reading comprehsnion in adolescent bilinguals learning … · two studies examined reading...

132
READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING ENGLISH AS A SECOND LANGUAGE by Adrian Dominic Kenneth Pasquarella A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development Ontario Institute for Studies in Education University of Toronto © Copyright by Adrian Dominic Kenneth Pasquarella (2014)

Upload: others

Post on 21-Mar-2020

3 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING ENGLISH AS A SECOND LANGUAGE

by

Adrian Dominic Kenneth Pasquarella

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Graduate Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development Ontario Institute for Studies in Education

University of Toronto

© Copyright by Adrian Dominic Kenneth Pasquarella (2014)

Page 2: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING ENGLISH AS A SECOND LANGUAGE

Adrian Dominic Kenneth Pasquarella Doctor of Philosophy

Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development University of Toronto

2014

Abstract

Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals

learning English as a second language (L2). The first study examined predictors of RC

among Chinese-English and Spanish-English bilinguals. The primary objective was to

identify universal and language specific aspects of RC in both the first language (L1) and

L2, across different groups of bilinguals. Measures of RC, vocabulary, multiple measure

of morphological awareness (e.g., derivational, compound), syntactic awareness, and

word-level reading were administered in the L1 and L2 to 117 Chinese-English and 71

Spanish-English bilinguals. Measures of non-verbal reasoning and working memory

were administered in English. Results indicated that vocabulary and morphological

awareness were direct predictors of RC across all models, suggesting these components

are universal aspects of RC. Indirect relationships between morphological awareness and

RC were mediated through vocabulary for all models. Interestingly, the patterns of

which measure of morphological awareness acted as a predictor of RC varied

systematically across languages, suggesting some language specific aspects between finer

grain morphological awareness skills and comprehension. An interesting interplay

between universal and language specific aspects of comprehension for diverse groups of

bilingual adolescents is discussed. The second study examined within- and cross-

ii

Page 3: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

language predictors of RC among groups of Chinese-English bilinguals in different

language learning environments. Fifty seven bilinguals learning English as a Foreign

Language (EFL) in China, 60 recent immigrants, and 57 long-term immigrants to Canada

completed parallel measure of word-level reading, vocabulary and RC in Chinese and

English. Results demonstrated that patterns of within- and cross-language predictors of

comprehension varied across groups, and for L1 or L2 comprehension. L1 and L2 RC

was uniquely associated across languages for the EFL bilinguals and recent immigrants,

but was not related for the long-term immigrants. Furthermore, L1 vocabulary was

uniquely associated with L2 comprehension for the EFL bilinguals only. Finally, only

within-language vocabulary was related to RC, in either Chinese or English, for the long-

term immigrants. The results demonstrate overlap between L1 and L2 reading skills in

the early stages of L2 acquisition. The influence of the language learning environment

on changing patterns of within- and cross-language relationships is discussed.

iii

Page 4: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Acknowledgements

First and foremost, I would like to thank my wonderful family for their love and support

over the years. To my wife, Courtney, you are my best friend and soul mate. You have always

shown me immense support, encouragement, and helped me enjoy life to the fullest. I know

together we can accomplish anything. To Eve, my beautiful daughter, you have brought so much

joy and happiness into my life. Your smile and laughter are sources of inspiration and

motivation. I am excited to watch you grow and experience all of life’s pleasures with you.

Thank you for making my life so rich and full of joy. To my Mom, Dad and brother, Anton, you

have given me so much support and helped me grow into the person I am today. I am thankful

for all your love, help, and a lifetime of great memories. I love you all more than words can

express.

I would also like to thank my advisor, Becky Chen-Bumgardner. You have given me

amazing opportunities and helped me grow as a researcher and scholar. My current and future

success is due to the effort, time, and hard work you have put in to help me develop and perfect

my skills. Most importantly, your friendship has made working with you an absolute pleasure. I

am so grateful to have met you and to be able to continue to collaborate. To my past advisor and

mentor, Alexandra Gottardo, you have been a constant source of support and motivation to help

me excel as a scholar but also as a person. You prepared me to be an independent researcher,

instilled a love for literacy, and the knowledge about how to help people through my passion. I

will be forever grateful for everything you have done, and continue to do, for me. To Esther

Geva, another excellent mentor, I am so grateful to have had the opportunity to get to know you

and to gain the valuable experience of collaborating with you. You have helped me create many

important opportunities that have greatly expanded the depth and breadth of my research skills

Page 5: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

and potential. Most importantly, all my mentors have instilled the confidence to explore new

and exciting ideas.

Additionally, there were a number of fantastic collaborators who were instrumental in the

development and execution of my dissertation research. Jie Zhang, Vedran Dronjic, Gloria

Ramirez, Karen Ay-Yueng, and Jin Xue, I owe all of you a great deal of thanks and admiration

for your support throughout the project in terms of measurement development, logistics, and data

collection. You were all a fantastic team to work with. Most importantly, thanks for being a

sounding board and providing excellent advice to help refine and specify the goals and intentions

of my dissertation research. Without you, I would not have been able to complete such an

intricate and multi-facetted study.

There were also a number of graduate and undergraduate students who helped with

recruitment, data collection, and data entry. A lot of hard work was done by an excellent group

of immerging scholars. I am very grateful for your help throughout the project. Furthermore, I

give thanks to all the schools in the Toronto Catholic District School board and the Toronto

District School Board for helping me promote the project and providing resources to complete it.

Finally, thank you to all the participants who devoted many hours to complete the study. There

was a coordinated effort among many people to complete my dissertation research, and I am

incredibly thankful to everyone who helped me accomplish a major life goal.

v

Page 6: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table of Contents

Abstract………………………………………………………………………………………. ii

Acknowledgements…………………………………………………………………………... iv

Lists of Tables...…………………………………………………………………………..….. ix

Lists of Figures.………………………………………………………………………………. x

Chapter 1: Introduction……………….…………………………………………………….… 1

Chapter 2: Study 1 - Models of RC in Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals: Universal and Language Specific Aspects of Comprehension in the L1 and L2.... 4

Theoretical Models of RC ……..…….……………………………..……………. 5

Typological characteristics of English, Spanish, and Chinese….……………..…. 7

Orthography and phonology………………………………………………….. 7

Morphology…………………………………………………………………... 8

Syntax……………………………..………………………………………….. 9

Oral Language Proficiency and RC………………………………………………. 10

Vocabulary……………………………………………………………………. 10

Morphological Awareness……………………………………………………. 12

Syntactic Awareness………………………………………………………….. 16

Objectives……..………………….………………………………………………. 20

Chapter 3: Method..……………………………………………………….………………….. 21

Participants..………….…………………………………….………………... 21

Procedures…………...…………………………………….………………… 22

Measures…………………………………………………………………….. 23

RC..…….……………..…….…………………………………………... 23

Vocabulary……………………………………………………………… 23

Morphological awareness……………….………..…………………….. 24

Compound awareness……………………………………..………... 24

Derivational awareness……………………………..………………. 25

Derivational production………………………………………… 25

Derivational structure………………………………………....... 25

Chinese homographic awareness………………...………………... 25

Syntactic awareness……………………..……………………………… 26

vi

Page 7: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Word reading and decoding………...…………………………………... 27

Nonverbal reasoning…………………..……………………………….. 28

Working memory……………………………………………………..... 28

Demographic questionnaire…………………………………………...... 29

Measurement comparability across languages………………………………. 29

Chapter 4: Results..……………………….……………………………………………..……. 30

Mean scores and group differences……..…………………………………… 30

Correlations…………………………………………………………….……. 31

Specification of Structural Models across Languages…………………....…. 32

Structural Equation Models in English.……………………………….. 35

Structural Equation Models in the L1……………………….………… 37

Chapter 5: Discussion………………………………………...………………………………. 40

The Universal Importance of Vocabulary and Morphological Awareness

for Comprehension.…………………………………………………………. 41

Language Specific Influences on Comprehension and Vocabulary………… 45

Limitations and Future Directions…………………………………………… 48

Conclusion and Implications………….…………………….……………….. 49

Chapter 6: Study 2 - Cross-language Relationships of L1 and L2 RC: A Cross-cultural Comparison of Learning to Read in Canada and China……………………….…. 52

Defining Different Language Learning Environments……………………… 53

Cross-language Relationships between L1 and L2 Reading Skills...……….. 53

Within- and Cross-language Relationships of Word-level Reading…….…... 54

Within- and Cross-language Relationships of Vocabulary…...……………... 57

Cross-language Relationships of RC……………………............................... 58

Objectives..…………………………………………………………….......... 60

Chapter 7: Method….………………………………………………………………………… 61

Participants...………………………………………………………………....... 61

Measures……...………………….……………………………………………. 63

RC……..………………………………………………………………. 63

Vocabulary…………………………………………………………….. 63

Word-level reading…………………………………………………….. 64

Demographic questionnaire…………………………………………… 64

Procedures……………………………….……………………………….…… 64

vii

Page 8: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 8: Results………………………………………………………………………..…… 66

Descriptive Statistics and Group Differences….……………………………… 66

Correlations……………………………………....……………………………. 67

Hierarchical Linear Regressions and Commonalities Analyses………………. 68

Chapter 9: Discussion.………………………………………………………………………... 74

The Role of Word-level Reading Within and Across languages..…….………. 76

The Role of Vocabulary Within and Across Languages………………………. 78

The Role of RC Across Languages…………………………….…………….... 79

Conditions that Support Cross-language Relationships...……………………... 81

Limitations and Future Directions………………..……………………….…… 81

Implications………………………………………………………………….… 83

Chapter 10: General Conclusions…………………………………………………………….. 85

References……………………………………………………………………………………. 87

viii

Page 9: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Lists of Tables

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and Analysis of Variance of age, length of residence, English and L1 variables.....…………………………………………………..……. 102

Table 2. Intercorrelations among English variables for Chinese-English bilinguals and Spanish-English bilinguals……………………………………………..…………... 103

Table 3. Intercorrelations among Spanish and control measures…………………………….. 104

Table 4. Intercorrelations among Chinese and control measures…………………………….. 105

Table 5. Coefficients for structural equation model of English RC………………………….. 107

Table 6. Coefficients of bootstrapping for indirect effects on English RC.………………….. 108

Table 7. Comparison of fit statistics by constraining parameter estimates to be equal across groups……………………………………………………………….………..…….. 109

Table 8. Coefficients for structural equation model of first language RC……...……………. 111

Table 9. Coefficients of bootstrapping for indirect effects L1 RC…………………………… 112

Table 10. Descriptive statistics and Analysis of Variance for Chinese EFL learners, Recent Immigrants, and Long-term Immigrants for age, months in Canada, English and Chinese variables…..………………..………....………………………………….. 115

Table 11. Intercorrelations among Chinese and English variables separated by group..…….. 116

Table 12. Hierarchical linear regression predicting English RC, separate for EFL, Recent Immigrant, and Long-term Immigrant groups..………………………………….. 117

Table 13. Hierarchical linear regression predicting Chinese RC, separate for EFL, Recent Immigrant, and Long-term Immigrant groups……………….…………………….. 118

Table 14. Commonality analyses beta weights, structural coefficients, proportions of total, common and unique variance for EFL, Recent Immigrant and Long-term Immigrant groups when predicting English RC…….……………………………… 119

Table 15. Summary of unique and common variance for independent variables when predicting English RC...…………..……………………….……………………….. 120

Table 16. Commonality analyses beta weights, structural coefficients, proportions of total, common and unique variance for EFL, Recent Immigrant and Long-term Immigrant groups when predicting Chinese RC………………………………….... 121

Table 17. Summary of unique and common variance for independent variables when predicting L1 RC….……………………………………………..….……………… 122

ix

Page 10: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Lists of Figures Figure 1. Theoretical structural equation model of RC in English, Spanish and Chinese…… 106

Figure 2. Structural models of English RC showing direct and indirect effects…...………… 110

Figure 3. Structural model for Spanish RC (Model 3) showing direct and indirect effects….. 113

Figure 4. Structural model for Chinese RC (Model 4) showing direct and indirect effects …. 114

x

Page 11: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 1: Introduction

Reading comprehension (RC) is critical for school success as it acts as a gateway to

academic and cultural knowledge. RC enables self-directed learning, and contributes to the

development of the mental lexicon (Cunningham & Stanovich, 2001). Bilingual children and

adolescents often experience delays in developing oral language and RC proficiency when

compared to native speakers of English (August, Carlo, Dressler & Snow, 2005; August &

Shanahan, 2006; Carlo, August, Snow, Dressler, Lippman, Lively, & White, 2004; Farnia &

Geva, 2011; Pasquarella, Gottardo, & Grant, 2012; Verhallen & Schoonen, 1993). Gaps in oral

language skills, especially vocabulary, do not close even after many years of schooling in

English (August et al., 2005; Farnia & Geva, 2011; Verhoeven, 2000). Researchers claim that to

close the gap in literacy skills by the beginning of post-secondary education, bilingual students

must show exponential growth in language and literacy skills throughout high school (e.g., Carlo

et al., 2004; Pasquarella et al., 2012). Thus, it is crucial to understand the underpinnings of oral

language and RC development of bilingual students, and to find ways to enhance these skills.

However, virtually no systematic research has been conducted in these areas with adolescent

bilingual learners (August & Shanahan, 2006; Pasquarella et al., 2012).

Two interrelated studies are presented here. Both studies examined the underpinning

skills of RC in adolescent bilinguals. The overall focus of the two studies was on the

interrelationships among component skills of RC, as there is a dearth of empirical research on

aspects of RC in bilinguals especially for adolescent populations (August & Shanahan, 2006;

Kirby & Savage, 2008; Pasquarella, Gottardo, & Grant, 2012). Currently, little is known about

how models of RC based on monolingual and younger bilingual children apply to adolescent

bilinguals (Pasquarella et al., 2012). This study will establish models of RC for adolescent

1

Page 12: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

bilinguals with different first language backgrounds. These models will help us understand the

learning needs of heterogeneous groups of bilinguals. Creating a more refined understanding of

the interrelationships among predictors of RC for adolescent bilinguals can act as a foundation

for evidence-based instructional interventions.

The focus of Study 1 was to examine the within-language predictors of RC for bilinguals

who spoke Spanish and Chinese as their first language (L1), and English as their second

language (L2). These two groups of bilinguals were chosen because Spanish and Chinese are

represented by typologically different scripts, with different degrees of similarity to English.

Predictors of RC (e.g., vocabulary, morphological awareness, syntactic awareness, word reading)

were examined in English, Spanish, and Chinese to understand how the predictive patterns

varied across the three languages. A central goal of Study 1 was to establish universal and

language-specific aspects of RC in different languages and orthographies.

The focus of Study 2 was to examine the cross-language relationships between RC for

Chinese-English bilinguals. Comparisons were made across three groups: (1) Chinese-English

bilinguals learning English in China, (2) recent Chinese-English immigrants learning English in

Canada, and (3) long-term Chinese-English immigrants learning English in Canada. These

comparisons allowed for an exploration of patterns of within- and cross-language predictors of

RC in different language learning environments. Furthermore, this study examined whether

factors such as exposure to and use of English, inherent in the language learning environment,

had an impact on the cognitive-linguistic models of RC.

Together, the two studies attempted to establish comprehensive and refined models of RC

for adolescent bilinguals. An overarching goal was to outline the specific cognitive and

linguistic components necessary for L1 and L2 RC in adolescent bilinguals from various

2

Page 13: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

backgrounds. Study 1 identified predictors of RC in English, Spanish and Chinese. As an

extension to Study 1, Study 2 demonstrated that predictors of comprehension may vary

depending on aspects of the language-learning context (i.e., L1- or L2-dominant educational

system and culture). Based on the findings, these studies discuss how literacy instruction can be

adapted to suit the needs of adolescent bilinguals from different backgrounds.

3

Page 14: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 2: Study 1 - Models of RC in Spanish-English and Chinese-English Bilinguals:

Universal and Language Specific Aspects of Comprehension in the L1 and L2

A fundamental aspect of learning to read is the understanding that printed symbols

represent oral language. Across the world, different scripts have been used to symbolize

meaning and to transcribe and transmit knowledge, events, and information (Olson, 1996). The

goal of reading in any language is to form coherent understandings of connected text. In order to

achieve this goal, a child must develop many cognitive and linguistic skills. Different theoretical

models have been developed to understand the cognitive and linguistic components involved in

RC, which include the ability to read words, knowledge of word meanings, and general oral

language proficiency (e.g., Cain, Oakhill & Bryant, 2004; Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Hoover &

Gough, 1990; Perfetti, 2007; Perfetti & Hart, 2001, 2002). While an increasing amount of

empirical research has been conducted in different languages, such as Spanish and Chinese (e.g.,

Chen, Hao, Geva, Zhu, & Shu, 2009; González, 1997; González & Valle, 2000; Gottardo, Yan,

Siegel, & Wade-Woolley, 2001; Shu, Chen, Anderson, Wu, & Xuan, 2006), relatively few

studies have simultaneously compared RC skills across a bilingual’s L1 and L2 (see van

Gelderen, Schoonen, Stoel, Glooper, & Hulstijn, 2007 for an exception). It would be useful to

extend previous research by identifying similarities and differences in predictors of RC across

different languages. This study addressed this research gap by comparing models of L1 and L2

RC in bilinguals who speak Spanish or Chinese as their L1 and English as their L2. I chose to

focus on adolescence as it is an age range critical for reading development for bilinguals, but is

accompanied by little empirical research (August & Shanahan, 2006; Pasquarella et al., 2012).

In terms of immigration demographics and statistics, it is important to examine Spanish-English

and Chinese-English bilinguals as they represent large percentages of the bilinguals in North

4

Page 15: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

American schools and these populations continue to grow (Capps, Fix, Murray, Ost, Passel, &

Herwantoro, 2005; Loeffer, 2007; Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development,

2000).

The primary goal of this study was to identify similarities and differences in predictors of

RC in the L1 and the L2 for Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals. Identifying

similarities in predictors across Spanish, Chinese and English may reveal universal aspects

important for comprehension in different languages. At the same time, identifying differences in

predictors of RC across the three languages will outline language-specific aspects involved in

reading in different languages. An additional purpose of the study was to compare predictors of

English RC for Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals to outline similarities and

differences in the skills these groups of students use to comprehend English text. Eventually,

informed educational strategies that highlight these similarities and differences can be used to

tailor instructional practices to increase English reading proficiency in diverse groups of

adolescent bilinguals.

Theoretical Models of RC

To understand the process of comprehending written language, different, but related,

theories have been proposed. One such theory, The Simple View of Reading (SVR), has been

established as a valid and reliable model that outlines the cognitive and linguistic skills involved

in RC (Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990). According to the SVR, RC can be

conceptualized as the product of skills in two areas, decoding and listening comprehension.

Decoding refers to the word-level skill that is demonstrated in word recognition and pseudoword

reading. Listening comprehension represents the more global skill of understanding spoken

language (Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990). Joshi, Tao, Aaron, and Quiroz

5

Page 16: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

(2012) validated the SVR in Spanish and Chinese, in addition to English, by examining the

contributions of decoding (operationalized as character recognition for the Chinese group) and

listening comprehension to RC across these three groups of monolinguals sampled in the primary

grades. For example, in grade 4 results indicated that measures of decoding (or character

recognition) and listening comprehension in combination explained 60% of the variance in RC

for Spanish monolinguals, 50% for English monolinguals, and 42% for Chinese monolinguals.

Overall, the results demonstrated that the SVR is useful for understanding individual differences

in children’s RC skills in English, Spanish, and Chinese. Pasquarella et al. (2012) applied the

SVR framework to understand the contributions of decoding and vocabulary to RC in adolescent

L1 and L2 speakers of English. Decoding, vocabulary and the interaction of the two were all

significant predictors of RC for the L2 learners, while only vocabulary was a significant

predictor for the L1 speakers. While the SVR has been applied to English RC for adolescent L2

speakers (Pasquarella et al., 2012), research has not examined whether the SVR can be applied to

understanding individual differences in Spanish and Chinese RC for adolescent bilinguals. To

extend previous research, Study 1 compared models of RC in Spanish, Chinese and English for

adolescent bilingual students.

While underpinnings of decoding and word reading have been extensively studied, a

major limitation of the SVR model is that the construct of listening comprehension is not clearly

defined. It has been theorized that listening comprehension (also referred to as oral language

proficiency) involves at least three main components, vocabulary, morphological awareness, and

syntactic awareness (Geva, 2006). Generally, vocabulary refers to the knowledge of word

meanings. Morphological awareness refers to the knowledge of, and the ability to manipulate

morphemes, which are the smallest units of meaning in a language. Finally, syntactic awareness

6

Page 17: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

is the ability to recognize and reflect on the grammatical structure of a language. Each of the

three constructs has been shown to uniquely contribute to English RC in monolingual and

bilingual learners (e.g., Cain et al., 2004; Carlisle, 2000; Pasquarella et al., 2012; Savage, 2006).

However, research has not examined the three components within the same framework for L1

and L2 RC in adolescent bilinguals. Separating the influences of vocabulary, morphological and

syntactic awareness (in conjunction with other cognitive and linguistics skills) on RC will lead to

a more refined understanding of the skills necessary for successful comprehension in adolescent

bilinguals from different linguistic backgrounds. Below I will discuss the typological

characteristics of the languages under study, and then review empirical studies on the

contributions of vocabulary, morphological and syntactic awareness to RC.

Typological characteristics of English, Spanish, and Chinese

Orthography and phonology. English and Spanish are both alphabetic orthographies

employing the Roman script, in which letters encode the phonetic components of oral language.

English and Spanish differ regarding grapheme (letter) to phoneme (sound) transparency.

Spanish has a shallow orthography (DeFior, Martos, & Cary, 2002), in which grapheme-and-

phoneme correspondences are highly regular. English, however, has a deep alphabetic

orthography (Wimmer & Goswami, 1994). While there is some regularity among grapheme-

and-phoneme correspondences, there is much less consistency as graphemes are often associated

with multiple phonemes (e.g., “ch” in chord, chore, chute) and grapheme-and-phoneme

correspondences can change depending on the addition of affixes (e.g., heal vs. health). Chinese,

on the other hand, is a logographic orthography. Each Chinese character corresponds to a

morpheme and a syllable in the spoken language (i.e., the character 兵 means soldier and is

pronounced /bing1/). Most Chinese characters are semantic-phonetic compound characters,

7

Page 18: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

which comprise a semantic radical and a phonetic radical. The semantic radical carries

information about the meaning of a compound character. The phonetic radical only accurately

represents the pronunciation of a compound character 39% of the time (Shu, Chen, Anderson,

Wu, & Xuan, 2003). Chinese characters that are not semantic-phonetic compound characters,

such as 兵, can only be read by mapping the whole character to its pronunciation. Another

unique feature of Chinese is the high level of homophone density. Tens of thousands of

morphemes are represented by fewer than 2000 syllables. For example, the tone syllable /yi4/

can correspond to characters 意,易,宜,益,亦,译,译,译,译,翼,异,译 and many

more (Duanmu, 2007). Additionally, many characters are homographs, meaning there are

multiple meanings associated with the same character. It is often the context of the sentence that

indicates the exact meaning of a homographic character.

