the role of the prefrontal cortex in the expression of impulsive- and premeditated ... · 2014. 11....

330
The Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in the Expression of Impulsive- and Premeditated-Aggression by Sarah Haberle BA (Hons) Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Clinical Psychology) School of Psychology University of Tasmania October 2011

Upload: others

Post on 26-Jan-2021

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • The Role of the Prefrontal Cortex in the Expression of Impulsive- and

    Premeditated-Aggression

    by

    Sarah Haberle BA (Hons)

    Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy

    (Clinical Psychology)

    School of Psychology

    University of Tasmania

    October 2011

  • i

    This thesis contains no material, which has been accepted for a degree or

    diploma by the University or any other institution, except by way of background

    information and duly acknowledged in the thesis. To the best of my knowledge and

    belief, this thesis contains no material previously published or written by another

    person except where due acknowledgement is made in the text of the thesis, nor does

    the thesis contain any material that infringes copyright.

    __________________________________ Date: _______________

    Sarah Haberle

    The research associated with this thesis abides by the international and

    Australian codes on human and animal experimentation, the guidelines by the

    Australian Government’s office of the Gene Technology Regulator and the rulings of

    the Safety, Ethics and Institutional Biosafety Committees of the University.

    __________________________________ Date: _______________

    Sarah Haberle

    This thesis may be made available for loan and limited copying in accordance

    with the Copyright Act 1968.

    __________________________________ Date: _______________

    Sarah Haberle

  • ii

    Abstract

    The notion that there is a relationship between frontal lobe damage and

    aggressive behaviour has been recognised in the clinical literature for over 50 years.

    However, although there is evidence for an association between general brain

    dysfunction and aggression, there is little evidence pertaining to subclinical

    impairment and the propensity for aggressive behaviour. Further to this, given the

    functionally heterogeneity of the prefrontal cortex, it is vital to delineate the specific

    roles of the dorsolateral, orbitofrontal and medial aspects of the prefrontal cortex in

    the expression of aggression.

    Two forms of aggression are distinguished: reactive, impulsive-aggression

    and goal-directed premeditated aggression. While impulsive-aggression is typically

    described as an emotionally-charged aggressive response characterised by a lack of

    control, premeditated aggression is considered to be a planned and controlled

    aggressive display that is instrumental in nature. The qualitative differences between

    these subtypes of aggression suggest distinct neuropsychological differences

    mediating the likelihood of their display.

    The aim of this thesis was to clarify the role of the prefrontal cortex in

    subclinical impulsive-aggression and premeditated aggression. More specifically,

    possible executive functioning deficits mediated by the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex,

    and emotion recognition, impulsivity, and response reversal capabilities mediated by

    the orbitofrontal cortex were explored. Participants included university

    undergraduate students identified as having high levels of trait aggression, classified

    as either predominantly impulsive, or predominantly premeditated in nature.

    Experiment 1 (n=85) explored possible executive deficits using a battery of

  • iii

    neuropsychological measures pertaining to dorsolateral functioning. It was found that

    impulsive-aggressive individuals performed significantly poorer on measures of

    cognitive flexibility, planning, problem-solving, and flexibility of verbal thought

    processes.

    Experiment 2 (n=87) sought to identify possible deficits in interpretations of

    facial expressions of emotion and hostile attribution biases. Contrary to expectations,

    the results indicated that while impulsive- and premeditated-aggressive individuals

    do not incorrectly interpret emotional expressions, premeditated-aggressive

    individuals attributed greater levels of aggression to neutral faces.

    Experiment 3 (n=87) investigated functions of the orbitofrontal cortex,

    namely impulsivity, response reversal, and decision-making capabilities. No

    differences between impulsive-aggressive and premeditated-aggressive individuals

    were found on any of these measures suggesting negligible involvement of the

    orbitofrontal cortex in subclinical aggression.

    Overall, the results from this thesis suggest distinct neuropsychological

    processes in individuals who display predominantly impulsive-aggressive behaviour

    compared to those who display predominantly premeditated-aggression. While

    impulsive-aggression may result from executive dysfunction pertaining to the

    dorsolateral region of the prefrontal cortex, the display of premeditated aggression is

    related to functioning of the orbitofrontal cortex mediating the interpretation of

    aggression in others. Such findings have important implications not only in the

    understanding of the causal features of such behaviour, but also in the development

    and implementation of successful treatment strategies.

  • iv

    Acknowledgements

    I would like to express my thanks to friends and colleagues whose support and

    encouragement have made this thesis possible. Most importantly, I would like to

    thank my supervisor Dr Frances Martin for her continued help and support throughout

    my candidature. I greatly appreciate your patience and flexibility, your honesty, and

    your continued encouragement. I would also like to thank Dr Raimondo Bruno, Dr

    Clive Skilbeck, and Dr Tess Crawley for their valued feedback on my research and

    other academic and support staff within the School of Psychology at the University of

    Tasmania, particularly Vlasti Broucek and David Chadderton for IT and technical

    support, and Sue Ross and Susan Jopling for answering all my questions and helping

    out in every way they possibly could. Thank you to all members of the annexe,

    especially my roomies Megan Laugher and Matt Treeby for the many laughs, songs,

    murals, mega-cards, and dances over the years. Lastly, I would like thank all of my

    friends and family for their patience, advice, and understanding, and being so

    supportive and encouraging especially when I needed it most.

  • v

    Table of Contents

    Abstract Acknowledgements List of Tables List of Figures Chapter 1. Overview of the Thesis p. 1 Chapter 2. Impulsive- and Premeditated-Aggression: A Review of

    the Literature p. 6

    2.1 Aggression p. 6 2.2 Distinguishing between impulsive- and premeditated-

    aggression p. 7 2.2.1 Psychobiological evidence p. 11 2.2.2 Psychophysiological evidence p. 12 2.2.3 Neuropsychological evidence p. 14 2.2.4 Psychopathy p. 15

    2.3 Importance of distinguishing between impulsive- and premeditated-aggression p. 17

    Chapter 3. Prefrontal Cortex & Aggression p. 20

    3.1 The prefrontal cortex p. 22 3.2 Prefrontal divisions p. 24 3.3 Prefrontal dysfunction and aggression p. 28

    3.3.1 Lesion studies p. 28 3.3.2 Neuroimaging and clinical neurological findings p. 32

    3.4 The specific roles of the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in aggression p. 35

    3.5 The role of the prefrontal cortex in impulsive- and premeditated-aggression p. 37

    3.6 Subcortical structures p. 40 3.7 Conclusion p. 41

    Chapter 4. Rationale p. 42 Chapter 5. Study 1: Executive Functioning p. 46

    5.1 Neuroanatomy of executive functions p. 47 5.2 Executive functioning measures p. 50

    5.2.1 Verbal Fluency Test p. 50 5.2.2 Trail Making Test p. 51 5.2.3 Tower of Hanoi p. 52 5.2.4 Stroop Colour-Word Interference Task p. 53

  • vi

    5.2.5 The Brixton Test p. 54 5.3 Executive functioning and aggression – a review of previous

    research p. 55 5.4 Executive functioning and impulsive- and premeditated-

    Aggression p. 57 5.5 The relationship between executive functioning deficits and

    aggression p. 60 5.6 Limitations of other studies p. 62 5.7 Aim and hypotheses p. 64 5.8 Method p. 65

    5.8.1 Participants p. 65 5.8.2 Materials p. 70

    5.8.2.1 Questionnaires p. 70 5.8.2.2 Executive function measures p. 73 5.8.2.3 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale –

    Third Edition p. 77 5.8.3 Procedure p. 78

    5.9 Results p. 80 5.9.1 Participants p. 80 5.9.2 Executive function measures p. 82 5.9.3 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition p. 83

    5.10 Discussion p. 85 5.10.1 Verbal Fluency Test p. 87 5.10.2 Trail Making Test p. 88 5.10.3 Tower of Hanoi p. 90 5.10.4 Stroop Colour-Word Interference Task p. 92 5.10.5 The Brixton Test p. 95 5.10.6 Personality measures p. 96 5.10.7 Psychopathy and Antisocial Personality Disorder p. 98 5.10.8 The link between executive functioning deficits and

    impulsive-aggression p. 100 5.10.9 Conclusion p. 103

    Chapter 6. Study 2: Emotion Recognition and Aggression Attribution p. 107

    6.1 Neural systems involved in emotion recognition p. 109 6.2 Separable neural systems for different emotional expressions p. 113 6.3 The relationship between emotion recognition and aggression p. 115 6.4 Aim and hypotheses p. 124 6.5 Method p. 125

    6.5.1 Participants p. 125 6.5.2 Materials p. 127

    6.5.2.1 Questionnaires p. 127 6.5.2.2 Facial recognition tasks p. 128 6.5.2.3 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale –

    Third Edition p. 129 6.5.3 Procedure p. 129

    6.6 Results p. 131 6.6.1 Participants p. 131 6.6.2 Emotion recognition task p. 132

  • vii

    6.6.3 Aggression rating task p. 142 6.6.4 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition p. 150

    6.7 Discussion p. 150 6.7.1 Emotion recognition task p. 150 6.7.2 Aggression rating task p. 155 6.7.3 Personality measures p. 157 6.7.4 The link between emotion recognition and aggression p. 157 6.7.5 Conclusion p. 159

