organic food consumers

45
ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS DOES MORAL THREAT AFFECT OUR PERCEPTIONS OF MORAL LY DRIVEN ORGANIC CONSUMERS? FINAL YEAR PROJECT CODE: EWI09 ROSA DAUMAL PLA UNIVERSITAT POMPEU F ABRA JUNE 2014 TUTOR: GERT CORNELIS SEN

Upload: others

Post on 28-Oct-2021

9 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

DOES MORAL THREAT AFFECT OUR PERCEPTIONS OF MORALLY DRIVEN

ORGANIC CONSUMERS?

F I N A L Y E A R P R O J E C T C O D E : E W I 0 9

R O S A D A U M A L P L A U N I V E R S I T A T P O M P E U F A B R A

J U N E 2 0 1 4 T U T O R : G E R T C O R N E L I S S E N

Page 2: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Table of Contents

ABSTRACT  ................................................................................................................................................  2  

INTRODUCTION  ......................................................................................................................................  3  

THE  FOOD  SECTOR  ................................................................................................................................  4  1.  THE  FACTS  ..............................................................................................................................................  4  2.  URGENCY  TO  CHANGE  .............................................................................................................................  6  

MORAL  THREAT:  A  THEORETICAL  APPROACH  ...........................................................................  8  A  MORAL  APPROACH  ...................................................................................................................................  8  

EMPIRICAL  STUDY  ...............................................................................................................................  13  MOTIVATIONS  AND  HYPOTHESIS  .............................................................................................................  13  DESIGN  AND  PARTICIPANTS  .....................................................................................................................  13  STIMULI  .....................................................................................................................................................  15  DEPENDENT  VARIABLES  ...........................................................................................................................  16  CONTROL  GROUP  .......................................................................................................................................  16  RESULTS  ....................................................................................................................................................  17  DISCUSSION  ...............................................................................................................................................  20  

CONCLUSIONS  .......................................................................................................................................  21  

AKNOWLEDGMENTS  ...........................................................................................................................  22  

NOTES  AND  REFERENCES  ..................................................................................................................  23  NOTES  ........................................................................................................................................................  23  REFERENCES  ..............................................................................................................................................  24  

APPENDICES  ..........................................................................................................................................  26  

APPENDIX  1  -­‐  SURVEY  DESIGN  .................................................................................................................  26  APPENDIX  2  -­‐  SPSS  ANALYSIS    ................................................................................................................  32  

   

1

Page 3: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

ABSTRACT

Centered in unfavorable upward moral comparison, the aim of this project is first to

analyze the different theories regarding moral threat from an external view, and then prove

that it can also be applied to the conscious organic consumer.

For this purpose, two experiments were done in two completely different settings in

Barcelona: (1) BioCultura 2014, a bio fair where conscious consumers and companies and

organizations related to the organic sector meet each year, with 82 participants asked to

answer a survey while waiting in line for a conference; and (2) an Architecture class from

Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, with 73 surveys identical to the first experiment

answered when students assisted in an exam review class.

Our results concluded that there is in fact a difference when perceiving a moral versus a

neutral driven man, and that him being moral made participants value him more positively in

both experiments, albeit lower in the second experiment. However, adding an extra

imperfection manipulation resulted in a higher likeability of the moral person compared to

the neutral one.

2

Page 4: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

INTRODUCTION

Although the organic food and cosmetic industry represents a very small percentage of

Catalonia’s GDP, its consumers may be identified not only as promising but also as an

already profitable segment.

It is understandable, then, how organic companies seek strategies to increase

population’s awareness regarding the negative consequences of consuming conventional

products, both for individuals and the environment, as a way to promote their organic

products.

Regardless of all their advertising efforts, the word-of-mouth, books as well as the

influence of social media and Internet in general, the recent food news and many

documentaries regarding the matter, there is still a wide group of consumers who, in spite of

knowing of their existence and their advantages, don’t purchase them.

Initially, we may associate their reluctance to the price they are sold for. It is true they are

in general much more expensive than conventional products, but in this statement we are

ignoring all the alternative ways that exist nowadays such as organic fruit and vegetable

delivery boxes [1], as well as the frozen food category. Other factors may be more or less

relevant, such as convenience and the features of the product itself and trust to their

certification authorities.

Perhaps another explanation could be their perception of moral threat when facing a

convinced organic consumer, resulting in their conviction to continue eating conventional

food. There are many theories that could explain why individuals feel this way that we will

see later on in this project. Afterwards, a study will follow to actually test the hypothesis that

non-organic consumers on average have a negative perception towards a moral (organic)

confident consumer.

3

Page 5: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

THE FOOD SECTOR

1. THE FACTS

There is no mistake in saying that the world we are living in nowadays is more polluted

than before, its varied species are diminishing each passing day but only one has all the

blame for it. Apart from the natural cycle of life, in order to live better we made inventions

and modified the environment to our own needs, without considering seriously all the long-

term consequences this would mean to our surroundings and to ourselves. As a result, we

now live longer but suffer the first symptoms of what we call Global Warming, and feel ill for

causes that could have been prevented.

According to a course held by Esther Vivas [2], the major part of the food industry is

partly responsible for it because it is responding to determined interests and in most cases it

does not consider their negative impact on the ecosystem and on individuals. Some

companies even present their food as the unique solution to global hunger thanks to the

incremented amount of produce resulting from new inventions. But truth be told, we are

living in a toxic food environment [3],  with increased cardiovascular, obesity, cancer and

other serious diseases as a result of this corrupted sector.

THE SERIOUSNESS OF THE PROBLEM

In her course, Esther Vivas also said that global hunger although commonly known as

an African problem is nearer than we think due to access restrictions, politic causes together

with the resulting pillaging of natural resources. She also said that since the 60s, thanks to a

modernization of the agriculture system, production has increased three fold and population

only twice. As a result, according to the UN there is food for everyone [4]. Evidently,

producing food for 12B people would be more than sufficient to fill the 7B inhabitants on

Earth. But in this world of abundance [5], there is still hunger. A response to this would be the

implementation of technology to solve the ecological crisis, and quoting UN, we need to

produce more food to solve hunger. However, this solution still assumes resources are

unlimited, and this is not realistic. Again, the same causes of the food crisis would be its

solutions.

4

Page 6: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

There is another relevant aspect that should be seriously rethought. According to Esther

Vivas, people have preferences for big, attractive food that tastes well, smells usually nice

and shows signs of freshness. In result, supermarkets effect on food waste (which according

to FAO (2013) [6], is “the food that is appropriate for human consumption that is being

discarded, whether or not after it is kept beyond its expiry date or left to spoil”) is enormous

(see Figure 1). Added to this is the farmers’ policy to burn or throw away food so as to

control pricing when produce is too large.

