shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · shape of the basal ganglia in...

10
Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandman a, , Kevin Head a , L. Tugan Muftuler b , Lydia Su c , Claudia Buss a,d,f , Elysia Poggi. Davis a,e a Early Human and Lifespan Development Program, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California Irvine, USA b Department of Neurosurgery, Medical College of Wisconsin, USA c Department of Radiological Sciences, University of California Irvine, USA d Department of Pediatrics, University of California Irvine, USA e Department of Psychology, University of Denver, USA f Institut für Medizinische Psychologie, Charité Centrum für Human- und Gesundheitswissenschaften, Charité Universitätsmedizin, Germany abstract article info Article history: Accepted 10 May 2014 Available online 17 May 2014 Keywords: Basal ganglia Putamen Intelligence Shape Volume Performance Current studies support the belief that high levels of performance and intellectual abilities are associated with increased brain size or volume. With few exceptions, this conclusion is restricted to studies of post-adolescent subjects and to cerebral cortex. There is evidence that bigger is bettermay not pertain to children and further, that there are areas of the brain in which larger structures are associated with cognitive decits. In 50 preadoles- cent children (21 girls) a structural survey of the brain (VBM) was conducted to determine and locate areas in which gray matter volume was associated with poor cognitive performance. Only increased gray matter volume in particular areas of the basal ganglia and specically the putamen was signicantly associated with poor perfor- mance on tests of memory, response speed and a general marker and subtests of intelligence. Based on the VBM ndings, volumetric analysis of basal ganglia structures was performed using FSL/FIRST. However, no signicant changes in total volume of putamen or other basal ganglia structures were detected with this analysis. The disagreement between measures of localized gray matter differences and volumetric analysis suggested that there might be local regional deformity rather than widespread volumetric changes of the putamen. Surface anal- ysis with FSL/FIRST demonstrated that bilateral outward deformation of the putamen, but especially the left, was associated with poor performance on several cognitive tests. Expansion of the globus pallidus and caudate nucleus also was associated with poor performance. Moreover a signicant association was detected between a reliable test of language-free intelligence and topographically distinct outward and inward deformation of the putamen. Expan- sion and contraction of the putamen as a predictor of intelligence may explain why this association was not ob- served with measures of total volume. These results suggest that deformity is a sensitive measure of function, and that distortion of the basal ganglia may be a neurophenotype for risk of developmental impairment. © 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Introduction Most studies in adults report that increased brain size or volume is associated with higher levels of intellectual abilities. Estimates suggest that increased gray matter volume can account for approximately 10% of the variation in adult intellectual ability (Toga and Thompson, 2005). The association between cognitive function and gray matter vol- ume or volumetric analysis of subcortical structures in preadolescent children however is largely unexplored (Luders et al., 2011; Wilke et al., 2003). In a study of children between the ages of 5 and 18 years, IQ ex- plained about 10% of variation in gray matter volume, primarily in the cingulate cortex and exclusively in the post-adolescent subjects (Wilke et al., 2003). Increased cortical thickness across a wide network of brain areas was associated with better performance on measures of general intelligence in a sample of subjects between the ages of 6 and 18, but the effects were primarily among the adolescent group (Karama et al., 2009) and were signicant but not observed to be specic for cognitive domains, such as verbal reasoning, calculation or vocabulary (Karama et al., 2011). In a multi-site study, Luders et al. (2011) reported primarily negative correlations between callosal thickness and intelligence in sub- jects between the ages of 6 and 17, however there were no signicant effects specic to the group of children between 6 and 8 years of age. A developmental shift from a negative association between intelligence and cortical thickness in early childhood to positive correlations later has been reported (Shaw et al., 2006) suggesting that the direction of the association between intelligence and cortical thickness may change with age. In support of this possibility, Wilke et al. (2003) reported NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93102 Corresponding author at: Early Human and Lifespan Development Program, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California Irvine, One University Drive, Orange, Ca 92866, USA. Fax: +1 714 628 8893. E-mail address: [email protected] (C.A. Sandman). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.020 1053-8119/© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect NeuroImage journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ynimg

Upload: others

Post on 28-Jul-2020

6 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

NeuroImage

j ourna l homepage: www.e lsev ie r .com/ locate /yn img

Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated withcognitive performance

Curt A. Sandman a,⁎, Kevin Head a, L. Tugan Muftuler b, Lydia Su c, Claudia Buss a,d,f, Elysia Poggi. Davis a,e

a Early Human and Lifespan Development Program, Department of Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of California Irvine, USAb Department of Neurosurgery, Medical College of Wisconsin, USAc Department of Radiological Sciences, University of California Irvine, USAd Department of Pediatrics, University of California Irvine, USAe Department of Psychology, University of Denver, USAf Institut für Medizinische Psychologie, Charité Centrum für Human- und Gesundheitswissenschaften, Charité Universitätsmedizin, Germany

⁎ Corresponding author at: EarlyHumanand LifespanDevof Psychiatry and Human Behavior, University of CalifornOrange, Ca 92866, USA. Fax: +1 714 628 8893.

E-mail address: [email protected] (C.A. Sandman).

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.0201053-8119/© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

a b s t r a c t

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Accepted 10 May 2014Available online 17 May 2014

Keywords:Basal gangliaPutamenIntelligenceShapeVolumePerformance

Current studies support the belief that high levels of performance and intellectual abilities are associated withincreased brain size or volume. With few exceptions, this conclusion is restricted to studies of post-adolescentsubjects and to cerebral cortex. There is evidence that “bigger is better”may not pertain to children and further,that there are areas of the brain inwhich larger structures are associated with cognitive deficits. In 50 preadoles-cent children (21 girls) a structural survey of the brain (VBM) was conducted to determine and locate areas inwhich graymatter volumewas associated with poor cognitive performance. Only increased graymatter volumein particular areas of the basal ganglia and specifically the putamenwas significantly associatedwith poor perfor-mance on tests of memory, response speed and a general marker and subtests of intelligence. Based on the VBMfindings, volumetric analysis of basal ganglia structures was performed using FSL/FIRST. However, no significantchanges in total volume of putamen or other basal ganglia structures were detected with this analysis. Thedisagreement between measures of localized gray matter differences and volumetric analysis suggested thatthere might be local regional deformity rather than widespread volumetric changes of the putamen. Surface anal-ysis with FSL/FIRST demonstrated that bilateral outward deformation of the putamen, but especially the left, wasassociated with poor performance on several cognitive tests. Expansion of the globus pallidus and caudate nucleusalsowas associatedwith poor performance.Moreover a significant associationwas detected between a reliable testof language-free intelligence and topographically distinct outward and inward deformation of the putamen. Expan-sion and contraction of the putamen as a predictor of intelligence may explain why this association was not ob-served with measures of total volume. These results suggest that deformity is a sensitive measure of function,and that distortion of the basal ganglia may be a neurophenotype for risk of developmental impairment.

