turner toward a general sociological theory of emotion

Upload: mariezetorres

Post on 08-Apr-2018

226 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    1/30

    Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour29:200218308

    Toward a General Sociological Theory ofEmotions

    JONATHAN H. TURNER

    Over the last two decades, the sociology of emotions has become one of the

    leading edges of micro-level theorizing. Building upon this theoretical momentum,

    this paper draws together key ideas from diverse theories and presents a synthetic

    theory of emotional dynamics in encounters of face-to-face interaction. Emphasis

    is on overt emotional arousalthat is, on emotional states visible to the parties

    involved. Such arousal shapes the mutual signaling and interpreting of gestures

    between individuals, especially gestures that can be read visually; and in so

    doing, emotionally-laden gesturing determines the flow of interaction. Yet, as

    will be emphasized, emotional displays are only surface manifestations of

    complicated and covert emotional dynamics, operating through the neurology

    of the brain. The theory to be developed pulls together arguments of several

    theoretical traditions, including mainstream symbolic interactionism (e.g., Mead,

    1934; McCall & Simmons, 1966; Stryker, 1980; Stryker & Serpe, 1982; Burke,

    1980, 1991; Heise, 1979; Smith-Lovin & Heise, 1988; Smith-Lovin, 1990),

    psychoanalytic variants of symbolic interactionism (Scheff, 1988; Scheff &

    Retzinger, 1992; Turner, 1994), interaction ritual theory (Collins, 1988, 1993),

    dramaturgical analysis (Goffman, 1959, 1967, 1974; Hochschild, 1979), powerand status theories (Collins, 1975; Kemper, 1978; Kemper & Collins, 1990),

    and expectation-states theories (Berger & Zelditch, 1985, 1998; Berger, Webster,

    Ridgeway & Rosenholtz, 1986; Wagner & Berger, 1993; Wagner & Turner,

    1998). My view is that each of these approaches has captured an important

    dynamic of emotions as they influence the flow of face-to-face interaction, but

    at the same time, none has delineated the full range of emotional forces operating

    on participants during an encounter.

    In Figure 1, a very skeletal outline of the argument is presented. For any

    given cycle of emotional arousal, the process begins with expectations frommany diverse sources; in turn, these expectations are collated as part of the

    definition of a situation into a generalized expectation about what will or should

    occur in the encounter. Self is a special type of expectation because when its

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999. Published by Blackwell Publishers 108 Cowley

    Road, Oxford, OX4 1JF UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA.

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    2/30

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    3/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 135

    expectation-state reserach has been conducted in task-oriented experimental

    groups, although there has been some effort to move to less controlled situations

    (e.g., Berger, 1988). In Table 1, some of the forces generating expectations in

    natural settings are listed. This listing does not mean, of course, that parties to an

    encounter always have well-defined expectations before they interact; obviously,people can enter interactions with few or vague expectations about what is to

    occur; and under these conditions, individuals only begin to form expectations

    as the interaction unfolds. And even when expectations are well defined before

    individuals enter the encounter, expectations may change, although there are

    limits and patterns to change as the large literature on expectation sttes

    documents. Thus, Table 1 only emphasizes that as expectations formwhether

    before, during, or after an episode of face-to-face interactionthey will become

    organized along the dimensions listed in the Table. That is, individuals will

    develop expectations with respect to demography, structure, culture, and trans-

    actional needs; and as will be emphasized shortly, individuals will also bring to

    an encounter, and develop within the flow of the interaction, expectations about

    their own self as well as the selves of others. For the moment, however, let me

    review just the forces enumerated in Table 1.

    (1) Demographic forces revolve around the number and nature of individuals in

    an encounter, particularly a focused encounter but an unfocused one as well

    (Goffman, 1961). The expectations for a situation are influenced by the number

    of individuals present, and equally important, the categories of individuals as

    friends, intimates, and strangers who exhibit other characteristics like gender,

    ethnicity, class, and age. Affect control theory (Heise, 1979; Smith-Lovin &

    Heise, 1988; Heise & Smith-Lovin, 1981; Heise & Smith-Lovin, 1995) is correct,

    I think, in emphasizing that others are also assessed with respect to their likely

    level of activity, their potency, and their relevance as objects of evaluation.

    (2) Structural forces include the nature, number and relationships among status

    positions that can become implicated in an encounter. As Kemper & Collins

    (1990) have stressed, power (authority) and status (prestige) considertions are the

    driving force behind the activation of emotions. As Table 1 suggests, power andstatus are only one set of many forces generating expectations about what

    individuals can and should do, a conclusion that follows from expectation-states

    theory (e.g., Ridgeway & Smith-Lovin, 1994). Moreover, power and status in a

    situation exert much of their influence on emotions by making self highly salient;

    that is, when encounters place individuals in hierarchies of power or status, they

    become more self aware. And as the salience of self increases, so does the

    potential for emotional reactions to behaviors that do not affirm the power and

    status orders (Ridgeway, 1982, 1994). Thus, it may be that the effect emphasized

    by Kemper & Collins (1990) of power and status on many emotions is due tothe increasing salience of self in power/status encounters. In addition to power

    and status, the density of connections among positions is important, because

    high density network ties create expectations that are very different than those

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    4/30

    136 Jonathan H. Turner

    Table 1. Sources of Expectations

    1 Demographic forces: the number and nature (categories) of others present in anencounter, especially with respect to the imputation of their activity, potency, andevaluation.

    2 Structural forces: the nature, number, and relationships among status positionsimplicated in an encounter, particularly with respect to:

    a differentiation of power/authority among positions or actorsb differentiation of prestige/honor among positions or actorsc density of connections among positions or actorsd diffuse status characteristics that actors in positions can claim and use

    3 Cultural forces: sets of symbols that individuals are cognizant of, or that they intendto invoke in an encounter, especially with respect to:

    a the appropriate ways of behaving (norms) and evaluating such behavior (values)b the appropriate type and intensity of sanctions to be employed

    c the appropriate type and intensity of sentiments, or feeling rulesd the appropriate type of ritualse the appropriate frames to be keyed and rekeyed

    4 Transaction forces: the intensity of, and relative proportions among, need states thatindividuals seek to meet in an encounter, especially with respect to:

    a the need for profitable pay-offs of relevant resourcesb the need for predictability in the actions of othersc the need to trust that others will meet obligations and expectationsd the need for intersubjectivity in order to presume that others experience a

    situation in similar ways, or if different, that these differences can be understoode the need to feel implicated and included in the ongoing flow of interaction in an

    encounter.

    for low density networks. At a minimum, as Collins (1975) has emphasized, high

    density networks make conformity to normative expectations more likely and

    sanctioning more effective. Finally, as expectation-states theory has documented,

    authority, prestige, and other social characteristics of actors outside the encounter

    are often brought into the interactions as diffuse status characteristics; and

    these characteristics will influence the power-status hierarchy and the behaviorsof individuals in this hierarchy.

    (3) Cultural forces are those systems of symbols that provide instructions and

    standards for behavior among those participating in an encounter. Some are

    behavioral or normative standards about how self and others are expected to

    behave; others are more evaluative and stress value standards about what self

    and others should do in a situation. Additional cultural sets indicate how

    demographic, structural, and self forces are to be deployed in an encounter.

    Related to both normative and value standards are sets of symbols indicating

    the appropriate type of sanctions (both positive and negative) as well as the formand intensity of sanctioning to be employed in a situation. A very special set of

    normative and evaluational standards is, as Hochschild (1979, 1983) first

    emphasized, the feeling rules or the expectations that individuals have about

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    5/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 137

    the nature and intensity of emotions that can, or should, be displayed in an

    encounter. Yet another set of symbols defines the appropriate use of rituals, or

    stereotyped sequences of behavior that guide the flow of interaction (Goffman,

    1967). Ritualized opening, closing, repairing, forming, tracking, keying, framing,

    totemizing and other interpersonal processes are conducted through rituals(Turner, 1988), with normative and value standards circumscribing the range of

    rituals to be employed in an encounter. Finally, again following Goffman (1974),

    cultural codes dictate frames demarking what is to be part of the interaction and,

    conversely, what is to be excluded; and particularly important, I think, are what I

    have termed body frames (the relevance of body to the flow of interaction) and

    personal information frames (the relevance of biography, intimacy, and self

    involvement) to be used to structure the boundaries of what is inside and outside

    the flow of interaction in an encounter (Turner, 1996b). All of these cultural forces,

    then, create expectations about what is to transpire in a situation.

