role of illness models

Upload: anonymous-dzttpbrp

Post on 30-May-2018

227 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    1/26

    Review article

    A review of the role of illness models in severe

    mental illness

    Fiona Lobban*, Christine Barrowclough, Steve JonesUniversity of Manchester, Manchester, UK

    Received 5 October 2001; received in revised form 19 September 2002; accepted 30 September 2002

    Abstract

    The ways in which people think about illness experiences have been associated with a variety of

    important behaviours and emotional responses in patients, carers, and professionals. Some of these

    responses have been shown to be related to outcome. Explicit models such as the self-regulation model

    (SRM) [Leventhal, H., Nerenz, D. R., & Steele, D. F. (1984). Illness representations and coping with

    health threats. In A. Baum & J. Singer (Eds.), A handbook of psychology and health. Hillsdale, NJ:

    Erlbaum, 219252.] have been shown to be useful in highlighting key beliefs across a wide range of

    different physical illnesses. The specific beliefs about mental illness that have been assessed have been

    varied and largely without a common theoretical framework. This has resulted in a literature from

    which it is difficult to draw firm conclusions. The central aim of this paper is to assess the applicability

    of the SRM to mental illness. To this end, we review studies to date that have examined the beliefs that

    people with a mental illness have about their experiences. In addition, we review studies that have

    examined the beliefs of relatives of people with a mental illness and professionals who work with thispopulation. We assess to what extent these studies are consistent with the SRM before suggesting ways

    in which the model could be further developed and tested. The SRM is presented as a useful

    framework for more advanced investigations into the function of beliefs about mental illness and how

    these can be modified in order to effect outcome. Developing psychological theories common to both

    0272-7358/03/$ see front matterD 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

    PII: S 0 2 7 2 - 7 3 5 8 ( 0 2 ) 0 0 2 3 0 - 1

    * Corresponding author. Academic Division of Clinical Psychology, School of Psychiatry and Behavioural

    Sciences, 2nd Floor Education and Research Centre, Wythenshawe Hospital, Manchester M23 9LT, UK. Tel.:

    +44-161-2915319.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (F. Lobban).

    Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    2/26

    physical and mental health may eventually result in an integrated approach in which mental illness

    becomes less stigmatised within the treatment setting.

    D 2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

    Keywords: Schizophrenia; Mental health; Illness models; Self-regulation model

    1. Introduction

    There is nothing either good or bad, but thinking makes it so. (Shakespeare, Hamlet, Act 2

    Scene 2)

    The way in which people think about events is important in determining their emotional

    and behavioural responses. This is true for a wide range of events, including health problems.

    The relationships between beliefs about illness and emotional and behavioural responses have

    been extensively explored in both physical and mental illness, but largely from different

    perspectives.

    In trying to understand variation in physical health- and illness-related behaviours,

    psychologists have focused primarily on social cognition models. These models attempt

    to identify the key cognitions that mediate between extrinsic factors that have been

    associated with health behaviours, such as demographics and social factors, and individualbehaviours. The models that have been most widely applied include the health belief model

    (Becker, 1974; Becker & Maimon, 1983; Janz & Becker, 1984), health locus of control

    (Seeman & Seeman, 1983; Wallston, Wallston, & DeVellis, 1978), protection motivation

    theory (Maddux & Rogers, 1983; van der Velde & van der Pligt, 1991), theory of reasoned

    action/planned behaviour (Ajzen, 1988, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980), self-efficacy theory

    (Bandura, 1982, 1991; Schwarzer, 1992), the transtheoretical model of change (Prochaska &

    DiClemente, 1984; Prochaska, DiClemente, & Norcross, 1992), and self-regulation theory

    (Leventhal, Nerenz, & Steele, 1984). All of these models assume that individuals are

    rational beings whose health-related behaviour depends upon their understanding of relevantinformation. They have all been widely applied to a range of physical illnesses and overall

    have been able to account for a significant amount of variance in illness-related behaviours,

    making the key cognitions in each model an important target for intervention (Connor &

    Norman, 1995).

    In trying to understand variation in health- and illness-related behaviours in severe mental

    illness, there have been very few attempts to draw on any of the developments made by the

    physical illness models. In mental illness, the study of beliefs has generally been focussed on

    peoples interpretations of internal and external experiences and how these interpretations

    contribute to the development and maintenance of psychotic symptoms. Examples of this area

    include studies of: (1) beliefs about intrusive thoughts, which may predispose them to beexperienced as auditory hallucinations (Morrison, 1998); (2) information processing bias such

    as jumping to conclusions that result in persecutory interpretations of innocuous events (Garety

    & Hemsley, 1994); and (3) tendency to make external and personal attributions for negative

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196172

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    3/26

    events (Kinderman & Bentall, 1997). There has been less exploration of other potentially

    important beliefs, highlighted in the physical health literature, such as the possible causes of theexperience, beliefs about control or treatment, perceived consequences, and how long the

    experience is likely to last for (though important exceptions to this are reviewed below).

    Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Freeman, and Bebbington (2001) have used the term secondary

    appraisals to refer to evaluations of the experience of psychosis itself (such as what caused

    it, and how an individual can control it, what are the consequences to the persons life), as

    opposed to primary appraisals of the meaning of anomalous experiences that may drive the

    core symptoms (such as people who are looking at me intend to do me harm). Some

    distinction between different types of appraisal is potentially very useful, especially when the

    individual is able to recognise that their primary appraisals are part of a mental illness. For

    example, their secondary appraisal may include beliefs such as these thoughts about peoplewanting to hurt me are caused by stress or I can control these experiences by taking some

    medication or reducing my stress. However, the distinction between primary and secondary

    appraisal becomes less clear when the individual does not recognise their experiences as

    being part of a mental illness. Their causal explanation may be that their thoughts about

    people wanting to harm them are due to the fact that people really are trying to harm them.

    The primary and secondary appraisals are not easy to distinguish in this scenario and

    therefore the model becomes less useful. In addition, the terms primary and secondary may be

    confusing as they are already used in the related literature on coping and have specific and

    somewhat different meanings in that context (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984).It is our suggestion that attempts to develop an understanding of the role of beliefs in

    mental illness would benefit from drawing on the extensive work that has been already done

    in understanding the role of beliefs in physical illness and, in particular, the self-regulation

    model (SRM) (Leventhal et al., 1984). This model is particularly useful for understanding

    severe mental illness for the following reasons.

    (1) The SRM emphasises the importance of emotional representations as well as cognitive

    representations. This may be particularly important in mental illness where dysregulation

    of emotion may be integral (Garety et al., 2001).(2) The SRM is currently the most widely used model because of its reliability and validity in

    exploring important patient beliefs across a range of physical illnesses, and the advances

    this has made in understanding self-management (Hampson, Glasgow, & Foster, 1995;

    Hampson, Glasgow, & Toobert, 1990), and recovery (Horne, James, Petrie, Weinman, &

    Vincent, 2000; Petrie, Weinman, Sharpe, & Buckley, 1996). The wide applicability of the

    model suggests it may also be appropriate for mental as well as physical illness.

    (3) Much of the work that has been done in mental illness is consistent with this model. This

    literature is reviewed below.

    (4) Preliminary studies in mental illness indicate that the SRM explains considerable variance

    in important outcome measures for both patients (Clifford, 1998) and relatives (Barrow-clough, Lobban, Hatton, & Quinn, 2001).

    (5) Finally, the SRM would have the added advantage of encouraging a wider exploration of

    beliefs about mental illness within clinical practice and research.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 173

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    4/26

    2. The SRM (Leventhal et al., 1984)

    The SRM is based on the premise that patients are active problem solvers whose health-

    related behaviours are attempts to close the perceived gap between their current health and a

    future goal state. The coping strategies they select (for example, whether or not to take

    medication) are guided by their interpretation and evaluation of their illness. The outcome of

    these behaviours is then evaluated and fed back into their model of the illness, and/or used to

    shape future coping responses. This dynamic aspect of the process is strongly emphasised in

    the model. The key beliefs identified in the SRM refer to a specific illness episode, rather than

    to intellectual beliefs about a possible illness occurrence. In applying the SRM to physical

    illness, five specific components have been identified as being key to guiding individual

    responses. These are the perceived identity of the illness (including a label and signs/

    symptoms), the perceived consequences (physical, social, and behavioural), the likely causes

    of the illness, and a likely timeline or sense of how long the illness will last for. A fifth belief

    identified by Lau and Hartman (1983) about potential for control or cure of the illness has

    also been added to the model. Validation for these beliefs is evident in the work of Kleinman

    (1980, 1988) who identified similar key beliefs when examining explanatory models of

    illness across different cultures.

    In addition to a cognitive representation of illness, Leventhal proposed an emotional

    representation, which he saw as existing in parallel. The emotional representation of illness

    has been less well developed and consequently, the relationship between these two systems isdoes not seem to have been fully elaborated (Fig. 1).

