soundsselves

Upload: adel-wang

Post on 01-Jun-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/9/2019 SoundsSelves

    1/8

    The exploration of how places are heard, ‘how they sound

    and resound’ (Ree 1999:53), has been very largely neg-

    lected in ethnographic enquiry. Academic literature on

     place has been dominated by a pervasive visual bias, with

    the result that a deaf ear has been turned to the acoustic

     properties of environments. Stephen Feld (1996) questions

    the sufficiency of visually-orientated enquiry into

    emplacement. Like Stoller (1997) and Ingold (2000), he

    suggests that the multi-sensory nature of perceptual expe-

    rience should logically require the multi-sensory concep-

    tualization of place. In his article ‘Waterfalls of song’ he

    argues ‘the potential of acoustic knowing’ (1996: 97).

    Sound, combined with an awareness of sonic presence, is

     posited as a powerful force in shaping how people inter-

     pret their experiences. Acoustemology, the ‘exploration of 

    sonic sensibilities’, brings the ethnographer closer tounderstanding the significance of sound to experiential

    truth (ibid ).

    This article is based on fieldwork carried out between

    May and August 1999 at the Edinburgh Royal Infirmary in

    Scotland. During this time I worked as a team member on

    Red Dot Radio, also known as the Edinburgh Hospital

    Broadcasting Service, which was housed in a small studio

    within the compound of the Edinburgh Royal Infirmary.

    My job was to talk to patients in all wards to find out what

    they enjoyed listening to, and what they felt could be

    improved about the radio service. However, it quickly

     became apparent that Red Dot Radio was a very minor 

    concern, and patients were far more preoccupied by the

    very busy and distinctive hospital soundscape which sur-rounded them. Surprised at the intensity of patient feeling

    about this soundscape, I began an enquiry into the acoustic

    existence of a distinctive hospital soundscape, created b

    the activities and work of care, which has come to charac

    terize the hospital environment. The sounds of medica

     practices, equipment and technology punctuate and pe

    vade hospital life, and have been endowed with particula

    significance by many of the patients whose thoughts ar

     present in these pages. They express an understanding tha

    the soundscape is produced through the enactment of

    code of medical practice (which anthropologists know a

     biomedicine) which requires them to be the passive recip

    ients of medical attention, the objects of medical tech

    niques, and accepting of certain systems of control. Th

    soundscape thus comes to be experienced as a symbol o

    this construction of ‘patienthood’, meaning that sounds ar

    central to the patients’ experience of themselves as ‘patien

    selves’. The hospital environment thus gives rise to a sonically constituted sense of self.

    Active soundscape

    Early on in my fieldwork, a patient told me he had writte

    a letter to the hospital administration suggesting tha

    nurses should be required to wear shoes which make n

    noise, as the constant sound of their footsteps up and dow

    the corridor was irritating, disturbed his rest and woke him

    early. ‘It’s all you can do to forget about it just to get t

    sleep, but before you know it the noise has woken you u

    again,’ he explained. He had numerous other complain

    concerning intrusive sounds. He was bothered by the nois

    of the television, which he could not see properly from hi

     bed. Hushed voices and whispers, the rasping of bed cutains and the squeaks of hospital trolleys also becam

    oppressive. Of course noise was muted in the interests o

    SoundselvesAn acoustemology of sound and self in the Edinburgh Royal Infirmary

    TOM RICE

    Tom Rice became interested 

    in patients' experience of hospital soundscapes while

    working as a volunteer at the

     Edinburgh Royal Infirmary.

     He wrote his BA dissertation

    on the subject, and this article

    engages with issues raised by

    that study. He is currently

     studying for a PhD in

    anthropology at Goldsmiths

    College, University of 

     London, looking at techniques

    of medical listening,

    conducting fieldwork among 

     patients and medical staff at 

    the Cardiology Unit of St 

    Thomas' Hospital in London.

     His email address is:

    [email protected]

     Fig. 1. An elderly man at a

    maternity clinic in Pakistan

     performing azan, a call to prayer, a traditional way of 

    celebrating the birth of a

    male child. Although the ears

    are important channels for 

    religious communication, he

    blocks them to exclude the

    extraneous sounds of the

    open hospital ward which

    make it difficult to create a

     sphere of privacy and 

    concentration.

