social isolation in america

23
T here are some things that we discuss only with people who are very close to us. These important topics may vary with the situation or the person—we may ask for help, probe for information, or just use the person as a sound- ing board for important decisions—but these are the people who make up our core network of confidants. How have these discussion networks of close confidants changed over the past two decades? We address that question here with data from a high-quality national probability survey that collected parallel data in 1985 and 2004. We find a remarkable drop in the size of core discussion networks, with a shift away from ties formed in neighborhood and com- munity contexts and toward conversations with close kin (especially spouses). Many more peo- ple talk to no one about matters they consider Social Isolation in America: Changes in Core Discussion Networks over Two Decades Miller McPherson Lynn Smith-Lovin University of Arizona and Duke University Duke University Matthew E. Brashears University of Arizona Have the core discussion networks of Americans changed in the past two decades? In 1985, the General Social Survey (GSS) collected the first nationally representative data on the confidants with whom Americans discuss important matters. In the 2004 GSS the authors replicated those questions to assess social change in core network structures. Discussion networks are smaller in 2004 than in 1985. The number of people saying there is no one with whom they discuss important matters nearly tripled. The mean network size decreases by about a third (one confidant), from 2.94 in 1985 to 2.08 in 2004. The modal respondent now reports having no confidant; the modal respondent in 1985 had three confidants. Both kin and non-kin confidants were lost in the past two decades, but the greater decrease of non-kin ties leads to more confidant networks centered on spouses and parents, with fewer contacts through voluntary associations and neighborhoods. Most people have densely interconnected confidants similar to them. Some changes reflect the changing demographics of the U.S. population. Educational heterogeneity of social ties has decreased, racial heterogeneity has increased. The data may overestimate the number of social isolates, but these shrinking networks reflect an important social change in America AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 2006, VOL. 71 (June:353–375) Please address correspondence to Miller McPherson at Department of Sociology, University of Arizona, 440 Social Sciences Bldg, Tucson, AZ 85621 ([email protected]) or Department of Sociology, Box 90088, Duke University, Durham, NC 27708 ([email protected]). Support for data collection was provided by National Science Foundation grant SES 0347699 to the first and sec- ond authors and by CIRCLE to Tom W. Smith. The first two authors presented earlier versions of these analyses at the 2005 Conference on Social Capital and Networks in Columbus, Ohio, and at the Social Capital Working Group at Duke University. The authors thank Howard Aldrich, Mark Chaves, Joe Galaskiewicz, Jerry A. Jacobs, Ken Land, S. Phil Morgan, Robert Putnam, Linda Renzulli, Barry Wellman, and three anonymous ASR reviewers for helpful comments. Peter V. Marsden provided details of his earlier work, allowing us to replicate his 1987 analyses; Tom W. Smith and Jimbum Kim at NORC provided valuable information about General Social Survey procedures and data issues.

Upload: alam-bakhtiar

Post on 09-Dec-2015

10 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

social solation

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Social Isolation in America

There are some things that we discuss onlywith people who are very close to us. These

important topics may vary with the situation orthe person—we may ask for help, probe forinformation, or just use the person as a sound-ing board for important decisions—but these arethe people who make up our core network ofconfidants. How have these discussion networksof close confidants changed over the past two

decades? We address that question here withdata from a high-quality national probabilitysurvey that collected parallel data in 1985 and2004. We find a remarkable drop in the size ofcore discussion networks, with a shift awayfrom ties formed in neighborhood and com-munity contexts and toward conversations withclose kin (especially spouses). Many more peo-ple talk to no one about matters they consider

Social IIsolation iin AAmerica: CChanges iin CCoreDiscussion NNetworks oover TTwo DDecades

Miller McPherson Lynn Smith-LovinUniversity of Arizona and Duke University Duke University

Matthew E. BrashearsUniversity of Arizona

Have the core discussion networks of Americans changed in the past two decades? In

1985, the General Social Survey (GSS) collected the first nationally representative data

on the confidants with whom Americans discuss important matters. In the 2004 GSS the

authors replicated those questions to assess social change in core network structures.

Discussion networks are smaller in 2004 than in 1985. The number of people saying

there is no one with whom they discuss important matters nearly tripled. The mean

network size decreases by about a third (one confidant), from 2.94 in 1985 to 2.08 in

2004. The modal respondent now reports having no confidant; the modal respondent in

1985 had three confidants. Both kin and non-kin confidants were lost in the past two

decades, but the greater decrease of non-kin ties leads to more confidant networks

centered on spouses and parents, with fewer contacts through voluntary associations and

neighborhoods. Most people have densely interconnected confidants similar to them.

Some changes reflect the changing demographics of the U.S. population. Educational

heterogeneity of social ties has decreased, racial heterogeneity has increased. The data

may overestimate the number of social isolates, but these shrinking networks reflect an

important social change in America

AMERICAN SOCIOLOGICAL REVIEW, 22006, VVOL. 771 ((June:353–375)

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

Please address correspondence to MillerMcPherson at Department of Sociology, Universityof Arizona, 440 Social Sciences Bldg, Tucson, AZ85621 ([email protected]) or Department ofSociology, Box 90088, Duke University, Durham,NC 27708 ([email protected]). Support fordata collection was provided by National ScienceFoundation grant SES 0347699 to the first and sec-ond authors and by CIRCLE to Tom W. Smith. Thefirst two authors presented earlier versions of theseanalyses at the 2005 Conference on Social Capital and

Networks in Columbus, Ohio, and at the SocialCapital Working Group at Duke University. Theauthors thank Howard Aldrich, Mark Chaves, JoeGalaskiewicz, Jerry A. Jacobs, Ken Land, S. PhilMorgan, Robert Putnam, Linda Renzulli, BarryWellman, and three anonymous ASR reviewers forhelpful comments. Peter V. Marsden provided detailsof his earlier work, allowing us to replicate his 1987analyses; Tom W. Smith and Jimbum Kim at NORCprovided valuable information about General SocialSurvey procedures and data issues.

Page 2: Social Isolation in America

important to them in 2004 than was the case twodecades ago.

Why is this question (and its disturbinganswer) significant? Social scientists know thatcontacts with other people are important in bothinstrumental and socio-emotional domains(Fischer 1982; Lin 2001). The closer andstronger our tie with someone, the broader thescope of their support for us (Wellman andWortley 1990) and the greater the likelihood thatthey will provide major help in a crisis (Hurlbert,Haines, and Beggs 2000). These are importantpeople in our lives. They influence us directlythrough their interactions with us and indirect-ly by shaping the kinds of people we become(Smith-Lovin and McPherson 1993).

Much of what we know about these core con-fidants comes from surveys that measure ego-centered networks—relationships from the pointof view of a single person. These data describethe interpersonal environment of an individual.They allow us to measure the degree to whichthat person is directly connected to differentparts of a social system and integrated into it atthe individual level.

Building on earlier network surveys (e.g.,Fischer 1982; Verbrugge 1977; Wellman 1979),the General Social Survey (GSS) measured thenational U.S. social system of ego-networks forthe first time in 1985 (Burt 1984; Marsden1987). Since then, our description of the coreinterpersonal environment for Americans hasbeen frozen in the mid-1980s. Of course, oneexpects major social indicators to change slow-ly, if at all. There is evidence, however, that thestructure of social relationships in the UnitedStates has shifted in recent decades. Putnam(1995; 2000), in particular, has heightened inter-est in networks by emphasizing links amonginterpersonal ties, voluntary association mem-bership, community well-being, and civic par-ticipation. He follows a rich tradition, datingback to de Tocqueville, in arguing that Ameri-cans’ ties to other members of their communi-ties help enhance our democratic institutions andpersonal well-being. While his ideas and evi-dence have generated much debate (Fischer2005; Sampson 2004), Putnam’s emphasis onthe importance of networks joins a wider liter-ature implicating social relationships in virtu-ally every important arena of life, from culturaltastes (McPherson 2004) to health (Bearman

and Moody 2004) to crime (Sampson and Laub1990).

To assess social change in American discus-sion networks, we replicated the 1985 networkquestions in the 2004 GSS, using the samequestion wording and highly similar data col-lection procedures. In this article, we first out-line what we know about the characteristics ofthe GSS questions—what kinds of networksthey tap, with what reliability and validity, andwhat kinds of issues they leave unanswered.We then compare the basic characteristics ofthese core discussion networks at the two timepoints, 1985 and 2004.1 Given that the differ-ences, especially in network size, are very large,we consider methodological factors that mightbe biasing our results, and we provide data onthese issues when possible. Finally, we decom-pose the differences in major network charac-teristics into meaningful methodological andsubstantive sources. We conclude with a dis-cussion of the potential importance of our find-ings.

CORE DDISCUSSION NNETWORKS: WWHATKINDS OOF TTIES AARE WWE MMEASURING?

THE QUESTIONS

When researchers study interpersonal environ-ments, a key issue is what type of relationshipthey want to measure. The ideal, of course,would be to assess several types of relationship(e.g., friend, coworker, advisor) and then to usethose multi-layered data to find common pat-terns (see Fischer 1982 for an excellent exam-ple). Given the time constraints of a nationalface-to-face survey, the 1985 GSS instead

354—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

1 The GSS asked the same question in 1987 as partof a module on political participation. In 1987, how-ever, the survey did not ask sociodemographic char-acteristics and interconnections among network alters.The only network tie characteristics assessed werepolitical party affiliation and whether or not therespondent discussed government or political matterswith the person mentioned. Therefore, while we notefindings from the 1987 data in a few comparisons,we base our primary analyses on the comparisonbetween 1985 and 2004. (For a more extensive analy-sis of 1987, see Online Supplement on ASR Website: http://www2.asanet.org/journals/asr/2006/toc051.html)

Page 3: Social Isolation in America

focused on a relation that was general, cogni-tively definable, and significant: it asked peo-ple with whom they discussed personallyimportant topics.

In his earlier study of California communi-ties, Fischer (1982) used a similar questionabout discussing personal matters. He foundthat this relationship elicited relatively strongpersonal ties with a good representation of bothkin and non-kin. These close relationships havetheoretical importance because they are cen-tral in social influence and normative pressures(Burt 1984:127), and have strong conceptualconnections to earlier survey measures of bestfriends and other close socio-emotional ties.Different ways of asking about important, closeinterpersonal relationships (often called strongties) tend to be convergent.2 Many ways of ask-ing such questions get the same close ties.