Morphology. Morphological awareness refers to the use of word formation rules to

construct and understand morphologically complex words (Kuo & Anderson, 2006; Stahl &

Nagy, 2006). Morphologically complex words can be categorized by different morphological

elements. Three types are: (1) inflections, (2) derivations, and (3) compounds. Inflections (e.g.,

-s, -ing) are word endings added to a base (root) word that denote meanings such as verb tense,

gender, or case. Derivations involve applying affixes (i.e., prefixes and suffixes un-, sub-, -ness,

-ly) to base words to form new words that have different meanings or word classes from the base

words (e.g., friend-friendship-friendly). Derivational suffixes also specify syntactic categories

(e.g., –ion indicates a noun, while –ify indicates a verb). Finally, compounds are the

combinations of two or more words to form new words (e.g., tomb + stone = tombstone) (Kuo &

Anderson, 2006; Lam, Chen, Geva, Luo, & Li 2012; McBride-Chang, 2004). With respect to

morphological characteristics of a language, this study investigated derivational and compound

8

Page 19: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

awareness as these types of skills are more important for understanding academic words and

sentence-level comprehension (e.g., Nagy & Anderson, 1984). Derivational and compound

morphological awareness may share different relationships with RC across English, Spanish and

Chinese. Therefore to create a parallel comparison in all three languages, I decided to focus on

both derivational and compound morphological awareness.

Within different languages the representation and frequency of morphologically complex

words differs substantially. Spanish and English both have a rich derivational morphology as

many morphologically complex words are derived. Compound words are present in Spanish and

English but they are encountered less often than derived words (Chen, Ramirez, Luo, Geva, &

Ku, 2012; Ramirez, Chen, Geva, & Luo, 2011). On the other hand, Chinese has a very rich

compound morphology as over 75% of words are formed through compounding (Kuo &

Anderson, 2006; Sun, Sun, Huang, Li & Xing, 1996). The morphological characteristics of

different languages may have important implications for how these skills are recruited during

reading.

Syntax. Syntax refers to the way words are put together to form sentences, clauses or

phrases. English and Spanish have many similar syntactic features. English and Spanish use

grammatical devices such as inflections to denote case, number, and tense. Additionally, both

languages use function words (e.g., of in English, de in Spanish) and articles (e.g., the in English

and el/la in Spanish for masculine/feminine forms, respectively). However, Spanish utilizes

inflections to a much greater degree than English. For example, Spanish includes inflections to

denote gender (e.g, common masculine nouns end in –o, common feminine nouns end in -a)

(Zagona, 2002), while English does not. Word order is similar in Spanish and English. Subject-

9

Page 20: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Verb-Object is the structure used in the majority of sentences (Muntendam, 2013; Zawiszewki,

Gutiérrez, Fernández, & Laka, 2011).

There are some common features shared across Chinese and English. For example,

uninflected modals are used in both English and Chinese. However, Chinese syntax differs from

English or Spanish because Chinese lacks the grammatical device of inflections, such as case

marking and tense. Additionally, function words are not used in Chinese (Li, 1996; Li &

Thompson, 1981). Instead, word compounding is used to show verb tense, number and degree.

For example, the character 猫 can denote either the singular or plural form of cat/cats.

Additional characters are added to a morpheme as quantifiers to indicate plurality, such as 一群

which means a group (Chik, Ho, Yeung, Chan, Chung, Luan, Lo & Lau, 2012). There is more

flexibility in word order in Chinese than Spanish or English. In Chinese, different word order

structures (e.g., Object-Subject-Verb or Subject-Verb-Object) can be used based on whether the

topic of the passage is an object or a subject. Given this flexibility, word order is often regarded

as the most important syntactic device necessary for RC in Chinese (Chang, 1992; Chao, 1968;

Chik et al., 2012; Huang, Li & Li, 2008; Li & Thompson, 1981; Yueng, Ho, Chan, Chung, &

Wong, 2013).

Oral Language Proficiency and RC

Vocabulary. Researchers have conceptualized vocabulary according to two dimensions:

breadth and depth. Vocabulary breadth generally refers to the number of words a person knows

in both oral (phonological) and written (orthographic) forms. Vocabulary depth refers to the

amount of knowledge about a particular word, which includes a root word’s multiple meanings

(e.g., lean meat vs. to lean against a wall), morphological derivations (e.g., leaner, leanest,

leanly), and the appropriate use of root words or morphologically complex words in different

10

Page 21: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

syntactic contexts (e.g., -ing is used to modify a noun to a verb in a sentence, while –est creates

an adjective, and –ly creates an adverb) (Nagy & Herman, 1987; Oullette, 2006; Proctor et al.,

2012; Proctor, Uccelli, Dalton, & Snow, 2009).

English vocabulary is a robust predictor of English RC in monolingual and bilingual

children, adolescents and adults (Braze, Tabor, Shankweiler, & Menclet 2007; Kieffer & Lesaux,

2008; Proctor et al., 2009; Pasquarella et al., 2012). For example, Proctor, Carlo, August, and

Snow (2006) examined English RC in 4th grade Spanish-English bilinguals in the United States.

Results indicated that vocabulary was strongly associated with English reading and listening

comprehension. Mancilla-Martinez and Lesaux (2010) examined longitudinal predictors of

English RC in low-achieving Spanish-English bilinguals from 4.5 to 11 years of age in the

United States. Initial status and the rate of growth of vocabulary, along with word reading,

explained virtually all of the variance (88%) in RC ability at age 11. In a study of adolescent L2

learners in Canada, Pasquarella et al. (2012) also identified vocabulary as a significant predictor

of English RC, after controlling for decoding.

Only one study, to my knowledge, has examined the relationships between Spanish

vocabulary and Spanish RC. Kim and Pallante (2012) studied first grade students from high

socio-economic status families in Chile. Children were tested three times during the year on

Spanish RC, vocabulary, word reading and pseudoword reading fluency. Vocabulary was a

significant predictor of RC after controlling for the influence of word reading and pseudoword

reading fluency. Additionally, students with larger vocabularies tended to experience faster

growth in RC performance over the course of the academic year, in comparison to children with

smaller vocabularies. Currently no studies have examined predictors of Spanish RC with

11

Page 22: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

adolescents. The current project will address this research gap by examining variables that

predict Spanish RC in adolescent Spanish-English bilinguals.

A few studies have examined the role of vocabulary in Chinese RC. For example, Zhang,

McBride-Chang, Tong, Anita, Shu, and Fong (2012) examined predictors of Chinese RC in a

one-year longitudinal study with 7-year-old Chinese children in Hong Kong. Chinese

vocabulary at time 1 was a significant predictor of RC at time 2 after controlling for age, rapid

automatized naming and phonological awareness. Wang, Cheng, and Chen (2006) also

identified Chinese vocabulary as a significant predictor of Chinese RC in a sample of Chinese-

English bilinguals in grade 2 attending education in US schools. Furthermore, Shu, McBride-

Chang, Wu, & Liu (2006) identified Chinese vocabulary as a significant predictor of Chinese RC

in Hong Kong Chinese children with and without reading difficulties in the 5th and 6th grades. In

summary, all studies reported that Chinese vocabulary was a significant predictor of Chinese RC.

Across studies of vocabulary breadth in English, Spanish and Chinese, vocabulary was

consistently a predictor of RC performance. However, previous research is almost exclusively

limited to studies involving children (see Pasquarella et al., 2012 for an exception), and the need

for research to examine adolescent populations is clear (August & Shanahan, 2006).

Considering bilinguals have shown pervasive gaps in vocabulary and RC compared with

monolingual speakers and norm referenced assessments (August et al., 2005; Carlo et al., 2004;

Pasquarella et al., 2012), disentangling the relationships of vocabulary, RC, and other cognitive

and linguistic skills may inform efforts to improve vocabulary and RC skills. In the current

study, vocabulary was measured with a multiple choice definitions task.

Morphological awareness. English morphological awareness shared strong

relationships with both English vocabulary and English RC in monolingual English speakers and

12

Page 23: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

L2 learners (Carlisle, 2000; Kuo & Anderson, 2006; Nagy, Beringer, Abbott, 2006; Ramirez,

Chen & Pasquarella, 2013; Stahl & Nagy, 2006). English derivational awareness was a

significant predictor of English RC for Spanish-English bilinguals reading English. In a sample

of 90 Spanish-English bilinguals in grades 4 to 7 in Canada, English derivational awareness

predicted both English RC and vocabulary (Ramirez et al., 2013). Kieffer and Lesaux (2008)

examined English morphological awareness, English vocabulary, and English RC in Spanish-

English bilinguals longitudinally from the fourth through fifth grade in the US. English

derivational morphological awareness emerged as a predictor of English RC, after controlling for

phonological awareness, word reading and vocabulary in grade 5. In a recent study (Kieffer &

Di Felice Box, 2013), morphological awareness made direct and indirect contributions to RC in

native English speakers and Spanish-English bilinguals in grade six in the US. The indirect

influence of morphological awareness on RC was partially mediated through vocabulary.

Specifically, derivational morphological awareness appeared to play an important role in English

RC for Spanish-English bilinguals. The current study will examine the direct and indirect

relationships of morphological awareness and RC with Spanish-English bilingual adolescents.

For Chinese-English bilingual children, researchers have investigated the relationships

between morphological awareness and RC. Since compound morphological awareness is an

important aspect of the Chinese language, researchers examined its role in English reading for

Chinese-English bilinguals. Studies have found differences in the predictive relationships

between English compound and derivational morphological awareness with English RC for

Chinese-English bilinguals. Wang, Cheng, and Chen (2006) reported English compound

morphological awareness to be an important predictor of English RC in Chinese-English

bilinguals who were sampled across grades 1 through 5 in the US. Wang et al. (2006) did not

13

Page 24: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

include a measure of derivational morphological awareness. Therefore, comparisons between

the relative contributions of derivational and compound morphological awareness to RC could

not be made. Pasquarella, Chen, Lam, Luo, and Ramirez (2011) included both measures of

morphological awareness in their examination of predictors of English RC in 137 Chinese-

English bilingual children in Canada. They found that both compound and derivational

morphological awareness were significant predictors of English RC after controlling for the

influences of word reading, vocabulary, phonological awareness and non-verbal reasoning. In

another study, Lam et al. (2012) examined the roles of English derivational and compound

awareness in the development of English comprehension skills in a small sample of Chinese-

English bilinguals in kindergarten and grade one. For both cohorts, derivational morphological

awareness, but not compound morphological awareness, was a significant longitudinal predictor

of RC tested one year later, after controlling for the influences of age, phonological awareness,

word reading and vocabulary. The lack of significant relationships between compound

morphological awareness and RC may be due to the sample size.

In addition to the direct relationship between morphological awareness and English RC,

morphological awareness has been found to be an indirect predictor of English RC for Chinese-

English bilinguals. Zhang and Koda (2012) examined direct and indirect effects of derivational

morphological awareness on English vocabulary and English RC with Chinese readers who were

learning English as a foreign language through graduate studies in China. Derivational

morphological awareness was only indirectly related to RC through vocabulary. Overall, the

results are somewhat inconsistent for Chinese-English bilinguals as to the role of derivational

and compound morphological awareness in English RC. Given inconsistencies across studies,

further research that compares both derivational and compound awareness is needed.

14

Page 25: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Substantially less research has been conducted on the role of morphological awareness in

Spanish or Chinese compared to English. Specifically, no studies have reported on the

relationship between Spanish RC and Spanish morphological awareness. The current study was

the first to investigate the links between Spanish vocabulary, morphological awareness and RC.

Alternatively, research has demonstrated compound morphological awareness to be a predictor

of Chinese RC. In a sample of Chinese children in Hong Kong, Zhang et al. (2012) showed that

morphological awareness at time 1 (age 7) was related to RC at time 2 (age 8), controlling for

age, word reading, rapid automatized naming, and phonological awareness. Additionally, Tong,

McBride-Chang, Shu, and Wong (2009) reported that morphological awareness was a

longitudinal and concurrent predictor of Chinese RC in a 1-year study of young Hong Kong

Chinese children. Furthermore, Shu et al. (2006) also demonstrated that Chinese morphological

awareness was a significant predictor of Chinese RC in 5th and 6th grade children in China.

Notably, the contribution of morphological awareness to RC was stronger than the contributions

of phonological awareness, vocabulary, and rapid digit naming. Overall, morphological

awareness appears to play a clear role in Chinese RC. However, none of the studies have

focused on the reading skills of adolescent Chinese-English bilinguals. A goal of the present

study was to extend research by addressing this issue.

In the present study, parallel tasks were created to the extent possible based on

characteristics of the languages. Comparable tasks of compound morphological awareness were

created across all languages. Additionally, derivational tasks were also parallel in Spanish and

English. However, specific to Chinese, derivational and compound morphological awareness

were also targeted through a homographic morphological awareness task. For example, 信服 is

a compound word meaning to convince and is pronounced /xin4 fu2/. Whereas, 弹性 is a derived

15

Page 26: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

word meaning flexibility and is pronounced /tan2 xing4/. Derivational morphology is often an

overlooked area of morphological awareness in Chinese because there are fewer derived words

than compound words. Therefore, including this task in the current study presented a highly

novel opportunity to examine the contributions of derivational and compound morphology

awareness in Chinese RC.

Syntactic awareness. Syntactic awareness refers to the ability to recognize and consider

the grammatical structure of a language. Researchers measure syntactic awareness with a variety

of tasks. Often, tasks of syntactic awareness required students to determine if spoken sentences

were grammatically correct or not. Alternatively, some studies had students repair incorrect

sentences (e.g., Johnston & Newport, 1989). Other researchers have used a sentence completion

task to assess syntactic awareness (e.g., Low & Siegel, 1995). Syntactic or grammatical cues are

important for comprehension as they help individuals establish a coherent representation of the

text (Cain, 2007; Fender, 2001; Givón, 1995; Kintsch, 1998; Tunmer & Bowey, 1984; Zhang,

2012).

Cain (2007) found little support for a direct relationship between syntactic awareness

(measured through a sentence correction task) and RC in a sample of monolingual English

speaking children who were 8 and 10 years old in England. Instead, indirect relationships were

mediated by vocabulary, grammatical knowledge and working memory. In another study, Low

and Siegel (2005) reported that syntactic awareness (measured by an oral cloze task) was a

unique predictor of RC, in addition to verbal working memory and phonological processing, in

both English monolingual and bilingual sixth graders in Canada. The differences between the

results of these two studies could be due to the sensitivity of the measures used. The task used

by Cain (2007) focused specifically on syntactic awareness, while the task used by Low and

16

Page 27: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Siegel (2005) may have been confounded with vocabulary knowledge. Additionally, the Low

and Siegel (2005) study did not control for vocabulary in their analyses. Therefore, the

significant relationship of syntactic awareness and RC in the Low and Siegel (2005) study may

be influenced by the students’ vocabulary knowledge.

Research among Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals has also demonstrated a

relationship between English syntactic awareness and English RC. Proctor, Silverman, Harring

and Montecillo (2012) examined the contributions of measures of vocabulary, morphological

awareness, syntactic awareness and a semantic relatedness task to the development of RC over

the course of one year in monolingual English-speaking and Spanish-English bilinguals children

sampled from grades 2 to 4 in the US. Results indicated that syntactic awareness was a unique

contributor to initial status, but not growth, in English RC above the contributions of vocabulary,

word identification and grade for monolingual and Spanish-English bilinguals. However, the

relationship between syntactic awareness and RC was weak for both groups. Zhang (2012)

examined predictors of English RC among Chinese young adults (mean age was approximately

24 years old) learning English as a foreign language (EFL) in China. Two measures of syntactic

awareness were used: (1) an implicit measure which required correct vs. incorrect judgment of a

sentence, and (2) an explicit measure that required students to correct grammatically incorrect

sentences. After controlling for the influence of vocabulary, syntactic awareness displayed a

significant, albeit weak, relationship with RC. Interestingly, it was the implicit measure of

syntactic awareness and not the explicit measure that was the significant predictor of RC. The

authors argue that the implicit awareness task requires more efficient and fluent processing of

syntactic information and word integration than the explicit task, which may account for its

17

Page 28: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

stronger relationship with RC. Overall the results suggested, at best, a weak relationship

between English syntactic awareness and English RC.

Currently no studies have investigated the role of Spanish syntactic awareness on Spanish

RC. Recently, a small number of studies have investigated the role of syntactic awareness in

Chinese monolingual children. For example, Chik, Ho, Yeung, Chan, Chung, Luan, Lo, and Lau

(2012) conducted a longitudinal study that involved 272 Chinese-speaking children in Hong

Kong across grades 1 and 2. In this study, syntactic awareness was measured with a sentence

completion task. Results indicated that syntactic skills in grade 1 made an independent

contribution to sentence comprehension in grade 2, after controlling for age, non-verbal

reasoning, reading related cognitive skills (including orthographic and phonological skills), word

reading, and morphological awareness. In another study, Yeung, Ho, Chik, Lo, Luan, Chan, and

Chung (2011) reported that syntactic awareness, assessed with a sentence completion task, was a

unique predictor of comprehension at the level of the sentence and passage in grade 1 Chinese-

speaking children living in Hong Kong. Overall, the results underscored the importance of

syntactic skills as an early predictor of Chinese comprehension. The authors argued that

syntactic knowledge facilitated text-information building and text coherence.

The majority of studies did not examine the relationship of syntactic awareness to RC in

conjunction with both vocabulary and morphological awareness in English, Spanish or Chinese

(see Proctor et al., 2012 for an exception in English). Additionally, the nature of the relationship

between syntactic awareness and RC among adolescent bilinguals remains unclear, as most

previous studies have looked at different populations of learners (e.g., monolingual children,

bilingual children, EFL adults). The current study explored whether and how these findings may

be applied to adolescent Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals. Additionally,

18

Page 29: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

measures of syntactic awareness differ between studies; some measures were implicit judgment

tasks (Zhang, 2012), others explicit correction tasks (Cain, 2007) and others sentence generation

(Proctor et al., 2012) or sentence completion tasks (Chik et al., 2012; Low & Siegel, 1995;

Yueng et al., 2011). It is likely that the different formats of syntactic awareness tasks require

different cognitive and linguistic skills. The current study uses implicit judgment tasks as they

require less vocabulary knowledge than other syntactic awareness tasks. Previous research has

suggested that syntactic awareness may share a unique relationship with RC, but this relationship

was weakened once vocabulary was factored into the equation (e.g., Zhang, 2012). Research is

needed to understand how the relationships between vocabulary, morphology and syntax jointly

influence RC in adolescent bilinguals to gain a clear understanding of each component’s unique

and shared influence on comprehension. Furthermore, examining the contributions of these

skills to RC in English, Spanish, and Chinese outlined similarities and differences of predictors

of RC across languages.

The current study created comparable measures of syntactic awareness to evaluate its

contribution to RC in English, Spanish and Chinese. For each language a judgment task that

required students to determine whether an orally presented sentence was grammatically correct

was used to assess syntactic awareness. The syntactic awareness tasks contained multiple

syntactic features that were representative of the languages understudy. Some of the features

were unique to a particular language, while other features were common to either Spanish and

English or Chinese and English. The goal was to create measures that assessed the participant’s

syntactic awareness regarding multiple representative features found within that language (e.g.,

tense, number, the use of articles or function words, word order, etc.), instead of focusing on one

particular element (e.g., word order).

19

Page 30: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Objectives

The current investigation examined models of RC for bilingual adolescents from two

different L1 backgrounds: Spanish and Chinese. The primary objective was to examine the

relationships of vocabulary, morphological awareness and syntactic awareness with RC.

Comparisons were made across English, Spanish and Chinese to identify how the components

varied across languages. Specifically, four different models were compared: (1) English RC for

Spanish-English bilinguals, (2) English RC for Chinese-English bilinguals, (3) Spanish L1 RC,

and (4) Chinese L1 RC. Identifying relationships across Spanish, Chinese, and English can

reveal language-universal and language-specific aspects of RC.

I hypothesized that vocabulary knowledge would be consistently related to RC in English,

Spanish and Chinese, and would therefore be universally important for RC. On the other hand,

language-specific relationships may also be present. For example, derivational morphological

awareness may be related to RC in Spanish and English but not Chinese, whereas compound

morphological awareness may demonstrate a stronger relationship with Chinese RC than with

Spanish or English RC. The salience and frequency of morphologically complex words

encountered in a language may have implications for how these skills are utilized to comprehend

different languages. Considering syntactic awareness is a predictor of English and Chinese RC

(Chik et al., 2012; Proctor et al., 2012; Yeung et al., 2011; Zhang, 2012) it may also be a

significant predictor for Spanish RC.

20

Page 31: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 3: Method

Participants

Participants included 117 Chinese-English bilinguals (84 Females; 33 Males) with a

mean age of 17.99 years old (SD = 3.4 years) and 71 Spanish-English bilinguals (43 Females; 28

Males) with a mean age of 19.65 years (SD = 3.36 years) enrolled in high school or in

undergraduate university programs in southern Ontario, Canada. All Chinese-English bilinguals

spoke Mandarin as their L1. The majority were from mainland China (80%), 10% were from

Taiwan, and the remaining 10% did not indicate a country of origin. On average, the Chinese-

English bilinguals had been living in Canada for 6.88 years (SD = 4.34 years). All Spanish-

English bilinguals spoke Spanish as their L1. On average, this group of students had been living

in Canada for 12.04 years (SD = 6.98 years). Ten percent of Spanish-English bilinguals were

from Colombia, 10% were from Mexico, 5% were from Ecuador, and 40% of participants were

distributed among Argentina, Chile, Cuba, Peru, Philippines, Spain, or the US. Additionally,

there were approximately 20% who were born in Canada and another 15% did not provide a

country of origin.

With respect to parental education, approximately 80% of the Chinese-English

bilingual’s parents had completed college or university at the undergraduate or graduate level,

while about 20% of the participant’s parents had completed high school or less. For the Spanish-

English bilinguals, approximately 70% of the participant’s parents had completed college or

university at the undergraduate or graduate level, while about 30% completed high school or less.

For the Chinese-English bilinguals, approximately 74% of the sample reported speaking

to their parents only in Chinese. Another 26% reported speaking with their parents frequently in

Chinese and occasionally in English. The majority of Chinese-English bilinguals (83%) reported

21

Page 32: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

conversations with their friends occurring mostly in English. The other 17% reported speaking

with their friends in both English and Chinese to a similar degree. For the Spanish-English

bilinguals, approximately 25% percent of the sample reported speaking to their parents only in

Spanish. Another 50% reported speaking with their parents frequently in Spanish and

occasionally in English. The remaining 25% reported speaking to their parents mostly in English

and rarely in Spanish. About 75% Spanish-English bilinguals reported conversations with

friends occurring mostly in English and occasionally in Spanish. The other 25% reported

speaking with their friends in English and Spanish to a similar degree.