    I would like to thank the

    members of the Edinburgh

    Hospital Broadcasting

    Service, in particular Tom

    Cornell and Malcolm Kirby,

    who created the opportunity

    for me to study the sounds of 

    the hospital. These thanks are

    extended to the many

     patients I met who lent their thoughts and feelings to the

     project. I am also very

    grateful to my supervisor, Dr 

    Di i i T i jil i f hi

  • 8/9/2019 SoundsSelves

    2/8

    hospital’s acoustic profile. Many had grown accustomed

    to the noise in ‘the way the sound of a waterfall soon

     becomes an unheard sound to those who live near it’

    (Kesey 1962: 75). But this patient’s feelings were echoed

     by others, and it became clear that the aural ‘scenery’was

    a distinctive feature of the hospital environment. As

    Priscilla, who referred to herself as ‘one of the bladder 

    ladies’, remarked: ‘With all the coughing and the noise and

     buzzing, where else could I be but in hospital?’

    In Western society, as Ree points out, hearing is cultur-

    ally constructed as a kind of supine passivity (1999: 53).

    But within the hospital, listening and hearing appear to be

    an active means of orientating oneself in the world.

    Patients come to know the hospital and understand its rou-

    tines and events through their ears. Hugh, commenting on

    the sounds in the male respiratory ward, told me: ‘You cantell what’s going on, you know. You can hear that they’re

    getting dinner ready, or that the medicine trolley’s coming

    round, and you can set your watch when you hear the

    nurses changing shift.’ It is clear that for some, hearing

     becomes an important means of gaining familiarity with

    the hospital environment.

    Recounting one of the most disturbing events of a 110-

    day stay in hospital, Gordon, a man who had recently been

    discharged after a liver transplant, described an experience

    which had been based entirely in sound. In the middle of 

    the night a patient in his ward had suffered a heart attack.

     Nursing staff had rushed in, and had pulled the curtains

    around all the beds in the ward before attempting to resus-

    citate the man. Gordon was unable to see what was hap- pening, he could only listen in his bed to the urgent

    struggle to keep his ward neighbour alive. The medics

    Gordon had the sounds of the occurrence re-echoing in hi

    mind. ‘I was shattered for days by these horrible noises

    he said. This violent eruption in the hospital soundscap

    was a deeply disturbing experience for him.

    The vividness with which Gordon experienced th

    sound in this account is heightened by an absence of othe

    available sensory information. Indeed, the sound took on

    more affective quality because of the dearth of other sen

    sory modalities. In her discussion of blind aesthetic

    Constance Classen describes how, for a blind ma

    standing in a park, the reality of his environment is createout of sound generated by the movements of other people

    ‘It is precisely when someone else enters the park – whe

    the tread of footsteps is felt, the sound of a voice heard

    that the world comes out of nothingness into being

    (Classen 1998: 141). She draws on the work of the blin

    writer John Hull, who explains that experience for him i

     brought into being through a ‘dynamic sequence o

    sounds’, the consequence of ‘world of happenings’ (Hu

    1990, cited ibid .: 142). For Gordon, an alarming and sin

    ister ‘world of happenings’ was manifested in a hauntin

    sonic presence which was never affirmed or dispelled b

    the proof of his other senses. His account is indicative o

    what many patients felt to be a more general impoverish

    ment of non-auditory stimuli within the hospital, aunusual atmosphere of sensory absences.

    The environment created in the ward appears to be on

    of stasis rather than the constant sensory flux one migh

    experience outside the hospital. The ward forms a spac

    characterized by a peculiar sensory monotony. Whe

    walking through a hospital one might be struck by the di

    ferences in smells between one ward and another, change

    in the colour of the paint or the texture of the floor, but fo

     patients who remain in the same ward for long periods o

    time the environment becomes one which they experienc

    as remarkably constant. An example of this stasis is foun

    in the ‘olfactive silence’, or rather the uniform scent of dis

    infectant which pervades the atmosphere (Howes 1988

    94). Odours are suppressed, a fact which Classen links t

    the symbolic odourlessness of sterility and scientific pre

    cision in Western culture (1993). Patients commente

    upon the remarkable fragrance which visitors would brin

    in with them on their clothes and possessions, or th

    amazing fresh scent which would be produced when the

    opened packages brought from home. These new smel

    were evocative and interesting in the olfactory blanke

    which the disinfectant of the ward created. Patients wer

    thus sensitive to the ‘deodorized’ environment of the hos

     pital, which Bubandt (1998) suggests is crucial to the sym

     bolic vocabulary of that institution.

