nonfamily living and the erosion of traditional family orientations among young adults

Upload: najmul-puda-pappadam

Post on 06-Jul-2018

218 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    1/15

     Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young

    Adults

    Author(s): Linda J. Waite, Frances Kobrin Goldscheider and Christina Witsberger

    Source: American Sociological Review , Vol. 51, No. 4 (Aug., 1986), pp. 541-554Published by: American Sociological Association

    Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2095586

    Accessed: 15-04-2016 17:54 UTC

     

    Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at

    http://about.jstor.org/terms

     

    JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted

    digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about

    JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

    Sage Publications, Inc., American Sociological Association are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize,preserve and extend access to American Sociological Review 

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    2/15

     NONFAMILY LIVING AND THE EROSION OF

     TRADITIONAL FAMILY ORIENTATIONS

     AMONG YOUNG ADULTS*

     LINDA J. WAITE FRANCES KOBRIN GOLDSCHEIDER

     The Rand Corporation Brown University

     CHRISTINA WITSBERGER

     The Rand Corporation

     Young adults in recent cohorts have been leaving the parental home earlier and marrying

     later now than they did several decades ago, resulting in an increased period of independent

     living. This paper explores the consequences of time spent in non-family living, using data

     from the National Longitudinal Surveys of Young Men and Young Women. We expect that

     experience in living away from home prior to marriage will cause young adults to change

     their attitudes, values, plans, and expectations, and move them away from a traditional

     family orientation. We find strong support for this hypothesis for young women; those who

     lived independently became more likely to plan for employment, lowered their expected

     family size, became more accepting of employment of mothers, and more non-traditional on

     sex roles in the family than those who lived with their parents. Non-family living had much

     weaker effects on young men in the few tests that we could perform for them. The paper also

     addresses the conditions under which living away increases individualism, and it discusses

     the implications of these findings.

     Beginning with the 1950s, the living arrangements

     of American adults have been undergoing major

     changes. More and more unmarried adults are

     living apart from their immediate families, forming

     non-family households' at a rate that far exceeds

     the growth in the adult population. During the

     1970s, non-family households increased 73 percent

     while family households increased only 13 percent

     (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 1980). This develop-

     ment can be seen as a further extension of the

     changes accompanying modernization that have

     resulted in a decline in the centrality of the family.

     Many have argued that this process may have

     accelerated over the past several decades, as

     indicated by a series of demographic changes,

     including rising ages at marriage, falling propor-

     tions of the population ever marrying, declining

     fertility and particularly marital fertility, the

     sizeable proportion of marriages expected to end in

     divorce, and the large numbers of children living

     with only one parent for at least part of their

     childhood (Westoff, 1978; Davis, 1982; Espenshade,

     1985).

     Wth these changes have come substantial

     changes in attitudes toward family life. For

     example, recent surveys show increasing accep-

     tance of divorce, permanent singleness and child-

     lessness (Thornton and Freedman, 1982). Changes

     in attitudes are normally explained by changes in

     the social structure. However, it is also possible

     that these effects are reinforced for individuals by

     their own experiences, causing further shifts in

     their family-related attitudes and behavior. Life

     course theory predicts that this would be particu-

     larly likely for experiences early in life-in

     childhood or early adulthood. Other things equal,

     parental divorce increases the likelihood of divorce

     for children (Pope and Mueller, 1976), teenage

     parenthood is more common among daughters of

     teenage mothers (Baldwin and Cain, 1980),

     number of siblings positively affects own actual

     and expected family size (Terhune, 1974; Waite and

     Stolzenberg, 1976) and children raised in non-

     intact families are less likely to marry at all but

     very young ages (Kobrin and Waite, 1984).

     In this context, perhaps the most critical

     characteristic of the rapid increase in non-family

     households is its concentration among very young

     adults. While non-family households increased

     their share of U. S. households from 15 to 22

     percent between 1960 and 1975 over all ages, the

     comparable increase for households with heads age

     25 or less was from 13 to 30 percent (Frey and

     Kobrin, 1982). Young adults are leaving the

     parental home substantially earlier now than they

     did in the past. In Rhode Island, for example, the

     American Sociological Review, 1986, Vol. 51 (August:541-554) 541

     * Please address all correspondence to Linda J. Waite,

     Senior Sociologist, The Rand Corporation, 1700 Main

     Street, P.O. Box 2138, Santa Monica, CA 90406-2138.

     The research reported here was supported by Contract

     No. NOI-HD-12814 from the Center for Population

     Research, National Institute of Child Health and Human

     Development. We would like to thank Lee Lillard, Julie

     DaVanzo, and the members of the Rand Population

     Research Center Workshop for helpful comments.

     l Non-family households consist of an individual

     living alone or with unrelated persons.

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    3/15

     542 AMERCANSOCOLOGCAL REVEW

     percentage of sons still at home at age 26 dropped

     from 50 percent for those who had reached that age

     between 1930 and 1950, to 20 percent for those

     reaching that age between 1976 and 1979. The

     comparable decline for daughters was from 37 to

     13 percent (Goldscheider and LeBourdais, 1986).

     When the trend toward earlier nestleaving

     began, it was associated with declining marriage

     age. However, it now is increasingly combined

     with delays in marriage, with the result that more

     and more young people are spending an important

     part of early adulthood in a context in which family

     roles may be much less salient, and are developing

     tastes and skills that are likely to reduce their

     orientation to family roles. The transition to

     adulthood for recent cohorts is following a

     distinctly different pattern from that of their

     parents, and could well have a substantial

     influence on their later lives. We have already

     documented that the experience of non-family

     living leads to a postponement of marriage, at least

     for women (Goldscheider and Waite, 1985). But

     does it have an influence on the kinds of marriages

     eventually formed through its effects on expecta-

     tions about the balance of work and family roles? It

     is these influences that we wish to explore.

     BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESES

     The centrality of family formation in all societies

     results in well-defined norms, rooted in an

     underlying sexual division of labor, norms that

     traditionally lead women to orient themselves more

     toward family roles than do men. In the United

     States the proper ages at which to marry and bear a

     first child are normatively well defined, and clearly

     differ for men and women, allowing men more

     time to acquire adult levels of independence and

     autonomy (Ryder and Westoff, 1971; Rindfuss and

     Bumpass, 1978; Modell, 1979). Scanzoni (1975)

     found that marriage, work, and fertility are

     strongly associated with sex-role norms: women

     who defined themselves through traditional female

     roles married earlier, were less likely to be

     employed full-time or to use effective contracep-

     tion, and expected to bear more children than

     women with a more non-familial orientation. He

     sees sex-role modernity primarily as an emphasis

     on individualism (although he makes this equation

     reluctantly), and summarizes his results by conclud-

     ing that The greater the individualism, the less

     the familism (p. 187).