    (4) Transactional forces are fundamental need-states that individuals activate in

    all encounters and that they expect to be met during the course of interaction.

    Previously, I saw these needs as what motivates individuals (Turner, 1987, 1988,

    1994), but more recently, I see motivation as a much more inclusive process,

    revolving around the emotional dynamics to be examined in this paper. Yet,

    individuals do have what I now term transactional needs, or needs for receiving

    payoffs of relevant resources (typically in accordance with cultural standards of

    equity and justice), for feeling that they can predict the responses of others, for

    trusting others to meet their obligations, for presuming (however inaccurately)

    that self and other are experiencing the world in similar ways for the purposes

    of the interaction, and for sensing that self is minimally included in the ongoing

    flow of the interaction. These transactional needs are listed in Table 1 in their

    order of importance to the rhythmic flow of an interaction and to individuals

    sense of satisfaction; and like the other forces in Table 1, they generate

    expectations about what should transpire in the encounter.

    In sum, we can begin to develop a theory of emotions by recognizing the

    importance of expectations. As a general proposition, we can say that the greateris the number of expectations that form an individuals definition of a situation

    and the higher is the salience of these expectations in an encounter, the greater

    is the potential for emotional arousal by an individual and for affective displays

    of emotions to others in the encounter. However, as we will see, this general

    proposition is qualified by the other forces outlined in Figure 1. For a review of

    the theory as a whole, consult the sixteen propositions in the conclusion.

    SELF AND IDENTITY

    My conceptualization of self follows the general line of argument in most

    theories within the symbolic interactionist tradition. Self is a set of images that

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    6/30

    138 Jonathan H. Turner

    individuals have about themselves with respect to (1) who they are, (2) what

    they can do, and (3) how they should be treated. Stated in this way, the

    formulation is rather vague and, hence, requires elaboration.

    The first issue to be addressed is whether or not self is a cognitive or emotional

    construct that individuals hold about themselves. So stated, the implied questionis rhetorical because the obvious answer is that self involves both cognitive and

    emotional elements. Indeed, as studies on the process of forming cognitions and

    remembering them now document, cognitions cannot be remembred without

    corresponding emotional tags (for accessible summaries of the neurological

    literature see: Damasio, 1994; Le Doux, 1996; Turner, 2000); and for a highly

    salient cognition like self, the cognitive and emotional dimensions are so

    neurologically embedded in each other that it is impossible conceptually and

    empirically to separate them. Thus for my purposes here, self is the activation

    of images about oneself as an object, both in the present and from long-term

    memory, variously charged with different types and valences of emotion.

    A second issue that needs to be addressed is the nature of these images.

    Symbolic interactionists have, at times, tended to emphasize self as a verbal

    construct. My view is that linguistic designations are only a special and limited

    property of self; and in fact, it would be impossible for self to be only verbal

    images because the human brain does not think in terms of conventional

    language. If humans did think only in words, they would seem very dim-witted,

    since language is an auditory mode of communication and, hence, sequential

    and very slow (Turner, 1996a, 1999). Humans are a primate and, by virtue of

    this fact, they are visually dominant (Maryanski & Turner, 1992:3368; Turner,

    1996a, 1996b, 2000); and I have little doubt that humans think in terms of

    patterns that better reflect the wiring of the brain around the visual sense

    modality that receives, processes, and stores information as gestalts. Indeed,

    regardless of the visual bias to thought, the neurology of consciousness in general

    appears to generate gestalts and configurations of sensory information. Sensory

    stimulation of visual, auditory, haptic, and olfactory receptors in the present are

    integrated in association cortices, such as the interior parietal lobule, and sentto working memory simultaneously, with this simultaneity giving consciousness

    the character of configurations of sensations and, if tagged with emotions from

    the activation of subcortical limbic systems, configurations of feelings as well

    (Geschwind, 1965a, 1965b; Geschwind & Damasio, 1984; Damasio, 1994). If

    working memory seeks to activate long-term memories, which have been created

    by the hippocampus in assigning emotional valences to past configurations of

    sensations and feelings, the process appears to involve stimulating clusters of

    neurons containing shorthand codes of an unknown nature that, once stimulated,

    organize a reactivation of sensory cortices in a manner that reproduces in anearly but far from exact way the previous inputs that had once entered

    working memory. This refiring of the sensory cortices leads to the simultaneous

    convergence of sensations and associated feelings in short-term or working

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    7/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 139

    memory, thereby imposing new images from long-term memory on those being

    immediately received in the present.

    Because primate neuroanatomy is organized in ways that make vision the

    dominant sense modality, it is most likely that the first languages among hominids

    were visual (e.g., hand, face, and body movements); and to this very day, humansrely upon visual cues as much as linguistic utterances in role taking and role

    making. The existence of two discrete areas on the left side of the brain

    Brocas area in the frontal lobe for speech production and Wernickes area next

    to the inferior parietal lobe for speech comprehensionindicates that the brain

    must use translation boxes to convert more gestalt-based brain thinking into

    sequential speech, although other areas along the Sylvian Fissure are also

    essential to speech comprehension and production (Kimura, 1993). Thus, self is

    mostly images of oneself couched in the brains way of thinking, revolving

    around coded neurons reactivating sensory cortices to reproduce images about

    self; and these images are organized as configurations of cognitive-emotional

    states. These images can be translated into linguistic modes, as when people

    talk to themselves. Indeed, this capacity to slow the process of thought down

    by translating brain thinking into linguistic constructions is important in

    understanding emotional dynamicsa point to which I will return.

    In this context, let me offer a side comment. One of the reasons that theories

    of self have proven so difficult to test empirically is that measurements rely on

    languagethat is, on utterances that represent translations of the brains way

    of thinking. In an old test like the Twenty Statements Test, for example,

    subjects were being asked to translate into words what they feel in the brains

    way of thinking, and given the nature of self images as cognitive-emotional

    constructs and the operation of defense mechanisms (to say nothing of diverse

    interpretations of the tests directions by subject), it is not surprising that TSTs

    only grossly measured some of the images that individuals have about themselves.

    Other tests of self, such as the use of the semantic differential in affect control

    theory, produce better results, but perhaps not for the reason that those who

    use the test generally assume (e.g., Heise, 1979; Smith-Lovin and Heise, 1988).In employing the semantic differential, subjects produce patterns of responses

    to dichotomous pairs of words which mix cognitions and affect. What makes

    this test more successful is the responses better reflect the way that the brain

    organizes information. Translation is still occurring but the product of this

    translation via Wernickes and Brocas area looks more like the way the brain

    configures cognitions (particularly given the way the statistical analyses of the

    tests configure individuals scores); and hence, we can have greater confidence

    in such an approach, although it is still a by-product of translating visually-

    organized images into words and, then via statistical analysis, back into scoresthat better approximate how the brain stores images and thoughts.

    A third related issue that has been hotly debated within interactionist circles

    is the way in which humans organize self images. One aspect of this issue is

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    8/30

    140 Jonathan H. Turner

    substantive: what are the dimensions along which cognitive-emotional images

    are arrayed? For example, in affect control theory, activity, potency, and

    evaluation are the critical dimensions; in other theories, we can find somewhat

    different, though overlapping, dimensions for arranging self images (e.g., Gecas,

    1989, 1991). For my purposes, however, I will resist the temptations to visualizesubstantive images or self as organized along a few dimensions, although as

    Table 1 suggests, humans probably organize expectations for others in this way.