    The notion that the hospital ward creates particular area

    of sensory ‘anaesthesia’ also applies to the visual dimension (Feldman 1994). Patients frequently expressed th

    sentiment that the ward set-up was visually dull, unstimu

    lating and even restrictive. Sitting in their beds, their eye

    were inevitably directed inwards towards the centre of th

    ward, forcing a kind of introspective gaze. Peter describe

    how when he first came into hospital and realized he wa

     bored of his surroundings, he would punctuate h

     boredom by taking slow walks to the toilet, stopping to tal

    to as many people as possible on his way. He gave up thes

    walks when he realized he was spending most of his day i

    the toilet. Televisions were sometimes provided, and news

     papers and magazines were available. These diversion

    though, were also introspective in terms of the ward layou

    and there was little scope for looking out or gaining different perspectives on one’s surroundings. The beds wer

     positioned in such a way that it was difficult to see out o

     Fig. 2. ‘What makes your 

    ears so long!’Cartoon by

    C.H. Bennett, published in1863 at the height of the

    evolutionist/creationist 

    debate. Bennett sends up

     Darwin's theory, using the

    ears to plot an absurd 

    morphology across species.

     However, eight years later, in

    the Descent of Man , Darwin

    compared the ears of men

    and monkeys and found them

    to be 'curiously alike' (2003:

    150). He noticed small points

    visible in 'the inwardly folded 

    margin, or helix' of the ears

    of both species ( ibid.: 16),

    which he believed to be

    ‘vestiges of the tips of 

     formerly erect and pointed 

    ears’( ibid. ), and asserted 

    that the points in question

    ‘are in some cases, both in

    man and apes, vestiges of a

     former condition’ ( ibid.: 16-

    17).

       T   H   E   W   E   L   L   C   O   M   E   L   I   B   R   A   R   Y ,   L   O   N   D   O   N

  • 8/9/2019 SoundsSelves

    3/8

    In his analysis of Bentham’s ‘Panopticon’, Foucault

    suggests that this lack of visual prospect is an essential ele-

    ment of the architecture of institutions such as hospitals

    (1975). In these institutions constant surveillance is used

    as a means of control by the ‘supervisors’ (an idea I

    explore in greater depth below). The patient/inmate/

    schoolch ild is made conscious of his or her permanent

    visibility. But while he ‘is seen… he does not see’ (ibid .:

    200; my italics). The patient’s ‘axial visibility’, meaning

    his or her being continually visible to the supervisors,

    implies a ‘lateral invisibility’, that is, an inability to gain a

    visual perspective on the system in which he or she is

    caught up. Patients are necessarily prevented from gaining

    an informed insight into the systems by which the hospital

    operates. Access to staff areas is forbidden, as it is to therooms in which the preparation and organization of care

    takes place. Patients are also prevented from seeking out

    other patients for widespread interpersonal communica-

    tion. Very strict regulations governed patients’ freedom to

    move around the Infirmary and to enter different wards in

    order to mix with other patients. As one man remarked,

    ‘You’re basically confined to the ward. The only people

    you see are those that come to see you.’Thus, ‘[t]he crowd,

    a compact mass, a locus of multiple exchanges, individu-

    alities merging together, a collective effect, is abolished

    and replaced by a collection of separated individualities…

    that can be numbered and supervised’ (Foucault 1975:

    201). The lack of visual opportunity, then, may be con-

    strued as part of a mechanism for the control of patients,inbuilt in the hospital’s architectural and social structure. I

    would suggest that the lack of opportunity for in sight is

    of taste. It was a favourite pastime of patients to compet

    for the most offensive terms in which to describe the

    food, which became a symbol of the mundane, bland an

    unappealing nature of the hospital routine. ‘The food sum

    up just about everything to do with hospital. It’s really

    really boring,’ I was told on one occasion. Many patien

    were on strict diets because of their illnesses, meaning tha

    the range of tastes available to them was severely curbed

    While conscious that the restrictions might be beneficial t

    their health, several patients found them highly frustrating

    Joanie, an elderly woman in the cardiology unit, told mshe would happily kill for a cup of coffee if only she coul

    ‘get the strength up’. Furthermore, some treatment

     patients were undergoing meant they did not really wan

    and certainly did not enjoy their food. A small numbe

    were on ‘nil-by-mouth’ order, so that their experience o

    taste was negligible during parts of their stay in hospita

    Some supplemented their diets with sweets and chocolat

     brought from home, and supplies were replenished by vi

    itors, but these were small gestures to change or counte

    the mundane flavour of hospital life.

