the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and … fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3),...

29
LANGUAGE AND SPEECH, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND GERMAN* ALLARD JONGMAN, MARIOS FOURAKIS and JOAN A. SERENO Central Institute for the Deaf, St. Louis The spectral characteristics of vowels in Modem Greek and German were examined. Four speakers of Modern Greek and three speakers of German produced four reperitions of words containing each vowel of their native language. Measurements of the fundamental frequency and the first three formants were made for each vowel token. These measurements were then transformed into log frequency ratios and plotted as points in the three-dimensional auditory-perceptual space proposed by Miller (1989). Each vowel token was thus repre- sented by one point, and the points corresponding to each vowel category were enclosed in three-dimensional target zones. For the present corpus, these zones differentiate the five vowels of Modern Greek with 100% accuracy, and the fourteen vowels of German with 94% accuracy. Implications for the distribution of common vowels across languages as a function of vowel density are discussed. Key words: vowel space, Greek, German INTRODUCTION The process of defining a vowel space for a language, or a universal space for all languages, can be characterized as having three facets corresponding to three different stages in the communication process (Lindblom, 1986). The articulatory stage, at which the vocal tract is shaped so as to produce the intended vowel, defines an articulatory vowel space by the possible positions of the tongue and the jaw, and by the shape of the lips. The acoustic stage, at which the sound radiating from the lips propagates through air, defines an acoustic space by the relative distribution of energy in the time and frequency domains. Finally, the auditory stage, at which the sound is processed by the The first and third authors are presently at the Max Planck Institute for Psycho- linguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands. A partial report of the results was presented at the 113th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America in Indianapolis, IN, May 11-15, 1987. The research was supported by NIH Grant ND21994and AFOSR Grant 860335 to Central Institute for the Deaf. The authors wish to thank James D. Miller, John W. Hawks, Steven J. Sadoff, Frank E. Kramer, and Melissa P. Piasecki for invaluable assistance. Address all correspondence to Allard Jongman, Max Planck Institute for Psycho- linguistics, P.O. Box 310, 6600 AH Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

Upload: tranthuy

Post on 14-Mar-2019

215 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

LANGUAGE AND SPEECH, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221

THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OFMODERN GREEK AND GERMAN*

ALLARD JONGMAN,

MARIOS FOURAKIS

andJOAN A. SERENO

Central Institute for the Deaf, St. Louis

The spectral characteristics of vowels in Modem Greek and German were examined. Fourspeakers of Modern Greek and three speakers of German produced four reperitions of wordscontaining each vowel of their native language. Measurements of the fundamental frequencyand the first three formants were made for each vowel token. These measurements werethen transformed into log frequency ratios and plotted as points in the three-dimensionalauditory-perceptual space proposed by Miller (1989). Each vowel token was thus repre-sented by one point, and the points corresponding to each vowel category were enclosed inthree-dimensional target zones. For the present corpus, these zones differentiate the fivevowels of Modern Greek with 100% accuracy, and the fourteen vowels of German with94% accuracy. Implications for the distribution of common vowels across languages as afunction of vowel density are discussed.

Key words: vowel space, Greek, German

INTRODUCTION

The process of defining a vowel space for a language, or a universal space for alllanguages, can be characterized as having three facets corresponding to three differentstages in the communication process (Lindblom, 1986). The articulatory stage, at whichthe vocal tract is shaped so as to produce the intended vowel, defines an articulatoryvowel space by the possible positions of the tongue and the jaw, and by the shape of thelips. The acoustic stage, at which the sound radiating from the lips propagates throughair, defines an acoustic space by the relative distribution of energy in the time andfrequency domains. Finally, the auditory stage, at which the sound is processed by the

The first and third authors are presently at the Max Planck Institute for Psycho-linguistics, Nijmegen, The Netherlands. A partial report of the results was presented atthe 113th Meeting of the Acoustical Society of America in Indianapolis, IN, May11-15, 1987. The research was supported by NIH Grant ND21994and AFOSR Grant860335 to Central Institute for the Deaf. The authors wish to thank James D. Miller,John W. Hawks, Steven J. Sadoff, Frank E. Kramer, and Melissa P. Piasecki forinvaluable assistance.

Address all correspondence to Allard Jongman, Max Planck Institute for Psycho-linguistics, P.O. Box 310, 6600 AH Nijmegen, The Netherlands.

Page 2: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

222 Vowels in Greek and German

ear and perceived as a linguistic unit, defines an auditory vowel space.A traditional problem has been to uniquely describe and distinguish phonemically

different vowels within this articulatory, acoustically, and auditorily defined vowelspace. Acoustically, it is often the case that vowel sounds perceived as representing thesame phonemic category differ in terms of their spectral and/or temporal properties.Several sources of variability may account for these differences. The acoustic charac-teristics of a specific vowel can be affected by: (1) the sex, age, and size of a speaker(inter-speaker variation); (2) the phonetic context in which the vowel is produced(coarticulatory effects); (3) the rate of speech at the moment the vowel is produced;(4) the stress value assigned to the syllable that contains the vowel; and (5) intonationcontour and phonation types. The last four sources collectively are usually referred to asintra-speaker variation. It is certainly a task for any theory of speech perception toaccount for the way in which a listener processes these differences during normal speechunderstanding. Both inter- and intra-speaker variations may cause instances ofphonemically identical vowels to occupy different regions in the vowel space, or instancesof phonemically different vowels to occupy overlapping or identical regions in the vowelspace (Peterson and Barney, 1952).

Several normalization schemes have been proposed in the literature in order to mapthe vowels of a language onto unique, non-overlapping regions in the vowel space. Suchnormalization algorithms attempt to extract invariant features of vowel sounds by takinginto account the frequency values of two or more formants and by transforming them invarious ways including mel scales (Fant, 1973), deviations from log means of vowelformant frequencies (Nearey, 1978), Bark versus sones/Bark representations (Lindblom,1986), Bark differences (Syrdal and Gopal, 1986), and multidimensional scaling tech-niques (e.g., Wright, 1986). The majority of such normalization schemes has focused onAmerican English vowels. However, even within a single language, normalizing forvariability due to speaker, phonetic context, and speaking rate proves to be quitedifficult. Most recently. Miller (1987a; 1989), in an attempt to account for intra- andinter-speaker differences, has proposed a general framework for the representation ofspeech sounds in an "auditory-perceptual space" in terms of log ratios of FO, Fl, F2,and F3. The present paper is an attempt to extend this framework, which was basedon the speech sounds of American English, to other languages. In particular, we addressthe problem o.f mapping vowels onto unique regions in the vowel space for ModernGreek, a language with five vowels, and for German, a language with 15 vowels, withinthis general approach.

The vowel space in the auditory-perceptual theory

In the auditory-perceptual theory (Miller, 1989), speech sounds, vowels in thisparticular case, are mapped onto an auditory-perceptual space (APS) of three dimensions.The vowel space proposed by Miller can be viewed as an outgrowth of proposals foundin earlier work by Peterson (1952), Shepard (1972), and Pols (1977). The dimensions ofthe auditory-perceptual space are defined by the short-term spectrum of a vowel soundin terms of log frequency ratios, and specifically by the positions of the first threeprominences in the spectrum relative to each other and to a low frequency reference.