    A related question about the organization of self images is structural: Do

    individuals organize images of themselves into hierarchies of identities (e.g.,

    McCall & Simmons, 1966; Stryker, 1980, 1991; Stryker & Serpe, 1982; Serpe

    & Stryker, 1987)? I suspect that some images of self are more salient and

    emotionally charged than others, but just which self images become salient is

    not just a reflection of clear rank ordering within the brain but also a response

    to purely situational cues activating particular images from long-term memory

    and, via working memory, integrating them with sensory images present in the

    immediate situation. Yet another question addresses how self images are organized

    and asks whether or not humans carry multiple identities and, relatedly, whether

    or not there are core identities (or self) and more peripheral and situational

    identities (or selves). My position is that individuals do have core selves or

    identities that they seek to confirm in all interaction situations; and these

    identities carry with them high-intensity emotional valences. Individuals also

    have situation-specific selves, tailored to images about who they are, what they

    can do, and what they can expect from others; and these too carry emotional

    tags but not to the same intensity as the emotions attached to core self. Moreover,

    there is a relatively high degree of consistency between core and peripheral

    selves, and if this is not the case, the individual suffers considerable emotional

    distress because of the incongruity (Higgins, 1987, 1989). Thus, in terms of how

    the cognitive-emotional images of self are organized, the theory to be developed

    here emphasizes that self is a series of emotional valences attached to images

    about oneself as an object, some of which are trans-situational and fundamental

    to a person and others of which are tied to particular types of situations. Ineither case, where others do not confirm self, emotions are aroused and defense

    mechanisms are activatedall of which influence the nature and level of covert

    and overt emotional arousal evident for a person during interaction (Higgins,

    1987, 1989; Heise, 1989; Smith-Lovin, 1990).

    A fourth issue is the behavioral responses that individuals make toward

    themselves as objects. One very important response to others and self is

    subcortical and involves activation of those of areas of the brain, sometimes

    loosely termed the limbic systems (Maclean, 1990), that generate unconscious

    emotional responses (Damasio, 1994; Le Doux, 1996). Another form of behavioralresponse is dramaturgical, revolving around the orchestration of gestures, both

    consciously and unconsciously, so as to elicit desired and expected responses

    from others or to achieve a particular goal or purpose (Goffman, 1959). Yet,

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    9/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 141

    another behavioral response is to react to oneself as an object before, during,

    and after interaction (Serpe, 1991). As I will argue, self reflection is a very

    important aspect of self, as all interactionist theories would emphasize, but it is

    important in a sense not always stressed by these theories. Much self reflection

    is, I believe, conducted in the brains mode of thinking and is not, therefore,linguistic (Turner, 1996a, 1996b); and in fact, self reflection is typically a blur

    of emotionally-charged images and feelings that are not so easily put into

    wordswhich is, perhaps, another way to phrase William James famous phrase,

    the stream of consciousness. This stream is being activated by various

    subcortical and unconscious limbic systems, thereby making emotionally-charged

    images about self surface manifestations of complex neurological forces that

    operate outside consciousness. The vast majority of self reflections in non-

    problematic interactions operates in this way, and for the most part, avoids the

    auditory mode of thought which immediately puts thinking into slow motion.

    Verbalized self reflection, as an internal conversation is, however, critical to

    understanding emotional arousal, especially since so many of the limbic forces

    causing the arousal are subcortical and, to an individual, not easily understood

    (although those interacting with an individual can often see the bodily changes

    occurring as subcortical limbic systems mobilize motor and visceral responses).

    Such self talk slows the process of thought and self reflection down so that

    individuals ponder why they are experiencing emotions and, if necessary, so that

    they can deal with problematic situations. Moreover, self talk gives individuals

    the ability to anticipate and re-live the emotions aroused in a situation,

    particularly when these emotions carry high valences. Additionally, and this is

    important for the theory that I seek to present, self talk can often delay and, if

    sufficiently frank and open, forestall the activation of defense mechanisms which

    will, if activated, compress and distort emotional responses.

    Yet, self talk can also sustain emotions that have been aroused in a situation,

    whether positive or negative; and if self talk is sufficiently vocal about negative

    experiences, emotions can be raised to a new amplitude, although eventually

    fatigue and defense mechanisms will lower the emotional voltage. When selfreflection about a situation allows an individual to re-experience positive

    outcomes of an interaction, however, the emotional energy generated can be

    sustained and periodically reactivated; and as Collins (1988) has emphasized,

    this kind of self reflection can become part of the particularized cultural capital

    of an individual and, hence, set up expectations for the next episode of

    interaction. The same is true for negative experiences that have been reflected

    upon, although in this case, such reflection will generally lower expectations

    unless defense mechanisms have so distorted the process of self talk that the

    person is no longer in touch with the interpersonal realities behind their ownexperiences. In this latter case, expectations can remain artificially high, thus

    setting off another round of negative experiences which only force the escalated

    use of defense mechanisms. But, as will be argued later, this use of defense

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    10/30

    142 Jonathan H. Turner

    mechanisms ultimately depresses emotional energy, along several fronts: repres-

    sion will eventually increase the valences for disappointment-sadness; projection

    will lead to aversion-fearful responses from others or aggressive-angry negative

    sanctioning from these others; and displacement of negative emotions onto

    others will elicit aggressive-angry forms of negative sanctioning from the victimsof such displacement.

    A final issue in the conceptualization of self is defense mechanisms, a topic to be

    examined in greater detail later. Defense mechanisms seek to protect self from

    unpleasant emotions, especially those directed at core self but peripheral selves as

    well. Repression, displacement, and projection all distort individuals perceptions

    of, and self reflections about, themselves in ways that change the overt emotional

    reactions and the level of emotional energy displayed in a situation. With some

    highly notable exceptions (e.g., Scheff, 1988; Scheff and Retzinger, 1992; Turner,

    1994), symbolic interactionists and many other micro theories have not adequately

    conceptualized the operation of defense mechanisms; and as we will see, they are

    central to a theory seeking to explain peoples level of emotional energy.

    Thus, when self is highly salient, the potential for activation of subcortical

    areas of the brain generating emotions increases. Just whether or not this

    potential is realized depends on the degree to which expectations in general and

    those about self in particular have been met during the course of interaction.

    To anticipate the theory summarized in the conclusions, when expectations have

    been met or exceeded and when others have offered positive sanctions, or both,

    people arouse a high level of positive effect towards themselves and others; and

    conversely, when expectations have not been met or others have given negative

    sanctions, or both, negative emotions toward self and others are activated.

    Attribution processes qualify this relationship. When success in meeting expecta-

    tions or receiving positive sanctions is attributed to self, self is evaluated positively,

    and individuals will sustain relations with others in the encounter; when success

    is attributed to internal states of others or categories of others, positive affect

    towards these others will be aroused; and when success is attributed to external

    causes in the situation, positive emotions will be of lower intensity. Conversely,when individuals do not meet expectations or receive negative sanctions from

    others, or both, negative affect towards self, and others ensues; and this process

    too is channelled by attributions. When self is blamed, powerful negative

    emotions toward self ensue; when internal states of others are seen as the cause

    of failure to meet expectations and receive negative sanctions, or both, aggressive

    emotions toward others will be evident; and when external causes are seen to

    be the cause, negative emotions toward self and others will be muted. Self talk

    can increase the intensity and duration of both positive and negative emotional

    arousal toward self and others; and when such self talk can target correctly thesources of emotional arousal, individuals will experience more positive emotions

    toward self and others in the present encounter and in other encounters as well.

    The activation of defense mechanisms generally lowers the modal level of overt

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    11/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 143

    emotional display by individuals, but increases the likelihood of short and

    periodic emotional outbursts. Of course, these generalizations, summarized by

    the propositions in the conclusions, get us ahead of the argument, but they give

    us a sense for the direction of the theory that I seek to develop.

    COLLATING EXPECTATIONS INTO A DEFINITION OF THE SITUATION

    Depending upon the situation, all or various subsets of the expectations listed

    in Table 1 can be operative. Moreover, these expectations are all interrelated.

    For example, transactional needs for payoffs in terms of standards or equity and

    justice are related to normative and value standards. Thus the listing in Table

    1 represents only a rough statement of research topics on expectation states

    rather than a refined and mutually exclusive categorization of variables. Yet, for

    the purposes of the argument to be developed, the critical consideration is that

    expectations exist along many dimensions, and humans can simultaneously

    activate all of these sources of expectations, although more typically some are

    more salient than others.

    Similarly, self as a special kind of expectation is also likely to be operative;

    and depending on the extent to which core or peripheral self is salient, the

    expectation-states for a situation will vary. Moreover, since the forces listed in

    Table 1 can increase the salience of selfas is the case, for example, when

    differentiation of power and prestige among interactants is high, when values

    dominate expectations, or when payoffs involve intrinsic resources from others.