For the Chinese-English bilinguals, 40% of the sample were enrolled in a Chinese

language class. The majority of students who were not enrolled in heritage language classes had

completed high school in China and came to Canada for the purpose of attending university. For

the Spanish-English bilinguals, only 8 students were enrolled in Spanish language classes. For

both groups, L1 languages classes lasted about three hours per week (the range was 2.5 to 3.5

hours per week).

Procedures

Participants completed measures in both their L1 and English. In total, testing took

approximately 4 hours to complete, which was 2.5 hours in English and 1.5 hours in the L1.

Generally, all the English measures were completed in one session and the L1 measures were

completed in another session, with the remainder of English or L1 measures completed in a third

session for a few participants. Tasks within each testing were administered by a trained research

assistant. Typically the English measures were administered before the L1 measures. Measures

of RC, vocabulary, morphological awareness and syntactic awareness in English and the L1 were

completed in groups. Additionally, the demographic and language use questionnaire was also

22

Page 33: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

administered during group testing. The measures of word reading, non-verbal reasoning and

working memory were administered individually.

Measures

Reading Comprehension. The Second Canadian Edition of the Gates MacGinitie RC

test (MacGinitie, MacGinitie, Maria, Dreyer, & Hughes, 1992) was used to assess RC in English,

Chinese and Spanish. Levels E and F were used for high school and undergraduate students

respectively to ensure that students worked through the test most appropriate for their age and

ability. Form 3 of Levels E and F were translated into Spanish and Chinese. Form 4 of Levels E

and F were used for English RC. Each test consisted of a series of short passages with multiple

choice questions. In total, each test had 48 multiple choice questions each with 4 or 5 response

options. Students were given 45 minutes to complete this task in each language. Reliabilities1 in

English for Forms E and F were .84 and .93 for the Chinese-English bilinguals, and .83 and .91

for the Spanish-English bilinguals. Reliabilities, based on translated Form 3 Levels E and F,

were .89 and .93 for the Chinese RC test and.87 and .89 for the Spanish RC test

Vocabulary. The Second Canadian Edition of the Gates MacGinitie Vocabulary

(MacGinitie et al., 1992) was used to assess vocabulary knowledge in English, Spanish and

Chinese. Form 4 of Levels E and F were used for high school and undergraduate students

respectively to ensure the participants worked through the test most appropriate for their age and

ability. Form 3 of Levels E and F were translated into Spanish and Chinese. To ensure that the

difficulty of items was similar across languages, word frequencies were tabulated based on the

SUBTLex databases for English (Brysbaert & New, 2009), Spanish (Cuetos, Glez-Nosti, Barbón,

& Brysbaert, 2011) and Chinese (Cai, & Brysbaert, 2010). First, word frequencies were

1 Cronbach’s alpha reliability ratings of internal consistency were calculated based on item-level data from the participants within the current study for all measures in the L1 and L2.

23

Page 34: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

calculated for the English tasks to be translated. Then the word frequencies for the translated

tasks were cross-referenced with the English version. For situations where differences between

frequencies were noted, the translated word was replaced with another word in the database that

had a similar frequency to the original target item. T-tests revealed non-significant differences in

the frequency ratings (using LOG frequency per million values) across languages for the

different forms, suggesting the L1 measures are comparable in difficulty to their English

counterparts (p range was .55 to .95). Each test consisted of target words with multiple choice

responses. Each test had 45 target words with 4 or 5 potential definitions. Students were given

35 minutes to complete this task in each language. Reliabilities in English for Form 4 Levels E

and F were .94 and .89 for the Chinese-English bilinguals and .90 and .91 for the Spanish-

English bilinguals. Reliabilities, based on translated Forms E and F, were .90 and .90 for

Spanish vocabulary, and .89 and .94 for Chinese vocabulary

Morphological awareness.

Compound awareness. Experimental compound awareness measures were created in

English, Spanish and Chinese following the same instructions and format. The tasks involved

the generation of the name (or label) of a novel noun based on a short description. The measures

contained non-recursive (two word compounds) and recursive compounds (more than two word

compounds) to increase the difficulty of the measures. A sample English item is: “A factory that

recycles sweaters is called a (sweater recycling factory)”. The task was administered with an

oral recording of the instructions and items. Participants followed along with a written copy and

provided a written responses for the answers. There were six practice trials for non-recursive

items followed by 15 test items. Then there were 4 practice items for recursive compounds

followed by another 10 test items for a total of 25 test items in both the English and Chinese

24

Page 35: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

tasks. The Spanish task was the same as the English and Chinese tasks except there were 5

recursive test items (instead of 10), tabulating to 20 test items in total. Participants were

encouraged to complete all items. Cronbach’s alpha reliabilities for the English task for

Chinese-English and Spanish-English bilinguals were .80 and .81, respectively. Cronbach’s

alpha reliabilities were .86 for the Spanish task, and .86 for the Chinese task.

Derivational awareness.

Derivational production. The derivational production tasks were adapted from Carlisle

(2000) for both English and Spanish (Ramirez, Chen, Geva & Kieffer, 2010). Participants were

presented with a root word (target word) and an incomplete sentence via an audio recording, and

followed along with a written copy. The participants were asked to write a written response by

modifying the target word to correctly complete the sentence. An example in English is: “Glory

(target word). The view from the hilltop was (glorious).” There were 31 items for the English

measure and 26 items for the Spanish measure. Participants were asked to complete all items.

Reliabilities of the English task for the Chinese-English and Spanish-English bilinguals were .84

and .90, respectively. Reliability for the Spanish task was .92.

Derivational structure. The derivational structure tasks (adapted from Ramirez et al.,

2010) were created for English and Spanish. The participants were asked to identify the

morphologically complex pseudoword that correctly completed the sentence. Participants

listened to a recording of the sentences with the multiple choice options. The participants then

circled the correct response. In each item, all the choices had the same root but different suffixes.

An example item in English was: “The young baby was sleeping so… a) terbious b) terbiously c)

terbiouses d) terbiousal”. There were 21 items on the English task and 21 items on the Spanish

25

Page 36: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

task. Reliabilities for the English task for Chinese-English and Spanish-English bilinguals

were .73 and .79, respectively. The reliability for the Spanish task was .90.

Chinese homographic awareness. An experimental homographic awareness task was

created in Chinese to examine derivational and compound awareness. In this task, participants

were shown a two-character word with a target morpheme [e.g., 商量 (discuss measure: discuss),

with 商 (discuss) as the target morpheme] followed by three two-character options containing

either the target morpheme [e.g., 商定 (discuss decide: decide)] or a morpheme that was a

homonym [e.g., 商店 (business shop: store) and 商人 (business person: businessman)]. The

participants were instructed to pick the option that contained the target morpheme. The task

contained two practice items and 40 test items. There were 20 derivational and 20 compound

items in the task. The test and practice items were recorded and read to participants as they

worked through the written test. The reliability of this task was .87.

Syntactic awareness. Experimental measures of syntactic awareness were constructed

for English, Spanish and Chinese. Participants were asked to judge sentences as being

grammatically correct or incorrect. Sentences were recorded and orally administered to keep

pronunciation and prosody consistent. Students were not shown sentences in the written form.

Students circled “T” if the sentences sounded grammatically correct or “F” if they sounded

grammatically incorrect. The task was modelled after Johnston and Newport’s (1989)

grammatical judgment task. Measures of the task in each language comprised multiple sentences

with representative grammatical features (e.g., third person singular, past tense, uninflected

modals) from that language. For each grammatical feature a short and long sentence (based upon

syllable length) was created, as well as a correct and incorrect version of each. Then all items

were randomized and counterbalanced across two alternate forms to create a balance of long vs.

26

Page 37: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

short and incorrect vs. correct items within each form. Participants received all items from both

forms, for a total of 72 items in English, 78 items in Chinese and 56 items in Spanish, with two

practice items in each language. An example of a correct item in English was, “Ann is the one

whose husband is wearing that funny hat”, and the incorrect counterpart was, “Ann is the one

who husband is wearing that funny hat”. Reliabilities for the English task for Chinese-English

and Spanish-English bilinguals were .84 and .87 respectively. Reliability was .80 for the Spanish

task, and .85 for the Chinese task.

Word reading and decoding. For English, the Word Identification subtest of the

Woodcock Reading Mastery Test–Revised (WRMT–R; Woodcock, 1987) was used to assess the

participants’ word reading accuracy. The test involved participants reading aloud a list of words

of increasing length and difficulty. The reliabilities of the Word Identification were .91 and .92

for the Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals respectively. The Word Attack subtest

of the WRMT–R was used to assess pseudoword reading. The test required the participants to

read aloud a list of pseudowords of increasing complexity. The Word Attack subtest was used to

assess proficiency with English phonological decoding. A stop rule of six consecutive words

read incorrectly was used for both tasks. The reliability ratings of the Word Attack subtest

were .79 and .78 for Chinese-English and Spanish-English bilinguals respectively.

The Identification de letras y palabras subtest of Woodcock Language Proficiency

Battery-Revised Spanish Edition (WLPB-S, Woodcock & Munoz-Sandoval, 1995) was used to

assess Spanish word identification skills. This task was analogous to the English version. The

participants were encouraged to read aloud words that increased in length and difficulty. The

same stop rule for the English tasks was used for the Spanish tasks. The reliability for the

Spanish word reading task was .86.

27

Page 38: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Considering that no standardized test of Chinese word reading was available, an

experimental test was created (based on the test in Luo, Chen, Deacon, Zhang, and Yin, 2013)

that included 240 items presented in order of decreasing word frequency based on the corpus

study conducted by Shu et al. (2003). The majority of items were selected from 12 volumes of

the Elementary School Textbooks (Elementary Education Teaching and Research Center, 1996)

used from grades 1 to 6 in Mainland China. Additional low frequency words were selected from

the Chinese SUBTLex database (Cai, & Brysbaert, 2010) to increase the difficulty of the task.

The test was discontinued when 10 consecutive characters were read incorrectly. The reliability

of this task was .99.

Non-verbal reasoning. The Matrix Analogies Reasoning Test (Naglieri, 1985) was

administered to measure Non-verbal reasoning. To shorten administration time, only subtests 2

and 4 were presented. Each subtest contained 16 items leading to a potential maximum score of

32. For each item, the participants were shown a pattern with one section missing and were

asked to choose one of six options that correctly completed the pattern. A stop rule of 4

consecutive incorrect items was used for each subtest. Reliabilities for the Chinese-English and

Spanish-English bilinguals were .76 and .86, respectively.

Working memory. The working memory task was adapted from Daneman and

Carpenter (1980) (see also Gottardo, Stanovich & Siegel, 1996). In this task, participants

listened to sets of pre-recorded statements containing familiar information (e.g., Cars have four

wheels; Fish swim in the sky). The participants indicated whether each sentence was true or

false. Additionally, the participants were required to recall the final word of each statement

(wheels, sky) at the end of a set. A set contained either two, three, or four statements. The

number of final words remembered was calculated, giving a possible maximum score of 42. The

28

Page 39: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

reliabilities of this task for the Chinese-English and Spanish-English bilinguals were .90 and .91

respectively. Only an English measure of working memory was developed and administered.

Demographic questionnaire. Participants were asked to fill out a questionnaire to

collect information on their age, grade, country of origin, age of immigration, and length of

residence in Canada. Also, the questionnaire asked students to indicate their parents’ level of

education. The questionnaire also asked to students to report how often they spoke English and

their L1 with family and friends, using a 5 point Likert-type scale (never-rarely-sometimes-

often-frequently). Finally, students reported whether they were currently enrolled in L1

language class (with a yes or no response) and how many hours each week they spent in the class.

Measurement comparability across languages

Central to the design and interpretation of the current study is the comparability of

measures across languages. To create measures that were parallel across languages different

techniques were employed for the various constructs. For RC and vocabulary, an alternate form

of a standardized task was translated into Spanish and Chinese. Translation tasks of RC and

vocabulary were used to ensure that the format and the method of questioning were comparable

across languages. To ensure accurate and authentic translation of the tasks, native speakers of

Chinese or Spanish who were highly proficient in English completed the translations.

Experimental measures were created for morphological awareness and syntactic awareness. To

ensure comparability across languages, the same instructions and format were used in all

languages when possible. Finally, standardized measures of decoding and word reading were

available for Spanish and English and were therefore used. An experimental Chinese character

reading task was adapted from a previously published study (Chen et al., 2009) by adding more

difficult items to ensure it was appropriate for adolescent Chinese speakers.

29

Page 40: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 4: Results

Mean Scores and Group Differences

Table 1 displays minimum and maximum scores, means, and standard deviations for age,

length of time in Canada, and all measures for the Spanish-English and Chinese-English

bilinguals. Skewness and kurtosis values were also examined for all measures. If the values fell

outside the range of +/- two standard errors then transformations to improve normality were

performed (as per Tabachnick & Fidell, 2007). Transformation was performed on measures of

non-verbal reasoning, word reading in the L1 and English, English derivational production,

English derivational structure for both groups, and the Chinese homographic awareness task2 for

the Chinese-speaking bilinguals. However, subsequent analysis revealed transforming data did

not change patterns of correlations or results of structural equation models. Therefore, only raw

score analyses are reported here3.

Standardized scores were calculated for English measures when norms were available.

For English word identification and word attack, both groups performed close to the standardized

mean. For English vocabulary and RC, grade equivalent scores are presented. Vocabulary and

comprehension grade equivalent scores were between a 9th and 10th grade. On average, the

bilingual students were performing approximately 2 grade levels below on these measures.

A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted to test for differences

among the Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals for age, months in Canada, and the

English variables. There was a statistically significant difference between the two groups on

2 The correlation between the derivational and compound items was .91 (p < .001). The combined score of all items was used in the current and subsequent analyses. 3 As an additional check to ensure administering both levels E and F within the same sample did not influence the results, subsequent analyses (i.e., t-tests, correlations, and structural models) were conducted using raw scores and extended scale scores. Extended scale scores are typically used to allow for different versions of the tasks to be combined for analysis (MacGinitie et al., 1992). The two methods produced identical findings. Therefore, to avoid redundancy only raw score analyses are presented below.

30

Page 41: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

these variables, Wilks’ λ = .61, F (12, 175) = 9.17, p < .001. One-way univariate analysis of

variance (ANOVA) by group (Chinese-English vs. Spanish-English bilinguals) conducted for

English measures, age, and length of residence are reported in Table 1. Scores on L1 measures

were not compared as measures were administered in different languages and contained different

items to reflect language specific characteristics important for Spanish or Chinese. The one-way

ANOVAs revealed that age was significantly different between the two groups. On average

Chinese-English bilinguals were about 15 months younger than the Spanish-English bilinguals.

Additionally, the Chinese-English bilinguals had been in Canada a significantly shorter amount

of time than the Spanish-English bilinguals. The Chinese-English bilinguals scored significantly

higher on the MAT but significantly lower on the English word identification and vocabulary

tasks than the Spanish-English bilinguals. Scores on all other measures, including English RC,

were not significantly different between groups.

Correlations

Table 2 presents the correlations among age, length of time in Canada, and all English

measures for the Chinese-English bilinguals above the diagonal and the Spanish-English

bilinguals below the diagonal. To safeguard against Type I error correlations significant at the

level of p < .01 and lower were considered meaningful. All English measures were positively

correlated with RC for both groups. Additionally, age was negatively correlated, and length of

residence in Canada was positively correlated, with RC for the Chinese-English bilinguals only.

For both groups, measures of English word reading, vocabulary, morphological awareness,

syntactic awareness, and word reading were all correlated.

Table 3 reports correlations among age, length of time in Canada, and all Spanish

measures for the Spanish-English bilinguals. All measures except for length of time in Canada,

31

Page 42: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

working memory, and word reading were significantly correlated with RC. Age was positively

correlated with the Spanish measures of vocabulary, morphological awareness, syntactic

awareness and RC. Length of residence in Canada was negatively related to vocabulary,

derivational production, compound awareness and syntactic awareness, but not RC, derivational

structure or word identification. Additionally, measures of vocabulary, morphological awareness

and syntactic awareness were all significantly correlated.

Table 4 displays correlations among age, length of residence in Canada, non-verbal

reasoning, English working memory and all Chinese measures for the Chinese-English bilinguals.

All variables except for non-verbal reasoning and working memory, were correlated with RC.

Length of residence in Canada was negatively correlated with RC and all other Chinese variables,

and age was positively correlated with all Chinese variables. Chinese word reading, vocabulary,

morphological awareness, and syntactic awareness were positively correlated with RC.

Additionally, measures of Chinese word reading, vocabulary, morphological awareness, and

syntactic awareness were all positively correlated with each other.

Specification of Structural Models across Languages

The relationships between RC and the underpinning constituent components were

examined with equivalent statistical models for the Spanish-English and Chinese-English

bilinguals’ L1 and L2. To this end, four models were compared to identify similarities and

differences among predictors of RC. The four models were: (1) English RC for Spanish-English

bilinguals, (2) English RC for Chinese-English bilinguals, (3) Spanish RC, and (4) Chinese RC.

Direct and indirect relationships were examined as previous research has shown that

morphological awareness was often highly associated with vocabulary, with documented

evidence of the mediation of morphological awareness on RC through vocabulary in Spanish-

32

Page 43: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

English bilingual children (Kieffer & DiFelice Box, 2013) and adult Chinese-speaking EFL

learners (Zhang & Koda, 2012). Furthermore, syntactic awareness has been indirectly related to

RC (Cain, 2007) and directly related to RC in Geva & Farnia’s (2012) longitudinal study of

elementary school children learning English as a second language. Additionally, other known

cognitive, linguistic, and socio-cultural predictors of RC were incorporated into the statistical

models to rule out the possibility that results could be due to spurious variables. Specifically,

decoding/word reading, non-verbal reasoning and working memory were added into the models

as various studies have shown these variables to be related to RC (e.g., Cain, 2007; Daneman &

Carpenter, 1980; Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Gathercole & Baddeley, 1993). Finally, the students’

length of residence in Canada was statistically controlled to approximate the amount of exposure

and instruction they had in the L1 and English.

Structural equation modelling (SEM) was used to examine predictors of RC in the

Chinese-English and Spanish-English bilinguals. A theoretical model (see Figure 1) grounded in

existing research was created to understand the direct and indirect relationships between RC and

the other cognitive and linguistic skills in English, Spanish and Chinese. Correlations reported

for each group helped inform the precise specification of the theoretical models of RC. Paths

were added from length of residence in Canada, non-verbal reasoning and working memory to

RC to control for these relationships. These control measures were also correlated with all other

measures (or respective residuals). Age was not added into the model as preliminary analyses

showed age to be a non-significant predictor once length of residence or vocabulary was entered

into the model. A path was added from word-level reading to RC. Decoding, as opposed to

word identification, was chosen as the word-level predictor for English because it allowed for

more precise approximation of students’ mastery of phonological decoding. Additionally, initial

33

Page 44: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

analyses indicated that English word identification was not a significant predictor of RC for

either group. Direct paths from vocabulary, morphological awareness and syntactic awareness to

RC were added. Paths from the morphological awareness measures to vocabulary were added to

examine mediation of morphological awareness on RC via vocabulary. Paths from syntactic

awareness were then added to the morphological awareness measures as an additional test for

mediation on RC. Covariances were added between measures of morphological awareness.

Additionally, covariances were added between word-level reading and vocabulary,

morphological awareness, and syntactic awareness. The theoretical structural model of RC was

then applied to the four models outlined earlier.

To compare the models of English RC for the Spanish-English and Chinese-English

bilinguals, a series of constraints was placed on the parameters to evaluate whether they were

significantly different across groups. In each step, a parameter estimate was fixed to be equal

across groups. The fit of the model with the constrained path was compared to that of the

unconstrained model. The Akaike information criterion (AIC) and the Browne-Cudeck criterion

(BCC) models were calculated to examine how constraining parameters influenced model fit.

Deviance statistics were calculated by taking the difference of the AIC, BCC, and df between the

unconstrained and constrained models. If the deviance statistic was significant, the model with

the lower values was the preferred model (Browne & Cudeck, 1993; Kenny, Kashy, & Cook,

2006). If constraining a parameter significantly reduced model fit, the parameter estimates were

significantly different across groups, and therefore should be free to vary. If constraining a

parameter improved or did not change model fit, then the parameter estimates under examination

were not statistically different across groups. Constraints were placed one at a time on each

predictor of RC, vocabulary, and the morphological awareness measures.

34

Page 45: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

The structural models were created and assessed with AMOS 21.0, and parameters were

estimated using the maximum likelihood fitting function and bootstrapping (with 2000 samples

and a 95% Bias-corrected confidence interval) to examine both direct and indirect effects.

Scores were mean centered before the analyses to increase interpretability when comparing

parameter values. Multiple fit indices were used to assess model fit, including the chi-square test,

root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the comparative fit index (CFI). A χ2 to

df ratio of < 3 suggested a good fit (Kline, 1998). CFI values can range from 0 to 1 with larger

numbers indicating better model fits. An acceptable model fit is indicated by a CFI value of .90

or larger. RMSEA values can range from 0 to 1 with smaller numbers indicating better model

fits. An acceptable model fit is indicated by a RMSEA value of .08 or less (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

However, concerns have been raised about the basis and generalization of setting cut-off values

for fit indices. The choice of setting cut-off values is highly sensitive to sample size, degrees of

freedom and model specification. Statisticians caution against strict reliance on using cut-off

scores to determine if a model should be accepted or rejected (Chen, Curran, Bollen, Kirby &

Paxton, 2008; Fan & Sivo, 2005; Kenny, Kaniskan & McCoach, 2011; Marsh, Hau & Wen,

2004). Thus, cut-off values should not be considered as absolute and models that do not satisfy

all fit statistics can still be informative regarding predictive relationships (Barrett, 2007).

Structural equation models in English. Table 5 presents the results for the models of

English RC (Models 1 and 2). Unstandarized coefficients, standard errors, standardized

coefficients, and associated significance levels are presented for both groups. The Spanish-

English bilinguals are on the right side of the table and the Chinese-English bilinguals on the left

side of the table. Model fit statistics for the joint analysis of the English models for Spanish-

English and Chinese-English bilinguals were: X2 (28) = 100.37, p < .001, X2/df = 3.59, RMSEA

35

Page 46: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

= .12, CFI = .90. Model fit was satisfactory for the CFI fit statistic and less than satisfactory for

the X2/df ratio and the RMSEA statistics. Statistical power of the model was excellent at .98

based on the RMSEA, using the procedures outlined by MacCallum, Browne, and Sugawara

(1996). Overall, the model explained 66% of the variance in RC for the Spanish-English

bilinguals and 71% of the variance in RC for the Chinese-English bilinguals. For the Spanish-

English bilinguals, vocabulary was a significant predictor of RC. Additionally, measures of

English derivational production and English working memory were direct predictors of RC. No

other variables were directly related to RC for the Spanish-English bilinguals. For the Chinese-

English bilinguals, vocabulary was a significant predictor of RC. Measures of derivational

production, derivational structure, decoding and non-verbal reasoning were also significant

predictors of English RC. No other predictors were directly related to RC for the Chinese-

English bilinguals.