These close ties are only a small subset of aperson’s complete interpersonal environment,which also includes a much larger array of weakties, which are more distant connections to peo-ple. Weak ties may occur in just one institutionalcontext or may connect us to people who are lesslike us in many ways (demographically, politi-cally, or culturally; see Granovetter 1973;McPherson and Smith-Lovin 1981). Estimatesof the larger network of weak ties range between150 (Hill and Dunbar 2003) to more than athousand (see review in Marsden 2005).

In 2004, we replicated a substantial subset ofthe network questions. Specifically, the 1985and 2004 GSS asked the following questions:

From time to time, most people discuss importantmatters with other people. Looking back over thelast six months—who are the people with whomyou discussed matters important to you? Just tellme their first names or initials. IF LESS THAN 5NAMES MENTIONED, PROBE: Anyone else?

Please think about the relations between thepeople you just mentioned. Some of them may betotal strangers in the sense that they wouldn’t rec-ognize each other if they bumped into each otheron the street. Others may be especially close, asclose or closer to each other as they are to you.

Are they especially close? PROBE: As close orcloser to each other as they are to you?

The survey then asked about demographiccharacteristics of the discussion partner: whetherthe partner was male or female, his or her race,his or her education and age, and some aspectsof the respondents’ relationship with the dis-cussion partner. Then, the interviewer askedmore about the character of the relationship:

Here is a list of some of the ways in which peopleare connected to each other. Some people can beconnected to you in more than one way. For exam-ple, a man could be your brother and he maybelong to your church and be your lawyer. WhenI read you a name, please tell me all of the waysthat person is connected to you.

How is (NAME) connected to you? PROBE:What other ways? (The options were presentedon a card: Spouse, Parent, Sibling, Child, Otherfamily, Co-worker, Member of group, Neighbor,Friend, Advisor, Other).

WHAT THESE QUESTIONS MEASURE

(AND MISS)

People’s reports of their connections with otherpeople are not perfect reflections of their actu-al interactions (Bernard, Killworth, and Sailer1982). On the other hand, people are quite goodat remembering long-term or typical patterns ofinteraction with other people (Freeman,Romney, and Freeman 1987). Therefore,answers that respondents give to network ques-tions on surveys often represent their typicalinterpersonal environment rather than whatev-er researchers specifically asked them. As onemight expect, respondents report frequentlycontacted, close, core network ties with thosewhom they have many types of relationshipsmore reliably than they do more distant, simplerelations (Kogovsek and Ferligoj 2004). Theseclose ties are also more accessible in memoryand tend to be listed first in a survey response(Brewer 1995; Burt 1986; Verbrugge 1977).Socio-emotional ties tend to be named morereliably than strictly instrumental relationships(Burt 1986).

Several studies have explicitly compared theGSS “important matters” question to other typesof network measures to see what types of peo-ple respondents are mentioning in response tothis very general inquiry. For example, Marin(2004) compared the GSS question with a more

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–355

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

2 Ruan (1998) reported that the wording was shift-ed from the “personal matters” of Fischer’s NorthernCalifornia Community Study to “important matters,”because respondents varied greatly in their under-standing of “personal” and sometimes interpretedthis term in very narrow ways.

Page 4: Social Isolation in America

complete network list generated with the helpof extensive probes. The people most likely tobe mentioned in the GSS question are strong,close ties who are more connected to others inthe network (because one name helps therespondent to recall others). Ruan (1998) exam-ined the overlap of names generated by the GSSquestion and other network questions based onexchange relations in China. She found that theGSS discussion question accounted for animportant part of a Chinese personal network.The people with whom the Chinese respon-dents discussed important matters were alsolikely to spend leisure time with the respon-dents and to be their confidants for personalmatters. The respondents expected them to offersubstantial help or to possess important socialresources. Similarly, Burt (1997), in a study ofmanagers, found that the GSS question elicit-ed high overlap with people whom the managerssocialized with informally and considered theirmost valued contacts, and who they would wantto ask for advice if they were considering a jobchange. These findings reinforce our sense thatthe important-matters question elicits the core,frequently accessed interpersonal environmentsthat people use for sociality and advice, andfor socio-emotional and instrumental support.

While clarifying what the GSS questionmeasures, we should also be clear about whatit does not measure. Most obviously, it doesnot measure what people talk about in theirrelationships. Several studies have asked thisinteresting question to help fill in the contentbehind these discussion networks (Bailey andMarsden 1999; Bearman and Parigi 2004;Straits 2000). The studies agree that importantmatters vary dramatically from respondent torespondent, encompassing relevant personalmatters (intimate relationships, finances, health,hobbies, and work problems), as well as moregeneral topics such as political issues. Theyalso agree that there are significant respon-dent–alter interactions in what types of topicsare considered important (Bearman and Parigi2004; Straits 2000). Not surprisingly, respon-dents talk about different things with theirspouses (children, education, finances) thanwith their coworkers and neighbors (communi-ty, politics, work). Indeed, Bailey and Marsden(1999) found that a sizable minority of theirsample interpreted the question in terms of fre-quency or intimacy of relationships, rather than

about a specific instance of discussion of a par-ticular important matter. Reinforcing this view,Bearman and Parigi (2004) found that somepeople cited apparently mundane matters likegetting a hair cut when asked the topic of theirlatest discussion about important matters.Luckily, Bailey and Marsden (1999) also foundthat measures of key network characteristics(e.g., density, range, heterogeneity) tended notto vary across different interpretations of thequestion.

In summary, the GSS network question aboutthose with whom one discusses important mat-ters elicits a close set of confidants who areprobably routinely contacted for talk about bothmundane and serious life issues, whatever thosemight be for a given respondent. They representan important interpersonal environment for thetransmission of information, influence, and sup-port. We would be unwise to interpret theanswers to this question too literally (e.g.,assuming that a specific conversation aboutsome publicly weighty matter had occurred inthe past six months), but these answers do giveus a window into an important set of close, rou-tinely contacted people who make up ourrespondents’ immediate social circle.

DATA AAND AANALYSES

The GSS is a face-to-face survey of the non-institutionalized United States adult popula-tion.3 The 1985 and 2004 surveys used the samequestions to generate the names of confidantsand identical procedures to probe for addition-al discussion partners. Therefore, the surveyresponses represent a very close replication ofthe same questions and procedures at two points

356—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

3 The GSS uses a multistage probability samplingdesign, based on the U.S. Census. Therefore, the1985 survey was based on the 1980 Census data,while the 2004 survey was based on the 2000 Census.In both years, an interviewer administered the surveyin English. Therefore, respondents who did not haveadequate language skills to cope with verbal Englishare effectively eliminated from the population. The1985 survey used a paper questionnaire, while the2004 survey used CAPI (computer-aided) technolo-gy. In both cases, however, the interviewers record-ed all answers for the respondent.

Page 5: Social Isolation in America

in time, representing the same underlying pop-ulation in 1985 and 2004.

MEASURES

We use the same measures of network charac-teristics that Marsden (1987:123–24) used in hisdescription of the structure of 1985 Americaninterpersonal environments. Size is the numberof names mentioned in response to the “namegenerator” question. Since family members andnon-kin are fairly distinct institutional bases ofconnectedness, Marsden (1987) focused hisanalysis on the kin and non-kin composition ofthese networks. We present these comparisons,and the distribution of ties across the entirerange of possible relationships measured by theGSS question (Spouse, Parent, Sibling, Child,Other family, Coworker, Member of group,Neighbor, Friend, Advisor, Other).

Marsden (1987) also was concerned about therange or concentration of the interpersonal envi-ronment, recognizing the well-known fact thattightly connected, closed interpersonal envi-ronments tend to be made up of similar othersand to provide fewer independent sources ofinformation. The contrast between range andconcentration also affects normative pressures—both in terms of pressure to conform and theresponsibility for support in times of need. LikeMarsden, we use density of the interpersonalnetwork as an indicator of network concentra-tion (the inverse of range). It is defined as themean intensity of tie strength among the dis-cussion partners mentioned. The GSS data arecoded 0 if the respondent reports that two of hisor her confidants are total strangers, 1 if they areespecially close, and 0.5 otherwise. We alsoinclude additional measures of tie strength,duration, and frequency of contact with the per-son mentioned. Tie duration was measured witha question about how long the respondent hadknown his or her confidant. Frequency of con-tact was measured by asking how often therespondent talked to the alter. Marsden (1987)also measured diversity of the interpersonalenvironment along several sociodemographicdimensions—age, education, race (Black,White, and Other), and sex—by taking the stan-dard deviation of the confidants in the case ofage and education and the Index of QualitativeVariation (IQV) for race and sex.

ANALYSES

We begin with an analysis that directly parallelsMarsden (1987), the much-cited description ofthe interpersonal environments published in theAmerican Sociological Review for the 1985data. In each case, we first replicated Marsden’s(1987) analyses on the unweighted 1985 GSSdata. We then applied weights to make the datarepresentative of the national population.4 Todescribe the basic parameters of discussion net-works, we replicate the Marsden (1987) tablesusing appropriate weights for both 1985 and2004. Then, we decompose the difference incore discussion network size using analysesthat allow us to control for demographic changesacross the two decades, to examine some pos-sible changes in reactions to the survey process,and to check for interactions of these variableswith year. The negative binomial regressionanalysis (a change from Marsden 1987),acknowledges the fact that our dependent vari-able is a count variable. Finally, we use logisticregression analysis to distinguish social iso-lates and those who report at least one discus-sion partner.

RESULTS

NETWORK SIZE

The major finding of this study is in the firsttwo columns of Table 1: the number of discus-

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–357

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

4 We note that the 1985 results in our tables differvery slightly from those of Marsden (1987). TheGSS sampling frame actually selects householdswithin blocks; the survey therefore must be weight-ed by the number of adults in the household eligiblefor the survey in order to constitute a representativesample of individuals in the population. Marsden(1987) presented statistics based on an unweightedsample. In 2004, the weighting scheme was slightlymore complicated. After an initial round of data col-lection was completed, half of the non-contactedrespondents were selected for intensive follow-up.These cases must therefore be weighted twice whatthe first round respondents are weighted. Since weare most interested in a representative description ofthe discussion networks of individual Americans in1985 and 2004, we use the appropriate weights inboth years. The weighting issues, while complex, donot influence the substantive conclusions of ouranalysis.

