the self prefers itself? self-referential versus parental ... · pdf filethat when...

15
Submitted 24 April 2014 Accepted 4 September 2014 Published 25 September 2014 Corresponding author Unni Sulutvedt, [email protected] Academic editor Maria Servedio Additional Information and Declarations can be found on page 11 DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 Copyright 2014 Sulutvedt and Laeng Distributed under Creative Commons CC-BY 4.0 OPEN ACCESS The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental standards in face attractiveness Unni Sulutvedt and Bruno Laeng Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Norway ABSTRACT Preference for phenotypic similarity in assortative mating may be influenced by either a preference for self-similarity or parent-similarity. The aim of the current study was to assess whether people’s preference in face attractiveness is influenced by resemblance to the opposite sex parent’s face (parental phenotype) or their own face (self-based phenotype or “self-imprinting”). We used computerized image manipulations of facial photographs of participants, their mothers and fathers. The original photographs were combined with 78% of the participants’ opposite sex prototype face (i.e., male and female prototypes made from equal contributions of a hundred faces), creating morphs where the contribution from the familiar faces went unnoticed. Female and male participants ranked these images together with the opposite-sex prototype dierent familiar morphs. Each participant did the same for the morphs generated with other same-sex participants’ faces and of their parents. We found that the female participants preferred the self-based morphs to the prototype faces. Male participants showed a general tendency towards self-referential standard. Parental face morphs were ranked low in attractiveness, which may be accounted for by the age dierence of the faces blended into the self-based versus parental face morphs, since we used present-time photographs of both the participants and their parents. Subjects Ecology, Evolutionary Studies Keywords Face attractiveness, Assortative mating, Phenotypic similarity INTRODUCTION According to current evolutionary thinking, a recognition process based on phenotype matching to specific individuals during early childhood provide the basis for whom will be considered a potential sexual mate later in life. Bateson (1978, p. 265) postulated an “optimal outbreeding” mechanism according to which “sexual imprinting sets the standard (or standards) of what immediate kin look like and the animals subsequently prefer to mate with an individual who looks slightly dierent”. He also surmised that sexual imprinting exists in humans and that self-stimulation with own phenotype (e.g., self ’s odor for some animals) may get sexually imprinted. Indeed, several researchers have used the term sexual or familial “imprinting” during childhood to describe an early learning process, often not in a strict Lorenzian sense (Lorenz, 1965) but as an “imprinting-like” type of mechanisms (e.g., Little et al., 2003) where early experiences may have a primacy How to cite this article Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental standards in face attractiveness. PeerJ 2:e595; DOI 10.7717/peerj.595

Upload: hoangxuyen

Post on 07-Mar-2018

222 views

Category:

Documents


7 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

Submitted 24 April 2014Accepted 4 September 2014Published 25 September 2014

Corresponding authorUnni Sulutvedt,[email protected]

Academic editorMaria Servedio

Additional Information andDeclarations can be found onpage 11

DOI 10.7717/peerj.595

Copyright2014 Sulutvedt and Laeng

Distributed underCreative Commons CC-BY 4.0

OPEN ACCESS

The self prefers itself? Self-referentialversus parental standards in faceattractivenessUnni Sulutvedt and Bruno Laeng

Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Norway

ABSTRACTPreference for phenotypic similarity in assortative mating may be influenced byeither a preference for self-similarity or parent-similarity. The aim of the currentstudy was to assess whether people’s preference in face attractiveness is influencedby resemblance to the opposite sex parent’s face (parental phenotype) or their ownface (self-based phenotype or “self-imprinting”). We used computerized imagemanipulations of facial photographs of participants, their mothers and fathers. Theoriginal photographs were combined with 78% of the participants’ opposite sexprototype face (i.e., male and female prototypes made from equal contributionsof a hundred faces), creating morphs where the contribution from the familiarfaces went unnoticed. Female and male participants ranked these images togetherwith the opposite-sex prototype different familiar morphs. Each participant didthe same for the morphs generated with other same-sex participants’ faces andof their parents. We found that the female participants preferred the self-basedmorphs to the prototype faces. Male participants showed a general tendency towardsself-referential standard. Parental face morphs were ranked low in attractiveness,which may be accounted for by the age difference of the faces blended into theself-based versus parental face morphs, since we used present-time photographs ofboth the participants and their parents.

Subjects Ecology, Evolutionary StudiesKeywords Face attractiveness, Assortative mating, Phenotypic similarity

INTRODUCTIONAccording to current evolutionary thinking, a recognition process based on phenotype

matching to specific individuals during early childhood provide the basis for whom

will be considered a potential sexual mate later in life. Bateson (1978, p. 265) postulated

an “optimal outbreeding” mechanism according to which “sexual imprinting sets the

standard (or standards) of what immediate kin look like and the animals subsequently

prefer to mate with an individual who looks slightly different”. He also surmised that sexual

imprinting exists in humans and that self-stimulation with own phenotype (e.g., self ’s

odor for some animals) may get sexually imprinted. Indeed, several researchers have used

the term sexual or familial “imprinting” during childhood to describe an early learning

process, often not in a strict Lorenzian sense (Lorenz, 1965) but as an “imprinting-like”

type of mechanisms (e.g., Little et al., 2003) where early experiences may have a primacy

How to cite this article Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental standards in face attractiveness.PeerJ 2:e595; DOI 10.7717/peerj.595

Page 2: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

over later ones so that exposure to individuals that happen to be in closest proximity during

childhood (typically, close kin) are most likely to provide “templates” for later mate choice

(see also Lieberman, Tooby & Cosmides, 2007). Such a kin recognition process is also at

the foundation for seeking a balance between incest avoidance (cf. Shepher, 1971) and an

opposite tendency to seek family resemblances.