    Chapter 7. Study 3: Inhibition, Response Reversal, and

    Decision-Making p. 161 7.1 Inhibition p. 161

    7.1.1 Inhibition and the frontal lobes p. 164 7.1.2 Inhibition measures p. 166 7.1.3 Inhibition and aggression p. 169

    7.2 Response reversal p. 171 7.2.1 Response reversal and the frontal lobes p. 172 7.2.2 Response reversal measures p. 174 7.2.3 Response reversal and aggression p. 177

    7.3 Decision-making p. 180 7.3.1 Decision-making and the frontal lobes p. 181 7.3.2 Decision-making measures p. 181 7.3.3 Decision-making and aggression p. 184 7.3.4 Somatic marker hypothesis p. 186

    7.4 Impulsivity, response reversal, decision-making, and aggression p. 187 7.5 Aim and hypotheses p. 188 7.6 Method p. 189

    7.6.1 Participants p. 189 7.6.2 Materials p. 189

    7.6.2.1 Questionnaires p. 189 7.6.2.2 Inhibition, response reversal, and

    decision-making tasks p. 189 7.6.2.3 Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale –

    Third Edition p. 191 7.6.3 Procedure p. 191

    7.7 Results p. 195 7.7.1 Stop Signal Task p. 195 7.7.2 Intra/Extra Dimensional Set Shift task p. 196 7.7.3 Cambridge Gambling Task p. 198

    7.8 Discussion p. 202 7.8.1 Stop Signal Task p. 203 7.8.2 Intra/Extra Dimensional Set Shift task p. 207 7.8.3 Cambridge Gambling Task p. 210 7.8.4 Limitations and directions for future research p. 212 7.8.5 Conclusion p. 213

  • viii

    Chapter 8. General Discussion p. 215 8.1 Overview of findings p. 215

    8.1.1 Executive functioning p. 215 8.1.2 Emotion recognition and aggression attribution p. 218 8.1.3 Inhibition, response reversal, and decision-making p. 219

    8.2 Theoretical implications p. 220 8.2.1 Inhibition p. 222 8.2.2 Premeditated-aggression p. 224

    8.3 Clinical implications p. 226 8.4 Limitations p. 228 8.5 Conclusion p. 229

    References p. 231 Appendices p. 307

    Appendix A: HREC approval letter p. 308 Appendix B: Information sheet for Study 1 p. 310 Appendix C: Consent form for Study 1 p. 312 Appendix D: Facial stimuli from Ekman and Friesen’s (1976)

    collection used for the emotion recognition task p. 313 Appendix E: Facial stimuli from Ekman and Friesen’s (1976)

    collection used for the aggression rating task p. 314 Appendix F: Information sheet for Study 2 and Study 3 p. 315 Appendix G: Consent form for Study 2 and Study 3 p. 317 Appendix H: Data analyses CD

  • ix

    List of Tables

    Chapter 5

    Study 1: Executive Functioning

    Table 5.1 The Impulsive-Premeditated Aggression Scale p. 67

    Table 5.2 Number of males and females in the three participant

    groups and total sample p. 68

    Table 5.3 Mean (and standard deviation) scores on the Aggression

    Questionnaire – Short Form and ages for the three

    participant groups and total sample p. 69

    Table 5.4 Means (and standard deviations) for the Aggression

    Questionnaire – Full Scale and I7 Impulsivity

    Questionnaire for the three participant groups p. 81

    Table 5.5 Means (and standard deviations) for the three participant

    groups on the executive function and WAIS-III measures p. 84

    Table 5.6 Results of ANOVAs for the executive function and

    WAIS-III measures p. 85

    Chapter 6

    Study 2: Emotion Recognition and Aggression Attribution

    Table 6.1 Number of males and females in the three participant

    groups and total sample p. 126

    Table 6.2 Mean (and standard deviations) scores on the Aggression

    Questionnaire – Short Form and ages for the three

    participant groups and total sample p. 127

    Table 6.3 Means (and standard deviations) for the subscales of the

    Aggression Questionnaire – Full Scale and I7 Impulsivity

    Questionnaire for the three participant groups p. 132

    Table 6.4 Mean (and standard deviations) number of correct

    responses (maximum = 4) on the emotion recognition task

    for the three participant groups p. 133

  • x

    Table 6.5 Paired samples t-test results for response for each face

    type p. 135

    Table 6.6 Mean (and standard deviations) frequency of responses to

    the neutral face on the emotion recognition task for the

    three participant groups p. 137

    Table 6.7 ANOVA results for responses to the neutral face for the

    main effect of participant group at the 1000ms and 2000ms

    stimulus durations p. 138

    Table 6.8 Paired samples t-test results for frequency of response for

    each face type when interpreting the neutral face p. 139

    Table 6.9 Mean (and standard deviations) reaction times on the

    emotion recognition task for the three participant groups p. 140-141

    Table 6.10 Paired samples t-test results for reaction time for each

    face type p. 142

    Table 6.11 Mean (and standard deviations) responses for the three

    participant groups on the aggressive rating task p. 144

    Table 6.12 Mean (and standard deviations) reaction times for the

    three participant groups on the aggressive rating task p. 145

    Table 6.13 Paired samples t-test results for face type at the 1000ms

    stimulus duration p. 148

    Table 6.14 Paired samples t-test results for face type at the 2000ms

    stimulus duration p. 149

    Table 6.15 Means (and standard deviations) for Vocabulary and

    Digit Span for the three participant groups p. 150

    Chapter 7

    Study 3: Inhibition, Response Reversal, and Decision-Making

    Table 7.1 Means (and standard deviations) for the Stop Signal Task

    for the three participant groups p. 196

    Table 7.2 Means (and standard deviations) on the Intra-Extra

    Dimensional Set Shift task for the three participant groups p. 198

    Table 7.3 Results of univariate ANOVAs for the Intra-Extra

    Dimensional Set Shift task p. 199

  • xi

    Table 7.4 Means (and standard deviations) on the Cambridge

    Gambling Task for the three participant groups p. 201

    Table 7.5 Results of univariate ANOVAs for the Cambridge

    Gambling Task p. 202

  • xii

    List of Figures

    Chapter 6

    Study 2: Emotion Recognition and Aggression Attribution

    Figure 6.1 Mean number of correct responses (maximum = 4) for each

    face type at the 1000ms and 2000ms stimulus durations p. 136

    Figure 6.2 Reaction times for each face type by each group at the

    1000ms stimulus duration p. 147

    Figure 6.3 Reaction times for each face type by each group at the

    2000ms stimulus duration p. 149

  • 1

    Chapter 1

    Overview of the Thesis

    While the underpinnings of human aggression are clearly multifactorial, including

    political, socioeconomic, cultural, and psychological factors, it is also clear that some

    forms of aggression, either impulsive or premeditated in nature, have an underlying

    neurobiology that is only just beginning to be understood. In this research the

    neurobiology of aggression is addressed, specifically the role of the prefrontal cortex

    in the expression of both impulsive- and premeditated-aggression.

    A significant body of evidence indicates that the likelihood of acting

    aggressively is related to the functional capacity of the frontal lobe. Using

    neuroimaging techniques, studies of violent offenders have consistently shown

    abnormalities in frontal lobe structures in individuals who have histories of violence

    (e.g., Raine, Lencz, Bihrle, LaCasse & Colletti, 2000; Raine et al., 1998).

    Additionally, lesion studies (e.g., Damasio, Grabowski, Frank, Galaburda & Damasio,

    1994) and neuropsychological studies (e.g., Stanford, Greve & Gerstle, 1997) have

    provided evidence of the relationship between prefrontal impairment and the

    propensity for aggressive behaviour. Unfortunately, however, the above studies have

    placed little emphasis on the separable regions of the prefrontal cortex. This is despite

    the fact that neuropsychological data strongly suggesting that only medial and

    orbitofrontal regions of the prefrontal cortex are involved in mediating aggression,

    while dorsolateral prefrontal cortex has little role (Grafman et al., 1996). The

    prefrontal cortex is functionally and anatomically heterogeneous and thus the

    separable regions may be differentially involved in the expression of aggression. The

    present thesis thus attempts to delineate the specific roles of these subregions in

    impulsive- and premeditated-aggression.

  • 2

    Many researchers have suggested that the relationship between prefrontal

    abnormalities and likelihood of aggression is mediated by the failure to adaptively use

    executive functions (Giancola, 2000). As outlined in Chapter 5, executive functions is

    a broad term used to describe those abilities which allow an individual to respond to

    situations in a flexible manner, creating and adapting plans, and not being governed

    exclusively by external stimuli (Hoaken, Allaby, & Earle, 2007). Such abilities are

    presumed to be mediated predominantly by the dorsolateral region of the prefrontal

    cortex.

    A further ability linked to the prefrontal cortex is the ability to correctly

    interpret emotional facial expressions. More specifically, patients with orbitofrontal

    cortex lesions are impaired in their ability to recognise facial expressions, particularly

    anger (Blair & Cipolotti, 2000; Hornak, Rolls & Wade, 1996). Neuroimaging studies

    support these findings, demonstrating activation in the orbitofrontal cortex by

    negative emotional expressions; in particular, anger, but also fear and disgust (Blair,

    Morris, Frith, Perrett & Dolan, 1999; Kesler-West et al., 2001). As described in

    Chapter 6, aberrations in the ability to identify facial expressions may result in the

    generation of inappropriate social responses, such as reacting aggressively to

    ambiguous social situations (Dodge, Laird, Lochman & Zelli, 2002). This hypothesis

    is based on Dodge (1986) who proposed that the accurate interpretation of social

    stimuli must be completed for prosocial behaviour to be manifested.