Page 3: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis andJ.A. Sereno 223

The following equations define the auditory-perceptual space;

X = log(SF3/SF2)

y = log(SFl/SR)

z = log(SF2/SFl)

Eq. 1

Eq.2

Eq.3

SFl, SF2, and SF3 represent the frequency locations of the first three significantprominences of the short term spectral envelope of the vowel waveform. SR is a referencefrequency, which is shifted slightly by the average spectrum of the current speaker. Thisreference is calculated as follows;

SR = 168(GMF0/168)V3 Eq. 4

where GMFO is the geometric mean of the speaker s FO for the utterance. The choiceof these variables, and the motivation for the sensory reference, are discussed extensivelyin Miller (1989).

Miller (1989) presented formant values of American English vowels taken from theliterature and from measurements in the C.I.D, laboratories. These values were convertedinto log frequency ratios, using Equations 1 through 4, and were then plotted in thethree-dimensional space. The 406 data points representing the nine non-diphthongizedvowels of English (/i, i, e, », a, A, a, u, u/) are shown plotted in Figure 1 (Panel A; frontview; Panel B; side view). As is clear from Panel B, these points fall in a narrow slab('the vowel slab') of the three-dimensional auditory-perceptual space, with the width ofthe slab being quite small.

The points in the auditory-perceptual space corresponding to the same phonetic vowelcategory can be grouped accordingly into target zones. Figure 2 shows the nine non-overlapping vowel target zones of American English which can account for 93% of thedata. (See Miller, 1989, for a complete listing of the individual data points and corres-ponding target zones.)

These target zones exhibit no overlap even though the data points that define themrepresent vowels spoken by speakers of different ages and gender, in various phoneticcontexts, and at different speaking rates. For expository purposes, the axes of the APSin Figure 2 have been rotated by a transformation to bring the vowel slab to a verticalposition using the following set of equations:

x' = 0.7071 l(y-x) Eq. 5

y' = 0.81622(z)-0.408 l (x+y) Eq. 6

z' = O.5772(x-Hy-Hz) Eq. 7

This transformation does not translate the origin, but only rotates the axes. We referto the coordinates x', y \ and z' defined by Equations 5-7, as slab coordinates.

These data suggest that the representation of vowels by means of the logarithmicratios of the first three formants and a reference frequency can uniquely characterizethe vowels of American English. Such a representation may thus account for much ofthe variability due to phonetic context and variations in speaking rate, as well as fordifferences among speakers in terms of gender and age.

Page 4: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

I 'levels in (ireek and (iernian

Fig , 1 . P a n e l A , F o r m a n t m e a s u r e m e n t s o t 4 0 6 A m e r i c a n Engl i s l i m o n o p l i t l i o n g a l v o w e lt o k e n s t r a n s f o r m e d i n t o p o i n t s in t h e t h r e e - d i m e n s i o n a l a u d i t o r \ - p e r c e p t u a ls p a c e , X, y , a n d z a x e s a re in 0,1 log u n i t s , a n d t l ie p o i n t o f o r ig in is ( 0 , 0 , 0 ) .T h e s y m b o l s a r e in A r p a b e t n o t a t i o n : h ' = | i ] , 111 = |i | , t l 1 = Ir, ] , A h = |iv | ,A ( ) = | , . | , A A = | a | , A l l = [ A | , I W = | u | , l i | l = | u | .

Pane l B. S ide v i e w o f t h e , ' \PS s h o w i n t ; h o w all v o w e l t o k e n s tall i n t o a n a r n i ws l ab ot t h e AP,S ( t h e "vowel s lab") . In t h i s o r i e n t a t i o n , t h e x at id \ a x e s a p p e a lt o lall oti t o p o l e a c h o t h e r , ( l - i g u r e t a k e n t~rotii F i g u r e 13 iti " A u d i t o r y - p e r -c e p t u a l i r i t e r p r e t a t i o t i ot^ t h e v o w e l " b y ,1 .0 , WxWux. Jtnir}ial t<f the AeousiieulSneietv iif Awerieu. 8 5 , 21 14 Z\^^. C o p y r i g h t 1')S^' b \ - .Acous t ica l S o c t e t \ ' otA m e r i c a , I ' s e d b y pe r i t i i s s i o t i . )

Page 5: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

.-1, J(>ni;nian, M. Fourakis and J..\. Sereno

0 6 -

0 4 -

/uw .

-0 2j-

-0 <N

\ A

AO _ ; .

J

EH

AA

1

AE '

- 0 2 0

Fig, 2, Target zones for the nine tnonophthongal vowels ot' .American hnglish iti slab

coordinates (see Equat ions 5 — ^ 1 , In this tVont vie\\\ the z' axis is perpendicular

to the \ ' y ' platie. x' y' and z' axes are in 0,1 log units, atid the point ot origin

is (0, 0, 0), (Figure based on Figure 14 in '".Auditory-perceptual in te rpre ta t ion

ot the vowel" by J.D. Miller, Jounial of the .Aeoustieal Soeietv ofAineriea. 85,2114- 2134. Cop\Tight l'- 'K'-i by Acoustical Societ\ ' of America. I 'sed by per-

mission. )

A eross-lansjiage evaluation of the auJitoiy-pereeptual theory

T h e a u d i t o r y - p e r c e p t u a l t h e o r y - n( s p e e c h p e r c e p t i o n w a s i n i t i a l h ' d e v e l o p e d f o r , a t i da p p l i e d t o , t h e v o w e l s o u n d s o f . A m e r i c a n E n g l i s h . H o w e v e r , f o r t h e t h e o r \ ' t o b eu t i i v e r s a l , at i i m p o r t a n t c h a l l e n g e is t o s e e h o w it a c c o t i i t i i o d a t e s v o w e l i t n e n t o r i e s t h a td i t t e r t r o m t h o s e o f E n g l i s h . F o r e x a t i i p l e , it is o f i n t e r e s t t o s e e h o w t h e v o w e l s ofl a n g u a g e s w i t h s m a l l e r v o w e l i t i v e n t o r i e s a r e o r g a n i z e d in t h e a u d i t o r \ - p e r c e p t u a l s p a c e .M o r e i t n p o r t a n t h - , t h e a u d i t o r y - p e r c e p t u a l t h e o r \ m u s t a l s o b e a b l e t o a c c o u n t f o r t i i o i ec o m p l e x i n v e n t o r i e s . In a first a t t e m p t , t h e p r e s e n t s t u d \ e x a t i i i n e s t h e v o w e l s p a c e r o tM o d e r n C r e e k a n d C e r m a n . M o d e r n C r e e k h a s a s i m p l e t i v e - v o w e l s \ s t e m , c o n s i s t m ' j o \| i , e , a, o , u ] ( H o u s e h o l d e r , K a z a z i s , a n d K o u J s o u d a s , I ' ' ( i 4 ) . G e r m a n , o n t h e o t h e r l i a i u l ,h a s a c o m p l e x \> v o w e l i n v e n t o r y w i t h b o t h t e t i s e a t i d l a x v o w e l s , a n d r o t m d e d a n d

Page 6: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

226 Vowels in Greek and German

unrounded vowels, consisting of [i:, i, e, e:, e, y:, y, 0:, 0, a:, a, o:, o, u:, u ] ' (Moulton,1962; J0rgensen, 1969);

To our knowledge, no studies of the spectral characteristics of Greek vowels have beenreported. For German, two studies were of interest for the purpose of the presentanalysis. In a spectrographic study, J(/)rgensen (1969) reported the formant values of the15 German vowels as produced by six male speakers. In addition, these vowels wereplotted in a linear Fl by F2 space for each speaker individually. In general, J0rgensenfound a tendency for the lax vowels to be more centrally located than their tensecounterparts. There was, however, considerable overlap among vowels even for a singlespeaker, and superimposing the data plots for all six speakers showed extensive overlapfor some vowels. Unfortunately, J0rgensen did not include a plot or any statisticalevaluation for the combined data of all six speakers. In a recent study, Iivonen (1987)reported formant measurements of the 15 German vowels produced by one male speaker(with exceptionally high FO, around 200 Hz). This study was not designed to uniquelycharacterize the German vowels; it primarily compared the accuracy of three methodsof spectral analysis.