© 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

Introduction

Most studies in adults report that increased brain size or volume isassociated with higher levels of intellectual abilities. Estimates suggestthat increased gray matter volume can account for approximately 10%of the variation in adult intellectual ability (Toga and Thompson,2005). The association between cognitive function and graymatter vol-ume or volumetric analysis of subcortical structures in preadolescentchildren however is largely unexplored (Luders et al., 2011; Wilke et al.,2003). In a study of children between the ages of 5 and 18 years, IQ ex-plained about 10% of variation in gray matter volume, primarily in the

elopment Program,Departmentia Irvine, One University Drive,

cingulate cortex and exclusively in the post-adolescent subjects (Wilkeet al., 2003). Increased cortical thickness across a wide network of brainareas was associated with better performance on measures of generalintelligence in a sample of subjects between the ages of 6 and 18, butthe effects were primarily among the adolescent group (Karama et al.,2009) and were significant but not observed to be specific for cognitivedomains, such as verbal reasoning, calculation or vocabulary (Karamaet al., 2011). In amulti-site study, Luders et al. (2011) reported primarilynegative correlations between callosal thickness and intelligence in sub-jects between the ages of 6 and 17, however there were no significanteffects specific to the group of children between 6 and 8 years of age. Adevelopmental shift from a negative association between intelligenceand cortical thickness in early childhood to positive correlations laterhas been reported (Shaw et al., 2006) suggesting that the direction ofthe association between intelligence and cortical thickness may changewith age. In support of this possibility, Wilke et al. (2003) reported

Page 2: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

94 C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

modest positive associations between deep gray matter structures andintelligence in younger children but stronger and significant associationsin the cingulate and higher brain structures in groups of adolescents andyoung adults.

These findings indicate that structure–function relations amongpreadolescent children are unclear at best and that the “bigger is better”hypothesismay not pertain to all areas of the brain. One candidate brainstructure that has not been examined in detail among preadolescentchildren is the basal ganglia. In a group of adolescent and post-adolescent subjects, increased brain volume in the orbitofrontal cortex,cingulate gyrus, cerebellum, and thalamus was significantly associatedwith higher IQ scores, but high density in the caudate nucleuswas asso-ciated with lower levels of intellectual function (Frangou et al., 2004).Complex findings were reported between cognitive function and basalganglia shape in a recent study of late adolescent subjects (Burgaletaet al., 2013). Patterns of expansion and inward displacement of subcor-tical areas including the putamen and caudate nucleus, especially in theright hemisphere, were associated with intellectual abilities. Howeveranother recent study (MacDonald et al., 2013) in a large sample of chil-dren collected from six laboratories reported that positive correlationsbetween intelligence and volumes of structures of the basal gangliaand striatum were significant in the left hemisphere and only in males.

There is evidence that impaired function among clinical groups isassociated with larger structures in the basal ganglia, specifically thecaudate and putamen. For instance bilateral enlargement of the puta-men and the left caudate nucleus was observed in a large group of 3–4 year old autistic children (Estes et al., 2011). Similarly, basal gangliavolumewas enlarged in schizophrenics, a group with well documentedimpaired executive function (Chakos et al., 1994; Hokama et al., 1995;Staal et al., 2000). Shape deformities in the basal ganglia have beenreported in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder including anoutward deformity in the superior, anterior portion of the bilateralcaudate and in the inferior, lateral portion of the left putamen(Choi et al., 2007). It is an important observation that medicationhistory does not account for these associations. Structures of basalganglia were increased in volume disproportional to total brain volumein medication-naïve children and adolescents with autism (Langenet al., 2007) and in untreated and unaffected siblings of schizophrenicpatients (Mamah et al., 2008). However there is evidence that childrendiagnosed with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) exhibitreduced volume of the caudate nuclei (Castellanos, 2002), perhapsrelated to delayed development arising at mid-adolescence (Silk et al.,2009).

The association between performance and basal ganglia size andstructure is not resolved especially related to preadolescent subjects.Many, but not all, studies suggest that increased size and outward defor-mity of structures in the basal ganglia, especially the caudate nucleusand putamen are linked to poor performance in healthy subjects andto clinical conditions with behavioral and cognitive impairments.These effects, with one exception (Estes et al., 2011), have been report-ed in post-adolescent subjects. However as reviewed above, severalstudies reported positive associations between performance andreduced basal ganglia size in clinical populations. The initial purposeof this study is to determine if there are areas of the brain in whichincreased size is associated with poor cognitive performance amongtypically developing young children. Significant associationswill be fur-ther assessed with specific topographical measures of size and shape.

Methods

Participants

Participants included 50 children (21 girls) whowere born at one oftwo hospitals in the greater Los Angeles area (UC IrvineMedical Center,or LongBeachMemorialMedical Center) andwere recruited fromongo-ing protocols of development. Children were eligible for participation if

they were between 6 and 10 (mean= 90.92mo± 12.26) years of age,right handed (determined using a modified version of the EdinburghHandedness Inventory) (Oldfield, 1971) and products of a single birth.Our typically developing sample comprised children with a stable neo-natal course (median Apgar score = 9, range 7 to 10) and withoutknown congenital, chromosomal, or genetic anomalies (e.g., trisomy21). Participants had no evidence in the newborn period of intraven-tricular hemorrhage (determined by ultrasound), periventricularleukomalacia, and/or low-pressure ventriculomegaly. Further, at thecurrent assessment, subjects had normal neurologicalfindings includingnormal ventricle size and there was no evidence of emotional, physicalor neurological problems reported in a structured interview usingthe MacArthur Health and Behavior Questionnaire (Armstrong andGoldstein, 2003). Parents and children gave informed consent andassent respectively for all aspects of the protocol, which was approvedby the Institutional Review Board for protection of human subjects.Because age at testing and sex of the child (Muftuler et al., 2011) can in-fluence brain structure and cognitive performance, both variables werecontrolled in all analyses.

MRI acquisition

Structural MRI scans were acquired on a 3-T Philips Achieva system.To minimize head motion, padding was placed around the head. Earprotection was given to all children. To further increase complianceand reduce motion, children were fitted with headphones and allowedto watch the movie of their choice while in the scanner. Following thescanner calibration and pilot scans, a high resolution T1 anatomicalscan was acquired in the sagittal plane with 1 mm3 isotropic voxeldimensions. An inversion-recovery spoiled gradient recalled acquisition(IR-SPGR) sequence with the following parameters was applied:Repetition rate (TR) = 11 ms, echo time (TE) = 3.3 ms, inversiontime (TI) = 1100 ms, turbo field echo factor (TFE) = 192, number ofslices: 150, no SENSE acceleration, and flip angle = 18°. Acquisitiontime for this protocol was 7 min.

