides a summary of all dissertation hypotheses and exploratory relationships

assessed. Additionally, Table 1 outlines all tables and models associated with each hypothesis

and exploratory relationship tested. Lastly Table 1 includes a brief summary of the findings and

provides additional information on figures associated with each tested relationship.In addition to

the synthesized information provided in Table 1, Figures 18, 19, 20, and 21 provide a graphical

representation of all the relationships supported from the Theoretical and Follow-Up Exploratory

Analyses Models 1, 2, and 3.

Page 115: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

97

CHAPTER FOUR: DISCUSSION

MTS are a growing norm in our lives (DeChurch & Zaccaro, 2010; DeChurch &

Mathieu, 2009; Zaccaro, Marks, & DeChurch, 2012). The goal of this dissertation was to shed

light on enabling the effectiveness of MTS. This dissertation makes three contributions to

knowledge in this area: 1)Two emergent states known to be important to teams – motivation and

affect- shape the dynamics of MTS (such as the development of behavioral processes and

cognitive emergent states); 2)Cognition–having a shared understanding of both where knowledge

is held and how to tackle a task–is a driver of MTS performance and the exchange of information

both within and across teams of a MTS, and 3) virtual communication– going beyond the

traditional way research in this area is conceptualized, comparing virtual teams and face-to-face

teams –distinct aspects of virtual tools (such as the ability to retrieve information at a later time)

influence the type of information being shared both within and across teams of a MTS.In the

following paragraphs each of these points will be discussed in further detail.

Motivational and Affective Emergent States Shaping the Dynamics in Multiteam Systems

The literature targeting the development of MTS processes has shown that MTS

transition processes (Marks et al., 2005) as well as leader training emphasizing coordination can

prompt both implicit and explicit coordination between teams (DeChurch & Marks, 2006).

Additionally work by Davison, Hollenbeck, Barnes, Sleesman, and Ilgen, (2011)suggests that

coordinated action across component teams by team boundary spanners and system level

Page 116: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

98

leadership positively influences MTS performance. The existing literature provides team level

inputs that promote MTS level behavioral processes, but our understanding of how emotional or

affective responses impact behavioral processes is limited. This dissertation opens avenues

focusing on motivational and affective emergent states (e.g., efficacy and trust)that are heavily

cited in teams research as proponents of team processes. It is concluded that both MTS level

efficacy and MTS level trust are catalyst of both MTS IS and MTS shared cognition.

Specifically, MTSefficacy was shown to predict MTSIS openness and both MTSTMS

(transactive memory) and SMM (shared mental models). Similarly, MTStrust was found to

predict MTSIS openness andMTSTMS.

The findings that both MTS efficacy and MTS trust promote MTS IS provides an answer

to a question posited by Mesmer-Magnus and colleagues (2011) in their meta-analytic work on

IS in virtual teams. The authors concluded that both IS uniqueness and openness were predictors

of virtual team performance. The authors also noted that in virtual teams IS uniqueness was

more common even though IS openness was found to be a stronger predictor of virtual team

performance. The question that the authors left us with was; how can we promote open IS in

virtual teams? Although team and MTS dynamics are not the same, one step for increasing MTS

IS openness is though enhancing MTS efficacy and MTS trust. To facilitate the emergence of

these states, leaders can play a pivotal role. Leaders can relay information in a timely manner,

act as boundary spanners, make MTS members aware of others members past experience and

success with similar tasks, and create new knowledge both within and across teams of an MTS.

The findings of MTS efficacy and MTS trust promoting MTS cognition provide us

insight into the how emergent states can be critical drivers of how MTS members conceptualize

Page 117: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

99

and frame the problem space before them. Both emergent states explained significant variance

in MTS TMS, or the understanding of where knowledge and expertise is within a MTS. The

context of MTS puts at the forefront the importance of understanding TMS as a tool that

enhances MTS effectiveness. When working in complex and dynamic environments, while

simultaneously collaborating with a number of teams to complete a shared goal, understanding

where knowledge is found and to whom critical task relevant information should be relayed are

essential for the functioning of MTS. This dissertation provides support that in order to build

these knowledge systems, promoting MTS efficacy and MTS trust is one step in the right

direction. Continuing to build onto the shared cognition literature, the following section goes

into more details about the contribution that this dissertation provides for shared cognition in

multiteam systems

Shared Cognition in Multiteam Systems

Since the initial work conducted on team cognition (Wegner et al., 1985; Liang,

Moreland, & Argote, 1995; Cannon-Bowers & Salas, 1997) research has accumulated noting the

importance of this emergent state (Mohammed et al.,2010; DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2011;

Ren & Argote, 2011; Peltokorpi, 2008). Much of the team cognition literature has focused on

two distinct types of team cognition shared mental models (task, team, and equipment; Cannon-

Bowers & Salas, 1997) and transactive memory (Wegner et al., 1985; Ren & Argote, 2011;

Lewis, 2003; Austin, 2003). The research thus far has shown the importance of these cognitive

emergent states in promoting team effectiveness (Zhang et al., 2007; Lewis, 2003; Austin,

2003).Recent reviews of the SMM and TMS literatures havenoteda thorough summary of the

Page 118: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

100

antecedents that promote such emergent state. For instance, Mohammed et al.’s (2010) review

on SMM suggests that team composition inputs (e.g., demographics, cognitive ability,

tenure/experience, organizational level similarity and educational level similarity, etc.), team

level inputs (e.g., planning, reflexivity, leadership, training, etc.), and organizational contextual

factors (e.g., stress, workload, novel versus routine environments, etc.)have been revealed as

antecedents promoting shared mental models. Similarly work by Ren and Argote (2011) has

noted that a number of team composition inputs (e.g., member demographics, member technical

competence, etc.), team level inputs (e.g., task interdependence, goal interdependence, group

training, shared experience, communication, technology, etc.), and organizational contextual

inputs (e.g., stress and geographic distributions, etc.) predict transactive memory systems in the

past.

As noted above, among all the distinct antecedents that promote the development of team

SMM and TMS, our understanding of how motivational and affective emergent states shape the

advancement of team and MTS cognitive structures has been a critical missing piece from our

shared cognition literature. This dissertation shed light on the importance of both collective or

MTS efficacy and MTS trust as key drivers that promote the development of both SMM and

TMS. Additionally, I went beyond emergent states as predictors of cognition and studied the

effect of behavioral processes, such as IS unique and open, as drivers of emergent cognition.

Initial hypotheses sought to uncover how distinct types of IS (open and unique) shape

emergent cognition (TMS and SMM). Results suggest that the relationship was not as originally

hypothesized. Alternatively, lagged correlation analyses presented suggests that shared

cognition impactsIS within and between teams of the MTS. SMM research has shown that SMM

Page 119: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

101

is positively related to the behavioral processes such as coordination (Marks et al., 2002) and

communication (Marks et al., 2000), but when we focus on the complex nature of MTS

communication and IS, the relationships transcend into a more complicated relationship. In

MTSmembers are not only communicating with their own teammates, but they also engage in

communication across team boundaries. This dissertation provided a stepping-stone for

understanding how shared schema of the task influencesIS in the context of MTS. More

specifically, the findings presented here provided insight into how cognition influences the

sharing of MTS IS openness and MTS IS uniqueness (within and across teams). Results showed

that task SMM were predictive of the IS uniqueness both within and across teams depending on

the virtual tool employed. In the subsequent section more details will be provided about the

influence of virtual toolsand how the findings presented in this dissertation expand the virtual

MTS literature.

Virtuality in Multiteam Systems

Over the years our organizations have experienced a surge in the use of alternative forms

of virtual communication in order to fulfill daily work-related responsibilities. Virtual

communication tools can be categorized as high or low onthree virtuality dimensions proposed

by Kirkman and Mathieu (2005). This dissertation sought to provide insight into the

effectsdistinct dimensions of the Kirkman and Mathieu taxonomy, media richness and an

adapted version of informational value (for instance, the ability to go back and review materials

that were discussed during a meeting) had on MTS dynamics. More specifically, this

dissertation focused on the impact that these dimensions had on the development of shared

Page 120: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

102

cognition in teams and IS both within and across teams of a MTS. The overall pattern of results

suggest that depending on the level of shared schema held by members of a MTS about the task

influenced the level of IS both within and across teams of a MTSwhen media retrievability tools

were employed to share task relevant information.

To be more specific, the results discussed earlier in this dissertation suggest that

MTSmembers that posses similar MTS SMMare more apt to share more unique IS with members

of their own component team as well as members of other component teams of the MTS.

Focusing first on the impact of media retrievability tools on the shared cognition and unique

information exchanged with members of other component teams. The relationship was stronger

when members of the MTS limited their use of media retrievability tools to exchange task

relevant information suggesting that MTS members may be effectively and efficiently using

these forms of tools for communication. In the event that MTS members do not share a similar

task SMM, their limited use of media retrievability tools to share task relevant information hurt

the quantity to unique IS exchanged with members of other teams of the MTS. Whereas, if the

MTS did not share a similar task SMM but did employee media retrievability tools more often to

exchange task relevant information, they were more likely to exchange unique IS with members

of other component teams.

Shifting our focus to the effects of media retrievability tools on the shared cognition and

unique information exchange with members of one’s team. The results suggest that similar to

the findings for sharing information with members of other component teams, MTS members

having a similar SMM results in more information exchanged with members of one’s own teams

if the amount of time employing media retrievability tools to exchange task relevant information

Page 121: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

103

is limited. In MTSs where members do not share similar mental models, the amount of unique IS

exchanged with members of one’s team is reduced.

Therefore,the relationship between MTS SMM and the unique ISexchanged, both within

and between teams, is impacted by the level and the type of information conveyed via

information retrievability tools. Based on these results expanding our understanding of how to

foster shared cognition within MTS and how it may influence the use of virtuality tools is a

critical next step for virtual MTS literature. Prior to voicing potential future research ideas for the

advancement of MTS theory, it is critical to acknowledge some of the shortcomings of this study

and recognize how they can be overcome.

Limitations and Potential Solutions

In every study there is room for improvement. In the following section a number of areas

will be discussed that could improve this line of research and potentially pave the way for the

advancement in the virtual MTS literature. A number of concerns with the current study will be

addressed: distinction between teams and MTS, measurement issues, and generalizability.

Differences Between Teams and MTSs

Our ability to fully understand the interplay between emergent processes and behavioral

states is the size of the component teams selected for this study. Much debate has evolved

around the literature on team size. Work has shown that as team size increases available

resources increase as well (Stewart, 2006). However as team size increases other aspects of

teams, such as coordination and other processes may be negatively impacted (Hackman, 2002).

Although the debate continues on the ideal team size, a starting point is necessary to initiate our

Page 122: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

104

understanding of how human processes evolve. For the purpose of this study, the Salas,

Dickinson, Converse, and Tannenbaum (1992) definition of teams was adopted to define the

component teams of a MTS, “a distinguishable set of two or more people who interact,

dynamically, interdependently, and adaptively toward a common and valued

goal/objective/mission, who have been assigned specific roles or functions to perform, and who

have a limited life-span of membership (p. 4). Noting this as one of the critical shortcomings of

this research, future research is highly encouraged to target this limitation and expand beyond a

two-person component team to understand how the dynamics evolve at the MTS level. More

recent work on MTS has already noted this limitation and as such have ventured to target this

concern (Davison et al., 2011).

A more critical question is whether MTS are truly a distinct entity from teams. In other

words are they more than simply a large team? To answer this question, first I draw from a

theoretical standpoint. I reference the goal structure of teams and MTS to have a better

understanding of how a team and MTS are distinct. In a team time devoted to a single task

contributes to the overall team goal. Whether team members are pooling their resources, are

sequentially contributing to a task, or are interdependently exchange task responsibilities, in the

end; each member’s contributions in any of the interdependences described all lead to the same

team goal. The MTS literature on the other hand has posited that a key driver of what

distinguished MTS from teams is the goal hierarchy that orchestrates and guides actions at

distinct levels (i.e., team and system level). For instance, in a MTS, component teams share at

least one similar distal goal, but simultaneously contribute towards a unique proximal team level

goal that is unique to each team. Therefore MTS component teams are faced with a dilemma of

Page 123: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

105

sorts. The time and resources that a component team allocates to the MTS/team goal are the time

and resources that cannot be devoted to the accomplishment of the team/MTS goal. The task

designed for this study was developed to mimic the MTS goal hierarchy parameters noted by

Mathieu et al. (2001) and DeChurch and Mathieu (2009).

Secondly, to show the distinction between teams and MTS, I reference the findings

presented in this dissertation. The findings presented in this dissertation may suggest that MTS

are similar to teams. For instance, the finding presented here are similar to findings mirrored in

the team literature over the last 20 years, trust (Polzer et al., 2006; Rico et al., 2009), efficacy

(Gully et al., 2002; Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006), IS (Mesmer-Magnus & DeChurch, 2009;

Mesmer-Magnus et al., 2011), and cognition (Ren & Argote, 2011; Mohammed et al., 2010;

DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2010) have all been found to be positive indicators of

performance. A further in depth look at the effect of IS suggests that a distinction is apparent in

how teams and MTS operate. For instance a recent meta-analytic review of the team IS literature

notes that IS uniqueness is most critical for the success of face-to-face teams (Mesmer-Magnus

& DeChurch, 2009). The findings presented in this dissertation show that IS openness is more

predictive of MTS performance. In other words, regarding IS, what defines team success (IS

unique) is distinct from what defines MTS success (IS open). Therefore, the breadth of

information exchanged among members of component teams results in greater performance.

A number of explanations can be considered for IS open to be more predictive of

performance, rather than IS unique in MTS. One potential explanation is that the means of

communication that were employed throughout this task included chats and video conversations.

Each means of communication can assist MTS members in understanding more about the task

Page 124: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

106

and the people through the distinct means. Not only can individuals with distinct personalities

(more gregarious versus shy) take advantage of employing the type of tool that is more in tune

with their personality and allow them to engage in greater IS, but it also provides a greater

amount of information being shared seeing as distinct communication mediums can be employed

simultaneously increasing the breadth of information exchanged.

Another potential explanation for this finding may be that with more IS open shared,

which can include a number of topics (e.g., such as social, task, backup behavior in nature, team

building, etc.), component teams of a MTS are actually developing emotional and affective

bonds through their written and verbal exchanges. Therefore, the open exchange of information

is acting as a social platform for MTS component team members to develop relationships with

other component team members.

Lastly the findings that IS open is a driver of MTS performance, provides us an important

view into how MTS operate. For the purpose of this dissertation IS unique was an objective

measure, whereas IS open was a subjective measure. The finding indicating that IS open is more

predictive of performance, can lead us to draw some conclusions about the culture of MTS. For

instance, individuals’ perceptions of the breadth of information shared among component teams

of the task at hand resulted in being a driver of performance. This finding posits important

implications for organizations, suggesting that perceptions of the communication culture,

specifically when the free flow of communication is encouraged (e.g., task, social, backup

behavior in nature, conflict resolution, etc.) can results in greater MTS performance. Therefore

our understanding of how individuals perceive the communication flow within their

organizations can provide insight into the behaviors they engage in and whether a MTS is likely

Page 125: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

107

to successfully accomplish its task.

Based on the above discussion it can be concluded that both theoretically and empirically,

we can draw a distinction between teams and MTS. Although there are similarities, out ability to

begin to understand MTS needs to be spurred from entities that share similar dynamics, teams.

Thanks to the advancement of the team literature over the past 30 plus years, we are able to

begin to uncover how MTS evolve over time and in distinct contexts. Our understanding of MTS

phenomena is not as simple as it sounds. Measurement of the constructs teams researchers have

posited to be critical for team success can provide to be difficult to target in MTS and is the

following limitation discussed.

Measurement Issues

A second limitation targeted in this discussion will be issues that arose via with the

measures selected. First issues will be discussed regarding the SMMmeasurement employed.

Over the years the measurement of SMM has received wide attention. A number of methods to

assess SMM have been reviewed and each has been noted to have its pros and cons (please see

Mohammed et al., 2010; Rentsch et al., 2009, DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2010; Resick et al.,

2010).The method put forth in this study has received mixed reviews. Among the positive

views, the pairwise comparison method provides for the ability to accurately illicit individuals’

perceptions as well as has been found to be more predictive of performance than other methods

employed (DeChurch & Mesmer-Magnus, 2010). Negative perceptions associated with this

methodology include: a)The degree of cognitive resources required to complete the measure, b)

The challenge it possess on participants to comprehend the instructions of what is being asked,

and c) The time it takes to complete these types of measures often fatigue participants. In order

Page 126: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

108

to counter these limitations the number of comparisons within teach SMM measure were reduced

and the SMM measures was the first set of measures completed during measure completion

sections of the study. Although these actions were taken to prevent fatigue and cognitive load, a

number of participants failed to fully complete all mental model measures. In many instances, as

analyses incorporating MTS SMM could not be interpreted with confidence due to the low

sample included in the analyses based on missing data. Targeting this problem in future research

is critical. Considering the ability to incorporate other means of mental model measurement,

such as card sort, ranking, rating, etc. may be a more effective and engaging way of capturing

taskforce SMM.

Along the same lines of measurement drawbacks, the scale employed to measure the trust

construct was not considered a reliable measure of trust when we take into consideration the

Cronbach alpha statistic of .58. This statistic is extremely low and it begs the question: was trust

truly targeting? Two potential explanations can be considered in light of this low Cronbach

alpha statistic. First, results may be due to the fact that the items were expected to measure trust,

but did not tap all aspects of the construct. Secondly, participants may not have understood what

was being asked of them with such measures. A solution to this issue would be to incorporate a

distinct measure of trust whose measures have a better reliability.

Going beyond the low Cronbach alpha level of the trust measure, it is important to

consider the level of analysis that questions were posited to participants. Note, this concern is

not exclusive to trust, but to all motivational and affective measures, such as efficacy, cohesion,

social identity, etc. Specific to this study, it could be that the trust measure targeted the trust

fostered towards one’s team, but the same feeling of willingness to be vulnerable to the acts of

Page 127: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

109

members of other component teams of the MTS may not be nurtured. If this is the case we need

to be cautious of findings, because we may not be measuring how much MTS level trust truly

exists. In other words we need to frame questions that provide us the ability to target the extent

to which one team’s perceptions of the other team’s trust capture’s MTS trust. Furthermore,

questions should be framed so that there is an explicit statement that the focus of interest is the

MTS level of trust and that in order to hone in on that level members need to take into

consideration both their own team level trust and how much they trust the other team. Therefore,

future MTS research should investigate how constructs measures should be framed to have a

better understanding of how questions should be administered to MTS members for the

advancement of MTS theory.

Design Issues

The third limitation that will be discussed has to do with the design of the current study.