    Individuals with lesions to the prefrontal cortex have also been shown to have

    deficits in inhibition, decision-making, and response reversal (Bechara, Damasio,

    Damasio & Anderson, 1994; Rolls, Hornak, Wade & McGrath, 1994). Such patients

    also display impairment in social functioning. The suggestion then follows that

    reappraisal will play an important role in social contexts in which one is required to

  • 3

    adapt to rapidly changing contexts (Happaney, Zelazo & Stuss, 2004). Furthermore,

    an inability to suppress previously rewarded responses due to inhibitory deficits, will

    in turn lead to inappropriate social responses.

    In research on aggression, it is vital to distinguish between impulsive- and

    premeditated-aggression. Impulsive-aggression is more reactive in nature and

    displayed without a self-generated goal. Premeditated-aggression, in contrast, appears

    to occur without provocation, is proactive, and is seen as a means to gain a valued

    outcome. This heterogeneity between impulsive- and premeditated-aggression

    suggests distinct cognitive mechanisms responsible for their display.

    Both the animal and human neuropsychological literature suggests that the

    prefrontal cortex is involved in the modulation of impulsive-aggression (Anderson,

    Bechara, Damasio, Tranel & Damasio, 1999; Gregg & Siegel, 2001). Certainly,

    damage to the medial frontal and orbitofrontal cortex is associated with increased risk

    for the display of impulsive-aggression in humans whether the lesion occurs in

    childhood (Anderson et al., 1999) or adulthood (Grafman et al., 1996). More

    specifically, individuals with orbitofrontal cortex lesions are typically described as

    disinhibited, socially inappropriate, impulsive, irresponsible, and as often

    misinterpreting others‟ moods (Rolls et al., 1994). In addition, there are considerable

    neuroimaging data assessing the neural functioning of patients with impulsive-

    aggression. These data have revealed reduced prefrontal functioning in patients

    presenting with impulsive-aggression (Søderstrom, Tullberg, Wikkelso, Ekholm &

    Forsman, 2000). Interestingly, this reduced prefrontal functioning is not observed in

    those with predominantly premeditated-aggression (Raine et al., 1998).

    Accounts of premeditated-aggression often lie in the related construct of

    psychopathy. Psychopathic individuals, individuals who present with marked

  • 4

    premeditated-aggression, do not present with deficits on neuropsychological measures

    which pertain predominantly to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Mitchell, Colledge,

    Leonard & Blair, 2002). Psychopathic individuals‟ high level of premeditated-

    aggression is thus completely unlike that of patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions

    (Cornell et al., 1996). It is likely then that such individuals show elevated levels of

    premeditated-aggression because they have been reinforced, and not punished, for

    committing such behaviour in the past (Blair, 2004). Such aversive conditioning has

    been shown to be mediated by the amygdala. In support of this, an MRI study by

    Tiihonen et al. (2000) found a strong negative correlation between level of

    psychopathy and amygdala volume.

    However, while there is clear evidence of amygdala involvement in

    premeditated-aggression through its role in aversive conditioning and instrumental

    learning, the orbitofrontal cortex may also be involved through its role in response

    reversal and extinction. That is, changing a response to a stimulus when the

    reinforcement contingencies change (Dias, Robbins & Roberts, 1996a). Moreover, the

    orbitofrontal cortex has been linked to decision-making when knowledge about

    potential positive and negative results is necessary to guide behavioural responding

    (Rogers et al., 1999b). On tasks such as the Intradimensional/Extradimensional

    (ID/ED) Set Shift Task which involves response reversal, adult psychopathic

    individuals show impairment (Mitchell et al., 2002). This suggests possible

    orbitofrontal dysfunction in individuals presenting with marked premeditated

    aggression.

    Investigating prefrontal impairment in antisocial behaviour more broadly is

    problematic due to its heterogeneity and comorbidity with several disorders, including

    drug and alcohol abuse, Antisocial Personality Disorder (APD), pathological

  • 5

    gambling, schizophrenia, and bipolar disorders, all of which may or may not involve

    an aggressive component. Frontal lobe functions have been implicated in all of these

    comorbid conditions, however frontal lobe deficits have also been found in disorders

    not readily associated with antisocial behaviour, such as obsessive-compulsive

    disorder. The question thus lies in whether there is a single common component of

    these disorders given that studies on such populations have been inconsistent with

    regard to demonstrated neuropsychological deficits.

    Through an investigation of prefrontal functioning in impulsive- and

    premeditated-aggressive individuals, the current study aimed to answer the following

    research questions:

    1. Do impulsive-aggressive and premeditated-aggressive individuals perform

    differently on measures of executive functioning known to relate to dorsolateral

    prefrontal cortex functioning?

    2. Do impulsive-aggressive and premeditated-aggressive individuals differ in their

    recognition of emotions in faces?

    3. Do impulsive-aggressive and premeditated-aggressive individuals demonstrate a

    hostile attributional bias in their interpretation of neutral facial expressions or

    overestimate the level of aggression in aggressive faces?

    4. Do impulsive-aggressive and premeditated-aggressive individuals present with

    deficits on measures of orbitofrontal cortex functioning, namely inhibition,

    decision-making, and response reversal?

  • 6

    Chapter 2

    Impulsive- and Premeditated-Aggression – A Review of the Literature

    2.1 Aggression

    Aggression can be defined as “any behaviour directed toward another

    individual that is carried out with the proximate (immediate) intent to cause harm”

    (Bushman & Anderson, 2001, p. 274). The aforementioned definition of aggression

    encompasses a variety of behaviours, which can range from verbal to relational to

    physical (Crick & Grotpeter, 1995). For example, a widely used definition of

    aggression is behaviour deliberately aimed at harming people and/or objects (Dodge,

    1991). In this definition, harm has implicitly been defined as hurting someone

    physically. However, other forms of harm, such as psychological harm (e.g.,

    humiliating), and relational harm (e.g., malevolent gossip), are just as important when

    discussing the notion of aggression.

    The aforementioned definition of aggression does not assume that all harmful

    behaviours are aggressive, rather, there are many instances in which harmful

    behaviours are prosocial. For example, the possible pain caused by a dentist to their

    patient is not aggressive because the proximate intent of the dentist is to help rather

    than hurt the individual. Similarly, both physical aggression in the context of self-

    defence and the selective use of verbal aggression by politicians, for example, are

    adaptive. Aggression, on the contrary, is problematic when it is a habitual behavioural

    pattern (Bushman & Anderson, 2001).

    The notion of aggression has been used to refer to a wide variety of concepts

    and phenomena which has led to much confusion within the aggression literature due,

    in part, to interchangeable use with other terms (Caprara et al., 1985). The related

  • 7

    theoretical constructs of anger and hostility continue to be used in lieu of aggression

    and it is therefore necessary to elucidate the conceptual distinctions between these

    constructs by reviewing their operational definitions. As noted above, aggression

    refers to a behavioural process that includes the goal of inflicting harm to another

    individual or object. In contrast, anger is conceptualised as an emotional state that can

    vary in intensity, from mild annoyances to rage (Spielberger, Jacobs, Russell &

    Crane, 1983). Moreover, the experience of anger lacks a specific goal (Berkowitz,

    1993) and is not necessary for aggression to occur. Unlike aggression or anger,

    hostility is an attitudinal or cognitive construct comprised of enduring cognitions that

    involve negative interpretations of the environment. As such, once hostile attitudes

    are verbally or physically expressed, they may be more appropriately labelled as

    aggression. Aggressive behaviour also needs to be distinguished from antisocial

    behaviour. Antisocial behaviour is defined as behaviour by which people are

    disadvantaged and basic forms and norms are violated (Merk, de Castro, Koops, &

    Matthys, 2005). Examples of such behaviour are lying, stealing and truancy.

    Aggressive behaviour, then, is a specific form of antisocial behaviour (Kempes,

    Matthys, de Vries & van Engeland, 2005).

    Caprara and colleagues (Caprara et al., 1985) suggest that the use of such

    concepts should be restricted to the use of two primary concepts: aggressiveness and

    aggression. According to their view, aggressiveness refers to a personality

    characteristic, while aggression refers to the aggressive behaviours manifested.

    2.2 Distinguishing between impulsive- and premeditated-aggression

    Traditionally, the study of violence and aggression has recognised the

    importance of distinguishing between different types of such behaviour. Aggression

  • 8

    can be classified in a number of ways, for example, by the target of aggression (e.g.,

    self-directed or directed towards another individual or object), mode of aggression

    (e.g., physical or verbal, direct or indirect), or cause of aggression (e.g., medical)

    (Siever, 2008). Although many individuals display more than one subtype of

    aggression (Barker, Tremblay, Nagin, Vitaro, & Lacourse, 2006), and correlations

    often exist among subtypes of aggression (Kempes et al., 2005), two distinct subtypes

    of aggressive behaviour consistently emerge; an affective or impulsive type, and a

    predatory or premeditated type. Characteristically, these two subtypes of aggression

    are distinguished by several features, but primarily by the amount of behavioural

    control exhibited during the incident.