After the images were acquired, initial quality control was per-formed by visual inspection of the acquired images while the subjectwas in the scanner. If there were noticeable motion artifacts, the scanwas repeated.

Voxel based morphometry (VBM) analysis

VBM is an automated structural analysis used to detect regionalmorphological differences in brain images obtained by MRI. Unlikevolumetric tracing methods, VBM does not depend on user-definedbrain regions, but rather presents a comprehensive localization ofdifferences in gray matter volume between subjects (Ashburner andFriston, 2000).

MRI data was processed for VBM analysis using SPM5 software(http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/) (SPM5 the Wellcome Departmentof Imaging Neuroscience, University College London). The structuralimages were bias field corrected, and segmented using an integratedgenerative model (Ashburner and Friston, 2005). Unified segmentationinvolves alternating between segmentation, bias field correction, andnormalization to obtain local optimal solutions for each process. Thepediatric Cincinnati Children's Hospital Medical Center a priori tem-plates (Wilke et al., 2002)were used to segment and spatially normalize(affine and 16 iteration non-linear transformations) the children'simages. The resulting images were modulated to correct voxel signalintensities for the amount of volume displacement during normaliza-tion. The normalized and segmented images were averaged across thechildren's datasets to produce sample specific graymatter,whitematter,and CSF a priori templates. The process was then repeated using thesample specific a priori templates resulting in subject specific deforma-tion maps for VBM analysis of 1 mm isotropic voxels. The normalized,segmented, and modulated images were then smoothed using a

Page 3: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

95C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

12 mm kernel to ensure that the data were normally distributed and tolimit the number of false positive findings (Salmond et al., 2002). Coor-dinates of themost significant voxel within each cluster were convertedfrom original Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates tothose of the Talairach brain atlas (Talairach and Tournoux, 1988) usingthe mni2tal utility (Matthew Brett 1999 GPL). Anatomical locations ofthe significant areas were based on the best estimate from the Talairachatlas using the Talairach Daemon Client (http://www.talairach.org/client.html).

Once thepreprocessingwas completed, the relation betweenperfor-mance on the neuropsychological tests and regional graymatter volumewas determined using regressionwith child age, sex and total graymat-ter volume as covariates. Consequently, the analysis detected regionaldifferences rather than overall, large-scale variations in gray matter.The statistics for VBM analyses were normalized to Z score. Clusterswere considered significant if they contained at least 100 voxels andremained significant after false discovery rate (FDR) correction for mul-tiple comparisons (p b .05) as recommended by Genovese et al. (2002).

Analytical strategy

Initially, VBM analysis of graymattermorphologywas implementedto survey areas of the brain associated with cognitive performance.Based on the findings from the VBM, direct measures of the shapesand volumes of subcortical structures were computed in areas of in-terest using FSL/FIRST software tools (FMRI Software Library [FSL]http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/). Those measures were used toinquire associations between cognitive performance and total volumeor shape differences in subcortical structures of interest.

Subcortical volume and shape analysis

For subcortical segmentation and shape analysis the FSL/FIRST soft-ware (http://fsl.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/fslwiki/FIRST) was used. FIRST is amodel-based segmentation/registration tool proposed by Patenaudeet al. (2011). It uses manually labeled brain image data as a trainingset that was provided by the Center for Morphometric Analysis (CMA),MGH, Boston. This training data comprises of 15 subcortical structuresthat were outlined by trained operators on T1 weighted MR imagesfrom 336 subjects. It contains a wide age range (4.2 to 72 years) andboth normal and pathologic brains. Therefore, the variations seen inthe typically developing brain were captured by this large database.

The software for analysis uses a two-stage affine process to registereach brain image onto anMNI152 template. The first stage of this regis-tration is performed on the whole brain. In the second stage, this inter-mediate brain image is weighted by a subcortical mask and affineregistration is applied one more time in order to achieve optimal regis-tration of the subcortical structures. Once the registration is estimated,inverse transformation is calculated and applied to the MNI152 tem-plate to bring it to the native space so that the original image voxelsdo not have to be interpolated. After a general registration is achieved,Bayesian Active Appearance Model (AAM) is used to register and seg-ment each subcortical structure precisely. AAM is an extension of ActiveShapeModel (ASM; Cootes et al., 1995) and it incorporates intensity in-formation in addition to the geometrical shape model. Each subcorticalstructure is parameterized as a surface mesh and then modeled as apoint distribution. The meshes can be reconstructed either in standard(useReconMNI option) or native space (useReconNative option). Inour experience, mesh generation in native space produced slightlymore accurate results. Hence that option was used for the analysespresented here. The useRigidAlign option was selected to remove poseeffects but scaling was not incorporated to preserve original sizes toensure that volumetric differences were preserved.

The developers of the FSL software applied thismethod to the exten-sive training set and eigenvectors of deformations were calculated. Tosegment the subcortical structures, the software searches through

eigenvectors to find the most probable shape instance. The softwareperforms the shape and appearance deformations along the eigenvec-tors, which represent a wide range of possible brain shapes and sizesas described above. Using a linear combination of these eigenvectors,the 3D mesh surface of a particular subcortical structure is deformedfrom the mean shape for the best registration onto the correspondingstructure in the subject's brain. Once this registration is done, the loca-tion of each vertex on the surface is recorded. This approach assumesthat each vertex corresponds to the same anatomical location acrosssubjects. Therefore, the mean shape of, and deviations from the mean,for each subject can be calculated and vertex-by-vertex statistical anal-yses can be performed to investigate various effects on the shape.

Once the brain images are processed through the FSL/FIRST pipeline,each segmentationwas inspected and found satisfactory. Then, statisticalanalyses were first conducted to study associations between putamenvolume and cognitive performance measures. Then similar analyseswere conducted on a per-vertex basis using multivariate GLM to studyshape differences. The results, adjusted for age, sex and total graymattervolume are shown at p b .05 FDR corrected. In addition, volumetrics forthe subcortical structures were derived using the scripts included inthe FIRST module.

Behavioral measures

Cognitive ability was assessed in two ways. First, children wereassessed with performance subtests of a standardized intelligence mea-sure (WISC IV). This assessment provides a language-free, performancebased assessment of general intelligence aswell as an assessment of spe-cific aptitudes. Second, childrenwere evaluatedwith a series of tasks se-lected to measure cognitive domains not specifically addressed in testsof general intelligence including declarative memory, motor and execu-tive functioning. These specific domains have established relations withfocused areas of the brain.

