reading fluency and its intervention - tufts...

29
This article was downloaded by: [Tufts University] On: 16 March 2012, At: 10:17 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Scientific Studies of Reading Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hssr20 Reading Fluency and Its Intervention Maryanne Wolf & Tami Katzir-Cohen Available online: 19 Nov 2009 To cite this article: Maryanne Wolf & Tami Katzir-Cohen (2001): Reading Fluency and Its Intervention, Scientific Studies of Reading, 5:3, 211-239 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_2 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Upload: danganh

Post on 22-Mar-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


3 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

This article was downloaded by: [Tufts University]On: 16 March 2012, At: 10:17Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

Scientific Studies of ReadingPublication details, including instructions forauthors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hssr20

Reading Fluency and ItsInterventionMaryanne Wolf & Tami Katzir-Cohen

Available online: 19 Nov 2009

To cite this article: Maryanne Wolf & Tami Katzir-Cohen (2001): Reading Fluency andIts Intervention, Scientific Studies of Reading, 5:3, 211-239

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_2

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone isexpressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make anyrepresentation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up todate. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should beindependently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liablefor any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damageswhatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connectionwith or arising out of the use of this material.

Page 2: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Reading Fluency and Its Intervention

Maryanne Wolf and Tami Katzir-CohenCenter for Reading and Language Research

Tufts University

This 3-part article represents an effort to confront 3 large lacunae in the research onreading fluency: definition, component structure, and theory-based intervention. The1st section describes several historical approaches to fluency and the components offluent reading that are implicit in these approaches. We then present our own develop-mental- and component-based definition of reading fluency. In the 2nd section wediscuss how different types of current fluency interventions correspond to particularcomponents in fluency’s structure and to particular phases of its development. Thelast section presents an overview of an experimental fluency program that attempts toaddress multiple components in the development of fluent reading. Finally, we arguethat increased exploration of the issues surrounding fluency and comprehension willcontribute to our understanding of both reading development and dyslexia subtypes.

The history of fluency research in the field of reading might best be characterized asintellectually spasmodic: There are periods of great effort and creativity, followedby fallow periods of relative disinterest. In 1983 fluency was described as the “mostneglected” reading skill (Allington, 1983). In 1997 Lyon and Moats summarizedthe state of intervention research with a call to refocus attention on fluency:

Improvements in decoding and word-reading accuracy have been far easier to obtainthan improvements in reading fluency and automaticity. This persistent finding indi-cates there is much we have to learn about the development of componential readingskills and how such skills mediate reading rate and reading comprehension. (p. 579)

The recent attention to fluency and fluency intervention is the result, we be-lieve, of a convergence of three major factors. First, the systematic research on therole of phonological processes in reading failure and intervention has proven both

SCIENTIFIC STUDIES OF READING, 5(3), 211–239Copyright © 2001, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Requests for reprints should be sent to Maryanne Wolf, Center for Reading and Language Research,Miller Hall, Tufts University, Medford, MA 02155. E-mail: [email protected]

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 3: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity of reading dis-abilities and the complexity of reading breakdown—particularly in the area of flu-ency. (For a recent comprehensive review, see Meyer & Felton, 1999; also seeBreznitz & Share, 1992; Rashotte & Torgesen, 1985; Torgesen et al., 1999;Torgesen, Rashotte, & Wagner, 1997; Young & Bowers, 1995.) The wish to ad-dress the needs of children who do not completely respond to phonological-basedtreatment is a motivating impulse in turning to additional explanatory principleslike fluency (Torgesen, Rashotte, & Alexander, in press).

The second factor is an increased awareness of the multiple underlying sourcesthat can contribute to or impede fluency development. The concerted effort to un-derstand the predictive ability of naming speed in reading failure is an example ofthis focus (see reviews in Wolf & Bowers, 1999, 2000), as are the extensive studiesthat explore a range of time-related deficits in children with reading disabilities invarious perceptual and motor areas (see reviews in Farmer & Klein, 1995;Nicolson & Fawcett, 1994; Stein, in press; Waber, in press; Wolf, Bowers, &Biddle, 2000; Wolff, 2000).

The third factor is related to naming-speed research and involves cumulative evi-dence from subtype research in developmental dyslexia. A growing body of workdemonstrates that there are discrete groups of children with reading disabilities whocan be characterized by single deficits in either naming speed or phonological pro-cesses or combined deficits in both areas (Badian, 1996; Compton, Chayna,DeFries, Gayan, & Olson, in press; Levy, in press; Lovett, 1987; Lovett, Steinbach,& Frijters, 2000; Manis, Doi, & Bhadha, 2000; Wolf, Bowers, & Biddle, 2000). Thisconceptualization, known as the Double-Deficit Hypothesis, has several fluency-re-lated implications (Wolf & Bowers, 1999, 2000). First, children with single nam-ing-speed deficits are frequently difficult to diagnose in early primary years, but goon to develop fluency and comprehension problems by the end of Grade 3. Second,children with both phonological and naming-speed deficits are consistently found topossess the most severe problems in reading and reading fluency. Third, and mostimportant, this work provides a theoretical rationale and foundation for interventionthat specifically addresses issues of speed of processing and fluency. Until very re-cently, intervention was largely directed to treatment for phonologically based de-coding problems. Children with either single processing-speed deficits or combineddeficits would be only partially served by such a focus, thus augmenting the ranks ofchildren who do not respond to treatment.

Represented by this special issue of Scientific Studies of Reading, the new,evolving work on reading fluency and fluency-based intervention has much tolearn from the 2 decades of work that precede it in theoretically based, phonologi-cally focused treatment. Specifically, there must be a greater concentration of ef-fort (a) in defining what we mean by fluency, (b) in charting the development of itscomponent structure, (c) in understanding the breadth and nature of process-ing-speed and fluency deficits in reading subtypes, and (d) in applying this knowl-

212 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 4: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

edge to the development of intervention programs. In the last years we havedirected considerable attention to understanding the third area of concern, the pro-cesses underlying naming speed and other rate-related deficits in developmentaldyslexia. We view this article as a working paper on the other three of these areas.The first section describes several historical approaches to fluency and the compo-nents of fluent reading implicit in these approaches. We then provide our ownevolving definition. In the second section we discuss how different types of cur-rent fluency interventions correspond to particular components in fluency’s struc-ture and particular phases of its development. Finally, the last section presents anoverview of our own first efforts to construct a theory-based, comprehensive flu-ency program that attempts to address multiple components in fluent reading.

TOWARD A DEFINITION OF FLUENCY AND ANUNDERSTANDING OF ITS COMPONENT STRUCTURE

We view this article as a working paper because there are still no consensual defini-tions of what is meant by fluency and what its relation might be to the subset oftime-related terms most frequently related to it (e.g., automaticity, speed of pro-cessing, reading rate/speed, and word recognition rate/proficiency). In this section,our goals are foundational for the rest of the article and for our future work. We re-view several historical and current definitions and approaches to fluency as the ba-sis for our own definition and as the context for examining the underlying compo-nents of fluency. These and other efforts toward gaining greater clarity aboutfluency will push forward not only our understanding of the component structure ofreading fluency, but also the development of better assessment tools and more com-prehensive interventions.

BACKGROUND: APPROACHES AND DEFINITIONS

Early Research

Perhaps predictably, any historical review of perspectives on fluency begins withthe work of William MacKeen Cattell and Sir Edmond Huey. Cattell (1886), a19th-century experimental psychologist, found that letters and words are namedfaster than other symbolic categories, such as colors, and other more concrete, se-mantic categories, such as pictured objects. Cattell was the first researcher to high-light the automatic-like rates of recognition achieved in letter naming and wordreading, with words read as fast as letters and reading speed increased when seman-tic and syntactic information are provided (as in sentences). This set of findings wascompletely replicated almost a century later by Doehring (1976).

READING FLUENCY 213

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 5: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Also a century ahead of his time, Huey (1908/1968) presciently described whatmost cognitive scientists refer to as automaticity in reading (LaBerge & Samuels,1974; Logan, 1988). More specifically, according to Huey, the development offluent reading involves the steady accumulation and synthesis of “more and morecomplex constituent acts as these are progressively welded together by practice”(p. 105). An integral aspect of this synthesis to Huey was the development of a rateof processing, which through “repetition progressively frees the mind from atten-tion to details, makes facile the total act, shortens the time, and reduces the extentto which consciousness must concern itself with the process” (p. 65).