    Many of my best informants were patients in the derma

    tology unit. They, in particular, reported an impoverish

    ment of their sense of touch. They wore ‘jammies’, pyjam

    suits which covered their skin disorders and applied thnecessary medication. Several patients remarked that the

    felt starved of contact because of these suits, and sensin

    through the skin became an unfamiliar experience. ‘Yo

    forget what things feel like sometimes, and then one tim

    you pick up an apple or an orange without the bandage an

    you think, “Wow”.’Patients ‘lost touch’with even the sim

     plest forms of contact. Even the act of shaking hands wa

     palpably different, altered by the barrier of cloth. Th

    ‘jammies’ became a second, thicker, less sensitive skin

     Northoff et al. observe this sensory privation in the

    analysis of the psychosomatics of atopic dermatitis: a wa

    is formed, reducing contact between the ‘lived body’ an

    the ‘lifeworld’(1992: 149).

    The hospital environment, then, is one in which sensor

    experience is ordered in ways which patients interpret a

     being restrictive. Experience is delimited in such a wa

    that particular areas of sensory anaesthesia are formed. Bu

    concurrently with the creation of these areas, other areas o

    sensitivity arise. For many patients, auditory informatio

    took on an unusual prominence in the construction o

    experience. Hearing came to be ‘privileged as a sensor

    mode’ (Howes 1988: 84). It is not my intention here t

    argue that the sense of hearing should be accorde

    supremacy over the other senses; neither am I trying t

    reinforce existing conceptual divisions between sensor

    categories. What this study indicates is that even within

    Western cultural milieu, sensory experience and intersensory emphasis is subject to variation. Within the partic

    ular environment represented by the hospital, sound i

    accorded a certain emphasis in the analysis of sensor

    interplay. It is lent an immediacy which might habituall

     be eclipsed by a visual bias in the ethnographic construc

    tion of reality.

    The presence of imposed sound was generally experi

    enced in negative terms. Patients felt it to be an unwel

    come intrusion which disrupted their privacy and mad

    rest fitful and difficult. Some used their hospital radi

    headphones to muffle or drown out the sounds aroun

    them. Others, exasperated by the constant noise, woul

    cover their heads with pillows. Michael Bull describe

    how personal stereo users employ headphones to excludor escape the chaotic urban soundscape. The headphone

    grant ‘users’ sanctuary from ‘the disenchanted and mun

       T   H   E   W   E   L   L   C   O   M   E   L   I   B   R   A   R   Y ,   L   O   N   D   O   N

     Fig. 3. Swiss theologian J. C.

     Lavater (1741-1801)

     popularized ‘physiognomy’,

    which suggested that a

     person’s character could be

    read from their external 

    appearance. ‘I am fully

    convinced that the ear... has

    its determinate signification,

    that it admits not of the

     smallest disguise, and that it 

    has particular analogy to the

    individual to which it 

    belongs… Examine this

     part... and you will soon see

    the distinctive differences

     pertaining to each character’ 

    (1789: 319).

  • 8/9/2019 SoundsSelves

    4/8

    in elaborating this contrast in experience: ‘Users describ

    the “bright, detailed imagery of the flow of time” exper

    enced through personal-stereo use, just as they describ

    the fear of that “long, dark hallway of time” without i

    (Bull 2000: 67, citing Adorno 1973). Here I deal with th

    subtext of Bull’s work on a culture of resistance to a dom

    inant soundscape. I explore how hospital patients confron

    conceptualize and appropriate a soundscape which per

    vades the environment in which they live, and which ou

    lasts their attempts at escape; I examine the thoughts o

    those who, in a very literal sense, are sitting in the ‘londark hallway’ of Adorno’s imagining. It is to this them

    that I now turn.

