prado, e.l., alcock, k.j., muadz, h., ullman, m.t., and shankar, a.h

13
DOI: 10.1542/peds.2012-0412 ; originally published online August 20, 2012; Pediatrics Anuraj H. Shankar Elizabeth L. Prado, Katherine J. Alcock, Husni Muadz, Michael T. Ullman and Randomized Trial in Indonesia Maternal Multiple Micronutrient Supplements and Child Cognition: A http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2012/08/15/peds.2012-0412 located on the World Wide Web at: The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275. Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2012 by the American Academy published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012 pediatrics.aappublications.org Downloaded from

Upload: dangliem

Post on 01-Feb-2017

222 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2012-0412; originally published online August 20, 2012;Pediatrics

Anuraj H. ShankarElizabeth L. Prado, Katherine J. Alcock, Husni Muadz, Michael T. Ullman and

Randomized Trial in IndonesiaMaternal Multiple Micronutrient Supplements and Child Cognition: A

  

  http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2012/08/15/peds.2012-0412

located on the World Wide Web at: The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is

 

of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2012 by the American Academy published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Pointpublication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly

at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 2: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

MaternalMultipleMicronutrient Supplements and ChildCognition: A Randomized Trial in Indonesia

WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: Micronutrients are essentialfor brain development during gestation and infancy. Fewrandomized trials of maternal multiple micronutrientsupplementation during pregnancy and postpartum haveexamined child outcomes beyond the neonatal period or testedwhich cognitive domains show long-term effects.

WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: Children of undernourished mothersgiven multiple micronutrients performed as well as children ofwell-nourished mothers in motor and visual attention/spatialability at age 42 months; children of undernourished mothersgiven iron/folic acid showed 4- to 5-month delays in theseabilities.

abstractOBJECTIVES: We investigated the relative benefit of maternal multiplemicronutrient (MMN) supplementation during pregnancy and until 3months postpartum compared with iron/folic acid supplementationon child development at preschool age (42 months).

METHODS: We assessed 487 children of mothers who participated inthe Supplementation with Multiple Micronutrients Intervention Trial, acluster-randomized trial in Indonesia, on tests adapted and validated inthe local context measuring motor, language, visual attention/spatial,executive, and socioemotional abilities. Analysis was according tointention to treat.

RESULTS: In children of undernourished mothers (mid-upper armcircumference ,23.5 cm), a significant benefit of MMNs was observedon motor ability (B = 0.39 [95% confidence interval (CI): 0.08–0.70]; P =.015) and visual attention/spatial ability (B = 0.37 [95% CI: 0.11–0.62];P = .004). In children of anemic mothers (hemoglobin concentration,110 g/L), a significant benefit of MMNs on visual attention/spatialability (B = 0.24 [95% CI: 0.02–0.46]; P = .030) was also observed. Norobust effects of maternal MMN supplementation were found in anydevelopmental domain over all children.

CONCLUSIONS: When pregnant women are undernourished or anemic,provision of MMN supplements can improve the motor and cognitiveabilities of their children up to 3.5 years later, particularly for bothmotor function and visual attention/spatial ability. Maternal MMNbut not iron/folic acid supplementation protected children from thedetrimental effects of maternal undernutrition on child motor andcognitive development. Pediatrics 2012;130:e536–e546

AUTHORS: Elizabeth L. Prado, PhD,a,b,c Katherine J. Alcock,DPhil,b Husni Muadz, PhD,a,d Michael T. Ullman, PhD,e andAnuraj H. Shankar, DSc,a,f for the SUMMIT Study GroupaSUMMIT Institute of Development, Mataram, Indonesia;bDepartment of Psychology, Lancaster University, Lancaster,United Kingdom; cUniversity of California at Davis Program inInternational and Community Nutrition, Davis, California;dMataram University Center for Research on Language andCulture, Mataram, Indonesia; eGeorgetown University Brain andLanguage Laboratory, Department of Neuroscience, Washington,DC; and fDepartment of Nutrition, Harvard University Schoolof Public Health, Cambridge, Massachusetts

KEY WORDSchild development, cognitive development, international publichealth, maternal health, maternal nutrition, motor development,multiple micronutrient supplementation

ABBREVIATIONSHb—hemoglobinIFA—iron/folic acidMMN—multiple micronutrientMUAC—mid-upper arm circumferenceSUMMIT—Supplementation with Multiple Micronutrients Inter-vention Trial

Dr Prado designed and supervised the developmental assessment,cleaned and analyzed the developmental data, and produced thefirst draft of the paper, as well as subsequent drafts andadditional analyses. Dr Prado and Dr Alcock were joint principalinvestigators of the preschool child development follow-up study.Dr Alcock designed the developmental assessment battery withDr Prado and advised on the training of the assessors. These dataformed part of Dr Prado’s doctoral thesis at Lancaster University.Dr Alcock supervised Dr Prado’s doctoral studies, advised onanalysis and interpretation of the data, and contributed to themanuscript. Dr Shankar was the principal investigator of theSupplementation with Multiple Micronutrients Intervention Trial(SUMMIT) study. He designed the protocols and directed andcoordinated execution of the study. Dr Shankar helped with fieldand data management of the preschool developmentalassessment and contributed to analysis and interpretation of thedata and preparation of the manuscript. Dr Muadz contributed tothe design and adaptation of the developmental assessment to thelocal context and helped with the field logistics and datamanagement of the developmental assessment. Dr Ullman advisedon the design of the developmental assessment, contributed toanalysis and interpretation of the data, and co-wrote sections ofthe paper with Dr Prado. All individual authors critiqued themanuscript and approved the final report. The SUMMIT StudyGroup designed and executed the SUMMIT study.

