sex typing and consumer behavior

Upload: amlunatic

Post on 02-Jun-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 Sex Typing and Consumer Behavior

    1/8

    Journal of Consumer Research Inc.

    Sex Typing and Consumer Behavior: A Test of Gender Schema TheoryAuthor(s): Bernd H. Schmitt, France Leclerc and Laurette Dub-RiouxSource: Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 15, No. 1 (Jun., 1988), pp. 122-128Published by: The University of Chicago PressStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2489178.

    Accessed: 24/11/2014 01:04

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at.http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

    .JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of

    content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms

    of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    .

    The University of Chicago PressandJournal of Consumer Research, Inc.are collaborating with JSTOR to

    digitize, preserve and extend access toJournal of Consumer Research.

    http://www.jstor.org

    http://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ucpresshttp://www.jstor.org/stable/2489178?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/stable/2489178?origin=JSTOR-pdfhttp://www.jstor.org/action/showPublisher?publisherCode=ucpress
  • 8/10/2019 Sex Typing and Consumer Behavior

    2/8

    Sex

    Typing

    and Consumer

    Behavior:

    A

    Test

    of Gender

    Schema

    Theory

    BERND

    H.

    SCHMITT*

    FRANCELECLERC

    LAURETTE

    DUBE-RIOUX

    Using

    the context of advertising, packaging,

    and consumer choice, three

    experi-

    ments were conducted

    to test the predictions

    of gender

    schema theory

    that gen-

    der-schematic

    (sex-typed)

    individuals and gender-aschematic

    (non-sex-typed)

    indi-

    viduals

    differ

    in

    the way

    they process

    and evaluate information

    related to gender.

    Results

    indicated strong

    sex effects,

    but the predicted

    sex-type effects were

    not

    obtained.

    ender

    serves

    as an

    importantsocial

    category

    in

    all cultures, and each culture has developed a

    rich

    network of

    associations

    that surround

    its con-

    cepts

    of

    maleness

    and femaleness. However,

    in-

    dividuals

    within

    a culture may

    differ from one

    an-

    other

    in

    the

    degree

    to which they

    utilize

    cultural

    definitions

    of masculinity

    and femininity

    as

    stan-

    dards against

    which they

    perceive,

    categorize,

    and

    evaluate

    gender-related

    information. According

    to

    Bem's

    (1981, 1985) gender

    schema

    theory,

    sex-typed

    individualstend to

    encode,

    and organize

    incoming

    in-

    formation

    in termsof a gender

    schema,

    using the

    tra-

    ditional bipolar

    masculinity/femininity

    dimension

    as

    the

    organizing

    principle.

    Non-sex-typed

    individuals,

    however,

    wheneverfeasible,

    use other,

    nongender-re-

    lated dimensions to organize information and are

    thus less likely than

    sex-typed

    individuals to engage

    in

    gender-schematic

    processing.

    Moreover,

    sex-typed

    individuals are concerned

    about keeping

    their

    behav-

    ior

    consistent with

    the culture's

    definitions

    of gender

    appropriateness;

    non-sex-typed

    individuals,

    in con-

    trast,

    are

    more

    likely

    to cross

    the traditional

    bound-

    aries

    of masculinity

    and femininity.

    Several studies

    have

    tested gender

    schema

    theory's

    basic claim that sex-typed

    individuals are more gen-

    der-schematic than non-sex-typed individuals (see

    Bem 1985 for a review). For example,

    to test whether

    gender-schematic individuals,

    as

    identified by the

    Bem Sex Role

    Inventory (BSRI;Bem 1974), sponta-

    neously encode

    and organize informationon the ba-

    sis

    of

    the

    sex-linked

    associations

    that constitute the

    gender schema, subjects

    were asked to remember

    words presented

    in random order (Bem 1981). These

    words included

    animal names, verbs, and articles of

    clothing;