Standardized intelligence measuresChildrenwere evaluatedwith a standardizedmeasure, the Perceptual

Reasoning Scale of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IV). The Perceptual Reasoning Scale (PRS) measured perceptual andfluid reasoning, spatial processing, and visual motor integration andrequired 25–35 min to complete. This instrument was standardized,validated, and acceptedwithin the developmental and pediatric commu-nity. The three subtests that comprise the PRS are block design, pictureconcepts, and matrix reasoning. These subtests were selected becausethey are highly correlated with general intelligence (Baron, 2004;Raven et al., 1998; Wechsler, 2002) and they are relatively culturallyfair and language free (Baron, 2004).

For each subtest a raw score was calculated by summing the totalnumber of points obtained including baseline items. The raw scoresfor each of the three subtests were converted to a scaled score byreference to a developmental table. Conventional scoring of the Percep-tual Reasoning Scale creates a composite index by summing the scaledscores for each of the three subtests.

Block design. Block designmeasures the ability to analyze and synthesizeabstract visual stimuli. It involves nonverbal concept formation, visualperception and organization, simultaneous processing, and visual-motor coordination (Cooper, 1995; Groth-Marnat, 1997; Sattler,2001). In this task, the child viewed a constructed model or a picturein a stimulus book, and used red andwhite blocks to recreate the designwithin a specific time limit. Across successive trials the design becomesmore complex. Block design contains 14 items and the task wasdiscontinued when the child missed three consecutive items.

Picture concepts. This taskmeasured abstract, categorical reasoning abil-ity (Deák andMaratsos, 1998; Shulman et al., 1995). The child was pre-sented with two or three rows of pictures and asked to choose one

Page 4: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

Table 1Mean performance and (standard deviation) for the total cohort (n = 50) and for girlsand boys separately.

Variables Total sample Girls Boys

Age (months) 90.9 (12.3) 91.2 (11.6) 90.7 (12.9)Go/no-go reaction time 526.3 (75.2) 529.2 (79.8) 524.2 (73.1)Single alternation 11.98 (2.8) 12.2 (3.3) 11.8 (2.5)Double alternation 26.2 (14.1) 23.8 (12.4) 28.1 (15.2)Tapping dominant hand 34.1 (6.4) 32.7 (5.4) 35.2 (7.0)Tapping non-dominant hand 30.2 (5.2) 28.8 (3.9) 31.2 (5.8)Continuous memory old abstract 64.9 (39.5) 58.5 (53.7) 70.1 (22.3)Continuous memory old concrete 69.0 (39.9) 64.1 (55.9) 72.9 (19.8)WISC perceptual reasoning index 107.8 (16.1) 108.1 (9.8) 107.7 (19.6)WISC perceptual reasoning percentile 63.8 (29.6) 67.6 (21.3) 61.0 (34.5)WISC block design 10.5 (2.8) 10.7 (2.2) 10.3 (3.2)WISC picture completion 11.8 (3.1) 12.2 (2.8) 11.5 (3.4)WISC matrix reasoning 11.5 (3.5) 11.0 (2.8) 11.9 (3.9)

96 C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

picture from each row to form a group with a common characteristic.With successive trials the common characteristic becomes increasinglyabstract. Picture concepts contain 28 items and are discontinued whenfive consecutive errors are made.

Matrix reasoning.Matrix reasoning provides a reliablemeasure of visualinformation processing and abstract reasoning (Raven et al., 1998). Foreach trial, the child viewed an incompletematrix and selected themiss-ing portion from five response options. Matrix reasoning contained 35items and was discontinued when the child failed to give a correctresponse on four of the last five items. The four types of matrices thatcomprised this task are: continuous and discrete pattern completion,classification, analogical reasoning, and serial reasoning.

Computerized neuropsychological testsEstablished taskswere adapted for computer application to a pediatric

population. Responses were recorded with specialized colored responsebuttons or a light pen. All task instructions were prerecorded for stan-dardized presentation and at the start of the session childrenwere orient-ed to the system with a series of exercises designed to familiarize themwith all components of the system. Prior to the start of each task thechild was given performance dependent practice trials to ensure thatthey could perform the task.

Go/no-go task. A variant of the continuous performance test (CPT), thego/no-go task required the execution of an anticipated motor responseor its active inhibition. A child's inhibitory ability is a measure of impul-sivity and is associated with executive functioning. Participants wereprimed to press a button as quickly as possible in response to the pre-sentation of every letter of the alphabet, except for the letter “X”. Inthis task, there were two blocks of trials. The first block consisted of25 trials containing 100% target stimuli, used to prime participants torespond to the target stimuli. The second block was the response in-hibition condition, consisting of 50 trials containing 25 target stimuli(go trials) and 25 non-target stimuli (no-go trials). Decision speedfor correct responses was the primary variable for this assessment.

Set shifting test (SST). This executive function task measured the abilityto first learn a simple pattern and then abandon it when it was no longerreinforced, in favor of a newpattern. Specifically, childrenwere instructedto “guess” into which doghouse an animated dog would move. After achoicewasmade, the dogmoved according to a preprogrammed pattern.The first pattern was “single alternation” (left, right, left, right). After thechild correctly learned this pattern, as indicated by guessing the correcthouse 10 times in a row, the pattern changed to “double alternation”(left, left, right, right). Total percent correctwas the primary score derivedfrom this test.

Continuous recognition memory test (CRMT). The continuous recognitionmemory test (CRMT) employed nonverbal, visual stimuli (bothconcrete and abstract) to assess the ability to discriminate betweenpreviously presented (“old”) and “new” stimuli. The CRMT includedpictures of common objects that could be verbally labeled (“concrete”stimuli) and shapes that were not easily labeled verbally (“abstract”stimuli). Half of the 210 trials displayed concrete and half of the trialsdisplayed abstract stimuli and were presented for 500 ms, with2000 ms intervening between lags. Eighty stimuli each were presentedtwice at either a lag of two or five items. The remaining trials comprised50 distracter stimuli (not analyzed) that either were not repeated orwere repeated at non-standard intervals. Scores derived from this testfor this analysis were percent correct for the abstract items.

Finger tapping test (FTT). The FTT required the child to tap a button withhis/her index finger as fast as possible for 10 s without moving his/herarm or wrist. The FTT is used to evaluate lateralized motor ability aswell as the capacity to sustain motor functioning over a short time.

This task provides important information because it allows the separa-tion of pure motor speed from reaction times in the cognitive tasks. Atotal of 6 trials are administered (3 for each hand). Scores derivedfrom this test were the mean number of taps for each hand.