In addition, Symons (1995) observed that, in current human environments, mirrors

are ubiquitous and they may affect attractiveness perception (particularly of same-sex

individuals) in evolutionarily unprecedented ways, and may even be responsible for some

positive assortative mating Symons (1995; p. 108). However, he also stressed the possibility

that already during the course of human evolutionary history our ancestors could see

their reflections in still water. Hence, the historically recent spread of use of mirrors could

have boosted the effects of an already selected “self-imprinting” mechanism and humans

did not need to wait for the appearance of mirrors to get knowledge of self-appearance

and be influenced by it. In fact, still water is highly reflective (consider the myth, at least

2,000 years old—of Narcissus falling in love with his image in a pond) and reflective

surfaces like still water have been present since the beginning of evolutionary time. Thus,

the potential for self-imprinting clearly predates the mass production of mirrors, as well as

that of portraits, homemade photos (e.g., “selfies”) and movies.

Remarkably, people tend to marry individuals who are similar to themselves. This

phenomenon has been widely documented within Western societies (e.g., Alvarez & Jaffe,

2004; Bereczkei et al., 2002; Zajonc et al., 1987), where individuals tend to pair and wed

willingly and not that commonly in pre-arranged marriages. Some of these studies showed

that when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown individuals of both sexes

(Zajonc et al., 1987), photos of actual partners were paired above chance. Moreover, Alvarez

& Jaffe (2004) found no difference between the matching of the ones most likely to be

siblings and the ones most likely to be married, which indicates the existence of a high

degree of similarity between partners. Finally, self-perception appears to modulate mate

preference (Buston & Emlen, 2003), which is consistent with the hypothesis that human’s

criterion for beauty is rooted on an image of self. These findings support the existence of a

detection mechanism of similarity to the self that influences attraction to others’ faces.

Regarding mate choices, “imprinting” to the father’s face has been shown to be relevant

for the daughter’s facial preference (e.g., Wilson & Barrett, 1987; Bereczkei et al., 2002; Little

et al., 2003; Watkins et al., 2011; Wiszewska, Pawlowski & Boothroyd, 2007). For example,

females found facial stimuli that resembled their father more attractive if they had a good

relationship with their father during childhood (e.g., Wiszewska, Pawlowski & Boothroyd,

2007). However, this imprinting-like process does not exclude that also the self ’s face may

have an impact on the formation of the kin template. Also, the child’s attachment to the

mother figure and early exposure to the mother’s face is essential in the social development

of humans (Bowlby, 1969; Belsky, Steinberg & Draper, 1991) and this could also influence

later affiliative choices. One possibility, which will be specifically explored in the present

study, is that experiences of faces in close proximity during childhood could influence, at a

later stage in life, adults towards a “parental/kin standard” of attractiveness, featuring the

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 2/15

Page 3: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

facial characteristics of self ’s parents. In addition, self-referential standards could become

established over a lifetime exposure with the self ’s own face and in fact reinforce kin-based

preferences.

It is likely that several early “face templates” are shaped by exposure to faces throughout

childhood which leave a lasting impression on an individual’s face processing mechanisms

(Perrett et al., 2002) and in turn influence mate choices in adulthood. Studies on

facial attractiveness are abundant and they have revealed that both the symmetry and

averageness of a face are crucial for attractiveness. However, if these were the sole

perceptual factors affecting people’s sense of facial beauty, then most people will be most

attracted to the average face (e.g., a prototype; Halberstadt & Rhodes, 2000).

A crucial assumption for the present study is that any deviation from the preference for

the prototype would support the idea that there exist important individual differences in

the sense of face aesthetics. Also, if facial preference is reduced to just a “mere exposure”

phenomenon (Hill, 1978; Moreland & Zajonc, 1982), where highly familiar faces are

regarded as more likeable and attractive, then faces resembling any of the family faces

(e.g., mother, father, as well as self) should be found to be equally attractive and possibly

preferred to the opposite-sex prototypes. To our knowledge, attempts to separate a parental

criterion from a self-referential have never been tested by requesting the same group of

participants to compare simultaneously faces similar to themselves and their parents.

Present studyAt the basis of the present research is evidence that lovers—when given the opportunity—

would prefer their partners to resemble themselves (Laeng, Vermeer & Sulutvedt, 2013).

