theoretical, information processing, and situational factors affecting attribution theory models of...

Upload: mirianalbertpires

Post on 06-Jul-2018

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    1/12

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    2/12

    ?Academy

    of Management

    Review

    1983,

    Vol.

    8,

    No.

    1,

    50-60

    Theoretical

    Information

    Processing

    n d

    Situational

    a c t o r s

    Affecting

    Attribution

    T h e o r y

    M o d e l s

    o

    rganizational

    ehavior

    ROBERT

    G.

    LORD

    JONATHAN

    E. SMITH

    University

    of

    Akron

    Attributional

    theories used

    to

    explain

    organizational

    behavior

    are

    overly

    restrictive.

    A

    ttributional

    processes

    may vary

    with

    the

    type of

    attributional

    question

    addressed

    and

    with

    the

    level

    of

    information

    processing

    consistent

    with situational

    or motivational

    factors.

    Several attributional

    principles

    are

    organized

    into a

    typology involving

    type of

    attributional

    question

    and

    level of information processing. Boundary conditions affecting the use of

    particular

    principles

    are

    identified

    and

    the

    importance

    of

    differences

    among

    attributional

    processes

    are discussed.

    Attribution

    theory

    recently

    has been

    applied

    to

    several

    topics

    of

    concern

    to

    organizational

    research-

    ers. For

    example,

    Mitchell

    and

    his

    associates

    (Green

    &

    Liden, 1980;

    Green

    &

    Mitchell,

    1979;

    Ilgen

    &

    Knowlton,

    1980;

    Knowlton

    &

    Mitchell,

    1980;

    Mitchell,

    Green,

    &

    Wood, 1981;

    Mitchell &

    Wood,

    1980)

    have used

    supervisors'

    causal

    attributions

    to

    explain

    their

    reactions

    to

    subordinates'

    perfor-

    mance. Other researchers have relied on attribu-

    tions

    and

    implicit

    theories

    to

    explain

    the effects of

    bogus

    performance

    feedback

    on

    a

    variety

    of

    percep-

    tions.

    Binning

    and

    Lord

    (1980)

    and Staw

    (1975)

    found

    that

    performance

    affected

    perceptions

    of

    numerous

    group

    processes.

    Several

    researchers

    (Lord,

    Binning,

    Rush,

    &

    Thomas,

    1978;

    Mitchell,

    Larson,

    &

    Green,

    1977;

    Rush,

    Thomas,

    &

    Lord,

    1977;

    Staw

    &

    Ross,

    1980)

    have

    shown

    that

    perfor-

    mance

    feedback

    affects

    leadership

    ascriptions

    or

    perceptions

    of

    leader behavior.

    Finally,

    several

    researchers

    concerned

    with motivation

    have linked

    attributions

    to

    internal

    factors

    to

    intrinsic

    motiva-

    tion

    (Deci,

    1975; Fisher,

    1978;

    Phillips

    &

    Lord,

    1980).

    Such

    works

    represent

    fruitful

    applications

    of

    social

    psychological

    theories

    to

    applied

    topics.

    It is

    'An earlier

    version

    of this article

    was

    presented

    at the annual

    meeting

    of the

    Academy

    of

    Management,

    San

    Diego,

    1981.

    useful

    to consider

    several

    factors

    that

    affect these

    applications

    of attributional

    theories. These restric-

    tive factors

    can be

    analyzed using

    a framework that

    focuses

    on both the

    type

    of attribution

    being

    made

    and

    the

    level of information

    processing

    compatible

    with constraints

    of the situation

    and

    the

    attributor.

    In

    this

    paper

    two

    dimensions

    will

    be

    used-type

    of

    attribution

    and level

    of

    information

    processing-to

    develop a typology of principles for forming attri-

    butions.

    Type

    of Attribution

    The

    type

    of attribution

    is

    important

    because

    several

    distinct

    questions

    have been

    explored by

    at-

    tribution

    theory

    research.

    In

    particular,

    attribution

    theory

    may

    be

    applied

    to

    understanding

    causality

    for a

    specific

    event

    (Kelley,

    1973),

    to

    assessing

    re-

    sponsibility

    for a

    particular

    outcome

    (Hamilton,

    1980),

    or to

    assess

    the

    personal

    qualities

    of

    persons

    involved

    in

    the event

    being

    considered

    (Jones

    & Da-

    vis, 1965).

    Attributions

    concerning

    causal

    assessments

    in-

    volve

    producing

    explanations

    for

    unexpected

    or

    puzzling

    events.

    Attributors

    typically

    are viewed as

    following logical

    or

    quasi-scientific

    processes

    through

    which

    they assign

    relative

    priority

    to

    poten-

    50

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    3/12

    tial causal

    agents

    such as

    actors,

    situations,

    or

    cir-

    cumstances.

    The

    key

    issue in

    assessing

    causality

    in-

    volves

    deciding

    whether

    the event

    in

    question

    would

    have occurred

    without

    the

    presence

    of

    a

    particular

    agent (Hamilton,

    1980).

    Thus,

    necessary

    factors

    are

    assigned

    highest

    causal

    priority.

    For

    example,

    Kel-

    ley's (1973)

    covariation

    principles

    can be viewed

    as

    describing rules for determining whether an event

    would

    have occurred

    in the

    absence

    of

    particular

    ac-

    tors, entities,

    or

    circumstances

    (time)

    because

    events would

    covary only

    with

    necessary

    factors.

    Attributions

    concerning

    responsibility

    assess-

    ments involve a somewhat

    different

    issue-whether

    sanctions

    should be administered

    to a

    particular

    ac-

    tor

    (Hamilton,

    1980).

    Although responsibility

    gen-

    erally

    is

    assigned

    to

    causally

    predominant

    sources,

    responsibility

    assessments

    may

    be

    mitigated by

    fac-

    tors such

    as

    foreseeability,

    intention,

    and

    justifica-

    tion

    (Fincham

    &

    Jaspars,

    1980; Heider,

    1958).

    Moreover,

    responsibility

    sometimes

    may

    be as-

    signed

    to sources of low

    priority

    in causal

    explana-

    tions.

    Here

    the

    key

    issue is whether an actor could

    have

    produced

    a

    different

    outcome

    by

    behaving

    dif-

    ferently

    (Hamilton,

    1980).

    Thus,

    even if

    most lead-

    ers would behave

    similarly

    in a

    given

    situation,

    leading

    to

    similarly

    bad

    outcomes and

    yielding

    cau-

    sal

    assessments

    to entities and

    circumstances,

    each

    would be held

    responsible

    if

    by behaving

    otherwise

    she/he could

    have

    produced

    different outcomes. It

    is

    to be noted that

    leadership

    positions

    often

    may

    involve

    responsibility

    for

    outcomes

    even

    though

    leaders

    may

    not be

    causally

    important

    (Pfeffer,

    1977).

    Attribution of

    personal

    qualities emphasizes

    a

    third

    issue-determining

    the

    qualities

    of

    individuals

    that

    generalize

    across

    situations

    and

    time.

    Thus,

    the

    key question

    involves

    determining

    how

    to

    charac-

    terize

    a

    particular

    social

    stimulus.

    Although

    per-

    sonal

    qualities

    may

    be

    inferred from

    behaviors

    or

    their

    outcomes

    (Jones

    &

    Davis,

    1965),

    such

    general-

    izations also

    may

    be

    based on

    characteristics

    such as

    race, sex,

    cultural

    background,

    or

    appearance.

    Thus,

    implicit

    theories

    and social

    stereotypes

    also

    are

    related

    to

    attributions of

    personal

    qualities.