     Family-related norms clearly affect the behavior

     of individuals; they may also respond to that

     behavior, as Marini (1984) argues. In either case,

     it is important to establish how such attitudes are

     formed and altered by experience. Although some

     researchers postulate that family-related norms are

     laid down early in life (Blake, 1972; Gustavus and

     Nam, 1970; Hoffman and Wyatt, 1960), other

     researchers have emphasized how experiences

     outside the family can induce changes in these

     family-derived values and tastes. Mason (1974)

     has argued that a role hiatus between the

     traditional role of daughter and the traditional roles

     of wife and mother gives young women an

     opportunity to develop tastes for roles alternative

     to motherhood-especially employment-that as a

     result alter family formation. Rubin (1976) cites

     impressionistic evidence that the rigidity of sex

     roles in working-class families may result from

     early marriage and men's lack of experience in

     living independently of women-either mother or

     wife-whose role it was to care for their physical

     needs. This suggests that a role hiatus may also

     affect young men's sex-role traditionality.

     Spitze (1978) tested the role hiatus hypothesis

     with longitudinal data, and found that those

     women who attended college increased their

     preference for employment, whereas those who

     worked increased their preference for full-time

     work in the home. However, a great many who

     work or attend college after high school remain in

     the parental home (Goldscheider and DaVanzo,

     1985). Non-family living prior to marriage may

     provide a more complete hiatus, and lead to

     changes in the family-related attitudes, plans and

     expectations of young men and women.

     We test this proposition by examining young

     women's plans for employment over the long run,

     their total expected family size, the number of

     children that they feel is an ideal family, men's and

     women's attitudes toward the employment of

     mothers under various conditions, and two dimen-

     sions of global sex-role attitudes. Together these

     measures reflect many aspects of young adults'

     orientation to family versus individual concerns. In

     these analyses, we hold constant their earlier plans'

     and attitudes, measured-to the extent possible-

     prior to any exposure to non-family living.

     However, we show as well that omitting such

     controls does not bias the results observed, since

     the likelihood of non-family living in early

     adulthood is not strongly related to underlying

     work and family attitudes.

     Specifically, we test the following propositions:

     -The experience of non-family living during

     early adulthood affects the attitudes, values,

     and plans of young adults, compared with the

     levels measured before the experience, and

     moves them away from a traditional family

     orientation.

     -Living away from home has stronger effects

     on personal plans and expectations than on

     more global ideals, although the latter may be

     affected as well, presaging normative change.

     -Non-family living will have stronger effects

     on the attitudes, expectations, and plans of

     women than of men, since more of a role

     hiatus is expected for men, even under

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    4/15

     ONAMLYLVNG543

     traditional sex-role regimes, but non-family

     living may affect men's views as well.

     -Nonfamily living interacts with education past

     high school to alter attitudes, expectations and

     plans. Both attending college and living away

     from parents influence young adults, but those

     who go away to college change their views

     more than those who go to college while

     living at home.

     -Those with more family-oriented attitudes are

     not less likely to live away from home during

     early adulthood than others, so that effects of

     non-family living will not change greatly with

     the addition or omission of controls for

     attitudes, expectations or plans at time one

     (prior to the risk period for nonfamily

     living).2

     DATA AND METHODS

     Data for this analysis come from the National

     Longitudinal Surveys of Young Women and

     Young Men. Conducted by the Ohio State

     University Center for Human Resource Research,

     these surveys include information over a recent

     15-year period on more than 10,000 young men

     and women. Personal interviews were conducted

     with national probability samples of the non-

     institutionalized population of females age 14 to 24

     in 1968, and males age 14 to 24 in 1966. Those

     included responded to lengthy interviews in many

     of the succeeding years through the early 1980s.3

     Attrition from the sample over the panel period has

     been relatively low; three-fourths of the original

     Young Women's sample was re-interviewed in

     1978, and seventy percent of the original Young

     Men's sample was re-interviewed in 1976. These

     are the last years used here.

     The analysis reported in this paper uses as

     control variables measures of stable respondent

     characteristics, such as race and characteristics of

     the parental family, taken at the initial survey

     (1966 for the Young Men, 1968 for the Young

     Women). Independent variables measuring the

     respondent's current situation-for example, em-

     ployment, education or marital status-were mea-

     sured contemporaneously with the dependent

     variable. Most models incorporate two measures of

     the relevant attitudes, plans, or expectations: one

     measured at the beginning of the period and one

     measured later and used as the dependent variable.

     Our sample is restricted to those age 14 to 17 in the

     first year of the survey to allow us to observe and

     measure a complete history of non-family living

     prior to marriage.4 Thus, we have data on Young

     Women from 1968 through 1978, and on Young

     Men from 1966 through 1976; each group was 24

     to 27 years old at the last interview analyzed here.

     The outcome measures include a wide range of

     young adults' family- and sex-role-related atti-

     tudes, plans, and expectations. First, young

     women were asked in each survey year about their

     plans to work at age 35, as an indicator of long-run

     plans for employment. We divided this measure

     into (a) those who planned to work and (b) those

     who planned not to or were undecided.5 Second,

     we include two measures of fertility asked in 1971,

     1973, and 1978: young women's expected family

     size and the number of children that they think is

     ideal for a family. Expected family size was

     measured from questions on children already born

     and additional children expected in the future.

     Taken together, these questions cover the primary

     roles-mother and worker-that young women

     must balance during their adult lives.

     Third, we examine three measures of respondents'

     views about the proper ways that mothers in

     general should balance work and family roles;

     these are all scales. One of these, called attitude

     toward mothers' working, consists of the sum of

     responses to three questions (coded as five-point

     Likert scales) about the conditions under which the

     respondent thinks that married mothers of pre-

     school children may work, given that a trusted

     relative is available for child care: (1) if she needs

     the money; (2) if she wants to and her husband

     agrees; and, (3) if she wants to but her husband

     doesn't particularly like it (see Appendix). The

     points from these three items were summed into a

     single scale which ranges from 3 (traditional) to 15

     (liberal).6 Young Women answered these questions

     2 If early attitudes and non-family living are not

     strongly related, then eliminating early attitudes from the

     equation for later attitudes does not introduce specifica-

     tion bias; hence, where early and later attitudes are

     strongly related, lack of change in the coefficient for

     non-family living when early attitudes are added or

     dropped indicates that young adults' likelihood of living

     away from home does not vary with their early attitudes.

     3 The NLS Young Women were interviewed annually

     from 1968 through 1973, again in 1975, 1977, 1978, the

     Young Men annually from 1966 through 1971, and again

     in 1973, 1975, 1976, the last years that we use in the

     analysis reported here.

     'Preliminary examination of the available data

     showed that none of the young women, and only a few

     young men, lived away from home before age 18. Since

     the initial survey did not ask about prior years' living

     arrangements, we could not know if those older than age

     17 at the first survey had already experienced some years

     of non-family living.

     5 Specifically, the young women were asked what kind

     of work they would like to be doing when they are 35.