    one-third

    of

    each category

    had been

    judged

    by undergraduates s masculine (e.g.,

    gorilla, hurling,

    trousers),

    one-third as feminine (e.g., butterfly,blush-

    ing, bikini), and one-third as neutral

    (e.g., ant, step-

    ping, sweater). In their recall of the items, gender-

    schematic subjects were more likely than gender-

    aschematic subjects

    to cluster the words

    on

    the basis

    of gender, i.e.,

    after

    having

    recalled

    a masculine item

    to

    recall

    another

    masculine

    item and after

    having

    re-

    called a

    feminine item to recall another feminine

    item. A relatedstudy measured subjects' response la-

    tencies

    when asked whether

    a

    series

    of attributes was

    self-descriptive(Bem 1981).

    Gender-schematic sub-

    jects

    were faster

    than

    gender-aschematic subjects

    when

    endorsing sex-appropriate

    attributes (e.g.,

    forceful

    and

    dominant

    for

    male

    subjects

    and

    gentle

    and warm for female subjects) and

    when

    rejecting sex-inappropriate

    attributes. Finally, gen-

    der-schematic subjects

    were more

    likely

    than

    gender-

    aschematic subjectsto choose sex-appropriateactiv-

    ities (e.g., using a

    drill if

    they

    are male and knitting

    and ironing

    if

    they

    are

    female) and

    to avoid

    sex-inap-

    propriatebehavior. Moreover,

    while performingsex-

    inappropriate

    activities, gender-schematic

    individu-

    als

    feel

    significantly

    worse

    than

    gender-aschematic

    n-

    dividuals

    (Bem

    and Lenney 1976).

    Recently, several

    studies have tested but failed to

    confirm

    vender

    schema

    theorv

    in

    the

    area

    of

    con-

    *

    Bernd

    H. Schmitt is a Ph.D.

    candidate

    in the Department

    of

    Psychology,

    France

    Leclerc is a Ph.D.

    candidate

    in the Johnson

    Graduate

    School of Management,

    and

    Laurette Dube-Rioux

    is

    a

    Ph.D. candidate

    in the School of Hotel

    Administration,

    all at

    Cor-

    nell University, Ithaca, NY 14853. The order of the second and

    third

    authors was determined

    by the flip of

    a coin. The authors

    wish

    to

    thank Sandra

    L. Bem, Pradeep

    Kakkar, J.

    Edward Russo,

    and

    three anonymous

    reviewers

    for

    their helpful comments

    on an ear-

    lier

    version of

    this article; the

    authors also

    wish to thank Isabelle

    Rougemont for

    her invaluable

    help in conducting

    Experiments

    1

    and 2,

    and Laurel Sgan

    and Deanna Silver for preparing

    the

    stimuli

    and running

    the

    subjects

    in

    Experiment

    3.

    A

    longer version

    of this

    article may

    be obtained by writing

    to

    the first author,

    who will be

    at

    the Columbia University

    Graduate School of Business,

    New

    York,

    NY

    10027, as

    of the 1988

    fall semester.

    122

    ?)

    JOURNAL OF CONSUMER

    RESEARCH

    *

    Vol. 15

    * June

    1988

    This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Sex Typing and Consumer Behavior

    3/8

    SEX TYPING AND

    CONSUMER

    BEHAVIOR 123

    sumer behavior.

    However, many

    of these studies

    were

    iimited

    in

    scope,

    and some were flawed

    by method-

    ological and conceptual

    shortcomings.

    Most studies

    (e.g.,

    Allison et al.

    1980; Coughlin

    and

    O'Connor

    1985; Gentry and

    Doering 1977; Gentry, Doering,

    and O'Brien 1978) tested only one aspect of market-

    ing (e.g., product

    choice or

    product

    perceptions)

    or

    used only

    one measure of

    advertising

    effectiveness

    (e.g.,

    ad

    recall or

    purchase intent).