    Level of

    Information

    Processing

    Level of

    information

    processing

    appropriate

    to

    the

    attributor

    or

    situation

    also is

    important

    because

    alternative

    theoretical

    explanations

    differ in

    the

    amount of

    information

    processing

    they

    assume.

    In-

    deed,

    conceptually,

    one

    might employ

    a

    continuum

    that

    ranges

    from the

    assumption

    that

    attributors are

    highly

    active,

    involved

    information

    processors

    to

    an

    assumption

    that

    they

    are

    passive

    and limited

    infor-

    mation

    processors

    as

    one describes

    differences

    in

    at-

    tributional theories.

    Recent research on

    cognition

    and social judgment supports such an approach.

    Schneider and

    Shiffrin

    (1977)

    and Shiffrin

    and

    Schneider

    (1977) argue

    that a

    stimulus can

    evoke

    either a

    controlled

    cognitive

    process

    in which the

    in-

    dividual

    consciously processes

    information

    (stimuli)

    as he

    or she makes

    decisions or an

    automatic

    pro-

    cess that does not

    require

    conscious

    monitoring

    of

    stimuli.

    Evidence from

    encoding

    information

    into

    memory

    also

    suggests

    that

    the

    process

    varies

    in

    re-

    gard

    to

    energy

    or

    attentional

    requirements

    (Hasher

    &

    Zacks,

    1979).

    Encoding

    may

    be

    relatively

    automatic and

    use little

    energy

    from the

    attentional

    mechanism, or it may be highly effortful and

    elaborate.

    Finally,

    this

    distinction also can be

    re-

    cognized

    in

    the area of

    attribution

    theory

    (Feldman,

    1981).

    Kelley

    (1973),

    Jones and Davis

    (1965),

    and

    Weiner

    (1972) postulate

    that

    attributions occur

    as a

    result

    of

    individuals

    seeking explanations

    for

    behavior.

    They

    emphasize

    a rational

    or

    effortful

    process

    in

    which the

    individual

    weighs

    or

    considers

    multiple pieces

    of

    information

    in

    a

    conscious

    at-

    tempt

    to

    explain

    situations or

    understand in-

    dividuals.

    However,

    recent

    research

    points

    out

    that

    sometimes

    one

    responds

    to

    cues in a

    mindless

    manner (Nisbett &

    Wilson,

    1977)

    or one

    responds

    via an

    established

    pattern

    or

    script

    (Abelson,

    1976).

    In

    these

    instances,

    the

    attributional

    process

    requires

    little

    effort.

    Moreover,

    as

    Lowe and

    Kassin

    (1980)

    have

    recently

    argued,

    attributions

    in

    everyday

    situa-

    tions

    may

    be

    guided

    as

    much

    by

    perceptual

    factors

    as

    by

    the

    cognitive

    factors

    emphasized

    in

    ex-

    perimental

    studies.

    These

    two

    dimensions-type

    of

    attribution

    and

    level

    of

    information

    process-

    ing-are

    used to

    develop

    the

    typology

    of

    attribu-

    tional

    principles

    presented

    in

    Table

    1.

    The

    prin-

    ciples

    associated with

    a

    high

    level of

    information

    processing

    have received

    widespread

    attention

    and,

    therefore,

    will

    not be

    discussed

    further.

    However,

    the

    attributional

    principles

    associated with

    a

    low

    level of

    information

    processing

    have

    not

    received

    sufficient

    consideration.

    51

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    4/12

    Tablel

    Principles

    Used

    by

    Attribution

    Theories

    Level of

    Type

    of

    Attribution

    Level

    f

    Information

    Personal

    Processing

    Causality

    Responsibility

    Characteristics

    High

    Covariation

    Intent

    Correspondence

    Configuration

    Control

    Justification

    Low Salience Formal role

    Prototype

    fit

    Temporal

    proximity

    Critical

    feature fit

    Spatial

    proximity

    Context

    Causality

    The amount of

    information

    processing

    needed to

    make

    causal

    assessments can be

    minimized

    in

    sev-

    eral

    ways.

    The

    foremost

    is that

    people generally

    may

    be satisfied

    with

    the first

    satisfactory

    causal

    explanation

    that comes

    to

    mind

    (Taylor

    &

    Fiske,

    1978).

    Thus,

    if

    there were some mechanism

    by

    which more

    plausible explanations

    would

    be

    consi-

    dered

    first,

    very

    few

    (and

    frequently

    only one)

    plausible

    explanations

    would be considered

    in

    most

    situations.

    Taylor

    and Fiske

    argue

    that,

    for

    the

    most

    part,

    attention is

    such

    a mechanism

    and

    that

    it,

    in

    turn,

    is directed to a

    large

    extent

    by

    salience.

    Accordingly,

    people

    attend

    to and see

    causality

    in salient sources. To

    Taylor

    and

    Fiske,

    such

    salience arises either from distinctive

    qualities

    of the

    social stimulus or from the

    centrality

    of

    a

    particular

    cause

    in

    an attributor's

    memory

    schema.

    Their

    point

    seems

    well

    taken,

    but there

    may

    be

    factors other

    than

    salience

    that

    direct attention

    to

    plausible

    causes.

    One

    may

    be

    a

    tendency

    to look for

    causes that are physically proximal to an effect; this

    would be

    particularly

    important

    when

    visual search

    for

    causes

    predominate.

    For

    example, equipment

    breakdowns

    often are

    attributed to

    the

    operator

    rather than defective

    materials or

    poor

    mainte-

    nance. Another

    factor,

    temporal

    proximity

    or

    closeness

    in

    time,

    would

    seem to

    be

    particularly

    im-

    portant

    when

    searching

    memory

    for

    possible

    causes. Because

    common

    sense

    suggests

    that

    causes

    must

    precede

    results,

    one

    may

    merely

    search back-

    ward

    from

    the event

    in

    question

    to the

    first

    plau-

    sible cause when

    making

    causal

    attributions.

    If,

    as

    Hasher

    and Zacks

    (1979) argue, temporal

    informa-

    tion

    is

    automatically

    encoded or

    if

    temporal

    order

    can

    be

    inferred from

    well learned

    scripts

    (Schank

    &

    Ableson,

    1977),

    such

    a

    limited

    search

    should be

    quite

    feasible and

    may

    not

    be

    consciously

    directed

    or monitored. For

    example,

    a

    change

    in

    organiza-

    tional

    performance

    often

    is

    attributed to

    im-

    mediately

    prior

    management

    decisions rather than

    to

    events

    in

    the more distant

    past

    (i.e.,

    the

    develop-

    ment of inferior

    products).

    In

    short,

    it

    is

    argued

    that

    causality

    often

    may

    be

    assessed

    by

    either

    a

    passively (salience)

    or

    heuris-

    tically

    (proximity)

    directed search for the first

    suffi-

    cient cause for an

    outcome.

    In

    other

    words,

    once

    a

    sufficient cause has been

    identified,

    search for or

    evaluation of other possible causes should be ter-

    minated. Because salient or

    proximal

    causes

    (such

    as

    people)

    tend to be considered first and often are

    plausible explanations, they

    should

    predominate

    as

    explanations

    of

    causality.

    Such

    processes

    should

    place

    limited demands on the human information

    processing

    system.

    The

    present

    authors feel that

    these

    may

    be better

    descriptions

    of humans'

    typical

    causal assessments

    than the

    quasiscientific

    mod-

    els-associated

    with

    a

    high

    level

    of

    information

    processing

    in Table

    1.