     Coded responses were: a) married, keeping home, raising

     a family; b) same as present job; c) don't know; and d) an

     occupation other than their current one.

     6 For each of the three questions, five points were

     assigned the response definitely all right, and one

     point to definitely not all right. The mid-point

     response was undecided. Although these three items

     comprise a formal Guttman scale, we report results for a

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    5/15

     544 AMERCANSOCOLOGCAL REVEW

     in 1968, 1972, and 1978; the Young Men did so in

     1971 and 1976.

     Finally, two more general indices of sex-role

     attitudes were included in the 1972 and 1978

     Young Women's interviews (see Appendix). These

     were Likert-type questions, also coded as five-

     point scales. The questions overlap to a substantial

     extent in the two years but are not identical. They

     deal with the impact of women's employment on

     their families, and include value judgements about

     appropriate roles for men and women. The

     variables used in our analyses are the composite

     indices (or factor scores) derived from the

     coefficient matrix of a factor analysis done with

     oblique rotation. Our factor analysis of each of the

     series produced two distinct dimensions. One,

     which we called sex-role attitudes (Family),

     reflects the importance of women's time at home to

     their children and families. The second dimension,

     sex-role attitudes (Jobs), measures views of the

     importance of women's employment to their

     self-esteem and to the economic well-being of their

     families. A higher score indicates greater accep-

     tance of non-familial roles for wives and mothers.

     Finding two distinct dimensions of sex-role

     attitudes in this series of questions replicates

     results from a number of other studies. Mason and

     Bumpass (1975) and Mason et al. (1976) found,

     using questions overlapping to some extent with

     those that we use, that respondents answered

     relatively consistently all questions on the appro-

     priateness of the traditional division of labor within

     the family, and those on rights of the sexes in the

     labor market, but apparently saw little need to

     connect their views across these two dimensions.

     Our key indicator of non-family living is the

     proportion of the years that the respondent lived

     outside the parental family between age 17 and

     first marriage or the terminal date for the analysis,

     whichever occurs first.7 Non-family living could

     include years spent in a college dormitory, military

     barracks, living with roommates of either sex and

     living alone. This measure uses the proportion of

     potential years of non-family living actually away

     from home, rather than the number of years, to

     control for the differences between individuals in

     the potential number of years. Since all respon-

     dents in our sample were living at home and

     unmarried as of the first interview, only those who

     married at age 18 had an opportunity to live away

     from home prior to marriage.

     One disadvantage of the use of proportion rather

     than number of years is that the smaller the number

     of potential years of non-family living, the easier it

     is to have spent either all or none of it actually

     away from home. If we assume that individuals

     have an underlying propensity to live away from

     home, then we have fewer observations on this

     propensity for individuals with a small number of

     potential years away. This gives us inefficient but

     unbiased estimates of these underlying propensities

     for these individuals.

     The use of proportion rather than number of

     years away implies that the first year of non-family

     living has a larger effect than subsequent years,

     with the effect of each year declining at a rate of

     (years - 1)/years, a slow to moderate decay

     function. This function makes sense theoretically,

     as we expect that the new experiences a young

     adult undergoes by living away, as well as the new

     skills acquired, are largest in the first year, with

     additional skills and experiences added at a

     declining rate with more years of non-family

     living.

     One could argue, alternatively, that family living

     should include both time living with parents and

     time living with a spouse. Just as young people

     may decide to leave the family to live away, so

     they may decide to form a new family of their

     own. This argument implies a specification of

     proportion of potential years of non-family living

     spent actually away, that includes in the denomi-

     nator all the years from age 17 until the measure of

     the dependent variable, including years after

     marriage, since the person could have lived away

     those years if he or she had not married. We

     created such a measure and include a discussion of

     it in our results.

     We measure living arrangements for respondents

     using the household listing available in each survey

     year. This gives the relationship to the respondent

     of all individuals living in the household, their age,

     sex, and certain other characteristics. Thus, we can

     summated rating scale of the items, as described above,

     to preserve maximum information about the respondents'

     views toward employment of mothers. However, we

     repeated all analyses after coding the items into a

     Guttman scale. We created this scale with two alternative

     coding schemes, either including or eliminating the

     undecided category as a pass. Both scales performed

     comparably. This Guttman scale has a coefficient of

     reproducibility ranging from 0.97 to 0.99 for the Young

     Women in 1968, 1972, and 1978, and a coefficient of

     scalability varying between 0.81 to 0.94 for the same

     years. For the Young Men, the Guttman scale on the

     same items has a coefficient of reproducibility of 0.97 in

     both years, and a coefficient of scalability of 0.85 and

     0.87 in 1971 and 1976, respectively. Thus, the scales

     appear to have identical-and excellent-measurement

     properties for Boys and Girls.

     7 This measure reflects the respondent's living arrange-

     ments at the survey date, the only time for which we have

     information about this variable. Living arrangements for

     young adults have never been measured-to our

     knowledge-at less than one-year intervals. We assume

     relatively little change between survey dates in family vs.

     non-family living. We weighted survey years to reflect

     the actual amount of time between surveys.

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    6/15

     ONAMLYLVNG545

     identify those respondents who reside with parents

     and those who live away at the survey date.8

     We have also included as controls other

     variables that are likely to affect the family- and

     sex-role-related plans and attitudes of young

     adults. These include family background character-

     istics (socioeconomic status and family structure)

     measured at the initial survey, and current

     socioeconomic and family characteristics, mea-

     sured in the same year as the dependent variable.

     Table 1 presents definitions, means, and standard

     deviations of all independent and dependent

     variables.

     Our analytic strategies vary depending on how

     often and in which years measures of the

     dependent variables were obtained. One measure,

     work plans, was asked of the young women in

     every survey year. The first response to this

     question, asked when the young women were 17

     years old, provides a Time 1 baseline before any

     non-family living could occur from which we

     measure change. Plans for work at age 35 stated at

     later ages are the dependent variable. We arbi-

     trarily selected these later ages to be 20, 22, and 24

     years old, at periods of 3, 5, and 7 years after the

     first measure, based on questions answered for

     20-year-olds between 1971 and 1973; for 22-year-

     olds between 1973 and 1975; and for 24-year-olds

     between 1975 and 1978. This portion of the

     analysis provides the strongest test of our hypoth-

     eses, in that work plans prior to any experience of

     non-family living can be fully controlled. Compar-

     ing the effects of non-family living with and

     without controlling work plans at age 17 tests the

     importance of controlling them.