This trial has been registered with the ISRCTN Register (http://isrctn.org) (identifier ISRCTN34151616).

www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2012-0412

doi:10.1542/peds.2012-0412

Accepted for publication May 14, 2012

(Continued on last page)

e536 PRADO et al at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 3: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

Micronutrients are necessary for braindevelopment during gestation and in-fancy. These are important periods forthe formation of the brain, laying thefoundation for the development of cog-nitive, motor, and socioemotional skillsthroughout childhood and adulthood.Children with restricted development ofthese skills during early life are at riskfor later neuropsychological problems,poor school achievement, low-skilledemployment, and poor care of theirown children, thus contributing to theintergenerational transmission of po-verty.1,2 More than 200 million childrenaged ,5 years in low- and middle-income countries, at a conservativeestimate, are not reaching their devel-opmental potential in these areas.3

Sufficient micronutrient intake is es-pecially important when mothers arepregnant and breastfeeding, which areperiods of increased micronutrientneeds and crucial periods for the braindevelopment of the infant.4 The neuraltube begins to form 16 days after con-ception and within 7 months takes ona form that resembles the adult brain.5

Micronutrients are necessary for manyof the biological processes that drivethis transformation, including neuronproliferation, axon and dendrite growth,synaptogenesis, and myelination. Forexample, in animal models, maternalvitamin B6 deficiency results in de-creased synaptic density in the neo-cortex,6 whereas maternal deficiencyin either vitamin B6 or zinc leads to re-duced dendritic branching.7,8 Maternaldeficiency in iron and vitamin B6, as wellas gestational hypothyroidism, whichcan be caused by deficiencies in iodineand selenium, result in reduced myeli-nation in the offspring.8,9 Few studieshave examined the effects of maternalmicronutrient deficiencies on brain de-velopment in humans.

Although the World Health Organizationrecommends distribution of iron/folicacid (IFA) supplements to pregnant

women, provision of multiple micro-nutrients (MMNs), including those im-portant for brain development such asvitamin B6, zinc, and iodine, may bemore beneficial for mothers and theirchildren. Three randomized trials haveexamined the effect of maternal MMNsupplementation on motor and cogni-tive development in infancy. Two of thesehave reported benefits on Bayley Scalesof Infant Development motor scores inchildren (age 7 months) of undernour-ished mothers in Bangladesh29 andchildren (age 6–18 months) of HIV-1–infected mothers in Tanzania.30 A thirdtrial in China found a benefit of mater-nal MMN supplementation comparedwith IFA on Bayley Scales of Infant De-velopment mental but not motor scoresat 12 months of age.10 These somewhatmixed results suggest that maternalMMN supplementation may improvemotor development, although perhapsonly in children of mothers who are atparticular risk (ie, with HIV-1 infectionor low BMI), and may also benefit othercognitive abilities.

Only 1 study has examined the effect ofmaternalMMNsupplementation inolderchildren: a randomized trial in Nepal,which assessed a cohort of 7- to 9-year-old children.11 Children whose mothershad received 15 micronutrients duringpregnancy scored higher on a test ofexecutive function than those whosemothers had received vitamin A alone.However, this benefit was found for only1 of 6 tests of motor and cognitivefunction. Children of mothers in thissame studywho received iron, folic acid,and vitamin A scored higher on 5 of 6cognitive and motor tests than thosewhose mothers received vitamin Aalone. In this study, children whosemothers received iron, folic acid, andvitamin A were not directly comparedwith those who received 15 micro-nutrients. In addition, this study usedan MMN formulation that did notcontain iodine, which is important for

child motor and cognitive development,nor did it assess the specific impact onchildren of undernourished or anemicmothers. Additional data are needed toinvestigate whether long-term benefitsof maternal MMN supplementation, overIFA alone, may be observed for childrenin other contexts or for the develop-ment of other cognitive and socioemo-tional skills. Analyses directly comparingthe current UNICEF/World HealthOrganization/United Nations UniversityMMN formulation with IFA, as reportedhere, are also important to inform ma-ternal health policy.

We examined the effect of maternalsupplementation with MMNs, comparedwith IFA, on motor and socioemotionalskills, aswell as several specific aspectsof cognitive ability: language, attention(the ability to focus and sustain atten-tion), visuospatial ability (the ability toperceive and mentally represent spa-tial relationships among objects), andexecutive function (cognitive self-regulation, such as planning, effort-ful control of attention, and inhibitionof automatic responses). In addition, inprespecified analyses, we examined in-teractions between maternal supple-ment type and 2 indicators of maternalnutritional status likely to be related topoor child development: undernourish-ment at enrollment (mid-upper armcircumference [MUAC] ,23.5 cm) andanemia at enrollment (hemoglobin [Hb]concentration,110 g/L).

METHODS

This study was conducted as part of theSupplementation with Multiple Micro-nutrients Intervention Trial (SUMMIT). Adetailed description of the design andprocedures of SUMMIT has been pub-lishedpreviously.12 SUMMITwas adouble-blind, cluster-randomized trial conductedon the Indonesian island of Lombok from2001 through 2004. Pregnant womenthroughout Lombok were enrolled inSUMMIT at prenatal care clinics held

ARTICLE

PEDIATRICS Volume 130, Number 3, September 2012 e537 at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 4: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

by local midwives. Written informedconsent was obtained from all partic-ipants. Consenting women received adaily supplement throughout the dura-tion of pregnancy and until 3 monthspostpartum. Midwives were randomlyassigned to distribute either IFA orMMNs. The contents of the 2 supple-ments are presented in Table 1. Allwomen who received prenatal carefrom the same midwife received thesame supplement. All SUMMIT scientistsand personnel, government staff, andparticipants were unaware of the allo-cation of MMNs and IFA. The study pro-tocol was approved by the NationalInstitutes of Health Research and De-velopment of the Ministry of Health ofIndonesia, the Provincial Planning De-partment of Nusa Tenggara BaratProvince, and the Johns Hopkins JointCommittee on Clinical Investigation(Baltimore, Maryland). Analysis wasaccording to intention to treat.

SUMMIT staff visitedparticipantswithin72 hours of enrollment to record base-line information, including MUAC andHb concentration, which were used toclassify mothers as undernourished oranemic, respectively, in stratified anal-yses. Because midwives had been pre-viously randomized to distribute IFA orMMNs, this classification was indepen-dent of randomization. Additional datawere also collected (see Supplemental

Information). The follow-up sample wasdrawn from a random sample of 2369women who provided blood samplesboth before and after supplementation.

Follow-up Sample

Figure 1 shows the trial profile. A totalof 28 426 children were born live, with26 228 being alive at 12 weeks post-partum after mortality and loss tofollow-up. To power the study to detecta difference of 0.3 SD and adding 15%for potential attrition, we targeted 549mothers, comprising all mothers whogave birth between September 24,2003, and March 31, 2004, and who hadtheir blood drawn both before and af-ter supplementation. We were able totest children of 484 of these mothers,including 3 sets of twins, for a total of487 children. Of the targeted partic-ipants, the proportion who were nottested did not differ between IFA (32 of272) and MMN (33 of 277) (P = .957).Fourteen motor scores (5 IFA, 9 MMN),8 visual attention/spatial scores (2 IFA,6MMN), and 11 executive function scores(4 IFA, 7 MMN) were missing because thechild refused to attempt the tests.