    Responsibility

    Although

    it is

    comforting

    to believe

    that

    respon-

    sibility

    assessments

    are

    guided by logical

    principles

    such as

    assessment

    of

    intent,

    control,

    or

    justifica-

    tion

    (and

    in

    legal proceedings

    they

    are),

    it is sus-

    pected

    that

    in

    many

    situations

    (or

    for

    many

    people)

    cognitively

    simpler,

    albeit

    less

    logical,

    principles

    predominate.

    For

    example,

    perceivers

    may assign

    responsibility

    for

    role

    related

    outcomes

    to role oc-

    cupants (Hamilton,

    1980),

    assuming

    that these

    in-

    dividuals,

    through

    different

    actions,

    could have

    produced

    different

    outcomes.

    In

    administering

    sanctions, mitigating

    circumstances

    may

    not

    even

    be

    considered.

    Thus,

    leaders

    may

    be

    replaced

    when

    their

    subordinates

    perform

    poorly,

    or

    employees

    may

    be sanctioned

    for failure

    to

    attain

    goals

    without

    extensive

    analysis

    on the

    part

    of

    the sanc-

    tioner. Such

    sanctions

    may

    be

    administered

    even

    52

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    5/12

    when

    recipients

    lack control over outcomes

    for

    which

    they

    are held

    responsible

    because sanctioners

    use

    cognitively simple

    rules for

    determining respon-

    sibility

    or

    because

    organizational policy

    determines

    sanctioning

    (Green

    &

    Mitchell,

    1979).

    Personal

    Characteristics

    Sometimes trait or personal characteristic assess-

    ments

    may

    reflect

    quasi-logical

    inferences

    from

    behavior as

    suggested by

    Jones and Davis

    (1965).

    However,

    it

    is

    suggested

    that less

    conscious

    and

    less

    demanding

    means of

    forming impressions

    of

    others

    may predominate.

    In

    many

    situations,

    attention

    and

    information

    processing capacity

    is

    directed

    at

    task,

    situational,

    or

    self-related

    factors while in-

    teraction

    takes

    place,

    thereby

    precluding

    elaborate

    or

    consciously

    controlled

    assessments of

    personal

    qualities.

    Nevertheless,

    trait

    assessments still

    seem

    to be formed in

    these

    situations,

    suggesting

    more

    automatic processes.

    How

    might

    such

    processing proceed?

    On the sim-

    plest

    level,

    people

    might

    be

    classified

    by

    the fit

    of

    their

    behavior,

    appearance,

    dress,

    and

    so on

    with

    category

    prototypes (Cantor

    &

    Mischel, 1979;

    Lord,

    Foti,

    &

    Phillips,

    1982).

    Alternatively,

    there

    may

    be

    critical

    features that

    map

    social

    stimuli

    into

    contrasting

    sets,

    as

    with

    sex.

    Also,

    people may

    use

    context

    as

    a

    means

    of

    assessing

    traits

    (people

    in

    libraries

    are

    intellectual).

    Such

    principles

    would

    equate

    the

    perceptual

    organization

    that

    makes stim-

    uli

    meaningful

    with

    trait

    ascriptions.

    That

    is,

    trait

    ascriptions would result from processes by which

    people

    recognize

    stimuli,

    not

    from

    logical

    analysis.

    On

    a

    somewhat

    more

    sophisticated level,

    subjects

    may

    generalize

    from

    ascribed

    traits to

    other

    traits.

    Thus,

    they

    may

    base

    inferences on

    implicit

    theories

    or

    stereotypes

    that

    are

    thought

    to

    be

    generally

    ap-

    plicable.

    Again,

    such

    inferences

    may

    not

    be

    con-

    scious,

    may

    not use

    much

    information

    processing

    capacity,

    and

    often

    may

    not

    be

    logical (or

    correct).

    A

    more

    cognitively

    demanding

    means

    to

    assess

    personal

    qualities

    would

    incorporate

    information

    on

    intent,

    reliability

    (consistency),

    or

    personal

    ver-

    sus situational determinism. At this level, one

    would

    expect

    conscious

    awareness of

    an

    attribu-

    tional

    process

    and

    a

    need

    to

    devote

    a

    substantial

    portion

    of

    one's

    controllable

    processing

    capacity

    to

    the

    attributional

    task.

    Boundary

    Conditions

    The

    model,

    as

    presented

    in Table

    1,

    argues

    that

    attributional

    processes

    differ

    in the

    type

    of infor-

    mation

    processing

    that is

    used.

    Among

    the

    potential

    determinants of this difference in

    effortfulness or

    consciousness

    of

    information

    processing

    are

    such

    factors as

    situational

    characteristics,

    the

    attributor's

    motives,

    and

    individual

    differences

    among

    attributors.

    Whether the

    attributor

    engages

    in an

    active,

    conscious

    effort to

    understand

    his

    world or

    whether she/he

    expends

    little

    energy,

    passively

    processing

    information

    may

    be

    deter-

    mined

    by

    such

    boundary

    conditions. It is

    believed

    that

    analysis

    of such

    boundary

    conditions can be

    best

    accomplished

    by

    assuming

    a

    minimum

    level of

    information

    processing

    and

    then

    articulating

    the

    factors that

    should

    increase the

    amount of

    process-

    ing

    capacity

    allocated to

    attributional

    questions.

    In

    other

    words,

    it

    is believed

    that

    attributional

    ques-

    tions generally are described by the passive, un-

    conscious

    processes

    described in

    the bottom row

    of

    Table

    1;

    the

    less

    common,

    controlled

    processes

    should

    occur

    only

    under

    the

    specific

    circumstances

    noted below.

    However,

    due to

    their more

    conscious

    nature,

    controlled

    processes

    should

    predominate

    in

    memory

    for

    or

    description

    of attributional

    pro-

    cesses.

    There

    are several

    situational or contextual

    factors

    that

    should

    increase the

    level of information

    pro-

    cessing

    involved in

    forming

    attributions. One

    con-

    cerns the

    extent to

    which

    the actions

    or

    outcomes

    being evaluated are consistent with past impressions

    of an

    actor.

    Actions that

    are

    inconsistent with

    prior

    expectations

    should

    lead

    attributors

    to conscious

    assessment of

    causality

    (Feldman,

    1981;

    Wong

    &

    Weiner,

    1981).

    A

    second

    factor concerns norms

    or

    cues

    inherent in a

    situation. For

    example,

    a

    perfor-

    mance

    evaluation

    context

    would

    probably produce

    more

    systematic,

    consciously

    directed

    attributions

    than occur

    in the

    day

    to

    day

    interactions

    between

    superiors

    and

    subordinates.

    Along

    these

    lines,

    Wil-

    son,

    Hull,

    and

    Johnson

    (1981)

    reported

    that

    reason-

    ing primes

    were

    required

    before

    subjects

    conscious-

    ly

    attended

    to

    attributional

    questions

    associated

    with the intrinsic interest of a task. A third factor

    concerns the

    informational

    context to which an

    at-

    tributor

    is

    exposed.

    When

    attributionally

    relevant

    information is

    easily

    available and is in

    a form

    con-

    sistent

    with

    conscious,

    reflective

    processes

    (e.g.,

    53

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    6/12

    written

    or

    graphic

    information),

    more

    cognitive

    modes

    of

    attributions

    should be elicited

    (Lowe

    &

    Kassin,

    1980).

    A final situational factor

    is the available

    process-

    ing capacity

    that can

    be allocated

    to

    the

    attribu-

    tional task.

    Hasher and

    Zacks

    (1979)

    have

    argued

    that controlled

    or effortful

    processing

    involves a

    limited

    capacity

    conscious

    system,

    whereas

    auto-

    matic

    processing

    is

    relatively independent

    of

    such

    contraints.

