ruth curtis bsc (hons) psychology dissertation

53
USPJED-40-3 10013482 1 Faculty of Health and Life Sciences PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH PROJECT USPJED-40-3 Cover sheet In submitting this project, I declare that it is my work, it contains no unreferenced or unacknowledged verbatim extracts from the works of others and it has not (either in whole or in part) been submitted towards any other award at UWE or elsewhere. Title: A mixed methods investigation testing the effectiveness of the Theory of Planned Behaviour constructs at predicting exercise behaviour Student Name: Ruth Curtis ................................... Student Number: 10013482 Supervisor: Dr. James Byron-Daniel .................... Date: 13/05/13

Upload: ruth-curtis

Post on 11-Apr-2017

103 views

Category:

Science


4 download

TRANSCRIPT

USPJED-40-3 10013482

1

Faculty of Health and Life Sciences

PSYCHOLOGY RESEARCH PROJECT

USPJED-40-3

Cover sheet

In submitting this project, I declare that it is my work, it contains no unreferenced or

unacknowledged verbatim extracts from the works of others and it has not (either in whole or in

part) been submitted towards any other award at UWE or elsewhere.

Title: A mixed methods investigation testing the effectiveness of the

Theory of Planned Behaviour constructs at predicting exercise behaviour

Student Name: Ruth Curtis ...................................

Student Number: 10013482

Supervisor: Dr. James Byron-Daniel ....................

Date: 13/05/13

USPJED-40-3 10013482

2

I would like to express my appreciation to all those who assisted me in the

making of this research project.

Firstly, I would like to thank my supervisor, Dr. James Byron – Daniel whose

brilliant suggestions and encouragement helped me through the making of

this project. Secondly, I would like to thank the module leader, Dr. Eamon

Fulcher whose unwavering patience in giving advice to all throughout the

year has been deeply appreciated. Lastly, I would like to thank all those who

took part in this research; without their participation this project would not

have been possible.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

3

Contents

Description Page Number

Abstract and Introduction 4

Method – Design 8

Participants 9

Materials 10

Procedure 12

Results – Section 1: Quantitative 13

Figure 1: Table of correlations 15

Section 2: Qualitative 15

Discussion 23

References 32

Appendices –

1: Information sheet, Consent form and Survey 36

2: De-brief sheet 44

3: Changes to data 44

4: Ethical Approval form 45

5: Example of qualitative coding 46

Project checklist 47

USPJED-40-3 10013482

4

A mixed methods investigation testing the effectiveness of the Theory of

Planned Behaviour constructs at predicting exercise behaviour

Abstract

The aim was to test the effectiveness of the Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB) constructs at

predicting exercise behaviour. A mixed methods design was utilised, 40 participants aged 18

to 30 years responded. It was hypothesised that all TPB variables would be positively

correlated with all exercise behaviour, in all settings as found previously (Hausenblas,

Carron and Mack, 1997). Only normative importance was found to significantly correlate

with walking behaviour. Partial support was found in the qualitative data – ‘Health’

supported the Attitude construct and ‘Acceptance’ supported Subjective Norm. It was

concluded that the quantitative methods used were not valid or reliable; qualitative data

suggests that the TPB should include personality, company, social support and ambivalence

in attitude as additional variables.

Introduction

The Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB, Ajzen, 1991) was proposed as a predictive model of

health behaviour. Ajzen theorised that 3 main variables or “beliefs” of an individual could

predict their subsequent behaviour. These three beliefs were: attitude towards the

behaviour, subjective norms, and perceived behavioural control which together were said to

shape an individual's behavioural intentions and ultimately their behaviours. Perceived

behavioural control is said to also have a direct effect on whether the behaviour is engaged

in or not (Ajzen, 1991). Ogden (2012) explains the theory behind each of the components;

Attitude refers to the individual’s evaluation of a behaviour and the outcomes associated

with engaging in it – either positive or negative. In theory the more positive the attitude the

USPJED-40-3 10013482

5

more likely the individual is to participate in this behaviour. Subjective norms refer to the

individual’s perceptions of significant others’ attitudes towards a behaviour, social pressures

to perform this behaviour and motivation to comply with this pressure. In theory if high

social pressure is perceived the individual is more likely to comply and engage in the

behaviour. Perceived behavioural control refers to the belief that the individual can perform

this behaviour based on internal and external factors (e.g. skills and obstacles respectively).

In theory if perceived behavioural control is high, the individual will feel they have control

over the obstacles that would stop them engaging in the behaviour and will therefore be

more likely to perform it. This theory was developed from the Theory of Reasoned Action

(TRA; Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975) which is the same theory minus the component of

perceived behavioural control.

Madden, Ellen and Ajzen (1992) investigated students’ reports of ten different behaviours

and found, when comparing the TRA and the TPB, that the TPB had better predictive power

than the TRA for both intention and behaviour across the behaviours. Armitage and Conner

(2001) conducted a meta-analytic review looking into the efficacy of the TPB. Each construct

was evaluated and it was found that the subjective norm construct is generally found to be

a weak predictor of intentions compared to attitude and perceived behavioural control;

subjective norm may have low efficacy at predicting behaviour. Godin and Kok (1996) also

studied the application of the TPB across many different health behaviours. These

behaviours included weight gain prevention, eating behaviour, exercise behaviour and

more. They found that the theory was applicable across health behaviours however the

efficacy of the theory varied for different behaviours - TPB could account for 42%, 32%,

46.8% of the variance in exercise, eating behaviour and oral hygiene intentions respectively.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

6

It could be interesting for a single health behaviour to be studied in detail; as efficacy varies

across behaviours it seems worthwhile to explore one behaviour in an attempt to

understand all underlying motivators to engage in that behaviour.

Hausenblas, Carron and Mack (1997) carried out a meta-analysis which investigated the

effectiveness of the application of both the TRA and the TPB when predicting exercise

behaviour. The paper concludes that there is strong evidence that the TRA is a ‘good’ theory

and the TPB is an even better theory in predicting exercise behaviour. These findings

suggest that the TPB is more effective at predicting exercise behaviour, than the TRA.

Spink, Wilson and Bostick (2012) however, hypothesised that the TPB constructs, specifically

perceived behavioural control would only predict intention to exercise in an unstructured

setting, rather than a structured one. They found support for this hypothesis; a simple

slopes analysis revealed that perceived behavioural control predicted intention only in the

unstructured setting. This research raises the interesting question of whether the TPB

constructs have the same efficacy at predicting exercise behaviour in a wide variety of

settings.

Many research journals look into expanding the TPB and adding additional constructs.

Knowles, Hyde and White (2012) found when using an extended version of the TPB,

incorporating moral norm and past behaviours, these two additional variables were

significant predictors of intention to donate money to charity, as well as the original

constructs. Bozionelos and Bennett (1999) when studying exercise behaviour also found

support for the inclusion of past behaviour as a predictor variable. 114 college students

completed questionnaires measuring TPB variables and later reported actual exercise

behaviour. Past behaviour was found to be the most predictive variable, while Subjective

USPJED-40-3 10013482

7

norm and Attitude were not significant predictors of intention to exercise and perceived

behavioural control was not found to significantly predict actual exercise behaviour. This

research does not provide support for the original constructs of the theory, however does

support past behaviour as an important predictor of exercise behaviour. Perhaps if this

variable were added to the TPB it would increase the efficacy of the theory.