Given the lack of any data on the Greek vowels, and the scarceness of data on theGerman vowels, the present study serves two purposes. First, it provides basic vowelmeasurements for Greek and German by listing values of fundamental frequency andthe first three formants for each vowel. Second, this study tries to uniquely characterizethese vowels by converting the formant values into log frequency ratios and plotting themas points in the three-dimensional auditory-perceptual space.

METHOD

Subjects

Subjects were recruited from the student body and faculty of Washington University,St. Louis, MO, with the exception of one Greek speaker, who was visiting the UnitedStates. Recruitment of native speakers who had only recently arrived in the UnitedStates drastically restricted the subject pool. The Greek subjects were two graduatestudents, one faculty member, and one visiting scholar, all native male speakers ofModern Greek (three from Athens, one from Patras). All four subjects had been in theUnited States between 10 days and four months at the time of the recordings. TheGerman speakers were three female graduate students, two from Cologne and one fromBonn, all native speakers of German. All three speakers can be classified as speaking the'Central Franconian' dialect (Wiesinger, 1983). At the time of the recordings, theyhad been in the United States for a duration of two to six months.

We have adopted the phonetic symbols used by J0rgensen (1969) so that his resultscan be directly compared to'the present results.

Page 7: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis andJ.A. Sereno 227

TABLE 1

Greek and German test words containing the 5 vowels of Greek and 14 vowels ofGerman, respectively. IPA symbols representing the vowel of the first syllable

of each word are given to the right of each column.

ModernGreek

Pita

Pet a

Pata

Pote

Puse

Gloss

pie

fiy!

step!

when

whereare you?

IPA

[i]

[e]

[a]

[o]

[u]

German

Stiele

StUle

stehle

Stelle

fiihle

mile

Hohle

HoUe

Buhle

BuUe

Sohle

solle

fahle

Falle

Gloss

handles

silence

(I) steal

place

(I) feel

(I) fill

cave

hell

lover

bull

sole

(I) ought to

pale (pi.)

trap

IPA

[ i ]

[i]

[e:]

[e]

[y:]

[y]

[</>:]

[4>]

[u:]

[ul[o:]

[0]

[a:]

[a]

Materials

Two word lists (see Table 1) were used: one for the Greek and one for the Germanspeakers. For Greek, stimulus words were selected such that the five target vowelsappeared in approximately the same context. For German, stimuU were taken fromMouhon (1962) such that words containing a tense-lax vowel opposition formed minimalpairs (e.g., 'stehle' - 'Stelle'); the following consonant was always /I/. The low, frontlong vowel [e] was excluded, since it is subject to much dialectal variation (Moulton,1962; J0rgensen, 1969). Thus, the number of German vowels in the present study was 14,Randomized lists were made that contained four repetitions of each word. Therandomized Greek and German words were placed in the following carrier sentences,respectively:

Greek; [6a po ksana]. "I will say again."

German: Ich sage < noch einmal. "I say one more time."

Page 8: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

228 Vowels in Greek and German

The subjects were instructed to familiarize themselves with the list of sentences beforethe recordings were made. The total number of tokens recorded was 80 (5 vowels X4 speakers X 4 repetitions) for Greek, and 168 (14 vowels X 3 speakers X 4 repetitions)for German.

Recording procedure

Subjects were recorded in an anechoic chamber using a special, low-noise microphone/preamplifier combination (Bruel and Kjaer 4179/2660). The microphone was placed ata height equal to, and 0.5 m in front of, the speaker's mouth (0° angle of incidence).Conversational speech levels were used. The microphone output was fed directly to aSony PCM-501ES digital audio recorder (16 bit mode) with a JVC 720 VCR serving asthe storage medium. A reading timer device, designed and built in-house, was used toregulate subjects' speed for recitation of the sentences.

Analysis and further processing

The recordings were digitized at 20 kHz with a 10-kHz low-pass filter setting with16 bit precision and stored as files to be processed by the commercial software packageILS (Interactive Laboratory System). The primary sampled data files were then high-pass filtered at 50 Hz to remove any incidental low frequency noise. First, LPC analysiswas performed using a 24 msec Hamming window moving in 1 msec steps, a preemphasisfactor of 98%, and 24 poles. In addition, a cepstrally based algorithm was used todetermine the fundamental frequency. Next, a frequency spectrum was derived at eachmillisecond of signal by means of a Fast Fourier Transform of the coefficients resultingfrom the linear prediction analysis. A program written in-house was used to extract theFO, Fl , F2, and F3 values from the ILS secondary file and store them in a table-formatfile along with the corresponding frame (msec) number. In the case of merged or missingformants, a root solving command was used to enhance the resonance calculations of theFFT,

The values of FO, Fl , F2, and F3 were converted into x, y, and z coordinates bymeans of Equations 1—3. These x, y, and z values were calculated and plotted at eachmillisecond of the waveform, thus generating a sequence of points, or path, through thethree-dimensional space. The distance between any two points in the path correspondedto the amount of spectral change that had taken place within that time period.

For each vowel, we selected the 'steady-state' portion in the following manner. Whenwe displayed a path in the APS on a graphics screen (Evans and Sutherland PS300),we could determine the portion of the path exhibiting very little movement in the space,i.e., corresponding to slow or no spectral change. Using cursors, we then were able todetermine the beginning and end of these portions. We extracted the correspondingsegment of the original waveform, smoothed it on both sides using a 12 msec half Kaiserwindow, and listened to it to verify that it sounded hke the intended vowel. Thisprocedure of excising the 'steady-state' portion was applied to all vowel utterances ofboth groups of speakers.

Once the tokens were verified, we took an average of the x, y, and z coordinates overthat part of the vowel showing little change, yielding one point per vowel token for

Page 9: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis andJ.A. Sereno 229

each speaker. Thus, each vowel token was represented by a single point in the APS, whichwas based on an average of the x, y, and z values corresponding to the 'steady-state'portion of the vowel and which had been perceptually verified to be the intended vowel.

Using the procedures outlined above, we collected 80 points for the Greek vowels,and 167 points for the German vowels. (One token of the German vowel [o:] wasdisqualified due to equipment malfunction during the recording session.) The Appendixlists all the absolute frequency values (FO, Fl , F2, F3) for each token of each vowel forboth the Greek and the German speakers. The values reported in the Appendix aregeometric means of FO, Fl , F2, and F3 over the 'steady-state' portion of the voweltokens as defined above.

