sree chitra tirunal institute for medical sciences and...

61
1 SREE CHITRA TIRUNAL INSTITUTE FOR MEDICAL SCIENCES AND TECHNOLOGY THIRUVANANTHAPURAM, KERALA Demographic, clinical and syndromic characteristics of Frontotemporal dementia: A descriptive study. Thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the rules and regulations for DM Degree Examination of Sree Chitra Tirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology By Dr Himanshu Soni DM Neurology Resident Month and Year of Submission: October 2012 Department of Neurology Sree Chitra Tirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology Thiruvananthapuram

Upload: others

Post on 10-Feb-2021

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 1

    SREE CHITRA TIRUNAL INSTITUTE FOR MEDICAL SCIENCES AND TECHNOLOGY

    THIRUVANANTHAPURAM, KERALA

    Demographic, clinical and syndromic characteristics of

    Frontotemporal dementia: A descriptive study.

    Thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the rules and regulations for DM

    Degree Examination of Sree Chitra Tirunal Institute for Medical Sciences

    and Technology

    By

    Dr Himanshu Soni

    DM Neurology Resident

    Month and Year of Submission: October 2012

    Department of Neurology

    Sree Chitra Tirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology

    Thiruvananthapuram

  • 2

    2010-2012

    DECLARATION

    I, Dr. Himanshu Soni, hereby declare that the projects in this book were

    undertaken by me under the supervision of the faculty, Department of

    Neurology, Sree Chitra Tirunal Institute for Medical Sciences and Technology.

    Thiruvananthapuram Dr. Himanshu Soni

    Date:

    Forwarded

    The candidate, Dr. Himanshu Soni, has carried out the minimum required

    project.

    Thiruvananthapuram Prof. Dr. Muralidharan Nair

    Date: Professor & Head, Dept of Neurology

  • 3

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENT

    I take this opportunity to express my sincere gratitude to Dr.P S Mathuranath,

    Additional Professor of Neurology and Cognitive Sciences expert, SCTIMST, my guide for

    the study, for his expert guidance, constant review, kind help and keen interest at each

    and every step during the completion of the study

    I am thankful to Dr.M.D.Nair, Professor and Head, Department of Neurology for his

    guidance, encouragement and valuable suggestions during the period of the study.

    I am extremely thankful to the CBNC team comprising of Neuropsychologists, Doctoral

    students, and Medical social worker for their valuable input and assistance to the study.

    I express my sincere thanks to Dr.Ravi Prasad Varma, Assistant Professor, Achutha

    Menon Centre for Health Science Studies for helping me with the statistical analysis of

    this study.

    Last but not the least, I extend my gratitudes to all my patients and their primary

    caregivers who participated in this study.

    Dr Himanshu Soni

    Senior Resident

    Department of Neurology

    SCTIMST, Trivandrum, Kerala

  • 4

    INDEX

    i. Introduction 1

    ii. Review of Literature 3

    iii. Aims of the Study 14

    iv. Materials and Methods 15

    v. Results 20

    vi. Discussion 36

    vii. Conclusions 41

    viii. References 45

    ix. Annexure 52

  • 5

    INTRODUCTION

    FTD encompasses a group of neurodegenerative diseases characterized by focal atrophy of

    frontal and anterior temporal lobes and non-Alzheimer pathology. In people under 65 years of

    age, FTD is as common as Alzheimer‟s disease (AD) and its prevalence has been estimated in

    15 per 100,000 patients between 45 to 64 years of age (Ratnavalli et al, 2002)

    Data on Frontotemporal dementia (FTD) from India is scarce in medical literature despite the

    projected rise in dementia prevalence in the subcontinent. A Pubmed/Medline search on

    publications on FTD revealed only few case reports and review articles.(Mohandas E et al,

    2009)

    There has been extensive research about the epidemiological features of Alzheimer disease,

    which has helped direct therapeutic approaches. Until recently, frontotemporal lobar

    degeneration (FTLD) was considered to be rare and difficult to diagnose, (Varma et al, 1999)

    and relatively little is known about the demographic features. Recent studies (Ratnavalli et al,

    2002) suggest that FTLD is the second most common diagnosis of dementia in individuals

    younger than 65 years. Many factors have limited research into FTLD. In particular, the size

    of FTLD cohorts at any one center is modest. In addition, until recently, low diagnostic

    accuracy for FTLD and related disorders has diminished enthusiasm for epidemiological

    research about this neurodegenerative disease. Finally, collaborative studies have not been

    possible because definitions of FTLD have varied between sites, thereby limiting the ability

    to combine cohorts. Only a few reports have explored the demographic features of FTLD. It

    is well accepted that FTLD is a presenile dementia with a strong genetic component, (Chow

    et al, 1999) but other risk factors for FTLD remain largely unexplored.

  • 6

    The recent establishment of consensus criteria for FTLD represents an opportunity to begin

    large-scale studies about the demographic features of FTLD. These criteria divide FTLD into

    3 major subgroups: frontotemporal dementia (FTD), semantic dementia, and progressive

    nonfluent aphasia (PNFA). Thus, this study analyzes the demographic features of a small

    group of patients diagnosed as having FTLD at a tertiary dementia referral clinic.

    The FrSBe (Grace J et al, 2001) is a 46-item behavior rating scale that is intended to measure

    behavior associated with damage to the frontal systems of the brain. The FrSBe has been

    demonstrated to have good reliability and is sensitive to behaviour change in focal frontal

    lesions when compared with patients with posterior lesions (Paulsen J et al 1996). The FrSBe

    has also been shown to be sensitive to behavior changes in AD, Huntington‟s disease,

    Parkinson‟s disease, vascular dementia, and mild cognitive impairment. We predicted that the

    FrSBe would detect the behavior changes that typify FTD, particularly the apathetic and

    disinhibited behaviors commonly displayed by patients with this disorder.

  • 7

    REVIEW OF LITERATURE

    Historical perspective:

    “Circumscribed atrophies” were first described by the Czech-German neurologist and

    psychiatrist Arnold Pick, who in 1892 reported the case of a 71-year-oldman with behavioral

    symptoms and a progressive aphasia associated with focal left temporal lobe atrophy (Pick,

    1892). Pick reported several other cases of circumscribed atrophies presenting with combined

    behavioral and language disturbances. In one initially unpublished case with a frontal

    syndrome, Alois Alzheimer observed swollen neurons and the argyrophilic “kugel”, later

    known as Pick‟s body. Onari and Spatz performed extensive pathological studies on Pick‟s

    case examined by Alzheimer and on similar cases. Significantly, Onari and Spatz noted that

    in many cases characteristic “Pick bodies”, were entirely absent, without altering the

    remainder of the clinical or pathologic picture.

    Another salient observation was that cases could be divided into those with primarily frontal

    or primarily temporal atrophy. Tissot and Constantinidis confirmed these data and recognized

    three types of so-called “Pick disease” (clinically defined): (A) with swollen neurons and

    Pick bodies, (B) with only swollen neurons and (C) without swollen neurons or Pick bodies

    (Constantinidis et al., 1974).

    In 1982, Marsel Mesulam coined the term “primary progressive aphasia” to describe patients

    with focal left hemisphere degeneration who presented with either non-fluent or fluent

    aphasia, initially without additional cognitive deficits (Mesulam, 1982). In the late 1970s,

    Arne Brun and Lars Gustafson in Sweden, and later David Neary and Julie Snowden in the

    UK, described the clinical, neuroimaging and pathologic features of a dementia syndrome

  • 8

    characterized by focal frontal lobe degeneration, coined “frontal lobe dementia of the non-

    Alzheimer‟s type” by the Swedish group (Brun, 1987), and “dementia of the frontal type”

    by the British group (Neary et al., 1988). Snowden and John Hodges described the language

    disturbance in patients with focal left anterior temporal atrophy, a syndrome they called

    “semantic dementia” (Snowden et al., 1989).

    Pathologically, these syndromes were all characterized by a typical gross and microscopic

    appearance, the absence of Alzheimer‟s pathology, and the variable presence of Pick bodies.

    In 1994 the Swedish and British groups published the first consensus clinical and

    neuropathological criteria for “frontotemporal dementia” (Clinical and neuropathological

    criteria for frontotemporal dementia; The Lund and Manchester Groups, 1994). The clinical

    criteria were refined in 1998, using the term “frontotemporal lobar degeneration” (FTLD) to

    encompass three distinct clinical syndromes: frontotemporal dementia (FTD), a primary

    disturbance of behavior and comportment; progressive non-fluent aphasia (PNFA), a

    disturbance of expressive language; and semantic dementia (semantic dementia), a syndrome

    characterized by loss of meaning of verbal and non-verbal concepts (Neary et al., 1998).

    Further insight into the biology of the disease was provided by Kirk Wilhelmsen and

    colleagues, who discovered the link between familial frontotemporal dementia and a locus on

    chromosome 17 containing the gene for the microtubule-associated protein The subsequent

    unveiling of multiple tau mutations associated with various FTLD phenotypes (Foster et al.,

    1997; Clark et al., 1998; Hutton et al., 1998) has strengthened the link between FTLD and

    other “tauopathies”, such as corticobasal degeneration (CBD) and progressive supranuclear

    palsy (PSP) (Kertesz et al., 1999). While a link between motor-neuron disease and FTLD had

    long been suspected (Hudson et al., 1993), this association was found to be much more

    common than previously thought (Lomen-Hoerth et al., 2002), and to be associated with a

    distinctive pathologic and genetic (Hosler et al., 2000) profile.

  • 9

    Clinical features of FTD:

    Patients with FTD display a heterogeneous clinical picture, which may include behavioral,

    cognitive, and motor manifestations.(Joseph KA et al, 2008) However, based on the

    predominant initial symptoms, FTD can be readily separated into two groups: the behavioral

    variant (bv -FTD), which is characterized by loss of insight, personality changes, and

    disturbances in social cognition(Neary et al,1998) and the language variant, also referred as

    primary progressive aphasia (PPA).(Mesulam 1982) The latter can be further divided into a

    well-defined clinical-pathological entity, semantic dementia (SD),(Hodges JR, 2007) and

    progressive nonfluent aphasia (PNFA).

