the role of parental bonding in depression

9
ORIGINAL ARTICLE Unique and Shared Aspects of Affective Symptomatology: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression and Anxiety Symptom Profiles Tiffany M. Meites Rick E. Ingram Greg J. Siegle Published online: 22 December 2011 Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011 Abstract Prior research has found an association between parental bonding and depression and anxiety. Specifically, low levels of care and high levels of over- protection have been associated with increased risk for developing depression and anxiety. However little research has explored the relationship between factors of parental bonding and specific aspects of depression and anxiety. The present study investigated these relationships in a sample of undergraduate students (n = 680) who reported a range of affective symptomatology. Lower levels of maternal care were associated with negative beliefs about the self, negative interactions with others and fatigue; lower levels of maternal and paternal care were associated with generalized fear. Maternal overprotection was asso- ciated with physical symptoms of anxiety and a fear of dying, whereas paternal overprotection was a significant predictor of negative beliefs about the self and difficulty maintaining steadiness when anxious. These findings highlight the importance of understanding the role of par- enting in the development of vulnerability to affective symptomatology. Keywords Depression Á Anxiety Á Bonding Á Parenting Á Attachment Á Cognitive vulnerability Introduction An extensive body of theory and research has suggested that early life stressors may play a critical role in creating cognitive vulnerability to anxiety and depression (Petchel and Pizzagalli 2011). For example, Beck’s original cogni- tive model of depression highlighted the importance of developmentally based cognitive factors in depression (1967). The theory stated that, ‘‘In childhood and adoles- cence, the depression-prone individual becomes sensitized to certain types of life situationswhen a person is sub- jected to situations reminiscent of the original traumatic experiences, he may then become depressed (p. 278; Beck 1967). A wealth of empirical data have supported this idea for depression (see Ingram et al. 2011 for review). Like- wise, theory and data suggest that early negative life events contribute to the development of vulnerability to anxiety disorders in adulthood (Malcarne et al. 2010). It appears clear that childhood adversity plays a critical role in the development of depression and anxiety. Parental bonding processes are associated with child- hood adversity and are broadly linked to ideas about attachment processes. Although a number of parenting behaviors can affect bonding, the core features of parental bonding are frequently conceptualized as the multidimen- sional constructs of care (warmth and nurturance) and protection (an appropriate level of concern for safety and security). Adequate bonding with key caregivers along the lines of care and protection is assumed to provide the necessary psychological foundation for healthy functioning in adulthood. Conversely, disrupted parental bonding, often defined as lower levels of care (rejection and neglect) and overprotection (controlling and intrusive behaviors), has been linked with the development of psychopathology (Enns et al. 2002). In particular, deficits in parental T. M. Meites Á R. E. Ingram (&) Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence, KS 66045, USA e-mail: [email protected] T. M. Meites e-mail: [email protected] G. J. Siegle University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, PA, USA e-mail: [email protected] 123 Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181 DOI 10.1007/s10608-011-9426-3

Upload: vladimir-jv

Post on 13-Apr-2015

31 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

DESCRIPTION

The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Unique and Shared Aspects of Affective Symptomatology:The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression and AnxietySymptom Profiles

Tiffany M. Meites • Rick E. Ingram •

Greg J. Siegle

Published online: 22 December 2011

� Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2011

Abstract Prior research has found an association

between parental bonding and depression and anxiety.

Specifically, low levels of care and high levels of over-

protection have been associated with increased risk for

developing depression and anxiety. However little research

has explored the relationship between factors of parental

bonding and specific aspects of depression and anxiety.

The present study investigated these relationships in a

sample of undergraduate students (n = 680) who reported

a range of affective symptomatology. Lower levels of

maternal care were associated with negative beliefs about

the self, negative interactions with others and fatigue;

lower levels of maternal and paternal care were associated

with generalized fear. Maternal overprotection was asso-

ciated with physical symptoms of anxiety and a fear of

dying, whereas paternal overprotection was a significant

predictor of negative beliefs about the self and difficulty

maintaining steadiness when anxious. These findings

highlight the importance of understanding the role of par-

enting in the development of vulnerability to affective

symptomatology.

Keywords Depression � Anxiety � Bonding � Parenting �Attachment � Cognitive vulnerability

Introduction

An extensive body of theory and research has suggested

that early life stressors may play a critical role in creating

cognitive vulnerability to anxiety and depression (Petchel

and Pizzagalli 2011). For example, Beck’s original cogni-

tive model of depression highlighted the importance of

developmentally based cognitive factors in depression

(1967). The theory stated that, ‘‘In childhood and adoles-

cence, the depression-prone individual becomes sensitized

to certain types of life situations…when a person is sub-

jected to situations reminiscent of the original traumatic

experiences, he may then become depressed (p. 278; Beck

1967). A wealth of empirical data have supported this idea

for depression (see Ingram et al. 2011 for review). Like-

wise, theory and data suggest that early negative life events

contribute to the development of vulnerability to anxiety

disorders in adulthood (Malcarne et al. 2010). It appears

clear that childhood adversity plays a critical role in the

development of depression and anxiety.