Data points from the literature

In order to expand our data base, we incorporated measurements taken from theavailable literature. For German, we converted J0rgensen's (1969) measurements intopoints in the APS by means of Equations 1-3, using a constant FO of 135 Hz(approximating the average male pitch of the Peterson and Barney (1952) data base)for all six male speakers. J0rgensen did not report F3 values for 14 out of 24 instancesof mid and high back vowels. For these cases, we used as an F3 value the geometric meanof all his reported F3 values for that vowel for all speakers. These procedures yielded84 points for the German vowels.

In addition, we converted formant values reported by Iivonen (1987) into points inthe APS. Iivonen included measurements for fundamental frequency and the threeformant frequencies at three different locations in the vowel: One taken at the "targetfor F l " one at the "target for F2", and one at "maximum volume velocity (Iivonen,1987, p. 128). In converting these values to points in the APS, we used the geometricmean of these three measurements for each vowel. This yielded 14 more points for theGerman vowels.

In sum, the Greek data base consisted of 80 tokens based on our own measurements,while the German data base consisted of 265 vowel points, of which 167 were based onour own measurements, 84 were taken from J0rgensen (1969), and 14 were taken fromIivonen (1987).

RESULTS

Modem Greek

The vowel productions of the Greek speakers are shown plotted in the APS (frontview) in Figure 3. Each symbol represents one vowel token by a speaker. The middleof each symbol represents the actual location of each token. Points belonging to thesame phonetic category are enclosed in target zones. These target zones are, in fact,three-dimensional objects, the plane projections of which are shown in Figure 3.^ All

The target zones were drawn on a high-resolution color graphics terminal (Evansand Sutherland PS300) which emulates a third dimension. No constraints were placedon the shape of the target "Zones, except for the fact that they had to be continuous.All zones were drawn by hand, using a computer-aided method with a resolution of0.01 log units. This resolution was chosen so that the present classification perfor-mance could be compared to that for American English as reported by Miller (1989),

Page 10: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

230 I 'owels in (ireek and German

' 111.' [el.:" [al» [01

. [u]

Y'

Fig, 3, Data points and target zones tor the live Greek vowels shown in front view usingslab coordinates. See Figure 2 for axis labels and units. Each sytnbol representsone vowel token produced by one speaker.

the present data points can be enclosed by distinct, non-overlapping zones. Fhus, thesezones enable us to describe the present corpus of Greek vowels with 1 OO'> accurac\.

It is remarkable that the target zones for [i] and [e | , in particular, leave a lot o\space unoccupied. One might expect much more variation in the vowel productions ^\Greek speakers since there are no competing photienies iti the area, as opposed toAtnerican English (see, for example, Lindblom, 1086), Of course, turther atiaUsts ot'vowels produced by both male and female speakers (since only male speakers were usedtn the present study) and iti tiiore phonetic contexts tnight result in target zoties forModern Creek which are less compact,

Gernian

The (jermati data present a more cotuplcx pattert i . Our Gertiian corpus otters tiot

only a greater vowel inventory, btit also vowel tokens from both male atul fetiiale

speakers. Ihe 2r>, points representing the Cerman vowels were plot ted in the APS. In

an a t t empt to mitiimize the amount o{ overlap among the 14 vowels, each set of points

Page 11: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

.-1, Jongman, M. Fourakis and J..A. Sereno

Fig, 4, Target zones for the 14 Gertnan vowels shown in front view using slab coor-dinates. See Figure ] for axis labels and units. The zones associated with each ofthe vowels are itidicated hy the labels.

belonging to a particular vowel category was enclosed by an outline of a correspondingtarget zone.

Figure 4 shows a front view of the auditory-perceptual space with the target zones forthe 14 German vowels.' As is apparent from Figure 4, these target zones are quiteirregular in shape, thus accommodating some differences in speaker and phonetic context.For the present stud>', we have sitnpl>- drawti the target zones such that the)' enclose asmany appropriate data points as possible. However, with this relatively small sample,it retrains an open question whether such zoties will prove to have atiy explatiatory value.In order t(.) address this question, perceptual veritication experitiients tnust be conductedto establish the "psychological reality of the target zone boundaries. Such experimentsare ctirrentl> being cotiducted in our laboratories to evaluate the target zones establishedtor American English. However, until this issue has been settled, we believe that thereis no a priori reason to prefer smooth zones (e.g., circles or ellipses) o\er the present

For the sake of clarity, individual data points are not sliown in tliis lMgure but areshown for each vowel in Figures ,S, d, and 7.

Page 12: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

232 Vowels in Greek and German

irregular zones.*For the 14 German vowels, the three-dimensional irregular zones were able to describe

the present corpus of German vowels with 94% accuracy. The zones group the datapoints according to phonetic category without any overlap. Since Figure 4 is a two-dimensional representation of the three-dimensional space, some target zones may appearto overlap. However, none of the target zones show any overlap when taking all threedimensions into account.

Table 2 summarizes the results of mapping the intended productions onto distincttarget zones. The overall accuracy is approximately 94% correct. Only 16 vowel tokensout of a total of 265 tokens were misclassified, with the majority of those points fallingin regions not belonging to any target zone. The vowel with the lowest score is [y](53%), which exhibits considerably more scatter than any of the other vowels. With theexception of this vowel, the percent correct classifications range from 89% to 100%.

The individual target zones with their corresponding data points are shown in Figures5, 6, and 7. Figure 5 shows a front view of the data points and target zones for thefront unrounded vowels [i:, i, e:, e] . Figure 6 shows a front view of the data points andtarget zones for the front rounded vowels [y:, y, 0:, 0 ] . Figure 7 shows a front viewof the data points and target zones for the back vowels [ u : , u , o : , o , a : , a ] .

With respect to the front-back distinction, there is a clear separation between frontvowels, on the one hand, and back vowels on the other. Front vowels occupy the APSquadrant with positive x' and y values, while back vowels occupy quadrants withnegative x' and either positive or negative y' values.

As for the unrounded-rounded distinction, the target zones for the front roundedvowels ([y:, y, 0:, 0]) fall behind those for the front unrounded ones ([i:, i, e:, e]).

* In addressing the issue of the irregular boundaries of the target zones. Miller (1987b)presented the 'third iteration' target zones for English, which classified 2051 voweldata points into the nine monophthongal categories of English with 95.8% accuracy.Of these 2051 points, 1420 were from Peterson and Barney (1952) and 631 werecollected at Central Institute for the Deaf. In order to examine the effect of highlyirregular borders on the percentage of correct classification. Miller (1987b) applied asmoothing algorithm to these target zones, which iteratively averaged the border linesevery three points. He applied this smoothing 50, 100, 400, and 1000 times. After100 times, for example, the target zone for [A] , which is highly irregular since in APSit is flanked by many adjacent target zones, became a three-dimensional oval-shapedobject with no appendages, about 0.25 log units long and 0.18 log units wide in frontview. The correct classifications for each of the smoothings are given in the followingtable as a percentage of the total 2051 points:

Smoothing iterations 0 50 100 400Correct classification (%) 95.8 72.5 69.5 45.1

After about 1000 iterations, the zones collapsed into single points and the classifi-cation percentage approached zero. We did not apply this algorithm to the Gerniantarget zones because of the small number of data points. However, we are currently inthe process of extending our German database and we hope to be able to refine theGerman target zones as well as get some estimate of the effect of irregular boundariesusing perception experiments.