Table 6 displays the bootstrapping results for the indirect effects for English RC. The

estimate effects, standard errors, the 95% Bias-corrected confidence intervals (lower bound and

upper bound), and associated significance levels are reported. For both groups of bilinguals,

derivational production and syntactic awareness were significant indirect predictors of RC.

Additionally, derivational structure was a significant indirect predictor of RC for the Chinese-

English bilinguals, but not the Spanish-English bilinguals. For both groups, vocabulary

mediated the relationship between measures of morphological awareness on RC. The influence

of syntactic awareness was mediated through the derivational structure and derivational

production measures for the Chinese-English bilinguals. For the Spanish-English bilinguals, the

indirect effect of syntactic awareness on RC was mediated through derivational production.

Compound awareness was not an indirect predictor of RC for either group. Additionally, length

36

Page 47: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

of residence was a significant indirect predictor of RC performance for both groups. The

mediation of length of residence on comprehension occurred through vocabulary. English

working memory was a significant indirect predictor of RC via vocabulary for the Spanish-

English bilinguals, but not for the Chinese-English bilinguals.

Table 7 displays the comparison of fit statistics for models with constrained parameters

against an unconstrained model. Overall, only constraining three parameters significantly

reduced model fit, strongly suggesting these parameters should be free to vary across groups.

Specifically, decoding was a significant predictor of RC for the Chinese-English but not the

Spanish-English bilinguals. Furthermore, derivational structure was a significant predictor of

RC for the Chinese-English bilinguals only. Additionally, the relationships between length of

residence in Canada and English vocabulary were significantly different between groups. While

the relationships were significant for both groups, length of residency appeared to be a stronger

predictor of vocabulary for the Chinese-English (standardized beta = .44, p < .001) than the

Spanish-English (standardized beta = .19, p < .05) bilinguals. Placing a constraint on the

relationship between working memory and RC resulted in a marginally significant reduction in

model fit. Working memory was a significant predictor of RC for the Spanish-English but not

the Chinese-English bilinguals. All other constraints placed on parameter estimates did not

significantly change model fit, suggesting all these relationships were functionally equivalent

between the two groups of bilinguals. Figure 2 summarizes the direct and indirect effects on

English RC for the Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals, and identifies the parameter

estimates that differed significantly across groups.

Structural equation models in the L1. Table 8 presents the results for models of L1

(Spanish or Chinese) RC (Models 3 and 4). Unstandarized coefficients, standard errors,

37

Page 48: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

standardized coefficients, and associated significance levels are presented for both groups. The

Spanish-English bilinguals are on the right side of the table and Chinese-English bilinguals on

the left side of the table. Model fit statistics for the Spanish RC model were: X2 (8) = 9.22, p

= .324, X2/df = 1.15, RMSEA = .05, CFI = .99. Fit statistics were acceptable for the Spanish RC

model. Statistical power of the model was excellent at .99 based on the RMSEA, using the

procedures outlined by MacCallum et al. (1996). Overall, 66% of the variance in RC was

explained by the Spanish model. For Spanish RC, vocabulary was a significant predictor.

Additionally, derivational structure was also a significant predictor of Spanish RC. Non-verbal

reasoning and English working memory were significant direct predictors of Spanish RC. No

other predictors were directly related to L1 RC performance.

Model fit statistics for the Chinese RC model were: X2 (5) = 30.71, p <.001, X2/df = 6.14,

RMSEA = .210, CFI = .97. For the Chinese model the CFI fit statistic suggested good fit, but

the X2/df ratio and the RMSEA were less than satisfactory. Statistical power of the model was

excellent at .97 based on the RMSEA, using the procedures outlined by MacCallum et al. (1996).

Overall, 82% of the variance in RC was explained by the Chinese model. For Chinese RC,

vocabulary and homographic morphological awareness were significant predictors. Additionally,

English working memory was a significant predictor of Chinese RC. No other predictors were

directly related to L1 RC performance.

Table 9 presents the bootstrapping results for the indirect effects for L1 comprehension.

The estimate effects, standard errors, the 95% Bias-corrected confidence intervals (lower bound

and upper bound), and associated significance levels are reported. For Spanish comprehension,

the significant indirect effects of derivational structure and length of residence in Canada were

mediated through vocabulary. Additionally, the significant indirect effect of Spanish syntactic

38

Page 49: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

awareness on Spanish RC was mediated through Spanish derivational structure. For Chinese

comprehension, both measures of morphological awareness, and syntactic awareness were

significant indirect predictors of Chinese RC. Consistent with the other mediation analyses, the

indirect effects of morphological awareness were related to comprehension through vocabulary.

Also consistent with the other mediation analyses, the indirect effect of syntactic awareness was

related to comprehension through morphological awareness (specifically the derivational

structure task for the Spanish group, and the homographic morphological awareness task for the

Chinese group). Figures 3 and 4 summarize the direct and indirect effects on Spanish and

Chinese RC, respectively.

39

Page 50: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 5: Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to examine the direct and indirect contributions of

vocabulary, morphological awareness, syntactic awareness and word-level reading to RC for

Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals. Four models were evaluated and compared: (1)

English RC for the Spanish-English bilinguals, (2) English RC for the Chinese-English

bilinguals, (3) Spanish RC and (4) Chinese RC. These relationships were examined while

controlling the influences of the students’ length of residence in Canada, non-verbal reasoning

and working memory. Comparable structural models were created for English, Spanish and

Chinese to examine similarities and differences in the component skills important for

comprehension across these three languages. Additionally, statistical comparisons were made

across the English models for the Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals to identify

similarities and differences in predictors of English RC for these two groups. The comparisons

made across all models revealed both language-universal and language-specific aspects of RC.

Comparisons across models revealed consistent similarities among predictors of RC. For

all models, vocabulary emerged as a significant direct predictor of RC, suggesting that

vocabulary is a universal aspect necessary for RC in all languages. For all models, at least one

measure of morphological awareness was a significant direct predictor of RC. Additionally,

morphological awareness was an indirect predictor of RC, with the relationship consistently

mediated through vocabulary in English, Spanish and Chinese. Thus, morphological awareness

appeared to play a universal role in comprehension and vocabulary in a general sense. However,

examination of these relationships on a finer level also revealed some language-specific

relationships. Finally, syntactic awareness was only an indirect predictor of RC, and the

40

Page 51: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

relationship between syntactic awareness and RC was mediated through morphological

awareness in all models.

Model comparisons also revealed differences between predictors of RC across the

Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals. Word-level reading, measured by pseudoword

decoding, was a significant predictor of English RC for the Chinese-English bilinguals, but not

for the Spanish-English bilinguals. Word-level reading was not a significant predictor of L1 RC

for either group. Furthermore, working memory was a significant predictor of RC for the L1

models and the English model for the Spanish-English bilinguals, but not the English model for

the Chinese-English bilinguals. Another difference was noted for models of English RC. Non-

verbal reasoning was a significant predictor for the Chinese-English bilinguals but not for the

Spanish-English bilinguals. However, group comparisons revealed that this difference was not

large enough to be statistically significant between groups. Non-verbal reasoning appeared to be

a more important predictor of English comprehension for the Chinese-English than the Spanish-

English bilinguals but overall this relationship appeared to be weak.

The Universal Importance of Vocabulary and Morphological Awareness for

Comprehension

The findings of the current study showed that vocabulary shared a significant relationship

with RC in both alphabetic and logographic languages in English, Spanish and Chinese. In

addition, morphological awareness served as a direct and indirect predictor of RC in both

alphabetic and logographic languages. The indirect relationships between morphological

awareness and RC was consistently mediated through vocabulary, which replicates the work of

Kieffer and DiFelice Box (2013) and extends the findings to adolescent Spanish-English and

Chinese-English bilinguals. Interestingly, different aspects of morphological awareness were

41

Page 52: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

related to RC in different ways. The measures of derivational awareness were significant direct

and indirect predictors of comprehension performance in English and Spanish. Compound

awareness was not a significant predictor of English RC or English vocabulary for either the

Spanish-English or Chinese-English bilinguals. This comes as somewhat of a surprise for the

Chinese-English bilinguals as several previous studies have demonstrated that compound

awareness was a predictor of RC (e.g., Pasquarella et al., 2011; Wang et al., 2009). However,

the results of the current study replicated and extended the findings of another published study

with younger Chinese-English bilinguals (Lam et al., 2012). Perhaps comprehension in English

and Spanish, especially in high school and beyond, relies more on derivational aspects of

morphology because unknown words encountered in this age range typically involve complex

derivations.

For Chinese RC, homographic and compound awareness were examined. Compound

awareness was a significant direct predictor of Chinese vocabulary and an indirect predictor of

Chinese RC. Considering Chinese vocabulary consists of many compound words, relationships

between compound awareness and vocabulary are expected. Additionally, homographic

awareness was both a direct and indirect predictor of RC. This result suggests that derivational

and compound awareness, measured through knowledge of homographs, are important

components of Chinese RC. Within this study, homographic awareness was a better predictor of

RC than compound awareness. The reasons for this may be that the homographic task focuses

on morpheme meaning whereas the compound task focuses more on the awareness of compound

structure. The results suggest that Chinese predictors that are connected with morpheme

meaning or semantics are robust predictors of Chinese RC.

42

Page 53: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

A strong connection between knowledge of the multiple meanings of a morpheme

represented by homographic characters (whether they are in derived or compound words) and

Chinese RC is expected because the context of the compound word helps determine the precise

meaning of a character, as well as the sentence. Notably, this study is the first to report that

knowledge of derivations, in addition to knowledge of compounds, is related to Chinese RC in

an adolescent sample. Similar to the English and Spanish results, the indirect effects of

morphological awareness on Chinese RC were mediated through vocabulary.

To summarize, morphological awareness was a unique predictor of RC in English,

Spanish, and Chinese and the effect of morphological awareness on RC was also consistently

mediated through vocabulary across all languages. These results po0int to strong and stable

relationships among these constructs across alphabetic and logographic languages.

Morphological awareness taps knowledge of the semantic and syntactic forms of words (Stahl &

Nagy, 2006; Tyler & Nagy, 1989) and is thereby considered to be an aspect of vocabulary depth

(see Kieffer & Lesaux, 2008; Leider, Proctor, Silverman, & Harring, 2013; Li & Kirby, 2013;

Proctor et al., 2012). Morphological awareness relates to both RC and vocabulary through a

deeper understanding of a target morpheme and the morphologically complex variations of a

target morpheme. In addition to its relationship with vocabulary, morphological awareness

appears to also relate to comprehension through syntactic elements (Stahl & Nagy, 2006; Tyler

& Nagy, 1989). However, this relationship may be more important for English and Spanish than

Chinese, as there is considerably less syntactic information presented in compound words in

Chinese. Overall, the current study shows that morphological awareness interacts with both

vocabulary and RC in different orthographies.

43

Page 54: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

The current study showed that syntactic awareness had an indirect effect on RC in

English, Spanish and Chinese and these indirect effects were consistently mediated by

morphological awareness in all languages. These mediation effects may result from the

grammatical knowledge carried in morphemes. In other words, the morphological awareness

measures may have assessed the ability to use morphological knowledge to analyze syntactic

information. These results replicated and extended the findings of Cain (2007) who reported

indirect relationships between syntactic awareness and RC with English monolingual children.

However, other research conducted with Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals reading

English (Proctor et al., 2012; Zhang, 2012), and with Chinese monolinguals (Chik et al., 2012;

Yueng et al., 2012), reported significant direct relationships between syntactic awareness and RC.

The different results reported between studies are likely due to differences in the measurement of

syntactic awareness. All studies except for Zhang (2012) used sentence generation, sentence

completion, or sentence correction tasks to measure syntactic awareness. It is possible that

productive tasks rely more on vocabulary and oral language proficiency than an implicit

judgement task. The Zhang (2012) study used an implicit judgement task, similar to the task

used in the current study. However, Zhang (2012) did not control for morphological awareness

in his analysis which may account for the different results. Future research should use multiple

measures of syntactic awareness within a larger battery of cognitive and linguistic skills to better

understand its contribution to RC.

The results of the current study offer some valuable insights on improving theoretical

models of RC in adolescent bilinguals. Regarding specification of the SVR (Gough & Tunmer,

1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990), not all components related to listening comprehension are

equally predictive of RC performance. The role of vocabulary was central and consistent across

44

Page 55: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

languages. While morphological awareness was a key component, the standardized betas with

comprehension were often not as large as vocabulary (except for Chinese comprehension).

Furthermore, morphological awareness also appeared to play a key role as an aspect of

vocabulary depth which affirms the importance of word-level semantics on comprehension

performance (Perfetti, 2007; Perfetti & Hart, 2001, 2002). Finally, syntactic awareness was only

indirectly related to comprehension suggesting that semantic and morphological skills may be

stronger predictors of RC in adolescent bilinguals. The reliance on word-level semantic

knowledge appears to be universally important for comprehension in alphabetic and logographic

languages, at least for bilingual adolescents, lending strong support for the assertion that a robust

lexical network is a universal aspect of RC (Perfetti, 2007; Perfetti & Hart, 2001, 2002).

Language Specific Influences on Comprehension and Vocabulary

Important differences in predictors of RC across the models were observed, suggesting

that there are language-specific aspects of comprehension. Predictors of English RC varied for

the Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals. Specifically, decoding was a significant

predictor of English RC for the Chinese-English bilinguals, but not the Spanish-English

bilinguals. However, there was no significant difference in decoding between the groups. The

Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals were similar in their ability to read English

pseudowords. Additionally, RC performance was also similar between these two groups. Thus,

reading proficiency may not be the reason for the differences observed. A possible explanation

in light of these results could be the level of automaticity of decoding ability between the groups.

Considering Spanish is a highly transparent orthography, the Spanish-English bilinguals likely

had some experience in decoding in their L1. By contrast, the Chinese-English bilinguals would

have no experience decoding words in their L1, which is a logographic orthography.

45

Page 56: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Additionally, the Spanish-English bilinguals lived in Canada longer than Chinese-English

bilinguals, and therefore received more English education and had more experience with

decoding in English than the Chinese-English bilinguals. Therefore, the Spanish-English

bilinguals may have mastered decoding to a point where it requires fewer cognitive resources

than for their Chinese-English peers. Of course, further research is warranted to replicate and

clarify this finding. Additional studies should include measures of pseudoword reading fluency,

and employ more time-sensitive tools such as reaction time measures and EEG.

As previously mentioned, morphological awareness emerged as a language-universal

aspect of RC in the L1 and L2 for the Spanish-English and Chinese-English bilinguals. However,

morphological awareness displayed different indirect relationships with L1 RC across groups.

Results indicated that compound awareness was a significant indirect predictor of Chinese RC,

but not Spanish RC. This result extended previous research on Chinese-English bilinguals by

demonstrating proximal influences of compound awareness on RC for adolescent bilinguals.

The results of the current study clarify the nature of this relationship by demonstrating that

compound awareness is an important component of vocabulary in Chinese and supports

comprehension through its association with vocabulary.

Interestingly, compound awareness was a significant predictor of Spanish vocabulary, but

not a direct or indirect predictor of Spanish comprehension. Given the relatively small number

of compounds in Spanish, perhaps this relationship demonstrates that compound morphological

awareness was positively associated with higher levels of Spanish vocabulary. Students who

have higher levels of Spanish vocabulary can reflect upon and manipulate Spanish compound

words to a greater extent than those who have lower levels of Spanish vocabulary. Given that

this is the first study to include a measure of compound awareness in Spanish, these findings

46

Page 57: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

need to be replicated by future research before any substantial claims can be made regarding the

nature of the relationship of compound morphology with vocabulary knowledge. Overall, the

differences presented in this study point to language-specific aspects involved in reading the L1

and L2 (i.e., English) and demonstrate that bilinguals with different L1s may employ different

strategies to comprehend text.

Finally, the students’ length of residence in Canada, an approximation of their language

exposure within an English speaking society, produced some differences in predictive power for

English and L1 comprehension between the two groups. Length of residence in Canada was an

indirect predictor of English RC, for both groups. Vocabulary knowledge meditated the

relationship between length of residence in Canada and English RC. However, the strength of

the relationship was significantly stronger for the Chinese-English than the Spanish English

bilinguals. Differences were also noted for the relationships between length of residence in

Canada and L1 RC and vocabulary. For Spanish-English bilinguals, length of residence in

Canada was negatively correlated with Spanish vocabulary and was an indirect predictor of RC.

No significant relationship or mediation occurred for the Chinese-English bilinguals.

Immigration experiences and patterns of language use may account for the relationships

observed. Since Chinese-English bilinguals have spent less time in Canada differences in

English language learning due to length of residence are likely to be more pronounced.

Alternatively, Spanish-English bilinguals have had more time to experience L1 language loss.

Furthermore, proportionally more Chinese-English bilinguals were enrolled in L1 language

classes than their Spanish-English peers, and a higher proportion of Chinese-English bilinguals

reported speaking to their parents only in Chinese. Therefore, formal and informal L1 language

learning experiences were still occurring to a greater degree for the Chinese-English than the

47

Page 58: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Spanish-English bilinguals. Overall, the results regarding immigration experiences demonstrate

that the socio-cultural context of language learning does have an impact on the relative strength

of the associations of linguistic skills on RC. The second study reported here examined how

changes in the within- and cross-language predictors of RC varied as a function of immigration

experiences.

Language-specific aspects of comprehension also emerged for the different groups under

study. Specifically, decoding was a more important predictor of comprehension for Chinese-

English and Spanish-English bilinguals. Additionally, aspects of morphological awareness

showed specific relationships inherent to the characteristics of the scripts being read.

Specifically, compound morphological awareness was more integral to Chinese vocabulary and

comprehension than it was in English. Overall, the results suggest that while universal aspects

are inherent to comprehension, one must understand the language-specific aspects as each

language embodies specific characteristics that influence the skills necessary for comprehension.

The theoretical implications of language-universal and language-specific aspects of

comprehension have implications for instruction of bilinguals with different language

backgrounds.

Limitations and Future Directions

While the current study presented novel findings on English and L1 RC for Spanish-

English and Chinese-English bilinguals, several limitations are noteworthy. First, RC and

vocabulary were each measured with a single task. Previous research has shown that different

measures of RC tap different underpinning skills for monolingual and bilingual readers. For

example, cloze tasks rely more on decoding, while reading short passages and answering

multiple choice questions appears to involve more oral language and vocabulary knowledge (e.g.,

48

Page 59: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Cutting & Scarborough, 2006; Keenan, Betjemann, & Olson, 2008; Leider et al., 2013). In a

similar vein, both breadth and depth are important for understanding the multifaceted nature of

vocabulary knowledge. Therefore, future studies should incorporate multiple measures of

vocabulary and RC to identify common and task specific aspects across different orthographies.

Second, the current study only found an indirect relationship between syntactic awareness and

RC. It is unclear whether the lack of a direct relationship is related to the way syntactic

awareness was measured. One way to potentially improve the syntactic awareness task would be

to offer a multiple-point scale of possible responses instead of a dichotomous “yes” or “no”

distinction regarding grammatical judgments. The improved measure may lead to better

estimation of syntactic awareness, and potentially improve predictive relationships with RC.

Finally, the sample size was relatively small in the current study, and model fit could have been

improved for the majority of fit indices. Future research should collect larger samples to allow

for more measures to be included in the analysis, which may also help improve model fit.

Conclusion and Implications

Results of the current study have both theoretical and practical implications. From a

theoretical standpoint, global constructs such as oral language proficiency are better understood

when underlying component skills are examined. The results of the current study echo a trend

that suggests an augmented SVR that includes more nuanced or discrete skills, and additional

cognitive and linguistic processes (such as non-verbal reasoning, working memory, and fluency),

creates a more accurate picture of the factors that underlie reading comprehension (Cain, Oakhill,

& Bryant, 2004; Farnia & Geva, 2013; Geva & Farnia, 2012; Kirby & Savage, 2008; Ouellette &

Beers, 2010). Specifically within the context of the current study, different aspects related to

listening comprehension were not equally important for RC performance. Vocabulary and

49

Page 60: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

morphological awareness emerged as consistently robust predictors, while syntactic awareness

played a more distal role in comprehension. Therefore, components of listening comprehension

should be considered separate (but interrelated) skills. Additionally, there were some language-

specific aspects found across groups, such as the importance of decoding for English RC for

Chinese-English but not Spanish-English bilinguals. Future research that investigates theoretical

models of RC should attempt to use measures that assess specific skills instead of global

proficiencies, and explore variations in predictor-outcome relationships across subgroups of

learners.

The results of the current study point towards universal aspects that are necessary for all

students to achieve proficiency in learning a language, especially a second language. Nurturing

vocabulary is an essential component of RC development. Explicit and systematic instruction is

critical for bilinguals considering the large vocabulary and achievement gap documented in

empirical research (Anglin, 1993; August et al., 2005; Verhallen & Schoonen, 1998). Increasing

morphological awareness may provide a fruitful venue to increase vocabulary and reading skills

as a growing body of research has demonstrated morphological awareness is intimately related to

vocabulary depth and comprehension performance in bilinguals (Kieffer & Lesaux, 2008; Leider

et al., 2013; Li & Kirby, 2013; Proctor et al., 2012). An instructional focus on vocabulary and

morphological awareness can occur in tandem for bilinguals from different language

backgrounds. Furthermore, the results suggest that bilingual students with different language

backgrounds may require a different balance of instructional practices to increase reading

proficiency. Regarding the differences, adolescent Chinese-English bilinguals may benefit from

explicit phonological awareness and decoding instruction to a greater extent than Spanish-

English bilinguals. Additionally, Chinese-English bilinguals may require added instruction for

50

Page 61: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

derivational aspects of morphological awareness as these features are encountered less often in

their L1. Overall, predictors of RC across English, Chinese and Spanish appear to be highly

similar among adolescent bilinguals, suggesting that RC is largely based upon universal skills

(e.g., vocabulary and morphological awareness) across different languages. Although there are

some language-specific aspects of RC, the similarities appear more substantial than the

differences.

51

Page 62: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 6: Study 2 - Cross-language Relationships of L1 and L2 RC: A Cross-cultural

Comparison of Learning to Read in Canada and China

The main focus of the present study was to explore the similarities and differences of

relationships between RC, vocabulary and word-level reading in Chinese and English for three

groups of bilinguals. Participants in this study comprised three groups of adolescents: (1)

Chinese-English bilinguals learning English in China (referred to as Chinese English as a

Foreign Language (EFL) learners), (2) recent Chinese-English bilingual immigrants to Canada

(referred to as recent immigrants), and (3) long-term Chinese-English bilingual immigrants to

Canada (referred to as long-term immigrants). The current study was situated within a model of

RC that examined the influences of word-level reading and vocabulary. Previous research has

identified that word-level reading and vocabulary explained individual differences in RC

performance for adolescent bilinguals (Pasquarella et al., 2012). However, studies have not

examined how predictors of comprehension vary as a function of students’ integration into an

English educational system. The current study aimed to extend this line of research by

examining how the components of the Simple View of Reading (SVR; Gough & Tunmer, 1986;

Hoover & Gough, 1990) are related to RC both within- and across-languages. An additional

purpose of the current study was to explore how predictors of RC may change as a function of

the immigration experience and the educational context for learning Chinese and English. In the

following section, different theoretical perspectives on cross-language relationships will be

discussed. Afterwards, previous research regarding the within- and cross-language relationships

for word-level reading, vocabulary and RC will be highlighted.