Information-Processing Models

Similar to Huey’s notions, more recent perspectives on fluency in the last quarter ofthe 20th century emphasize automatic, effortless rates of processing that allow thereallocation of attention. For example, LaBerge and Samuels (1974), whose workushered in an era of renewed attention to fluency, proposed a model of automaticitywith principles derived from information-processing theory (e.g., Norman, 1968;Posner, Lewis, & Conrad, 1972). According to their model, reading becomes in-creasingly fluent as the result of the development of automaticity of the subskills:“When one describes a skill at the macro level as being automatic, it follows that thesubskills at the micro level and their interrelations must also be automatic” (p. 295).The first stage of these subskills in their model involves the visual code and the unit-ization of visual stimuli. These stimuli may include letters, spelling patterns,words, and highly frequent word groups (e.g., high school). With exposure andpractice, the visual features in stimuli like letters become unitized and then per-ceived as a single unit. As these units accumulate and letter perception becomes in-creasingly automatic, attention to early visual coding processes decreases. This de-crease allows attentional resources to be reallocated to other areas, such as thesemantic (or meaning) code.

The earlier physiological work of Donald Hebb (1949) undergirds the conceptof unitization used here. An example from the visual system is helpful. When anunknown visual stimuli is first seen by the retina, there is an activation in the vi-sual cortex of multiple individual cells. These cells correspond to each aspect ofwhat the retina sees and are responsible for very specific types of information(e.g., curved lines, diagonals, etc.). After multiple exposures to the same stimu-lus, the individual cells in the visual areas become a working unit, or cell assem-bly. These unified groups of neurons learn to work together in synchrony so thatrecognition of frequently viewed stimuli such as letters becomes so efficient, it isvirtually automatic. Hebb argued that the ultimate result of these cell assembliesin the visual area is a reservoir of mental representations of practiced, frequentlyviewed visual stimuli.

214 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 6: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

LaBerge and Samuels (1974) argued that a similar principle is found at morecomplex visual levels (and also in other modalities, such as phonological represen-tations). For example, the orthography of every language permits particular, fre-quently seen spelling patterns or letter combinations (e.g., th in English, schr inGerman). With sufficient exposures and practice, these orthographic patterns alsobecome quickly recognized. A similar principle, they believed, may apply to manyfrequently read words, although LaBerge and Samuels raised questions about howthese larger units are perceived and processed by different readers (see, e.g.,Gough, 1972).

The primary contributions of the LaBerge and Samuels model for work in flu-ency (McCormick & Samuels, 1979) include several principles about how we canprocess written information at highly rapid rates: (a) the concept of unitization; (b)the key notion that, with the increased speed of lower level subskills, attention canbe reallocated elsewhere; and (c) the more reading-specific concept that withautomaticity, attention can be shifted from lower level decoding to higher levelcomprehension skills. A fundamental premise of our work is based on their con-ceptualization that macrolevel fluency is based on the automaticity of microlevelsubskills and their connections.

Consciously building on the work of Cattell, Hebb, and LaBerge and Samuels,Doehring (1976) provided one of the most systematic studies of the developmentof fluency in reading subskills in children from kindergarten until the end of highschool. In his seminal monograph “Acquisition of Rapid Reading Responses,”Doehring studied the acquisition of rates of processing in symbols (colors and ob-jects), letters, letter combinations, words, random word sequences, and sentences.In so doing, he sought to map

the relative course of acquisition of skills for processing the graphological features ofletters, the orthographic regularities of letter combinations, the semantic features ofwords, and the semantic-syntactic constraints of word sequences. These are the skillsthat must be mastered beyond the level of simple accuracy to the point where accurateprocessing becomes rapid enough to be classified as fluent reading. (p. 2)

The data in this study went well beyond a replication of Cattell’s first findings aboutthe “time it takes” human beings to acquire automatic-like rates in various pro-cesses; to our minds, Doehring described the first data in a developmental, compo-nent-process approach to reading fluency.

Perfetti’s (1977, 1985) verbal efficiency theory represents another highly influ-ential perspective in the history of fluency-related research. Like LaBerge andSamuels (1974) and Doehring (1976), Perfetti worked within an information-pro-cessing approach. Perfetti did not refer to the word fluency, and he did not equateverbal efficiency with fluency. Rather, Perfetti discussed fluency-related conceptsin terms of an overall account of reading ability in which verbal efficiency theory

READING FLUENCY 215

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 7: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

played a major role. Within this context, fluency is closest to what Perfetti andwhat Jackson and McClelland (1979) called “effective reading speed,” construedas the outcome of comprehension accuracy and reading speed (words per minute).

Perfetti’s (1985) account of verbal efficiency was a theoretical effort to explainhow “individual differences in reading comprehension are produced by individualdifferences in the efficient operation of local processes. The local processes arethose by which temporary representations of text are established” (p. 100; here heincluded orthographic, phonological, and semantic processes). Some of the criticalcomponents in Perfetti’s model that contributed to the efficiency of local processeswere (a) general symbol activation and retrieval, (b) recognition processes, (c) lex-ical access and retrieval, and (d) working memory.

Learning and practice also were thought to play a pivotal role in the acquisitionof efficiency in the local processes. This efficiency was considered necessary toconstruct high-quality mental representations, a central concept in the theory. Thequality of representations was hypothesized to affect both the further developmentof efficiency (by enhancing the ease of lexical retrieval) and working memory:“To the extent that these codes are retrieved rapidly and are high in quality, the sys-tem is efficient” (Perfetti, 1985, p. 118). When the underlying systems were effi-cient, the individual was considered able to free cognitive resources to focus onhigher level demands in reading, which was important for comprehension. Theconverse was also predicted here. For example, Perfetti suggested that an ineffi-cient system resulting in a slow rate of word recognition could obstruct the individ-ual’s ability to hold large units of text in working memory, which, in turn wouldaffect comprehension and recall.

Important to work today, Perfetti (1985) stressed that the quality of representa-tions and of each of the local processes, components, and their various forms of in-tegration (depending on the type of reading task) were potentially rate-limitingfactors in reading acquisition. Furthermore, as Doehring (1976) suggested earlier,different emphases would be placed on processing systems, depending on devel-opment. We emphasize LaBerge and Samuels’ (1974), Doehring’s (1976), andPerfetti’s (1985) focus on the development of efficiency in lower levels of process-ing because this aspect of fluency development is frequently neglected in presentfluency interventions.

There were immediate and long-term effects of this model on reading and read-ing disabilities research, particularly concerning short-term memory and lexicalretrieval. Shankweiler and Crain’s (1986) influential article on reading disabilityincorporated aspects of Perfetti’s theory by hypothesizing that the demands of or-thographic decoding and limited working memory capacity contribute to compre-hension difficulties of poor readers. Swanson (as cited in Johnston & Anderson,1998) proposed that many poor readers have difficulty in reading because they failto establish effective, efficient visual–verbal connections, which may lead to diffi-culty in accessing or laying down long-term memory traces.

216 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 8: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Perfetti’s description of the role of an efficient lexical retrieval process in read-ing development has influenced much of our own work on word-retrieval and itsrelation to reading disability (Wolf, 1982, 1991; Wolf & Goodglass, 1986; Wolf &Obregon, 1992). Perfetti used knowledge about retrieval and all components inverbal efficiency theory as ways to explicate individual differences in readingcomprehension. In doing so, Perfetti’s work provides the figure–ground perspec-tive from which to view fluency today—as a means to reading comprehension.

Rauding Theory

Carver (1991, 1997), who also focused on the links between fluency and compre-hension, introduced an emphasis on the different purposes of reading and their re-spective rates. According to Carver, most reading is done in the rauding mode,which involves the fastest rate at which an individual can successfully understandcomplete thoughts in each sentence.

Carver’s work includes a model that depicts four levels of factors that affect theachievement of optimal reading rate and accuracy. Some of these factors are age,teaching variables, “aptitude” factors (i.e., verbal knowledge, decoding processes,and cognitive speed), decoding speed, and naming speed (see Spring & Capps,1974). Carver reintroduced the complex concept of cognitive speed to reading flu-ency theory, a line of research (Kail, 1991, 1992) in reading and in other areas ofcognitive development (see also Breznitz, in press; Marcus, 1997).