Page 13: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

Pro-ducedvowel

i:

i

e:

e

y:

Mapped

i: i

19 -

- 17

- -

- -— —

onto:

e:

_

-

19

-

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis andJ.A. Sereno 233

TABLE :

Classification of German vowels by means of perceptual target zones.The rows show the intended vowels and the columns the target zones

onto which they map

Un-claimed %

y: y 0 : 0 u: u o: o a: a areas correct

- - - - - - - - - - - 100

- - 1 - - - - - - - - - 1 89

- - - - - - - - - - - 100

- - - 1 9 - - - - - - - - - - 100

19 - - - - - - - - - 100

y — 2 — — — 10 1 — — — — — — — 6 53

0: - - - - - 1 18 - - - - - - - - 95

0 - - - - - - - 1 9 - - - - - - - 100

u: - - - - - - - - 1 8 - - - - - 1 95

u _ - _ - - - - - - 1 8 - - - - 1 95

o: - - - - - - - - - - 1 7 - - - 1 94

o - - - - - - - - - - - 1 9 - - - 100

a: - - - - - - - - - - - - 1 9 - - 100

a - - - - - - - - - - - - - 1 8 1 95

Total correct:249/265 = 94

as shown in Figure 8. A comparison of the front unrounded and front rounded vowelsin terms of z' values revealed that the front unrounded vowels have a mean z' value ofapproximately 0.70 log units, whereas the front rounded vowels are further back with amean z' value of approximately 0.65 log units. The distinction between the front roundedand front unrounded vowels in terms of z' values eliminates the apparent overlap in thetwo-dimensional front view among the vowels [ i : ] , [ e : ] , and [y : ] . As shown in Figure4, the target zone for rounded [y:] seems to overlap with those for unrounded [i:]and [e:] , However, when the vowel slab is rotated 90 degrees into side view, it can beseen that these target zones-do not overlap. Figure 9 shows such a side view of the data

Page 14: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

234 in (ireek and (jernian

[J

+

111

le)[ i l

[e ]

F i g , 5 , D a t a p o i n t s a n d t a r g e t z o n e s t o r t h e G e r t n a t i tVont u n r o u n d e d v o w e l s ( | i : .

i, e :, e j ) s h o w t i in I r o n t v i e w u s i n g s l a b c o o r d i n a t e s . S e e F i g u r e 2 t o r a x i s l a b e l s

a n d u n i t s . E a c h s y m b o l r e p r e s e n t s o n e v o w e l t o k e n p r o d u c e d b y o t i e s p e a k e r .

p o i n t s t o r | i : J a n d | e : ] ( w h i c h o v e r l a p in s i d e v i e w o t i l y ) a t i d t h e p o i t i t s r e p r e s e n t t t i g

| \ ' : J . ,As c a n b e s e e n , all | y : | p o i n t s a r e b e h i n d t h e d a t a p o i n t s f o r | i : ] a t i d | e : ] . T h u s ,

t h o s e u n r o u n d e d - r o u n d e d v o w e l p a i r s t h a t o v e r l a p in t h e t r o u t v i e w ca t i b e s e e n t o b e

n o n o v e r l a p p i n g in t h e s i d e v i e w .

F i t i a l l y , C e r m a n t e t i s e a n d l ax v o w e l s t i i a p o n t o d i s t i t i c t t a r g e t z o n e s , w i t h t a r g e t / o n e s

l o r t e n s e v o w e l s g e n e r a l l \ - h a v i n g l a r g e r y v a l u e s t h a n t h o s e f o r t h e i r l ax c o u n t e r p a r t s .

S i n c e t h e v a l u e o t y ' is l a r g e l y d e p e n d e n t o n t h e v a l u e o f z ( s e e E q u a t i o n d ) , t h i s t i n d i n g

is in a c c o r d w i t h t h e t r a d i t i o t i a l o b s e r v a t i o n ( e . g . , J 0 r g e t i s e n , I '- 'd'- ') t h a t t e t i s e ( l o n g )

v o w e l s t e n d t o h a v e a l o w e r E l ( i . e . , a r e less c e n t r a l i z e d ) t h a t i t h e i r l ax ( s h o r t ) c o u n t e r -

p a r t s .

In o r d e r t o c o t n p a r e t h e p e r t o r m a n c e o t t h e A P S t o t h a t o f o t h e r c l a s s i t T c a t i o n

s c h e m e s , w e c o n d u c t e d l i n e a r d i s c r i m i n a n t a t i a K s e s u s i n g c o t i i b i n a t i o n s o f F O , E l , E 2 ,

a i u l E 3 , a s w e l l as t h e A P S p a r a m e t e r s x , y , a n d z , a s t h e c l a s s i f i c a t o r \ ' v a r i a b l e s . ' ^ 1 m c a i

W f t l i a i i k o n e o t t t i e r e v i e w e r s , l e r r y N e , i r e v , t o r n i a k i n p h i s l i n e a r d i s c r i t n i n a n ta i i a h ' s i s p n . i g r a n i a v a i l a b l e t o t i s .

Page 15: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M Fourakis and J. A. Sereno

Y'

AD

[ y i[0]

"7T• —'-•f-

0

/>\ff

Eig. (T. Data points and target zones t"or the Certnan tVont rounded vowels ([>• , y,

0 . , 01 ) shown in front view using slab coordit iates. See Eigure 2 for axis labels

and units. Each symbol represetits one vowel token produced b\ ' one speaker.

discritninant analysis is a technique to verify that apparent clusters are real, and to decide

to which cluster a new data point should be assigned. The discritiiinatit atiaK'sis m e t h o d

as applied to sitnilar issues is described in ,Assmann, Xearey, and Hogan (l'-^S2), Syrdal

11*^)85), and Syrdal and Gopal ( l ' i S b ) , Brietlv', this analysis uses group tiieans to assign

each individual token to a group on the basis of the es t imated a posteriori p robabt l i t \

ot group membership . The results presented here are tVom the R ( resubs t i tu t ion) m e t h o d

ot dassificaticMi, which provides an index of the resolution into groups of the present

data set. In addit ion, the a posteriori probabil i ty (APP) indicates the relative s trength of

group metiibership. For modera te satiiple sizes such as the present one , this index nia\- be

more intormative than percent correct classification (.Assniann et a!.. U''S2),

The results of the discriminant analyses for the variables FO, F l . E2, atid E3 , as well

as X, y, and z, are shinvn in Table 3. It can be seen that \ari inis combina t ions o\ these

variables produce reasonably similar results. However, the additioti of EO to ei ther the

tirst two or the first three formants improves resolut ion, increasin!: the APP to O.'^S

and O.fiO, respectively. The x , y , z coordinate system yields a ver\- similar pe r fo rmance .

Page 16: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

Vowels in (ireek and Gernian

Eig. 7. Data points and target zones for the German back vowels ( | u : , u, o : , o, a :, a | )

shown in front view usina slab coordinates . See Eigure 2 for axis labels atid

units . Each symbol represents one vowel token produced by one speaker.