    Impulsive-aggression is described as a reactive or emotionally charged

    aggressive response characterised by a loss of behavioural control (Barratt, 1991;

    Raine et al., 1998). These aggressive acts are unplanned and spontaneous in nature

    and are either unprovoked or out of proportion to the provocation. Impulsive-

    aggression is usually accompanied by an agitated or irritated mood, poor modulation

    of physiological arousal and loss of behavioural control (Houston, Stanford,

    Villemarette-Pittman, Conklin & Helfritz, 2003). Interpersonal communication is

    often non-adaptive during the agitated state and information processing appears to be

    inefficient (Elliot, 1992). This subtype of aggression can result in sudden, heightened,

    or inappropriate aggressive responses, and probably accounts for most societal

    problems that are associated with aggression. Coccaro (1998) emphasises that while

    the impulsive-aggressive individual does not necessarily have the intention to cause

    harm either to themselves or to others, their behaviour nevertheless has the potential

    to do so. Perpetrators of impulsive-aggression often report regret after the act,

    however often lack the self control to refrain from committing such acts again

  • 9

    (Barratt, 1994). Barratt proposes that the personality traits of impulsiveness and

    anger/hostility are related to most impulsive-aggressive acts.

    The theoretical roots of impulsive-aggression lie in the frustration-aggression

    model (Berkowitz, 1993). According to this theory, aggression is displayed as a

    consequence of frustration, actual or perceived threat, and heightened arousal in the

    form of anger. Aggression is displayed in reaction to aversive events with the

    subjective experiences of the individual central to the situation. The subjective

    experience of, for example, feeling threatened and not necessarily being threatened is

    a principal concept in this theory. Frustration may not immediately lead to aggression,

    but generate such emotions as anger, which can then augment the readiness to display

    aggression (Merk et al., 2005).

    Premeditated-aggression, on the other hand, is considered a purposeful,

    controlled aggressive display that is usually instrumental in nature. These acts require

    forethought and planning and are generally executed with low autonomic arousal

    (Stanford, Houston, Villemarette-Pittman, & Greve, 2003b). Premeditated-aggressive

    acts are carried out with a high degree of behavioural control and are directed toward

    a goal, such as external reinforcers (e.g., money oriented) or intimidation (Dodge &

    Coie, 1987; Hubbard, Dodge, Cillessen, Coie, & Schwartz, 2001; Vitiello, Behar,

    Hunt, Stoff, & Ricciuti, 1990).

    Premeditated-aggression can be understood by the principles of operant

    conditioning, where the probability of aggression is increased by prior history of

    subsequent reward or reinforcement (Dodge, 1991). An act of aggression occurs

    because of the expectancy of the reinforcement or reward that is to follow. From this

    model, one can see that the likelihood of committing an aggressive act may increase

    as a function of social reinforcement emanating from an environment where gangs are

  • 10

    present or where aggression is viewed in a more positive light (Kingsbury, Lambert,

    & Hendrickse, 1997). A deficit in the ability to experience or anticipate remorse or the

    aversive outcomes increases the risk of premeditated-aggression, which appears to be

    the case in aggressive individuals with psychopathy who have difficulty anticipating

    and experiencing negative feelings of remorse or guilt (Hare, 1999).

    The utility of this impulsive-premeditated classification has been indicated in

    predictive validity studies (Barratt, Stanford, Felthous, & Kent, 1997a; Barratt,

    Stanford, Kent & Felthous, 1997b; Heilbrun, Heilbrun, & Heilbrun, 1978; Linnoila et

    al., 1983; Mungas, 1988). In a study involving male and female college students,

    Barratt and colleagues (Barratt, Stanford, Dowdy, Liebman, & Kent, 1999) found

    impulsive and premeditated acts of aggression are independent constructs, however,

    emphasised that these two subtypes of aggressive behaviour may also coexist to

    varying degrees in „normal‟ persons. It is rare in practice that the aggressive acts

    exhibited by an individual can be classified as entirely premeditated or entirely

    impulsive (Stanford et al., 2003b; Weinshenker & Siegel, 2002). For example, an

    individual who habitually loses his temper, exhibits an irritable mood and often

    responds out of proportion to the provoking stimulus might be characterised as

    displaying impulsive-aggression. However, it is possible that this individual may also

    demonstrate some incidences of premeditated-aggression. Conversely, an individual

    whose aggressive displays are usually planned and consciously executed may

    experience some instances in which he loses control of his behaviour. Thus the

    proposed dimensional as apposed to categorical classification scheme permits the

    behaviour to be characterised as predominantly premeditated or predominantly

    impulsive, allowing for the heterogeneity that naturally occurs within an individual.

  • 11

    Although there has been some criticism of the dichotomous method of

    characterising aggressive behaviour (see Bushman & Anderson, 2001; Parrott &

    Giancola, 2007), a dichotomous approach is supported by a number of important

    distinctions between individuals who express predominantly impulsive- or

    predominantly premeditated-aggression. More specifically, researchers have found

    that impulsive-aggressive individuals experience disruptions across a variety of

    domains including verbal ability and intelligence, physiological reactivity, biological

    function and treatment response (Barratt et al., 1997b; Coccaro, 1992). In contrast,

    individuals demonstrating premeditated-aggression tend to have more circumscribed

    disturbances on measures of personality (Barratt et al., 1997a; Stanford et al., 2003b).

    2.2.1 Psychobiological evidence

    The neurotransmitter that has received the most attention in regards to

    aggressive behaviour is serotonin. Serotonin facilitates prefrontal cortical regions,

    such as the orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex that are involved in

    modulating and often suppressing the emergence of aggressive behaviours (Siever,

    2008). In both humans and animals, it appears that serotonin is primarily associated

    with impulsive-aggression in comparison to premeditated-aggression, and it appears

    that its effects may be receptor specific (Miczek, 1987; Shaikh, De Lanerolle, &

    Siegel, 1997). In humans, a relationship between decreased serotonergic function and

    impulsive-aggressive behaviour has consistently been demonstrated using a number

    of strategies, including central neurochemical measures (CSF 5-HIAA; Linnoila et al.,

    1983; Roy, Adinoff and Linnoila, 1988; Virkkunen, De Jong, Bartko, Goodwin, &

    Linnoila, 1989a), platelet binding (Kent et al., 1988), prolactin response

    (Fenfluramine; Coccaro et al., 1989), pharmacological treatment (sertraline; Kavoussi,

  • 12

    Liu & Coccaro, 1994; fluoxetine; Coccaro, Kavoussi & Hauger, 1997) and regional

    metabolic activity in response to serotonergic agonist (m-CPP; New et al., 2002).

    Barratt, Kent, Bryant and Felthous (1991) found that phenytoin reduces the frequency

    of aggressive acts. Replication studies have shown that phenytoin may reduce

    incidences of impulsive-aggression, but not premeditated-aggression (Barratt et al.,

    1997b; Barratt, Felthous, Kent, Liebman & Coates 2000). Such findings support the

    hypothesis that impulsive- and premeditated-aggression have different underlying

    biological substrates that respond differently to pharmacologic agents with specific

    modes of action.

    2.2.2 Psychophysiological evidence

    Psychophysiological techniques also provide a practical measure of

    neuropsychological functioning, however while a substantial literature exists on

    autonomic correlates of antisocial behaviour (Raine, 2002a, 2000b), there are a

    limited number of studies which have compared these measures in groups whose

    aggression was explicitly classified according to an impulsive-premeditated scheme.

    Pitts (1997) measured heart rate in aggressive children whose behaviour was

    classified as reactive (impulsive) or proactive (premeditated) in nature. Those

    characterised by reactive aggressive behaviour responded to a challenging task with

    increasing heart rate while those characterised by proactive aggression did not.

    Similarly, in a study of domestic batterers, heart rate activity was compared during

    marital interaction (Jacobson & Gottman, 1998). The batterers were divided into those

    whose heart rates lowered during the interaction and those whose heart rate increased.

    The men exhibiting decreasing heart rates were characterised as calm, calculated,

    antisocial and sadistic (premeditated). Those exhibiting increasing heart rates were

  • 13

    described as being more emotional, angry and unstable (impulsive). Finally, a more

    recent study of aggressive children indicated a significant increase in skin

    conductance reactivity in those rated high in reactive aggression during a laboratory-

    based measure of induced anger (Hubbard et al., 2002). Again, those individuals

    exhibiting primarily reactive or impulsive aggression responded differently

    autonomically than those deemed more proactive or premeditated.

    Electroencephalography (EEG) abnormalities are also present among those

    who engage in impulsive-aggression. For example, Drake, Hietter and Pakalnis

    (1992) found a greater incidence of EEG slowing in a group of patients described as

    having episodic dyscontrol syndrome as compared to depressed patients and controls.