    Thus,

    other

    cognitive

    tasks that also

    re-

    quire

    use of this

    limited

    attentional

    capacity

    would interfere with controlled

    processing

    but not

    with automatic

    processing.

    Therefore,

    it

    is

    argued

    that

    people

    will

    use controlled

    processes

    only

    when

    there

    is

    available

    capacity-that

    is,

    capacity

    not be-

    ing

    used

    by

    task,

    self,

    or

    socially

    related

    cognitive

    activities.

    Motivational

    factors

    also

    may

    serve to

    increase

    the level of information

    processing

    directed to

    at-

    tributional

    questions.

    For

    example,

    the level

    of in-

    formation processing may be dependent on the in-

    volvements

    of

    the

    observer.

    This

    contingency

    as-

    pect

    of information

    processing

    has

    been

    suggested

    in

    several

    decision contexts

    (Chaiken,

    1980;

    Langer,

    Blank,

    &

    Chanowitz, 1978;

    McAllister, Mitchell,

    &

    Beach,

    1979).

    Research

    that has

    manipulated

    pro-

    spect

    of

    future interaction

    (Berscheid,

    Graziano,

    Monson,

    &

    Dermer,

    1976)

    also

    lends

    some

    limited

    support

    to the

    notion

    that

    involvement

    may

    be a de-

    terminant

    of

    the extent

    of information

    processing.

    Persons

    who

    anticipate

    future

    interactions

    (involve-

    ment)

    attend

    more to

    the

    other

    and

    remember

    more

    about the other than do persons who anticipate no

    interaction-suggesting

    that

    they

    differ

    in

    informa-

    tion

    processing.

    In

    a

    related

    vein,

    Jones

    and Davis

    (1965)

    argue

    that

    hedonic

    relevance

    leads

    to

    disposi-

    tional

    attributions.

    Perhaps

    hedonic

    relevance,

    per-

    sonal

    importance

    or

    involvement,

    also increases

    at-

    tentional

    energy

    or the

    effort

    expended

    in informa-

    tion

    processing.

    An

    individual

    who

    depends

    on

    another for

    rewards

    probably

    engages

    in

    conscious

    efforts

    to understand

    that other

    individual.

    Another

    constraint

    related

    to

    involvement

    might

    focus

    on the

    need for

    control.

    Kelley (1971)

    postu-

    lates

    that one

    not

    only

    seeks to

    understand

    the

    world but also seeks to control it.

    Wong

    and Wei-

    ner's

    (1981)

    assertion

    that

    individuals

    do

    not think

    in

    terms of

    attributions

    unless

    confronted

    with

    failure

    or

    unexpected

    events

    would

    be consistent

    with

    Kelley's argument.

    Faced

    with

    failure

    or sur-

    prised by

    unanticipated

    results,

    individuals

    might

    search

    for

    ways

    to avoid future failures or

    unplea-

    sant

    surprises.

    Such an

    argument

    has

    implications

    regarding

    information

    processing.

    In

    situations in

    which

    individuals feel

    in

    control,

    they may process

    information

    automatically.

    However,

    in

    situations

    in

    which individuals

    perceive

    a

    need to

    establish

    control

    of the

    environment, they probably engage

    in

    controlled

    or

    effortful

    processing.

    Thus,

    when

    introduced to new

    co-workers,

    others should active-

    ly

    attend to the new workers' behaviors

    and

    attempt

    to size them

    up.

    Still another motivational

    constraint

    may

    be the

    need

    for

    cognitive

    consistency.

    Festinger's

    (1957)

    theory

    of

    cognitive

    dissonance

    postulates

    that

    peo-

    ple

    find

    cognitive

    dissonance

    stressful and seek to

    be

    cognitively

    consonant.

    When dissonance does

    occur,

    it can be reduced

    by

    changing

    a

    cognition,

    adding

    new

    cognitions,

    or

    reducing

    the

    importance

    of a

    cognition.

    These

    cognitive

    changes

    can

    occur

    unconsciously; however, in situations of extreme

    stress

    and when

    both

    cognitions

    are

    important,

    the

    individual

    may

    have

    to

    engage

    in

    an active

    search or

    review

    of information to reduce

    the dissonance.

    The vast

    body

    of research

    on individual

    dif-

    ferences

    in information

    processing

    is

    beyond

    the

    scope

    of this

    paper.

    However,

    the

    boundary

    condi-

    tion

    or

    moderating

    effect

    of individual

    differences

    upon

    the

    extent

    of

    required

    information

    processing

    must

    be

    recognized.

    For

    example,

    field

    dependence

    (Witkin

    &

    Goodenough,

    1977),

    repression-sensiti-

    zation

    (Byrne,

    1964),

    and

    self-esteem

    (Weiss

    &

    Knight, 1980) are just three of the many areas that

    may

    have

    implications

    for

    the

    effortfulness

    of

    in-

    formation

    processing

    related

    to

    attributional

    ques-

    tions.

    Ordering

    of

    Attributional

    and

    Behavioral

    Processes

    Up

    to

    this

    point,

    the

    analysis

    has avoided

    the

    question

    of

    how attributions

    tie into

    behavior

    or at-

    titude

    change.

    In most

    typical

    attributional

    models,

    attributional

    assessments

    precede

    behavior.

    For

    ex-

    ample,

    the

    Green

    and Mitchell

    (1979)

    model

    depicts

    leaders' causal attributions as preceding their

    behavior

    toward

    subordinates.

    This

    is

    fully

    consis-

    tent

    with the

    logical,

    cognitive

    emphasis

    found

    in

    the

    attributional

    models associated

    with a

    high

    level

    of

    information

    processing

    in

    Table

    1.

    However,

    54

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    7/12

    there

    may

    be

    many

    instances

    in which behavior

    is

    contrained or determined

    by

    external

    factors,

    and

    in

    those situations

    attributions

    may

    follow

    behaviors as

    part

    of a

    justification

    or

    rationaliza-

    tion

    process (Staw,

    1980). Experimental

    evidence

    il-

    lustrating

    such

    ordering

    has

    been

    provided

    by

    Strickland

    (1958)

    in

    the area of

    monitoring

    behavior and more recently by Wilson, Hull, and

    Johnson

    (1981)

    in

    assessing

    the

    relationship

    of

    over-

    justification

    to

    intrinsic

    motivation.

    Kipnis,

    Schmidt,

    Price,

    and

    Stitt

    (1981)

    also found

    that

    behavioral

    differences

    in

    the

    leadership styles

    and

    influence

    tactics

    of

    managers

    of

    experimental

    work

    crews

    induced

    managers

    to make different

    attribu-

    tions

    concerning

    workers' motivation and

    different

    evaluations

    of

    workers'

    performance.

    A

    somewhat

    more

    complex picture

    of

    ordering

    emerges

    when

    one

    considers the

    relations

    among

    the

    various

    types

    of

    attributional

    principles presented

    in

    Table 1.

    For

    example,

    consider

    the

    question

    of

    whether an individual

    possesses

    leadership

    quali-

    ties.

    Highly

    cognitive

    models

    explaining

    leadership

    perceptions (Calder,

    1977)

    might

    build

    on

    Jones

    and

    Davis's

    (1965)

    principles

    of

    correspondent

    in-

    ference to

    explain

    attributional

    processes,

    whereas

    less

    cognitive

    models

    might rely

    on

    principles

    such

    as the match

    of the

    stimulus

    person

    in

    question

    to

    the

    perceiver's

    leadership

    prototype (Lord

    et

    al.,

    1982).