Page 6: Social Isolation in America

Table 1. Size of Discussion Networks, 1985 and 2004b

Total Discussion Network Kin Networka Non-Kin Networka

Network Size 1985 2004 1985 2004 1985 2004

0 10.0% 24.6% 29.5% 39.6% 36.1% 53.4%1 15.0% 19.0% 29.1% 29.7% 22.4% 21.6%2 16.2% 19.2% 21.0% 16.0% 18.1% 14.4%3 20.3% 16.9% 11.7% 9.4% 13.2% 6.0%4 14.8% 8.8% 5.8% 4.0% 6.8% 3.1%5 18.2% 6.5% 2.8% 1.3% 3.4% 1.4%6+ 5.4% 4.9% .— .— .— .—

Mean 2.94 2.08 1.44 1.12 1.42 .88Mode 3.00 .00 .00 .00 .00 .00SD 1.95 2.05 1.41 1.38 1.57 1.40

Note: N (1985) = 1,531; N (2004) = 1,467.a Information on kinship was collected on the first five alters cited. Therefore, the sum of kin and non-kin altersis not equal to the overall network size distribution.bIn all tables for this paper, cases are weighted to reflect the population. Weight variable for 1985 is a function ofthe number of adults in the household (ADULTS), while the weight variable for 2004 is WT2004NR.

358—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

sion partners in the typical American’s interper-sonal environment has decreased by nearly oneperson (from a mean of 2.94 to a mean of 2.08).The modal number of discussion partners hasgone from three to zero, with almost half of thepopulation (43.6 percent) now reporting that theydiscuss important matters with either no one orwith only one other person. The decrease is espe-cially marked among those who report four orfive discussion partners: these respondents havegone from a third of the population (33.0 percent)to only 15.3 percent of the population. The smallnumber of people who report very large discus-sion networks (six or more) has decreased lessmarkedly, from 5.4 to 4.9 percent.

The next columns of Table 1 show that thismarked social change has occurred in both kinand non-kin discussion partners.5 Both havedropped from a mode of 1 to a mode of 0. Sinceboth kin and non-kin discussion partners havegone down, the proportion kin has increasedonly moderately across the 19-year span. The

average proportion kin has gone up from 0.49to 0.54). Marsden’s (1987) generalization thatAmerican’s core discussion networks are heav-ily constituted by family still holds.

All of the changes described in Table 1 arestatistically significant (as is the change in pro-portion kin). The distributions on all three vari-ables differ significantly from 1985 to 2004, andthe means are all significantly smaller in 2004.Indeed, it is easier to list the few things thathaven’t changed: the standard deviations of allthree variables have remained relatively stable,and are not different by year.

TYPES OF RELATIONSHIPS

Table 2 looks in more detail at the types of rela-tionships that the respondents have with theirconfidants, to allow us to see where this largesocial change is occurring. The top panel showsthe percentages of respondents who mentionedat least one discussion relationship of each type.Since the overall discussion network size hasgone down dramatically, we expect that the rep-resentation of each type of relationship willalso go down. The interesting aspects of Table2 are the deviations from that expected pattern.Most notably, more people in 2004 report dis-cussing important matters with a spouse (38.1percent) than in 1985 (30.1 percent). The pro-portion of respondents who mention at leastone parent as a core discussant has decreased

5 Marsden (1987) coded the respondents whoreported six or more discussion partners as 6.5. Wefollow that practice here. Marsden based his decisionon personal communications with Tom Smith, theProject Director of the General Social Survey, aboutthe distribution of these respondents. In 2004, thatinformation about networks above size 5 was notrecorded (Smith, personal communication).

Page 7: Social Isolation in America

only slightly (from 23.0 to 21.1 percent).Notable for their sizeable decreases are co-member of a voluntary group and neighbor,both representing types of community ties thathave been stressed in the public policy debateover civic engagement (e.g., Putnam 2000).

The relationships labeled “other,” while smallin number, are an interesting residual category.While unmarried partners are included in thespouse/partner relationship, some respondentsdo not consider the family of a partner to be partof the respondent’s own family. So, a boyfriend’smother, a girlfriend’s mother, or a partner’s son-in-law appear here as an uncoded relationshiptype (rather than being placed by the respondentinto the category “other family”). Similarly, ex-family members no longer have family status forsome respondents. Respondents reported dis-cussing important matters with ex-spouses,spouse’s ex-spouses, son’s father, and such.Several respondents mentioned support peopleor professional service workers (e.g., mother’scaretaker, child’s teacher).6

Since our interest in these close personalcontacts is driven partly by their ability to shapeflows of information, influence, and affiliation,the bottom panel of Table 2 shows the percent-ages of respondents who have networks with dif-ferent levels of reach. In addition to the largeproportion of respondents who have no one totalk to, we find that the percentage of peoplewho depend totally on a spouse for such closecontact has increased from 5.0 to 9.2 percent.The proportion of people who talk to non-spousekin (who are likely to reside outside their ownhousehold) has dropped (58.8 to 42.9 percent).The most striking drop, however, is in the per-centage of people who talk to at least one per-son who is not connected to them throughkinship, a decline from 80.1 to 57.2 percent.These latter ties are the most likely to bridgesocially distinct parts of the community struc-ture, since we know that marriage and familyties are more homophilous on class, religion,race, and several other social attributes thanties formed in other ways (McPherson, Smith-Lovin, and Cook 2001).

NETWORK DENSITY AND RANGE

After the dramatic social changes representedin Tables 1 and 2, Table 3 seems a picture of sta-bility. The core discussion networks remain

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–359

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

6 In addition, a few respondents had recently talkedto people who were probably not parts of their reg-ular core discussion network about important matters(e.g., a state senator, the previous owner of my prop-erty, a landlord, or an acquaintance).

Table 2. Respondents Who Had Various Relationships with at Least One Confidant

Type of Relationship to Respondenta 1985, % (N = 1,531) 2004, % (N = 1,467)

No Confidant 10.0 24.6**Spouse 30.2 38.1**Parent 23.0 21.1**Sibling 21.1 14.1**Child 17.9 10.2**Other Family Member 18.2 11.8**Coworker 29.4 18.0**Comember of group 26.1 11.8**Neighbor 18.5 7.9**Friend 73.2 50.6**Advisor 25.2 19.2**Other 4.5 3.1**

Spouse is only Confidant 5.0 9.2**At Least One Non-spouse Kin 58.8 42.9**At Least One Non-kin Confidant 80.1 57.2**

Note: The table displays, for example, “What percent of the sample mentioned a spouse/parent/etc. as a personwith whom they discussed important matters?”a Since more than one type of relationship can be mentioned for any given discussion partner (e.g., a coworkercan also be a co-member of a group, an advisor and a friend), the percentages do not sum to 100.** p < .01 (two-tailed tests).

Page 8: Social Isolation in America

Table 3. Structural Characteristics of Core Discussion Networks

1985 (N = 1,167a) 2004 (N = 788b)

Network Density—<.25 9.9% 7.3%—.25–.49 18.5% 11.8%—.50–.74 37.9% 39.5%—>.74 33.7% 41.4%——Mean .60 .66——SD .33 .33Mean Frequency of Contact (days per year)—6–12 3.7% 3.0%—>12–52 15.3% 10.6%—>52–365 81.0% 86.4%——Mean 208.92 243.81——SD 117.08 114.86Length of Association (in years)—>0–4.5 12.1% 10.7%—>4.5–8+ 87.9% 89.3%——Mean 6.72 7.01——SD 1.34 1.00Age Heterogeneity (standard deviation of age of alters)—<5 25.8% 29.1%—5–<10 24.6% 19.7%—10–<15 24.3% 23.9%—>15 25.3% 27.3%——Mean 10.35 10.34——SD 6.96 8.1——Population Age Heterogeneity 20.89 18.37Education Heterogeneity (standard deviation of alters’ educations)—0–1 31.9% 34.7%—>1–2.5 41.0% 45.2%—>2.5 27.0% 20.1%——Mean 1.77 1.48——SD 1.52 1.38——Population Educ Heterogeneity 3.59 3.17Race Heterogeneity (Index of Qualitative Variation)c

—0 91.1% 84.5%—>0 8.9% 15.4%——Mean .05 .09——SD .18 .26——Population IQV .34 .53Sex Heterogeneity (Index of Qualitative Variation)0 23.8% 24.2%.01–.90 39.9% 37.6%>.90 36.3% 38.1%——Mean .67 .68——SD .43 .46——Population IQV .99 1.00

a Density and heterogeneity measures are meaningful only for respondents who mentioned more than one alter.The actual Ns for different analyses vary somewhat because of missing data, ranging from 1167 for race and sexto 1132 for education.b The number of respondents is considerably lower in 2004 than in 1985 because fewer respondents mentionedtwo or more alters. Again, the actual Ns vary because of missing data, from 788 for race and sex to 776 for edu-cation.c Different race categories are used in 1985 and 2004 (because the 2004 GSS was changed to conform to the new2000 Census usage. For these analyses, we have re-coded the 2004 categories to match the 1985 codes.

360—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

Page 9: Social Isolation in America

very densely interconnected, with mean densi-ties of 0.60 and 0.66 respectively in 1985 and2004. This density is the average level of inter-connection among named confidants. Recallingthat a code of 1 represents the confidants beingcloser to each other than they are to the respon-dent, these networks are quite tightly woven.This pattern was noted by Marsden (1987:126)and remains strong in 2004. There is a slightshift toward even more interconnected networksin 2004, a pattern that is supported by analysesof frequency of contact and duration of tie. Thetypical respondent now sees his or her closeconfidant more than once a week, on average,and has known him or her for more than sevenyears. In general, the core discussion networksin 2004 are more closely tied to each other, aremore frequently accessed, and are longer-termrelationships. Even more than in 1985, the dis-cussion networks we measure in 2004 are theclosest of close ties.