    Although originally developed around physical illness, Leventhal, Diefenbach, and

    Leventhal (1992) have briefly described how the model could be usefully applied to mental

    illness. They use the example of hypochondriasis, and argue this originates when an

    individual builds a model of illness in response to perceived bodily sensations. The

    Fig. 1. Outline of the self-regulation model (Leventhal et al., 1984).

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196174

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    5/26

    individuals model is that they have a serious illness, but the medical investigations do not

    confirm this and the medical practitioners model is that they are experiencing anxiety.Repeated reassurance does not work in the long-term because it fails to provide the individual

    with an alternative coherent model that explains why they experience the bodily sensations

    that they do. Until this is done, re-experiencing of the bodily sensations will trigger the old

    model of illness and the accompanying concern. This formulation is entirely consistent with

    specific cognitive models of hypochondriasis (Salkovskis, 1989; Warwick & Salkovskis,

    1990).

    3. Assessing beliefs about illness/health

    Since Leventhal originally proposed the SRM, there has been considerable support for

    both the specific beliefs that constitute the way in which people think about physical illness,

    and their utility in accounting for variation in outcome in a number of areas. A questionnaire

    that assesses these dimensions; the Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ) (Weinman, Petrie,

    Moss-Morris, & Horne, 1996) has been used with people with a wide range of health

    problems. Using this and similar measures, and controlling for degree of disease severity,

    associations have been demonstrated between illness perceptions and emotional adjustment

    (e.g., Murphy, Dickens, Creed, & Berstein, 1999), overall functioning levels (e.g., Scharloo,

    Kaptein, Weinman, Willems, & Rooijmans, 2000), coping and managing symptoms (e.g., deValle & Norman, 1992; Hampson, Glasgow, & Zeiss, 1994), adherence to health care

    regimens (e.g., Wichowski & Kubsch, 1997), and cognitive processing of illness-related

    information (e.g., Croyle & Ditto, 1990). The importance of relatives/carers beliefs about

    illness and/or treatment have also been investigated and shown to be associated with patient

    outcome (Heijmans, de Ridder, & Bensing, 1999), and treatment adherence by the patient

    (Heinzelman & Baguley, 1970). Relatives models are also likely to influence the patients

    own models, as evidence suggests that models can be influenced by information provided by

    others, even strangers (e.g., Croyle, 1992).

    The IPQ has recently been modified to include subscales assessing emotional repres-entation and illness coherence (a sense of having a comprehensive understanding of the

    illness). The dimensions of control/cure and timeline have also been subdivided to

    differentiate personal and treatment control, and an acute/chronic and episodic timeline

    (Moss-Morris et al., 2001). Although the IPQ has been extensively used in assessing illness

    beliefs in physical illness, it has not been used to explore beliefs about mental illness,

    except in one study described below, which found a version modified for relatives of people

    with a diagnosis of schizophrenia to be a valid and reliable tool (Barrowclough et al.,

    2001).

    Before continuing to assess the reliability of the IPQ in measuring beliefs about mental

    illness, more in-depth assessment of the validity of focusing on the beliefs highlighted in theSRM is required. This requires a more open-ended methodology, such as semistructured

    interviews, to establish whether or not the key constructs that people use about mental illness

    are similar to those used to understand a range of physical illnesses, i.e., those described by the

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 175

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    6/26

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    7/26

    4.1.1.1. Signs and symptoms. Most of the research about the role of cognitive factors in

    mental illness, has focussed on the identity component, i.e., how individuals define theirexperiences. Many studies support the idea of the role of an information processing bias,

    including an externalising bias in people with paranoid delusions (Kinderman & Bentall,

    1996), self-serving bias (Kaney & Bentall, 1989), attentional bias (Bentall & Kaney, 1989),

    and recall bias (Kaney, Wolfenden, Dewey, & Bentall, 1992). In addition, there is

    experimental evidence that people with delusional beliefs are more likely to jump to

    conclusions (Huq, Garety, & Hemsley, 1988), and that voice hearers have higher levels of

    private self-consciousness (Morrison & Haddock, 1997). These biases may explain why

    internal events such as thoughts may be experienced as voices, or why harmless events

    such as someone talking on a phone, may be experienced as a conspiracy to harm. Garety

    et al. (2001) and Morrison (2001) attempt an integration of these processes into coherent

    models.

    The interpretations that are made about experiences have been clearly linked to beha-

    vioural responses. Chadwick and Birchwood (1994), extending Becks cognitive model for

    emotional disorders, proposed that beliefs about voices would be more important in

    predicting emotional and behavioural responses, than their content per se. The beliefs that

    they focused on were the omnipotence of the voice (how powerful it was) and whether it was

    perceived as malevolent (wishing to do evil) or benevolent (wishing to do good). They found

    that the individual actively engaged with voices perceived as benevolent, whereas those

    perceived as malevolent were resisted. Beliefs about voices, rather than the content predictedresponse. More recent work in this area has highlighted additional beliefs that may be

    important including the control that the individual feels they have over the voice. These

    studies are discussed below.

    Understanding individual beliefs about identity of voices may also be important in

    preventing future harm. For example, Junginger (1990) found that people who experienced

    command hallucinations were more likely to act on these if they recognised the identity of the

    voice as someone familiar to them, and if they held delusions related to their voices. Acting

    on commands was not associated with the dangerousness of the command.

    Label. How the individual then summarises and labels these experiences may also havean important impact on their responses. In particular, whether or not the individual labelstheir experience as being disease-related. Within mental illness, how the individual labels

    their experiences has been closely linked to insight. Traditionally, insight has been seen as

    on all or nothing phenomenon and patient insight would be assessed by clinical

    descriptions of patient free responses (Amador, Strauss, Yale, & Gorman, 1991). People

    who show insight are more likely to be compliant with medication (Bartko, Herczeg, &

    Zador, 1988; Nageotte, Sullivan, Duan, & Camp, 1997), and have a better treatment

    outcome (McEvoy et al., 1989; McEvoy, Howe, & Hogarty, 1984). However, more

    recently, there has been recognition of the construct of insight as complex, multidimen-

    sional, and fluid over time (Amador & David, 1998). The need to understand the way inwhich individual appraises his or her own experiences, and the function of this appraisal,

    has been recognised. Moreover, the assumption that understanding psychotic symptoms

    within a strict medical model is beneficial to the patient has been challenged. White,

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 177

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    8/26

    Bebbington, Pearson, Johnson, and Ellis (2000) found a strong positive association between

    insight as measured by adherence to a medical model, and depression. Jackson et al.(1998) found that people who integrated their experiences more fully, accepting that they

    had experienced a psychotic episode, actually showed higher levels of depression, than

    those who adopted a more sealing over approach. They argue that this may reflect the

    individual becoming engulfed by the demoralization and stigma that they associate with

    mental illness.

    The link between medication adherence and insight is also more complex than initially

    suggested. Overall, there is a positive association between traditionally measured insight, and

    adherence to medication (Bartko et al., 1988). However, there are many individuals who do

    not accept the assertion that they have a mental illness, and yet are adherent to treatment. This

    suggests that the relationship between insight and adherence is influenced by other factors. Arecent review of nonadherence in affective disorders highlights the importance of attitudes

    and beliefs a bout medication, and argues for more research to identify specific targets for

    intervention (Scott & Pope, 2002).

    Finally, the way in which individuals label their experiences has been associated with

    perceived quality of life. Mechanic, McAlpine, Rosenfield, and Davis (1994) found that

    people who attributed their mental illness to a physical/medical/biological problem as

    opposed to a psychological problems, scored higher on a perceived quality of life measure,

    and reported less personal stigma and greater self-esteem. However, the results of this study

    need to be interpreted in light of the fact that the participants were recruited via membershipof an organisation that promotes a disease model of schizophrenia.

    These studies suggest that the way in which an individual identifies their experiences is

    associated with their emotional response, and important behaviours such as taking medica-

    tion. However, to fully understand these responses, the pattern of beliefs across a number of

    dimensions is important. The signs/symptoms described or the label used is not sufficient on

    its own.

    4.1.2. Cause

    This dimension of the model assesses what the individual thinks has caused their illness.This dimension has mainly been explored in relation to depressed mood. It is likely that

    beliefs about cause are important in many areas, especially adherence to treatment, but no

    studies were identified that explicitly assessed this.