     The second set of dependent variables includes

     those measured only occasionally. These are

     expected and ideal fertility, attitudes toward

     mothers who work under certain specific condi-

     tions, and the more global set of questions on

     sex-role attitudes (Family and Jobs). For these

     dependent variables we modified our analytic

     strategy to use the later measure as the dependent

     variable, with the earlier measure included as an

     independent variable to provide an approximate

     baseline. Thus, in our analysis of fertility expecta-

     tions and ideal fertility, we predict 1973 expecta-

     tions and ideals (when the young women were

     aged 19 to 22) from 1971 expectations and ideals

     (when they were 17 to 20), plus characteristics and

     experiences of the individual. This latter analytic

     strategy does not always allow us to measure the

     attitude in question before any experience with

     non-family living. We test the importance for our

     conclusions of this limitation.

     For clarity, we have included a brief table that

     gives the ages of the young women and the survey

     years included for various outcome measures. The

     young men were ages 24 to 27 when we measured

     their attitudes toward mothers' work in 1976.

     Equations with continuous dependent variables,

     including both measures of fertility and all the

     attitude scales, are estimated with ordinary least

     squares. Equations for work plans, a dichotomy,

     use logistic regression, estimated with maximum-

     likelihood techniques (Goodman, 1976). To permit

     comparison of the effects of the independent

     variables across equations, we transformed the

     logit coefficients to yield measures analogous to

     unstandardized ordinarily least squares (OLS)

     regression coefficients (Hanushek and Jackson,

     1977). The transformed logit coefficients reflect

     the estimated effect of a unit change in the

     independent variable on the probability of planning

     to work at age 35, evaluated at the sample means.

     RESULTS

     We discuss the results of the analyses described

     above as they provide evidence on each of the

     hypotheses presented earlier. We begin with the

     first and most general hypothesis-that nonfamily

     living changes young adults' plans, expectations

     and attitudes about family and work roles away

     from a traditional family orientation. Before we

     discuss the individual models, however, we must

     note the strikingly consistent picture that emerges

     across the wide range of measures analyzed for

     females: living away from parents tends to alter

     young women's attitudes, expectations and plans

     toward non-familial roles. The more experience

     that young women have with independent living,

     the more they change their plans for later work,

     their family size expectations, their attitude toward

     work by mothers, and their family-related sex-role

     attitudes away from a traditional family orienta-

     tion. In contrast, no significant effects appear in

     the one analysis possible for young men, although

     the coefficients have the expected sign.

     Turning to the detailed results, we see in Table 3

     that after controlling for young women's plans for

     employment as reported when they were 17 years

     old, the higher the proportion of years lived away

     from parents between age 17 and the age in

     question (or first marriage, whichever came first),

     the greater the likelihood that the young women

     planned to work at age 35. This effect is large and

     significant for age 20, although it attenuates in size

     with increasing age, suggesting that the effects of

     non-family living may be short-lived. The coeffi-

     cients imply that a young woman who lived away

     all of the time between age 17 and 20 or her

     marriage (whichever came first) was about 10

     percentage points more likely to plan for later

     employment, net of her plans stated only 3 years

     8 For further details, on measures of living arrange-

     ments available from these data see Goldscheider and

     Waite (1985) and Goldscheider (1985).

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    7/15

     546 AMERCANSOCOLOGCAL REVEW

     Table 1. Description of Independent Variables Used in the Analysis*

     Means (standard

     deviations for

     continuous variables)

     Nonfamily living Proportion of years spent in non-family living between .199

     age 17 and first marriage or age in question (.309)

     Work 35 at 17 (D) Plans to work when age 35 as measured at age 17 .416

     Back(D 0=Whte andotherraces .299

     Non-Intact (D) Did not live wth 2 natural parents at age 14 .186

     Parents Education Average years of school competed 10397

     byprens 3225

     outhD Cenus regon396

     Size of Labor Scale ranging from1 (rural) to 8 3.431

     Force in Area (urbanzed areas of 3 mllion or more) (2.356)

     YearYearbecam age inquestion74.019

     (1.001)

     EducationYears of schoolingcomleted12581

     (2.189)

     Enrolled(D Enrolledinschool ful-tim .119

     Empoyed (D) Current full-time or part-time empoyment .573

      usbands total income In 1967 constant dollars (thousands) 2.504

     (3.664)

     Mrried (D) Currently married for the first time .466

     Divorced (D) Currently divorced, separated, or wdowed 062

     Remrred(D Currentlyremrred.014

     Kds (D Livngwthownchldren.368

     Emloyed*kds Interactionterm.132

     Standard deviation

     (for continuous

     Dependent Variables Means variables)

     Plans to hold a job at age 35

     (I=yes, 0=no):

     ag20551

     ag22554

     ag24652

     Attitude toward mothers working

     (3 to 15, high = more liberal):

     Girls

     196897332577

     1972106822400

     Boys

     1971106722926

     1976117852780

     Sex-role attitudes (Family)

     (-2.9 to 1.6, high = more liberal):

     972005875

     978025923

     Sex-role attitudes (Jobs)

     (-2.3 to 1.8, high = more liberal):

     1972 008711

     1978 003715

     Expected family size

     (in children, 0 to 13):

     197125091561

     197319491253

     Ideal family size

     (in children, 0 to 14):

     197129441251

     197326201130

     (D) Dummy variable.

     * The statistics for the independent variables come from the Young Women's sample at age 22.

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    8/15

     NONAMLYLVNG547

     Table 2. Ages and Years Measured for Young Women

     urveyYars Aes

     Work Plans Attitude toward

     at Ages: Mothers Wrkng Fertility Sex Roles

     ge n1968 20 22 24 1972 1973 1978

     14 1978

     5197319751977 18-21 19-22 2427

     16 1972

     17 1971 1973 1975

     No surveys were done in 1974 or 1976.

     earlier, compared with a woman who did not live

     away during this period.9

     The analysis of young women's attitudes toward

     mothers' working, shown in the first column of

     Table 4, also shows strong, statistically significant

     positive effects of non-family living on acceptance

     of employment of married mothers with young

     children. A young woman who lived away from

     home all of the possible years between age 17 and

     marriage, or Time 2, increased her score on this

     scale by about 1 point on a 13-point scale

     compared with someone who never lived away.

     We find similar patterns in our analysis of the total

     number of children expected by young women.

     Living away from home reduces fertility expecta-

     tions, with a decline for those who experience

     non-family living throughout the interval of about

     a quarter of a child, compared with those who

     either always lived with parents or moved straight

     from parents to husband. '0

     Ideal family size (Table 4, column 4), in

     contrast, shows no effect of non-family living.

     This result reinforces the theoretical and analytic

     differences between these measures. Ideals seem

     quite stable, as reflected by the somewhat larger

     effect of the prior measure for this than for birth

     expectations, and by the lack of effect from all the

     new experiences except school enrollment.

     The right-most columns of Table 4 present the

     two sex-role attitudes scores (Family and Jobs),

     measured in 1978. The results show that non-

     family living increases both young women's

     acceptance of non-family roles for women with

     family responsibilities (Family) and their view of

     the benefits of women's employment to their

     self-esteem and to their families' economic posi-

     tion (Jobs). When we analyze these two scales as

     measured in 1972, without a Time 1 control, we

     find an even stronger effect of non-family living on

     Family, but no effect on Jobs (results not shown).