All testing was conducted from April toSeptember 2007 at the homes of theparticipants within 3 weeks of the daythe child turned 42 months. Written in-formed consent was obtained from aparent. Ethicalapproval for the informed

consent and research procedures forthe follow-up study was obtained fromthe Lancaster University Ethics Com-mittee and the Mataram University Eth-ical Research Committee.

Developmental Tests

Developmental tests were selected thatassess specific abilities which developduring early childhood and are likely tobe sensitive to nutritional influences.Research in maternal and child un-dernutrition in humans and animalssuggests possible effects on motor de-velopment,13 language development,14,15

and nonverbal cognitive development,including visual attention, visuospatialability, and executive function,16 as wellas socioemotional development.17,18

Tests that assess each of these domainswere adapted to the local language,culture, and setting in Lombok, andwere evaluated for reliability and val-idity. A full description of the test se-lection criteria, adaptations, and thereliability and validity results is repor-ted in Prado et al.19 The tests are de-scribed in Table 2.

Additional Measures

The following data were also collectedat the time of child testing: an adaptedversion of the HomeObservation for theMeasurement of the Environment in-ventory,20 the child’s Hb concentration,and maternal depression. For details,see Supplemental Information.

Statistical Analyses

Group Characteristic Comparisons

First, we examined whether childrenwhose mothers received MMNs and IFAwere matched on the characteristicslisted in Table 3. For the continuousvariables, the difference was estimatedinmixed effectsmodels. A randomeffectof midwife code on intercept was in-cluded in all analyses because the ran-domization was allocated by midwiferather than by individual participant;

TABLE 1 Contents of the 2 Maternal Supplements

IFA MMNs

Iron (ferrous fumarate) 30 mg Iron (ferrous fumarate) 30 mgFolic acid 400 mg Folic acid 400 mg

Retinol (retinyl acetate) 800 mgVitamin D (ergocalciferol) 200 IUVitamin E (a-tocopherol acetate) 10 mgAscorbic acid 70 mgVitamin B1 (thiamine mononitrate) 1.4 mgVitamin B2 (riboflavin) 1.4 mgNiacin (niacinanide) 18 mgVitamin B6 (pyridoxine) 1.9 mgVitamin B12 (cyanocobalamin) 2.6 mgZinc (zinc gluconate) 15 mgCopper 2 mgSelenium 65 mgIodine 150 mg

e538 PRADO et al at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 5: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

specification of this random effectaccounts for variation between midwifeclusters.21 For the categorical variables,the difference between groups was es-timated by using generalized linearmodels, with midwife code as a repeatedmeasure. For details, see SupplementalInformation.

Calculation of Test Scores

Foreach test score (described in Table 1),z scores were computed on the basisof the distribution of our sample. Thecomputation of z scores harmonizedevery test score to the same scale.This allowed us to compute compositedomain scores as the average of eachchild’s test z scores from that domain,in the following domains: language

development, Picture Vocabulary andSentence Complexity z scores; socio-emotional development, socioemotionalcompetence and problem z scores; ex-ecutive function, Snack Delay and Win-dows z scores; and visual attention/spatial ability, Visual Search and BlockDesign z scores. For details, see Sup-plemental Information. For the motorscale, 9.4% of item scores were missing.These missing items were imputed byusing the sequential regression imputa-tion method described in Raghunathanet al.22 For details, see Supplemental In-formation. We additionally performedthe analyses on motor scores bycounting refused (missing) items as afailure, rather than using imputation,and found a similar pattern of results.

Effect of Maternal MMN Versus IFA

The effect of MMN versus IFA supple-mentation on each domain score wasestimated in mixed effects models witha fixed effect of supplement type anda random effect of midwife code. Thismodel was estimated first with a fixedeffect of supplement type as the onlyindependent variable, and second, withfixed effects of supplement type and anyvariables from Table 3 that indepen-dently predicted each domain score. Fora detailed description of covariate se-lection, see Supplemental Information.Because gestational age at birth (pre-term, full-term, or post-term) did not sig-nificantly predict any domain score, thisvariable was not included as a covariate.

To examine the effect on each domainscore in children of mothers who wereundernourished (MUAC ,23.5 cm) atenrollment and mothers who wereanemic (Hb ,110 g/L) at enrollment,the interaction between each of these 2variables and supplement type was(separately) added to the model. If thisinteraction was significant at the P, .1level, the effect of MMNs was estimatedfor each subgroup.

RESULTS

The effect of MMN supplementation onthe composite score for each develop-mental domain is reported in Table 4.The estimate (B) represents the un-standardized estimate of the differencein developmental scores between chil-dren of mothers who received MMNsand IFA, expressed as a fraction of thevariation (SD) of the developmentalscore. For example, children of motherswho received MMNs scored 0.12 SDhigher in motor development comparedwith those who received IFA (P = .253).Although this effect on motor develop-ment was significant when adjusting forthe covariates (P = .036), it was not sig-nificant in the unadjusted analysis. Nosignificant effects of MMNs were foundin any other developmental domain,

FIGURE 1Trial profile.

ARTICLE

PEDIATRICS Volume 130, Number 3, September 2012 e539 at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 6: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

suggesting that there were no robusteffects of maternal MMN supplementa-tion in the full group of children.

Children of UndernourishedMothers

The interaction between maternal sup-plement typeandmaternalundernutrition(MUAC ,23.5 cm) was significant formotor development (B = 0.412; SE = 0.199;

t(399) = 2.07; P = .039) and visualattention/spatial ability (B = 0.454, SE =0.168, t(420) = 2.71, P = .007) but not forlanguage, executive function, or socio-emotional development.

In motor development, children of un-dernourished mothers who receivedMMNs scored 0.35 SD higher than thosewho received IFA (P = .044) (Table 5).In a group of children aged 30 to 55

months in Lombok tested to establishthe developmental sensitivity of thetests,19 the estimate of the effect of age(in months) on the motor developmentz score was 0.08, indicating that motordevelopment increased ∼0.08 SD witheach additional month of age. Thus, thiseffect of maternal MMN supplementa-tion on motor development (0.35 SD)represents an advantage equivalent to

TABLE 2 Motor, Cognitive, and Socioemotional Tests

Domain/Test Method and Scores

Motor DevelopmentFine and Gross Motor

Development ScalesThe fine and gross motor scales were developed by selecting age-appropriate items (10 fine motor items

and 10 gross motor items) from both the Bayley Scale of Motor Development42 and the Ages and StagesQuestionnaire43; for example, “Jumps over a rope 20 cm high” and “Threads three beads on a string.”The motor score was calculated as the total number of items, out of 20, that a child performed successfully.