    Recent

    experimental

    studies now

    suggest

    that

    this

    latter

    more

    automatic

    process

    may precede

    the

    former,

    more

    controlled,

    process

    in

    explaining

    leadership perceptions

    (Cronshaw

    &

    Lord,

    1982;

    Phillips & Lord, 1981).

    These

    results

    are

    consistent with

    an

    information

    processing

    model in

    which

    social

    information is en-

    coded

    and

    organized

    via

    automatic

    processes

    that

    required

    minimal

    processing

    capacity

    and

    minimal

    conscious

    attention. If

    subsequent

    causal

    assess-

    ments

    were

    made,

    they

    would

    be based

    on the

    infor-

    mation

    input

    and

    organized

    by

    such

    automatic

    pro-

    cesses.

    This

    model

    also is

    consistent

    with

    recent

    work

    by

    Wilson et al.

    (1981)

    and

    Wong

    and

    Weiner

    (1981)

    that used

    unobtrusive

    methodologies

    that

    did

    not

    force

    subjects

    to

    think

    in

    attributional

    terms.

    They found that

    subjects

    did not think in attribu-

    tional

    terms

    unless

    induced

    to

    do

    so

    by

    inconsisten-

    cies

    of

    results

    with

    expectations,

    failures,

    or ex-

    perimenter

    provided

    reasoning primes

    that

    engage

    inferential

    processes.

    Implications

    of

    Attributional

    Typology

    It has

    been

    argued

    that

    researchers

    often have

    failed to

    distinguish adequately

    among

    different

    at-

    tributional

    questions

    and

    among

    the

    various

    infor-

    mational

    processes by

    which

    these

    questions

    could

    be

    addressed

    by

    attributors.

    Moreover,

    attribu-

    tional

    processes usually

    have

    been

    considered

    ante-

    cedents

    to

    associated

    cognitive

    or

    behavioral

    pro-

    cesses

    of

    attributors.

    The

    possibility

    that

    attribu-

    tions

    follow

    related

    cognitions

    or

    behaviors,

    as

    they

    may

    in

    rationalization or

    reconstructive

    processes,

    rarely

    has

    been

    considered.

    Theoretical

    Implications

    One obvious

    implication

    of this

    typology

    is that

    no

    single

    model of

    attributional

    processes

    seems

    adequate

    for

    describing

    the

    thought processes

    grouped

    under the

    rubric

    of

    attribution

    theory.

    Humans

    seem

    quite

    eclectic in the

    principles they

    can use to form attributions. In order to describe at-

    tributional

    processes

    accurately,

    therefore,

    re-

    searchers

    must be

    equally

    eclectic.

    Further,

    they

    must be

    careful

    in

    generalizing

    models of

    attribu-

    tional

    processes

    developed

    in a

    particular

    context

    to

    other

    types

    of

    attributional

    questions

    or other

    situa-

    tions.

    The

    potential

    for

    overgeneralization

    can

    easily

    be

    illustrated

    by

    considering superiors'

    attributions

    concerning

    their

    subordinates.

    Superiors'

    responses

    to

    poor

    performing

    subordinates

    might

    best

    be de-

    scribed

    by

    principles

    related

    to

    causality

    if

    their

    focus is on understanding poor performance or on

    generating

    remedial

    responses

    (e.g.,

    training pro-

    grams).

    Alternatively,

    if

    the

    emphasis

    is on

    admini-

    stering

    sanctions to

    poor

    performing

    employees,

    superiors

    may

    be

    described

    better

    by

    the

    principles

    related to

    responsibility

    attributions in

    Table 1.

    Finally,

    if

    superiors

    are

    required

    to

    predict

    future

    subordinate

    behavior,

    it would

    be

    expected

    that

    principles pertinent

    to

    assessing

    personal

    qualities

    would be

    most

    descriptive.

    The

    problem

    of

    overgeneralization

    becomes

    more

    severe

    if one

    simultaneously

    considers

    both

    dimen-

    sions of Table

    1,

    for

    superiors may

    address

    some

    at-

    tributional

    questions

    in

    a

    conscious,

    logical

    fashion

    while

    using

    more

    automatic

    processes

    for other

    questions.

    For

    example, personal

    qualities

    (traits)

    may

    be

    continually

    assessed

    during

    normal

    interac-

    tions

    with

    subordinates

    using fairly

    automatic

    pro-

    55

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    8/12

    cesses,

    but

    more controlled

    processes

    may predomi-

    nate

    under the

    infrequent

    situations

    in

    which

    sanc-

    tions

    are

    considered. The

    consequences

    of

    this

    ex-

    ample

    for

    researchers

    concerned with

    superior

    at-

    tributional

    processes

    are

    straightforward.

    That

    is,

    one must

    not

    only

    identify

    attributional

    principles,

    but

    must also

    specify

    the

    type

    of

    questions

    and

    situational

    factors that determine which principles

    are

    likely

    to

    be

    used.

    A

    second

    implication

    for scientific

    inquiry

    con-

    cerns

    the

    chameleon-like

    nature of human

    attribu-

    tors.

    It is

    maintained

    that the

    type

    of

    information

    processes

    used to

    form attributions varies with

    the

    motivation

    of

    attributors,

    the

    form and

    amount of

    information

    presented

    them,

    the

    available

    process-

    ing capacity

    to

    devote

    to

    attributional

    questions,

    and the

    primes

    or cues inherent in

    particular

    situa-

    tions. Such

    factors,

    alone

    or

    in

    combination,

    may

    easily

    determine whether controlled or

    more

    automatic

    processes

    are

    used

    in

    forming

    attribu-

    tions.

    Thus,

    the troublesome

    problem

    of demand

    characteristics

    (Orne, 1969)

    seems

    particularly

    ger-

    mane

    to

    studies of attributional

    processes.

    As

    Lowe

    and

    Kassin

    (1980) point

    out,

    experimenters

    in-

    terested

    in

    testing

    more

    cognitive

    (controlled)

    models of attributional

    processes

    can

    easily

    induce

    such

    processes by

    their choice of

    methodologies.

    A

    related

    problem

    concerns

    the obtrusiveness and

    limitations of data collection

    strategies.

    Wong

    and

    Weiner

    (1981) point

    out

    that most data on attribu-

    tional

    processes

    are collected via

    self-report

    pro-

    cedures.

    Such

    procedures

    may prime

    attributional

    processes, leading subjects to think in terms of at-

    tributional

    questions

    they

    otherwise

    would not ad-

    dress.

    Moreover,

    such measures

    may

    be biased in

    that the more automatic

    processes

    may

    be

    less

    ac-

    cessible

    to

    subjects (Nisbett

    &

    Wilson,

    1977;

    Wilson

    et

    al.,

    1981)

    and, therefore,

    may

    not

    be

    adequately

    measured

    by self-report techniques.

    There is a

    clear

    need

    for

    development

    of

    more

    varied methodolo-

    gies

    for

    measuring

    attributions. Such methodo-

    logies

    should be

    designed

    to

    tap

    both

    perceptual

    and more

    cognitive processes

    and

    should not be

    based

    solely

    on

    subjects' retrospective

    insight

    into

    their own information

    processing.

    One

    potential

    approach

    would be

    to

    investigate

    attributions

    by

    analyzing

    protocols

    generated by

    subjects

    while

    performing

    the behavior

    in

    question.

    As Ericsson and

    Simon

    (1980)

    note,

    to be

    accurate

    such verbal

    reports

    should be concurrent rather

    than

    retrospective

    and should

    describe

    ongoing pro-

    cesses

    rather than

    judgments

    requiring

    inferences or

    generalizations.