52

Page 63: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Defining Different Language Learning Environments

The three groups involved in the current study were: (1) Chinese EFL learners, (2) recent

immigrants, and (3) long-term immigrants. These groups were primarily defined by

demographic characteristics, such as educational experiences, exposure, language use, and length

of immigration inherent in their language learning environments. The Chinese EFL learners

were native Chinese nationals who were learning English as a foreign language at a university in

Beijing, China. Chinese was the societal language and the primary language of instruction in

school. English exposure, instruction and use were largely limited to the English language

classes. Very little English exposure and language use occurred outside the classroom. The

recent immigrants arrived during high school or upon graduation to complete an undergraduate

degree at a Canadian university. These students completed most of their education in China.

Once they had immigrated, these students were immersed in an English-speaking society and

were required to learn English at a rapid pace to keep up with the demands of an English

educational system. The long-term immigrants, on the other hand, typically arrived in Canada

during early elementary school or before. These students received most, if not all, of their

education in an English-speaking system. Long-term immigrants are bilinguals because their

parents were native Chinese speakers and the language spoken at home was Chinese. For both

immigrant groups, exposure to and use of Chinese predominantly occurred at home or through

heritage language classes outside of regular school hours. English was often used to

communicate with peers in and outside the classroom.

Cross-language Relationships between L1 and L2 Reading Skills

Cross-language relationships are defined as the use of previously acquired resources in

one language, typically the L1, to develop proficiency in another language, typically the L2

53

Page 64: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

(Odlin, 1989; Genesee, Geva, Dressler, & Kamil, 2006). Cummins (1979) posited the

developmental interdependence hypothesis, which states that L2 skills (or competence) are

partially a function of the types of skills (or competence) that are already developed in the L1.

Additionally, Cummins (1979) claimed that a “threshold” may exist where a certain (or

minimum) level of linguistic competence must be attained in the L1 and L2 to allow the potential

benefits of bilingualism to influence cognitive growth and linguistic proficiency, or to avoid

cognitive deficits in developing higher-level comprehension skills (e.g., inferencing). To extend

and refine the language interdependence hypothesis, specific underlying cognitive abilities that

support L1 and L2 literacy acquisition have been used to investigate cross-language relationships

(e.g., Geva & Ryan, 1993).

More recently, Koda (2007) proposed that cross-language relationships are influenced by

the linguistic distance between the L1 and L2. When there is a high degree of similarity between

the L1 and L2, cross-language relationships occur with little adjustment. However, when two

languages are distantly related, L1 skills do not facilitate L2 reading. By examining relationships

of L1 and L2 literacy skills with Chinese-English bilinguals, the primary objective of the current

study was examine the conditions that specify transfer for word-level reading, vocabulary and

RC. This theory posed that cross-language associations are thought to occur more easily

between languages that use the same alphabet and writing system (e.g., Spanish and English)

than between two languages that belong to two different writing systems (e.g., Chinese and

English).

Within- and Cross-language Relationships of Word-level Reading

Within this study, word-level reading is broadly defined and can refer to decoding, word

identification, and/or pseudoword reading. The orthographic representation of words varies

54

Page 65: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

substantially across English and Chinese. As a result, the nature of word-level reading differs.

English words can be read in primarily two different ways: (1) whole word identification and (2)

decoding (Ehri, 1997; Stanovich, 1991). Since Chinese is a logographic script where a character

represents a morpheme and a syllable, word reading primarily occurs through identification of

the whole character or identifying the semantic and radicals (Shu et al., 2003). Given the

differences between reading an alphabetic versus a logographic script, word-level reading was

chosen as the term used in the current study. Word-level reading is a more inclusive term than

decoding because it can be applied to both Chinese and English reading.

Studies with monolingual and bilingual readers have shown that the strength of

relationships between word-level reading and RC changes overtime. For young readers, word-

level reading explains a large amount of variance in RC (Catts, Hogan & Adolf, 1995; Cutting &

Scarborough, 2006; Joshi et al., 2012; Hoover & Gough, 1990; Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Keenan

et al., 2008; Lesaux, Lipka, & Siegel, 2006; Perfetti & Hogaboam, 1975). The association

decreases over time as word-level reading becomes automatized and text complexity increases

(Catts et al., 1995; Joshi et al., 2012; Perfetti, 2007). Research has only recently examined

whether decoding is a predictor of RC in bilingual adolescents. Pasquarella and colleagues

(2012) demonstrated that English word-level reading was an important predictor of English RC

for a group of recently immigrated bilingual adolescents. Word-level reading was a significant

predictor because this group of readers was still in the early stages of learning English and had

not yet mastered English decoding and word identification skills. Regarding Chinese RC,

Pasquarella et al. (2011) identified Chinese word-level reading as a significant predictor of

Chinese RC in Chinese-English bilinguals sampled across the first to fourth grades of elementary

55

Page 66: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

school. At least within languages, word-level reading skills appeared to be an important

component of Chinese and English RC for bilingual students.

Relatively few studies have reported cross-language associations of word-level reading to

RC. Furthermore, studies have predominately been conducted with children in the early stages

of learning to read, and have produced interesting results. In a study of Chinese-English

bilingual children in North America, Pasquarella et al. (2011) demonstrated a weak correlation

between English word reading and Chinese RC (r = .21, p < .05). However, English word

reading was not a significant predictor of Chinese RC, when controlling for Chinese word

reading, vocabulary and morphological skills. Furthermore, Chinese character reading was not

correlated with English RC. In another study with young Chinese-English bilinguals in North

America, Wang et al. (2006) also reported a significant correlation between English word

reading and Chinese RC (r = .38, p < .01). However, Chinese character reading and English RC

were not correlated. Finally, Li, McBride-Chang, Wong, and Shu (2012) examined longitudinal

predictors of RC in Chinese and English in 8-year-old Hong Kong Chinese-English bilinguals.

The authors report that English word reading was correlated with Chinese RC (r = .36, p <.05),

and Chinese word reading was correlated with English RC (r = .30, p <.05). However, word

reading was not a significant cross-language predictor of RC when controlling for gender,

father’s income, mother’s education, phonological skills, rapid automatized naming, Chinese

word reading or vocabulary.

Across studies, English word reading was consistently correlated with Chinese RC.

However, Chinese word reading was correlated with English RC in the Li et al. (2012) study but

not in the Pasquarella et al. (2011) and Wang et al. (2006) studies. The different patterns of

results across studies may be related to the educational context of learning English and Chinese.

56

Page 67: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

The Pasquarella et al. (2011) and Wang et al. (2006) studies were conducted in North America

where English was the language of instruction and children were learning decoding strategies to

read English. The Li et al. (2012) study was conducted in China where Cantonese was the

medium of instruction, and children were not exposed to English phonics instruction. Therefore,

it is possible the participants in the Li et al. (2012) study were relying more on a whole word

strategy to read English, which may have heightened the cross-language connection between

Chinese word reading and English RC. Given that different patterns of cross-language

associations were found across studies, comparisons across demographically different groups of

bilinguals are warranted. A goal of the current study was to extend research by examining cross-

language relationships of word-level reading with RC with adolescent bilinguals.

Within- and Cross-language Relationships of Vocabulary

Vocabulary is a robust predictor of RC performance and a critical component in the

acquisition and development of within-language comprehension skills (e.g., Nagy & Herman,

1987; Oullette, 2006; Pearson et al., 2007; Proctor et al., 2009). Results from Study 1 confirmed

vocabulary as an essential within-language component of English and Chinese comprehension

for adolescent bilinguals. Length of residence for the group of Chinese-English bilinguals was

directly related to vocabulary and indirectly related to English RC suggesting there may not be a

uniform influence of the relationship between these variables for students with different lengths

of residency in Canada. The current study further explored the effect of length of residence on

learning by comparing the relationships between vocabulary and RC among the three groups of

Chinese-English bilinguals: those who were in China, and recent or long-term immigrants to

Canada.

57

Page 68: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Research regarding cross-language relationships between vocabulary and RC is limited

for Chinese-English bilinguals. In their study of Chinese-English bilingual children in Hong

Kong, Li et al. (2012) indicated that Chinese vocabulary was a significant predictor of English

RC after accounting for the influence of English word reading, vocabulary, phonological

awareness, and mother’s education. The authors suggested that their results reveal a role of L1

processes and abilities in second language reading. Considering students were in the early stages

of learning English and had very limited English word reading and vocabulary knowledge, it was

not surprising that there was no cross-language prediction of either English word reading or

vocabulary to Chinese RC. Overall, the results demonstrated that vocabulary in one language

can contribute to RC in the other language for Chinese-English bilinguals.

On the other hand, a study with Chinese-English children conducted within an English

speaking country reported non-significant relationships between vocabulary and RC across

languages (Pasquarella et al., 2011). Given the dearth of empirical research of the relationship

between vocabulary and RC across languages further research in this area is warranted. We

cannot yet determine why the existing studies are producing different findings. To help solve

this problem, the current study examined how vocabulary contributed to RC, both within- and

across-languages, for groups of adolescent bilinguals within different language learning

experiences.

Cross-language Relationships of RC

Of the relatively few studies that have examined RC in the L1 and L2, results have shown

positive associations across languages. In a longitudinal study following children from the 3rd to

6th grade, Nakamoto, Lindsay and Manis (2008) assessed Spanish-English bilinguals on

comparable measures of decoding, oral language, and RC in English and Spanish. Results

58

Page 69: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

indicated that there were strong associations between RC factors in Spanish and English (r = .83,

p < .001), suggesting L1 and L2 comprehension skills are distinct but highly related factors. In

series of studies, Van Geledren, Schoonen, Glopper, Hulstijin, Simis, Snellings and Stevenson

(2004) examined Dutch-English bilinguals in the 8th grade who had received an average of 1.5

years of English instruction in school. Given this limited exposure to English, these students had

relatively low levels of English vocabulary. Results indicated significant relationships between

RC in the L1 and L2. In a longitudinal follow-up of the same participants, van Gelderen and

colleagues (2007) confirmed significant relationships between RC in the L1 and L2 in grade 10.

Interestingly, the relationships between L1 and L2 RC appeared to strengthen over time,

suggesting that L1 and L2 comprehension became more aligned as time progressed. The results

of these studies on Dutch-English bilinguals were consistent with the significant relationship

between Spanish and English RC reported by Nakamoto et al. (2008).

Two studies have examined the relationships between RC skills in English and Chinese.

Pasquarella et al. (2011) reported a significant correlation between Chinese and English RC

skills in their study of bilingual children in the first to fourth grades. Li et al. (2012) extended

the Pasquarella et al. (2011) study by reporting that English RC was a significant predictor of

Chinese RC, and vice versa, in a sample of Chinese-English bilingual children in Hong Kong.

The significant bi-directional relationships between L1 and L2 RC survived within-language

controls of phonological awareness, processing speed, word reading, and vocabulary in Chinese

and English.

Across all studies, a consistent pattern of significant relationships between RC in the L1

and L2 were reported. It is important to note that the Li et al. (2012) and Van Geledren and

colleagues (2004, 2007) studies were conducted within a language learning environment where

59

Page 70: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

English exposure was confined to instruction within the classroom, and the societal language was

the L1. The Nakamoto et al. (2008) and Pasquarella et al. (2011) studies were conducted within

an English speaking society and educational system. Despite these cultural differences

significant relationships between L1 and L2 RC were reported. The extent to which these

findings apply to adolescent Chinese-English bilinguals from different language learning

environments is unknown. The current study extended previous research by examining patterns

of cross-language relationships of RC in groups of bilinguals with remarkably different L1 and

L2 learning experiences.

Objectives

The purpose of the current study was to explore the within- and cross-language

relationships between word-level reading, vocabulary, and RC for L1 and L2 reading skills of

Chinese-English bilinguals. The current study extended previous research by examining how

within- and cross-language predictors of L1 and L2 RC may vary for adolescent bilinguals in

different language learning environments. Comparisons were made across three groups that

varied in their exposure to and use of the L1 and L2. The three groups of Chinese-English

bilinguals were: (1) Chinese EFL learners, (2) recent immigrants, (3) long-term immigrants.

To examine within- and cross-language relationships, measures of word-level reading,

vocabulary and RC were administered in both English and Chinese. Hierarchical linear

regression and commonality analyses were used to identify the unique and shared aspects of RC

in the L1 and L2. The advantage of conducting a commonality analysis in addition to the linear

regressions is that it can help understand common and unique contributions of L1 and L2 skills

to RC.

60

Page 71: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 7: Method

Participants

Participants comprised 57 English majors in Beijing, China (Chinese EFL learners), 60

Chinese-English bilinguals who were recent immigrants to Canada (recent immigrants), and 57

Chinese-English bilinguals who were long-term immigrants to Canada (long-term immigrants).

Chinese-English bilinguals who had lived in Canada for six years or less were classified as

recent immigrants; those who had lived in Canada for more than 6 years were classified as long-

term immigrants. Previous research has indicated that, on average, it takes approximately 6

years of schooling for most immigrants to reach average levels on standardized measures of

academic achievement and to gain proficiency in academic English (Collier, 1987). Therefore, a

cut-score of 6 years was chose to help distinguish groups of immigrants. Both immigrant groups

included students enrolled in high school or in undergraduate university programs in southern

Ontario.

The Chinese EFL learners had a mean age of 20.20 years (SD = 0.66 years), and the

majority (91%) were female. All students spoke Mandarin as their first language and had been

living in a Chinese-speaking society for their entire lives. Approximately half of the sample had

parents who had completed a high school education or less, another 40% had completed college,

and the remaining 10% completed professional or post-graduate degrees. The recent immigrants

had a mean age of 19.22 years old (SD = 2.16) and 80% were females. The recent immigrants

had been living in Canada for approximately 3.42 years (SD = 1.58 years). All students spoke

Mandarin as their first language, and 86% were from China, 10% from Taiwan, and the

remainder were from Indonesia and Malaysia. Approximately 60% of the group had parents

who had completed college or university, 15% of parents had completed only high school or less,

61

Page 72: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

and the remaining 25% of parents had completed a post-graduate or professional degree. For the

long-term immigrants, the average age was 17.36 years old (SD = 1.88 years) and 63% were

female. On average, this group had spent 10.53 years in Canada (SD = 3.12 years). All students

spoke Mandarin as their L1, and 90% were originally from China with the other 10% were from

Taiwan. Fifty-five percent of the group reported parents who had completed college or

university, 20% who had completed high school or less, and 25% who had completed a

professional or post-graduate degree.

The use of Chinese varied across the three groups. For the Chinese EFL learners,

approximately 96% of the group reported always speaking to their parents in Chinese. The other

4% reported some conversations in English, but the majority occurred in Chinese. Eighty-eight

percent of the sample reported always speaking to their friends in Chinese. The other 12%

reported speaking to their friends occasionally in English, but the majority of conversations

occurred in Chinese. For the recent immigrants, 82% of the group reporting always speaking to

their parents in Chinese. The other 18% reported frequently speaking to their parents in Chinese,

and occasionally in English. Peer interactions, on the other hand, were more balanced in this

group. The majority (60%) reported speaking to their peers in both English and Chinese. Only

20% reported always speaking with their peers in Chinese, while almost 20% reported always

speaking with their peers in English. Finally, for the long-term immigrants, 67% reported

speaking with their parents only in Chinese. Another 25% reported frequently speaking with

their parents in Chinese. The remainder of the group (8%) reported rarely speaking with their

parents in Chinese. Interactions with peers, on the other hand, occurred predominately in

English, with 72% of the sample reported speaking with their friends only in English. Another

62

Page 73: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

23% reported frequently using English and occasionally using Chinese, while the remaining 5%

reported using both Chinese and English some of the time.

Measures

The same Chinese and English measures described in Study 1 were used in Study 2.

Reading Comprehension. The Second Canadian Edition of the Gates MacGinitie RC

(MacGinitie et al., 1992) test was used to assess RC in English, and the alternative form

translation was used for Chinese.

For this study, the Chinese EFL learners received Level F of the Chinese translation of the Gates

MacGinitie and Level E for the English test. Pilot testing revealed Level F of the English task

was too difficult as the majority of students responded at the level of chance. For the recent and

long-term immigrants, both levels E and F were used for the Chinese and English tasks to ensure

that students worked through the test most appropriate for their age and ability (Level E was used

for students in High school and Level F was used for students in University). For the English

RC measures, Cronbach’s alpha was .71 for the Chinese EFL learners, .84 for the recent

immigrants and .93 for the long-term immigrants. Cronbach’s alpha calculated for the Chinese

comprehension task was .70 for the Chinese EFL learners, .89 for the recent immigrants, and .93

for the long-term immigrants.

Vocabulary. The Second Canadian Edition of the Gates MacGinitie Vocabulary test

(MacGinitie et al., 1992) was used to assess vocabulary knowledge in English. As with the

comprehension tests, the EFL students received Level F for Chinese and Level E for English. For

the recent and long-term immigrants, both levels E and F were used to ensure that students

worked through the test most appropriate for their age and ability. For the English vocabulary

measures, Cronbach’s alpha was .77 for the Chinese EFL learners, .89 for the recent immigrants,

63

Page 74: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

and .94 for the long-term immigrants. Cronbach’s alpha for the Chinese vocabulary task for the

EFL group was .76, and .89 for the recent immigrants, and .93 for the long-term immigrants,

respectively.

Word-level reading. To measure English decoding, the pseudoword reading subtest,

Word Attack from the WRMT-R (Woodcock, 1987), was administered. Cronbach’s alpha

was .77 for the EFL group, .72 for the recent immigrant group, and .86 for the long term

immigrant group.

To measure Chinese character reading a 240 item experimental task was used.

Cronbach’s alpha was .76 for the EFL group, and .99 for both the recent immigrant and the long-

term immigrant groups.

Demographic questionnaire. Participants were asked to fill out a questionnaire to

collect information on their age, grade, country of origin, age of immigration, and length of

residence in Canada. Also, the questionnaire asked students to indicate their parents’ level of

education. The questionnaire also asked students to report how often they spoke English and

their L1 with family and friends, using a 5 point Likert-type scale (never-rarely-sometimes-

often-frequently). Finally, students reported whether they were currently enrolled in L1

language class (with a yes or no response) and how many hours each week they spent in the class.

Procedures

The participants completed the Chinese and English measures in two or three testing

sessions. The vocabulary and RC tests and demographic questionnaire, were given as part of a

larger battery of group-administered measures. The English decoding and Chinese character

reading tasks were given as part of a larger individually-administered testing session. For the

EFL group, the Chinese measures were administered first, followed by the English measures.

64

Page 75: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

The English measures were administered before the Chinese measures to the two immigrant

groups. The total testing time was 2 hours, including about 1 hour for English and 1 hour for

Chinese. All measures were administered by a trained research assistant.

65

Page 76: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 8: Results

Descriptive Statistics and Group Differences

Table 10 displays minimum and maximum scores, raw means and standard deviations for

age, length of residence in Canada, English and Chinese word-level reading, vocabulary and RC.

Skewness and kurtosis values were examined for all measures. Chinese word reading was

negatively skewed for the EFL group. Additionally, Chinese vocabulary was negatively skewed

for the long-term immigrant group. All other variables were normally distributed. Corrections

to normality, as per Tabachnick and Fidell (2007), did not change patterns of correlations,

regressions or commonality analyses. As such, subsequent analyses were conducted on

untransformed scores.

To check whether administering both levels E and F to the recent and long-term

immigrant groups influenced the results, analyses (i.e., t-tests, correlations, regressions and

commonality analyses) were conducted using both raw scores and extended scale scores. These

analyses produced identical findings. Therefore, for simplicity only raw score analyses are

presented below.

A multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted to test for differences

among the three groups (EFL students, recent immigrants, long-term immigrants) in age, months

in Canada, English and Chinese word reading, vocabulary and RC. There was a statistically

significant difference between the three groups on these variables, Wilks’ λ = .074, F (16, 328) =

55.09, p < .001. Univariate ANOVAs and Tukey’s post-hoc comparisons (see Table 10)

revealed that age was significantly different across the three groups. On average, the Chinese

EFL learners were the oldest by approximately one year. The long-term immigrants were almost

two years younger than the recent immigrants and three years younger than the Chinese EFL

66

Page 77: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

learners. Length of residence was significant different across the three groups. As expected, the

recent immigrants had been in Canada significantly shorter time that the long-term immigrants.

It is important to note that length of residence was normally distributed for both groups, and a

few participants (n = 10 in total) were centered on the cut-point of 6 years. Regarding English

decoding, vocabulary and RC, the long-term immigrants scored highest, followed by the recent

immigrants and then the Chinese EFL learners. For Chinese character reading and vocabulary

measures, the Chinese EFL learners scored highest, followed by the recent immigrants and then

the long-term immigrants. For Chinese RC, the Chinese EFL learners and the recent immigrants

scored similarly, and both outperformed the long-term immigrants.

Correlations

Table 11 presents correlations for age, English and Chinese word reading, vocabulary and

comprehension for the Chinese EFL learners in the top panel, the recent immigrants in the

middle, and the long-term immigrants in the bottom panel. For the Chinese EFL learners,

English RC was correlated with both English decoding and vocabulary. Chinese RC was

correlated with Chinese character reading and vocabulary. All Chinese measures were positively

correlated with English RC. Age was not correlated with any variable in English or Chinese.

For the recent immigrants, English RC was related to English decoding and vocabulary.

Chinese RC was related to Chinese character reading and vocabulary. English RC was

positively related to Chinese RC. Additionally, English vocabulary was negatively related to

Chinese character reading. Age was positively correlated with Chinese character reading,

vocabulary and comprehension.

For the long-term immigrants, English RC was related to English decoding and

vocabulary. Likewise, Chinese RC was related to Chinese character reading and vocabulary.

67

Page 78: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

English RC was not related to Chinese RC. Instead, English comprehension was negatively

related to Chinese character reading and vocabulary. Additionally, English vocabulary was

negatively related to Chinese character reading, vocabulary and comprehension. Age was

negatively related to English vocabulary and comprehension, and positively related to Chinese

character reading, vocabulary and RC.

Hierarchical Linear Regressions and Commonality Analyses

Tables 12 and 13 present hierarchical regressions which were used to identify within- and

cross-language predictors of RC in English (Table 12) and Chinese (Table 13) for the three

groups of bilinguals. For each regression, within-language relationships of word-level reading

and vocabulary were entered before cross-language predictors.4 For example, when English RC

acted as the outcome variable, English pseudoword reading and vocabulary were entered into the

first and second steps, respectively. Chinese character reading, vocabulary and RC were entered

in the third, fourth and fifth steps, respectively. Table 12 displays unstandarized coefficients,

standard errors, and standardized coefficients when English RC acted as the outcome variable.

For the Chinese EFL learners, 41% of the variance was explained by the regression model.