Cognitive DevelopmentLanguage AbilityPicture Vocabulary Test Based on the British Picture Vocabulary Scale,44 the child was presented 50 items, each consisting of 4 pictures

(1 target and 3 distracters) and a word spoken by the tester. The child was asked to point to the picturecorresponding to the word. Item scores were summed for a total score.

Sentence Complexity Scale Based on the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory–Level III,45 the child’s parent or anothercaregiver was asked 16 questions concerning the child’s expressive language ability. For example, the parentwas asked, given an appropriate context, “Which one more closely resembles your child’s speech: “Tas te kadu”(I’m using my bag) or “Tas te kadu jauq robot” (I’m using my bag to carry a robot)? A score of 0 was given for theless complex structure and a score of 1 for the more complex structure. These items were adapted to the Sasaklanguage based on transcripts of child speech.19 For each test, item scores were summed for a total score.

Visuospatial AbilityBlock Design Test Based on aspects of both the British Ability Scale46 and the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence–Third

Edition,47 the child was presented 7 shapes made with wooden blocks and was asked to build a copy of each.Thirty seconds were allowed per attempt. Children who failed on the first attempt were given a second attempt,only for the first 4 items. Children were scored on the average amount of time per correct item completed,which was then reciprocal transformed, reversing the score to match the direction of theother scores (higher is better).

Visual AttentionVisual Search Test This test was based on the NEPSY Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment visual search subtest.48

In each of 2 items, the child was shown an array of pictures and asked to point to every instance of a targetpicture in the array, while the tester circled each picture the child indicated. The score was calculated as thenumber of hits (number of targets correctly indicated) minus the number of false alarms(number of distracter pictures incorrectly indicated).

Executive FunctionSnack Delay Test In the Snack Delay Test,49,50 the tester placed a snack in the child’s hand and told the child to wait until given permission

to eat the snack. Four trials were administered, with delays of 5, 15, 30, and 45 seconds. If the child ate the snackbefore the allotted time, the amount of time the child waited before eating the snack was recorded. The averageamount of time the child waited before eating the snack across the four trials was calculated.

Windows Test In the Windows Test,51 the tester placed a treat inside 1 of 2 boxes, each of which had a clear window through whichthe child could see the treat. The child was initially instructed to point to the box with the treat to obtain the treat.After 2 trials, the rule was reversed and the child was instructed to point to the box without the treat to obtain it.The score was calculated as the number of correct trials after the rule was reversed, out of 6 trials.

Socioemotional DevelopmentSocioemotional Development Scale A Socioemotional Development Scale was developed based on the Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional

Assessment.52 In an interview format, the child’s parent or caregiver rated 29 items probing specific aspects of thechild’s behavior on a scale from 0 to 2 (0 = not true/rarely, 1 = somewhat true/sometimes, 2 = very true/often).Both competence items (eg, “Plays well with other children”) and problem items (eg, “Does not make eye contact”)were presented. A competence score was calculated as the sum of the competence item ratings and a problemscore was calculated as the sum of the problem item ratings. The problem score was reversed, to match thedirection of the other scores (higher scores representing better functioning), by subtracting the raw scorefrom the maximum possible score.42

e540 PRADO et al at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 7: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

∼4.5 months of age. No difference inmotor scores was found in children ofmothers who were not undernourishedduring pregnancy. This finding suggeststhat the effect of MMNs on motor de-velopment observed over all children in

the adjusted analysis (Table 4) was dueto this effect in children of under-nourished, rather than well-nourished,mothers.

In visual attention/spatial ability, chil-dren of undernourished mothers who

received MMNs scored 0.35 SD higherthan those who received IFA (P = .011)(Table 5). Children of mothers whowere not undernourished during preg-nancy did not show this advantage. Theeffect of age in months on this score, in

TABLE 3 Group Characteristic Comparisons of the Preschool Follow-up Sample Who Received IFA and MMNs

Characteristic IFA MMN IFA Versus MMN

n Mean 6 SD or % n Mean 6 SD or % T or za P

Baseline maternal age, y 234 25.7 6 6.0 241 25.5 6 5.5 0.28 .777Baseline maternal height, cm 216 149.6 6 4.9 217 149.6 6 4.9 0.11 .915Mean compliance (percentage of supplements consumed) 209 79.6 6 21.3 201 75.5 6 21.3 1.15 .254Maternal cognitive z score 82 20·04 6 0.56 93 0·10 6 0·66 1.52 .133Maternal mood score at cognitive testing 41 41.4 6 7.7 50 39.8 6 6.9 1.08 .280Maternal mood score at child testing 160 42.0 6 5.5 160 42.1 6 5.7 0.16 .877Child’s birth weight, g 197 3199.5 6 519.2 195 3265.4 6 497.9 1.32 .188Child’s Hb concentration, g/L 218 109 6 13 208 110 6 13 0.44 .661HOME inventory score 241 39.3 6 5.3 246 38.8 6 5.7 1.05 .296Child’s hours of sleep in the 24 hours before testing 241 11.3 6 1.3 246 11.3 6 1.4 0.25 .804Baseline maternal Hb concentration 0.24 .622,110 g/L 109/240 45 117/243 48$110 g/L 131/240 55 126/243 52

Baseline maternal MUAC 0.19 .665,23.5 cm 68/212 32 75/220 34$23.5 cm 144/212 68 145/220 66

Baseline maternal education (completed years of formaleducation)

0.10 .917

0 18/236 8 22/243 91–6 121/236 51 121/243 507–9 66/236 28 59/243 24.9 31/236 13 41/243 17

Baseline paternal education (completed years of formaleducation)

0.42 .675

0 16/225 7 21/231 91–6 94/225 42 96/231 427–9 47/225 21 50/231 22.9 68/225 30 64/231 28

Baseline socioeconomic indexb 0.71 .4790 85/237 36 98/243 401 69/237 29 66/243 272 48/237 20 43/243 18.2 35/237 15 36/243 15

Gestational age at enrollment 0.26 .795First trimester 93/241 39 96/246 39Second trimester 112/241 47 105/246 43Third trimester 36/241 15 45/246 18

Parity (birth order of the tested child) 0.67 .4111 81/237 34 78/243 322–3 109/237 46 123/243 514–5 40/237 17 32/243 13$6 7/237 3 10/243 4