Specifically, a 22% self-based morph with the partner’s face was preferred over the morph

of the partner’s face with its same-sex’s prototype. Thus, when given the opportunity,

partners will make aesthetic choices towards increased resemblance to self, either for the

whole face or some distinctive elements (e.g., the eyes’ color; Laeng, Mathisen & Johnsen,

2007). Nevertheless, it remains unclear whether these findings reflect a self-referential

mechanism or a parental mechanism or a combination of the two. Thus, the present study

attempted to tease apart the contribution of familial imprinting to phenotype matching

to self. To our knowledge, this is the first time that the latter effects have been studied with

graphic manipulation methods based on the face images of the participants’ parents.

Since the learning process of kin detection and imprinting may influence both sexual

and non-sexual preferences (e.g., trust or friendship), the effects of family resemblance

may be better captured by asking participants to rank images according to their “attrac-

tiveness”, without explicitly mentioning sexual attractiveness. Thus, we used computerized

image manipulations, morphs, of facial photographs of self, and self ’s biological mother

and father in order to assess this dilemma. That is, stimuli were made by a weighted average

of every pixel in the two images, so that all aspects of the faces (shape and pigment) were

moved towards an intermediate representation which was to all effect a novel face stimulus.

We used the opposite-sex prototype face (of the participant’s age cohort) as the base image

into which the same small amount of information (22%) of self, father, and mother were

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 3/15

Page 4: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

morphed. All images were presented simultaneously as high-quality color prints and

participants were asked to rank them for their attractiveness. In addition, the same images

from another subject’s protocol were presented for comparisons between Family and

Other. The opposite-sex prototype face (i.e., the morphed average of 100 people of the

participants’ age-cohort) was also presented, so as to provide a baseline for comparing

increase or decreases in the sense of beauty due to the other image manipulations.

We used morphs with a blending of 22% because they have previously been shown, by

use of “objective criteria of awareness” based on forced-choice tasks and signal detection

analysis (Merikle, Smilek & Eastwood, 2001), to be perceived below consciousness, as

opposed to morphs with larger contributions, e.g., 33% (Laeng, Vermeer & Sulutvedt,

2013). Moreover, Laeng, Vermeer & Sulutvedt (2013) found that attractiveness judgments

on self-morphs combined with attractive strangers peaked at 22%, relative to 11% or 33%

morphs. This may indicate that 22% approximates the optimal amount of likeness in order

to prevent avoidance mechanisms of primary incest. As seen in a pilot study, this 22% peak

effect was not observed if the stranger’s face was previously rated as unattractive, in which

case other evolutionary “constraints” may dominate—e.g., the variations in asymmetry

of individual faces (cf. Rhodes, 2006; Miller & Todd, 1998). Therefore, when morphing, we

used the opposite-sex prototype faces, in order to achieve a face of optimal averageness and

symmetry.

We need to stress that we are not suggesting that average faces are optimally attractive

but only that average facial configurations are more attractive than most faces (Rhodes,

2006). The logic behind our study is simply based on the well-documented fact that the

symmetrical averaged face obtained when morphing multiple faces typically score very

high in attractiveness and when adding to a prototype a sizable proportion of a randomly

chosen face, this does not cause an increase of attractiveness but typically reduces it a bit.

Based on the above evidence, we hypothesize that adding to a prototype a 22% of oneself

or kin should result in no gain in attractiveness unless self ’s appearance or kin’s are actually

“liked”.

The predictions are quite straightforward: If the preference for the phenotype is strongly

based on a self-referential process the self-morph should be preferred to all other morphs.

In contrast, the self-morph should be ranked as less attractive than the morphs based on

kin’s faces if parental imprinting plays an important role. Since our participants were

heterosexual, we also predicted that the maternal morph should be preferred to the

paternal by male participants while the reverse should happen for the female participants.

Finally, if facial preference is caused by a “mere exposure” phenomenon, all the morphs

based on the familiar faces (e.g., mother, father, as well as self) should be found to be

equally attractive.

However, we need to point out from the outset a caveat of the present experiment: we

were able to use only ‘present-time’ images of the participant’s parents, which may not be

ideal for studying the influence of parental effects, since parents do not look exactly the

same today as they did when a parental standard could have been established. Nevertheless,

the present study is an initial and direct test of the parental-template hypothesis. Since a

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 4/15

Page 5: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

few previous studies have also shown that individuals’ preferences in attractiveness may be

influenced by attachment experiences in adults’ upbringing (e.g., Wiszewska, Pawlowski &

Boothroyd, 2007; Nojo, Tamura & Ihara, 2012), we also used the short form of the EMBU

scale (Arrindell et al., 1999) to assess positive versus negative attachment to the mother and

the father figure.

MATERIALS AND METHODSParticipantsThe study included a total of 36 heterosexual participants (18 females) between the age

of 18 and 33 with normal or corrected to normal vision (mean age = 25.50; SD = 4.24).

The participants were recruited in the Oslo area, either at seminars at the Department of

Psychology or by word of mouth.

Psychological research in Norway is subject to ethical review by the regional medical

research board only if the research involves patients, children or animals and involves

drugs, genetic samples or invasive techniques. Since none of these conditions applied

to the present study, the academic institution demanded only that the project comply

with Declaration of Helsinki guidelines and that informed consent be obtained from the

participants. We obtained written consent from all participants, and participants were free

to withdraw from the project.