    Despite this classification, there is a clinical, pathological, and genetic overlap. For instance,

    SD cases may develop features of bv-FTD,(Rosen HJ et al,2006) and patients with the

    clinical variant often have common areas of brain atrophy and family history of another

    variant. Moreover, there is increasing evidence of overlap between FTD and other

    neurodegenerative disease, notably Motor Neuron disease (MND),(Bak TJ et al, 2001)

    Progressive Supranuclear Palsy (PSP), and Corticobasal degeneration (CBD).(Beove BF et al

    2003), For example, cases initially diagnosed as PNFA may end up showing a clinical

    picture and pathology of CBD.[Kertesz et al,2003) Indeed, some argue that those entities

    should all be included under the rubric of Pick‟s complex.

  • 10

    Differentiating one variant of FTD from another, as well as from other neurodegenerative and

    nondegenerative diseases (particularly psychiatric conditions) remains challenging.(Rosness

    TA et al, 2008) Fortunately, recent advances in molecular pathology and genetics, improved

    imaging techniques, and better clinical descriptions have contributed enormously to our

    understanding of these conditions and are offering new insights, which we hope will be

    helpful for improved diagnosis and management of patients with these devastating disorders.

    Clinical Features

    Behavioral variant (bvFTD): The clinical hallmark of bvFTD is a disturbance in the

    personality and behavior, with changes of mood, motivation, and inhibition, leading to

    profound social disruption.(Boxer AL et al, 2005) As the initial symptoms are

    neuropsychiatric, without impairment on cognitive screening tests, or overt changes on

  • 11

    structural imaging, these patients may be inappropriately diagnosed as suffering from a

    psychiatric disease, usually, depression or personality disorder.(Rosness TA et al, 2008)

    These changes become gradually evident to relatives, colleagues, and friends, because of

    disruption in their work performance, social, and family relationships. The effect on care is

    vivid with a high level of burden and stress.(Mioshi E et al,2007) Patients may perform

    normally on standard neuropsychological tests of memory, language, attention, and visual

    spatial ability, but more recent tests designed to assess emotion processing, social cognition,

    theory of mind,(Gregory C, 2002) and complex decision making are more sensitive and may

    show deficits in early cases, even if standard cognitive battery are normal.(Wittenberg et al

    ,2008)

    In order to simplify the clinical picture, this myriad of neuropsychiatric manifestations may

    be classified in three main groups

    Positive symptoms: These include disinhibition with lack of concern about social norms or

    embarrassment, impulsivity, outburst of violence, stereotypic and ritualistic behavior,

    abnormal appetite for sweets or gluttony, and impaired emotional judgment. When they are

    present, they strongly suggest the diagnosis of FTD.(Rascovsky K et al; 2007, Wittenberg et

    al ,2008)

    Negative symptoms: These include apathy and inertia, emotional blunting, impaired insight,

    lack of interest in usual or leisure activities, decline in the amount of speech (adynamism or

    laconic speech), and reduce self-care for complex instrumental activities.( Isabelle Le Ber et

    al; 2006) These symptoms are less specific to FTD and also occur in depression.

    Cognitive symptoms: These often appear later and include mental rigidity; loss of flexibility

    and abstraction; impairment in the pragmatic level of the discourse, with disorganization and

    distractibility; and poor planning and organization. At this stage, most patients fail in

    executive tasks and may show frontal release sign, such as grasping.(Libon DJ et al; 2007)

  • 12

    This cluster of symptoms (positive, negative, and dysexecutive) have putative anatomical

    correlate to the orbitofrontal, medial, and dorsolateral frontal cortices,

    respectively.(Cummings JL;1995), Moreover, the progression of the atrophy, and

    consequently the clinical manifestations, may follow a predictable fashion, beginning in

    orbitofrontal and medial aspects of frontal cortex, and then involving the dorsolateral cortex

    and temporal anterior structures and basal ganglia.(Kril JJ et al;2004, Perry RJ et al;2006] It

    has become increasingly apparent that some patients presenting with symptoms suggestive of

    bvFTD fall to progress even over many years.(Davies RR et al;2006) These “phenocopy”

    cases may actually constitute an unusual presentation of other conditions, such as late onset

    of bipolar disorder, personality disorder, or Asperger spectrum disorder. A number of

    features set apart these nonprogressive patients; normal performance on test of executive

    function and of emotion processing; better activities of daily living; and absence of brain

    atrophy. The underlying pathophysiology in this group is unclear, but we assume that it

    reflects function disruption of orbitomesial frontal regions in the absence of neurodegeneration

    and importantly a lack of atrophy on MRI and hypometabolism on FDC-PET.

    Semantic dementia

    Patients typically present with “loss of memory for words” and show impairment on tests of

    word comprehension, although the underlying deficit is the amodal store of semantic memory

    or knowledge about words, objects, people, and sounds.(Hodges JR; 2007) Patients show a

    gradual reduction of vocabulary and use high frequency terms (thing, boy), although speech

    is fluent and well-articulated, without phonological or syntactic errors.(Knibb JA et al;2005)

    A consistent feature is the impairment of naming objects or anomia. The performance is

    influenced by the level of familiarity and specificity of items asked. In other words, if the

    item is extensively encountered by the patient, it is likely to be forgotten later. Likewise, the

    patient will tend to name objects that are prototypic of their category. For instance, patients

  • 13

    are able to name cat, dog, and horse, but not tiger or zebra, and use superordinate or general

    labels, calling the latter also a cat and horse, and may be just animal. Impairment of single

    word comprehension can be assessed by asking the patient to match the word with the

    corresponding object or to define the meaning of words.(Knibb JA et al, 2005)] There is a

    striking dissociation between repetition (preserved) and meaning (loss), best demonstrated by

    asking patients to repeat words such as “hippopotamus” or “catastrophe” and then to say what

    they mean. These patients also show surface dyslexia where all words are read according to

    general rules of pronunciation, regardless of word meaning. In spite of the fact that word-

    based tests show clear deficits, actually there is a deterioration of central, amodal knowledge

    about objects or people which is apparent when nonverbal tests, such as “Pyramid and Palms

    test ” are employed, which requires subjects to match the two pictures that go together. Some

    SD patients present prominent deficits in identification of famous people. This deficit

    represents a general impairment on the “knowledge of people” than merely prosopagnosia

    (i.e., loss of ability to recognize faces), since patients are unable to produce any information

    from their name or voice. Such patients typically show predominant right temporal atrophy

    together with behavioral symptoms and poor insight.(Chan D et al; 2009)

    Progressive Nonfluent Aphasia

    Unlike SD, the presenting features of PNFA are more varied and may reflect breakdown at

    various stages of speech production, from alterations in lexical retrieval, misarrangements of

    the words according to grammatical rules, or impaired motor programming of the intended

    utterance.(Ash S et al;2009) Generally speaking, there are problems with the syntactic or

    motor aspects of speech, causing speech to be halting, slow, and distorted. Severe

    agrammatism causes oversimplification of the language production, lack of function words

    (e.g., prepositions, auxiliary verbs, or articles), or words inflections (i.e., endings of verb or

    noun according to conjugation or number, respectively).(Hodges JR et al;1996). But in the

  • 14

    early stages, grammatical errors are subtle and may be difficult to distinguish from common

    errors or detect in a short interview. Syntactic problems are usually best assessed by testing

    sentence comprehension. Breakdown in the motor programming is referred to as apraxia of

    speech, which causes distortion of output with pauses, speech errors, and loss of melody.

    Patients may have difficulty in repeating or pronouncing polysyllabic words and strings of

    syllables (such as Pa- Ta-Ka), producing distortions and aprosodic intonation. Orobuccal

    apraxia may develop and some patients evolve into a picture of CBD or PSP.

    A third language variant: Logopenic/phonological

    Gorno-Tempini et al. described cases of language variant that fulfilled neither SD nor PFNA

    criteria.(Gorno-Tempini ML, et al; 2004) These cases showed reduced speech output with

    frequent pauses and impaired naming; preservation of grammar, motor speech, and semantic

    knowledge. A remarkable feature was profound impairment on repetition of sentences or

    string of words and difficulties in understanding complex instructions, despite of sparing

    single word repetition and comprehension. This has been attributed to a reduction of working

    memory resources, due to impairment of phonological loop. Interesting enough, this group

    showed a distinctive pattern of brain atrophy that involved the left temporoparietal junction.

    There is growing evidence that the underlying pathology is Alzheimer‟s disease, suggesting

    that this variant is in fact, an atypical presentation of AD.( Rabinovici GD et al; 2008)

    Imaging

    The advent of high resolution MRI and of methods of automated qualification such as Voxel-

    based morphometry (VBM) and cortical thickness measures has enhanced our knowledge of

    the anatomical changes in the variants of FTD. Patients with bv-FTD show atrophy of the

    orbitobasal and medialfrontal lobes, together with anterior temporal and insular

    involvement.(Rosen HJ, et al;2002). SD is associated with atrophy of the anterior temporal

    lobe involving particularly polar, anterior parahippocampal, and fusiform regions including

  • 15

    the perirhinal cortex. The atrophy is bilateral, but typically asymmetric and often more severe

    on the left. In PNFA, the changes are subtler and involve the left inferior frontal lobe and

    anterior insula cortex. In logopenic/phonological variant the atrophy involves the left

    hemisphere, particularly the posterior temporal lobe (superior and middle temporal gyri) and

    inferior parietal lobe and lesser involvement of the precuneus. These changes can also be

    detected using simpler MRI-based visual rating scales, which simply use standard coronal

    cuts. These scales aid diagnosis and monitoring of progression.