T A B L I : 3

Results of discriminant analysis for Gernian vowels

Variables

E l , E2

F l , E 2 , E3

EO, E l , E2

EO, E l , E 2 ,

X, \ . z

' ' correct

(ll M

a posteriori probability (APP)

O., O

()„> I

0,58

O,(iO

O,SS

Page 17: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

,•1, Jongman, M. Fourakis and J..A. Sereno

• Front Unrounded® Front Rounded

Z'

Fia. 8, Data pomts for the German front unrounded vowels ([t:, i, e :, e] ) and the trontrounded vowels {[y.. y. 4>:. (^\) shown in side view using slab coordinates. Thefront unrounded vowels are represented b>' cubes, their rounded counterpartsby spheres. In this orientation, the x' axis is perpendicular to the y z' plane,x', >•' and z' axes are in 0,1 log units and the point of origin is (0, 0, 0). Thefront of the space is to the right, with the data points tiu the rounded vowelsfalling behind those t'or the unrounded vowels.

with an APP of 0.58, While the percent correct classification of the x, y, and z variablesdoes not reach the level of the hatid-drawn target zones, this tiiay be due to the factthat tokens are assigned to groups on the basis of group means, Eor target zones thatenvelope other zones, the group means will be ver)' similar. In this sense, then, litieardiscriminant analysis would not be able to difterentiate points tailing, tor exatnple. itithe target zoties for Gernian [a] and [a:] (see Eigure 7), since these zones would havesimilar means. Nevertheless, the present analysis shows that vowel classitlcatioti oti thebasis of X, y, and z roaches a level cotnparable to that on the basis of either EO, E l , E2,or EO, El , F2, and F3. For another approach to the comparison of the ,APT scheme toother normalization schetiies (e.g., Koenig, mel, and Bark), see Miller ( U>8^)),

Page 18: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

I 'owels in (ireek and German

Y

n [ i ]A [e-l

T

Fig. Q, Data points tor the Gernian unrounded vowels | i : ] and [e: ] along with the

data points tor the German rounded vowel [y : ] shown in side view using slab

coordinates . See Fiaure 8 for axis labels atid units.

DISCUSSION

The present s tudy a t t emp t s to characterize the vowels of Greek and Gerniati b\ '

conver t ing FO, E l , F2 , and E3 values into log frequency ratios and plott ing them \\\

the APS, Represent ing the relatively simple vowel inventory of Greek in the audi tor \ -

perceptual space enabled us to describe the five Greek vowels with lOO'i' accuracy. The

data points tor the five Greek vowels are enclosed by non-contiguous target zones.

Represent ing the much niore complex German vowel system in the three-ditiiensional

space enabled us to describe the vowels with ' '4 '- ' accuracy. The data points tor the

14 Cerman vowels are enclosed by adjacent target zones, with little or no unclaimed

space be tween them. This representat ion of the German vowels yielded good separation

along the t ront -back , rouiuled-Lmrotuuled, and tense-lax dimensions.

In his spectrographic s tudy ot the Cerman vowels, ,l0rgensen (l'^'d'-') pointed out that

instead of representing the vowels in an Fl by F2 space, it might be advantageous to

take 13 into account , especially tor laiigtiages with both unrounded and routided front

vowels. Since, according to J^igeiisen, three-dimensional displa>'s are usualU' quite

Page 19: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis andJ.A. Sereno 239

hard to grasp, J0rgensen plotted the vowels in a two-dimensional space, using an effectiveF2' (Korlen and Malmberg, 1960), a measure that takes F3 into account. However, whenJ0rgensen plotted the German vowels in an Fl by F2' space, it did not enhance theseparation of the unrounded and rounded front vowels at all, so that J0rgensen used anFl by F2 space for the remainder of the paper (J0rgensen, 1969). Although J0rgensendid not quantify the amount of overlap in his two-dimensional space, it does seem that inthe present analysis taking a third dimension (F3) into account as an independentdimension, rather than a derived dimension, substantially reduces the amount of overlap,particularly that between front unrounded and rounded vowels. Furthermore, theintroduction of a low-frequency reference (SR) for speaker normalization and for thedisambiguation of certain vowels may also have improved the classification of the 14German vowels in the present study.

In addition, the three-dimensional space used in the auditory perceptual theoryaffords a way of comparing vowel systems across languages which is not subject to anyof the objections brought forth by Disner (1980; 1986). Disner (1986) argues againstnormalization schemes which use mean formant frequencies as the correction factorwhen comparing vowel systems from different languages. She states that, when usingoverall formant frequency means for a language like French (or German) with frontrounded vowels, the normalization procedure is likely to overnormalize the data, sincethere are more front vowels than back vowels. However, the auditory-perceptual theorydoes not use overall formant means as the correction factor for normalization. Thus,the vowels of each language can be mapped onto the same space, bounded by anatomicaland physiological constraints, independent of vowel density in any area of the space.

It is possible, then, to represent and compare the vowels of all languages in a vowelspace that is defined independently of vowel inventory. For example, the /i/ vowelof one language can be compared with the /i/ vowel of another language, regardless ofthe density of vowels in the language. It does seem to be the case that those vowelphonemes that the three languages discussed in this paper (Greek, German, and AmericanEnglish) have in common occupy similar locations in APS. Figure 10 shows a front viewof APS with the three vowels that Greek, German, and American Enghsh have in common([i, a, u]). For each language, each vowel is represented by a single point which is basedon the average of the x', y', and z' values for all tokens of a particular vowel. The x',y', and z' values for American English [i, a, u] were taken from the database describedin Miller (1989). As can be seen, these common vowels occupy similar regions in APS.However, it seems that the locations of these common vowels do, in fact, vary as afunction of the phoneme inventory of each individual language, with the Greek vowelstending to be more centrally located and the German and American English vowels moreperipherally located.

In general, it would seem advantageous for a given language to have vowels that aremaximally distinct acoustically (see, for example, Liljencrants and Lindblom, 1972;Stevens, 1972; Lindblom, 1986) for reasons of communicative efficiency. Greek providesan example with its five vowels being quite far from each other in APS. Interestingly,five-vowel inventories similar to that of Greek are much more frequent than any othertype of vowel inventory (Crpthers, 1978; Maddieson, 1984). The question remains, then.

Page 20: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

240 I'owels in Greek and German

Greek

German

American English

4a]

Fig, 10, Data points t~or three common vowels of Greek, German, and American English([i, u, a] ) shown in front view using slab coordinates. Each data point is theaverage of the x' y' and z' values of all tokens for each vowel in each language.See Figure 2 for axis labels and units.

how the vowels of languages with larger vowel inventories are organized in this samevowel space. The vowel spaces tor Gernian and American Hnglish are much more dense.It seems that the larger the vowel inventory, the more peripheral the location of theextreme vowels (in terms of x' and y'), relative to vowels of languages with smalleiinventories.

As shown in Figure 10, this general trend holds for the vowels |i] and |u | ot German,a language with 15 niotiophthotigal vowels, as compared to those of American Enghsh,a language with ^ tnonophthongal vowels, which, in turn, are relativelv' more extretiiethan those ot Greek, a language with 5 vowels. The exception is | a ] , which is mostextremely located for American English, Of ctnirse, these prelitiiinary findings will haveto be replicated with a much larger sample.