Figure 1: Food wastage volumes, at world level by phase of the food supply chain [FAO, 2013]

The consequence of transitioning from local food to a larger and normally more distant

food chain is evident as well, since nowadays farmer-consumer information transfer is less

easy and a limited amount of companies own almost the whole market.

All in all, the major impact on nature has been due to the Green Revolution [7] starting

from the 60s. Thanks to the financing or managers provided by Ford and Rockfeller

Foundations, the Development Advisory Service, the World Bank and USAID on one hand,

it increased food production and its major contributors have even won a Nobel of

Agriculture [8] for aiding at solving global hunger. However, due to the phytosanitary

chemicals used in production and the hybrid seeds companies like Monsanto sell to farmers,

land fertility has decreased [9], seed variety has lessened, vital species such as bees are now

endangered [10] and there have been several cases of farmer suicides [11]. And the safety of

such products is also questionable [12&13].

Genetically Modified Foods, also known as GMO, have also taken a major role in the

second Green Revolution phase, the Biotechnology Revolution [14], contributing to put an

5

Page 7: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

end to starvation even more according to its inventors [Montsanto, one of the leading

companies in producing GMO, has the slogan “Produce more. Conserve more. Improve

lives.”]. However, many studies show they also are questionably safe for our health and that

of the environment [15].

It is obvious that due to all the previous reasons, the food system must be changed. In

addition to continuous research on the effect of such practices, some criticals such as Prince

Charles and Al-Gore favor alternatives over this system.

2. URGENCY TO CHANGE

A way to save the environment would be to start applying the principle of precaution in all

the food we eat. By demanding safer food, we will help change the way it is produced, and

thus the planet will benefit from it. It is obvious that eating out or precooked food won’t be

a good way to start. We need to change our habits and start taking conscience of everything

we do. It won’t be easy, that is true.

A transition towards local food is quite approachable. Buying food from local farmers or

markets that clearly state the origin of their products is a good start. Considering that,

according to Esther Vivas, nowadays food travels around 5000km before we eat it and most

of it is harvested before its optimum point of maturity, its impact is unquestionable.

However, a change towards organic food is also needed to ensure food security, health,

and more importantly diversity and life in a long-term horizon.

ORGANIC FOOD

Originating from organic agriculture, a farming and production system of food that

combines beneficial environmental practices, high level of biodiversity, the preservation of

natural resources and the utilization of natural raw materials and processes, its aim is to not

only respond to consumer demands, but also produce common goods that contribute to

protecting the planet, the animals and the development of the rural sector.

As a consequence of the lack of intensive crops and phytosanitary products, organic

food is exempt of toxics (although in many cases some may still be found in studies) [16],

they have an optimum maturity which results in better taste, flavor, nutritional value, less

water content and are more digestible. Given that they are being mostly sold as whole foods,

6

Page 8: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

they are balanced and adequate to the needs of human consumption, due to their higher

nutritional value.

But the Organic movement affects not only food. In general, the organic sector applies a

more social responsible model regarding an ethical management, fair trade, and beneficiary

and reentry-into-society labor policies.

The way to identify organic products is to look for their organic logo. In fact, since 2010

the European Commission has established that all products certified in Europe must include

the European Organic Certificate [17] (shown in Figure 2), while they optionally may include

another from the country or autonomy they are certified in. Among others, we may find the

following logos:

Figure 2: European Comission certification [18]

Figure 3: Consell Català de la Producció Agrària Ecològica in

Catalonia [19]

Figure 4: Organic Trade Association in USA [20]

Figure 5: BIO-SIEGEL in Germany [21]

Figure 6: Japanese Agricultural Standard certification [22]

Figure 7: Agence BIO’s logo in France [23]

Additionally, organic products may include other certificates such as

Figure 8: Fairtrade International [24]

Figure 9: Demeter International logo [25]

Figure 10: ECOCERT logo [26]

 All in all, consuming organic food may be an initiative completely driven by the

conviction that a better future is possible, or due to alternative reasons that are not

supported by this “moral” approach. Given this, now that we know about the need and

benefits of consuming organic products apart from the ways to clearly differ them from

conventional products, it would be interesting to analyze the theories that surround being or

not moral from an external perspective, and their reactions.

7

Page 9: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

MORAL THREAT: A THEORETICAL APPROACH

Consumers have several concerns that may influence their consumption of organic food

and these concerns may be more or less influenced by literature.

Given the similarity of the motives and applying Ibarra and Kitsuse’s (1993) study

regarding vegetarians, vegetarians may defend themselves with arguments based on either (1)

entitlement-driven motives, where they emphasize “the virtue of securing for all persons

equal institutional access and freedom of choice” or (2) endangerment-driven, “emphasizing

the optimal bodily function and health care”. In fact, several European studies focused on

this topic, and one specifically exposed the various motivations to purchase organic food.

These factors [27] are organic knowledge, environmental concern, price consciousness,

health consciousness, availability, quality, subjective norms, risk aversion, perceived control,

convenience and familiarity. Additionally, all the factors can fall into two main motives:

individualistic and altruistic [28]. However Siderer et al (2005) classified their justifications

into health, environment or social status driven motives.

According to Lund et al (2013), there exists a segment of organic consumers that is fully

convinced of the whole organic concept and it is through their consumption behavior that

they seek to show and maintain their motives. Such consumers, as stated by Sirieix et al

(2011), are generally driven by altruistic values and universalism.

There might be some cases where these “concerned consumers” state their reasons

without any negative repercussions from the audience, being more plausible when the

audience has some green-consciousness. However, not everyone willingly accepts his or her

motivations. Depending on how the person explains them, the listener may feel morally

threatened. In the following section we will further analyze this concept, and see how it is

related to the Organic food movement.

A MORAL APPROACH

According to the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy [29], the term morality may be

used when referring to:

o “Codes of conduct that are put forward by a society or some other group […], or are

accepted by an individual for his or her own behavior”

8

Page 10: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

o “Code of conduct that, given specified conditions, would be put forward by all rational

persons”

Thus, the term morality is highly important and widely accepted in our lives and it

applies to “all who can understand it and can govern their behavior by it”. We might

consider something immoral than another culture might not, and even within the same

culture people differ in their morality driven behavior.

When applying this concept to green-conscious individuals and especially to organic

food consumers, we might believe that they have created a moral code of their own. It is for

this reason that it would be interesting to analyze the different perceptions external people

have of these individuals when being faced with their moral statements.

EXTERNAL VIEW

Although inoffensively expressed, moral statements such as Leo Tolstoy’s “A man can

live and be healthy without killing animals for food; therefore, if he eats meat, he participates

in taking animal life merely for the sake of his appetite. And to act so is immoral.” may

trigger many positive and negative reactions to the audience surrounding said speaker. Kelley

(1971) states that when we face a person, we evaluate his or her morality according to

o A reality system, based on opinions, when we determine whether an action is right or

wrong

o An achievement system, based on abilities, which give the means to follow said

conviction

Nonetheless, the same author concludes that both elements are strictly necessary to be

present when perceiving someone as moral.