We can also examine the character of theinterpersonal environments by examining thediversity of the people mentioned as core dis-cussion partners. Table 3 also looks at the het-erogeneity of conf idants in terms of age,education, race, and sex. Here, again, we see apicture of relative stability. The mean hetero-geneity of the discussion networks is signifi-cantly less than the heterogeneity of the overallpopulation, reaffirming the well-known findingthat networks are homophilous (McPherson etal. 2001). The relatively subtle changes in thediversity of the discussion networks seem tomirror the demographic changes in the popu-lation. Age and education heterogeneity havegone down in the general population, mainlybecause of cohort succession, and the diversi-ty of discussion networks has gone down slight-ly to reflect that fact. Racial diversity has goneup in the population (through immigration anddisparate fertility rates), and has increased in dis-cussion networks as well. (Analyses not report-ed here also indicate that more people now havea confidant of another race. That is, respondentand his/her confidant are more likely to differby race in 2004 than in 1985.) Sex heterogene-ity has not changed significantly in the overallpopulation, and is remarkably stable in corediscussion networks as well.

Kin structures create definite patterns in net-work diversity, of course. Family members knoweach other, and they may be close (even closer

than they are to the respondent). In addition,having kin in one’s network tends to increasecontacts across age categories (through con-tacts with grandparents, parents, or children),educational strata (because of cohort differ-ences in educational stock), and sex (because ofthe heterosexual nature of marital unions and thesex composition of sibship), while it reducesheterogeneity of network ties on race, religion,geographic origin, and other matters(McPherson et al. 2001; Marsden 1987:Table 2).

Comparing 1985 and 2004, we see that mostof the effects of the proportion of kin in one’score discussion network on the interconnect-edness and diversity of network contacts arequite stable over the time period. Since these pat-terns are relatively well known, we present themin an Online Supplement and comment onlyon signif icant changes here (see OnlineSupplement on ASR Web site: http://www2.asanet.org/journals/asr/2006/toc051.html).Having kin as confidants tends to make one’snetwork more interconnected and dense—sincekin tend to know each other and perhaps beclose. This effect, however, is somewhat lessmarked in the 2004 data than in the 1985 data.Regressing density on proportion kin producesan OLS coefficient of .26 in 2004, as comparedto .36 in 1985; the proportion kin coefficientinteracts significantly with year).7 Furthermore,the predicted value of density when a networkhas no kin in it has increased in 2004 comparedto 1985, indicating that even non-family dis-cussion partners are now more likely to knoweach other and be close.

The effect of kin on age heterogeneity in dis-cussion networks has increased, probablybecause of changes in cohort structure.Networks of kin are more age diverse now thanin the 1980s, while discussion networks with-out kin are more age homogeneous. The largestchange, by far, is in the coefficient-related pro-portion kin in the discussion network to educa-tional heterogeneity. Marsden (1987: Table 2)

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–361

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

7 Following Marsden, we use OLS, although onemight make an argument for more sophisticated tech-niques that reflect that some of the data are bound-ed at 0 and 1. We suspect that Marsden used OLSbecause it is more familiar to most journal readers.None of the substantive conclusions varies by method.

Page 10: Social Isolation in America

finds a large positive OLS coefficient (.42, p <.01). The weighting by adults in the householdchanges this coefficient much more than mostother findings, reducing the effect to .30 (stillstatistically significant at p < .01). The impactof kin on educational diversity is much lower in2004 (a coefficient of .20) and is no longer sta-tistically significant. Both kin and non-kin net-works have gotten less educationallyheterogeneous by 2004—primarily because ofcohort succession and the increasing educa-tional stock of the population as a whole. Thedifference in cross-educational contact, poten-tially important for both the framing of issuesand the flow of information, no longer variessignificantly if one has only kin for confidantsor no kin at all in one’s discussion network.

DEMOGRAPHIC VARIATION IN NETWORKS

Marsden (1987) also examined how importantdemographic categories varied in terms of theirinterpersonal environments. Table 4 here repro-duces some of the most important analysesshown in Marsden’s (1987) Tables 3 and 4. Weuse OLS to see how age, education, race, andsex influence the size, kin composition, anddensity of one’s core discussion networks.

Age, which structured networks significant-ly in 1985, has very little impact on contempo-rary conf idant networks. Marsden (1987:127–28, Table 3) found a curvilinear pattern,with network size (especially non-kin confi-dants) dropping off quite precipitously withincreasing age and the proportion kin beingsomewhat higher among younger respondentsand the elderly ages. In contrast, age is notstrongly related to size or kinship compositionin 2004. None of the nonstandardized coeffi-cients regressing the network characteristics onage and age squared is statistically significant,and the multiple correlation between age, agesquared, and network characteristics is not sig-nificantly different from zero. Clearly, therehas been cohort succession since 1985; the verysocially active generation that fought WorldWar II is getting less numerous (especially in thenon-institutionalized population).

More highly educated people have more peo-ple to talk to about things that are important tothem. In fact, the impact of education on thenumber of family confidants has actuallyincreased since 1985. It is still true, however, that

more educated people have a lower proportionof kin in their networks than people with lesseducation.

In the confidant networks of men and women,we see that women still have significantly morekin in their networks than men do, but they nolonger have fewer non-kin confidants than men.Since the size of both the kin and non-kin coef-ficients has gotten smaller from 1985 to 2004,we find that women no longer have a signifi-cantly higher proportion of kin in their net-works when compared with men. Since thekin-dominated nature of women’s networks isone of the staples of the social capital literature(c.f., Moore 1990), this social change is poten-tially important. It is especially noteworthy thatthe shift occurs not because women are droppingkinship ties, but rather because they are achiev-ing equality with men in non-kinship ties.Unfortunately, as with growing wage equality,the equity is being achieved by men’s shrinkinginterconnection with non-kin confidants ratherthan by women’s greater connection to the worldoutside the family.

Race continues to have a broad impact on net-works in American society. Both blacks andother-race respondents have smaller networks ofconfidants than white Americans (the referencecategory). This pattern is most apparent in kin-ship networks, which are markedly smalleramong non-whites.

A PPRELIMINARY SUMMARY

OF SOCIAL CHANGE IN NETWORKS

In spite of a large literature on declining civicengagement and neighborhood involvement,we began this analysis with the expectation thatnetworks of core confidants would be a stablefeature of one’s interpersonal environment.Given the close, densely interconnected natureof the ties generated by the GSS question, itseemed unlikely that the typical American wouldnot mention several people in response. Wewere clearly wrong. The number of confidantsmentioned in 2004 is dramatically smaller thanin 1985. Both kin and non-kin ties havedecreased, although the change is larger in non-family ties. In the past two decades, discussionnetworks have focused on the very close fami-ly ties of spouse/partner and parent, while thepotentially integrative ties of voluntary group

362—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

Page 11: Social Isolation in America

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–363

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

Tab

le 4

.D

iffe

renc

es b

y A

ge, E

duca

tion

, Sex

and

Rac

e in

Net

wor

k S

ize

and

Kin

/Non

kin

Com

posi

tion

Dep

ende

nt V

aria

bles

Net

wor

k S

ize

# of

Kin

# of

Non

-Kin

Pro

port

ion

Kin

Den

sity

Inde

pend

ent V

aria

bles

1985

2004

1985

2004

1985

2004

1985

2004

1985

2004

Age

—A

ge.0

2NS

.02N

S–.

02N

S.0

1NS

.03

.00N

S–.

01.0

0NS

–.00

NS

–.01

—A

ge2

–.00

–.00

NS

.00N

S–.

00N

S–.

00–.

00N

S.0

0–.

00N

S.0

0NS

.00

—C

onst

ant

3.15

1.65

2.06

.86N

S1.

17.8

8NS

.69

.48N

S.6

0.8

2—

R2

.07

.00N

S.0

1.0

0NS

.07

.00N

S.0

3.0

0NS

.02

.01

Edu

cati

on—

Edu

c (y

rs)

.19

.15

.02

.05

.15

.08

–.03

–.01

–.02

–.01

—C

onst

ant

.57

.03

1.15

.45

–.47

–.28

.87

.79

.86

.86

—R

2.1

2.0

5.0

0.0

1.1

2.0

4.0

5.0

1.0

5.0

2S

exS

ex (

f=1)

–.05

NS

.19N

S.2

8.2

3–.

30–.

02N

S.0

7.0

1NS

.04N

S–.

00N

S

—C

onst

ant

3.02

1.78

1.28

1.00

1.59

.89

.49

.59

.58

.67

—R

2.0

0NS

.00N

S.0

1.0

1.0

1.0

0NS

.01

.00N

S.0

0NS

.00N

S

Rac

e/et

hnic

(W

hite

is r

efer

ence

cat

egor

y)—

Bla

ck–.

78–.

66–.

58–.

53–.

19N

S–.

12N

S–.

08N

S–.

08N

S.0

2NS

.00N

S

—O

ther

–.43

NS

–.64

–.45

–.49

.00N

S–.

11N

S–.

08N

S–.

11.0

7NS

.05N

S

—C

onst

ant

3.03

2.22

1.51

1.23

1.44

.91

.54

.61

.60

.66

—R

2.0

2.0

2.0

2.0

3.0

0NS

.00N

S.0

0NS

.01N

S.0

0NS

.00N

S

Not

e:D

ata

show

uns

tand

ardi

zed

OL

S r

egre

ssio

n co

effi

cien

ts o

f ne

twor

k va

riab

les

on r

espo

nden

ts’d

emog

raph

ic c

hara

cter

isti

cs. A

ll c

oef f

icie

nts

sign

ific

ant a

t p<

.01,

unl

ess

indi

cate

d as

not

sig

nifi

cant

(N

S).

Mar

sden

(19

87)

also

ana

lyze

d di

ffer

ence

s in

net

wor

k si

ze a

nd k

in c

ompo

siti

on b

y si

ze o

f pl

ace,

but

this

var

iabl

e ha

s no

t yet

bee

n co

ded

for

2004

so c

ompa

rabl

e an

alys

es a

re n

ot p

ossi

ble

at th

is ti

me.

(T

he s

ize

of p

lace

var

iabl

e is

add

ed to

the

data

set

aft

er th

e da

ta a

re c

olle

cted

, usi

ng th

e re

spon

dent

s’ad

dres

ses

and

curr

ent

Cen

sus

trac

t inf

orm

atio

n.)

Page 12: Social Isolation in America

membership and neighbor have decreased dra-matically.