    Alternatively,

    one

    could

    use

    open-

    ended

    questions

    to

    probe

    attributions rather

    than

    the more

    common

    ratings

    of

    several

    experimenter

    provided

    causal

    sources

    (e.g., ability).

    Ratings

    of

    explicit

    causal

    sources

    are

    troublesome

    because

    items may prime the causal source being rated and

    thereby

    may

    affect

    ratings.

    One

    recent

    study (Kro-

    eck,

    1982)

    compared

    open-ended

    responses

    that

    were content

    analyzed

    by

    a

    procedure

    based

    on

    Elig

    and Frieze

    (1979)

    to

    ratings

    of a

    list of

    plausible

    causal sources

    (e.g.,

    effort,

    ability, luck).

    Kroeck

    found

    frequent

    mention of

    perceptually

    based

    in-

    formation

    (e.g.,

    appearance)

    when

    using

    an

    open

    format measure of

    attributions.

    Moreover,

    such

    in-

    formation

    was

    more

    predictive

    of

    rating

    on

    some

    dimensions

    (leadership

    potential

    and

    promotability)

    than

    was a

    structured format in

    which

    subjects

    rated

    experimenter provided

    causes.

    A final theoretical concern stems from the

    poten-

    tial for what can be

    labeled

    hybrid

    attributional

    processes.

    Such

    processes

    would involve

    combina-

    tions

    of

    the

    principles presented

    in

    Table 1.

    Hybrid

    processes

    would seem

    particularly

    likely

    when at-

    tributional

    questions

    are

    addressed

    over

    a

    period

    of

    time in which the informational or

    motivational

    perspective

    of the

    attributor

    may

    change

    or

    when

    attributional

    processes

    are related to other

    behavioral

    or attitudinal

    processes.

    One

    example

    of

    a

    hybrid process may

    be when information that is

    encoded,

    stored,

    and retrieved

    by

    automatic

    pro-

    cesses is evaluated by more controlled processes at

    some later

    point

    in time. For

    example,

    workers

    may

    be

    categorized

    as either

    high

    or low

    performer types

    over

    many

    interactions

    and

    with

    little

    systematic

    or

    conscious

    processing

    by superiors.

    Later

    on,

    super-

    iors

    may consciously

    assess

    subordinates

    ability,

    a

    process

    that on the surface resembles

    a controlled

    process.

    However,

    as

    Lingle

    and

    Ostrom

    (1979)

    have

    demonstrated,

    this latter

    judgment

    may

    be

    derived

    from

    prior judgments.

    Futhermore,

    prior

    judgments

    may

    lead to selective

    recall of informa-

    tion

    consistent

    with

    already

    formed

    opinions

    (Hastie, 1981).

    The

    reverse

    process

    also

    may

    occur.

    Once a

    quality

    is

    attributed

    to

    an individual

    by

    some

    conscious,

    controlled

    process,

    this label

    may

    lead

    to

    subsequent

    automatic

    encoding, storage,

    and retrieval

    of information consistent

    with

    such

    a

    label. For

    example,

    if

    good performance

    is ex-

    56

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    9/12

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    10/12

    hybrid processes

    suggests

    that

    such

    attributions

    must

    be

    considered as

    evolving

    across

    time rather

    than as a

    momentary phenomena

    occuring

    in

    or at a

    single experimental

    setting.

    One

    potentially

    impor-

    tant

    question

    raised

    by

    this

    perspective

    is

    whether

    attributions

    formed

    immediately

    after

    observations

    of

    behavior or

    outcomes would

    be

    qualitatively

    dif-

    ferent from

    delayed

    attributions. It

    is

    suspected that

    immediate

    attributions would

    be more

    dependent

    on

    perceptual

    factors,

    whereas

    delayed

    attributions

    may depend

    primarily

    on the

    ease with

    which attri-

    butors can recall

    information,

    as is

    suggested by

    the

    findings

    of Ross and

    Sicoly

    (1979).

    A

    related

    con-

    sideration is that

    attributions that

    emerge

    as a

    response

    to

    novel stimuli

    may

    differ from

    attribu-

    tions

    in

    response

    to familiar stimuli.

    Perhaps

    novel

    attributions

    require

    a

    more active mode of

    informa-

    tion

    processing

    than do

    attributions

    that

    only

    con-

    firm

    or

    modify

    previous

    assessments.

    It has been

    suggested

    here that attributions often

    may

    involve various levels of information

    process-

    ing

    occuring

    at

    multiple points

    in

    time. This

    sugges-

    tion raises

    questions

    concerning

    the

    dynamic

    as-

    pects

    of

    attributional

    processes.

    More

    specifically,

    do

    changes

    in

    factors such as information

    load,

    perceptual

    salience,

    or an

    attributor's

    motivation

    produce

    shifts

    in

    the

    level of

    attributional

    processes

    used in

    addressing

    a

    single

    attributional

    question?

    Also,

    if

    such

    changes

    do

    occur,

    are

    attributors

    aware that

    they

    are

    using qualitatively

    different

    processes?.

    Finally,

    the

    methodological

    issue of

    how

    best to

    measure attributions deserves attention. Less ob-

    trusive

    measures

    than

    questionnaire

    based

    self-

    report

    techniques

    need

    to be

    developed. Moreover,

    attention should be

    directed

    at

    the

    problem

    of

    ade-

    quate

    measurement of

    automatic

    attributional

    pro-

    cesses

    when

    attributors

    may

    not be

    consciously

    aware of

    their use.

    In

    conclusion,

    it

    has

    been

    argued

    that

    researchers

    need

    to

    consider both

    the

    type

    of

    attributional

    ques-

    tion and

    the

    amount of

    information

    processing

    con-

    sistent with

    situational

    factors or

    attributor motiva-

    tion

    when

    developing

    models

    of

    attributional

    pro-

    cesses. A

    typology

    of

    principles

    organized

    along

    these two dimensions and the

    research

    implications

    of this

    typology

    have been

    presented.

    The

    authors

    hope

    this

    leads to a

    more

    comprehensive

    view

    of at-

    tributional

    processes

    than is

    typically

    found in

    the

    applied

    literature.

    References

    Abelson,

    R. P.

    Script processing

    in attitude formation

    and deci-

    sion

    making.

    In

    J. S. Carrol & J. W.

    Payne (Eds.),

    Cognition

    and social behavior.

    Hillsdale,

    N. J.:

    Erlbaum,

    1976.

    Berscheid, E., Graziano,

    W.

    G., Monson,

    T.

    C.,

    &

    Dermer,

    M.

    Outcome

    dependency:

    Attention, attribution,

    and attraction.

    Journal

    of Personality

    and Social

    Psychology,

    1976,

    34,

    978-989.

    Binning,

    J.

    F.,

    &

    Lord,

    R.

    G.

    Boundary

    conditions

    for

    perfor-

    mance cue effects on

    group process ratings: Familiarity

    versus

    type

    of feedback.

    Organizational

    Behavior and

    Human Per-

    formance,

    1980,

    26,

    115-130.

    Byrne,

    D.

    Repression-sensitization

    as

    a

    dimension

    of

    personal-

    ity. Progress

    in

    Experimental Personality

    Research,

    1964, 1,

    169-220.

    Calder,

    B. J. An

    attribution

    theory

    of

    leadership.

    In B. M.

    Staw

    &

    G.

    R.

    Salancik

    (Eds.),

    New directions

    in

    organizational

    behavior.

    Chicago:

    St. Clair

    Press, 1977,

    179-204.

    Cantor, N., & Mischel, W. Prototypes in person perception. In

    L.

    Berkowitz

    (Ed.),

    Advances

    in

    experimental

    social

    psy-

    chology

    (Vol. 12).