     These results for 1978, an outcome measured at the

     latest survey year and at the oldest ages, suggest

     that the effects of non-family living are not

     necessarily short-lived, despite the attenuation

     apparent on active work plans; its influence

     persists up to a decade for fundamental attitudes

     about women's work and family roles.

     We performed several checks on the susceptibil-

     ity of our findings to changes in the measurement

     of non-family living. First, we re-estimated all the

     9 We were concerned with the possibility that the

     effects we observed for non-family living might be

     spurious, resulting from the unmeasured effects of work

     experience, since our models include only current

     employment status. We thought this unlikely, since

     Spitze (1978) found that holding a job rather than going

     to college during early adulthood caused a decrease in

     young women's taste for employment at age 35. We also

     knew that research on the relationship of employment and

     non-family living early in adulthood has shown them to

     be only weakly related. Many young adults leave home

     for non-work related reasons, and many others remain at

     home and work after finishing school. McElroy (1983)

     found a weak positive relationship between employment

     and non-family living for a sample of high school

     dropouts and Goldscheider and DaVanzo (1985) found a

     weak negative relationship over the seven years after

     completing high school in a sample that was restricted to

     those who reached twelfth grade.

     Nevertheless, reasoning that the problem should be

     most acute for employment-related attitudes, we re-

     estimated the models of plans for work at age 35 (shown

     in Table 3) three times, substituting various detailed

     measures of employment history for current employment.

     These measures were: (1) the proportion of years since

     age 17 that the respondent worked at all at any time

     during the year; (2) the proportion of years that the

     respondent worked at least 1000 hours (approximately half

     time); and (3) the proportion of years that the respondent

     worked at least 26 weeks (approximately a half-year).

     Each of these models showed exactly the same pattern of

     effects of non-family living on changes in work plans,

     with the same pattern of significance. In fact, the

     coefficients for our measure of non-family living were

     larger in each of the alternate specifications than in that

     with current employment. We conclude that the use of

     current employment does not mask effects of employ-

     ment history in the model that theoretically should have

     the strongest effects of employment on the outcome

     measure, and that our results with current employment

     are probably somewhat conservative. To keep the

     complexity of the reported results within reasonable

     bounds we report only the analyses with current

     employment, but the alternative specifications are

     available from the authors on request.

     10 As we noted earlier, we repeated all these analyses

     with this scale coded into a Guttman scale, coded from 0,

     for those who agreed that mothers should work under

     none of the stated conditions, to 3, for those who agreed

     that mothers could work under all the conditions. The

     substantive conclusions were identical.

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    9/15

     548 AMERCANSOCOLOGCAL REVEW

     Table 3. Effects of Non-family Living and Other Variables on Young Women's Plans to Work at Age 35

     Age

     xpantoryVrabes 20 22 24

     nfamyLvng0101** 0083 0042

     Wrk pans for 35 measured at age 17 0279** 0213** 0179**

    Back0138**0128**0021

     nntact 0075**-0051 0116**

     arens Education-0004 0006 -0003

     moyd0042 -00330158**

     nroed0284**0186**0132*

     Sze 00020010-0002

     Souh0053*00510057*

     Yar-0037**-00070021*

     rred-00030031 -0066

     vorced/Separated 0023 0049 -0069

     mrred-0031 0111 0065

     Husbands Total ncom -0006 -0011 0001

     Kd 0080*00570161**

     moyd*Kd0091 0068 -0079

     Educational Attainmnt 0037**0059**0039**

    227740928

     2 - log-likelihood ratio (df= 18) 240.43 113.93 103.01

     One-tailed significance:

     * p. 05

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    10/15

     NONAMLYLVNG549

     Second, we re-estimated these models using a

     specification of non-family living that allows for

     separate effects for each number of years of

     independent living, given years at risk. It also

     controls for number of years at risk, that is, the

     number of years between age 17 and the measure

     of the dependent variable. In no case did this

     fully saturated model significantly improve on the

     fit of the restricted model that we estimated, as

     measured by F-statistics for the regressions and

     Chi-squared statistics for the logits (all p > .08).

     Among the control variables, a result worth

     noting is that for NONINTACT, which also

     measures non-traditional living arrangements.

     NONINTACT shows a positive effect on plans for

     work at age 35 at two of the three ages presented in

     Table 3; in the equations for the other measures

     NONINTACT tends to operate in the same

     direction as non-family living. Where it has any

     effect, it decreases family orientation, suggesting

     that living in a non-traditional family during

     adolescence moves young women away from plans

     for family roles later in life. This finding is

     consistent with our proposition that experiences in

     non-family living during young adulthood erode an

     orientation toward traditional family roles.

     We also find that education is less associated

     with family-oriented attitudes and plans, as is

     current employment, but we see effects of marital

     status only for sex-role attitudes and expected

     family size. Not surprisingly, women who experi-

     ence marital disruption have decreased their

     expected number of children. Married women and

     working mothers hold more liberal views on the

     Jobs measure, whereas divorced and remarried

     women are more liberal on the Family scale. Black

     women seem to hold more positive attitudes

     toward work, however this is measured, and to

     have a somewhat larger expected and ideal family

     size, but do not differ from whites on family-

     related sex-role attitudes.

     Our second proposition states that non-family

     living not only affects personal plans and expecta-

     tions, but also influences global ideals, although

     less strongly, since individuals are likely to use the

     extenuating circumstances in their particular case

     to justify deviations from the ideal that they hold,

     at least for a while. The results presented in Tables

     3 and 4 offer mixed support for this reasoning. We

     find no significant effect of non-family living on

     ideal family size. However, we see significant

     effects of living away on attitudes toward mothers'

     working and both the Family and Jobs dimensions

     of the Sex-role attitudes measure. All these

     represent global, rather than personal views. We

     conclude that non-family living affects some but

     not all dimensions of global attitudes.