Parental bonding processes are associated with child-

hood adversity and are broadly linked to ideas about

attachment processes. Although a number of parenting

behaviors can affect bonding, the core features of parental

bonding are frequently conceptualized as the multidimen-

sional constructs of care (warmth and nurturance) and

protection (an appropriate level of concern for safety and

security). Adequate bonding with key caregivers along the

lines of care and protection is assumed to provide the

necessary psychological foundation for healthy functioning

in adulthood. Conversely, disrupted parental bonding, often

defined as lower levels of care (rejection and neglect) and

overprotection (controlling and intrusive behaviors), has

been linked with the development of psychopathology

(Enns et al. 2002). In particular, deficits in parental

T. M. Meites � R. E. Ingram (&)

Department of Psychology, University of Kansas, Lawrence,

KS 66045, USA

e-mail: [email protected]

T. M. Meites

e-mail: [email protected]

G. J. Siegle

University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh,

PA, USA

e-mail: [email protected]

123

Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181

DOI 10.1007/s10608-011-9426-3

Page 2: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

bonding have been found to predict both depression and

anxiety (e.g., Blatt and Homann 1992; Grotmol et al. 2010;

Lima et al. 2010).

Although depression and anxiety are conceptualized as

distinct disorders, there is considerable overlap between the

two. In the categorical framework of the Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-Text Revision,

depression and anxiety can co-exist and thus be comorbid

(DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association 2000).

Some estimates suggest that as many as half of those

diagnosed with major depressive disorder also suffer from

an anxiety disorder (e.g., Hirschfeld 2001; Kessler et al.

2003). Such comorbidity complicates efforts to understand

how variables such as deficits in parental bonding may

contribute to the development of these affective states,

particularly since anxiety has emerged as a significant

predictor of the development of depression (Olino et al.

2010; Wittchen et al. 2003) and because high levels of

one syndrome are correlated with high levels of the other

(Ingram 1990).

Although depression and anxiety are viewed as separate

affective syndromes with unique factors, they are none-

theless characterized by factors that are common to each

disorder (e.g., Ahrens and Haaga 1993; Cropley and

MacLeod 2003; MacLeod et al. 1997; Reardon and

Williams 2007). For example, evidence derived from the

tripartite model of depression and anxiety suggests that

they differ on features such as anhedonia, physiological

reactivity (e.g., Watson et al. 1995a, b), and level of

positive affect (e.g., Williams et al. 2004), but that both

conditions involve a high level of negative affect. Addi-

tionally, even though depression and anxiety are nosolog-

ically distinct syndromes within the DSM-IV-TR, their

diagnostic symptoms overlap; specifically fatigue, con-

centration problems, and sleep difficulties are criteria for

both categories.

The overlap of depression and anxiety has implications

for understanding the role of parental bonding in the

development of these syndromes. Despite empirical evi-

dence linking parental bonding variables with depressive

and anxious states, the exact nature of this relationship

remains unclear. Although data have shown that parental

bonding is related to the global states of depression and

anxiety, it is unclear if they have varying relationships with

the different patterns of symptoms. Similarly it is unknown

whether the core features of parental bonding (i.e., care and

overprotection) are associated with distinct affective fac-

tors. A number of studies have suggested that a lack of care

and heightened overprotection are associated with psy-

chopathology in general (Blatt and Homann 1992;

Gladstone and Parker 2005; Ingram et al. 1998; Lima et al.

2010; Rapee 1997), but with few exceptions, it has yet to

be determined whether these dimensions are uniquely

associated with the features of depressed or anxious states.

Moreover, lack of care and heightened overprotection can

characterize either or both maternal or paternal behaviors;

maternal and paternal bonding may act independently and

may be associated with a unique set of affective factors. To

date, however, there is little evidence as to whether these

specific parent behaviors are associated with the unique

elements of depressive and anxious states. Exploration of

these relationships may yield evidence with significant

implications for understanding the development of the

distinct features of affective states.

One consideration when exploring these relationships is

identifying the appropriate samples to study. Diagnosti-

cally assessed samples of individuals with affective disor-

ders are obviously important in this regard, but the high

degree of comorbidity between patients with substantial

degrees of symptomatology also makes disentangling var-

ious features both difficult and possibly misleading (Ingram

1990). An alternate strategy is to test these questions in

nonclinical samples with a range of affective symptom-

atology, which may permit the distinct features of anxiety

and depression to be more clearly distinguishable. More-

over, as long as the commonly recognized affective fea-

tures of depression and anxiety exist in these samples, this

strategy may yield insights into how parental bonding is

related to the important features of depressive and anxious

disorders.

Given these considerations, the present study sought to

examine the association between dimensions of parental

bonding and the distinct features of depressive and anxious

symptoms. Because relationships of depressive and anx-

ious symptoms cannot be evaluated in participants who do

not report any symptoms, analyses were confined to those

reported at least some symptomatology on both measures.

We then examined the association between these factors

and scores on measures of maternal and paternal care and

overprotection. In order to examine this issue, a sample of

undergraduates was administered the Beck Anxiety

Inventory (BAI; Beck and Steer 1990), the Beck Depres-

sion Inventory (BDI; Beck and Steer 1993) and the

Parental Bonding Instrument (Parker et al. 1979).

To derive depressive and anxious symptoms profiles,

factor analyses were conducted on the BDI and on the BAI.

Prior research has suggested that the BDI and the BAI are

comprised of individual sub-factors (e.g., Osman et al.

1997; Shafer 2006) but it is important to note that dis-

crepancies have arisen as to the number of factors each

measure contains. Helm and Boward (2003) reported a

two-factor structure for the BDI whereas other studies have

reported only a single factor (e.g., Welch et al. 1990), a

four factor structure (Bennett et al. 1997), or a three factor

structure (Byrne and Baron 1993). Similar findings have

been reported for the BAI, with some evidence for a four

174 Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181

123

Page 3: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

factor model with two higher order factors (Osman et al.