Stimuli and apparatusStandardized frontal view photographs were taken of the faces of all the 36 participants

and their biological mothers and fathers. A total of 108 photographs, using a Panasonic

Lumix DMC digital camera, were collected. When parents lived far away, high quality

digital photographs were sent by e-mail. The photographs were taken from a distance

of 1 m, and participants and their parents were asked to display a neutral, emotionless

facial expression. Facial hair was removed and deep creases, folds, and lines of the skin

were retouched using Adobe Photoshop© to reduce age effects. We chose to use pictures

of parents from present time, after failing to collect pictures of parents from the time

their children were young. The old pictures were of too poor quality and not sufficiently

standardized (e.g., some showed smiling faces, eye-glasses, different poses, etc.), so that

morphing these old pictures with the high quality prototypes would not yield satisfactory

images. Hence, we opted to using current photos taken with the same digital camera and in

standard pose, and with neutral expressions.

Using image-morphing software (Morpheus Photo Morpher©), 22% of all the original

photographs were blended in with the opposite sex prototype face of the participant’s age

cohort (made from 100 faces) to create self-referential, paternal, and maternal morphs

(see Fig. 1). On all images, 71 points were placed on different features of the faces before

morphing. As in Lie, Rhodes & Simmons (2008) these color morphs (239 × 316 cm) were

digitally fitted with an oval black mask (not shown in the figure) to reduce any effects of

clothing and hairstyle, leaving only the face and minimal hair visible.

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 5/15

Page 6: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

Figure 1 Examples of morphed image-set and original images. Top row: Examples of the morphedimage-set based on the author: (A) Self, (B) Mother, and (C) Father. Bottom row: Original images of theauthor (D) Self, (E) Mother, and (F) Father.

The s-EMBU retrospective attachment test (Arrindell et al., 1999; Arrindell et al., 2001)

was used to control for perceived parental attachment in childhood. The s-EMBU is a

23-item inventory with 4-point Likert type scales, to be answered separately for mother

and father. This test consists of three scales: Emotional Warmth, Rejection, and (Over)

Protection each of which comprises eight items. Following Gyuris, Jarai & Bereczkei (2010)

only Emotional Warmth and Rejection was used to assess perceived positive and negative

emotions towards parents during upbringing.

ProcedureEach participant was matched by age to another participant of the same sex so as to obtain

a set of morphs for the “Other” condition. Four morphs of were presented side by side for

each separate sessions for “Family” or “Other” conditions. The participants were asked to

rank them from the most attractive (i.e., 1st place) to the least (i.e., 3rd place). The order of

the images was random within each block. All the stimulus images were presented centered

on the computer screen with a resolution of 1024 × 768 pixels. The software “Experiment

Center” by SensoMotoric Instruments® (SMI, Teltow, Germany) was used to present the

stimuli.

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 6/15

Page 7: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

Table 1 Mean ranks for Family morphs.

Female participants Mean rank

Self morph 1.53

Male prototype 2.36

Self ’s mother morph 2.97

Self ’s father morph 3.14

Male participants Mean rank

Female prototype 1.89

Self morph 2.33

Self ’s mother morph 2.72

Self ’s father morph 3.06

Participants were tested individually and were seated in front of a computer screen

at a distance of 60 cm. A blank screen was presented for 500 ms followed by a fixation

cross presented for 1000 ms and then the image-set was viewed as long as it took to make

a decision, and the participants pressed space on the keyboard to move on to the next

image-set. The experimenter manually recorded the attractiveness responses (hence we did

not analyze response times), and the experiment lasted approximately 30 min, followed

by the short questionnaire. After the experiment all the participants were debriefed and

asked if they had recognized any of the faces. Only two participants thought they might

have recognized their opposite-sex parent, and said they had consequently ranked these

morphs in 3rd place. However, removing these participants from the analyses did not affect

the overall results and, therefore, we present below results from all participants’ responses.

ResultsAll data were analyzed using StatView® statistical package. t-tests for independent samples

were conducted on the s-EMBU to compare differences between sexes in perceived parental

behavior. There were no significant differences in the ratings between females and males

in any of the elements in the s-EMBU; mother rejection (p = .68), father rejection

(p = .32), mother emotional warmth (p = .57), and father emotional warmth (p = .40).

All participants were high on parental emotional warmth and low on parental rejection,

thus, the s-EMBU was not used in further analyses.

Descriptive statistics were calculated for each participant, obtaining mean attractiveness

ranks for each combination of the variables (Tables 1 and 2). Rankings from female and

male participants were analyzed separately, since the parental hypothesis predicts opposing

differences in attractiveness between parental sexes. The attractiveness rankings were

analyzed using the Friedman’s Rank Test and pair-wise comparisons were carried out with

the Paired Sign Test.

The analysis of the female participants in the Family group showed significant difference

in ranking of morphs: χ2 = 17.23, df = 3, p < .0005 (see Table 1, top). The self−

morph was ranked first and the male prototype was ranked second place, these differed

significantly from each other according to a Paired Sign Test (p < .03), confirming the

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 7/15

Page 8: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

Table 2 Mean ranks for Other morphs (i.e., non-familiar faces). The ‘Other’ morphs were the sameimages labeled in Table 1 as ‘Self ’ morphs.