As social scientist we tend to rely on experimentation in order to target desired construct and

study them in the laboratory where we can control for extraneous factors. As this study was a

correlational study, the constructs of interest evolved naturally, which in many instances is more

characteristic of the real world, but simultaneously limits our comprehensive power of such

phenomena. Additionally, with correlational studies it is often difficult to assign causality due to

the number of cross sectional nature of the data. In the hopes of ameliorating this issue, all study

variables were measured at distinct points in time, except for the MTS cognition variables MTS

IS openness variables. Although most constructs were measured at distinct points in time

throughout the lifecycle of the MTS, an experimental setting that manipulates the two variables

of interest (MTS efficacy and MTS trust) would be the ideal setting to determine their effects on

Page 128: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

110

subsequent MTS processes and emergent states. By designing experimentation around distinct

levels of MTStrust and MTSefficacy, we can truly dissect the effects of each on behavioral

processes and cognitive emergent states.

Along the same lines of the point noted above studying MTS under laboratory settings

allows us to exercise some control similar to experimentation. This study provided the

opportunity for all MTS to be compared across the same task, aiding a convoy equipped with

humanitarian aid supplies across a war-torn region. Although studies such as these often put in

question the generalizability of findings, our ability to study MTS in the “wild” introduces a

number of limitations that set us back in fully grasping how they evolve in their natural

environments. For instance when we consider MTS in the field it is very difficult to draw

conclusions across MTS that vary in number of component teams and team size; that are exposed

to distinct situations, environmental constraints, varying degrees of training, that have long vs.

short tenure of component teams working together, etc. Although not limited to the

aforementioned, these limitations provide a brief overview of how any two MTS can be

substantially different. These distinctions put into question what are the true drivers of

developing mechanisms and performance. As such, studying MTS in the laboratory is a

solution to the overwhelming natural limitations caused by field samples. In order to

compensate for these limitations, which are important to understanding how MTS function, these

real world situational events are incorporated in laboratory simulations and studies. In the

attempt at designing such simulations and studies to resemble real world scenarios, our ability to

draw conclusions about real world MTS is enhanced.

In order to represent the real world situational factors in this study the task selected was

Page 129: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

111

one that represented a military action MTS. This task was designed in order to assert that the

properties of a MTS were intact. By properties I include the distinct aspects of the Mathieu et al.

(2001) definition: a) MTS are comprised of two or more discernable teams, b) the component

teams have input, process, and/or outcome interdependence, and c) there is a shared set of distal

goals across teams and unique proximal goals within teams. The task designed for this study

provided for each individual member of the taskforce across teams to have responsibilities that

only he/she could accomplish. Next, at the team level the goal for each team was unique and the

subsequent team could not assist in the successful attainment of the other team’s goal. Lastly,

both teams’ goals contributed to a unique MTS level goal.

Along similar lines of targeting real world MTS, another limitation of this study is the

extent to which it provides a realistic tenure for MTS members to fully evolve as they would in a

real world MTS. For instance, although the study lasted 240 minuets (4 hours), MTS members

had only 100 minutes during which they could interact as a MTS. The commonly arising

argument from this time limitation is whether 100 minutes is sufficient time for trust, efficacy,

and cognitive states to thoroughly flourish as they would in natural settings or in MTS with

longer time spans. In an effort to tackle this concern the study was designed with two

performance episodes, one allowing participants to become acquainted with the task and

software and a second where performance was measured. Additionally, all MTS were promoted

to undergo planning (where goal development, task management, and task strategizing take

place) and action phases (where implementing goals, coordinating action, providing backup

behavior take place) to stimulate realistic episodes that take place in MTS. These distinct

episodes provided MTS members the ability to review their performance during the action

Page 130: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

112

phases of the task and make adjustments where they thought they were necessary. The

aforementioned concernsbring into question theecological validity of the current findings.

The ecological validity of these findings is a true concern of this study where two dyadic

teams were selected to represent a MTS. As noted by Marks et al. 2005, smaller MTS provide a

situation where greater demands are placed on members. In larger MTS, distinct processes or

linking mechanisms may be more apparent and critical for the function of MTS. For instance, an

MTS comprised of two teams with two-person teams will have fewer networks of

communication channels that can be accessed throughout a task than a MTS comprised of 5

teams with 6-person teams. Here the possibility for open communication channels are

extensively augmented and can provide for distinct patterns of communication to arise.

Although it is clear and I do not intend to argue that the patterns of communication will be

distinct, the finding that the type of communication, for instance IS open will be more predictive

of MTS performance will likely translate to larger MTS. The reason for this argument is that IS

open, in other words the breadth of information shared, suggests that there may be a social aspect

intertwined with the breadth of information being shared via virtual tools that is currently not

being targeted. When we think of breadth of IS, distinct types of information can be targeted:

social chitchat, providing backup behavior, resolving arguments, motivational encouragement,

etc. All these distinct forms of information being exchanged provide members of component

teams to initiate the process of building bonds or relationships with other component team

members. Therefore testing similar propositions in larger MTS, where component teams may

be comprised of more than 2 individuals and the MTS are comprised of more than 2 teams we

are likely to see similar findings where breadth of information is more critical for performance.

Page 131: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

113

With greater number of individuals some form of communication across component teams to

initiate conversation and communication between members and potentially provide a baseline for

getting to know each other’s strengths and weaknesses can be captured via breadth of

information. Therefore, as noted by Marks et al. (2205) “the influence of size, design, and

operations of different types of MTS are ripe areas for future investigation” (p. 970). More

importantly, understanding what is incorporated in IS open, as noted in earlier discussions of this

dissertation, targeting distinct processes via communication tools may be a fruitful avenue for

future research. Furthermore, as noted by Moreland (2010) although the dyads and groups

undergo a number of distinct influential situations that impact the development of phenomena,

dyad research should be carefully interpreted when generalizing to group/team research.

Moreland also noted that many of the phenomena that occur in groups also occur in dyads. More

importantly although drawing cautionary attention to dyad and group research, Moreland did

articulate “dyad research can help us understand group research” (p. 261), therefore an initial

look at the development of MTS phenomena in small MTS comprised of dyad teams should be

viewed as a stepping stone that will allow us to begin to understand how phenomena evolve in

large scale MTS.

Another limitation that feeds into the ecological validity is the makeup of MTS and how

they interact. For instance, we should consider the linking mechanisms (i.e., “mechanism that

connect component teams,” p.18) of MTS and howthey impact the interplay of component teams

(Zaccaro et al., 2012). Let us take a step back and consider the current MTS. The component

teams were essentially homogeneous in that all members were from the same age group, same

educational background (psychology majors), but also the teams identities were both military in

Page 132: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

114

nature with similar skills at their disposal. Zaccaro et al. (2012) noted that distinct linkage

attributes, such as power distinct groups within an MTS hold, impact the dynamics within a

system. Within the MTS created for this study power was equally distributed to each

component team. For instance both teams were equal in size and within the MTS they both

possessed the same hierarchical level. Thus the constancy provided across the MTS in this study

may have impacted the development of some linking mechanisms to develop in distinct ways

than would likely be the case in MTS described by Mathieu et al., (2001). Mathieu and

colleagues described an emergency response MTS which included two county governmental

component teams and two hospital component teams (Zaccarro et al.). In this example the power

distributed to the component teams would differ based on the size of each and the position each

held within their organization (Zaccaro et al.). Therefore, our ability as researchers to advance

the science of MTS requires us to take into consideration the distinct linking mechanisms that

connect MTS component teams, such as size and power that component teams have,and

understand how each plays a role in the evolving nature of emergent states and behavioral

processes of a MTS.

Although a number of solutions targeting the existing limitationin this study have been

provided. In the subsequent section ideas for future advancement of MTS research is provided.

Researchers are encouraged to consider the following ideas when developing MTS theory and

designing MTS studies.

Implications for Future Research

Two areas on which future research in MTS theory could benefit from expanding will be

Page 133: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

115

targeted within this section of the discussion. The first incorporates distinct aspects of a MTS,

for instance, individuals within the system and the processes that develop over time. The second

will include methodological factors, such as measurement of processes.

The findings from this dissertation provide a stepping-stone for a number of areas within

the organizational psychology literature. First the findings provide new insight into emergent

states that leaders and boundary spanners can aim to foster in virtual MTS. Recent work by

Davison and Hollenbeck (2011) note that boundary spanners engage in three types of behaviors

outbound information flow (“communications activities aimed specifically at representing the

unit to outsiders, such as resource owners upon which the team is critically dependent and other

external constituencies that hold power and influence over the unit” p.328); inbound information

flow (“encompasses the set of communications activities specifically associated with informing

the focal unit about the environment, both external and internal to the organization to which the

unit belongs” p. 329); and behavioral integration which has been subdivided into three potential

areas in organizational structure a) across external boundaries, b)across internal subunit

boundaries and c)within internal subunits. We must consider that with exchanging information

across boundaries, boundary spanners must establish some form of trust with the members he or

she will engage in collaborating with as well as show those individuals that the teams with which

they are going to collaborate or depend on for information are not only trust worthy but capable

of successfully contributing to the task in a timely manner. Work by Davison and Hollenbeck

(2011) and Davison et al. (2011) have begun to explore the impact boundary spanners have on

coordinating efforts in MTS, but the impact boundary spanners can have onencouraging

knowledge creation and the development of MTS efficacy and MTS trustare rich avenues for the

Page 134: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

116

advancement of MTS theory.

As noted in Chapter 1 of this dissertation, a number of motivational and affective states

exist. Within the teams literature the following affective and motional emergent states have been

linked to team performance: collective orientation, collective efficacy, team learning orientation,

team cohesion, trust, team empowerment, and team goal commitment to name a few (Salas,

Rosen, Burke, & Goodwin, 2009). For the sake of this dissertation collective efficacy and trust

were targeted to determine the effect on behavioral processes and cognitive emergent states. Our

understanding of what motivates action in MTS is essential for their effectiveness. Future

research should expand the range of motivational and affective emergent states to obtain a more

thorough understanding of their effect. Building onto this point, exploring other critical

behavioral processes, such as understanding how MTS members learn, share and create

knowledge is vital for the functioning of individuals, teams, systems, and organizations.

Understanding the motives that foster such developments constitute next steps within the MTS

literature.

Forthe purpose of this study MTS efficacy and MTS trust were selected based on three

reasons. The first being the challenge that developing MTS efficacy and MTS trust posit in these

complex network of teams. For instance, MTS are comprised of component teams that usually

collaborate within team boundaries but venturing outbeyond team boundaries is essential for the

success of MTS. Being able to pinpoint or at least begin to uncover what motivates this

behavior, such as level of efficacy and trust is just the beginning. The second reason these two

emergent states were selected was because of the challenge they create to develop in virtual

contexts. Within the virtual team literature trust has been one of the more studied emergent

Page 135: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

117

processes commonly cited as a reason is the challenges that lack of accessibility provides(Rico et

al., 2009; Cramton, 2001; Jarvenpaa & Linder, 1999; Jarvenpaa et al., 1998). Drawing a parallel

to the virtual team trust literature, lack of exposure to other team’s success when they are not in

the geographical space or simply due to lack of exposure, as can be the case for virtual MTS, is

another challenge posed to MTS in the development of MTS efficacy. Lastly, although existing

research posits that emotionally driven emergent states provided are the reason why shared

cognition impacts team performance, this dissertation sought to make a case for the importance

of emotional and affective emergent states to be the driving mechanisms of subsequent

processes.

Work by Mathieu, Rapp, Maynard and Mangos (2010) and Liu and Zang (2010) have

found that affective and motivational states serve as mediators to the cognitive emergent states

and performance relationship. Specifically Mathieu et al. (2010) found that team efficacy

mediated the task SMM and performance relationship, whereas Liu and Zang (2010) found that

team efficacy mediated the TMS and team performance relationship. Although in the current

study, mediational analyses were not conducted to test the effects of shared cognition as the

reason for MTS efficacy influencing MTS performance, results do suggest that MTS efficacy

and trust were positively related to shared cognition. The findings presented in this study in

conjunction with previous work, suggest that MTS efficacy and MTS trust are essential emergent

states that should be fostered throughout the lifecycle of virtual MTS. Furthermore

understanding whether MTS efficacy and MTS trust are precursors to shared cognition or the

reason why shared cognition is predictive of performance are avenues that should be explored in

the future.

Page 136: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

118

Thus far, points that have been addressed as future research avenues for MTS work

include the study of key players of a MTS, such as boundary spanners or leaders. Additionally,

attention has been placed on the importance of expanding our knowledge of the distinct emergent

states teams researchers have shed light on over the years and how each may impact MTS

dynamics. In the following paragraphs, I would like to draw attention to another important

avenue for MTS research, the role of contextual factors.

Distinct contextual characteristics can mitigate or exacerbate the effective functioning of

MTS. For instance, the level of interdependence among the teams in a system, whether

collaboration is defined as pooled (i.e., where individuals first work independently and later

combine their work), sequentially (where a pre-established order is determined for task

completion and individuals further down the line tend to be more dependent on those that come

before them), reciprocal (i.e., all members within in a team tend to be dependent on one another,

not just in a linear fashion), or team (i.e., group members diagnose, problem solve, and

collaborate to complete a task, p.75; Thompson 2004, Saavedra, Early, & Van Dyne, 1993) can

have differential impact on how processes and emergent states manifest themselves in MTS. For

instance, meta-analytic work on the effects of team cohesion on team performance suggests that

as team workflow becomes more interdependent the team cohesion and team performance

relationship becomes stronger (Gully, Devine, & Whitney, 1995). This is but one example of

how task interdependence influences the dynamics of team phenomena. Taking this knowledge

and translating it to more complex system of teams, such as MTS, will allow us researchers to be

more diagnostic of how to target the development of distinct phenomena, such as cohesion, when

it begins to dwindle in MTS.

Page 137: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

119

Another contextual factor that is placed at the forefront of MTS theory, and which is the

means via which highly interdependent teams coordination action, is virtual communication.

Based on the accessibility, the growing market, and the necessity for rapid communication

virtual tools have integrated themselves as part of our work life. Technology has provided us the

ability to collaborate across time zones, countries, and oceans. The literature on virtuality has

quickly been thrusting forward and paving the way for practitioners to understand the potential

advancement of such tools. In 2002 Baltes et al. meta-analysis compared face-to-face teams to

virtual teams. In 2004 Fjermestad also focused on the comparison of face-to-face versus

virtuality but categorized the literature by considering the synchronicity of information

exchanged via virtual tools. In 2005 Kirkman and Mathieu focused on pushing the literature to

move beyond face-to-face teams versus virtual teams and to consider the 3 dimensions (use of

virtual tools, synchronicity, and informational value provided by virtual tools) on which virtual

tools could be categorized.In 2011 Mesmer-Magnus et al. reviewed the virtual team literature

taking into consideration the Kirkman and Mathieu virtual tool taxonomywhen identifying teams

as being more or less virtual. Building on the work by Kirkman and Mathieu and Mesmer-

Magnus et al. this dissertation focused on expanding our understanding of how distinct aspects of

virtual tools impact the development of MTS dynamics. Moving beyond the current study

further investigation into: the types virtual tools, the information conveyed via virtual tools, the

amount oftime employing virtual tools and how each of these influences MTS dynamics is a

fruitful avenue for future research.

Up to this point, the importance of key players and how they can be of value to the

development of emergent states and behavioral processes has been addressed. Additionally, the

Page 138: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

120

importance of taking into consideration and potentially manipulating contextual factors in future

studies has also been stressed. The last section that I would like to draw attention to for future

MTS research is the methodology used to test research questions.

Although not a principal driver of this dissertation, it was attempted to target the best way

to frame questions that would provide participants the ability to conceptualize constructs at MTS

level. For instance, the multiteam efficacy questions were framed in two distinct ways with the

hopes of providing insight into which is the best way to frame questions pertaining to the MTS

level: a)My team and the other team will be able to achieve most of the goals that are set for the

taskforce and b) Our taskforce will be able to achieve most of the goals that are set for the

taskforce. In all cases both means of assessing the construct were equally predictive of the

dependent variable. Future research in this area could benefit form expanding the findings in this

area to other construct and provide more insight into how to best frame the assessment of MTS

level constructs.

In an attempt to better understand the MTS measurement literature a number of

researchers have drawn attention to the benefits of incorporating network analyses as a tool to

assess the relationships that arises in MTS (Poole & Contractor, 2011). Virtual MTS provide a

platform in which information exchanged between teams can be traced in order to understand

how influential members (i.e., leaders)shape information exchange and development of team

cognition. By employing network methodology we may be able to better assess the relationships

that form in MTS. Additionally, being able to pinpoint individuals that are key catalyst of

organizational change can serve many purposes within organizations. Not only can we target

these individuals with new interventions,but also they may be the critical players to relay new

Page 139: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

121

knowledge throughout the organization.

Thus far, the measurement discusses have been more obtrusive in nature. By obtrusive, I

mean requesting individuals to take time out of their natural daily tasks to complete measures

that allow scientist to capture important phenomena of interest. Many times in experimentation,

requiring participants to pause to complete measures will influence distinct motivational,

affective, and cognitive emergent states as well as behavioral processes that are naturally

developing in teams and MTS. Our ability to assess these relationships in a way where we do

not break the natural development of these mechanisms forming is an area where this research

can improve. Through the review of chat logs, I was able to obtain social and task relevant

information, but the data provides a number of other important emergent states and behavioral

processes that can be detected, such as aggression, dislike, conflict, backup behavior, etc. Our

ability to use these types of measures in conjunction with existing psychometric measures can

help us build MTS theory.

Some of the latest work in the field has been targeting the use of unobtrusive measures to

capture both team and MTS level mechanisms. Recent work by Baard et al. (2012); DeCostanza

and Dirosa (2012); Contractor and DeChurch (2012) have sought to tackle this issue by

employing unobtrusive measures of team processes, such as, using physiological measures, and

reviewing past gaming history of online gaming communities todiagnose emergent trends.

These innovative ways of targeting behavioral processes and emergent states appears to capture a

direction that our science is rapidly moving towards. These means of capturing critical

mechanisms for the functioning of teams, MTS, and organization allow us to be more efficient

with participants’ time devoted to our studies, limit the fatigue we cause participants from the

Page 140: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

122

lengthy measures we request them to complete, and lastly allow us to go about understanding

behavior and emergent phenomena as it naturally occurs without interruptions. As noted this new

trend of assessing MTS mechanisms promises a number of benefits that can provide insight into

MTS dynamics.

Although a number of alternative ideas for future research are in mind and the list can go

on and on, the above-mentioned are the critical next steps for the advancement of MTS theory.

In the following section of this discussion a highlight of the theoretical contributionsthis

dissertation provides the MTS literature will be discussed. This section will be followed by the

contributions that these findings provide organizations and practitioners (i.e., practical

implications). Lastly, I will conclude with closing remarks and key take away points of this

dissertation.