The purpose of the current study is to test the effectiveness of the TPB constructs, including

the past behaviour construct, at predicting exercise behaviour in a number of different

settings. Due to the different foci when studying exercise behaviour, moral norm cannot be

included as a predictor variable or component, as there is no real ‘moral norm’ to exercising.

Moral norm concerns moral choices made by an individual in order to fit in with a social

group such as choosing not to steal, however choosing to exercise is not a moral choice.

However, past behaviour can be applied to this research and thus is included as an

additional predictor of behaviour in this study.

The effectiveness of the Theory is tested both quantitatively and qualitatively. Quantitative

questions measure all constructs of the TPB individually and a correlation is carried out to

test each construct’s effectiveness at predicting exercise behaviour in different settings.

Intention to exercise was not measured as it would predict future exercise behaviour and

exercise behaviour data is only collected at one time. Based on previous research supporting

the TPB, it is predicted that each of the TPB constructs will significantly positively correlate

with actual exercise behaviour in all settings and thus support the notion that the theory is

effective at predicting exercise behaviour. As past research surrounding the TPB tends to be

quantitative this theory has not been explored much qualitatively. This may be because the

theory is easier to test quantitatively, especially across many different behaviours that may

USPJED-40-3 10013482

8

be qualitatively different from one another. There seems to be a gap in the literature,

therefore qualitative data is also collected in this study. Qualitative data is collected through

open-ended questions. This data is thematically analysed and motivations to exercise and

barriers preventing exercise are identified. The motivations and barriers are compared to

the TPB constructs to assess whether these motivations ‘fit in’ to these constructs. Ajzen

(1991) argued that behavioural intent reflects how hard a person is willing to try or how

motivated he or she is, to perform the behaviour; behavioural intent is thought to be

influenced by Attitude, Subjective Norm (SN) and Perceived Behavioural Control (PBC)

therefore studying motivations to exercise qualitatively was thought to be appropriate in

this research. If the constructs in the TPB are effective at predicting exercise behaviour, it is

expected to take all motivations to exercise and barriers preventing exercise into account.

Method

Design

Once ethical approval was obtained (see Appendix 4), a mixed methods design was

employed. A mixed methods approach was needed for this research as one methodology

was insufficient to both analyse relationships between exercise behaviour and the TPB

constructs and to scrutinise whether participants themselves qualitatively identified

attitude, subjective norms, PBC and past behaviour as the only factors affecting whether

they choose to exercise or not. Intention to exercise was not measured as this would predict

future exercise behaviour and actual exercise behaviour was only collected at one time.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

9

In this study several correlations were carried out to test for a positive relationship between

The TPB constructs (Attitude, Subjective Norms, Perceived Behavioural Control (PBC) and

past behaviour) and exercise behaviour, in different settings (work, travel, home, garden

and leisure) at different intensities (vigorous, moderate or walking). A positive relationship

would indicate that the construct correlated with exercise behaviour would have a

significant influence on whether the participant exercises or not.

Qualitative data were also collected in this survey through open-ended questions. Data was

analysed using Thematic Analysis to identify themes in the data relating to participants’

motivations to exercise. This method was utilised to assess whether these motivations

would make sense in terms of The TPB constructs. The type of thematic analysis conducted

was deductive, semantic and essentialist. Thematic Analysis was considered a ‘good’

method of analysis as it allows for unanticipated responses; is relatively quick and easy and

Braun and Clarke (2006) state it is a good method to use for researchers with little

experience of qualitative research. The qualitative questions on the survey were designed

using Ajzen’s (2002) ‘Constructing a Theory of Planned Behaviour Questionnaire’. The

statements were put to participants to respond to quantitatively first and then they were

asked to explain why they felt this way qualitatively e.g. ‘why do you see physical activities

to be a good/bad thing?’

Participants

There were 40 participants in the sample; 11 males, 28 females and 1 ‘undisclosed’. The age

range was between 18 and 30 years; this was an inclusion criteria to ensure the sample

represented a young population. A smaller sample was thought to be appropriate as both

quantitative and qualitative data were being analysed and it would have been time

USPJED-40-3 10013482

10

consuming to analyse both kinds of data from more Ps. 40 Ps was also thought to be a large

enough sample to represent the population. All Ps were students attending the University of

the West of England. The sampling method was opportunity sampling; Ps accessed the

survey via the participant pool at university and completed it in order to obtain credit

counting towards their degree. This method was thought to be appropriate as it was less

time-consuming; data was collected quickly and easily and, as Ps accessed the survey via

university, it was easier to obtain a student sample of the desired age range.

Materials

A single survey was created to measure all variables (See Appendix 1). It consisted of 49

questions. It was formulated by combining the International Physical Activity Questionnaire

(IPAQ) (long version) and questions formulated using Ajzen’s (2002) ‘Constructing a Theory

of Planned Behaviour Questionnaire’. The IPAQ data measured the respondents’ actual

behaviours quantitatively. Craig et al. (2003) found when using the IPAQ across 12 different

countries the questionnaires produced repeatable data (Spearman’s rho clustered around

0.8). The long form of the IPAQ was used in this research as it is more suited for research

requiring more detailed assessment and the short form is more suitable for national

monitoring. Since this research was on a much smaller scale, the longer version was chosen.

As this questionnaire also surveys exercise behaviour in a number of different settings, it

was thought to be more useful when testing whether the constructs have the same

predictive power in different settings. This questionnaire measured exercise behaviour by

setting (e.g. work) and by intensity (e.g. vigorous). In each of these settings Ps were required

to report days of exercise in the last 7, and minutes of exercise in that setting on one of

those days. This number of minutes was then multiplied by the number of days the

USPJED-40-3 10013482

11

participant reported exercising at that level in that setting. For example, if a P reported

exercising for 40 minutes vigorously during their leisure time on one day in the last 7 days,

and reported exercising this way 4 days in the last 7 their overall score for vigorous leisure

exercise would be 160. It is assumed that if the P exercised for 40 minutes on one of those 4

days they would exercise for 40 minutes on the other days as well.

As previously mentioned this questionnaire was integrated with questions, both

quantitative and qualitative, formulated using Ajzen’s (2002) ‘Constructing a Theory of

Planned Behaviour Questionnaire’. These questions tested the constructs of the TPB

individually, including the past behaviour construct which will be included in this study.

Attitude, Subjective Norms, PBC and past behaviour were all measured by means of likert

scales. Ps were shown several statements and given the option to respond with 1 of 7

responses. An example question would be ‘I believe frequent exercise to be a good thing’,

Ps could then respond with one of the following options - strongly disagree, disagree,

somewhat disagree, neither agree nor disagree, somewhat agree, disagree, strongly agree.

Each response was represented numerically with numbers 1 – 7; 1 meaning strongly

disagree, 7 meaning strongly agree.

Other likert scales were used measuring importance, likelihood, and similarity to self.

Several questions were used to measure each construct so that scores from the questions

measuring the same constructs could then be added together to produce overall Attitude,

Subjective Norm, PBC and past behaviour ‘scores’. Subjective Norm was split into 4 separate

sections as the construct appeared to be multifaceted. These sub-sections were - normative

importance (how important are your peers’ opinions of your exercise habits?), similar habits

(how similar are your exercise habits to your peers’?), important similar (how important it is

USPJED-40-3 10013482

12

to have similar exercise habits to your peers’?) and likely encourage (how likely are peers to

encourage you to exercise?). A higher score for Attitude indicates a more positive attitude

towards exercise; for Subjective Norm indicates subjective norms are more important; for

PBC indicates high PBC and for past behaviour indicates typically higher levels of exercise

than reported.