    Passive soundselves

    In his article ‘Sound and senses’, Sullivan (1986) explore

    sonic symbolism among the peoples of the highlands an

    eastern mountain regions of the Andes. He describes how

    the screeching of industrial machinery is imagined to b

    the cries of a monstrous being named ‘Pishtaco

    ‘Pishtaco’ screams as he sucks out the body fat of Indian

    in order to lubricate car and aeroplane engines, boa

    motors and mining equipment. His scream is the sound o

    the invading industrial society and its machinery, whic

    displaces, enslaves and ultimately destroys indigenoucommunities. Sullivan’s analysis links the significance o

    sound to its integrality with a wider social process, namel

    the influx of modern industrial technology and its terribl

    impact on those who are affected by it. Thus ‘the metalli

    whining of technological societies prolongs the deat

    agony of indigenous populations’ (1986: 15-16). The ho

     pital soundscape does not represent a cultural metaphor o

    the same power or directness as the harrowing exampl

    described by Sullivan. However, here I briefly explor

    how powerful cultural symbols may be disguised in th

     background noises of a society. I suggest that the sounds o

    the hospital have become symbols of patienthood and th

    regime which patienthood entails for some within th

    Royal Infirmary.

    Residents of the hospital’s dermatology unit saw them

    selves as being a generally active and alert group o

     patients because, as James pointed out: ‘We’re not reall

    ill, as such; we have skin conditions of course, but that

    not the same, and our minds are pretty much OK.’A ver

    interesting remark made by a woman on the same war

    was that she wished she could just leave her body in th

    hospital and get on with things, as she felt her mind wa

    still active, and her spirits were high. These people, then

    observe a clear-cut mind/body dualism. They conside

    themselves to be in hospital because of a physical ma

    function which does not affect them mentally. But th

    activities of the hospital served to remind them of their ‘ilness’, and impose on them an awareness of their situatio

    as patients. James expressed this in the following com

    ment: ‘Sometimes when I’ve just woken up I lie there an

    think “I’d like to go for a nice walk”, but hearing every

    thing going on in the ward chases that idea away becaus

    I realize “Oh no, of course I can’t, I’m sick aren’t I?”’Th

    soundscape forced James to recognize his ‘patienthood

    More specifically, it was influential in making him accep

    and consequently live out one of the structures whic

    underpins the workings of the biomedical model, namel

    the distinct mind/body dualism implicit in the under

    standing of illness as an organic reality, a consequence o

     biochemical changes.

    As Helman observes, biomedicine increasingly undestands the body as ‘an animated machine’ (1984: 104). Th

    doctor’s project is to link observed symptoms to

     Fig. 4. The ear is implicated 

    in punishment and control. In

    1883, anthropologist 

     Alphonse Bertillion launched 

    a system for the identification

    of persistent criminals based 

    on a series of body

    measurements including the

    'length of the right ear'. It 

     proved very successful, and 

    was adopted by police in

     Britain and the United States,

     some even going so far as to

     suggest that all citizens should be physically

    inspected for such signs of 

    delinquency. The ears thus

    became integral to the

    development of a projected 

    bio-panopticon (see

    www.cmsu.edu/cj/ 

    alphonse.htm and

    www.oreilly.com/catalog/ 

    dbnationtp/chapter/ch03.html).

     Fig. 5. In traditional Chinese

    medicine the ear is closely

    connected with the meridians

    (energy pathways) relating to supplementary vessels and 

    organs throughout the body.

     It is both part of the body and 

    an organic representation of 

    the whole body.

  • 8/9/2019 SoundsSelves

    5/8

    of machines and monitors were an ever-present feature of 

    the hospital soundscape, their sounds impassive and clin-

    ical, representing what Sullivan describes as ‘phonetic dis-

    memberment’, atonal electronic sound which is ‘parcelled

    out one phoneme at a time’ (1986: 24). The soundscape

    was characterized by signals and notifiers, conversation

    was punctuated by their pulsing. These sounds generated

    feelings of alienation in some patients. ‘When I lie there

    and listen to those bleeps and blinks I feel as though I were

    a lab rat, something in a laboratory, you know’ said Carrie

    from the female cardiology unit. ‘It’s very strange when

    even in the night all these machines are bleeping away,’remarked Brian, who was recovering from a stroke. ‘They

    never stop, and you think to yourself “What kind of place

    am I in?”’ The repetitive electronic noise served as a con-

    stant reminder to patients of their own illness and that of 

    others around them. One man recalled a time when the

     bleeping had merged strangely with the sobbing and

    wailing of a woman in the bed opposite, creating a bizarre

    contrast between the noise of a machine and that of human

    suffering. The sound added a frightening drama to his sit-

    uation, making him conscious of his vulnerability in the

     presence of technology, his exposure in a domain of scien-

    tific medical interest.