Connectionist Models

The last body of research with implications for fluency in this review involves theevolution of connectionist models of reading. These models are profoundly differ-ent from earlier information-processing models, particularly in the role of retrievaland unitization. Connectionist models emphasize the continuous, distributed inter-action of phonological, orthographic, syntactic, and semantic-processing codesduring word recognition (Adams, 1990; Caramazza, 1997; Foorman, 1994;Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989). There are no retrieval mechanisms, as such;rather, all codes are computed for every word. In these powerful models, theamount and level of activation in each system and the speed of processing withinand among these systems are products of internal factors (e.g., learning) and exter-nal factors (e.g., the consistency of correspondence between a given letter and asound pattern). These models add to any discussion of fluency the importance ofaccounting not only for intra- and intersystem factors in the learner (the emphasis inearlier information-processing models), but also for such linguistic features as fre-quency, regularity, and the amount of processing-code connections among individ-

READING FLUENCY 217

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 9: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

ual words. The latter factor, we believe, has special importance for learning and, byextension, for the design of fluency interventions.

Current Research and Definitions

Throughout the period between LaBerge and Samuels (1974) and Carver (1997),the general consensus was that fluent reading could be defined as “that level ofreading competence at which textual material can be effortlessly, smoothly, and au-tomatically understood” (Schreiber, 1980, p. 177). Current research is largely con-sistent with this view. Indeed Meyer and Felton (1999), in a recent review of the lit-erature, defined fluency in a similar manner as “the ability to read connected textrapidly, smoothly, effortlessly, and automatically with little conscious attention tothe mechanics of reading such as decoding” (p. 284). Along similar lines, Hudson,Mercer, and Lane (2000) defined fluency as “accurate reading at a minimal ratewith appropriate prosodic features (expression) and deep understanding” (p. 16).

Although this approach to fluency effectively captures the end goal of flu-ency—that is, effortless reading with good comprehension—it does not yet permitease in validation or in conceptualizing the components underlying fluency. Forexample, Torgesen et al. (in press) argued that most previous fluency definitionsdo not allow empirical validation; they preferred, therefore, the minimalist defini-tion of “rate and accuracy in oral reading” used in curriculum-based assessment re-search (Shinn, Good, Knutson, Tilly, & Collins, 1992). The National ReadingPanel’s (2000) definition of fluency as “the immediate result of word recognitionproficiency” (pp. 3–5) allows the simple procedure of testing for a particular levelof proficiency in word recognition, just as Torgesen et al.’s view can be assessedby performance on an oral reading measure that incorporates rate and accuracy(e.g., the Gray Oral Reading Test; Wiederholt & Bryant, 1992).

Although we concur in principle with Torgesen et al.’s (in press) general admo-nition to use definitions that can be validated, their emphasis on oral reading andthe National Reading Panel’s emphasis on word recognition ignore the multipleother dimensions of fluency, particularly lower level subskills, that were empha-sized in earlier accounts. Few current approaches attempt to define fluency interms of either its component parts or its various levels of reading subskills—thatis, letter, letter pattern, word, sentence, and passage.

More to the point, almost all of the definitions until this time posit fluency as theoutcome of learned skills. Kame’enui, Simmons, Good, and Harn (in press) sug-gested a figure–ground shift for the field. In a broad-ranging article, they concep-tualized fluency in a more developmentally encompassing manner both as thedevelopment of proficiency in underlying lower level and component skills ofreading (e.g., phoneme awareness) and as the outcome of proficiency in higherlevel processes and component skills (e.g., accuracy in comprehension; see also

218 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 10: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Logan, 1997). Such a perspective, we believe, has profoundly different implica-tions for prevention, intervention, and assessment than the traditional view. For,within a developmental perspective, efforts to address fluency must begin at thebeginning of the reading process—that is, during acquisition—not after reading isalready acquired. The importance of working proactively to prevent the develop-ment of difficult fluency problems is a major theme in the recent studies byTorgesen et al. (in press) and by Kame’enui et al. (in press).

Berninger, Abbott, Billingsley, and Nagy (in press) took an equally multidi-mensional view with a systems approach to fluency. In their conceptualization,fluency is influenced by (a) the characteristics of stimulus input (e.g., rate and per-sistence of a visual signal or speech signal), (b) the efficiency and automaticity ofinternal processes (e.g., the development of phonological, orthographic, and mor-phological systems), and (c) the coordination of responses by the executive func-tions system. As we discuss later, Berninger is one of the few researchers to placespecial importance on the role of morphological knowledge about words in facili-tating the development of orthographic rate and overall fluency.

We regard Kame’enui’s (in press) developmental and Berninger’s (in press)systems-analysis approaches as the context for our own working definition:

In its beginnings, reading fluency is the product of the initial development ofaccuracy and the subsequent development of automaticity in underlyingsublexical processes, lexical processes, and their integration in single-wordreading and connected text. These include perceptual, phonological, ortho-graphic, and morphological processes at the letter, letter-pattern, and wordlevels, as well as semantic and syntactic processes at the word level and con-nected-text level. After it is fully developed, reading fluency refers to a levelof accuracy and rate where decoding is relatively effortless; where oral read-ing is smooth and accurate with correct prosody; and where attention can beallocated to comprehension.

THE COMPONENT STRUCTURE OF FLUENCY

This developmental and component-based definition of fluency poses a difficultchallenge for empirical validation; at the same time it provides a foundation for thespecification of processes, skills, and factors that underlie or influence reading flu-ency and that can guide assessment and validation efforts. In this subsection we un-pack the previously discussed definitions and approaches in terms of the compo-nents that are implied as necessary in the development of fluency.

Most researchers describe three major types of processes as prominent in flu-ency’s development: orthographic, phonological, and semantic. Berninger et al.(in press) and Adams (1990) added emphases on morphological and syntactic

READING FLUENCY 219

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 11: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

knowledge systems. For example, Adams proposed that both orthographic and se-mantic information are necessary for learning morphological knowledge about thepatterns and roots that make up many words (e.g., derivational and compoundwords; Latin- and Greek-derived roots). The direct learning of these morphemepatterns enhances vocabulary acquisition, fluency, and reading comprehension.First, such knowledge provides rich semantic associations that can be applied toacquiring new vocabulary words. Second, morphological knowledge makes ortho-graphic chunks more visible, more familiar, and more quickly retrieved. Third, thecombination of rapid recognition of orthographic units and faster retrieval ofmeaning facilitates comprehension. The working notion here (and in the interven-tion described later) is that one retrieves faster what one knows better, thus contin-uously emphasizing the connections that link orthographic, semantic,phonological, and morphological systems. This conclusion is reinforced by recentresearch showing that morphological awareness is significantly related to readingfluency in third and fourth grades (Berninger et al., in press).

Multiple Components

The implications of a developmental, component-based conceptualization of flu-ency are far from simple. At a minimum such an approach implies three related pre-mises: first, that multiple processes contribute to fluency development; second, thatmultiple sources of dysfluency are possible; and third, that assessment and inter-vention should be based on knowledge about the development and breakdown ofthese components. With regard to the first premise, the fluency-related processesand components described in the research summarized here include lower level at-tention and visual perception, orthographic (letter-pattern) representation and iden-tification, auditory perception, phonological representation and phoneme aware-ness, short-term and long-term memory, lexical access and retrieval, semanticrepresentation, decoding and word identification, morphosyntactic and prosodicknowledge, and connected-text knowledge and comprehension. Berninger et al. (inpress) went further and argued that a systems approach to fluency would include theexecutive function’s coordination of all these discussed internal-processing sys-tems so that they perform smoothly and in synchrony. Wood, Flowers, andGrigorenko (in press) added to the set of executive functions an emphasis on therole of anticipatory facilitation for tasks that become fluent.

In other words, the unsettling conclusion is that reading fluency involves everyprocess and subskill involved in reading. We do not shy away from this conclusion;rather, we wish to underscore it. Unlike reading accuracy, which can be executedwithout utilizing some important reading components like semantic processes, weargue that fluency is influenced by the development of rapid rates of processing in allthe components of reading. Kame’enui et al. (in press) would emphasize that reading

220 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 12: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

fluency involves the development of accuracy and proficiency in every underlyingcomponent. Researchers within connectionist approaches (Adams, 1990; Foorman,1994; Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989) would stress the explicit linkages or connec-tions among the orthographic, semantic, and phonological processes. Berninger etal. (in press) and Adams (1990) would add the connections betweenmorphosyntactic knowledge and these other processes.