    Equally important, multiple expectations are combined, as expectation-states

    research suggests (e.g., Wagner and Turner, 1998). Just how this combining occurs

    is not fully understood, but at some level, it represents a mechanism for simplifying

    actors definitions of the situation. In combining expectations, some become a

    more significant subset than others; and in terms of arousing emotional reactions,

    Kemper and Collins (1990) are probably correct in seeing authority and status as

    highly salient, but the effect of these structural forces operates, to some degree,through self and identity forces. Thus, authority/status and self would be more

    likely, I believe, to dominate the formation of a subset of expectations and, thereby,

    to circumscribe the inclusion or exclusion into the subset of other forces listed in

    Table 1. Still, this point should not be pushed too far; and for the theory presented

    here, self-expectations are the most influential on an individuals level of emotional

    arousal, use of defense mechanisms, and emotional energy.

    EXPECTATIONS AND EXPERIENCE

    Several theoretical traditions document that incongruities between what indi-

    viduals expect in a situation and what they experience generate emotional

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    12/30

    144 Jonathan H. Turner

    responses. For example, B.F. Skinner observed that pigeons under a reinforcement

    schedule appeared to become angry when not reinforced, a finding that

    Homans (1961) originally elaborated into a law of distributive justice (i.e.,

    rewards to individuals should be proportional to their investments relative to

    the investments of others in a situation) and later (Homans, 1974) an aggression-approval proposition (i.e., anger and punishment are likely responses to not

    receiving an expected reward, satisfaction-approval for receiving a reward, and

    happiness-approval for receiving more than expected). Molm (1991) in her

    ongoing research program on punishment in exchanges has reported that

    satisfaction and affect in exchanges of resources are mediated by expectations

    and that units of punishment have greater effects on actors satisfaction because

    rewards are more common than punishments, giving punishment a kind of

    shock (or surprise) value. This effect, however, is reduced as punishments

    become as frequent as rewards, although the basic relationship among expecta-

    tions, experiences, and emotions remains. Within the expectation states theoretical

    research program, it is consistently reported that emotions escalate when

    individuals do not honor the expectations associated with the distribution of

    status (Ridgeway, 1982, 1994; Ridgeway and Johnson, 1990; Berger, 1988).

    Among the symbolic interactionist theories, the affect control approach developed

    by David Heise and Lynn Smith-Lovin (Heise, 1979, 1989; Smith-Lovin and

    Heise, 1988; Smith-Lovin, 1990) is built around the level of deflection between

    fundamental sentiments and transient sentiments which escalates emotional

    arousal and motivates actors to reduce the deflection. Within a somewhat

    different wing of symbolic interactionism, Sheldon Stryker (1980) and collabor-

    ators (Stryker and Serpe, 1982) have argued that emotions are a marker or sign

    that a particular identity, which an individual expects to confirm, has not

    received such confirming responses. Similarly, other interpersonal theories

    emphasize congruence between self and responses of others (e.g., Backman,

    1985, 1988; Swann, 1983, 1990; Higgins, 1987, 1989). And, within the power-

    models of Kemper and Collins (1990), the activation of either negative or

    positive emotions is related to whether or not anticipated confirmations ofrelative status or power have been realized.

    Thus, it is clear that emotional arousal is related to the relationship between

    expectations and experience. I will simply term this force the degree of congruity

    or incongruity between expectations and experiences in a situation. If there is

    an incongruity below expectations for reinforcing outcomes or associative

    experiences, then the three primary emotions of assertion-anger, aversion-fear,

    and disappointment-sadness are activated in varying degrees of intensity and in

    different combinations and ratios, as will be discussed shortly (see Tables 2 and

    3). If there is an incongruity that exceeds expectations for either positive ornegative outcomes, a variant of the primary emotion of satisfaction-happiness is

    stimulated. And if expectations and experiences are congruent, then comparat-

    ively low levels of emotion are aroused: if expectations were for positive

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    13/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 145

    Table 2. Variants of Primary Emotions

    Satisfaction-Happiness

    Low intensity Moderate intensity High intensity

    content cheerful joysanguine buoyant blissserenity friendly rapturegratified amiable jubilant

    enjoyment gaiety

    Aversion-Fear

    Low intensity Moderate intensity High intensity

    concern misgivings terror

    hesitant trepidation horrorreluctance anxiety high anxiety

    Assertion-Anger

    Low intensity Moderate intensity High intensity

    annoyed displeased dislikeagitated frustrated loathingirritated belligerent disgust

    vexed contentious hate

    perturbed hostility despisenettled ire desestrankled animosity hatred

    seethingwrath

    Disappointment-Sadness

    Low intensity Moderate intensity High intensity

    discouraged dismayed sorrowdowncast disheartened heartsickdispirited glum despondent

    resigned anguishedgloomywoefulpained

    Startlement-Surprise

    Low intensity Moderate intensity High intensity

    sudden-awareness sudden-alertness amazed

    astonishedastoundedincredulous

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    14/30

    146 Jonathan H. Turner

    Table 3. First-order combinations of primary emotions

    Satisfaction-Happiness

    Satisfaction-Happiness aversion-fear wonder, hopeful, relief, gratitude, pride,

    reverenceSatisfaction-Happiness assertion-anger vengeance, appeased, calmed, soothed,

    relish, triumphant, bemusedSatisfaction-Happiness disappointment-sadness nostalgia, yearning, hopefulnessSatisfaction-Happiness startlement-surprise elation, exhilaration, thrilled

    Aversion-Fear

    Aversion-Fear satisfaction-happiness awe, reverence, venerationAversion-Fear assertion-anger revulsed, repulsed, antagonism, dislike,

    envyAversion-Fear disappointment-sadness dread, warinessAversion-Fear startlement-surprise panic, consternation, alarmed, scared

    Assertion-Anger

    Assertion-Anger satisfaction-happiness snubbing, mollified, rudeness, placated,righteousness

    Assertion-Anger aversion-fear abhorrence, jealousy, suspiciousnessAssertion-Anger disappointment-sadness bitterness, depression, betrayed

    Assertion-Anger startlement-surprise offended, incited, perturbed, piqued,outraged, irate, insulted, appalled,provoked, indignant, furious, inflamed,incensed

    Disappointment-Sadness

    Disappointment-Sadness satisfaction-happiness acceptance, moroseness, solace,melancholy

    Disappointment-Sadness aversion-fear regret, forlornness, remorseful, miseryDisappointment-Sadness assertion-anger aggrieved, discontent, dissatisfied,

    unfulfilled, boredom, grief, envy,sullenness

    Disappointment-Sadness startlement-surprise crestfallen, shaken, dejected, stunned,upset

    Startlement-Surprise

    Startlement-Surprise satisfaction-happiness delightStartlement-Surprise aversion-fear shock, alarmed, unnerved, unsettled,

    agitated, vigilantStartlement-Surprise assertion-anger disgust, upset, abhorrence, irritatedStartlement-Surprise disappointment-sadness dejection, incredulity, dismay

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    15/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 147

    experiences, then satisfaction is activated; and if expectations for negative

    experiences are met, then relatively low level variants of disappointment-sadness

    are likely to ensue.

    Aside from this gross relationship between the level of emotional arousal

    and the congruity/incongruity between expectations and experiences, anotherprimary emotionstartlement-surprisecan increase the intensity of the emo-

    tions aroused. Of course, incongruity between expectations and experience is,

    per se, a type of surprise; and so surprise is always involved in emotional

    reactions. Yet, there can be varying degrees of surprise, and the more one is

    surprised by an incongruity the greater is the intensity of the emotions

    experienced, at least in the short term. Part of the reason for this enhanced

    intensity is that surprise activates motor and visceral system responses in ways

    that feed back and feed forward to stimulate subcortical emotion systems as well

    as cortical attention to stimuli (Le Doux, 1996; Damasio, 1994); another part is

    that surprise highlights the incongruity between expectations and experiences;

    and most significantly, surprise makes individuals more self aware, and as a

    consequence, surprise increases the salience of self. And, escalating the visibility

    of self in a situation raises the emotional stakes. Even when self is not originally

    salient, surprise as it activates other emotions pulls at least situational selves into

    the emotional response; and if more core cognitions-emotions about self become

    salient, then the emotional intensity increases considerably. Some have argued,

    it should be noted, that surprise may not be a primary emotion; yet, whatever

    its status in this respect, surprise as a primary or learned response intensifies

    emotional experiences.