Gestational age at birth 0.54 .587Preterm (28–36 wk) 49/241 20 59/246 24Full-term (37–42 wk) 157/241 65 157/246 64Post-term (.42 wk) 35/241 14 30/246 12

Child’s gender (number of girls) 108/241 45 123/246 50 1.19 .236

All baseline measures were taken within 72 hours of enrollment. Maternal cognitive and mood scores were taken from Prado et al.39a Comparisons using mixed effects models for continuous variables (t is reported) and generalized linear models for categorical variables (z is reported).b The socioeconomic index was derived from a survey administered at enrollment to determine whether participants owned 12 household items. Subsequently, we discarded 6 items withcorrelations with the total score of ,0.1. Removing these items improved the internal consistency of the index from Cronbach’s a = 0.41 to 0.57. The 6 remaining items (owned a radio;a television; a refrigerator; a bike; a motorbike; and a small sales business, such as selling snacks, commonly operated out of the home) were summed for a total score.

ARTICLE

PEDIATRICS Volume 130, Number 3, September 2012 e541 at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 8: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

the aforementioned sample of childrenaged 30 to 55 months, was 0.07, in-dicating that this score increased by∼0.07 SD with each month of age.Therefore, the effect of MMNs in chil-dren of undernourished mothers rep-resents an advantage equivalent to ∼5months of age. Both the effects onmotorand visual attention/spatial ability inchildren of undernourished motherswere slightly stronger in the analysesadjusting for the covariates (B = 0.39and B = 0.37, respectively).

The mean z score for each domain inchildren of mothers who received IFAand MMNs stratified on the basis of themother’s MUAC is presented in Fig 2.

Children of undernourishedmothers whoreceived IFA scored substantially lowerin motor development (mean: –0.28) andvisual attention/spatial ability (mean: –0.35) than all other groups (mean:0.01–0.10). Children of undernourishedmothers who received MMNs had scores(mean: 0.07 for motor ability and 0.01 forvisual attention/spatial ability) similar tochildren of mothers who were not un-dernourished in either supplement group(mean: 0.01–0.10). This finding suggeststhat maternal MMN supplementationprotected children of undernourishedmothers from negative developmentaleffects of the mother’s poor nutritionalstatus during pregnancy.

Children of Anemic Mothers

The interaction between maternalsupplement type and maternal anemia(Hb ,110 g/L) was significant at theP , .1 level for visual attention/spatialability (B = 0.292, SE = 0.157, t(469) =1.86, P = .063) but not for any otherdomain score.

Children of anemic mothers who re-ceived MMNs scored 0.23 SD higher invisual attention/spatial ability thanthose who received IFA, an effect whichapproached significance in the un-adjusted analysis (P = .055) and wassignificant in the adjusted analysis (B =0.24, P = .030) (Table 5). This effect sizerepresents an advantage equivalent to∼3 months of age. Children of motherswho were not anemic during preg-nancy did not show a benefit of ma-ternal MMN supplementation on visualattention/spatial ability.

DISCUSSION

In a randomized trial in Indonesia thatexamined the effects of maternal MMNsupplementation, compared with IFA,on the motor, cognitive, and socio-emotional abilities of children at age 42months, the following results werefound. No significant effectswere foundfor children overall, except in the ad-justed analysis on motor development.Stratified analysis showed that this

TABLE 4 Effect of MMNs on Motor, Cognitive, and Socioemotional Domain Scores

Domain

Adjusted for Cluster Randomization

Adjusted for Cluster Randomizationand Other Covariates That

Independently Predicted EachDomain Score

n Estimate (95% CI) P n Estimate (95% CI) P

Motor development 473 0.12 (–0.08 to 0.32) .253 425 0.19 (0.02 to 0.37)a .036Language development 487 0.00 (–0.20 to 0.19) .971 380 0.07 (–0.11 to 0.25)b .463Visual attention/spatial ability 479 0.08 (–0.09 to 0.24) .355 424 0.07 (–0.08 to 0.22)c .342Executive function 476 20.06 (–0.21 to 0.09) .427 385 20.04 (–0.19 to 0.11)d .634Socioemotional development 487 20.02 (–0.16 to 0.12) .777 410 0.00 (–0.15 to 0.15)e .981

The estimate of the effect of MMNs represents the difference in scores between children of mothers who received MMNs andIFA, expressed as a fraction of the variation (SD) of the score. Positive estimates indicate that those who received MMNsscored higher than those who received IFA. CI, 95% confidence interval.a Adjusted for Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment Inventory (HOME) inventory score and child’s Hbconcentration.b Adjusted for HOME inventory score, child’s Hb concentration, and mother’s MUAC ,23.5 cm.c Adjusted for HOME inventory score and mother’s MUAC ,23.5 cm.d Adjusted for HOME inventory score and birth weight.e Adjusted for HOME inventory score and compliance (mean percentage of supplements consumed).

TABLE 5 Effect of MMNs on Motor Development and Visual Attention/Spatial Ability in Stratified Analyses

DomainAdjusted for Cluster Randomization

Adjusted for Cluster Randomization and Other Covariates ThatIndependently Predicted Each Domain Score

n Estimate (95% CI) P Interaction Term P n Estimate (95% CI) P Interaction Term P

Motor developmentMother’s MUAC ,23.5 cm 139 0.35 (0.01 to 0.69) .044 .039 128 0.39 (0.08 to 0.70)a .015 .073Mother’s MUAC $23.5 cm 280 20.06 (–0.31 to 0.19) .633 251 0.04 (–0.18 to 0.27)a .703

Visual attention/spatial abilityMother’s MUAC ,23.5 cm 140 0.35 (0.08 to 0.63) .011 .007 140 0.37 (0.11 to 0.62)b .004 .005Mother’s MUAC $23.5 cm 284 20.10 (–0.29 to 0.09) .312 284 20.08 (–0.26 to 0.10)b .399Mother’s Hb ,110 g/L 222 0.23 (0.00 to 0.46) .055 .063 188 0.24 (0.02 to 0.46)c .030 .038Mother’s Hb $110 g/L 253 20.07 (–0.28 to 0.15) .560 232 20.07 (–0.27 to 0.13)c .488

The estimate of the effect of MMNs represents the difference in scores between children of mothers who received MMNs and IFA, expressed as a fraction of the variation (SD) of the score.Positive estimates indicate that those who received MMNs scored higher than those who received IFA. The interaction term P value represents the P value associated with the interactionbetween maternal supplement type and the stratification variable (mother’s MUAC or Hb). CI, 95% confidence interval.a Adjusted for Home Observation for the Measurement of the Environment Inventory (HOME) inventory score and child’s Hb concentration.b Adjusted for HOME inventory score.c Adjusted for HOME inventory score and mother’s MUAC ,23.5 cm.

e542 PRADO et al at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 9: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

effect onmotor developmentwas foundonly in children of mothers who wereundernourished during pregnancy andwas equivalent to an advantage of∼4.5months of age. Maternal MMN supple-mentation also yielded advantages invisual attention/spatial ability in 2 sub-groups of children: children of motherswho were undernourished during preg-nancy, equivalent to ∼5 months of age,and children of mothers who wereanemic during pregnancy, equivalentto ∼3 months of age. These findingshave several implications.