Quantitative questions were followed by qualitative open-ended questions allowing the Ps

to explain why he/she felt, for example, a friend’s opinion on their exercise habits was

important or unimportant to them. These questions were formulated by rewording the

quantitative questions simply allowing Ps to express why they held this opinion. Ajzen

himself explains how to test the TPB constructs (Ajzen, 2002), this could allow for bias when

making this questionnaire; however as there are no standardised questionnaires to test the

TPB, formulating questions following these instructions was one of the few viable options

when collecting data through a survey.

Procedure

Participants were recruited by means of the University participant pool through opportunity

sampling. Before beginning the survey Ps were shown an information sheet (see Appendix

1), divulging all information about the study; Ps were not deceived at all. Ps were given a

link to a website explaining what exercise addiction is and another link in case, although this

is highly unlikely, anyone became worried by taking this survey that they may have an

addiction to exercise. Ps were then shown a consent form (see Appendix 1) that informs the

Ps they can withdraw at any point without giving a reason for doing so; all information will

be kept confidential; the survey will bring them no physical or psychological harm and asks

USPJED-40-3 10013482

13

for their consent to participate. The Ps must have understood and agreed to the terms of

the consent form before continuing to take the survey.

Ps were required to complete one online survey which should have taken no more than 30

minutes to complete. Each participant was assigned a unique code so no data may be traced

personally back to any individual. It was the participants’ responsibility to ensure their data

was kept private whilst they completed the questionnaire e.g. complete it in a confidential

setting such as at home on their home computer with no one watching. After Ps had

completed the online survey they were shown a debrief sheet (see Appendix 2) attached to

the end of the survey reiterating the purpose of the study and informing them of previous

research in the area.

Once all data was collected, it was downloaded to SPSS for statistical analysis and the

qualitative data was transferred to Microsoft Word for coding. The qualitative data was

analysed using a deductive, semantic and essentialist approach. Deductive as the TPB is

being tested so the analysis was theory driven; essentialist as the responses coded were

taken to represent the participants’ ‘true’ reality and semantic as the entire data set was

coded in a descriptive manner. The data was read several times so it was familiar.

Everything concerning motivations or barriers to exercise was coded and similar codes were

grouped together if they were considered to portray the same underlying motivation or

barrier to exercise (see Appendix 5 for an example of coded data).

Results

Section 1 : Quantitative

USPJED-40-3 10013482

14

Before analysis some of the ambiguous data had to be altered in order for analysis to be

possible. A few participants stated the time they had spent doing exercise in hours,

consequently this was changed to minutes. 3 participants also stated the time they spent

exercising as a range e.g. 30 – 60 minutes, these entries were changed to the middle value

of the range – 45 minutes in this instance (full details in Appendix 3). A high value for these

variables indicated more minutes spent exercising.

As previously mentioned, scores from questions measuring the same TPB construct were

added together to produce an overall score for that construct. Furthermore for each setting

minutes of exercise was multiplied by days of exercise in that setting in the last 7 days to

create an overall number of minutes of exercise for the last 7 days. Individual correlations

were carried out between minutes of exercise in each setting and the TPB constructs. If the

TPB is effective at predicting exercise behaviour all constructs should be positively

correlated to actual behaviour. The hypothesis that all the TPB constructs would correlate

with actual exercise behaviour in all settings was not supported. A Pearson’s r test showed a

statistically significant positive correlation between leisure walking and SN, specifically

normative importance i.e. the importance of peers’ opinions on how much the individual

exercises (r=0.35, N=35, p=0.039). However, this was the only statistically significant

correlation found; none of the TPB constructs were significantly correlated with any other

exercise variable. Exercise through working, bicycle riding, walking as a means of travel,

gardening, housework and leisure activities (vigorous and moderate) were not significantly

correlated to the TPB constructs (p>0.05, See Figure 1).

USPJED-40-3 10013482

15

Figure 1. A table of correlations between TPB variables and exercise behaviour (p values to 2 decimal places)

Type of exercise

TPB variables

Attitude Normative importance

Similar habits

Important similar

Likely encourage

PBC Past exercise

Vigorous work 0.50 0.69 0.22 0.27 0.35 0.55 0.22

Moderate work 0.32 0.66 0.43 0.15 0.36 0.17 0.26

Walking work 0.20 0.94 0.28 0.55 0.32 0.89 0.54

Bicycle riding 0.13 0.54 0.54 0.32 0.54 0.80 0.13

Walking to travel 0.63 0.48 0.78 0.47 0.69 0.90 0.92

Vigorous garden 0.25 0.07 0.58 0.80 0.62 0.54 0.64

Moderate garden 0.45 0.18 0.58 0.45 0.79 0.54 0.85

Moderate housework

0.70 0.25 0.83 0.38 0.75 0.51 0.16

Leisure walking 0.22 0.04* 0.25 0.17 0.45 0.76 0.30

Vigorous leisure 0.19 0.16 0.52 0.64 0.11 0.32 0.12

Moderate leisure 0.11 0.28 0.53 1.00 0.70 0.14 0.30

Section 2 : Qualitative

A thematic analysis was used to analyse the qualitative data, and 8 themes surrounding

motivations to exercise were identified. The themes identified were - Health, Competition,

Friendship, Performance, Appearance, ‘Feeling Good’ and finally Independence. A further 5

themes surrounding barriers to exercise were also identified. These themes were - Exercise

as not a priority; The body as a barrier; Motivation; Weather and Money. Of these, themes

of Health, Friendship, Independence and Exercise as not a priority will be discussed and

analysed in terms of whether they fit into the TPB constructs.

Health

A number of Ps identified health as a motivator to exercise. Ps responses suggest that they

believe exercise improves their health therefore hold a positive attitude towards it and are

more likely to engage in it. P19 stated that “It (exercise) is a good thing because it… keeps

USPJED-40-3 10013482

16

me physically and mentally healthy”. This response indicates the P’s belief that by exercising

she will remain healthy, both physically and mentally. This broadens the term of health to

include mental health as well as physical health. P28 also seemed to share this belief about

exercise – “(Exercise) contributes to keeping a healthy weight and healthy mental state”.

P10 reported that “Exercise is just as important as a good balanced diet”; this extract

suggests that this P believes that incorporating exercise is just as important as a healthy diet

in order to maintain a healthy lifestyle. It seems that Ps tend to hold positive attitudes

towards exercise behaviour and believe that by exercising they will experience positive

health outcomes. This implies that the P holds a positive belief towards exercise before

engaging in it; the expectation of positive health outcomes is the motivator to engage in

exercise. These beliefs strongly resemble the Attitude construct of the TPB – positive

evaluation of a particular behaviour due to expected positive outcomes from it.

P41 stated that “It will make me become fitter because I am doing more exercise, it effects

my attitude positively”. This supports the assumption that the expectation of positive

outcomes is what drives the P to exercise, and the relevance of the Attitude construct in this

theory. However, it could be interpreted that, this positive attitude towards exercise

behaviour has been produced through exercising. Therefore it is not the Ps attitude that has

made them intend to exercise, it is in exercising itself that this positive attitude towards it

has been produced. P23 articulates that “I force myself to go to the gym almost every day …

so my fitness improves, this has changed my attitude towards exercise because I can see the

changes in myself and makes me feel a lot more positive about it”. As the P reported she has

to force herself to exercise, this implies that the positive belief she holds towards exercise

was not there originally; it was after she began to see fitness and health improvements

USPJED-40-3 10013482

17

through exercising that she began to feel positively about exercise. This supports the

alternative interpretation that, through exercising and experiencing positive health

outcomes, the positive attitude is induced.