    Postpsychotic Depressive Disorder of Schizophrenia was listed in the Diagnostic and

    Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders4th ed. (DSM IV; American Psychiatric Association,

    1994) as a possible diagnostic category requiring further research. Roughly one third of

    patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia, also have full depressive syndromes, and over a 7-

    year period, nearly half will experience significant depression (Sands & Harrow, 1999). A

    large number of factors have been suggested to account for why some individuals develop

    depression, including genetic factors or family history (e.g., Galdi, 1983; Galdi, Reider,Silber, & Bonato, 1981), the use of neuroleptics (e.g., Van Putten & May, 1978), personality

    (e.g., Liddle, Barnes, Curson, & Patel, 1993), and substance abuse (e.g., Bartels & Drake,

    1989). More recently, the suggestion that post psychotic depression is associated with the

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196178

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    9/26

    individuals secondary appraisal of their experiences has been made. Birchwood, Iqbal,

    Chadwick, and Trower (2000a, 2000b) used the PBIQ and followed up 115 patients over a12-month period following an acute psychotic episode. They found that over a third of these

    people became depressed following the episode and this was not directly related to the

    psychotic symptoms they had experienced. Depression, however, was related to beliefs about

    the illness. In addition to beliefs about the consequences of the illness (see Section 4.1.3),

    postpsychotic depression was also associated with attribution of the cause of psychosis to

    factors internal to the self. This suggests that for some people depression may have been a

    psychological reaction to the experience of having a psychotic episode, rather than a

    symptom of the illness per se.

    4.1.3. Consequences

    This dimension assesses what the individual believes will be the consequences of having

    the health problem on their lives. The perceived consequences of the individuals experience

    have been shown to be associated with variations in coping (Kinney, 1999; Tarrier, Harwood,

    Yusopoff, Beckett, & Baker, 1990), depression (Birchwood et al., 1993, 2000a, 2000b), and

    medication adherence (Adams & Scott, 2000; Budd, Hughes, & Smith, 1996).

    Patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia are able to identify strategies that they use to

    cope with their symptoms (Fallon & Talbot, 1981). The beliefs that they have about the likely

    consequences of symptoms appear to mediate this relationship. Kinney (1999) showed that

    patients who perceived their symptoms as more taxing and burdensome were more likely toreport effortful attempts to cope. A cognitive behavioural style intervention known as Coping

    Strategy Enhancement (CSE) developed by Tarrier et al. (1990) includes an assessment of the

    individuals appraisal of their experiences, and uses belief modification strategies to aid the

    development of existing and new coping skills, where necessary. This intervention has shown

    to be effective at reducing positive symptoms.

    Some authors have suggested that beliefs about the consequences of symptoms can lead to

    adoption of behaviours that although intended by the individual to prevent harm, in fact

    exacerbate the problem. Morrison (1998) proposed that the experience of psychotic

    symptoms regularly results in the use of safety behaviours such as distraction or avoidance.Some support for this theory comes from a study by Freeman, Garety, and Kuipers (2001) in

    which they found that higher levels of anxiety were associated with greater use of safety

    behaviours. Morrison argues that strategies to reduce the use of safety behaviours, such as

    challenging negative beliefs about the consequences of symptoms, can be used to help people

    to drop safety behaviours and test out their beliefs associated with feared consequences of the

    symptoms.

    Birchwood et al. (1993) found a significant association between the perception of

    negative social consequences of symptoms, including stigmatisation and social contain-

    ment, and depression in people with a diagnosis of depression. More recently, Birchwood

    et al. (2000a, 2000b), in the study described previously, found that individuals whoappraised their illness as constituting a loss of autonomy and valued role, as humiliating,

    and who felt entrapped by their illness, were more likely to become depressed than those

    who did not.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 179

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    10/26

    Finally, perceived consequences have been associated with medication adherence. Budd et

    al. (1996), using the health belief model as a basis to investigate adherence, found that perceivedseverity of symptoms was associated with greater medication adherence. In addition, percep-

    tions of susceptibility and beliefs about the benefits of the medication were also important.

    Adams and Scott (2000), also using the health belief model, found perceptions of severity of

    symptoms and perceived benefits from mediation to be important in medication adherence.

    4.1.4. Timeline: acute/chronic

    No studies were identified that explored patients beliefs about how long their experiences

    were likely to last and the impact of these beliefs. This is surprising in light of the very

    variable outcome for people who experience a psychotic episode (e.g., Mason et al., 1995).

    One reason for a lack of research in this area may be that the timeline for any individual isvery hard to predict, therefore, challenging beliefs associated with distress would be very

    difficult.

    4.1.5. Control

    This dimension of the SRM assesses beliefs about how the problems can be controlled. A

    useful distinction has been made recently between beliefs about control by treatments, and

    beliefs about personal control (Moss-Morris et al., 2001).

    4.1.5.1. Personal control. Appraisals of controllability of symptoms by the individual havebeen investigated and shown to be associated with a number of important outcomes.

    One of the first studies to investigate peoples beliefs about their symptoms in severe

    mental health was undertaken by Romme, Honig, Noorthoorn, and Escher (1992) and

    focussed on how voice hearers coped with their experiences. Following a TV program about

    hearing voices, they contacted people who telephoned in to the programme, and sent them a

    questionnaire about their voices. They found that over half of the sample felt that they were

    unable to cope with their voices. This subgroup was more likely to be in psychiatric care and

    was more likely to use distraction as their main strategy. Those who did feel able to cope were

    more likely to use strategies that reflect the belief that they could control their voicesincluding setting limits on the voices. More recently, Hoffman, Kupper, and Kunz (2000) in a

    prospective study found that negative self-concepts, external loci of control, and depression

    correlated to a greater extent with depressive-resigned coping strategies such as self-pity,

    resignation and brooding, than did schizophrenic symptoms. Controlling for negative

    symptoms, external control beliefs still had a predictive value for the outcome.

    Beliefs about the ability to control psychotic experiences have also been shown to be

    directly associated with level of distress. Freeman and Garety (1999) identified factors

    implicated in the meta-cognitive model of anxiety as also present in a group of people

    experiencing persecutory delusions. The level of distress reported by individuals in relation to

    a belief was not only associated with its content, but also with beliefs about not being able tocontrol their thinking about the feared persecution. Similarly, it has been suggested by

    Morrison, Wells, and Nothard (2000) that hallucinations become distressing only when

    appraised as uncontrollable and dangerous.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196180

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    11/26

    Finally, expanding the work discussed previously by Beck-Saunder, Birchwood, and

    Chadwick (1997) and Chadwick and Birchwood (1994) found that in addition to beliefs aboutmalevolence/benevolence of the voice, beliefs about ability to control the voice were

    significantly associated with compliance with command hallucinations. People who experi-

    enced these but who believed that they retained subjective control over their voices were less

    likely to comply with all types of demands.

    Appraisals of controllability of symptoms have been shown to be associated with type of

    coping strategies used (Romme et al., 1992) hopelessness (Hoffman et al., 2000), distress

    (Freeman & Garety, 1999), and compliance with auditory hallucinations (Beck-Sander et al.,

    1997). These findings are particularly important as perceived controllability has been shown

    to be amenable to treatment using CBT interventions (Chadwick, Sambrooke, Rasch, &

    Davies, 2000; Drury, Birchwood, & Cochrane, 2000).

    4.1.5.2. Treatment. Beliefs about the potential for various treatments to control unwanted

    symptoms have been associated with treatment adherence. This is extremely important as

    nonadherence in mental illness is an extensive problem ranging from 11% to 80% [see

    review by Corrigan, Lieberman, & Engel, 1990, and a review by Fenton, Blyler, &

    Heinssen, 1997 suggests a median 1-month to 2-year nonadherence rate of 55% (range

    24%88%)]. Nonadherence is strongly associated with admission to hospital (Green,

    1988), relapse (Linn, Klett, & Caffey, 1982), increased length of admission (Caton, Koh,

    Fleiss, Barrow, & Goldstein, 1985; McEvoy et al., 1984), and trouble with the police(Smith, 1989).

    In two studies which used a different social cognition model, the health belief model

    (Becker, 1974), to investigate medication adherence, perceived benefits of medication came

    out as a significant predictor of adherence to medication (Adams & Scott, 2000; Budd et al.,

    1996). Nonadherence to psychological treatment, usually assessed by drop out, is also

    associated with beliefs about the ability of the treatment to control the symptoms. Tarrier,

    Yusupoff, McCarthy, Kinney, and Wittkowski, (1998) questioned people who had dropped

    out of cognitive behavioural treatment: the most common reason given was that the patients

    did not perceive the treatment as suitable for their problems.

    4.1.6. Coherence

    This dimension refers to a sense of having a comprehensive understanding of the illness.

    This does not seem to have been explicitly investigated in people with a mental illness. A

    related concept, however, is that described by McGlashan, Levy, and Carpenter (1975) as a

    continuum of recovery styles ranging from full integration of the experience into their sense

    of self, to sealing over in which the psychotic experience is spilt off from the sense of self

    and high levels of denial regarding the severity of the experience are evident. Integrators

    tend to be curious about their symptoms, regard them as part of their life pattern, and gain

    information about them. In contrast, people who seal over tend to isolate the psychoticexperience and view it as alien to themselves. Contrary to predictions, there is some

    evidence that at least for people experiencing first episode of psychosis, integration may not

    be the best style. Integration has been associated with an increase in depression in

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 181

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    12/26

    individuals undergoing psychological intervention for early psychosis (Jackson et al.,

    1998).The acceptance of the role of an individual with psychosis is likely to be associated with

    stigma, negative stereotypes, and lack of hope about returning to premorbid levels of

    functioning (McGorry & McConville, 1999).