1997) and other evidence suggesting a two-factor model

(Chapman et al. 2009). Because no definitive factor

structure has been identified for either the BDI or BAI, and

because this study sought to maximize the descriptiveness

of symptoms when exploring relations between parenting

and symptomatology, exploratory factor analyses were

conducted.

Method

Participants and Procedure

Participants received credit in partial completion of a

course requirement and completed the measures in mass

testing procedure. Nine hundred sixty-two participants

initially completed the study, but because the present study

sought to explore the relation between anxiety and

depressive symptoms and parental bonding, participants

who did not endorse any symptoms on the BDI and the

BAI were excluded from analyses. The final sample con-

sisted of 680 participants, with women comprising 73.9%

of the final sample. The mean BDI score of the sample was

14.51, with individual scores ranging from 1 to 59. The

mean BAI score of the sample was 8.43, with individual

scores ranging from 1 to 58. Descriptive data for the

sample are presented in Table 1.

Measures

Parental Bonding

The Parental Bonding Instrument (PBI; Parker et al. 1979)

is a self-report questionnaire that measures parenting atti-

tudes and behaviors (i.e., caring and overprotection) from

the first 16 years of life. Participants rate each parent on

two four-point Likert subscales: a 12-item caring subscale

and a 13-item overprotection subscale. Higher scores on

the care scales represent more positive parental behavior,

whereas higher scores on the overprotection scales indicate

less positive parental behavior (e.g., more parental intru-

siveness). Although the PBI is a retrospective measure,

several studies have established that the PBI has adequate

reliability and validity (Brewin et al. 1993; Parker et al.

1979; Parker 1989), as well as temporal stability (Wilhelm

et al. 2005). In the present study, all four subscales had

adequate internal consistency, such that the Maternal and

Paternal Care subscales had Chronbach’s a = 0.91 and

a = 0.93 respectively, while the Maternal and Paternal

Protection subscales had Chronbach’s a = 0.87 and

a = 0.86 respectively.

Depressive Symptomatology

The BDI (Beck and Steer 1993) was used to assess current

depressive symptomatology. The BDI is a widely used

21-item self-report inventory that measures a range of

depressive symptoms. Each item is rated on a 0–3 scale

with total scores ranging from 0 to 63; higher scores are

assumed to reflect increased levels of depressive symp-

tomatology. Research has demonstrated that the BDI has

acceptable reliability and validity (e.g., Beck et al. 1988).

In the present study, the BDI had adequate internal con-

sistency, Chronbach’s a = 0.87.

Anxious Symptomatology

The BAI (Beck and Steer 1990) is a 21-item self-report

questionnaire that parallels the BDI in structure and scoring

(e.g., a 0–3 scale). It assesses the presence and severity of a

variety of symptoms associated with anxiety, with higher

scores indicating higher levels of anxiety. The BAI has

adequate reliability and validity (Steer and Beck 1997).

The BDI had adequate internal consistency, Chronbach’s

a = 0.89.

Results

Analyses

Principal components factor analyses with varimax rotation

were conducted on the individual items to identify the

underlying factor structure. Items were considered to load

onto a specific factor if their loading was larger than 0.400.

However, if items cross-loaded on more than one factor,

item content was reviewed and the items were assigned to

the factor most representative of that content. Composite

Table 1 Demographic information for the present sample

Mean (SE) Minimum Maximum

Age 18.44 (0.05) 16.25 33.75

BAI 8.43 (0.27) 1.00 58.00

BDI 14.51 (0.36) 1.00 59.00

PBIMC 27.62 (0.28) 0.00 36.00

PBIMP 14.10 (0.30) 0.00 38.00

PBIFC 22.45 (0.36) 0.00 36.00

PBIFP 12.81 (0.30) 0.00 37.00

BAI beck anxiety inventory, BDI beck depression inventory, PBIMCparental bonding instrument maternal care, PBIMP parental bonding

instrument maternal overprotection, PBIFC parental bonding instru-

ment paternal care, PBIFP parental bonding instrument paternal

overprotection

Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181 175

123

Page 4: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

scores were created for each identified factor by summing

the scores of items on that factor. To assess the relation

between these factors and parental bonding, multiple

regression analyses were used. Parental bonding factors

(maternal and paternal care and overprotection) were

entered simultaneously in multiple regression analyses to

predict the factors identified from the BDI and the BAI. All

analyses were conducted using PASW 18.0 software.

Missing Data

Because less than one percent of data were missing, a

single imputation was conducted to estimate missing data.

Prior research suggests that one imputation is sufficient to

reduce error estimates when there is less than 1% of

missing data (Fichman and Cummings 2003). All further

analyses were conducted on the data set including this

imputation.

Factor Analyses

Factor analyses conducted independently on the BDI and

BAI together and separately suggested the same underlying

factor structure; results presented here represent separate

factor analyses for each measure. Primary factor structures

were not reduced to secondary factor structures in order to

provide greater specificity in analyses. A four factor structure

provided the best fit for the BDI data, explaining 46.38% of

total variance. These factors were identified as: negative

feelings about the self (e.g., ‘‘I feel sad;’’ explaining 18.54%

of the total variance; eigenvalue = 6.25), negative interac-

tions with others (e.g., ‘‘I am less interested in other people

than I used to be;’’ explaining 12.62% of the total variance;

eigenvalue = 1.33), sleep difficulties and fatigue (e.g., ‘‘I

don’t sleep as well as I used to;’’ explaining 7.24% of the total

variance; eigenvalue = 1.14), and weight related concerns

(e.g., ‘‘My appetite is not as good as it used to be;’’ explaining

8.00% of the total variance; eigenvalue = 1.02). Seven

items had loadings greater than 0.300 on two factors; items

were assigned to specific factors based on thematic content.