Female observers Mean rank

Other morph 1.93

Male prototype 2.15

Other’s mother morph 2.84

Other’s father morph 3.08

Male observers Mean rank

Female prototype 1.50

Other morph 2.50

Other’s mother morph 2.58

Other’s father morph 3.42

presence of a self-referential preference over and above a preference for average and

symmetric faces of the opposite sex. The self-morph also differed significantly from

both the mother (p < .002) and father (p < .006) morphs. The prototype was preferred

significantly to the mother morph (p = .049). Interestingly, comparing the male prototype

and the father’s morph showed no significant difference between their ranks (p = .21),

which shows that the parental face did not invariably resulted in an unattractive face

morph.

When the same female participants judged morphs in the Other set, they also showed

significant differences in their ranking of attractiveness; χ2 = 9.22, df = 3, p < .03

(see Table 2, top). However, the difference between the Other morph (i.e., the morph

replacing the self-morph of the above analyses, Table 2) and the male prototype failed

to be significant (p = .212), which confirms that only when the morph contains a bit

of self-resemblance such a morph is preferred over the prototype. The prototype was

significantly preferred to both the Other’s mother morph (p = .03) and the Other’s father

morph (p = .001). Finally, Other was preferred to both Other’s mother morph (p = .005)

and the Other’s father morph (p = .052). There was no difference in the ranks of the

Other’s mother morph versus the Other’s father morph (p = .091).

Analysis of male participants in the Family group revealed the presence of a a significant

difference among ranks, χ2 = 8,20, df = 3, p = .04 (see Table 1, bottom). This was

entirely accounted, according to the Paired Sign Tests, by a significant difference for the

morph ranked first versus the one ranked last, i.e. the female prototype over the father

morph (p = .041). When the same male participants judged morphs in the Other set,

they showed significant differences in their ranking of attractiveness, χ2 = 19.95, df = 3,

p < .0001 (see Table 2, bottom). They clearly preferred the female prototype over the

Other morph (p = .0013) as well as the Other’s father (p = .0001) or mother morphs

(p = .01). Moreover, the Other morph was significantly preferred to the Other’s father

morph (p = .0001) as well as to the Other’s mother morph (p = .01).

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 8/15

Page 9: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

DISCUSSIONThe present results are consistent with the hypothesis that people possess a self-referential

standard of face attractiveness, since self-based morphs were preferred to the prototype

morphs, especially by the female participants. They are also in line with several previous

findings of “narcissistic” preferences for faces (e.g., Alvarez & Jaffe, 2004; Bereczkei et al.,

2002; Zajonc et al., 1987; Laeng, Vermeer & Sulutvedt, 2013). Interestingly, a previous study

with morphs (Laeng, Vermeer & Sulutvedt, 2013) also revealed stronger preferences for

self-morphs in females than in males. It is possible that these effects may be caused by a

greater tolerance for same-sex blends (i.e., feminization) with female participants than

for the males (i.e., masculinization). Another possibility is that females are more exposed

on average than males to the self ’s phenotype by use of mirrors. In the present study, the

male participants’ choices were also consistent with a self-referential standard, since for

male participants the female prototype did not differ significantly in attractiveness from

the self-morph.

However, we failed to show evidence in favor of the parental template hypothesis or

family imprinting hypothesis, because the self-morph was preferred over the maternal

and paternal morphs and the opposite-sex prototype was preferred over both parental

morphs. In addition, the Other morphs were ranked above their parents’ morphs. When

taken together these results suggest that there was an effect of the age of the face morphed

into the prototype, where the older ages of parents to both self ’s and other’s reduced the

attractiveness of all parental morphs. Clearly, a caveat of the present experiment is that

it used ‘present-time’ images of parents which may have counter-acted the influence of

possible parental effects, since parents do not look exactly the same today as they did when

a possible parental standard could have been “imprinted”. Although we digitally retouched

skin creases, folds, and lines to reduce the cues of age, the wide age difference may still

create confounds in the images. If a parents’ template is established during the first years

of life, individuals might actually be mostly attracted to faces resembling what the parent

looked like at the time self was a child.

However, it is interesting to note that while the females significantly preferred the male

prototype to both Other’s mother and father morphs, this difference disappeared when the

male prototype was compared to the self ’s father morph. This suggests that family resem-

blance to the opposite-sex parent may at least mitigate the negative “age effect” on females’

preference. A few previous studies have found the presence of facial similarities between

the faces of the spouse and the parent of the opposite sex (Little et al., 2003; Marcinkowska

& Rantala, 2012; Wilson & Barrett, 1987; Wiszewska, Pawlowski & Boothroyd, 2007).

One study on facial similarity between spouses and their parent (Nojo, Tamura & Ihara,

2012) used images of parents from when the participants were children, and found no

clear implications of the similarity between the couples and their mother/father-in-law.

Thus, the present results only allow us to draw tentative conclusions about parental versus

self-referential tendencies in face attractiveness, since it is difficult to disentangle the effects

due to the age of the face morphed into the prototype from that of its family status and the

present results are largely consistent with younger faces being preferred to older ones.