    4.1.7. Other dimensions

    Two important areas of work do not initially seem to fit easily within any one of the SRM

    dimensions. Birchwood et al. (2000a, 2000b) attempted to understand the impact of perceived

    social rank on voice hearing. The second is beliefs about treatment, which have been shown

    to be a necessary addition to understand adherence responses (Horne & Weinman, 1997;

    Horne, 1998). These are not beliefs about whether or not the illness is amenable to treatment(which is included in the SRM), but beliefs about specific treatments such as their perceived

    advantages and disadvantages.

    Birchwood et al. (2000a, 2000b) investigated the use of ranking theory in understanding

    the relationship that people have with their voices, and how this may impact on outcome.

    This work builds on the theory that how an individual views himself or herself in relation

    to significant others in their lives can have a determining role in their emotional state

    (Gilbert, 1992). Using a range of questionnaire measures to assess social rank and power of

    the dominant voice and significant others in their lives, the authors found that the perceived

    difference in rank between the voice and voice hearer is mirrored in their perceptions ofsocial rank differences between self and others. Greater perceived differential was associated

    with depression in patients. Birchwood et al. argue that interpersonal schemata are

    responsible for both the experience of auditory hallucinations as dominant, and the

    depression.

    The attempt to understand peoples beliefs within a social context is an important step

    forward in our view, and is entirely consistent with the SRM, which places a clear

    emphasis on the importance of the sociocultural context (Leventhal et al., 1997). However,

    the concepts of social rank and power may benefit from being more clearly defined. Both

    concepts seem to incorporate issues of identity, control, and consequences. Exploring theseconcepts within the SRM may actually help define more clearly the beliefs behind these

    concepts.

    The SRM focuses on beliefs about health/illness. Research into adherence with

    medication has highlighted the need to investigate beliefs about treatment as well. Day,

    Bentall, and Warner (1996) found that patients reported experiences of neuroleptic

    medication reflected a complex interrelationship between costs and benefits of taking

    medication, interactions with the prescribers, and issues of autonomy. Horne and Wein-

    man (1999) have suggested that this may be generally true for a range of treatments and

    have begun to investigate beliefs about treatment as an addition to beliefs about illness in

    a range of physical health problems. In particular, beliefs about the necessity ofmedication and concerns about the potential adverse effects seem to be important and

    need to be addressed in any intervention focused on increasing adherence (Horne &

    Weinman, 1995).

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196182

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    13/26

    4.1.7.1. Using the SRM to investigate beliefs about mental illness. Two attempts to

    investigate beliefs outlined in the SRM in relation to schizophrenia have both used theIPQ. Clifford (1998) administered the IPQ to a sample of 38 psychotic patients. The subscales

    showed acceptable levels of internal reliability (as .60 .92). There was a positive

    association between nonadherence to medication and a perception of fewer and less severe

    symptoms, a shorter duration of illness, external attribution of cause, and more severe

    negative consequences.

    The IPQ was also used with people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia by Talley (1999).

    She found that only the subscales measuring consequences and symptoms were internally

    reliable. The consequences subscale also showed some concurrent validity in correlating with

    other measures of the impact of the illness.

    These studies suggests that the SRM is a potentially useful model for understanding mentalillness, but that the IPQ may require some modification before it can be reliably used with this

    population.

    Summary. All of the above research supports the assertion that models of mental illness

    held by individuals are an important area for investigation if we are to understand variation in

    outcome. There is a clear need to focus more on each individuals appraisal of their

    experiences, and to develop more sophisticated ways to investigate these. To quote Rogers

    et al. (1998, p. 1322), future practice and research might fruitfully examine the way in which

    placing value on patients own definitions of their problems, experience and management

    strategies presents an opportunity for rethinking the nature of professional/patient encountersabout medication and mental health policy more generally. In order to do this a framework

    for examining patients own definitions of their problems, experiences and management

    strategies is needed.

    4.2. Beliefs held by relatives/carers

    Cognitive representations of mental illness held by relatives/carers are likely to be

    important for a number of reasons. Evidence reviewed below suggests that models can have

    an impact on the emotional response of the relative and on the way in which relatives respondto the person experiencing the mental illness, which in turn may impact on their progress. In

    addition, because beliefs are likely to be influenced by cultural and social norms (Leventhal et

    al., 1997), relatives beliefs are likely to play some role in shaping the representation held by

    the person experiencing the mental illness, especially give the often long duration of

    untreated illness (Beiser et al., 1993; Larsen, McGalshan, & Moe, 1996; Loebel et al.,

    1992). There is also some evidence from the physical health literature to suggest that

    relatives models can have an impact on the adaptive outcome for patients (Heijmans et al.,

    1999; Heinzelman & Baguley, 1970).

    The importance of relatives beliefs about mental illness in designing treatment

    interventions to help relatives coping responses has been strongly emphasized in the workof Tarrier, Barrowclough, and colleagues (Barrowclough et al., 1987; Tarrier, 1991; Tarrier

    & Barrowclough, 1986). Their development of interventions for families with an individual

    with schizophrenia have been guided by evidence suggesting that providing information

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 183

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    14/26

    alone can have very little impact (Berkowitz, Eberlein-Fries, Kuipers, & Leff, 1984;

    McGill, Fallon, Boyd, & Wood-Swerio, 1983). They argue that this is because relativesalready have models that will be used to actively process additional information that is

    provided, leading to rejection and accommodation of incongruent facts. Therefore,

    successful intervention requires the therapist to . . .assess and take into account both

    patients and relatives perceptions of the illness prior to embarking on the task of giving

    information (Tarrier & Barrowclough 1986, p. 462, lines 79). This allows the amount,

    type, and speed of presentation of information to be tailored to the individual. Failu re to do

    this is likely to be one reason for high nonadherence to psychological interventions (Tarrier,

    1991).

    Most of the work in this area has been focused on the attributions that relatives make about

    the symptoms and problems of the person experiencing a mental illness. Weiners theory ofattribution (Weiner, 1985a, 1985b, 1988, 1990) suggests that our attributional appraisals play

    a key role in our emotional and behavioural reactions to another individuals behaviour.

    Specific causal dimensions have been linked to discrete affects. In particular, Weiner has

    suggested that anger is linked to beliefs that the individual can control their behaviour, pity to

    beliefs that the individual is unable to control their behaviour, and guilt to beliefs about

    personal responsibility. Weiner has also suggested that causal attributions are associated with

    behavioural responses. In experimental situations the belief that a person is drunk is

    associated with attributions of personal control to the person and neglect behaviour on the

    part of the observer, whereas beliefs that an individual has a disability/illness is associatedwith perceived lack of personal control to the person and helping behaviour on the part of the

    observer. This theory has been developed and used to explore how relatives attributions

    about the patients behaviour can impact on their responses. Results of these studies are

    reviewed below. Some work exploring the impact of the social consequences perceived by the

    relative is also described in a study that explicitly used the SRM (Barrowclough et al., 2001).

    We were unable to identify studies in which beliefs about the identity, or a sense of coherence,

    were explored in relation to outcome for the patient or the relative.

    The main outcomes that have been assessed for relatives include distress, expressed

    emotion (EE), subjective level of burden, and relatives coping strategies.EE is a measure of the emotional climate within the home environment. It is assessed using a

    structured interview with a relative at the time of patient hospitalisation. The taped interview

    can be reliably rated to classify relatives as high or low EE on the basis of criticism, hostility, or

    emotional over-involvement. High EE has been reliably associated with higher rates of relapse

    in schizophrenia (see Butzlaff & Hooley, 1998 for a meta-analytical review), as well as being

    associated with patient outcome in numerous other mental and physical health problems (see

    Wearden, Tarrier, Barrowclough, Zastowny, & Rahill, 2000 for a review of EE in health care

    research). Recent research and theory about the attributions of relatives of people with a mental

    health problem (Hooley, 1987) and relatives of patients with schizophrenia (Barrowclough,

    Johnston, & Tarrier, 1994; Brewin, 1994; Brewin, MacCarthy, Duda, & Vaughn, 1991;Greenley, 1986; Hooley, 1985; Lopez, Nelson, Snyder, & Mintz, 1999; Weisman, Lopez,

    Karno, & Jenkins, 1993) suggests that relatives beliefs about the nature and symptomatology

    of the mental illness may help to understand the high EE response in some relatives.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196184

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    15/26

    4.2.1. Cause

    Attributions of causality for symptoms have also been shown to be associated with levelsof relatives distress (Barrowclough, Tarrier, & Johnston, 1996; Heinrichsen & Lieberman,

    1999), emotional response to the patient (Barrowclough et al., 1994, 1996), and relatives

    coping (Chesla, 1989; Rose, 1983).

    In general, there is some preliminary evidence to suggest that relatives are more likely to

    attribute the cause of schizophrenia to biological factors than psychosocial factors, when

    compared to a general population sample (Angermeyer & Matschinger, 1996). This study was

    done on a German sample of people in a self-help organization, and the authors suggested that

    it could be due to increased exposure to a medical model via contact with psychiatric services,

    or an attempt to reduce feelings of guilt that may be associated with a psych osocial model.