Performance on each of the tests is presented in Table 1. Data arepresented for the total sample and boys and girls separately. Therewere no significant differences between boys and girls on any of themeasures. Despite the large number of tests administered the associa-tion was low and nonsignificant for all the tests in the computerizedbattery. Subtests of the WISC shared ~25% variance and between 62%and 75% variance with WISC percentile.

Results

VBM analysis of gray matter morphology initially was implementedto address the central question of whether specific areas of increasedgray matter volume were associated with impaired cognitive perfor-mance in typically developing preadolescent children. Regression anal-yses indicated that only areas of increased gray matter volume in thebasal ganglia and specifically the putamenwere significantly associatedwith impaired performance. Significant associations illustrated in Fig. 1,indicate that lower scores on the WISC measure of intelligence(p b 0.05) and poorer scores on the block design (p b 0.05) from theWISC (which contributed significantly to the WISC score) were associ-ated with larger gray matter volumes in the putamen. Slower responsespeedmeasured from the dominant hand (p b 0.05) and poorer perfor-mance on the abstract (difficult) stimuli in the test of declarative mem-ory (CRMT; p b 0.05) also were significantly associated with increasedgray matter of the putamen. No significant negative associations wereobserved between gray matter volume and performance on pictureconcepts, matrix reasoning, non-dominant finger tap and the concrete(easy) trials of the CMRT.

The statisticalmaps that indicated brain areaswith higher graymattervolume associatedwith impaired cognitive performance on the four taskslisted above were loaded into xjView with a p b .05 uncorrected thresh-old. This analysis detected areas of the brain in which there was overlap-ping significance (e.g., Haier et al., 2008). The only area to survive thisanalysis was the putamen. The finding was associated with a lowp value and unlikely due to chance. If the performance measureshad been completely independent (there was a positive relationbetween two of the tests) the probability of this occurrence wouldhave been close to p b .054. Fig. 2 illustrates that poor performanceacross all tests was associated with increased volume only in the basalganglia and primarily the putamen.

Based on the findings from the VBM analysis, we expected thatvolumes in the putamen/basal ganglia would be largest in poorperforming children. However, one criticism of imaging procedures in-cluding VBM is that errors of misregistration can result from abnormal

Page 5: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

Fig. 1. VBM analysis illustrating areas of the basal ganglia, primarily the putamen in which greater gray matter volume is associated with impaired performance. Significant relations areobserved for WISC score (PRI), block design, right finger tapping and CMRT percentage correct for abstract stimuli.

97C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

anatomy and/or systematic shape differences in structures (Mechelliet al., 2005). We directly assessed with geometric algorithms, whethervolume of the putamen was associated with cognitive performance.There were no significant correlations between any of the tests andtotal volume or between performance and the volume of any basalganglia structures (p's all N .10).

The inconsistent findings between VBM and volumetric analysissuggested a different approach. Examination of Fig. 1 indicated thatthe increased gray matter volume in the putamen associated withpoor performance was topographically distributed. About 30% of theputamen volume was associated with performance, localized primarilyin the dorsal medial area. The possibility that the VBM effects werelocalized and related to the shape or regional deformity of the putamenwas assessed with FSL/FIRST and GLMmodeling.

Bilateral shape of the putamenwas significantly associated (GLM, allFDR corrected) with performance of the block design, matrix reasoningand the perceptual index of the WISC. As presented in Table 2, a

remarkable percentage of the putamen was expanded in associationwith poor test performance. Fig. 3 shows the relation between theVBM volume findings (insert) and the results of the shape analysis forblock design. Impaired performance of the block design subtest wasassociated with greater volume and expanded shape of the putamen.Thefigure clearly shows that poor cognitive performancewas associatedwith greater expansion of the left putamen (50.6%) than the right (26%)structure. Fig. 4 shows that in addition to the putamen, areas of theglobus pallidus and caudate nucleus also were significantly expandedin association with poorer performance on the block design subtest(Table 2).

Therewereno significant associations betweenmeasures of brain den-sity or graymatter volume and thematrix reasoning subtest but complexand significant relations were discovered with analysis of shape. As seenin Fig. 5, topographically discrete areas of expansion and compression ofthe putamenwere associatedwith performance on thematrix reasoning.Significant (F3,45 = 3.03, p = .04 [left], F3,45 = 3.31, p = .03 [right])

Page 6: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

Fig. 2. Common areas of the basal ganglia, primarily the putamen in which increased volume is associated with impaired cognitive performance.

98 C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

expansion of the head and tail and compression of the dorsal and ventralareas of the putamen were associated with good performance.

None of the computerized performance measures were associatedwith shape of the basal ganglia.

Discussion

The principal purpose of this study was to determine if there areareas in the preadolescent brain in which bigger is not always better.The general consensus is that increased gray matter in a large range ofbrain areas is associated with better cognitive performance, but thisconclusion has been restricted largely to post-adolescent subjects(Karama et al., 2009; Luders et al., 2011; Wilke et al., 2003). No studieshave examined brain–behavior associations in an exclusive, typicallydeveloping preadolescent subject population and the few studies thathave included children among their samples have not observed signifi-cant associations between brain size and performance in the youngersubgroups (Karama et al., 2009, 2011; Luders et al., 2011; Wilke et al.,2003). The evidence from the present study, utilizing three differentmethods of analysis, indicates that larger or outward surface deforma-tion of the areas of the basal ganglia and primarily expansion of themedial dorsal putamen is associated with impaired cognitive functionin typically developing young children.

Table 2Outward deformation of the putamen associated with impaired cognitive performance.

Block design F ratio DF p value % of structuredeformed

L-putamen 3.415 (3,45) 0.025 50.623R-putamen 4.032 (3,45) 0.013 26.012

Matrix reasoningL-putamen 3.031 (3,45) 0.039 77.882R-putamen 3.305 (3,45) 0.029 57.788

Perceptual indexL-putamen 3.063 (3,45) 0.038 78.972R-putamen 3.251 (3,45) 0.030 62.305

Outward deformation of globus pallidus and caudate nucleus associated withimpaired performance on the block design subtest

L-globus pallidus 3.659 (3,45) 0.019 38.629R-globus pallidus 3.322 (3,45) 0.028 57.321L-caudate 3.827 (3,45) 0.015 32.474R-caudate Not significant

The association between size and shape of the basal ganglia and cog-nitive performancemay be surprising because it is widely accepted thatthe basal ganglia are responsible for sensorimotor coordination, includ-ing response selection and initiation (Grahn et al., 2009) and are centersof integration for the execution of habitual or automatic movements(Herrero et al., 2002). Dysfunction of basal ganglia is associated withneurological disorders (Romero et al., 2008) especially those withprofound motor impairment including Parkinson's and Huntington'sdisease (HD) (Albin et al., 1995; DeLong, 1990; DeLong and Wichmann,2007; Graybiel et al., 2000; Wichmann and DeLong, 2011). However,consistent with our findings of cognitive but not motor associationswith structures of the basal ganglia, criticalmotivational, motor planning,procedural learning, associative and cognitive functions independentfrom motor skill or function have been reported for this brain area(Alexander et al., 1986; Brown et al., 1999; Ducharme et al., 2011;Knowlton et al., 1996; Nakano, 2000; Nicola, 2007; Rolls, 1994; Salinaset al., 2000; Yin and Knowlton, 2006). Moreover, electrophysiologicalstudies have demonstrated that cells of the putamen and caudate nucleusare active when primates perform an associative learning task that doesnot require a motor response (Romero et al., 2008).