It is important to remark that moral comparisons may be directed upwards, when a

moral person exposes his or her views and/or actions, which may threaten the observer in

some way; or downwards, which is the way the moral person perceives the others compared

to him or herself. This project centers on unfavorable upward moral comparison, given the

open ground for research on the matter.

It would be a mistake to disregard all possible reactions this upward moral comparison

may result in. Monin (2007) described a more positive outcome as “the best of human

nature, which provides life templates to aspire to”. A different way to explain the same

9

Page 11: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

concept would be elevation, described as “a warm, uplifting feeling that people experience

when they see unexpected acts of human goodness, kindness and compassion” [Haidt,

2000]. In this case, people feel attracted to such behavior and admire it, wanting perhaps to

reassemble it in some way. Keltner and Haidt (2003) also mentioned that such perception

“makes the moral other socially attractive, and inspires people to do good themselves”

[Monin, 2007].

But not all reactions are supportive. They may trigger some negative reactions that give

reason to perceive the other person as offensive and unpleasant. Monin (2007), who

describes it as resentment, proposes that “moral behavior can constitute a threat when it

suggests to the perceiver that [said person] is not as moral as [he or she] could be, and that

the specific action called for by the self resembles reactions documented in other cases of

self-threat” [Major, Testa & Isma, 1991; Salovey, 1991].

The next possible step is to understand what directs us towards one of the two

directions. Alicke (2000) concluded it was in fact moderated by “the nature of the subject

and the target’s relationship, the importance of the comparison dimension for the subject’s

self-concept, the comparison’s ambiguity, and the subject’s need for accuracy. Festinger

(1954) described that “superior others are more threatening when they are similar, whereas

dissimilar others can be dismissed as irrelevant [Wheeler, 1991; Mettee & Risking, 1974;

Wood, 1989; Salovey, 1991; Major et al, 1991; Tesser, 1991].

A very interesting approach is that of Mussweiler (2003) and Mussweiler, Rüter &

Epstude (2004), where they suggest that whether “social comparison results in assimilation

or contrast is a result of the initial ‘holistic’ assessment of the comparison, and the resulting

selective accessibility of features”, that is, when we see ourselves similar to the speaker, we

look for information to confirm this conclusion in order for us to embrace his or her

convictions. Monin (2007) added this is also restrained by whether we are referring to

objective performance (such as “How many push-ups can you do?”) against subjective

assessment (such as “How athletic are you?”). And according to Mussweiler (2003), the

selective accessibility model results in that right after we consider moral others, we tend to

think about our own moral behavior as we retrieved it for the comparison. However he

concludes that this may result in lower moral assessment of ourselves given the higher

reference point.

10

Page 12: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

What are the exact causes of such negative views of others who behave more morally

than we do? As stated by Monin (2007), we can differ between three possible sources, and

different focuses of comparison, in line with Smith’s (2000) research: moral inferiority and

moral confusion, focused on the self; and anticipated moral reproach, focused on others.

o Moral inferiority assumes that “threat of morally superior others can resemble other threats

by others who seem better than us” [Tesser, 1991; Beach & Tesser, 2000]. In line with

this, “people can feel less moral relative to others whom they see as more moral than

themselves”. All in all, as stated by Monin (2007), we may think that upward moral

comparison could result in self-threat, and as a consequence people may activate defense

mechanisms.

o Moral confusion. Monin (2007) argues that in upward moral comparison, we may even

come to the point where we doubt our own morality, that is, when we believe it is not at

a desirable level, which in turn causes an uncomfortable situation with the self.

Additionally, sometimes moral others may open our eyes to surprising paths we didn’t

know existed.

o Anticipated moral reproach. In many cases, when being confronted by a moral person we

may think he or she is judging our own morality. Monin (2007) expresses this may cause

resentment, but authors such as Sabini and Silver (1982) see the difficulties in expressing

moral reproach due to coming to an undesired conclusion: moral superiority. And, as the

same author explains, in the moral domain “it seems less acceptable to comment on

other’s choices unless one’s choices are at least as moral”. In that sense, he explains that

regardless of our own morality state caused by moral others, as long as they are

perceived as judgmental and conceited our reaction would be resentment. Also, Monin

(2007) remarks, “even then they don’t agree that the domain of judgment is of moral

relevance, people resent being reproached by others”.

Of course these causes imply some consequences. In this case, we may refer to a

defensive strategy to ease a threatening upward moral comparison. There are in fact 3 main

reactions to such circumstances: suspicion, trivialization and resentment, and we will pick

one or a combination depending on the context.

o Suspicion - denying virtue. As explained by Monin (2007), this approach is based on the

denial of the other person’s righteousness entirely, and gives little respect to said

11

Page 13: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

behavior. In fact, research proves that a common response would be to be suspicious

and skeptical as for the real intentions of the moral actor concern, attributing them to

false and selfish virtues.

o Trivialization - do-gooder derogation. Given that in this approach disregarding goodness

of the actor’s behavior would be too apparent, a solution would actually be to allusively

remove the threat by humiliating moral others on other aspects by suggesting a lack of

capability, treating their moral gesture as unimportant, judging morally driven individuals

as good intentioned but ingenuous, feeble and ignorant with a lack of perspicacity and

no reality consciousness. In the case of vegetarians, Monin & Minson (2007) found that

omnivores indeed “think they are seen as morally inferior by vegetarians, and that the

extent of this anticipated moral reproach correlates with the negative valence of words

associated with vegetarians”, such as being notably less intelligent and moral to a lesser

degree.

o Resentment - disliking and distancing. In the event said morally driven person is

unmistakably moral and benefits from strong arguments, both previous strategies

become practically ineffective. When all else has failed, perhaps because of our

convincement of the other’s morality and potency, we might defer to distancing

ourselves from the moral other while feeling a lack of aspiration to associate with him or

her, who is in fact threatening us with his convictions [SEM model by Tesser, 1991].

However, it’s important to consider Monin’s (2007) conclusion that when being

confronted by a moral person, admiration is the most probable outcome as long as the moral

other does not threaten his or her audience, or gives room to judgments upon their morality.

In the following section we will see in depth how an scenario of a moral person is

perceived by two groups, a more morally driven (BioCultura) and another which may be

considered as neutral (Architecture class).

12

Page 14: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

EMPIRICAL STUDY

MOTIVATIONS AND HYPOTHESIS

Given the lack of abundant research on the field, and especially in that of the organic

food sector, we want to better understand the nature of our perceptions when facing a moral

organic consumer who defends his or her motives. As presented in the last section, there are

different reactions to such a statement, but we want to do research on resentment, that is,

the Likeability of the refuser and how Moral Threat affects it. Thus, profiting from the lack

of abundant work on the matter, a study has been performed to confirm the theory that

behaving morally may result in negative external evaluations. However, we have added an

imperfection factor to assess if the degree of morality is indeed important when evaluating

such person, as it would trigger the moral statement’s power.