    Abnormalities in P1 amplitude have been reported in impulsive-aggressive college

    students (Houston & Stanford, 2001) and youths characterised by explosive

    aggressive behaviours (Bars, Heyrend, Simpson & Munger, 2001). In addition, adults

    classified as impulsive-aggressive exhibit decreased P1-N1-P2 latency (Houston &

    Stanford, 2001), reduced P3 amplitude (Barratt et al., 1997b; Gerstle, Mathias &

    Stanford 1998; Mathias & Stanford, 1999), increased P3 latency (Mathias & Stanford,

    1999), and reduced amplitude and increased latency on the late positive potential, a

    purported measure of emotional processing (Conklin & Stanford, 2002). These

    differences reflect a number of sensory and information processing deficits specific to

    impulsive-aggression, as well as preliminary evidence for emotional processing

    impairment.

    Volkow et al. (1995), using positron emission tomography (PET), found that

    psychiatric patients with a history of repetitive, purposeless violent behaviour showed

    significantly lower relative metabolic values in medial temporal and prefrontal

    cortices compared to normal controls. Similarly, Raine et al. (1998) reported that

  • 14

    affective (impulsive) murderers have significantly reduced prefrontal activation when

    compared to predatory (premeditated) murderers and controls.

    The literature regarding premeditated-aggression though sparse is consistent.

    Individuals who engage in acts of premeditated-aggression show few differences from

    non-aggressive controls on psychophysiological measures, including P3 (Barratt et al.,

    1997b; Stanford et al., 2003b). Stanford et al. found that the P3 latency difference did

    approach significance (p = .06), suggesting a trend toward a longer P3 latency in the

    premeditated-aggressive group. Such prolonged P3 latency has been linked to

    increased attitudinal hostility (Bond & Surguy, 2000). Thus, the high levels of

    anger/hostility evidenced in the premeditated-aggressive group may have played a

    role in the latency trend observed in the sample.

    2.2.3 Neuropsychological evidence

    While small in number, neuropsychological studies comparing modes of

    aggression have established a correlation between increased impulsive-aggression and

    decreased executive functioning, while few deficits have been found in those who are

    premeditated in their aggressive behaviour (Houston et al., 2003). Dolan and

    Anderson (Dolan, Deakin, Roberts & Anderson, 2002) grouped male personality

    disordered offenders into high and low impulsive aggressors using the Belligerence

    Scale of the Special Hospital Assessment of Personality and Socialization (SHAPS:

    Blackburn, 1982). They found that both impulsivity and aggression were negatively

    related to executive function, and aggression was negatively related to memory

    function. Similarly, Giancola, Moss, Martin, Kirisci and Tarter (1996) found that

    problems in executive functioning were a predictor of reactive aggression in

    adolescent boys at risk for substance abuse. Most recently, Villemarette-Pittman,

  • 15

    Stanford and Greve (2002) found that verbal deficiencies varied according to

    executive demands of the task in a sample of impulsive-aggressive college students.

    In the first study to compare premeditated-aggressive subjects with controls on

    a variety of neuropsychological tests, Stanford et al. (2003b) found no significant

    differences except for a single subscale of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST),

    where the premeditated group exhibited greater failure to maintain set than controls.

    In contrast, there were pronounced differences on a range of personality measures,

    including impulsivity, verbal and physical aggression, anger, hostility, psychoticism

    and neuroticism. The authors concluded that the difference between the premeditated-

    aggressive group and controls was a result of an impulsive personality style rather

    than a significant cognitive deficit.

    In summary, neuropsychological assessment has shown a clear link between

    impulsive-aggressive behaviour and problems in executive functioning, while few if

    any deficits have been demonstrated in premeditated-aggressive individuals.

    2.2.4 Psychopathy

    The concept of psychopathy has provided some utility in further distinguishing

    between impulsive- and premeditated-aggression. Psychopathy refers to a

    constellation of personality and behavioural characteristics marked by low baseline

    arousal, dishonesty, absence of remorse, empathy, and conscience, antisocial

    behaviour, and impersonal relationships (Hare, 2003). Interpersonally, they are often

    described as grandiose, arrogant, callous, superficial and manipulative (Hare, 1999).

    The concept of psychopathy has been operationalised by the work of Hare

    (1991, 1999), and its assessment is now highly reliable and valid. There is a growing

    body of research to show that psychopathic criminals engage in more premeditated

  • 16

    and predatory violence than non-psychopathic criminals (Cornell et al., 1996; Serin,

    1991). Williamson, Hare and Wong (1987) found that incarcerated psychopaths had

    higher rates (45.2%) than incarcerated non-psychopaths (14.6%) of committing their

    crime for material gain (i.e., proactive in nature), and that non-psychopaths had higher

    rates (31.7 vs. 2.4%) of emotional arousal leading to their offences (i.e., impulsive in

    nature). Likewise, Cornell et al. (1996) found premeditated-aggressive offenders

    could be distinguished from non-premeditated offenders by higher total psychopathy

    specifically concerning: pathological lying; manipulative actions; lack of empathy;

    parasitic lifestyle; irresponsibility; criminal versatility; and superficiality. The authors

    concluded that “the link between psychopathy and instrumental violence supports the

    distinction between instrumental and reactive violence, and raised the possibility that

    the presence of instrumental violence could be an associated characteristic of

    psychopathic offenders” (p. 790). Similarly, Woodworth and Porter (2002), in a study

    of 125 homicide offenders, found that the great majority of homicides committed by

    psychopaths were instrumental (i.e., premeditated), whereas only 48.4% of homicides

    committed by non-psychopaths were instrumental. Meloy (1988) theorised that a

    predisposition to engage in premeditated violence in psychopaths would be due to

    their low levels of autonomic arousal and reactivity, their disidentification with the

    victim, their emotional detachment and their lack of empathy.

    Similar differences have been found in children. A growing body of evidence

    indicates that children with conduct problems represent a group that can be further

    divided into discrete subtypes based on the presence of callous and unemotional traits

    (Frick, O‟Brien, Wootton & McBurnett, 1994; Frick & Ellis, 1999). Callous and

    unemotional traits are similar to psychopathic traits such as lack of guilt and empathy,

    superficial charm, and constricted emotion. In a sample of children with conduct

  • 17

    problems, Christian, Frick, Hill and Tyler (1997) found that a subgroup of children

    exhibiting symptoms of Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) and Conduct Disorder

    (CD) and callous and unemotional traits differed from those without such traits in the

    number and variety of conduct problems. Pardini, Lochman and Frick (2003) found

    that the presence of callous and unemotional traits in adjudicated juveniles was

    associated with the use of aggression to obtain rewards and dominate (i.e.,

    premeditated). Frick et al. (2003), in a sample of non-referred children, found that

    children demonstrating conduct problems and callous and unemotional traits were

    more likely to demonstrate high levels of proactive aggression than those without

    these traits, whose aggression was predominantly impulsive. It thus appears that it is

    the presence of callous and unemotional traits that distinguish this subgroup and its

    associated problems.

    2.3 Importance of distinguishing between impulsive- and premeditated

    aggression

    There is evidence in both children and adults that impulsive- and

    premeditated- aggression are distinguishable forms of aggressive behaviour with

    important clinical implications. For example, longitudinal studies in children and

    adolescents rated on reactive and proactive aggressive behaviour have shown that

    proactive, but not reactive, aggression predicts later delinquent behaviour (Vitaro,

    Gendreau, Tremblay & Oligny, 1998). Nouvion and colleagues found that proactive

    aggressive subjects had a greater incidence of personality disorders, including CD and

    APD, compared to reactive aggressive and non-aggressive control subjects (Nouvion,

    Cherek, Lane, Tcheremissine & Lieving, 2007).

  • 18

    These findings are in line with previous studies that have found proactive, but

    not reactive, aggression to be predictive of ODD, CD and externalising problems

    (e.g., Conner, Steingard, Anderson & Melloni, 2003; Pulkkinen, 1996; Vitaro et al.,

    1998). Given that individuals diagnosed with APD must have evidence of CD before

    the age of 15, early onset behavioural problems leading to adult antisocial behaviour

    was prevalent in the premeditated group. This provides evidence that premeditated-

    aggressive individuals may have increased personality psychopathology and be at

    increased risk for early aggressive and antisocial behaviours relative to impulsive-

    aggressive or non-aggressive individuals. The fact that CD was a distinguishable

    factor between groups is of significance due to the fact that CD is stable over time

    (Bassarath, 2001a; Kazdin, 2000), and is associated with criminal behaviour and

    substance abuse (Hser, Grella, Collins & Teruya, 2003; Mueser et al., 2006;

    Tcheremissine & Lieving, 2006).

    Heilbrun et al. (1978) found that murderers whose violence was classified as

    impulsive were more likely to fail on parole that those whose murders were

    premeditated in nature. In a pharmacological treatment study comparing aggressive

    subtypes, inmates whose aggressive behaviour was classified as impulsive showed

    significant reductions in the frequency and intensity of aggressive acts, normalisation

    of event-related potentials (P3) and improvement in mood state measures during a six

    week trial of anticonvulsant phenytoin (Dilantin) when compared to placebo (Barratt

    et al., 1997a). Inmates whose aggressive behaviour was classified as premeditated in

    nature showed no improvement during the same trial. Similarly, Malone et al. (1998)

    examined the effect of lithium carbonate in CD children whose repeated aggressive

    behaviour was categorised as either predatory or affective in nature. Treatment

    response was associated with affective rather than predatory aggressive behaviour.