Through complex cortical connections with prefrontal associationcortex and limbic cortex, the basal ganglia are thought to play a role incognitive function that is similar to their role in motor control. That is,the basal ganglia are involved in selecting and enabling various cogni-tive, executive, or emotional programs that are stored in cortical areas.The putamen and caudate nucleus are the main recipients of afferentsarising from the entire cerebral cortex and from the intralaminar nucleiof the thalamus (Kreitzer andMalenka, 2008). The projections from thecortex to basal ganglia are topographic. For instance the frontal lobeprojects predominantly to the caudate head and the putamen. Specifi-cally, the most dorsal aspect of the lateral prefrontal cortex (DLPC) pro-jects to the dorsal and central part of the caudate nucleus, and theventral DLPC projects to the ventral and central caudate nucleus. Themedial PFC preferentially projects to themedial caudate nucleus, nucleusaccumbens and ventral putamen and the orbital PFC projects to centraland lateral parts of the caudate nucleus and to the ventromedial puta-men (Tanji andHoshi, 2008). Thus the basal gangliamaymodulate a net-work of cortical and thalamic projections and pathways that contributeto the significant associations with behavior reported here.

The association between performance on the matrix reasoning testand basal ganglia deformation was especially interesting. Of the testsadministered to the children, it was the only one inwhich both compres-sion and expansion of the putamen and caudate nucleus were bilaterallyand topographically related to improved performance. The finding that

Page 7: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

Fig. 3. Areas from green to blue survive FDR p b .05 correction and showwhere larger areas of the putamen correlate negatively with performance on block design. Comparisonwith VBMfindings in the insert.

99C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

both expansion and compression were related to this test may explainwhy associations with performance were not observed with measuresof size or volume. The matrix reasoning test based on the Raven's pro-gressive matrices (Raven, 1976), is a widely accepted test of non-verbalfluid reasoning and intelligence that examines cognitive flexibility toinfer rules and perform high-level abstractions. The neural substratesfor these abilities include a frontoparietal network (Soulières et al.,2009), and as suggested here, structures of the basal ganglia. It is possiblethat performance on the matrix reasoning test in part reflects the integ-rity of topographic frontal connections with the caudate and putamen.

The findings with matrix reasoning and the results in general invitefurther discussion about the differences in the methods employed inthis study to examine structure–function relations. There was disagree-ment among the approaches regarding the associations between vari-ous structures and performance. With VBM, larger areas of the basalganglia and especially the putamen, were associated with poorermotor performance with the dominant hand, visual memory, blockdesign and a general index of intellectual ability. All of these associationsdisappeared and no new ones were generated when a specific, geomet-ric analysis of volume was applied. Thus, with VBM, the conclusionwould be that a larger putamenwas associated with impairment in sev-eral specific abilities and with general intelligence. When differences intotal volume of subcortical structures with respect to performancewereinvestigated, the conclusion would be that there are no associations

between the putamen/basal ganglia and performance. Even thoughthere are problems of misregistration with VBM because surface defor-mities can masquerade as variation in volume (Mechelli et al., 2005) inthis caseVBMalerted us to brain regions of interest to examine structureshape.

There may be several reasons that structure–function relations havenot been observed among preadolescent subjects. It has been suggestedthat the absence of relations between measured behavioral perfor-mance and gray matter volume in preadolescent children is related totheir “pre-pruning” brain inefficiency (Wilke et al., 2003). Adolescencehas been associated with a period of rapid brain maturation duringwhich there are significant reductions (pruning) of synaptic connec-tions resulting in greater mental efficiency (Gogtay et al., 2004)and therefore much less “noise.” This argument suggests that the “pre-pruned” preadolescent nervous system is inefficient and highly variable.The variability, bothwithin and between subjects obscures the ability todetect reliable associations between brain areas and function. Recentfindings from our studies however, indicate that cortical thinning doesoccur bilaterally within a narrow age range of preadolescent children(Muftuler et al., 2011) and although puberty may be a primary triggerfor brain maturation, it probably is a more gradual process and maynot completely explain the absence of findings. A second, obvious possi-bility is that the basal ganglia simply have been overlooked in mostprevious studies of typically developing preadolescent children.

Page 8: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

Fig. 4. Top panel is a superior view and the bottom panel an inferior view of the basal ganglia. The structures are color coded to illustrate significance (p values are coded with hot colorsreflecting highest levels of significance). Significance indicates that areas of expansion are associated with poorer cognitive performance.

100 C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

One conclusion from these findings is that shape and not size orvolume of basal ganglia may be a more sensitive measure of the associ-ation with performance. If differences in a subcortical structure acrosssubjects are focal and not distributed throughout the whole volume,then a measure of total volumemight not be sensitive enough to detectthose differences. On theother hand, a vertex-by-vertex surface analysisshould capture those localized differences. Moreover, measures ofshape, as opposed to measures of volume, provide the opportunity toassess associations between behavioral performance and distinctive to-pographic regions of the structures.

In summary, the findings from our study indicated that typically de-veloping children with generally expanded shape of the putamen andcaudate nucleus perform more poorly on tests of cognitive function.

Deformity of the basal ganglia may be a neurophenotype that is associ-atedwith risk for cognitive impairment and perhaps syndromes that in-clude impaired thinking and reasoning. This possibility is consistentwith recent studies that reported deformity of structures in the basalganglia was linked to developmental disorders such as autism andattention deficit disorder (Estes et al., 2011; Mamah et al., 2008; Qiuet al., 2009, 2010) and other psychiatric conditions in adults that areassociated with impaired cognitive function (Choi et al., 2007).

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the National Institute of Healthgrants NS-41298, HD-51852 and HD-28413 to CAS and HD-50662 and

Page 9: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

Fig. 5. Illustrates the negative relations (compression— inward arrows) and positive relations (expansion— outward arrows) of the putamen with performance on the matrix reasoningsubtest of the WISC.