Following this, the main hypothesis to be tested is

H1: Compared to participants facing a neutral scenario, those that are faced with

a moral scenario value the moral person more negatively

Additionally and to complement our analysis we also wanted to test the following two:

H2: Relative to participants in a neutral scenario, those in a moral scenario will

evaluate the moral person more negatively when he is not described with the

imperfection than when the imperfection is present.

H3: Participants will evaluate the neutral person more negatively when he is

described with the imperfection than when the imperfection is not present.

DESIGN AND PARTICIPANTS

To better understand its possible precursors, two experiments were conducted in two

different places: (1) BioCultura 2014, an organic food fair held each year in Barcelona among

other cities in Spain, with many conferences as well as exhibitors presenting their products

to both consumers and retailers; and (2) an Architecture university course, called

“Condicionament i Serveis III”, with students of 4th course from Escola Tècnica Superior

d’Arquitectura in Barcelona focused on lightening systems, electroacoustics and

acclimatization. Thus, the nature of both groups is expected to be different.

13

Page 15: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

EXPERIMENT 1: BIOCULTURA BARCELONA 2014

The experiment consisted on asking a total of 100 respondents to answer a Spanish

written three-page survey with 12 questions (see Appendix for more information) while they

were waiting on a line to attend a conference held by Olga Cuevas regarding cholesterol.

Among them, only 97 returned the survey, and only 81 are considered in the analysis

[consisting of 24 men and 57 women] due to unanswered important questions and

inconsistency in the results.

First, participants were asked some demographic questions such as sex, age, education

level, city of residence and job status. Additionally, they were inquired whether they knew

about organic products, because before then we assumed that everybody present in

BioCultura knew of these products. Actually, from our participants 90.24% knew of organic

food and 3.66% did not. We also tested the frequency to consume them as well as the

influence of other people in their decision to consume. A final open question asking to add

some comment was also present.

In the following page, they were introduced to one scenario with two independent

variables: morality and imperfection; and were required to assess the likeability of the other

person using a 7-point Likert scale on 12 items based on Monin et al (2008), that will be

explained in the section “Dependent variables”. Another open question to add comments

was present as well.

In the third and last page, they were asked to draw themselves sitting below a tree. Their

drawing was measured with the aid of a compass, and its aim was to test if participants

facing a moral scenario were more likely to draw bigger drafts compared to those facing a

neutral scenario, and whether the imperfection factor influenced the results as well. Our

prediction was they would compensate their resentment with a bigger drawing of

themselves.

EXPERIMENT 2: ARCHITECTURE COURSE

For this experiment, the same method introduced in Experiment 1 was applied, with the

only difference that participants were students from Architecture in an exam revision class

held in May 2014. A total of 76 students participated in it and only 73 are considered for the

analysis, consisting of 36 men and 37 women.

14

Page 16: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

In this case, there was no assumption on whether they knew of organic food or not, but

the same questions regarding this matter were asked this time as well (for more information

on the survey see Appendix). This time 76.71% knew of organic food and 13.7% did not.

STIMULI

Participants in both experiments were randomly assigned to one out of four scenarios,

each one containing a specific stimulus:

Scenario 1 (moral , with imperfection): “Imagine you are having dinner with some friends and one of them brought a friend of his. The conversation turns out to be quite lively until one of your friends brings out the employment situation of the present days, and mentions the difficulties he is facing in his job position. The invited friend, who is actively seeking for a job, comments that on this day he received a job offer from a

Consumer Panel company, specialized in monitoring the consumer’s behavior of a group of consumers, with multinational companies being their main clients. He commented that he re fused the offer because in case he accepted it he would be betraying his values as a conscious and responsible consumer. He also mentions that

in fact some of the clients from that company were those that financed the contra-legislation to label Genetically Modified Organisms (GMO). He adds that he regularly uses his bike, and finishes off by saying

that today he jumped some traffic lights.”

Scenario 2 (moral , no imperfection): “Imagine you are having dinner with some friends and one of them brought a friend of his. The conversation turns out to be quite lively until one of your friends brings out the employment situation of the present days, and mentions the difficulties he is facing in his job position. The invited friend, who is actively seeking for a job, comments that on this day he received a job offer from a

Consumer Panel company, specialized in monitoring the consumer’s behavior of a group of consumers, with multinational companies being their main clients. He commented that he re fused the offer because in case he accepted it he would be betraying his values as a conscious and responsible consumer. He also mentions that

in fact some of the clients from that company were those that financed the contra-legislation to label Genetically Modified Organisms (GMO). He adds that he regularly uses his bike, and finishes off by saying

that he does not jump any traffic light.”  Scenario 3 (neutral , with imperfection): “Imagine you are having dinner with some friends and one of them

brought a friend of his. The conversation turns out to be quite lively until one of your friends brings out the employment situation of the present days, and mentions the difficulties he is facing in his job position. The invited friend, who is actively seeking for a job, comments that on this day he received a job offer from a

Consumer Panel company, specialized in monitoring the consumer’s behavior of a group of consumers, with multinational companies being their main clients. He commented that he re fused the offer because it was too far away from the place he is currently living in. He adds that he regularly uses his bike, and finishes

off by saying that today he jumped some traffic lights.”  

Scenario 4 (neutral , no imperfection): “Imagine you are having dinner with some friends and one of them brought a friend of his. The conversation turns out to be quite lively until one of your friends brings out the employment situation of the present days, and mentions the difficulties he is facing in his job position. The invited friend, who is actively seeking for a job, comments that on this day he received a job offer from a

Consumer Panel company, specialized in monitoring the consumer’s behavior of a group of consumers, with

15

Page 17: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

multinational companies being their main clients. He commented that he re fused the offer because it was too far away from the place he is currently living in. He adds that he regularly uses his bike, and finishes

off by saying that he does not jump any traffic light.”  

DEPENDENT VARIABLES

Each of the experiments measured the following Dependent Variables (DV)

o Likeability of the refuser, by answering a 7-Likert scale (with 1 being “not at all” and

7 “completely”) regarding twelve factors introduced by Monin et al (2008): smart,

kind, warm, pleasant, generous, reasonable, stupid*, cold*, dishonest*, stingy*,

obnoxious* and to what extend they thought it would be pleasant to cooperate with

the refuser (all items marked with * were reverse coded in the analysis). A reliability

analysis was performed to identify the relevant Likeability items, and afterwards only

those were considered to form an average scale called Likeability.

o Draft of the participant. A compass was used to measure the farthest length of the

drawing of the “person” (i.e. from head to toes, if that was the longest distance) and

by doing a circle with that distance, its diameter was measured using a millimeter-

precise ruler. Using the same method, the tree was measured as well resulting in a

circle of 15.4 cm of diameter. Additionally, a percentage of the drawing compared to

the tree (used as a point of reference) was also calculated, and it will indeed be the

variable considered in the analysis.