    Pathology

    Definitive diagnosis of FTD requires neuropathological examination. Unlike other dementia

    syndromes, notably AD, FTD encompasses considerable pathological heterogeneity.(Cairns

    NJ et al; 2007) The classification is based on the identification of intracellular protein

    inclusions by means of inmunohistochemistry. Accordingly, three broad subdivisions have

    been recognized:

    FTD with tau-positive inclusions: This includes classic Pick‟s disease, Progressive

    Supranuclear Palsy, Corticobasal degeneration, argyrophilic grain disease, and patients with

    mutation of the Microtubuleassociated protein tau (MAPT) gene on chromosome 17 (FTDP-

    17).

    FTD with tau-negative, ubiquitin-positive inclusions: This is the commonest pathological

    finding in FTD and includes these with progranulin gene mutations. The ubiquitinated protein

    has been identified as the transactive response DNA-binding protein with Mr 43 (TDP-43)

    which is also found in MND, (Roeber S et al; 2008) strengthening the association between

    FTD and MND. Interestingly, cases with ubiquitinated lesions without TDP-43 have been

    recently identified, which appear to have abnormal deposits of another protein called fused in

    sarcoma protein (FUS).

  • 16

    Dementia lacking distinctive histology. Includes cases that do not show any particular or

    distinctive inclusion or histology, besides neuronal loss, superficial spongiosis, and gliosis.

    With the advent of newer inmunohistochemical techniques such cases are now increasingly

    rare. The correspondence between clinical phenotype and underlying pathological subtype

    has been a topic of considerable interest in recent years. SD has been consistently associated

    with Ubiquitin (TDP-43) positive pathology. In contrast, PNFA cases show variable results in

    different clinicopathology series, due probably to different diagnostic criteria employed as

    well as inclusion of logopenic cases in older series. Despite that, PNFA more often associated

    to tau pathology, particularly if there is motor speech disorder. In bv-FTD approximately a

    half has tau-positive and the other TDP-43-positive pathology.

    Genetic

    Around 40% of patients report a family history of dementia, although in many instances this

    is almost certainly unrelated, but 10–20% have a clear pattern of autosomal dominant

    inheritance, with at least two relatives having young onset dementia or MND.(Seelaar H, et

    al. 2008) The heritability, however, varies according to the variant FTD: SD showing the

    least, whereas bv-FTD and FTD with MND the most inheritable. The commonest identified

    mutations are MAPT and progranuline (PGRN), both in chromosome 17q21. (Baker M, et al;

    2006) Although the prevalence of mutations varies among studies, the two mutations have a

    similar frequency, being found in around 5–10% of patients. Other mutations involve the

    Valosincontaining protein (VCP) and CHMP2B genes, but are very rare. The mutations of

    MAPT lead to abnormal intracellular accumulation of hyperphosphorylated tau. While

    mutation of PGRN gene results in reduced expression of progranulin and is associated with

    Ubiquitin-TDP-43 pathology. MAPT mutations typically give rise to the clinical phenotype

    called Frontotemporal dementia and parkinsonism linked to Chromosome 17 (FTDP-17). As

    its name suggests, the clinical picture embraces isolated behavioral and personality changes,

  • 17

    initial extrapyramidal signs, suggesting of PSP or CBD, or a combination of behavioral and

    extrapyramidal syndromes. In contrast, PGRN mutations appear to produce striking clinical

    heterogeneity which includes CBD, PNFA, or bv-FTD. Moreover, some cases depict an

    anmestic syndrome compatible with initial AD and a logopenic aphasia.

  • 18

    AIMS OF THE STUDY

    1. To study and compare demographic, neurological characteristics of patients in the 3

    FTD syndromes i.e. behavioural FTD, PNFA and Semantic Dementia

    2. To objectively study and compare various behavioural characteristics and its

    progression in FTD patients using a five point Likert rating scale (FrSBe).

  • 19

    MATERIALS AND METHODS

    Study Setting and Patient selection

    This study is a ambidirectional cohort study which was carried out at SCTIMST, Trivandrum,

    Kerala, India. Study cases were selected from the patients visiting the Cognition and

    Behavioral Neurology Center (CBNC), Department of Neurology which is a tertiary referral

    speciality clinic running once a week for neurocognitive problems and it caters to patients

    with amnesia, mild cognitive impairment, dementia, aphasia, neuropsychiatry syndromes and

    related conditions. The study had the approval of the Institutional Ethics Committee.

    We selected consecutive patients who met the criteria of Neary et al for FTD. Data was

    collected from patients which were assessed between January 1, 2005, and August, 2012.

    Data of patients from 2005 to 2009 was collected retrospectively from our medical records

    department and the Neuropsychology testing unit. Whenever possible, their follow up data

    was screened and if required next of kin/ primary caregiver were contacted on phone or mail

    to know the current status of the individual. Data was collected prospectively 2010 onwards.

    Diagnosis and Diagnostic criteria

    The diagnosis was based on historical information, neurological examination, and

    neuropsychological assessment and supported by findings on magnetic resonance imaging.

    Patients in whom the clinical diagnosis was equivocal were excluded from the study. The

    inclusion criteria for this study were based on the „Neary‟ criteria (Neary et al., 1998), The

    criteria focussed on a predominance of a frontal or temporal lobe cognitive/behavioural

    syndrome and the absence or insignificance of an anterograde amnesia and visuospatial

    impairment in the initial clinical presentations (i.e. within the first 2 years of symptoms). In

  • 20

    addition, all patients were required to have imaging studies demonstrating focal cerebral

    atrophy of at least one of the following: the anterior temporal lobes, frontal lobes and insula

    or caudate nuclei. The criteria have been previously reported (Knopman et al., 2007).

    Patients were separated on clinical grounds into those showing a predominantly behavioural

    presentation (bvFTD) and those presenting with language impairment (PNFA, Semantic

    dementia). The bvFTD group was further classified into those with disinhibited, distractible

    picture (DbvFTD) and those showing an apathetic, inert pattern (AbvFTD).

    Neuropsychological testing: The clinical diagnosis was based on the neurologic and

    physical examination results, medical history, informant interview, a neuropsychological

    evaluation, laboratory screening, and brain imaging. The Mini-Mental State Examination

    (MMSE) was administered to all patients. The neuropsychological tests administered were a

    1-hour neuropsychological battery that measures memory, language, executive function,

    visuospatial skills, and praxis.

    Age at onset was queried as part of the clinical interview, and is defined as the age at which

    the first change in cognition or behavior is noted by the caregiver or the patient. Education

    was coded as the number of years of formal education. The diagnosis was determined by

    consensus, including the neurologist and/or psychiatrist and neuropsychologist.

    The exclusion criteria (Knopman et al., 2007) were common to all three syndromes and

    included the following:

    (i) The syndrome is due to cerebrovascular disease.

    (ii) The syndrome is due to traumatic brain injury.

    (iii) By clinical history or neuropsychological testing, anterograde amnesia is a principal

    symptom or sign.

  • 21

    (iv) By clinical history or neuropsychological testing, visuospatial deficits are principal

    symptoms/signs.

    (v) Rapid eye movement sleep behaviour disorder is present.

    (vi) Imaging findings are diagnostic of another neurological disease, including the presence

    of lacunar infarction.

    (vii) Prior history dating from early adulthood of schizophrenia, bipolar disease, mental

    retardation and severe personality disorder.

    (viii) The clear, unequivocal presence of another neurological disease such as Parkinson‟s

    disease, Huntington‟s disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, multiple sclerosis; an inherited

    disorders such as metachromatic leukodystrophy, or any other defined neurological disorder

    other than an FTD.

    Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe): The FrSBe (Grace J et al, 2001) is a 46-item

    behavior rating scale of the Likert type that is intended to measure behavior associated with

    damage to the frontal systems of the brain. 12 items/questions pertain to apathy, 15 items

    assess disinhibition and 19 items deal with executive dysfunction. Minimun score is 46 and

    maximum is 230 which is later converted to a T score based on normative data for age,

    gender. According to the manual, the FrSBe was designed (a) to be brief, reliable, and valid;

    (b) to assess adult behavior before (ie, premorbid baseline) and after frontal systems damage

    occurs; and (c) to permit multiple observers to provide behavior ratings. It consists of two

    rating forms: a self-rating form to be completed by the patient and a family rating form to be

    completed by an informant who has regular contact with the patient, such as spouse, child,

    relative, or significant other. Professionals (eg, rehabilitation staff) seeking to track patient

    behavior over time also can use the family form to rate a patient‟s current behavior. Each

    FrSBe form yields a Total score and scores for subscales measuring Apathy, Disinhibition,

    and Executive Dysfunction. Scores are obtained on each scale for baseline behavior and

  • 22

    current behavior. Thus, behavior change can be indexed by comparing T scores based on

    normative data of the ratings of prior and current patient behavior.

    Reliability: High internal consistency has been demonstrated in multiple studies. The FrSBe

    manual(Grace J et al, 2001) reports a coefficients of 0.92, 0.78, 0.80, and 0.87 for the Total,

    Apathy, Disinhibition, and Executive scales of the family form and 0.88, 0.72, 0.75, and 0.79

    for the self-form in a normative sample.

  • 23

    Biostatistics

    -Data was summarized as mean, median or percentages with range or standard deviation for

    dispersion. Fisher‟ exact test was performed to compare groups of small size.

    -From the FrSBe main and subscores, composite Z scores were calculated and using the Z

    score, T score for the main and subscores was calculated using the following formula:

    T score = 10 x(Z score) + 50.

    -T scores were used for further analysis for comparing between diagnostic subgroups which

    was done using one way ANOVA (Analysis of Variance). A p value of < 0.05 was taken as

    significant. Post hoc analysis of the subgroups was carried out using the Scheffe test.

    -SPSS (15.0 for PC) was used to analyze the correlation between FrSBe subscales and

    demographical and behavioral variables included in the model and to carry out statistical

    comparisons between diagnostic subgroups.