Theoretical Contributions

This study provides a number of theoretical contributions to the following literatures:

multiteam systems, information sharing, and virtuality. First this study provides insight into a

number of mechanisms that over the last 25 plus years have been critical drivers of team

functioning (Kozlowski & Ilgen, 2006, Mathieu et al., 2008). Here they are unmasked yet in

another context, MTS, and their importance noted. For instance, understanding that MTS

members’ perspectives on the system’s potential to successful accomplish it’s tasks is a driving

force of both the behavior members engage in as well as the potential for developing cognitive

architecture of the situation at hand.

Second, this study extends the IS literature to a new context that is ever growing in our

Page 141: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

123

daily lives, MTS. Building on the team IS literature, the extension of IS taking place both

between and within teams is a critical difference that must be targeted in future research in order

to advance our understanding of MTS IS. In addition through this study IS characterized as

unique and open were separately analyzed to focus on the contribution of each onMTSbehavioral

processes, emergent states and performance. Additionally, the study of these constructs was done

through a virtual context lens, which brings me to the final theoretical contribution of this study,

the advancement of the virtuality literature.

Going beyond the traditional way of studying virtuality (Baltes et al., 2002) where a

comparison between face-to-face teams and virtual teams takes place, this dissertation sought to

incorporate the Kirkman and Mathieu’s (2005) taxonomy of virtual teams and consider distinct

characteristics the virtual tools encompass. For instance, the virtual tool employed provided

participants the ability to use a number of distinct virtual functions such as video conference

calls, audio conference call, or chat. This study focused on both the type of information

exchanged through these mechanisms (social versus task) as well as the time devoted to the use

of each tool and how it influenced the development of MTS behavioral processes and emergent

states. Advancing the science through theoretical contributions is important for the growing

science of MTS, but being able to translate these findings to our everyday lives is also essential.

Practical Implications

Results from this dissertation provide a number of suggestions for practitioners to

incorporate in order to make MTS more effective. First, our understanding of what motivates

individuals is critical. Our ability to train organizational leaders in promoting MTS level

Page 142: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

124

efficacy is one avenue in which real world MTS can benefit. One place in which MTS are

prevalent and in which effective coordination is vital is in the Armed Forces. Effective

coordinationbetween teams within a platoon is critical for the life of the men that comprise each

team, the system, and the nation. Thus, through this research our ability to understand that

efficacy is a critical driver of action, such as sharing information and a precursor of developing

shared cognition, we can hone the skills of our military officers to promote and target the

development of this emergent state. Along the same lines, incorporating training that provides

awareness and prescriptive solutions on building collective efficacy can provide a great deal of

difference when it comes to the advancement of these systems.

Again emphasizing the importance of training, as organizations turn to MTS as a means

of organizing work managers ability to provide a shared schema of the situation can prove to be

fruitful for the functioning of MTS. The work presented here suggests that possessing a shared

schema was positively related to communicating across team boundaries of a MTS. Therefore,

our ability to train managers and component MTS teams in the importance of establishing an

accurate and similar understanding of the situation can be of value to a MTS’s future

collaborative efforts.

Second, this dissertation provides new advancements in our understanding of how virtual

tools, which are inundating our daily work lives, can be beneficial and detrimental for the

exchange of information both within and between teams. The ability to promote the use of tools

that enhance the informational value conveyed and that provide for information to be retrieved at

a later time is a key driver in promoting sharing of information across team boundaries and

essentially promoting the effectiveness of MTS. Organizations can foster a work environment in

Page 143: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

125

which the use of these tools is a standard means via which meetings take place. Additionally,

promoting not only the use of the tools but the value they provide in better grasping project

details, gaining a system level perspective, as well as assisting with the sharing of valuable

information between teams of a MTS is a way to enhance buy-in by organizational members that

comprise these systems and employee virtual tools. Through these forms of tools, organizations

can promote and provide an open communication culture via virtual tools.

Lastly, in this dissertation open information sharing was more predictive of MTS

performance when compared to other forms of information sharing. It is important to note that

open information sharing was assessed using a subjective measure, whereas both unique and

commonly held information sharing was assessed via objective measures. These findings

suggest that individual’s perceptions of openly sharing information are an important driver of

MTS performance. Thus promoting an organizational climate where individuals are not hesitant

toshare information or seek information via virtual communication tools is important for the

functioning of virtual MTS.

Conclusion

The findings provided in this dissertationalludeto a number of important take away.

First, three important conclusions of this research: a)Both MTS level efficacy and trust are

important drivers of exchanging information in MTS and the development of MTS cognition,

b)Possessing a similar MTS SMM is an important driver of exchanging information across and

within teams of a MTS, and c)Employing virtual tools that provide for the retrievability of

information at a later time can enhance information exchange across teams in a MTS. Second,our

Page 144: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

126

ability as scientist to advance our understanding ofhow to make MTS more effective via

behavioral processes and emergent states that evolve in MTS is important with the advancing

nature of MTS work in our organizations. One means via which we can do this is by employing

unobtrusive tools such as the virtual communication tools to further understand these dynamics

as they naturally evolve in MTS. Lastly,as practitioners our ability to translate scientific findings

into effective organizational strategies and trainingis important for the success of MTS. The

findings and study limitations presented here outline a number of promising avenues for

practitioners to tackle in the near future with the goal of advancing our knowledge and forging

effective MTS.

Page 145: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

127

APPENDIX A

FIGURES

Page 146: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

128

Figure 1: Theoretical model.

Page 147: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

129

Figure 2: Taskforce DELTA goal hierarchy.

Kazbar

Region

Recon Officer

Goals:

• Plot

intelligence of

enemy threats

and targets in

the region

accurately

• Identify

Insurgent

Hotspots

• Zone

Insurgent

Hotspots as

Red on the

Strategic Map

Field

SpecialistGoals:

• Neutralized

Insurgent

Hotspots

following

Rules of

Engagement

• Zone

neutralized

Insurgent

Hotspot areas

as green so the

convoy can

move through

the region

safely

Team Level Goal Clear the region of

both Insurgent and

IED Hotspots

MTS Level Goal Move the Convoy

carrying humanitarian

aid through the war-

torn region while

limiting the damage

inflicted on its trucks

Recon Officer

Goals:

• Plot

intelligence of

enemy threats

and targets in

the region

accurately

• Identify IED

Hotspots

• Zone IED

Hotspots as

Blue on the

Strategic Map

Field Specialist

Goals:

• Neutralized

IED Hotspots

following

Rules of

Engagement

• Zone

neutralized

IED Hotspot

areas as green

so the convoy

can move

through the

region safely

Individual Level Goals

Page 148: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

130

Figure 3: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 1.

Page 149: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

131

Figure 4: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 2.

Page 150: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

132

Figure 5: Follow-up exploratory analyses Model 3.

Page 151: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

133

Figure 6: Information sharing uniqueness within team and multiteam TMS relationship moderated by media retrievability task

information exchanged – depiction of Model 31.

Page 152: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

134

Figure 7: Information sharing uniqueness within team and multiteam TMS relationship moderated by media retrievability

social and task information exchanged – depiction of Model 33.

Page 153: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

135

Figure 8: Information sharing uniqueness between teams and multiteam task SMM relationship moderated by media

retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 66.

Page 154: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

136

Figure 9: Information sharing uniqueness within team and multiteam task SMM relationship moderated by media retrievability

task information exchanged – depiction of Model 67.

Page 155: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

137

Figure 10: Information sharing uniqueness between teams and multiteam task SMM relationship moderated by media

retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 68.

Page 156: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

138

Figure 11: Information sharing uniqueness within team and multiteam task SMM relationship moderated by media

retrievability task information exchanged– depiction of Model 69.

Page 157: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

139

Figure 12: Multiteam TMS and information sharing uniqueness between teams relationship moderated by media retrievability

social information exchanged – depiction of Model 123.

Page 158: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

140

Figure 13: Multiteam task SMM and information sharing uniqueness between teams relationship moderated by media

retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 169.

Page 159: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

141

Figure 14: Multiteam task SMM and information sharing uniqueness within team relationship moderated by media

retrievability task information exchanged – depiction of Model 178.

Page 160: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

142

Figure 15: Multiteam task SMM and information sharing uniqueness within team relationship moderated by media

retrievability social and task information exchanged – depiction of Model 181.

Page 161: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

143

Figure 16: Theoretical model findings.

Page 162: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

144

Figure 17: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 1 findings.

Page 163: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

145

Figure 18: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 2 findings.

Page 164: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

146

Figure 19: Follow-up exploratory analysis Model 3 findings.

Page 165: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

147

APPENDIX B

TABLES

Page 166: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

148

Table 1: Dissertation Hypotheses Summarized

No. Hypothesis Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

Theoretical Model

1 MTS efficacy is positively related

to MTS IS uniqueness.

NS 12 1-10

2 MTS IS uniqueness is positively

related to MTS performance.

S 13 11-14 Not supported

for efficiency

(Model 11), but

supported for

effectiveness

(Models 13 and

14).

3 MTS IS uniqueness is positively

related to MTS performance

through (i.e., mediated by) the

MTS TMS.

NS 14 15-18

4 MTS trust is positively related to

MTS IS openness.

S 15 19 Model 19

depicts the

supported

relationship.

5 MTS IS openness is positively

related to MTS performance.

S 16 20 & 21 Supported for

multiteam

efficiency and

effectiveness.

6 MTS IS openness is positively

related to MTS performance

through MTSSMM.

NS 17 22-27

Page 167: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

149

Table 1

No. Hypothesis Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

7 Media retrievability moderates the

relationship between MTS IS

uniqueness and MTS TMS. When

MTS engage in more MTS IS via

high information friendly

retrievable tools, such as chat

messages (i.e., high retrievability

media), the relationship between

MTS IS uniqueness and MTS TMS

will be stronger than when

multiteam systems utilize low

information friendly retrievable

tools, such as video and voice

communication (i.e., low

retrievability media) to exchange

MTS IS uniqueness.

S 18 28-33 6 & 7 Model 31 depicts

the media

retrievability

task moderator

and model 33

depicts the

media

retrievability

social and task

aggregate

moderator.

8 Media richness moderates the

relationship between MTS IS

openness and MTSSMM, such that

when multiteam systems engage in

MTS IS Open using more rich

media (e.g., video communication),

MTS SMM will be more similar

than when open information

exchange occurs through less rich

media (e.g., text messaging).

NS 19 34-36

Exploratory Analyses

MTS efficacy will be positively

related to MTS IS openness

S 20 37-41 Models 37-40

depict the

supported

relationships and

model 16 depicts

the non-

supported

relationship.

Page 168: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

150

Table 1

Relationship Studied Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

Follow-up Exploratory Analyses Model 1

MTS efficacy will be positively

related to MTS IS openness

S 20 37-41 Models 37-40

depict the

supported

relationships and

model 16

depicts the non-

supported

relationship.

MTS IS openness will be

positively related to MTS

performance through MTS TMS.

NS 21 42 & 43

MTS trust will be positively

related to MTS IS uniqueness.

NS 22 44 & 45

MTS IS uniqueness will be

positively related to MTS

performance through MTS SMM.

NS 23 46-57

Media retrievability will moderate

the relationship between MTS IS

uniqueness and MTS SMM.

S 24 58-75 8-11 Models 66-69

depict supported

relationships for

Task SMM. Was

not supported

for Goal SMM

or Team SMM.

Media retrievability will moderate

the relationship between MTS IS

openness and MTS TMS.

NS 25 76-78

Page 169: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

151

Table 1

Relationship Studied Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

Media retrievability will moderate

the relationship between multiteam

information sharing openness and

multiteam SMM.

NS 26 79-87

Media richness will moderate the

relationship between MTS IS

openness and MTS TMS.

NS 27 88

Media richness will moderate the

relationship between MTS IS

uniqueness and MTS SMM.

NS 28 89-94

Media richness will moderate the

relationship between MTS IS

uniqueness and MTS TMS.

NS 29 95 & 96

Follow-UpExploratory Analyses Model 2

MTS efficacy will be positively

related to MTS TMS.

S 31 97-101 Models 97-100

depict the

supported

relationships.

MTS TMS will be positively

related to MTS performance.

S 32 102 &

103

Model 103

depicts the

supported

relationship for

multiteam

effectiveness.

Not supported

for multiteam

efficiency.

Page 170: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

152

Table 1

Relationship Studied Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

MTS TMS will be positively

related to MTS performance

through MTS IS uniqueness.

S 33 104-

107

Model 106 MTS

TMS is

positively

related to

performance via

MTS IS Unique

between teams

MTS trust will be positively

related to MTS SMM.

NS 34 108-

110

MTS SMM will be positively

related to MTS performance.

S 35 111-

116

Not supported

for multiteam

efficiency or

goal and team

SMM. Model

115 depicts the

supported

relationship for

Task SMM

MTS SMM will be positively

related to MTS performance

through MTS IS openness.

S 36 117-

122

Model 121

depicts full

mediation. MTS

Task SMM

MTS IS

Openness

MTS

Performance

(Effectiveness)

Page 171: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

153

Table 1

Follow-UpExploratory Analyses Model 3

Relationship Studied Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

Media retrievability will moderate

the relationship between MTS

TMS and MTS IS uniqueness.

S 37 123-

128

12 Model 123 MTS

TMS is

positively

related to MTS

IS Unique

Between teams

via Media

Retrievability

Social

Media richness will moderate the

relationship between MTS SMM

and MTS IS openness.

NS 38 129-

131

MTS efficacy will be positively

related to MTS SMM.

S 39 132-

146

Models 132-134

depict the

supported

relationships.

MTS trust will be positively

related to MTS TMS.

S 40 147 Model 147

depicts the

supported

relationship

MTS TMS will be positively

related to MTS performance

(efficiency and performance)

through MTS IS openness.

S 41 148 &

149

Model 148 and

149 depict the

supported

relationship for

MTS efficiency

and MTS

effectiveness.

Page 172: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

154

Table 1

Relationship Studied Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

MTS SMM will be positively

related to MTS performance

(efficiency and effectiveness)

through MTS IS uniqueness.

NS 42 150-

161

Models 158 and

depict full

mediation. MTS

SMM MTS IS

Uniqueness

Between

TeamsMTS

Performance

(Effectiveness).

Media retrievability will moderate

the relationship between MTS

TMS and TS IS openness.

NS 43 162-

164

Page 173: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

155

Table 1

Relationship Studied Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

Media retrievability will

moderate the relationship

between MTS SMM and

MTS IS uniqueness.

S 44 165-

182

13 -15 Model 169 depicts the

relationships between

multiteam task SMM

and multiteam

information sharing

unique between team

moderated by Media

retrievability task

specific.

Model 178 depicts

the relationship

between multiteam

task SMM and

multiteam

information sharing

unique within team

moderated by

Media retrievability

task specific.

Model 181 depicts

the relationship

between multiteam

task SMM and

multiteam

information sharing

unique within team

moderated by

Media retrievability

social and task

specific.

Page 174: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

156

Table 1

Relationship Studied Supported

vs. Not

Supported

Table Model Figure Notes

Media retrievability will moderate

the relationship MTS SMM and

MTS IS openness.

NS 45 183-

191

Media richness will moderate the

relationship between MTS SMM

and MTS IS uniqueness.

NS 46 192-

197

Media richness will moderate the

relationship between MTS TMS

and MTS IS openness.

NS 47 198

Media richness will moderate the

relationship MTS TMS and TMS

IS uniqueness.

NS 48 199 &

200

Page 175: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

157

Table 2: Zero Order Correlations of Study Variables

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

1.PC/Video Play Experience 1.52 .37 1

2.Iming 4.42 .48 .16 1

3.TaskEfficacy 4.17 .46 .27* .19 1

4.Taskforce Efficacy Both Teams 3.76 .54 .10 -.01 .74** 1

5.TaskForce Efficacy 3.79 .54 .16 .03 .74** .95** 1

6.Taskforce Efficacy Percentage 80.25 9.94 .11 .13 .72** .61 .62** 1

7.Taskforce Efficacy Total Yes 3.34 .85 -.04 .20 .07 .05 .08 .03 1

8.Multiteam Trust 3.37 .23 .16 -.05 .52** .48** .51** .38** .12 1

9.IS Uniqueness Between 4.16 2.62 .06 .10 .11 .02 .01 .05 .13 .08 1

10.IS Uniqueness Within 3.30 1.83 .22* .06 .15 .17 .18 .13 -.02 .09 .67** 1

11.IS Common Between 2.22 1.67 -.02 .02 .07 -.05 -.00 .05 .16 -.10 .43** .36** 1

12.IS Common Within 15.17 7.95 .06 .06 .05 .03 .06 .04 .05 -.03 .61** .66** .48** 1

13.IS Openness 3.34 .43 .10 .25* .51** .47** .43** .29** .16 .37** .24* .26* .09 .09 1

14.TMS 4.02 .26 .15 .25* .54** .47** .45** .42** .05 .37** .01 .01 -.07 .00 .58**

15.Goal SMM .33 .33 .21 .20 .26* .31** .24* .19 .19 .17 .10 .01 -.00 .01 .28*

16.Task SMM .16 .22 .24* -.04 .14 -.05 -.04 -.02 -.00 .14 .26* .22 .05 .04 .35**

Page 176: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

158

Table 2: Zero Order Correlations of Study Variables

M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

17.Team SMM .10 .16 .05 .10 -.04 -.06 -.04 .06 .22 -.09 .05 -.07 .05 .12 -.08

18.MTS Efficiency 58.34 12.54 .29** .29** .40** .21 .25* .27* .10 .14 .24* .23* .10 .04 .38**

19.MTS Effectiveness 6.08 2.91 .24* .16 .37 .23* .26* .23* .07 .27* .26* .26* .06 .02 .38**

Page 177: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

159

Table 2: Zero Order Correlations of Study Variables

14 15 16 17 18

1.PC/Video Play

Experience

2.Iming

3.TaskEfficacy

4.Taskforce Efficacy Both

Teams

5.TaskForce Efficacy

6.Taskforce Efficacy

Percentage

7.Taskforce Efficacy Total

Yes

8.Trust

9.IS Uniqueness Between

10.IS Uniqueness Within

11.IS Common Between

12.IS Common Within

13.IS Openness

14.TMS 1

15.Goal SMM .40** 1

16.Task SMM .29* .28* 1

17.Team SMM -.03 .24 -.02 1

18.MTS Efficiency .28* .20 .17 -.09 1

19.MTS Effectiveness .33** .10 .27* -.13 .62**

Note. N = 82; † = p >.10; * = p <.05; ** = p <.01

Page 178: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

160

Table 3: Cronbach Alpha and Rwg Statistics for Study Variables

Variable α Rwg

Trust .58 .78

Task Efficacy .81 .88

Taskforce Efficacy (teams) .97 .90

Taskforce Efficacy .97 .93

Bandura Yes/No .84 -

Bandura Percentage .87 .89

TMS .95 .95

IS Open .90 .98

Page 179: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

161

Table 4: Multiteam Efficacy

Task Efficacy

1. How far do you think your team and the other team can move the convoy in the upcoming trial?

2. How far do you think your team and the other team can move the convoy without getting much

damage in the upcoming trial?