Factor loadings are presented in Table 2.

For the BAI, a four factor structure was identified as

providing the best fit, explaining 51% of the total variance.

These four factors were conceptually defined as general

fear (e.g., ‘‘Fear of the worst happening;’’ explaining

15.41% of the total variance; eigenvalue = 6.97)., physical

symptoms (e.g., ‘‘Numbness or tingling;’’ explaining

11.63% of the total variance; eigenvalue = 1.29), lack of

steadiness (e.g., ‘‘Dizzy or lightheaded;’’ explaining

14.41% of the total variance; eigenvalue = 1.49), and fear

of dying (e.g., ‘‘Fear of dying;’’ explaining 9.85% of the

total variance; eigenvalue = 1.03). Information on these

factors is presented in Table 3.

Parental Bonding and Depressive Symptoms

Significant associations for parental bonding and depres-

sive and anxious symptom factors are reported in Table 4.

Approximately 10.3% of total variance for participants’

negative feelings about the self factor was explained by

parental bonding. Maternal care and paternal overprotec-

tion were significant predictors, such that lower levels of

maternal care, b = -0.253, t(675) = -6.005, P \ 0.01,

and higher levels of paternal overprotection, b = 0.089,

t(675) = 2.03, P = 0.042, were associated with higher

levels of self-related negativity.

Similarly, 7.2% of the total variability in the negative

beliefs about interactions with others factor was explained by

Table 2 Component matrix for the BDI

Item Self Neg.

interactions

Weight

concerns

Fatigue

1 0.389

2 0.559

3 0.735

4 0.593

5 0.684

6 0.565

7 0.692

8 0.527

9 0.504

10 0.394

11 0.333

12 0.651

13 0.546

14 0.589

15 0.534

16 0.769

17 0.447

18 0.655

19 0.707

20 0.357

21 0.453

Loadings are reported from the rotated component matrix. Self (‘‘I do

not feel sad;’’ ‘‘I am not particularly discouraged about the future;’’ ‘‘I

do not feel like a failure;’’ ‘‘I don’t feel particularly guilty;’’ ‘‘I don’t

feel I am being punished;’’ ‘‘I don’t feel disappointed in myself;’’ ‘‘I

don’t feel any worse than anybody else;’’ ‘‘I don’t have any thoughts

of killing myself;’’ ‘‘I make decisions about as well as I ever could;’’

‘‘I can work about as well as before.’’). Negative interactions (‘‘I get

as much satisfaction out of things as I used to;’’ ‘‘I am no more

irritated now than I ever am;’’ ‘‘I have not lost interest in other

people;’’ ‘‘I don’t feel I look any worse than I used to;’’ ‘‘I am more

worried about my health than usual;’’ ‘‘I have not noticed any recent

changes in my interest in sex’’). Weight concerns (‘‘My appetite is no

worse than usual;’’ ‘‘I haven’t lost much weight, if any, lately’’).

Fatigue (‘‘I don’t cry any more than usual;’’ ‘‘I can sleep as well as

usual;’’ ‘‘I don’t get more tired than usual’’)

176 Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181

123

Page 5: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

parental bonding. Maternal and paternal care were signifi-

cant predictors, with lower levels of maternal, b = -0.194,

t(675) = -4.52, P \ 0.01, and paternal, b = -0.105,

t(675) = -2.544, P = 0.011, care associated with higher

levels of negative beliefs about interactions with others.

For fatigue, 2.5% of the total variability was explained

by parental bonding, indicating that lower levels of

maternal care were significantly associated with higher

levels of fatigue, b = -0.088, t(675) = -2.01,

P = 0.045. Approximately 1.8% of total variability in

weight-related symptoms was explained by parental

bonding; however none of the components of parental

bonding were significant predictors.

Parental Bonding and Anxiety Symptoms

Results of the multiple regression analysis using general

fear as the dependent variable suggested that approxi-

mately 2.4% of variability in fear was explained by

parental bonding. Of the four components of maternal

bonding, only maternal care was a significant predictor of

fear, b = -0.098, t(675) = -2.218, P = 0.027, such that

as maternal care decreased, participant fear increased. For

participants’ reported physical symptoms of fear, parental

bonding explained approximately 1.0% of the total vari-

ability; however none of the parental bonding factors were

a significant predictor of participants’ reported physical

symptoms of fear.

For lack of steadiness, 3.4% of the total variability was

accounted for by parental bonding, with parental bonding

factors nonsignificant predictors of this construct. Finally

parental bonding explained approximately 3.9% of the total

variability for fear of dying. Maternal overprotection was

the only significant predictor, such that higher levels of

maternal overprotection were associated with increased

fears of dying, b = 0.151, t(675) = 3.235, P \ 0.01.

Discussion

Previous research has examined the overall association

between PBI factors and depression and anxiety and found

that PBI factors are correlated with both depression and

anxiety. In the current study, we examined more specific

associations between parenting behaviors and depressive

and anxious symptoms. Regression analyses suggested that

participants’ reported parental bonding was significantly

associated with several symptom patterns identified from

the BDI and BAI. In particular, lower levels of maternal

care were associated with negative self-beliefs, negative

interactions with others, fatigue, and generalized fear;

lower levels of paternal care were also related to negative

interactions with others. Maternal overprotection was

associated with a fear of dying, whereas paternal over-

protection was associated with negative beliefs. Weight-

related issues in depression and autonomic nervous system

activity and lack of steadiness in anxiety were not signifi-

cantly associated with any dimension of parental bonding.