    Self-blaming attributions are generally accompanied by high levels of distress (Barrowcloughet al., 1996). In addition, Heinrichsen and Lieberman (1999) found that relatives who

    attributed symptoms to the patients moral failings or psychological problems from earlier life

    were more likely to report poor emotional adjustment.

    Attributions of cause that are internal to the patient are generally found in relatives who are

    hostile in their response, as rated on the CFI, whereas relatives who tended to attribute all

    behaviours to illness were rated as emotionally overinvolved (Barrowclough et al., 1996).

    Support for the importance of the role of carer beliefs is increased by the finding from one

    study that such beliefs are better predictors of outcome than EE ratings (Barrowclough et al.,

    1994).Chesla (1989) and Rose (1983) have shown an association between attributions of cause of

    mental illness and the type of coping strategies used by relatives. Relatives who saw mental

    illness as being caused by a chemical imbalance strongly reinforced the use of medication,

    whereas those who saw irrational thoughts as the cause tried to reason with the patient to

    think differently and avoided reinforcing any delusional ideas.

    4.2.2. Consequences

    When asking relatives about the consequences of the mental illness, it is important to

    recognize the distinction between consequences for the relative and consequences for theperson experiencing the mental health problem. Barrowclough et al. (2001) used a modified

    version of the IPQ to assess both of the dimensions. As would be expected, relatives who

    perceived greater consequences for themselves also scored higher on measures of distress and

    sense of burden. In addition, the relatives perception of consequences for the patient was

    associated with the patients perception of the their relationship with their relative. The more

    consequences of the illness that the relative perceived for the patient, the more the patient both

    expressed negative feelings towards the relative and perceived negative feelings from them.

    4.2.3. Timeline: acute/chronic

    In the study referred to above, Barrowclough et al. (2001), an association was also foundbetween the relatives perception of how long the symptoms would last for and their level of

    EE. Relatives who had a more chronic timeline perception of the illness were more likely to

    be rated as lower EE.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 185

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    16/26

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    17/26

    4.3.1. Identity

    No studies were identified that measured the relationship between professionals beliefsabout the label and/or symptoms of mental illness, and their responses. However, it has been

    hypothesized that belief in the validity of the concept of schizophrenia may be one factor

    associated with whether or not professionals choose to work with this population. Bellack

    (1986) argued that the reason why so few clinical psychologists in the USA were working

    with people with a diagnosis of schizophrenia was that they held certain beliefs about the

    disorder, including not believing in the validity of the concept. Beliefs about schizophrenia

    held by psychologists working in Ireland and the UK partially support this hypothesis, with

    33% disagreeing with the validity of the concept (Gallagher, Gernez, & Baker, 1991).

    However, the hypothesized association between such beliefs and choosing to work with the

    population of people who receive such a diagnosis has not been demonstrated and there are

    examples of extensive work done in this area by people who question the validity of the

    concept (e.g., Bentall, 1990; Bentall, Jackson, & Pilgrim, 1988).

    4.3.2. Cause

    Beliefs about the cause of mental illness have been associated with treatment plans. Cape,

    Antebi, Standen, and Glazebrook (1994) found a strong correlation between belief in a causal

    role for psychosocial factors and use of nonmedical treatments, such as psychotherapy,

    among psychiatrists. Psychiatrists who felt that psychosocial factors were important in the

    aetiology of schizophrenia were more likely to rate social support as important prognosticallyand were less likely to see the patient as needing medication for the rest of his/her life.

    4.3.3. Control

    4.3.3.1. Personal. Beliefs that professionals have about the amount of control that an

    individual has over their mental illness have been associated with the patients perception of

    the quality of the therapeutic relationship. Barrowclough et al. (2001) assessed feelings and

    thoughts of both staff and patients on a low secure inpatient facility for people with severe

    mental illness. They found that patients were very sensitive to staff feelings for them, andthere was an association between less positive feelings reported by the staff and the staff

    attributing more control to the patients over their symptom-related behaviour. This finding is

    particularly important given that Weiners theory would predict that this attribution of

    controllability would result in less helping behaviour by the staff (Weiner, 1985a, 1985b,

    1988).

    4.3.3.2. Treatment. Beliefs about how well treatment can control symptoms have been

    explored but have not been directly linked to any important outcome variables.

    Jorm et al. (1997) found that both Australian GPs and psychiatrists believed that patients

    with schizophrenia or depression had a better prognosis if they were to receive professionalhelp. In a large survey of Australian mental health professionals (Caldwell & Jorm, 2000),

    nurses were significantly more positive than psychiatrists about many interventions including

    seeing a naturopath or herbalist, taking vitamins or sedatives, courses on relaxation,

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 187

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    18/26

    meditation or yoga, seeing a counseller, stress management, close friends, physical activity,

    getting out more, hypnosis, diet, etc. They were generally more negative than psychiatristsabout a visiting a GP, admission to a psychiatric ward, and ECT.

    Gallagher et al. (1991) found that the majority of psychologists they surveyed in Ireland

    and the UK believed that schizophrenia was not adequately treated with medication, and

    strongly disagreed with the view that schizophrenia is too severe a condition for psychol-

    ogists to work with. This suggests that negative beliefs about the potential effectiveness of

    psychological interventions are not dominant and therefore unlikely to reduce the likelihood

    of psychologists working with this population.

    How these beliefs about the potential effectiveness of treatments impact on actual practice

    has not been tested. However, this is an important area for future research due to the likely

    impact of such beliefs on the treatments that patients are offered.Using the SRM to investigate beliefs about mental illness in professionals. To our

    knowledge, the SRM has not been explicitly used to assess how beliefs held by mental health

    professionals impact on their responses to their patients. However, some important lessons

    can be learned from studies using the SRM to assess beliefs held by professionals working

    with physical illness. This work has suggested that what may be more important than either

    the patients beliefs about their mental illness, or those of the professional, is the amount of

    agreement between the two models. There is considerable evidence to suggest that there is

    often a poor fit between patient and practitioner models and, more importantly, that this is

    often not addressed or even assessed. Helman (1985) studied people with chronic physicalproblems but for whom there was a recognised psychosomatic component. They found that

    there was substantial variation in agreement levels between the doctors and patients models.

    In general, doctors had a very poor understanding of patients models and this was worse for

    patients in lower socioeconomic and educational groups. They suggest that the accuracy of

    the doctors understanding of the patient model will predict future adherence.

    4.3.3.3. Summary. Some studies cited above have suggested that beliefs about cause,

    control, and likely response to treatment may vary within and across professional groups, and

    that these beliefs may impact on the treatment that patients receive. However, studies that areable to assess the direct impact of beliefs on practice, rather than relying on self-report would

    greatly increase the validity of findings in this area. Such studies would benefit from a clear

    theoretical model to guide exploration of which beliefs may be key. Important beliefs within

    the SRM that have not been explored in this area include beliefs about identity, timeline, and

    consequences. In addition, exploration of the importance of similarity of models between

    client and practitioner in mental health is clearly warranted.

    5. Limitations of illness models

    It is hypothesised that models of illness that have been found to be helpful in physical

    illness may be usefully applied to mental illness to further our understanding of peoples

    responses to illness. It is likely that modifications will be necessary and that different

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196188

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    19/26

    dimensions of understanding to those identified in physical illness may be critical. However,

    the work on illness perception in physical illness provides us with a methodology to developand test a framework that is needed to further work in this area.

    One of the main criticisms of the models of illness literature has been a lack of testing of

    proposed causal links between beliefs and outcomes, most of the evidence being based on

    associations. Early attempts to directly impact on beliefs and demonstrate a clear link between

    change in belief and change in outcome are beginning to be reported (e.g., Yardley, Beech, &

    Weinman, 2001). Cognitive behaviour therapy, more commonly discussed in the clinical

    psychology literature, could be more extensively used to modify beliefs and demonstrate

    directly the causal role of beliefs in outcome.

    Other shortcomings in the methodology of models of illness need to be acknowledged and

    overcome. Firstly, although Leventhals original model highlights beliefs about identity,cause, consequences, timeline, and cure/control, many studies have explored single beliefs in

    relation to outcome. The pattern of beliefs across the dimensions are likely to have a very

    different impact from single beliefs in isolation; the examination of isolated variables may

    prove valuable in particular situations, but will do little to advance understanding or aid in the

    development of a communicable body of knowledge (Leventhal & Cameron, 1987, p. 134).

    Again, notable exceptions to this criticism can be found in studies that have attempted to

    explore patterns of beliefs (Buick, 1997). Secondly, most studies have used peoples self-

    report of what they think or what they say they would do. There is generally very little

    correlation between this and what they actually do (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980) so more validassessments are required.