3. How much intel do you think your team and the other team will share in the upcoming trial?

4. What is the percentage that you can neutralize hotspots in the upcoming trial?

Taskforce Collective Efficacy (Chen et al., 2001 Team-Based)

1. My team and the other team will be able to achieve most of the goals that are set for the taskforce.

2. When facing difficult tasks, I am certain that my team and the other team will accomplish them.

3. In general, I think that my team and the other team can obtain outcomes that are important.

4. I believe that my team and the other team can succeed at most any task we encounter in our

mission.

5. My team and the other team will be able to successfully overcome challenges in the mission

environment.

6. I am confident that my team and the other team can perform effectively on our tasks.

7. Compared to other taskforces, my team and the other team can do most tasks very well.

8. Even when things are tough, my team and the other team can perform quite well.

9. I am confident that my team and the other team can perform effectively on our tasks.

10. Compared to other taskforces, my team and the other team can do most tasks very well.

11. Even when things are tough, my team and the other team can perform quite well.

Taskforce Collective Efficacy (Chen et al., 2001 Multiteam System-Based)

1. Our taskforce will be able to achieve most of the goals that are set for the taskforce.

2. When facing difficult tasks, I am certain that our taskforce will accomplish them.

3. In general, I think that our taskforce can obtain outcomes that are important.

Page 180: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

162

Table 4: Multiteam Efficacy

4. I believe that our taskforce can succeed at most any task we encounter in our mission.

5. Our taskforce will be able to successfully overcome challenges in the mission environment.

6. I am confident that our taskforce can perform effectively on our tasks.

7. Compared to other taskforces, our taskforce can do most tasks very well.

8. Even when things are tough, our taskforce can perform quite well.

9. Our taskforce will be able to achieve most of the goals that are set for the taskforce.

10. When facing difficult tasks, I am certain that our taskforce will accomplish them.

11. In general, I think that our taskforce can obtain outcomes that are important.

Multiteam Efficacy Bandura

1a. I believe that our taskforce team can finish the mission in the upcoming mission

1b. Based on your above answer, how certain are you?

2a. I believe that our taskforce can neutralize all hotspots in the upcoming mission.

2b. Based on your above answer, how certain are you?

3a. I believe that our taskforce can successfully exchange all intelligence required to neutralize

enemies in the upcoming mission.

3b. Based on your above answer, how certain are you?

4a. I believe that our taskforce can successfully move the convoy through the war-torn region

without causing it damage in the upcoming mission.

4b. Based on your above answer, how certain are you?

5a. I believe that our taskforce can finish the upcoming mission.

5b. Based on your above answer, how certain are you?

6a. I believe that our taskforce can neutralize all hotspots in the upcoming mission.

6b. Based on your above answer, how certain are you?

7a. I believe that our taskforce can exchange all intelligence required to neutralize enemies in the

upcoming mission.

7b. Based on your above answer, how certain are you?

Page 181: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

163

Table 4: Multiteam Efficacy

8a. I believe that our taskforce can successfully move the convoy through the war torn region

without causing it damage in the upcoming mission.

8b. Based on your above answer, how certain are you?

Note. For scales the Task Efficacy scale, the response scale was 1 = 0%, 2 = 25%, 3 = 50%, 4 =

75%, and 5= 100%.For the Taskforce Collective Efficacy (Chen et al., Team-Based), and

Taskforce Collective Efficacy (Chen et al., Multiteam System-Based) the Response Scale was: 1

= Strongly Disagree, 2 = Disagree, 3 = Neutral, 4 = Agree, and 5 = Strongly Agree. For the

Bandura scalea the response scale was Yes/No and for the Bandura scale b the response scalewas

a percentage provided by the participant from 0-100.

Page 182: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

164

Table 5: Trust

1. The other team will keep our team in mind when performing the mission.

2. I would be willing to let the other team have complete control over our team’s decisions in the

upcoming mission.

3. If the other team asked why a problem occurred during the mission, I would tell them even if I

were partly to blame.

4. I feel comfortable taking risks in the mission knowing that even if we fail, the other team will

support our decision.

5. It is important for my team to keep an eye on what the other team is doing.

6. Increasing my team’s vulnerability to the other team would be a mistake.

7. If I had my way, I wouldn’t let the other team influence our task force.

Note. Response Scale: 1 = Completely Disagree, 2 = Disagree, 3 = Neutral, 4 = Agree, and 5 =

Completely Agree

Page 183: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

165

Table 6: Information Sharing Psychometric Scale Items

Information Sharing Uniqueness

Between Teams: Information exchanged between team members on distinct teams that was initially

distributed across all four taskforce members.

Within Team: Information exchanged between team members of the same team that was initially

distributed across the two team members.

Information Sharing Openness

The members of the other team:

Information used to make key decisions was freely shared among my team and the members of the

other team.

The members of the other team worked hard to keep our team up to date on their activities.

The members of the other team were kept “in the loop” about key issues affecting the taskforce.

Information Sharing Commonly Held/Shared

Between Teams: Information exchanged between team members on distinct teams that was initially

distributed to both taskforce members engaging in the communication exchange. In other words each

taskforce member engaging in the exchange of information possessed the information prior the

exchange taking place.

Within Team: Information exchanged between team members of the same team that was initially

distributed to both team members engaging in the communication exchange. In other words the two

team members possessed the information prior to the exchange taking place.

Note. Respond as honestly as possible, using the response scale provided. Response Scale: 1= Very

Strongly Disagree, 2= Disagree

3= Neither Disagree or Agree, 4= Agree, and 5= Very Strongly Agree

Page 184: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

166

Table 7: Transactive Memory Systems -Psychometric Scale

Specialization

1. Each member of the taskforce has specialized knowledge of some aspect of our task.

2. I have knowledge about an aspect of the task that no other member of the taskforce has.

3. Different members of the taskforce are responsible for expertise in different areas.

4. The specialized knowledge of several different members of the taskforce is needed to complete the

task.

5. I know which members of the taskforce have expertise in specific areas.

Credibility

1. I am comfortable accepting procedural suggestions from other members of the taskforce.

2. I trust that task knowledge of the other members is credible.

3. I am confident relying on the information that members of the taskforce brought to the discussion.

4. When other members of the taskforce give information, I want to double-check it for myself.

5. I do not have much faith in other members of the taskforce’s “expertise.”

Note. Please answer the following question using the scale provided. Response Scale: 1 = Completely

Disagree, 2 = Disagree, 3 = Neutral, 4 = Agree, and 5 = Completely Agree

Page 185: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

167

Table 8: Shared Mental Models

Multiteam Goal Shared Mental Model

Locating CI targets (Civilians, etc.) and threats (AFVs, etc.)

Locating OD targets and threats

Neutralizing Insurgents

Disarming IEDs

Clearing hotspots in Kazbar

Advancing the Executive Convoy

Multiteam Task Shared Mental Model

Clearing Insurgent hotspots

Sharing intelligence with the Stinger team

Communicating with the Stinger team about the mission status

Assisting the Stinger team in completing their goals

Clearing IED/Counter Insurgent hotspots

Multiteam Team Shared Mental Model

Obtaining Intelligence

Sharing Intelligence with your teammate

Updating the strategic map

Communicating with your team mate about the mission status

Reporting the locations of threats and targets

Requesting engagement authorization

Clearing hotspots of enemy threats

Page 186: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

168

Note. Within each of the boxes not marked with a slash, please enter a number 1 through 5

indicating the extent to which the item directly above and the item directly to the left are related.

The numbers indicate the following levels of relationships:1 = Minimal or no relationship, 2 =

Weakly related, 3 = Moderately related, 4 = Somewhat strongly related, and 5 = Very strongly

related.

Table 9: Performance Indices

Multiteam System Efficiency

The amount of time it took the humanitarian aid convoy to travel to its final destination.

Multiteam System Effectiveness

While neutralizing the war torn region did the MTS follow the Rules of Engagement.

1) All threats in a cell were reported

2) Threat within a cell is correctly neutralized

3) Convoy travels safely through the cell

Note. These indices were task derived

Page 187: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

169

Table 10: Controls

Gaming Related Questions

If yes, how frequently do you play video games? (Please circle one)

Have you played video games in the past 5 years?

Virtual Communication Related Questions

How confident are you in using Skype?

How often do you use messenger (e.g., Facebook chat, GChat, AIM, and others) every day?

Note. For the gaming questions the response scale was 1 = Only once or twice in the last 5 years, 2 = A

few times per year, 3 = A few times per month, 4 = A few times per week, 5 = Daily. For the virtual

communication related questions the response scale was 1= not at all, 2 = very little, 3 = to some

extent, 4 = to a great extent, and 5 = to a very great extent.

Page 188: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

170

Table 11: Zero Order Correlations between Control Variables and Study Variables

PCVidPlay VidPlay Skype Iming

TaskEfficacy -.29** .34** .11 .19

TeamEfficacy -.30** .18 .01 -.01

Trust4T1 -.10 .18 .02 -.05

TMS -.37** .25* .10 .25*

Multiteam Goal SMM -.06 .21 -.10 .20

Multiteam Task SMM -.09 .25* -.17 -.04

Multiteam Team SMM .04 .04 .12 .10

IS Uniqueness Between Team -.12 .09 .08 .10

IS Uniqueness Within Team -.08 .23* -.02 .06

IS Commonly Held Between Teams .09 -.04 .09 .02

IS commonly Held Within Team -.02 .07 -.03 .06

IS Open -.20 .16 -.02 .25*

Media Rich .02 .09 .09 .35*

Media Retrievability Social -.20 .07 -.01 -.09

Media Retrievability Task -.14 .08 -.19 -.05

Media Retrievability Social and Task Aggregate -.14 .09 -.19 -.05

Multiteam Efficiency -.26* .35** -.03 .29**

Multiteam Effectiveness -.34** .33** -.07 .16

Note. N = 82, † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 189: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

171

Table 12: Analyses Regressing MTS IS Uniqueness on MTS Efficacy

Hypothesis 1

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

1 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .50 .01 - 1 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .33 .01 .00

Iming .10 Iming .10

MTS Task Efficacy .00

N = 82

2 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .50 .01 - 2 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .33 .01 .00

Iming .10 Iming .10

MTS Taskforce Efficacy-tm .02

N = 82

3 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .50 .01 - 3 AvgPC&VidPPlay .02 .53 .02 .01

Iming .10 Iming .08

MTS Taskforce Efficacy .09

N = 82

Page 190: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

172

Hypothesis 1

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

4 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .50 .01 - 4 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .36 .01 .0

Iming .10 Iming .09

MTS Bandura Percent .04

N = 82

5 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .50 .01 - 5 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 .68 .03 .02

Iming .10 Iming .07

MTS Bandura Y/N .12

N = 82

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

6 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 2.11 .05 - 6 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20† 2.03 .07 .02

Iming .03 Iming .03

N = 82 MTS Task Efficacy .15

Page 191: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

173

Hypothesis 1

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

7 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 2.11 .05 - 7 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20† 2.06 .07 .02

Iming .03 Iming .03

MTS Taskforce Efficacy-tm .15

N = 82

8 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 2.11 .05 - 8 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20† 1.64 .06 .01

Iming .03 Iming .01

MTS Taskforce Efficacy .10

N = 82

9 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 2.11 .05 - 9 AvgPC&VidPPlay .21† 1.72 .06 .01

Iming .03 Iming .01

MTS Bandura Percent .11

N = 82

Page 192: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

174

Hypothesis 1

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

10 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 2.11 .05 - 10 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 1.40 .05 .00

Iming .03 Iming .03

MTS Bandura Y/N -.02

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 193: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

175

Table 13: Analyses Regressing MTS Performance on MTS IS Uniqueness

Hypothesis 2

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Efficiency

11 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 6.76 .15** - 11 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24* 5.93 .18† .03†

Iming .25* Iming .23*

MTS IS Uniqueness Between

Teams .20†

N = 82

12 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 6.76 .15** - 12 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 5.43 .17 .02

Iming .25* Iming .24*

MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team .17

N = 82

DV = MTS Effectiveness

13 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 3.18 .07* - 13 AvgPC&VidPPlay .21* 3.86 .13* .06*

Iming .13 Iming .10

MTS IS Uniqueness Between

Teams .23*

N = 82

Page 194: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

176

Hypothesis 2

Step 1 Step 1

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV= MTS Effectiveness

14 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 3.18 .07* - 14 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 3.54 .12* .05*

Iming .13 Iming .12

MTS IS

Uniqueness

Within Team .22*

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 195: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

177

Table 14: Mediator Analyses: The MTS IS and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by MTS TMS

Hypothesis 3

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

15 Path A -.02 .11

Path B 2.51† 1.30

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Between Teams

2.50† 1.28

DIRECT EFFECT 2.55* 1.26

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS TMS -.06 .31 -.79 .48

N = 82

16 Path A .00 .11

Path B 2.52† 1.31

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Within Team

2.09 1.32

DIRECT EFFECT 2.16† 1.30

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS TMS -.07 .35 -.94 .51

N = 82

Page 196: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

178

Hypothesis 3

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

17 Path A -.03 .11

Path B .85** .31

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Between Teams

.68* .31

DIRECT EFFECT .70* .30

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS TMS -.02 .10 -.23 .17

N = 82

18 Path A -.03 .11

Path B .85** .31

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Within Team

.63* .32

DIRECT EFFECT .66* .30

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS TMS -.02 .10 -.27 .16

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; Path A = IVMediator and Path B = Mediator DV

Page 197: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

179

Table 15: Analyses Regressing MTS IS Openness on MTS Trust

Hypothesis 4

DV = MTS IS Openness

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

19 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 2.86 .07† - 19 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 6.91 .21** .14**

Iming .24* Iming .27**

MTS Trust .38**

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 198: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

180

Table 16: Analyses Regressing MTS Performance on MTS IS Openness

Hypothesis 5

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Efficiency

20 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 6.76 .15** - 20 AvgPC&VidPPlay .23* 8.07 .24** .09**

Iming .25* Iming .17†

MTS ISOpenness .31**

N = 82

DV = MTS Effectiveness

21 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 3.18 .07* - 21 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20† 5.98 .19** .12**

Iming .13 Iming .04

MTS ISOpenness .35**

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 199: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

181

Table 17: Mediator Analyses: The MTS IS Openness and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by MTS SMM

Hypothesis 6

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

22 Path A .24* .12

Path B .57 1.49

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Open 3.71* 1.48

DIRECT EFFECT 3.57* 1.54

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Goal SMM .14 .43 -.47 1.26

N = 70

23 Path A .36** .11

Path B .22 1.53

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Open 3.88** 1.39

DIRECT EFFECT 3.80** 1.51

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Task SMM .08 .55 -1.04 .98

N = 68

Page 200: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

182

Hypothesis 6

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

24 Path A -.13 .15

Path B -1.22 1.45

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Open 5.07** 1.66

DIRECT EFFECT 4.91** 1.67

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Team SMM .17 .44 -.22 2.18

N = 61

DV= MTS Effectiveness

25 Path A .24* .12

Path B -.19 .34

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Open 1.14** .34

DIRECT EFFECT 1.18** .35

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Goal SMM -.05 .10 -.29 .10

N = 70

Page 201: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

183

Hypothesis 6

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

26 Path A .36** .11

Path B .36 .36

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Open 1.16** .33

DIRECT EFFECT 1.03** .35

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Task SMM .13 .13 -.11 .39

N = 68

27 Path A -.13 .15

Path B -.37 .34

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Open .97* .40

DIRECT EFFECT .92 .40

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Team SMM .05 .10 -.05 .42

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; Path A = IVMediator and Path B = Mediator DV

Page 202: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

184

Table 18: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS Cognition Relationship Moderated by Media Retrievability

Hypothesis 7

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

28 AvgPC&VidPPlay .14 2.07 .10† - 28 AvgPC&VidPPlay .14 1.66 .10 .0

Iming .26* Iming .26*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.01

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.01

Media Retrievability

Social .03

Media Retrievability Social .10

Interaction Term -.08

N = 78

Page 203: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

185

Hypothesis 7

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

29 AvgPC&VidPPlay .15 2.09 .10† - 29 AvgPC&VidPPlay .19 2.07 .12 .02

Iming .26* Iming .25*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team

-.02 MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team

-.08

Media Retrievability

Social

.03 Media Retrievability Social .36

Interaction Term -.36

N = 78

30 AvgPC&VidPPlay .15 2.16 .11† - 30 AvgPC&VidPPlay .15 1.81 .11 .0

Iming .26* Iming .25*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.00

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.02

Media Retrievability Task -.07 Media Retrievability Task -.07

Interaction Term -.08

N = 78

Page 204: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

186

Hypothesis 7

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

31 AvgPC&VidPPlay .15 2.17 .11† - 31 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 2.80 .16* .05*

Iming .26* Iming .26*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.02

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.04

Media Retrievability Task -.07 Media Retrievability Task -.03

Interaction Term -.24*

N = 78

32 AvgPC&VidPPlay .12 1.74 .08 - 32 AvgPC&VidPPlay .12 1.43 .09 .01

Iming .22* Iming .22*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.02

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .01

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.09

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.10

N = 82 Interaction Term -.07

Page 205: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

187

Hypothesis 7

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

33 AvgPC&VidPPlay .12 1.74 .08 - 33 AvgPC&VidPPlay .14 2.23 .13* .05*

Iming .22* Iming .23*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.02

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .08

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.09

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.05

Interaction Term -.24*

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 206: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

188

Table 19: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Openness and MTS SMM Relationship Moderated by Media Richness

Hypothesis 8

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

34 AvgPC&VidPPlay .15 2.46 .16† - 34 AvgPC&VidPPlay .19 2.82 .22† .06†

Iming .24† Iming .14

MTS ISOpenness .06 MTS ISOpenness .05

Media Richness .16 Media Richness .16

Interaction Term -.26†

N = 55

Page 207: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

189

Hypothesis 8

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Task SMM

35 AvgPC&VidPPlay .21 3.19 .20* - 35 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24† 2.80 .22 .02

Iming -.22 Iming -.27†

MTS ISOpenness .39** MTS ISOpenness .36**

Media Richness .01 Media Richness .03

Interaction Term -.15

N = 54

DV = MTS Team SMM

36 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.12 1.35 .11 - 36 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.13 1.09 .11 .0