    Self is always hightly salient if individuals expect reinforcing and confirming

    responses from others. Thus, while self can be pulled into the emotional

    dynamics, thereby increasing the emotional valences involved, interactions that

    begin with a high sense of self as an object in relation to others will produce

    more intense emotional reactions when incongruity occurs. When self is not

    confirmed, individuals will experience higher levels of assertion-anger, aversion-

    fear and disappointment-sadness; when self is viewed negatively but actuallyreinforced positively, the sense of relief (and other mixtures of fear, satisfaction,

    and sadness) will be more acutely experienced (see Tables 2 and 3); when more

    positive views of self are confirmed, the sense of satisfaction will move to the

    happiness side of this primary emotion.

    As anticipated earlier, a key factor in increasing the salience of self and the

    intensity of the emotions experienced is the level of direct sanctioning by others.

    Often individuals simply do not realize their expectations or goals with no

    sanctioning from others directly involved, but with active sanctioning by others,

    whether on the positive or negative side, the emotional stakes are raised becausesanctions focus the responses of others on self, making self even more of an

    object to itself. Thus, when congruities and incongruities between expectations

    and experiences follow from active sanctioning by others, the emotions experi-

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    16/30

    148 Jonathan H. Turner

    enced will be more intense. If others praise self, satisfaction-happiness is more

    acutely felt; and if such praise is a surprise and exceeds expectations, then the

    emotional reactions are escalated further. If others react negatively to behaviors

    of self, then more intense mixes of assertion-anger, aversion-fear, and disappoint-

    ment-sadness ensue; and if surprise is involved, then anger or fear is even morestrongly felt, often drowning out the sense of disappointment-sadness that also

    accompanies negative sanctioning.

    As noted earlier attribution processes are also involved in the relationship

    among expectations, experiences, and emotions. If incongruity occurs, and the

    incongruity is attributed by the individual to self, then the emotions experienced

    are intensified, whether positively or negatively. If the incongruity is attributed

    to the behaviors and motives of others (as a kind of internal attribution), and

    the experience has been negative, then more heightened variants of assertion-

    anger are aroused, often mixed with aversion-fear if the other is in a position

    to apply negative sanctions. If the experience is positive, however, then other

    will be viewed, and responded to, in terms of combinations and variants of

    satisfaction-happiness. If the incongruity is attributed to situational forces without

    reference to others or, if others are involved, to forces beyond their control,

    then the emotions experienced will be lower key. Under these conditions of

    external attribution, incongruities on the negative side will be more likely to

    produce reactions on the disappointment end of this primay emotion and its

    variants, and if anger and fear are activated, these too will have lower valances;

    and incongruities on the positive side under conditions of external attribution

    will qualify and mitigate happiness (see Tables 2 and 3).

    To sum up, the emotional arousal stimulated by congruity or incongruity

    between expectations and experiences is influenced by a number of variables:

    the nature of expectations, the extent to which the incongruity exceeds or fails

    to meet expectations, the extent to which surprise has been activated, the degree

    to which self is salient and implicated in the experience of congruity or

    incongruity, the extent to which congruity or incongruity is related to direct

    sanctioning by others, and the nature of the attributions made for the cause ofthe incongruity or congruity. Yet, these variables interact in complex ways in

    generating emotions, per se, and in activating defense mechanisms that distort

    emotional experiences. Moreover, there are complex feedback and feed forward

    relations with subcortical systems involved in the production of emotional

    responses. At this point, then, we can only see the generalizations presented

    above as first approximations of a very complicated process.

    EMOTIONAL AROUSAL

    Emotional arousal always involves activation of at least one, and typically several,

    primary emotions. This arousal begins subcortically, as sensory inputs move to

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    17/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 149

    the thalamus which then activates the hypothalamus as well as various limbic

    systems, such as the amygdala, septum, and cingulate gyrus; but as working

    memory experiences these subcortical inputs, while activating coded neurons to

    restimulate sensory cortices to reproduce images from long-term memory,

    individuals experience feelings organized in terms of variants and combinationsof primary emotions. Following Kempers (1987) review of the large literature

    on the primary emotions, or what can be reasonably seen as biologically-

    based emotions in humans, five such emotions appear to be hard-wired into

    humans neuroanatomy and, indirectly, into the endocrine system (Ekman, 1982).

    Table 2 is my own adaptation of the literature on primary emotions (see

    Turner, 2000, for a review). As is evident I see each primary emotion as

    potentially moving from a low to a higher intensity state: satisfaction to happiness,

    aversion to fear, assertion to anger, disappointment to sadness, and startlement

    to surprise. This is not an issue of mere labels, because the low intensity variant

    (the left side of each pair in Table 2) is, I believe, the more prevalent biological

    state for most mammals; in contrast, humans and other higher mammals have

    the neurological capacity to elaborate this lower intensity state to a much higher

    level of amplitude. Thus, for humans, satisfaction can move from contentment

    through buoyancy and cheerfulness to joyfulness and, as we will see in Table 3,

    to many first-order combinations of primary emotions, such as pride; aversion

    moves from hesitancy through anxiety and trepidation to terror and many first-

    order combinations as well as becoming implicated in complex second-order

    emotions like shame and guilt as is delineated in Table 4; assertiveness goes

    through such moderate states as displeasure, frustration, and hostility to high

    intensity states like loathing, disgust, and hate, while becoming implicated in the

    many first-and second-order combinations listed in Tables 3 and 4; disappoint-

    ment moves from a sense of being discouraged through pain and dismay to

    sorrow and anguish as well as many first- and second-order combinations;

    startlement goes from sudden awareness through sudden alertness to amazement

    and incredulity, while being implicated in more complex combinations of

    emotions and, as emphisized earlier, in the intensification of all emotions.As a basic hypothesis, I would argue that when expectations and experiences

    are congruent, whether activating positive or negative emotions, the low intensity

    state is the most likely node of activationthat is, satisfaction, mild aversion, mild

    assertiveness, disappointment, and startlement. Conversely, as the incongruence

    between expectations and experiences increases, especially as self becomes ever

    more salient, the high intensity end of these emotions is activated, as are the

    various first- and second-order combinations portrayed in Tables 3 and 4.

    As is evident in Table 3, I see not only variants but first-order combinations

    of primary emotions as crucial. Following Kempers (1987) lead, emotions arecombined in first-order combinations revolving around the mixing of two

    primary emotions in varying proportions or ratios (Turner, 1996a, 1996b, 1997a,

    1997b, 1999, 2000). Thus, the emotions that humans use in their role taking

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    18/30

    150 Jonathan H. Turner

    and role making efforts are variants of primary emotions and combinations of

    these basic emotions; it is the capacity to combine primary emotions that makes

    humans emotional repertoire so large compared to other animals. Table 3

    presents my current candidates for some first-order combinations.

    In Table 3, each of the primary emotions is combined with another primaryemotion to form a first-order combination. This process is very much like the

    mixing of colors, as Plutchik (1980, 1993) has made explicit in his theory of

    basic and derived emotions, but I do not think that the mixing is quite as orderly

    and systematic as implied by Plutchiks emotions wheel. Rather, the neural

    mechanisms that perform the mixing are, themselves, complex systems, revolving

    at a minimum around feed forward and feed back relations among (1) thalamus

    and hypothalamus, (2) cortical and subcortical motor areas, (3) subcortical visceral

    responses and neurotransmitters like dopamine, norepinephrine, serotonin, and

    acetylcholine, (4) subcortical emotion centers such as the septum, cingulate

    gyrus, and amygdala, (5) basal forebrain, (6) hippocampus, and (7) neocortex.

    Thus, the neurological capacity to generate and mix emotions is vast, far greater

    than the actual numbers and range of emotions that structure human interaction.

    The neuro substrate of emotions is an important topic itself, but for my purposes

    here, the critical point is that humans emotional repertoire revolves around a

    complex process of activating interconnected systems in the brain and the body

    in ways that generate mixes among primary emotions.

    Table 3 lists combinations that involve one dominant primary emotion mixed

    with lesser but varying proportions of a second primary emotion. For example,

    when the more intense variant of satisfaction-happiness is dominant, but mixed

    with varying levels of aversion-fear, emotions such as wonder, hopefulness, relief,

    gratitude, reverence and, most importantly, pride are generated. As a first-order

    combination of mostly happiness (toward self as an object), tinged with a small

    amount of fear (about potential failure), pride needs to be emphasized because,

    following Cooley (1916) and Scheff (1988, 1990), its activation will be very

    important in understanding self and the relation of self to other emotional states.