    Other limitations are more difficult to rectify. Firstly, models such as the SRM are dynamic

    models in which the variables all impact on one another. Beliefs that influence responses are

    themselves modified in light of the appraisal of the outcome. This can make the identification

    of dependent and independent variables in research very difficult, and models need to be

    assessed over time as they are constantly evolving. Secondly, it could be argued that

    investigating illness models in people with a diagnosis of a mental illness is less relevant

    that in those with a physical illness because the nature of the problem may interfere with their

    ability to reflect on their experiences and form meaningful models. However, this argumentimplies a distinction between physical and mental experiences that is not supported by

    evidence. Problems identified as physical in their diagnosis, such as diabetes, can have a

    significant impact on an individuals cognitive ability and their psychological functioning

    (see Frier, 2001). Psychological factors also play a large part in outcome via their impact on

    help seeking behaviour (see Kreitler, 1999). Similarly, patients diagnosed with a problems

    considered by many to be psychological, experience very real physical symptoms, as in the

    case of fibromyalgia, chronic fatigue syndrome, irritable bowel syndrome, and atypical facial

    pain (see Cameron, 2001 for further discussion of this issue). Our own clinical experience

    with people with a mental illness suggests that they clearly do form cognitive representations

    about their experiences, even if these are at odds with the representations held by their keyworker or psychiatrist.

    Finally, it is important that where illness models are used to increase our understanding of

    peoples responses to mental illness, it is recognised that they are theoretical models that are

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 189

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    20/26

    tools to help testing of hypotheses and to guide us to potentially important areas for

    intervention. Models are inevitably limited and cannot replace individual formulations,which provide a far more complex and useful guide to the individuals understanding of

    their illness and the meaning it has for them.

    References

    Adams, J., & Scott, J. (2000). Predicting medication adherence in severe mental disorders. Acta Psychiatrica

    Scandinavica, 101, 119124.

    Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality and behaviour. Milton Keynes: Open Univ. Press.

    Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behaviour. Organisational Behaviour and Human Decision Processes, 50,179211.

    Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and predicting social behaviour. Englewood Cliffs, NJ:

    Prentice Hall.

    Amador, X. F., & David, A. S. (Eds.) (1998). Insight and psychosis (pp. 1384). New York, NY: Oxford Univ.

    Press.

    Amador, X. F., Strauss, D., Yale, S. A., & Gorman, J. M. (1991). Awareness of illness in schizophrenia.

    Schizophrenia Bulletin, 17, 132133.

    American Psychiatric Association (1994). DSM-IV; Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

    (4th ed.). Washington, DC: The Association.

    Angermeyer, M. C., & Matschinger, H. (1996). Lay beliefs about mental disorders: a comparison between western

    and the eastern parts of Germany. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemiology, 34(5), 275281.

    Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American Psychologist, 37, 122147.

    Bandura, A. (1991). Self-efficacy mechanism in physiological activation and health promoting behaviour. In J.

    Madden (Ed.), Neurobiology of learning, emotion and affect (pp. 229270). New York: Raven Press.

    Barrowclough, C., Johnston, M., & Tarrier, N. (1994). Attributions, expressed emotion, and patient relapse: an

    attributional model of relatives response to schizophrenic illness. Behaviour Therapy, 25, 67 88.

    Barrowclough, C., Lobban, F., Hatton, C., & Quinn, J. (2001). Investigation of models of illness in carers of

    schizophrenia patients using the Illness Perception Questionnaire. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 40,

    371385.

    Barrowclough, C., Tarrier, N., & Johnston, M. (1996). Distress, expressed emotion, and attributions in relatives of

    schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22(4), 691701.

    Barrowclough, C., Tarrier, N., Watts, S., Vaughn, C., Bamrah, J. S., & Freeman, L. (1987). Assessing the func-

    tional value of relatives knowledge about schizophrenia: a preliminary report. British Journal of Psychiatry,

    151, 1 8.

    Bartels, S. J., & Drake, R. E. (1989). Depression in schizophrenia: current guidelines to treatment. Psychiatric

    Quarterly, 60(4), 337357.

    Bartko, G., Herczeg, I., & Zador, G. (1988). Clinical symptomatology and drug compliance in schizophrenia

    patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 77, 74 76.

    Becker, M. H. (1974). The health belief model and sick role behaviour.Health Education Monographs, 2, 409 419.

    Becker, M. H., & Maimon, L. A. (1983). Models of health-related behaviour. In D. Mechanic (Ed.), Handbook of

    health, health care and the health professions (pp. 539568). New York: Free Press.

    Beck-Sander, A., Birchwood, M., & Chadwick, M. (1997). Acting on command hallucinations: a cognitive

    approach. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36, 139148.

    Beiser, M., Erickson, D., Fleming, J. A. E., et al. (1993). Establishing the onset of psychotic illness. American

    Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 13491354.

    Bellack, A. S. (1986). Schizophrenia; behaviour therapys forgotten child. Behaviour Therapy, 17, 199 214.

    Bentall, R. P. (1990). Reconstructing schizophrenia. London: Routledge.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196190

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    21/26

    Bentall, R. P., Jackson, H. F., & Pilgrim, D. (1988). Abandoning the concept of schizophrenia: some implications

    of validity arguments for psychological research into psychotic phenomena. British Journal of Clinical Psy-

    chology, 27, 303 324.

    Bentall, R. P., & Kaney, S. (1989). Content specific information processing in persecutory delusions: an inves-

    tigation using the emotional stroop test. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 62, 355364.

    Berkowitz, R., Eberlein-Fries, R., Kuipers, L., & Leff, J. (1984). Educating relatives about schizophrenia. Schiz-

    ophrenia Bulletin, 7, 418429.

    Birchwood, M., Iqbal, Z., Chadwick, P., & Trower, P. (2000a). Cognitive approach to depression and suicidal

    thinking in psychosis: I. Ontogeny of post-psychotic depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 516521.

    Birchwood, M., Iqbal, Z., Chadwick, P., & Trower, P. (2000b). Cognitive Approach to depression and suicidal

    thinking in psychosis: II. Testing the validity of a social ranking model. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177,

    522528.

    Birchwood, M., Mason, R., MacMillan, F., et al. (1993). Depression, demoralisation and control over psychotic

    illness: a comparison of depressed and non-depressed patients with a chronic psychosis. Psychological Med-icine, 23, 387395.

    Brewin, C. R. (1994). Changes in attribution and expressed emotion among the relatives of patients with schiz-

    ophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 24, 905 911.

    Brewin, C. R., MacCarthy, B., Duda, K., & Vaughn, C. E. (1991). Attribution and expressed emotion in the

    relatives of patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 546554.

    Budd, R. J., Hughes, I. C. T., & Smith, J. A. (1996). Health beliefs and compliance with antipsychotic medication.

    British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 35, 393 397.

    Buick, D. L. (1997). Illness representations and breast cancer; coping with radiation and chemotherapy. In Petrie, &

    Weinman (Eds.), Perceptions of health and illness (pp. 379411). Harwood Academic Publishers.

    Butzlaff, R. L., & Hooley, J. M. (1998). Expressed emotion and psychiatric relapse. Archives of General Psy-

    chiatry, 55, 547 552.Caldwell, T. M., & Jorm, A. F. (2000). Mental health nurses beliefs about interventions for schizophrenia and

    depression: a comparison with psychiatrists and the public. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,

    34, 602611.

    Cameron, O. G. (2001). Introception; the inside story; a model for psychosomatic processes. Psychosomatic

    Medicine, 63(5), 697 710.

    Cape, G., Antebi, D., Standen, P., & Glazebrook, C. (1994). Schizophrenia; the views of a sample of psychiatrists.

    Journal of Mental Health, 3, 105113.

    Caton, C. L., Koh, S. P., Fleiss, J. L., Barrow, S., & Goldstein, J. M. (1985). Re-hospitalisation in chronic

    schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 173, 139148.

    Chadwick, P., & Birchwood, M. (1994). Theomnipotence of voices. A cognitive approach to auditory hallucinations.

    British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 190201.Chadwick, P., & Birchwood, M. (1995). The omnipotence of voices: II. The Beliefs About Voices Ques-

    tionnaire (BAVQ). British Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 773776.

    Chadwick, P., Sambrooke, S., Rasch, S., & Davies, E. (2000). Challenging the omnipotence of voices: a group

    cognitive behaviour therapy for voices. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 9931003.

    Chesla, C. A. (1989). Parents illness models of schizophrenia. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 2(4), 218225.

    Clifford, C. (1998). Compliance with medication in psychiatric patients: the self-regulatory model and the health

    belief models. Master of Health Psychology Thesis, University of Surrey.

    Connor, M., & Norman, P. (1995). The role of social cognition in health behaviours. In M. Connor, & P. Norman

    (Eds.), Predicting health behaviour: research and practice with social cognition models (pp. 123). Buck-

    ingham, PA: OUP.

    Corrigan, P. W., Lieberman, R. P., & Engel, J. D. (1990). From non-compliance to collaboration in the treatment ofschizophrenia. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 41, 12031211.