Some Ps also discussed their concerns about the negative health effects of not exercising.

The concern that by not exercising there will be negative health repercussions may be

another important motivator to exercise. P7 reported that he felt exercise was important

“because I don’t want to be unhealthy or unfit”. This extract supports the notion that health

may also motivate participants to exercise in order for them to avoid becoming unhealthy or

unwell. P32 stated that exercising was important to him “Because I want to be active now in

order to have a long and healthy life”; this implies that it is believed that, by exercising, life-

threatening illness is less likely and health is maintained in the future. Ps appear to hold the

attitude that exercise prevents bad health and thus is a good thing, supporting the inclusion

of the Attitude component in the TPB. However, Ps attitudes may not be entirely positive –

they may not enjoy exercise but see the behaviour as necessary to remain in good health.

Friendship

Another motivator to exercise identified across the data was ‘Friendship’. Ps seem to

identify exercise as important as they want others to feel positively towards them and feel

exercise will help to produce this positive opinion. Others appear to feel that exercising is a

fundamental requirement of being in their friendship group. Ps also reported that they

enjoy exercising with friends - for some as it keeps them motivated, and for others as they

enjoy the company. This theme was split into two sub-themes – Acceptance and Company.

Acceptance

USPJED-40-3 10013482

18

Acceptance was a recurrent sub-theme throughout the data. A number of participants

reported pressure to engage in physical activity from their peers. A number of Ps reported

caring about their peers’ opinions of their exercise habits; P2 stated “I care about these

people’s opinions as it reflects what they think of me”. Ps seemed to feel that through

exercising their peers would think more positively about them and they would be accepted;

P38 “I care what they think everyone wants to be accepted its human nature”. It would

appear that a motivator to exercise is Ps wanting to conform to group norms by engaging in

exercise and thus to be accepted. A number of other Ps expressed their desire to appear

how they perceived their peers wanted them to i.e. active; P33 reported “I don’t want to

seem lazy”. This extract implies the P cares about others’ opinions of her exercise habits as

she doesn’t want to be perceived badly. The SN construct in the TPB refers to the

perception of normative amounts of a behaviour (e.g. exercise) and perceived pressure to

perform that behaviour and motivation to comply with these norms. As previously shown in

the data extracts, it appears some Ps perceive social pressure to engage in exercise and are

thus more likely to participate as they don’t want to be disapproved of. P26 said “(I) Don’t

want to be looked down on for not exercising”. This implies the P perceives exercise habits

to be important to her social status; she feels she must exercise in order for others to not

think badly of her and is therefore motivated to comply to these norms, supporting the

relevance of the SN construct.

Some Ps reported they felt they had to adhere to the amount of exercise their peers were

engaging in - P32“I feel pressure to keep up with them”. Some described feeling badly if

they did not conform to the amount of exercise their peers do - P33 “If I don’t adhere to the

amount they do I feel guilty”. It seems that some Ps felt a sense of need to engage in the

USPJED-40-3 10013482

19

‘expected’ amount of exercise that those around them engage in, and comply with social

norms. P7 stated that “Because they are your friends they are going to have similar interests

as you”. This suggests this P may believe that peers are supposed to do as much exercise as

one another; it is almost as if it is believed to be a requirement of friendship to engage in

the same levels of exercise as each other so you have that in common. This implies that by

conforming to the normative amount of exercise in the group, it is believed you are

accepted within the friendship group. This would be the individual’s motivation to comply

with exercise norms if this was to be understood in terms of the SN construct.

Company

Another sub—theme identified under the broader theme of ‘Friendship’ was Company. It

was interpreted that peer company and encouragement whilst exercising was a motivator

to exercise. P4 “I train with a friend in the gym, and he helps keep me motivated in going to

the gym and when we are there to keep pushing me”. It appears that it may not just be

social pressures that make people exercise but their company that motivates them – P14

“My family sometimes encourage me to exercise. It motivates me if they are also doing the

activity.” This statement implies the encouragement only works coupled with company of

others also engaging in the activity, otherwise encouragement alone would not work. This P

doesn’t seem to feel the opinions of others on his exercise habits are important; it would

seem company is his encouragement. The influence of company is not addressed in the

TPB, social norms are the only motivator mentioned. When asked if the amount their peers

exercise influences their exercise habits P1 reported “they (peers) come with me and make

me go with them when I am less inclined”; this suggests that if this P didn’t have his peers

USPJED-40-3 10013482

20

for company whilst exercising he would not go as often. P12 reported she was motivated by

company - “Friends for company at the gym”.

Throughout the data it was noticed that Ps sometimes used exercise as a means to meet up

with, and socialise with friends - P10 “I find exercise to be a social thing as well so spending

time with family and friends at the same time”. This data implies that peers sometimes

make time for seeing their friends through exercising together - P2 “Same interests, stuff to

talk about and fitting in time for your friends via exercise”. It could be interpreted that if

peers did not exercise the same amount as this P they may not be ‘fit in’ to this P’s life as

easily; friendships could be being maintained through exercising together socially. This

influence is not directly referred to in the TPB motivation to exercise as it is a social activity

– social influences are only mentioned in the SN construct referring to others’ attitudes

about the behaviour affecting the individual’s.

P38 stated “It is easier to work out with a buddy then alone”. This response implies that

exercise seems less challenging when you have a “buddy” to exercise with. Several Ps

reported a preference to exercising with peers – P8 “I prefer to exercise with friends” and

that it was more enjoyable – P37 “it is more fun to exercise with friends”. These responses

seem to suggest that Ps are more motivated to exercise if with someone else, as it is more

fun which makes them want to exercise more - P27 “playing sports with friends is always

more enjoyable and you are more likely to want to do it again”. These extracts seem to

support the Attitude construct in the TPB as Ps feel more positive about the behaviour if

they have company and are therefore more likely to participate in it; however there is no

direct mention of company as a motivator in the TPB and it seems to be, for these Ps, of

some importance.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

21

Independence

On the other hand, many Ps deemed exercise to be a personal activity and seemed to feel

the exercise habits of others or others’ opinions on their exercise habits were unimportant.

This is a completely contrasting opinion to those previously mentioned. P35 reported “my

social life doesn’t revolve around exercise”, this response implies that for this P social

acceptance does not depend upon how much exercise they do. P27 reported “I am my own

person and keep what I do to myself and don’t let other influence me” this statement was

interpreted to mean this P does not care about others’ influences or opinions on their

(undisclosed gender) exercise habits, it was interpreted that they feel they exercise as much

as they want regardless of others. This opinion seemed to be shared – P6 “I don’t care that

much about other people’s opinions”. Others expressed the failure of others’ pressures to

persuade them to exercise - P37 “Family have (tried to motivate me) but doesn’t motivate

me”. It would seem that some feel their exercise habits are personal and, even when under

pressure from others to exercise, they will exercise or not exercise as they would have done

without outside influences. These extracts completely contest the importance of the SN

construct as these Ps felt others opinions had no influence on their exercise behaviours and

felt no pressure to comply.

Some participants wrote about how exercise was for personal gain, therefore it seemed

these Ps felt others’ opinions were irrelevant - P34 “I exercise for my benefit not theirs”.