  • 24

    RESULTS

    Out of the 65 patients initially suspected to have FTD in the CBNC Clinic, 22 (33.8%) cases

    were excluded, as on further evaluation in the form of Neuroimaging, Neuropsychological

    testing, treatment response, these 22 cases had an alternate diagnosis as mentioned in Fig 1.

    65 patients suspected to have FTD on

    screening

    43 cases included in study

    35 Behavior variant

    8 Language variant

    22 cases excluded:

    8 mixed, 4 Alzheimer's,

    3 Pseudodementia, 2 MND

    1 each of meningioma, Acom

    artery aneurysm, NPH, CJD

    Craniopharyngioma, PSP,

    Amnestic MCI

    Data of 20 cases collected retrospectively and 23 cases prospectively

    Figure 1: Flow chart showing initial data collection and excluded cases. MND- Motor Neuron Disease, NPH- Normal Pressure Hydrocephalus, CJD- Creudzfeldt Jacob

    Disease, PSP- Progressive Supranuclear Palsy, MCI- Minimal Cognitive Impairment

  • 25

    Out of the 43 cases included in the study, 35 (81%) were of the behavioural variant of FTD

    (bvFTD). Of the 8 patients which were labelled as language variant of FTD, 6 patients had

    PNFA and 2 had Semantic Dementia. Figure 2 depicts the patient classification.

    35 (81%)

    6 (14%)

    2 (9%)

    Number of patients(%)

    bvFTD

    PNFA

    SD

    Total FTD Patients= 43

    Figure 2 : bvFTD- Behavioral variant, PNFA- Progressive Non Fluent Aphasia, SD- Semantic Dementia

  • 26

    Figure 3: AbvFTD- Apathetic variant, DbvFTD- Disinhibited variant

    The behavioral variant cases of FTD were further divided into 3 clinical profiles i.e Apathetic

    (AbvFTD), Disinhibited (DbvFTD) and mixed (mixed bvFTD). Out of 35 bvFTD cases, 13

    (37%) were AbvFTD, 14 (40%) were DbvFTD and 8 cases (23 %) were of mixed bvFTD

    type. Figure 3 above shows the subtype distribution of the bvFTD cases.

    1314

    8

    0

    2

    4

    6

    8

    10

    12

    14

    16

    AbvFTD DbvFTD Mixed bvFTD

    bvFTD subtypes

    No. of patients

  • 27

    Figure 4 : Number of FTD cases diagnosed per year along with total no. of new

    dementia/MCI patients seen in the CBNC Clinic. The Annualized hospital incidence rate

    was 4.83 new cases/year

    Table 1: Annualized hospital incidence rate = 4.83 per 100

    As mentioned previously, the cases included in the study were taken over a time period from

    2005 to 2012. An average of 111 new patients attended the CBNC Clinic per year from 2005

    to 2012 apart from the follow up cases and the number of FTD cases diagnosed per year

    ranged from 3 to 10 cases per annum. The annualized hospital incidence rate was 4.83 per

    100 dementia/MCI cases i.e approximately 5 cases of FTD were diagnosed per year which is

    shown in Figure 4 and Table 1

    9098

    115110

    99

    125130

    126

    6 5 4 4 4 3 7 100

    20

    40

    60

    80

    100

    120

    140

    2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012

    No

    . of

    pat

    ien

    ts

    Year

    new dementia/MCI

    FTD

    2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012

    Total New patients 90 98 115 110 99 125 130 126

    FTD patients 6 5 4 4 4 3 7 10

    Hospital incidence

    rate 6.67 5.10 3.48 3.64 4.04 2.40 5.38 7.94

  • 28

    Gender:

    Of the 43 cases, 28 (65%) were male and 15 (35%) were female. Similar distribution of

    gender was seen in the bvFTD and PNFA subtype. There was one each patient in the

    semantic dementia subtype. (Table 2)

    Diagnosis No. of cases Male (%) Female (%)

    bvFTD 35 23 (65%) 12 (35%)

    PNFA 6 4 (66%) 2 (34%)

    Semantic dementia 2 1 (50 %) 1 (50%)

    Total 43 28 (65%) 15 (35%)

    Table 2: Gender distribution of the study population.

    Age at Onset:

    Age of onset ranged from 29 to 79 years. The youngest case was of a 29 yr old male

    diagnosed with apathetic variant of FTD who had initial presentation as impairment of

    attention without any family history and neuroimaging (MRI Brain) showed characteristic

    bifrontal and temporal atrophy. In this study 11 of 43 (25%) had onset of disease symptoms

    beyond 65 yrs age. Among the variants of FTD, the behavioral variant showed a similar

    proportion of early vs late onset {77 % (27 of 35) patients had early onset, 23 % ( 8 of 35)

    had late onset dementia}.

    Figure 5 shows the proportion of early Vs late onset FTD in the total study population as

    well as in the behavioral and the language variant.

  • 29

    Figure 5: 25 % (11 out of 43) of total cases had onset of illness at > 65 yrs age.

    FTD type

    N Mean Std. Deviation Minimum Maximum

    ANOVA p

    value

    bvFTD 35 55.94 10.541 29 79

    0.334 PNFA 6 57.33 12.291 41 76

    SD 2 67.50 .707 67 68

    Total 43 56.67 10.679 29 79

    Table 3: Analysis of each FTD subtype on the basis of age of onset

    Table 3 shows the age of onset data of the study population, the mean age of onset for the

    entire study population was 56.67 years with a standard deviation of 10.5 years. Mean age of

    onset for bvFTD, PNFA & SD was 55.94, 57.33 and 67.5 respectively. On applying one way

    32

    27

    5

    0

    11

    8

    1 2

    0

    5

    10

    15

    20

    25

    30

    35

    total bvFTD PNFA SD

    Presenile Vs Senile onset

    < 65 yrs

    > 65 yrs

  • 30

    ANOVA, there was no statistical significance between the age of onset of the three main

    types of FTD although mean age of onset of Semantic dementia was later as compared to that

    of bvFTD. Even on subgroup analysis among patients of bvFTD (apathetic vs disinhibited vs

    mixed bvFTD) statistical significance was not reached (Table 4)

    FTD Subtype N Mean

    Std. Deviation

    (Std D) Minimum Maximum

    ANOVA p

    value

    A-bvFTD 13 53.23 11.973 29 75 0.476

    D-bvFTD 13 56.77 9.139 47 69

    Mixed 9 58.67 10.512 50 79

    Total 35 55.94 10.541 29 79

    Table 4: Subgroup analysis of bvFTD patients according to age of onset

    Onset to Diagnosis:

    The range of duration of onset of symptoms to diagnosis of the syndrome was 1 to 7 years

    with mean time interval from onset of symptoms to diagnosis was 3.14 years (Std D of 1.65).

    Among the broad type ( bvFTD, PNFA, SD), largest time interval was for the SD group pf

    patients i.e 4 years. Among the bvFTD subtype, mean interval to diagnosis ranged from 2.62

    yrs (for DbvFTD) and 3.67 yrs (for mixed bvFTD). There was no statistically significant

    difference in the time interval to diagnosis either between the broad syndrome or the subtypes

    of bvFTD. Table 5 & 6.

  • 31

    FTD N Mean Std. Deviation Minimum Maximum

    ANOVA p

    value

    bvFTD 35 3.14 1.734 1 7

    0.699

    (NS)

    PNFA 6 2.83 1.472 1 5

    SD 2 4.00 .000 4 4

    Total 43 3.14 1.656 1 7

    Table 5: mean interval from onset to diagnosis of FTD type. ( NS- not significant)

    N Mean

    Std.

    Deviation Minimum Maximum

    ANOVA

    p value Subtype

    AbvFTD 13 3.31 1.653 1 6

    0.353

    (NS)

    DbvFTD 13 2.62 1.938 1 7

    Mixed

    bvFTD

    9 3.67 1.500 1 5

    Total 35 3.14 1.734 1 7

    Table 6: mean interval from onset to diagnosis of bvFTD subtype

    Education, Handedness, Family History:

    Mean education years of the study population was 10.8 years with range from 2 to 18 years.

    Mean years of education for the bvFTD and the language FTD group was 10.02 and 14.12

    years respectively. Since the number of SD & PNFA cases were not high, this difference was

    not statistically significant. Handedness information was available in 40/43 patients and all of

    them were right handed. Family history was available in 37 of 43 patients (86%). Three

    patients of 37 (8.1%) had a positive family history of which 2 had bvFTD and one had

    PNFA.

  • 32

    Neurological signs:

    At the time of inclusion, a parkinsonian syndrome was present in 18% of the patients. An

    akinetorigid form (60%) was more frequent than rest tremor (40%). Other movement

    disorders were less common (8%). The most frequent was postural tremor, present in 3% of

    the patients. Oculomotor disturbances were noted in 5% of the patients and consisted mainly

    in slow saccades (4%) and up-gaze limitation (1%), after verification that none of these

    patients had parkinsonian syndrome or fulfilled the international consensus diagnosis criteria

    for progressive supranuclear palsy (Litvan et al., 1996).

    Motor Neuron Disease was seen in two patients of the 43 cases (4.65%), spinal form was

    seen in one patient which was of the disinhibited bvFTD subtype presenting with loss of

    social conduct, addiction and sexual disinhibition at 48 years of age and diagnosed at our

    institute 3 years into the illness in 2012 at the age of 51 years. The patient did not have any

    family history suggestive of amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. The other patient was a 42 year old

    female presenting with progressive language dysfunction in the form of reduced word output,

    grammatical mistakes. Two years into the illness she developed progressive bulbar palsy

    requiring PercutaneousEnteroGastrostomy for severe swallowing disability.

    Table 7 lists the neurological signs seen in the study population and its frequency.