    New York: Academic

    Press, 1979,

    3-52.

    Chaiken,

    S. Heuristic versus

    systematic

    information

    processing

    and

    the use

    of

    source versus

    message

    cues

    in

    persuasion.

    Jour-

    nal

    of

    Personality

    and Social

    Psychology,

    1980, 39,

    752-766.

    Cronshaw,

    S.

    F.,

    &

    Lord,

    R.

    G.

    Perceputal

    versus

    cognitive

    determinants

    of causal

    attributions and

    leadership. Paper

    presented

    at

    the

    meeting

    of the

    Midwest

    Academy

    of

    Manage-

    ment, Columbus, Ohio,

    1982.

    Deci,

    E. L. Intrinsic

    motivation.

    New

    York: Plenum

    Press,

    1975.

    Elig,

    T.

    W.,

    &

    Frieze,

    I.

    H.

    Measuring

    causal attributions

    for

    success and failure. Journal

    of

    Personality

    and

    Social

    Psychology,

    1979, 37,

    621-624.

    Ericsson,

    K.

    A.,

    &

    Simon,

    H. A. Verbal

    reports

    as

    data.

    Psychological

    Review,

    1980,

    87,

    215-251.

    Feldman,

    J.

    Beyond

    attribution

    theory: Cognitive

    processes

    in

    performance

    appraisal.

    Journal

    of Applied

    Psychology,

    1981,

    66,

    127-148.

    Festinger,

    L. A

    theory of

    cognitive

    dissonance.

    New York:

    Row,

    Peterson,

    1957.

    Fincham,

    F.

    D.,

    &

    Jaspars,

    J.

    M.

    Attribution of

    responsibility:

    From man the scientist to man the lawyer. In L. Berkowitz

    (Ed.),

    Advances

    in

    experimental

    social

    psychology

    (Vol.

    13).

    New York:

    Academic

    Press, 1980,

    81-138.

    Fisher,

    C. D. The effects of

    personal

    control,

    competence,

    and

    extrinsic reward

    systems

    on intrinsic motivation.

    Organization

    Behavior and Human

    Performance,

    1978, 21,

    273-288.

    58

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    11/12

    Foti,

    R. J.

    Fraser,

    S.

    L.,

    &

    Lord,

    R. G. The effects

    of

    leadership

    labels and

    prototypes

    on

    perceptions

    of

    political

    leaders.

    Jour-

    nal

    of Applied Psychology,

    1982,

    67,

    326-333.

    Green,

    S.

    G.,

    &

    Liden,

    R. C. Contextual and attributional

    in-

    fluences on control decisions.

    Journal

    of Applied Psychology,

    1980, 65,

    453-458.

    Green,

    S.

    G.,

    &

    Mitchell,

    T. R.

    Attributional

    processes

    of

    leaders in leader-member

    interactions.

    Organizational

    Behavior and Human Performance, 1979, 23, 429-458.

    Hamilton,

    V.

    L.

    Intuitive

    psychologist

    or

    intuitive

    lawyer?

    Alter-

    native

    models

    of

    the

    attribution

    process.

    Journal

    of

    Personal-

    ity

    and Social

    Psychology,

    1980, 39,

    767-772.

    Hasher, L.,

    &

    Zacks,

    R. T.

    Automatic

    and effortful

    processes

    in

    memory.

    Journal

    of

    Experimental

    Psychology:

    General,

    1979,

    108,

    356-388.

    Hastie,

    R. Schematic

    principles

    in

    human

    memory.

    In E.

    T.

    Hig-

    gins,

    C. P.

    Herman,

    & M. P.

    Zanna

    (Eds.),

    Social

    cognition:

    The

    Ontario

    symposium

    on

    personality

    and

    social

    psychology.

    Hillsdale,

    N. J.:

    Erlbaum,

    1981,

    39-88.

    Heider,

    F.

    The

    psychology of

    interpersonal

    relations. New York:

    John

    Wiley,

    1958.

    Ilgen, D. R., & Knowlton, W. A., Jr. Performance attributional

    effects on feedback from

    superiors.

    Organizational

    Behavior

    and

    Human

    Performance,

    1980, 25,

    441-456.

    Jones,

    E.

    E.,

    &

    Davis,

    K. E.

    From

    acts to

    dispositions:

    The

    at-

    tribution

    process

    in

    person

    perception.

    In L.

    Berkowitz

    (Ed.),

    Advances

    in

    experimental

    social

    psychology

    (Vol.

    2).

    New

    York:

    Academic

    Press,

    1965,

    219-266.

    Kelley,

    H. H.

    Attribution

    in

    social

    interaction.

    In E.

    E.

    Jones

    (Ed.),

    Attribution:

    Perceiving

    the

    causes

    of

    behavior.

    Mor-

    ristown,

    N.

    J.:

    General

    Learning

    Press, 1971,

    1-26.

    Kelley,

    H. H.

    The

    process

    of

    causal

    attribution.

    American

    Psychologist,

    1973, 28,

    107-128.

    Kipnis,

    D.,

    Schmidt, S.,

    Price, K.,

    &

    Stitt,

    C.

    Why

    do I

    like

    thee:

    Is it

    your

    performance

    or

    my

    orders.

    Journal

    of Applied

    Psychology,

    1981,

    66,

    324-328.

    Knowlton,

    W.

    A.,

    &

    Mitchell,

    T.

    R. Effects of

    causal

    attribu-

    tions

    on

    a

    supervisor's

    evaluation of

    subordinate

    performace.

    Journal

    of

    Applied

    Psychology,

    1980, 65,

    459-466.

    Kroeck,

    K. G.

    Supervisory

    responses

    to

    subordinate

    goal

    attain-

    ment.

    Doctoral

    dissertation,

    The

    University

    of

    Akron,

    1982.

    Langer,

    E.,

    Blank, A.,

    &

    Chanowitz,

    B.

    The

    mindlessness of

    ostensibly

    thoughtful

    action: The

    role of

    placebic

    informa-

    tion

    in

    interpersonal

    interaction.

    Journal

    of

    Personality

    and

    Social

    Psychology,

    1978, 36,

    635-642.

    Lingle,

    J.

    H.,

    &

    Ostrom,

    T.

    M.

    Retrieval

    selectivity

    in

    memory-

    based

    judgments.

    Journal

    of Personality

    and

    Social

    Psychology,

    1979,

    37,

    180-194.

    Lord,

    R.

    G.,

    Foti,

    R.

    J.,

    &

    Phillips,

    J. S. A

    theory

    of

    leadership

    categorization.

    In

    J. G.

    Hunt,

    U.

    Sekaran,

    & C.

    Schriesheim

    (Eds.),

    Leadership:

    Beyond

    establishment

    views.

    Carbondale:

    Southern

    Illinois

    University

    Press,

    1982,

    104-121.

    Lord,

    R.

    G.,

    Binning,

    J.

    F.,

    Rush,

    M.

    C.,

    &

    Thomas,

    J. C.

    The

    effect

    of

    performance

    cues and

    leader

    behavior on

    question-

    naire

    ratings

    of

    leadership

    behavior.

    Organizational

    Behavior

    and

    Human

    Performance,

    1978, 21,

    27-39.

    Lowe,

    C.

    A.,

    &

    Kassin,

    S. M. A

    perceptual

    view of

    attribution:

    Theoretical and

    methodological

    implications. Personality

    and

    Social

    Psychology

    Bulletin, 1980, 6,

    532-542.

    McAllister,

    D.

    W., Mitchell,

    T.