    By looking down the right side of Table 3 under stiuations where one primaryemotion is dominant, the varieties of first-order combinations of emotions can

    be seen. I am not sure that the combinations presented in Table 3 are exactly

    right, but the general point is that humans have the neurological capacity in

    their covert thoughts, in their self presentations and role making, in their self

    talk, and in their role taking to draw upon a very large repertoire of first-order

    combinations of primary emotions, thereby giving human interaction, thought,

    decision making, sanctioning, and moral coding a subtle and fine-tuned quality.

    But one critical difference with Kempers and my earlier work with first-order

    combinations is that some of the most important emotions in human interactionare what I term second-order combinations of three primary emotions mixed in

    different ratios. As Table 4 emphasizes, the emotions of shame and guilt are the

    most important combinations of three primary emotions. Shame is an emotion

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    19/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 151

    Table 4. The Structure of Shame and Guilt

    Rank-order of primary emotions Second-order emotionsShame

    1 Disappointment-sadness (at self)2 Assertion-Anger (at self)3 Aversion-fear (about consequences to self)

    Guilt

    1 Disappointment-sadness (at self)2 Aversion-fear (about consequences to self)3 Assertion-anger (at self)

    in which self is highly salient; the most prominent emotion in shame is

    disappointment-sadness at self for behaving inadequately in the eyes of others

    and in ones own self-assessments, coupled with anger at self and some fear

    about the consequences to self for having engaged in behaviors are seen as

    inadequate and, perhaps, incompetent. Guilt activates the same three primary

    emotions but in a slightly different combination: the dominant emotion is still

    disappointment-sadness at self for having failed to meet expectations, but the

    second most important emotion is now fear about the consequences of this

    failure, coupled with some anger at self for having so failed. Many have argued

    (e.g., Lewis, 1971; Scheff, 1988) that guilt is simply extreme shame, but the

    subjective feelings for shame and guilt are quite different, especially in extreme

    form. Others have suggested different ways to combine or distinguish shame,

    guilt, and related emotions like embrarrassment (e.g., Sabini & Silver, 1997,

    1998; Tangney, et al., 1996). I may be wrong in this conclusion, but the general

    point remains the same: shame and guilt are at least a second-order emotion,

    mixing three primary emotions; and the different rankings among the primary

    emotions give shame and guilt distinctive guidelines and make their experience

    by an individual distinctive. Moreover, the causes of the two emotions vary:guilt is activated with failure to meet moral expectations, while shame is

    generated when one has behaved incompetently. Because shame and guilt are

    such unpleasant emotions, they are very likely to mobilize defense mechanisms.

    Since disappointment-sadness is the dominant emotion in shame and guilt,

    the depression that often accompanies shamed and guilty individuals may not

    reflect the use of repression and other defense mechanisms; rather, the depression

    may simply be an artifact of the dominance of disappointment-sadness in both

    shame and guilt. Yet, as I will argue below, the activation of defense mechanisms

    accelerates and strengthens this depressive effect. For, it takes a great deal ofemotional energy to repress; and when one uses other defense mechanisms, such

    as attributions to others and situations, projections onto others of ones own

    emotions, and displacement of the anger and fear contained in shame and guilt

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    20/30

    152 Jonathan H. Turner

    onto others, one also tends to receive back negative sanctions, making self feel

    even more disappointed and angry as well as fearful about the consequences of

    ones actions. In the end, the effort to control these unpleasant feelings and

    responses of others lowers the overt emotional energy in interaction.

    ACTIVATION OF DEFENSE MECHANISMS

    Subcortical systems generating emotions are, to a degree, controlled by the

    centers for rational and purposive thinking in the neocortex, especially the

    prefrontal cortex; and this was probably so long before Homo sapiens sapiens

    emerged on the hominid line. And it may well be that the hominid neocortex

    became ever larger over the last few million years not so much for language-

    production and comprehension as for cortical control of hominids expanding

    emotional capacities.

    Such control enables humans to present self and fronts in ways that do not

    disrupt the flow of interaction; and in fact, cortical control of emotions as well

    as the inadvertent leaking of emotions from such control facilitates interpersonal

    attunement and sanctioning. Yet, as hominids ability for seeing self as an object

    (an ability that is probably an unselected by-product of an expanding of the

    neocortex, especially the prefrontal neocortex), cortical control could also be

    used to defend self from particularly unpleasant emotions, especially those

    building upon and combining fear, anger, and sadness. If surprise was also

    involved, neocortical efforts to protect self from unpleasant emotions would

    simply intensify. Emotional variants of happiness and first-order combinations

    such as pride did not generate such protective reactions, and so the cortical

    system of defense that evolved was designed to protect individuals from highly-

    charged elaborations and combinations of fear, anger, and sadness. As a relevant

    aside, I think that humans can read in others anger and fear as well as all of its

    variants far more readily and subtly than they can read satisfaction-happiness

    because these are the emotions that bring pain. Such emotions are activated bynegative sanctions from others as well as failures to meet expectations; and as a

    result, these emotions activate powerful subcortical limbic responses (principally,

    the systems revolving around the amygdala) which, in turn, can activate efforts

    at cortical control.

    The most commonly discussed defense mechanismsrepression, projection,

    and displacementare simply ways to protect self from experiencing negative

    emotions, particularly second-order emotions such as shame and guilt (Freud,

    1900 [1960]; Horney, 1950; Sullivan, 1953). Ironically, these are also the very

    emotions that are essential for social control, because if individuals do not feelshame at themselves for disassociative and inadequate behaviors or for failing

    to meet order-sustaining expectations as well as guilt for such behaviors, social

    organization would not be possible. The world would be filled with sociopaths

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    21/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 153

    who would soon become extinct. Thus, defense mechanisms are maladaptive to

    the social order only if they keep individuals from experiencing those very

    emotions that are essential to the smooth flow of interaction, while being

    maladaptive to the individual only if they depress emotional energy and lower

    the capacity to role take and role make effectively. Although the causes of shameand guilt as well as the subjective experience of these emotions differ, they are

    both subject to the same defense mechanisms. Let me offer some general and,

    I grant, fairly obvious generalizations as they relate to understanding defense

    mechanisms and emotional energy in human interaction.

    Activation of defense mechanisms such as projection and displacement in the

    face of either guilt and shame tends to be counterproductive in the long run

    because use of these mechanisms invites negative sanctions from others. If an

    individual is seen as imputing his or her own negative emotions on others or as

    venting on others negative emotions, especially versions of anger, people will

    respond in ways that increase an individuals sense of shame and guilt, or

    alternatively, force a person to repress to an even greater degree. Repression is

    often necessary before other defense mechanisms can be activated, but it also

    operates by itself. Highly repressed individuals often have episodes of emotional

    outburst, typically anger but anxiety as well, which once again invite negative

    responses from others, thereby heightening the shame for engaging in such

    outbursts and the guilt over having done so.

    As Lewis (1971) and Scheff (1988) have emphasized, repression operates at a

    number of levels, from simply bypassing shame and not acknowledging it to

    burying negative feelings about self from conscious reflection. If individuals

    habitually bypass shame, and I would add, guilt, as well, repressive responses

    must become ever more mobilized. The end result is that persons are drained

    of emotional energy, except for disproportionate and typically inappropriate

    outburst of emotion, especially anger but fear as well, that escape the cortical

    censors. These emotional outbursts only feed back and require the censors to

    increase their pressure which, in turn, lowers peoples emotional energy in ways

    that can disrupt interaction. A biography of such cycles will, in the end, decreasethe modal levels of emotional energy generated by individuals, even when they

    are in situations, such as those giving them power and authority, that normally

    produce positive emotional arousal.

    Self thought and self talk are important elements in these dynamics. If second-

    order variants of primary emotions causing shame and guilt are only experienced

    in the brains mode of processing information as images and gestalts, there is a

    greater likelihood that the emotional arousal will go unacknowledged or that

    more active repression will occur. The key to dealing with negative emotions is

    self talk, where the blur of cognitive-emotional images can be slowed down formore careful inspection and introspection. Of course, the goal of most therapy

    is to get individuals to engage in such self talk, but the difficulties that therapists

    have are really built into the very nature of human thinking.