  • 19

    While distinguishing between these forms of aggression can not only lead to a

    better theoretical understanding of aggression (Coie & Dodge, 1998; Poulin & Boivin,

    2000; Vitiello & Stoff, 1997), it can also to better prognostication. Such a distinction

    is also assumed to lead to the development of more specific interventions and

    treatment, which may then prove more effective than interventions aimed at

    aggression in general (McAdams, 2002; Vitello & Stoff, 1997).

  • 20

    Chapter 3

    Prefrontal Cortex & Aggression

    Research into the antecedents of violence and aggression indicates that there

    are many factors which contribute to the development of these behaviours. It is

    important to note that while there are general predictors of violent and aggressive

    behaviour, no single theory can account for causation in all situations. It is accepted

    that the causes of aggression are multi-faceted and that neurological deficit may be a

    factor in only a small percentage of those who demonstrate such behaviour. However,

    given that aggression – like any behaviour – ultimately derives from the normal and

    abnormal operations of the brain, closer examination of the aspects of brain structure

    and function relevant to aggressive behaviour are required.

    Numerous studies, in both animals and humans, have supported an association

    between abnormalities in brain function and aggressive and violent behaviour (Filley

    et al., 2001; Golden, Jackson, Peterson-Rohne & Gontkovsky, 1996; Krakowski,

    2003). Case studies of patients with neurological disorders or those who have suffered

    traumatic brain injury provide provocative insights into which brain regions, when

    damaged, might predispose to irresponsible, aggressive behaviour.

    Psychophysiological and neuropsychological assessments have also demonstrated that

    violent and/or aggressive individuals have lower brain functioning than controls,

    including lower verbal ability and diminished executive functioning (Barratt et al.,

    1997b; Dolan & Park, 2002; Hoaken et al., 2007).

    The availability of new functional and structural neuroimaging techniques,

    such as PET, magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) and functional MRI (fMRI), has

    broadened our understanding of the neural circuitry that underlies aggressive and

  • 21

    antisocial behaviours. More specifically, as reviewed by Davidson and colleagues, a

    circuit that includes several regions of the prefrontal cortex, the amygdala,

    hippocampus, hypothalamus, anterior cingulate cortex, ventral striatum, and other

    interconnected structures has been implicated in various aspects of emotion regulation

    and affective style (Davidson & Irwin, 1999; Davidson, Jackson & Kalin, 2000a).

    Emotion regulation includes those processes which amplify, attenuate, or maintain an

    emotion, and thus incorporates the expression of aggressive behaviours. Related to

    this is evidence which suggests that individuals who are vulnerable to faulty

    regulation of negative emotion may be at increased risk for aggression and/or violent

    behaviour (Davidson, Putnam & Larson, 2000b).

    Prefrontal cortical dysfunction has been implicated as a possible anatomical

    correlate of aggressive behaviour (Convitt et al., 1996; Critchley et al., 2000; Pietrini,

    Guazzelli, Basso, Jaffe & Grafman, 2000; Raine et al., 1994). Both cognitive and

    behavioural similarities have been noted between individuals who have frontal lobe

    damage and those who show characteristics of antisocial behaviour (Price, Daffner,

    Stowe & Mesulam, 1990). For example, Eslinger and Damasio (1985) noted that

    damage to the prefrontal area is associated with heightened aggression, emotional

    outbursts, disorganisation, and risk-taking behaviour. This observation has been a

    major impetus for the research on neuropsychological abnormalities in antisocial

    individuals. In particular, damage to the prefrontal regions of the brain and the

    resulting impairment in executive functions is considered to be associated with

    increased aggressive and antisocial behaviour (Brower & Price, 2001; Tateno, Jorge

    & Robinson, 2003).

  • 22

    3.1.1 The prefrontal cortex

    The frontal cortex encompasses the brain areas anterior to the central sulcus

    and comprises approximately one third of the cerebral cortex. The frontal cortex can

    be divided into three principle regions: the primary motor cortex, the prefrontal

    cortex, and the limbic cortex (Duke & Kaszniak, 2000). The prefrontal cortex refers to

    the most anterior regions of the frontal lobes and it is functionally and anatomically

    heterogeneous (Fuster, 2001). The prefrontal cortex has a rich supply of connections

    with other neural regions. Cortically, it is connected with association cortex in the

    temporal, parietal and occipital lobes, and subcortically with the hippocampus,

    amygdala, thalamus, hypothalamus, subthalamus, septum, striatum, pons, and

    mesencephalon (Fuster, 2001; Pandya & Barnes, 1987). Given that the prefrontal

    cortex is connected to more brain regions that any other cortical region, its position

    allows the integration of information processed at lower levels, including input from

    the limbic circuits, as well as being the major target of the basal ganglia-

    thalamocortical circuits (Royall et al., 2002).

    The prefrontal cortex, along with its underlying subcortical regions, is

    extensively interconnected with the major sensory and motor systems of the brain.

    Connections from the posterior cortical areas, particularly areas of multimodal

    convergence, bring information regarding the external environment. Subcortical

    pathways, including the amygdala, hippocampus, midbrain area, and thalamus, bring

    details about internal states (Duke & Kaszniak, 2000).

    The prefrontal cortex has direct neural projections from a variety of limbic

    structures that are directly linked to the amygdala. There are input projections from

    the thalamus to the prefrontal cortex, and these connections contain information

    arising from the temporal cortex and the amygdala. Direct reciprocal connections

  • 23

    from the prefrontal cortex to the amygdala have also been identified (Afifi &

    Bergman, 1998). Output projections to the amygdala are both excitory and inhibitory

    in nature. However, damage to the prefrontal cortex results in an overactivation of the

    amygdala, suggesting that the effect of the prefrontal cortex on the amygdala is

    predominantly inhibitory (Gerwitz, Falls & Davis, 1997; Morgan, Romanski &

    LeDoux, 1993). With regard to the functionality of this connection, lesions to the

    prefrontal cortex in rats reduce the prefrontal inhibitory action on the amygdala,

    resulting in an increased difficulty in the extinction of aversive responses (Morgan et

    al., 1993), as well as impairing the ability to anticipate future negative consequences

    (Bechara, Tranel, Damasio & Damasio, 1996). Therefore, it appears that the

    prefrontal cortex plays an important role in regulating the acquisition of new

    responses, and the extinction of aversive responses (Lopez, Vazquez & Olson, 2004).

    The prefrontal cortex also plays a central role in many aspects of social

    cognition (Rilling et al., 2002), including perspective taking (Frith & Frith, 1999), and

    also in the regulation of emotions such as aggression (see Blair, 2004 for review).

    Early descriptions of frontal lobe syndromes arose from several 19th century

    investigators, described in a number of reviews (Damasio et al., 1994; Macmillan,

    2002; Tranel, Anderson & Benton, 1994), highlighting the changes displayed in social

    behaviour, personality, and emotional regulation that occurred after frontal lobe

    pathology. Subsequent investigators continued to elaborate on the nature and extent of

    these deficits, their causes and management (Miller & Cummings, 1999; Stuss &

    Knight, 2002), firmly establishing a vital role for the frontal lobes, particularly the

    prefrontal cortex, in such processes.

    In an overview of neuroanatomy and neuropathology, Stuss and Benson

    (1984) described six specific manifestations of prefrontal damage: (1) inability to use

  • 24

    knowledge to regulate behaviour; (2) impaired ability to handle sequential behaviour;

    (3) impaired ability to establish or change a mental set; (4) impaired ability to

    maintain a mental set; (5) impaired ability to monitor personal behaviour; and (6)

    attitudes of apathy.

    3.2 Prefrontal divisions

    For clinical purposes, the prefrontal cortex can be divided into three distinct

    neuroanatomical regions: 1) dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (Brodmann‟s areas 9, 10,

    46); 2) medial prefrontal cortex (including the functionally related anterior cingulate

    cortex and Brodmann‟s area 24); and 3) orbital prefrontal cortex (Brodmann‟s areas

    11 and 12), corresponding to the most inferior and ventral parts of the prefrontal

    cortex (behind the eyes, or orbits). Both medial prefrontal and orbitofrontal are part of

    a frontostriatal circuit that has strong connections to the amygdala and other parts of

    the limbic system. Consequently, these regions are anatomically well suited for the

    integration of affective and non-affective information, and for the regulation of

    appetitive/motivated responses. Functionally, these regions are often considered

    together, as when researchers focus on effects of damage to ventromedial prefrontal

    cortex (Happaney et al., 2004).

    The prefrontal cortex is a heterogeneous region of the brain and the three

    principal frontal-subcortical circuits are involved in cognitive, emotional, and

    motivational processes. The primary focus of the current research will be on the roles

    of the dorsolateral and orbital divisions of the prefrontal cortex, which manifest quite

    distinct anatomical and functional properties (Fuster, 1989; Stuss & Benson, 1986).

    The dorsolateral prefrontal cortex projects primarily to the dorsolateral head

    of the caudate nucleus, which receives input from the posterior parietal cortex and

  • 25

    premotor areas. The dorsolateral circuit then connects to the dorsolateral part of the

    globus pallidus and rostral substantia nigra reticulate, and continues to the

    parvocellular area of the medial dorsal and ventral anterior portions of the thalamus.

    Projections from the thalamus back to the dorsolateral prefrontal circuit close the

    circuit (Cummings, 1993).