    Tyler (1984) equates the scientific discourse with

    Western visualism, an idea supported by Bronowski, who

    states that the ‘world of science is dominated by the sense

    of sight’ (1978: 11). The discussion of a soundscape,

    then, must represent something of an anomaly in an envi-

    ronment ordered according to the visual metaphors of a

    scientific medical discourse. But when looking at, or 

    rather, listening to the scheme of optical control

    employed within the hospital, this opposition apparently

    collapses. Indeed, sound appears to reinforce and com-

     plement the visual mechanism of authority rather than

    undermine it.

    In exploring the unlikely collusion of the sonic and the

    visual I draw on the words of Anthony, a man of 32, who

    had been given six weeks’ bed rest in order to allow asmashed elbow and a fractured pelvis to heal. This meant

    he was unable to get off his bed, except to wheelchair 

    himself to the toilet and to his daily physiotherapy ses-

    sions. He proudly informed me that he was known as the

    ward troublemaker. He had twice been caught smoking

    and once got drunk on vodka which one of his friends had

    smuggled in disguised as mineral water. He took great

     pleasure in recounting how he had succeeded in buying

    hash from someone. Anthony expressed a willingness to

    exchange information for pornographic magazines.

    Three Fiestas and a couple of Knaves later he agreed to a

    formal interview. His mischievous activities, he said, had

    only been possible since his transfer from the main ward

    to a small side ward. In the main ward he had been awareof being constantly watched by the nurses. ‘They were all

    over the bloody place,’ he said. ‘Always saying “What do

    reminded him that he was under observation, and durin

    the day their voices and activity meant he was unable t

    forget that authority was not far away. Rolling a cigarett

    on the windowsill one day, he thought he heard a nurs

    approaching; panicking slightly, he pushed his paper

    and tobacco out of the window. It was a false alarm.

    Earlier I considered Foucault’s analysis of Bentham’

    Panopticon in relation to the visually restrictive environ

    ment of the hospital. But the introspective organization o

    ward space represents only one dimension of the panopti

    system and its application to that particular institution. Thmainstay of the Panopticon is the principle of surveillance

    Conscious of the possibility that his every move is bein

    watched, the observed modifies his behaviour to conform

    to the dictates of a particular system. Thus he begins t

    watch himself, internalizing the disciplinary gaze. In th

    hospital environment, Foucault suggests, surveillanc

    ensures that patients observe the regulations.

    The Panopticon’s efficiency lies in its preventative char

    acter. The institution need never exercise its strength b

    intervening. Power is exercised spontaneously, and inter

    estingly, Foucault remarks, ‘without noise’ (1975: 206; m

    italics). His implication is that control may be exercise

    silently, stealthily and with precision. However, there is

    definite inconsistency here, for while sound is understooto be indicative of disorder and imprecision, contrastin

    sharply with silent optical control, the patient abov

    clearly experiences sound as an auditory complement t

    surveillance, reminding him of its omnipresence. Just a

    the click of a camera asserts that a picture has been taken

    or the whir of a closed-circuit TVcamera informs a perso

    that his or her movements are being observed, the sound o

    nurses moving suggests to patients that they are bein

    watched. The soundscape reinforces the system of visua

    surveillance. It implies the presence of panoptic contro

    creating in patients like Anthony a sense of being a con

    trolled, monitored patient self. Indeed, in Anthony’s cas

    the sound of the nurse reached him at a point when he wa

    deliberately concealing his actions from view. It pervade

    a space which the gaze could not reach. Sound thu

    extends the scope of panoptic possibility. We see th

    emergence of a Panaudicon, an acoustics of power which

    unlike the Orwellian notion of the ‘never-sleeping ear

    (1949: 174), is not manifested in the possibility of bein

    heard by a listening presence, but in hearing an authori

    tarian presence. The Panaudicon operates actively throug

    the subtle infiltration of sound into the patient’s awareness

    The patient becomes the bearer of a receptive ear of powe

    as well as the object of an active eye of power. The channe

    of the ear thus becomes an important ‘channel of power

    (Foucault 1975: 205).