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    22/30

    154 Jonathan H. Turner

    The key to using self talk effectively is seeing the sources of emotional reactions

    and recognizing the differences between guilt and shame. Ultimately, these

    sources are the expectations listed in Table 1 as well as the degree to which

    experiences, especially those which make self highly salient, correspond to

    expectations. If self talk can be reflectively analytical, seeing incongruities forwhat they are and making selfattributions about failures to meet expectations,

    then the emotional reactions can be culled from the brains mode of organizing

    cognitions in terms of fields and gestalts. In turn, if open and honest, such

    reflection can stave off the activation of defense mechanisms, with the result

    that the negative emotions experienced, particularly anxiety, shame and guilt,

    can be managed and become a part of an individuals cultural capital and

    used to more effectively present self and role make in future interactions. Such

    analytical reflection, as it creates cultural capital, raises emotional energy in

    subsequent interactions. One way in which analytical self reflection works is to

    lower or alter expectations, and as a result, the emotional stakes are lessened

    because incongruities between expectations and experiences, even those where

    self is salient, will be either eliminated or, at the very least, greatly reduced.

    Self talk can also focus on more positive emotions, especially those variants

    and combinations of primary emotions that generate pride. Pride not only

    enhances peoples sense of well being, it charges emotional energy because pride,

    like its more negative counterparts, becomes a part of the cultural capital of

    individuals that can be invested in interactions (Collins, 1988). Indeed, high

    levels of pride are often necessary for individuals to engage in self talk about

    shame or guilt, and under ideal circumstances, frank self talk about shame or

    guilt can become yet another source of pride. For this to occur, however, a

    persons cultural capital must have an existing reservoir of prideful cognitions

    accumulated in past interactions of a given type or, more generally, in his or

    her cumulative biography. Highly depressed individuals, who have cultural

    capital filled with shame and guilt, generally have such lowered pride that they

    cannot engage in effective self talk; and indeed, their self talk can often turn

    into self-recriminations which, in the end, will reactivate defense mechanisms.Moreover, self talk can also be used for self deception; and as a result,

    individuals can simply employ talk to impose a defensive regime that protects

    self. When confronted with shame and guilt, especially a biography of bypassed

    and repressed experiences, talk can make external or internal attributions of

    emotions, projecting them onto particular people, categories of people, and

    situations; self talk can justify displacement, once again through internal and

    external attributions to others and situations; and self talk can jabber around

    guilt and shame in order to drown out awareness of those emotions that have

    been pushed below conscious reflection (such individuals are often compulsivetalkers not only to themselves but to others as well).

    If humans did not have the capacity to bypass minor moments of shame and

    guilt, however, they would be overwhelmed by emotions. Any given moment

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    23/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 155

    of emotionally-charged interaction or any legacy of accumulated emotional

    experiences in an individuals biography would make a person too emotionally

    charged to function effectively in interactions. Thus, defense mechanisms evolved,

    no doubt, as part of the neocortical package to keep emotions from getting out

    of control and for enabling individuals to use them in associative ways. Moreover,most emotions are read visually, bypassing the translation boxes for speech

    comprehension and production, and feeding directly into the brains mode of

    organizing memories and images, thereby keeping emotions from becoming

    ready topics of slow self talk. Again, this is a useful way to avoid being

    overwhelmed by emotions. It is, therefore, a matter of how active defense

    mechanisms are. As a rule of thumb, if the activation of defense mechanisms

    and the natural operation of the brain in terms of imaging prevent individuals

    from retrieving painful cognitions and making them available for self talk, a

    decrease in modal levels of emotional energy will ensue.

    CONCLUSIONS: STATING THE THEORY FORMALLY

    I have delayed to the end presenting the theory because the dynamics of

    emotions discussed thus far are interrelated in direct, indirect, and reverse causal

    patterns, thereby requiring that I first describe the operative processes behind

    the labels presented in Figure 1 before pulling these together into a more formal

    and parsimonious theory.

    The propositions enumerated below formalize the basic lines of argument

    stated discursively thus far. Given the highly speculative nature of the argument,

    however, these propositions should only be considered tentative hypotheses,

    although they do summarize various empirical findings from well-known

    theoretical traditions. As speculative extensions of existing theories and research

    findings on these theories, the proposition do not represent a refined theory.

    Rather, the propositions are only a first step in trying to develop a more general

    sociological theory of emotions in interpersonal encounters. When stated aspropositions, however, the argument can more readily be assessed and criticized

    in light of other theoretical positions or empirical findings, thereby paving the

    way for correction and increased refinement. I offer these sixteen propositions

    in the hopes of stimulating critique and theoretical cumulation.

    1. The greater is the number of expectations that form an individuals

    definition of a situation and the higher is the salience of these expectations

    in an encounter, the greater is the potential for emotional arousal by an

    individual and for affective displays of emotions to others in the encounter.2. If individuals have received positive sanctions from others and have met

    or exceeded expectations, or both, in an encounter, they will experience at

    least satisfaction and more typically happiness; and, as a consequence, they

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    24/30

    156 Jonathan H. Turner

    will display associative affect towards others and will receive similar affect

    from others in the encounter.

    2a. If self is salient in an encounter and if attributions for success in

    meeting expectations and/or for receiving positive sanctions are placed

    on self, then these individuals will experience pride; and, as a consequence,they will seek to develop and sustain associative relations with others in

    this encounter.

    2b. If attributions for success in meeting expectations and for receiving

    positive sanctions are attributed in an encounter to the internal states of

    others or categories of others, then these individuals will experience

    variants of satisfaction-happiness; and, as a consequence, they will display

    towards and receive from others these positive emotions, and, thereby,

    develop and sustain associative relations with others in this encounter.

    2c. If attributions for success in meeting expectations and for receiving

    positive sanctions are attributed in an encounter to external causes in the

    situation, then the level of satisfaction-happiness expressed by individuals

    towards others will be lower, but nonetheless, individuals will still

    seek to interact and to create associative relations with others in the

    encounter.

    3. Both hapiness and pride under conditions of positive attribution increase

    the level of associative affect emitted by an individual towards others in an

    encounter who, reciprocally, will tend to increase their display of affect to

    the individual in an escalating cycle of ever-more associative and emotionally-

    charged relations until fatigue and satiation set in, or the encounter is

    terminated.

    4. Self talk about interactions in which variants and combinations of

    satisfaction, happiness, and pride have been experienced will sustain the

    positive flow of emotions previously displayed and become a part of

    individuals cultural capital which, in turn, will set expectations for the

    subsequent interaction, making individuals more inclined to pursue associat-

    ive interaction with particular others in encounters now and in the future;and, as a consequence, the level of associative affect displayed towards and

    received from others will increase in these encounters.

    5. Such self talk of variants and combinations of satisfaction-happiness will

    often be generalized to similar others or similar encounters, thereby raising

    the expectations for positive emotions and making individuals more likely

    to seek associative relations with others or to enter encounters like those

    which, in the pasts, were rewarding to the individual; and, as a consequence,

    the level of associative affect displayed towards others and received from

    others will increase in such encounters.6. If, however, individuals receive negative sanctions from others or are

    unsuccessful in meeting expectations, or both, in an encounter, they will

    experience varying combinations of assertion-anger, aversion-fear, and

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    25/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 157

    disappointment-sadness, which, in turn, will activate the converse of processes

    (2) through (5) above.

    6a. If self is salient in an encounter and if attributions by individuals for

    failure to behave competently and for receiving associated negative

    sanctions are placed on self, then these individuals will experience shamewhich will eventually lower their modal level of emotional arousal; and,

    as a consequence, their modal level of affective display will decline and,

    thereby, dampen the associative affect in relations with others in the

    encounter.

    6b. If self is salient in an encounter and if attributions by individuals for

    failure to meet normative expectations and for receiving associated

    negative sanctions are placed on self, then these individuals will experience

    guilt; and as a consequence their modal level of emotional arousal will

    decline and, thereby, dampen the associative affect in relations with others.

    6c. If attributions for failure to meet expectations and for receiving

    negative sanctions in an encounter are attributed by individuals to the

    internal states of others or categories of others, then variants of assertion-

    anger toward these others or categories of others will be aroused and

    displayed by these individuals up to the point of fatigue, or escalated

    negative sanctions by others, or termination of the encounter.