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 9/15

Page 10: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

In addition, we found no significant differences in ratings of perceived Emotional

Warmth or Rejection from fathers or mothers during upbringing. All the participants

had quite similar mean scores, as both parents were rated rather high on Emotional

Warmth and low on Rejection. Despite the participants’ seemingly good relationship to

their parents, the self-resembling faces were chosen over the parents faces, suggesting

that positive feelings towards parents do not indicate a greater parental tendency in

face attraction. Wiszewska, Pawlowski & Boothroyd (2007) compared facial proportions

of fathers’ faces to the proportions of the facial stimuli chosen by females as the most

attractive. They found that there was no concordance overall between the father’s face

and chosen stimuli, but if the women rated their fathers highly the proportions of the

attractive stimuli was more similar to the fathers’ faces. They did not, however, test for

similarity between the daughters’ and the fathers’ faces. There is a possibility that the

females that rated their fathers most positively also were more facially like their fathers, so a

self-referential preference may be a contributing factor. Other studies have found evidence

that daughters of older men tend to choose older husbands (Wilson & Barrett, 1987), and

prefer age cues in male faces compared to cues of youth (Perrett et al., 2002). This effect may

be the result of a parental tendency, but it could also simply be due to familiarity with older

people. In addition, the above studies did not investigate whether the chosen husbands and

stimuli had any facial resemblance with the females’ fathers or the self.

Although the presence of mirrors, photos, films, and drawings in modern times may

have considerably boosted, compared to the evolutionary past, sexual self-imprinting

in humans, the empirical evidence in humans for sexual imprinting (either positive or

negative) remains weak (e.g., Marcinkowska & Rantala, 2012; Rantala & Marcinkowska,

2011). Moreover, it remains unclear if self-imprinting should be considered an adaptive

strategy or it could have become maladaptive by promoting an excessive tendency towards

inbreeding. However, there is empirical evidence suggesting that the ‘like mate with

like’ strategy (whether this may based on kin’s or self ’s phenotypes) can confer adaptive

value. Remarkably, it has been shown in a study on the whole population of Iceland

that a moderate degree of genetic similarity increases reproductive success and genetic

compatibility in humans; specifically, there was a positive association between kinship

and fertility, so that Icelandic couples that were mildly related (i.e., third or fourth

cousins) had the greatest reproductive success and the highest number of children who

further reproduced (Helgason et al., 2008). Moreover, self-resemblance in both physical

and psychological phenotypes may indicate suitability to one’s environment and, in

humans, could promote the partners’ successful cooperation, relevant for the survival

of the offspring (Godoy et al., 2008). There are also examples of use in humans of physical

cues based on ‘kin’ resemblance that are strategical in evolutionary terms, for example

for detecting cuckoldry (for example in the face, see Bovet et al., 2012; Laeng, Mathisen &

Johnsen, 2007; Platek & Thomson, 2007).

The well-documented phenotypic similarity among partners in many human

societies may thus reflect inclusive fitness mechanisms, where there can be increased

gene duplication without an increase in reproductive investment and a reduced cost

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 10/15

Page 11: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

of altruism (Epstein & Guttman, 1984; Thiessen & Gregg, 1980). Given that phenotypic

facial characteristics are known to be highly heritable in humans (Baydas et al., 2007;

Weinberg et al., 2013), faces may be one of the best visual clues to genetic similarity (Kazem

& Widdig, 2013; Holmes, 2004; Bovet et al., 2012). Indeed, for both sexes, ratings of facial

attractiveness have been found to be a better predictor of general physical attractiveness

than ratings on body images (Currie & Little, 2009; Peters, Rhodes & Simmons, 2007).

Whether people could see themselves having a relationship with a stranger can be

predicted by the stranger’s face attractiveness (Currie & Little, 2009), which implies that

face attractiveness is an important feature in human mate choice decisions and that faces

may provide relevant signals of heritable quality (Tregenza & Wedell, 2000).

To conclude, although a universal preference for the human prototype is well-

documented (e.g., Rhodes, 2006), such a preference could derive from general processing

mechanisms or from self ’s exposure to other non-related individuals. Thus, a key

difference from parental or self-referential “imprinting” and general prototype learning

is that the latter may be based on self ’s exposure to non-related and mostly unknown

individuals over a lifetime; whereas the former would be linked to early exposure to figures

of attachment and self. Thus, mate choice could be the result of a delicate balance between

opposing tendencies: inbreeding versus outbreeding; universal “ideals” of beauty versus

highly idiosyncratic preferences based on one’s own appearance. The interplay between

choices based on universal standards of beauty (i.e., symmetry and averageness versus

the personal self-based ones) is well expressed in an aphorism by poet Auden (1962):

“Narcissus does not fall in love with his reflection because it is beautiful, but because

it is his”.

ADDITIONAL INFORMATION AND DECLARATIONS

FundingThe study was funded by the University of Oslo through a student grant. The funders had

no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of

the manuscript.

Grant DisclosuresThe following grant information was disclosed by the authors:

University of Oslo.

Competing InterestsThe authors declare there are no competing interests.

Author Contributions• Unni Sulutvedt conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments,

analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper,

prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 11/15

Page 12: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

• Bruno Laeng conceived and designed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed

reagents/materials/analysis tools, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables,

reviewed drafts of the paper, initiated the study and supervised Unni during her master

studies.