     Our third hypothesis deals with sex differences

     in the effects of independent living. The NLS data

     contain very few measures of young men's

     attitudes toward family life or appropriate roles for

     men and women. The one measure available (for

     1971 and 1976) corresponds exactly to one of the

     scales analyzed for young women: attitude toward

     mothers' working. This is shown in the second

     column of Table 4. We see no significant effect of

     non-family living on men's views, in contrast to

     those we found for women. Note, however, that

     the coefficient for young men has the expected sign

     and is nearly significant at the p

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    11/15

     550AMERCANSOCOLOGCAL REVEW

     Table 5. Transformed Logit Coefficients for Models of Young Women's Plans, Attitudes and Expectations

     (Controlling for All Variables in Table 2)

     Plans for Work at Age 35

      202224

     Model 1

     nfamyLvng0101** 0083 0042

     Model 2

     oege * awy0173** 0215** 0148**

     oege * hom 0118** 0107** 0046

     oege other awy0161* 0131 0139*

     ncoege awy0000 0007 -0014

     Model 3

     oege * awy0140** 0078 0026

     oege * hom 0088** 0003 -0047

     oege other awy0141 0052 0079

     ncoege awy0002 0023 0003

     Educational attainmnt 0029**0056**0041*

     titude towrd Sex-roe

     Mothers Wrkng Expected Ideal Attitudes

     Girs Boys Famly Size Famly Sizes Famly Jobs

     1972 1976 197319731978 1978

     Model 1

     Nonfamly Living 0979** 0261 -.225** -.066 0161** 0112*

     Model 2

     College awy0328*0400*0026 0036 0241**0027

     College hom -0145 0016 0038 0076 0115*-0061

     College other away -0382 0230 -0307* -0282* 0382** 0099

     Non-college away 0.551** -0.077 -0.199** -0.083 0.055 0.043

     Model 3

     College awy0204 -0342 -00090027 0166**0071

     College hom -0235 -0567*0012 00690061 -0030

     College other away -0427 -0308 -0322* -0285* 0344** 0121

     Non-college away 0581** -0089 - 0193** -0081 0067 0036

     Educational attainment 0094* 0204** 0020 0005 0024* -0014

     models, of a measure of educational attainment.

     The two sets of analyses point to the same

     conclusion: young women who live away from

     home while attending college are more likely to

     change their work plans than those who attend

     college while living at home. These women, in

     turn, are significantly more likely to change their

     work plans toward employment than those who

     remain at home and do not go to college. Those

     who live at home while in college, but live

     independently at some other time, also exhibit an

     increased preference for working at age 35,

     although the effect is not significant. Those who

     live away from home without attending college

     show no difference from the omitted category.

     Adding educational attainment to the model

     controls for the effect of years of school completed

     by the age in question. This differentiates between

     young women who attended college only briefly

     and those who completed more years of higher

     education. This control attenuates but does not

     eliminate the effects of the joint events. The

     coefficients suggest that living independently

     reinforces the lessons learned at college, pointing

     to the socializing effect of time spent with other

     students outside the classroom, away from parental

     control and influence.

     The other panels of Table 5 show the joint

     measures of college-going and non-family living

     described above for the other outcome measures.

     These coefficients suggest that both college

     attendance and non-family living affect the magni-

     tude of attitude change, whether or not these occur

     together. These patterns are not always fully

     consistent; in general, however, both those who

     lived at home while attending college and then

     lived away (a relatively small group), and those

     who lived away at college showed more change

     than others on the Family factor analysis scores.

     As before, adding educational attainment attenu-

     ates but does not eliminate these relationships.

     The final hypothesis is related to the importance

     of including a Time 1 control measuring a given

     attitude prior to any experience of non-family

     living. One could argue that the relationship we

     observe between attitudes and plans and non-

     family living arises because those who grew up

     with the most non-familial views may be more

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    12/15

     ONAMLYLVNG55

     Table 6. Effects of Non-Family Living on Attitudes and Expectations About Family Roles at Time 2 With

     and Without the Inclusion of a Baseline Measure

     Work Plans for Age 35

     ge 20 Age 22 Age 24

    Wthout Tm I 110** 120 068

    WthTm 1 101** 083 042

     Attitudes toward Famly Size Sex-Role Attitudes

     Mothers' Working Expected Ideal Family Jobs

     Wthout Tm 1 .995**-.211 - .084 .192**.114

     WthTm 1 .979**-.225**-.066 .161**.112*

     likely than others to live away from home. This

     potential problem arises for all analyses where the

     initial attitude measure may have occurred after

     some experience with non-family living, as is the

     case with sex-role scores. If so, the relationship

     between non-family living and non-familial atti-

     tudes that we observe could be spurious and only

     reflect this relationship with the omitted variable.

     Our controls for early attitudes, measured prior to

     any non-family living, eliminate this possibility for

     those outcomes for which we have this baseline;

     these are work plans for age 35 and attitudes

     toward mothers' working for young women. Both

     of these models show the hypothesized effect,

     providing strong tests of our central proposition.

     These models also allow us to determine the extent

     of the strength of the association between non-

     family living and early attitudes, and hence, the

     likely extent of specification bias in the analyses

     without Time 1 controls.

     To test our reasoning, we re-estimated all the

     young women's models in Tables 3 and 4,

     eliminating the initial measure of the dependent

     variable. The resulting coefficients for non-family

     living are presented in Table 6. Comparison with

     the coefficients for non-family living in Tables 3

     and 4 shows that adding or deleting the initial

     measure makes little difference in the conclusions;

     generally the results for non-family living without

     controls for the initial measure are slightly stronger

     than those with controls, but these differences are

     quite small. In only one case, expected family size,

     does adding or deleting the initial measure change

     the significance of non-family living. Given the

     strong relationships between the initial measure

     and the later one shown in Tables 3 and 4, it is

     clear that including the initial measure improves

     the overall fit of the model; however, it does not

     eliminate the effects of non-family living.

     As a second test of this hypothesis, regression

     models were estimated with the non-family living

     measure at Time 2 as the dependent variable and

     the Time 1 attitudes or work plans variable

     included as an explanatory variable. All other

     explanatory variables were retained as measured at

     Time 2, which allowed us to isolate the effect the

     attitudinal measure alone has on the propensity to

     live away from home.'3 These results, in Table 7,

     show that only the initial work plans variable has a

     significant effect on the subsequent propensity for

     non-family living-and only for age 22 or later.

     The other attitude scales had no effect on

     non-family living. These results corroborate those

     in Table 6 that show that the percentage change in

     the non-family living coefficient is relatively

     small, except for the models for work plans at age

     22 and age 24, where the coefficients increase 45

     to 62 percent when the Time 1 baseline measure is

     omitted.

     These results provide strong support for our

     hypothesis that living away during young adult-

     hood is not highly selective with respect to initial

     attitudes regarding values or belief systems. Those

     with the least family-oriented views to begin with

     are not substantially more or less likely to live

     away from their parents during the transition to

     adulthood, so that these and other analysis of the

     effects of non-family living on attitudes and

     behavior should not be unduly concerned about

     issues of selectivity. However, there is indication

     that some selectivity may exist with respect to

     expressions of intentions or plans, if these plans

     may produce actions that affect living arrange-

     ments. In our analysis, specific work plans for age

     35 might well be expected to interact with living

     arrangements more than judgments regarding

     appropriate familial sex roles. In this case, it is

     prudent to include a baseline measure, which we

     have done in this analysis.

     DISCUSSION

     We argued earlier in this paper that living away

     from home prior to marriage changes young adults'

     attitudes, expectations, and plans away from

     13 For example, measuring marital status at age 20 in

     the work plans model adjusts for the effect marriage had

     on the proportion of years lived away from home between

     age 17 and 20. If explanatory variables were measured at

     Time 1 when all women were single, the effects of the

     attitudinal measure would be confounded with the effects

     of marriage on the non-family living variable.