CONTROL GROUP

As both experiments wanted to examine whether a person being morally driven was

liked more negatively, we randomly introduced the neutral group of scenarios (3 and 4) in

50% of the surveys to assess if the results were indeed caused because of the manipulations,

and the implications resulting from that. Scenario 4, neutral and no imperfection, was the

control group.

16

Page 18: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

RESULTS

EXPERIMENT 1: BIOCULTURA

Before proceeding to analyze the results, a Factor analysis (A) was conducted to better

explain all Likeability items and how they were correlated. It resulted in 2 factors, but only

the first one is considered as it gave a power of ! = 5.86. All items that were negatively

correlated were reverse coded for our analysis, following the same method as the original

experiment by Monin et al (2008). Note that all results can be found in Appendix 2.

Next, a reliability analysis (B) followed giving Cronbach’s alpha !! = 0.91 and thus

indicated a high reliability of the factor in our analysis.

We later on analyzed independently our 12 Likeability items for each scenario (C). Only

the factors “Smart” (F=3.93), “Generous” (F=5.19), “Reasonable” (F=8.81), “To what

extent do you want to cooperate with the refuser?” (F=2.33), “Stingy” (F=2.37) and

“Dishonest” (F=1.714, although significant later on) were significant for our analysis. In

general, the refuser is perceived more positively when being moral than neutral. Additionally,

the not imperfect moral refuser is seen as smarter, more reasonable, less stingy and less

dishonest than the imperfect moral refuser. Resulting from a Post-hoc analysis (C3), almost

in all cases where the refuser was imperfect and moral were significantly better perceived

than when being imperfect and neutral.

After this, we conducted a Univariate Variance analysis of Likeability for each scenario

(D), resulting in a significant mean difference (F=3.842). We can deduce from the graph

below participants indeed considered more positively a moral refuser and additionally not

being imperfect.

17

Page 19: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Afterwards, by performing a 1-way ANOVA analysis (E) we confirmed the relevance of

Likeability items “Smart” (F=4.37), “Generous” (F=5.42), “Reasonable” (F=8.42),

“Cooperate” (F=1.88, although significant in previous analysis), “Stingy” (F=2.78) and

“Dishonest” (F=2.06).

A mean and ANOVA analysis was conducted (F), indicating the significant (F=3.84)

effect of each scenario on Likeability, in line with the Univariate variance analysis. Given that

we also included a second measure in our survey, the % of the drawing compared to the

reference tree, we wanted to see whether it could be considered in our conclusions.

Unfortunately, although the means indicated a difference between being moral or neutral,

and being imperfect and not, with F=0.23 we couldn’t conclude our hypothesis that a moral

refuser would influence a bigger drawing as a consequence of feeling moral threat.

We also run a Crosstab analysis (G), both separately and with all Likeability items

included considering each scenario. Although Likeability and % of reference tree were not

significant with Pearson Chi-squares of 136.85 and 139.69 respectively, if we analyzed all

items separately only “Reasonable” (35.26) and “Dishonest” (31.47) were significant. The

items “Smart” (26.47), “Kind” (25.05), “Generous” (28.15), “Stupid” (24.40) and “Stingy”

(26.45) were marginally significant.

EXPERIMENT 2: ARCHITECTURE COURSE

As in the first experiment, all analysis procedures were replicated for this experiment.

Thus, a Factor analysis was conducted (H), resulting in 3 factors, but only the first one is

considered as it gave a power of ! = 4.76. All items that were negatively correlated were

reverse coded for our analysis as well, following the same method as the original experiment

by Monin et al (2008). Analogously, all results can be found in Appendix.

Next, a reliability analysis (I) gave Cronbach alpha !! = 0.87 and thus indicated a high

reliability of the factor in our analysis.

When running a multivariate general linear model (J), we analyzed our 12 Likeability

items for each scenario. In this experiment only the factors “Smart” (F=4.97), “Kind”

(F=3.51), “Generous” (F=3.71), “Reasonable” (F=4.41), “Stupid” (F=4.49), “Stingy”

(F=6.58), “Obnoxious” (F=2.06) and “Dishonest” (F=4.48) were significant for our

analysis. As in the previous experiment, in this case the refuser is generally perceived more

18

Page 20: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

positively when being moral than neutral. In items such as “Generous”, “Reasonable”,

“Stupid” and “Obnoxious” our neutral not imperfect refuser is surprisingly better

considered than the rest, although in almost all cases we can appreciate how the moral not

imperfect refuser is seen as smarter, kinder, more generous, more reasonable, less stupid, less

stingy, more obnoxious and less dishonest compared to the moral imperfect refuser. A Post-

hoc analysis (J3) confirmed that the difference between the moral imperfect and the neutral

imperfect refuser is significant for each item, although in most cases we can appreciate a

significant difference between the second scenario and the third.

After this, we conducted a Univariate variance analysis of Likeability for each scenario

(K), resulting in a significant mean difference (F=4.786). As shown in the graph below, we

can deduce participants perceived more positively the not imperfect refuser, but the moral

manipulation was not clearly different from the neutral manipulation.

Afterwards, by performing a 1-way ANOVA analysis (L) we confirmed the relevance of

Likeability items “Smart” (F=5.35), “Kind” (F=2.86), “Generous” (F=3.73), “Reasonable”

(F=4.03), “Stupid” (F=4.02), “Stingy” (F=2.63) and “Dishonest” (F=3.35).

Again, we run a mean and ANOVA analysis (M), resulting in a significant (F=4.79)

effect of each scenario on Likeability. As in the previous experiment, the % of the drawing

with reference to the tree did still not have a significant effect (F=0.29), and results were

confluent between scenarios.

Finally, we performed a Crosstab analysis (N) as in the previous experiment. This time,

Likeability and % of reference tree were still not significant with Pearson Chi-squares of

129.97 and 133.02 respectively, but if we analyzed all items separately only items “Smart”

19

Page 21: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

(28.1), “Stupid” (24.98), “Stingy” (24.95) and “Dishonest” (24.45) were marginally

significant.

DISCUSSION

When we first designed both experiments, we had in mind that a moral refuser would be

regarded less positively than a neutral refuser. Although this was our first hypothesis and the

base of our project, we have seen how in both experiments it is the exact opposite in most

of the Likeability items as well as the Likeability factor by itself. For our second experiment

the results are less clear in some items, but they are concluding nonetheless.

Our predictions were also that supposedly our moral refuser would be perceived more

negatively if he did not have an imperfection rather than if he did. Our results refute our

second hypothesis as well, as we have seen how in our experiments when he does not have

the defect he is generally perceived more positively than if he does. A reason for this would

be a lack of sufficient understanding of the scenario by the participants, although perhaps a

real situation where participants were confronted with the refuser in person could be more

concluding.