    Some studies did

    not use the BSRI

    in

    determining individuals'

    sex

    type

    (e.g.,Gentryand

    Doering 1977; Gentry,Doering,

    and

    O'Brien

    1978; Gentry

    and Haley 1984).'

    Most

    impor-

    tantly, the hypotheses of some studies are not deriv-

    able

    from genderschema theory.

    McIntyre et al. con-

    tended

    that

    they provided partial support

    for gender

    schema

    theory

    because emphasis on traditional

    roles and on sexist

    products may . . . produce a com-

    mercial

    that is extremely

    salient, and offensive to peo-

    ple with a

    liberal gender schema (1986,

    p. 190)-a

    type of schema that does not exist in Bem's theory.

    Similarly,

    Kahle

    and

    Homer

    hypothesized,

    on the ba-

    sis of

    gender schema theory as they

    claimed, that

    males ought to valuefood more based on

    the 'size of

    the

    kill'

    (e.g., cattle

    are

    harder to kill than

    chickens),

    whereas the

    nutritional

    aspects

    of

    food

    ought

    to be

    more

    important

    for

    women, who have

    historically

    planned

    menus and prepared food

    (1985, p. 243).

    This

    hypothesis cannot be derived from

    gender

    schema

    theory,

    because

    the

    theory does

    not

    make

    claims about how role

    assignimients

    t different

    times

    of

    human

    history

    influence the selection of

    lunch

    items today.

    In

    sum, it is

    not

    clear

    whether gender

    schema

    theory

    has been

    given

    a fair

    test

    in

    consumer

    research.

    This

    article's three

    experiments

    were

    designed

    to

    test

    the

    theory

    extensively.

    In

    designing

    the three ex-

    periments,

    we tried

    to adhere as

    closely

    as

    possible

    to

    experimental paradigms

    employed

    in

    previous psy-

    chological

    research that revealed

    positive

    results

    for

    gender

    schema

    theory.

    In

    Experiment 1,

    subjects judged jeans

    advertise-

    ments that depicted

    a man

    and

    a woman

    either

    in

    a

    sex-role conformist

    or

    in

    a

    sex-role nonconformist

    way.

    In

    a second

    part

    of the

    experiment,

    subjects pro-

    vided appeal ratings

    for

    fragrance

    lacons

    (two

    mascu-

    line,

    two

    feminine).

    We

    predicted

    that

    gender-sche-

    matic

    subjects, compared

    to

    gender-aschematic

    ndi-

    viduals,

    would show a more

    positive

    attitude toward

    sex-role conformist advertisements than toward sex-

    role nonconformist advertisements.

    Additionally,

    gender-schematic

    individuals

    should

    show

    stronger

    preferences

    than

    gender-aschematic

    individuals for

    sex-appropriate

    ragrance

    lacons.

    In

    Experiment2,

    subjectswere shown

    the names

    of

    18 magazines

    (one-third

    masculine,

    one-third femi-

    nine,

    one-third neutral).

    We

    predicted that gender-

    schematic individuals would be more likely than

    gender-aschematic

    individuals to

    remember

    sex-ap-

    propriate

    magazines

    and,

    in

    their

    recall of the

    maga-

    zines,

    to cluster

    them on the

    basis of gender.After

    the

    recall task,

    subjects

    were asked whether

    they would

    be

    interested

    in

    reading

    each

    magazine. Gender-sche-

    matic

    subjects should be

    more likely than

    gender-

    aschematic subjects

    to choose sex-role

    conformist

    magazines.

    Moreover,

    gender-schematic subjects

    should

    be faster

    than

    gender-aschematic

    subjects

    when

    making

    sex-role

    conformist choices

    and slower

    when

    making sex-role

    nonconformist

    choices.

    Experiment 3 was

    designed to test whether

    the two

    groupswould

    differ from one

    another

    in

    their

    mem-

    oryforsex-roleconformist andsex-rolenonconform-

    ist

    advertisements.