First, these results demonstrate thatadequate maternal micronutrient in-take is necessary for intact braindevelopment, a conclusion that in thepast has largely depended on animalmodels. Although acute maternal defi-ciencies in certain micronutrients areknown to result in severe neurologicimpairment (eg, maternal iodine de-ficiency resulting in cretinism),4 theeffect of mild to moderate micro-nutrient deficiencies during preg-nancy in humans has not yet beenclarified. The finding that children ofundernourished mothers who re-ceived IFA performed at substantially

lower motor and cognitive levelscompared with those who receivedMMNs and compared with children ofwell-nourished mothers (Fig 2) sug-gests that supplementation with IFAalone is insufficient to protect againstnegative long-term developmental ef-fects of maternal undernutrition. TheMMN supplement in the current studycontained iodine, zinc, and vitamin B6,all of which are important for braindevelopment. Maternal iodine supple-mentation has been found to improvemotor and cognitive development inseveral studies.14,23,24 Maternal zincsupplementation has been found toimprove muscle mass in 12-month-oldinfants,25 which could lead to improvedmotor development, although improve-ments in motor development have notbeen observed in 2 other studies ofmaternal zinc supplementation.26,27 Thus,adequate intake of other micronutrientsin addition to iron/folic acid duringpregnancy and lactation seems to beimportant for healthy motor and cog-nitive development.

Second, the finding that MMN sup-plementation benefited these child-ren’s motor and cognitive abilities in

undernourished mothers is consistentwith previous evidence that maternalMMN supplementation can provide oth-er important advantages, such as re-ductions in low birth weight28 and infantmortality 3 months postpartum,12 andsuggests that these advantages extendbeyond gestational and early infanthealth outcomes. The finding is alsoconsistent with the positive findings ofthe 3 previous studies that examined theeffect of MMN supplementation on Bay-ley Scales of Infant Development mentaland motor scores10,29,30 and providesimportant additional elements, such asthe evaluation of multiple domains ofearly child development and the care-ful adaptation of the tests to the localsetting.19 Perhaps most importantly,whereas those studies focused on in-fants (up to age 18 months), the currentstudy examined children at 42 monthsof age. Infant development scores aregenerally poor predictors of later-cognitive abilities31; in contrast, thosescores of children ages 3 to 7 yearspredict later outcomes such as schoolachievement,32–34 which in turn leads tohigher skilled employment and higherwages in adulthood. The fact that wefound a benefit of MMN supplementation

FIGURE 2Mean z score in each developmental domain for children of mothers who received IFA and MMNs stratified on the basis of the MUAC of the mother.Error bars show the SEM.

ARTICLE

PEDIATRICS Volume 130, Number 3, September 2012 e543 at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 10: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

in visual attention/spatial ability is in-triguing because focusing and sus-taining attention are important skills forschool success.35 Moreover, attentionmeasured before and during elemen-tary school has been found to predictschool achievement up to 5 yearslater.36–38 Thus, the results obtained heresuggest that maternal MMN supple-mentation might have much longer-termadvantages for children of under-nourished and anemic mothers.

Third, the finding that the motor andcognitive benefits were observed pri-marily in the children of mothers whowereundernourishedoranemicduringpregnancy is consistent with previousstudies that have observed a particularbenefit of MMN supplementation inthese subgroups on infant survival,12

maternal cognition,39 and infant motordevelopment,29 although not on birthweight.28,40 Together, these findings sug-gest that micronutrient deficiency maybe especially problematic among thesesubgroups, and that these mothersand their children particularly benefitfrom MMN supplementation duringpregnancy and postpartum in certainimportant outcomes. Given that somestudies have found no differences be-tween maternal MMN and IFA supple-mentation on neonatal infant mortality41

or on cognitive performance at age 7 to9 years,11 this highlights the importance

of examining effects in subgroups ofmothers who are particularly likely tobenefit from MMN supplementation.

We found no effects on language, ex-ecutive, or socioemotional skills. Onelimitation of this study was that thechildren were tested at an age in whicha wide variation in performance isnormal. For example, because a widevariation in language ability falls withina normal range at age 3 years, it may bedifficult to detect effects in languagedevelopment at this age. Another limi-tation was that the tests of executivefunction in the current study primarilyassessed inhibition ability (in both theSnack Delay and Windows tests) and,to some extent, task switching (in theWindows test). In later childhood, it be-comes easier to assess other aspects ofexecutive function, including planningand task monitoring, which might bemore sensitive to maternal MMN sup-plementation. A third limitation was thatour measure of socioemotional devel-opment relied on parent report ratherthan direct assessment of the child,which may also be more sensitive to ef-fects ofmaternalMMNsupplementation.

CONCLUSIONS

Our findings suggest that, in a popula-tion such as that examined here in In-donesia,maternalMMNsupplementation

compared with IFA supplementation canimprove the motor and cognitive abili-ties of children as late as 3.5 years, inparticular when women are under-nourished oranemic duringpregnancy,and especially for motor function andvisual attention/spatial ability. The lossof developmental potential in earlychildhood is a critical problem in low-and middle-income countries. Thefindings suggest that providing MMNsupplements to mothers during preg-nancy and lactation, particularly tothose who are undernourished oranemic, is an effective interventionto address this loss and to promotehealthy motor and cognitive devel-opment in children.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTSThe SUMMIT Study Group designed andexecuted the SUMMIT study. We ac-knowledge the substantial contribu-tion of Dr Abas Jahari in the supportand completion of the study. Sri Hartini,Astri Hidayanti, Siti Nurul Hikmah, BaiqElfa Ismayani, Atik Rahmawati, andFitriati conducted pilot testing, test ad-aptation, and data collection. Nurhafni,Indah Qoriana, Rosmawarty, and Nuniks I.Gayatri provided administrative sup-port. Farhiyah, Nurmawati, and Maryatitranscribed child speech samples to val-idate the parent-reported languagemeasure.

