subjective well-being, eudemonic well-being and perception of self and others marie-claire ellsmore,...

23
Subjective well-being, eudemonic well-being and perception of self and others Marie-Claire Ellsmore, RoseAnne Misajon & Tom Whelan Monash University, Melbourne

Post on 19-Dec-2015

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Subjective well-being, eudemonic well-being and

perception of self and others

Marie-Claire Ellsmore, RoseAnne Misajon & Tom Whelan

Monash University, Melbourne

Subjective well-being and eudemonic well-being:

complementary constructs

Eudemonic well-being (EWB) evolves from pursuing contexts and relationships that fulfill intrinsic human needs while continually extending the self, resulting in personal growth (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2001; Ryff, 1989; Waterman, 1993)

Both Subjective well-being (SWB) and EWB research traditions provide complementary insight into well-being phenomena (e.g., Sirgy et al., 2006)

Well-being and positively biased self-perception

Both SWB and EWB research traditions also converge at the social-cognitive level of analysis (e.g., Fujita & Diener, 2005; Reis, et al., 2000)

Positively biased self-perception Social comparison perspective: perception of

self and “average other” (Festinger, 1954)

Self-insight perspective: perception of self compared with external evaluation (Kwan et al., 2004)

Well-being and positively biased self-perception

Positively biased self-perception measured using social comparison methodologies has been found to correlate with greater SWB and EWB (e.g., Boyd-Wilson et al., 2002; Cummins & Nistico,

2002) Baumeister’s (1989) “optimal margin of illusion” to account for the paradox:SWB and positively biased self-perception = linear relationship (e.g., Taylor et al., 2003)EWB and positively biased self-perception = curvilinear relationship?

Well-being and perception of others

Greater SWB and EWB is associated with positive interpersonal relationships (e.g., Cummins, Lau & Davern, in press; Ryff, 1989)

Evidence also suggests that how positively individuals perceive others affects their interpersonal functioning (e.g., Mikuliner & Horesh, 1999; Sacco, 1999)

Therefore, how individuals perceive others is likely to be directly predictive of both SWB and EWB

Self- and other-perception

What about the interdependence of self- and other-perception? (e.g., Baumeister & Twenge, 2003; Mussweiler, 2003)

Unlikely that both positively biased self-perception and positive other-perception can both predict greater well-being

1. Clarify relationships between positively biased self-perception, SWB and EWB

1.a.Curvilinear relationship between positively biased self-perception and EWB?

Aims of research

2. Explore relationships between other-perception, SWB and EWB

3. Explore impact of other-perception on a social comparison measure of positively biased self-perception

Participants 133 ‘hardcopy’ respondents 23 e-mail respondents Aged 18 – 83 (M = 42.8 years; SD = 14.9) Female n = 99 (63.0%); Male n = 58 (37.0%) University educated n = 86 (54.8%) Married or living with partner n = 95 (60.5%) Children/ dependent relatives n = 73 (46.5%) English spoken at home n = 141 (89.9%) Non-English speakers’ average time in Australia of 26

years (range 10 – 47 years) Metropolitan residents n = 140 (89.2%)

Measures Demographic measure = age, sex,

dependent relatives or children at home, language spoken at home, number of years spent in Australia, postcode, highest level of education completed, relationship situation

SWB (Life Satisfaction) = Personal Wellbeing Index (International Wellbeing Group, 2006)

EWB (Self-actualisation) = Short Index of Self-Actualization (Jones & Crandall, 1986)

Measures Positive bias in self-perception 8 positive personality descriptors: “Friendly, Reliable, Imaginative, Interesting,

Considerate, Intelligent, Sincere, Humorous”

8 negative personality descriptors: “Unkind, Insecure, Dishonourable, Mean,

Dishonest, Phony, Deceitful, Liar” Participants asked to rate both themselves and

the “average” person of same age and sex Methodology adopted from Boyd-Wilson et al.,

2002; 2004.

Measures Bias in self-perception =

(∑Self positive - ∑Other positive) +

(∑Other negative - ∑Self negative)

Index > 0 = positively biased self-perception Index of 0 = absence of bias in self-perception Index < 0 = negatively biased self-perception

Measures

“Self-positivity” = ∑Self positive ratings - ∑Self negative ratings

“Other-positivity” = ∑Other positive ratings - ∑Other negative

ratings

Other-perception = Revised Philosophies of Human Nature Scale (Robinson, Shaver, & Wrightsman, 1992)

jbrooker
See underlined text in bullet point 1 in Notes section - should "of" be "and"?