Multiple Sources of Dysfluency

The second premise is no less complex and is a logical outgrowth of the first prem-ise and of time-allocation principles. Impairment in any one or more of the underly-ing processes could increase the processing time both within that process and inreading outcome behaviors (Wolf, 1991; Wolf, Bowers, & Biddle, 2000). Further,it appears that inefficiency not only can arise from accuracy or timing problemswithin a processing system but also may be based on the coordination and integra-tion of information across processes. An example of the latter is found in recent re-search by Breznitz (in press), who proposed that a temporal discrepancy (i.e.,asynchrony) between the speed of processing of visual information and the speed ofprocessing of auditory information prevents the efficient cross-modal integrationnecessary for visual–verbal processing in reading.

If the two premises of a developmental, component-based conceptualization ofreading fluency are true, a long and complex set of processes and variables couldbe implicated when fluency is not attained. What is the evidence that impairedreaders break down across this continuum of processes? A full discussion of all theevidence that might be brought to bear on this large question is well outside thescope of this article. Meyer and Felton (1999), however, provided a first summaryof the existing research on explanations of dysfluency. They divided this researchinto three major areas:

1. A breakdown in the lower level processes. At this level, dysfluent reading isbased on deficits in phonological processing and/or orthographic processing sys-tems that affect the timing and coordination of these systems. More specifically,deficits may arise from phonological, visio-spatial, and/or working memory pro-cesses. Breznitz (in press) used extensive evoked potential and behavioral data toindicate that the largest contribution to slowed word-reading rate among dyslexicreaders is the slowed speed of processing in perceptual stages (see also Farmer &Klein, 1995; Stein, in press; Tallal, Miller, & Fitch, 1993).

2. A failure to make higher order semantic and phonological connections be-tween words, meaning, and ideas (Adams, 1990). In this level, dysfluent reading isbased on deficits that occur after perceptual identification has been completed.One manifestation may involve the slowed retrieval of names, meaning, or both

READING FLUENCY 221

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 13: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

(see work on confrontation naming and receptive vocabulary by German, 1992;Haynes, 1994; Segal & Wolf, 1993; for naming speed, see Wolf, Bowers, &Biddle, 2000).

3. A breakdown in syntactic processing (Schreiber, 1980), with deficits exhib-ited in a lack of prosody and rhythm in oral reading and a lack of sensitivity toprosodic cues. These deficits may become apparent only at the level of connectedtext for reading but may be able to be assessed much earlier in speech development(e.g., through measures of prosody and syntax).

Assessment and Intervention

Meyer and Felton’s (1999) organization of possible areas of deficit implies thatbreakdown in fluent reading could originate at the sublexical, lexical, sentence, andhigher-conceptual integration levels. In future efforts we will examine evidence forpossible sources of dysfluency at the component level as well. There are significantimplications for diagnosis, as just sketched, that can utilize the application of manyexisting measures. The most important implication, however, of Meyer and Felton’sreview—and of our working definition of fluency—concerns a new, more compre-hensive approach to intervention. In the second section of this article we briefly ex-amine how current fluency interventions correspond to the different phases of flu-ency development and to the range of component processes (e.g., orthographic) andlevels (e.g., sublexical, lexical) incorporated in this view of reading fluency.

THE CORRESPONDENCE BETWEEN CURRENTFLUENCY INTERVENTION AND THE DEVELOPMENTAL,

COMPONENT-BASED VIEW OF READING FLUENCY

As discussed in work by Stahl and his colleagues (Stahl, Heubach, & Cramond,1997), the oldest and most commonly used method for facilitating fluency is the re-peated reading technique. Here readers are simply asked to read a passage of con-nected text (at a level appropriate to the learner) several times until a particularreading rate (words per minute) is attained. At each juncture the reader is given fur-ther passages at that level until the optimal rate is reached. Dowhower (1994)showed that practicing a series of passages helps build a large repertoire of quicklyidentified words, a principle also emphasized in the development of fluency byTorgesen et al. (in press) and by Stahl et al. (1997). Repeated reading can be as-sisted and thus necessitate oral reading (see Young, Bowers, & MacKinnon, 1996;also see their use of prosodic assistance methods) or unassisted and involve silentreading. First described in the literature by Dahl (1974) and named by Samuels(1985), repeated reading methods were designed to increase reading rate for the

222 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 14: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

given material and other similar materials and to improve comprehension. (As dis-cussed earlier, this is based on the principle that comprehension can be allocatedmore time when the rate of lower level decoding skills is increased.)

First, does repeated reading achieve its own specified goals? Second, how doesit fall on a developmental continuum of fluency’s growth, and what processes doesit address? In their careful review of the most well conducted repeated readingstudies, Meyer and Felton (1999) underscored the

complexity involved in assessing comprehensive gains from fluency instruction, andsuggest factors that need to be considered such as student age, reading level, instruc-tional method type (types of repeated reading or rapid decoding of single words), andcueing. Furthermore, the question of whether fluency and comprehension recipro-cally influence one another is unanswered. (p. 294)

There is by now a considerable history of studies documenting the relation be-tween fluency and comprehension (Fuchs, Fuchs, & Maxwell, 1988; Jenkins,Fuchs, Espin, van den Broek, & Deno, 2000; Levy, in press; Levy, Abello, &Lysynchuk, 1997; Torgesen et al., in press). As Meyer and Felton (1999) sug-gested, however, whether the reciprocal direction also exists between comprehen-sion and fluency remains unresolved. Equally unresolved to our minds is whetherrepeated reading or any existing fluency instruction significantly changes accu-racy, rate, and comprehension of trained materials with transfer to other materials.

AN EXAMINATION OF FLUENCY STUDIES

To address this latter question, as well as issues about developmental stages andcomponents that different interventions emphasize, we have taken a second look atmany of the studies included in Meyer and Felton’s (1999) review and added sev-eral new fluency intervention studies. Depicted in Table 1, our framework exam-ines the following variables: (a) the intensity of the intervention and the nature ofthe gains (i.e., rate, accuracy, comprehension, transfer), (b) the developmental levelof reading subskills addressed (e.g., letter, letter-pattern, word, or passage) and thegrade of the individuals, and (c) the components of fluency focused on in the inter-vention. In the remainder of this section we summarize what we found about eachof the variables.

Intensity of Interventions

The length of interventions in the existing literature is extremely limited (mostrange between 1 and 15 days). Most of the existing studies were intended to be

READING FLUENCY 223

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 15: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

TABLE 1Fluency Studies in Chronological Order: Gains Across Rate, Accuracy, Comprehension, and Transfer to New Materials

Study Population NLength of

InterventionNature of

Intervention

Level andComponentEmphasized

No. ofRereadings

inIntervention

Reading RateGains

AccuracyGains

ComprehensionGains

Transfer toUntrainedPassages

O’Shea,Sindelar,andO’Shea(1985)

Average toabove-averagereaders

30 thirdgradersread at70 to119wpm

3 sessions Half cued forcomprehen-sion, half forrate andaccuracy

PassagePhonolog-icaldecoding/rate

1, 3, and 7 Rate group1–3: 24.7 wpm3–7: 13.6 wpmComp group1–3: 22.6 wpm3–7: 10.3 wpm

+ For rategroup

Comprehensiongains onlyfrom 1 to 3rereading.Comprehension

NA

RashotteandTorgesen(1985)

Nonfluentreaderswithreadingdisabilities

12 2–5gradesread at65 wpmor lessonGrade 2level

7 days, 21sessions insmall-groupinstructionsettings

The threeconditions:1. Repeatedreading withinsession ofunrelatedstories.2. Repeatedreading ofstories withshared words.3. Fourdifferentstories a day.