Others wrote about their individuality - P9 “Everyone is different and need different levels

of exercise”. It seemed some Ps believe exercise habits are subjective to the needs of the

individual and a personal activity and therefore social norms were of no importance to

them. It was interpreted that many Ps felt they themselves were the only people important

USPJED-40-3 10013482

22

in deciding their own exercise habits - P21 “it is my choice what I do with my body”. P17

reported “I don’t try and do the same as them the lack of exercise is based on my time”, this

extract was interpreted to mean this Ps exercise habits are nothing to do with others’ habits

or opinions; it is this P’s time that controls how much exercise she engages in, which is

personal to the individual.

Exercise as not a priority

The main barrier Ps reported for not engaging in exercise was time; P10 “Lack of time is a

barrier that stops me from exercising”. However it was interpreted that throughout the data

when Ps mentioned having a lack of ‘time’ it actually meant that exercise was not a priority

and other pressures took precedence; P18 “Time is a major barrier in trying to fit everything

in”. It seemed that it was not time itself that stood in the way of exercising, rather the

engagement in other activities. P4 reported “I have a lot of studying to do and that is out of

my control” it was interpreted that Ps chose to blame other commitments on their lack of

exercise and perceived to have no control over these other commitments as barriers. P17

shared the belief that other commitments were a barrier to exercise - “My workload stands

in the way, I have no control over deadlines and amount of work”. These responses support

the inclusion of PBC in the TPB as Ps perceive that these other commitments stop them

from engaging in exercise and they have no control over these barriers.

Interestingly however, when asked about the barriers that prevent exercise, P25 reported

“social activity and uni work … limit the time I can spend doing exercise. I have control over

these but choose to allocate my time this way”; this P writes of the other commitments

preventing exercise, however states that he has control over his time as he chooses which

activities to engage in at any time or not. This extract supports the notion that Ps reporting

USPJED-40-3 10013482

23

having a lack of time may just feel exercise is not a priority in their lives and use other

commitments as an excuse to not exercise, as they have a choice which activities they

engage in. This interpretation is also supported by P15’s entry “It’s (exercise is) not that

important to me because I find other things to do”. These two Ps seem to feel there is

always a choice of how to allocate your own time. They were interpreted to feel they have

control over their barriers to exercise however still do not want to engage in it. This

contests the importance of the PBC construct in the TPB for these Ps.

Discussion

It was found that only the SN construct was related to walking exercise behaviour in a

leisure setting. No other significant correlations were found. The hypothesis that the TPB

constructs would all relate to all types of exercise behaviour in all settings was not

supported. This is not in-keeping with what other researchers have found; the literature

mainly supports the TPB (Madden, Ellen and Ajzen, 1992; Armitage and Conner, 2001; Godin

and Kok, 1996; Hausenblas, Carron and Mack, 1997). As there is vast support for the TPB in

the literature and the current research largely contests what has been found before, the

methods used to collect data in this study may need to be questioned.

The scales used to calculate total exercise behaviour for the week may have influenced the

results. Days of exercising were multiplied by the time the P reported to have exercised on

one of those days; the P may not have necessarily exercised for that amount of time on each

of the occasions they exercised that week. Therefore this scale may not be valid as it may

not be a true representation of the time each P exercised in that week; the amount of

USPJED-40-3 10013482

24

exercise is likely to be overestimated. The quantitative questions in the survey measuring

PBC and past exercise may also have limited validity. The question supposed to be

measuring PBC only asks the P to rate if they feel their current fitness level will allow them

to exercise a healthy amount. As PBC is composed of the Ps perception of both internal

(skills( i.e. fitness level)) and external control factors (i.e. obstacles), this question may only

have allowed for the former to be measured. The question measuring past exercise only

asks the P if they normally do more or less exercise than they have stated in the survey. This

does not adequately represent whether the P has exercised regularly in the past or not; it

only tells whether they currently do more or less than was reported. It should also be noted

that as the survey only asked P to recount the amount the P exercised within the last 7 days

this may not be a true representation of how much the P exercises typically. If the test were

carried out again completely different results could be obtained, consequently the data may

not be reliable.

Another major limitation of this research is that data was analysed using several

correlations; this does not allow for any causal relationship to be established between

variables, the significant result found only implies a relationship. There could be another

variable influencing both levels of SN and walking exercise behaviour. Other confounding

influences could have been that, as the information sheet informed Ps exactly what was

being measured, they could have answered the survey differently than they would have

done had they not known why the questions were being asked. Although this method allows

for the Ps not to be deceived in any way, the knowledge of what the research is about could

have influenced their responses. A second confounder could have been that data was

collected by means of self-report. Ps were trusted to truthfully and accurately account their

USPJED-40-3 10013482

25

exercise behaviours of the past week. Social desirability bias could have interfered with how

truthful the accounts of exercise behaviour were; Ps may have reported more exercise than

they actually participated in as they may view exercise as a socially desirable activity, or may

have reported a positive attitude towards exercise when they may actually feel indifferent.

The qualitative data provided partial support for TPB constructs. Attitude seemed to be an

important motivator; SN was interpreted to be an important motivator but the theme of

Independence contested it’s relevance for some Ps. PBC appeared to be a factor as to

whether Ps exercised or not, however some participants seemed to feel that they did have

control over the barriers preventing exercise and chose not to exercise. This raised the

question as to whether the Ps in fact did not have control over the barriers preventing

exercise or did have control over these barriers but used them as excuses not to exercise as

it was not a priority.

Although you cannot generalise qualitative research, some researchers have also theorised

that barriers reported may not be “real” barriers, or even perceived as real, but excuses to

not exercise as it is not a priority to them. Dishman (2001) theorised that Ps reporting

barriers to exercise were not necessarily reporting real obstacles to exercise. It was found

that exercisers and non-exercisers reported the same barriers; if this is the case then it

raises the question of how some Ps in the current study felt they had control over these

barriers and some did not? Dishman also noted that some drop-outs from an exercise

program lived closer to exercise facilities and had more leisure time than their counterparts

who continued with the exercise program. Barriers that Ps are reporting may well be

excuses more than actual barriers. Ps may have wanted to report they had no control over

the barriers to exercise due to the social desirability effect; they may not have wanted to

USPJED-40-3 10013482

26

appear ‘lazy’. Morgan (1977) found that even those living in environments where time

restraints are minimal (e.g. prison), do not have exercise habits that are substantially

different from those of people in the general population. The findings of this study suggest

that for some, other commitments and time are just an excuse not to engage in exercise, as

people with large amounts of free time still do not do significantly more exercise than the

general population. Ps in the current study may have believed that they had no control over

the barriers to exercise – “I have a lot of studying to do and that is out of my control”, which

would support the inclusion of PBC in the TPB. However, some reported having control over

the same barriers but choose to engage in other activities in their leisure time – “I have

control over these but choose to allocate my time this way” these extracts support the

notion that some barriers that are reported may just be excuses not to engage in exercise.

PBC is partially supported however, it will prove very challenging to tell whether Ps are

reporting real barriers or just making excuses. This should be explored in further research.