English vocabulary was a significant within-language predictor and Chinese vocabulary and RC

were unique cross-language predictors. For the recent immigrants, 73% of the variance was

explained by the regression model. English decoding and vocabulary were significant within-

language predictors and Chinese RC was a significant cross-language predictor. For the long-

4 Initially, age was added as a control variable in the first step within the regressions. However,

it was always a non-significant predictor across the three groups and did not influence any other

predictor-outcome relationships. Therefore, it was removed from the subsequent analyses to

simplify the regressions and commonality analyses.

68

Page 79: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

term bilingual immigrants, 71% of the variance was explained by the regression model. Only

English vocabulary knowledge was a significant predictor of English RC.

Table 13 reports unstandarized coefficients, standard errors, and standardized coefficients

when Chinese RC acted as the outcome variable. For the EFL and recent immigrant groups,

43% and 77% of the variance was respectively explained by the regression models. Also for

both groups, Chinese vocabulary was a significant within-language predictor, and English RC

also positively predicted Chinese RC. On the other hand, English vocabulary was a negative

predictor of Chinese RC for both groups. Considering there was no significant zero-order

correlation between English vocabulary and Chinese RC, the relationships reported in the

regression need further clarification as variable suppression may be occurring (Zientek &

Thompson, 2010). For the long-term immigrant group, 77% of the variance was explained by

the regression models. Chinese vocabulary was the only significant predictor of Chinese RC.

To gain a better understanding of the cross-language relationships, commonality analyses

were conducted separately for the three groups of bilinguals to clarify aspects of shared and

unique variance. Examining both beta weights and structural coefficients – where structural

coefficients are Pearson correlation coefficients between a predictor variable and the predicted

outcome score (i.e., Ŷ) - is a more useful data analytic technique than just examining one or the

other (Mood, 1971; Newton & Spurrell, 1967; Pedhazur, 1997; Nimon & Reio, 2011; Zientek &

Thompson, 2010). The commonality analyses contained the same variables as the regression

models described previously.

Table 14 displays beta weights, structural coefficients, total variance, common variance

and unique variance for the within- and cross-language predictors of English RC. For the

Chinese EFL learners, the beta weights were largest for English vocabulary, Chinese vocabulary

69

Page 80: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

and Chinese RC, and structural coefficients were moderate-to-large for all within- and cross-

language variables. For the significant cross-language predictors, variance was mostly shared

with other predictors. For the recent immigrants, beta weights were largest for English

vocabulary and Chinese RC, and moderate for English decoding and Chinese vocabulary.

However, the beta weight for Chinese vocabulary and English RC was negative. Given that the

structural coefficient was near-zero, Chinese vocabulary appeared to act as a suppressor variable

when predicting English RC for the recent immigrants. For the long-term immigrants, English

vocabulary knowledge shared a near perfect relationship with English RC.

Table 15 displays the variance coefficient (proportion of variance explained) and the

percentage of explained variance for each predictor and all combinations of predictors when

English RC acted as the dependent variable. I was most interested in the unique aspect of the

cross-language predictors and the combinations of common variance components among the

independent variables that included the cross-language predictors. Therefore, I focused on cross-

language vocabulary and RC because these relationships varied across groups.

For the Chinese EFL learners, Chinese vocabulary and RC uniquely explained 8% and

14% of the variance in English RC. Additionally, approximately 13% of the common variance

was shared with Chinese vocabulary, 16% with Chinese comprehension, and 40% with the

combination of Chinese vocabulary and Chinese RC and the within-language predictors. For the

recent immigrants, Chinese vocabulary and RC accounted for approximately 2% and 14% of the

variance in English RC respectively. Twenty-four percent of the common variance was shared

with Chinese vocabulary and 27% with Chinese comprehension. Interestingly, the combination

of shared variance accounted by Chinese vocabulary and Chinese RC was -26%. Considering

that Chinese vocabulary was acting as a suppressor variable and explained negative variance

70

Page 81: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

when combined with Chinese RC, it seemed that Chinese vocabulary suppressed the relationship

between Chinese and English RC for the recent immigrants. Further analyses revealed that not

including Chinese vocabulary in the regression predicting English comprehension decreased the

strength of the association between English and Chinese RC, however this cross-language

relationship was still significant5. For the long-term immigrants, Chinese vocabulary and RC

explained virtually no unique variance. The amount of common variance among the cross-

language predictors was also low at 19% for vocabulary, 7% for comprehension, and -4% for the

combination of L1 vocabulary and RC.

Table 16 reports beta weights, structural coefficients, total variance, common variance

and unique variance for the within- and cross-language predictors of Chinese RC. For the

Chinese EFL learners, the beta weights were largest for Chinese vocabulary and English RC.

English vocabulary produced a negative beta weight with Chinese comprehension. Structural

coefficients were moderate-to-large for all within- and cross-language variables, except for a

weak negative coefficient for English vocabulary. For the significant cross-language predictors,

English vocabulary and English RC, variance was mostly shared with other predictors. For the

recent immigrants, beta weights were largest for Chinese vocabulary and English RC. Similar to

the Chinese EFL learners, English vocabulary produced a negative beta weight with Chinese RC.

Given that the structural coefficient was near-zero for both the Chinese EFL learners and recent

immigrants, English vocabulary acted as a suppressor variable when predicting Chinese RC for

both groups. Further analyses also revealed that not including English vocabulary in the

regression predicting Chinese comprehension underestimated the strength of the association

5Given that it is strongly suggested to retain suppressor variables in the regression analyses (e.g., Padey & Elliott, 2010; Thompson, 2006; Zientek & Thompson, 2010), omitting Chinese vocabulary when predicting English reading comprehension would lead to underestimating the relationship between reading comprehension across languages for the recent immigrants.

71

Page 82: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

between English and Chinese RC, however this cross-language relationship was still significant

for both groups. For the recent immigrants, there was more unique than shared variance

explained by the cross-language predictors. Finally for the long-term immigrant group, Chinese

vocabulary shared the strongest relationships with Chinese comprehension.

Table 17 displays the variance coefficient (proportion of variance explained) and the

percentage of explained variance for each predictor and all combinations of predictors when

Chinese RC acted as the dependent variable. For the Chinese EFL learners, English vocabulary

and English RC uniquely explained 16% and 14% of the variance in Chinese RC, respectively.

The common variance shared with English vocabulary, and the combination of English

vocabulary and English RC was negative (-1% and -10% respectively), whereas a large amount

of common variance was shared with English comprehension (46%). There appeared to be

considerable overlap between L1 and L2 comprehension skills. Alternatively, English

vocabulary knowledge acted as a suppressor variable when predicting Chinese RC for the EFL

group. The suppression effect resulted in underestimation of the relationship between Chinese

and English RC if English vocabulary was not included in the model.

For the recent immigrants, English vocabulary and English RC explained approximately

2% and 11% of the unique variance in Chinese RC. Additionally, the common variance shared

with English vocabulary was approximately 17%, and 21% for English RC. Like the Chinese

EFL learners, the common variance for the combination of English vocabulary and English RC

was negative (-21%), suggesting that English vocabulary was acted as a suppressor variable for

the recent immigrant group. For the long-term immigrants, English vocabulary and RC

explained virtually no unique variance with Chinese RC. English vocabulary shared 35% of the

72

Page 83: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

variance with other predictors, English comprehension shared 13% and the combination of

English vocabulary and comprehension was -12%.

73

Page 84: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 9: Discussion

The purpose of the current study was to identify how the within- and cross-language

contributions of word-level reading, vocabulary and RC in Chinese, the L1, and English, the L2,

varied as a function of the bilingual students’ language learning environment. Comparable

measures in the L1 and L2 were administered to three groups of bilinguals with distinctly

different experiences regarding exposure to and use of the L1 and L2. The Chinese EFL learners

experienced L2 learning within the confines of instruction only, where the L1 was the societal

language and was almost solely used to communicate with family and peers. The recent

immigrants received most of their education in China and moved to Canada during high school

or to receive an education at a Canadian university. Generally, these students were integrated

into an English education system during adolescence. The long-term immigrants moved to

Canada during elementary school or earlier and received most, if not all, of their education in an

English system. They came from families where the home language was Chinese.

Performance on word-level reading, vocabulary and RC across groups generally followed

trends that would be expected given the differences in the groups’ language learning

environments. The long-term immigrants were most proficient in their English skills and least

proficient in their Chinese skills. Also as expected, the Chinese EFL learners performed the

highest on all Chinese skills and scored the lowest on all English skills (with the exception of the

Chinese EFL learners and the recent immigrants having comparable performance on Chinese

RC). The recent immigrants’ performance was typically in-between the other two groups,

barring the exception noted above.

The regressions and commonality analyses produced similarities and differences among

predictors of English RC across groups. For the Chinese EFL learners, English RC was

74

Page 85: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

positively predicted by English vocabulary, Chinese vocabulary and Chinese RC. When

predicting English RC for the recent immigrants, English decoding, English vocabulary and

Chinese RC were positive predictors. No other significant relationships were identified. Finally,

for the long-term immigrants, the only significant predictor of English RC was English

vocabulary.

The results for Chinese RC also produced similarities and differences across groups. For

the Chinese EFL learners, significant positive predictors of Chinese RC were Chinese

vocabulary and English RC. English vocabulary was a significant negative predictor of Chinese

RC. However, the supplemental commonality analyses revealed that English vocabulary was a

suppressor variable in the regression. For the recent immigrants, Chinese vocabulary and

English RC were positive predictors of Chinese RC. Similar to the Chinese EFL learners,

English vocabulary was a negative predictor of Chinese RC. The commonality analyses also

revealed a suppression effect for the recent immigrants. For both suppression results, not

including English vocabulary in the model underestimated the strength of the relationships

between Chinese and English RC. For the long-term immigrants, Chinese vocabulary was the

only significant predictor of Chinese RC.

The SVR (Gough & Tunmer, 1986; Hoover & Gough, 1990) is a useful framework for

understanding the within- and cross-language predictors of RC in adolescent bilinguals coming

from different backgrounds. At the same time, the results suggest that models of RC are

distinctly different across groups of Chinese-English bilinguals in diverse language learning

environments, and these groups of bilinguals utilized their L1 and L2 skills differently to

comprehend text. The following sections will outline the within- and cross-language

relationships of word-level reading, vocabulary and RC for the three groups of bilinguals.

75

Page 86: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

The Role of Word-level Reading Within and Across Languages

English word-level reading was only a significant within-language predictor of English

RC for the recent immigrants. The results appear to suggest that recent immigrants were still

mastering decoding and individual differences were predictive of RC performance. Word-level

reading was not a unique predictor of RC in English or Chinese for the Chinese EFL learners or

long-term immigrants. The lack of relationships between English decoding and English RC may

be interpreted differently for the two groups. For the Chinese EFL learners, English education

often does not include phonics instruction (Keung & Ho, 2009). Therefore, the Chinese EFL

learners may be relying on a whole-word strategy during the RC task, which would result in

decoding skills being less related to English RC. On the other hand, the long-term immigrants

may have mastered decoding to a point where it was no longer predictive of RC performance.

Chinese word-level reading was a significant within-language predictor of Chinese RC

for the recent immigrants. However, word-level reading was not a predictor of Chinese RC for

the Chinese EFL learners or the long-term immigrants. The significant relationship for the

recent immigrants and the lack of relationships for the Chinese EFL learners and the long-term

immigrants may be understood by considering the scores obtained on the Chinese word-reading

task. For the recent immigrants there was substantial variability in the measure and no evidence

of floor or ceiling effects. On the other hand, the Chinese EFL learners did very well and there

was hardly any variability in performance which would have prevented the measure from

significantly predicting RC. Alternatively, the long-term immigrants did poorly, and there was

large variability in performance on the Chinese word reading task. Therefore, a floor effect may

have prevented the measure from significantly predicting RC for the long-term immigrants.

76

Page 87: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Consistent with other research regarding cross-language relationships of word-level

reading skills in Chinese and English (e.g. Gottardo et al., 2001), there were no instances of

cross-language relationships within the regression results. Considering there is a large degree of

linguistic distance between reading Chinese logographic characters and reading English words, it

is not surprising that there is no relationship across the L1 and L2. However, examination of

structural coefficients across groups revealed different patterns. Moderate to strong structural

coefficients between word-reading skills and comprehension across languages were evident for

the Chinese EFL learners. Specifically, a strong structural coefficient (.63) was noted for

Chinese word-level reading and English RC, while the structural coefficient between English

word-level reading (a decoding task) and Chinese comprehension was moderate (.36). At the

same time, near zero (or negative) coefficients were evident for the recent and long-term

immigrants. Perhaps the Chinese EFL learners are utilizing more familiar skills, such as a

reliance on a whole word activation strategy, for reading both Chinese and English. While this

strategy is useful for Chinese word-level reading, it is not as helpful when reading English

pseudowords. This interpretation may help clarify the Chinese EFL learners’ poorer

performance on the English word-level reading task in comparison to the other groups, and the

moderate relationship observed between English word-level reading and Chinese RC. This

interpretation is tentative and requires further investigation. Most importantly, a trend emerged

showing that as integration into an L2 system occur word-level reading skills in Chinese and

English become less related. Perhaps, as integration into a L2 system increases, L1 and L2

reading separate and become modularized and more language specific.

77

Page 88: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

The Role of Vocabulary Within and Across Languages

Vocabulary was a within-language predictor of RC across groups. Within-language

semantic knowledge was critically important for L1 and L2 comprehension across diverse groups

of language learners. Vocabulary appears to be a stable predictor of comprehension and

consistently important in the L1 and L2 throughout the process of integration into a L2 society.

However, cross-language associations of vocabulary knowledge were far less consistent.

Patterns of cross-language associations differed when either the L1 or L2 acted as the outcome

variable, and also differed across groups.

Chinese vocabulary and English RC were positively related for the EFL group, which

replicated and extended the work of Li et al. (2012). The positive cross-language association in

the current study suggested that the Chinese-English EFL learners were able to utilize semantic

knowledge in their L1 to help comprehend English. The Chinese EFL learners had poor

performance in English vocabulary and superior performance in Chinese vocabulary when

compared to the other Chinese-English bilingual groups. L1 semantic skills appear to be

important in L2 reading during early stages of L2 acquisition. Considering that Chinese and

English do not have cognates, it may be semantic knowledge in the L1 that is related to reading

in the L2. Students who have well-developed semantic knowledge in the L1 may be more

proficient at producing L2 to L1 translations when reading the L2 than students with less

developed L1 semantic knowledge. Additionally, students who have well developed semantic

knowledge in the L1 may also have an easier time understanding a similar concept encountered

in the L2. However, given that only the current study and the Li et al. (2012) study have

reported positive associations between Chinese vocabulary and English RC, further investigation

is necessary to understand the nature of these cross-language associations. No positive

78

Page 89: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

associations between L1 vocabulary and L2 RC were found for the other groups. It appears that

L1 semantic knowledge facilitates L2 comprehension only for Chinese EFL learners. Perhaps

the language learning environment may have influenced the cross-language associations. It may

be that English instruction in China emphasizes translations and using well-developed L1

knowledge to build L2 vocabulary and reading skills, while in Canada there may be less reliance

and instruction on using translation knowledge to build L2 reading and language skills.

Within this study, positive associations between L1 vocabulary and L2 comprehension

and negative associations between L2 vocabulary and L1 comprehension were demonstrated.

While the differences in these relationships confirm that asymmetrical relationships between

vocabulary and RC across languages may exist, the negative association is interpreted as

statistical suppression. The suppression effect of English vocabulary underestimated the strength

of the association between Chinese and English comprehension. Since within-language

vocabulary skills are an important predictor of within-language comprehension skills it appears

to be necessary to consider these two constructs together when examining cross-language

relationships.

The Role of RC Across Languages

The current study reported bidirectional associations of RC across Chinese and English

for the Chinese EFL learners and recent immigrants. Cross-language associations, however,

were not found among the long-term immigrants, who had immigrated to Canada at a young age.

The results of the current study showed cross-language relationships of comprehension for

students in the early stages of learning English who were proficient in their L1. These results

replicate and extended the work of Li et al. (2012), who reported bidirectional cross-language

relationships between L1 and L2 RC in younger Chinese-English bilinguals.

79

Page 90: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

The bi-directional transfer of comprehension skills for the Chinese EFL learners and the

recent immigrants indicated that L1 and L2 comprehension was closely related for learners who

were developing proficiency in their L2. L1 RC predicted unique variance for the Chinese EFL

learners and recent immigrants but not for the long-term immigrants. Likewise, shared variance

among English and L1 predictors was substantial for all groups except the long-term immigrants.

However, as residency in an L2 educational system and culture increased, comprehension

appeared to become more language specific. The finding that L1 and L2 comprehension for the

long-term immigrants was only predicted by within-language vocabulary skills supported the

idea that L1 and L2 RC became a language specific skill. Additionally, proportions of unique

and common variance explained by cross-language RC were virtually non-existent for the long-

term immigrants. These results suggest that as exposure in an L2 environment increases L1 and

L2 skills become less related.

What is the nature of the cross-language relationships between L1 and L2 comprehension

for the Chinese EFL learners and recent bilingual immigrants? Van Gelderen et al. (2007)

proposed that the locus of transfer is most likely metacognitive in nature, when explaining the

relationship between L1 and L2 comprehension skills of Dutch-English bilinguals. Higher-level

comprehension skills such as strategy use and the ability to construct meaning from text are

skills that may transfer and explain why comprehension is related in the L1 and L2. It is highly

plausible that comprehension strategies also transfer between Chinese and English, and this

transfer explains the results in the current study. Bilingual students with relatively low L2

proficiency may rely heavily upon metacognitive skills to comprehend an unfamiliar language.

However, as L2 proficiencies develop, within-language skills become much better predictors of

L2 performance. Chinese EFL learners, recent, and long-term immigrants may adopt different

80

Page 91: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

strategies and utilize different skills to read the L1 and L2. Future studies should examine the

use of comprehension strategies in Chinese-English bilinguals to examine whether a strategy

learned in one language can be used during reading in another language. Additionally,

examining how L1 and L2 proficiency interacts with transfer of strategies across languages

would improve our understanding regarding the nature of the relationships between RC in the L1

and L2.

Conditions that Support Cross-language Relationships

Patterns of cross-language relationships were substantially different across groups of

Chinese-English bilinguals from different language-learning environments. The patterns of

cross-language relationships that emerged in the current study demonstrates that transfer is more

likely to occur for students who are proficient in their L1 and in the early stages of learning an

L2. Even though there is considerable linguistic distance (Koda, 2007) between the L1 and L2,

cross-language relationships were still evident for students in the relatively early stages of L2

acquisition. Furthermore, cross-language relationships varied as a function of the skill under

examination. Strongest patterns of cross-language relationships were found for RC while no

cross-language relationships were found for word-level reading. These patterns suggest that

higher-level skills may transfer more easily than lower-level reading skills in cases where there is

a large degree of linguistic distance between L1 and L2 orthography.

Limitations and Future Directions

A limitation of the current study may be the specific cut-point of length of residence used

to help determine the groups. It is possible that the immigrant groups could have been divided

differently. Using a latent class analysis to sub-divide the immigrant participants would have

been helpful to inform specific cut-points, however the modest sample size of the current study

81

Page 92: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

prevented this from being an option. Also, the age differences between the groups and the

unbalanced proportions of males and females within each group is another limitation of the study.

Future research should attempt to compare groups of bilinguals who are more similar in age.

Additionally, having a balanced proportion of males and females would allow us to rule out

differences due to gender in terms of performance or predictors of RC. An additional limitation

is the use of experimental translation tasks to assess L1 vocabulary and RC. While great care

was taken to ensure the translations were as accurate as possible, the authenticity of these

measures is not validated. Future research should attempt to develop and standardize Chinese

vocabulary and RC measures that are appropriate for adolescents with a wide range of language

and reading proficiency in Chinese. Furthermore, the Chinese word-level reading task had a

floor effect for the long-term immigrants and did not capture enough variance for the Chinese

EFL learners. Further development of this task should include higher frequency words when

used with long-term immigrants or students with low levels of proficiency. Alternatively,

additional lower frequency words should be included for students with high levels of Chinese

proficiency. Another suggestion is to add measures of word-reading fluency, as they may

capture more variance and help prevent floor effects in low-proficiency speakers. A future

direction is to include other groups of bilinguals who spoke different L1s. Including bilinguals

with an alphabetic L1 who also varied in terms of differences in their language-learning

environments would have improved the generalizability of the current study.

Given that there are stark differences between patterns of within- and cross-language

contributions to literacy skills in different learning environments, the next step is to investigate

the ways in which these different environments influence models of RC and cross-language

associations. For example: how do acculturation, engagement, and language use interact on an

82

Page 93: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

individual level with within- and cross-language predictors of reading performance, and more

importantly reading development? Understanding individual differences in socio-cultural and

motivational factors and how they interact with cognitive and linguistic models of RC is a

natural extension of the current findings presented within this study. A longitudinal design for

the research would increase the clarity of L1 and L2 language and literacy development across

different cultural contexts and conditions of language use and exposure.

Implications

The results from the current study have theoretical and practical importance. Instances of

cross-language associations appear to be influenced by the language-learning environment and

the context for learning an L2. Failure to acknowledge these differences when designing

empirical studies will lead to difficulties improving our understanding of L2 acquisition.

Additionally, these differences should not be neglected when trying to provide the best

educational opportunities for bilinguals. Educators should be mindful of these differences and

structure educational practices accordingly. Different strategies to improve L2 (or even L1)

comprehension can occur for students with different immigration experiences. For example,

students who are learning English in a non-English speaking environment can be taught how to

best utilize existing vocabulary to increase L2 vocabulary and comprehension skills.

Additionally, students in the early stages of L2 acquisition can continue to develop higher-order

comprehension strategies in their L1, while learning L2 decoding skills and vocabulary.

Alternatively, instruction should focus primarily on teaching within-language vocabulary skills

to improve L1 and L2 comprehension for long-term immigrants. Another implication is that

assessment of RC in the L1 can inform future potential in L2 proficiency and development, or

reveal the possibility of difficulties in developing L2 comprehension. However, the results also

83

Page 94: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

suggest that assessing L1 proficiency is most useful for students who intend to immigrate or

recently immigrated to an English speaking society.

Additionally, research must increase understanding of the integration between L1 and L2

semantic knowledge as a method to increase L2 vocabulary knowledge. L2 vocabulary

acquisition is a formidable challenge in second language acquisition because it is difficult and

time consuming to improve (e.g., Carlo et al., 2004; Farnia & Geva, 2011). While only the EFL

group could capitalize on L1 vocabulary to improve L2 comprehension, this result does point to

the possibility of students accessing their existing semantic knowledge to increase word learning

experiences in the L2 in the initial stages of second language learning. Across the groups,

comprehension appears to become a language specific skill as time spent in a L2 environment

increases. The results suggest that we can initially support L2 acquisition with L1 skills but

intensive and systematic instruction in the L2 is necessary for students to achieve high levels of

proficiency in a new language.