  • 33

    Neurological signs Frequency

    Primary reflexes

    Grasping

    Sucking

    36%

    10%

    Parkinsonian syndrome 18%

    Pyramidal signs 13%

    Postural tremor 3%

    Oculomotor disorders 5%

    Eyelid apraxia 2%

    Table 7: Clinical characteristics of 43 patients with FTD

    Survival time/ onset to death:

    Survival( alive/dead) data was available for 40 patients on follow up or on telephonic enquiry

    with 3 patients being lost to follow up. 10/40 patients (25%) had died during the course of the

    study with mean time in years of disease onset to death being 4.3 years. All the 10 deaths

    occurred in the bvFTD variant.

    Alive Dead Total

    Fishers exact p

    FTD type

    bvFTD

    22(68%) 10 (31%) 32 0.16 NS

    PNFA +

    SD 8 0 8

    Total 30(75%) 10(25%) 40(100%)

    Only 44.51% power to detect a difference

    Table 8 : number and proportion of deaths and comparison within FTD subtypes

    None of the patient was institutionalized prior to death and at follow up none of the alive

    patients are being managed in a hospice or an institution. Cause of death was aspiration

    pneumonia in 2 patients and cause in rest of the patients could be recorded in others.

  • 34

    Neuropsychological evaluation:

    MMSE was available in 24 of 43 patients, 17 patients (39.5%) were untestable and MMSE

    was not available in 2 patients studied retrospectively. Mean MMSE of the study population

    was 15.91 with a range of 2 to 27. Mean MMSE of the bvFTD group was 14.38 whereas that

    of the language variant was 20.5. Detailed neuropsychological testing in the testable patients

    revealed significant fronto-temporal function impairment with preservation of visuospatial

    function. As compared to published western literature (Isabelle Le Ber et al, Brain 2006)

    which report a mean MMSE of 21.5 (± 5.1), our study patients had a much lower MMSE

    score, more so for the bvFTD subtype. The lower MMSE score cannot be explained by the

    years of education parameter as the mean years of education was 10.8 years in the study

    group (Kerala has the highest Literacy rate among all Indian states approaching 100 %).

    Other factors probably responsible for this observation may be the delay in diagnosis and

    relatively advanced state in which the patient first reaches the neurologist, especially because

    the patients are prone to be treated as psychiatric/mood disorder patients in view of the

    typical neuropsychiatric profile and relatively younger age of onset.

    Behavioral symptoms at the onset of disease:

    Among the study population of 43 cases, the most common initial presenting symptom at the

    onset of disease as reported by the patient‟s primary care giver or next of kin was apathy (9 of

    43 patients, 21%). Frequency of other symptoms at the onset of disease is as follows:

    language disturbance 18.6 %, executive dysfunction 16.2 %, aggression 9.3%, addiction 7 %,

    food faddism, attention loss, excessive talk- each 4.6 % and 2.3 % each of obsessive

    compulsive behaviour, selfish behaviour, hoarding and mental rigidity. Figure 6 gives the

    absolute values of the symptoms at disease onset.

  • 35

    Frequency of Various Behavioral symptoms:

    The study evaluated 15 symptoms characteristically described in FTD, more so in the bvFTD

    category. Figure 8 enumerates the frequency of these individual symptoms present during the

    course of the illness of the 43 patients of the study. More than 50 % cases had loss of insight,

    apathy and social bladder incontinence which are characteristic symptoms described in

    frontal lobe dysfunction. Other commonly noted symptoms (> 25 % incidence) were

    restlessness, anger outbursts/agitation, food faddism, hallucinations/delusions and

    hyperorality/utilization behaviour. Other less common ( ~ 10 to 20 %) features were seen in

    emotional incontinence, sexual disinhibition, stereotypy, addictions, perseveration, hoarding

    and sign board reading. Figure 9 shows the absolute numbers of various behavioural

    symptoms seen in the study population.

    9

    8

    7

    4

    3

    2

    2

    2

    1

    1

    1

    1

    0 2 4 6 8 10

    Apathy

    Language

    Executive dysfunction

    Aggression

    Addiction

    Food fadism

    Attention

    Excessive talk

    OCD

    Selfish Behavior

    Hoarding

    Mental rigidity

    no.of patients

    Initial presenting symptom

    Figure 6: Presenting symptom at disease onset as reported by caregiver.

  • 36

    81

    67

    51

    39.5 39.534.9

    30.225.6 23.3

    18.6 18.6 16.3 16.39.3 7

    0

    10

    20

    30

    40

    50

    60

    70

    80

    90

    Frequency of behavioral symptoms in %

    35

    29

    17

    17

    7

    15

    8

    22

    4

    11

    7

    8

    3

    13

    10

    8

    14

    26

    26

    36

    28

    35

    21

    39

    32

    36

    35

    40

    30

    33

    0 10 20 30 40 50

    Loss of insight

    Apathy

    Restlessness

    Anger outbursts/agitation

    Addiction

    Food fadism

    Sexual disinhibition

    Social incontinence

    Hoarding

    Utilization/Hyperorality

    Perseveration

    Stereotypy

    Sign board

    Hallucination/Delusions

    Emotional incontinence

    Frequency of Behavior symptoms in 43 patients

    present

    absent

    Figure 8 : Frequency of characteristic symptoms described in FTD

    Figure 9 (below): Relative proportion of FTD symptoms

  • 37

    Figure 10 depicts the frequency of these 15 symptoms in the subtypes of bvFTD.

    Comparison of diagnostic groups on FrSBe (Frontal system behavioural

    scale):

    Table 9 presents the average T scores of the total and each of the three FrSBe subscale for

    the 3 diagnostic groups namely bvFTD, PNFA and SD. FrSBe score estimation was done in

    25 of the 43 patients. These 25 patients were included and prospectively followed up year

    2010 onwards and hence FrSBe data was not available on the remaining 18 patients.

    Table 10 presents the comparison of the total and the subscale FrSBe score among the three

    subtypes of bvFTD i.e. AbvFTD, DbvFTD and mixed bvFTD. The post hoc analysis of the

    bvFTD subtype data is given below table 10.

    13

    13

    3

    4

    2

    3

    2

    7

    1

    2

    1

    1

    0

    4

    4

    14

    6

    10

    8

    3

    7

    5

    10

    1

    4

    5

    5

    0

    7

    2

    8

    8

    4

    5

    2

    5

    1

    5

    2

    3

    1

    2

    3

    2

    2

    0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40

    Loss of Insight

    Apathy

    Restlessness

    Anger …

    Addiction

    food fadism

    Sexual disinhibition

    Social incontinence

    Hoarding

    Utilization/Hyperorality

    Perseveration

    Stereotypy

    Sign board

    Hallucination/Delusions

    Emotional incontinence

    AbvFTD DbvFTD mixed bvFTD

  • 38

    FTD type N Mean Std.

    Deviation Minimum Maximum

    ANOVA p

    value

    FrSBe total

    score

    bvFTD 20 52.84 7.67 39.57 70.22

    0.008 PNFA 3 41.18 14.36 28.28 56.67

    SD 2 34.73 2.73 32.80 36.67

    Total 25 50.00 10.00 28.28 70.22

    FrSBe Apathy

    score

    bvFTD 20 51.86 9.27 35.08 71.56

    0.120 PNFA 3 45.71 13.09 35.99 60.62

    SD 2 37.81 .00 37.82 37.82

    Total 25 50.00 10.00 35.08 71.56

    FrSBe

    Disinhibition

    score

    bvFTD 20 52.41 9.16 34.92 69.72

    0.034 PNFA 3 42.97 9.18 33.95 52.32

    SD 2 36.36 .68 35.89 36.85

    Total 25 50.00 10.00 33.95 69.72

    FrSBe Executive

    dysfunction

    score

    bvFTD 20 52.96 7.44 42.83 70.51

    0.006 PNFA 3 39.37 14.58 25.53 54.59

    SD 2 36.25 5.38 32.45 40.06

    Total 25 50.00 10.00 25.53 70.51

    Table 9: Total and subscale FrSBe score for the 3 FTD types Mean differences are not significant in post-hoc Scheffe tests at 0.017 levels

  • 39

    FTD subtype N Mean Std. Deviation Minimum Maximum ANOVA p

    value

    FrSBe total

    score

    AbvFTD 8 53.44 6.68 45.70 63.77

    0.491

    DbvFTD 6 49.78 6.20 41.51 59.58

    Mixed

    bvFTD 6 55.11 10.22 39.57 70.22

    Total 20 52.84 7.67 39.57 70.22

    FrSBe Apathy

    score

    AbvFTD 8 57.99 7.23 47.85 71.56

    0.004

    DbvFTD 6 42.83 6.44 35.08 50.58

    Mixed

    bvFTD 6 52.71 7.31 42.38 64.26

    Total 20 51.86 9.27 35.08 71.56

    FrSBe

    Disinhibition

    score

    AbvFTD 8 47.60 5.15 39.75 53.29

    0.122

    DbvFTD 6 57.47 8.55 47.49 69.72

    Mixed

    bvFTD 6 53.77 11.77 34.92 66.82

    Total 20 52.41 9.16 34.92 69.72

    FrSBe Executive

    dysfunction

    score

    AbvFTD 8 53.03 7.91 42.83 64.28

    0.327

    DbvFTD 6 49.63 4.09 44.91 55.98

    Mixed

    bvFTD 6 56.20 8.95 44.21 70.51

    Total 20 52.96 7.44 42.83 70.51

    Mean differences are significant in post-hoc Scheffe tests at 0.017 levels.

    Table 10: FrSBe total and subscale score comparison among bvFTD sybtypes and below- Post hoc

    analysis.

    Dependent Variable bvFTD

    subtype

    bvFTD

    subtype Sig.

    FrSBe Apathy score

    AbvFTD DbvFTD .004

    mixed .400

    DbvFTD Mixed .079

  • 40

    DISCUSSION

    The present study describes the demographic, clinical characteristics of a study population of

    43 FTD patients seen at a single centre. This study, to our knowledge, is the first study

    devoted to clinical, neuropsychiatric characteristics of FTD from an Indian population and it

    provides valuable insight regarding the demographic data of FTD in this part of the world.