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    13/15

     52 AMRCANSOCOLOGCA REVEW

     Table 7. Effects of Time 1 Measure on Non-Family

     Living Variable at Time 2

      me 1 Masure Coefficient Tme 2

     Wrk Plans-age 17 .017 age 20

     Wrk Plans-age 17 .052** age 22

     Wrk Plans-age 17 .090** age 24

     Attitudes, Mothers

     Workng 1968 0011972

     Sex-role Attitudes,

     Famly 1972 008 1978

     Sex-role Attitudes,

     Jobs, 1972 001 1978

     family and toward individual concerns. Our results

     support this hypothesis consistently, indicating that

     young women who lived independently became

     more likely to plan for employment, lowered their

     expected family size, became more accepting of

     employment of mothers, and became more non-

     traditional on sex roles in the family than those

     who lived with their parents. We found much

     weaker effects of non-family living in the tests we

     could perform for young men. Our results suggest

     that personal plans and expectations respond in the

     expected way to experiences in non-family living,

     but that some dimensions of ideals and global

     attitudes change as a result of this experience, as

     well. We also find evidence that living away from

     home reinforces the effects of attending college on

     young women's attitudes, expectations and plans.

     Given these results, it is important to learn more

     about why non-family living should have such an

     effect on attitudes about new family formation and

     the kinds of families likely to be formed. We

     suggest the following possibilities. First, living

     away from home lessens parental control over their

     children's activities and may weaken the link

     between parents' values, attitudes, and behavior

     and those of their children. Young adults in their

     own apartment are freed from parental curfews and

     supervision of their friends and behavior, can

     manage their own household in the way that they

     choose, and may have a sexual freedom impossible

     in their parents' house. Second, experience in

     independent non-family living may equip young

     adults with new social and domestic skills (such as

     housekeeping, maintenance of a dwelling unit or

     car, household finance, health care, and entertain-

     ing). Living away from home may give both young

     men and women the self-confidence that they can

     get along without a family and may enable them to

     acquire the skills that they need to maintain their

     independence as long as they wish. Third,

     non-family living potentially exposes young adults

     to a wider variety of experiences and influences

     than those encountered while they live with their

     parents. These may cause individuals to change

     their plans and expectations for themselves, and

     their views of appropriate behavior for men and

     women, in general. We do not test any of these

     possible mechanisms directly, but our results

     suggest that such tests might prove a fruitful

     avenue for further research.

     Previous research on the development and

     revision of sex-role attitudes has focused on

     education, employment (Spitze, 1978; Dambrot et

     al., 1983; Morgan and Walker, 1983), marriage,

     becoming a parent, and divorce (Spitze and Waite,

     1981). We find that education and employment are

     associated with less family-oriented attitudes and

     plans, but we see effects of current marital status

     primarily for sex-role attitudes. Married women

     hold less traditional views on the economic

     benefits to the family of women's employment,

     and divorced women and working mothers are

     more liberal on the appropriate division of roles

     within the family. Non-family living, by contrast,

     seems to have broader effects than marital status,

     with coefficients sometimes larger than those of

     employment and education. Non-family living is a

     previously ignored but potentially important expe-

     rience in the transition to adulthood, especially as

     it influences young adults' attitudes and plans for

     their own lives.

     One particularly significant result of this re-

     search is the finding that the omission of Time 1

     controls has little effect on measuring the impact of

     non-family living on later orientations toward work

     and family life. This provides increased confidence

     that results obtained previously that show the

     impact of non-family living on the likelihood of

     marriage were not biased by the omission of some

     underlying anti-family attitude among those who

     both live away from parents early in adulthood and

     are also unlikely to marry. It also opens the way to

     research on other possible sequelae of non-family

     residence in early adulthood, such as marital

     dissolution, or less traditional assignments of tasks

     within marriage, allowing greater range in the

     analysis of the forces leading to change in marriage

     and the family in the late twentieth century.

     These changes in the nature of the family, many

     of which we discussed earlier, have altered some

     of the bases on which traditional, gender-based

     divisions of labor rested. These divisions may have

     served some important functions under earlier

     conditions (Marwell, 1975). But as Marini (1984)

     has argued, norms about the transition to adulthood

     tend to reflect behavior, as well as affect it, and

     thus may change as behavior changes. The shifts in

     individual attitudes, plans, and expectations that

     our findings link with experiences with non-family

     living may be a first step in the process of

     alteration of norms. Thus, our finding that

     non-family living influenced all our measures of

     behavioral plans and evaluations, but did not affect

     some indicators of global ideals, should not be

     interpreted as showing that non-family living may

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    14/15

     NONAMLYLVNG553

     only have a transient effect, one with impacts only

     on those experiencing it. For if Marini is correct,

     the increase in non-family living, by changing

     plans relating to male and female roles in the

     family, may presage larger shifts than we have

     already observed in norms about family life.

     APPENDIX

     Attitudes towards mothers' working

      Now I'd like you to think about a family where there is

     a mother, a father who works full-time, and several

     children under school age. A trusted relative who can

     care for the children lives nearby. In this family situation,

     how do you feel about the mother taking a full-time job

     outside the home-

     a. If it is absolutely necessary to make ends meet?

     b. If she wants to and her husband agrees?

     c. If she prefers to work, but her husband doesn't

     particularly like it?

     Sex-role attitudes scores #1 and #2, 1972

     a. Modern conveniences permit a wife to work

     without neglecting her family.

     b. A woman's place is in the home, not in the office

     or shop.

     c. A job provides a wife with interesting outside

     contacts.

     d. wife who carries out her full family responsibilities

     doesn't have time for outside employment.

     e. A working wife feels more useful than one who

     doesn't hold a job.

     f. The employment of wives leads to more juvenile

     delinquency.

     g. Working wives help to raise the general standard of

     living.

     h. Working wives lose interest in their home and

     families.

     i. Employment of both parents is necessary to keep up

     with the high cost of living.

     Sex-role attitudes scores #1 and #2, 1978

     The 1978 series includes items a, b, d, e, f, and i. It also

     includes the questions listed below.

     j. It is much better for everyone concerned if the man

     is the achiever outside the home and the woman

     takes care of the home and family.

     k. Men should share the work around the house with

     women, such as doing dishes, cleaning, and so

     forth.

     1. A working mother can establish just as warm and

     secure a relationship with her child as a woman who

     doesn't work.

     m. Women are much happier if they stay at home and

     take care of their children.

     n. A woman should not let bearing and rearing

     children stand in the way of a career if she wants it.

     Items a and c had no relationship with either dimension in

     the 1972 scales, and were dropped; items a and k were

     deleted from the 1978 scores for the same reason. Items

     e, g and i loaded highly on score #2 (jobs), and the

     remaining items loaded highly on score #1 (family).