Ps seemed to hold positive attitudes towards exercise due to it being perceived as being

good for their health – “It is a good thing because… it keeps me physically and mentally

healthy”. However it seemed to be unclear whether the positive attitude preceded the

exercise behaviour and motivated the Ps to engage in it or was induced by “seeing results”

through participating in exercise and therefore feeling more positively about it. Sutton

(2002) argues that the TPB constructs are assumed to causally influence behaviour and this

causality cannot be assumed unless experimental methods are used to conclude this. The

theory does not consider that an attitude could be induced by the engagement in a

behaviour or that relationships between variables may not be causal. Sniehotta (2009)

tested the TPB experimentally using a behaviour change intervention and found that

USPJED-40-3 10013482

27

although the constructs successfully predicted intention, behaviour change was not in line

with the theory; there appeared to be an intention-behaviour gap. He concluded that the

TPB leading position in behavioural science should be questioned. However, as this

experiment took place over 2 months, complete control over all extraneous variables was

unlikely to have been maintained; this could have affected the results of this study.

This intention-behaviour gap has been addressed in the research; Conner and Armitage

(1998) suggest that past behaviour can account for 13% future behaviour. Ouellette and

Wood (1998) suggest possible ways past behaviour could influence future behaviour. The

first suggestion is that past behaviour may cause a conscious change in cognition e.g. “I

exercised and it felt good so I will again today”. This pathway seems to explain why some Ps

were interpreted to have induced their positive attitude towards exercise by engaging in it –

“I force myself to go to the gym almost every day … so my fitness improves, this has

changed my attitude towards exercise because I can see the changes in myself and makes

me feel a lot more positive about it”. This additional variable in the TPB may account for

more of the variability in behaviour, and should be included in the model in future studies.

Although this construct was not found to relate to exercise behaviour in the current study,

as was discussed earlier, the quantitative question measuring past behaviour may not have

been valid.

Another question raised by the study was that as Ps only spoke a little about why they felt

positively or negatively towards exercise, it was unclear whether their opinions were

entirely either positive or negative. Breckler (1994) theorised that individuals may hold

“competing evaluative predispositions” towards a behaviour; meaning their attitudes would

not entirely be either positive of negative towards exercise. Sparks et al. (2001) found when

USPJED-40-3 10013482

28

taking both negative and positive attitudes into account, when using the TPB to predict food

consumption, participants holding more ambivalent attitudes were less likely to translate

their overall attitude into an intention to eat and actual consumption. These findings

suggest that taking both positive and negative attitudes towards a behaviour into account

and creating an overall attitude variable that considers ambivalence, could account for more

variability in intention and behaviour. Ambivalence should be considered in future studies in

order to get a ‘true’ representation of a participant’s attitude. This could also be possible if

qualitative interviews took place so the researcher could collect rich in-depth data

conveying the participant’s full attitude towards exercise e.g. it is good for my health so I

feel positively about it, however it tires me out and is boring so I feel negatively about

engaging in it.

Support was found for the SN construct within the data – Acceptance was a common sub-

theme throughout. Some Ps seemed to feel that if they listened to their peers’ opinions and

engaged in physical activity they would be accepted by them - “I care what they think

everyone wants to be accepted its human nature”. Some also appeared to feel that they

had to engage in exercise in order to be accepted as if it were a requirement of friendship -

“I feel pressure to keep up with them”. However, Independence was also a common sub-

theme throughout the data. Some Ps felt others’ opinions on their exercise habits were

unimportant - “I don’t care that much about other people’s opinions” and others reported

the failed attempts of others to encourage them to exercise - “Family have (tried to

motivate me) but doesn’t motivate me”. These are contradictory findings suggesting that SN

are not an important motivator to exercise for all; there is an element of subjectivity

possibly depending on the Ps personalities. It appears that a few of the Ps seemed to be

USPJED-40-3 10013482

29

self-motivated to engage in exercise behaviour, without external influences - “I am my own

person and keep what I do to myself and don’t let other influence me”. Self-determination

theory (Deci and Ryan, 2000) is a theory of human motivation concerned with how

individuals motivate themselves to engage in a behaviour without outside influence.

Ingledew, Markland and Sheppard (2004) speculated, when investigating personality and

self-determination of exercise behaviour, that extroverted individuals feel self-determined

to exercise as engaging in exercise satisfies their need for relatedness. It was also theorised

that conscientious individuals are able to feel self-determined to exercise as it satisfies their

need for competence. This suggests that the SN construct may bear less motivational

influence on those extroverted or conscientious individuals. Perhaps personality should be

taken into account when using the TPB to predict exercise behaviour as the constructs may

be less influential to some than others.

In a wide variety of studies SN has been found to be the weakest predictor of behaviour in

the TPB (Armitage and Conner, 2001). However some studies have found that similar

influences that are not mentioned in the TPB are influential. As was discussed in the study

Company was a common sub-theme. This is not mentioned directly in the theory and in this

data appeared to be very influential - “Friends for company at the gym”. Ball, Bauman, Leslie

and Owen (2001) found walking with a companion, peers or pets was positively correlated

with engaging in more walking exercise behaviour. Although this is only a correlational study

meaning there was only a relationship between variables, it was not necessarily causal.

However, Courneya, Plotnikoff, Hotz, and Birkett (2000) found when conducting telephone

interviews that social support was a superior predictor of exercise intention than SN. In the

current study many Ps commented on their peers providing social support to continue with

USPJED-40-3 10013482

30

exercise behaviour - “I train with a friend in the gym, and he helps keep me motivated in

going to the gym and when we are there to keep pushing me”. These findings suggest that

the SN construct should be revised or these additional variables should be considered in the

theory when being used to predict exercise behaviour.

Although qualitative data cannot be generalised it appears that the TPB cannot account for

all the motivators Ps report to engage in exercise behaviour in the current study. In future

studies, past behaviour, ambivalence of attitude, personality, company and social support

should be taken into account to investigate whether these additional variables will increase

the predictive power of the TPB.

The current study takes a fairly novel approach to this research area, qualitative studies are

not common in exploring theories of behaviour. The study allows for quantitative analysis as

well as analysis of in-depth qualitative data allowing the Ps to explain why they felt for

example a peer’s opinions on their exercise behaviour was important or unimportant. The

method allowed for explanation of underlying mechanisms of influence on exercise

behaviour. By using two methodologies this allows for triangulation; if quantitative

methodology alone had been used it may have been concluded that the TPB constructs

have little relation to exercise behaviour however, the qualitative data found partial support

for the constructs therefore the quantitative methodology was questioned.

Despite the study’s strengths, there were several methodological issues in addition to those

stated earlier. The qualitative questions asked to Ps were leading and could have affected

responses e.g. ’Does the amount your peers exercise influence how much you exercise?

Why?’. This question directly asks the P to think about whether his/her peers influence how

much he/she exercises; if this study were to be carried out again more general questions

USPJED-40-3 10013482

31

should be asked to Ps such as ‘what motivates you to exercise?’ . This way a true account

may be given as to what is important to the P when it comes to influences to exercise.

However, when using leading questions, Ps still reported other motivators other than the

TPB constructs – Company, Appearance and so on. In future studies perhaps conducting a

focus group asking Ps about general motivations to exercise would be more beneficial. As

there was only one significant result when conducting a quantitative study perhaps the

constructs may be ‘captured’ better qualitatively. Ps can then freely report all motivators

and barriers to exercise without such leading questions restricting what they can discuss.

In conclusion, this study provides partial support for the effectiveness of the TPB in

predicting exercise behaviour. However, in future studies the theory should be tested

qualitatively so all motivations to exercise can be explored in an in-depth fashion. Additional

variables should also be considered when using this theory to predict exercise behaviour as

this research and a number of studies support the inclusion of these variables – past

behaviour, ambivalence of attitude, personality, company and social support.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

32

References

Armitage, C. J., Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-

analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471-499.

Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human

Decision Processes, 50, 179-211.