    FTD type distribution:

    81 % of cases in this study were of the behavioural presentation (bvFTD) whereas the

    proportion of PNFA & Semantic dementia(SD) was very low ( 14 & 9 % respectively).

    Previous studies have reported that around quarter of the patients are of PNFA and SD type.

    Whether this observation is true or as a result of a selection bias, it has to be seen in

    subsequent studies in the similar population or any other part of the country.

    Gender distribution:

    In terms of gender, overall there was a predominance of men diagnosed as having FTD ( 65

    % men vs 35 % women) and similar representation was seen in the subtypes of Behavioral

    FTD and PNFA. In semantic dementia, equal prevalence was found.

    A few studies (Diehl et al, 2002) document a predominance of men in patients with FTD.

    However, others report a predominance of women,(Gustafson et al, 2001) and yet others find

    an equal sex distribution.( Hodges JR et al, Neurology 2003, Rosso SM et al, 2003) Fewer

    studies have evaluated the sex distribution in those with semantic dementia and PNFA.

    Snowden et al, 1996 demonstrated a 2:1 ratio of women to men with semantic dementia,

    while Hodges et al, 2003 found a predominance of women in 8 PNFA patients and a

  • 41

    predominance of men in 9 patients with semantic dementia. The confusing pattern of sex

    differences may be due to the small samples previously described. Johnson et al 2005 studied

    the demographic characteristics of 353 patients and reported male predominance for bvFTD

    and semantic dementia and the female predominance for PNFA which they hypothesize that

    it may reflect differences in biological vulnerability to the 3 anatomically distinct syndromes.

    This cortical asymmetry may reflect different vulnerabilities to neurodegeneration between

    women (left frontal) and men (right frontal and/or bilateral temporal).

    Age of onset of FTD:

    Frontotemporal lobar degeneration comprises 5–6% of all dementias, but 8–17% of early-

    onset (under 70) dementias in autopsy series (Barker et al.,2002). The disease most

    commonly presents in the sixth decade, though presentation may occur as early as the third

    and as late as the ninth decade of life (Knopman et al., 1990; Barker et al.,2002; Ratnavalli et

    al., 2002; Hodges et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005). FTLD is generally considered an early-

    onset dementia, but up to one-quarter of cases can present after age 65 (Rosso et al., 2003;

    Johnson et al., 2005). Progressive non-fluent aphasia presents later than other clinical

    syndromes in some series (Hodges et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005).

    In this study too 75 % patients presented with illness starting in the 6th

    decade (

  • 42

    FTD, suggesting that this disorder also occurs in older patients. The age at onset for FTLD is

    an important issue to resolve because several studies have used an age of 65 or 70 years as

    the cutoff to estimate the prevalence of FTD. By using these definitions, there is an inherent

    diagnostic bias against the diagnosis of FTLD in the very old.

    A positive family history is present in 20–56% of the patients with FTLD (Ratnavalli et al.,

    2002; Hodges et al., 2003). The mode of inheritance is autosomal dominant in the majority of

    cases. Interest in FTLD has increased during the past decade, since hereditary bvFTD was

    found in some cases to be associated with mutations in the microtubule associated protein tau

    (MAPT) gene (Hutton et al., 1998). Two other genes, VCP (Watts et al., 2004) and CHMP2B

    (Skibinski et al., 2005) are responsible for a minority of familial cases. Recently, mutations in

    the PGRN gene, coding for progranulin were found in FTLD-U families (Baker et al., 2006).

    Our study showed a positive family history in 3 of 37 patients (8.1 %). Family history was

    not available in 6 patients. Out of the three patients who had a positive family history, 2 had

    bvFTD and one had PNFA. Genetic studies would have thrown more light on the inheritance

    of these cases, which however could not be done.

    Survival time/ symptom onset to death:

    Various studies have described median survival from symptom onset and diagnosis as 6 and 3

    years respectively, with the clinical syndrome of FTD-MND associated with a more rapidly

    progressive course (2 and 1 years respectively) (Hodges et al., 2003; Johnson et al., 2005;

    Roberson et al., 2005). Significantly, mean time from diagnosis to institutionalization can be

    as short as 1 year, probably due to the high prevalence of behavioral symptoms. Survival is

    shorter and cognitive and functional decline are much more rapid than in AD (Roberson et

  • 43

    al., 2005). In our study, 10/40 patients (25%) had died during the course of the study with

    mean time in years of disease onset to death being 4.3 years. This figure is less as compared

    to the median survival from symptom onset as described by Hodges et al, 2003 was 6 ± 1.1

    years (95% CI) for bvFTD. Probable explanation to this finding could be lack of

    institutionalization facilities for care of FTD patients of the Indian population, increasing

    prevalence of smaller, nuclear families, poor support infrastructure for neurological

    rehabilitation. Further studies probing into this specific aspect of patient survival in and out

    of dedicated patient care setting may throw further light on the issue.

    Behavioral symptoms at the onset and in the course of the disease:

    Among the study population of 43 cases, the most common initial presenting symptom at the

    onset of disease as reported by the patient‟s primary care giver or next of kin was apathy (9 of

    43 patients, 21%). Frequency of other symptoms at the onset of disease is as follows:

    language disturbance 18.6 %, executive dysfunction 16.2 %, aggression 9.3%, addiction 7 %,

    food faddism, attention loss, excessive talk- each 4.6 % and 2.3 % each of obsessive

    compulsive behaviour, selfish behaviour, hoarding and mental rigidity. Figure 6 gives the

    absolute values of the symptoms at disease onset.

    More than 50 % cases had loss of insight, apathy and social bladder incontinence which are

    characteristic symptoms described in frontal lobe dysfunction. Other commonly noted

    symptoms (> 25 % incidence) were restlessness, anger outbursts/agitation, food faddism,

    hallucinations/delusions and hyperorality/utilization behaviour. Other less common ( ~ 10 to

    20 %) features were seen in emotional incontinence, sexual disinhibition, stereotypy,

    addictions, perseveration, hoarding and sign board reading.

  • 44

    Comparison of FrSBe scores among FTD patients:

    FrSBe T scores (total and subscale scores of Apathy, Disinhibition and Executive

    dysfunction) were compared across the three syndromes of FTD i.e. bvFTD, PNFA, Semantic

    dementia as well as subtypes of bvFTD namely Apathetic(AbvFTD), Disinhibited(DbvFTD)

    and MixedbvFTD. FrSBe scores were available in 25 out of 43 study cases. Among the 25

    cases in which FrSBe scores were available, 20 were of bvFTD type( 8 AbvFTD, 7 DbvFTD,

    5 mixed bvFTD) and 5 were of the language variant ( 3 PNFA, 2 SD).

    The mean FrSBe total as well as the three subscale scores for the bvFTD group were higher

    than the PNFA and SD group. On applying ANOVA, significant difference was noted among

    the three FTD syndromes in the total, Disinhibition and Executive dysfunction scores,

    whereas the apathy score difference was not statistically significant. This suggests that the

    language variant of FTD differs from the behavioural variant more in terms of disinhibition

    and executive dysfunction but is similar to the latter in terms of presence of apathy. As the

    number of PNFA and SD patients in the current study was low, post hoc analysis using

    Scheffe test failed to show any statistical significance.

    When comparison using the FrSBe scale was done among the bvFTD subtypes (AbvFTD,

    DbvFTD, Mixed bvFTD), it was evident that the mean total and subscale FrSBe scores were

    similar in the three subtypes but statistically significant difference was noted on the Apathy

    score (p value = 0.004, ANOVA) among the three subtypes, on post hoc analysis this

    difference was sustained only when comparing the apathetic and disinhibited group which

    can be explained by the fact the mixed bvFTD patients also had high Apathy scores in view

    of presence of symptoms of both the groups. Although the mean disinhibition score was more

    in the DbvFTD subtype (T score- 69.72) as compared to that of AbvFTD ( T score- 53.29), it

    failed to reach a level of statistical significance.

  • 45

    Apathy was the most common behavioral problem in AbvFTD, and dysexecutive problems

    was found in both AbvFTD and DbvFTD, particularly as the severity of the disorders

    increases . Both of our groups displayed increases in apathy and dysexecutive behaviors after

    the onset of dementia, but the DbvFTD group showed more dramatic change in disinhibition.

    The FrSBe Disinhibition scale contains items assessing behaviors commonly observed in

    FTD, such as swearing, silly or childlike behavior, impulsivity, inappropriate sexual

    behavior, and labile emotionality.

    Similar findings have been reported when the FrSBe has been used with other disorders

    involving disinhibition as a prominent feature. (Spinella et al, 2004) have conducted a series

    of studies showing that high FrSBe scores were associated with polysubstance abuse, binge

    eating, and difficulty delaying gratification in simulated gambling tasks.

    In addition to its utility in differential diagnosis, the FrSBe correlates with real-world

    outcomes in dementia patients. FrSBe Apathy scores predict performance on activities of

    daily living (Norton LE et al, 2001), whereas higher FrSBe Disinhibition and Executive

    scores are associated with caregiver burden (Davis J et al, 2007). The present study extends

    these findings to FTD and bolsters the notion that quantified measurement of behavioural

    change provides useful information in dementia diagnosis. The FrSBe might be a useful

    addition to the armamentarium of clinicians by allowing objective evaluation of behaviour

    change over time. The FrSBe is not meant to replace a comprehensive, multidisciplinary

    evaluation. Rather, it can provide a standardized method for evaluating behavior change,

    complementing cognitive assessment, neuroimaging, and the neurologic examination. It is

    low in cost and in burden on the patient, requiring only about 15 minutes of caregiver time to

    complete.