Such a large, unexpected social change rais-es immediate questions. Therefore, in the nextsection we explore some reasons why the appar-ent difference between 1985 and 2004 might beartifactual. We also review other trends thatmight support or question our results.

COULD SSUCH AA LLARGESOCIAL CHANGE BBE RREAL?

Social change is best measured when bench-marks are frequent. Since our measurementsare 19 years apart, we have no way to assessdirectly whether or not the dramatically small-er 2004 networks are part of a slowly develop-ing trend. We therefore must consider threats tovalidity and look at related data to see if othertrends might show similar patterns.

STUDY DESIGN

The most common threat to trend measurementis change in the questions themselves. The GSSasked the same question in 1985 and 2004.While the important matters that respondentsdiscussed may have shifted with demographiccharacteristics or historical context, there is noreason to expect that the 2004 important-mat-ters question would not elicit the close, fre-quent confidants that it did in 1985. Interviewertraining and probe patterns also were very sim-ilar across the two surveys. The GSS imple-mented a number of changes in sampling frameand survey procedures during the two-decadeperiod in question, but these seem very unlike-ly to have created the observed pattern.8

CONTEXT

Question order is a vexing, important, andunderstudied aspect of survey design (see reviewin Smith 1989). Context effects are generally not

large, however, and tend to be concentratedwithin modules of questions on similar con-tent. In methodological experiments conductedin 1988, when the GSS core questions werechanged, Smith (1989) estimated that only sixout of 358 questions showed real context effects.

For questions like the ones of interest here,however, preceding questions can influencewhat one thinks of as important matters (Baileyand Marsden 1999; Bearman and Paragi 2004)and, to a lesser extent, which alters one names(Straits 2000). In 1985, the network questionswere preceded by a battery of questions on reli-gion. In 2004, they were preceded by a moduleof questions on voluntary group membership.While not identical, the fact that a large pro-portion of the voluntary sector is composed ofreligiously affiliated associations (Bonikowskiand McPherson 2006) means that the connec-tions that would be cognitively primed would besomewhat similar in both cases. If there were abias introduced by this contextual feature, onesuspects that it would lead to overreporting ofco-membership relationships in the 2004 net-work data (since the groups of which the respon-dent and his/her alters might have beenco-members had just been reviewed, and thetopics that they invoked presumably primed).Recall from Table 2, however, that co-mem-bership relations declined more than other typesof relations.

A more serious possibility is that the volun-tary organization questions in 2004 had a train-ing effect on respondents—effectively teachingthem that mentioning a larger number of affil-iations in response to an initial question wouldthen lead to more questions about each men-tioned connection.9 Luckily, the GSS networkquestions were partially repeated in 1987 in amodule on political participation. In 1987, thenetwork question appeared just after the batteryof questions on voluntary association. (In thiscase, the network question was not followed byqueries about the alter’s characteristics, butinstead was narrowed to a focus on politicaldiscussions. The wording of the initial namegenerator, however, was identical to that used in

364—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

8 The GSS shifted from PAPI (paper-recorded) toCAPI (computer-assisted) data collection during thisperiod and changed from a sampling frame based onthe 1980 Census to the 2000 Census. Response ratesalso fell somewhat beginning in 1998, although GSSrates are still much higher than comparable surveys.The response rate was 70.4 percent in 2004 and 78.7percent in 1985.

9 In 2004, each voluntary association type in whichthe respondent reported a membership generatedanother question about how many memberships ofthat type he or she had.

Page 13: Social Isolation in America

1985 and 2004.) In supplement data, we com-pare the limited analyses that can be replicatedcomparing 1987 and 2004, separated by 16years and both preceded by voluntary associa-tion modules (see Online Supplement on ASRWeb site). In these replications, we again finda dramatic drop in network size (from 2.63 in1987 to 2.08 in 2004, p < .01) and a dramaticincrease in the proportion of respondents withno core confidants (4.5 percent in 1987 and24.6 percent in 2004, p < .01). There may havebeen some tendency for the voluntary associa-tion context effect to suppress very large net-works. Comparing the 1987 data to the 1985data, we see fewer networks of sizes three, four,five, and more. Yet the voluntary associationcontext decreased the number of people whoreported no confidants; that proportion is actu-ally smaller in the 1987 data than in the 1985data (4.5 percent as compared with 10.0 per-cent).

The relationship between voluntary associa-tion membership and network size is positiveand roughly the same size in both surveys (a cor-relation of .22 in 1987 compared to a correla-tion of .18 in 2004). This relationship is asubstantively reasonable one: there is a large lit-erature on the interrelationship of networks andvoluntary groups (McPherson 1983, 2004;McPherson and Ranger-Moore 1991;McPherson, Popielarz, and Drobnic 1992).Ideally, of course, one would want an experi-ment embedded in the survey design thatassessed how context affected the network ques-tions. In time, such a measure of context effectshould be possible. The National ScienceFoundation has funded a re-interview of the2004 GSS respondents to further link their net-works and voluntary association membershipsthrough a life history calendar (BCS 0527671,“Niches and Networks: Studying the Co-evo-lution of Voluntary Groups and SocialNetworks,” $746,000). These interviews willbe conducted in the fall of 2006, two years afterthe original interview, and will allow both apanel study of the network questions and aninvestigation of context effects.

FATIGUE AND COOPERATIVENESS

With any question that requires a respondent-generated list, one must be concerned that peo-ple who are tired, uncooperative, or hostile

might attempt to speed the survey process alongby saying that they have few (or no) entries inthe list. The GSS is a long survey, lasting overan hour for many respondents. Therefore, onemust be concerned with fatigue effects, espe-cially if these effects differed in 1985 and 2004.

The network items occurred near the end ofthe survey in both years. The GSS asks a coreof sociodemographic and social trend questionsin each year,10 followed by modules of questionson various topics. In 1985, the first networkquestion was question 127 out of 148 total ques-tions. In 2004, the name generator question wasalso numbered 127, but this has less meaningin a CAPI survey where different questions takeon different positions depending on skip pat-terns. It occurred, however, after 109 questionsin the core and a module of questions aboutmembership in voluntary associations.

The GSS has the interviewer rate the coop-erativeness of the respondent immediately afterthe face-to-face session is completed (soon afterthe network questions in both years).Respondents are categorized as interested/friendly, cooperative, restless/impatient, or hos-tile. The 2004 respondents were no more like-ly to be impatient or hostile than were the 1985respondents (less than 4 percent in both years).The great majority of respondents were rated inthe most positive category (interested/friendly)in both years (79.3 and 82.2 percent in 1985 and2004 respectively).

As we expected, cooperativeness is stronglyrelated to the number of people who are report-ed as confidants, with hostile respondentsreporting almost two fewer confidants thaninterested and friendly respondents. There wasno statistical interaction, however, between thecooperativeness variables and survey year inpredicting the number of discussion partnersmentioned. To the extent that uncooperative-ness leads to underreporting of network ties, thisfactor seems to have operated in similar waysin both survey years. We also note that some ofthe relationship between cooperativeness andnetwork size might not be an artifact. Peoplewho are friendly and interested in the social

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–365

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

10 The core module was somewhat longer in 1985than in 2004, because of the major cut in NationalScience Foundation funding for the survey in themid-1980s.

Page 14: Social Isolation in America

situation of a face-to-face interview may also bemore sociable in other settings.

We also constructed an index of how manyquestions prior to the network module had miss-ing data for each respondent. Our logic wasthat refusal to answer preceding questions mightbe a behavioral indicator of fatigue or non-cooperativeness. Indeed, the number (out of 10)questions coded missing immediately prior tothe network module is correlated -0.16 with thenumber of network alters mentioned (p < .01).We therefore control for this index of missingdata in our multivariate analyses of networksize.11

CONVERGENT DATA FROM OTHER SOURCES

In the case of most major social changes,researchers can triangulate from multiple datasources at multiple time points to establish anoverall pattern with some certainty. Since schol-ars have rarely measured networks in a way thatcan be generalized to the national population,we have fewer resources here. There are, how-ever, two types of evidence that might reinforcethe data that we present.

The f irst source of convergent data isBearman’s and Parigi’s (2004) finding that 20percent of the North Carolinians that they inter-viewed in 1997 have no one with whom they dis-cuss important matters. The proportion of peoplewho report no confidants in the North Carolinastudy is consistent with the trend between the10.0 percent estimated from the 1985 GSS sam-ple and the 24.6 percent estimated from the2004 sample. In supplemental data, we alsonote that the 1987 GSS data show a movementtoward a lower network size (see OnlineSupplement on ASR Web site).

On the other hand, some telephone surveysof the national population asking questionsabout the number of close friends show rather

different results. In 1990, for example, theGallup Poll found that only 3 percent of theirsample reported no close friends; only 16 per-cent had less than three friends. While thereare many differences between the Gallup andGSS surveys, this raises the interesting questionof whether the important-matters question getsat closer, core ties than the concept of closefriend. Another recent telephone survey by Pewalso found much larger numbers of core or closefriends, when it asked about a combination oftypes of contact (Boase et al. 2006). Both ofthese surveys alert us to the possibility thatrespondents might be interpreting “discuss” ina literal way, and not including some types ofpersonal contact (see Conclusions section). Onthe other hand, the Pew survey has a responserate of 35 percent, while the GSS consistentlygets more than 70 percent of its sampled units.Our analyses (not reported here, but availablefrom the authors) of the 2004 weights used inthe GSS indicate that easily reached respon-dents are quite different from difficult-to-inter-view people in terms of their interpersonalenvironments. This fact reinforces the impor-tance of response rates in studies of affiliation,social networks, and civic participation.

The second area where we look for conver-gence is other trend data reported by the large,hotly contested literature on civic engagement.Putnam (1995, 2000) raised the issue of declin-ing embeddedness in civic and neighborhoodassociations to the attention of both policy-makers and scholars (especially in political sci-ence, where networks had not been a centraltopic previously). While there has been sub-stantial debate about his data and the down-ward trends that they indicate (c.f., Fischer2005; Paxton 1999; Rotolo 1999; Rotolo andWilson 2004; Sampson 2004), the decline thathe reports in socializing among neighbors andgeneral participation in social life beyond thelevel of the nuclear family fits well with ourobservations that association co-members,neighbors, and extended family are mentionedmuch less often as confidants in our surveydata. While researchers have contested Putnam’s(1995, 2000) reported declines in voluntaryassociation memberships, scholars have gen-erally confirmed his observations about thedownward trends in socializing with friendsand neighbors (e.g., Paxton 1999:114). Still,these declines have been very small compared

366—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

11 Since the index considers different questions in1985 and 2004, the actual levels of missing data can-not be directly compared. There is no sign, howev-er, that the overall levels of missing data were higherin the 2004 survey. In general, CAPI administrationleads to lower levels of missing data, because skip pat-terns are more efficient (Smith, personal communi-cation). The index of preceding questions that aremissing does not interact with survey year in pre-dicting the number of network alters.