Procedure

Measures were collated in the following order:1. Self-ratings on the 16 personality descriptors2. Personal Wellbeing Index 3. Ratings of others on the 16 personality

descriptors4. Short Index of Self-Actualization5. Revised Philosophies of Human Nature Scale6. Demographic information 56 (35.7%) respondents returned questionnaires

with sections 1 and 3 reversed

Results

SWB underwent a square root transformation and reflection to improve normality, therefore increased SWB scores indicate lower SWB

SWB and EWB r = -.23, p < .01(i.e., greater life satisfaction scores correlated with greater self-actualisation scores)

SWB and positively biased self-perception r = .05, p = ns

EWB and positively biased self-perception r = .07, p = ns

Results Self-positivity and other-positivity

r = .50, p < .001

Positively biased self-perception and self-positivity r = .22, p < .01

Positively biased self-perception and other-positivity

r = -.73, p < .001

Self-positivity and SWB r = -.46, p <.001 (i.e., SWB scores increased with greater self-positivity)

Self-positivity and EWB r = .27, p <.001

Results Apart from respondents’ age significantly

correlating with other-positivity (r = .32, p <.0001), none of the other demographic or procedural variables (i.e., questionnaire format or rating order) significantly correlated with the variables of interest

To partition variance as a function of age, sequential multiple regression was used for all subsequent analyses with age entered as step 1 of each analysis

Results

1. A sequential polynomial regression found no evidence of the hypothesised curvilinear (quadratic) relationship between positively biased self-perception and EWB

2. The sequential regression of age, trust and cynicism onto SWB found trust and cynicism predicted approx. 21% of variance in SWB (sr² = .21, p < .001); trust positively, and cynicism negatively correlating with greater SWB

Results3. The sequential regression of age, trust and

cynicism onto EWB found trust and cynicism predicted approx. 7% of variance in EWB (sr² = .07, p < .01); trust positively, and cynicism negatively correlating with greater EWB

4. The sequential regression of age, trust and cynicism onto positively biased self-perception found trust and cynicism predicted approx. 12% of variance in positively biased self-perception (sr² = .12, p < .001); trust negatively and cynicism positively correlating with positive bias in self-perception

Conclusions SWB and EWB significantly positively correlated

Positively biased self-perception, SWB and EWB non-significantly correlated

Positive other-perception (i.e., greater trust & less cynicism) significantly positively correlated with both SWB and EWB

Negative other-perception (i.e.,greater cynicism & less trust) significantly positively correlated with positively biased self-perception

Self-positivity significantly positively correlated with other-positivity, SWB and EWB

Limitations Exclusive reliance on self-report measures

Lack of control over test-taking behaviour

Causal interpretation not possible

Relationship between other-perception and well-being may be better accounted for by personality (e.g., agreeableness) or attachment style

ReferencesBaumeister, R. F. (1989). The optimal margin of illusion. Journal of Social and Clinical

Psychology, 8(2), 176-189.

Baumeister, R. F., & Twenge, J. M. (2003). The social self. Retrieved Jun-7-2007, from http://www.mrw.interscience.wiley.com.ezproxy.lib.monash.edu.au/hop/hop_contents_fs.html

Boyd-Wilson, B. M., McClure, J., & Walkey, F. H. (2004). Are wellbeing and illusory perceptions linked? The answer may be yes, but ... Australian Journal of Psychology, 56(1), 1-9.

Boyd-Wilson, B. M., Walkey, F. H., & McClure, J. (2002). Present and correct: We kid ourselves less when we live in the moment. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 691-702.

Cummins, R. A., Lau, A. L. D., & Davern, M. (Eds.). (in press). Homeostatic mechanisms and subjective wellbeing. New York: Springer.

Cummins, R. A., & Nistico, H. (2002). Maintaining life satisfaction: The role of positive cognitive bias. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3, 37-69.

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117-140.

ReferencesFujita, F., & Diener, E. (2005). Life satisfaction set point: Stability and change. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1), 158-164.

International Wellbeing Group. (2006). Personal Wellbeing Index: 4th Edition. Melbourne: Australian Centre on Quality of Life, Deakin University.

Jones, A., & Crandall, R. (1986). Validation of a short index of self-actualization. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12(1), 63-73.

Kwan, V. S. K., John, O. P., Kenny, D. A., Bond, M. H., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Reconceptualizing individual differences in self-enhancement bias: An interpersonal approach. Psychological Review, 111(1), 94-110.

Mikulincer, M., & Horesh, N. (1999). Adult attachment style and perception of others: The role of projective mechanisms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 1022-1034.

Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110(3), 472-489.

Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). Daily well-being: The role of autonomy, competence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(4), 419-435.

ReferencesRobinson, J. P., Shaver, P. R., & Wrightsman, L. S. (1991). Measures of personality and social

psychological attitudes (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press.

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141-166.

Ryff, C. D. (1989a). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explorations on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(6), 1069-1081.

Sacco, W. P. (1999). A social-cognitive model of interpersonal processes in depression. In T. Joiner & J. C. Coyne (Eds.), The interactional nature of depression: Advances in interpersonal approaches (pp. 329-362). Washington: American Psychological Association.

Sirgy, M. J., Michalos, A. C., Ferriss, A. L., Easterlin, R. A., Patrick, D., & Pavot, W. (2006). The quality-of-life (QOL) research movement: Past, present, and future. Social Indicators Research, 76, 343-466.

Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & McDowell, N. K. (2003). Portrait of a self-enhancer: Well adjusted and well liked or maladjusted and friendless? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(1), 165-176.

Waterman, A. S. (1993). Two conceptions of happiness: Contrasts of personal expressiveness (eudaimonia) and hedonic enjoyment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(4), 678-691.