101C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

HD-65823 to EPD. The funding agencies did not contribute to the designof the study, collection of the data, analysis and interpretation of thedata, and writing of the report or decision to submit this manuscriptfor publication. The authors have no actual or potential personal orother relationships that could inappropriately influence this work. Weare grateful for the expert assistance of Kendra Leak, Cheryl Crippen,Megan Blair, Christina Canino andNatalie Hernandez and to the familieswho participated in our studies.

References

Albin, R.L., Young, A.B.., Penney, J.B., 1995. The functional anatomy of disorders of thebasal ganglia. Trends Neurosci. 18, 63–64.

Alexander, G.E., DeLong, M.R., Strick, P.L., 1986. Parallel organization of functionally segre-gated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 357–381.http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041.

Armstrong, J.M., Goldstein, L.H., 2003. Manual for the MacArthur Health and BehaviorQuestionnaire (HBQ 1.0). MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Psychopa-thology and Development, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.

Ashburner, J., Friston, K.J., 2000. Voxel-based morphometry: the methods. NeuroImage11, 805–821. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0582.

Ashburner, J., Friston, K.J., 2005. Unified segmentation. NeuroImage 26, 839–851.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.018.

Baron, I.S., 2004. Neuropsychological Evaluation of the Child. Oxford University Press,New York, NY.

Brown, J., Bullock, D., Grossberg, S., 1999. How the basal ganglia use parallel excitatoryand inhibitory learning pathways to selectively respond to unexpected rewardingcues. J. Neurosci. 19, 10502–10511.

Burgaleta, M., MacDonald, P.A., Martinez, K., Roman, F.J., Alvarez-Linera, J., Gonzalez, A.R.,Karama, S., Colom, R., 2013. Subcortical regionalmorphology correlateswith fluid andspatial intelligence. Hum. Brain Mapp. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22305.

Castellanos, F.X., 2002. Anatomicmagnetic resonance imaging studies of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 4, 444–448.

Chakos, M.H., Lieberman, J.A., Bilder, R.M., Borenstein, M., Lerner, G., Bogerts, B., Ashtari,M., 1994. Increase in caudate nuclei volumes of first-episode schizophrenic patientstaking antipsychotic drugs. Am. J. Psychiatry 151, 1430–1436.

Choi, J.S., Kim, S.H., Yoo, S.Y., Kang, D.H., Kim, C.W., Lee, J.M., Kwon, J.S., 2007. Shape deformityof the corpus striatum in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Res. 155, 257–264.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.02.004.

Cooper, S., 1995. The Clinical Use and Interpretation of theWechsler Intelligence Scale forChildren — Revised. , C.C. Thomas, Springfield, IL.

Cootes, T.F., Taylor, C.J., Cooper, D.H., Graham, J., 1995. Active shapemodels: their training andapplication. Comput. Vis. ImageUnderst. 61, 38–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/cviu.1995.1004.

Deák, G.O., Maratsos, M., 1998. On having complex representations of things: pre-schoolers use multiple words for objects and people. Dev. Psychol. 34, 224–240.

DeLong, M.R., 1990. Primate models of movement disorders of basal ganglia origin.Trends Neurosci. 13, 281–285.

DeLong, M.R.,Wichmann, T., 2007. Circuits and circuit disorders of the basal ganglia. Arch.Neurol. 64, 20–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archneur.64.1.20.

Ducharme, S., Hudziak, J.J., Botteron, K.N., Ganjavi, H., Lepage, C., Collins, D.L., Karama, S.,2011. Right anterior cingulate cortical thickness and bilateral striatal volume correlatewith child behavior checklist aggressive behavior scores in healthy children. Biol.Psychiatry 70, 283–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.015.

Estes, A., Shaw, D.W., Sparks, B.F., Friedman, S., Giedd, J.N., Dawson, G., Dager, S.R.,2011. Basal ganglia morphometry and repetitive behavior in young childrenwith autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 4, 212–220. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/aur.193.

Frangou, S., Chitins, X., Williams, S.C., 2004. Mapping IQ and gray matter density in healthyyoung people. NeuroImage 23, 800–805. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.027.

Genovese, C.R., Lazar, N.A., Nichols, T., 2002. Thresholding of statistical maps in functionalneuroimaging using the false discovery rate. NeuroImage 15, 870–878. http://dx.doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.1037.

Gogtay, N., Giedd, J.N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K.M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A.C., Thompson,P.M., 2004. Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood

Page 10: Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is ... · Shape of the basal ganglia in preadolescent children is associated with cognitive performance Curt A. Sandmana,⁎,

102 C.A. Sandman et al. / NeuroImage 99 (2014) 93–102

through early adulthood. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 101, 8174–8179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0402680101.

Grahn, J.A., Parkinson, J.A., Owen, A.M., 2009. The role of the basal ganglia in learning andmemory: neuropsychological studies. Behav. Brain Res. 199, 53–60. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2008.11.020.

Graybiel, A.M., Canales, J.J., Capper-Loup, C., 2000. Levodopa-induced dyskinesias anddopamine-dependent stereotypies: a new hypothesis. Trends Neurosci. 23, S71–S77.

Groth-Marnat, G., 1997. Handbook of Psychological Assessment, 3rd ed. J. Wiley, NewYork, NY.

Haier, R.J., Head, K., Head, E., Lott, I.T., 2008. Neuroimaging of individuals with Down'ssyndrome at-risk for dementia: evidence for possible compensatory events.NeuroImage 39, 1324–1332. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.064.

Herrero,M.T., Barcia, C., Navarro, J., 2002. Functional anatomy of thalamus and basal ganglia.Childs Nerv. Syst. 18, 386–404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00381-002-0604-1.

Hokama, H., Shenton, M.E., Nestor, P.G., Kikinis, R., Levitt, J.J., Metcalf, D., McCarley, R.W.,1995. Caudate, putamen, and globus pallidus volume in schizophrenia: a quantitativeMRI study. Psychiatry Res. 61, 209–229.

Karama, S., Ad-Dab'bagh, Y., Haier, R.J., Deary, I.J., Lyttelton, O.C., Lepage, C., Evans, A.C.,2009. Positive association between cognitive ability and cortical thickness in arepresentative US sample of healthy 6 to 18 year-olds. Intelligence 37, 145–155.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.09.006.

Karama, S., Colom, R., Johnson, W., Deary, I.J., Haier, R., Waber, D.P., Evans, A.C., 2011.Cortical thickness correlates of specific cognitive performance accounted for by thegeneral factor of intelligence in healthy children aged 6 to 18. NeuroImage 55,1443–1453. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.01.016.