    R.,

    &

    Beach,

    L. R.

    The con-

    tingency model for the selection of decision strategies: An em-

    pirical

    test of the

    significance,

    accountability,

    and

    reversibil-

    ity. Organizational

    Behavior and

    Human

    Performance,

    1979,

    24,

    228-244.

    Mitchell,

    T.

    R.,

    &

    Wood,

    R. E.

    Supervisor's

    response

    to

    subor-

    dinate

    poor performance:

    A test of

    an attributional

    model.

    Organizational

    Behavior and

    Human

    Performance,

    1980,

    25,

    123-138.

    Mitchell,

    T.

    R.,

    Green,

    S.

    G.,

    &

    Wood,

    R. E.

    An attributional

    model of

    leadership

    and

    the

    poor

    performing

    subordinate:

    Development

    and

    validation. In B. M.

    Staw & L.

    L. Cumm-

    ings (Eds.),

    Research in

    organizational

    behavior

    (Vol.

    3).

    Greenwich,

    Conn.: JAI

    Press,

    1981,

    197-234.

    Mitchell,

    T.

    R., Larson,

    J.

    R.,

    &

    Green,

    S. G. Leader

    behavior,

    situational

    moderators,

    and

    group performance:

    An

    attribu-

    tional

    analysis.

    Organizational

    Behavior and

    Human

    Perfor-

    mance, 1977,

    18,

    254-268.

    Nisbett,

    R.

    E.,

    &

    Wilson,

    T.

    D.

    Telling

    more than

    we can

    know:

    Verbal

    reports

    on

    mental

    processes.

    Psychological

    Review,

    1977,

    84,

    231-259.

    Orne,

    M. Demand

    characteristics

    and the

    concept

    of

    quasi-

    controls. In

    R. Rosenthal

    and R.

    Rosnow

    (Eds.),

    Artifacts

    in

    behavioral

    research.

    New York:

    Academic

    Press, 1969,

    143-179.

    Pfeffer,

    J. The

    ambiguity

    of

    leadership. Academy of

    Manage-

    ment

    Review,

    1977,

    2,

    104-112.

    Phillips,

    J.

    S.,

    &

    Lord,

    R. G.

    Determinants of

    intrinsic

    motiva-

    tion: Locus of control and competence information as com-

    ponents

    of

    Deci's

    cognitive

    evaluation

    theory.

    Journal

    of

    Ap-

    plied

    Psychology,

    1980, 65,

    211-218.

    Phillips,

    J.

    S.,

    &

    Lord,

    R.

    G. Causal

    attributions

    and

    percep-

    tions of

    leadership.

    Organizational

    Behavior

    and Human

    Per-

    formance,

    1981, 28,

    143-163.

    Ross, M.,

    &

    Sicoly,

    F.

    Egocentric

    biases

    in

    availability

    and at-

    tribution. Journal

    of Personality

    and Social

    Psychology,

    1979,

    37,

    322-336.

    Rush,

    M.

    C.,

    Thomas,

    J.

    C.,

    &

    Lord,

    R. G.

    Implicit

    leadership

    theory:

    A

    potential

    threat to

    the internal

    validity

    of

    leader

    behavior

    questionnaires.

    Organizational

    Behavior and

    Human

    Performance, 1977, 20,

    756-765.

    Schank, R., & Ableson, R. P. Scripts, plans, goals, and under-

    standing:

    An

    inquiry

    into

    human

    knowledge

    structure.

    Hillsdale,

    N. J.:

    Erlbaum,

    1977.

    Schneider, W.,

    &

    Shiffrin,

    R.

    M.

    Controlled and

    automatic

    hu-

    man

    information

    processing:

    I.

    Detection,

    search,

    and

    atten-

    tion.

    Psychology

    Review,

    1977, 84,

    1-66.

    59

    This content downloaded from 194.29.185.139 on Mon, 23 Jun 2014 16:38:14 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

  • 8/18/2019 Theoretical, Information Processing, And Situational Factors Affecting Attribution Theory Models of Organizational B…

    12/12

    Shiffrin,

    R.

    M.,

    &

    Schneider,

    W. Controlled and automatic

    hu-

    man

    information

    processing:

    II.

    Perceptual learning,

    automatic

    attending,

    and a

    general

    theory.

    Psychological

    Review,

    1977,

    84,

    127-190.

    Snyder,

    M.

    Self-monitoring processes.

    In

    L.

    Berkowitz

    (Ed.),

    Advances

    in

    experimental

    social

    psychology

    (Vol. 12).

    New

    York:

    Academic

    Press,

    1979,

    86-128.

    Staw,

    B. M.

    Attribution of the

    causes of

    performance:

    A

    general alternative interpretation of non-sectional research on

    organizations.

    Organizational

    Behavior

    and Human

    Perfor-

    mance,

    1975,

    13,

    414-432.

    Staw,

    B. M.

    Rationality

    and

    justification

    in

    organizational

    life.

    In

    B. M. Staw &

    L. L.

    Cummings

    (Eds.),

    Research

    in

    organizational

    behavior

    (Vol.

    2).

    Greenwich,

    Conn.:

    JAI

    Press,

    1980,

    45-80.

    Staw,

    B.

    M.,

    &

    Ross,

    J. Commitment

    in an

    experimenting

    soci-

    ety:

    A

    study

    of

    the attribution of

    leadership

    from ad-

    ministrative

    scenarios.

    Journal

    of

    Applied

    Psychology,

    1980,

    65,

    249-260.

    Strickland,

    L.

    H.

    Surveillance

    and trust. Journal

    of Personality,

    1958,

    26,

    200-215.

    Weiner,

    B.

    Theories

    of

    motivation:

    From mechanism

    to

    cogni-

    tion.

    Chicago:

    Markham,

    1972.

    Weiss,

    H.

    M.,

    &

    Knight,

    P.

    A.

    The

    utility

    of

    humility:

    Self-

    esteem,

    information

    search,

    and

    problem-solving

    efficiency.

    Organizational

    Behavior and Human

    Performance,

    1980, 25,

    216-223.

    Wilson,

    T.

    D., Hull,

    J.

    G.,

    &

    Johnson,

    J. Awareness and self-

    perception:

    Verbal

    reports

    of internal states. Journal

    of

    Per-

    sonality and Social Psychology, 1981, 40, 53-71.

    Witkin,

    H.

    A.,

    &

    Goodenough,

    D. R.

    Field

    dependence

    and in-

    terpersonal

    behavior.

    Psychological

    Bulletin,

    1977, 84,

    661-689.

    Wong,

    P. T.

    P.,

    &

    Weiner,

    B. When

    people

    ask

    why ques-

    tions,

    and the heuristics of attributional search. Journal

    of

    Personality

    and Social

    Psychology,

    1981, 40,

    650-663.

    Wood,

    R.

    E.,

    &

    Mitchell,

    T.

    R.

    Manager

    behavior

    in

    social

    con-

    text:

    The

    impact

    of

    impression management

    on attributions

    and

    disciplinary

    actions.

    Organizational

    Behavior and Human

    Performance,

    1981, 28,

    356-378.

    Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. Salience, attention, and attribution:

    Top

    of the head

    pehnomena.

    In L. Berkowitz

    (Ed.),

    Advances

    in

    experimental

    social

    psychology

    (Vol.

    11).

    New York:

    Academic

    Press, 1978,

    249-288.

    Robert

    G. Lord

    is Associate

    Professor

    of

    Psychology

    in

    the

    Department

    of Psychology,

    University

    of

    Akron.

    Jonathan

    E. Smith

    is

    Assistant

    Professor of

    Psychology

    in

    the

    Department of Psychology,

    University of

    Akron.

    60