Ajzen, I., (2002). Constructing a TPB questionnaire: Conceptual and methodological

considerations. Retrieved from http://www-unix.oit.umass.edu/~aizen/pdf.

tpbmeasurement.pdf.

Ball, K., Bauman, A., Leslie, E., Owen, N. (2001). Perceived environmental aesthetics and

convenience and company are associated with walking for exercise among Australian

adults, Preventive Medicine, 33( 5), 434–40.

Bozionelos, G., & Bennett, P. (1999). The Theory of Planned Behaviour as predictor of

exercise: The moderating influence of beliefs and personality variables. Journal of Health

Psychology, 4, 517–529.

Braun, V., Clarke, V. (2006). Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative Research in

Psychology, 3(2), 77-10.

Breckler, S. J. (1994). A comparison of numerical indices for measuring attitudinal

ambivalence. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 54, 350-365.

Conner, M., Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the Theory of Planned Behaviour: a review and

avenues for further research, Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429-1464.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

33

Courneya, K. S., Plotnikoff, R. C., Hotz, S. B., Birkett, N. J. (2000). Social support and the

theory of planned behavior in the exercise domain. American Journal of Health Behavior, 24,

300-308

Craig, C. L., Marshall, A. L., Sjorstrom, M., Bauman, A. E., Booth, M. L., Ainsworth, B. E.,

Pratt, M., Ekelund, J. F., Oja, P. (2003) International physical activity questionnaire: 12-

country reliability and validity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 35 8: 1381-

1395.

Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human

behavior. New York: Plenum.

Dishman, R.K. (2001). The problem of exercise adherence: Fighting sloth in nations with

market economies, Quest, 53, 279-294.

Fishbein, M., Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction to

Theory and Research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

Godin, G., Kok, G. (1996). The theory of planned behavior: A review of its applications in

health-related behaviors. American Journal of Health Promotion, 11, 87-98.

Hausenblas, H. A., Carron, A. V., Mack, D. E. (1997). Application of the theories of reasoned

action and planned behavior to exercise behavior: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport &

Exercise Psychology, 19, 36-51.

Ingledew, D.K., Markland, D., Sheppard, K.E. (2004). Personality and self-determination of

exercise behaviour. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1921-1932.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

34

Knowles, S., Hyde, M., White, K. (2012) Predictors of Young People’s Charitable Intentions to

donate Money: An Extended Theory of Planned Behavior Perspective. Journal of Applied

Social Psychology, 42, 9, pp. 2096–2110.

Madden, T. J., Ellen, P. S., Ajzen, I. (1992). A comparison of the theory of planned behavior

and the theory of reasoned action. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18, 3-9.

Morgan, W. P. (1977). Involvement in vigorous physical activity with special reference to

adherence. In L I. Gedvilasand M.W. Kneer (Eds.) Proceedings of National College Physical

Education Association, pp. 235-246.

Ogden, J. (2012) Health Psychology: a textbook: Open University Press: Buckingham

Ouellette, J. A., Wood, W. (1998). Habit and Intention in Everyday Life: The Multiple

Processes by Which Past Behavior Predicts Future Behavior. Psychology Bulletin, 124(1), 54-

74.

Sniehotta, F. (2009). An Experimental Test of the Theory of Planned Behavior. Applied

Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 1, 257–270.

Sparks, P., Conner, M., James, R., Shepherd, R., Povey, R. (2001). Ambivalence about health-

related behaviours: An exploration in the domain of food choice. British Journal of Health

Psychology, 6, 53-68.

Spink, K., Wilson, K., Bostick, J. (2012). Theory of Planned Behavior and Intention to Exercise:

Effects of Setting. American Journal of Health Behavior. 36 (2), 254-264.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

35

Sutton, S. (2002). Testing attitude–behaviour theories using non-experimental data: An

examination of some hidden assumptions. European Review of Social Psychology, 13, 293–

323.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

36

Appendices

Appendix 1: Information sheet, Consent Form, Survey - example of a completed survey

USPJED-40-3 10013482

37

USPJED-40-3 10013482

38

USPJED-40-3 10013482

39

USPJED-40-3 10013482

40

USPJED-40-3 10013482

41

USPJED-40-3 10013482

42

USPJED-40-3 10013482

43

USPJED-40-3 10013482

44

USPJED-40-3 10013482

45

Appendix 2: De-brief sheet

Thank you for taking the time to complete this survey! A revised version of Azjen's (1991) The Theory of Planned Behaviour was tested in this research. Ajzen thought 3 main variables or “beliefs” of an individual could predict their subsequent behaviour. These three beliefs were: attitude towards the behaviour, subjective norms , and perceived behavioural control which together were said to shape an individual's behavioural intentions and ultimately their behaviours. Perceived behavioural control is also seen to have a direct effect on behaviour. Past behaviour was also included as a predictor variable. The aim of this study was to test the effectiveness of Ajzen's 'Theory of Planned Behaviour' in relation to exercise behaviours. A second aim of the research is to test if past behaviour is a significant predictor of intention and behaviour in relation to exercise. Hausenblas, Carron and Mack (1997) is a meta-analysis which investigates the effectiveness of the application of both the Theory of Reasoned Action (which is the Theory of Planned Behaviour minus the Perceived Behavioural Control construct) and the Theory of Planned Behaviour when predicting exercise behaviour. The paper concludes that there is strong evidence that the Theory of Reasoned Action is a ‘good’ theory and the Theory of Planned Behaviour is an even better theory in predicting exercise behaviour. Armitage and Conner (2001) conducted a meta-analytic review looking into the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Each construct was evaluated and it was found that the subjective norm construct is generally found to be a weak predictor of intentions compared to attitude and perceived behavioural control. In this research project questions are asked in order to measure all constructs of the Theory of Planned Behaviour separately and evaluate the efficacy of the constructs qualitatively. If you would like any further information or a copy of the results email [email protected]

Appendix 3: Changes to data

R_0U7XZRDAzMaD6At (8) – walk travel time 10-15 changed to 12.5minutes.

R_eWXHSrXQPvETuAZ (10) – walk travel time 30 – 60 changed to 45 minutes.

R_6VxSxIMh0Hbmqr3 (18) – sitting week 4-5 hours changed to 270 minutes (4.5 hrs),

sitting weekend changed from 3-4 hours to 210 minutes (3.5 hours).

USPJED-40-3 10013482

46

Appendix 4: Ethical Approval

USPJED-40-3 10013482

47

Appendix 5: Qualitative coding example Q29. Why do you see physical activities to be a good/bad thing?

1. When you push too hard and cause injury

-Exercise associated with injury.

2. Improves health and confidence

- Exercise as improving health and confidence.

3. Can put unnecessary strain on the body and do more harm than good, must be moderated

for effectiveness.

- Exercise associated with negative health outcomes. “Right” amount of exercise needed.

4. They help to reduce fat, improve cardiovascular system, improve health generally.

- Exercise associated with health improvement.

5. Keeps me fit

- Exercise good for fitness and health.

6. Because it is good for your health

- Exercise as good for your health.

7. Because it gets you fit and healthy

- Exercise making you become fit and healthy.

8. I see physical activities as a good thing as they help keep you healthy

- Exercise as good. Keeping you healthy.

9. increases confidence and relieves stress

- Exercise as confidence boosting and mood improving.

10. Exercise is just as important as a good balanced diet

- Exercise as just as important for health as a good diet.

11. Healthy, good for your body

- Exercise as good for the body. Health improving.