84

Page 95: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Chapter 10: General Conclusions

What emerged from Study 1 was a more refined understanding of the constituent

components necessary for RC in adolescent bilinguals, and the degree to which these

components were consistent predictors across the L1 and L2 for Spanish-English and Chinese-

English bilinguals. Vocabulary and morphological awareness emerged as consistently important

predictors of comprehension in English, Spanish and Chinese. Specifically, the results suggest a

central role of word knowledge, in terms of semantics and morphological awareness, as a critical

and universal aspect of comprehension across languages (Perfetti, 2007; Perfetti & Hart, 2001,

2002). Fostering word knowledge is essential to achieve reading proficiency in alphabetic and

logographic languages alike. Additionally, English decoding was only a significant predictor of

English RC for the Chinese-English, not the Spanish-English, bilinguals. Overall, there was a

large degree of universality among the within-language predictors of RC across Spanish, Chinese

and English. The common predictors across languages (e.g., vocabulary) were much stronger

predictors of RC performance than the language specific predictors.

While strong instances of universal components of RC were found across two distinctly

different groups of bilinguals, this should not lead to the suggestion that all bilinguals are alike.

In addition to the universal aspects of comprehension, there were also multiple language specific

findings. Understanding how the two work together, for different groups of bilinguals, is

necessary to develop a comprehensive understanding of how people comprehend text in different

languages. Additionally, these studies outline what skills should be the focus of evidence-based

instructional programs for students from different linguistic and cultural backgrounds.

Study 2 identified the degree to which within- and cross-language predictors varied as a

function of changes in the Chinese-English bilinguals’ language learning environment. In the

85

Page 96: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

early stages of L2 acquisition, comprehension was highly related across languages, and skills in

the L1 were related to comprehension in the L2. As bilinguals became more integrated into a L2

educational system and culture, reading across languages was less related and became reliant

upon within-language skills. The interconnections between reading skills in the L1 and L2 are

sensitive to educational experiences and the language learning environment, and are a dynamic

process that is redefined as skills and knowledge within each language are developed or

forgotten.

Predictors of comprehension also appear to vary as a function language-learning

experiences. This observation suggests that we should consider the socio-cultural context of

language learning as an important methodological factor that influences how empirical studies

are constructed, how results are interpreted, and how groups of bilinguals are defined. Even

more important are the accompanying educational implications. The research presented in these

two studies strongly suggests that all bilinguals would benefit from common instructional

practices, such as enhancing vocabulary and word study instruction. However, a one-size-fits-all

approach would be a gross overgeneralization of these findings. Not only do bilinguals with

different language backgrounds require a different balance of instruction, but instructional

practices should vary based on their prior educational experiences. Understanding how and

when to capitalize on L1 language and literacy skills and how L2 instruction should be promoted

at different stages of L2 learning is critical. This will ensure we are engaged in the best practices

to reach the societal and moral obligations to ensure all bilingual students are equipped with the

language and literacy skills needed to succeed.

86

Page 97: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

References

Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the

Society for Research in Child Development, 238, 58.

August, D., Carlo, M., Dressler, C., & Snow, C. (2005). The critical role of vocabulary

development for English language learners. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice,

20, 50–57.

August, D., & Shanahan, T., (2006). Developing Literacy in Second-Language Learners: A

Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth. NJ:

Erlbaum.

Braze, D., Tabor, W., Shankweiler, D. P., & Mencl, W. E. (2007). Speaking up for vocabulary:

Reading skills differences in young adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 266-243.

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K. A. Bollen

and J. S. Long (Eds.) Testing Structural Equation Models. CA: Sage.

Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of

current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word

frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977-990.

Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based

on film subtitles. PLoS ONE 5(6) e10729.

Cain, K. (2007). Syntactic awareness and reading ability: Is there any evidence for a special

relationship? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 679–694.

Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s RC ability: concurrent prediction by

working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 96, 31-42.

87

Page 98: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Capps, R., Fix, M., Murray, J., Ost, J., Passel, J., & Herwantoro, S. (2005). The new demography

of America’s schools: Immigration and the No Child Left Behind Act. Washington, D.C:

The Urban Institute.

Carlo, M. S., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C. E., Dressler, C., Lippman, D. N., Lively, T.

J., & White, C. E. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English-

language learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly,

39, 188–215.

Catts, H. W., Hogan, T. P., & Adlof, S. M. (2005). Developmental changes in reading and

reading disabilities. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), The connections between

language and reading disabilities (pp. 25–40). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex

words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing, An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169-

190.

Chen, X., Hao, M., Geva, E., Zhu, J. & Shu, H. (2009). The role of compound awareness in

Chinese children’s vocabulary acquisition and character reading. Reading and Writing:

An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 615–631.

Chen, X., Ramirez, G., Luo, Y., Geva, E. & Ku, Y.-M. (2012). Comparing vocabulary

development in Spanish- and Chinese-Speaking ELLs: The effects of metalinguistic and

sociocultural factors. Reading and Writing, 25, 1991-2020.

Chik, P. P., Ho, C. S., Yeung, P., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K., Luan, H., Lo, L., & Lau, W.

(2012). Syntactic skills in sentence RC among Chinese elementary school children.

Reading and Writing, 25, 679-699.

Collier, V. P. (1987). Age and rate of acquisition of second language for academic purposes.

88

Page 99: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

TESOL Quarterly, 21, 617-641.

Courville, T., & Thompson, B. (2001). Use of structure coefficients in published multiple

regression articles: ß is not enough. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 61,

229-248.

Cuetos, F., Glez-Nosti, M., Barbon, A., & Brysbaery, M. (2011). SUBTLEX-ESP: Spanish word

frequencies based on film subtitles. Psicológica, 32, 133-143.

Cunningham, A., E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2001). What reading does for the mind. Journal of

Direct Instruction, 1, 137-149.

Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of RC: Relative contributions of word

recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how

comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277–299.

Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading.

Journal of Verbal Learning and Behavior, 19, 450-466.

Deacon, S.H. & Kirby, J.R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just ‘more phonological

awareness’? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading

development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238.

DeFoir, S., Martos, F., & Cary, L. (2002). Differences in reading acquisition development in two

shallow orthographies: Portuguese and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 135-148.

Ehri, L. C. (1997). Sight word learning in normal readers and dyslexics. In B. Blachman (Ed.),

Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia (pp. 163–186). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Elementary Education Teaching and Research Center, Beijing Education and Science Institute.

(1996). Elementary school textbooks. Beijing, China: Beijing Publishers

89

Page 100: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Fan, X., & Sivo, S. A. (2005). Sensitivity of fit indices to model misspecification and model

types. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 42, 509-529.

Farnia, F., & Geva, E. (2011). Cognitive correlates of vocabulary growth in English language

learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 711-738.

Farnia, F., & Geva, E. (2013). Growth and predictors of change in English language learners’

reading comprehension. Journal of Research in Reading, 36, 389-421.

Fender, M. (2001). A review of L1 and L2/ESL word integration skills and the nature of L2/ESL

word integration development involved in lower-level text processing. Language

Learning, 51, 319-396.

Garcia-Vázquez, E. (1995). Acculturation and academics: Effects of acculturation on reading

achievement among Mexica-American students. Bilingual Research Journal, 19, 305-315.

Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. NJL Erlbaum.

Genesee, F., Geva, E., Dressler, D. & Kamil, M. (2006). Synthesis: Cross-linguistic relationships.

In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners:

Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth (pp. 153–

174). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Givón, T. (1995). Functionalism and grammar. PA: John Benjamins.

González, J. E. J. (1997). A reading-level match study of phonemic processes underlying reading

disabilities in a transparent orthography. Reading and Writing, 9, 23-40.

González, J. E. J., & Valle, I. H. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish

language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 44-60.

90

Page 101: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Gottardo, A., &Mueller, J. (2009). Are first and second language factors related in predicting L2

RC? A study of Spanish speaking children acquiring English as a second language from

first to second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 330–344.

Gottardo, A., Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. S. (1996). The relationships between phonological

sensitivity, syntactic processing, and verbal working memory in the reading performance

of third-grade children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 563–582.

Gottardo, A., Yan, B., Siegel, L., & Wade-Woolley, L. (2001). Factors related to English reading

performance in children with Chinese as a first language: More evidence of cross-

linguistic transfer of phonological processing. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93,

530-542.

Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading ability. RASE: Remedial

& Special Education, 7, 6-10.

Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An

Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160.

Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The

influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language.

Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60-99.

Joshi, R. M., Tao, S. T., Aaron, P. G., & Quiroz, B. (2012). Cognitive component of

componential model of reading applied to different orthographies. Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 45, 480-486.

Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., & Olson, R. K. (2008). RC tests vary in the skills they assess:

Differential dependence on decoding and oral comprehension, Scientific Studies of

Reading, 12, 281-300.

91

Page 102: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Kenny, D.A., Kashy, D.A. & Cook, W.L. (2006). Dyadic data analysis. New York: Guilford

Press.

Keung, Y. C., & Ho, C. S. H. (2009). Transfer of reading-related cognitive skills in learning to

read Chinese (L1) and English (L2) among Chinese elementary school children.

Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 103-112.

Kieffer, M. J., & Di Felice Box, C. (2013). Derivational morphological awareness, academic

vocabulary, and RC in linguistically diverse sixth graders. Learning and Individual

Differences, 24, 168-175.

Kieffer, M., J. & Lesaux, N. K. (2008). The role of derivational morphology in the RC of

Spanish-speaking English language learners. Reading and Writing, 21, 781–804.

Kim, Y-S., & Pallante, D. (2012). Predictors of reading skills for kindergarteners and first grade

students in Spanish: a longitudinal study. Reading and Writing, 25, 1-22.

Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. NY: Cambridge.

Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading?

Literacy, 42, 75-81.

Koda, K. (2007). Reading and language learning: Crosslinguistic constraints on second language

reading development. Language Learning, 57, 1–44.

Ku, Y. & Anderson, R.C. (2003). Development of morphological awareness in Chinese and

English. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 399–422.

Kuo, L. & Anderson, R.C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to read: A cross-

linguistic perspective. Educational Psychologist, 41, 161–180.

92

Page 103: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Lam, K., Chen, X., Geva, E., Luo, Y. C., & Li, H. (2012). The role of morphological awareness

in reading achievement among young Chinese-speaking English language learners: a

longitudinal study. Reading and Writing, 25, 1847-1872.

Leider, C. M., Proctor, C. P., Silverman, R. D., & Harring, J. R. (2013). Examining the role of

vocabulary depth, cross-linguistic transfer, and types of reading measures on the RC of

Latino bilinguals in elementary school. Reading and Writing. DOI: 10.1007/s11145-013-

9427-6.

Lervag, A., & Aukrust, V. G. (2010). Vocabulary knowledge is critical determinant of the

difference in RC growth between first and second language learners. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 612–620.

Lesaux, N. K. Lipka, O., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). Investigating cognitive and linguistic abilities

that influence the RC skills of children from diverse linguistic backgrounds. Reading and

Writing, 19, 99-131.

Li, T., McBride-Chang, C., Wong, A. M. -Y., & Shu, H. (2012). Longitudinal predictors of

spelling and RC in Chinese as an L1 and English as an L2 in Hong Kong Chinese

children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(2), 286-301.

Li, M., & Kirby, J. R. (2013). Unexpected poor comprehenders among adolescent ESL students.

Scientific Studies of Reading. DOI: 10.1080/10888438.2013.775130.

Loeffer, M. (2007). NCELA Fast FAQ 4: What languages do ELLs speak? National Clearing

House for English-Language Acquisition and Language Instruction. Washington, D.C

Low, P. B., & Siegel, L. S. (2005). A comparison of the cognitive processes underlying RC in

native English and ESL speakers. Written Language & Literacy, 8, 207-231.

93

Page 104: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Luo, Y., Chen, X., Deacon, H., Zhang, J., & Yin, L. (2013). The role of visual processing in

learning to read Chinese characters. Scientific Studies of Reading, 17, 22-40.

MacGinitie, W. H., MacGinitie, R. K., Maria, K., Dreyer, L. G., & Hughes, K. E. (2006) Gates-

MacGinitie Reading Tests (GMRT) Second Canadian Edition. Riverside Publishing

Mancilla-Martinez, J., & Lesaux, N. K. (2010). Predictors of reading comprehension for

struggling readers: The case of Spanish-speaking language minority learners. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 102, 701-711.

Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Wen, Z. (2004). In search of golden rules: Comment on hypothesis-

testing approaches to setting cutoff values for fit indexes and dangers of overgeneralizing

Hu and Bentler;s (1999) findings. Structural Equation Modeling, 11, 320-341.

McBride-Chang, C. (2004). Children's Literacy Development. London: Edward Arnold/Oxford

Press.

Mood, A. M. (1971). Partitioning variance in multiple regression analysis as a tool for

developing learning models. American Educational Research Journal, 8, 191-202.

Muntendam, A. G. (2013). On the nature of cross-linguistic transfer: A case study of Andean

Spanish. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 111-131.

Naglieri, J. A. (1985). Matrix analogies test – Expanded form. San Antonio, TX : The

Psychological Corporation.

Nagy, W. & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English?

Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304-330.

Nagy, W.E., Berninger, V.W. & Abbott, R.D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond

phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal

of Educational Psychology, 98, 134–147.

94

Page 105: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Nagy, W., García, G. E., Durgunoglu, A., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. (1993). Spanish-English bilingual

students' use of cognates in English reading. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 241-259.

Nagy, W., & Herman, P. A. (1987). Breadth and depth of vocabulary knowledge: Implications

for acquisition and instruction. In M. McKeown & M. Curtis (Eds.), The nature of

vocabulary acquisition (pp. 19-59). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Nakamoto, J., Lindsey, K. A., & Manis, F. R. (2008). A cross-linguistic investigation of English

language learners’ RC in English and Spanish. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 351–371.

Newton, R. G., & Spurrell, D. J. (1967). A development of multiple regression for the analysis of

routine data. Applied Statistics, 16, 51-64.

Nimon, K., & Reio, T. G., (2011). Regression commonality analysis: A technique for

quantitative theory building. Human Resource Development Review, 10, 329-340.

Odlin, T. (1989). Language transfer: Cross-linguistic influence in language learning. UK:

Cambridge University Press.

Olson, D. R. (1996). The world on paper. NY: University of Cambridge.

Organization for Economic Cooperation and Development. (2000). Database on Immigrants in

OECD Countries: Immigrants by citizenship and age. Demography and Population:

Migration Statistics, retrieved from http://webnet.oecd.org/wbos/ on 2008/07/30

Ouellette, G. P. (2006). What’s meaning got to do with it: The role of vocabulary in word

reading and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 554–566.

Padey, S., & Elliott, W. (2010). Suppressor variables in social work research: Ways to identify in

multiple regression models. Journal of Society for Social Work Research, 1, 28-40.

95

Page 106: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Pasquarella, A., Chen, X., Lam, K., Yang, C. L., & Ramirez, G. (2011). Cross-language Transfer

of Morphological Awareness in Chinese-English Bilinguals. Journal of Research in

Reading, 34, 23-42.

Pasquarella, A., Grant, A., & Gottardo, A. (2012). Comparing factors related to RC in

adolescents who speak English as a first (L1) or second (L2) language. Scientific Studies

of Reading, 16, 475-503.

Pedhazur, E. J. (1997). Multiple regression in behavioral research: explanation and prediction

(3rd ed.). Orlando, FL: Holt, Rinehart and Winston

Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of

Reading, 11, 357-383.

Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2001). The lexical basis of comprehension skill. In D.S. Gorfien (Ed.),

On the consequences of meaning selection: Perspectives on resolving lexical ambiguity

(pp. 67-86). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven (Ed.),

Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189–213). Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.

Perfetti, C. A., & Hogaboam, T. (1975). Relationship between single word decoding and RC

skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 67, 461-469.

Proctor, C. P., August, D., Carlo, M. S., & Snow, C. E. (2006). The intriguing role of Spanish

language vocabulary knowledge in predicting English RC. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 98, 159–169.

Proctor, C. P., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking children

reading English: Towards a model of comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology,

97, 246–256.

96

Page 107: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Proctor, C. P., & Mo, E. (2009). The relationship between cognate awareness and English

comprehension among Spanish-English bilingual fourth grade students. TESOL

Quarterly, 43, 126-136.

Proctor, C. P., Silverman, R. D., Harring, J. R., & Montecillo, C. (2012). The role of vocabulary

depth in predicting RC among English monolingual and Spanish-English bilingual

children in elementary school. Reading and Writing, 25, 1635-1664.

Proctor, C. P., August, D., Carlo, M. S., & Snow, C. (2006). The intriguing role of Spanish

language vocabulary knowledge in predicting English RC. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 98, 159– 169.

Proctor, C. P., Uccelli, P., Dalton, B., & Snow, C. E. (2009). Understanding depth of vocabulary

online with bilingual and monolingual children. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 25, 311-

333.

Protopapas, A., Sideridis, G., D., Mouzaki, A., & Simos, P., G. (2007). Development of lexical

mediation in the relation between RC and word reading skill in Greek. Scientific Studies

of Reading, 11, 165-197.

Ramirez, G., Chen, X., & Pasquarella, A. (2013). Cross-linguistic transfer of morphological

awareness in Spanish-speaking ELLs: The facilitating effect of cognate knowledge.

Topics in Language Disorders, 33, 73-92.

Ramirez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E., & Kiefer, H. (2010). Morphological awareness in Spanish-

speaking English language learners: Within and cross-language effects on word reading.

Reading and Writing, 23, 337-358.

97

Page 108: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Ramirez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E., & Luo, Y. (2011). Morphological awareness and word reading

in English language learners: Evidence from Spanish- and Chinese-speaking children.

Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 601-618.

Savage, R. (2006). RC is not always the product of nonsense word decoding and linguistic

comprehension: Evidence from teenagers who are extremely poor readers. Scientific

Studies of Reading, 10, 143-164.

Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese:

Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74, 27–47.

Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental

dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 98, 122-133.

Snow, C. E., & Hoefnagel-Hӧhle, M. (1978). The critical period for language acquisition:

Evidence for second language learning. Child Development, 49, 1114-1128.

Stahl, S. A., & Nagy, W. E. (2006). Teaching word meanings. NJ: Mahwah.

Stanovich, K. E. (1991). Word recognition: Changing perspectives. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P.

Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 418-52).

New York: Longman.

Sun, H.L., Sun, D.J., Huang, J.P., Li, D.J. & Xing, H.B. (1996). The description on the corpus

system of modern Chinese studies. In Z.S. Luo & S.L. Yuan (Eds.), Studies of Chinese

and Chinese character in the computer era. (pp. 459–466). Tsing Hua: Tsinghua

University Publisher.

Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics. Boston, MA: Allyn and

Bacon.

98

Page 109: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Thompson, B. (2006). Foundations of behavioral statistics: An insight-based approach. New

York, NY: Guilford.

Tong, X., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., & Wong, A., M-Y. (2009). Morphological awareness,

orthographic knowledge and spelling errors: Keys to understanding early Chinese literacy

acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 426-452.

Tunmer, W. E., & Bowey, J. A. (1984). Metalinguistic awareness and reading acquisition.

Metalinguistic Awareness in Children, 15, 144-168.

Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology, Journal of

Memory and Language, 28, 649-667.

van Gelderen, A., Schoonen, R., de Glopper, K., Hulstijin, J., Simis, A., Snellings, P., &

Stevenson, M. (2004). Linguistic knowledge, processing speed, and metacognitive

knowledge in first- and second-language RC: A componential analysis. Journal of

Educational Psychology, 96, 19-30.

van Gelderen, A., Schoonen, R., Stoel, R. D., de Glooper, K., & Hulstijn, J. (2007).

Development of adolescent RC in language 1 and language 2: A longitudinal analysis of

constituent components. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 477–491.

Verhallen, M., & Schoonen, R. (1993). Lexical knowledge in L1 and L2 of third and fifth

graders. Applies Linguistics, 4, 452-470.

Verhoeven, L. (2000). Components in early second language reading and spelling. Scientific

Studies of Reading, 4, 313-330.

Wang, M., Yang, C. & Chen, C. (2009). The contribution of phonology, orthography, and

morphology in Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30,

291–314.

99

Page 110: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Wang, M., Cheng, C. & Chen, S. (2006). Contribution of morphological awareness to Chinese–

English biliteracy acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 542–553.

Whitley, M. S. (2002). Spanish/English contrasts: A course in Spanish linguistics. Washington,

D.C.: Georgetown University Press.

Wigfield, A. & Guthrie, J. T. (1997). Relations of children’s motivation for reading and the

amount and breadth of their reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 420-432.

Wiley, E. W., Bialystok, E., & Hakuta, K. (2005). New approaches to using census data to test

the critical-period hypothesis for second-language acquisition. Psychological Science, 16,

341-343.

Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading

development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91-103.

Woodcock, R. W. (1987). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery-Revised. Itasca, IL:

Riverside Publishing.

Woodcock, R. W., & Munoz-Sandoval, A. F. (1995). Woodcock Proficiency Battery-revised:

English and Spanish Forms. Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

Yeung, P., Ho, C., Chik, P., Lo, L., Luan, H., Chan, D., & Ching, K. (2011). Reading and

spelling Chinese among beginning readers: What skills make a difference? Scientific

Studies of Reading, 15, 285-313.

Zawiszewski, A., Gutiérrez, E., Fernández, B. & Laka, I. (2011). Language distance and non-

native syntactic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Bilingualism:

Language and Cognition, 14, 400-411.

100

Page 111: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Zientek, L. R., & Thompson, B. (2010). Using commonality analysis to quantify contributions

that self-efficacy and motivational factors make in mathematics performance. Research

in the Schools, 17, 1-11.

Zhang, D. (2012). Vocabulary and grammar knowledge in second language RC: A structural

equation modeling study. The Modern Language Journal, 96, 558-575.

Zhang, D., & Koda, K. (2012). Contributions of morphological awareness and lexical

inferencing ability to L2 vocabulary knowledge and RC among advanced EFL learners:

testing direct and indirect effects. Reading and Writing, 25, 1195-1216.

Zhang, J., McBride-Chang, C., Tong, X., Anita, M.-Y., Shu, H., & Fong, C. Y.-C. (2012).

Reading with meaning: the contributions of meaning-related variables at the word and

subword levels to early Chinese RC. Reading and Writing, 25, 2183-2203.