REFERENCES

1. Campbell FA, Ramey CT, Pungello EP,Sparling J, Miller-Johnson S. Early child-hood education: young adult outcomesfrom the Abecedarian Project. Appl Dev Sci.2002;6(1):42–57

2. Reynolds AJ, Temple JA, Robertson DL,Mann EA. Long-term effects of an earlychildhood intervention on educationalachievement and juvenile arrest: a 15-yearfollow-up of low-income children in publicschools. JAMA. 2001;285(18):2339–2346

3. Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S,Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B; Interna-tional Child Development Steering Group.

Developmental potential in the first 5 yearsfor children in developing countries. Lancet.2007;369(9555):60–70

4. Institute of Medicine. Nutrition DuringPregnancy. Washington, DC: National Acad-emy Press; 1990

5. Couperus JW, Nelson CA. Early brain de-velopment and plasticity. In: McCartney K,Phillips D, eds. The Blackwell Handbook ofEarly Childhood Development. Malden, MA:Blackwell Publishing; 2006

6. Groziak SM, Kirksey A. Effects of maternalrestriction in vitamin B-6 on neocortex de-velopment in rats: neuron differentiation

and synaptogenesis. J Nutr. 1990;120(5):485–492

7. Chang SJ, Kirksey A, Morré DM. Effects ofvitamin B-6 deficiency on morphologicalchanges in dendritic trees of Purkinje cellsin developing cerebellum of rats. J Nutr.1981;111(5):848–857

8. Georgieff MK, Rao R. The role of nutrition incognitive development. In: Nelson CA, LucianaM, eds. Handbook of Developmental Cogni-tive Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: MITPress; 1999:491–504

9. Morré DM, Kirksey A, Das GD. Effects ofvitamin B-6 deficiency on the developing

e544 PRADO et al at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 11: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

central nervous system of the rat. Myeli-nation. J Nutr. 1978;108(8):1260–1265

10. Li Q, Yan H, Zeng L, et al. Effects of maternalmultimicronutrient supplementation on themental development of infants in ruralwestern China: follow-up evaluationof a double-blind, randomized, controlledtrial. Pediatrics. 2009;123(4). Available at:www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/123/4/e685

11. Christian P, Murray-Kolb LE, Khatry SK, et al.Prenatal micronutrient supplementationand intellectual and motor function in earlyschool-aged children in Nepal. JAMA. 2010;304(24):2716–2723

12. Shankar AH, Jahari AB, Sebayang SK, et al;Supplementation with Multiple Micro-nutrients Intervention Trial (SUMMIT) StudyGroup. Effect of maternal multiple micro-nutrient supplementation on fetal loss andinfant death in Indonesia: a double-blindcluster-randomised trial. Lancet. 2008;371(9608):215–227

13. Gorman KS. Malnutrition and cognitive de-velopment: evidence from experimental/quasi-experimental studies among the mild-to-moderately malnourished. J Nutr. 1995;125(suppl 8):2239S–2244S

14. O’Donnell KJ, Rakeman MA, Zhi-Hong D,et al. Effects of iodine supplementationduring pregnancy on child growth anddevelopment at school age. Dev Med ChildNeurol. 2002;44(2):76–81

15. Pollitt E, Gorman KS, Engle PL, Martorell R,Rivera J. Early supplementary feeding andcognition: effects over two decades. MonogrSoc Res Child Dev. 1993;58(7):1–99, discus-sion 111–118

16. Hughes D, Bryan J. The assessment ofcognitive performance in children: consid-erations for detecting nutritional influen-ces. Nutr Rev. 2003;61(12):413–422

17. Strupp BJ, Levitsky DA. Enduring cognitiveeffects of early malnutrition: a theoreticalreappraisal. J Nutr. 1995;125(suppl 8):2221S–2232S

18. Black MM. The evidence linking zinc de-ficiency with children’s cognitive and motorfunctioning. J Nutr. 2003;133(5 suppl 1):1473S–1476S

19. Prado EL, Hartini S, Rahmawati A, et al. Testselection, adaptation, and evaluation: a sys-tematic approach to assess nutritional in-fluences on child development in developingcountries. Br J Educ Psychol. 2010;80(pt 1):31–53

20. Caldwell BM, Bradley RH. Home Observationfor Measurement of the Environment: Ad-ministration Manual. Tempe, AZ: Family &Human Dynamics Research Institute, ArizonaState University; 2003

21. Wears RL. Advanced statistics: statisticalmethods for analyzing cluster and cluster-randomized data. Acad Emerg Med. 2002;9(4):330–341

22. Raghunathan TE, Lepkowski JM, Van Hoe-wyk J, Solenberger P. A multivariate tech-nique for multiply imputing missing valuesusing a sequence of regression models.Surv Methodol. 2001;27(1):85–95

23. Thilly CH, Lagasse R, Roger G, Bourdoux P,Ermans AM. Impaired foetal and postnataldevelopment and high perinatal death-rate in a severe iodine deficient area. In:Stockigt JR, Nagataki S, eds. Thyroid Re-search VIII. Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press;1980:20–23

24. Connolly KJ, Pharoah PO, Hetzel BS. Fetal io-dine deficiency and motor performance dur-ing childhood. Lancet. 1979;2(8153):1149–1151

25. Iannotti LL, Zavaleta N, León Z, Shankar AH,Caulfield LE. Maternal zinc supplementationand growth in Peruvian infants. Am J ClinNutr. 2008;88(1):154–160

26. Hamadani JD, Fuchs GJ, Osendarp SJ, HudaSN, Grantham-McGregor SM. Zinc supple-mentation during pregnancy and effects onmental development and behaviour ofinfants: a follow-up study. Lancet. 2002;360(9329):290–294

27. Tamura T, Goldenberg RL, Ramey SL, NelsonKG, Chapman VR. Effect of zinc supple-mentation of pregnant women on themental and psychomotor development oftheir children at 5 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr.2003;77(6):1512–1516

28. Fall CH, Fisher DJ, Osmond C, Margetts BM;Maternal Micronutrient SupplementationStudy Group. Multiple micronutrient supple-mentation during pregnancy in low-incomecountries: a meta-analysis of effects onbirth size and length of gestation. Food NutrBull. 2009;30(suppl 4):S533–S546