Table 3 Component matrix for the BAI

Item General

fear

Lack of

steadiness

Physical

fear

Fear of

dying

1 0.554

2 0.560

3 0.606

4 0.647

5 0.728

6 0.518

7 0.520

8 0.549

9 0.626

10 0.589

11 0.615

12 0.656

13 0.697

14 0.528

15 0.487

16 0.723

17 0.697

18 0.411

19 0.522

20 0.726

21 0.759

All loadings are reported from the rotated component matrix. General

fear (‘‘Unable to relax;’’ ‘‘Fear of the worst happening;’’ ‘‘Terrified;’’

‘‘Nervous;’’ ‘‘Fear of losing control;’’ ‘‘Scared.’’). Steadiness

(‘‘Numbness or tingling;’’ ‘‘Wobbliness in legs;’’ ‘‘Dizzy or light-

headed;’’ ‘‘Unsteady;’’ ‘‘Hands trembling;’’ ‘‘Shaky’’). Physical fear

(‘‘Feeling hot;’’ ‘‘Heart pounding or racing;’’ ‘‘Indigestion/discomfort

in abdomen;’’ ‘‘Face flushed;’’ ‘‘Sweating (not due to heat)’’). Fear of

Dying (‘‘Feeling of choking;’’ ‘‘Difficulty breathing;’’ ‘‘Fear of

dying;’’ ‘‘Faint’’)

Table 4 Standardized significant beta coefficients for parental

bonding and identified factors of anxiety and depressive symptoms

BDI

negative

self

Negative

interactions

Fatigue BAI

general

fear

Dying

fear

Maternal care -0.25 -0.19 -0.09 -0.10

Maternal

overprotection

0.15

Paternal care -0.11

Paternal

overprotection

0.09

Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181 177

123

Page 6: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

Clearly parenting behaviors do not account for all the

variance in these affective factors; the present pattern of

findings brings into sharper relief the broader links that

have been found between parenting and depression and

anxiety. When the overall relationship is decomposed into

separate factors, unique relationships were obtained

between maternal and paternal parenting behaviors and

aspects of depressive and anxious symptoms. Thus

depressive and anxious syndromes are not simply related to

parenting but also to particular parenting patterns, at least

for some aspects of these syndromes. As such, these data

provide clues about how specific types of parenting may be

related to particular aspects of these affective states, with

implications for understanding some of the causal factors

associated with the development of depression and anxiety.

The pattern of findings may also have implications for

understanding some of the causal factors associated with

the development of depression and anxiety. For example,

results indicated that deficits in maternal care were asso-

ciated with more self-related negative cognitions and more

negative cognitions about interactions with others. These

results are broadly consistent with prior research showing a

unique association between maternal care and cognitive

vulnerability to depressive symptoms, including finding an

attention bias for negative stimuli in individuals with lower

reported levels of maternal care (Ingram and Ritter 2000).

The current results are also consistent with findings of

elevated levels of automatic negative thoughts in individ-

uals with lower levels of maternal bonding (Ingram et al.

2001). Parental bonding deficits may thus influence the

way children (and later on adults) process information from

the world (McGinn et al. 2005; Ingram et al. 2004); an

individual with perceived low levels of maternal care may

begin to assume that she is unlovable and unworthy of

others’ care and consideration. With repeated exposure to

similar patterns of parental caregiving, these thoughts may

be consolidated into schemas, much in the same way as

posited by Beck’s (1967) cognitive theory of depression.

These negative schemas may then be translated into cog-

nitive vulnerabilities; parental bonding may function as a

mediating factor in the development of anxiety and

depression through the emergence of these schemas.

Alternatively, parental bonding may mediate the rela-

tionship between stressful life events and the development

of depression or anxiety through coping styles. For exam-

ple, positive levels of maternal bonding have been asso-

ciated with increased use of problem-solving coping

strategies and decreased use of emotion-focused coping

strategies (Matheson et al. 2005). Inasmuch as emotion-

focused coping strategies have been associated with

increased risk of developing depression or anxiety (e.g.,

Matheson et al.), it seems likely that poor maternal bonding

may result in the development of less adaptive coping

strategies and increased risk for poor psychological health.

Together these findings suggest that maternal bonding may

be particularly important in the development of both cog-

nitive vulnerability and coping strategies.

Deficits in maternal care were also associated with sleep

difficulties/fatigue and generalized fear. These specific

factors are not typically addressed in theories pertaining to

parenting and affective distress, and thus the processes

underlying the relationship been maternal care deficits and

these problems are unclear. If deficits in maternal care

result in the development of negative cognitive schemas,

however, poor maternal care may indirectly affect sleep.

That is, sleep difficulties can be driven by cognitive pro-

cesses, and to the extent that a process like rumination

takes place, difficulty sleeping may occur and consequently

cause fatigue. Failure to use problem-solving coping

strategies may also perpetuate rumination and continued

sleep and energy problems. Likewise, when considering

anxiety and maternal care deficits, individuals whose

mothers are perceived as rejecting and neglectful may be

more prone to experience heightened fearfulness when

anxiety is provoked, lacking maternal comfort and direc-

tion in developing strategies for managing anxiety.

Whereas a comforting mother might reduce fears, an

individual whose mother is critical and lacking in warmth

may develop a schema of the world as a fearful place with

regular and uncontrollable negative events; this schema

may become prominent when stressful events occur,

resulting in heightened and persistent anxiety.