    I have tried to establish here that the hospital soundscape is not composed of meaningless scraps of sound

    Rather, the acoustic elements which constitute the sound

    scape are the products of particular medical practice

    embedded in the discourse of biomedicine. Patients hav

    endowed these sounds with complex meanings, such tha

    the soundscape has become a symbol, a sonic articulatio

    of the patients’position. In some patients, then, we see th

    emergence of a sonically ordered sense of self, a ‘self

    hood’shaped by the acoustic dimension.

    Conclusion

    Echoing Sullivan (1986) and Feld (1996), this stud

    emphasizes the significance of sound in human experienc

    in terms of both knowledge and imagination. In doing soit stresses the immediate relevance of sonic meaning t

    ethnographic inquiry, and the need to nurture auditor

     Fig. 7. Van Gogh may well 

    have experienced auditoryhallucinations during the

     psychotic attack in which he

    cut off the lower half of his

    left ear. He noted that in the

     sanatorium where he stayed 

    other patients heard strange

     sounds and voices as he had,

    and he speculated in one

    case that this was probably

    due to a disease of the nerves

    in the ear. He may have cut 

    off his ear to silence the

    disturbing sounds.

    Bull, M. 2000. Sounding out 

    the city: Personal stereos

    and the management of 

    everyday life. Oxford,

     New York: Berg.

    Bronowski, J. 1978. The

    origins of knowledge and 

    imagination. New Haven:

    Yale University Press.

    Bubandt, N. 1998. The odour of things: Smell and the

    cultural elaboration of 

    disgust in Eastern

    Indonesia. Ethnos 63(1):

    48-80.

    Classen, C. 1990. Sweet

    colours, fragrant songs.

     American Ethnologist 17:

    722-735.

    Classen, C. 1993. Worlds of 

     sense: Exploring the

     senses in history and 

    across cultures. London,

     New York: Routledge.

    Classen, C. 1998. The color 

    of angels: Cosmology,

     gender and aesthetic

    imagination. London,

     New York: Routledge.

    Darwin, C. 2003 [1871]. The

    d t f d

     Fig. 6. In his etchings,

     Leonardo da Vinci ensured 

    that the features of the face

    were in correct proportion by

    matching them to the

    dimensions of the ears, nose

    or eyes. This image is of da

    Vinci's framework of facial 

     proportions superimposed 

    onto a tracing of a sketch by

    him.

  • 8/9/2019 SoundsSelves

    6/8

    endeavour, the discussion is focused more on how patients

    experience sound in terms of the construction and ordering

    of the self than on the manner in which the senses are

    engaged in emplacement. It is through exploring the

    acoustemology of the self that this study attempts to sound

    out new ethnographic depths.

    I have sought to demonstrate here how the acoustic

    dimension of the hospital is heightened by a relative dep-

    rivation of other sensory modalities. Thus, the sense of 

    hearing gains a particular sensitivity and force. Much is

    heard while little is seen, smelled, touched or tasted. Butis not my intention to create new ‘sensory centrisms’

    (Bubandt 1998: 73), nor to argue the case for an avenue of 

    auditory supremacy, subverting the primacy of visualism

    in Western conceptual models through the construction of 

    an anti-visualism. My point is that for some patients,

    experiencing the hospital creates a slightly different per-

    ceptual emphasis, ‘a re-evaluation of the senses from the

    standpoint of their interplay’ (Feld 1996: 96). I have dis-

    cussed this re-evaluation of the senses, exploring the

    meanings and associations patients accord to the sound-

    scape. It is through experience of the sonic dynamics of 

    hospital that patients become familiar with and under-

    stand the environment and practices in which they are

    caught up.Sullivan makes the following observation concerning

    the creative power of sound: ‘Sound achieves creation in

    different ways. The presence of a new sound or song can

    create a new form of existence’ (1986: 24). This creative

     power is evident in the accounts of the patients to whom I

    spoke. They suggest that the sonic structure of the hospita

     plays an integral role in the creation of the reality of tha

     place, and in the way they perceive themselves to exi

    within it. Hearing and the interpretation of sounds are thu

    understood to be vital to orientation in a social, as well a

    a material and spatial sense. After all, patients have no

    always been patients. It is partly through the hospita

    soundscape that they are able to situate themselves withi

    the biomedical discourse of which they have become

     part. In a sense the soundscape may be construed as a coe

    cive influence, imposing upon patients a role which themay consider disagreeable or inappropriate. Sound there

    fore becomes implicated in the subtle articulation an

    exercise of power.