Additionally, we also forecasted that a neutral person would be evaluated better if he did

not have an imperfection than if he did. This third hypothesis turned out to be true, and

even though it is not statistically significant in the first experiment, in the second one we may

differentiate it.

All in all, our participants in the first experiment had more positive views of the moral

refuser compared to participants in the second experiment. We expected this, as the morality

of the scenario would be less threating in their case due to similar grounds, but we also

forecasted they would have more extreme perceptions, which was not the case.

However, we could not assess how morally driven the interviewed participants were,

despite the fact that it could have proven beneficial for our conclusions. The comments in

the survey were a way to differentiate them, but it turned out they were not widely used,

even in the BioCultura experiment. Additionally, the questions should be rethought to make

the scenarios clearer to participants, and the control (neutral) group should also be

reconsidered due to the results we got from the groups exposed to the scenario.

20

Page 22: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

CONCLUSIONS

In this project we have seen how adopting a more conscious consumption behavior

could solve a problematic food system and most likely save the planet. That being said, one

of the main constraints or problems when adopting such behavior is the fact that individuals

or groups are going against the flow, facing many obstacles on the way and many people

contrary to their beliefs. Thus, how people perceive them, especially when they defend their

ideals against arguments from the others, is specially a delicate matter. If we analyze it from

an external view in the event moral threat is present, that is, when we may feel the other is

more moral than us and we take it negatively, we may respond in three different ways: we

may (1) feel morally inferior to said person, influencing defense mechanisms which could

either be suspicion, trivialization or disliking and distancing; (2) be confused with our own

behavior, and surprised as well for finding that our morality was not as clear as we though;

or (3) feel they are judging our own morality and thus we feel dissatisfied (and it may not

only be for morality topics, but in general).

When trying to apply it in a practical experiment of a job offer moral refuser compared

to a neutral refuser, we saw that in general those facing a moral refuser tend to perceive

more positively the refuser compared to neutral ones, although we first predicted it would be

the other way around. We introduced the imperfection manipulation as well, as we tried to

define if an imperfect moral refuser would be better perceived and less extreme. However,

those that were presented with a moral and not imperfect refuser valued him more positively

than if he was moral but imperfect. Additionally, a neutral refuser was valued more positively

when he was not imperfect, in line with our predictions.

For further research on the art, we would suggest to run an improved experiment in a

more real situation where manipulations are more accurate with the results we are trying to

look for. The lack of a manipulation check before running the experiments could be one of

the answers of our results being so surprising. Additionally, we could study if age influences

our perceptions of the moral refuser, and if perhaps sex is also indicative in a sense given

that in our study we did not have a sufficient sample size for each of the sexes when

considering them separately in each scenario. The fact that our moral individual was a man

instead of sex undetermined person could also change to see if it affected the results.

21

Page 23: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

AKNOWLEDGMENTS

I would like to give a special thank you to all the people that accompanied me during this

project, an idea that started four years ago.

To Estève Giraud, for guiding me not only in this project but also in life. To Gert

Cornelissen, because apart from gladly accepting to tutor this work and providing many

useful ideas on how to focus it he also taught extremely useful knowledge during his classes

in Consumer Behavior. To Maria Galli, my Market Research II professor during the current

term, for giving her support when designing and analyzing the experiments, and her

monumental patience and dedication. To Carola Camañés, my godmother, and to Ruth

Muñoz, my Traditional Chinese Medicine doctor, who have given me proof of the benefits

of an organic food diet as well as life changing counsel. To Veritas, for their monthly

newsletter as an introductory source of knowledge and for being my first reference during

my transition to organic products in 2009. To BioSpace, OBBIO, L’Hort d’en Climent, and

Ecobotiga.cat for being my current ones. To all the initiatives I support that encouraged this

project: my cousin Paul Domènech’s land conversion to an organic farm, Etselquemenges’s

blog, club and projects, Club de Probadores Bio by TVBio, BioCultura, BioFach… who

make organic food more accessible and known, as well as all the institutions, CCPAE and

EU among them, ensuring organic food to be possible. To holaBIO!, for offering me the

opportunity to have an inside view of the sector during my internship. To all the people who

contributed in the experiments study, giving this project a consistent conclusion. To

everyone who agreed to let me run both experiments, including Vida Sana (for the first

experiment in BioCultura) and my father (for the second experiment in the Architecture

class). To Thaïs Morillas, for suggesting a more appropriate way of measuring the drawings,

given the variety of results. To Pompeu Fabra University and Inholland Hogeschool, for

their excellent information resources databases.

And last but not least, to my family and friends for being there when most needed, and

because if not for them this project would not have been possible.