Although deficits in maternal care were associated with

the greatest number of affective factors, maternal over-

protection was associated with a heightened fear of dying.

Some of the items that children with overprotective

mothers were more likely to endorse included feeling of

choking and difficulty breathing. Parents who are over-

protective of their children seem likely to manage problems

themselves and not permit children to explore solutions to

concerns, providing an invalidating message of incompe-

tence. By sending the message that children are incapable

of handling problems themselves and excessively limiting

the development of control over their lives, an overpro-

tective mother may send create a fertile climate for

developing schemas of incompetence and low self-efficacy,

both of which may increase and perpetuate anxiety.

Even though maternal behaviors may be particularly

virulent in certain facets of depression and anxiety, pater-

nal behaviors may also play an important role, albeit a

different one. Specifically, higher levels of paternal over-

protection were associated with a more negative view of

the self. An overprotective/intrusive father may be likely to

convey the message that the world is a dangerous place and

that the child is unable to manage these concerns inde-

pendently. Likewise, an intrusive father may play a role in

178 Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181

123

Page 7: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

creating negative self-views; to the extent that fathers’

intrusiveness inhibits the child’s ability to develop an

independent view of the self, the child is likely to develop a

schema of disappointment, failure and guilt, all aspects of

the ‘‘negative view of self’’ factor. Interestingly, the neg-

ative view of self was associated with paternal overpro-

tection but not with deficits in paternal care. Hence,

although mothers and fathers both play a role in creating a

negative sense of the self, the specific pathways may differ:

criticalness for mothers and intrusiveness for fathers.

Similarly, the association between care and negative

interactions with others involves deficits in both maternal

and paternal care. Parental care likely plays the same role

in the development of negative interactions with others that

maternal care does.

Inasmuch as the data show that mothers and fathers play

related but unique roles in certain dimensions of depression

and anxiety, it seems likely that the combination of dys-

functional parenting may be particularly troublesome for

children’s cognitive and emotional development. Further,

although the effects of parenting can be examined sepa-

rately, they likely interact to produce the vulnerability. For

example, is a father’s overprotection interpreted as criti-

cism when the other parent is in fact critical or neglectful?

If so, a child with a critical mother might be primed to

develop negative views of the self that are potentiated by

an intrusive father. Moreover, to the extent that both a lack

of maternal care and controlling paternal behavior broad-

ens and strengthens negative self-schemas, individuals

raised with these parenting styles may be at particular risk

for developing affective symptoms in the face of stress. Of

course, a father and mother who are both critical and

lacking in warmth would create a poor care-giving envi-

ronment; it is perhaps not surprising that these factors were

related to negative interactions with others in this study.

Caregivers who are critical and rejecting have been long

thought to shape the development of a generalized schema

that others who will also be critical and rejecting (e.g.,

Bowlby 1980). This may be particularly the case when both

caregivers exhibit these behaviors; interactions with others

are viewed through the lens of the child’s interactions with

caregivers.

Although deficits in care and excesses in protection for

both mothers and fathers appear to create considerable

difficulties for the individual, a greater number of, and

stronger, relationships with the depression and anxiety

factors were observed for maternal parenting factors. Such

findings are broadly in line with data previously reported

data showing that maternal care deficits are specifically

linked to negative self-schemas (e.g., Ingram et al. 2001;

Ingram and Ritter 2000). As such, maternal care may play a

particularly significant role in the development of schemas,

likely due in part to unique factors associated with mothers.

It seems likely that mothers in most families play the pri-

mary role in care giving. Accordingly, levels of maternal

bonding may reflect the relative time spent with a primary

caregiver. With more exposure to maternal care, partici-

pants’ schemas may be more reflective of their interactions

with their mothers. Alternatively, there may be additional

social expectations of mothers that are less prevalent for

fathers (e.g., taking on a nurturing, caring role); violations

of these expectations may be more unexpected for the child

and result in broad reappraisal of the situation. Disrupted

maternal bonding may thus result in more pervasive cog-

nitive biases and vulnerability to symptoms than disrupted

paternal bonding.

The present study suggested that parental bonding may

play a particularly important role in some symptoms of

anxiety and depression. However several limitations should

be acknowledged. The factor analytic strategy used may

have overfactored the BAI and BDI, resulting in additional

factors that are not well explained by parental bonding. To

some extent, this may have been unavoidable, because

these factors are conceptually related and are highly cor-

related with each other (see Table 5). Additionally, the

present study sought to maximize descriptiveness of factors

in order to identify relations between subtle differences in

symptomatology and parental bonding. Although visual

Table 5 Correlations between

BDI and BAI factors

** P \ 0.01

Self Negative

interactions

Weight

concerns

Fatigue General

fear

Physical

fear

Fear of

dying

Negative

interactions

0.62**

Weight concerns 0.35** 0.27**

Fatigue 0.52** 0.46** 0.30**

General fear 0.42** 0.33** 0.13** 0.37**

Physical fear 0.19** 0.18** 0.11** 0.22** 0.56**

Fear of dying 0.26** 0.21** 0.24** 0.26** 0.52** 0.50**

Lack of

steadiness

0.31** 0.24** 0.27** 0.31** 0.59** 0.57** 0.61**

Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181 179

123

Page 8: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

analysis of the scree plot might suggest a two-factor

solution for each inventory, the factors selected each

explain a minimum of 7% of the total variance. Each factor

had face validity, such that items loading on each factor

were conceptually related. Given theoretical basis for

maintaining those factors and the relatedness of items on

each factor, as well as the desire for greater specificity of

these analyses, these factors were not evaluated for a sec-

ond order structure. The finding of a four factor structure

for these inventories is consistent with prior research on the

BDI (e.g., Bennett et al. 1997; Shafer 2006) and BAI (e.g.,

Osman et al. 1997). However, given that prior research has

failed to find consistent factor structures for these inven-

tories, replication of the present factor structure and find-

ings is recommended.