Iming .11 Iming .12

MTS ISOpenness -.28† MTS ISOpenness -.26

Media Richness .19 Media Richness .18

Interaction Term .06

N = 49

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 208: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

190

Table 20: Analyses Regressing MTS IS Openness on MTS Efficacy

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

37 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 2.86 .07† - 37 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.06 8.33 .29** .22**

Iming .24* Iming .17†

MTS TaskEfficacy .50**

N = 82

38 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 2.86 .07† - 38 AvgPC&VidPPlay .02 10.39 .28** .21**

Iming .24* Iming .25**

MTS Taskforce Efficacy-tm .47**

N = 82

39 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 2.86 .07† - 39 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.00 10.88 .24** .18**

Iming .24* Iming .24*

MTS Taskforce Efficacy .42**

N = 82

Page 209: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

191

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

40 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 2.86 .07† - 40 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 4.08 .13* .06*

Iming .24* Iming .21*

MTS Bandura Percent .26*

N = 82

41 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 2.86 .07† - 41 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 2.28 .08 .02

Iming .24* Iming .22†

MTS Bandura Y/N .12

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 210: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

192

Table 21: Mediator Analysis: The MTS IS Openness and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by MTS TMS

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

42 Path A .55** .09

Path B .45 1.54

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Open 3.90** 1.28

DIRECT EFFECT 3.65* 1.54

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS TMS .24 .89 -1.53 2.08

N = 82

DV= MTS Effectiveness

43 Path A .55** .09

Path B .40 .37

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Open 1.01** .31

DIRECT EFFECT .79* .37

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS TMS .22 .19 -.12 .63

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; Path A = IVMediator and Path B = Mediator DV

Page 211: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

193

Table 22: Analyses Regressing MTS IS Uniqueness on MTS Trust

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

44 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .50 .01 - 44 AvgPC&VidPPlay .03 .48 .02 .01

Iming .10 Iming .10

MTS Trust .08

N = 82

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

45 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 2.11 .05 - 45 AvgPC&VidPPlay .21† 1.49 .05 .0

Iming .03 Iming .03

MTS Trust .06

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 212: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

194

Table 23: Mediator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by MTS SMM

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

46 Path A .06 .12

Path B 1.21 1.47

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams 2.89* 1.42

DIRECT EFFECT 2.82* 1.43

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Goal SMM .07 .29 -.20 1.13

N = 70

47 Path A -.04 .12

Path B 1.47 1.49

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team 1.85 1.44

DIRECT EFFECT 1.91 1.44

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Goal SMM -.06 .26 -.94 .30

N = 70

Page 213: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

195

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

48 Path A .26* .12

Path B .83 1.50

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams

3.20* 1.43

DIRECT EFFECT 2.98* 1.49

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Task SMM .22 .43 -.40 1.42

N = 68

49 Path A .22† .12

Path B 1.14 1.47

TOTAL EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team

2.99* 1.45

DIRECT EFFECT 2.79† 1.47

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Task SMM .25 .43 -.21 1.43

N = 68

Page 214: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

196

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

50 Path A .05 .13

Path B -1.85 1.47

TOTAL EFFECT

MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

3.26* 1.43

DIRECT EFFECT 3.33* 1.43

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Team SMM -.09 .38 -1.26 .33

N = 61

51 Path A -.07 .12

Path B -1.50 1.51

TOTAL EFFECT

MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

2.56 1.49

DIRECT EFFECT 2.42 1.50

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Team SMM .11 .34 -.19 1.72

N = 61

Page 215: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

197

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

52 Path A .10 .12

Path B .03 .35

TOTAL EFFECT

MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

.77* .34

DIRECT EFFECT .76* .34

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Goal SMM .00 .06 -.09 .19

N = 70

53 Path A .01 .12

Path B .10 .35

TOTAL EFFECT

MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

.56 .34

DIRECT EFFECT .00 .04

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Goal SMM .00 .04 -.06 .12

N = 70

Page 216: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

198

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

54 Path A .27* .12

Path B .53 .35

TOTAL EFFECT

MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

.93 .34

DIRECT EFFECT .79 .35

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Task SMM .14 .12 -.01 .49

N = 68

55 Path A .22† .12

Path B .60† .34

TOTAL EFFECT

MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

.95** .34

DIRECT EFFECT .84* .34

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Task SMM .13 .11 -.01 .43

N = 68

Page 217: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

199

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

56 Path A .05 .12

Path B -.49 .34

TOTAL EFFECT

MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

.71* .34

DIRECT EFFECT .73* .33

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Team SMM -.02 .08 -.25 .09

N = 61

57 Path A -.07 .12

Path B -.42 .35

TOTAL EFFECT

MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

.55 .35

DIRECT EFFECT .51 .35

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS Team SMM .03 .08 -.05 .34

N = 61

Page 218: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

200

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; Path A = IVMediator and Path B = Mediator DV

Page 219: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

201

Table 24: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS SMM Relationship Moderated by Media Retrievability

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

58 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20† 1.81 .10 - 58 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20 1.49 .11 .01

Iming .17 Iming .18

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.04

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.05

Media Retrievability

Social .19

Media Retrievability

Social .05

Interaction Term .15

N = 66

Page 220: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

202

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

59 AvgPC&VidPPlay .23† 2.13 .12† - 59 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 1.71 .12 .0

Iming .17 Iming .17

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.14

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.13

Media Retrievability

Social .20

Media Retrievability

Social .10

Interaction Term .11

N = 66

60 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20 1.20 .07 - 60 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20 .96 .07 .0

Iming .15 Iming .15

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .00

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .01

Media Retrievability

Task .04

Media Retrievability

Task .04

Interaction Term .03

N = 66

Page 221: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

203

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

61 AvgPC&VidPPlay .23† 1.43 .09 - 61 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24† 1.38 .10 .01

Iming .15 Iming .16

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.12

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.12

Media Retrievability

Task .04

Media Retrievability

Task .07

Interaction Term -.13

N = 66

62 AvgPC&VidPPlay .18 1.37 .08 - 62 AvgPC&VidPPlay .18 1.08 .08 .0

Iming .17 Iming .17

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .06

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .05

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .02

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .02

Interaction Term .01

N = 70

Page 222: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

204

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

63 AvgPC&VidPPlay .19 1.35 .08 - 63 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20† 1.42 .10 .02

Iming .18 Iming .18

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.05

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .03

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .02

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .05

Interaction Term -.18

N = 70

64 AvgPC&VidPPlay .23† 2.32 .13† - 64 AvgPC&VidPPlay .23† 1.87 .14 .01

Iming -.10 Iming -.11

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .28*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .28*

Media Retrievability

Social -.08

Media Retrievability

Social .03

Interaction Term -.12

N = 65

Page 223: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

205

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Task SMM

65 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20 1.94 .11 - 65 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 1.69 .12 .01

Iming -.08 Iming -.09

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .24†

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .21

Media Retrievability

Social -.05

Media Retrievability

Social .19

Interaction Term -.26

N = 65

66 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24* 2.93 .16* - 66 AvgPC&VidPPlay .23* 4.12 .26** .10**

Iming -.10 Iming -.10

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .26*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .25*

Media Retrievability

Task -.19

Media Retrievability

Task -.20†

Interaction Term -.31**

N = 65

Page 224: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

206

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Task SMM

67 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20 2.72 .15* - 67 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 4.95 .29** .14**

Iming -.08 Iming -.07

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .25*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .26*

Media Retrievability

Task -.20†

Media Retrievability

Task -.15

Interaction Term -.38**

N = 65

68 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 3.11 .16* - 68 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 4.18 .25** .09**

Iming -.12 Iming -.13

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .26*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .46**

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.18

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.19†

Interaction Term -.35**

N = 68

Page 225: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

207

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Task SMM

69 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 2.35 .13† - 69 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 3.75 .23** .20**

Iming -.10 Iming -.09

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .19

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .36**

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.19

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.13

Interaction Term -.37**

N = 68

Page 226: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

208

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Team SMM

70 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 .17 .01 - 70 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 .16 .02 .01

Iming .08 Iming .08

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.01

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.01

Media Retrievability

Social .05

Media Retrievability

Social -.06

Interaction Term .12

N = 59

71 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 .42 .03 - 71 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 .36 .03 .0

Iming .08 Iming .07

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.14

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.15

Media Retrievability

Social .06

Media Retrievability

Social .18

Interaction Term -.14

N = 59

Page 227: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

209

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Team SMM

72 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 .16 .01 - 72 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 .13 .01 .0

Iming .07 Iming .07

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .00

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .00

Media Retrievability

Task .04

Media Retrievability

Task .04

Interaction Term .01

N = 59

73 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 .42 .03 - 73 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 .34 .03 .0

Iming .07 Iming .07

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.14

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.14

Media Retrievability

Task .06

Media Retrievability

Task .05

Interaction Term .03

N = 59

Page 228: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

210

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Team SMM

74 AvgPC&VidPPlay .03 .20 .01 - 74 AvgPC&VidPPlay .03 .16 .01 .0

Iming .09 Iming .09

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .04

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .04

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .02

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .02

Interaction Term -.01

N = 61

75 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 .30 .02 - 75 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 .24 .02 .0

Iming .10 Iming .10

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.10

MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team -.10

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .03

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .03

Interaction Term .01

Page 229: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

211

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Table 25: Moderator Analyses: The IS Openness and MTS TMS Relationship Moderated by Media Retrievability

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

76 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 11.72 .39** - 76 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 9.50 .39 .0

Iming .12 Iming .13

MTS ISOpenness .56** MTS ISOpenness .55**

Media Retrievability

Social -.07

Media Retrievability

Social .07

Interaction Term -.16

N = 78

77 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 11.66 .39** - 77 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 9.35 .39 .0

Iming .13 Iming .12

MTS ISOpenness .55** MTS ISOpenness .56**

Media Retrievability

Task -.06

Media Retrievability

Task -.05

Interaction Term .06

Page 230: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

212

N = 78

Page 231: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

213

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

78 AvgPC&VidPPlay .08 10.80 .36** - 78 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 8.69 .36 .0

Iming .09 Iming .08

MTS ISOpenness .54** MTS ISOpenness .51**

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.08

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.07

Interaction Term .08

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 232: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

214

Table 26: Moderator Analysis: The MTS IS Openness and MTS SMM Relationship Moderated by Media Retrievability

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

79 AvgPC&VidPPlay .19 2.26 .13† - 79 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 2.05 .14 .01

Iming .12 Iming .12

MTS ISOpenness .17 MTS ISOpenness .18

Media Retrievability

Social .14

Media Retrievability

Social -.09

Interaction Term .26

N = 66

80 AvgPC&VidPPlay .19 1.91 .11 - 80 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 2.01 .14 .03

Iming .11 Iming .07

ISOpen .20 ISOpen .22†

Media Retrievability

Task .04

Media Retrievability

Task .07

Interaction Term .19

N = 66

Page 233: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

215

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

81 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 2.36 .13† - 81 AvgPC&VidPPlay .14 2.28 .15 .02

Iming .13 Iming .10

MTS ISOpenness .23* MTS ISOpenness .16

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .03

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .06

Interaction Term .18

N = 70

DV = MTS Task SMM

82 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 4.21 .22** - 82 AvgPC&VidPPlay .21† 3.36 .22 .00

Iming -.18 Iming -.18

MTS ISOpenness .42** MTS ISOpenness .43**

Media Retrievability

Social -.10

Media Retrievability

Social -.19

Interaction Term .10

N = 65

Page 234: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

216

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Task SMM

83 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 4.72 .24** - 83 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24* 3.84 .24 .00

Iming -.17 Iming -.16

MTS ISOpenness .39** MTS ISOpenness .38**

Media Retrievability

Task -.17

Media Retrievability

Task -.19

Interaction Term -.08

N = 65

84 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24* 4.43 .22** - 84 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 3.56 .22 .0

Iming -.19 Iming -.18

MTS ISOpenness .36** MTS ISOpenness .39**

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.16

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.17

Interaction Term -.07

N = 68

Page 235: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

217

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Team SMM

85 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 .57 .04 - 85 AvgPC&VidPPlay .08 .50 .04 .00

Iming .13 Iming .13

MTS IS Openness -.18 MTS IS Openness -.18

Media Retrievability

Social .08

Media Retrievability

Social .20

Interaction Term -.14

N = 59

86 AvgPC&VidPPlay .08 .50 .04 - 86 AvgPC&VidPPlay .08 .49 .04 .0

Iming .11 Iming .13

MTS IS Openness -.16 MTS IS Openness -.17

Media Retrievability

Task .03

Media Retrievability

Task .02

Interaction Term -.09

N = 59

Page 236: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

218

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Team SMM

87 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 .37 .03 - 87 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 .43 .04 .01

Iming .13 Iming .15

MTS IS Openness -.12 MTS IS Openness -.06

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .01

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .01

Interaction Term -.13

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 237: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

219

Table 27: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Openness and MTS TMS Relationship Moderated by Media Richness

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

88 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 8.82 .37** - 88 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 6.96 .37 .0

Iming .22† Iming .23†

MTS ISOpenness .51** MTS IS Openness .52**

Media Richness -.07 Media Richness -.08

Interaction Term .03

N = 64

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 238: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

220

Table 28: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS SMM Relationship Moderated by Media Richness

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

89 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 2.46 .16† - 89 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 2.05 .17 .01

Iming .26† Iming .23

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.06

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.06

Media Richness .15 Media Richness .18

Interaction Term -.10

N = 55

90 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20 2.81 .18* - 90 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20 2.22 .18 .0

Iming .27† Iming .26†

MTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team -.15

MTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team -.16

Media Richness .14 Media Richness .14

Interaction Term -.04

N = 55

Page 239: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

221

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Task SMM

91 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22 1.90 .13 - 91 AvgPC&VidPPlay .21 1.62 .14 .01

Iming -.15 Iming -.13

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .26†

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .28*

Media Richness .09 Media Richness .07

Interaction Term .11

N = 54

92 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 1.84 .13 - 92 AvgPC&VidPPlay .15 1.48 .13 .0

Iming -.13 Iming -.12

MTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team .26†

MTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team .28†

Media Richness .09 Media Richness .09

Interaction Term .06

N = 54

Page 240: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

222

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Team SMM

93 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.14 .47 .04 - 93 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.14 .49 .05 .01

Iming .03 Iming .00

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .01

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams -.02

Media Richness .15 Media Richness .16

Interaction Term -.12

N = 49

94 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.11 .63 .05 - 94 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.10 .51 .06 .01

Iming .03 Iming .03

MTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team -.12

MTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team -.13

Media Richness .14 Media Richness .14

Interaction Term -.05

N = 49

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 241: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

223

Table 29: Moderator Analyses: The MTS IS Uniqueness and MTS TMS Relationship Moderated by Media Richness

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

95 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 2.23† .13 - 95 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 1.91 .14 .01

Iming .24** Iming .32*

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .03

MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .02

Media Richness -.03 Media Richness -.01

Interaction Term -.10

N = 64

96 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 2.26 .13† - 96 AvgPC&VidPPlay .14 2.09 .15 .02

Iming .34** Iming .33**

MTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team -.05

MTS IS Uniqueness Within

Team -.06

Media Richness -.03 Media Richness -.02

Interaction Term -.14

Page 242: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

224

N = 64

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 243: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

225

Table 30: Lagged Correlation Analysis of MTS Behavior and MTS Cognition Relationship

1 2 4 5 6

1. MTS IS Unique Between Team Time 1 -

2. MTS IS Unique Within Team Time 1 - -

4. MTS TMS Time 2 .01 .01 -

5. MTS Goal SMM Time 2 .10 .01 - -

6. MTS Task SMM Time 2 .26* .22† - - -

7. MTS Team SMM Time 2 .05 -.07 - - -

1 2 4 5 6

1. MTS IS Unique Between Team Time 3 -

2. MTS IS Unique Within Team Time 3 - -

4. MTS TMS Time 2 .20† .10 -

5. MTS Goal SMM Time 2 .16 .09 - -

6. MTS Task SMM Time 2 .34** .31** - - -

7. MTS Team SMM Time 2 .06 .05 - - -

Note. N= 61-82, * = p <.05; ** = p <.01

Page 244: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

226

Table 31: Analyses Regressing MTS TMS on MTS Efficacy

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

97 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 3.14 .07* - 97 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.01 12.00 .31** .24**

Iming .23* Iming .15

MTS TaskEfficacy .51**

N = 82

98 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 3.14 .07* - 98 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 10.53 .29** .22**

Iming .23* Iming .24*

MTS Taskforce Efficacy-

tm .46**

N = 82

99 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 3.14 .07* - 99 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 9.02 .26** .19**

Iming .23* Iming .22*

MTS Taskforce Efficacy .43**

N = 82

Page 245: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

227

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

100 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 3.14 .07* - 100 AvgPC&VidPPlay .08 7.29 .22** .15**

Iming .23* Iming .18†

MTS Bandura Percent .38**

N = 82

101 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 3.14 .07* - 101 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 2.07 .07 .00

Iming .23* Iming .23*

MTS Bandura Yes/No .01

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 246: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

228

Table 32:Analyses Regressing MTS Performance on MTS TMS

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Efficiency

102 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 6.76 .15** - 102 AvgPC&VidPPlay .23* 5.78 .18† .03†

Iming .25* Iming .20†

MTS TMS .20†

N = 82

DV = MTS Effectiveness

103 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22* 3.18 .07* - 103 AvgPC&VidPPlay .19† 4.62 .15** .08**

Iming .13 Iming .06

MTS TMS .29**

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 247: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

229

Table 33: Mediator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by MTS IS Uniqueness

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

104 Path A .20† .11

Path B 4.75 1.26

TOTAL EFFECT MTS TMS 2.45† 1.33

DIRECT EFFECT 1.87 1.24

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .95 .50 -.04 2.03

N = 82

105 Path A .10 .11

Path B 1.46 1.29

TOTAL EFFECT MTS TMS 2.45† 1.33

DIRECT EFFECT 2.33† 1.33

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .15 .26 -.19 1.11

Page 248: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

230

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; Path A = IVMediator and Path B = Mediator DV Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

106 Path A .20† .11

Path B .92 .31

TOTAL EFFECT MTS

TMS

.83** .31

DIRECT EFFECT .72* .30

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS

IS Uniqueness Between

Teams .18 .11 .03 .48

N = 82

107 Path A .10 .11

Path B .40 .30

TOTAL EFFECT MTS

TMS

.83** .31

DIRECT EFFECT .80** .32

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS

IS Uniqueness Within .04 .06 -.03 .27

Page 249: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

231

Team

N = 82

Table 34: Analyses Regressing MTS SMM on MTS Trust

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

108 AvgPC&VidPPlay .18 2.68 .07† - 108 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 2.43 .10 .03

Iming .18 Iming .20†

MTS Trust TF .16

N = 70

DV = MTS Task SMM

109 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 2.26 .06 - 109 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24† 1.73 .07 .01