    Finally, I would support Classen’s assertion that ‘[m]or

    than any other discipline, anthropology should seek t

    counter our tendency to perceive other cultures throug

    our own sensory model by attempting to understand them

    through their cultural model’ (1990: 722). But the explo

    ration of the hospital soundscape indicates a case in whic

    our sensory model is not an entirely appropriate tool fo

    understanding certain aspects of our own culture. If sen

    sory interplay is to be analysed meaningfully, even withi

    our own cultural milieu, changes are required in ethno

    graphic representation. There is a need to amplify and harmonize aural idioms in ethnographic texts in order t

    explore the complex interplay of senses in cultural prac

    tice. The descriptive power and vocabulary of soun

    studies must be developed in order to allow proper explo

    ration of the depths of sonic knowledge. !

    This drawing, of which a detail is reproduced on the front cover, is illustrator Miriam Navarro's response to the article 'Soundselves' by Tom Rice. The article explores the ways in which

    hospital patients are affected by the sounds which surround them. Far from being simply irritating by-products of the activities taking place within the hospital, sounds carry important 

    meanings for those patients who are compelled to listen, and whose daily lives are therefore filled with noise. The exploration of soundscapes provides an interesting challenge for 

    th l i t i th hi hl i l t f th di i li ’ t l d th ti l i i ti Y t th l f th hi h d t d th f h i t b

    Feld, S. 1996. Waterfalls of 

    song: An acoustemology

    of place resounding in

    Bosavi, Papua New

    Guinea. In Feld, S. &

    Basso, K.H. (eds) Senses

    of place, pp.91-135. Santa

    Fe: School of American

    Research Press.

    Feldman, A. 1994. From

    Desert Storm to Rodney

    King via ex-Yugoslavia:

    On cultural anaesthesia.

    In Serematakis, N. (ed.)The senses still:

     Perception and memory

    as material culture in

    modernity, pp. 87-107.

    University of Chicago

    Press.

    Foucault, M. 1975.

     Discipline and punish:

    The birth of the prison.

    London: Penguin.

    Helman, C.G. 1984. Culture,

    health and illness: An

    introduction for health

     professionals. Oxford:

    Butterworth-Heinemann.

    Howes, D. 1988. On the

    odour of the soul: Spatial

    representation and

    olfactory classification in

    Eastern Indonesia and

    Western Melanesia.

     Bijdragen 144: 84-113.

    Ingold, T. 2000. The

     perception of the

    environment: Essays on

    livelihood, dwelling and 

     skill . New York:

    Routledge.

    Kesey, K. 1973. One flew

    over the cuckoo’s nest .

    London: Picador.

    Lavater, J. C. 1789. The

    whole works of Lavater 

    on physiognomy (vol IV).

    London: W. Simmonds. Northoff, G, Schwartz, M.A.

    & Wiggins, O.P. 1992.

    Psychsomatics, the lived

     body, and anthropological

    medicine: Concerning a

    case of atopic dermatitis.

    In Leder, D. (ed.) The

    body in medical thought 

    and practice, pp. 139-154.

    Dordrecht, Boston,

    London: Kluwer 

    Academic Publishers.

    Orwell, G. 2000. Nineteen

    eighty-four . London:

    Penguin.

    Ree, J. 1999. I see a voice.

    London: Harper Collins.

    Sullivan, L. 1986. Sound andsenses: Towards a

    hermeneutics of 

     performance. History of 

     Religions 26(1): 1-33.

    Stoller, P. 1997. Sensuous

     scholarship. Philadelphia:

    University of 

    Pennsylvania Press.

    Tyler, S. 1984. The vision

    quest in the West, or what

    the mind’s eye sees.

     Journal of 

     Anthropological Research

    40: 23-40.

  • 8/9/2019 SoundsSelves

    7/8

  • 8/9/2019 SoundsSelves

    8/8

    Copyright of Anthropology Today is the property of Wiley-Blackwell and its content may not be copied or

    emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission.

    However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.