Page 15: Social Isolation in America

Table 5. Multivariate Models of Discussion Network Size and Social Isolation

Model

Dependent Variable: Dependent Variable:Discussion Network Size No Discussion Partners

(Negative Binomial Regression) (Logistic Regression)

Independent Variable .I .II .III .IV .V

Constant 1.078 1.150 .477 –2.144 –1.297Wave (1 =2004) –.356 –.329 –.407 1.374 .214NS, b

Cooperative (Compared to Friendly/Interested) .— –.225 –.145 .126 NS .132 NS

Restless/Impatient .— –.667 –.585 1.295 1.308Hostile .— –1.121 –.985c 2.005 2.016Number Missing in Previous Module .— –.257 –.198 .372 .376Education (in yrs) .— .— .059 –.087 –.158Education* Wave .— .— .— .— .099Female .— .— .071c –.194NS –.195NS

Agea .— .— –.002 .016 .015Currently Married .— .— .061c –.256 –.253Black .— .— –.233 .942 .918Other Race .— .— –.308 .375NS .360NS

Alpha (Heterogeneity Coef.) .188 .153 .089 .N/A .N/AF 126.07 43.41 45.63 23.48 21.35

Note: N = 2,998. All coefficients significant at p < .01 unless indicated as not significant (NS).a The squared term for age was not significant.b Coefficient represents 1985 vs. 2004 difference for zero years of education, not the general effect of the 1985-2004 comparison, which is very strong, and statistically significant. See Figure 1 and Equation IV.c Significant at the p < .05 level.

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–367

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

to the social changes that we observe. For exam-ple, the decline in socializing with neighbors hasbeen about 3 percent over the past two decades.Respondents in 2004 are somewhat less likelythan those in 1985 to report that they can trustother people, think that they are fair (as opposedto taking advantage), and think that they arehelpful (as opposed to looking out for them-selves). The changes in these variables, howev-er, are in the order of 2 percent (fair) to 9.6percent (helpful)—again, small relative to thedrop that we see in core network size.

DEMOGRAPHIC CHANGE AS A

SOURCE OF NETWORK CHANGE

Of course, the demographic characteristics ofthe country have changed considerably in thetwo decades. Some of those changes could haveresulted in a shrinking network size even in theabsence of non-demographic social change. Asthe population gets older and more raciallydiverse, we would expect networks to get small-er, since older people and racial minorities havesmaller networks, on average. On the other

hand, the increasing education of the populationshould tend to increase network size. To assessthe extent to which basic demographic changeshave altered the landscape of interpersonal envi-ronments, we now move to a multivariate modelto examine change from 1985 to 2004.

CHANGE NNET OOF DDEMOGRAPHIC AANDMETHODOLOGICAL FFACTORS

We use negative binomial regression to modelthe size of discussion networks, because ourdependent variable is a count of network alters.Here, data from both the 1985 and 2004 GSSare combined, with the survey year acting as anindependent variable in the analysis. Table 5,Model I, illustrates the most important socialchange documented by our earlier analyses ofdiscussion networks: the number of confidantshas decreased significantly over the periodbetween the two surveys. This negative binomialcoefficient of –.356 (evaluated with the Y-inter-cept) corresponds to a drop of .86 network altersby 2004 (c.f., row 8 of Table 1, results round-ed). The coefficients in all models for Wave

Page 16: Social Isolation in America

(the 1985–2004 contrast) are a test of the nullhypothesis that differences in network sizebetween the two surveys are due to samplingerror.

Model II adds the indicators of fatigue andhostility that we suspect may lead respondentsto underreport their network ties. The more hos-tile the respondent gets, the more he or she islikely to report a small network. Having miss-ing data on questions that precede the networkquestions serves as an additional indicator ofsurvey problems. Controlling for these dataissues does not, however, significantly reducethe drop in discussion network size from 1985to 2004.

Controlling for demographic factors actual-ly increases the estimated difference in net-work size over the 19–year period (Model III).This effect occurs because education is posi-tively associated with network size, and educa-tional levels have increased over time. Thiseffect more than offsets the declines in networksize due to other factors such as the decliningproportion of the population that is married andthe growing minority population.

More educated and younger people have sig-nificantly larger discussion networks, as dowomen. Network size gradually shrinks withaging, and non-white Americans have fewernetwork resources. Marriage draws one intonetworks of people with whom one discussesimportant matters (notably one’s spouse, themost often-named type of relationship for thediscussion partner).12

Of course, there are many controls that wecould implement. The results in Table 5 repre-sent the major, stable, statistically significantdemographic sources of confidant networks.Some of the logically plausible socio-demo-graphic variables are not important sources ofnetwork variation in these data. For example, theopportunity structures represented by number ofsiblings, number of children, and number ofadults in the household do not significantlyaffect the number of confidants (possiblybecause these variables are highly correlatedwith marital status). Work status (whether rep-

resented as hours worked per week or as dummyvariables for full-time and part-time work) doesnot have an effect. Geographic mobility does notappear to have an impact, although our abilityto explore this factor is limited by the fact thatthe “size of place” variable has not yet beenadded to the 2004 GSS.13 Neither size of placeof residence at age 16 nor whether or not therespondent has moved geographically since age16 has an effect. While a full exploration of thenon-demographic sources of confidant networksis beyond the scope of this article, some com-monly used predictors like the number of hoursspent watching television are also unimportant(Putnam 2000). Therefore, we conclude thatthe large drop in confidant networks between1985 and 2004 in these data is unlikely to be aresult of population shifts on other variables.

Since negative binomial regression coeffi-cients are not as intuitively interpretable as OLScoefficients, we offer the following predictedvalues from Table 5 as illustration of our mainresult. In 1985, a white married 25-year-oldmale high school graduate who was an inter-ested, friendly respondent to the survey, andwho had no missing data on any of the 10 itemspreceding the network module, would be expect-ed to have slightly more than three confidants(3.3) with whom he discussed important mat-ters. In 2004, an interested, friendly fellow withthe same demographic characteristics wouldhave reported a network more than one altersmaller (2.2). Another way of viewing the samecomparison would be to age our friendly fellowby the 19 years of the study (from 25 to 44),leading to an even smaller network of 2.1 alters.

The resources represented by core networksmirror other major class divides in our society.Net of all other factors, increasing educationsharply increases the number of discussion part-ners that a respondent reports, from roughly1.5 alters for a person with the lowest level ofeducation in 1985, to around five alters for sucha person at the highest level of education. Thedifferences for 2004 are smaller, but just asstriking: from about one alter to over three

368—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

12 The marital statuses that indicate the absence ofa spouse (never married, widowed, separated,divorced) do not differ significantly from one anoth-er in their impact on confidant networks.

13 Size of place is coded directly from Censusrecords and the sampling unit designation, ratherthan being reported by the respondent. As of thisdate (January 2006), NORC has not yet completedthis coding.

Page 17: Social Isolation in America

alters. The differences between 1985 and 2004,however, remain salient even in the face of thismajor divide. In 1985, high school dropouts(with 10 years of education) had a network withroughly 2.8 discussion partners—in the rangeof a college graduate in 2004.

Figure 1 brings these stark differences ineducational trajectory to bear on the issue of kincomposition and network range. In both timeperiods, education promotes discussion withboth kin and non-family members, with tiesoutside the family affected most markedly. Thedifferent slopes of these two curves mean thatat some level of educational achievement thetwo curves cross. Discussion networks becomedominated by people outside one’s immediatefamily. In 1985, this cross occurred at around13 years of education, a little more than a highschool diploma. Discussion networks of thosewith some college comprised more non-familythan family; college graduates had more confi-dants outside their kin group than inside it. In2004, the non-kin ties have dropped so muchthat this crossover is not predicted to occur untila respondent has acquired post-college educa-tion. Clearly, the net effect of these changes is

to focus and limit the reach of core discussionnetworks in the general population.

THE SSHAPE OOF SSOCIAL IISOLATION

Given the close, dense nature of core discussionnetworks, one might argue that the crucial dis-tinction is not among different network sizes butbetween those who have someone to talk to andthose who report no one with whom they candiscuss matters that are important to them. InTable 5, Model IV, we present a logistic regres-sion analysis that contrasts those who did notname anyone in answer to the name generator(even after the obligatory probe by interview-ers) and those who did name a discussion part-ner. Most of the effects are what we would haveexpected from our earlier analysis of networksize. The data issues operate in the same man-ner—more cooperative respondents are lesslikely to be socially isolated, while those whohaving lots of missing data are more likely to beisolates. More highly educated, younger, cur-rently married people are less likely to be socialisolates.

The only notable change from Model IIIhere is that men are not significantly more

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–369

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

Figure 1. Ego Network Size for Kin and Non-Kin Ties, 1985 and 2004

Page 18: Social Isolation in America

likely than women to be social isolates in corediscussion networks. They may have fewer dis-cussion partners than women, but theynonetheless are as likely to have at least oneconfidant. Similarly, other-race people are notsignificantly different from whites (althoughblacks are still more likely than whites to beisolates).

In the analyses reported in Table 5, we test-ed for all possible two-way interactions betweensurvey year and predictor variable. In the logis-tic regression analysis of the probability ofbeing a social isolate, we found an interactionbetween survey year and education. Model Vshows that interaction. The effect of educationon the probability of being a social isolate isstrong and negative in 1985 (a coefficient of -0.158), and becomes somewhat less negative in2004 (–0.158 + 0.099 = –0.059).