    We predicted

    that

    gender-sche-

    matic

    individuals would be

    more likely to

    remember

    sex-role

    conformist

    advertisements than

    sex-role

    nonconformist

    advertisements.

    Gender-aschematic

    individuals,

    however, should be less

    likely to

    show

    such a differential

    recall.2

    EXPERIMENT 1

    Method

    Experiment

    1

    was

    presented as a

    study

    on

    advertis-

    ing

    and packaging,

    and 151

    students from an

    under-

    graduatemarketing course atCornellUniversity par-

    ticipated. At the

    end of the

    study,

    subjects completed

    the

    BSRI,

    which was

    used

    to

    determine

    subjects'

    sex

    types.3 The

    sample

    contained 39

    androgynous

    sub-

    '

    Bem (1985) has not excluded the use of other sex role invento-

    ries in a test of gender schema theory. However, Kelly, Furman,

    and Young

    (1978)

    have

    shown

    that a

    large proportion

    of

    subjects

    may be classified nto differentsex-type categories, depending on

    the inventoryused.

    2

    Recent research

    n

    social cognition has shown

    that people are

    often better at

    remembering chema-consistent

    nformation (Tay-

    lor and

    Crocker 1981). The results of

    some studies, however, are

    inconsistent with this prediction. For

    example, based on research

    by Hastie and Kumar

    (1979),

    we

    would predict that

    gender-sche-

    matic

    individualswould be betterat

    remembering ex-rolenoncon-

    formist rather than

    sex-role conformist

    advertisements,

    but

    gen-

    der-aschematic ndividualswould be less

    likely to

    show

    this differ-

    ential recall. In either

    case,

    we would

    predict differencesbetween

    gender-schematic

    nd

    gender-aschematic

    ndividuals.

    3

    The BSRI is a

    paper-and-pencil est that asks

    subjects to indi-

    cate the extent to which each of 60 attributesdescribeshimself or

    herself.The BSRIconsists

    of two 20-item scales that

    reflectAmeri-

    can

    culture's definition of

    masculinity (e.g.,

    self-reliant, inde-

    pendent ) and of

    femininity (e.g.,

    affectionate, tender ), and

    20 neutral tems (e.g.,

    happy, sincere ).In

    various psychomet-

    ric analyses, Bem

    (1974) demonstrated

    hat the masculinity (M)

    and

    femininity (F) scales are

    orthogonal,

    hat the BSRI s

    internally

    consistent (coefficient

    alpha

    >

    0.8)

    and

    test-retestreliable

    r

    >

    0.9),

    and

    that high scores do not reflect a

    general

    tendency

    to

    respond

    in a socially

    desirabledirection.

    Recently, Qualls (1987) provided

    evidence for the BSRI's

    reliability alpha

    >

    0.85) and for ts

    conver-

    gent and discriminant

    validity.

    Each respondent's

    composite

    M

    and

    F

    scores

    were calculated,

    This content downloaded from 202.84.40.92 on Mon, 24 Nov 2014 01:04:47 AMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

    http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsphttp://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp
  • 8/10/2019 Sex Typing and Consumer Behavior

    4/8

    124

    THE

    JOURNAL OF CONSUMER RESEARCH

    jects (21 males, 18 females), 37 undifferentiatedsub-

    jects (18 males, 19 females), and

    56 traditionally sex-

    typed subjects (26 males, 30 females).

    According to

    Bem (1985), androgynous and undifferentiated

    sub-

    jects are gender-aschematic, and

    traditionally sex-

    typed subjects are gender-schematic. Because gender

    schema theory

    does

    not make

    specific predictions

    about cross-sex-typed

    individuals

    (Bem 1985, p.

    195), they were excluded from subsequent analyses.

    In the first part of Experiment 1, subjects saw

    12

    jeans advertisements.