In conclusion, the spectral characteristics of the tive vowels in Modern Greek and 14vowels in German were examined. For each vowel token, trequenc\' values of EO, El , E2,and E3 were obtained. A transtorination was tised to convert these measurements intolog frequency ratios which were then plotted as points in the APS, Einally, target /ones

Page 21: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis and J. A. Sereno 241

were drawn around points representing phonemically identical vowels. These target zonescould correctly categorize the present Greek and German corpora with 100% and 94%accuracy, respectively. Using this preliminary set of data, the present approach appearsto normalize for a variety of inter- and intra-speaker variables and allow a unique charac-terization of vowel sounds in different languages, as well as a way to compare vowelsacross languages. In this manner, such an approach may provide insights not only intothe language-specific organization of vowel systems, but also into cross-languagecomparisons.

(Received March 6, 1989: accepted October 6, 1989)

REFERENCES

ASSMANN, P.F., NEAREY, T.M., and HOGAN, J.T. (1982). Vowel identification: Orthographic,perceptual, and acoustic aspects. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 71, 975-989.

CROTHERS, J. (1978). Typology and universals of vowel systems. In J.H. Greenberg, C.A, Ferguson,and E.A. Moravcsik (eds.), Universals of Human Language, Vol. 2: Phonology (pp, 93-152).Stanford: Stanford University Press,

DISNER, S.F, (1980). EvaluationofvowelnormaUzationprocedures.yoMrnfl/o/f/ie/IcoMsricfl/5ocief>'of America, 67,253-261,

DISNER, S.F. (1986). On describing vowel quality. In J.J. Ohala and J.J. Jaeger (eds.), ExperimentalPhonology (pp. 69-79). New York: Academic Press.

FANT, G. (1973). Speech Sounds and Features. Cambridge: MIT Press.HOUSEHOLDER, F.W., KAZAZIS, K., and KOUTSOUDAS, A. (1964). Reference Grammar of

Literary Dhimotiki. UAL 30/2 Part II, PubUcations of the Indiana University ResearchCenter in Anthropology, Folklore, and Linguistics, 31, Bloomington: Indiana University.

IIVONEN, A. (1987). A set of German stressed monophthongs analyzed by RTA, FFT, and LPC.In R. Channon and L, Shockey (eds,). In Honor of Use Lehiste (pp. 125-138). Dordrecht:Foris Publications.

J<I)RGENSEN, H. (1969). Die gespannten und ungespannten Vokale in der norddeutschen Hoch-sprache, mit einer spezifischen Untersuchung der Struktur ihrer Formantenfrequenzen,Phonetica, 19,217-245.

KORLEN, G., and tVIALIVIBERG, B. (1960). Tysk Fonetik. Lund: Gleerups,LILJENCRANTS, J., and LINDBLOM, B. (1972). Numerical simulation of vowel quality systems:

The role of perceptual contrast. Language, 48, 839-862.LINDBLOM, B. (1986). Phonetic universals in vowel systems. In J.J. Ohala and J.J, Jaeger (eds).

Experimental Phonology (pp. 13-44). New York: Academic Press.MADDIESON, I, (1984). Patterns of Sounds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.MILLER, J.D. (1987a). Auditory-perceptual processing of speech waveforms. In W.A. Yost and C.S.

Watson (eds.). Auditory Processing of Complex Sounds (pp. 257-266). HiUsdale: Erlbaum,MILLER, J.D. (1987b). Classification of vowel production by means of perceptual target zones: A

response to Ladefoged and Studdert-Kennedy. Journal of the A coustical Society of A merica,82, Suppl. 1,S82.

MILLER, J.D. (1989). Auditory-perceptual interpretation of the vowel. Journal of the AcousticalSociety of America, 85,2114-2134.

MOULTON, W.G.( 1962). The Sounds of German and English. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.NEAREY, T.M. (1978). Phonetic Feature Systems for Vowels. Bloomington: Indiana University

Linguistics Club.

Page 22: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

242 Vowels in Greek and German

PETERSON, G.E. (1952). The information bearing elements of speech. Journal of the AcousticalSociety of America, 24, 629-637.

PETERSON, G.E., and BARNEY, H.L. (1952). Control methods used in a study of the vowels. Journalof the .Acoustical Society of America, 24, 175-184.

POLS, L.C.W. (1977). Spectral Analysis and Identification of Dutch Vowels in Monosyllabic Words.Soesterberg, The Netherlands: Institute for Perception TNO,

SHEPARD, R.N. (1972). Psychological representation of speech sounds. In E.E. David and P.B. Denes(eds.). Human Communication: A Unified View (pp. 67-113). New York: McGraw Hill.

STEVENS, K.N. (1972). The quantal nature of speech. In E,E, David and P.B, Denes (eds.). HumanCommunication: A Unified View (pp. 51-66). New York: McGraw Hill.

SYRDAL, A.K.(1985). Aspects of a model of the auditory representation of American English vowels.Speech Communication, 4, 121-135,

SYRDAL, A,K., and GOPAL, H.S. (1986). A perceptual model of vowel recognition based on theauditory representation of American English vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society ofAmerica, 79, 1086-1100.

WIESINGER. P. (1983). Die Einteilung der deutschen Dialekte, In W. Besch, U. Knoop, W. Putschke,and H.E. Wiegand (eds.), Dialektologie: Ein Handbuch zur deutschen und allgemeinenDialektforschung, Vol 2 (pp. 807-900). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter,

WRIGHT, J.T. (1986). The behavior of nasalized vowels in the perceptual vowel space. In J.J, Ohalaand J.J. Jaeger (td%.). Experimental Phonology (pp, 45-67) , New York: Academic Press.

APPENDIX

Tables Al and A2 list the fundamental frequency and formant frequency values for eachof the vowel tokens plotted in the APS. Each measurement reported represents thegeometric mean of the relevant parameter over the total duration of the steady-stateportion of the vocalic section transcribed as the intended vowel. To replicate the plotsin the main paper, one needs to convert the FO values into SR values using Equation 4and then use Equations 1 through 3, or Equations 5 through 7, to derive x, y, z or x',y', and z' coordinates, respectively. Table Al lists the values for the five Greek vowels

and Table A2 for the 14 German vowels.

Spkr.

1.

Token

12

3

4

FO

192

203

216

201

Vowel

Fl

317

319

311

3.17

TABLE A1

[i]

F2

2226

2337

2345

2088

F3

2550

2635

2603

2320

FO

182

197

206

200

Vowel

Fl

475

477

484

481

le]

F2

1806

1821

1854

1818

F3

2247

2300

2276

2130

Page 23: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis andJ.A. Sereno 243

4.

1

2

3

4

1-)

3

4

1• )

3

4

169

157

197

195

128

129

130

130

135

143

124

125

311 2099 2374

308 2093 2354

311 2134 2362

312 2145 2332

310 1725 2188

303 1729 2183

304 1777 2075

300 1716 2266

310 2037 2549

311 2045 2644

301 2103 2655

310 2042 2617

172 469 1626 2056

157 468 1657 1989

193 510 1625 2043

202 479 1614 2038

124 468 1407 2425

130 473 1408 2422

123 469 1407 2418

127 456 1414 2466

131 471 1676 2322

135 473 1703 2460

125 472 1667 2273

143 466 1749 2565

Spkr. Token

1. 1

3

4

3.