PassageWordrecognitionExposureto words

4 1. 35.3 wpm2. 33.0 wpm3. 5.2 wpm(all signifi-cant)

1. 2.52errordecrease2. 2.16errordecrease3. .82errordecrease(only 1&2signifi-cant)

Comprehensionwas high tobegin with,therefore maybe ceiling effect

1. Moretransfer whendegree of wordcommonalityamong storiesis high.2. Wordcommonalityhad less effecton accuracy orcomprehen-sion.D

ownl

oade

d by

[T

ufts

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:17

16

Mar

ch 2

012

Page 16: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Levy,Nicholas,and Kohen(1993)

PoorreadersGoodreaders

24 poorreaders24 goodreadersGrade3–5

One or twosessions

Read silentlystories, crossout misspelledwords

PassageOrtho-graphicpatterns

4 Across gradesboth groupsimproved,poor readersshowed moreimprove-ment

+ Forbothgroups

Gains in bothgroups

Only on wordrecognition,not oncomprehen-sion

Stoddard,Valcante,Sindelar,O’Shea,andAlgozzine(1993)

Readingdisabled

30grades4–5(read atleast 70wpm)

15 Half receivedtraining inintonation.Half receivedtraining insentencesegmentation.

PassageProsody/Syntax

7 Reading rateimprovedsignificantlyfor bothgroups from 1to 3 to 7repetitions.Intonation: 30wpm;Segmentation:33 wpm

NA Improved from1 to 3 readings,but not to 7;did not differamong groups

NA

Lemoine,Levy, andHutchinson(1993)Study 1

PoorreadersGrade 4

32 4 sessions Blockedpresentationof wordsfamilies orscrambledpresentationof wordfamilies

SublexicalLexicalMorphol-ogy

Wordrecognition

34repetitions

Blocked-150msec;Scrambled-350 msec

+ Forbothgroups

Number orerrors decreasedequally for thetwo trainingmethods

Nogeneralization

(continued)

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 17: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

TABLE 1 (Continued)

Study Population NLength of

InterventionNature of

Intervention

Level andComponentEmphasized

No. ofRereadings

inIntervention

Reading RateGains

AccuracyGains

ComprehensionGains

Transfer toUntrainedPassages

Lemoine etal. (1993)Study 2

GoodreadersPoorreadersGrade 3

40 5 sessions Readers weretrained on 50regular wordsor 50 irregularwords

LexicalWordrecognition

25 Good readers:Named bothequally quick-ly, decrease of100 msec overthe first 4 tri-als. Poor read-ers: The dif-ference be-tween regularand irregulardecreased overtrials

+ Forbothgroups

+ For bothgroups

Number ofrepetitions intraining affect-ed retentionfor bothgroups on bothsets of words.Poor readersbenefited morefrom repeti-tions, both inrate gains as inaccuracy gains

Young,Bowers,andMacKinnon(1996)

PoorreadersGrade 5

40 1 session ModelingText practiceBothNeither

Passage 3 Practice oftext: 36.9wpm. Both: 29wpm. Onlymodeling: 17.7wpm. Neither:25.5 wpm.

+ In allgroupsapprox.2.5

Practice of text:10, Modeling:4, Both: 7,Neither: 7

+ Only for therepeatedreading group,in accuracygains

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 18: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Levy,Abello, andLysynchuk(1997)

Poorreaders

20 (FN)20 (SN)Grade 4

4 sessions Repeated oralreading ofisolatedcontent words

LexicalPassageWordrecognition

5 of isolatedwords 4 ofstories

Fast RAN:25.3, SlowRAN: 33.5 (onstories), FastRAN: 80 sec,Slow RAN: 60sec (on singlewords)

Trainedstorieswere readmore ac-curately,no gainsfromrereadingin bothgroups

+ For bothgroups

+ Wordrecognition

+ Rate+Comprehen-sion

Breznitz(1997)Study 1

Averagereaders,agematched(age 6.9).Poor-dyslexicreaders(age 9.1),mostlyboys

52 2 sessions Reading attwo rateconditions:Self-paced orfast-paced +auditorymasking

PassageRate

3 of paralleltexts:SelfFastSelf

Averageachievers: 9.13sec; Poorreaders: 8.43sec (both sig.increase of20%)

Bothgroupsmade theleast er-rors in themasked,fast-pacecondition.Sig. dif-ferenceonly forpoorreader.

In fast pace bothgroups gainedon comp. forpoor readersmore marked.

NA

Breznitz(1997)Study 2

Dyslexicreaders(age 9.3)

23:18 boys5 girls

1 session Reading attwo rates:self-paced, fastpaced

PassageRate

3 parallelsets ofmaterials

0.1 letters persecond (sig.)

Decreasein 7.9 er-rors perpassage(sig.)

Recall increasedin 8% (sig.)

NA

Note. Most of these studies are from experimental studies; intervention was done in a 1:1 setting, except Rashotte and Torgesen (1985).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 19: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

brief, experimental explorations of what different versions of repeating readingmethods could do to facilitate accuracy and rate gains in oral reading. Several of thestudies measured comprehension, but in ways that varied widely from study tostudy. It is not our intention to criticize the goals of these studies, but to indicate theneed for more intensive studies that allow a more meaningful evaluation of compre-hension and transfer.

Developmental Level of Reading SubskillsAddressed by Interventions

The second general need that can be inferred from Table 1 is that the majority of theexperimental studies focus on passage-level reading with no attention to lexi-cal-level, much less sublexical-level, instruction to increase rate. This means thatfluency was being addressed late in its development at a time when only a few un-derlying processes could be addressed (e.g., prosody). The few fluency studies thatdid emphasize increased single-word rate showed mixed results for comprehension(e.g., Tan & Nicholson, 1997, found gains). Levy is one of the few researchers toconduct systematic, experimental rate studies on sublexical levels, although hermajor interest in these studies was in increasing accuracy and rate of word recogni-tion, not comprehension (Levy, in press; Levy, Bourassa, & Horn, 1999). Interest-ingly, in a recent study Levy et al. (1999) found little difference between sin-gle-word and connected-text repeated reading practice on the rate of reading.Torgesen et al. (in press) concluded from studies like these that “the primary locusof the repeated reading effect is on individual word reading efficiency.” Torgesenet al. (in press) argued that this sight vocabulary is key to closing the large gap be-tween the words read by average or good readers—estimated by Nagy and Ander-son (1984) to be 1 million words a year—and those read by less skilled readers, esti-mated at 100,000 words.

Several questions emerge from such a conclusion: first, whether an emphasissolely on word-level efficiency is sufficient to change comprehension; second,whether increases in word-reading efficiency are enough to build a sufficientlylarge sight vocabulary. As seen in Table 1, there are real gains that are possible inoral reading accuracy and rate from various repeated reading methods at both theconnected-text and word levels. It appears reasonable to us that such gains inword-reading rate—if accumulated over time within more intensive interventionprograms—could result in an expanded working sight vocabulary for children.Whether there are concomitant gains in comprehension remains as unresolved forthis summary as for Meyer and Felton’s (1999).

These kinds of questions and the enormity of the existing vocabulary discrep-ancy between good and impaired readers have led some researchers, includingTorgesen et al. (in press), to conclude that intervention efforts are best invested inthe prevention of such a gap. Many, including us, are in agreement concerning the

228 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 20: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

need for developmental prevention models that address the growth of accuracyand fluency before fluency problems ever have an opportunity to develop. AsTorgesen et al. (in press) and Kame’enui et al. (in press) have begun to demon-strate, there are impressive ways we can prevent an enormous number of at-riskchildren from developing later reading and reading fluency problems.

Despite all our best prevention efforts, however, some children—for variouswell-discussed reasons (see Wolf & Bowers, 1999, 2000)—will continue to de-velop serious reading fluency and comprehension problems. The fluency needs ofmost of these children, we believe, will be insufficiently addressed by most of theexperimental interventions described in Table 1. These studies address only the fi-nal levels of fluency development and few of the components underlying it.

Levels of Subskills and Component Processesof Reading Fluency Instruction

The most difficult question in this article concerns what components that fluencyintervention should emphasize. The most difficult implication of our view of read-ing fluency is that each component and each level of subskills in reading should beaddressed in fluency intervention. Following connectionist principles (see Adams,1990; Foorman, 1994; Seidenberg & McClelland, 1989), we believe that explicitinstruction is necessary to link phonological, orthographic, semantic, and morpho-logical processes to sublexical and word-level subskills (see Berninger et al., inpress). As seen in Table 1, only a very small number of intervention programs in-clude any systematic attention to the explicit linking of phonological, orthographic,and semantic processes in the acquisition of reading (see discussion of DecodingPilot Program and RAVE-O program in Meyer & Felton, 1999). Furthermore, webelieve that systematic instruction should be directed to accuracy and then to rate ateach developmental level of the acquisition of reading subskills—that is, at thelevel of the phoneme, grapheme, letter, letter pattern (orthographic chunk), word,phrase and sentence, and passage.