    Jeans

    were

    selected as a

    prod-

    uct category because they

    are

    popularamong college

    students and because they qualify

    as a neutral

    prod-

    uct

    (Gentry, Doering,

    and O'Brien

    1978). Each

    ad-

    vertisement consisted of a male or female describing

    himself or herself

    with a

    masculine

    statement (e.g.,

    I

    seek out

    positions

    of

    authority. ),

    a feminine state-

    ment (e.g.,

    I

    try to get

    others to notice the

    way

    I

    dress. ),

    or

    a neutral

    statement

    (e.g.,

    I admire

    free,

    spontaneous people. ).The personin the picturewas

    always

    the same

    male

    or female. Each

    subject

    saw

    each person

    in

    the

    advertisement

    in

    connection with

    two

    masculine, two feminine,

    and two neutral state-

    ments, resulting

    n 12advertisements.The masculine

    and feminine

    statements

    in

    the advertisements were

    selected from the

    ANDRO

    PRF,

    a sex role

    inventory

    that includes

    self-descriptive

    statements of

    the

    Per-

    sonality

    Research Form

    (PRF);

    items of the PRF

    that

    are not

    part

    of

    the

    ANDRO PRF and that had

    been

    found

    to be non-sex-typed (Berzins, Welling, and

    Wetter

    1978)

    were selected

    as neutral

    statements. The

    order of

    presentation

    of the 12

    advertisements was

    randomized across subjects.

    Using seven-point

    Lik-

    ert-type scales, subjects indicated for each of the 12

    advertisementshow

    much

    they

    liked the

    type

    of

    per-

    son

    portrayed

    n the

    advertisement,

    how similar

    they

    thought they

    were to

    that

    type

    of

    person,

    and how

    effective the advertisement

    might

    be

    in

    convincing

    people

    to

    buy the product.

    This

    part

    of the

    experi-

    ment took

    the form

    of a

    2

    (sex

    of

    subject)

    X

    3 (sex

    type)

    X

    2

    (sex

    of

    person

    in

    the

    advertisement)

    X

    3

    (type

    of

    statement) experimentaldesign.

    The first wo

    factors

    were

    between-subjectsvariables,

    and the latter

    factors were

    within-subjects

    variables.

    The stimuli of the

    second

    part

    of

    Experiment

    1

    were

    professionally rendered drawings of four fragrance

    flacons

    of

    different

    shapes. Subjects

    ndicated on

    sev-

    en-point scales how appealingthey would find

    each

    flacon

    if

    they were to purchasea

    fragrance for their

    own

    use,

    for a

    male friend,

    and for a

    female

    friend.

    The four flacons

    were selected on the basis of a

    pre-

    test;

    20 pretest subjects had rated two flacons

    as mas-

    culine and two flacons as feminine. The shapesof

    the two

    masculine flacons

    were

    rectangular

    and

    the

    shapes of the

    feminine flacons

    were

    round and

    more

    refined. The second

    part took the form of a 2 (sex)

    X

    3

    (sex

    type)

    X

    4

    (flacon shape) design. The

    presentation

    order of flacon

    shape, the

    within-subjects

    variable,

    wascounterbalanced.

    Results

    Did

    gender-schematic and gender-aschematicsub-

    jects

    differ

    in

    the way that they

    responded

    to the sex-

    typed items of the PRF that

    were

    used as

    self-descrip-

    tive statements

    in

    the

    jeans

    advertisements?To

    pro-

    vide such a manipulation check, subjects' responses

    on the

    similarity

    measure

    were

    analyzed by

    a

    2

    (sex

    of

    subject)

    X

    3 (sex

    type)

    X

    2

    (sex

    of

    person

    in

    the

    ad)

    X

    3

    (type

    of

    statement)

    ANOVA,

    which

    revealed a

    significant

    three-way

    interaction of

    sex,

    sex

    typing,

    and

    type

    of statement

    (F(6,270)

    =

    76.92, p

    0.25). Instead,

    the main effect of sex of reader (F(1,55) = 16.20, p