22

Page 24: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

NOTES AND REFERENCES

NOTES

1.  Ets el que menges, Qué hago con mi cesta de verduras. Available at: http://www.etselquemenges.cat/blogs/que-hago-con-mi-cesta-22869 [Accessed April 20, 2014aa]. 2. Esther Vivas, Curs a Barcelona: Del camp a la taula. Qui decideix què mengem? | More info at: http://esthervivas.com/intern/curs-a-barcelona-del-camp-a-la-taula-qui-decideix-que-mengem/ [Accessed February 20, 2014d]. 3. Battle EK; Brownell KD, 1996. Confronting a rising tide of eating disorders and obesity: treatment vs. prevention and policy. Addict Behav., (21), pp.755–65. Brownell, K. & Horgen, K.B., 2003. Food Fight, Available at: http://books.google.es/books/about/Food_Fight.html?id=Zs4QWm9kaosC&pgis=1 [Accessed May 1, 2014]. 4. WEP, United Nations World Food Programme - Fighting Hunger Worldwide. Available at: http://www.wfp.org/ [Accessed April 28, 2014ag]. 5. Nierenberg, D. & Massey, A., In a world of abundance, food waste is a crime - USATODAY.com. Available at: http://usatoday30.usatoday.com/news/opinion/forum/2010-06-16-column16_ST1_N.htm [Accessed April 28, 2014q]. 6. FAO, 2013. Food wastage footprint. Impacts on natural resources 7. For more information, visit: Hazell, P.B.R., 2003. Green Revolution: Curse or Blessing  ? The Oxford Enclyclopedia of Economic History, pp.1–4. Available at: http://www.ifpri.org/sites/default/files/pubs/pubs/ib/ib11.pdf. Pingali, P.L., 2012. Green revolution: impacts, limits, and the path ahead. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(31), pp.12302–8. Available at: http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?artid=3411969&tool=pmcentrez&rendertype=abstract [Accessed May 27, 2014]. 8. Anon, “Nobel of agriculture” goes to Monsanto executive — RT USA. Available at: http://rt.com/usa/world-food-prize-monsanto-executive-971/ [Accessed April 11, 2014v]. 9. Anon, Special Report: Are regulators dropping the ball on biocrops? | Reuters. Available at: http://www.reuters.com/article/2010/04/13/us-usa-gmos-regulators-idUSTRE63C2AJ20100413 [Accessed April 28, 2014ad]. 10. Gillam, C., Bees crucial to many crops still dying at worrisome rate: USDA | Reuters. Available at: http://www.reuters.com/article/2014/05/15/us-usda-honeybees-report-idUSKBN0DV12120140515 [Accessed May 20, 2014b]. 11. Anon, India’s farmer suicides: are deaths linked to GM cotton? – in pictures | Global development | theguardian.com. Available at: http://www.theguardian.com/global-development/gallery/2014/may/05/india-cotton-suicides-farmer-deaths-gm-seeds [Accessed May 05, 2014r]. 12. Lupkin, S., The Health Effects of GMO Foods - ABC News. Available at: http://abcnews.go.com/Health/health-effects-gmo-foods/story?id=23459884 [Accessed April 28, 2014ae]. 13. Gillam, C., Heavy use of herbicide Roundup linked to health dangers-U.S. study | Reuters. Available at: http://www.reuters.com/article/2013/04/25/roundup-health-study-idUSL2N0DC22F20130425 [Accessed April 28, 2014p]. 14. Vidal, J., GM foods: a “biotech revolution”? | Environment | theguardian.com. Available at: http://www.theguardian.com/environment/2011/oct/19/gm-foods-a-biotech-revolution [Accessed April 28, 2014m]. 15. Some of the studies include: - Aris A., Leblanc S., Maternal and fetal exposure to pesticides associated to genetically modified foods in Eastern Townships of Quebec, Canada, Reproductive Toxicology, Volume 31, Issue 4, May 2011, Pages 528-533. - Spisák, S. et al., 2013. Complete genes may pass from food to human blood. A. Dewan, ed. PloS one, 8(7), p.e69805. Available at: http://dx.plos.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069805 [Accessed June 3, 2014]. - IRT, Institute for Responsible Technology - Are Genetically Modified Foods a Gut-Wrenching Combination? Available at: http://responsibletechnology.org/glutenintroduction [Accessed April 28, 2014s]. - Séralini, G.-E. et al., 2012. Long term toxicity of a Roundup herbicide and a Roundup-tolerant genetically modified maize. Food and chemical toxicology  : an international journal published for the British Industrial Biological Research Association, 50(11), pp.4221–31. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0278691512005637 [Accessed May 23, 2014]. - Anon, Detection of Glyphosate Residues in Animals and Humans. Available at: http://omicsonline.org/open-access/detection-of-glyphosate-residues-in-animals-and-humans-2161-0525.1000210.pdf [Accessed April 28, 2014e]. - Schubert, D., 2002. A different perspective on GM food. Nature Biotechnology, 20(10), pp.969–969. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nbt1002-969 [Accessed June 7, 2014]. 16. Burke, M., Don’t worry, it's organic. Available at: http://www.rsc.org/chemistryworld/Issues/2004/June/organic.asp [Accessed April 20, 2014f]. 17. European Commission, Organic Legislation. Available at: http://ec.europa.eu/agriculture/organic/eu-policy/eu-legislation/brief-overview/index_en.htm [Accessed April 20, 2014y].

23

Page 25: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

18. Figure 2 at http://ec.europa.eu/agriculture/organic/downloads/logo/index_en.htm [Accessed April 20, 2014i]. 19. Figure 3 at http://www.ccpae.org/index.php?option=com_content&task=view&id=35&Itemid=209&lang=en [Accessed April 20, 2014h]. 20. Figure 4 at http://www.usda.gov/wps/portal/usda/usdahome?navid=organic-agriculture [Accessed April 20, 2014x] 21. Figure 5 at http://www.oekolandbau.de/bio-siegel/ [Accessed April 20, 2014w]. 22. Figure 6 at http://www.maff.go.jp/e/jas/specific/organic.html [Accessed April 20, 2014ah]. 23. Figure 7 at http://www.agencebio.org/la-marque-ab [Accessed April 20, 2014t]. 24. Figure 8 at http://www.fairtrade.net/ [Accessed April 20, 2014j]. 25. Figure 9 at http://www.demeter.net/certification [Accessed April 20, 2014c]. 26. Figure 10 at http://www.ecocert.com/en/certification [Accessed April 20, 2014g]. 27. Smith, S. & Paladino, A., 2010. Eating clean and green? Investigating consumer motivations towards the purchase of organic food. Australasian Marketing Journal (AMJ), 18(2), pp.93–104. Available at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S144135821000011X [Accessed February 10, 2014]. 28. Sirieix, L., Kledal, P.R. & Sulitang, T., 2011. Organic food consumers’ trade-offs between local or imported, conventional or organic products: a qualitative study in Shanghai. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 35(6), pp.670–678. Available at: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1470-6431.2010.00960.x [Accessed February 8, 2014]. 29. Using: - Doris, J. & Stich, S., 2006. Moral Psychology: Empirical Approaches. Available at: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/moral-psych-emp/#IntWhaMorPsy [Accessed June 1, 2014]. - Gert, B., 2002. The Definition of Morality. Available at: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/morality-definition/ [Accessed June 1, 2014].

REFERENCES

Alicke, M., 2000. Evaluating Social Comparison Targets. In Handbook of Social Comparison. pp. 271–293. Beach, S.H. & Tesser, A., 2000. Self-Evaluation Maintenance and Evolution. In J. Suls & L. Wheeler, eds.

Handbook of Social Comparison SE - 7. The Springer Series in Social Clinical Psychology. Springer US, pp. 123–140. Available at: http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4615-4237-7_7.

Cramwinckel, F.M. et al., 2013a. The threat of moral refusers for one’s self-concept and the protective function of physical cleansing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(6), pp.1049–1058. Available at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S002210311300142X [Accessed April 4, 2014].

Cramwinckel, F.M. et al., 2013b. The threat of moral refusers for one’s self-concept and the protective function of physical cleansing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, pp.1049–1058.

Festinger, L., 1954. A Theory of Social Comparison Processes. Human Relations, 7, pp.117–140. Greene, J. & Haidt, J., 2002. How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, pp.517–

523. Haidt, J., Rosenberg, E. & Holly, H., 2003. Differentiating diversities: Moral diversity is not like other kinds.

Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33, pp.1–36. Han, H. et al., 2011. Are lodging customers ready to go green? An examination of attitudes, demographics, and

eco-friendly intentions. International Journal of Hospitality Management, 30(2), pp.345–355. Available at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0278431910000903 [Accessed February 25, 2014].

Ibarra, P.R., Kitsuse, J.I. & Best, J., 1993. Vernacular Constituents of Moral Discourse: An Interactionist Proposal for the Study of Social Problems. In Constructionist Controversies. Issues in Social Problems Theory. pp. 21–54.