     REFERENCES

     Baldwin, Wendy and Virginia S. Cain. 1980. The

     Children of Teenage Parents. Family Planning

     Perspectives 12:34-43.

     Blake, Judith. 1972. Coercive Pronatalism and Ameri-

     can Population Policy. Pp. 84-108 in Aspects of

     Population Growth Policy, edited by Robert Parke, Jr.

     and Charles Westoff. Washington, D.C.: The Commis-

     sion on Population Growth and the American Future

     Research Reports, U.S. Government Printing Office,

     Volume 6.

     Davis, Kingsley. 1982. Changes in Marriage since

     World War II. Paper presented at the conference on

      Contemporary Marriage: Comparative Perspectives

     on a Changing Institution. Center for Advanced

     Study in the Behavioral Sciences, Stanford, California.

     Dambrot, Faye H., Mary E. Papp, and Cheryl Whitmore.

     1983. The Sex-Role Attitudes of Three Generations

     of Women. Personality and Social Psychology

     Bulletin 10:469-73.

     Espenshade, Thomas J. 1985. Marriage Trends in

     America: Estimates, Implications, and Underlying

     Causes. Population and Development Review

     11:193-245.

     Frey, William H. and Frances Kobrin. 1982. Changing

     Families and Changing Mobility: Their Impact on the

     Central City. Demography 19:261-77.

     Goldscheider, Frances Kobrin. 1985. Crossvalidating

     Data from National Longitudinal Surveys Measuring

     the Transition to Adulthood N:9999:NICHD. Santa

     Monica, CA: The Rand Corporation.

     Goldscheider, Frances Kobrin and Julie DaVanzo. 1985.

      Living Arrangements and the Transition to Adult-

     hood. Demography 22:545-63.

     Goldscheider, Frances Kobrin and Celine LeBourdais.

     1986. The Decline in Age at Leaving Home:

     1920-1979. Sociology and Social Research 70:99-102.

     Goldscheider, Frances Kobrin and Linda J. Waite. 1985.

      Effects of Nestleaving Patterns on the Transition to

     Marriage. Paper presented at the 1985 annual

     meetings of the Population Association of America,

     Boston.

     Goodman, Leo A. 1976. The Relationships Between

     Modified and Usual Multiple-Regression Approaches

     to the Analysis of Dichotomous Variables. Pp.

     83-110 in Sociological Methodology, edited by David

     Heise.

     Gustavus, Susan 0. and Charles B. Nam. 1970. The

     Formation and Stability of Ideal Family Size Among

     Young People. Demography 7:43-52.

     Hanushek, Erik A. and John E. Jackson. 1977. Statistical

     Methods for Social Scientist. New York: Academic.

     Hoffman, Lois W. and Frederick Wyatt. 1960. Social

     Change and Motivations for Having Larger Families:

     Some Theoretical Considerations. Merrill-Palmer

     Quarterly 6:235-44.

     Kobrin, Fances E. and Linda J. Waite. 1984. Effects of

     Family Stability on the Transition to Marriage.

    Journal of Marriage and the Family 46:807-16.

     Marini, Margaret Mooney. 1984. Age and Sequencing

     Norms in the Transition to Adulthood. Social Forces

     63:229-44.

     Marwell, Gerald. 1975. Why Ascription? Parts of a

     More or Less Formal Theory of Functions and

     Dysfunctions of Sex Roles. American Sociological

     Review 40:445-55.

     Mason, Karen 0. 1974. Women's Labor Force Partici-

    This content downloaded from 195.229.156.148 on Fri, 15 Apr 2016 17:54:17 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

  • 8/17/2019 Nonfamily Living and the Erosion of Traditional Family Orientations Among Young Adults

    15/15

     554 AMERCANSOCIOLOGCAL REVEW

     nation and Fertility. Research Triangle Park, NC:

     Research Triangle Institute.

     Mason, Karen Oppenheim, and Larry L. Bumpass.

      U.S. Women's Sex-Role Ideology. American

     Journal of Sociology 80:1212-19.

     Mason, Karen Oppenheim, John L. Czajka, and Sara

     Arber. 1976. Change in U.S. Women's Sex-Role

     Attitudes, 1964-1974. American Sociological Re-

     view 41:573-96.

     McElroy, M. 1983. The Joint Determination of

     Household Membership and Market Work: The Case

     of Young Men. Discussion Paper 83-1, Economics

     Research Center, NORC, Chicago.

     Modell, John. 1979. The Marriage Transition: Norma-

     tive Aspects of Change Since World War II. Paper

     presented at the meetings of the Population Association

     of America, Philadelphia.

     Morgan, Carolyn Stout, and Alexis J. Walker. 1983.

      Predicting Sex Role Attitudes. Social Psychology

     Quarterly 46:148-51.

     Pope, Hallowell, and Charles W. Mueller. 1976. The

     Intergenerational Transmission of Marital Instability:

     Comparisons by Race and Sex. Journal of Social

     Issues 32:49-66.

     Rindfuss, Ronald R. and Larry Bumpass. 1978. Age

     and the Sociology of Fertility: How Old is Too Old?

    Pp. 43-56 in Social Demography, edited by Karl

     Taeuber, Larry Bumpass and James Sweet. New York:

     Academic.

     Rubin, Lillian Breslow. 1976. Worlds of Pain: Life in the

     Working-Class Family. New York: Basic Books.

     Ryder, Norman and Charles F. Westoff. 1971. Reproduc-

     tion in the United States, 1965. Princeton, NJ

     Princeton University Press.

     Scanzoni, John H. 1975. Sex Roles, Life Styles and

     Childbearing. New York: Free Press.

     Spitze, Glenna D. 1978. Role Experiences of Young

     Women: A Longitudinal Test of the Role Hiatus

     Hypothesis. Journal of Marriage and the Family

     40:471-80.

     Spitze, Glenna D. and Linda J. Waite. 1981. Young

     Women's Preferences for Market Work: Resources to

     Marital Events. Research in Population Economics

     3: 147-66.

     Terhune, Kenneth W. 1974. A Review of the Actual and

     Expected Consequences of Family Size. Washington

     D.C.: U.S. Government Printing Office.

     Thornton, Arland, and Deborah Freedman. 1982.

      Changing Attitudes Toward Marriage and Single

     Life. Family Planning Perspectives 14:297-303.

     U.S. Bureau of the Census. 1980. Household and

     Family Characteristics: March 1980. Current Popu-

     lation Reports, Series P-20, No. 366, Washington,

     D.C., U.S. Government Printing Office.

     Waite, Linda J. and Ross M. Stolzenberg. 1976.

      Intended Childbearing and Labor Force Participation

     of Young Women: Insights from Nonrecursive Mod-

     els. American Sociological Review 41:235-52.

     Westoff, Charles F. 1978. Some Speculations on the

     Future of Marriage and the Family. Family Planning

     Perspectives 10:79-83.