Iming -.09 Iming -.09

MTS Trust TF .10

N = 68

Page 250: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

232

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Team SMM

110 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .35 .01 - 110 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 .43 .02 .01

Iming .10 Iming .10

MTS Trust -.10

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 251: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

233

Table 35: Analyses Regressing MTS Performance on MTS SMM

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Efficiency

111 AvgPC&VidPPlay .26* 5.03 .13** - 111 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24* 3.64 .14 .01

Iming .22† Iming .20†

MTS GoalSMM .11

N = 70

112 AvgPC&VidPPlay .27* 6.49 .16** - 112 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24* 4.75 .18 .02

Iming .26* Iming .27*

MTS Task SMM .13

N = 68

113 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 5.71 .15** - 113 AvgPC&VidPPlay .28* 4.25 .18 .03

Iming .25* Iming .28*

MTS Team SMM -.14

N = 61

Page 252: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

234

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Effectiveness

114 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24* 2.76 .08† - 114 AvgPC&VidPPlay .24† 1.83 .08 .0

Iming .10 Iming .10

MTS GoalSMM .03

N = 70

115 AvgPC&VidPPlay .19 3.11 .09* - 115 AvgPC&VidPPlay .13 3.69 .15* .06*

Iming .19 Iming .21†

MTS Task SMM .25*

N = 68

116 AvgPC&VidPPlay .27* 3.66 .11* - 116 AvgPC&VidPPlay .28* 3.05 .14 .03

Iming .16 Iming .18

MTS Team SMM -.16

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 253: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

235

Table 36: Mediator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by MTS IS Openness

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

117 Path A .27* .11

Path B 3.57 1.54

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Goal SMM 1.39 1.50

DIRECT EFFECT .57 1.49

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Openness .95 .56 .12 2.48

N = 70

118 Path A .35 .12

Path B 3.80 1.51

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Task SMM 1.63 1.48

DIRECT EFFECT .22 1.53

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Openness 1.34† .72 .23 3.15

N = 68

Page 254: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

236

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

119 Path A -.07 .12

Path B 4.91 1.67

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Team SMM -1.72 1.53

DIRECT EFFECT -1.23 1.45

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Openness -.34 .67 -2.00 .81

N = 61

DV= MTS Effectiveness

120 Path A .27 .11

Path B 1.18 .35

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Goal SMM .08 .36

DIRECT EFFECT -.19 .34

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Openness .32† .14 .10 .67

N = 70

Page 255: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

237

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

121 Path A .35** .12

Path B 1.03** .35

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Task SMM .75* .35

DIRECT EFFECT .36 .36

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Openness .36* .16 .13 .73

N = 68

122 Path A -.07 .12

Path B .92* .40

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Team SMM -.47 .35

DIRECT EFFECT -.37 .34

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Openness -.06 .13 -.35 .16

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; Path A = IVMediator and Path B = Mediator DV

Page 256: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

238

Table 37: Moderator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS IS Uniqueness Relationship Moderated by Media Retrievability

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

123 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 2.99 .14* - 123 AvgPC&VidPPlay .13 3.41 .19* .05*

Iming .21† Iming .26*

TMS .14 TMS .01

Media Retrievability Social .14 Media Retrievability Social .15

Interaction Term -.26*

N = 78

124 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 2.58 .12* - 124 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 2.42 .14 .02

Iming .20† Iming .21†

TMS .14 TMS .13

Media Retrievability Task -.05 Media Retrievability Task -.07

Interaction Term -.14

N = 78

Page 257: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

239

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

125 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 2.62 .12* - 125 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 2.42 .14 .02

Iming .21† Iming .22*

MTS TMS .12 MTS TMS .19

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.06

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.08

Interaction Term -.15

N = 82

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

126 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 .69 .04 - 126 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 .92 .06 .02

Iming -.03 Iming .00

MTS TMS .12 MTS TMS .03

Media Retrievability Social .11 Media Retrievability Social .11

Interaction Term -.18

N = 78

Page 258: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

240

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

127 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 .87 .05 - 127 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 1.11 .07 .02

Iming -.05 Iming -.04

MTS TMS .11 MTS TMS .10

Media Retrievability Task -.14 Media Retrievability Task -.17

Interaction Term -.17

N = 78

128 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 .73 .04 - 128 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 .95 .06 .02

Iming .01 Iming .02

MTS TMS .07 MTS TMS .15

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.14

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.16

Interaction Term -.17

N = 82

Page 259: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

241

Table 38: Moderator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS IS Openness Relationship Moderated by Media Richness

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS ISOpenness

129 AvgPC&VidPPlay .08 1.21 .09 - 129 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 1.71 .15† .06†

Iming .25 Iming .20

MTS Goal MM .06 MTS Goal MM .13

Media Richness -.01 Media Richness .04

Interaction Term -.26†

N = 55

130 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 3.89 .24** - 130 AvgPC&VidPPlay .01 3.19 .25 .01

Iming .28* Iming .25†

MTS Task MM .38** MTS Task MM .38**

Media Richness .09 Media Richness .09

Interaction Term -.10

N = 54

Page 260: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

242

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS ISOpenness

131 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 2.81 .20* - 131 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 2.25 .20 .0

Iming .28* Iming .29*

MTS Team MM -.25† MTS Team MM -.24†

Media Richness .18 Media Richness .17

Interaction Term .06

N = 49

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 261: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

243

Table 39: Analyses Regressing MTS SMM on MTS Efficacy

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

132 AvgPC&VidPPlay .18 2.68 .07† - 132 AvgPC&VidPPlay .14 2.80 .11† .04†

Iming .18 Iming .15

MTS TaskEfficacy .20†

N = 70

133 AvgPC&VidPPlay .18 2.68 .07† - 133 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 4.69 .17** .10**

Iming .18 Iming .21†

MTS Taskforce Efficacy-

tm .32**

N = 70

134 AvgPC&VidPPlay .18 2.68 .07† - 134 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 3.29 .13* .06*

Iming .18 Iming .19†

N = 70 MTS Taskforce Efficacy .24*

Page 262: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

244

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Goal SMM

135 AvgPC&VidPPlay .18 2.68 .07† - 135 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 2.41 .10 .03

Iming .18 Iming .16

MTS Bandura Percent .16

N = 70

136 AvgPC&VidPPlay .18 2.68 .07† - 136 AvgPC&VidPPlay .20† 2.50 .10 .03

Iming .18 Iming .14

MTS Bandura Yes/No .17

N = 70

DV = MTS Task SMM

137 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 2.26 .06 - 137 AvgPC&VidPPlay .23† 1.78 .08 .02

Iming -.09 Iming -.11

MTS TaskEfficacy .11

N = 68

Page 263: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

245

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Task SMM

138 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 2.26 .06 - 138 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 1.55 .07 .01

Iming -.09 Iming -.09

MTS Taskforce Efficacy-

tm -.05

N = 68

139 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 2.26 .06 - 139 AvgPC&VidPPlay .26* 1.57 .07 .01

Iming -.09 Iming -.08

MTS Taskforce Efficacy -.06

N = 68

140 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 2.26 .06 - 140 AvgPC&VidPPlay .26* 1.49 .06 .0

Iming -.09 Iming -.09

MTS Bandura Percent -.02

N = 68

Page 264: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

246

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Task SMM

141 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25* 2.26 .06 - 141 AvgPC&VidPPlay .26* 1.50 .07 .01

Iming -.09 Iming -.09

MTS Bandura Yes/No .03

N = 68

DV = MTS Team SMM

142 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .35 .01 - 142 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 .37 .02 .01

Iming .10 Iming .11

MTS TaskEfficacy -.09

N = 61

143 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .35 .01 - 143 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 .32 .02 .01

Iming .10 Iming .10

MTS Taskforce Efficacy-

tm -.07

N = 61

Page 265: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

247

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS Team SMM

144 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .35 .01 - 144 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 .30 .02 .01

Iming .10 Iming .10

MTS Taskforce Efficacy -.06

N = 61

145 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .35 .01 - 145 AvgPC&VidPPlay .03 .26 .01 .0

Iming .10 Iming .09

MTS Bandura Percent .04

N = 61

146 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .35 .01 - 146 AvgPC&VidPPlay .02 1.05 .05 .04

Iming .10 Iming .05

MTS Bandura Yes/No .21

N = 61

Page 266: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

248

Table 40: Analyses Regressing MTS TMS on MTS Trust

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS TMS

147 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 3.14 .07* - 147 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 6.86 .21** .14**

Iming .23* Iming .25*

MTS Trust .37**

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 267: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

249

Table 41: Mediator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by MTS IS Openness

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

148 Path A .58** .09

Path B 3.56* 1.54

TOTAL EFFECT MTS TMS 2.45 1.33

DIRECT EFFECT .45 1.54

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Openness 2.11* .90 .36 3.87

N = 82

DV= MTS Effectiveness

149 Path A .58** .09

Path B .79* .37

TOTAL EFFECT MTS TMS .83** .31

DIRECT EFFECT .40 .37

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Openness .46* .18 .12 .85

Page 268: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

250

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; Path A = IVMediator and Path B = Mediator DV

Page 269: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

251

Table 42: Mediator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS Performance Relationship Mediated by MTS IS Uniqueness

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

150 Path A .16 .12

Path B 5.21** 1.44

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Goal SMM 1.39 1.50

DIRECT EFFECT .92 1.38

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness Between

Teams .81 .64 -.20 2.43

N = 70

151 Path A .33 .11

Path B 4.99 1.52

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Task SMM 1.63 1.48

DIRECT EFFECT -.01 1.47

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness Between

Teams 1.65* .71 .48 3.29

N = 68

Page 270: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

252

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

152 Path A .06 .13

Path B 5.61** 1.44

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Team SMM -1.72 1.53

DIRECT EFFECT -1.87 1.37

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .33 .84 -1.20 2.16

N = 61

153 Path A .09 .12

Path B 1.25 1.48

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Goal SMM 1.39 1.50

DIRECT EFFECT 1.31 1.50

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .11 .30 -.18 1.16

N = 70

Page 271: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

253

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Efficiency

154 Path A .31** .11

Path B 1.68 1.52

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Task

SMM

1.63 1.48

DIRECT EFFECT 1.10 1.55

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Within Team .52 .55 -.40 1.72

N = 68

155 Path A .05 .13

Path B 3.09* 1.54

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Team

SMM

-1.72 1.53

DIRECT EFFECT -1.89 1.49

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Within Team .16 .44 -.57 1.26

N = 61

Page 272: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

254

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

156 Path A .16 .11

Path B 1.17** .35

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Goal SMM .08 .36

DIRECT EFFECT -.03 .34

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Between Teams .18 .16 -.06 .58

N = 70

157 Path A .33** .11

Path B 1.09** .36

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Task SMM .75* .35

DIRECT EFFECT .39 .35

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Between Teams .36 .18 .10 .84

N = 68

Page 273: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

255

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

158 Path A .06 .13

Path B 1.05 .35

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Team SMM -.47 .35

DIRECT EFFECT -.49 .33

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness

Between Teams .06 .16 -.24 .42

N = 61

159 Path A .09 .12

Path B .50 .35

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Goal SMM .08 .36

DIRECT EFFECT .04 .36

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS Uniqueness

Within Team .05 .07 -.05 .25

N = 70

Page 274: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

256

Bootstrap Coefficients

Model Coefficient SE Lower 95% BCa Upper 95% BCa

DV= MTS Effectiveness

160 Path A .31** .11

Path B .48 .36

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Task SMM .75* .35

DIRECT EFFECT .60 .37

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Within Team .15 .14 -.03 .55

N = 68

161 Path A .05 .13

Path B .70* .36

TOTAL EFFECT MTS Team SMM -.47 .35

DIRECT EFFECT -.51 .15

INDIRECT EFFECT MTS IS

Uniqueness Within Team .04 .11 -.13 .34

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01; Path A = IVMediator and Path B = Mediator DV

Page 275: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

257

Table 43: Moderator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS IS Openness Relationship Moderated by Media Retrievability

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

162 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.00 11.50 .38** - 162 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 9.16 .39 .01

Iming .16† Iming .09

MTS TMS .57** MTS TMS .59**

Media Retrievability Social .16† Media Retrievability Social .15†

Interaction Term .06

N = 78

163 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 10.39 .36** - 163 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 8.21 .36 .0

Iming .08 Iming .08

MTS TMS .57 MTS TMS .57

Media Retrievability Task .02 Media Retrievability Task .02

Interaction Term .01

N = 78

Page 276: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

258

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

164 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 10.35 .35** - 164 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 8.19 .35 .0

Iming .12 Iming .11

MTS TMS .55** MTS TMS .54**

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .02

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .03

Interaction Term .02

N = 82

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 277: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

259

Table 44: Moderator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS IS Uniqueness Relationship Moderated by Media Retrievability

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

165 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 1.89 .11 - 165 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 1.56 .11 .00

Iming .22† Iming .22†

MTS Goal MM -.01 MTS Goal MM -.01

Media Retrievability Social .17 Media Retrievability Social .26

Interaction Term -.11

N = 66

166 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 5.03 .25** - 166 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 4.42 .27 .02

Iming .28* Iming .26*

MTS Task SMM .36** MTS Task SMM .36**

Media Retrievability Social .16 Media Retrievability Social .29*

Interaction Term -.20

N = 65

Page 278: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

260

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

167 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25† 2.11 .13† - 167 AvgPC&VidPPlay .25† 1.66 .13 .00

Iming .20 Iming .20

MTS Team SMM -.02 MTS Team SMM -.03

Media Retrievability Social .13 Media Retrievability Social .13

Interaction Term -.01

N = 59

168 AvgPC&VidPPlay .17 1.37 .08 - 168 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 1.09 .08 .00

Iming .20 Iming .20

MTS Goal SMM .02 MTS Goal SMM .02

Media Retrievability Task -.03 Media Retrievability Task -.03

Interaction Term -.03

N = 66

Page 279: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

261

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

169 AvgPC&VidPPlay .07 4.36 .22** - 169 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 4.41 .27† .05

Iming .26* Iming .25*

MTS Task SMM .37** MTS Task SMM .35**

Media Retrievability Task .03 Media Retrievability Task -.04

Interaction Term -.23*

N = 65

170 AvgPC&VidPPlay .26* 1.82 .12 - 170 AvgPC&VidPPlay .27* 1.43 .12 .00

Iming .19 Iming .19

MTS Team SMM -.02 MTS Team SMM -.01

Media Retrievability Task .00 Media Retrievability Task -.00

Interaction Term .02

N = 59

Page 280: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

262

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

171 AvgPC&VidPPlay .15 1.62 .09 - 171 AvgPC&VidPPlay .14 1.36 .10 .01

Iming .19 Iming .20

MTS Goal SMM .09 MTS Goal SMM .15

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.05

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.06

Interaction Term -.09

N = 70

172 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 3.86 .19** - 172 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 3.77 .23† .04†

Iming .27* Iming .25*

MTS Task SMM .33** MTS Task SMM .44**

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.01

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.07

Interaction Term -.23†

N = 68

Page 281: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

263

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

173 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 1.78 .11 - 173 AvgPC&VidPPlay .22† 1.40 .11 .00

Iming .22† Iming .22†

MTS Team SMM .03 MTS Team SMM .03

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.03

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.03

Interaction Term -.01

N = 61

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

174 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 .47 .03 - 174 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 .37 .03 .00

Iming .03 Iming .03

MTS Goal SMM -.02 MTS Goal SMM -.02

Media Retrievability Social .14 Media Retrievability Social .14

Interaction Term -.01

N = 66

Page 282: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

264

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

175 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 2.52 .14* - 175 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 2.35 .16 .02

Iming -.05 Iming -.07

MTS Task SMM .34** MTS Task SMM .34**

Media Retrievability Social .14 Media Retrievability Social .27†

Interaction Term -.20

N = 65

176 AvgPC&VidPPlay .12 .54 .04 - 176 AvgPC&VidPPlay .16 1.18 .10† .06†

Iming -.10 Iming -.04

MTS Team SMM .01 MTS Team SMM .09

Media Retrievability Social .12 Media Retrievability Social -.06

Interaction Term .32†

N = 59

Page 283: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

265

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

177 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 .77 .05 - 177 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 .84 .07 .02

Iming -.00 Iming .01

MTS Goal SMM .01 MTS Goal SMM .02

Media Retrievability Task -.19 Media Retrievability Task -.21†

Interaction Term -.14

N = 66

178 AvgPC&VidPPlay .02 2.39 .14† - 178 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.02 3.30 .22* .08**

Iming -.07 Iming -.10

MTS Task SMM .32** MTS Task SMM .30*

Media Retrievability Task -.11 Media Retrievability Task -.20

Interaction Term -.30*

N = 65

Page 284: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

266

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

179 AvgPC&VidPPlay .14 .65 .05 - 179 AvgPC&VidPPlay .15 .56 .05 .00

Iming -.10 Iming -.09

MTS Team SMM .02 MTS Team SMM .05

Media Retrievability Task -.14 Media Retrievability Task -.15

Interaction Term .07

N = 59

180 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 .88 .05 - 180 AvgPC&VidPPlay .06 1.09 .08 .03

Iming .04 Iming .05

MTS Goal SMM .07 MTS Goal SMM .19

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.18

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.20†

Interaction Term -.21

N = 70

Page 285: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

267

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

181 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.02 2.03 .11 - 181 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.04 2.65 .17* .06*

Iming -.02 Iming -.04

MTS Task SMM .29* MTS Task SMM .44**

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.12

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.20

Interaction Term -.30*

N = 68

182 AvgPC&VidPPlay .11 .60 .04 - 182 AvgPC&VidPPlay .12 .50 .04 .00

Iming -.07 Iming -.06

MTS Team SMM .06 MTS Team SMM .03

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.16

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.16

Interaction Term .06

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 286: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

268

Table 45: Moderator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS IS Openness Relationship Moderated by Media Retrievability

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

183 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 2.26 .13† - 183 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 1.80 .13 .0

Iming .20 Iming .20

MTS Goal SMM .17 MTS Goal SMM .16

Media Retrievability Social .19 Media Retrievability Social .13

Interaction Term .07

N = 66

184 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.02 5.47 .26** - 184 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 4.49 .27 .01

Iming .29** Iming .28*

MTS Task SMM .39** MTS Task SMM .39**

Media Retrievability Social .21† Media Retrievability Social .29*

Interaction Term -.12

N = 65

Page 287: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

269

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

185 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 2.55 .16* - 185 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 2.07 .16 .0

Iming .29* Iming .28*

MTS Team SMM -.16 MTS Team SMM -.18

Media Retrievability Social .21† Media Retrievability Social .26†

Interaction Term -.08

N = 59

186 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 1.59 .09 - 186 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 1.94 .14 .05