    Crisp, A. H., Gelder, M., Rix, S., Meltzer, H. I., & Rowlands, O. J. (2000). Stigmatisation of people with mental

    illnesses. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 4 7.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 191

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    22/26

    Croyle, R. T. (1992). Appraisal of health threats; cognition, motivation and social comparison. Cognitive Therapy

    and Research, 16, 165 182.

    Croyle, R. T., & Ditto, P. H. (1990). Illness cognition and behaviour: an experimental approach. Journal of

    Behavioural Medicine, 13(1), 3152.

    Day, J. C., Bentall, R. P., & Warner, S. (1996). Schizophrenic patients experiences of neuroleptic medication: a Q-

    methodological investigation. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 93, 397402.

    de Valle, M. N., & Norman, P. (1992). Causal attributions, health locus of control beliefs and lifestyle changes

    among the pre-operative coronary patients. Psychology and Health, 7, 201 211.

    Drury, V., Birchwood, M., & Cochrane, R. (2000). Cognitive therapy and recovery from acute psychosis:

    acontrolled trial. Five-year follow-up. British Journal of Psychiatry, 177, 814.

    Fallon, I. R. H., & Talbot, R. E. (1981). Persistent auditory hallucinations: coping mechanisms and implications

    for management. Psychological Medicine, 11, 329339.

    Fenton, W. S., Blyler, C. R., & Heinssen, R. K. (1997). Determinants of medication compliance in schizophrenia:

    empirical and clinical findings. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23(4), 637651.Freeman, D., & Garety, P. A. (1999). Worry, worry processes and dimensions of delusions: an exploratory

    investigation of a role for anxiety processes in the maintenance of delusional distress. Behavioural and

    Cognitive psychotherapy, 27, 47 62.

    Freeman, D., Garety, P. A., & Kuipers, E. (2001). Persecutory delusions: developing the understanding of belief

    maintenance and emotional distress. Psychological Medicine, 31, 12931306.

    Frier, B. M. (2001). Hypoglycaemia and cognitive function in diabetes (Review). International Journal of Clinical

    Practice, 123, 30 37.

    Galdi, J. (1983). The causality of depression in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 142, 1149 1154.

    Galdi, J., Reider, R. O., Silber, D., & Bonato, R. R. (1981). Genetic factors in the response to neuroleptics in

    schizophrenia; a psychopharmacogenetic study. Psychological Medicine, 11, 713728.

    Gallagher, A. G., Gernez, T., & Baker, L. J. V. (1991). Beliefs of psychologists about schizophrenia and their rolein its treatment. Irish Journal of Psychology, 12(4), 393405.

    Garety, P. A., & Hemsley, D. R. (1994). Delusions: investigations into the psychology of delusional reasoning.

    Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.

    Garety, P. A., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., Freeman, D., & Bebbington, P. E. (2001). A cognitive model of the positive

    symptoms of psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 31, 189195.

    Gilbert, P. (1992). Depression; the evolution of powerlessness. Hove: Lawerence Erlbaum Associates.

    Green, J. H. (1988). Frequent re-hospitalisation and non-compliance with treatment. Hospital and Community

    Psychiatry, 38, 55 60.

    Greenley, J. R. (1986). Social control and expressed emotion. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 174,

    2430.

    Hampson, S. E., Glasgow, R. E., & Foster, L. S. (1995). Personal models of diabetes among older adults: relationto self-management and other variables. Diabetes Educator, 21, 300 307.

    Hampson, S. E., Glasgow, R. E., & Toobert, D. J. (1990). Personal models of diabetes and their relations to self-

    care activities. Health Psychology, 9, 632 646.

    Hampson, S. E., Glasgow, R. E., & Zeiss, A. (1994). Personal models of osteoarthritis and their relation to self-

    management activities and quality of life. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 17, 143 158.

    Heijmans, M., de Ridder, D., & Bensing, J. (1999). Dissimilarity in patients and spouses representations of

    chronic illness; exploration of relations to patient adaptation. Psychology and Health, 14(3), 451466.

    Heinrichsen, G. A., & Lieberman, J. A. (1999). Family attributions and coping in the prediction of emotional

    adjustment in family members of patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica,

    100, 359 366.

    Heinzelman, F., & Baguley, R. (1970). Responses to physical activity programs and their effects on healthbehaviour. Public Health Reports, 85, 905911.

    Helman, C. G. (1985). Communication in primary care: the role of patient and practitioner explanatory models.

    Social Science and Medicine, 20(9), 923931.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196192

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    23/26

    Hoenig, J., & Hamilton, M. W. (1966). The schizophrenic patient in the community and his effect on the

    household. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 12, 165 176.

    Hoffman, H., Kupper, Z., & Kunz, B. (2000). Hopelessness and its impact on rehabilitation outcome in schizo-

    phreniaan exploratory study. Schizophrenia Research, 43, 147 158.

    Hooley, J. M. (1985). Expressed Emotion: a review of the critical literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 5,

    119139.

    Hooley, J. M. (1987). The nature and origins of expressed emotion. In M. J. Goldstein, & K. Hahlweg (Eds.),

    Understanding major mental disorder: the contribution of family interaction research (pp. 176194). New

    York: Family Process Press.

    Horne, R. (1998). Adherence to medication: a review of existing research. In L. Myers, & K. Midence (Eds.),

    Adherence to treatment in medical conditions (pp. 285310). London: Harwood.

    Horne, R., & Weinman, J. (1995). The Beliefs About Medicines Questionnaire (BMQ): a new method for

    assessing lay beliefs about medicines. Proceedings of the Special Group in Health Psychology: British

    Psychological Society.Horne, R., & Weinman, J. (1997). Patients beliefs about prescribed medicines and their role in adherence to

    treatment in chronic physical illness. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47, 555 567.

    Horne, R., & Weinman, J. (1999). Patients beliefs about prescribed medicines and their role in adherence to

    treatment in chronic illness. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 47(6), 555567.

    Horne, R., James, D., Petrie, K., Weinman, J., & Vincent, R. (2000). Patients interpretation of symptoms as a

    cause of delay in reaching hospital during acute myocardial infarction. Heart, 83(4), 388393.

    Huq, S. F., Garety, P. A., & Hemsley, D. R. (1988). Probabilistic judgements in deluded and non-deluded subjects.

    Quaterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 801 812.

    Jackson, H. J., McGorry, P. D., Edwards, J., Hulbert, C., Henry, L., Francey, S., et al. (1998). Cognitively-oriented

    psychotherapy for early psychosis (COPE): preliminary results. British Journal of Psychiatry, 172(suppl. 33),

    9299.Janz, N. K., & Becker, M. H. (1984). The health belief model: a decade later.Health Education Quarterly, 11,147.

    Jorm, A. F., Korten, A. E., Jacomb, P. A., et al. (1997). Helpfulness of interventions for mental disorders; beliefs of

    health professionals compared with the general public. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 233 237.

    Junginger, J. (1990). Predicting compliance with command hallucinations. American Journal of Psychiatry,

    147(2), 245247.

    Kaney, S., & Bentall, R. P. (1989). Persecutory delusions and attributional style. British Journal of Medical

    Psychology, 62, 191 198.

    Kaney, S., Wolfenden, M., Dewey, M. E., & Bentall, R. P. (1992). Persecutory delusions and recall of threatening

    propositions. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 85 87.

    Kinderman, P., & Bentall, R. P. (1996). Self-discrepancies and persecutory delusions: evidence for a defensive

    model of paranoid ideation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 105111.Kinderman, P., & Bentall, R. P. (1997). Causal attributions in paranoia and depression: internal, personal, and

    situational attributions for negative events. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(2), 341345.

    Kinney, C. F. (1999). Coping with schizophrenia; the significance of appraisal. Unpublished PhD thesis, Faculty

    of Medicine, University of Manchester.

    Kleinman, A. (1980). Patients and healers in the context of culture: an exploration of the borderline between

    anthropology, medicine and psychiatry. USA: University of California Press.

    Kleinman, A. (1988). The illness narratives: suffering, healing and the human condition. USA: Basic Books.

    Kreitler, S. (1999). Denial in cancer patients (Review). Cancer Investigation, 17(7), 514534.

    Larsen, T. K., McGalshan, T. H., & Moe, L. C. (1996). First-episode schizophrenia: I. Early course parameters.

    Schizophrenia Bulletin, 22, 241 256.

    Lau, R. R., & Hartman, K. A. (1983). Common sense representations of common illnesses. Health Psychology, 2,167185.

    Lazarus, R. S., & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer.

    Leventhal, H., Benjamin, Y., Brownlee, S., Diefenbach, M., Leventhal, E. A., Patrick-Miller, L., & Robitaille, C.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 193

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    24/26

    (1997). Illness representations; theoretical foundations. In K. J. Petrie, & J. Weinman (Eds.), Perceptions of

    health and illness (pp. 1947). London: Harwood Academic Publishers.