Human EthicsThe following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body

and any reference numbers):

Psychological research in Norway is subject to ethical review by the regional medical

research board only if the research involves patients, children or animals and involves

drugs, genetic samples or invasive techniques. Since none of these conditions applied

to the present study, the academic institution demanded only that the project comply

with Declaration of Helsinki guidelines and that informed consent be obtained from the

participants. We obtained written consent from all participants. All information was

handled and stored anonyomously, and participants were free to withdraw from the

project. In addition, participants gave their written informed consent to publication of

their photographs.

Supplemental InformationSupplemental information for this article can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/

10.7717/peerj.595#supplemental-information.

REFERENCESAlvarez L, Jaffe K. 2004. Narcissism guides mate selection: humans mate assortatively, as revealed

by facial resemblance, following an algorithm of “self seeking like”. Evolutionary Psychology2:177–194.

Arrindell WA, Richter J, Eisemann M, Garling T, Ryden O, Hansson SB, Kasielke E, Frindte W,Gillholm R, Gustafsson M. 2001. The short-EMBU in East Germany and Sweden: across-national factorial validity extension. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology 42:157–160DOI 10.1111/1467-9450.00226.

Arrindell WA, Sanavio E, Aguilar G, Siva C, Hatzichristou C, Eisemann M, Recinos LA,Gaszner P, Peter M, Battagliese G, Kallai J, Van der Ende J. 1999. The development of theshort form of the EMBU: its appraisal with students in Greece, Guatemala, Hungary and Italy.Personality and Individual Differences 27:613–628 DOI 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00192-5.

Auden WH. 1962. “Hic et Ille”, pt. 3, sct. A, The Dyer’s Hand. Random House. 1990 Edition.

Bateson P. 1978. Sexual imprinting and optimal outbreeding. Nature 273:659–660DOI 10.1038/273659a0.

Baydas B, Erdem A, Yavuz I, Ceylan I. 2007. Heritability of facial proportions and soft-tissueprofile characteristics in Turkish Anatolian siblings. American Journal of Orthodontics andDentofacial Orthopedics 131:504–509 DOI 10.1016/j.ajodo.2005.05.055.

Belsky J, Steinberg L, Draper P. 1991. Childhood experience, interpersonal development, andreproductive strategy: an evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development 62:647–670DOI 10.2307/1131166.

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 12/15

Page 13: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

Bereczkei T, Gyuris P, Koves P, Bernath L. 2002. Homogamy, genetic similarity, and imprinting;parental influence on mate choice preferences. Personality and Individual Differences 33:677–690DOI 10.1016/S0191-8869(01)00182-9.

Bovet J, Barthes J, Durand V, Raymond M, Alvergne A. 2012. Men’s preference for women’s facialfeatures: testing homogamy and the paternity uncertainty hypothesis. PLoS ONE 7:e49791DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0049791.

Bowlby J. 1969. Attachment and loss, vol. 1: attachment. New York: Basic Books.

Buston PM, Emlen ST. 2003. Cognitive processes underlying human mate choice: therelationship between self-perception and mate preference in Western society. Proceedingsof the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100:8805–8810DOI 10.1073/pnas.1533220100.

Currie TE, Little AC. 2009. The relative importance of the face and body in judg-ments of human physical attractiveness. Evolution of Human Behavior 30:409–416DOI 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.06.005.

Epstein E, Guttman R. 1984. Mate selection in man: evidence, theory, and outcome. Social Biology31:243–278.

Godoy R, Eisenberg DTA, Reyes-Garcıa V, Huanca T, Leonard WR, McDade TW, Tanner S.2008. Assortative mating and offspring well-being: theory and empirical findings froma native Amazonian society in Bolivia. Evolution and Human Behavior 29:201–210DOI 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.003.

Gyuris P, Jarai R, Bereczkei T. 2010. The effect of childhood experiences on mate choice inpersonality traits: homogamy and sexual imprinting. Personality and Individual Differences49:467–472 DOI 10.1016/j.paid.2010.04.021.

Halberstadt J, Rhodes G. 2000. The attractiveness of nonface averages: implications for anevolutionary explanation of the attractiveness of average faces. Psychological Science 11:285–289DOI 10.1111/1467-9280.00257.

Helgason A, Palsson S, Guðbjartsson DF, Kristjansson Þ, Stefansson K. 2008. Anassociation between the kinship and fertility of human couples. Science 319:813–816DOI 10.1126/science.1150232.

Hill WF. 1978. Effects of mere exposure on preferences in nonhuman mammals. PsychologicalBulletin 85:1177–1198 DOI 10.1037/0033-2909.85.6.1177.

Holmes WG. 2004. The early history of Hamiltonian-based research on kin recognition. AnnalesZoologici Fennici 41:691–711.

Kazem ANJ, Widdig A. 2013. Visual phenotype matching: cues to paternity are present in rhesusmacaque faces. PLoS ONE 8(2):e55846 DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0055846.

Laeng B, Mathisen R, Johnsen JA. 2007. Why do blue-eyed men prefer women with the same eyecolor? Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 61:371–384 DOI 10.1007/s00265-006-0266-1.