Iming .17 Iming .15

MTS Goal SMM .20 MTS Goal SMM .19

Media Retrievability Task -.02 Media Retrievability Task .01

Interaction Term .22†

N = 66

Page 288: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

270

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

187 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.02 4.34 .22** - 187 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.02 3.48 .23 .01

Iming .27* Iming .27*

MTS Task SMM .40** MTS Task SMM .40**

Media Retrievability Task .05 Media Retrievability Task .04

Interaction Term -.06

N = 65

188 AvgPC&VidPPlay .13 1.83 .12 - 188 AvgPC&VidPPlay .12 1.48 .12 .0

Iming .27* Iming .26*

MTS Team SMM -.15 MTS Team SMM -.17

Media Retrievability Task -.06 Media Retrievability Task -.06

Interaction Term -.07

N = 59

Page 289: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

271

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

189 AvgPC&VidPPlay .03 2.04 .11† - 189 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 2.16 .14 .03

Iming .17 Iming .16

MTS Goal SMM .24* MTS Goal SMM .10

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.04

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.02

Interaction Term .23

N = 70

190 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.03 4.09 .20** - 190 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.03 3.24 .20 .0

Iming .29** Iming .29**

MTS Task SMM .37** MTS Task SMM .39**

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .03

Media Retrievability

Aggregate .02

Interaction Term -.04

N = 68

Page 290: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

272

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Openness

191 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 1.87 .12 - 191 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 1.54 .12 .0

Iming .20* Iming .29

MTS Team SMM -.11 MTS Team SMM -.07

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.08

Media Retrievability

Aggregate -.07

Interaction Term -.08

N = 61

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 291: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

273

Table 46: Moderator Analyses: The MTS SMM and MTS IS Uniqueness Relationship Moderated by Media Richness

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

192 AvgPC&VidPPlay .27* 1.77 .12 - 192 AvgPC&VidPPlay .27* 1.51 .13 .01

Iming .19 Iming .18

MTS Goal SMM .02 MTS Goal SMM .04

Media Richness -.08 Media Richness -.06

Interaction Term -.10

N = 55

193 AvgPC&VidPPlay .12 3.19 .20* - 193 AvgPC&VidPPlay .12 2.61 .21 .01

Iming .29* Iming .26†

MTS Task SMM .32* MTS Task SMM .32*

Media Richness -.09 Media Richness -.08

Interaction Term -.09

N = 54

Page 292: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

274

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

194 AvgPC&VidPPlay .30* 1.59 .12 - 194 AvgPC&VidPPlay .29* 1.40 .14 .02

Iming .15 Iming .16

MTS Team SMM -.03 MTS Team SMM -.02

Media Richness -.03 Media Richness -.05

Interaction Term .12

N = 49

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

195 AvgPC&VidPPlay .03 .21 .02 - 195 AvgPC&VidPPlay .04 .33 .03 .01

Iming -.02 Iming .01

MTS Goal SMM .08 MTS Goal SMM .05

Media Richness -.11 Media Richness -.14

Interaction Term .13

N = 55

Page 293: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

275

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

196 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.10 5.30 .30** - 196 AvgPC&VidPPlay -.10 4.17 .30 .00

Iming -.06 Iming -.05

MTS Task SMM .54** MTS Task SMM .54**

Media Richness -.07 Media Richness -.07

Interaction Term .03

N = 54

197 AvgPC&VidPPlay .09 .39 .03 - 197 AvgPC&VidPPlay .10 .35 .04 .01

Iming -.18 Iming -.19

MTS Team SMM .00 MTS Team SMM -.01

Media Richness .00 Media Richness .02

Interaction Term -.07

N = 49

Page 294: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

276

Table 47: Moderator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS IS Openness Relationship Moderated by Media Richness

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS ISOpenness

198 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 7.59 .34** - 198 AvgPC&VidPPlay .00 6.09 .34 .0

Iming .05 Iming .04

MTS TMS .54** MTS TMS .54**

Media Richness .10 Media Richness .10

Interaction Term -.07

N = 64

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 295: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

277

Table 48: Moderator Analyses: The MTS TMS and MTS IS Uniqueness Relationship Moderated by Media Richness

Step 1 Step 2

Model Variable b F R2 ∆R

2 Model Variable b F R

2 ∆R

2

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Between Teams

199 AvgPC&VidPPlay .26* 2.61 .15* - 199 AvgPC&VidPPlay .26* 2.23 .16 .01

Iming .16 Iming .15

MTS TMS .13 MTS TMS .13

Media Richness -.02 Media Richness -.01

Interaction Term -.11

N = 64

DV = MTS IS Uniqueness Within Team

200 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 .77 .05 - 200 AvgPC&VidPPlay .05 .70 .06 .01

Iming -.15 Iming -.16

MTS TMS .22 MTS TMS .21

Media Richness .04 Media Richness .04

Interaction Term -.09

N = 64

Note. † = p<.10, * = p<.05; ** = p<.01

Page 296: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

278

APPENDIX C

SESSION TIMELINE

Page 297: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

279

Session Timeline

10 min 20 min 10 min 35 min 25 min

Practice

Transition

Practice

Action

Performance

Transition Performance

Action 1

Performance

Action 2 End

45 min

Role

Training

• Efficacy

• Trust

• TMS

• SMM

• IS Open

• IS Unique

• Virtuality

• IS Unique

• Virtuality

• Efficiency

• Effectiveness

Page 298: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

280

APPENDIX D

PERFORMANCE MISSION INTELLIGENCE DISTRIBUTION

Page 299: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

281

Performance Mission Intelligence

Intelligence PR PS SR SS

A4 – Tire Fire (238, 305) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

A4 – Van (232, 207) Non-Hotspot Intel O O X X

A9 – AFV (244, 432) Hotspot Intel X X O O

A9 – AFV (321, 149) Hotspot Intel X O X O

A9 – Tent (178, 432) Hotspot Intel O X X X

C3 – Barrel (247, 229) Hotspot Intel X O X O

C3 – Tire Fire (110, 233) Hotspot Intel X X O X

C3 – Tire Fire (397, 234) Hotspot Intel O O X X

C7 – Checkpoint (102, 446) Non-Hotspot Intel X X O O

C7 – RPG (106, 334) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

E1 – Radio Tower (304, 436) Hotspot Intel X O O X

E1 – Tire Fire (235, 82) Hotspot Intel O O X O

E1 – Tire Fire (442, 203) Hotspot Intel O X O X

E1 – Van (314, 180) Hotspot Intel O X X O

E2 – AFV (394, 241) Non-Hotspot Intel X O X O

E2 – Civilian (420, 150) Non-Hotspot Intel X O O X

E4 – Radio Tower (406, 129) Non-Hotspot Intel X O X O

E4 – Sedan (225, 267) Non-Hotspot Intel X O O X

E8 – Civilian (117, 445) Hotspot Intel O X X O

E8 – Humanitarian Tent (100,

156) Hotspot Intel X O O X

E8 – RPG (74, 86) Hotspot Intel O X O X

E8 – RPG (89, 379) Hotspot Intel X O O O

F10 – Radio Tower (113, 436) Hotspot Intel X X O O

Page 300: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

282

F10 – Sedan (223, 265) Hotspot Intel O X O X

F10 – Tire Fire (276, 97) Hotspot Intel X X X X

F10 – Tire Fire (91, 324) Hotspot Intel O O O X

F3 – AFV (119, 412) Hotspot Intel O X O O

F3 – AFV (356, 178) Hotspot Intel X X X X

F3 – Checkpoint (135, 349) Hotspot Intel O O X X

F3 – Civilian (415, 178) Hotspot Intel O X O X

F4 – RPG (127, 411) Non-Hotspot Intel O O X O

F4 – AFV (366, 186) Non-Hotspot Intel O X O X

F4 – Civilian (367, 82) Non-Hotspot Intel X O O O

G4 – Radio Tower (88, 152) Hotspot Intel O X O O

G4 – Tire Fire (438, 326) Hotspot Intel X O O X

G4 – Tire Fire (62, 325) Hotspot Intel O X X O

G4 – Van (245, 332) Hotspot Intel X O X O

G8 – AFV (52, 304) Hotspot Intel O X X O

Intelligence PR PS SR SS

G8 – Civilian (115, 291) Hotspot Intel X O O X

G8 – Civilian (345, 285) Hotspot Intel X O X O

G8 – RPG (432, 304) Hotspot Intel O O O X

G9 – Sedan (125, 414) Non-Hotspot Intel O O O X

G9 – Tire Fire (221, 295) Non-Hotspot Intel O X O X

G9 – Tire Fire (296, 429) Non-Hotspot Intel O X X O

G9 – Tire Fire (71, 300) Non-Hotspot Intel O O X O

H3 – Checkpoint (431, 107) Hotspot Intel X X X X

H3 – Humanitarian Tent (98, 411) Hotspot Intel O O X O

H3 – RPG (100, 357) Hotspot Intel X X O O

Page 301: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

283

H3 – RPG (428, 279) Hotspot Intel X O X O

H4 – Fire (121, 284) Non-Hotspot Intel O X O X

H4 – Radio Tower (420, 106) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

H4 – Radio Tower (74, 117) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

H4 – Van (334, 270) Non-Hotspot Intel O X X O

H5 – AFV (356, 381) Non-Hotspot Intel O X O O

H5 – AFV (74, 233) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

H5 – Checkpoint (360, 440) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

H5 – Checkpoint (57, 141) Non-Hotspot Intel O X O X

H7 – Barrel (240, 349) Hotspot Intel X O X O

H7 – Tire Fire (236, 299) Hotspot Intel X O O O

H7 – Tire Fire (69, 428) Hotspot Intel X X X X

H7 – Tower (427, 295) Hotspot Intel O O X X

H9 – Barrel (222, 215) Hotspot Intel X O O O

H9 – Tire Fire (176, 155) Hotspot Intel X O O X

H9 – Tire Fire (212, 298) Hotspot Intel O X X O

H9 – Tire Fire (337, 182) Hotspot Intel O X O X

I1 – AFV (237, 109) Hotspot Intel O X X O

I1 – AFV (385, 377) Hotspot Intel X O O X

I1 – Civilian (231, 65) Hotspot Intel X O O O

I1 – Civilian (379, 422) Hotspot Intel O X O X

I3 – Checkpoint (329, 71) Non-Hotspot Intel X O X X

I3 – Checkpoint (87, 267) Non-Hotspot Intel O O O X

I3 – Civilian (328, 305) Non-Hotspot Intel O O X O

I3 – RPG (93, 322) Non-Hotspot Intel O X X O

I4 – Radio Tower (197, 90) Hotspot Intel X O O X

Page 302: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

284

I4 – Radio Tower (357, 94) Hotspot Intel X O X O

I4 – Radio Tower (70, 84) Hotspot Intel O X X O

I4 – Sedan (404, 241) Hotspot Intel O X O X

I4 – Tire Fire (203, 282) Hotspot Intel X X X X

I4 – Tire Fire (347, 325) Hotspot Intel X X O O

I4 – Tire Fire (97, 337) Hotspot Intel O O O X

Intelligence PR PS SR SS

I8 – AFV (103, 326) Hotspot Intel O X O O

I8 – AFV (401, 290) Hotspot Intel X X X X

I8 – Checkpoint (433, 215) Hotspot Intel O X O X

I8 – Humanitarian Tent (63, 361) Hotspot Intel X O X X

I8 – Humanitarian Tent (97, 92) Hotspot Intel O X X O

I8 – RPG (102, 169) Hotspot Intel O O X X

I9 – AFV (198, 421) Non-Hotspot Intel O O X X

I9 – AFV (66, 425) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

I9 – Checkpoint (352, 87) Non-Hotspot Intel O O X X

I9 – RPG (357, 155) Non-Hotspot Intel X X O O

J6 – Radio Tower (304, 54) Non-Hotspot Intel X X O O

J6 – Radio Tower (367, 135) Non-Hotspot Intel O O X X

J6 – Radio Tower (416, 60) Non-Hotspot Intel X X O O

J6 – Tire Fire (62, 220) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

J6 – Van (182, 218) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

J7 – Checkpoint (423, 224) Hotspot Intel O O X X

J7 – Civilian (169, 26) Hotspot Intel X X O O

J7 – RPG (282, 43) Hotspot Intel O O X O

J7 – RPG (428, 279) Hotspot Intel X X X X

Page 303: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

285

J7 – RPG (91, 216) Hotspot Intel O O O X

J8 – Barrel (417, 260) Hotspot Intel X X O O

J8 – Radio Tower (434, 94) Hotspot Intel X X X X

J8 – Radio Tower (96, 313) Hotspot Intel O O X X

J8 – Tire Fire (103, 235) Hotspot Intel O X O O

J8 – Tire Fire (225, 200) Hotspot Intel X O O O

J9 – Radio Tower (231, 49) Non-Hotspot Intel X O O O

J9 – Radio Tower (235, 137) Non-Hotspot Intel O X O O

J9 – Radio Tower (52, 134) Non-Hotspot Intel X O O X

J9 – Radio Tower (55, 57) Non-Hotspot Intel X O X O

J9 – Sedan (254, 213) Non-Hotspot Intel X X X X

Note. X = Individual is provided this piece of intelligence via mission briefings from

command at the start of the mission. O = Individual does not have this piece of intelligence. He

or she may

Page 304: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

286

APPENDIX E

INTERNAL REVIEW BOARD HUMAN SUBJECTS PERMISSION

LETTER

Page 305: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

287

Page 306: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

288

Page 307: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

289

REFERENCES

(2012). Pamela [computer software]. Available from http://www.pamela.biz/.

(2012). Skype Premium (Version 5.3) [computer software]. Available from

http://www.skype.com.

Aiken, L.S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions.

Newbury, CA: Sage.

Akgun, A.E., Byrne, J., Keskin, H., Lynn, G.S., & Imamoglu, S.Z. (2005). Knowledge networks

in new product development projects: A transactive memory perspective. Information &

Management, 42, 1105-1120.

Aubé, C., & Rousseau, V. (2005). Team goal commitment and team effectiveness: The role of

task interdependence and supportive behaviors. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and

Practice, 93, 189-204.

Austin, J. R. (2003). Transactive memory in organizational groups: The effects of content,

consensus, specialization, and accuracy on group performance. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 88, 866-878.

Baard, S.K., Kozlowski, S.W.J., DeShon, R.P., …. (2012). Assessing team process dynamics: An

innovative methodology for team research. In G.F. Goodwin & A.H. DeCostanza, Get out

of the way! Unobtrusive measures of team constructs. Symposium accepted at the Annual

Conference of Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA.

Baltes, B., Dickson, M., Sherman, M., Bauer, C., & LaGanke, J. (2002). Computer mediated

Page 308: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

290

communication and group decision-making: A meta-analysis.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 87, 156–179.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.

Englewood Cliffs, NY: Prentice-Hall.

Beal, D.J., Cohen, R.R., Burke, M.J., & McLendon, C.L. (2003). Cohesion and performance in

groups: A meta-analytic clarification of construct relations. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 88, 989-1004.

Bell, B.S., & Kozlowski, S.W.J. (2002). A typology of virtual teams: Implications for effective

leadership. Group & Organization Management. 27, 14-49.

Borgatti, S., Everett, M. G., & Freeman, L. C. (1992). UCINET IV network analysis software.

Columbia, SC: Analytic Technologies.

Bunderson, S. J., & Sutcliffe, K. M. (2002). Comparing alternative conceptualizations of

functional diversity in management teams: Process and performance effects. Academy of

Management Journal, 45, 875 – 893.

Bunderson, J.S., & Sutcliffe, K.M. (2003). Mangment team learning orientation and business unit

performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 552-560.

Campion, M. A., Medsker, E. M., & Higgs, A. C. (1993). Relations between work group

characteristics and effectiveness: Implications for designing effective work groups.

Personnel Psychology, 46, 823 – 850

Campion, M. A., Papper, E. M., & Medsker, G. J. (1996). Relations between work team

Page 309: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

291

characteristics and effectiveness: A replication and extension. Personnel Psychology, 49,

429 – 452.

Cannon-Bowers, J. A., Salas, E., & Converse, S. (1993). Shared mental models in expert team

decision making. In N. J. Castellan (Ed.), Individual and group decision making: Current

issues (pp. 221-246). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Chen, G., Gully, S.M., & Eden, D. (2001). Validation of a new general self-efficacy scale.

Organizational Research Methods, 4, 62-83.

CNN, 1999 http://articles.cnn.com/1999-09-30/tech/9909_30_mars.metric_1_mars-orbiter-

climate-orbiter-spacecraft-team?_s=PM:TECH

Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/correlation

analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Colquitt, J.A., Scott, B.A., & LePine, J.A. (2007). Trust, trustworthiness, and trust propensity: A

meta-analytic test of their unique relationships with risk taking and job performance.

Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 909-927.

Contractor, N., & DeChurch, L.A. (2012). Leveraging digitial trace technologies to nderstand

network dynamics in teams. In G.F. Goodwin & A.H. DeCostanza, Get out of the way!

Unobtrusive measures of team constructs. Symposium accepted at the Annual Conference

of Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA.

Cooke, N. J., Kiekel, P. A., & Helm, E. E. 2001. Measuring team knowledge during skill

acquisition of a complex task.International Journal of Cognitive Ergonomics, 5: 297-315.

Costa, A.C. (2003). Work team trust and effectiveness. Personnel Review, 32, 605-622.

Page 310: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

292

Costa, AC., Roe, R.A and Taillieu, T. (2001), "Trust within teams: the relation with

performanceeffectiveness", European Journal of Work and Organizational Psychology,

Vol. 10 No.3,pp.225-44.

Cramton, C.D. (2001). The mutual knowledge problem and its consequences for dispersed

collaboration. Organization Science, 12, 346-371.

Davison, R.B., & Hollenbeck, J.R. (2011). Boundary spanning in the domain of multiteam

systems. In S.J. Zaccaro, M.A. Marks, & L.A. DeChurch (Eds.), Multiteam systems:An

organizational form for dynamic and complex environments. Taylor & Francis.

Davison, R. B., Hollenbeck, J. R., Barnes, C. M., Sleesman, D. J., & Ilgen, D. R. (2011,

December 26). Coordinated Action in Multiteam Systems. Journal of Applied Psychology,

Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/a0026682

DeCostanza, A.H,. & Dirosa, G. (2012). Unobtrusively tracking army unit cohesion over time. In

G.F. Goodwin & A.H. DeCostanza, Get out of the way! Unobtrusive measures of team

constructs. Symposium accepted at the Annual Conference of Society for Industrial and

Organizational Psychology, San Diego, CA.