Knowlton, B.J., Mangels, J.A., Squire, L.R., 1996. A neostriatal habit learning system inhumans. Science 273, 1399–1402.

Kreitzer, A.C., Malenka, R.C., 2008. Striatal plasticity and basal ganglia circuit function.Neuron 60, 543–554. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.11.005.

Langen, M., Durston, S., Staal, W.G., Palmen, S.J., van Engeland, H., 2007. Caudate nucleusis enlarged in high-functioningmedication-naive subjects with autism. Biol. Psychiatry62, 262–266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.09.040.

Luders, E., Thompson, P.M., Narr, K.L., Zamanyan, A., Chou, Y.Y., Gutman, B., Toga, A.W., 2011.The link between callosal thickness and intelligence in healthy children and adolescents.NeuroImage 54, 1823–1830. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.09.083.

MacDonald, P.A., Ganjavi, H., Collins, D.L., Evans, A.C., Karama, S., 2013. Investigating therelation between striatal volume and IQ. Brain Imaging Behav.. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s11682-013-9242-3.

Mamah, D., Harms, M.P., Wang, L., Barch, D., Thompson, P., Kim, J., Csernansky, J.G., 2008.Basal ganglia shape abnormalities in the unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients.Biol. Psychiatry 64, 111–120. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.004.

Mechelli, A., Price, C.J., Friston, K.J., Ashburner, J., 2005. Voxel-based morphometry of thehuman brain: methods and applications. Curr. Med. Imaging Rev. 1, 105–113.

Muftuler, L.T., Davis, E.P., Buss, C., Head, K., Hasso, A.N., Sandman, C.A., 2011. Cortical andsubcortical changes in typically developing preadolescent children. Brain Res. 1399,15–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.018.

Nakano, K., 2000. Neural circuits and topographic organization of the basal ganglia andrelated regions. Brain Dev. 22, S5–S16.

Nicola, S.M., 2007. The nucleus accumbens as part of a basal ganglia action selectioncircuit. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 191, 521–550. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00213-006-0510-4.

Oldfield, R.C., 1971. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.Neuropsychologia 9, 97–113.

Patenaude, B., Smith, S.M., Kennedy, D.N., Jenkinson, M., 2011. A Bayesian model of shapeand appearance for subcortical brain segmentation. NeuroImage 56, 907–922.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.046.

Qiu, A., Crocetti, D., Adler, M., Mahone, E.M., Denckla, M.B., Miller, M.I., Mostofsky, S.H.,2009. Basal ganglia volume and shape in children with attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 166, 74–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08030426.

Qiu, A., Adler, M., Crocetti, D., Miller, M.I., Mostofsky, S.H., 2010. Basal ganglia shapes pre-dict social, communication, and motor dysfunctions in boys with autism spectrumdisorder. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 49, 539–551. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2010.02.012 (551.e531-534).

Raven, J.C., 1976. Raven Progressive Matrices. , The Psychological Corporation, Toronto,CA.

Raven, J., Raven, J.C., Court, J.H., 1998. Manual for Raven's Progressive Matrices and Vocabu-lary Scales. , Oxford Psychologists Press, Oxford, UK.

Rolls, E.T., 1994. Neurophysiology and cognitive functions of the striatum. Rev. Neurol.(Paris) 150, 648–660.

Romero, M.C., Bermudez, M.A., Vicente, A.F., Perez, R., Gonzalez, F., 2008. Activity ofneurons in the caudate and putamen during a visuomotor task. Neuroreport 19,1141–1145.

Salinas, E., Opris, I., Zainos, A., Hernandez, A., Romo, R., 2000. Motor and non-motor rolesof the cortico-basal ganglia circuitry. In: Miller, R., Wickens, J. (Eds.), Brain Dynamicsand the Striatal Complex: Conceptual Advances in Brain Research. Harwood Academic,Amsterdam, Netherlands, pp. 237–256.

Salmond, C.H., Ashburner, J., Vargha-Khadem, F., Connelly, A., Gadian, D.G., Friston, K.J.,2002. Distributional assumptions in voxel-based morphometry. NeuroImage 17,1027–1030.

Sattler, J.M., 2001. Assessment of Children: Cognitive Applications, 4th ed. , J.M. Sattler,San Diego, CA.

Shaw, P., Greenstein, D., Lerch, J., Clasen, L., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., Giedd, J., 2006. Intellec-tual ability and cortical development in children and adolescents. Nature 440,676–679. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nature04513.

Shulman, C., Yirmiya, N., Greenbaum, C.W., 1995. From categorization to classification: acomparison among individuals with autism, mental retardation, and normal develop-ment. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 104, 601–609.

Silk, T.J., Vance, A., Rinehart, N., Bradshaw, J.L., Cunnington, R., 2009. Structural develop-ment of the basal ganglia in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a diffusion tensorimaging study. Psychiatry Res. NeuroImage 172, 220–225.

Soulières, I., Dawson, M., Samson, F., Barbeau, E.B., Sahyoun, C.P., Strangman, G.E.,Mottron, L., 2009. Enhanced visual processing contributes to matrix reasoning inautism. Hum. Brain Mapp. 30, 4082–4107. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20831.

Staal, W.G., Hulshoff Pol, H.E., Schnack, H.G., Hoogendoorn, M.L., Jellema, K., Kahn, R.S.,2000. Structural brain abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia and their healthysiblings. Am. J. Psychiatry 157, 416–421.

Talairach, J., Tournoux, P., 1988. Co-planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain: 3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging. Thieme, NewYork, NY.

Tanji, J., Hoshi, E., 2008. Role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in executive behavioralcontrol. Physiol. Rev. 88, 37–57. http://dx.doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00014.2007.

Toga, A.W., Thompson, P.M., 2005. Genetics of brain structure and intelligence. Annu. Rev.Neurosci. 28, 1–23. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135655.

Wechsler, D., 2002. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence— Third Edition(WPPSI-III). , Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX.

Wichmann, T., Delong, M.R., 2011. Deep-brain stimulation for basal ganglia disorders.Basal Ganglia 1, 65–77. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.baga.2011.05.001.

Wilke, M., Schmithorst, V.J., Holland, S.K., 2002. Assessment of spatial normalization ofwhole-brain magnetic resonance images in children. Hum. Brain Mapp. 17, 48–60.http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/hbm.10053.

Wilke, M., Sohn, J.H., Byars, A.W., Holland, S.K., 2003. Bright spots: correlations of graymatter volume with IQ in a normal pediatric population. NeuroImage 20, 202–215.http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00199-X.

Yin, H.H., Knowlton, B.J., 2006. The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation. Nat. Rev.Neurosci. 7, 464–476. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nrn1919.