Additionally, the PBI is a retrospective self-report

inventory and even though data support its reliability and

validity, causal conclusions between maternal and paternal

factors and patterns of affective symptomatology cannot be

drawn. For instance, rather than deficits in maternal care

creating a negative self-schema, it may be that those indi-

viduals who were inclined to endorse a more negative self

view attributed these deficits in maternal care and are

attributing blame to their mother retrospectively. Although

such a possibility cannot be discounted, two factors argue

against this interpretation. First, specific deficits in maternal

care tend to be consistently linked to negative self-views

across studies, including one which assessed maternal

bonding several weeks before assessment of other factors

(Ingram and Ritter 2000). Second, if blaming after the fact

was occurring, a broader pattern of negative bias might be

seen across all factors of affective symptomatology. Instead,

a unique pattern of relationships between parenting factors

and facets of depression and anxiety was observed and the

only overlap was the association between maternal and

paternal care deficit and negative interactions with others.

In sum, the present study provides evidence linking

parental bonding with anxious and depressive symptom-

atology. Aspects of parental bonding were stronger pre-

dictors of cognitive symptomatology than physiological

symptomatology. Maternal care predicted the most factors

and was significantly associated with four of the eight

factors of affective symptomatology, suggesting a unique

role for maternal care in these symptoms. Future studies

should consider which aspects of symptomatology are of

interest when exploring parental bonding and vulnerability

to anxiety and depression. Additionally, this study suggests

that examining common factors between anxious and

depressive symptomatology is a viable method for under-

standing the development of vulnerability to these disor-

ders. Such an approach, particularly when conducted with a

nonclinical population with a range of symptomatology,

may yield additional information on the relation between

the disorders. Commonalities between individuals with

these disorders and with subclinical features may also be

elucidated using this method. Understanding this relation-

ship may provide greater insight into the importance of

early life events in the development of cognitive vulnera-

bility to anxiety and depression.

References

Ahrens, A. H., & Haaga, D. A. (1993). The specificity of attributional

style and expectations to positive and negative affectivity, depres-

sion, and anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 17, 83–98.

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statisticalmanual of mental disorders-IV-text revision. Washington, D.C.:

American Psychiatric Association.

Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Causes and treatment. Philadelphia:

University of Philadelphia Press.

Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1990). Manual for the beck anxietyinventory. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Beck, A. T., & Steer, R. A. (1993). Beck Depression Inventorymanual. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Garbin, M. G. (1988). Psychometric

properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years

of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8, 77–100.

Bennett, D. S., Ambrosini, P. J., Bianchi, M., Barnett, D., Metz, C., &

Rabinovich, H. (1997). Relationship of Beck Depression Inven-

tory factors to depression among adolescents. Journal ofAffective Disorders, 45, 127–134.

Blatt, S. J., & Homann, E. (1992). Parent-child interaction in the

etiology of dependent and self-critical depression. ClinicalPsychology Review, 12, 47–91.

Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss. New York: Basic Books.

Brewin, C. R., Andrews, B., & Gotlib, I. H. (1993). Psychopathology

and early experience: A reappraisal of retrospective reports.

Psychological Bulletin, 113, 82–98.

Byrne, B. M., & Baron, P. (1993). The Beck Depression Inventory:

Testing and cross-validating a hierarchical factor structure for

nonclinical adolescents. Measurement and Evaluation in Coun-seling and Development, 26, 164–178.

Chapman, K., Williams, S. R., Mast, B. T., & Woodruff-Borden, J.

(2009). A confirmatory factor analysis of the Beck Anxiety

Inventory in African American and European American young

adults. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23, 387–392.

Cropley, M., & MacLeod, A. K. (2003). Dysphoria, attributional

reasoning and future event probability. Clinical Psychology andPsychotherapy, 10, 220–227.

Enns, M. W., Cox, B. J., & Clara, I. (2002). Parental bonding and

adult psychopathology: Results from the US national comorbid-

ity survey. Psychological Medicine: A Journal of Research inPsychiatry and the Allied Sciences, 32, 997–1008.

Fichman, M., & Cummings, J. N. (2003). Multiple imputation for

missing data: Making the most of what you know. Organiza-tional Research Methods, 6, 282–308.

Gladstone, G. L., & Parker, G. B. (2005). The role of parenting in the

development of psychopathology: An overview of research using

the Parental Bonding Instrument. In J. Hudson & R. M. Rapee

(Eds.), Psychopathology and the family (pp. 21–33). New York,

NY: Elsevier Science.

Grotmol, K. S., Ekeberg, O., Finset, A., Gude, T., Moum, T., &

Tyssen, R. (2010). Parental bonding and self-esteem as predic-

tors of severe depressive symptoms. Journal of Nervous andMental Disease, 198, 22–27.

180 Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181

123

Page 9: The Role of Parental Bonding in Depression

Helm, H. W., & Boward, M. D. (2003). Factor structure of the Beck

Depression Inventory in a university sample. PsychologicalReports, 92, 53–61.

Hirschfeld, R. M. A. (2001). The comorbidity of major depression

and anxiety disorders: recognition and management in primary

care. Primary Care Companion, Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,2, 244–254.