DeDreu, C.K.W., Nijstad, B.A., van Knippenberg, D. (2008). Motivated information processing

in group judgment and decision making. Personality Social Psychology Review, 12, 22-

49.

DeChurch, L.A. (2002). Team’s leader teams: Examining the role of leadership in multi-team

systems.

DeChurch, L. A., & Marks, M. A. (2006). Leadership in multiteam systems. Journal of Applied

Page 311: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

293

Psychology, 91, 311 – 329.

DeChurch, L. A., & Mathieu, J. E. (2008). Thinking in terms of multiteam systems. In E. Salas,

G. F. Goodwin, & C. S. Burke, Team Effectiveness in Complex Organizations: Cross-

Disciplinary Perspectives and Approaches, pp. 267-292. Taylor & Francis: New York.

DeChurch, L. A., & Mesmer-Magnus, J. R. (2010). The cognitive underpinnings of effective

teamwork: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95, 32 - 53.

DeChurch, L. A., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2010). Teams won’t solve this problem. Human Factors, 52,

329 – 334.

DeChurch, Burke, Mathieu, Resick, Murase, Jimenez, Doty, & Benishek, (2010).

DeSanctis, G., Monge, P. (1999). Introduction to the special issue: Communication processes for

virtual organizations. Organization Science, 10, 693-703.

Driskell, J.E., Salas, E., & Johnston, J. (1999). Does stress lead to a loss of team perspective?

Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 3, 291-302.

Edwards, B. D., Day, E. A., Arthur, W., & Bell, S. T. (2006). Relationships among team ability

composition, team mental models, and team performance. Journal of Applied Psychology,

91, 727 – 736.

Faraj, S., & Sproull, L. (2000). Coordinating expertise in software development teams.

Management Science, 46, 1554-1568.

Gibson, C. B., & Cohen, S. G. (2003). Virtual Teams That Work: CreatingConditions for

VirtualCollaboration Effectiveness.San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Gibson, C.B., & Gibbs, J.L., (2006). Unpacking the concept of virtuality: The effects of

Page 312: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

294

geographic dispersion, electronic dependence, dynamic structure, and national diversity on

team innovation. Administrative Science Quarterly, 51, 451-495.

Gibson, C.B. (1999). Do they do what they believe they can? Group efficacy and group

effectiveness across tasks and cultures. Academy of Management Journal, 42, 138-152.

Goodwin, G.F., Essens, P.J.M.D., Smith, D. (2012). Multiteam Systems in the Public Sector. In

S.J. Zaccaro, M.A. Marks, & L.A. DeChurch (Eds.), Multiteam systems:An organizational

form for dynamic and complex environments. New York., NY: Taylor & Francis.

Gully, S.M., Devine, D.J., & Whitney, D.J. (1995). A meta-analysis of cohesion and performance:

Effects of Level of analysis and task interdependence. Small Group Research, 26, 497-

520.

Gully, S.M., Incalcaterra, K.A., Joshi, A., & Beaubien, J.M. (2002). A meta-analysis of team-

efficacy, potency, and performance: Interdependence and level of analysis as moderators

of observed relationships. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 819-832.

Hackman, J. R. (1987). The design of work teams. In L. Hoch (Ed.), Handbook of organizational

behavior (pp. 315–342). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Hackman, J. R. (2002). Leading teams: Setting the stage for great performances. Boston: HBS

Press.

He, J., Butler, B. S., & King, W. R. (2007). Team cognition: Development and evolution in

software project teams. Journal of Management Information Systems, 24(2), 261-292.

*Hedlund, J., Ilgen, D. R., & Hollenbeck, J. R. (1998). Decision accuracy in computer-mediated

Page 313: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

295

versus face-to-face decision-making teams. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes,76, 30–47.

Hiltz, S.R., Johnson, K., & Turoff, M. (1986). Experiments in group decision making:

Communication processes and outcomes in face-to-face versus computerized conferences.

Human Communication Research, 13, 225-252.

Hinds, P.J., & Weisband, S.P. (2003). Knowledge Sharing and Shared Understanding in Virtual

Teams. In CB Gibson & SG Cohen, Virtual Teams That Work: Creating Conditions For

Virtual Team Effectiveness. pp. 21-36. Josey Bass: San Francisco.

Hollingshead, A.B., & Brandon, D.P. (2003). Potential benefits of communication in transactive

memory systems. Human Communication Research, 29, 607-615.

Ilgen, D. R., Hollenbeck, J. R., Johnson, M., & Jundt, D. (2005). Teams in organizations: From

input-process-output models to IMOI models. Annual Review Psychology, 56, 517 – 543.

Jarvenpaa, S.L., & Leidner, D.E. (1999). Communication and trust in global virtual teams.

Organization Science, 10, 791-815.

Jarvenpaa, S.L., Knoll, K., & Leidner, D.E. (1998). Is anybody out there? Antecedents of trust in

global virtual teams. Journal of Management Information Systems, 14, 29-64.

Kanawattanachai, P., & Yoo, Y. (2007). The impact of knowledge coordination on

virtual team performance over time. MIS Quarterly, 31, 783–808.

Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (1989). Motivation and cognitive abilities: An integrative/

aptitude-treatment interaction approach to skill acquisition. Journal of Applied

Psychology—Monograph, 74, 657–690.

Page 314: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

296

Keyton, J., Ford, D.J., & Smith, F.L. (2012). Communication, collaboration, and identification as

faciltators and constraints of multiteam systems. In S.J. Zaccaro, M.A. Marks, & L.A.

DeChurch (Eds.), Multiteam systems:An organizational form for dynamic and complex

environments. New York., NY: Taylor & Francis.

Kim, P.H., Dirks, K.T., Cooper, C.D., & Ferrin, D.L. (2006). When more blame is better than

less: The implications of internal vs. external attributions for the repair of trust after a

competence -vs-integrity-based trust violation. Organizational Behavior and Human

Decision Process, 99, 49-65.

Kirkman, B.L., & Mathieu, J.E. (2005). The dimensions and antecedents of team virtuality.

Journal of Management, 31, 700-718.

Kirkman, B.L., Rosen, B., Tesluk, T., & Gibson, C. (2004). The impact of team empowerment on

virtual team performance: The moderating role of face-to-face interaction. Academy of

Management Journal, 47, 175-192.

Klimoski, R., & Mohammed, S. (1994). Team mental model: Construct or metaphor? Journal of

Management, 20, 403 – 437.

Kozlowski, S. W. J., & Ilgen, D. R. (2006). Enhancing the effectiveness of work groups and

teams. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7, 77 – 124.

Krackhardt, D. 1988 Predicting with Networks: A Multiple Regression Approach to

Analyzing Dyadic Data Social Networks December lO(4): 359--381.

Krackhardt, D. (1992). The strength of strong ties: The importance of philos in organizations.

In N. Nohria, & R. Eccles (Eds.), Networks and organizations: Structure, form and

Page 315: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

297

action(pp. 216-239). Boston: Harvard Business School Express.

Kramer, R.M. (1999). Trust and distrust in organizations: Emerging perspectives, enduring

questions. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 569-598.

Langferd, C.W. (2004). Too much of a good thing? Negative effects of high trust and individual

autonomy in self-managing teams. Academy of Management, 47 (3), 385-399.

Lenth, R. V. (2006-9). Java Applets for Power and Sample Size [Computer software]. Retrieved

June 14, 2012, from http://www.stat.uiowa.edu/~rlenth/Power.

Lewis, K. (2003). Measuring transactive memory systems in the field: Scale development and

validation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 587 – 604.

Lim, B.C., & Klein, K.J. (2006). Team mental models and team performance: A field study of

the effects of team mental model similarity and accuracy. Journal of Organizational

Behavior, 27, 403-418.

Liu, B., & Zang, Z. (2010). The mediating effects of team efficacy on the relationship between a

transactive memory system and team performance. Social Behavior and Personality, 38,

865-870.

MacKinnon, D.P., Fairchild, A.J., & Fritz, M.S. (2007). Mediation analysis. Annual Review of

Psychology, 58, 593-614.

MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation,

confounding and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1(4), 173-181.

Marks, M. A., DeChurch, L. A., Mathieu, J. E., Panzer, F. J., & Alonso, A. (2005). Teamwork in

multiteam systems. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 964 – 971.

Page 316: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

298

Marks, M.A., Luvison, D. (2012). Product launch and strategic alliance MTSs. In S.J. Zaccaro,

M.A. Marks, & L.A. DeChurch (Eds.), Multiteam systems:An organizational form for

dynamic and complex environments. New York., NY: Taylor & Francis.

Marks, M. A., Mathieu, J. E., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2001). A temporally based framework and

taxonomy of team processes. Academy of Management Review, 26, 356 – 376.

Marks, M. A., Sabella, M. J., Burke, C. S., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2002). The impact of cross-training

on effectiveness. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 2 – 13.

Marks, M. A., Zaccaro, S. J., & Mathieu, J. E. (2000). Performance implications of leader

briefings and team-interaction training for team adaptation to novel environments. Journal

of Applied Psychology, 85, 971 – 986.

Mathieu, J.E. (2011). Reflections on the evolution of the multiteam systems concept and a look to

the future. In SJ Zacarro, MA Marks, and LA DeChurch eds. Multiteam systems: An

organizational form for dynamic and complex environments. Taylor & Francis.

Mathieu, J. E., Cobb, M. A., Marks, M. A., Zaccaro, S. J., & Marsh, S. (2004). Multiteam

ACES: A research platform for studying multi-team systems. In S. G. Schiflett, L.

R. Elliott, E. Salas, & M. Coovert (Eds.), Scaled worlds: Development, validation and

applications (pp. 297–315). Burlington, VT: Ashgate.

Mathieu, J.E., Gilson, L.L., & Ruddy, T.M. (2006). Empowerment and team effectiveness: An

empirical test of an integrated model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 97-108.

Mathieu, J. E., Heffner, T. S., Goodwin, G. F., Cannon-Bowers, J. A., & Salas, E. (2005). Scaling

the quality of teammates’ mental models: equifinality and normative comparisons. Journal

Page 317: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

299

of Organizational Behavior, 26, 37 – 56.

Mathieu, J. E., Heffner, T. S., Goodwin, G. F., Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A. (2000). The

influence of shared mental models on team process and performance. Journal of Applied

Psychology, 85, 273 – 283.

Mathieu, J. E., Marks, M. A., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2001). Multi-team systems. In N. Anderson, D.

Ones, H. K. Sinangil, & C. Viswesvaran (Eds.), International handbook of work and

organizational psychology (pp. 289–313). London: Sage Publications.

Mathieu, J. E., Maynard, M. T., Rapp, T., & Gilson, L. (2008). Team effectiveness 1997 – 2007:

A review of recent advancements and a glimpse into the future. Journal of Management,

34, 410 – 476.

Mathieu, J.E., Rapp, T.L., Maynard, M.T., & Mangos, P.M. (2010). Integrative effects of team

and task shared mental models as related to air traffic controllers’ collective efficacy and

effectiveness. Human Performance, 23, 22-40.

Mayer, R. C., Davis, J. H., & Schoorman, F. D. (1995). An integrative model of organizational

trust. Academy of Management Review, 20, 709-734.

McGrath JE, Arrow H, Berdahl JL. 2000. Thestudy of groups: past, present, and future.

Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 4:95–105.

McGrath, J.E. (1964). Social psychology: A brief introduction. New York:

Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Mesmer-Magnus, J.R., & DeChurch, L.A. (2009). Information sharing and team performance: A

meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94, 535-546.

Page 318: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

300

Mesmer-Magnus, J.R., DeChurch, L.A., Jiménez, M., Wildman, J.L., & Shuffler, M.L. (2011).

Teams in virtual organizations: A meta-analytic examination of virtuality and information

sharing. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115, 214-225.

Mohammed, S., & Angell, L.C. (2004). Surface-and deep-level diversity in work groups:

exaiming the moderating effects of team orientation and team process on relationship

conflict. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25 1015-1039.

Mohammed, S., & Dumville, B. 2001. Team mental models in a team knowledge framework:

Expanding theory and

measurement across disciplinary boundaries. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22: 89-

106.

Mohammed, S., Ferdanzi, L., & Hamilton, K. (2010). Metaphor no more: A 15 year review of the

team mental model construct. Journal of Management, 36, 879 – 910.

Moreland, R. L., Argote, L., & Krishnan, R. (1996). Socially shared cognition at

work:Transactive memory and group performance. In J. L. Nye & A. M. Brower (Eds.),

What's social about social cognition? Research on socially shared cognition in small

groups (pp. 57-84). Thousand Parks, CA.: Sage.

Murase, T., Sanz, E., Jiménez, M., Benishek, L. E., DeChurch, L. A., Salas, E., & Martinez, D.

(2010, June). Teams in the 21st century: Progress and perspective for the IPO model.

Presented at the 4th Annual Conference of INGRoup, DC.

Peltokorpi, V. (2008). Transactive memory systems: Review and extension. European

Psychologist, 17, 11-20.

Page 319: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

301

Poole, M.S., & Contractor, N. (2011). Conceptualizing the multiteam system as an ecosystem of

networked groups. In S.J. Zaccaro, M.A. Marks, & L.A. DeChurch (Eds.), Multiteam

systems:An organizational form for dynamic and complex environments. Taylor &

Francis.

Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects

in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,

36(4), 717-731.

Rentsch, J. R., & Klimoski, R. J. (2001). Why do ‘great minds’ think alike? Antecedents of team

member schema agreement. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22, 107 – 120.

Ren, Y. & Argote, L. (2011). Transactive memory systems 1985-2010: An integrative framework

of key dimensions, antecedents, and consequences. The Academy of Management Annals,

5, 189-229.

Rico, R., Alcover, CM, Sachez-Manzanares, M., & Gil, F. (2009). The joint relationship of

communication behaviors and task interdependence on trust building and change in virtual

project teams. Social Science Information, 48(2), 229–255.

Ridings, C.M., Gefen, D., & Arinze, B. (2002). Some antecedents and effects of trust in virtual

communities. Journal of Strategic Information Systems, 11, 271-295.

Saavedra, R., Early, P.C., & Van Dyne, L. (1993). Complex interdependence in task-performing

groups. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 61-72.

Salas, E., Rosen, M.A., Burke, C.S., & Goodwin, G.F. (2009). The wisdom of collectives in

organizations: An update of the teamwork competencies. In E. Salas, G. F. Goodwin, & C.

Page 320: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

302

S. Burke, Team Effectiveness in Complex Organizations: Cross-Disciplinary Perspectives

and Approaches, pp. 267-292. Taylor & Francis: New York.

Salas, E., Sims, D.E., & Burke, C.S. (2005). Is there a big five in teamwork? Small Group

Research, 36, 555-599.

Serva, M.A., Fuller, M.A., & Mayer, R.C. (2005). The reciprocal nature of trust: A longitudinal

study of interacting teams. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, 625-648.

Shea, G.P., & Guzzo, R.A. (1987). Groups as human resources. In K.M.Rowland & G.R. Ferris

(Eds.), Research in personnel and humanresource management (Vol. 5, pp. 323–356).

Greenwich, CT: JAIPress.

Sierra Entertainment(2007)

Smith-Jentsch, K.A., Kraiger, K., Cannon-Bowers, J.A., & Salas, E. (2009). Do familiar

teammates request and accept more backup? Transactive memory in air traffic control.

Human Factors, 51, 181-192.

Staples, D. S., & Webster, J. (2008). Exploring the effects of trust, task

interdependence, and virtualness on knowledge sharing in teams. Information

Systems Journal, 18, 617–640.

Stasser, G., & Titus, W. (1985). Pooling of unshared information in group decision making:

Biased information sampling during discussion. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 48, 1467-1478.

Stasser, G. D., & Titus, W. (1987). Effects of information load and percentages of

shared information on the dissemination of unshared information during group

Page 321: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

303

discussion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 81–93.

Stasser, G., Taylor, L. A., & Hanna, C. (1989). Information sampling in

structured and unstructured discussions of three- and six-person groups.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 67–78.

Stewart, G. L. (2006). A meta-analytic review of relationships between team design features and

team performance. Journal of Management, 32, 29 – 55.

Thompson, L.L. (2004). Making the team: A guide for managers (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River,

NJ: Pearson.

Thompson, L.L. (2010). Making the team: A guide for managers (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River,

NJ: Pearson.

Webley, K. (2010). Top 10 NASA Flubs. TIME.

http://www.time.com/time/specials/packages/article/0,28804,1982672_1982673_1982667,

00.html

Wegner, D. M. (1986). Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group mind. In B.

Mullen & G. R. Goethals (Eds.), Theories of group behavior (pp. 185-208). New York:

Springer-Verlag.

Wegner, D. M. (1995). A computer network model of human transactive memory. Social

Cognition,13, 319-339.

Wegner, 1987Wegner, D. M. 1987. Transactive memory: A contemporary analysis of the group

mind. In I. B. Mullen & G. R. Goethals(Eds.), Theories of group behavior: 185-208. New

York: Springer-Verlag.

Page 322: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

304

Wegner, D.M., Giuliano, T., & Hertel, P. (1985). Cognitive interdependencein close relationships.

In W.J. Ickes (Ed.), Compatibleand incompatible relationships (pp. 253–276). New York:

Springer-Verlag.

Weisband, S. (1992). Group discussion and first advocacy effects in computer-mediated and face-

to-face decision making groups. Organizational and Human Decision Processes, 53, 352-

380.

Yoo, Y., & Kanawattanachai, P. (2001). Developments of transactive memory systems and

collective mind in virtual teams. The International Journal of Organizational Analysis, 9,

187-208.

Yuan,Y.C., Fulk, J., Monge, P.R., & Contractor, N. (2010). Expertise directory development,

shared task interdependence, and strength of communication network ties and multilevel

predictors of expertise exchange in transactive memory work groups. Communication

Research, 37, 20-47.

Zaccaro, S.J., Blair, V. , Peterson, C., & Zazanis, M. (1995). Collective efficacy. In J.E. Maddux

(Ed.), Self-Efficacy, Adaptation, and Adjustment: Theory , Resarch, and Application. New

Work, NY: Plenum.

Zaccaro, S.J., Gualtieri, J., Minionis, D. (1995). Task cohesion as a facilitator of team decision

making under temperal urgency. Military Psychology, 777-93.

Zaccaro, S.J., Marks, M.A., & DeChurch, L.A., eds. (2011). Multiteam systems: An introduction.

In S.J. Zaccaro, M.A. Marks, & L.A. DeChurch (Eds.), Multiteam systems:An

organizational form for dynamic and complex environments. New York., NY: Taylor &

Page 323: Two Pathways To Performance: Affective- And Motivationally

305

Francis.

Zhang, Z., Hempel, P. S., Han, Y., & Tjosvold, D. (2007). Transactive memory system links work

team characteristics and performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 1722 – 1730.