    6d. If attributions for failure to meet expectations and for receiving

    sanctions in an encounter are attributed by individuals to external causes

    in the situation, then variants of assertion-anger will be less intense and

    the level of negative emotional arousal and display by these individuals

    will be relatively low; and, as a consequence, they will sustain relations

    with lessened associative affect with others in the encounter.

    7. All of the effects described in (2) through (6) will be magnified under

    conditions of increasing surprise; and the greater is the level of surprise

    experienced by individuals, the higher wil be their levels of emotional

    arousal and display in an encounter.

    8. All of the effects listed in (1) through (7) will feed back and changeexpectations; and the more intense are the emotions experienced by

    individuals, whether positive or negative, the greater will be the change in

    their expectations for subsequent interactions in similar encounters.

    9. The more expectations escalate among individuals in an encounter,

    whether positive or negative, the more likely are incongruities to emerge in

    subsequent encounters, thereby raising the level of emotional arousal and

    display towards others in subsequent interactions in similar encounters.

    10. To the degree that defense mechanisms have been used by individuals

    in an encounter to repress disappointment-sadness, aversion-fear, andassertion-anger, especially as these combine to produce shame and guilt,

    then the modal level of overt emotional energy arousal by these individuals

    will be lowered in this encounter; and, as a consequence, the level of

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    26/30

    158 Jonathan H. Turner

    associative affect displayed towards others and received from others will be

    correspondingly dampened in the encounter.

    11. To the degree that defense mechanisms have been used habitually and

    over a long period in an individuals biography, then the modal level of

    emotional displays by this individual will be low; and, as a consequence,the individuals level of associative affect displayed towards others and

    received from others will be dampened in all encounters.

    12. To the extent that attributions for disappointment-sadness, aversion-

    fear, and assertion-anger are directed by individuals toward others and

    categories of others in an encounter, their modal level of overt emotional

    arousal and display will increase, especially episodes of anxiety and anger,

    up to the point where fatigue, shame, or guilt about such responses reduce

    the level of emotional display; and the more negative sanctions from others

    lead these individuals to experience shame and guilt, the more powerful

    will be the effect of shame and guilt on reducing their emotional displays

    toward others in the encounter.

    13. To the extent that disappointment-sadness, aversion-fear, and assertion-

    aggression are projected or displaced by individuals on others, the more

    likely are such individuals to experience negative sanctions from these

    others; and, as a consequence, the more likely are the dynamics in (6a6d)

    and (12) above are to be operative.

    14. To the degree that the processes described in (12) and (13) have occurred

    over a long period of time and typify the biography of an individual, the

    less will be this individuals modal level of emotional display in an encounter

    when not engaged in periodic outbursts of assertive displays revolving

    around attribution, projection, and displacement; and, as a consequence,

    this individuals modal level of associative affect displayed towards and

    received from others will be dampened.

    15. To the extent that individuals can engage in analytical self talk that

    correctly targets the sources for their emotional experiences in encounters

    whether the target be self, others, or situationsthe greater will be theirsense of satisfaction and, hence, the more this sense of satisfaction will

    mitigate the dampening affects of negative emotions and, thereby, raise

    these individuals modal level of positive emotional arousal in encounters;

    and, as a consequence, they will display toward others and receive from

    others associative affect in all encounters.

    16. If self talk by individuals is effective in reducing negative emotional

    arousal, especially those negative emotions that have accumulated over

    longer periods of time and that revolve around anxiety, guilt, and shame,

    the more likely will these individuals experience pride in encounters, andhence, the greater will be these individuals modal level of positive emotional

    arousal; and, as a consequence, they will display toward others and receive

    from others associative affect in all encounters.

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    27/30

    Sociological Theory of Emotions 159

    Jonathan A. Turner

    Department of Sociology

    University of California, Riverside

    Riverside, CA 92521-0719

    USA

    REFERENCES

    BACKMAN, C.W. (1985). Identity, Self-presentation, and the Resolution of Moral Dilemmas:Toward A Social Psychological Theory of Moral Behavior. In The Self and Social Life,ed. B.R. Schlenker. New York: McGraw-Hill, pp. 23656.

    BACKMAN, C.W. (1988). The Self: A Dialectical Approach. Advances in Experimental Social

    Psychology, 21, 22960.BERGER, J. (1988). Direction in Expectation States Research. In Status Generalization: New

    Theory and Research, eds. M. Webster and M. Foschi. Stanford, CA: Stanford UniversityPress, pp. 45074.

    BERGER, J . & ZELDITCH M. Jr. (1985). Status Rewards and Influence. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.BERGER, J . & ZELDITCH M. Jr. (1988). Status, Power, and Legitimacy. New Brunswick:

    Transaction Books.BERGER, J., WEBSTER, M.A. Jr., Ridgeway, C. & ROSENHOLTZ, S.J. (1986). Status Cues,

    Expectations, and Behavior. Advances in Group Processes, 3, 122.BURKE, P.J. (1980). The Self: Measurement Implications From a Symbolic Interactionist

    Perspective. Social Psychology Quarterly, 43, 1829.

    BURKE, P.J. (1991). Identity Processes and Social Stress. American Sociological Review, 56,83649.

    COLLINS, R. (1975). Conflict Sociology: Toward An Explanatory Social Science. New York: Academic Press.

    COLLINS, R. (1988). Theoretical Sociology. San Diego: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.COLLINS, R. (1993). Emotional Energy and the Common Denominator of Rational

    Action. Rationality and Society 5, 20330.COOLEY, C.H. (1916). Social Organization: A Study of the Larger Mind. New York: Scribners.DAMASIO, A. (1994). Descartes Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York:

    G.P. Putnam.EKMAN, P. (1982). Emotions in the Human Face. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    FREUD, Sigmund. (1990) [1960]. The Interpretation of Dreams. New York: Wiley.GECAS, V. (1989). The Social Psychology of Self-efficacy. Annual Review of Sociology, 8, 133.GECAS, V. (1991). The Self Concept as a Basis for A Theory of Motivation. In The Self-

    Society Dynamic: Cognition, Emotion and Action, eds. J.A. Howard and P.L. Callero.Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, pp. 17188.

    GESCHWIND, N. (1965a). Disconnection Syndromes in Animals and Man, Part I. Brain,88, 23794.

    GESCHWIND, N. (1965b). Disconnection Syndromes in Animals and Man, Part II. Brain,88, 585644.

    GESCHWIND, N. & DAMASIO, A. (1984). The Neural Basis of Language. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 7, 12747.

    GOFFMAN, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. Garden City, NY: Anchor Books.GOFFMAN, E. (1961). Encounters. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill.GOFFMAN, E. (1967). Interaction Ritual: Essays on Face-to-Face Behavior. Garden City, NY:

    Anchor Books.

    The Executive Management Committee/Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999

  • 8/7/2019 TURNER Toward a General Sociological Theory of Emotion

    28/30

    160 Jonathan H. Turner

    GOFFMAN, E. (1974). Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organization of Experience. New York:Harper and Row.

    HEISE, D. (1979). Understanding Events. New York: Cambridge University Press.HEISE, D. (1989). Effects of Emotion Displays on Social Identification. Social Psychology

    Quarterly, 53, 1021.

    HEISE, D . & SMITH-LOVIN, L. (1981). Impressions of Goodness, Powerfulness, andLiveliness From Discerned Social Events. Social Psychology Quarterly, 44, 93106.

    HIGGINS, E.T. (1987). Selfdiscrepancy: A Theory Relating Self and Affect. PsychologicalReview, 94, 31940.

    HIGGINS, E.T. (1989). Self-discrepancy Theory: What Patterns of Self-beliefs CausePeople to Suffer? Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 93136.

    HOCHSCHILD, A.R. (1979). Emotion Work, Feeling Rules and Social Structure. AmericanJournal of Sociology, 85, 55175.

    HOCHSCHILD, A.R. (1983). The Managed Heart: Commercialization of Human Feeling. Berkeley:University of California Press.

    HOMANS, G.C. (1961). Social Behavior: Its Elementary Forms. New York: Harcourt Brace.HOMANS, G.C. (1974). Social Behavior: Its Elementary Forms, revised edition. New York:

    Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.HORNEY, K. (1950). Neurosis and Human Growth: The Struggle Toward Self Realization. New

    York: Norton.KEMPER, T.D. (1978). An Interactional Theory of Emotions. New York: Wiley.KEMPER, T.D. (1987). How Many Emotions Are There? Wedding The Socia