  • 46

    FTLD is a relatively rare disorder, and our sample was therefore small, despite recruitment

    from 2005 to 2012. Consensus diagnosis by expert clinicians with research diagnostic criteria

    was chosen as the gold standard for accurate diagnosis for this study. The only more accurate

    method of diagnosis of which we are aware is autopsy. These data were unfortunately not

    available in this cohort. We hope to investigate the FrSBe further with a larger sample of

    FTLD patients and to correlate scores with pathology and/or neuroimaging changes in future

    studies.

    Divisions between clinical syndromes are not absolute. Clinical overlap would inevitably

    have the effect of reducing behavioural differences between groups. The fact that strong

    behavioural differences can nevertheless be identified, which accords with previous clinical

    experience, suggests that such differences are real and robust. The study draws attention to

    behavioural distinctions between the clinical syndromes of bvFTD, PNFA and semantic

    dementia in the domains of emotion and insight, social behaviour, response to sensory

    stimuli, eating, addictions and oral behaviour, and repetitive behaviours and compulsions.

    Further correlative studies of behavioural change and structural and functional change on

    brain imaging would provide further understanding of the anatomical substrate of the

    behavioural disorders of frontotemporal lobar degeneration.

  • 47

    CONCLUSIONS

    1. The proportion of patients with onset after 65 was high in our study (25%)which was

    consistent with earlier observations, The diagnosis of FTD should therefore be considered

    even in elderly patients.

    2. Incidence of PNFA and Semantic dementia was low in our study as compared to western

    data.

    3. Time to death from symptom onset was low (4.3 years) as compared to published literature

    (6.1 years) which may suggest lack of neurorehabilitation and institutionalization facilities.

    4. Patients enrolled in the study presented in a relatively advanced state at the time of

    enrolment and diagnosis

    5. The language variant of FTD (PNFA and SD) differs from the behavioural variant more in

    terms of disinhibition and executive dysfunction but is similar to the latter in terms of

    presence of apathy.

    6. At an early state in the disease, clinical/neuropsychological differentiation into an apathetic

    vs disinhibited bvFTD is possible, however this distinguishability cannot be maintained as

    the disease advances.

    7. The FrSBe Scale might be a useful addition to the armamentarium of clinicians by

    allowing objective evaluation of behaviour change over time and it can provide a

    standardized method for evaluating behavior change, complementing cognitive assessment,

    neuroimaging, and the neurologic examination. It is low in cost and in burden on the patient,

    requiring only about 15 minutes of caregiver time to complete.

  • 48

    Limitations of the study

    1. In view of lesser numbers of language variant of FTD, comparative analysis was not

    significant regarding few parameters due to lack of power of study.

    2. Pathological confirmation of the FTD diagnosis could not be made due to

    unavailability of consent from the patients.

    3. Genetic studies could not be possible due to resource and infrastructure limitations.

    4. Although on follow up, few patients had development of new signs like parkinsonism,

    bulbar palsy, definite documentation of evolving into a 2nd

    or 3rd

    syndrome could not

    be made as reported in few previous studies (Kertesz A et al., Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2007).

    5. Although VBM correlation was contemplated as a part of this study, sufficient

    number of cases with follow up neuroimaging was not possible in the stipulated time

    period in view of advanced disease state of patients, uncooperativeness for imaging

    and hazards of imaging under sedation/anesthesia. This arm of the study is being

    continued.

  • 49

    REFERENCES

    1. Ash S, Moore P, Vesely L, Gunawardena D, McMillan C, Aderson C, Avants B,

    Grossman M. Nonfluent speech in frontotemporal lobar degeneration. J

    Neurolinguistics. 2009;22:370–83.

    2. Bak TH, Hodges JR. Motor neurone disease, dementia and aphasia: Coincidence, co-

    occurrence or continuum? J Neurol. 2001;248:260–70.

    3. Baker M, et al. Mutations in progranulin cause tau-negative frontotemporal dementia

    linked to chromosome 17. Nature 2006; 442: 916–9.

    4. Barker WW, Luis CA, Kashuba A, et al. (2002). Relative frequencies of Alzheimer

    disease, Lewy body, vascular and frontotemporal dementia, and hippocampal

    sclerosis in the State of Florida Brain Bank. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 16: 203–

    212.

    5. Boeve BF, Lang AE, Litvan I. Corticobasal degeneration and its relationship to

    progressive supranuclear palsy and frontotemporal dementia. Ann Neurol.

    2003;54:S15–9.

    6. Boxer AL, Miller BL. Clinical features of frontotemporal dementia. Alzheimer Dis

    Assoc Disord.2005;19:S3–6.

    7. Brun A (1987). Frontal Lobe Degeneration of the Non-Alzheimer Type. I.

    Neuropathology Arch Gerontol Geriatrics 6: 193–208.

    8. Cairns NJ, et al. Neuropathologic diagnostic and nosologic criteria for frontotemporal

    lobar degeneration: Consensus of the Consortium for Frontotemporal Lobar

    Degeneration. Acta Neuropathol. 2007;114:5–22.

    9. Chan D, Anderson V, Pijnenburg Y , Whitwell J, Barnes J, Scahill R, et al. The

    clinical profile of right temporal lobe atrophy. Brain. 2009;132:1287–98.

  • 50

    10. Chow TW, Miller BL, Hayashi VN, Geschwind DH. Inheritance of frontotemporal

    dementia. Arch Neurol. 1999;56:817-822.

    11. Clark LN, Poorkaj P, et al. (1998). Pathogenic implications of mutations in the tau

    gene in pallido-pontonigral degeneration and related neurodegenerative disorders

    linked to chromosome 17. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 13103–13107.

    12. Constantinidis J, Richard J, Tissot R (1974). Pick‟s disease: histological and clinical

    correlations. Eur Neurol 11: 208–217.

    13. Cummings JL. Anatomic and behavioral aspects of frontal-subcortical circuits. Ann N

    Y Acad Sci. 1995;769:1–13.

    14. Davies RR, Kipps CM, Mitchell J, Kril JJ, Halliday GM, Hodges JR. Progression in

    frontotemporal dementia: Identifying a benign behavioral variant by magnetic

    resonance imaging. Arch Neurol. 2006;63:1627–31.

    15. Davis J, Tremont G. Impact of frontal systems behavioral functioning in dementia on

    caregiver burden. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007;19:43–9.

    16. Diehl J, Kurz A. Frontotemporal dementia: patient characteristics, cognition, and

    behaviour. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2002;17:914-918.

    17. Foster NL, Wilhelmsen K, Sima AA, et al. (1997). Frontotemporal dementia and

    parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17: a consensus conference. Conference

    Participants. Ann Neurol 41: 706–715.-

    18. Gislason TB, Sjogren M, Larsson L, Skoog I. The prevalence of frontal variant

    frontotemporal dementia and the frontal lobe syndrome in a population based sample

    of 85 year olds. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003;74:867-871.

    19. Gorno-Tempini ML, et al. Cognition and anatomy in three variants of primary

    progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol. 2004;55:335–46.

  • 51

    20. Grace J, Malloy PF. Frontal Systems Behavior Scale (FrSBe): professional manual.

    Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 2001.

    21. Gregory C, Lough S, Stone V, Erzinclioglu S, Martin L, Baron-Cohen S, et al. Theory

    of mind in patients with frontal variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer‟s

    disease: Theoretical and practical implications. Brain. 2002;125:752–64

    22. Gustafson L, Passant U, Grasbeck A, Brun A. Clinical variability of frontotemporal

    dementia. In: Miyoski K, Shapiro C, Gavilia M, Morika Y, eds. Contemporary

    Neuropsychiatry. Tokyo, Japan: Springer-Verlag; 2001:152-157.

    23. Hodges JR, Patterson K. Nonfluent progressive aphasia and semantic dementia: A

    comparative neuropsychological study. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1996;2:511–24.

    24. Hodges JR, Davies R, Xuereb J, et al. Survival in frontotemporal dementia.

    Neurology. 2003;61:349-354.

    25. Hodges JR, Patterson K. Semantic dementia: A unique clinicopathological syndrome.

    Lancet Neurol. 2007;6:1004–14.

    26. Hosler BA, Siddique T, Sapp PC, et al. (2000). Linkage of familial amyotrophic

    lateral sclerosis with frontotemporal dementia to chromosome 9q21-q22. JAMA 284:

    1664–1669.

    27. Hudson LP, Munoz DG, Miller L, et al. (1993). Amygdaloid sclerosis in temporal

    lobe epilepsy. Ann Neurol 33: 622–631

    28. Hutton M, et al. Association of missense and 50-splice site mutations in tau with the

    inherited dementia FTDP-17. Nature 1998; 393: 702–5.

    29. Isabelle Le Ber et al., Demographic, neurological and behavioural characteristics and

    brain perfusion SPECT in frontal variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 2006

    Nov; 129(Pt 11):3051-65.

  • 52

    30. Johnson JK, Diehl Jet al; Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: demographic

    characteristics of 353 patients. Arch Neurol. 2005 Jun;62(6):925-30.

    31. Josephs KA. Frontotemporal dementia and related disorders: Deciphering the enigma.

    Ann Neurol. 2008;64:4–14.

    32. Kertesz A. Pick complex: An integrative approach to fronto-temporal dementia:

    Primary progressive aphasia, corticobasal degeneration, and progressive supranuclear

    palsy. Neurologist. 2003;9:311–7.

    33. Kertesz A, Blair M, McMonagle P, Munoz DG , The diagnosis and course of

    frontotemporal dementia.. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2007 Apr-Jun;21(2):155-63.

    34. Knibb JA, Hodges JR. Semantic dementia and primary progressive aphasia: A

    problem of categorization? Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2005;19:S7–14.

    35. Knopman D, et al. Longitudinal Tracking of FTLD. Toward developing Clinical Trial

    Methodology. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 2007; 21: S58–63.