Keltner, D. & Haidt, J., 2003. Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition & Emotion, 17, pp.297–314.

Kelley, H.H., 1971. Moral evaluation. American Psychologist, 26, pp.293–300. Kreps, T.A. & Monin, B., 2011. “Doing well by doing good”? Ambivalent moral framing in organizations.

Research in Organizational Behavior, 31, pp.99–123. Lund, T.B., Andersen, L.M. & O’Doherty Jensen, K., 2013. The Emergence of Diverse Organic Consumers:

Does a Mature Market Undermine the Search for Alternative Products? Sociologia Ruralis, 53(4), p.n/a–n/a. Available at: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/soru.12022 [Accessed March 2, 2014].

Magaña-Gómez, J.A. & de la Barca, A.M.C., 2009. Risk assessment of genetically modified crops for nutrition and health. Nutrition reviews, 67(1), pp.1–16. Available at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19146501 [Accessed June 11, 2014].

Major, B., Testa, M. & Blysma, W.H., 1991. Responses to upward and downward social comparisons: The impact of esteem-relevance and perceived control. In Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research. pp. 237–260).

24

Page 26: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Mettee, D.R. & Riskind, J., 1974. Size of defeat and liking for superior and similar ability competitors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 10(4), pp.333–351. Available at: http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/0022103174900304 [Accessed June 11, 2014].

Minson, J.A. & Monin, B., 2012. Do-Gooder Derogation: Disparaging Morally Motivated Minorities to Defuse Anticipated Reproach. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3, pp.200–207.

Monin, B., 2007. Holier than me? Threatening social comparison in the moral domain. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 20(1), pp.53–68.

Monin, B., Sawyer, P.J. & Marquez, M.J., 2008. The rejection of moral rebels: resenting those who do the right thing. Journal of personality and social psychology, 95, pp.76–93.

Monin, B., Pizarro, D., & Beer, J., 2007. Deciding vs. reacting: Conceptions of moral judgment and the reason-affect debate. Review of General Psychology.

Monin, B., Sawyer, P., & Marquez, M., 2007. Resenting those who do the right thing: Moderators of liking for moral rebels. Unpublished manuscript.

Mussweiler, T., 2003. Comparison processes in social judgment: Mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review, 110, pp.472–488.

Mussweiler, T., Rüter, K. & Epstude, K., 2004. The ups and downs of social comparison: mechanisms of assimilation and contrast. Journal of personality and social psychology, 87, pp.832–844.

Rozuel, C., 2011. The Moral Threat of Compartmentalization: Self, Roles and Responsibility. Journal of Business Ethics, 102(4), pp.685–697. Available at: http://link.springer.com/10.1007/s10551-011-0839-4 [Accessed June 11, 2014].

Ruby, M.B., 2012. Vegetarianism. A blossoming field of study. Appetite, 58, pp.141–150. Ruby, M.B. & Heine, S.J., 2011. Meat, morals, and masculinity. Appetite, 56, pp.447–450. Sabini, J. & Silver, M., 1982. Moralities of everyday life, Oxford University Press. Available at:

http://books.google.ca/books?id=7Ua7AAAAIAAJ. Salovey, P., 1991. Social comparison processes in envy and jealousy. In Social comparison: Contemporary theory and

research. pp. 261–285. Siderer, Y., Maquet, A. & Anklam, E., 2005. Need for research to support consumer confidence in the growing

organic food market. Trends in Food Science & Technology, 16(8), pp.332–343. Available at: http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0924224405000361 [Accessed February 25, 2014].

Smith, R.H., 2000. Reactions to upward and downward social comparisons. Handbook of Social Comparison: Theory and Research, pp.173–201.

Tesser, A., 1991. Emotion in social comparison and reflection processes. In Social comparison contemporary theory and research. pp. 115–145.

Tung, S.-J. et al., 2012. Attitudinal inconsistency toward organic food in relation to purchasing intention and behavior: An illustration of Taiwan consumers. British Food Journal, 114(7), pp.997–1015. Available at: http://www.emeraldinsight.com/10.1108/00070701211241581 [Accessed February 14, 2014].

Wheeler, L., 1991. A brief history of social comparison theory. In Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research. pp. 3–21.

Wood, J. V., 1989. Theory and research concerning social comparisons of personal attributes. Psychological Bulletin, 106, pp.231–248.

Zepeda, L. & Deal, D., 2009. Organic and local food consumer behaviour: Alphabet Theory. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 33(6), pp.697–705. Available at: http://doi.wiley.com/10.1111/j.1470-6431.2009.00814.x [Accessed February 22, 2014].

25

Page 27: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

APPENDICES

Appendix 1. Survey design

26

Page 28: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Scenario 1 (Moral, Imperfect)

27

Page 29: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Scenario 2 (Moral, not Imperfect)

28

Page 30: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Scenario 3 (Neutral, Imperfect)

29

Page 31: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Scenario 4 (Neutral, not Imperfect)

30

Page 32: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Drawing of oneself with a reference of a tree

31

Page 33: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Appendix 2. SPSS Analysis

Part 1: Experiment 1 – BioCultura

A. Factor Analysis

B. Reliability Analysis

32

Page 34: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

C. General linear model (Multivariate)

C1. Gender analysis in each scenario

C2. Do you know about organic food?

Moral 1, Defect 1

Moral 1, Defect 0

Moral 0, Defect 1

Moral 0, Defect 0 Total

Woman 12 16 14 15 57

Man 5 6 7 6 24

SCENARIO

Gender

33

Page 35: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

C3. Post-hoc analysis (continues in next page)

34

Page 36: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

 

C4. Likeability items depending on scenario (statistically significant items only)

35

Page 37: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

D. Univariate Variance Analysis

 

 

E. ANOVA Analysis

36

Page 38: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

F. Means Analysis

 

G. Crosstab Analysis (Chi-square) – All items  

Smart

Kind

Generous

Reasonable

Stupid

Stingy

Dishonest

37

Page 39: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

Part 2: Experiment 2 – Architecture Class

H. Factor Analysis

 

 

I. Reliability Analysis

 

38

Page 40: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

J. General linear model (Multivariate)

 

J1. Gender analysis in each scenario

J2. Do you know about organic food?

Moral 1, Defect 1

Moral 1, Defect 0

Moral 0, Defect 1

Moral 0, Defect 0 Total

Woman 7 10 12 8 37

Man 12 7 7 10 36

SCENARIO

Gender

39

Page 41: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

J3. Post-hoc analysis (continues in the next page)

40

Page 42: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

 

J4. Likeability items depending on scenario (statistically significant items only)

41

Page 43: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

 

K. Univariate Variance Analysis

 

42

Page 44: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

L. ANOVA Analysis

 

M. Means Analysis

43

Page 45: ORGANIC FOOD CONSUMERS

N. Crosstab Analysis (Chi-Square) – All items

Smart

Stupid

Stingy

Dishonest

44