On the surface, this is a great deal to ask. In actuality it is asking that each level inthe teaching of reading incorporate both an accuracy and a rate of processing compo-nent. For example, following Ehri’s (1998) framework, we believe that instructionfor fluency development should begin with an emphasis on the accuracy of underly-ing representations (i.e., phonological, orthographic, semantic, and morphological)for each level up to the word level. In the second phase the focus should be on facili-tating the rate of processing in these same lower level processes and subskills, untilthey become automatic and can be orally read within the context of connected text.In what we refer to as the fluency outcome stage (the equivalent of Ehri’s speedphase), instruction would emphasize attainment of an efficient reading speed withgood comprehension of passage-level connected text (see Biemiller, 1977; Chall,1983; Doehring, 1976, for average reading rates at different ages).

READING FLUENCY 229

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 21: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

In the last section of this article we discuss an experimental fluency programbased on the developmental, componential view of reading fluency articulated inthis article. This program represents our first efforts to squarely confront the com-plexity and developmental changes in fluency development. We do not conceptu-alize this program as the only solution to the questions raised in this article. Rather,we see it as an evolving approach that will be empirically tested and refined as ourcollective understanding of fluency increases.

TOWARD A COMPREHENSIVE APPROACH TOFLUENCY INTERVENTION: THE RAVE-O

PROGRAM PRINCIPLES

Following concepts outlined in the last section, the RAVE-O program (Retrieval,Automaticity, Vocabulary, Engagement, and Orthography; see complete descrip-tion in Wolf, Miller, & Donnelly, 2000) was designed as a comprehensive, flu-ency-based intervention with three major goals: (a) accuracy and automaticity inthe components and subskills underlying reading; (b) fluency in word identifica-tion, word attack, connected text, and comprehension; and (c) a transformed atti-tude in children with reading disabilities toward written language. As such, the pro-gram is one example of a developmental, component-based approach to fluencydevelopment. The program emerged out of theoretical-based knowledge aboutdyslexic children whose particular deficits go beyond phonological-based deficits.The RAVE-O program was specifically designed to address the needs for increasedrate of processing and reading fluency among two well-represented subtypes ofchildren with reading disabilities (Wolf & Bowers, 1999).

RAVE-O is aimed at increasing processing speed both in underlying compo-nents (i.e., visual and auditory recognition, orthographic pattern recognition, lexi-cal retrieval, and semantic activation processes) and in three reading outcomebehaviors (i.e., word identification, word attack, and comprehension). Drawing onresearch by Doehring (1976) and connectionist models of the reading process (Ad-ams, 1990; Caramazza, 1997; Foorman, 1994), RAVE-O simultaneously ad-dresses the need for automaticity in phonological, orthographic, and semanticsystems and the importance of teaching explicit connections among these threesystems.

PROGRAM IN PRACTICE

Goals 1 and 2

The program is to be taught only in combination with a program that teaches sys-tematic, phonological analysis and blending. Children are taught a group of core

230 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 22: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

words each week that exemplify critical phonological, orthographic, and semanticprinciples. Each core word is chosen on the basis of (a) shared phonemes with thephonological-treatment program, (b) sequenced orthographic patterns, and (c) se-mantic richness (e.g., each core word has at least three different meanings). First,the multiple meanings of core words are introduced within their varied possible se-mantic contexts. Second, children are directly taught to connect the phoneme anal-ysis skills (taught in the phonological program) with the trained orthographic pat-terns in RAVE-O. For example, children are taught individual phonemes in thephonological program (like a, t, and m) and orthographic chunks with the samephonemes in RAVE-O (e.g., at and am, along with their word families).

A central program emphasis is on rapid recognition and practice of the most fre-quent orthographic letter patterns in English. A special set of computerized games(see Speed Wizards; Wolf & Goodman, 1996) was designed both to allow formaximal practice and to increase the speed of orthographic pattern recognition(i.e., onset and rime) in a fun fashion. In addition, the format of the games was de-signed to enhance the speed and accuracy of the multiple underlying componentslike visual scanning, auditory discrimination at the phoneme and phoneme clusterlevels, and letter and word identification (see more complete description in Wolf,Miller, & Donnelly, 2000).

Another key emphasis in the program involves the direct integration of vocabu-lary development and increased lexical retrieval skills. The dual emphasis on vo-cabulary and retrieval is based on earlier work in vocabulary development thatsuggested that one retrieves fastest what one knows best (see Beck, Perfetti, &McKeown, 1982; German, 1992; Kame’enui, Dixon, & Carnine, 1987; Wolf &Segal, 1999). Vocabulary growth is conceptualized as essential both to rapid re-trieval (in oral and written language) and to improved comprehension, an ultimategoal in the program.

The combination of the vocabulary and retrieval areas also helps address sev-eral clinical and theoretical issues in research on children with reading disabilities.First, in the clinical domain, although many dyslexic children begin with adequateto superb vocabularies, their vocabulary falls behind the level of average readerswho are reading increasingly sophisticated texts with words unavailable in oraldiscourse. This is a key part of the sight word vocabulary discrepancy discussed byTorgesen et al. (in press) and Nagy and Anderson (1984). Second, although a childwith reading disabilities may well know a given word, our work has demonstratedthat many dyslexic readers often cannot retrieve known words (Wolf &Goodglass, 1986; Wolf & Obregon, 1992). Third, whether it is an antecedent or aconsequence of reading and fluency problems, or a combination of the two, somechildren with reading disabilities appear to have a less flexible set toward deter-mining the often multiple meanings of words. They simply may not be able to allo-cate more time to processing more than one meaning to a known word, thusaffecting later comprehension development.

READING FLUENCY 231

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 23: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

RAVE-O’s daily structure combats flagging vocabulary development,word-retrieval problems, and inflexibility in word usage by teaching an imagina-tive set toward language from the start. Every core word in the program with itsvaried meanings and syntactic uses is practiced every day in different ways. Forexample, “Word-webs” are created for many of the words as a teaching tool to il-lustrate the principle “If you know one word, you know a hundred!”

To enhance the speed and accuracy of lexical retrieval, four metacognitivestrategies for word retrieval—called Sam Spade Strategies—are taught. Eachstrategy incorporates some of the phonological, orthographic, and semantic princi-ples of the program (see earlier RAVE program in Wolf & Segal, 1999). For exam-ple, children are taught during “Tip of the Tongue” occurrences to try to rememberan onset, rime, or semantic associate of the missed word.

Sam Spade also appears as a character in the series of timed and untimed com-prehension stories (e.g., Minute Mysteries). These stories accompany each weekof RAVE-O and directly address fluency in comprehension in several ways. Thecontrolled vocabulary in the stories incorporates the week’s particular ortho-graphic patterns and emphasizes the multiple meanings and semantically relatedwords of the week’s core words. In addition, the stories provide a good vehicle forrepeated reading practice, which facilitates fluency in connected text. Thus, theMinute Mysteries facilitate fluency in phonological, orthographic, and semanticsystems at the same time that they build comprehension skills.

The Third Goal: Engagement With Language

An important, implicit goal of the RAVE-O program concerns a whole new cogni-tive–affective set toward language use and is specifically geared toward childrenwho have begun to feel disenfranchised from their own language. As Chukovsky(1963) once stated in another context, many of our children have begun to feel “likestrangers in the realm of their own language” (p. 9). RAVE-O is built around the no-tion that a combination of systematic goals, incremental successes, and whimsicalimaginative activities go hand in hand with building a new attitude to language,reading, and learning in general. From colorfully animated computer games towebs of words, children learn they can master this task of reading. The motivationalcomponent of RAVE-O is one of its real strengths for both teacher and learner, par-ticularly in overcrowded urban schools.

PRELIMINARY ANALYSES

Preliminary data on the RAVE-O program are now available for 200 second- andthird-grade children with severe reading impairments who participated in the large

232 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 24: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

National Institute for Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) interven-tion project by Morris, Lovett, and Wolf (1995). In this project (described in Wolf,Miller, & Donnelly, 2000), children were selected if they met either low achieve-ment or ability–achievement (regressions corrected) discrepancy criteria for read-ing disability on the Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests (Woodcock, 1987). Allchildren in the program were taught in small-group (4:1), pullout settings for 70sessions, with 30 min of RAVE-O and 30 min of the phonological program (seeLovett et al., 2000, for description of Phonological Analysis and Blending).