    Leventhal, H., & Cameron, L. (1987). Behavioural theories and the problem of compliance. Patient Education and

    Counselling, 10, 117138.

    Leventhal, H., Diefenbach, M., & Leventhal, E. A. (1992). Illness cognition: using common sense to understand

    treatment adherence and affect cognition interactions. Cognitive Therapy and research, 16(2), 143163.

    Leventhal, H., & Nerenz, D. R. (1985). The assessment of illness cognition. In P. Karody (Ed.), Measurement

    strategies in health psychology (pp. 517554). Canada: Wiley.

    Leventhal, H., Nerenz, D. R., & Steele, D. F. (1984). Illness representations and coping with health threats. In A.

    Singer, & J. Singer (Eds.), A Handbook of psychology and health (pp. 219252). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Liddle, P. F., Barnes, T. R., Curson, D. A., & Patel, M. (1993). Depression and the experience of psychological

    deficits in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 88, 243 247.

    Linn, M. W., Klett, C. J., & Caffey, E. M. (1982). Relapse of psychiatric patients in foster care. American Journal

    of Psychiatry, 139, 778 783.Lloyd, K. R., Jacob, J. S., Patel, V., St. Louis, L., Bhugra, D., & Mann, A. H. (1998). The development of a Short

    Explanatory Model Interview (SEMI) and its use among primary care attenders with common mental disor-

    ders. Psychological Medicine, 28, 12311237.

    Loebel, A. D., Lieberman, J. A., Alvir, J. M. J., et al. (1992). Duration of psychosis and outcome in first-episode

    schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 1183 1188.

    Lopez, S. R., Nelson, K. A., Snyder, K. S., & Mintz, J. (1999). Attributions and affective reactions of family

    members and course of schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108, 307 314.

    Maddux, J. E., & Rogers, R. W. (1983). Protection motivation and self-efficacy: a revised theory of fear appeals

    and attitude change. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 469479.

    Magliano, L., Fadden, G., Madianos, M., Caldas de Almedia, J. M., Held, T., Guarneri, M., Marasco, C., Tosini,

    P., & Maj, M. (1998). Burden on the families of patients with schizophrenia: results of the BIOMED I study.Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, 405 412.

    Mason, P., Harrison, G., Glazebrook, C., Medley, I., Dalkin, T., & Croudace, T. (1995). Characteristics of outcome

    in schizophrenia at 13 years. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 596 603.

    Maurin, J. T., & Boyd, C. (1990). Burden of mental illness on the family: a critical review. Archives of Psychiatric

    Nursing, 4, 99107.

    McEvoy, J. P., Apperson, L. J., Appelbaum, P. S., Ortlip, P., Breckosky, J., & Hammill, K. (1989). Insight in

    schizophrenia: its relationship to acute psychopathology. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177,

    4247.

    McEvoy, J. P., Howe, A. C., & Hogarty, G. E. (1984). Differences in the nature of relapse and subsequent inpatient

    course between medication-compliant and noncompliant schizophrenia patients. Journal of Nervous and

    Mental Disease, 172, 412 416.McGill, C. W., Fallon, I. R. H., Boyd, J. L., & Wood-Swerio, C. (1983). Family education intervention in the

    treatment of schizophrenia. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 34(10), 934 938.

    McGlashan, T. H., Levy, S. T., & Carpenter, W. T. (1975). Integration and sealing over: clinically distinct recovery

    styles from schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32, 12691272.

    McGorry, P. D., & McConville, S. B. (1999). Insight in Psychosis: an elusive target. Comprehensive Psychiatry,

    40(2), 131142.

    Mechanic, D., McAlpine, D., Rosenfield, S., & Davis, D. (1994). Effects of illness attribution and depression on

    the quality of life among persons with serious mental illness. Social Science in Medicine, 39, 155 164.

    Morrison, A. P. (1998). A cognitive analysis of auditory hallucinations: are voices to schizophrenia what bodily

    sensations are to panic? Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 26, 289 302.

    Morrison, A. P. (2001). The interpretation of intrusions of in psychosis: an integrative cognitive approach tohallucinations and delusions. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29(3), 257277.

    Morrison, A. P., & Haddock, G. (1997). Self-focussed attention in schizophrenic patients and normal subjects: a

    comparative study. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 937941.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196194

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    25/26

    Morrison, A. P., Wells, A., & Nothard, S. (2000). Cognitive factors in predisposition to auditory and visual

    hallucinations. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 67 78.

    Moss-Morris, R., Weinman, J., Petrie, K., Horne, R., Cameron, L. D., & Buick, D. (2001). The revised illness

    perception question. Psychology and Health, 17(1), 116.

    Murphy, H., Dickens, C., Creed, F., & Berstein, R. (1999). Depression, illness perception and coping with

    rheumatoid arthritis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 46(2), 155164.

    Nageotte, C., Sullivan, G., Duan, N., & Camp, P. L. (1997). Medication compliance among the seriously mentally

    ill in a public mental health system. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 32, 49 56.

    Petrie, K. J., Weinman, J., Sharpe, N., & Buckley, J. (1996). Predicting return to work and functioning following

    myocardial infarction: the role of the patients view of their illness. British Medical Journal, 312, 11911194.

    Prochaska, J. O., & DiClemente, C. C. (1984). The transtheoretical approach: crossing traditional bounda-

    ries of therapy. Homewood, IL: Dow Jones Irwin.

    Prochaska, J. O., DiClemente, C. C., & Norcross, J. C. (1992). In search of how people change: applications to

    addictive behaviours. American Psychologist, 47, 11021114.Rogers, A., Day, J. C., Williams, B., Randall, F., Wood, P., Healy, D., & Bentall, R. P. (1998). The meaning and

    management of neuroleptic medication; a study of patients with a diagnosis of schizophrenia. Social Science

    and Medicine, 47(9), 1313 1323.

    Romme, M. A. J., Honig, A., Noorthoorn, E. O., & Escher, A. D. M. A. C. (1992). Coping with hearing voices: an

    emancipatory approach. British Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 99103.

    Rose, L. E. (1983). Understanding mental illness: the experience of families of psychiatric patients. Journal of

    Advanced Nursing, 8, 507511.

    Salkovskis, P. M. (1989). Somatic problems. In K. Hawton, P. M. Salkovskis, J. Kirk, & D. M. Clark (Eds.),

    Cognitive therapy for psychiatric problems: a practical guide (pp. 235276). Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.

    Sands, J. R., & Harrow, M. (1999). Depression during the longitudinal course of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia

    Bulletin, 25(1), 157171.Scazufca, M., & Kuipers, E. (1996). Links between expressed emotion and burden of care in relatives of patients

    with schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 580 587.

    Scharloo, M. A., Kaptein, A. A., Weinman, J. A., Willems, L. N. A., & Rooijmans, H. G. M. (2000). Physical and

    psychological correlates of functioning in patients with chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Journal of

    Asthma, 37(19), 1729.

    Schwarzer, R. (1992). Self-efficacy in the adoption and maintenance of health behaviours: theoretical approaches

    and a new model. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-efficacy: thought control and action (pp. 217 243). London:

    Hemisphere.

    Scott, J., & Pope, M. (2002). Non-adherence with mood stabilizers: prevalence and predictors. Journal of Clinical

    Psychiatry, 63(5), 384391.

    Seeman, M., & Seeman, T. E. (1983). Health behaviour and personal autonomy: a longitudinal study of the senseof control in illness. Journal of Health and Social Behaviour, 24, 144160.

    Smith, L. D. (1989). Medication refusal and the rehospitalised mentally ill inmate. Hospital and Community

    Psychiatry, 40, 491 496.

    Talley, S. (1999). Self-regulation activities used by individuals with schizophrenia. Dissertation Abstracts Interna-

    tional: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 59 (12-B), 6265. Univ. Microfilms International, US.

    Tarrier, N. (1991). Some aspects of family interventions in schizophrenia: I. Adherence to intervention pro-

    grammes. British Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 475 480.

    Tarrier, N., & Barrowclough, C. (1986). Providing information about schizophrenia: some comments. British

    Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 458 463.

    Tarrier, N., Harwood, S., Yusopoff, L., Beckett, R., & Baker, A. (1990). Coping Strategy Enhancement (CSE): a

    method of treating residual schizophrenic symptoms. Behavioural Psychotherapy, 18, 283 293.Tarrier, N., Yusupoff, L., McCarthy, E., Kinney, C., & Wittkowski, A. (1998). Some reasons why patients

    suffering with chronic schizophrenia fail to continue in psychological treatment. Behavioural and Cognitive

    Psychotherapy, 26, 177 181.

    F. Lobban et al. / Clinical Psychology Review 23 (2003) 171196 195

  • 8/14/2019 role of illness models

    26/26

    van der Velde, W., & van der Pligt, J. (1991). AIDS related behaviour: coping, protection motivation, and

    previous behaviour. Journal of Behavioural Medicine, 14, 429451.

    Van Putten, T., & May, P. R. (1978). Akinet