Again, to make the logistic regression coef-ficients somewhat more vivid, we compute thepredicted probability that our white married25-year-old male high school graduate who isenthusiastically participating in the survey andleaving no missing data would have someone totalk to about important matters in 1985. He

would be virtually assured of a discussion part-ner (predicted probability of being an isolate =0.04). The same type of person in 2004 wouldhave a more a ten percent chance of being anisolate (predicted probability = 0.16). Do thesame mental experiment and age our 25-year-old to 44 years of age in 2004: we find that hisprobability of being an isolate would have quin-tupled from 0.04 to 0.20.

UNEVENNESS IIN TTHE SSOCIALCHANGE

Given that social change rarely affects the entirepopulation simultaneously, the relative lack ofinteractions seems somewhat strange. We there-fore explore in more depth possible uneven-ness in the network changes that we observe.While the change is unusually pervasive (prob-ably because of the 19-year gap in our assess-ment), there are some hints about uneven changein different social groups.

First, there is the statistical interactionbetween education and year in affecting theprobability of social isolation. This interactionis made clearer by inspection of Figure 2, which

370—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

Figure 2. Social Isolation Increases 1985–2004

Page 19: Social Isolation in America

plots the fitted probability of social isolationacross years of education in 1985 and 2004. Asthe figure shows, there is a very sharp increasein the probability of social isolation for all lev-els of education, but the greatest change occursin the middle range of education. In 1985,increasing education led to a sharp decline insocial isolation, while that effect is much lessevident in 2004. This change is one of the fewareas where inequality has gone down in oursociety. Unfortunately, the inequality is decreas-ing because everyone is getting worse off (if weassume that social isolation is bad).

We also inspected the data for interestingsubgroup differences, using the intersection ofrace, class, and gender as a general guide. Thedecline in networks is quite uniform, but young(ages 18–39), white, educated (high schooldegree or more) men seem to have lost more dis-cussion partners than other population groups(from 3.5 in 1985 to 2.0 in 2004). In the nextsection, we discuss the possible impact ofInternet usage on this demographic group.Young, poorly educated (less than high school),white women also experienced a large decline(3.2 to 1.4 alters).

Among African Americans, a gender differ-ence is striking. Older (60+) African Americanmen’s networks have declined the most (from3.6 to 1.8). Among black women, the change ismore uniform, with the young experiencing alarger decline than the old. Indeed, black menover 60 are the only sector of the older popula-tion that experienced a major decline between1985 and 2004. Otherwise, the elderly havebeen more stable than most other groups intheir core social connections.14 This articleleaves these possible subgroup changes in corediscussion networks to future analyses.

DISCUSSION AAND CCONCLUSIONS

If we assume that interpersonal environmentsare important (and most sociologists do), thereappears to have been a large social change in thepast two decades. The number of people whohave someone to talk to about matters that areimportant to them has declined dramatically,and the number of alternative discussion part-ners has shrunk. In his groundbreaking study ofsocial networks, To Dwell among Friends,Claude Fischer (1982:125–27) labeled thosewho had only one or no discussion ties withwhom to discuss personal matters as havingmarginal or inadequate counseling support. Bythose criteria, we have gone from a quarter ofthe American population being isolated fromcounseling support to almost half of the popu-lation falling into that category.

The American population has lost discussionpartners from both kin and outside the family.The largest losses, however, have come from theties that bind us to community and neighbor-hood. The general image is one of an alreadydensely connected, close, homogeneous set ofties slowly closing in on itself, becoming small-er, more tightly interconnected, more focused onthe very strong bonds of the nuclear family(spouses, partners, and parents). The educationlevel at which one is more connected throughcore discussion ties to the larger communitythan to family members has shifted up into thegraduate degrees, a level of education attainedby only a tiny minority of the population. Highschool graduates and those with some collegeare now in a very family-dominated social envi-ronment of core confidants.

Some of the basic parameters of discussionnetwork structure have moved very little in 19years. Age and sex heterogeneity of ties hasremained remarkably constant, and the declinein educational diversity seems directly linked tothe increasing education level of the population.Racial contact in these discussion networks hasactually increased. Having a network dominat-ed by family members still increases one’s con-tact with other ages and the other sex, while itmakes the interpersonal environment morehomogeneous with regard to race. The distinc-tion between family and non-family ties haslost its importance only for education. Wherefamilies used to link the more highly educatedyounger generations to less educated elders,

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–371

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

14 We should note that these observations are effec-tively discussing a post-hoc five-way (race/class/gen-der/age/year) interaction. Furthermore, the inspectionof such subgroups is complicated by this being acount variable: simple comparisons of means do nothave the same interpretation as for interval variables.For example, a change of 1 is a 50 percent change ina network size of 2, but only a 25 percent change ina network of size 4.

Page 20: Social Isolation in America

now kin and non-kin look similar in their edu-cational composition.

If core discussion networks represent animportant social resource, Americans are stillstratified on education and race. Higher edu-cation people have larger networks of both fam-ily and non-family members, and their networkshave more of the range that tends to bring newinformation and perspective into the interper-sonal environment. Non-whites still have small-er networks than whites. Sex, on the other hand,seems to have lost some of its interpersonalstratifying power in the past 19 years. Whilewomen still have marginally larger networksthan men and have more discussions aboutimportant matters with kin, they no longer showa significant deficit in the number of core con-tacts outside the family. As a result, women nolonger have a signif icantly more kinship-focused discussion network than men; nor arethey significantly less likely than men to besocial isolates.

Our final estimates, corrected for responseproblems and demographic shifts, are that (1)the typical American discussion network hasslightly less than one fewer confidant in it thanit did in 1985, and (2) that in 2004 an adult, non-institutionalized American is much more like-ly to be completely isolated from people withwhom he or she could discuss important mat-ters than in 1985. Given the size of this socialchange, we remain cautious (perhaps even skep-tical) of its size. The limited network data in1987 indicate that the proportion of people whoanswer “no one” and who list relatively largenumbers of confidants may be especially sen-sitive to context effects (see Online Supplementon ASR Web site). Given our analyses of thehighest-quality nationally representative dataavailable, however, our best current estimate isthat the social environment of core confidantssurrounding the typical American has becomesmaller, more densely interconnected, and morecentered on the close ties of spouse/partner.The types of bridging ties that connect us tocommunity and neighborhood have witheredas confidant networks have closed in on a small-er core group.

Since the GSS has few measures other thandemographic characteristics that were asked atboth points in time, we are not well positionedto explore the reasons behind the social change.Still, it is useful to speculate (with help from

other literature) to guide future research. Threeexplanations seem most likely.

The first two possibilities concern how peo-ple interpret the question that we asked them,in view of historical and cultural change. WhatAmericans considered important might wellhave shifted over the past two decades, perhapsas a result of major events (the attacks of 9/11and the wars that followed). If people think of“important” more in terms of national andworld-level events, more people might nowthink that they have nothing important to say.15

Since many people interpret the question assimply asking about their close confidants(rather than a particular discussion of importantmatters), it seems unlikely that such a shift incultural meaning would have produced such astrong effect. It may, however, have contributedto the pattern.

The second possibility is that the use of theword “discuss” in the question was interpretedby respondents to exclude other forms of com-munication that are becoming dominant in ourcontacts with core confidants. Many more peo-ple now use cell phones and Internet (email, listserves, chat rooms, and instant messaging) tocontact core network members (Wellman et al.2006; Boase et al. 2006). If people excludethese types of communications when answeringthe question, it could reduce the number ofalters reported.16

The third possibility is the most substantive-ly interesting. Shifts in work, geographic, andrecreational patterns may have combined to cre-ate a larger demarcation between a smaller coreof very close confidant ties and a much largerarray of less interconnected, more geographi-

372—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

15 Bearman and Parigi (2004) found that roughlyhalf of their respondents who reported discussingimportant matters with no one in the past six monthssaid that they had nothing to say.

16 The fact that cell phones and Internet commu-nications tend to mirror other channels of commu-nication makes this explanation less plausible as asource of major change. Still, the Pew Internet andAmerican Life project report shows that Americansreport an average of 23 core or very close ties (witha median of 15) when the questions includes three ele-ments—the people to whom Americans turn to dis-cuss important matters, with whom they are infrequent contact, or from whom they seek help (Boaseet al. 2006).

Page 21: Social Isolation in America

cally dispersed, more unidimensional relation-ships. Families, especially families with chil-dren, may face a time bind that comes fromlonger commutes and more work time(Hochschild 1997). As more women haveentered the labor force, families have added 10to 29 hours per week to their hours working out-side the home (Jacobs and Gerson 2001; Houtand Hanley 2002). The increase has been themost dramatic among middle-aged, better-edu-cated, higher-income families—exactly thedemographic group that fuels the voluntaryassociation system (McPherson 1983;McPherson and Ranger-Moore 1991). The nar-rowing of the education gap suggests that thisgroup—highly educated middle-class fami-lies—is where the declines in the number of corediscussion ties have been sharpest. Such fami-lies can use new technologies to stay in touchwith kin and friends—most notably cell phonesand the Internet. While these technologies allowa network to spread out across geographic spaceand might even enhance contacts outside thehome (e.g., arranging a meeting at a restaurantor bar), they seem, however, to lower the prob-ability of having face-to-face visits with fami-ly, neighbors, or friends in one’s home (Boaseet al. 2006; Gershuny 2003; 2and Erbring 2000;Nie, Hillygus, and Erbring 2002).17 Wellman etal. (2006:10–13) note that Internet usage mayeven interfere with communication in the home,creating a post-familial family where familymembers spend time interacting with multiplecomputers in the home, rather than with eachother. They suggest that computer technologymay foster a wider, less-localized array of weakties, rather than the strong, tightly intercon-nected confidant ties that we have measuredhere.

This may not be all bad, of course, since weknow that weak ties expose us to a wider rangeof information than strong, close ties. We alsoknow, however, that strong ties offer a widerarray of support, both in normal times (Wellmanand Worley 1990) and in emergencies (Hurlburt

et al. 2000). Only geographically local ties canoffer some services and emotional support withease (Wellman and Worley 1990).

Whatever the reason, it appears thatAmericans are connected far less tightly nowthan they were 19 years ago. Furthermore, tieswith local neighborhoods and groups have suf-fered at a higher rate than others. Possibly, wewill discover that it is not so much a matter ofincreasing isolation but a shift in the form andtype of connection. Just as Sampson et al. (2005)discovered a shift in the type of civic partici-pation, and the Pew Internet and AmericanSociety Report (Boase et al. 2006) showed ashift in modes of communication, the evidencethat we present here may be an indicator of ashift in structures of affiliation.