29. Tofail F, Persson LA, El Arifeen S, et al.Effects of prenatal food and micronutrientsupplementation on infant development:a randomized trial from the Maternal andInfant Nutrition Interventions, Matlab(MINIMat) study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2008;87(3):704–711

30. McGrath N, Bellinger D, Robins J, Msa-manga GI, Tronick E, Fawzi WW. Effect ofmaternal multivitamin supplementation onthe mental and psychomotor developmentof children who are born to HIV-1-infectedmothers in Tanzania. Pediatrics. 2006;117(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/117/2/e216

31. Snow CE, Van Hemel SB, eds. Early Child-hood Assessment: Why, What, and How.Washington, DC: The National AcademiesPress; 2008

32. Gorman KS, Pollitt E. Does schooling bufferthe effects of early risk? Child Dev. 1996;67(2):314–326

33. Stith AY, Gorman KS, Choudhury N. Theeffects of psychosocial risk and gender onschool attainment in Guatemala. AppliedPsychology: An International Review. 2003;52(4):614–629

34. Duncan GJ, Dowsett CJ, Claessens A, et al.School readiness and later achievement.Dev Psychol. 2007;43(6):1428–1446

35. Rowe KJ, Rowe KS. The relationship be-tween inattentiveness in the classroom andreading achievement (Part B): an explana-tory study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psy-chiatry. 1992;31(2):357–368

36. Rabiner D, Coie JD; The Conduct ProblemsPrevention Research Group. Early attentionproblems and children’s reading achieve-ment: a longitudinal investigation. J AmAcad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(7):859–867

37. Rapport MD, Scanlan SW, Denney CB.Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder andscholastic achievement: a model of dualdevelopmental pathways. J Child PsycholPsychiatry. 1999;40(8):1169–1183

38. Spira EG, Fischel JE. The impact of preschoolinattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity onsocial and academic development: a review.J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(7):755–773

39. Prado EL, Ullman MT, Muadz H, Alcock KJ,Shankar AH; SUMMIT Study Group. The effectof maternal multiple micronutrient supple-mentation on cognition and mood duringpregnancy and postpartum in Indonesia:a randomized trial. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(3):e32519

40. Sebayang SK, Dibley MJ, Kelly P, Shankar AV,Shankar AH. Modifying effect of maternalnutritional status on the impact of mater-nal multiple micronutrient supplementa-tion on birthweight in Indonesia. Eur J ClinNutr. 2011;65(10):1110–1117

41. Ronsmans C, Fisher DJ, Osmond C,Margetts BM, Fall CHD; Maternal Micro-nutrient Supplementation Study Group.Multiple micronutrient supplementationduirng pregnancy in low-income countries:a meta-analysis of effects on stillbirths andon early and late neonatal mortality. FoodNutr Bull. 2009;30(suppl 4):S547–S555

42. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Devel-opment. 2nd ed. San Antonio, TX: Psycho-logical Corporation; 1993

43. Schaefer CE, DiGeronimo TF. Ages and Stages:A Parent’s Guide to Normal Childhood De-velopment. New York, NY: John Wiley &Sons, Inc; 2000

ARTICLE

PEDIATRICS Volume 130, Number 3, September 2012 e545 at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 12: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

44. Dunn LM, Dunn LM, Whetton C, Burley J.British Picture Vocabulary Scale. 2nd ed.London, UK: NFER-Nelson Publishing Co, Ltd;1997

45. Fenson L, Marchman VA, Thal D, Dale PS,Reznick JS, Bates E. The MacArthur-BatesCommunicative Development Inventories,Second Edition: User’s Guide and TechnicalManual. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. BrookesPublishing Co; 2007

46. Elliot CD. British Ability Scales. 2nd ed.London, UK: NFER-Nelson Publishing Co, Ltd;1996

47. Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Pri-mary Scale of Intelligence—Third Edition(WPPSI-III). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt As-sessment, Inc; 2002

48. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp S. NEPSY: A De-velopmental Neuropsychological Assess-ment. Orlando, FL: The PsychologicalCorporation; 1998

49. Kochanska G, Murray KT, Harlan ET. Effortfulcontrol in early childhood: continuity andchange, antecedents, and implications forsocial development. Dev Psychol. 2000;36(2):220–232

50. Carlson SM. Developmentally sensitivemeasures of executive function in pre-school children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;28(2):595–616

51. Russell J, Mauthner N, Sharpe S, Tidswell T.The ’windows task’ as a measure of stra-tegic deception in preschoolers and autis-tic subjects. Br J Dev Psychol. 1991;9(2):331–349

52. Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS. Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment(BITSEA) Manual, Version 2.0. New Haven,CT: Yale University; 2002

(Continued from first page)

Address correspondence to Elizabeth L. Prado, PhD, SUMMIT Institute of Development, Kantor Bappeda Provinsi Nusa Tenggara Barat, Lantai 2, Jalan Flamboyan No 2,Mataram, NTB, Indonesia. E-mail: [email protected] and Katherine Alcock, DPhil, Department of Psychology, Lancaster University, Bailrigg, Lancaster, LA1 4YF,United Kingdom. E-mail: [email protected]

PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275).

Copyright © 2012 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

FUNDING: Support for this project was provided by the Allen Foundation, a National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship, the Turner Foundation,UNICEF, the Centre for Health and Human Development, and the United States Agency for International Development–Indonesia (grant 497-G-00-01-00001-00). Thesponsors of the study had no role in the study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. The corresponding author had fullaccess to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

e546 PRADO et al at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from

Page 13: Prado, E.L., Alcock, K.J., Muadz, H., Ullman, M.T., and Shankar, A.H

DOI: 10.1542/peds.2012-0412; originally published online August 20, 2012;Pediatrics

Anuraj H. ShankarElizabeth L. Prado, Katherine J. Alcock, Husni Muadz, Michael T. Ullman and

Randomized Trial in IndonesiaMaternal Multiple Micronutrient Supplements and Child Cognition: A

  

ServicesUpdated Information &

/peds.2012-0412http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2012/08/15including high resolution figures, can be found at:

Supplementary Material

5/peds.2012-0412.DCSupplemental.htmlhttp://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/suppl/2012/08/1Supplementary material can be found at:

Permissions & Licensing

tmlhttp://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xhtables) or in its entirety can be found online at: Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures,

Reprints http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

Information about ordering reprints can be found online:

rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2012 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elkpublication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly

at Dahlgren Medical Library on August 25, 2012pediatrics.aappublications.orgDownloaded from