The preliminary data indicate significant gains in word attack, word identifica-tion, oral reading rate and accuracy, and passage comprehension. Although thesedata will be presented in full report in a subsequent study, the preliminary analysesoffer our first hint that a developmental–componential approach to fluency inter-vention can change the reading rate of severely impaired readers on standardizedoral reading measures and, most important, increase their comprehension on stan-dardized measures. The findings, therefore, provide one promising piece of evi-dence that severe processing speed deficits in individuals with reading disabilitiesmay be amenable to treatment. That said, these data do not yet represent an empiri-cal validation of the developmental–componential view to reading fluency that weoffer in this article; rather, they offer a starting place for further efforts.

SUMMARY

There are many unresolved theoretical and applied questions in research on readingfluency and fluency-based deficits in individuals with reading disabilities. Promi-nent among the theoretical questions are issues of definition and clarification oftime-related terms. There is no consensus concerning how we use such basic termsas rate, automaticity, speed of processing, temporal processing, dynamic process-ing, or precise timing, much less fluency. Yet, as illustrated here, our definitionshave critical implications for how and who we diagnose and for how we constructand evaluate intervention. We argue strongly for a definition of fluency that is de-velopmental- and component-based, where rate and speed are the characteristics ofthe components and subskills of reading, and where accuracy and automaticity areassessable outcome stages of reading and reading fluency. Within this admittedlycomplex view, there are large lacunae in our understanding of how and how muchindividual component processes (e.g., orthographic, semantic, morphosyntactic)contribute to fluency and how fluency and comprehension influence each other.There are also frank omissions even in this broad view of fluency concerning thecontributions of basic processes like general cognitive speed (see Breznitz, inpress; Carver, 1991; Kail, 1992). There remains much to understand.

At such a moment it is good to reflect on our own recent history as a field.Thirty years ago, powerful questions about the role of phonological processes in

READING FLUENCY 233

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 25: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

reading led to a systematic body of research on phonological-core deficits, diag-nostic measures, and phonological-based treatments. In a similar vein, we believethat the exploration of the basic structure and development of reading fluency willlead to a heightened understanding of fluency-based reading problems in children,their assessment, and their amelioration. The articles in this special issue are a partof that exploration.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We acknowledge the support of National Institute for Child Health and Human De-velopment Grant OD30970–01A1 and the Haan Foundation for Children duringongoing intervention research. We thank present and past members of the Centerfor Reading and Language Research: Heidi Bally, Kathleen Biddle, TheresaDeeney, Katharine Donnelly, Wendy Galante, Calvin Gidney, Julie Jeffery, TerryJoffe, Cynthia Krug, Lynne Miller, Mateo Obregon, Beth O’Brien, AlyssaO’Rourke, and Maya Rom. The work cited in this article could never have beendone without their efforts. We also are indebted to Pat Bowers, Zvia Breznitz,Robin Morris, and Maureen Lovett for their intellectual collaborations on this topicover many years, and to our colleagues whose contributions to the National Dys-lexia Research Foundation’s conference—Time, Fluency, and Dyslexia—are citedthroughout this article.

REFERENCES

Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MITPress.

Allington, R. L. (1983). Fluency: The neglected reading goal in reading instruction. The ReadingTeacher, 36, 556–561.

Badian, N. (1996, November). Dyslexia: Does it exist? Dyslexia, garden-variety poor reading, and theDouble-Deficit Hypothesis. Paper presented at the Orton Dyslexia Society, Boston.

Beck, I. L., Perfetti, C. A., & McKeown, M. G. (1982). Effects of long-term vocabulary instruction onlexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 506–521.

Berninger, V. W. (in press). Specific reading and writing disabilities in young children: Assessment,prevention, and intervention (2nd ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic.

Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Billingsley, F., & Nagy, W. (in press). Processes underlying timing andfluency of reading: Efficiency, automaticity, coordination, and morphological awareness. In M.Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. York Press.

Biemiller, A. (1977). Relationship between oral reading rates for letters, words, and simple text in thedevelopment of reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 13, 223–253.

Breznitz, Z. (1997). Effects of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dyslexic readers.Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 289–297.

234 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 26: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Breznitz, Z. (in press). The role of inter-modality temporal features of speed of information processingin asynchrony between visual-orthographic and auditory-phonological processing. In M. Wolf(Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. York Press.

Breznitz, Z., & Share, D. L. (1992). Effects of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dys-lexic readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 289–297.

Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? CognitiveNeuropsychology, 14, 177–209.

Carver, R. P. (1991). Using letter-naming speed to diagnose reading disability. Remedial and SpecialEducation, 12(5), 33–43.

Carver, R. P. (1997). Reading for one second, one minute, or one year from the perspective of raudingtheory. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 3–43.

Cattell, M. (1886). The time it takes to see and name objects. Mind, 2, 63–85.Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.Chukovsky, K. (1963). From two to five. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Compton, D. L., Chayna, J. D., DeFries, D. J., Gayan, J., & Olson, R. K. (in press). Genetic and environ-

mental influences on reading and RAN: An overview of results from the Colorado twin study. In M.Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. York Press.

Dahl, P. (1974). An experimental program for teaching high speed word recognition and comprehensionskills (Final Rep. Project No. 3–1154). Washington, DC: National Institute of Education.

Doehring, D. G. (1976). Acquisition of rapid reading responses. Monograph of the Society for Researchin Child Development, 165(2).

Dowhower, S. L. (1994). Repeated reading revisited: Research into practice. Reading and WritingQuarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 10, 343–358.

Ehri, L. (1998). Grapheme–phoneme knowledge is essential for learning to read words in English. In J.Metsala & L. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning reading (pp. 3–40). Mahwah, NJ: Law-rence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Farmer, M. E., & Klein, R. M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia:A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 460–493.

Foorman, B. R. (1994). The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental is-sues. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer.

Fuchs, L. S., Fuchs, D., & Maxwell, L. (1988). The validity of informal reading comprehension mea-sures. Remedial and Special Education, 9(2), 20–29.

German, D. J. (1992). Word finding intervention for children and adolescents. Topics in Learning Dis-orders, 13(1), 33–50.

Gough, P. B. (1972). One second of reading. In J. F. Kavanaugh & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.), The relation-ship between speech and hearing (pp. 331–358). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Haynes, C. (1994). Differences between name recognition and name retrieval abilities in relationship toreading performance. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA.

Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behaviour. New York: Wiley.Hudson, R., Mercer, C. D., & Lane, H. (2000). Exploring reading fluency: A paradigmatic overview.

Unpublished manuscript, University of Florida, Gainesville.Huey, S. E. (1968). The psychology and pedagogy of reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original

work published 1908)Jackson, M. D., & McClelland, J. L. (1979). Processing determinants of reading speed. Journal of Ex-

perimental Psychology: General, 108, 151–181.Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., Espin, C., van den Broek, P., & Deno, S. L. (2000, February). Effects of task

format and performance dimension on word reading measures: Criterion validity, sensitivity to im-pairment, and context facilitation. Paper presented at Pacific Coast Research Conference, SanDiego, CA.

READING FLUENCY 235

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 27: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Johnston, R. S., & Anderson, M. (1998). Memory span, naming speed, and memory strategies in poorand normal readers. Memory, 6, 143–163.

Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood and adolescence. Psy-chological Bulletin, 109, 490–501.

Kail, R. (1992). Processing speed, speech rate, and memory. Developmental Psychology, 28, 899–904.Kame’enui, E. J., Dixon, R. C., & Carnine, D. W. (1987). Issues in the design of vocabulary instruction.

In M. G. McKeown & M. E. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of vocabulary acquisition (pp. 129–145).Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Kame’enui, E. J., Simmons, D. C., Good, R. H., & Harn, B. A. (in press). The use of fluency-based mea-sures in early identification and evaluation of intervention efficacy in schools. In M. Wolf (Ed.),Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. York Press.

LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading.Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293–323.

Lemoine, H. E., Levy, B. A., & Hutchinson, A. (1993). Increasing the naming speed of poor readers:Representations formed across repetitions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55,297–328.

Levy, B. A. (in press). Moving the bottom: Improving reading fluency. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, flu-ency, and the brain. York Press.