    Functionally, the high-level cognitive abilities mediated by the dorsolateral

    prefrontal cortex and its connections are those referred to as „executive functions‟,

    including cognitive flexibility, temporal ordering of events, planning, monitoring and

    inhibiting pre-programmed behaviour, set-shifting, working memory and concept

    formation (Smith & Jonides, 1999). According to Cummings (1995), dysfunction in

    the dorsolateral prefrontal circuit is associated with circuit-specific problems

    including decreased verbal fluency, perseveration, difficulty shifting set, poor

    recall/retrieval of information, reduced mental control, limited abstraction ability, and

    poor response inhibition. However, while patients with lesions restricted to this region

    are concrete and perseverative and show impairments in reasoning and mental

    flexibility (Benton, 1986), they typically demonstrate intact perception, calculation,

    language abilities and storage of memories (Duke & Kaszniak, 2000).

    The orbitofrontal cortex occupies the ventral region of the prefrontal cortex

    (Kringelbach & Rolls, 2004), which is reciprocally connected with the amygdala

    (Ghashghaei & Barbas, 2002). The orbitofrontal cortex projects to the ventromedial

    caudate nucleus, which receives input from other cortical association areas and

    brainstem regions, and has open interconnections with the dorsolateral prefrontal

    cortex, the temporal pole, and the amygdala (Davis & Whalen, 2001). The

    orbitofrontal cortex contains the secondary taste cortex, in which the reward value of

    taste is represented. It also contains the secondary and tertiary olfactory cortical areas,

  • 26

    in which the identity and also the reward value of odours are represented. The

    orbitofrontal cortex also receives information about the sight of objects from the

    temporal lobe cortical visual areas (Rolls, 1999).

    The orbitofrontal-subcortical circuit is said to underlie social behaviour and

    appears to play a critical role in the representation of the reward value of a stimulus

    and the way in which this representation guides goal-directed behaviour (Rolls, 1999).

    Lesions specific to the circuit have been found to result in marked changes in

    personality, including disinhibition, impulsivity, and antisocial behaviour, and

    irritability and lability are often prominent (Cummings, 1995). Some of the changes

    may be related to difficulty in the learning and reversal of stimulus-reinforcement

    associations, and thus the correction of behavioural responses when they are no longer

    appropriate due to changes in reinforcement contingencies (Rolls, 2004; Hornak et al.,

    2004). Indeed, investigations in macaques have shown that lesions to the orbitofrontal

    cortex impair reversal learning (Dias et al., 1996a). Consistent with this, the

    orbitofrontal cortex is activated by monetary rewards and punishments, and the

    magnitude of the reinforcers (O‟Doherty, Kringelbach, Rolls, Hornak & Andrews,

    2001). The visual input to neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex is in many cases the

    reinforcement association of visual stimuli, one of which is information about faces.

    Such facial stimuli convey information that is important in social reinforcement

    (Rolls, 2004).

    The medial circuit, begins in the anterior cingulate and projects to the nucleus

    accumbens. The anterior cingulate has interconnections with dorsolateral prefrontal

    cortex and the amygdala, and it also receives input from the ventral tegmental area

    (Duke & Kaszniak, 2000). The medial frontal-subcortical circuit is involved in

  • 27

    motivation. Lesions to this region often produce apathy, lack of motivation, decreased

    social interaction, and psychomotor retardation (Sbordone, 2000).

    The ventromedial prefrontal region includes the medial and varying sectors of

    the lateral orbitofrontal cortex, encompassing Brodmann‟s areas 25, lower 24, 32, and

    medial aspect of 11, 12, and 10, and the white matter subjacent to all of these areas

    (Bechara, 2004). Patients with bilateral lesions of the ventromedial cortex develop

    severe impairments in personal and social decision-making, in spite of otherwise

    largely preserved intellectual abilities. Following damage to this region of the

    prefrontal cortex, patients develop difficulties in daily and future planning, and

    difficulties in choosing friends and activities (Bechara, Damasio & Damasio, 2000a;

    Bechara, Tranel & Damasio, 2002).

    The identification of these adjacent circuits provides insight as to the

    similarities of behavioural changes caused by lesions to different brain regions. Whilst

    focal lesions to the areas of the prefrontal cortex have led to what have been labelled

    “frontal lobe syndrome”, the involvement of multiple circuits in subcortical lesions

    has resulted in variable behavioural manifestations (Cummings, 1995). For example,

    studies of lesions to the globus pallidus have described patients with marked changes

    in personality and reduced activity levels with memory and executive function

    deficits, but with normal intelligence and language abilities (e.g., Strub, 1989).

    In summary, the frontal–subcortical circuits are extensively connected to each

    other at the level of the frontal lobes. The circuits are discrete in subcortical regions.

    The dorsolateral circuit, because of its neuroanatomy, is uniquely able to integrate

    information from all three frontal–subcortical circuits. Here, the integrated

    information from the external world and the cognitive and emotional states of the

    individual can be used in the production of social behaviour.

  • 28

    3.3 Prefrontal dysfunction and aggression

    Frontal lobe dysfunction in particular, has been invoked to explain the actions

    of individuals convicted of violent crimes, who appear to fail to inhibit impulsive,

    trivially motivated, or habitual aggression. Case studies as far back as 1935 have

    reported the onset of antisocial personality traits after frontal lobe injury (Blumer &

    Benson, 1975). Such cases typically involve damage to the orbitofrontal cortex, which

    clinical observations have associated with poor impulse control, explosive aggressive

    outbursts, inappropriate verbal lewdness, jocularity, and lack of interpersonal

    sensitivity (Duffy & Campbell, 1994). This dysregulation of affect and behaviour may

    occur while cognitive, motor, and sensory functioning remains relatively intact

    (Mesulam, 1986).

    3.3.1 Lesion studies

    Research on individuals who have suffered traumatic brain injury is of key

    importance in investigating the neural substrates of aggressive behaviour. The critical

    role of the prefrontal cortex in aggressive behaviour was initially recognised by case

    reports that prefrontal brain lesions could result in the emergence of antisocial

    behaviours or psychopathic traits in previously normal subjects (Damasio, Tranel &

    Damasio, 1990). A prime example of this disinhibition is found in the often cited case

    of Phineas Gage, a dependable and responsible stable railroad worker who was

    injured by a tamping rod that penetrated his skull through his orbital frontal cortex.

    After the accident he became irresponsible and impulsive, despite preserved general

    cognitive and motor skills (Damasio et al., 1994).

    McAllister and Price (1987) evaluated 20 psychiatric patients with diverse

    types of frontal cortical pathology as identified by computed tomography scans and

  • 29

    EEG. Results indicated that 60% of the patients displayed disinhibited behaviour with

    affective lability and 10% displayed violent outbursts. However, the results of this

    study are difficult to interpret given that, in addition to having a frontal lobe

    pathology, all of the patients had at least one psychiatric diagnosis, and the exact

    neuroanatomical location of the pathology for each patient was not reported. In

    another study, Heinrichs (1989) found that a frontal cortical lesion was the best

    predictor of violent behaviour in a sample of 45 neuropsychiatric patients. Again,

    many of the patients in this study had other psychiatric diagnoses and the exact

    anatomical locations of the frontal neuropathologies were not specified.

    Further data from neurological case reports have provided much useful

    information regarding the relationship between prefrontal cortical functioning and

    aggression. Thompson (1970) reported a case of a 33-year-old male with a history of

    violent behaviour subsequent to a head injury at the age of 12. A

    pneumencephalography revealed bilateral cortical atrophy in the prefrontal regions.

    Price et al. (1990) studied the adult behaviour patterns of two patients who acquired

    brain damage during childhood. While both patients developed relatively normally

    until the damage was sustained, following the damage these patients displayed an

    inability to respond to punishment or delay gratification, irresponsibility, sexual

    promiscuity, grand larceny, drug involvement, angry outbursts, arson, suspected rape,

    and physical violence. Although the exact location of the lesions in both cases is

    equivocal, neurological and neuropsychological examination indicated bilateral

    lesions in the prefrontal cortex.

    Boone et al. (1988) described a 13-year-old female suffering from partial

    complex seizures localised primarily in the prefrontal cortex. Six weeks before being

    admitted to hospital she began exhibiting prominent behavioural changes consisting

  • 30

    of bizarre speech, sexual disinhibition, disobeying parental orders, and verbal and

    physical aggression. An EEG revealed activity in the frontal lobes and

    neuropsychological tests demonstrated deficits on prefrontal tests involving attention,

    alternation between tasks, performance on mazes, response inhibition, and

    distractibility. In a similar case, Eslinger and Damasio (1985) noted personality

    changes in patient EVR subsequent to surgical ablation of the orbital and mesial areas

    of the prefrontal cortex. Following the surgery, while his level of intelligence was

    above average, EVR began to engage in what the authors termed „sociopathic

    behaviour‟, including difficulties in decision-making, adjustment problems, poor

    judgement, and employment problems. The patient performed well on prefrontal

    cortical tests such as the WCST which the authors attributed to the fact that the

    dorsolateral prefrontal regions and superior mesial regions were left undamaged.

    Meyers, Berman, Scheibe and Hayman (1992) noted similar behavioural

    sequelae involving disinhibition, poor judgement, and irresponsibility subsequent to

    surgical damage to the left orbital prefrontal cortex in a 33-year-old male.