101

Page 112: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and Analysis of Variance of age, length of residence, English and L1 variables

Notes. Min = Minimum, Max = Maximum, SD = Standard Deviation * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

Spanish-English bilinguals Chinese-English bilinguals

Min Max Mean SD Min Max Mean SD

F (1, 186)

Partial η2

Age in months 177 413 235.86 40.31 174 286 221.00 25.44 8.53** .05 Length of Residence 6 276 144.54 83.86 6 204 82.51 52.13 39.16*** .17 Non-verbal Reasoning 11 32 24.51 5.41 13 32 28.00 3.27 30.44*** .14

English Measures Working Memory 12 40 26.14 7.88 11 41 27.46 7.59 1.30 .01 Word Reading 63 105 92.92 8.61 59 103 89.30 9.84 6.54* .03 Standard Score 61 126 99.98 12.65 47 121 94.16 15.57 - - Decoding 20 44 36.39 4.97 19 44 35.50 5.06 1.38 .01 Standard Score 74 146 105.46 12.99 72 131 101.03 11.62 - - Vocabulary 7 44 30.27 7.86 7 45 26.75 9.73 6.64* .03 Grade Equivalent 4 13 10.19 2.69 3 13 9.30 2.68 - - Derivational Structure 6 21 17.01 3.20 8 21 18.06 2.7 1.72 .02 Derivational Production 2 31 25.80 5.39 10 31 25.20 4.57 0.68 .00 Compound Awareness 3 24 16.85 5.17 5 25 17.95 4.36 2.64 .01 Syntactic Awareness 30 68 59.58 8.92 30 68 57.67 7.75 2.39 .01 Reading Comprehension 6 44 31.30 8.57 11 48 32.94 8.38 1.67 .01 Grade Equivalent 2 13 9.90 3.05 3 13 10.57 2.77 - -

First Language Measures Word Reading 38 96 55.66 11.87 0 288 144.10 76.84 - - Vocabulary 7 40 25.52 8.77 7 55 39.81 15.81 - - Homographic Awareness - - - - 7 40 30.35 8.37 - - Derivational Structure 2 21 16.45 4.15 - - - - - - Derivational Production 1 26 18.65 6.29 - - - - - - Compound Awareness 1 19 10.42 4.77 0 25 15.97 5.97 - - Syntactic Awareness 15 52 39.94 8.25 0 77 63.86 11.70 - - Reading Comprehension 9 44 26.42 8.47 1 45 28.98 11.33 - -

102

Page 113: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 2. Intercorrelations among English variables for Chinese-English bilinguals (above the diagonal) and Spanish-English bilinguals (below the diagonal)

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 1. Age in months - -.49*** .10 .15 -.08 -.17 -.40** -.01 -.17 -.10 -.25*** -.24*** 2. Length of Residence -.12 - -.16 .01 .39*** .36*** .56*** .09 .34*** .16 .31*** .37** 3. Non-verbal Reasoning .05 -.07 - .19* .08 .16 .12 .32*** .11 .34*** -.04 .31*** 4. Working Memory -.13 .04 .34*** - .26** .34*** .23* .35*** .30*** .43*** .06 .27** 5. Word Reading .21 .28* .37** .31** - .70*** .64*** .42*** .64*** .42*** .32*** .59*** 6. Decoding -.04 .18 .29* .38*** .68*** - .54*** .39*** .45*** .50*** .31*** .56*** 7. Vocabulary .07 .31** .42*** .43*** .63*** .47*** - .53*** .65*** .40*** .40*** .78*** 8. Derivational Structure .21 .26* .44*** .24* .59*** .43*** .62*** - .58*** .51*** .24** .66*** 9. Derivational Production .09 .05 .18 .16 .48*** .34** .46*** .37** - .45*** .41*** .70*** 10. Compound Awareness -.15 .06 .28* .30* .35** .29* .42*** .34** .63*** - .17 .46*** 11. Syntactic Awareness -.06 .14 .33** .46*** .44*** .33** .46*** .43*** .32** .28* - .36*** 12. Reading Comprehension -.10 .24* .39*** .52*** .51*** .31** .75*** .44*** .49*** .45*** .50*** -

103

Page 114: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 3 Intercorrelations among Spanish and control measures

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 1. Age in months 1 -.12 .05 -.13 .08 .35** .40*** .34** .35** .34** .34** 2. Length of Residence 1 -.07 .04 -.18 -.52*** -.15 -.44*** -.29* -.32** -.23 3. Non-verbal Reasoning 1 .40*** -.07 .16 .24* .20 .22 .04 .39*** 4. Working Memory 1 .08 .03 .11 -.01 -.04 -.08 .29* 5. Word Reading 1 .05 .09 .13 .05 .06 .09 6. Vocabulary 1 .70*** .79*** .65*** .50*** .69*** 7. Derivational Structure 1 .74*** .60*** .43*** .66*** 8. Derivational Production 1 .66*** .59*** .61*** 9. Compound Awareness 1 .51*** .59*** 10. Syntactic Awareness 1 .36** 11. Reading Comprehension 1

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

104

Page 115: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 4. Intercorrelations among Chinese and control measures

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1. Age in months 1 -.49*** .10 .07 .58*** .52*** .54*** .38*** .36*** .50*** 2. Length of Residence 1 -.16 .01 -.69*** -.64*** -.67*** -.47*** -.37*** -.59*** 3. Non-verbal Reasoning 1 .19* .14 .20* .21* .27** .15 .24* 4. Working Memory 1 .01 .06 .11 .12 .15 .18 5. Word Reading 1 .92*** .89*** .64*** .45*** .84*** 6. Vocabulary 1 .88*** .62*** .34*** .88*** 7. Homographic Awareness 1 .61*** .48*** .88*** 8. Compound Awareness 1 .42*** .57*** 9. Syntactic Awareness 1 .43*** 10. Reading Comprehension 1

105

Page 116: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Figure 1. Theoretical structural equation model of RC in English, Spanish and Chinese

Note. Derivational Structure was not included in the Chinese RC model.

106

Page 117: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 5. Coefficients for structural equation model of English RC

Spanish-English bilinguals Chinese-English bilinguals

Unstandardized

Estimate Standard

Error Standardized

Beta Unstandardized

Estimate Standard

Error Standardized

Beta English Reading Comprehension

Length of Residence 0.008 0.007 .08 0.001 0.010 .01 Non-verbal Reasoning 0.087 0.109 .06 0.381 0.121 .16** Working Memory 0.225 0.090 .21* -0.048 0.057 -.05 Decoding -0.266 0.126 -.16 0.203 0.094 .13* Vocabulary 0.598 0.111 .54*** 0.381 0.066 .46*** Derivational Structure -0.248 0.235 -.10 0.612 0.197 .21** Derivational Production 0.284 0.137 .18* 0.430 0.128 .25*** Compound Awareness 0.191 0.147 .12 0.035 0.122 .02 Syntactic Awareness 0.116 0.082 .12 0.001 0.058 .00

English Vocabulary Length of Immigration 0.017 0.007 .20* 0.082 0.011 .46*** Working Memory 0.241 0.087 .25** -0.010 0.080 -.01 Derivational Structure 0.918 0.218 .39*** 0.887 0.260 .26*** Derivational Production 0.323 0.140 .23* 0.682 0.165 .32*** Compound Awareness 0.127 0.158 .09 0.070 0.165 .03

English Derivational Production Syntactic Awareness 0.179 0.069 .29** 0.236 0.048 .41***

English Derivational Structure Syntactic Awareness 0.153 0.040 .42*** 0.077 0.031 .22*

English Compound Awareness Syntactic Awareness 0.175 0.066 .30** 0.099 0.048 .19*

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

107

Page 118: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 6. Coefficients of bootstrapping for indirect effects on English RC

Notes. CI = Confidence Interval * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

Spanish-English bilinguals Chinese-English bilinguals 95% Bootstrapping CI 95% Bootstrapping CI

Unstandardized

Estimate Standard

Error Lower Bound

Upper Bound p

Unstandardized Estimate

Standard Error

Lower Bound

Upper Bound p

Length of Residence 0.100 0.046 0.024 0.178 * 0.202 0.048 0.142 0.302 ** Working Memory 0.151 0.063 0.069 0.282 ** 0.016 0.032 -0.038 0.067 Derivational Production 0.214 0.061 0.112 0.319 * 0.121 0.039 0.053 0.188 ** Derivational Structure 0.122 0.066 0.024 0.239 0.154 0.039 0.096 0.227 ** Compound Awareness 0.046 0.055 -0.040 0.146 0.005 0.039 -0.075 0.058 Syntactic Awareness 0.204 0.096 0.049 0.384 * 0.251 0.070 0.121 0.360 *

108

Page 119: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 7. Comparison of fit statistics by constraining parameter estimates to be equal across groups

Notes. df = degrees of freedom, CFI = Comparative Fit Index, RMSEA = Root Mean Square Error of Approximation, AIC = Akaike Information Criterion, BCC = Brown-Cudeck Criterion + p < .06, * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

Χ2 df Χ2/df p CFI RMSEA AIC BCC ∆AIC ∆BCC ∆df p Unconstrained 100.37 28 3.59 *** 0.90 0.12 264.37 288.40

Constraints Placed on Parameters Predicting English Reading Comprehension Length of Residence 100.67 29 3.47 *** 0.90 0.12 262.67 286.41 1.70 1.99 1 Non-verbal Reasoning 102.83 29 3.55 *** 0.90 0.12 264.83 288.57 -0.46 -0.17 1 Working Memory 106.13 29 3.66 *** 0.90 0.12 268.13 291.87 -3.76 -3.47 1 + Decoding 107.49 29 3.71 *** 0.90 0.12 269.49 294.22 -5.12 -5.82 1 * Vocabulary 103.15 29 3.56 *** 0.90 0.12 265.15 288.88 -0.78 -0.48 1 Derivational Structure 107.14 29 3.70 *** 0.90 0.12 269.14 292.87 -4.77 -4.47 1 * Derivational Production 100.98 29 3.48 *** 0.90 0.12 262.98 286.72 1.39 1.68 1 Compound Awareness 101.05 29 3.48 *** 0.90 0.12 263.04 286.78 1.33 1.62 1 Syntactic Awareness 101.71 29 3.51 *** 0.90 0.12 263.71 287.44 0.66 0.96 1

Constraints Placed on Parameters Predicting English Vocabulary Length of Residence 119.15 29 4.11 *** 0.88 0.13 281.15 304.88 -16.78 -16.48 1 *** Working Memory 105.52 29 3.64 *** 0.89 0.12 267.52 291.25 -3.15 -2.85 1 Derivational Structure 100.38 29 3.46 *** 0.90 0.12 262.38 286.12 1.99 2.28 1 Derivational Production 103.05 29 3.55 *** 0.90 0.12 265.05 288.79 -0.68 -0.39 1 Compound Awareness 101.71 29 3.63 *** 0.90 0.12 265.71 289.74 -1.34 -1.34 1

Constraints Placed on Parameters Predicting English Derivational Structure Syntactic Awareness 102.67 29 3.54 *** 0.90 0.12 264.67 288.41 -0.3 -0.01 1

Constraints Placed on Parameters Predicting English Derivational Production Syntactic Awareness 100.82 29 3.48 *** 0.90 0.11 262.82 286.55 1.55 1.85 1

Constraints Placed on Parameters Predicting English Compound Awareness Syntactic Awareness 101.42 29 3.49 *** 0.90 0.12 263.42 287.15 0.95 1.25 1

109

Page 120: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Figure 2. Structural models of English RC showing direct and indirect effects Spanish-English bilinguals (Model 1) Chinese-English bilinguals (Model 2)

Notes. Bolded paths refer to parameters estimates that are significantly different between groups; dotted paths denoted significant indirect effects on RC.

110

Page 121: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 8. Coefficients for structural equation model of first language RC

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

Spanish-English bilinguals Chinese-English bilinguals

Unstandardized

Estimate Standard

Error Standardized

Beta Unstandardized

Estimate Standard

Error Standardized

Beta First Language Reading Comprehension

Length of Residence 0.011 0.009 0.11 0.006 0.011 0.03 Non-verbal Reasoning 0.266 0.123 0.18* 0.120 0.136 0.04 Working Memory 0.198 0.085 0.19* 0.139 0.059 0.10* Word Reading 0.042 0.053 0.06 -0.003 0.017 -0.02 Vocabulary 0.460 0.134 0.49*** 0.395 0.078 0.54*** Derivational Structure 0.570 0.210 0.29** - - - Derivational Production -0.016 0.190 -0.01 - - - Homographic Awareness - - - 0.552 0.121 0.40*** Compound Awareness 0.329 0.178 0.19 -0.081 0.090 -0.05 Syntactic Awareness -0.012 0.093 -0.01 0.058 0.045 0.06

First Language Vocabulary Length of Residence -0.030 0.007 -0.29*** -0.020 0.016 -0.07 Working Memory 0.014 0.070 0.01 -0.102 0.090 -0.05 Derivational Structure 0.701 0.209 0.34*** - - - Derivational Production 0.435 0.157 0.31** - - - Homographic Awareness - - - 1.438 0.121 0.76*** Compound Awareness 0.305 0.153 0.17* 0.348 0.132 0.14***

First Language Derivational Structure Syntactic Awareness 0.218 0.054 0.43*** - - -

First Language Derivational Production or Homographic Awareness Syntactic Awareness 0.446 0.073 0.59*** 0.342 0.055 0.50***

First Language Compound Awareness Syntactic Awareness 0.295 0.059 0.51*** 0.213 0.043 0.42***

111

Page 122: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 9. Coefficients of bootstrapping for indirect effects L1 RC

Spanish-English bilinguals Chinese-English bilinguals 95% Bootstrapping CI 95% Bootstrapping CI

Unstandardized Estimate

Standard Error

Lower Bound

Upper Bound p

Unstandardized Estimate

Standard Error

Lower Bound

Upper Bound p

Length of Residence -0.142 0.080 -0.302 -0.031 * -0.038 0.043 -0.103 0.049 Working Memory 0.006 0.042 -0.067 0.065 -0.028 0.025 -0.084 0.005 Derivational Structure 0.164 0.087 0.045 0.324 ** - - - - Derivational Production 0.153 0.089 0.016 0.307 - - - - Homographic Awareness - - - - 0.415 0.115 0.215 0.585 *** Compound Awareness 0.082 0.060 0.007 0.191 0.075 0.043 0.017 0.170 * Syntactic Awareness 0.378 0.100 0.268 0.639 *** 0.423 0.097 0.273 0.611 **

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

112

Page 123: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Figure 3. Structural model for Spanish RC (Model 3) showing direct and indirect effects

Notes. Dotted paths denoted significant indirect effects on RC

113

Page 124: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Figure 4. Structural model for Chinese RC (Model 4) showing direct and indirect effects

Notes. Dotted paths denoted significant indirect effects on RC

114

Page 125: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 10. Descriptive statistics and Analysis of Variance for Chinese EFL learners, Recent Immigrants, and Long-term Immigrants for age, months in Canada, English and Chinese variables.

EFL (F) learners Recent Immigrants (R) Long-term Immigrants (L)

Min. Max. Mean SD Min. Max. Mean SD Min. Max. Mean SD F (2,171)

Partial η2

Pairwise Comparisonsa

Age in Months 209 270 242.42 7.88 170 286 230.62 25.92 177 286 208.35 22.58 40.75*** .19 F > R > L Length of Residence 0 0 0 0 6 72 40.63 18.73 84 204 126.59 37.45 410.93*** .83 L > R > F English Decoding 22 42 32.35 5.20 21 44 34.23 4.86 19 43 36.84 4.96 11.57*** .12 L > R > F English Vocabulary 3 32 18.00 6.16 8 45 22.32 8.20 7 42 31.42 9.05 42.91*** .33 L > R > F English RC 16 38 26.51 5.92 14 48 30.70 7.99 11 46 35.30 8.20 19.84*** .19 L > R > F Chinese Word Reading 209 238 222.18 6.48 0 214 187.57 50.69 30 288 98.35 73.28 87.69*** .51 F > R > L Chinese Vocabulary 54 77 69.40 5.42 7 54 48.43 8.69 11 55 30.74 16.58 169.97*** .67 F > R > L Chinese RC 25 44 36.39 4.59 1 41 34.67 8.18 10 45 23.00 11.14 43.02*** .34 (F = R) > L

Notes. Min = Minimum, Max. = Maximum, SD = Standard Deviation * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001 a Equal sign indicates nonsignificant difference, and greater-than sign indicates p < .05

115

Page 126: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 11. Intercorrelations among Chinese and English variables separated by group.

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

1 2 3 4 5 6 7

Chinese EFL learners 1. Age in months 1 .-11 .05 -.27 -.23 -.19 -.07 2. English Decoding 1 .24 .36** .32* .19 .24 3. English Vocabulary 1 .30* .25 .17 -.08 4. English RC 1 .41** .51*** .48*** 5. Chinese Word Reading 1 .71*** .39** 6. Chinese Vocabulary 1 .55*** 7. Chinese RC 1

Recent Immigrants 1. Age in months 1 -.03 -.03 .02 .35** .27* .26* 2. English Decoding 1 .41*** .53*** -.23 -.23 -.01 3. English Vocabulary 1 .73*** -.30* -.21 -.03 4. English RC 1 -.08 .01 .33** 5. Chinese Word Reading 1 .80*** .65*** 6. Chinese Vocabulary 1 .80*** 7. Chinese RC 1

Long-term Immigrants 1. Age in months 1 -.25 -.43*** -.28* .43*** .42*** .35** 2. English Decoding 1 .56*** .53*** -.25 -.23 -.07 3. English Vocabulary 1 .81*** -.60*** -.59*** -.44*** 4. English RC 1 -.34* -.33* -.17 5. Chinese Word Reading 1 .93*** .83*** 6. Chinese Vocabulary 1 .86*** 7. Chinese RC 1

116

Page 127: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 12.

Hierarchical linear regression predicting English RC, separate for EFL, Recent Immigrant, and Long-term Immigrant groups

Chinese EFL learners Recent Immigrants Long-term Immigrants

Step Variable Final B SE (B) β Final B SE (B) β Final B SE (B) β 1 English Decoding .186 .129 .19 .394 .131 .24** .084 .156 .05 2 English Vocabulary .268 .115 .24* .568 .078 .58*** .822 .103 .91*** 3 Chinese Word Reading -.082 .111 -.04 -.003 .019 -.02 .010 .023 .09 4 Chinese Vocabulary .347 .148 .28* -.229 .142 -.25 -.003 .111 -.01 5 Chinese RC .400 .171 .30* .553 .122 .57*** .123 .115 .17

Notes. B = Unstandardized Beta, SE = Standard Error, β = Standardized Beta * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

117

Page 128: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 13.

Hierarchical linear regression predicting Chinese RC, separate for EFL, Recent Immigrant, and Long-term Immigrant groups

Chinese EFL learners Recent Immigrants Long-term Immigrants

Step Variable Final B SE (B) β Final B SE (B) β Final B SE (B) β 1 Chinese Word Reading .068 .086 .01 .011 .018 .07 .042 .027 .28 2 Chinese Vocabulary .292 .115 .41* .659 .105 .70*** .425 .120 .63*** 3 English Decoding .118 .102 .11 -.022 .134 -.01 .245 .185 .11 4 English Vocabulary -.269 .087 -.28* -.206 .100 -.21* -.080 .185 -.07 5 English RC .243 .104 .30* .499 .110 .49*** .178 .167 .13

* p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001

118

Page 129: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 14. Commonality analyses beta weights, structural coefficients, proportions of total, common and unique variance for EFL, Recent Immigrant and Long-term Immigrant groups when predicting English RC

English

Decoding English

Vocabulary Chinese

Word Reading Chinese

Vocabulary Chinese

RC Chinese EFL learners

Beta Weights .190 .242 -.036 .288 .301 Structural Coefficient .560 .466 .630 .785 .734 Total Variance .131 .091 .167 .258 .225 Common Variance .101 .041 .166 .224 .166 Unique Variance .030 .050 .001 .034 .059

Recent Immigrants Beta Weights .240 .583 -.021 -.249 .564 Structural Coefficient .621 .845 -.092 .010 .387 Total Variance .284 .526 .006 .000 .110 Common Variance .234 .267 .006 -.013 .011 Unique Variance .044 .259 .000 .013 .100

Long-term Immigrants Beta Weights .051 .906 .087 -.007 .168 Structural Coefficient .627 .966 -.399 -.392 -.197 Total Variance .279 .661 .113 .109 .028 Common Variance .277 .294 .112 .109 .021 Unique Variance .002 .367 .001 .000 .007

119

Page 130: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 15. Summary of unique and common variance for independent variables when predicting English RC

Chinese EFL learners Recent Immigrants Long-term Immigrants

Coefficient % R2 Coefficient % R2 Coefficient % R2

Unique to English Decoding .030 7.20 .044 6.01 .002 0.24 Unique to English Vocabulary .050 11.89 .259 35.12 .367 51.75 Unique to Chinese Word Reading .001 0.14 .000 0.02 .001 0.14 Unique to Chinese Vocabulary .034 8.06 .013 1.71 .000 0.00 Unique to Chinese RC .059 14.16 .010 13.54 .007 0.93 Common with Chinese Vocabulary (without Chinese RC) .054 12.81 .176 23.83 .134 18.90

Common with Chinese RC (without Chinese Vocabulary) .067 15.87 .200 27.19 .048 6.70

Common with Chinese Vocabulary and Chinese RC .171 40.71 -.188 -25.53 -.025 -3.53

120

Page 131: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 16. Commonality analyses beta weights, structural coefficients, proportions of total, common and unique variance for EFL, Recent Immigrant and Long-term Immigrant groups when predicting Chinese RC

Chinese Word Reading

Chinese Vocabulary

English Decoding

English Vocabulary

English RC

Chinese EFL learners Beta Weights .017 .410 .110 -.280 .303 Structural Coefficient .608 .843 .364 -.130 .729 Total Variance .157 .301 .056 .007 .226 Common Variance .157 .229 .046 -.059 .167 Unique Variance .000 .073 .010 .006 .059

Recent Immigrants Beta Weights .064 .701 -.014 -.206 .487 Structural Coefficient .743 .915 -.014 -.031 .378 Total Variance .426 .646 .000 .001 .110 Common Variance .425 .481 .000 -.017 .024 Unique Variance .001 .165 .000 .018 .086

Long-term Immigrants Beta Weights .276 .633 .110 -.066 .131 Structural Coefficient .944 .978 -.083 -.500 -.189 Total Variance .689 .738 .005 .192 .028 Common Variance .678 .682 -.003 .191 .023 Unique Variance .011 .056 .008 .001 .005

121

Page 132: READING COMPREHSNION IN ADOLESCENT BILINGUALS LEARNING … · Two studies examined reading comprehension (RC) among adolescent bilinguals learning English as a second language (L2)

Table 17. Summary of unique and common variance for independent variables when predicting L1 RC

Chinese EFL learners Recent Immigrants Long-term Immigrants

Coefficient % R2 Coefficient % R2 Coefficient % R2

Unique to Chinese Word Reading .000 0.03 .001 0.18 .011 1.37 Unique to Chinese Vocabulary .073 17.12 .165 21.38 .056 7.31 Unique to English Decoding .010 2.31 .000 0.02 .008 1.03 Unique to English Vocabulary .066 15.58 .018 2.27 .001 0.11 Unique to English RC .059 13.84 .086 11.14 .005 0.67 Common with English Vocabulary (without English RC) -.005 -1.22 .134 17.30 .271 35.08

Common with English RC (without English Vocabulary) .196 45.99 .166 21.48 .102 13.20

Common with English Vocabulary and English RC -.043 -10.23 -.165 -21.37 -.090 -11.60

122