12. Releases serotonin/makes you feel better/helps with fitness

- Exercise as mood and fitness improving.

13. keeps you motivated and fit

- Exercise as fitness. Exercise as motivating.

14. I see them as a good thing because as well as health benefits, you tend to feel happier

afterwards.

- Exercise as good – beneficial to health - and mood.

15. Good thing: To stay healthy and physically fit.

- Exercise as health and fitness; a positive thing.

16. it keeps you fit.

- Exercise as keeping you fit and healthy.

17. I don't see them as either good or bad, I just can't find the time to exercise with all the

course work i have to do.

- Neutral feelings about exercise. – Coursework as barrier to exercise.

18. maintain health, and weight

- Exercise as health. – Exercise to maintain current weight.

19. It is a good thing because it releases stress and keeps my physically and mentally healthy.

- Exercise as good for physical and mental health. – Exercise as stress relief.

USPJED-40-3 10013482

48

Research Project Schedule of Work Check List The idea behind this check list is to help you organise your project work and so that you can see at a glance the work that you need to do. It will also remind your supervisor of your progress and will help them to provide an academic reference for you when you apply for jobs or further study. It is also useful for keeping a record of your supervisory meetings and to identify the level of support you have received.

Separate copies to be retained by the student and the supervisor. Please synchronise your copies during your supervisory meetings.

Student’s Name Ruth Curtis

Student’s ID 10013482

Student’s Contact Details [email protected]

Working Title of Project

A mixed methods investigation testing the effectiveness of the Theory of Planned Behaviour constructs at predicting exercise behaviour

Please check off the following items as you achieve them. There is room to insert comments and dates against each item if necessary.

Section 1. Student Declaration

Comments

I have read and understood what to hand-in and on which dates

Yes

I have read the Research Project in Psychology module handbook

Yes

I understand that I should attempt to keep in regular contact with my supervisor

Yes

I understand that if I intend to use human participants that I must not begin testing until I have received ethical approval (in the form of an approved Project Certificate)

Yes

I understand that if I intend to use human participants that it is an assessment offence to provide false information on them

Yes

I understand that it is an assessment offence to invent, covertly manipulate, or provide false information about the data I have collected and presented in my project

Yes

Section 2. The Planned Study

I have identified a topic area Yes

I have identified a purpose for the study Yes

I have defined the research question or hypothesis

Yes

I have identified the basic design of the study Yes

USPJED-40-3 10013482

49

Section 3. Research Design

For an experimental or quantitative study: I can describe each independent variable and its levels N/A Correlational – no IV or DV I can describe the dependent variable(s) and how it is

(they are) to be measured N/A Correlational – no IV or DV

I can state the particular statistical test to be used to analyse the data

YES

I know how to use SPSS to analyse this data YES I have identified a useful text to help me if necessary YES

For a qualitative study: I am aware of the particular approach for gathering and

analysing the data YES

I am aware of the method of analysis YES I know how to apply the method of analysis on my

particular data YES

I have identified a useful text to help me if necessary YES

I have discussed which materials to use for the study (photographs, text, music, questionnaires etc) and how to develop or obtain them

YES

Section 4. Health & Safety and Ethical Issues

I have discussed ethical issues with my supervisor

YES

I have discussed any permissions needed with my supervisor

YES

I have discussed any relevant health and safety issues with my supervisor

YES

I know how to access and use RAGS YES

I have developed an information sheet for my study

YES

I have developed a consent form for my study YES

I have developed a debrief sheet for my study YES

I understand that if I carry out my research project without ethical approval then it may not be marked

YES

I have checked that I have approval to proceed with my study before testing participants

YES

I have printed of my Project Certificate from RAGS

YES

Section 5. Data Collection and Analysis

USPJED-40-3 10013482

50

I have carried out my testing or I have collected my data for qualitative analysis

YES

I have analysed the data YES

I know how to interpret the results of the study

YES

I know how to report my findings appropriately

YES

I know how to evaluate its findings YES

I have identified what to include as an appendix

YES

I understand how to cite and reference properly

YES

Section 6. Feedback on your Draft Report

I have obtained general feedback on the following sections/aspects of my report

Your supervisor will not provide very detailed feedback (e.g., they will not rewrite your text for you or identify all of your grammatical, spelling or typographical errors)

Abstract YES Introduction YES

Research question or hypothesis YES Method YES

Results/Analysis YES

Discussion YES Conclusions YES Referencing YES Appendices -

Communication and grammar YES Critical comments and reasoning YES

Section 7. GDP and References

I have written a curriculum vitae (CV) N/A

I have sent my supervisor a copy of my CV N/A

I have received feedback on my CV from my supervisor

N/A

I have attempted the GDP Level 3 work N/A

I have completed a personal statement for the purposes of an academic reference

N/A

USPJED-40-3 10013482

51

Section 8. RAGS Check, tick, or comment as appropriate

Project Outline

H & S

Ethics

Animals Genetic

Mod. Consent

Form

Information Sheet

Debrief

Student completed

YES YES YES YES YES N/A N/A

Tutor checked

and signed off

YES YES YES YES YES YES YES

Section 9. Supervisory Record

Date Time Discussed Student’s To Do

27/09/12

3:00

Discussed a number of ideas related to health psychology. Decided on a rough plan to

conduct a qualitative survey that looks at the reasons why some people engage in healthy

and un-healthy behaviours and the contradictions inherent in this body of work

Write a rough plan – procedure and

rationale for next meeting

24/10/12

12:00

Discussed the project at length and decided to change the topic to the Theory of

Planned Behaviour and exercise behaviour; studying one health behaviour thought to

be easier. Decided on mixed methods approach. Ajzen’s TPB questionnaire

combined with exercise questionnaire

Work on RAGs and questionnaire – have draft for next meeting

26/11/12

3:00

Discussed RAGs and questionnaire

development – putting into qualtrics

Finish RAGs and questionnaire over

Xmas

07/02/13

3:00

Discussed questionnaire

put de-brief sheet on questionnaire and

upload to participant pool

USPJED-40-3 10013482

52

25/02/13

3:00

Data collected. Discussed quantitative

analysis

To attempt to analyse quantitative data in

SPSS

13/03/13

3:00

Trouble analysing using SPSS – advice given on logistics of software.

Analyse statistical data

09/04/13

3:00

Qualitative analysis discussed. Advice to

start write up given.

Analyse qualitative data and begin write

up

02/05/13

3:00

Meeting about progression of write up.

Continue as was. Complete dissertation.

Section 10. Statement about the Level of Assistance Obtained Comment on or tick the relevant box about the level of assistance with each of the following: [“I had a lot of step-by-step help on this” would mean that without help you would not have been

able to analyse the data at all; “I had some specific help on this” would mean that you could have done at least some analysis without any help but you would have been unsure about aspects of your analysis; “I only had some general advice on this” would mean that you could have done the analysis without any help and that you just needed to confirm you were on the right tracks.]

I had a lot of step-by-step help on this

I had some specific help

on this

I only had some general advice on this

Comments

Design of the study

YES

Statistical or qualitative

analysis

YES

Interpretation of the results

or findings

YES

Section 11. Plagiarism Please tick the following before handing in your project (add a comment if necessary):

USPJED-40-3 10013482

53

I understand the use of SafeAssign to detect plagiarism on this module

YES

I have uploaded a draft copy of my report onto SafeAssign to check for plagiarism

YES

I have tried to avoid plagiarism to the best of my ability

YES