Miller McPherson is Professor of Sociology at theUniversity of Arizona and Research Professor ofSociology at Duke University. His current projectsinclude a test of his evolutionary model of affiliationwith nationally representative data funded by theHuman and Social Dynamics Initiative at theNational Science Foundation. The project will createa representative sample of voluntary groups andstudy the co-evolution of group memberships andnetworks over time.

Lynn Smith-Lovin is Robert L. Wilson Professor ofSociology at Duke University. She received the 2006Cooley-Mead Award from the ASA’s SocialPsychology Section and the 2005 LifetimeAchievement Award in the Sociology of Emotions. Herresearch examines the relationships among socialassociation, identity, action, and emotion. Her cur-rent projects involve an experimental study of justice,identity, and emotion as well as work with McPhersonon an ecological theory of identity (both funded bythe National Science Foundation).

Matthew E. Brashears is a Ph.D. candidate inSociology at the University of Arizona. A past win-ner of the Pacific Sociological Association and theSocial Psychology Section’s Graduate Student PaperAwards, he is interested in social networks and theirrole in information transmission and transformation.His dissertation focuses on examining the reciprocaleffects of attitudinal similarity and network formation.His past research has examined sex-based differ-ences in the strength of homophily. His currentresearch includes a cross-national examination ofStatus Construction Theory and a study of resist-ance to new ideas.

REFERENCES

Bailey, S. and Peter V. Marsden. 1999. “Interpretation

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–373

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

17 In our data, we find some suggestion of this timebind effect. Among people who worked either full-time or part-time, the number of hours worked has asignificant effect on increasing social isolation. Thateffect seems to be stronger in 2004 than in 1985 and1987.

Page 22: Social Isolation in America

and Interview Context: Examining the GeneralSocial Survey Name Generator Using CognitiveMethods.” Social Networks 21:287–309.

Bearman, Peter S. and James Moody. 2004.“Adolescent Suicidality.” American Journal ofPublic Health 94:89–95.

Bearman, Peter and Paolo Parigi. 2004. “CloningHeadless Frogs and Other Important Matters:Conversation Topic and Network Structure.” SocialForces 83:535–57.

Bernard, H. R., P. D. Killworth, and L. Sailer. 1982.“Informant Accuracy in Social Network Data: AnExperimental Attempt to Predict ActualCommunication from Recall Data.” Social ScienceResearch 11:30–66.

Boase, Jeffery, John Horrigan, Barry Wellman, andLee Rainie. 2006. “The Strength of Internet Ties.”Washington, DC: Pew Internet and American LifeProject, January.

Bonikowski, Bart and Miller McPherson. 2006.“Voluntary Associations.” In Handbook of 21stCentury Sociology, edited by Dennis L. Peck andClifton Bryant. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Brewer, D. D. 1995. “The Social Structural Basis ofthe Organization of Persons in Memory.” HumanNature 6:379–403.

Burt, Ronald S. 1984. “Network Items and theGeneral Social Survey.” Social Networks6:293–339.

———. 1986. “A Note on Socio-metric Order in theGeneral Social Survey Data.” Social Networks8:149–74.

———. 1997. “A Note on Social Capital andNetwork Content.” Social Networks 19:355–73.

Fischer, Claude S. 1982. To Dwell among Friends:Personal Networks in Town and City. Chicago,IL: University of Chicago Press.

———. 2005. “Bowling Alone: What’s the score?”Social Networks 27:155–67.

Freeman, L. C., A. K. Romney, and S. C. Freeman.1987. “Cognitive Structure and InformantAccuracy.” American Anthropologist 89:311–25.

Gershuny, Jonathan. 2003. “Web Use and Net Nerds:A Neofunctionalist Analysis of the Impact ofInformation Technology in the Home.” SocialForces 82:141–68.

Granovetter, Mark. 1973. “The Strength of WeakTies.” American Journal of Sociology 78:1360–80.

Hill, R. A. and R. I. M. Dunbar. 2003. “SocialNetwork Size in Humans.” Human Nature14:53–72.

Hochschild, Arlie. 1997. The Time Bind. New York:Metropolitan Books.

Hout, Michael and Caroline Hanley. 2002. “TheOverworked American Family: Trends and Non-trends in Working Hours, 1968–2001.” WorkingPaper, Survey Research Center, University ofCalifornia, Berkeley, CA.

Hurlbert, Jeanne S., Valerie A. Haines, and John J.

Beggs. 2000. “Core Networks and Tie Activation:What Kinds of Routine Networks AllocateResources in Nonroutine Situations?” AmericanSociological Review 65:598–618.

Jacobs, Jerry A. and Kathleen Gerson. 2001.“Overworked Individuals or OverworkedFamilies.” Work and Occupations 28:40–63.

Kogovsek, Tina and Anuska Ferligoj. 2004. “TheQuality of Measurement of Personal SupportSubnetworks.” Quality and Quantity 38:517–32.

Lin, Nan. 2001. Social Capitol: A Theory of SocialStructure and Action. New York: CambridgeUniversity Press.

Marin, A. 2004. “Are Respondents More Likely toList Alters with Certain Characteristics?Implications for Name Generator Data.” SocialNetworks 26:289–307.

Marsden, Peter V. 1987. “Core Discussion Networksof Americans.” American Sociological Review52:122–31.

———. 2005. “Recent Developments in NetworkMeasurement.” Pp. 8–30 in Models and Methodsin Social Network Analysis, edited by Peter J.Carrington, John Scott, and Stanley Wasserman.New York: Cambridge University Press.

McPherson, Miller. 1983. “An Ecology ofAff iliation.” American Sociological Review48:519–32.

———. 2004. “A Blau Space Primer:Prolegomenon to an Ecology of Affiliation.”Industrial and Corporate Change 13: 263–80.

McPherson, Miller, Pamela A. Popielarz, and SonjaDrobnic. 1992. “Social Networks andOrganizational Dynamics.” American SociologicalReview 57:153–70.

McPherson, Miller and James Ranger-Moore. 1991.“Evolution on a Dancing Landscape: Organizationsand Networks in Dynamic Blau Space.” SocialForces 70:19–42.

McPherson, Miller and Lynn Smith-Lovin. 1981.“Women and Weak Ties: Sex Differences in theSize of Voluntary Associations.” American Journalof Sociology 87:883–904.

McPherson, Miller, Lynn Smith-Lovin, and James M.Cook. 2001. “Birds of a Feather: Homophily inSocial Networks.” Annual Review of Sociology27:415–44.

Moore, Gwen. 1990. “Structural Determinants ofMen’s and Women’s Personal Networks.” AmericanSociological Review 55:726–35.

Nie, Norman H. and Lutz Erbring. 2000. “OurShrinking Social Universe.” Public Perspective11:44–45.

Nie, Norman H., D. Sunshine Hillygus, and LutzErbring. 2002. “Internet Use, InterpersonalRelations and Sociability: A Time Diary Study.”Pp. 215–43 in The Internet in Everyday Life, edit-ed by Barry Wellman and CarolynHaythornthwaite. Oxford, England: Blackwell.

374—–AMERICAN SSOCIOLOGICAL RREVIEW

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson

Page 23: Social Isolation in America

Paxton, Pamela. 1999. “Is Social Capital Decliningin the United States? A Multiple IndicatorAssessment.” American Journal of Sociology105:88–127.

Putnam, Robert D. 1995. “Bowling Alone: America’sDeclining Social Capital.” Journal of Democracy6:65–78.

———. 2000. Bowling Alone: The Collapse andRevival of American Community. New York: Simonand Schuster.

Rotolo, Thomas. 1999. “Trends in VoluntaryAssociation Participation.” Nonprofit and VoluntarySector Quarterly 28:199–212.

Rotolo, Thomas and John Wilson. 2004. “WhatHappened to the ‘Long Civic Generation’?Explaining Cohort Differences in Volunteerism.”Social Forces 82:1091–121.

Ruan, Danching. 1998. “The Content of the GeneralSocial Survey Discussion Networks: AnExploration of the General Social SurveyDiscussion Name Generator in a Chinese Context.”Social Networks 20:247–64.

Sampson, Robert J. 2004. “Neighborhood andCommunity: Collective Efficacy and CommunitySafety.” New Economy 11:106–13.

Sampson, Robert J. and John H. Laub. 1990. “Crimeand Deviance over the Life Course: The Salienceof Adult Social Bonds.” American SociologicalReview 55:609–27.

Sampson, Robert J., Doug McAdam, HeatherMacIndoe, and Simon Weffer-Elizondo. 2005.“Civil Society Reconsidered: The Durable Nature

and Community Structure of Collective CivicAction.” American Journal of Sociology.111:673–714.

Smith, Tom W. 1989. “Thoughts on the Nature ofContext Effects.” Methodological Report No. 66,National Opinion Research Center, September,Chicago, IL.

Smith-Lovin, Lynn and Miller McPherson. 1993.“You Are Who You Know: A Network Perspectiveon Gender.” Pp. 223–51 in Theory onGender/Feminism on Theory, edited by PaulaEngland. New York: Aldine.

Straits, Bruce C. 2000. “Ego’s Important Discussantsor Significant People: An Experiment in Varyingthe Wording of Personal Network NameGenerators.” Social Networks 22:123–40.

Verbrugge, L. M. 1977. “The Structure of AdultFriendship Choices.” Social Forces 56:576–97.

Wellman, Barry. 1979. “The Community Question:The Intimate Networks of East Yorkers.” AmericanJournal of Sociology 84:1201–31.

Wellman, Barry, Bernie Hogan, Kristen Berg, JefferyBoase, Juan-Antonio Carrasco, Rochelle Cote,Jennifer Kayahara, Tracy L.M. Kennedy and PhuocTran. 2006. “Connected Lives: The Project.” InNetworked Neighborhoods, edited by PatrickPurcell. London: Springer.

Wellman, Barry and Scot Wortley. 1990. “DifferentStrokes from Different Folks: Community Tiesand Social Support.” American Journal ofSociology 96:558–88.

SOCIAL IISOLATION IIN AAMERICA—–375

#2855-ASR 71:3 filename:71301-McPherson