    36. Knopman DS, Mastri AR, Frey WHd, et al. (1990). Dementia lacking distinctive

    histologic features: a common non- Alzheimer degenerative dementia. Neurology 40:

    251–256.

    37. Kril JJ, Halliday GM. Clinicopathological staging of frontotemporal dementia

    severity: Correlation with regional atrophy. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord

    2004;17:311–5.

    38. Libon DJ, Xie SX, Moore P, Farmer J, Antani S, McCawley G, et al. Patterns of

    neuropsychological impairment in frontotemporal dementia. Neurology.

    2007;68:369–75

    39. Litvan I,et al. Clinical research criteria for the diagnosis of progressive supranuclear

    palsy (Steele–Richardson–Olszewski syndrome): report of the NINDS-SPSP

    international workshop. Neurology 1996; 47: 1–9.

  • 53

    40. Lomen-Hoerth C, Anderson T, Miller B (2002). The overlap of amyotrophic lateral

    sclerosis and frontotemporal dementia. Neurology 59: 1077–1079.

    41. Mesulam MM (1982). Slowly progressive aphasia without generalized dementia. Ann

    Neurol 11: 592–598.

    42. Mioshi E, Kipps CM, Dawson K, Mitchell J, Graham A, Hodges JR. Activities of

    daily living in frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease. Neurology.

    2007;68:2077–84

    43. Mohandas E, Rajmohan V. Frontotemporal dementia: An updated overview. Indian J

    Psychiatry 2009;51:65-9

    44. Neary D, Snowden JS, Northen B, et al. (1988). Dementia of frontal lobe type. J

    Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 51:353–361.

    45. Norton LE, Malloy PF, Salloway S. The impact of behavioral symptoms on activities

    of daily living in patients with dementia. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 2001;9:41– 8.

    46. Paulsen JS, et al. Frontal behavioral syndromes in cortical and subcortical dementia.

    Assessment 1996;3:327–37.

    47. Perry RJ, Graham A, Williams G, Rosen H, Erzinçlioglu S, Weiner M, et al. Patterns

    of frontal lobe atrophy in frontotemporal dementia: a volumetric MRI study. Dement

    Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2006;22:278–87

    48. Pick A (1892). Uber die Beziehungen der senilen Hirnatrophie zur Aphasie. Prager

    Medizinische Wochenschrift 17: 165–167

    49. Rabinovici GD, et al. Abeta amyloid and glucose metabolism in three variants of

    primary progressive aphasia. Ann Neurol. 2008;64:388–401.

    50. Rascovsky K, Hodges JR, et al. Diagnostic criteria for the behavioral variant of

    frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD): Current limitations and future directions.

    Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2007;21:S14–8

  • 54

    51. Ratnavalli E, Brayne C,DawsonK, et al. (2002). The prevalence of frontotemporal

    dementia. Neurology 58: 1615–1621.

    52. Roeber S, Mackenzie IR, Kretzschmar HA, Neumann M. TDP-43-negative FTLD-U

    is a significant new clinico-pathological subtype of FTLD. Acta Neuropathol.

    2008;116:147–57.

    53. Roberson ED, Hesse JH, Rose KD, et al. (2005). Frontotemporal dementia progresses

    to death faster than Alzheimer disease. Neurology 65: 719–725.

    54. Rosen HJ, Allison SC, Ogar JM, Amici S, Rose K, Dronkers N, et al. Behavioral

    features in semantic dementia vs other forms of progressive aphasias. Neurology.

    2006;67:1752–6.

    55. Rosen HJ, et al. Patterns of brain atrophy in frontotemporal dementia and semantic

    dementia. Neurology. 2002;58:198–208.

    56. Rosness TA, Haugen PK, Passant U, Engedal K. Frontotemporal dementia: A

    clinically complex diagnosis. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2008;23:837–42

    57. Rosso SM, Landweer EJ, Houterman M, et al. Medical and environmental risk factors

    for sporadic frontotemporal dementia: a retrospective case-control study. J Neurol

    Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2003;74:1574-1576.

    58. Seelaar H, et al. Distinct genetic forms of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology.

    2008;71:1220–6

    59. Skibinski G, et al. Mutations in the endosomal ESCRTIII-complex subunit CHMP2B

    in frontotemporal dementia. Nat Genet 2005; 37: 806–8.

    60. Snowden JS, Neary D, Mann DMA. Fronto-Temporal Lobar Degeneration: Fronto-

    Temporal Dementia, Progressive Aphasia, Semantic Dementia. New York,

    NY:Churchill Livingstone Inc; 1996.

  • 55

    61. Snowden J, Goulding P, Neary D (1989). Semantic dementia: a form of circumscribed

    cerebral atrophy. Behav Neurol 2: 167–182.

    62. Spinella M, Lyke J. Executive personality traits and eating behavior. Int J Neurosci

    2004;114:83–93.

    63. Varma AR, Snowden JS, Lloyd JJ, et al. Evaluation of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria

    in the differentiation of Alzheimer‟s disease and frontotemporal dementia. J Neurol

    Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1999;66:184-188.

    64. Watts GD, et al. Inclusion body myopathy associated with Paget disease of bone and

    FTD is caused by mutant valosin-containing protein. Nat Genet 2004; 36: 377–81.

    65. Wittenberg D, Possin KL, Rascovsky K, Rankin KP, Miller BL, Kramer JH. The

    early neuropsychological and behavioral characteristics of frontotemporal dementia.

    Neuropsychol Rev. 2008;18:91–102.

  • 56

    ANNEXURE

    Consensus Criteria for Diagnosis of bvFTD (Neary et al, 1998)

    The clinical diagnostic features of FTD: Clinical profile

    Character change and disordered social conduct are the dominant features initially and

    throughout the disease course. Instrumental functions of perception, spatial skills, praxis, and

    memory are intact or relatively well preserved.

    I. Core diagnostic features

    A. Insidious onset and gradual progression

    B. Early decline in social interpersonal conduct

    C. Early impairment in regulation of personal conduct

    D. Early emotional blunting

    E. Early loss of insight

    II Supportive diagnostic features

    A. Behavioral disorder

    1. Decline in personal hygiene and grooming, 2. Mental rigidity and inflexibility

    3.Distractibility and impersistence 4. Hyperorality and dietary changes 5. Perseverative and

    stereotyped behaviour 6. Utilization behavior

    B. Speech and language

    1. Altered speech output: a. Aspontaneity and economy of speech b. Press of speech

    2. Stereotypy of speech 3. Echolalia 4. Perseveration 5. Mutism

    C. Physical signs

    1. Primitive reflexes 2. Incontinence 3. Akinesia, rigidity, and tremor 4. Low and labile blood

    pressure

    D. Investigations

    1. Neuropsychology: significant impairment on frontal lobe tests in the absence of severe

    amnesia, aphasia, or perceptuospatial disorder 2. Electroencephalography: normal on

    conventional EEG despite clinically evident dementia 3. Brain imaging (structural and/or

    functional): predominant frontal and/or anterior temporal abnormality

  • 57

    Consensus Criteria for Diagnosis of PNFA (Neary et al, 1998)

    The clinical diagnostic features of progressive nonfluent aphasia:

    Clinical profile- Disorder of expressive language is the dominant feature initially and

    throughout the disease course. Other aspects of cognition are intact or relatively well

    preserved.

    I. Core diagnostic features

    A. Insidious onset and gradual progression

    B. Nonfluent spontaneous speech with at least one of the following: agrammatism, phonemic

    paraphasias, anomia

    II Supportive diagnostic features

    A. Speech and language:

    1. Stuttering or oral apraxia 2. Impaired repetition 3. Alexia, agraphia 4. Early preservation of

    word meaning 5. Late mutism

    B. Behavior

    1. Early preservation of social skills 2. Late behavioral changes similar to FTD

    C. Physical signs: late contralateral primitive reflexes, akinesia, rigidity, and tremor

    D. Investigations

    1. Neuropsychology: nonfluent aphasia in the absence of severe amnesia or perceptuospatial

    disorder

    2. Electroencephalography: normal or minor asymmetric slowing

    3. Brain imaging (structural and/or functional): asymmetric abnormality predominantly

    affecting dominant (usually left) hemisphere

  • 58

    Consensus Criteria for Diagnosis of Semantic Dementia (Neary et al, 1998)

    Clinical profile: Semantic disorder (impaired understanding of word meaning and/or object

    identity) is the dominant feature initially and throughout the disease course. Other aspects of

    cognition, including autobiographic memory, are intact or relatively well preserved.

    I. Core diagnostic features

    A. Insidious onset and gradual progression

    B. Language Disorder characterized by

    1. Progressive, fluent, empty spontaneous speech 2. Loss of word meaning, manifest by

    impaired naming and comprehension 3. Semantic paraphasias and/or

    C. Perceptual disorder characterized by

    1. Prosopagnosia: impaired recognition of identity of familiar faces and/or

    2. Associative agnosia: impaired recognition of object identity

    D. Preserved perceptual matching and drawing reproduction

    E. Preserved single-word repetition

    F. Preserved ability to read aloud and write to dictation orthographically regular words

    II. Supportive diagnostic features

    A. Speech and language

    1. Pressure of speech 2. Idiosyncratic word usage 3. Absence of phonemic paraphasias

    4.Surface dyslexia and dysgraphia 5. Preserved calculation

    B. Behavior

    1. Loss of sympathy and empathy 2. Narrowed preoccupations 3. Parsimony

    C. Physical signs

    1. Absent or late primitive reflexes 2. Akinesia, rigidity, and tremor

    D. Investigations

    1. Neuropsychology: Profound semantic loss, manifest in failure of word comprehension

    and naming and/or face and object recognition

    2. Electroencephalography: normal

    3. Brain imaging (structural and/or functional): predominant anterior temporal abnormality

    (symmetric or asymmetric)

  • 59

    FrSBe Score

  • 60

    FrSBe Score continued…

  • 61

    End of FrSBe Score.