Ingram, R. E. (1990). Attentional nonspecificity in depressive and

generalized anxious affective states. Cognitive Therapy andResearch, 14, 25–35.

Ingram, R. E., Atchley, R. A., & Segal, Z. V. (2011). Vulnerability todepression: From cognitive neuroscience to prevention andtreatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Ingram, R. E., Bailey, K., & Siegle, G. J. (2004). Emotional

information processing and disrupted parental bonding: Cogni-

tive specificity and avoidance. Journal of Cognitive Psychother-apy, 18, 53–65.

Ingram, R. E., Miranda, J., & Segal, Z. V. (1998). Cognitivevulnerability to depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Ingram, R. E., Overby, T., & Fortier, M. (2001). Individual

differences in dysfunctional automatic thinking and parental

bonding: Specificity of maternal care. Personality and IndividualDifferences, 30, 401–412.

Ingram, R. E., & Ritter, J. (2000). Vulnerability to depression:

Cognitive reactivity and parental bonding in high-risk individ-

uals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109, 588–596.

Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Koretz, D., & Wang,

P. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive disorder:

Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication

(NCS-R). Journal of the American Medical Association, 289,

3095–3105.

Lima, A. R., Mello, M. F., & de Jesus Mari, J. (2010). The role of

early parental bonding in the development of psychiatric

symptoms in adulthood. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 23,

383–387.

MacLeod, A. K., Tata, P., Kentish, J., & Jacobsen, H. (1997).

Retrospective and prospective cognitions in anxiety and depres-

sion. Cognition and Emotion, 11, 467–479.

Malcarne, V. L., Hansdottir, I., & Merz, E. L. (2010). In R. E. Ingram

& J. Price (Eds.), Vulnerability to psychopathology: Risk acrossthe lifespan (2nd ed., pp. 291–333). New York: Guilford Press.

Matheson, K., Kelly, O., Cole, B., Tannenbaum, B., Dodd, C., &

Anisman, H. (2005). Parental bonding and depressive affect: The

mediating role of coping resources. British Journal of SocialPsychology, 44, 371–395.

McGinn, L. K., Cukor, D., & Sanderson, W. C. (2005). The

relationship between parenting style, cognitive style, and anxiety

and depression: Does increased early adversity influence symp-

tom severity through the mediating role of cognitive style.

Cognitive Therapy and Research, 29, 219–242.

Olino, T. M., Klein, D. N., Lewinsohn, P. M., Rohde, P., & Seeley, J.

R. (2010). Latent trajectory classes of depressive and anxiety

disorders from adolescence to adulthood: Descriptions of classes

and associations with risk factors. Comprehensive Psychiatry,51, 224–235.

Osman, A., Kopper, B. A., Barrios, F. X., Osman, J. R., & Wade, T.

(1997). The Beck Anxiety Inventory: Reexamination of factor

structure and psychometric properties. Journal of ClinicalPsychology, 53, 7–14.

Parker, G. (1989). The parental bonding instrument: Psychometric

properties reviewed. Psychiatric Developments, 4, 317–335.

Parker, G., Tupling, H., & Brown, L. B. (1979). A parental bonding

instrument. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 52, 1–10.

Petchel, P., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2011). Effects of early life stress on

cognitive and affective function: An integrated review of human

literature. Psychopharmacology, 214, 55–70.

Rapee, R. M. (1997). Potential role of childrearing practices in the

development of anxiety and depression. Clinical PsychologyReview, 17, 47–67.

Reardon, J. M., & Williams, N. L. (2007). The specificity of cognitive

vulnerabilities to emotional disorders: anxiety sensitivity, loom-

ing vulnerability and explanatory style. Journal of AnxietyDisorders, 21, 625–643.

Shafer, A. B. (2006). Meta-analysis of the factor structures of four

depression questionnaires: Beck, CES-D, Hamilton, and Zung.

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62, 123–146.

Steer, R. A., & Beck, A. T. (1997). Beck Anxiety Inventory. In C.

P. Zalaquett & R. J. Wood (Eds.), Evaluating stress: A book ofresources (pp. 23–40). Lanham, MD: Scarecrow Education.

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Strauss, M.

E., & McCormick, R. A. (1995a). Testing a tripartite model: II.

Exploring the symptom structure of anxiety and depression in

student, adult, and patient samples. Journal of AbnormalPsychology, 104, 15–25.

Watson, D., Weber, K., Assenheimer, J. S., Clark, L. A., Strauss, M.

E., & McCormick, R. A. (1995b). Testing a tripartite model: I.

Evaluating the convergent and discriminant validity of anxietyand depression symptom scales. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-ogy, 104, 3–14.

Welch, G., Hall, A., & Walkey, F. (1990). The replicable dimensions

of the Beck Depression Inventory. Journal of Clinical Psychol-ogy, 46, 817–827.

Wilhelm, K., Niven, H., Parker, G., & Hadzi-Pavlovic, D. (2005). The

stability of the Parental Bonding Instrument over a 20-year

period. Psychological Medicine: A Journal of Research inPsychiatry and the Allied Sciences, 35, 387–393.

Williams, J., Peeters, F., & Zautra, A. (2004). Differential affect

structure in depressive and anxiety disorders. Affect, Stress, andCoping: An International Journal, 17, 321–330.

Wittchen, H. U., Beesdo, K., Bittner, A., & Goodwin, R. (2003).

Depressive episodes–Evidence for a causal role of primary

anxiety disorders? European Psychiatry, 18, 384–393.

Cogn Ther Res (2012) 36:173–181 181

123