Levy, B. A., Abello, B., & Lysynchuk, L. (1997). Transfer from word training to reading in context:Gains in reading fluency and comprehension. Learning Disability Quarterly, 20, 173–188.

Levy, B. A., Bourassa, D. C., & Horn, C. (1999). Fast and slow namers: Benefits of segmentation andwhole word training. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73, 115–138.

Levy, B. A., Nicholas, A., & Kohen, D. (1993). Repeated readings: Process benefits for good and poorreaders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56, 303–327.

Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instant theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492–527.Logan, G. D. (1997). Automaticity and reading: Perspectives from the instance theory of automation.

Reading and Writing Quarterly, 13, 146.Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of

normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development, 58, 234–260.Lovett, M. W., Steinbach, K. A., & Frijters, J. C. (2000). Remediating the core deficits of developmental

reading disability: A double-deficit perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 334–358.Lyon, G. R., & Moats, L. C. (1997). Critical conceptual and methodological considerations in reading

intervention research. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 578–588.Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and ortho-

graphic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.Marcus, D. (1997). An investigation of the relationship of naming speed, processing speed, and reading

in young, reading-impaired children. Unpublished master’s thesis, Tufts University, Boston.McCormick, C., & Samuels, S. J. (1979). Word recognition by second graders: The unit of perception

and interrelationships among accuracy, latency, and comprehension. Journal of Reading Behaviour,11, 107–118.

Meyer, M. S., & Felton, R. H. (1999). Repeated reading to enhance fluency: Old approaches and new di-rections. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 283–306.

Morris, R., Lovett, M., & Wolf, M. (1995). Treatment of developmental reading disabilities. NationalInstitute for Child Health and Human Development Grant proposal #1R55HD/OD30970–01A1.

Nagy, W., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English? Reading Re-search Quarterly, 19, 304–330.

National Reading Panel. (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scien-tific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC:National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

236 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 28: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Nicholson, R., & Fawcett, A. J. (1994). Reaction times and dyslexia. Quarterly Journal of ExperimentalPsychology, 47, 29–48.

Norman, D. A. (1968). Toward a theory of memory and attention. Psychological Review, 75,522–536.

O’Shea, L. J., Sindelar, P. T., & O’Shea, D. J. (1985). The effects of repeated readings and attentionalcues on reading fluency and comprehension. Journal of Reading Behavior, 17, 129–142.

Perfetti, C. (1977). Language comprehension and fast decoding: Some psycholinguistic prerequisitesfor skilled reading comprehension. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.), Cognition, curriculum, and comprehen-sion (pp. 20–41). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford Press.Posner, M. I., Lewis, J. L., & Conrad, C. (1972). Component processes in reading: A performance analy-

sis. In J. F. Kavanaugh & I. G. Mattingly (Eds.), Language by ear and by eye (pp. 159–192). Cam-bridge, MA: MIT Press.

Rashotte, C., & Torgesen, J. (1985). Repeated reading and reading fluency in learning disabled children.Reading Research Quarterly, 20, 180–188.

Samuels, S. J. (1985). Automaticity and repeated reading. Lexington, MA: Lexington.Schreiber, P. A. (1980). On the acquisition of reading fluency. Journal of Reading Behavior, 12,

177–186.Segal, D., & Wolf, M. (1993). Cognitive, linguistic, and developmental perspectives on learning disor-

ders. Boston: Little, Brown.Seidenberg, M., & McClelland, J. (1989). A distributed developmental model of word recognition and

naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.Shankweiler, D., & Crain, S. (1986). Language mechanisms and reading disorder: A modular approach.

Cognition, 24, 139–168.Shinn, M. R., Good, R. H., Knutson, N., Tilly, W. D., & Collins, V. L. (1992). Curriculum based mea-

surement of oral reading fluency: A confirmatory analysis of its relation to reading. School Psychol-ogy Review, 21, 459–479.

Spring, C., & Capps, C. (1974). Encoding speed, rehearsal, and probed recall of dyslexic boys. Journalof Educational Psychology, 66, 780–786.

Stahl, S. A., Heubach, K., & Cramond, B. (1997). Fluency-oriented reading instruction (Reading Re-search 79). Ahtens, GA: National Reading Research Center.

Stein, J. (in press). The neurobiology of reading difficulties. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and thebrain. York Press.

Stoddard, K., Valcante, G., Sindelar, P., O’Shea, L., & Algozzine, B. (1993). Increasing reading rate andcomprehension: The effects of repeated readings, sentence segmentation, and intonation training.Reading Research and Instruction, 32, 53–65.

Tallal, P., Miller, S., & Fitch, R. H. (1993). Neurobiological basis of speech: A case for the preeminenceof temporal processing. In P. Tallal, A. Galaburda, & C. Von Euler (Eds.), Temporal processing inthe nervous system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences (Vol. 682, pp. 7–47). New York:New York Academy of Sciences.

Tan, A., & Nicholson, T. (1997). Flash cards revisited: Training poor readers to read words faster im-proves their comprehension of text. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 276–288.

Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C., & Alexander, A. (in press). The prevention and remediation of reading flu-ency problems. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. York Press.

Torgesen, J., Rashotte, C., & Wagner, R. (1997, November). Research on instructional interventions forchildren with reading disabilities. Paper presented at the International Dyslexia Association Confer-ence, Chicago.

Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., Rashotte, C., Rose, E., Lindamood, P., Conway, T., & Garven, C. (1999). Pre-venting reading failure in young children with phonological processing disabilities: Group and indi-vidual responses to instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 1–15.

READING FLUENCY 237

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2

Page 29: Reading Fluency and Its Intervention - Tufts Universityase.tufts.edu/crlr/documents/2001SSR-ReadingFluency.pdf · highly successful and insufficient in dealing with the heterogeneity

Waber, D. P. (in press). Aberrations of timing in children with impaired reading: Cause or effect? In M.Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain. York Press.

Wiederholt, J. L., & Bryant, B. (1992). Gray Oral Reading Test (GORT–3). Austin, TX: PRO-ED.Wolf, M. (1982). The word-retrieval process and reading in children and aphasics. In K. Nelson (Ed.),

Children’s language (pp. 437–486). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading

Research Quarterly, 26, 123–141.Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (1999). The “Double-Deficit Hypothesis” for the developmental dyslexias.

Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 1–24.Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. (2000). The question of naming-speed deficits in developmental reading dis-

abilities: An introduction to the Double-Deficit Hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33,322–324.

Wolf, M., Bowers, P., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptualreview. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.

Wolf, M., & Goodglass, H. (1986). Dyslexia, dysnomia, and lexical retrieval. Brain and Language, 28,154–168.

Wolf, M., & Goodman, G. (1996). Speed Wizards [Computerized reading program]. Tufts University,Boston, and Rochester Institute of Technology, Rochester, NY.

Wolf, M., Miller, L., & Donnelly, K. (2000). The Retrieval, Automaticity, Vocabulary Elaboration, Or-thography (RAVE-O): A comprehensive fluency-based reading intervention program. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 33, 375–386.

Wolf, M., & Obregon, M. (1992). Early naming deficits, developmental dyslexia, and a specific deficithypothesis. Brain and Language, 42, 219–247.

Wolf, M., & Segal, D. (1999). Retrieval-rate, accuracy and vocabulary elaboration (RAVE) in read-ing-impaired children: A pilot intervention program. Dyslexia, 5, 1–27.

Wolff, P. H. (2000). Impaired temporal resolution in developmental dyslexia. Annals of the New YorkAcademy of Sciences, 682, 87–103.

Wood, F., Flowers, L., & Grigorenko, E. (in press). On the functional neuroanatomy of fluency, or whywalking is just as important to reading as talking is. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and thebrain. York Press.

Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests. Circle Pines, MN: American GuidanceService.

Young, A., & Bowers, P. G. (1995). Individual differences and text difficulty determinants of readingfluency and expressiveness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 60, 428–454.

Young, A. R., Bowers, P. G., & MacKinnon, G. E. (1996). Effects of prosodic modeling and repeatedreading on poor readers’ fluency and comprehension. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 59–84.

Manuscript received November 9, 2000Final revision received December 4, 2000Accepted December 4, 2000

238 WOLF AND KATZIR-COHEN

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tuf

ts U

nive

rsity

] at

10:

17 1

6 M

arch

201

2