    Interestingly, this patient performed in the above average range on prefrontal cortical

    tests such as the WCST; however this is again likely due to the preservation of the

    dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. These findings reflect those of Phineas Gage described

    earlier, who subsequent to his injury, was described as being untrustworthy,

    irresponsibly, and disrespectful. Again, the majority of the neural damage was located

    in the orbital and mesial prefrontal regions, whereas the dorsolateral area was found to

    be spared (Damasio et al., 1994).

    Of the eight studies reviewed in the preceding section, six document a

    relationship between prefrontal cortical pathology and aggressive behaviour.

    However, none of these reports specified the exact location of the neuropathology

  • 31

    within the frontal lobes. As a result, these data do not provide evidence to implicate

    more specifically the dorsolateral or orbital prefrontal regions in the expression of

    aggressive behaviour. The remaining two reports indicate that their patients had

    lesions in the orbital prefrontal cortex and not the dorsolateral area.

    Other case studies of patients who have sustained damage to the orbitofrontal

    region, such as EVR, resemble Gage in manifesting a behavioural profile that has

    been referred to as „acquired sociopathy‟ (Saver & Damasio, 1991; Tranel, 1994;

    Meyers et al., 1992; Blair & Cipolotti, 2000). Damasio et al. (1994) describe acquired

    sociopathy as a reactive, emotionally driven violence toward a person that is related to

    emotional inhibitory dyscontrol. Although showing minimal impairments on standard

    neuropsychological tests of intelligence and executive functions, these patients

    display marked deficits in real life tasks involving judgement, awareness of socially

    appropriate conduct, and the capacity to assess future consequences (Bechara et al.,

    2000b).

    Blair and Cipolotti (2000) reported on JS who sustained damage to the

    orbitofrontal cortex and some damage to the left amygdala. Premorbidly, JS was

    described as being a quiet, withdrawn person who was never aggressive. Following

    the damage, JS showed unpredictable, impulsive-aggression and violence, and

    demonstrated deficits in the recognition of facial expression, particularly in the

    recognition of anger and disgust. He also produced significantly lower skin

    conductance responses (SCR) to the anger and disgust expressions compared with

    comparison groups.

    Further clinical data demonstrate that lesions in the orbitofrontal cortex and

    adjacent prefrontal regions produce syndromes characterised by impulsivity and

    aggression. Anderson et al. (1999) reported on two individuals, tested in their

  • 32

    twenties, who suffered early damage to orbital and lateral sectors of the prefrontal

    cortex. Both exhibited a significant deficit in moral reasoning, a history of verbal and

    physical aggression, and intermittent, explosive bursts of anger. A further study of

    two adults who sustained frontal lobe injury in childhood suggests that early damage

    to orbitofrontal regions may lead to a “comportmental learning disability” that closely

    resembles sociopathy and includes a diminished capacity to inhibit violence (Price et

    al., 1990).

    Further evidence implicating the orbitofrontal regions comes from a large

    retrospective study of Vietnam veterans with penetrating head injuries, which found

    that ventromedial frontal and orbitofrontal lesions, as assessed by computed

    tomography scans, specifically increased the risk of aggressive and violent behaviour.

    (Grafman et al.,1996). Data have also been reported showing higher rates of antisocial

    behaviour (including stealing, physical assault and sexual comments or advances) in

    patients with frontotemporal dementia, even when compared with equally cognitively

    impaired patients with Alzheimer‟s disease (Miller, Darby, Benson, Cummings, &

    Miller, 1997; Stip, 1995).

    3.3.2 Neuroimaging and clinical neurological findings

    Brain imaging studies are now beginning to confirm the role of the prefrontal

    cortex in modulating and controlling violence in humans. Reviews of brain imaging

    studies of violent and psychopathic populations completed by Raine (1993), Mills and

    Raine (1994), Raine and Buchsbaum (1996), Henry and Moffitt (1997), and Bufkin

    and Luttrell (2005), while showing some variability across studies, concur in

    indicating that violent offenders have functional deficits in the anterior regions of the

    brain, particularly the frontal region.

  • 33

    Most recently, in their review of 17 neuroimaging studies, Bufkin and Luttrell

    (2005) found that the areas associated with aggressive and/or violent behaviour,

    particularly impulsive acts, are located in the prefrontal cortex and the medial

    temporal regions. Of the 17 studies reviewed, 14 specifically examined possible links

    between frontal lobe pathology and aggressive and/or violent behaviour. In the 10

    single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) and PET studies, 100%

    reported deficits in either prefrontal (8 of 10 studies) or frontal (2 of 10 studies)

    functioning in aggressive, violent and/or antisocial groups compared to non-

    aggressive patients or healthy controls. Analyses of specific regions in the prefrontal

    cortex revealed that individuals who were aggressive and/or violent had significantly

    lower prefrontal activity in the orbitofrontal cortex (4 of 10 studies), anterior medial

    cortex (2 of 10 studies) and/or superior frontal cortex (1 of 10 studies). In the four

    MRI studies, half reported decreased grey matter volume in prefrontal or frontal

    regions, and 25% reported non-specific white matter abnormalities, not localised to

    the frontal cortex.

    Initial studies demonstrate anterior brain dysfunction in individuals with a

    history of violence. Goyer et al. (1994), using PET in an auditory activation condition,

    showed that an increased number of aggressive impulsive acts were associated with

    reduced glucose in the anterior medial and left anterior orbitofrontal frontal cortex of

    17 personality disordered patients. Volkow and colleagues in two PET studies which

    compared forensic psychiatric patients with normal controls, documented decreased

    frontal cortical blood flow or metabolism associated with „repetitive‟ and

    „purposeless‟ violent behaviour (Volkow & Tancredi, 1987; Volkow et al., 1995).

    Søderstrom et al. (2000) using SPECT, found reduced blood flow in both frontal and

    temporal lobes of 21 individuals convicted of impulsive violent crimes.

  • 34

    In a PET study evaluating responses to the probe metachlorophenylpiperazine,

    decrements in the lateral, medial and orbital frontal cortices were found at baseline in

    men with a history of physical aggression and in the orbital frontal cortex for both

    men and women with a history of physical aggression (New et al., 2009).

    Furthermore, a series of studies demonstrated that while normal subjects show

    increased relative glucose metabolism in orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate

    gyrus following acute serotonergic stimulation, impulsive-aggressive personality

    disordered patients show decreased relative metabolism in this area (New et al., 2002;

    Siever et al., 1999; Soloff et al., 2003). These studies suggest that orbitofrontal and

    adjacent regions may exert an inhibitory influence on aggression, perhaps through a

    serotonergic mechanism.

    Interictal episodes of impulsive aggression have also been observed in patients

    with temporal lobe epilepsy. Such patients who display episodes of impulsive

    aggression have a highly significant reduction (approximately 17%) in left prefrontal

    gray matter compared with temporal lobe epilepsy patients with no history of

    aggression or controls (Woermann et al., 2000).

    One particular set of studies was undertaken by Raine and colleagues on a

    heterogeneous group of suspected murderers pleading not guilty by reason of insanity.

    In a first study Raine et al. (1994) conducted a PET study on 22 subjects accused of

    murder and 22 matched controls. The offender group had significantly lower glucose

    metabolism in both medial and lateral prefrontal cortex relative to the controls. Raine,

    Buchsbaum and LaCasse, (1997) in a study of 41 murderers found hypoactivation in

    prefrontal territories including lateral and medial regions of the prefrontal cortex, as

    well as activation in the right amygdala, compared with age- and sex-matched

    controls. In a subsequent reanalysis of these data, murderers were classified as those

  • 35

    who commit planned, predatory murder or those who committed affective, impulsive

    murder. The impulsive murderers showed reductions in lateral prefrontal metabolism

    compared with controls, whereas the predatory group did not (Raine et al., 1998).

    Findings from a more recent structural MRI study indicated that individuals with a

    diagnosis of APD recruited from the community showed an 11% reduction in the

    volume of gray matter in the prefrontal cortex, compared to both normal controls and

    a substance dependence control group (Raine et al., 2000).

    A number of studies have also found abnormal frontal EEG activity, as well as

    diminished frontal event related potentials (ERP), correlating with antisocial

    personality disorder or histories of aggression (Bauer, O‟Connor & Hesselbrock,

    1994; Bernat, Hall, Steffen, & Patrick, 2007; Finn, Ramsey & Earleywine, 2000;

    O‟Connor, Bauer, Tasman & Hesselbrock, 1994).

    3.4 The specific roles of the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in

    aggression

    The cumulative evidence from the studies reviewed above indicates that

    clinically significant frontal lobe dysfunction is associated with aggressive behaviour.

    Subjects with both traumatic brain injury and neurodegenerative disorders primarily

    involving the prefrontal cortex display increased rates of aggressive and antisocial

    behaviour compared to subjects who have no, or non-frontal brain injury. Studies

    employing neuropsychological testing, neurological examination, EEG, and

    neuroimaging have also found evidence for increased rates of prefrontal network

    dysfunction among aggressive and antisocial subjects (see Brower & Price, 2001;

    Bufkin & Luttrell, 2005 for reviews).

  • 36

    Unfortunately, however, the above studies have placed little emphasis on

    considering the separable regions of frontal cortex. Data based on acquired damage to

    the prefrontal cortex