Laeng B, Vermeer O, Sulutvedt U. 2013. Is beauty in the face of the beholder? PLoS ONE 8:e68395DOI 10.1371/journal.pone.0068395.

Lie HC, Rhodes G, Simmons LW. 2008. Genetic diversity revealed in human faces. Evolution62:2473–2486 DOI 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2008.00478.x.

Lieberman D, Tooby J, Cosmides L. 2007. The architecture of human kin detection. Nature445:727–731 DOI 10.1038/nature05510.

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 13/15

Page 14: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

Little AC, Penton-Voak IS, Burt DM, Perrett DI. 2003. Investigating an imprinting-likephenomenon in humans, partners and opposite-sex parents have similar hair and eye colour.Evolution of Human Behavior 24:43–51 DOI 10.1016/S1090-5138(02)00119-8.

Lorenz K. 1965. Evolution and modification of behaviour. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Marcinkowska UM, Rantala MJ. 2012. Sexual imprinting on facial traits of opposite-sex parentsin humans. Evolutionary Psychology 10:621–630.

Merikle PM, Smilek D, Eastwood JD. 2001. Perception without awareness: perspectives fromcognitive psychology. Cognition 79:115–134 DOI 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00126-8.

Miller GF, Todd PM. 1998. Mate choice turns cognitive. Trends in Cognitive Science 2:190–198DOI 10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01169-3.

Moreland RL, Zajonc RB. 1982. Exposure effects in person perception: familiarity,similarity, and attraction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 18:395–415DOI 10.1016/0022-1031(82)90062-2.

Nojo S, Tamura S, Ihara Y. 2012. Human homogamy in facial characteristics: doesa sexual-imprinting-like mechanism play a role? Human Nature 23:323–340DOI 10.1007/s12110-012-9146-8.

Perrett D, Penton-Voak IS, Little AC, Tiddeman BP, Burt DM, Schmidt N, Oxley R, Kinloch N,Barrett L. 2002. Facial attractiveness judgments reflect learning of parental age characteristics.Proceedings of the Royal Society London B 269:873–880 DOI 10.1098/rspb.2002.1971.

Peters M, Rhodes G, Simmons LW. 2007. Contributions of the face and body to overallattractiveness. Animal Behavior 73:937–942 DOI 10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.07.012.

Platek SM, Thomson JW. 2007. Facial resemblance exaggerates sex-specific jealousy-baseddecisions. Evolutionary Psychology 5:223–231.

Rantala MJ, Marcinkowska UM. 2011. The role of sexual imprinting and the Westermarkeffect in mate choice in humans. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65:859–873DOI 10.1007/s00265-011-1145-y.

Rhodes G. 2006. The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology57:199–226 DOI 10.1146/annurev.psych.57.102904.190208.

Shepher J. 1971. Mate selection among second generation kibbutz adolescents andadults: incest avoidance and negative imprinting. Archives of Sexual Behavior 1:293–307DOI 10.1007/BF01638058.

Symons D. 1995. Beauty is in the adaptations of the beholder: The evolutionary psychology ofhuman female sexual attractiveness. In: Abramson PR, Pinkerton SD, eds. Sexual nature/sexualculture. The University of Chicago Press, 80–120.

Thiessen D, Gregg B. 1980. Human assortative mating and genetic equilibrium: an evolutionaryperspective. Ethology and Sociobiology 1:111–140 DOI 10.1016/0162-3095(80)90003-5.

Tregenza T, Wedell N. 2000. Genetic compatibility, mate choice and patterns of parentage: invitedreview. Molecular Ecology 9:1013–1027 DOI 10.1046/j.1365-294x.2000.00964.x.

Watkins CD, DeBruine LM, Smith FG, Jones BC, Vukovic J, Fraccaro P. 2011. Likefather, like self: emotional closeness to father predicts women’s preferences forself-resemblance in opposite-sex faces. Evolution and Human Behavior 32:70–75DOI 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.09.001.

Weinberg SM, Parsons TE, Marazita ML, Ma-her BS. 2013. Heritability of face shape in twins: apreliminary study using 3D sterophotogrammerty and geometric morphometrics. Dentistry3000(1):a004 DOI 10.5195/d3000.2013.14.

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 14/15

Page 15: The self prefers itself? Self-referential versus parental ... · PDF filethat when participants were asked to pair pictures of unknown ... photos of actual partners were paired above

Wilson GD, Barrett PT. 1987. Parental characteristics and partner choice: some evidence forOedipal imprinting. Journal of Biosocial Science 19:157–161 DOI 10.1017/S0021932000016758.

Wiszewska A, Pawlowski B, Boothroyd LG. 2007. Father–daughter relationship as a moderatorof sexual imprinting: a facialmetric study. Evolution and Human Behavior 28:248–252DOI 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.02.006.

Zajonc R, Adelmann P, Murphy S, Niedenthal P. 1987. Convergence in the physical appearanceof spouses. Motivation and Emotion 11:335–346 DOI 10.1007/BF00992848.

Sulutvedt and Laeng (2014), PeerJ, DOI 10.7717/peerj.595 15/15