3

4

3

4

Vowel [a]

FO Fl F2 F3

182 770 1300 2329

180 771 1368 2320

180 773 1414 2397

198 736 1351 2177

156 739 1138 2970

148 718 1171 3027

146 674 1146 3078

195 626 1175 2899

123

118

122

117

626

595

594

625

1125 2457

1188 2509

1151 2509

FO

191

194

198

197

150

174

175

197

130

130

131

129

Vowel

Fl

470

476

474

482

471

473

470

481

473

476

480

473

lo]

F2

802

814

811

825

750

856

887

762

945

837

825

878

F3

2433

2428

2492

2484

1857

1853

1979

2086

2519

3239

2733

2676

Page 24: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

244 Vowels in Greek and German

4.

Spkr.

1.

2.

3.

4.

1-)

3

4

Token

1

2

3

4

1

2

3

4

1

2

3

4

1

2

3

4

132

134

128

129

FO

215

212

225

215

168

165

209

216

139

133

139

131

148

148

134

147

683

645

663

648

Vowel

Fl

421

421

350

384

312

312

342

328

313

312

311

312

358

315

312

313

1242

1242

1248

1245

lu]

F2

831

728

616

609

855

771

749

730

907

1509

1000

1001

1004

1065

850

836

2326

2275

2380

2307

F3

2490

2491

2491

2461

2240

2107

2139

2203

2336

2302

2300

2295

2648

2471

2361

2338

136

133

138

140

486

478

482

477

938

959

866

903

2198

2224

2303

2351

Page 25: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis and J.A. Sereno 245

Spkr.

1.

-I

3.

Spkr.

1.

2.

3.

Token

1

3

4

1~>

3

4

1t

3

4

Token

1

2

3

4

1

•)

3

4

1

2

3

4

FO

198

199

188

194

214

213

205

202

249

238

258

249

FO

176

202

181

177

204

190

206

189

254

258

255

241

Vowel

Fl

315

310

307

306

302

311

317

315

304

299

308

308

Vowel

Fl

347

388

315

311

430

344

441

324

470

465

468

469

TABLE A2

F2

2556

2672

2507

2607

2562

2526

2576

2625

2594

2652

2504

2646

[e.l

F2

2463

2379

2312

2499

2539

2573

2689

2539

2483

2469

2509

2468

F3

3306

3452

3372

3117

3479

3494

3447

3445

3400

3476

3301

3515

F3

3262

2997

3277

3377

3061

3025

3043

3099

2969

3113

3106

3160

FO

184

184

185

Vowel

Fl

463

353

329

li]

F2

2048

1798

1769

F3

2633

2415

2227

195 453 2030 2458

177

199

206

188

269

244

260

459

434

443

360

465

465

472

2327

2297

2390

2138

1926

1915

1927

2787

2795

2855

2802

2937

2801

2814

244 468 1939 3011

FO

177

180

184

188

204

200

196

195

229

224

237

Vowel

Fl

432

624

629

617

648

642

680

629

474

596

574

[e]

F2

1971

1963

2034

2006

2062

2120

2065

2032

2077

2039

2029

F3

2546

2525

2607

2638

2930

2970

2932

2968

2933

2998

3002

249 488 2045 3028

Page 26: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

246 Vowels in Greek and German

Vowel [y:]

Spkr.

1.

-)

3.

Token

1

-)

3

4

1

3

4

1

2

3

4

FO

195

196

188

193

227

220

222

220

243

264

265

257

Fl

340

333

310

309

304

306

307

308

312

308

309

310

F2

1727

1583

1509

1717

1535

1551

1558

1353

1872

1879

1875

1842

F3

2296

2199

2352

2453

2500

2486

2496

2517

2464

2427

2409

2392

FO

192

191

184

Vowel

Fl

476

333

467

[y]

F2

1686

1783

1700

F3

2296

2417

2568

189 498 1570 2149

219 459 1565 2502

221 456 1506 2478

220 470 1666 2575

215 468 1653 2649

212 398 1569 2230

292 464 1614 2150

279 464 1703 2249

263 467 1562 2392

Vowel [0:]

Spkr.

1.

2.

3.

Token

1

2

3

4

1

2

3

4

1

3

4

FO

189

194

184

180

213

206

213

204

266

269

254

246

Fl

319

333

314

313

460

445

459

437

427

466

465

467

F2

1579

1559

1436

1429

1477

1411

1406

1412

1576

1701

1629

1712

F3

2335

2338

2306

2215

2315

2365

2327

2402

2341

2309

2344

FO

186

184

188

Vowel

Fl

637

557

616

10]

F2

1747

1888

1846

F3

2429

2566

2551

181 589 1797 2392

199

216

205

201

274

232

234

232

628

627

629

625

611

659

666

505

1725

1708

1662

1708

1712

1712

1560

1711

2653

2646

2599

2609

2469

2337

2532

2341

Page 27: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

A. Jongman, M. Fourakis andJ.A. Sereno 247

Spkr.

1.

2.

3.

Spkr.

1.

2.

3.

Token

1

• )

3

4

1

2

3

4

1

2

3

4

Token

1

2

3

4

1

2

3

4

1

2

3

4

FO

185

179

182

180

207

214

198

202

274

253

250

245

FO

182

171

171

166

189

-

180

189

271

236

242

247

Vowel [u:]

Fl

317

306

316

315

329

305

317

357

312

314

313

315

F2

840

1189

956

843

1026

808

632

708

713

589

568

598

Vowel [o:]

Fl

332

317

316

321

371

325

354

483

468

472

469

F2

756

598

600

834

758

626

721

718

964

1017

917

F3

1935

1954

2047

1883

2567

2641

2600

2488

2552

2532

2568

2496

F3

2917

2610

2442

2567

2894

2820

2834

2406

2437

2466

2485

FO

182

183

187

177

205

208

209

203

265

248

237

242

FO

168

167

178

172

182

187

181

175

259

228

217

Vowel [u]

Fl

345

347

333

318

458

467

463

456

491

478

469

468

F2

853

742

758

761

801

800

798

755

739

931

932

894

Vowel [o]

Fl

541

528

487

611

697

737

668

658

809

637

636

F2

1391

1262

1251

1332

1104

1191

1129

1123

1244

1133

1109

F3

2227

2305

1932

2239

2770

2778

2730

2804

2552

2529

2451

2418

F3

2491

2483

2324

2435

2667

2678

2653

2735

2510

2558

2522

238 642 1184 2413

Page 28: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,

248 Vowels in Greek and German

Vowel [a:]

Spkr. Token FO Fl F2 F3

1.

3.

12

3

4

1

2

3

4

1

2

3

4

174176

180

172

190

184

175

192

231

228

249

235

754800

777

800

796

802

805

819

957

849

1188

982

15051577

1409

1350

1259

1258

1289

1370

1288

1167

1316

1315

2464

2602

2592

2693

2703

2653

2732

2739

2514

2527

2662

2604

FO

177

180

169

189

204

199

191

203

261

252

240

249

Vowel [a]

Fl

676

723

654

727

816

813

795

974

1084

1003

967

995

F2

1708

1851

1610

1531

1361

1405

1491

1439

1485

1420

1405

1408

F3

2534

2537

2542

2500

2752

2671

2791

2675

2670

2524

2522

2566

Page 29: THE ACOUSTIC VOWEL SPACE OF MODERN GREEK AND … Fourakis... · language and speech, 1989,32(3), 221 -248 221 the acoustic vowel space of modern greek and german* allard jongman,